You are on page 1of 15

See discussions, stats, and author profiles for this publication at: https://www.researchgate.

net/publication/336740535

Predicting intention to recycle on the basis of the theory of planned behaviour

Article  in  International Journal of Nonprofit and Voluntary Sector Marketing · October 2019


DOI: 10.1002/nvsm.1653

CITATIONS READS

15 1,197

11 authors, including:

Denni Arli Foluké Abigail Badejo


University of Tasmania Griffith University
107 PUBLICATIONS   2,427 CITATIONS    15 PUBLICATIONS   112 CITATIONS   

SEE PROFILE SEE PROFILE

Joan Carlini Cassandra France


Griffith University Griffith University
18 PUBLICATIONS   139 CITATIONS    6 PUBLICATIONS   402 CITATIONS   

SEE PROFILE SEE PROFILE

Some of the authors of this publication are also working on these related projects:

Call for Book Chapters Transforming Relationship Marketing View project

Call for Book Chapters: Developing Digital Marketing: Relationship Perspectives View project

All content following this page was uploaded by Denni Arli on 11 June 2020.

The user has requested enhancement of the downloaded file.


Received: 22 May 2019 Revised: 5 August 2019 Accepted: 15 August 2019
DOI: 10.1002/nvsm.1653

RESEARCH ARTICLE

Predicting intention to recycle on the basis of the theory of


planned behaviour

Denni Arli1 | Abi Badejo2 | Joan Carlini2 | Cassandra France3 |


Charles Jebarajakirthy2 | Kathy Knox2 | Robin Pentecost2 | Helen Perkins2 |
Park Thaichon2 | Tapan Sarker4 | Owen Wright2

1
Department of Marketing, Labovitz School of
Business and Economics, University of Governments worldwide have launched various schemes to promote recycling by
Minnesota Duluth, Duluth, Minnesota
individuals, from legislation to voluntary and mandatory policies, waste charging,
2
Department of Marketing, Griffith Business
School, Griffith University, Nathan, kerbside collection, waste separation bins, and promotional campaigns. Much remains
Queensland, Australia to be done, however, in terms of understanding the psychological relationships
3
UQ Business School, University of
among consumers' attitudes, intentions, and behaviours when it comes to recycling.
Queensland, Brisbane, Queensland, Australia
4
Department of Business Strategy and
This study was designed to examine recycling intention through the lens of the the-
Innovation, Griffith Business School, Griffith ory of planned behaviour (TPB). Using online survey, we recruited participants (n =
University, Nathan, Queensland, Australia
827) through an online survey platform (mturk.com). The results show that TPB can
Correspondence predict consumers' intention to recycle. It was further found, however, that attitude
Denni Arli, Department of Marketing, Labovitz
School of Business and Economics, University
towards recycling did not predict intention to recycle. The findings presented here
of Minnesota Duluth, Duluth, MN 55812. have significant implications for policymakers and practitioners who are interested in
Email: darli@d.umn.edu
inculcating recycling intention and behaviours in members of the public. This study
Funding information extends the TPB in the context of recycling. There is a need to examine the theories'
Sustainability Small Grants Griffith Centre for
Sustainable Enterprise (GCSE)
explanatory power in different research settings and context. Moreover, regulations
and policies on recycling continue to evolve. For example, recently in 2018, Australia
is banning plastic bag. Consequently, the new policy will affect people's perception
towards recycling. Hence, continued research on recycling is needed.

1 | I N T RO D UC T I O N the present century (Varotto & Spagnolli, 2017). As a consequence of


all of this waste, environmental issues have now become a major con-
Over the past three decades, waste management has become a critical cern. In many parts of the world, materialistic consumption denotes
issue for governments (both nationally, from a policy development “the good life,” “freedom,” and “progress” and is associated with social
perspective, and locally, through the implementation of waste interaction, social inclusion, and self-expression (Bentley, Fien, & Neil,
recycling measures), the private sector (through efforts to incorporate 2004; Hume, 2010). These lifestyle decisions have been identified as
“green” practices), and the general public (as the largest source of one of the drivers of climate change (Roy & Pal, 2009). Urban waste
waste). Although attitudes towards the environment (and recycling) has become a critical environmental concern, with the reduction of
have improved over this time (e.g., Thomas & Sharp, 2013), efforts to pollution through recycling being key to the preservation of earth's
investigate and thereby improve recycling behaviour are ongoing natural resources (Varotto & Spagnolli, 2017).
(Organisation for Economic Co-operation and Development, 2014). As environmental issues become more important to individuals
As the population has increased, so has consumerism and with it and to society in general, interest in recycling as a form of waste man-
waste, which is now calculated at around 2 billion tons per year glob- agement has increased as part of the effort to reduce human impact
ally and is only expected to increase as the population grows through on the environment (Chen & Tung, 2009; Strong, 1998). Governments

Int J Nonprofit Volunt Sect Mark. 2019;e1653. wileyonlinelibrary.com/journal/nvsm © 2019 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. 1 of 14
https://doi.org/10.1002/nvsm.1653
2 of 14 ARLI ET AL.

around the world have launched various schemes to promote 2000), though there are of course many other approaches to the
recycling practices (Wilson, Rodic, Scheinberg, Velis, & Alabaster, study of human behaviour (Chan & Bishop, 2013), such as the norm
2012), ranging from mandatory recycling policies, waste charging activation model (Schwartz, 1973, 1977) and value–belief norm the-
schemes, kerbside recycling collection, community waste separation ory (Stern, 2000). Thus, TPB has been used to explain phenomena
bins, to promotional campaigns for recycling practices (De Jaeger, such as public transport use (Bamberg & Schmidt, 2003) and general
Eyckmans, Rogge, & Van Puyenbroeck, 2011; Goddard, 1995; Phau & conservation behaviour (Kaiser & Gutscher, 2003) and has been
Ong, 2007; Tencati, Pogutz, Moda, Brambilla, & Cacia, 2016; Wan, shown to predict behaviour better than demographics alone (Oreg &
Qiping Shen, & Choi, 2017; Wilson, 1996). Much remains to be done, Katz-Gerro, 2006; Trumbo & O'Keefe, 2001).
however, in terms of understanding the psychological relationships According to TPB, the primary antecedent of an individual's
among consumers' attitudes, intentions, and behaviours when it behaviour is his or her intention. Intention in this respect is deter-
comes to recycling (Varotto & Spagnolli, 2017; Wan et al., 2017). This mined by three constructs: first, individuals' overall attitude towards
study built on previous environmental research through the inclusion the behaviour; second, their subjective norms, which refer to the
of self-efficacy (intrinsic motivation) and situational factors (extrinsic social pressure felt in relation to performing the behaviour; and third,
motivation) as predictors of perceived behavioural control (PBC). The PBC, which refers to the individual's perception of the difficulty of
argument is that intrinsic and extrinsic motivations lead to different performing the behaviour or, to put it another way, the extent to
outcomes (Deci & Ryan, 2008) and, thus, are likely to have different which an individual believes that he or she has self-efficacy to per-
effects on PBC. Intrinsically motivated behaviours are produced and form the behaviour (Ajzen, 1985, 1991).
maintained because enjoyment of them reinforces them (Lavigne et Over time, further constructs have been added to the TPB frame-
al., 2009). Extrinsic motivation includes both external regulation, work. Thus, Chan and Bishop (2013), in a study of recycling, argued
which affects an individual's behaviour through contingencies of for the inclusion of moral norms, defined as “the reflection of a per-
reward or punishment, and self-regulation, which occurs when the sonal value system in a given situation” by Klockner (2013, p. 1030).
external regulation has been partially internalized and is energized by These researchers, reacting to suggestions that discriminant validity
such factors such as desire for approval, avoidance of shame, contin- was lacking for attitudes within the context of conservation research,
gent self-esteem, and ego-involvement (Deci & Ryan, 2008; Ryan & found that replacing attitudes with moral norms increased the predic-
Deci, 2017). Thus, one objective of this study was to investigate ante- tive validity of both intentions and actual behaviour. So also a meta-
cedents of consumers' intention to recycle, specifically through the analysis by Bamberg and Möser (2007) indicated that as much as 52%
lens of theory of planned behaviour (TPB; Ajzen, 1985). The study has of the variance in intentions could be accounted for through the
several theoretical and managerial contributions. This study extends incorporation of personal moral norms into the TPB framework. There
the TPB in the context of recycling. Despite critics on TPB (e.g., has in fact been some disputes regarding whether moral norms should
Sniehotta et al. 2014), Ajzen (2015) suggested that TPB is still relevant replace or rather should be included alongside attitudes in the TPB
and needed in order to understand human behaviour. There is a need model (Wan et al., 2017). Thus, Chan and Bishop (2013), in testing the
to examine the theories' explanatory power in different research set- new construct, found the removal of attitudes to improve its predic-
tings and context (Armitage & Conner, 2001; Black, 2010; Paul, Modi, tive power, whereas Botetzagias et al. (2015) rejected the substitution
& Patel, 2016). Moreover, regulations and policies on recycling con- of the latter term with the former on the grounds that both are the
tinue to evolve. For example, recently in 2018, Australia is banning same from a psychological perspective. As a consensus has yet to
plastic bag (CNN 2018). Consequently, the new policy will affect peo- emerge on the issue (Wan et al., 2017), the decision was accordingly
ple's perception towards recycling. Hence, continued research on made to test both constructs in this study.
recycling is needed.
The following section establishes the general background for
2.2 | Factors implicated in recycling behaviours
recycling behaviour, with contemporary consumerism serving as a
frame of reference. Next, we develop the theoretical framework used Early investigations of recycling behaviour identified attributes of
for the investigation. The data are then presented, followed by the “recyclers” (e.g., Coggins, 1994; Thieme, Royne, Jha, Levy, & Barnes
main findings. The study concludes with a discussion of policy implica- McEntee, 2015; Vining & Ebreo, 1990), including shared characteris-
tions and avenues for future research. tics unique to consumers in this context (Schultz, Oskamp, & Mainieri,
1995). In light of these findings, recycling behaviours can be associ-
ated with three broad factors. The first involves environmental values,
2 | REVI EW O F T H E L IT ERAT URE
which is to say underlying attitudes towards the environment; thus,
recycling behaviours can be driven by recognition of the inherent
2.1 | Theory of planned behaviour
value of the environment (Vining & Ebreo, 1992). Second, situational
One method for promoting proenvironmental behaviour has been to or contextual factors promote or discourage recycling behaviours
draw on social psychological theories. The TPB (Ajzen, 1985, 1991; (Derksen & Gartrell, 1993; Guagnano, Stern, & Dietz, 1995); such fac-
Greaves, Zibarras, & Stride, 2013) has proved particularly useful in this tors can be socio-demographic (Berger, 1997), knowledge based
respect (Botetzagias, Dima, & Malesios, 2015; Shaw, Shiu, & Clarke, (Schahn & Holzer, 1990), or experience based (Daneshvary,
ARLI ET AL. 3 of 14

Daneshvary, & Schwer, 1998). The third type of factors associated intentions predict their likelihood to engage in a behaviour (Ajzen &
with environmentally conscious behaviours is psychological in nature, Fishbein, 2000). TPB has capacity to help explain a variety of
involving individuals' unique emotional attributes, such as altruism behaviours, including, in one study, proenvironmental behaviours at
(Hopper & Nielsen, 1991), intrinsic and extrinsic motivations (De hotels (Han & Kim, 2010; see also Barr et al., 2001; Tonglet et
Young, 1986), perceptions of environmental hazards (Baldassare & al., 2004).
Katz, 1992), beliefs regarding outcomes (Boldero, 1995; Hopper & Also as mentioned, attitudes towards recycling represent a key
Nielsen, 1991), subjective norms or social influence (Chan, 1998), factor, ranging from the extreme scepticism of individuals who only
logistics and self-efficacy (Steel, 1996), and the notion of environmen- perform the desired behaviours under coercion to the complete
tal “citizenship” (Selman, 1996). For this research, we focused on spe- acceptance of individuals who regularly and willingly perform the
cific aspects of these factors in order to identify predictors of desired behaviours (Guagnano et al., 1995). Positive consequences
recycling behaviours. of recycling appear to promote recycling behaviours, and external
conditions can affect the relationship between attitudes and behav-
iours (cf. Steg, Bolderdijk, Keizer, & Perlaviciute, 2014). Based on
3 | D E V E L O P M E N T OF HY P O T H E S E S
the theory, one of the main factors influencing people's attitude is
awareness. Studies found that awareness towards recycling leads
3.1 | Attitude towards recycling ! intention to
to various behaviour such as positive attitude towards recycling
recycle
(Ramayah, Lee, & Lim, 2012); overcoming barriers towards the collec-
Over the past few decades, solid waste has become a major issue tion and utilization of recovered paper (Miranda & Blanco, 2010);
for industrialized countries (Wan et al., 2016). Among the many fac- increasing the knowledge on the importance and benefits of
tors shown to impact environmentally conscious behaviour, public recycling (Sidique, Joshi, & Lupi, 2010); and increasing consumers'
awareness of environmental problems has been shown, not surpris- intention to recycle e-waste (Dwivedy & Mittal, 2013; Tanskanen,
ingly, to be especially important. This being the case, much of the 2013). On the basis of these considerations, we formulated the fol-
public policy relating to recycling has focused on information cam- lowing hypothesis:
paigns (Barr, Gilg, & Ford, 2001). More specifically, because individ-
ual consumers and households can play a major role in waste H2 : Awareness of recycling significantly influences attitudes towards
reduction and recycling, campaigns have been designed to increase recycling.
individual awareness of waste issues and thereby to reinforce atti-
tudes that positively affect behaviours that impact the environment
(Barr et al., 2001).
3.3 | Subjective norms ! the intention to recycle
Informing consumers about the benefits of recycling is, then, an
important strategy in shaping their attitudes (Vicente & Reis, 2008). In a study of waste recycling in Hong Kong, Chan (1998) found that
Awareness of the difficulty of finding space for landfills, for example, subjective norms regarding the use of waste recycling receptacles cor-
can increase the mindfulness of households about waste reduction related positively with behavioural intention and that those who per-
and recycling. Numerous studies have shown that awareness of the ceived significant social pressure were more likely to use the
well-documented benefits of recycling—which include, along with receptacles. Thus, mass communication was found to be a major
reducing the need for landfills, conserving energy and natural source of influence in establishment of subjective norms. On the
resources and creating new jobs—is a significant predictor of positive other hand, Mannetti, Pierro, and Livi (2004) found that subjective
attitudes towards it (Tonglet, Phillips, & Read, 2004, Wan et al., 2014). norms were a weak predictor of the intention to recycle and attrib-
Naturally enough, the conviction that recycling provides a tangible uted this result to the specific weakness of agree–disagree measures.
benefit is associated with engaging in recycling behaviours (Blok, This finding was consistent with previous research, including a meta-
Wesselink, Studynka, & Kemp, 2015). On the basis of these consider- analytic review by Armitage and Conner (2001) of 185 studies that
ations, we formulated the following hypothesis: identified subjective norms as the weakest predictor in the TPB
model. On the other hand, Fornara, Carrus, Passafaro, and Bonnes
H1 : Attitudes towards recycling significantly influence the intention (2011) reported that various social norms make distinct contributions
to recycle. to such place-related behaviour as household waste recycling and that
descriptive (i.e., subjective and local) norms were powerful predictors
of the target proenvironmental behaviour in specific spatial–physical
settings. Then again, Park and Ha (2014), in developing a comprehen-
3.2 | Awareness of recycling ! attitudes towards
sive model of consumers' recycling intentions, found that subjective
recycling
norms did not directly influence recycling intention but that subjective
The TPB, introduced above, is one of the most widely used rational norms did so indirectly through the mediation of attitudes, personal
choice models for explaining individuals' decision-making processes norms, and PBC. These researchers' failure to find a direct impact
(Han, 2015). Simply put, from the perspective of TPB, individuals' does not necessarily mean that social consensus and social pressure
4 of 14 ARLI ET AL.

play no role in increasing consumers' recycling intention; rather, sub- not to be a significant independent predictor of intentions (p =
jective norms play an essential role in recycling intention by promot- .77)—though the lack of physical facilities was a barrier to household
ing favourable attitudes towards recycling, strong personal obligations recycling and, therefore, exerted a significant moderating effect on
to engage in it, and the perception that recycling is not difficult (Park the relationship between PBC and recycling intentions. Likewise, in
& Ha, 2014). On the basis of these considerations, we formulated the a cross-sectional survey conducted in Glasgow, Scotland, at a time
following hypothesis: when recycling facilities were relatively undeveloped, Knussen, Yule,
MacKenzie, and Wells (2004) found that PBC was moderately posi-
H3 : Subjective norms significantly influence the intention to recycle. tively correlated with intention to recycle (p = .44); thus, the PBC–
intention relationship was significantly stronger when facilities were
not perceived to be lacking. However, PBC has not always proved
to be a significant independent predictor of recycling intentions.
3.4 | Moral norms ! subjective norms
Thus, for example, in a study of a mature kerbside recycling scheme
Given the research indicating that subjective norms are weak predic- in the United Kingdom, the items used to measure PBC were tai-
tors of intentions in the context of TPB (e.g., Armitage & Conner, lored to the specific context, for example, “I have plenty of opportu-
2001), calls have been made for the incorporation of additional nor- nities to recycle” and “I know what items of household waste can
mative variables relevant to the predictive validity of TPB (e.g., be recycled” (Tonglet et al., 2004); again, PBC was not a significant
Manstead, 2000; Rivis & Sheeran, 2003), for instance, moral norms independent predictor of recycling intentions and recycling behav-
(again, Chan & Bishop, 2013). The influence of moral norms—also iour. On the basis of these considerations, we formulated the fol-
referred to as personal norms—has been shown to increase the lowing hypotheses:
variance in intentions over and above that afforded by other TPB
variables across a range of behavioural contexts (see Rivis, Sheeran, H5 : PBC significantly influences the intention to recycle.
& Armitage, 2009). Thus, for example, Matthies, Selge, and Klöckner
(2012), in a study of the influence of parents on their children's reuse H6 : PBC significantly influences recycling behaviour.
and recycling of paper, found both personal and subjective norms to
be relevant (cf. Bamberg & Möser, 2007; Hunecke, Blöbaum,
Matthies, & Höger, 2001). A study by Bamberg and Möser (2007)
3.6 | Self-efficacy ! perceived behaviour control
also pointed to moral norms as being responsible for a significant
part of the variance in proenvironmental behaviours. As moral norms A theoretical distinction has been made between self-efficacy (as
are activated when individuals become aware of the impact of defined by Bandura, 1977; 1982) and PBC (as defined by Ajzen,
their actions on others and accept personal responsibility for them 1991). In the integrated waste management model proposed by Tay-
(Schwartz, 1977), it was expected that moral norms would signifi- lor and Todd (1995), which was based on TPB, self-efficacy is a key
cantly influence subjective norms, as stated in the following determinant of waste management intentions and behaviour, exerting
hypothesis: an effect indirectly through its relationship with PBC. In these
scholars' study of household recycling behaviour in a city in Canada,
H4 : Moral norms significantly influence subjective norms. self-efficacy was a predictor of PBC within the integrated waste man-
agement model, and fit was tested using structural equation model-
ling. The path coefficient from self-efficacy to PBC was weak but
significant (p < .01), and that from PBC to behavioural intention was
3.5 | PBC ! intention to recycle
even stronger (p < .001). Thus, the empirical precedent in the context
PBC, as discussed, refers to an individual's perception of the diffi- of household recycling behaviour indicates support for the theoreti-
culty of performing a behaviour, which is to say the perception of cally derived prediction that self-efficacy is linked to PBC. We accord-
self-efficacy to perform the behaviour (intrinsic motivation) and of ingly formulated the following hypothesis:
control over it (extrinsic motivation). Previous field studies of eco-
logical behaviours have suggested that PBC predicts H7 : Self-efficacy significantly influences PBC.
proenvironmental behavioural intentions and self-reported behav-
iours (Davis, O'Callaghan, & Knox, 2009). Thus, for example, for a
study of intentions to recycle household waste, Carrus, Passafaro,
3.7 | Situational factors ! PBC
and Bonnes (2008) used a survey conducted in Italy to test the
model of goal-directed behaviour (an extension of TPB) and deter- Beyond the influence of self-efficacy, the TPB suggests that situa-
mined that PBC correlated moderately but significantly with inten- tional factors play a significant role in the perception of behavioural
tion to engage in household recycling (r = .44). Similarly, in a cross- control (Ajzen, 1991). Although an individual's internal disposition
sectional survey conducted in Taiwan, Chen and Tung (2014) found influences his or her decision to engage in a behaviour, features of a
PBC to correlate significantly with recycling intention (r = .44) but given situation may act as enablers or disablers of that behaviour
ARLI ET AL. 5 of 14

(Ajzen, 2005; Barr, 2007). Thus, for example, individuals with a posi- H8 : Situational factors significantly influence perceived behaviour
tive attitude towards recycling but little access to recycling facilities control.
are constrained by the situation in which they find themselves. Situa-
tional influences add a level of complexity to behavioural responses
owing to the dynamic nature of the forces that are potentially at play
3.8 | Intention to recycle ! behaviour
(Flannery & May, 2000).
Situational factors, then, obtain within a specific time and space There is a robust theoretical basis for the direct influence of intent
(Carrington, Neville, & Whitwell, 2010); such factors are beyond indi- on behaviour (Ajzen, 1991). Specifically, according to TPB, the
viduals' control but nevertheless influence their behaviour (Flannery & strength of an individual's intention towards a behaviour correlates
May, 2000). So it is that such demographic variables as age, gender, with the likelihood of performing that behaviour (Ajzen, 2005).
and income are known to exert a significant influence in behavioural Intentions thus reflect an individual's willingness to exert the effort
contexts (Echegaray & Hansstein, 2017; Wang, Guo, & Wang, 2016). to perform a behaviour (Ajzen, 1991) and are impacted by a range
An individual's personal situation, including environmental conditions, of direct and indirect factors, including the individual's nature, social
knowledge, and experience, is also influential in behavioural contexts influences, and issues of control, all of which create the intent to
(Barr, 2007; Mahmud & Osman, 2010). Effort, logistics, and inconve- engage in a behaviour (Ajzen, 2005). Indeed, the concept of intent
nience associated with recycling may also be important considerations is at the centre of TPB (Oztekin, Teksoz, Pamuk, Sahin, & Kilic,
(Boldero, 1995), along with situational constraints, self-awareness, 2017) and has been shown to be a fairly reliable predictor actual
and competence (Ajzen, 2005). behaviour (Ajzen, 1991). Thus, when individuals have a strong intent
More specifically, TPB holds that situational influences directly to engage in recycling behaviour, they are far more likely to perform
impact PBC, which in turn relates to individuals' perceptions of the it than those with vague or weak intent. Various models confirm
ease of a behaviour. Thus, for example, control of recycling is often this relationship between intention and behaviour, particularly in the
associated with perceptions of the situational influences represented environmental behaviour arena, as Chao (2012) showed in the case
by the cost and convenience of recycling behaviour (Wang et al., of Taiwanese students, Boldero (1995) in the case of Australian stu-
2016). Therefore, in the case of recycling, situational barriers may dents, and Latif et al. (2012) in the case of Malaysian consumers.
diminish individuals' perception of behavioural control (Pop, On the other hand, Carrington et al. (2010) found that, at least in
Romonti-Maniu, & Zaharie, 2014): When these barriers increase the relation to environmentally ethical purchasing behaviour, individuals'
effort required and create inconveniences (Boldero, 1995), individ- intent was a less than effective predictor, as did Echegaray and
uals' behavioural control with respect to recycling is directly Hansstein (2017) in the context of e-waste in Brazil. Therefore,
impacted. although there is strong evidence for a relationship between intent
Situational factors contribute directly to PBC in a range of con- and behaviour, uncertainty remains regarding the specifically envi-
texts, including environmental ethical behaviours (Flannery & May, ronmental context (Echegaray & Hansstein, 2017) that requires fur-
2000) and recycling (Echegaray & Hansstein, 2017; Pop et al., 2014). ther examination. Nevertheless, the prevailing view appears to be
The effects of situational influences are somewhat inconsistent in the that individuals with strong intentions are more likely to engage in a
recycling environment (Flannery & May, 2000; Latif, Omar, Bidin, & given behaviour (see Figure 1). We accordingly formulated our last
Awang, 2012), but solid theoretical reasoning supports the conceptual hypothesis, as follows:
link between situational factors and perceived control. We accord-
ingly formulated the following hypothesis: H9 : Intention to recycle significantly influences recycling behaviour.

F I G U R E 1 The theory of
planned behaviour (adapted from
Ajzen, 1991)
6 of 14 ARLI ET AL.

4 | METHODOLOGY variable to test for internal consistency reliability and assessed con-
vergent validity by determining whether factor loadings were greater
4.1 | Measure and data collection than.50 and, therefore, statistically significant (p ≤ .05). The minimum
factor loading was.56 (for moral norm, Item 1; and awareness of
Participants (n = 827) were recruited through an online survey plat-
recycling, Item 4; Bagozzi & Yi, 2012), which met the threshold of
form (MTurk). The extant literature supports the use of this platform
internal consistency (Malhotra & Birks, 2007). The confidence inter-
in social science research (Goodman & Paolacci, 2017; Hauser &
vals around the correlation estimates between any two constructs
Schwarz, 2016; Smith, Roster, Golden, & Albaum, 2016). Participants were all significantly different from one another (Anderson & Gerbing,
received USD$1.0 in their Amazon.com account as a reward for suc- 1988). To assess discriminant validity among the constructs, we com-
cessfully completing the survey. The demographic information is sum- pared the average variance extracted (AVE) values for each with the
marized in Table 1. squared correlation estimates of paired measured constructs in the
All of the measures used 5-point Likert-type scales, adapted from model (Fornell & Larcker, 1981); the AVE ranged from .47 (subjective
previous research, with anchors ranging from 1 (strongly disagree) to 5 norm) to .89 (intention to recycle). We further calculated composite
(strongly agree). Attitudes towards recycling, subjective norms, moral reliability (CR) values and compared them with a threshold benchmark
norms, PBC, intention to recycle, and situational factors were adapted of.60 (Bagozzi & Yi, 1988); the minimum CR of this study ranged
from Ajzen (1991) and Tonglet et al. (2004). Awareness of recycling from.73 to.96. The variance extracted was tested, and the AVE for
was adapted from Guagnano et al. (1995) and self-efficacy from each factor was greater than the square of the correlation coefficient
Bandura (1994). The scale items that we used in this study are sum- for each of the other factors (see Table 3). Thus, the test confirmed
marized in Table 2. the measurement model discriminant validity (Fornell &
Larcker, 1981).

4.2 | Reliability
5 | RESULTS
Using structural equation modelling (AMOS), we ran a confirmatory
factor analysis (CFA) on the measures. The fit of the model was good, Having established the unidimensionality and reliability of the models,
χ 2(df) = 1,819.985 (538); comparative fit index = .93; Tucker–Lewis we next estimated the hypothesized relationships between the vari-
index = .92; nonnormed fit index = .91; root mean square error of ous constructs (see Table 4). The results indicated that attitudes
approximation = .05; standardized root mean square residual = .04, towards the behaviour did not influence the intention to recycle; thus,
being well above the recommended values (Jöreskor & Sörbom, 2003; H1 was not supported. However, H2 was supported, as awareness of
Steiger, 1990). We also calculated Cronbach's alpha for each key recycling significantly influenced attitudes towards it (β = .538; p <
.001). H3 and H4 were also supported, as subjective norms or social

TABLE 1 Demographic profile (N = 210) pressure significantly influenced the intention to recycle (β = .593; p <
.001), and the subjective norm was influenced by the moral norm (β =
Demographic Percentage
.593; p < .001), respectively.
Gender Further, PBC, or the ease of recycling, significantly influenced
Male 53.3 intention to recycle (β = .695; p < .001) and recycling behaviour (β =
Female 46.7 .174; p < .001), so H5 and H6 were supported. So also were H7 and
Marital status H8, as PBC was significantly influenced by self-efficacy (β = .229, p <
Married 56.7 .001) and by situational factors (β = −.667, p < .001), respectively.
Single 43.3 Finally, intention to recycle significantly influenced recycling behav-

Age iour (β = .639, p < .001), so H9 was supported too.

18–24 years 13.8


25–34 years 54.3 6 | DISCUSSION
35–44 years 21.4
45–54 years 6.2 Moral norms are higher order constructs, being bound up in an indi-
Over 55 years 4.3 vidual's sense of self, and are relatively stable. These norms, therefore,
Ethnicity drive behaviour across a range of contexts (Bandura, 2012), particu-

Caucasian 37.1 larly those that have an “oughtness” quality or ethical element
(Rokeach, 1973), which includes proenvironmental behaviour
African American 4.8
(Schwartz, 1977). Whereas moral norms constitute a general individ-
Asian 50
ual standard of behaviour (Rokeach, 1973; Schwartz, 1977), social
Latino 3.3
norms emerge from collective agreement within important referent
Other 4.8
groups in regard to acceptable behaviour in a given context (Rokeach,
ARLI ET AL. 7 of 14

TABLE 2 Confirmatory factor analysis

Convergent validity Reliability

Factor Item Factor loading CR AVE


Awareness of recycling
Recycling substantially reduces the use of landfills. AR01 .81 .87 .63
Recycling conserves natural resources. AR02 .88
Recycling will make a difference in the quality of the AR03 .86
environment
Recycling creates jobs. AR04 .56
1 = strongly disagree; 5 = strongly agree
Moral norm
I feel I should not waste anything if it could be used again. MN01 .56 .86 .50
It would be wrong of me not to recycle my household MN02 .75
waste.
I would feel guilty if I did not recycle my household waste MN03 .76
Not recycling goes against my principles. MN04 .76
I am concerned with maintaining a good place to live. MN05 .58
I have a strong interest in the health and well-being of the MN06 .65
community in which I live
Everybody should share the responsibility to recycle MN07 .83
household waste.
1 = strongly disagree; 5 = strongly agree
Self-efficacy
I can always manage to solve difficult problems if I try SE01 .80 .84 .57
hard enough.
It is easy for me to stick to my aims and accomplish my SE02 .67
goals.
I am confident that I could deal efficiently with SE03 .78
unexpected events.
Thanks to my resourcefulness, I know how to handle SE04 .86
unforeseen situations.
I can solve most problems if I invest the necessary effort. SE05 .80
1 = strongly disagree; 5 = strongly agree
Situational factor
Recycling is too complicated. SF01 .80 .87 .63
Recycling takes up too much room. SF02 .79
Recycling programmes are a waste of money. SF03 .72
Recycling takes up too much time. SF04 .86
1 = strongly disagree; 5 = strongly agree
Attitude towards recycling
1 = bad; 5 = good ATR01 .67 .88 .59
1 = waste of time; 5 = useful ATR02 .83
1 = deceptive; 5 = rewarding ATR03 .73
1 = not sensible; 5 = sensible ATR04 .83
Subjective norm
Most people think I should recycle. SN01 .64 .73 .47
Most people would approve of me recycling. SN02 .59
Those people who are important to me would want me to SN03 .82
engage in recycling.
(Continues)
8 of 14 ARLI ET AL.

TABLE 2 (Continued)

Convergent validity Reliability

Factor Item Factor loading CR AVE


1 = strongly disagree; 5 = strongly agree
Intention to recycle
How likely are you to recycle your waste at home in the IR01 .94 .96 .89
next 4 weeks.
I intend to recycle my waste at home every day in the IR02 .93
forthcoming month.
I will try to recycle my waste at home each day in the IR03 .96
forthcoming month.
1 = strongly disagree; 5 = strongly agree
Perceived behavioural control
I have complete control over whether I engage in PBC01 .55 .89 .78
recycling.
I am confident that I could engage in recycling. PBC02 .74
It would be difficult for me to engage in recycling. PNC03 .77
1 = strongly disagree; 5 = strongly agree
Recycling behaviour
In the past 3 months, how frequently did you recycle RB01 .61 .59 .77
household waste that can be put into the recycling bins
provided by the council or local authorities?
In the past 3 months, how frequently did you recycle at RB02 .90
your workplace?
1 = never; 5 = always

Abbreviations: AVE, average variance extracted; CR, composite reliability.

TABLE 3 Discriminant validity

Construct 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9
1. Awareness of recycling .63
2. Moral norm .56** .50
**
3. Self-efficacy .29 .29** .57
**
4. Situational factors −.50 −.53** −.24** .63
** ** **
5. Attitude towards recycling .49 .47 .14 −.47** .59
** ** **
6. Subjective norm .43 .57 .24 −.44** .37** .47
** ** ** ** **
7. Intention to recycle .34 .68 .23 −.51 .35 .42** .89
** ** ** ** **
8. Perceived behavioural control .31 .40 .24 −.50 .28 .28** .54** .48
** ** ** ** ** ** **
9. Recycling behaviour .25 .55 .21 −.47 .28 .37 .73 .45** .59
M 4.14 4.01 3.81 1.87 4.59 4.12 3.91 4.03 3.65
SD 0.65 0.71 0.64 0.81 0.62 0.65 1.07 0.78 1.12

Note. Values below the diagonal are bivariate correlations between the constructs; bold diagonal elements represent the average variance extracted for
the relevant construct; values above the diagonal represent squared correlations; and values below the diagonal represent correlations.
**p < .001.; *p < .05.

1973, Schwartz, 1977). Moral and social norms are, thus, distinct con- measures of moral norms (which are individual) and subjective norms
structs, as the evaluations of discriminant validity for this study con- (which are group based) found in this study was consistent with theo-
firmed (see Table 3). However, precisely because social norms are retical expectations.
viewed as expressions of underlying, individual, and internally stable Ajzen (2012) argued that behaviour is determined in part by nor-
cognitive structures, including morals and values (Rokeach, 1973; mative beliefs concerning the expectations of important referent
Schwartz, 1977), the significant positive association between the others, an idea introduced above, and that these normative beliefs
ARLI ET AL. 9 of 14

TABLE 4 Results of the structural model

Hypothesis Path Path coefficient p value Result


H1 Attitude towards the behaviour ! intention to recycle .025 .635 Not supported
H2 Awareness of recycling ! attitude towards recycling .538 .000 Supported
H3 Subjective norm ! intention to recycle. .593 .000 Supported
H4 Moral norm ! subjective norm .631 .000 Supported
H5 Perceived behavioural control ! influence intention to recycle .695 .000 Supported
H6 Perceived behavioural control ! recycling behaviour .174 .000 Supported
H7 Self-efficacy ! perceived behavioural control .229 .000 Supported
H8 Situational factors ! perceived behaviour control −.667 .000 Supported
H9 Intention to recycle ! recycling behaviour .639 .000 Supported

Note. Fit statistics: χ (df) = 1819.985 (538); comparative fit index = .93; Tucker–Lewis index = .92; nonnormed fit index = .91; root mean square error of
2

approximation = .05; standardized root mean square residual = .04.

combine to produce a subjective norm or perceived social pressure to unique contribution of attitudes relating to intention to recycle
perform a particular behaviour (see in particular p. 443). The finding might, as per previous TPB models, actually be subsumed by the
presented here that a subjective norm in support of recycling behav- inclusion of a measure of beliefs about the outcomes (i.e., measures
iour significantly predicted recycling intention is, thus, consistent with of awareness, as included in the current model) seen in the results
Ajzen's theory. Furthermore, subjective norms are considered concep- of the model analysed here. If a measure of beliefs regarding the
tually independent of attitudes towards a behaviour, in that an indi- outcomes of a behaviour were included in a TPB-based model, this
vidual can have a positive attitude about it but perceive little social effect would merit investigation in future studies using similar meth-
pressure to engage in it and, therefore, choose not to do so. It is, odology and measures. Were such further empirical evidence to be
therefore, important to consider the unique contribution of each of found, then, measures of beliefs about behavioural outcomes could
the other variables in the extended TPB model. prove to be effective substitutes for attitudes in TPB-based predic-
These results confirm the expected association between aware- tive models of behaviour. Such a finding could be particularly impor-
ness of the positive consequences of recycling (i.e., beliefs about the tant, as beliefs of this type can be readily taught through evidence-
outcomes) and attitudes towards it. Ajzen (2012) argued that a behav- based proenvironmental initiatives and, therefore, may be more eas-
iour is guided in part by evaluations of its likely outcomes (see in par- ily influenced than attitudes when it comes to increasing the likeli-
ticular p. 448), which in turn produce either favourable or hood of proenvironmental behaviour.
unfavourable attitudes towards it. The significant positive association Beyond self-efficacy, the findings presented also indicate that sit-
between awareness of the consequences of recycling and attitudes uational factors positively influence perceived behaviour control, as
towards recycling found here is, thus, also consistent with Ajzen's was expected based on TPB (Bandura 1982). Thus, for instance, Tay-
theory. lor and Todd (1995) proposed that self-efficacy is a key determinant
Unlike the meta-analysis by Bamberg and Möser (2007) of psy- of waste management intentions and in particular intention to recycle
cho-social determinants of proenvironmental behaviour, which owing to its relationship with PBC. On the other hand, Ajzen (1991)
found significant correlations between proenvironmental attitudes suggested that the situational factors should play an influential role in
and behaviours across nine studies, the CFA model tested here the perception of behavioural control. Thus, Echegaray and Hansstein
showed no significant unique contribution of attitudes towards (2017) identified age, gender, and income as situational influences
intention to recycle. This result seems counter-intuitive given the more commonly thought to exert significant influence on behaviour
empirical evidence to the contrary reported in a number of previous (cf. Wang et al., 2016). In the case of recycling, situational barriers
environmental studies and the broader evidence for the validity of may diminish individuals' perceptions of behavioural control (Pop et
TPB and component constructs, including attitudes. However, it is al., 2014).
noteworthy that, in previous studies, neither measures of awareness Likewise, the present study showed that PBC positively influences
of consequences nor beliefs about the outcomes of the target intention to recycle, thus mirroring the finding of a cross-sectional
behaviours were used in the models. As has been seen, Ajzen's survey conducted in Taiwan by Chen and Tung (2014). Carrus et al.
(1991, 2012) theory emphasizes readily accessible beliefs about the (2008) also reported that PBC predicted proenvironmental behav-
likely outcomes of a behaviour (e.g., the belief that recycling con- ioural intentions in their study of household waste recycling. Further-
serves natural resources) as a foundation for attitude formation. It more, unlike Echegaray and Hansstein (2017), who found that
could accordingly be argued that a CFA model—one accounting for intention to recycle did not predict the behaviour of recycling of e-
the relative influence of all of the variables used in predicting inten- waste in Brazil, we found a direct link between the two. This finding
tion to recycle—has the potential to reveal whether any predicted corroborates the previous work in Australia (Boldero, 1995), Taiwan
10 of 14 ARLI ET AL.

(Chao, 2012), and Malaysia (Latif et al., 2012) discussed above, which 8 | LIMITATIONS AND FUTURE RESEARCH
revealed a positive relationship between intention and
proenvironmental behaviour. These results are again consistent with Although samples of U.S. respondents recruited through Amazon's
the tenet of TPB that the strength of an individual's intention towards Mechanical Turk are generally considered to be of high quality and
a behaviour correlates with the likelihood of performing it (Ajzen, demographically diverse—and the approach is certainly cost-effec-
2005; Oztekin et al., 2017). tive—as a source of data, this platform has a few limitations. Among
these limitations are restricted access, because Mechanical Turk
reaches only tech-savvy consumers, and the tendency of consumers

7 | MANAGERIAL IMPLICATIONS to become distracted by uncontrollable factors in their personal


physical environments (Minton et al., 2015). The very fact that the
The findings presented here have significant practical implications for respondents were all in the United States represents another limita-
policymakers and practitioners interested in inculcating intention and tion on the generalizability of the findings, one that could be obvi-
behaviour relating to recycling in the public through a review of the ated in future studies by the inclusion of respondents from other
relevant regulations. One such implication is that, in general, interven- countries. Finally, the data for this study were cross-sectional.
tions by governmental agencies and organizations should concentrate Owing to the rapidity of technological developments, including
on enhancing awareness of the benefits of recycling, increasing self- increased access to information through the Internet, attitudes, per-
efficacy, and being attentive to perceived norms. Mass media and ceptions, and intention relating to recycling are likely to change over
non-profit organizations can also promote recycling behaviours time. As a consequence, replicating this study using longitudinal
through green campaigns. (rather than cross-sectional) data could reveal further interesting
In addition, the public should be made keenly aware of ethical results.
norms relating to recycling behaviour, including the purchase and use This study has paved the way for future research in other respects
of recycling products. Thus, for instance, environmental programmes as well. To begin with, attitudes, perceptions, and behaviours relating
could highlight the norms and moral aspects of recycling behaviour to such environmental issues as recycling tend to vary with demo-
while at the same time discussing risks to the safety of people and graphic characteristics, including gender, income, religion, and educa-
animals posed by poor waste management practices. Similarly, media tion (Arli & Tjiptono, 2017; Gifford & Nilsson, 2014; Martínez-
channels and governmental agencies should seek to remain active in Espiñeira, García-Valiñas, & Nauges, 2014; Liobikienė & Juknys,
sharing insights regarding the negative impacts of poor waste 2016). Hence, a similar study could be conducted comparing various
management. groups (e.g., younger with older or more with less educated respon-
At the same time, because the intention to recycle represents dents). Further, although we treated norms as a single construct,
an individual's willingness to exert effort and is influenced by direct those enforced by family and friends also drive behavioural intentions
and indirect factors, such as social influences (Ajzen, 1991, 2005), (Jebarakirthy & Lobo, 2015), including with respect to
other communities, especially families and schools, have the poten- proenvironmental issues (Gronhoj & Thogersen, 2012; Joung & Park-
tial to be effective in fostering recycling habits. One example is by Poaps, 2013). The norms of both family members and friends could
creating a recycling club within a school, which will help normalize accordingly be factored into future research as separate determinants
the recycling behaviour. The club will create the norm by “promot- of intention to recycle. Finally, we examined only direct effects, leav-
ing the significance of recycling and portraying recycling as a socially ing it to future researchers to examine the influence of moderating
desirable behavior” (Wan, Shen, & Yu, 2014, p. 148). These steps factors, such as those affecting the association between awareness of
stand to enhance the attitudes and behaviours of individuals regard- and attitudes towards recycling.
ing recycling. Furthermore, parents and teachers could instruct chil-
dren on the use of recycling bins and recyclable products in order
ET HICS S TAT E MENT
to plan for a cleaner and healthier society. Mass media and social
media can contribute by drawing attention to the admiration of Informed consent was obtained from all individual participants
family members and friends that recycling can engender. Members included in the study. All procedures performed in studies involving
of the public should encourage family and friends to support gov- human participants were in accordance with the ethical standards of
ernment recycling efforts. Watching similar people or hearing their the institutional and/or national research committee and with the
success stories in keeping the environment clean will motivate them 1964 Declaration of Helsinki and its later amendments or comparable
to believe that if other could, they can too. It will boost their self- ethical standards.
efficacy. Lastly, government can incentivize the process of recycling,
for example, by giving a coupon to residents who they can recycle
OR CID
their waste for free, hence creating a sense of control in regard to
their recycling behaviour. Denni Arli https://orcid.org/0000-0002-6320-3994
ARLI ET AL. 11 of 14

RE FE R ENC E S Department of Family and Community Services on behalf of National


Youth Affairs Research Scheme.
Ajzen I. (1985). From intentions to actions: A theory of planned behaviour. Berger, I. E. (1997). The demographics of recycling and the structure of
In Kuhl J. & Beckmann J.. (Eds.), Action-control: From cognition to environmental behavior. Environment and Behavior, 29(4), 515–531.
behaviour (pp. 11–39). Heidelberg, Germany: Springer. https://doi.org/ https://doi.org/10.1177/001391659702900404
10.1007/978-3-642-69746-3_2. Black, I. (2010). Sustainability through anti-consumption. Journal of Con-
Ajzen, I. (1991). The theory of planned behaviour. Organisational Behaviour
sumer Behaviour, 9(6), 403–411. https://doi.org/10.1002/cb.340
Human Decision Process, 50(2), 179–211. https://doi.org/10.1016/
Blok, V., Wesselink, R., Studynka, O., & Kemp, R. (2015). Encouraging sus-
0749-5978(91)90020-T
tainability in the workplace: A survey on the pro-environmental behav-
Ajzen, I. (2005). Attitudes, personality and behavior. New York, NY, Maiden-
iour of university employees. Journal of Cleaner Production, 106, 55–
head and Berkshire, UK: Open University Press.
67. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jclepro.2014.07.063
Ajzen, I. (2015). The theory of planned behaviour is alive and well, and not
Boldero, J. (1995). The prediction of household recycling of newspapers:
ready to retire: A commentary on Sniehotta, Presseau, and Araújo-
The role of attitudes, intentions, and situational factors. Journal of
Soares. Health Psychology Review, 9(2), 131–137.
Applied Social Psychology, 25(5), 440–462. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.
Ajzen, I., & Fishbein, M. (2000). Attitudes and the attitude–behavior rela-
1559-1816.1995.tb01598.x
tion: Reasoned and automatic processes. European Review of Social
Botetzagias, I., Dima, A., & Malesios, C. (2015). Extending the theory of
Psychology, 11(1), 1-33, 1, DOI: https://doi.org/10.1080/
14792779943000116. planned behaviour in the context of recycling: The role of moral norms
Ajzen, I., Albarracin, D., & Hornik, R., eds. Prediction and change of health and of demographic predictors. Resources, Conservation and Recycling,
behavior: Applying the reasoned action approach. Psychology Press; 95, 58–67. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.resconrec.2014.12.004
2012. Carrington, M. J., Neville, B. A., & Whitwell, G. J. (2010). Why ethical con-
Anderson, J. C., & Gerbing, D. W. (1988). Structural equation modeling in sumers don't walk their talk: Towards a framework for understanding
practice: A review and recommended two-step approach. Psychological the gap between the ethical purchase intentions and actual buying
Bulletin, 103(3), 411. behaviour of ethically minded consumers. Journal of Business Ethics, 97
Arli, D., & Tjiptono, F. (2017). God and green: Investigating the impact of (1), 139–158. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10551-010-0501-6
religiousness on green marketing. International Journal of Nonprofit and Carrus, G., Passafaro, P., & Bonnes, M. (2008). Emotions, habits and ratio-
Voluntary Sector Marketing, 22(3), e1578. https://doi.org/10.1002/ nal choices in ecological behaviours: The case of recycling and use of
nvsm.1578 public transportation. Journal of Environmental Psychology, 28(1), 51–
Armitage, C. J., & Conner, M. (2001). Efficacy of the theory of planned 62. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jenvp.2007.09.003
behaviour: A meta-analytic review. British Journal of Social Psychology, Chan, K. (1998). Mass communication and pro-environmental behaviour:
40(4), 471–499. https://doi.org/10.1348/014466601164939 Waste recycling in Hong Kong. Journal of Environmental Management,
Bagozzi, R. P., & Yi, Y. (1988). On the evaluation of structural equation 52(4), 317–325. https://doi.org/10.1006/jema.1998.0189
models. Journal of the Academy of Marketing Science, 16, 74–94. Chan, L., & Bishop, B. (2013). A moral basis for recycling: Extending the
https://doi.org/10.1007/BF02723327 theory of planned behaviour. Journal of Environmental Psychology, 36,
Bagozzi, R. P., & Yi, Y. (2012). Specification, evaluation, and interpretation 96–102. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jenvp.2013.07.010
of structural equation models. Journal of the Academy of Marketing Sci- Chao, Y.-L. (2012). Predicting people's environmental behaviour: Theory of
ence, 40(1), 8-34. planned behaviour and model of responsible environmental behaviour.
Baldassare, M., & Katz, C. (1992). The personal threat of environmental Environmental Education Research, 18(4), 437–461. https://doi.org/10.
problems as predictor of environmental practices. Environment and 1080/13504622.2011.634970
Behaviour, 24(5), 602–616. https://doi.org/10.1177/ Chen, M.-F., & Tung, P.-J. (2014). Developing an extended theory of
0013916592245002 planned behaviour model to predict consumers’ intention to visit green
Bamberg, S., & Möser, G. (2007). Twenty years after Hines, Hungerford,
hotels. International Journal of Hospitality Management, 36, 221–230.
and Tomera: A new meta-analysis of psycho-social determinants of
Chen, M. F., & Tung, P. J. (2009). The moderating effect of perceived lack
pro-environmental behaviour. Journal of Environmental Psychology, 27,
of facilities on consumers' recycling intentions. Environment and Behav-
14–25. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jenvp.2006.12.002
iour, 42(6), 824–844.
Bamberg, S., & Schmidt, P. (2003). Incentives, morality, or habit? Predicting
Coggins, P. C. (1994). Who is the recycler? Journal of Waste Management
students' car use for university routes with the models of Ajzen,
and Resource Recovery, 1(2), 1–7.
Schwartz, and Triandis. Environment and Behavior, 35, 264–285.
Daneshvary, N., Daneshvary, R., & Schwer, R. K. (1998). Solid-waste
https://doi.org/10.1177/0013916502250134
recycling behavior and support for curbside textile recycling. Environ-
Bandura, A. (1994). Self-efficacy. In V. S. Ramachaudran (Ed.), Encyclopedia
ment and Behavior, 30(2), 144–161. https://doi.org/10.1177/
of human behavior (Vol. 4) (pp. 71–81). New York, NY: Academic Press.
0013916598302002
Bandura, A. (2012). On the functional properties of perceived self-efficacy
Davis, G., O'Callaghan, F., & Knox, K. (2009). Sustainable attitudes and
revisited. Journal of Management, 38(1), 9–44.
behaviours amongst a sample of non-academic staff: A case study
Bandura, A, & Walters, R. H. (1977) Social learning theory Vol. 1. Engle-
wood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-hall. from an Information Services Department, Griffith University, Bris-
Barr, S. (2007). Factors influencing environmental attitudes and behaviors: bane. International Journal of Sustainability in Higher Education, 10(2),
A U.K. case study of household waste management. Environment and 136–151. https://doi.org/10.1108/14676370910945945
Behavior, 39(4), 435–473. https://doi.org/10.1177/0013916 De Jaeger, S., Eyckmans, J., Rogge, N., & Van Puyenbroeck, T. (2011).
505283421 Wasteful waste reducing policies? The impact of waste reduction pol-
Barr, S., Gilg, A. W., & Ford, N. J. (2001). A conceptual framework for icy instruments on collection and processing costs of municipal solid
understanding and analysing attitudes towards household-waste man- waste. Waste Management, 31(7), 1429–1440. https://doi.org/10.
agement. Environment and Planning, 33(11), 2025–2048. https://doi. 1016/j.wasman.2011.02.021
org/10.1068/a33225 De Young, R. (1986). Some psychological aspects of recycling. Environment
Bentley, M., Fien, J., & Neil, C. (2004). Sustainable consumption: Young and Behavior, 18(4), 435–449. https://doi.org/10.1177/
Australians as agents of change (No. 0975249843). Canberra, Australia: 0013916586184001
12 of 14 ARLI ET AL.

Deci, E. L., & Ryan, R. M. (1985). The general causality orientations scale: of World Business, 45, 385–394. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jwb.2009.
Self–determination in personality. Journal of research in personality, 19, 08.007
109–134.. Hunecke, M., Blöbaum, A., Matthies, E., & Höger, R. (2001). Responsibility
Deci, E. L., & Ryan, R. M. (2008). Self-determination theory: A macro the- and environment: Ecological norm orientation and external factors in
ory of human motivation, development, and health. Canadian Psychol- the domain of travel mode choice behavior. Environment and Behavior,
ogy, 49(3), 1823–1185. 33(6), 830–852. https://doi.org/10.1177/00139160121973269
Derksen, L., & Gartrell, J. (1993). The social context of recycling. American Jebarakirthy, C., & Lobo, A. (2015). A study investigating attitudinal per-
Sociological Review, 58, 434–442. https://doi.org/10.2307/2095910 ceptions of microcredit services and their relevant drivers in bottom of
Dwivedy, M., & Mittal, R. K. (2013). Willingness of residents to participate pyramid market segments. Journal of Retailing and Consumer Services,
in e-waste recycling in India. Environmental Development, 6, 48–68. 23, 39–48. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jretconser.2014.12.005
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.envdev.2013.03.001 Joung, H. M., & Park-Poaps, H. (2013). Factors motivating and influencing
Echegaray, F., & Hansstein, F. V. (2017). Assessing the intention–behavior clothing disposal behaviours. International Journal of Consumer Studies,
gap in electronic waste recycling: the case of Brazil. Journal of Cleaner 37, 105–111. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1470-6431.2011.01048.x
Production, 142, 180–190. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jclepro.2016. Jöreskog, K. G., & Sörbom, D. (2003). LISREL 8: Structural equation modeling
05.064 with the SIMPLIS command language. Scientific Software International;
Flannery, B. L., & May, D. R. (2000). Environmental ethical decision making 1993.
in the U.S. metal-finishing industry. The Academy of Management Jour- Kaiser, F., & Gutscher, H. (2003). The proposition of a general version of
nal, 43(4), 642–662. https://doi.org/10.2307/1556359 the theory of planned behavior: Predicting ecological behavior. Journal
Fornara, F., Carrus, G., Passafaro, P., & Bonnes, M. (2011). Distinguishing of Applied Social Psychology, 33, 586–603. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.
the sources of normative influence on proenvironmental behaviors: 1559-1816.2003.tb01914.x
The role of local norms in household waste recycling. Group Processes Klockner, C. (2013). A comprehensive model of the psychology of environ-
& Intergroup Relations, 14(5), 623–635. https://doi.org/10.1177/ mental behavior: A meta-analysis. Global Environmental Change, 23,
1368430211408149 1028–1038. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.gloenvcha.2013.05.014
Fornell, C., & Larcker, D. F. (1981). Evaluating structural equation models Knussen, C., Yule, F., MacKenzie, J., & Wells, M. (2004). An analysis of
with unobservable variables and measurement error. Journal of Mar- intentions to recycle household waste: The roles of past behaviour,
keting Research, 18, 39–50. https://doi.org/10.1177/ perceived habit, and perceived lack of facilities. Journal of Environmen-
002224378101800104 tal Psychology, 24(2), 237–246. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jenvp.2003.
Gifford, R., & Nilsson, A. (2014). Personal and social factors that influence 12.001
pro-environmental concern and behaviour: A review. International Latif, S. A., Omar, M. S., Bidin, Y. H., & Awang, Z. (2012). Environmental
Journal of Psychology, 49(3), 141–157. problems and quality of life: Situational factor as a predictor of
Goddard, H. C. (1995). The benefits and costs of alternative solid waste recycling behaviour. Procedia - Social and Behavioral Sciences, 35, 682–
management policies. Resources, Conservation and Recycling, 13(3), 688. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.sbspro.2012.02.137
183–213. https://doi.org/10.1016/0921-3449(94)00021-V Lavigne, G. L., Hauw, N., Vallerand, R. J., Brunel, P., Blanchard, C.,
Goodman, J. K., & Paolacci, G. (2017). Crowdsourcing consumer research. Cadoretta, I., & Angot, C. (2009). On the dynamic relationships
Journal of Consumer Research, 44(1), 196–210. https://doi.org/10. between contextual (or general) and situational (or state) motivation
1093/jcr/ucx047 toward exercise and physical activity: A longitudinal test of the top-
Greaves, M., Zibarras, L., & Stride, C. (2013). Using the theory of planned down and bottom-up hypotheses. International Journal of Sport and
behaviour to explore environmental behavioral intentions in the work- Exercise Psychology, 7, 147–168. https://doi.org/10.1080/1612197X.
place. Journal of Environmental Psychology, 34, 109–120. https://doi. 2009.9671897
org/10.1016/j.jenvp.2013.02.003 Liobikienė, G., & Juknys, R. (2016). The role of values, environmental risk
Gronhoj, A., & Thogersen, J. (2012). Action speaks louder than words: The perception, awareness of consequences, and willingness to assume
effect of personal attitudes and family norms on adolescents' pro-envi- responsibility for environmentally-friendly behaviour: The Lithuanian
ronmental behaviour. Journal of Economic Psychology, 33, 292–302. case. Journal of Cleaner Production, 112, 3413–3422. https://doi.org/
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.joep.2011.10.001 10.1016/j.jclepro.2015.10.049
Guagnano, G. A., Stern, P. C., & Dietz, T. (1995). Influences on attitude– Mahmud, S. N. D., & Osman, K. (2010). The determinants of recycling
behavior relationships: A natural experiment with curbside recycling. intention behavior among the Malaysian school students: An applica-
Environment and Behavior, 27(5), 699–718. https://doi.org/10.1177/ tion of theory of planned behaviour. Procedia - Social and Behavioral
0013916595275005 Sciences, 9, 119–124. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.sbspro.2010.12.123
Han, H. (2015). Travelers' pro-environmental behavior in a green lodging Malhotra, N., & Birks, D. (2007). Marketing Research: an applied approach:
context: Converging value-belief-norm theory and the theory of 3rd European Edition. Pearson education.
planned behavior. Tourism Management, 47, 164–177. https://doi.org/ Mannetti, L., Pierro, A., & Livi, S. (2004). Recycling: Planned and self-
10.1016/j.tourman.2014.09.014 expressive behaviour. Journal of Environmental Psychology, 24(2), 227–
Han, H., & Kim, Y. (2010). An investigation of green hotel customers' deci- 236. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jenvp.2004.01.002
sion formation: Developing an extended model of the theory of Manstead, A. S. (2000). The role of moral norm in the attitude–behavior
planned behavior. International Journal of Hospitality Management, 29 relation. In D. J. Terry, & M. A. Hogg (Eds.), Applied social research. Atti-
(4), 659–668. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ijhm.2010.01.001 tudes, behavior, and social context: The role of norms and group member-
Hauser, D. J., & Schwarz, N. (2016). Attentive Turkers: MTurk participants ship (pp. 11–30). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates
perform better on online attention checks than do subject pool partici- Publishers.
pants. Behavior Research Methods, 48(1), 400–407. Martínez-Espiñeira, R., García-Valiñas, M. A., & Nauges, C. (2014). House-
Hopper, J. R., & Nielsen, J. M. (1991). Recycling as altruistic behavior: Nor- holds' pro-environmental habits and investments in water and energy
mative and behavioral strategies to expand participation in a commu- consumption: Determinants and relationships. Journal of Environmental
nity recycling programme. Environment and Behavior, 23(2), 195–220. Management, 133, 174–183. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jenvman.2013.
https://doi.org/10.1177/0013916591232004 12.002
Hume, M. (2010). Compassion without action: examining the young con- Matthies, E., Selge, S., & Klöckner, C. A. (2012). The role of parental behav-
sumers consumption and attitude to sustainable consumption. Journal iour for the development of behaviour specific environmental norms:
ARLI ET AL. 13 of 14

The example of recycling and re-use behaviour. Journal of Environmen- Schwartz, S. H. (1973). Normative explanations for helping behavior: A cri-
tal Psychology, 32(3), 277–284. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jenvp.2012. tique, proposal, and empirical test. Journal of Experimental Social Psy-
04.003 chology, 9, 349–364. https://doi.org/10.1016/0022-1031(73)90071-1
Minton, E. A., Kahle, L. R., Kim, C. H. (2015). Religion and motives for sus- Schwartz, S. H. (1977). Normative influences on altruism. Advances in
tainable behaviors: A cross-cultural comparison and contrast. Journal Experimental Social Psychology, 10, 221–279.
of Business Research, 68(9), 1937–1944. Selman, P. (1996). Local sustainability: Managing and planning ecologically
Miranda, R., & Blanco, A. (2010). Environmental awareness and paper sound places. London, UK: Chapman.
recycling. Cellulose Chemistry & Technology, 44(10), 431. Shaw, D., Shiu, E., & Clarke, I. (2000). The contribution of ethical obligation
Oreg, S., & Katz-Gerro, T. (2006). Predicting pro-environmental behavior and self-identity to the theory of planned behaviour: An exploration of
cross-nationally: Values, the theory of planned behavior, and value- ethical consumers. Journal of marketing management, 16(8), 879–894.
belief-norm theory. Environment and Behavior, 38, 462–483. https:// https://doi.org/10.1362/026725700784683672
doi.org/10.1177/0013916505286012 Sidique, S. F., Joshi, S. V., & Lupi, F. (2010). Factors influencing the rate of
Organisation for Economic Co-operation and Development (2014). Green- recycling: An analysis of Minnesota counties. Resources, Conservation
ing household behaviour: Overview from the 2011 survey (Revised ed.). and Recycling, 54(4), 242–249. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.resconrec.
OECD Studies on Environmental Policy and Household Behaviour: 2009.08.006
OECD Publishing. https://doi.org/10.1787/9789264214651-en Smith, S. M., Roster, C. A., Golden, L. L., & Albaum, G. S. (2016). A multi-
Oztekin, C., Teksoz, G., Pamuk, S., Sahin, E., & Kilic, D. S. (2017). Gender group analysis of online survey respondent data quality: Comparing a
perspective on the factors predicting recycling behavior: Implications regular USA consumer panel to MTurk samples. Journal of Business
from the theory of planned behavior. Waste Management, 62, 290– Research, 69(8), 3139–3148. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jbusres.2015.
302. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.wasman.2016.12.036 12.002
Park, J., & Ha, S. (2014). Understanding consumer recycling behavior: Sniehotta, F. F., Presseau, J., & Araujo-Soares, V. (2014). Time to retire the
Combining the theory of planned behavior and the norm activation theory of planned behaviour. Health Psychology Review, 8(1), 1-7.
model. Family and Consumer Sciences Research Journal, 42(3), 278–291. Steel, B. S. (1996). Thinking globally and acting locally? Environmental atti-
https://doi.org/10.1111/fcsr.12061 tudes, behaviour and activism. Journal of Environmental Management,
Paul, J., Modi, A., & Patel, J. (2016). Predicting green product consumption 47, 27–36. https://doi.org/10.1006/jema.1996.0033
using theory of planned behavior and reasoned action. Journal of retail- Steiger, J. H. (1990). Structural model evaluation and modification: An
ing and consumer services, 29, 123–134. https://doi.org/10.1016/j. interval estimation approach. Multivariate Behavioral Research, 25(2),
jretconser.2015.11.006 173–180.
Phau, I., & Ong, D. (2007). An investigation of the effects of environmental Steg, L., Bolderdijk, J. W., Keizer, K., & Perlaviciute, G. (2014). An inte-
claims in promotional messages for clothing brands. Marketing Intelli- grated framework for encouraging pro-environmental behaviour: The
gence & Planning, 25(7), 772–788. https://doi.org/10.1108/ role of values, situational factors and goals. Journal of Environmental
02634500710834214 Psychology, 38, 104–115. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jenvp.2014.
Pop, C.-M., Romonti-Maniu, A.-I., & Zaharie, M.-M. (2014). How do 01.002
moderating effects predict recycling behaviour? An examination of Stern, P. C. (2000). Toward a coherent theory of environmentally signifi-
situational factors and satisfaction with the local council. The Proceed- cant behaviour. Journal of Social Issues, 56, 407–424. https://doi.org/
ings of the International Conference, Marketing: from Information to 10.1111/0022-4537.00175
Decision. Strong, C. (1998). The impact of environmental education on children's
Ramayah, T., Lee, J. W. C., & Lim, S. (2012). Sustaining the environment knowledge and awareness of environmental concerns. Marketing Intel-
through recycling: An empirical study. Journal of Environmental Man- ligence & Planning, 16(6), 349–355. https://doi.org/10.1108/
agement, 102, 141–147. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jenvman.2012. 02634509810237523
02.025 Tanskanen, P. (2013). Management and recycling of electronic waste. Acta
Rivis, A., Sheeran, P., & Armitage, C. J. (2009). Expanding the affective and materialia, 61(3), 1001–1011. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.actamat.2012.
normative components of the theory of planned behavior: A meta- 11.005
analysis of anticipated affect and moral norms. Journal of Applied Social Taylor, S., & Todd, P. (1995). An integrated model of waste management
Psychology, 39(12), 2985–3019. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1559- behavior: A test of household recycling and composting intentions.
1816.2009.00558.x Environment and Behavior, 27(5), 603–630. https://doi.org/10.1177/
Rivis, A., & Sheeran, P. (2003). Descriptive norms as an additional predictor 0013916595275001
in the theory of planned behaviour: A meta-analysis. Current Psychol- Tencati, A., Pogutz, S., Moda, B., Brambilla, M., & Cacia, C. (2016). Preven-
ogy: Developmental Learning Personality Social, 22(3), 218-233. tion policies addressing packaging and packaging waste: Some emerg-
Rokeach, M. (1973). The nature of human values. Free press. ing trends. Waste Management, 56, 35–45. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.
Roy, J., & Pal, S. (2009). Lifestyles and climate change: Link awaiting activa- wasman.2016.06.025
tion. Current Opinion in Environmental Sustainability, 1, 192–200. Thieme, J., Royne, M. B., Jha, S., Levy, M., & Barnes McEntee, W. (2015).
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cosust.2009.10.009 Factors affecting the relationship between environmental concern and
Ryan, R. M., & Deci, E. L. (2017). Self-determination theory: Basic psychologi- behaviors. Marketing Intelligence & Planning, 33(5), 675–690. https://
cal needs in motivation, development, and wellness. New York, NY: Guil- doi.org/10.1108/MIP-08-2014-0149
ford Publications. Thomas, C., & Sharp, V. (2013). Understanding the normalisation of
Schahn, J., & Holzer, E. (1990). Studies of individual environmental con- recycling behaviour and its implications for other pro-environmental
cern: The role of knowledge, gender, and background variables. Envi- behaviours: A review of social norms and recycling. Resources, Conser-
ronment and Behavior, 22(6), 767–786. https://doi.org/10.1177/ vation and Recycling, 79, 11–20. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.resconrec.
0013916590226003 2013.04.010
Schultz, P. W., Oskamp, S., & Mainieri, T. (1995). Who recycles and when? Tonglet, M., Phillips, P. S., & Read, A. D. (2004). Using the theory of planned
A review of personal and situational factors. Journal of Environmental behaviour to investigate the determinants of recycling behaviour: A
Psychology, 15(2), 105–121. https://doi.org/10.1016/0272-4944(95) case study from Brixworth, UK. Resources, Conservation and Recycling,
90019-5 41(3), 191–214. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.resconrec.2003.11.001
14 of 14 ARLI ET AL.

Trumbo, C., & O'Keefe, G. (2001). Intention to conserve water: Environ- Wan, C., Shen, G. Q., & Yu, A. (2014). The role of perceived effectiveness
mental values, planned behavior, and information effects. A compari- of policy measures in predicting recycling behaviour in Hong Kong.
son of three communities sharing a watershed. Society and Natural Resources, Conservation and Recycling, 83, 141–151. https://doi.org/
Resources, 14, 889–899. 10.1016/j.resconrec.2013.12.009
Varotto, A., & Spagnolli, A. (2017). Psychological strategies to promote Wang, Z. H., Guo, D. X., & Wang, X. M. (2016). Determinants of residents'
recycling: A systematic review with a meta-analysis of validated field e-waste recycling behaviour intentions: Evidence from China. Journal
interventions. Journal of Environmental Psychology, 51, 168–188. of Cleaner Production, 137, 850–860. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jenvp.2017.03.011 jclepro.2016.07.155
Vicente, P., & Reis, E. (2008). Factors influencing households' participation Wilson, D. C. (1996). Stick or carrot? The use of policy measures to move
in recycling. Waste Management and Research, 26(2), 140–146. waste management up the hierarchy. Waste Management and Research,
https://doi.org/10.1177/0734242X07077371 14(4), 385–398. https://doi.org/10.1177/0734242X9601400406
Vining, J., & Ebreo, A. (1990). What makes a recycler? A comparison of Wilson, D. C., Rodic, L., Scheinberg, A., Velis, C. A., & Alabaster, G. (2012).
recyclers and nonrecyclers. Environment and Behavior, 22(1), 55–73. Comparative analysis of solid waste management in 20 cities. Waste
https://doi.org/10.1177/0013916590221003 Management and Research, 30(3), 237–254. https://doi.org/10.1177/
Vining, J., & Ebreo, A. (1992). Predicting recycling behavior from global 0734242X12437569
and specific environmental attitudes and changes in recycling opportu-
nities. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 22(20), 1580–1607. https://
doi.org/10.1111/j.1559-1816.1992.tb01758.x
Wan, C., Qiping Shen, G., & Choi, S. (2017). Experimental and instrumental
attitudes: Interaction effect of attitude and subjective norm on How to cite this article: Arli D, Badejo A, Carlini J, et al.
recycling intention. Journal of Environmental Psychology, 50, 69–76. Predicting intention to recycle on the basis of the theory of
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jenvp.2017.02.006
planned behaviour. Int J Nonprofit Volunt Sect Mark. 2019;
Wan, W., Zhang, S., Huang, H., & Wu, T. (2016). Occurrence and distribu-
tion of organophosphorus esters in soils and wheat plants in a plastic e1653. https://doi.org/10.1002/nvsm.1653
waste treatment area in China. Environmental Pollution, 214, 349–353.

View publication stats

You might also like