You are on page 1of 8

International Journal of Psychology

International Journal of Psychology, 2018


Vol. 53, No. 6, 426–432, DOI: 10.1002/ijop.12403

Predictors of developing mathematics anxiety among


middle-school students: A 2-year prospective study

Nir Madjar1 , Gil Zalsman2,3,4,5 , Abraham Weizman2,3,5 , Shaul Lev-Ran3,6 , and


Gal Shoval2,3
1 School of Education, Bar-Ilan University, Ramat Gan, Israel
2 Child and Adolescents’ Psychiatry Division, Geha Mental Health Center, Petach Tikva, Israel
3
Sackler Faculty of Medicine, Tel Aviv University, Tel Aviv, Israel
4 Division of Molecular Imaging and Neuropathology, Department of Psychiatry, Columbia

University, New York, NY, USA


5 Felsenstein Medical Research Center, Sackler Faculty of Medicine, Tel Aviv University, Rabin

Medical Center, Petah Tikva, Israel


6
Sheba Medical Center, Ramat Gan, Israel

W hile there is an abundance of research pertaining to the development of anxiety disorders, there is still a dearth
of knowledge regarding the development of anxiety in the general population. The objective of this study was to
longitudinally explore the development of mathematics anxiety among normative middle-school students, and to identify
the moderating role of gender, school transition and scholastic achievements on these trajectories. Subjects included 413
sixth grade students (53.3% females, mean age 11.27 ± 0.38 years). Participants were evaluated for their level of anxiety
in the context of mathematics, using the Value of Education scale, in four time-points with a 6-month gap between each
time-point. Data regarding subjects’ grades and school transition were also collected. A growth curve analysis using
hierarchical linear modelling revealed that girls, students who transitioned between schools and high achievers (each
independently) reported a significant increase in mathematics related anxiety towards the end of sixth grade, which later
decreased during seventh grade. The findings of this prospective study on factors affecting the development of mathematics
anxiety among normative adolescents may be important in planning focused primary prevention school-based strategies.

Keywords: Mathematics anxiety; Educational psychology; Gender; School transition; Normative population.

Worries and concerns regarding academic tasks and some individuals, normal childhood fears may be an
achievements are important aspects of general function- antecedent to the development of future anxiety disorders
ing and behaviour among adolescents. While previous (Muris et al., 2000). Recent findings indicate that, among
research has explored the characteristics and develop- undergraduate students, some levels of anxiety increase
ment of pathological anxiety, less emphasis has been attention and control, but only under conditions of "high
given to levels of anxiety within the general population, cognitive load" (Najmi, Wegner, & Nock, 2007). These
although these may substantially impact prospective findings further emphasise the need to explore the devel-
adjustment and well-being (Eccles, 2005). Therefore, opment and potential implications of anxious symptoms
mental health practitioners and researchers can benefit among students.
from understanding the antecedents and developmental The expectancy-value theory (Eccles, 2005) is a theo-
trajectories of anxiety regarding academic tasks in the retical framework which established a validated measure
general adolescent population. to assess aspects of domain-specific anxiety in the general
Maladaptive worries and concerns may be precursors population. The expectancy-value theory posits that out-
to future development of an anxiety disorder (Muris, come expectancies and the values ascribed to a given task
Merckelbach, Mayer, & Prins, 2000). Moreover, for determine one’s choices and performance in that specific

Correspondence should be addressed to Nir Madjar, School of Education, Bar-Ilan University, Ramat-Gan, 5290002 Israel. (E-mail:
nir.madjar@biu.ac.il).

© 2016 International Union of Psychological Science


DEVELOPMENT OF MATHEMATICS ANXIETY 427

context. One of these values is the subjective psychologi- suggested that this transition is a fundamental crossroad
cal cost one is expected to experience while conducting a in peoples’ lives with implications on several psycho-
given task. For example, the feeling that one is spending logical aspects, such as increase in depressive symp-
too much time or effort, risking social relationships, etc. in toms and decrease in self-esteem (see reviews: Hanewald,
order to properly accomplish a task, would be considered 2013; Symonds & Galton, 2014). Specifically within the
as perceived psychological cost. (Flake, Barron, Hulle- mathematics domain it was found that students reported
man, McCoach, & Welsh, 2015). Our study is focused on lower levels of motivational orientations throughout mid-
the emotional aspect of psychological cost, which include dle school (Middleton, Kaplan, & Midgley, 2004).
the worries and concerns towards a specific task (Conley, However, the effect of school transition on students’
2012; Perez, Cromley, & Kaplan, 2014), within the con- well-being is still under dispute. For instance, although
text of mathematics (i.e., mathematics-related anxiety). students who transitioned at the end of sixth grade
The expectancy-value theoretical framework describes reported lower self-esteem and higher rates of being bul-
several personal and contextual factors that may explain lied by other children compared to students who remained
the development of subjectively perceived psychological at the same elementary school, there were no signifi-
cost and the magnitude of these subjectively perceived cant differences in students’ achievements, school absen-
costs. Factors found to be most significantly associated teeism, suspensions, senses of safety, or positive attitudes
with psychological cost among the general adolescent towards school between early and late transition (Weiss
population include gender, school transitions and previ- & Kipnes, 2006). Therefore, it is still important to explore
ous domain-specific achievements (Eccles, 2005). the implications of school transition in the context of MA,
and especially to distinguish between changes occurring
as a result of normal developmental and changes related to
Gender and anxiety
the school transition itself. Such distinctions may require
Anxiety disorders are generally more prevalent among longitudinal studies following students at similar devel-
women compared to men (Pigott, 1999). Previous studies opmental stages, in which some experience a transition
show that girls report higher levels of mathematics anx- and others do not.
iety (MA) compared to boys; however, when controlling
for test anxiety it was negatively associated with school Prior achievements
performance only for girls (Devine, Fawcett, Szűcs, &
Dowker, 2012), and when controlling for general anxi- Past successful or unsuccessful experiences have a
ety it was negatively associated with performance only great impact on students’ self-concept in general and
for secondary school students (Hill et al., 2016). In other self-efficacy in particular (Bandura, 2001). In the context
words, some personal and contextual factors may mod- of formal learning, grades are the most explicit feedback
erate the relationships between gender and MA. Accord- on the quality of performance; therefore, students’ pre-
ingly, these complex relationships, as well as other related vious performance in school is highly associated with
outcomes, still require additional research (for review, see their worries regarding prospective learning activities
Dowker, Sarkar, & Looi, 2016). (Eccles & Roeser, 2011). When a student experiences
Gender difference also persists in the evaluation of repeated failures over time, an increase in maladap-
self-competence and anxiety in various domains besides tive concerns and anxiety to engage in similar task in
mathematics. For example, as children mature, girls tend the future is expected. Mathematics-related anxiety
to rate their language abilities higher than boys, whereas was consistently found associated with mathemat-
boys rate their abilities in mathematics higher (Wigfield, ics performance—lower levels of anxiety are related
Eccles, Schiefele, Roeser, & Davis-Kean, 2007). Further- with better achievements—although not related with
more, girls and women are generally more likely to expe- verbal-related competence or performance (see review:
rience a decrease in their self-evaluation following neg- Dowker et al., 2016).
ative feedback, ascribe their failures to internal reasons In the current study, we included gender, school tran-
and report higher levels of general academic-related anx- sition and students’ self-reported grade point average
iety (e.g., regarding tests, homework assignments, etc.) (GPA) to examine whether they would be associated with
compared to boys (for review, see Butler, 2014). These MA, as well as predict the trajectory across time.
findings suggest that gender may play an important role
in the development of MA as well.
The current study

School transitions In this study, we followed a sample of elementary school


students from the general population over 2 years. The
Students worldwide experience at least one school tran- sample consisted of two cohorts: a cohort of students
sition during their formal education period. It has been who transitioned to middle school between sixth and

© 2016 International Union of Psychological Science


428 MADJAR ET AL.

seventh grade (i.e., transition), and another cohort in grade (K-8). The transition of most students in Israel
which students continued for the seventh grade in the to middle-school occurs after the sixth grade (Resh &
same school (i.e., no-transition). Dar, 2012); therefore, we first sampled three schools that
The focus of the current study was mathematics, as is represent the population of students who do not undergo
considered a compulsory topic in almost all school sys- a transition, and then matched three schools from a geo-
tems, and a source of stress and anxiety for a substantial graphically close area and with similar socioeconomic
proportion of students (Luttrell et al., 2009). Accordingly, characteristics.
MA among adolescents has been a focus of research,
and several validated measures have been developed. For
Materials and procedures
example, the Abbreviated Math Anxiety Scale (AMAS;
Hopko, Mahadevan, Bare, & Hunt, 2003), which focuses The students completed self-report surveys during class
on students’ worries about activities related to specific
hours, with the guidance of a trained research assistant.
mathematics-related learning tasks (such as listening to
The surveys were administered four times over two con-
a mathematical lecture or starting a new chapter in a
secutive school years: at the beginning of sixth grade
mathematics textbook), and concerns regarding exter-
(October–November 2012), at the end of sixth grade
nal evaluation of mathematical abilities (such as think-
(May–June 2013), at the beginning of seventh grade
ing about or taking a mathematics exam). However, the
(October–November 2013) and at the end of seventh
Value of Education scale (VOE; Battle & Wigfield, 2003)
grade (May–June 2014).
was developed based on the expectancy-value theoreti-
Each survey included several additional measures that
cal framework, and therefore was chosen as the primary
were not in the scope of the current study, taking approxi-
assessment tool. We hypothesised that:
mately 25 minutes to complete. The surveys were anony-
mous and were matched using a unique code that did not
H1. Initial levels of MA would be associated with gender
allow identification of the students to any of the research
(higher for girls), school transition (higher for transition)
or educational personnel. The study was approved by the
and GPA (higher for low GPA).
Head Scientist in the Ministry of Education, which is the
H2. MA will generally increase along time. official body in charge of ethical approval within formal
education system in Israel, in accordance with the Decla-
H3. The increase rate would be higher among girls com- ration of Helsinki (1964).
pared to boys.
Mathematics anxiety
H4. The increase rate would be higher for students who
transitioned between schools. The assessment of MA was based on a 4-item subscale
adapted from the VOE scale (Battle & Wigfield, 2003),
H5. The increase rate would be higher for students with which focuses on student maladaptive worries regarding
initial low GPA. learning tasks. In this scale, students are asked to rank
their level of agreement with items measuring their sub-
jective perceptions of negative thoughts and worries asso-
METHODS ciated with mathematics in school. Students reported their
level of agreement with each item on a Likert scale rang-
Participants ing from 1 to 5.
The scale was originally developed within undergrad-
Participants (N = 413; 53.3% female) were sixth grade uate population and have been utilised in previous stud-
students (mean age =11.27, SD = 0.38) sampled from six ies (Perez et al., 2014); however, items were adapted to
different elementary schools located within an urban area middle-school students as well (Conley, 2012). Within
characterised by high-socioeconomic status according the current sample, it was found to have acceptable inter-
the Israeli National Bureau of Statistics. Three schools nal reliability in the current sample at each time-points
were "transition" (n = 226), and three schools were (Cronbach’s α ranged from .64 to .79). Furthermore,
"no-transition" (n = 187) institutions. The timing of exploratory factor analyses using Varimax rotation con-
transition is predetermined by the Ministry of Education ducted at each time-point indicated that single-factor
and does not depend on the students or parental choice, solutions are best fit to data (a range from 42.7 to 51.5%
which reduced likelihood of selection bias or other a of sun squared loadings). The Eigen values of addi-
priori differences between the two cohorts. The entire tional factor were below 1 at each time-point. There-
education system is divided into districts, and all students fore, the measure yielded a consistent and acceptable
in the same district transit at the same stage—either at construct validity and internal reliability within the spe-
the end of sixth grade (K-6) or and the end of eighth cific sample. To create the final measure, the mean of the

© 2016 International Union of Psychological Science


DEVELOPMENT OF MATHEMATICS ANXIETY 429

four items was calculated for each student (MA ranged a 1–5 scale) regardless of gender and type of school;
from 1 to 5). see Table 1 for descriptive statistics. The correlation
with GPA was consistently negative and signifi-
cant at all four time-points (Pearson r’s ranged from
Independent variables
−.10 to −.34).
Students’ estimation of their school GPA was assessed The non-linear growth curve indicated that
using a single item (assessed at the first time-point at self-reported GPA negatively predicted initial levels
which a student participated) asking the students to indi- (i.e., intercept) of MA (see Table 2 for all curve analysis
cate the range of the grades they typically received, on a results). Gender and school transition were not signifi-
Likert scale of 1 to 4 (1 = D, 2 = mostly C, 3 = mostly B cantly associated with initial levels of MA. Accordingly,
and 4 = A). This single item was adopted from the Mary- H1 was partially supported. When controlling for gender,
land Adolescent Development in Context (MEDICS; see school transition and self-reported GPA, both linear and
e.g., Wang & Eccles, 2013), and students were also asked quadratic slopes were not significant, meaning that H2
to indicate their gender (male/female). Data were based was not supported (i.e., MA did not significantly increase
on self-report measures as the Ministry of Education did over time).
not allow access to students’ school records. Findings revealed that gender, school transition and
GPA were, independently, significant predictors of the tra-
jectories of MA over time (H3, H4 and H5, respectively).
Statistical analyses The significant coefficient of gender for both linear and
quadratic slopes indicated that male students reported
The primary method of analysis was non-linear growth stable MA, whereas female students started with simi-
curve analysis (Hox, 2010) using hierarchical linear mod- lar level, but increased before moving to seventh grade,
elling (HLM) (Raudenbush & Bryk, 2002). The analysis
models the developmental trajectory of each participant
TABLE 1
and it is less susceptible to missing values at random com- Descriptive statistics of MA levels across all 4 time-points
paring to other longitudinal analyses methods. The results (6-month intervals) and correlation with GPA
enable us to identify the linear and non-linear trajectories
across time, while examining factors that were hypothe- Gender School transition
Correlation
sised to predict the initial levels (i.e., intercepts) and the Male Female Yes No with GPA
trajectories (i.e., slopes).
Baseline 1.92 (.75) 1.83 (.62) 1.85 (.70) 1.90 (.67) −.33***
Equation (1) describes the Level 1 model, in which 6 Months 1.86 (.72) 1.98 (.66) 1.98 (.73) 1.88 (.66) −.34***
linear and quadratic slopes are modelled as predictors of 12 Months 1.94 (.69) 2.03 (.80) 2.04 (.80) 1.93 (.70) −.10*
MA across time (centred on the first time-point). Equation 18 Months 1.85 (.71) 1.89 (.76) 1.85 (.77) 1.89 (.71) −.33***
(2) describes the Level 2 model of the intercept, in which Note: Data are presented as mean (SD).
gender (0 for boys and 1 for girls), school transition (0 *p < .05, ***p < .001; one-tailed.
for no-transition and 1 for transition) and GPA (grand
mean centred) are the independent variables. Equation (3) TABLE 2
describes the Level 2 model for both slopes, in which Growth curve analysis coefficients of the study variables
gender, transition and GPA were modelled as predictors
Coefficients (SE)
of the slopes (Level 2 moderators).
For intercept
[MA] = π0 + π1 (Linear slope) Intercept 1.95 (0.07)***
Gender −0.10 (0.07)
+ π2 (Quadratic slope) + e (1) Transition −0.04 (0.07)
GPA −0.35 (0.06)***
π0 = β00 + β01 (Gender) + β02 (Transition) For linear slope
Intercept −0.13 (0.09)
+ β03 (GPA) + r0 (2) Gender 0.22 (0.10)*
Transition 0.23 (0.10)*
Πx = βx0 + βx1 (Gender) + βx2 (Transition) GPA 0.18 (0.07)**
For quadratic slope
+ βx3 (GPA) + r1 (3) Intercept 0.04 (0.03)
Gender −0.06 (0.03)*
Transition −0.08 (0.03)**
RESULTS GPA −0.06 (0.02)**

Note: Gender coded 0 for males and 1 for females; Transition coded 0
The sampled normative population students (N = 413) for no school transition and 1 for school transition.
reported low levels of MA ranging from 1.85 to 2.04 (in *p < .05, **p < .01, ***p < .001.

© 2016 International Union of Psychological Science


430 MADJAR ET AL.

Figure 1. Growth curve of MA trajectory by gender.


Figure 3. Growth curve of MA trajectory by GPA levels. Note: low,
1 SD below mean; high, 1 SD above mean).

criteria for an anxiety disorder. Yet, this maladaptive


emotional state may have significant impact and might
evolve into a clinical syndrome in the future.
Our first hypothesis was partially supported, with data
indicating that self-reported GPA is associated with initial
level of MA. This means that students who reported
lower level of scholastic achievements in school also
reported higher levels of MA. The second hypothesis was
not supported, as MA within a normative population of
elementary school students was found to be relatively
low and stable over the 2-year period of the present
study. Mean-level differences at the beginning of sixth
Figure 2. Growth curve of MA trajectory by students who transitioned grade were not significant when comparing boys and girls,
school and students who did not. or transition and non-transition students. However, the
changes in the average that occurred depended on gender,
remained higher at the beginning of the year and then school transition and self-reported GPA.
returned close to their initial levels (Figure 1). These find- The third hypothesis focused on gender as a moder-
ings reveal less stable patterns of MA among girls com- ator of the development of MA among school students.
paring with boys. Findings indicated that while boys reported similar levels
Similarly, school transition also predicted the lin- of anxiety over time, girls reported higher level towards
ear and quadratic slopes—students from no-transition the end of the school year and at the beginning of the
schools reported stable MA, whereas students from tran- new school year, and then these levels decreased. As
sition schools started lower but increased before moving hypothesised, girls were more susceptible to changes
to middle school, remained higher at the beginning of in their perceived worries at this age, a finding that is
the first year at middle school, and then returned close aligned with the existing literature on pathological anx-
to their initial levels (Figure 2). Lastly, students with rela- iety and gender differences in older populations (Leach,
tively higher GPA started with lower level of MA, which Christensen, Mackinnon, Windsor, & Butterworth, 2008).
increased over time and then decreased to the initial levels Recent studies demonstrated that teachers’ rate male stu-
(Figure 3). dents as more skilled in mathematics comparing with
female students (Robinson-Cimpian, Lubienski, Ganley,
& Copur-Gencturk, 2014), and that they may express
DISCUSSION their approach and opinion of students in their interac-
tion with them. Therefore, boys and girls can react differ-
The current study aimed to provide a prospective obser- ently within the same context. For instance, it has been
vation on the development of MA among middle-school found that in classrooms with higher instructional sup-
students. While many teenagers experience intense emo- port, boys reported higher social engagement compared to
tions, such as fear or worries regarding their performance, girls (Rimm-Kaufman, Baroody, Larsen, Curby, & Abry,
most do not meet the clinical psychiatric diagnostic 2015).

© 2016 International Union of Psychological Science


DEVELOPMENT OF MATHEMATICS ANXIETY 431

School transition also played an important role in evaluation, such as formal clinical interviews, in order to
explaining students’ changes of MA across time, as spec- identify and assess maladaptive perception. Other mea-
ulated (H4). Similar to previous research in the field, stu- sures of MA may also be implemented in order to repli-
dents who experienced school transition were at higher cate our results (see review: Dowker et al., 2016). Second,
risk to have higher levels of MA. Moreover, although MA the study is limited to the normative population of ele-
increased before and right after the transition, it decreased mentary school students within the mathematics domain.
during the first year in middle school. It is possible that Future research may also extend the scope of the study
once students get accustomed to the new setting, their fear to include additional contexts and various populations.
and worries decrease. These findings are consistent with Third, anxiety was relatively stable over time, but the
previous studies that focused on the psychological impact effect sizes were moderate to low. However, it should be
of school transition, generally reporting negative influ- noted that this study is based on naturalistic assessment of
ences on students’ well-being and self-perception at this the outcome variable, which is expected to reveal smaller
particular stage (Symonds & Galton, 2014). When indi- effect sizes comparing to an experimental interventional
viduals anticipate even an expected and common change design or case–control studies of clinical populations.
in their life, such as school transition, this may impact Our study had several strengths. The study is a longi-
their fears and worries regarding mathematics. Increased tudinal investigation of an important aspect of adolescent
awareness by educational psychologists, counselors and functioning with a relatively large sample size, and based
other practitioners at this time may help in early identi- on a solid theoretical framework. We followed a sample
fication of personal difficulties and provide timely inter- of middle-school students over two consecutive school
vention. years, and implemented advanced analyses to examine
Findings regarding student GPA do not support our our research questions.
last hypothesis (H5). Although high achievers were In conclusion, the longitudinal study provides prospec-
more likely to report increased MA levels, this finding tive evidence of the development of anxiety, at least in
appears to be ascribed to very low levels of anxiety at the context of mathematics, among normative adolescent
first-time point, which is then expected to increase over students population. The findings shed light on maladap-
time—known as ’regression to the mean’ (Barnett, van tive worries that may be precursors of clinical anxiety and
der Pols, & Dobson, 2005). The high achievers remained may have developmental implications on the function-
lower on their MA level during the entire study period, ing of normative students. Primary school-based preven-
but they had more malleable perceptions of anxiety. In tive strategies can be constructed to cope with evolution
other words, the GPA may explain the variability of MA of MA.
over time in a way that low achievers reported higher
and more stable MA levels over time. It is possible that Manuscript received February 2016
even those who did not feel generally anxious increase Revised manuscript accepted November 2016
First published online December 2016
their anxiety level towards the end of the school year and
at the beginning of the new school year, and after some
adjustment period it returns to the lower levels. There- REFERENCES
fore, paying attention to students with high achievements
may also be advised as they may be more susceptible to Bandura, A. (2001). Social cognitive theory: An agentic per-
spective. Annual Review of Psychology, 52(1), 1–26.
changes over time, and therefore, should not be ignored
Barnett, A. G., van der Pols, J. C., & Dobson, A. J. (2005).
by education personnel and mental health providers. Regression to the mean: What it is and how to deal with it.
Adolescent students’ choices regarding many aspects International Journal of Epidemiology, 34(1), 215–220.
in life, including future career or personal relationships, Battle, A., & Wigfield, A. (2003). College women’s value ori-
may be determined by specific concerns and worries entations toward family, career, and graduate school. Journal
(Eccles, 2005). Moreover, school is an important context of Vocational Behavior, 62(1), 56–75.
that can shape these individual perceptions and aspira- Butler, R. (2014). Motivation in educational contexts: Does gen-
tions (Eccles & Roeser, 2011). Therefore, this study, con- der matter? Advances in Child Development and Behavior,
ducted in the context of MA during the transition from 47, 1–41.
elementary to middle school, provides important infor- Conley, A. M. (2012). Patterns of motivation beliefs: Combining
mation regarding the developmental trajectory and factors achievement goal and expectancy-value perspectives. Jour-
nal of Educational Psychology, 104(1), 32–47.
that can moderate these trajectories across time.
Devine, A., Fawcett, K., Szűcs, D., & Dowker, A. (2012).
There are several limitations that should be considered Gender differences in mathematics anxiety and the relation to
when interpreting the results of the current study. First, mathematics performance while controlling for test anxiety.
the study is based on a self-report measure that represents Behavioural and Brain Functions, 8(33), 2–9.
the subjective perception of the participants about their Dowker, A., Sarkar, A., & Looi, C. Y. (2016). Mathematics
achievements and levels of anxiety. Future studies should anxiety: What have we learned in 60 years? Frontiers in
aim to use more comprehensive and sensitive methods of Psychology, 7, 1–16.

© 2016 International Union of Psychological Science


432 MADJAR ET AL.

Eccles, J. S. (2005). Subjective task value and the Eccles et al. Najmi, S., Wegner, D. M., & Nock, M. K. (2007). Thought
model of achievement-related choices. In A. J. Elliot & C. suppression and self-injurious thoughts and behaviors.
S. Dweck (Eds.), Handbook of Competence and Motivation Behaviour Research and Therapy, 45(8), 1957–1965.
(pp. 105–121). New York, NY: Guilford Press. Perez, T., Cromley, J. G., & Kaplan, A. (2014). The role of iden-
Eccles, J. S., & Roeser, R. W. (2011). Schools as developmental tity development, values, and costs in college STEM reten-
contexts during adolescence. Journal of Research on Adoles- tion. Journal of Educational Psychology, 106(1), 315–329.
cence, 21(1), 225–241. Pigott, T. A. (1999). Gender differences in the epidemiology and
Flake, J. K., Barron, K. E., Hulleman, C., McCoach, B. D., & treatment of anxiety disorders. Journal of Clinical Psychia-
Welsh, M. E. (2015). Measuring cost: The forgotten compo- try, 60, 4–15.
nent of expectancy-value theory. Contemporary Educational Raudenbush, S. W., & Bryk, A. S. (2002). Hierarchical linear
Psychology, 41, 232–244. models: Applications and data analysis methods (Vol. 2).
Hanewald, R. (2013). Transition between primary and sec- Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
ondary school: Why it is important and how it can be sup- Resh, N., & Dar, Y. (2012). The rise and fall of school integration
ported. Australian Journal of Teacher Education, 38, 62–74. in Israel: Research and policy analysis. British Educational
Hill, F., Mammarella, I. C., Devine, A., Caviola, S., Passolunghi, Research Journal, 38(6), 929–951.
M. C., & Szücs, D. (2016). Maths anxiety in primary and Rimm-Kaufman, S. E., Baroody, A. E., Larsen, R. A., Curby, T.
secondary school students: Gender differences, developmen- W., & Abry, T. (2015). To what extent do teacher–student
tal changes and anxiety specificity. Learning and Individual interaction quality and student gender contribute to fifth
Differences, 48, 45–53. graders’ engagement in mathematics learning? Journal of
Hopko, D. R., Mahadevan, R., Bare, R. L., & Hunt, M. Educational Psychology, 107(1), 170–185.
K. (2003). The abbreviated math anxiety scale (AMAS) Robinson-Cimpian, J. P., Lubienski, S. T., Ganley, C. M., &
construction, validity, and reliability. Assessment, 10(2), Copur-Gencturk, Y. (2014). Teachers’ perceptions of stu-
178–182. dents’ mathematics proficiency may exacerbate early gen-
Hox, J. (2010). Multilevel analysis: Techniques and applica- der gaps in achievement. Developmental Psychology, 50(4),
tions. Routledge: Mahwah, NJ. 1262–1281.
Leach, L. S., Christensen, H., Mackinnon, A. J., Windsor, T. D., Symonds, J. E., & Galton, M. (2014). Moving to the next school
& Butterworth, P. (2008). Gender differences in depression at age 10–14 years: An international review of psychological
and anxiety across the adult lifespan: The role of psychoso- development at school transition. Review of Education, 2,
cial mediators. Social Psychiatry and Psychiatric Epidemi- 1–27.
ology, 43(12), 983–998. Wang, M.-T., & Eccles, J. S. (2013). School context,
Luttrell, V. R., Callen, B. W., Allen, C. S., Wood, M. D., Deeds, achievement motivation, and academic engagement: A
D. G., & Richard, D. C. (2009). The mathematics value longitudinal study of school engagement using a multi-
inventory for general education students: Development and dimensional perspective. Learning and Instruction, 28,
initial validation. Educational and Psychological Measure- 12–23.
ment, 70, 142–160. Weiss, C. C., & Kipnes, L. (2006). Reexamining middle school
Middleton, M. J., Kaplan, A., & Midgley, C. (2004). The change effects: A comparison of middle grades students in middle
in middle school students’ achievement goals in mathematics schools and K–8 schools. American Journal of Education,
over time. Social Psychology of Education, 7(3), 289–311. 112(2), 239–272.
Muris, P., Merckelbach, H., Mayer, B., & Prins, E. (2000). How Wigfield, A., Eccles, J. S., Schiefele, U., Roeser, R. W., &
serious are common childhood fears? Behaviour Research Davis-Kean, P. (2007). Development of achievement moti-
and Therapy, 38(3), 217–228. vation. In Handbook of child psychology. Wiley.

© 2016 International Union of Psychological Science


Copyright of International Journal of Psychology is the property of John Wiley & Sons, Inc.
and its content may not be copied or emailed to multiple sites or posted to a listserv without
the copyright holder's express written permission. However, users may print, download, or
email articles for individual use.

You might also like