You are on page 1of 27

Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 137 (2022) 104633

Contents lists available at ScienceDirect

Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews


journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/neubiorev

Popular interventions to enhance sustained attention in children and


adolescents: A critical systematic review☆
Eadaoin J. Slattery a, b, *, 1, Eoin O’Callaghan c, Patrick Ryan b, Donal G. Fortune b,
Laura P. McAvinue d
a
Centre for Assessment Research, Policy and Practice in Education, Institute of Education, Dublin City University, Ireland
b
Dept. of Psychology, University of Limerick, Ireland
c
Independent Researcher, Dublin, Ireland
d
School of Education, University College Dublin, Ireland

A R T I C L E I N F O A B S T R A C T

Keywords: There are a myriad of interventions promoting activities designed to help enhance sustained attention in children
Attention and adolescents. In this systematic review, we critically evaluate the evidence behind three popular sustained
Intervention attention training approaches – cognitive attention training, meditation, and physical activity. Seven databases
Children
were searched in addition to secondary searches. Cognitive attention training, meditation training or physical
Adolescence
activity intervention studies aimed at improving sustained attention (randomised-controlled or non-randomised-
controlled designs) in samples of children and adolescents (3–18 years) were included. We screened 3437 unique
articles. Thirty-seven studies satisfied inclusion criteria. In general, cognitive attention training (n = 14) did not
reliably improve sustained attention. Physical activity (n = 15) and meditation interventions (n = 8) demon­
strated somewhat more potential in enhancing sustained attention, but these effects should be considered pre­
liminary and need to be replicated with greater methodological rigour. Cognitive attention training
demonstrated very limited transfer to other aspects of attention. Notably, mindfulness training had rather
consistent positive effects on selective attention. Across all three intervention types, there was very weak evi­
dence for transfer to other aspects of cognition, behaviour, and academic achievement. The paper concludes with
methodological recommendations for future studies to strengthen the evidence base.

1. Introduction enhance sustained attention and other cognitive functions in children


have bourgeoned. In this paper, we review the evidence for three
Attention is a cognitive process that is fundamental to learning and common intervention approaches (cognitive attention training, physical
functioning (e.g., Lundervold et al., 2017; Slattery et al., 2022). It activity, and meditation training) for improving sustained attention in
comprises multiple independent but related components (Raz and children and adolescents and provide a systematic review of the studies
Buhle, 2006). Sustained attention (commonly referred to as ‘concen­ in this field.
tration’ or ‘focus’ in everyday life) refers to the ability to continuously
maintain focus on goal-directed activity over time (Sturm and Willmes,
1.1. Sustained attention
2001). From reading a book to listening to a talk, sustained attention is
an elementary cognitive function that underlies more complex forms of
Sustained attention refers to the ability to maintain focus and
attention, such as divided or selective attention, and other cognitive
engagement to task goals over time, particularly in conditions of
domains, such as memory and learning (Sarter et al., 2001). In the last
monotony and repetition (Unsworth and Robison, 2020). Robertson and
few years, studies of interventions promoting activities designed to
colleagues (1997) defined sustained attention as “the ability to

This research has been funded by the Irish Research Council under the Government of Ireland Postgraduate Programme (GOIPG/2018/796).

* Correspondence to: Centre for Assessment Research, Policy and Practice in Education, Institute of Education, St. Patrick’s Campus, Dublin City University,
Drumcondra, Dublin 9, Ireland.
E-mail address: eadaoin.slattery@dcu.ie (E.J. Slattery).
1
https://orcid.org/0000-0002-5513-8635

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2022.104633
Received 23 January 2022; Received in revised form 15 March 2022; Accepted 17 March 2022
Available online 23 March 2022
0149-7634/© 2022 The Author(s). Published by Elsevier Ltd. This is an open access article under the CC BY license (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
E.J. Slattery et al. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 137 (2022) 104633

self-sustain mindful, conscious processing of stimuli whose repetitive, Breslau et al., 2009). Moreover, problems with attention have been
non-arousing qualities, would otherwise lead to habituation” (p. 747). shown to undermine traditional academic interventions (e.g., tutoring;
An important characteristic of sustained attention is that performance Rabiner and Malone, 2004) and early attention problems have even
tends to decline over time (called the ‘vigilance decrement’; Parasura­ been shown to predict graduation from high school (Pingault et al.,
man, 1979). These time-on-task effects have been demonstrated over 2011). Given the importance of sustained attention in childhood, the
both long durations (e.g., Mackworth, 1948) and short durations (e.g., possibility of training sustained attention (i.e., improving the ability to
Gillberg and Åkerstedt, 1998). Another important aspect of sustained pay attention in typically developing children and ameliorating sus­
attention is that attention fluctuates from moment-to-moment. Sus­ tained attention deficits in children with neurodevelopmental condi­
taining an appropriate amount of attention is critical for task perfor­ tions) has received considerable interest and several training methods
mance, but our ability waxes and wanes. Sustained attention fluctuates have been employed. Currently, there are a myriad of attention training
between ‘in the zone’ periods of low response variability and higher interventions promoting activities designed to help enhance sustained
accuracy and ‘out of the zone’ periods of higher response variability and attention.
lower accuracy (Esterman et al., 2013, 2014). At any given moment, our
capacity to sustain attention is determined by a dynamic interplay be­ 1.2. Approaches for improving sustained attention
tween cognitive factors, motivation factors, emotional factors, and
arousal factors (McAvinue et al., 2015). Lapses in sustained attention Two broad approaches for improving attention have been identified:
can occur from a deficit in any one or a combination of these factors, cognitive attention training and state training (Tang and Posner, 2009).
which cause the individual to disengage from the task at hand. Cognitive attention training involves the repetitive practice of a cogni­
A number of separate neural and neurophysiological networks have tive task thought to exercise neural networks related to attention (Pos­
been linked to sustained attention. These include the default network, ner et al., 2015). This type of training is often called attention training,
the frontoparietal network, the salience network, and the locus coeru­ cognitive training, network training or brain training. Cognitive atten­
leus norepinephrine system (Buckner and DiNicola, 2019; Langner and tion training focusses on improving the cognitive control aspect of sus­
Eickhoff, 2013; Petersen and Posner, 2012; Sridharan et al., 2008). The tained attention involving the frontoparietal network with the practice
default mode network is a large-scale brain system, which includes the of tasks designed to exercise this system. The alternative approach, state
medial prefrontal cortex, precuneus and posterior cingulate, lateral training, involves practice designed to develop a brain state that is
temporoparietal cortex and medial temporal lobe. This system tends to thought to influence attention and other networks (Posner et al., 2015).
be active during periods of rest and/or periods of internally-directed This type of training may also involve networks but does not include
thought (e.g., mind wandering) and ‘deactivated’ during cognitive tasks designed to specifically train an attentional network
externally-oriented sustained attention tasks (Buckner and DiNicola, (Posner et al., 2015). A range of methods have been suggested for
2019). The right frontoparietal network plays an important role in the training a brain state that promotes attention; physical activity and
cognitive control of sustained attention (Petersen and Posner, 2012). meditation are two examples of state training approaches (Posner et al.,
This network demonstrates increased activity during externally-directed 2015; Tang and Posner, 2009). State training methods likely put the
sustained attention tasks and decreased activity with time on task (Coull brain and body into an optimal state for sustaining attention. Physical
et al., 1996; Paus et al., 1997). Thus, when sustained attention is activity likely primarily targets sustained attention via physiological
externally directed, the frontoparietal network is typically activated, arousal though may also target cognitive control when the kind of ex­
while the default mode network is typically deactivated. The salience ercise involves a requirement for continued focus on goal-directed
network, which includes the insula, amygdala, dorsal anterior cingulate stimuli. Meditation training likely targets both physiological and
and the frontal poles detects salient information (e.g., information cortical arousal. It may also work through the salience network, calming
related to emotion and reward; Seeley et al., 2007) and maintains the the emotions and enabling a reappraisal of what should be attended to,
focus of attention on relevant stimuli for further processing by regu­ and the frontoparietal network, maintaining focus on certain stimuli
lating activity within other networks (Sridharan et al., 2008). It is while excluding others. In practice and school settings, cognitive
thought that the salience network engages the frontoparietal network to attention training, meditation and physical activity are among the most
focus on goal-directed activity (Sridharan et al., 2008). In addition to popular intervention approaches for enhancing ‘concentration’ or
these neural networks, the locus coeruleus norepinephrine system plays ‘focus’. A recent commentary on behavioural interventions for cognitive
a critical role in sustained attention, subserving arousal. The system has enhancement highlighted the need for well-defined and precise termi­
widespread connections to the frontoparietal and salience networks nology to describe interventions and intervention outcomes (Green
(Unsworth and Robison, 2017). Optimal locus coeruleus norepinephrine et al., 2019). To date, the lack of precision in the literature precludes a
activity is thought to reduce background noise and increase definitive conclusion on whether cognitive attention training, medita­
signal-to-noise ratios, ensuring greater processing of target stimuli tion and physical activity enhance sustained attention capacity. The
(Esterman and Rothlein, 2019). This is consistent with the Yerkes current review provides a fine-grained critical analysis of these sus­
Dodson Law, which proposes that any task will have an optimal level of tained attention training interventions and attempts to answer that
arousal below and beyond which performance will decrease (i.e., there question.
is an inverted-U relationship between arousal and task performance;
Yerkes and Dodson, 1908). 1.3. Cognitive attention training
Sustained attention develops through childhood and into adulthood
with a period of accelerated development occurring in early and middle In recent years, cognitive attention training has been a common
childhood (approximately 6–9 years; Betts et al., 2006; Lewis et al., intervention approach for targeting children’s sustained attention (e.g.,
2017; Lin et al., 1999; McAvinue et al., 2012; Yan et al., 2018). The Rapport et al., 2013). The premise of training is that the repetitive
ability to sustain attention is a key factor influencing children’s aca­ practice of a specific task leads to improvements in the underlying
demic achievements (e.g., Steinmayr et al., 2010). However, poor sus­ neural network linked to the task (i.e., the frontoparietal network),
tained attention is a relatively common problem in childhood (Döpfner which enhances the cognitive function(s) underpinned by the targeted
et al., 2008) and certain neurodevelopmental disorders and learning neural network (Kerns et al., 1999; Tamm et al., 2013). The objective is
disorders are characterised by deficits in sustained attention. Attention that these improvements would be sustained on a long-term basis. It is
problems at school entry (age 5–6 years) have been shown to predict posited that if one’s sustained attention capacity increases, then per­
success in reading and mathematics at the end of primary school (age formance in other cognitive or behavioural domains that are related to
11–12; Duncan et al., 2007) and at the end of formal schooling (age 17; sustained attention should also increase (Peng and Miller, 2016). Several

2
E.J. Slattery et al. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 137 (2022) 104633

cognitive attention training programmes claim to train sustained 1.5. Physical activity
attention in children (e.g., Pay Attention!, Aixtent, Attention Process
Training), including some commercialised programmes (e.g., Tali Train, Physical activity is another popular approach for enhancing sus­
BrainTrain). Typically, training requires children to practice tained attention in children (e.g., Posner et al., 2015). Empirical and
videogame-like attention tasks (among other tasks) delivered via com­ meta-analytic studies have shown broad effects of chronic physical ac­
puters or tablets with adaptive procedures (training task difficulty is tivity on children’s cognition, including attention (e.g., Vazou et al.,
automatically adapted to an individual’s level of performance) and 2019). Several mechanisms have been proposed to explain the effects of
reward systems to encourage motivation. It is widely accepted that physical activity on cognition. From a neurophysiological perspective, it
practice on any cognitive task leads to improved performance on those is posited that exercise stimulates the synthesis and release of neuro­
same activities (Katz et al., 2018), but the extent to which those im­ transmitters such as norepinephrine, dopamine and serotonin which
provements matter for other untrained tasks (e.g., academic achieve­ may improve cognitive functioning (Best, 2010; Moreau and Conway,
ment) is a matter of ongoing debate (Shipstead et al., 2012). 2013). The synthesis and release of norepinephrine is particularly
In a selective meta-analysis, Peng and Miller (2016) investigated the important for sustained attention as it regulates the arousal factors that
effects of cognitive attention training programmes on attention and are required for maintaining optimal sustained attention (Sarter et al.,
other cognitive outcomes (all non-trained academic and cognitive 2001). The positive effects of exercise on sustained attention may also be
measures) in populations of ADHD, learning difficulties and typically attributed to increases in neuro-trophins such as brain-derived neuro­
developing individuals. The results indicated that training improved tropic factor (Cotman et al., 2007; Hillman et al., 2008), which play a
performance on attention measures (objective and subjective combined) critical role in hippocampus functioning, synaptic plasticity, neuro­
compared to controls with a medium effect size (Hedges’ g =.25) and genesis, and neuroprotection (Cotman et al., 2007). However, recently,
transferred to measures of far transfer (Hedges’ g =.24). The findings led some researchers (e.g., Diamond and Ling, 2016) have argued that
the authors to conclude that “attention is a malleable construct” (p. 83). cognitively engaging physical activity (e.g., physical activity that re­
However, the term ‘attention’ is a broad category and has limited quires the allocation of attention and cognitive effort) is more beneficial
descriptive value. This is because attention is not a unitary construct but for cognition than ‘mindless’ or ‘simple’ physical activity (e.g., physical
rather comprises several distinct components (Raz and Buhle, 2006). activity without a cognitive component). Physical activity that is
Thus, training may generate different effects on the various components cognitively engaging (e.g., basketball, which requires children to focus
of attention (e.g., sustained attention, selective attention, divided their attention, scan the environment, anticipate the behaviour of
attention). The pertinent question is then: What aspect or aspects of teammates/opponents and so on) may have more of an effect on
attention are amenable to cognitive attention training? Notably, these attention than physical activity with low cognitive engagement (e.g.,
findings are in direct contrast with an earlier review by Rapport et al. running). This is because physical activity with a cognitive component is
(2013) which found no significant effects of training targeting attention assumed to activate multiple mechanisms simultaneously (Best, 2010).
on attention outcome measures in children and adolescents with ADHD Thus, this type of physical activity may impact arousal factors and
(i.e., no evidence of near transfer) though only 6 attention training top-down systems of attentional control underlying sustained attention.
studies were included. In a recent meta-analytic review, de Greeff et al. (2018) investigated
the effects of long-term physical activity on multiple cognitive domains,
1.4. Meditation including attention (selective, divided and sustained attention), execu­
tive functions (inhibition, working memory, cognitive flexibility and
Another widespread approach for enhancing sustained attention in planning), and academic performance (mathematics, spelling and
children is meditation, particularly mindfulness meditation (e.g., Posner reading) in children aged 6–12 years. However, only one study exam­
et al., 2015). Mindfulness has been defined as “the awareness that ined the effect of long-term physical activity on attention, which used a
emerges through paying attention on purpose, in the present moment, measure of selective attention and found a large positive effect (Hedges’
and nonjudgmentally to the unfolding of experience moment by g = 0.90). Notably, no studies focused on sustained attention or divided
moment” (Kabat-Zinn, 2003, p. 145). During mindfulness practice attention. Overall, there were small-to-moderate positive effects on
meditators typically select a point of focus (e.g., the breath) and direct several cognitive domains as well as on academic performance but there
their attention to that point over time (Bishop et al., 2004). If one’s mind was no significant effect for each subdomain of academic performance
wanders from the point of focus, thoughts or sensations are acknowl­ (mathematics, reading and spelling). The authors noted larger effects for
edged and further processing is inhibited as attention is directed back to cognitively engaging physical activity compared to aerobic physical
the point of focus (Bishop et al., 2004). Mindfulness training may activity.
improve children’s self-regulation as it targets both top-down
(controlled) and bottom-up (automatic) processes (Zelazo and Lyons, 1.6. Criteria for evaluating approaches for improving sustained attention
2012). That is, the effortful regulation of attention that is key to mind­
fulness practice trains children’s cognitive control (e.g., sustained Unfortunately, various methodological issues are prevalent in the
attention is required to maintain awareness of the present moment) literature evaluating the impact of behavioural interventions for
while simultaneously modulating potential bottom-up influences on cognitive enhancement (Green et al., 2019). In an excellent review, Si­
self-regulation (e.g., arousal and motivation). These bottom-up in­ mons et al. (2016) found insufficient evidence for the benefits of
fluences interact with top-down pathways in complex ways to facilitate cognitive training on real-world cognition after finding major method­
the recruitment of top-down processes (Zelazo and Lyons, 2012). ological shortcomings in published studies. The authors recommend a
Recently, Mak et al. (2018) reviewed the efficacy of mindfulness-based set of best practices for cognitive training interventions and use these
interventions for improving attention and executive function in children standards to evaluate intervention studies cited on the websites of
and adolescents aged 5–18 years. The studies recruited child­ various cognitive training companies. These standards included
ren/adolescents that were typically developing, diagnosed with ADHD, adequate sample sizes, pre-registration, active control groups, matched
orphans, in correctional schools/institutions or had reading difficulties. groups at baseline, random assignment, outcome measures fully re­
The authors found that out of 13 studies, 5 studies demonstrated a sig­ ported and analysed, outcome measures different to the trained task and
nificant positive effect of training on at least one measure of attention or blind assessors when using subjective outcomes (for more detail, see
executive function; however, only one study found a positive effect of Simons et al., 2016). While Simons et al. (2016) focused specifically on
mindfulness on sustained attention (assessed using the Test of Variables cognitive training studies, these criteria are relevant for any interven­
of Attention). tion study aiming to enhance cognitive capacity. In this review, we

3
E.J. Slattery et al. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 137 (2022) 104633

evaluate the quality of scientific evidence for each intervention study for 2.2. Eligibility criteria
enhancing sustained attention using these standards.
Studies meeting the following criteria were included: a) randomised
or non-randomised studies that compared an intervention group and at
1.7. The current review least one control group, b) intervention designs with a stand-alone
cognitive attention training / physical activity / meditation pro­
The current review updates and expands previous reviews by gramme aiming to enhance sustained attention capacity (studies had to
examining popular intervention approaches (cognitive attention describe a specific aim to improve attention or sustained attention), c)
training, physical activity, meditation) for specifically improving sus­ samples of children or adolescents aged 3–18 years (this age range was
tained attention in a single paper. We focused on two main questions: chosen to broadly capture the years characterised by development of
sustained attention capacity), including typically developing children
1) Do interventions designed to enhance sustained attention improve and children with neurological conditions / neurodevelopmental dis­
performance on sustained attention tasks? If so, what intervention orders / learning difficulties, d) the intervention was delivered over
methods are most effective? more than one session, e) employed at least one cognitive measure of
2) Do interventions designed to enhance sustained attention improve sustained attention. Multi-domain training programmes (e.g., in­
performance on other cognitive tasks (e.g., executive functions, ac­ terventions training sustained, selective and divided attention) could
ademic outcomes) and behavioural measures (e.g., ADHD also be included. Multiple papers based on the same study were
symptoms)? compiled and included. For example, if multiple manuscripts reported
data from the same trial, they were compiled and included for full
Question 1 addresses whether attention training interventions are evaluation. Studies were excluded when: a) samples comprised psychi­
effective in improving performance on sustained attention tasks. atric patients (psychiatric groups can have sustained attention diffi­
Examining whether cognitive attention training, exercise and medita­ culties for a variety of reasons other than weaknesses in the cognitive
tion improves performance on measures of sustained attention is of systems underpinning sustained attention and, thus, were excluded from
theoretical importance because it provides insight into whether sus­ the review to make the groups more comparable), b) pre- and post- data
tained attention is malleable. Question 2 addresses transfer to other for sustained attention was not reported or different measures of sus­
cognitive, academic and behavioural domains. This question is partic­ tained attention were used at pre- and post-test, c) they were written in a
ularly relevant for practitioners, teachers, and applied researchers language other than English, d) training effectiveness in enhancing
designing interventions targeting real-world outcomes/clinically rele­ sustained attention capacity could not be established due to explicit
vant outcomes. To answer these questions, we synthesise studies of multicomponent interventions (this is because their design does not
cognitive attention training, physical activity and meditation that aim to permit determining the effect of each stand-alone intervention, exam­
improve sustained attention in children and adolescents. We then ples of such interventions were cognitive attention training + physical
discuss evidence for improvements in sustained attention and other as­ activity or cognitive attention training + strategy-based training or
pects of cognition/behaviour from these attention training approaches mindfulness training + parent training) or e) they examined the atten­
and identify whether sustained attention and other cognitive/behav­ tion training technique (used for treating repetitive negative thinking)
ioural skills can be enhanced. Lastly, we make recommendations for or attention bias modification training (used to modify attention bias to
future research. threat).

2. Method
2.3. Screening
The protocol for this review was registered with PROSPERO:
CRD42020193922. Following de-duplication in EndNote, the titles and abstracts of all
studies were screened. Screening was conducted using Rayyan software.
An initial sample of 10% were independently screened by two reviewers
2.1. Search strategy (ES, EOC) to ensure consistency in the screening process (n = 260).
Inter-rater reliability was high (Cohen’s Kappa = 0.86). The remaining
The following bibliographic databases were systematically searched studies were single screened by ES. Where the title and abstract met the
on June 19, 2020: EMBASE (via Elsevier), Medline (via Ovid), Medline criteria (or if this was unclear), the full text article was retrieved and
(via EBSCO), APA PsychInfo (via EBSCO), The Cochrane Library (via screened. Full-text screening was undertaken by ES and a random
Cochrane Library), Web of Science Core Collection (via ISI), and sample of articles (10%) was checked by another reviewer (EOC). Any
PubMed (via National Library of Medicine). The search strategy took the disagreements between the reviewers regarding study eligibility were
following form: (terms for physical activity) or (terms for meditation) or resolved through discussion and consensus. A second reviewer (EOC)
(terms for cognitive attention training) and (terms for sustained atten­ was consulted if study eligibility was unclear at any stage. The screening
tion) and (terms for training). No time restrictions were placed on the process is graphically represented in Fig. 1.
search strategy. Supplementary searches were also performed. A grey
literature search was conducted via Open Grey and the references of
included studies/relevant systematic reviews were hand searched for 2.4. Data extraction
studies. Where possible, authors of registered trials, protocol papers,
conference proceedings were contacted to identify any unpublished Data from the included studies were extracted by ES and entered into
studies. Searches were updated on June 5, 2021, using the same search an Excel spreadsheet. Under the following headings: publication details,
strategy as the original search. The reproducible searches for all data­ methods, number of participants, participant characteristics, interven­
base searches are available at https://osf.io/8m2nh/. All identified tion, comparator, setting, statistical analyses, outcomes, results and
studies were loaded into EndNote. other.

4
E.J. Slattery et al. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 137 (2022) 104633

Fig. 1. Prisma flow.

2.5. Quality assessment reported at least one positive near transfer effect to a measure of sus­
tained attention; however, of these, five studies (1, 7, 10, 13, 14) had
The PEDro scale was used to assess the quality of all included full text serious methodological limitations. Six studies (2, 5, 8, 9, 11, 12) found
studies. ES completed quality assessment. The scale is widely used to no statistically significant effects of training on sustained attention.
rate the methodological quality of clinical trials included in systematic Three studies with few methodological limitations (3, 4, 6) reported
reviews across health and medical research (Cashin and McAuley, statistically significant improvements in sustained attention following
2019). This scale was chosen because it can be used to evaluate many training. Study 3 found that the AKL-T01 programme designed to train
different types of study designs (e.g., RCT, cluster RCT, crossover RCT). attention and cognitive control led to improvements in sustained
It comprises 11 items assessing external validity (item 1), internal val­ attention, assessed using the TOVA Attention Performance Index, in a
idity (items 2–9) and statistical reporting (items 10–11). Items are rated sample of children with ADHD (N = 348; Kollins et al., 2020). This study
yes (1) or no (0). A total score is computed by adding the ratings of items has many strengths (e.g., randomised design, large sample size, active
2–11. Scores range from 0 to 10 with higher scores indicating greater control, prospective registration, participants with deficits in cognitive
methodological quality. Total PEDro scores are included in Table 3 (see attention, between group statistical comparisons) and provides the
Appendix A for each study’s ratings on individual items). strongest evidence for an improvement in sustained attention following
cognitive attention training. Study 4 reported that the Tali Train atten­
3. Review and discussion tion training group demonstrated greater improvements in sustained
attention from baseline to follow up compared to the passive control
In this section, we summarise the results of studies evaluating three group (Kirk et al., 2019). In this study, typically developing children
popular intervention approaches (cognitive attention training, medita­ (N = 107) were cluster randomised to the training group, active control,
tion, physical activity) for enhancing sustained attention and discuss or passive control. The Tali Train group completed adaptive game-based
whether certain methodological concerns apply. The evaluation of exercises designed to train sustained, selective, and executive attention.
methodology is based on criteria outlined by Simons et al. (2016), which The active control completed non-adaptive game-based exercises that
is described in the section ‘Criteria for Evaluating Approaches for required minimal attention skills. Notably, there was no improvements
Improving Sustained Attention’ (see Introduction). Table 1 provides the in sustained attention in the intervention group relative to the active
details of the 37 studies included in the review and Table 2 lists the control group. That is, the active control who received no cognitive
relevant measures included in each study. Table 3 indicates the extent of attention training demonstrated improvements in sustained attention.
positive effects on sustained attention and other cognitive/behaviour­ Thus, this study provides limited evidence that cognitive attention
al/academic outcome measures. In the table, we use colours to represent training is effective in improving sustained attention. Study 6 found that
whether the intervention yielded 1) positive effects with few methodo­ participants (N = 129) who received NeuroTracker training demon­
logical concerns (dark blue), 2) positive effects with significant meth­ strated improvements in sustained attention, assessed using d prime
odological concerns (striped blue), 3) no effects with few scores from the Conners’ Continuous Performance Test 3, compared to
methodological concerns (dark yellow) or 4) no effects with significant participants in both the active and passive control groups (Tullo et al.,
methodological concerns (striped yellow). 2018). However, it is worth noting that an improvement was reported
on only one outcome (d prime) out of several possible outcome variables
on the CPT-3. Therefore, considering the inconsistent findings and
3.1. Summary of intervention study results
methodological weaknesses of the studies evaluating cognitive attention
training, the evidence supporting cognitive attention training for
An overview of each study can be found in Appendix B.
improving sustained attention is sparse.
Seven studies (1, 4, 5, 8, 11, 13, 14) examined near transfer effects to
3.1.1. Cognitive attention training studies
other measures of attention (e.g., selective attention, divided attention).
In total, fourteen studies examined cognitive attention training in
Four of these studies (1, 4, 8, 14) found evidence for such effects on
children and adolescents. Eight of these studies (1, 3, 4, 6, 7, 10, 13, 14)

5
E.J. Slattery et al.
Table 1
Characteristics of intervention studies.
ID First Year Sample (N) Age % Place Design Intervention (n) Setting Duration (minutes) Comparator (n) Follow up
Author (years) Male

Cognitive Attention Training


1 Nejati 2021 ADHD 8–14 53% Iran RCT (2) Program for Attention Rehabilitation Hospital 600–750 min (12–15 1. Passive control (n = 15)
(N = 30) & Strengthening (PARS; n = 15): sessions over 4–5
Adaptive, non-computerised tasks weeks)
targeting sustained attention,
selective attention and shifting
attention.
2 Slattery 2021 Typically 9–11 50% Ireland Cluster Keeping Score! (n = 18): Training School 270 min (18 sessions 1. Active control (n = 18): Same 6 weeks
developing RCT (2) was based on sustained updating and over 6 weeks) as the training group except the
(N = 36) required participants to silently keep score was called out by the
score while playing table tennis. researcher as each point was
won.
3 Kollins 2020 ADHD 8–12 71% United RCT (2) AKL-T01 (n = 180): Adaptive, video Home 500 min (20 sessions 1. Active control (n = 168):
(N = 348) States game-based exercises designed to over 4 weeks) Online activities that required
train to attention and cognitive participants to spell as many
control administered via a tablet. words as possible, by
connecting letters in a game-
like grid.
4 Kirk 2019 Typically 5–9 61% Australia Cluster Tali Train (n = 39): Adaptive, tablet- School 500 min (25 sessions 1. Active control (n = 35): Non- 6 months
developing RCT (3) based exercises designed to train over 5 weeks) adaptive, tablet-based exercises
(N = 107) sustained, selective, and executive that required minimal
attention. attention.
2. Passive control (n = 33)
5 Bikic 2018 ADHD 6–13 84% Denmark RCT (2) ACTIVATE™ Cognitive Training Home 1920 min (48 1. Passive control (n = 35) 24 weeks
(N = 70) (n = 35): Computerised exercises sessions over 8
6

designed to train multiple cognitive weeks)


functions.
6 Tullo 2018 Neurodev 6–18 71% Canada RCT (3) 3D Multiple Object-Tracking School 105 min (15 sessions 1. Active control (n = 43):
disorders Paradigm, NeuroTracker (n = 43): over 5 weeks) Computer game 2048.
(N = 129) Adaptive training designed to train 2. Passive control (n = 43)
visual attention including sustained
and selective attention.
7 Bikic 2017 ADHD 15.6 75% Denmark RCT (2) Scientific Brain Training (n = 9): Home 1050 min (35 1. Active control (n = 9): Non-
(N = 18) (0.99) Adaptive computerised programme sessions over 7 adaptive version of Tetris.
designed to train multiple cognitive weeks)
functions.

Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 137 (2022) 104633


8 Kirk 2016 IDD and 4–11 60% Australia RCT (2) Tali Train (n = 38): See study 1 for Home 500 min (25 sessions 1. Active control (n = 38): See 3 months
attentional description. over 5 weeks) study 1 for description.
difficulties
(N = 76)
9 Mishra 2016 ADHD 12 (1.9) 93% India RCT (2) Online Neuroplasticity Targeted Home 1800 min (60 1. Active control (n = 10):
(N = 31) Remediation of Attention Deficits in sessions over 6 Played the Hoyle puzzle game
Children (ONTRAC; n = 21): Online months) online.
adaptive exercises that either focused
on attended-signal training or
distractor-suppression training.
Exercises trained sustained attention,
working memory and response
inhibition.
10 Sarzyńska 2017 Typically 8 43% Poland CT (2) Adaptive computerised tasks NR 300 min (10 sessions) 1. Active control (n = 24): 3 months
developing designed to train multiple attentional Adaptive computerised tasks (intervention
(N = 54) functions (n = 30) that targeted problem solving group only)
(continued on next page)
E.J. Slattery et al.
Table 1 (continued )
ID First Year Sample (N) Age % Place Design Intervention (n) Setting Duration (minutes) Comparator (n) Follow up
Author (years) Male

and perceptual skills and


required minimal attention.
11 Schrieff- 2017 Low SES 7–13 80% South CT (3) Pay Attention! (n = 5): Non- School 900 min (20 sessions 1. Active control (n = 5). –
Elson (N = 15) Africa computerised tasks designed to train over 10 weeks) Played physical games like
sustained, selective, alternating, and hopscotch.
divided attention. 2. Passive control (n = 5)
12 Rueda 2012 Typically 64.7 54% Spain RCT (2) Adaptive computerised exercises School 450 min (10 sessions 1. Active control (n = 18). 2 months
developing (3.2) designed to train tracking, over 5 weeks) Watched cartoons.
(N = 37) months discrimination, conflict resolution,
inhibition, and sustained attention
(n = 19).
13 Tucha 2011 ADHD 10.9 69% Germany RCT (2) Aixtent (n = 16): Adaptive NR 360 min (8 sessions 1. Active control (n = 16) –
(N = 32) computerised programme designed to over 4 weeks) Adaptive non-computerised
trained sustained, selective, and visual perception training.
divided attention.
14 Kerns 1999 ADHD 7–11 57% Canada CT (2) Pay Attention! (n = 7): Adaptive, School or 480 min (16 sessions 1. Active control (n = 7): –
(N = 14) non-computerised exercises targeting lab over 8 weeks) Played computer games.
sustained, selective, alternating, and
divided attention.
Meditation Training
15 Bauer 2020 Typically 11.76 30% United RCT (2) Aimed to target focused attention and School 1440 min (32 1. Active control (n = 16): –
developing (.40) States change students’ mindsets about their sessions over 8 Computerised coding
(n = 40) stress and beliefs using mindfulness weeks) programme.
exercises requiring participants to
attend to the present moment and re-
focus their attention (n = 15).
7

16 Mishra 2020 History of 10–18 60% India Cluster Internal attention intervention After 900 min (30 sessions 1. External attention 1 year (for
neglect RCT (3) (n = 15): Practiced attending to the school over 6 weeks) intervention (n = 15): Adaptive behaviour and
(N = 45) sensations of their breath using the group computerised exercises from academic
Meditrain app. brainhq.com targeting focused measures)
attention, divided attention,
selective attention and working
memory.
2. Passive control (n = 15)
17 Tarrasch 2018 Typically 8–11 50% Israel CT (2) MBSR (n = 58): Exercises aimed to School 450 min (10 sessions Passive control (n = 43) –
developing raise children’s awareness of physical over 10 weeks)
(N = 101) processes, feelings, and thoughts.

Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 137 (2022) 104633


18 Kiani 2017 Females with 13–15 0% Iran RCT (2) Mindfulness meditation training NR 720 min (8 sessions Passive control (n = 15) –
ADHD (n = 15): Sessions focused on over 8 weeks)
symptoms attention and the five senses.
(N = 30)
19 Wimmer 2016 Typically 10.80 47% Germany CT (3) MBSR method (n = 16): Sitting School Approx. 2700 min 1. Active control (n = 8): –
developing (0.53) meditation and body scan formed the (25 sessions over 18 Concentration skills training
(N = 34) core the training programme. weeks) 2. Passive control (n = 10)
20 Ricarte 2015 Typically 6–13 54% Spain RCT (2) Mindfulness Emotional Intelligence School 450 min (30 sessions 1. Passive control (n = 45) –
developing years Training (n = 45): Exercises included over 6 weeks)
(N = 90) breathing meditation, synchronised
breathing meditation, mandala
meditation, present moment
meditation on sounds and body scan.
21 Leonard 2013 Imprisoned 16–18 100% United Cluster Power Source cognitive behavioural/ Prison 750 min (Approx. 10 1. Active control (n = 117): –
youth years States RCT (2) mindfulness meditation (n = 147): sessions over 3–5 Cognitive perception training
(N = 264) Combined aspects of cognitive weeks)
behavioural therapy with the
(continued on next page)
E.J. Slattery et al.
Table 1 (continued )
ID First Year Sample (N) Age % Place Design Intervention (n) Setting Duration (minutes) Comparator (n) Follow up
Author (years) Male

attentional and response modification


elements of mindfulness meditation.
22 Napoli 2005 Typically 1st-3rd 53% United RCT (2) Attention Academy (n = 114): School 540 min (12 sessions 1. Active control (n = 144) –
developing graders States Exercises included paying attention to over 24 weeks) completed “reading and other
(N = 228) (age NR) breath, movement activities and quiet activities”.
sensory stimulating activities.
Physical Activity
23 Meijer 2021 Typically 9.2 49% Netherlands Cluster 1. Aerobic exercise (n = 221): School 1680 min (56 1. Passive control (n = 430) –
developing RCT (3) Activities designed to target sessions over 14
children moderate-to-vigorous intensity while weeks)
(N = 891) avoiding high cognitive demands.
The focus was on highly repetitive
and automated exercises (e.g.,
running, squats).
2. Cognitively demanding exercise
(n = 240): Team games or exercises
that required complex movement,
strategic play, cooperation between
children, anticipating behaviour and
dealing with changing task demands.
24 Cherriere 2020 Diagnosis of 7–12 22% Canada CT (2) 1. Dance (n = 5): Various dance forms NR 1200 min (20 1. Passive control (n = 4) –
Charcot-Marie- years (African-contemporary, jazz, break sessions over 10
Tooth disease dance, and tap) with the goal of weeks)
(N = 9) addressing the specific clinical needs
of the children. Dance forms were
chosen for their particular emphasis
8

on musical rhythm and their inclusion


of complex motor movements.
25 Duarte 2020 Typically 12–14 NR NR Cluster 1. Qigong (n = 22): Qigong integrates School 40 min (8 sessions 1. Active control: Performed a –
developing years RCT (3) movement and breathing exercises + home over 4 weeks) sham Qigong (n = 22).
children that require and induce a special kind 2. Passive control (n = 22)
(N = 66) of mental state of ‘awareness’ or
‘attention’. This group performed the
White Ball Qigong exercise, according
to the Heidelberg Model of
Traditional Chinese Medicine.
26 García- 2020 Typically 8–10 57% Chile Cluster Active Start Intervention (n = 100): School 1200 min (40 1. Passive control (n = 70) –

Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 137 (2022) 104633


Hermoso developing years RCT (2) Physical games that focused on group sessions over 8
(N = 170) cooperation. weeks)
27 Hedayatjoo 2020 Hearing 7–12 44% Iran RCT (2) Balance training (n = 18): Completed School 540 min (12 sessions 1. Passive control (n = 18) –
deficits years various balance exercises. + home over 4 weeks)
(N = 36) practice
28 Månsson 2019 ADHD and 10–14 85% Denmark CT (2) Target shooting (n = 64) Shooting NR (sessions were 1. Passive control (n = 66) –
ADHD years range approx. 60 min; 1
symptoms session per week for 6
(N = 130) months)
29 Buchele 2018 Typically 4–5th 49% United Cluster Coordinated-bilateral physical School 120 min (20 sessions 1. Fitness tracker only group –
Harris developing graders States CT (3) activity (n = 31): Exercises involved over 4 weeks) (n = 29)
(N = 116) (age NR) bilateral body movement and 2. Passive control (n = 56)
participants wore fitness trackers.
30 Rezaei 2018 ADHD 7–14 NR Iran RCT (3) Yoga (n = 7): Yoga and relaxation NR 1080 min (24 1. Neurofeedback intervention –
(N = 21) years exercises. sessions over 8 (n = 7) 2. Passive control
weeks) (n = 7)
31 Chou 2017 78% Taiwan CT (2) 1. Passive control (n = 25) –
(continued on next page)
E.J. Slattery et al.
Table 1 (continued )
ID First Year Sample (N) Age % Place Design Intervention (n) Setting Duration (minutes) Comparator (n) Follow up
Author (years) Male

ADHD 10.50 Yoga (n = 25). Yoga exercises and After 640 min (16 sessions
(N = 50) (1.05) attention / breath / body awareness. school over 8 weeks)
group -
dance
studio
32 Sabel 2017 Childhood 7–16 46% Sweden Cross- Active video gaming (Nintendo Wii) Home 1500–1800 min (5 1. Passive control –
brain tumour years over sessions over 10–12
survivors RCT (2) weeks)
(N = 13)
33 Gallotta 2015 Normal weight 8–11 NR Italy Cluster 1. Coordinative physical activity School NR (2 1 hour sessions 1. Passive control (n = 69) –
and obese years RCT (3) (n = 83): Focused on improving per week over 5
children coordination and dexterity of months)
(N = 230) participants.
2. Traditional physical activity
(n = 78): Focused on endurance,
strength, flexibility exercises and
9

circuit training for cardiovascular


health.
34 Zach 2015 Typically 4–5 years 51% Israel Cluster 1. Orienteering (n = 44) School NR (9 weekly 1. Passive control (n = 39) –
developing CT (3) 2. Dance (n = 40) sessions)
(N = 123)
35 Spitzer 2013 Typically 12.72 63% Germany Cluster Activities like basketball, soccer, and School 1080 min (36 1. Passive control (n = 20) –
Study 1 developing CT (2) dance (n = 24). sessions over 4
(N = 44) months)
36 Spitzer 2013 Typically 12.36 61% Germany Cluster Games that could be played with 3–4 School 900 min (30 sessions 1. Passive control (n = 33) –
Study 2 developing CT (2) members on each team (n = 55). over 3 months)
(N = 148)

Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 137 (2022) 104633


37 Verret 2012 ADHD 7–12 90% Canada CT (2) Activities such as basketball, soccer, School 1350 min (30 1. Passive control (n = 11) –
(N = 21) years and tag (n = 10). sessions over 10
weeks)

Note. RCT = randomised controlled trial, CT = controlled trial, Neurodev = neurodevelopmental, IDD = intellectual and developmental disabilities, NR = not reported, MBSR = mindfulness-based stress reduction.
E.J. Slattery et al. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 137 (2022) 104633

Table 2 Table 2 (continued )


Relevant measures assessed in each intervention study. ID First Author Year Objective Outcomes Subjective Outcomes
ID First Author Year Objective Outcomes Subjective Outcomes
(CANTAB; 3 measures) measures)
Cognitive attention training 2. Attention Switching 2. Executive function
1 Nejati 2021 1. Sustained attention: (CANTAB; 2 measures) behaviour: BRIEF
Persian Attention 3. Spatial Working (parent/teacher; 22
Registration Test (3 Memory (CANTAB; 1 measures)
measures) measure) 3. Functional behaviour:
2. Selective attention: 4. Spatial planning: Weiss Function
Stroop Test (4 measures) Stockings of Cambridge Impairment scale
3. Attention shifting: (CANTAB; 2 measures) (parent; 1 measure)
Trail Making Test (2 5. Movement/visual/
measures) comprehension
4. Inhibitory control: difficulties: Motor
Go/No-Go Test (3 Screening Task
measures) (CANTAB; 1 measure)
5. Working memory: 1- 6. Rule acquisition/
Back Task (2 measures) flexibility: Intra-Extra
2 Slattery 2021 1. Sustained attention: 1. Executive function: Dimensional Set Shift
Vigil, Cerberus and BRIEF (parent; 4 (CANTAB; 1 measure)
Sustained Attention to measures) 7. Response inhibition:
Response Task subtests Stop Signal Task
(TEA-Ch2; 3 measures) (CANTAB; 2 measures)
2. Short-term memory: 8. Reaction Time
Forward Span subtest (CANTAB; 2 measures)
WISC-V (1 measure) 6 Tullo 2018 1. Inattentiveness/
3. Working memory: inhibition: Conners’ CPT
Digit Span Backward (1 measure)
subtest WISC-V and the 7 Bikic 2017 1. Sustained attention: 1. ADHD symptoms:
Operation Span and Rapid Visual ADHD Rating Scale
Symmetry Span subtests Information Processing (parent/teacher/
of the Adaptive (CANTAB; 2 measures) participant; 3 measures)
Composite Complex 2. Speed accuracy:
Span (3 measures) Match to Sample Visual
3 Kollins 2020 1. Sustained attention: 1. Functional Search (CANTAB; 1
TOVA (1 measure) impairment: measures)
2. Working memory: Impairment Rating 3. Visual Memory:
CANTAB-Spatial Scale (parent; 1 Delayed Matching to
Working Memory test (8 measure) Sample (CANTAB; 1
measures) 2. ADHD symptoms: measure)
3. Inhibitory function: ADHD Rating Scale IV (3 4. Short-term memory:
TOVA (1 measure) measures) Spatial Span (CANTAB; 1
3. Clinical Global measure)
Impressions- 5. Spatial working
Improvement (clinician; memory: Spatial
measured at post working memory
intervention only) (CANTAB; 2 measures)
4. Executive function: 6. Spatial planning
BRIEF (parent; 3 ability: Stocking of
measures) Cambridge (CANTAB; 1
4 Kirk 2019 1. Sustained attention: 1. Inattention/ measure)
Simple reaction time hyperactivity: SWAN 7. Rule acquisition/
(TEA-Ch; 1 measure) (parent/teacher; 4 flexibility: Intra-Extra
2. Executive attention: measures) Dimensional Set Shift
SART (TEA-Ch; 1 (CANTAB; 1 measure)
measure) 8 Kirk 2016 1. Sustained attention: 1. ADHD symptoms:
3. Visual selective Vigilan task (2 measures; SWAN (parent/teacher;
attention: Balloon Hunt WATT) 2 measures)
+ Hide & Seek Visual 2. Selective attention: 2. Executive function:
(TEA-Ch; 2 measures) Visearch task (2 BRIEF (parent; 9
3. Auditory selective measures; WATT) measures)
attention: Hide & Seek 3. Attentional control: 3. Working memory:
Auditory (TEA-Ch; 1 Visearch task (1 WMRS (teacher; 1
measure) measure; WATT) measure)
4. Verbal working 4. Cardinality: Give-a- 4. Behavioural
memory: Backward Digit Number protocol (1 difficulties: DBC
Span (AWMA; 1 measure) (parent; 6 measures)
measure) 5. Informal/ formal
5. Visuospatial working numerical concepts:
memory: Odd One Out TEMA-3 (1 measure)
(AWMA; 1 measure) 6. Receptive vocabulary:
6. Numeracy: Test of PPVT-4 (1 measure)
Early Mathematics 7. Phonological abilities:
Ability (1 measure) Letter Knowledge and
5 Bikic 2018 1. Sustained attention: 1. ADHD symptoms: Rhyme Detection (PAT;
Rapid Visual ADHD Rating Scale IV 1 measure)
Information Processing (parent/teacher; 8 9 Mishra 2016 1. Sustained attention: 1. ADHD symptoms:
TOVA (1 measure) ADHD Rating Scale
(continued on next page)

10
E.J. Slattery et al. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 137 (2022) 104633

Table 2 (continued ) Table 2 (continued )


ID First Author Year Objective Outcomes Subjective Outcomes ID First Author Year Objective Outcomes Subjective Outcomes

2. Response inhibition: (parent; 1 measure) WISC-III (1 measure)


TOVA (1 measure) 2. Illness severity: and Coding subtest
3. Short-term memory: Clinical Global WISC-III (1 measure)
Spatial Capacity Task (1 Impression of Severity 5. Impulsivity/executive
measure), Verbal of Illness Scale function: Matching
Capacity Task (1 (clinician; 1 measure) Familiar Figures Test (2
measure) measures)
4. Executive function: 6. Inhibition/executive
Stroop Colour and Word function/selective
Interference Test (1 attention: Day-Night
measure) Stroop Test (1 measure)
10 Sarzyńska 2017 1. Attention: d2 7. Academic efficacy:
Attention test (7 Age- and grade-
measures) appropriate arithmetic
2. Intelligence: Raven’s problems (1 measure)
Progressive Matrices (1 8. Visual-spatial ability:
measure) Hooper Visual
11 Schrieff- 2017 1. Sustained attention: Organisation Test (1
Elson Score! (TEA-Ch; 1 measure)
measure) and Numbers Meditation Training
Forward (CMS; 1 15 Bauer 2020 1. Sustained attention:
measure) SART (1 measure)
2. Selective attention: 2. Response inhibition:
Sky Search (TEA-Ch; 3 SART (1 measure)
measures) 3. Seed of response:
3. Attentional control: SART (2 measures)
Opposite Worlds and 16 Mishra 2020 1. Sustained attention: 1. ADHD symptoms:
Creature Counting (TEA- Test of Variables of ADHD-RS IV (caregiver;
Ch; 3 measures) Attention (1 measure) 2 measures)
4. Divided attention: Sky 2. Interference 2. Academic
Search DT (TEA-Ch; 1 resolution: Flanker Test performance: APRS
measure) (1 measure) (teacher; 1 measure)
5. Inhibition/Switching: 17 Tarrasch 2018 1. Sustained attention:
Inhibition NEPSY-II (1 CPT (3 measures)
measure) and switching 2. Selective attention:
NEPSY-II (1 measure) Conjunctive Search task
6. Working memory: (6 measures)
Numbers Backwards 18 Kiani 2017 1. Sustained attention:
(CMS; 1 measure) CPT (1 measure)
12 Rueda 2012 1. Attention: ANT (3 2. Impulsivity: CPT (1
measure) measure)
2. Delay gratification: 3. Working memory:
Delay of gratification Digit Span and Letter-
task (4 measures) Number Sequencing
3. Decision making: subtests of the WISC-V (5
Children gambling task measures)
(2 measures) 4. Inhibition: Stroop-
4. Intelligence: K-BIT (2 Word-Colour
measures) Interference Test (1
13 Tucha 2011 1. Tonic alertness (TAP; measure)
2 measures) 5. Planning: Tower of
2. Phasic alertness (TAP; London Test (1 measure)
2 measures) 19 Wimmer 2016 1. Sustained attention:
3. Vigilance (TAP; 3 Vigilance test (3
measures) measures)
4. Selective attention 2. Cognitive flexibility:
(TAP; 3 measures) Reversible Figures task
5. Divided attention and WCST-64 (2
(TAP; 3 measures) measures)
6. Flexibility (TAP; 2 3. Inhibition: Stroop
measures) Color Word Interference
14 Kerns 1999 1. Auditory sustained Inattention/impulsivity: Test (2 measures)
attention: Attention ADDES (parent/ 4. Information
Capacity Test (1 teacher; 2 measures) processing: Recognition
measure) task and Visual Search
2. Visual sustained task (7 measures)
attention: Underlying 20 Ricarte 2015 1. Sustained attention/
Task subtest 14 (1 eye-hand coordination
measure) and Children’s speed: TMT Part A (1
CPT (2 measures) measure)
3. Selective attention: 2. Mental flexibility:
Underlying Task subtest TMT Part B (1 measure)
2 and 4 (1 measure) 3. Focused attention:
4. Planning/attention: Perception of Differences
Mazes subtests WISC-III Test-Faces (1 measure)
(1 measure), Digit Span 4. Memory span/
(continued on next page)

11
E.J. Slattery et al. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 137 (2022) 104633

Table 2 (continued ) Table 2 (continued )


ID First Author Year Objective Outcomes Subjective Outcomes ID First Author Year Objective Outcomes Subjective Outcomes

sustained attention/ R; 1 measure)


working memory: Digit 3. Short-term memory/
Span (WISC-III; 3 attention: Letter Number
measures) Sequencing Test (WISC-
21 Leonard 2013 1. Attentional networks R; 1 measure)
(alerting, orienting, 4. Short-term memory/
conflict monitoring): attention/hand-eye
ANT (9 measures) coordination: Coding
22 Napoli 2005 1. Sustained attention: 1. ADHD symptoms: Test (WISC-R; 1
Score! subtest, Walk ADD-H Comprehensive measure)
Don’t Walk subtest and Teacher Rating Scale (4 31 Chou 2017 1. Sustained attention/
Code Transmission measures) selective attention:
subtest (TEA-Ch; 1 Visual Pursuit Test (2
measure) measures)
2. Selective attention: 2. Discrimination:
Sky Search and Map Determination Test (2
Mission (TEA-Ch; 1 measures)
measure) 32 Sabel 2017 1. Sustained attention: 1. Execution of
Physical Activity Conners’ CPT II (1 Activities of Daily
23 Meijera 2021 1. Attention/sustained measure) Living: AMPS (clinician;
attention: ANT (5 2. Disinhibition: 2 measures)
measures) Conners’ CPT II (1
2. Verbal short-term/ measure)
working memory: Digit 3. Mean reaction time:
Span Task (2 measures) Conners’ CPT II (1
3. Visuospatial short- measure).
term/working memory: 4. Selective attention:
Grid Task (2 measures) Map Mission (1 measure)
4. Inhibition: Stop Signal 5. Visual attention:
Task (1 measure) Visual Scanning (D-
24 Cherriere 2020 1. Sustained attention: KEFS; 1 measure)
Score! subtest (1 6. General working
measure) and Score DT memory: Digit Span
subtest (1 measure; TEA- WISC-IV (1 measure)
Ch) 7. Verbal working
2. Short-term memory/ memory: Auditory
working memory: Digit Consonant Trigrams (1
Span subtest (WISC-IV; 1 measure)
measure) 8. Immediate memory:
RAVLT (1 measure)
25 Duarte 2020 1. Attention/ 9. Complex word span:
concentration: d2 Test of RAVLT (1 measure)
Attention (5 measures) 10. Simple span: Spatial
26 García- 2020 1. Concentration: d2 Test span (WNV; 1 measure)
Hermoso (1 measure) 11. Complex spatial
2. Selective attention: d2 span: Spatial span (WNV;
Test (1 measure) 1 measure)
3. Academic grades: 12. General spatial
Mathematics working memory:
performance (1 Spatial span (WNV; 1
measure) and language measure)
performance (1 13. Verbal learning:
measure) RAVLT (1 measure)
27 Hedayatjoo 2020 1. Attention/sustained 14. Immediate recall:
attention: Persian Form RAVLT (1 measure)
of the CPT (1 measure) 15. Delayed recall:
28 Månsson 2019 1. Inattention/ 1. ADHD symptoms: RAVLT (1 measure)
hyperactivity/ ADHD Rating Scale 16. Information: WISC-
impulsiveness: Qb Test (parent/teacher; 2 IV (1 measure)
(4 measures) measures) 17. Copying capacity:
2. Behavioural Rey Complex Figure Test
Problems: SDQ (parent/ (1 measure)
teacher; 2 measures) 18. Copying time: Rey
3. Quality of Life: Complex Figure Test (1
Kidscreen-27 (self- measure)
report; 1 measure) 19. Immediate recall:
29 Buchele 2018 1. Sustained attention/ Rey Complex Figure Test
Harris selective attention/ (1 measure)
concentration: d2 Test of 20. Delayed recall: Rey
Attention (5 measures) Complex Figure Test (1
30 Rezaei 2018 1. Sustained attention/ measure)
impulsivity: CPT (4 21. Recognition: Rey
measures) Complex Figure Test (1
2. Attention/ measure)
concentration/maths: 22. Psychomotor
Arithmetic Test (WISC- processing speed/
(continued on next page)

12
E.J. Slattery et al. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 137 (2022) 104633

a
Table 2 (continued ) Principal components analysis was performed on all measures to derive
composite scores. Six components were extracted: 1) information processing and
ID First Author Year Objective Outcomes Subjective Outcomes
control (information processing speed, lapses of attention and motor inhibition),
implicit learning: Coding 2) interference control (speed and accuracy of interference control), 3) attention
(WISC-IV; 1 measure) accuracy (accuracy of alerting attention and spatial orientation), 4) visuospatial
23. Verbal phonemic
working memory (visuospatial working memory and visuospatial short-term
fluency: COWAT (1
memory), 5) verbal working memory (verbal working memory and verbal
measure)
24. Verbal sematic short-term memory), and 6) attention efficiency (speed of alerting attention and
fluency: COWAT (1 spatial attention to information).
measure)
25. Interference: Stroop
task (1 measure)
26. TMT (1 measure) selective attention, though two of these studies (1, 14) had serious
27. Picture arrangement methodological limitations. Study 4 and Study 8 examined the efficacy
(WNS; 1 measure) of the Tali Train programme in typically developing children (4) and
28. IQ: WISC-IV (1 children with IDD (8). Study 4 reported that the passive control group
measure)
33 Gallotta 2015 1. Sustained attention/
made greater gains in auditory selective attention (assessed using the
concentration: d2-R test Hide and Seek Auditory subtest from the TEA-Ch) from baseline to post-
of attention (3 measures) training compared to the intervention group (Kirk et al., 2019). In
34 Zach 2015 1. Attention: MOXO contrast, Study 8 reported that the intervention group showed signifi­
Continuous Performance
cantly greater improvements in selective attention (assessed using the
Test (4 measures;
sustained attention, Visearch task from the Wilding Attention Battery) from baseline to
timing, impulsivity, and post-training and from baseline to 3-month follow up compared to the
hyperactivity) active control group (Kirk et al., 2016). These contradictory findings
2. Spatial perception: highlight the influence of the task used to assess study outcomes (i.e.,
Reproduction of Patterns
(CMB; 4 measures)
intervention effects may depend on the task used rather than on
35 Spitzer 2013 1. Sustained attention: 1. Study behaviour: enhanced capacity). Moreover, it also highlights that potential inter­
Study 1 d2 test of attention (4 Teacher’s List for Social vention effects may depend on the sample of participants selected (i.e.,
measures) and Studying behaviour enhancing capacity in children with developmental cognitive deficits vs
2. Academic (10 measures)
improving capacity in typically developing children). Two other studies
performance: German,
Math & English (tests not reported improvements in other aspects of attention. Study 13 reported
reported) improvements in divided attention and alternating flexibility following
36 Spitzer 2013 1. Sustained attention: 1. Study behaviour: training though this study had methodological limitations, including a
Study 2 d2 test of attention (4 Teacher’s List for Social small sample size and no correction for multiple comparisons despite a
measures) and Studying Behaviour
2. Academic (10 measures)
large number of pairwise comparisons (Tucha et al., 2011). Study 11
performance: German, reported improvements in the intervention group in attentional control;
Math & English (tests not however, the group comprised five participants (Schrieff-Elson et al.,
reported) 2017). Considered collectively, these studies at best provide limited
37 Verret 2012 1. Auditory sustained 1. Behavioural
evidence for the efficacy of cognitive attention training in improving
attention: Score (TEA- problems: CBCL (parent
Ch; 1 measure) and teacher; 22 other aspects of attention.
2. Visual search: Sky measures) Twelve studies (1, 2, 3, 4, 5, 7, 8, 9, 10, 11, 12, 14) examined far
search (TEA-Ch;1 1 transfer effects (i.e., any other cognitive, academic, or behavioural
measure) measure). Eight studies report far transfer; effects varied considerably
3. Divided attention: Sky
Search (TEA-Ch; 1
across studies. Studies reported improvements in working memory (1),
measure) planning (5, 14), IQ (12), decision making (12), ADHD symptoms (4, 9),
4. Response inhibition: interference (9), inhibition (1, 9, 11, 14), numeracy (8) and academic
Walk/Don’t Walk (TEA- efficacy (14). Some studies reported far transfer effects in the absence of
Ch; 1 measure)
reliable near transfer (e.g., 4). These effects are difficult to explain in the
Note. TEA-Ch = Test of Everyday Attention for Children, BRIEF = Behavioural absence of improvements in the trained cognitive abilities. That is, ef­
Rating Inventory of Executive Function, SART = Sustained Attention to fects of training on measures of far transfer are typically seen as
Response Task, WISC = Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children, TOVA = Test of dependent on increases in cognitive capacity. Thus, in these studies, far
Variables of Attention, SWAN = Strengths and Weaknesses of ADHD Symptoms transfer effects are likely not attributable to the training component of
and Normal Behaviour, AWMA = Automated Working Memory Assessment,
the intervention and may be due to non-specific factors or factors un­
CANTAB = Cambridge Neuropsychological Test Automated Battery, CPT =
related to the intervention. Alternatively, a theoretical account of why/
Continuous Performance Test, WATT = Wilding Attention Battery, TEMA = Test
how cognitive attention training improves performance on measures of
of Early Mathematics Ability, PPVT = Peabody Picture Vocabulary Task, PAT =
Phonological Abilities Test, WMRS = Working Memory Rating Scale, DBC = far transfer in the absence of near transfer is required to explain these
Developmental Behaviour Checklist, CMS = Children’s Memory Scale, NEPSY = findings.
Developmental Neuropsychological Assessment, ANT = Attention Network Test,
K-BIT = Kaufman Brief Intelligence Test, TAP = Test Battery for Attentional 3.1.2. Meditation studies
Performance, ADDES = Attention Deficit Disorder Evaluation Scale, APRS = Eight studies examined meditation training in children and adoles­
Academic Performance Rating Scale, WCST = Wisconsin Card Sorting Test, TMT cents. All of these studies investigated mindfulness-based interventions.
= Trail Making Test, SDQ = Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire, D-KEFS = Five studies (15, 16, 17, 19, 21) reported positive intervention effects on
Delis-Kaplan Executive Function System, RAVLT = Rey Auditory Verbal at least one measure of sustained attention. Though these results are
Learning Test, WNV = Wechsler Nonverbal Scale of Ability, COWAT =
encouraging, all meditations studies had methodological/statistical is­
Controlled Oral Word Association Test; AMPS = Assessment of Motor and Pro­
sues (e.g., small sample sizes, non-randomised designs, no correction for
cess Skills, CMB = Cognitive Modifiability Battery, CBCL = Child Behaviour
Checklist.
multiple comparisons, no long-term follow up). Out of the 8 meditation
studies, five studies (17, 19, 20, 21, 22) examined the impact of training

13
E.J. Slattery et al. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 137 (2022) 104633

Table 3
Intervention effects and methodological characteristics.

Note. WM = Working Memory, EFs = Executive Functions, IN/HY = Inattention/Hyperactive-Impulsive behaviour. Dark blue = positive effects with few methodo­
logical concerns; striped blue = positive effects with significant methodological concerns; dark yellow = no effects with few methodological concerns; striped yellow =
no effects with significant methodological concerns; blank = not tested.
a
Results are split across Kirk et al. (2016) and Kirk et al. (2017) and also reported in Kirk et al. (2020).
b
Results of this study are also reported in Lange et al. (2012).
c
Principal components analysis was performed on all measures to derive composite scores. Some composite scores could not be categorised according to the above
table. The results indicated no statistically significant differences between the two intervention groups and the control group for any of the components.
d
Studies with less than 20 participants in each group were categorised as having significant methodological concerns despite having a high Pedro score. This is because
the Pedro scale does not account for the number of participants in each group.

on other measures of attention. Four of these studies reported positive efficacy of mindfulness in improving attention.
effects on other aspects of attention / other indices of attention,
including overall attention performance (ANT % correct; 21), selective 3.1.3. Physical activity studies
attention (conjunctive visual search task; 17; visual search task; 19; TEA- Fifteen studies examined the impact of a physical activity interven­
Ch; 22). These findings suggest that mindfulness training may be tion. Physical activity interventions included yoga, target shooting,
particularly helpful for improving selective attention. Five studies Nintendo Wii, coordinated-bilateral physical activity, and traditional
examined other outcomes (16, 18, 19, 20, 22), out of which four studies physical activity. Eleven of these studies reported improvements in
reported positive effects on aspects of cognition and/or behaviour sustained attention following physical activity interventions (24, 25, 27,
(ratings of attention or hyperactivity; 16, 18, 20, 22). However, it is 28, 29, 30, 31, 33, 34, 36, 37). One of the most promising of these was
difficult to ascertain far transfer effects due to the small number of far Study 33, which used a cluster-randomised parallel group design to
transfer measures employed in studies and these measures varied across examine the impact of traditional physical activity, coordinative phys­
studies. Overall, these studies provide some promising evidence for the ical activity, and a passive control on sustained attention (d2-R test;

14
E.J. Slattery et al. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 137 (2022) 104633

Gallotta et al., 2015). Improvements between groups were analysed by improvements (Simons et al., 2016). In an effectiveness study, a passive
comparing pre-to-post absolute difference and percentage difference. A control (no-contact control or treatment-as-usual) may be more appro­
main effect of group was found on three measures. Post hoc tests indi­ priate to align with the study’s aims (i.e., measure the extent of
cated that traditional physical activity led to an improvement in pro­ improvement under real-world conditions; Green et al., 2019). Partici­
cessing speed compared to the coordinative group, while coordinative pants in these control groups simply complete pre- and
physical activity led to improvements in concentration and % error post-assessments. In a feasibility study, a passive control may be
scores relative to both the traditional physical activity and control group appropriate as the aim of this study, as the name suggests, is to
(exact p values not reported just p < .05). The other ten studies had demonstrate feasibility (Green et al., 2019). However, of the six feasi­
several limitations including non-randomised designs (e.g., 24, 28, 29, bility or pilot studies, three studies also aimed to evaluate the efficacy of
31, 32, 34, 35, 36, 37), significant differences between the groups at their respective interventions (7, 9, 32). Thus, it is important to consider
baseline on some outcome measures that were not controlled for (e.g., the study goals when evaluating the control groups. From the ten studies
28, 29), passive controls only (e.g., 27, 30, 31, 34) and small sample that seemed to identify as efficacy studies in this review, seven studies
sizes (e.g., 24, 27, 30, 37), which complicate the interpretation of included active control groups (2, 3, 4, 6, 8, 14, 21). Two of these studies
intervention effects. Overall, while exercise-based interventions included both an active and passive control group (4, 6). The inclusion of
demonstrate some potential, further, more rigorous research is required both an active and passive control group can aid in the interpretation of
to definitively determine the efficacy of physical activity in improving study results (Green et al., 2019). From the four studies that seemed to
sustained attention. identify as effectiveness studies, three studies included passive control
groups (17, 18, 30). Three out of six feasibility / pilot studies included
3.2. Issues that impact the comparison of studies active controls (7, 9, 19; Study 19 included both an active and passive
control group). Notably, in several studies there were some differences
Many methodological factors complicate the comparison of study between the intervention and control groups on outcome measures prior
results. In this section, we summarise these factors and explain the to the intervention (e.g., 17, 28, 29) which complicates the interpreta­
impact of these issues on findings. Across studies there was large vari­ tion of intervention effects. Additionally, some studies did not randomly
ability in study types and designs (randomised vs non-randomised allocate participants to groups (e.g., 17, 28, 31). Group differences on
studies, active vs passive control groups), sample characteristics, variables other than those of interest might drive or suppress any effects
training characteristics, experimental protocols, construct measure­ seen in these studies (Simons et al., 2016).
ment, and statistical analyses and reporting. It is our hope that this
analysis will result in better methodology and reporting in future 3.2.2. Sample size
attention training research. Many studies had small sample sizes, which make the results less
reliable. Some intervention groups consisted of 15 or fewer participants
3.2.1. Study types and designs (e.g., 1, 7, 11, 16, 24, 30). This is problematic as a study with a small
It is possible to differentiate between four broad types of intervention sample size is likely to produce an exaggerated effect size, mis­
studies for cognitive enhancement: 1) feasibility studies (identify the representing the strength of an intervention (Button et al., 2013). Large
feasibility of an intervention), 2) mechanistic studies (demonstrate an sample sizes provide stronger and more reliable evidence about the ef­
intervention’s mechanisms of action), 3) efficacy studies (determine ficacy of an intervention (Redick et al., 2015). It has been recommended
whether the intervention results in the desired effect under controlled that intervention studies should include pre-test and post-test data for a
circumstances) and 4) effectiveness studies (determine whether an minimum of 20 participants per group (Redick et al., 2015; Simmons
intervention results in the desired effect under real-world settings/ et al., 2011). Twenty studies satisfied this criterion (out of 37 studies).
clinical practice; Gartlehner et al., 2006; Green et al., 2019). Each type The type of intervention study being conducted may also influence de­
of study is characterised by different goals, which impacts the meth­ cisions around optimal sample size. For example, it has been suggested
odological approaches employed and the conclusions that can or should that efficacy and effectiveness studies require larger sample sizes than
be drawn from the study (for detailed review, see Green et al., 2019). feasibility studies (Green et al., 2019). Further sample size consider­
Note, in practice studies may not neatly fall into these categories or may ations may include practicalities or factors that may limit sample size
overlap multiple categories. Many studies in this review seemed to such as whether a study requires costly neuroimaging outcomes in
identify as either an efficacy study (2, 3, 4, 5, 6, 8, 14, 16, 21, 33) or addition to behavioural outcomes.
effectiveness study (13, 17, 18, 30); six studies clearly identified as a
feasibility or pilot study (7, 9, 19, 24, 32, 37). Notably, in most instances 3.2.3. Construct measurement
the precise type of study being conducted was not clear (e.g., 1, 10, 20, Studies used a wide range of tasks to measure sustained attention.
22, 25, 26, 31). This was particularly the case for meditation and Common tasks included the Conners’ Continuous Performance Test, the
physical activity studies. As such, we would like to echo the call of Green Test of Everyday Attention for Children, the d2 Test of Attention and the
et al. (2019) for more precise terminology at the outset of a paper so that Attention Network Test. Typically, multiple variables were extracted
studies can clearly be distinguished, and the methodology employed from each task such as errors of omission, errors of commission, reaction
evaluated relative to the type of study being conducted. time and reaction time variability. Some studies used questionable
An important consideration in the design of any intervention study is sustained attention tasks, which heavily rely on other cognitive pro­
the selection of a control group or groups. The adequacy of a control cesses. For example, Study 20 used the Trail Making Test Part A to assess
group depends on the type of intervention study being conducted and its sustained attention (Ricarte et al., 2015). This test is typically regarded
underlying research question. In an efficacy study, an appropriate con­ to be a test of visual search and motor speed skills (Bowie and Harvey,
trol is identical to the intervention group except for the “active” ingre­ 2006; Crowe, 1998). The majority of studies measured changes in sus­
dient (i.e., an active control group; Simons et al., 2016). If an efficacy tained attention based on only one task, rather than using multiple
study does not employ an active control, then differences between the measures. Performance on any task reflects variance specific to 1) the
intervention and control group could account for any observed task, 2) its method of administration, and 3) the construct it is intended

15
E.J. Slattery et al. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 137 (2022) 104633

to measure (Redick et al., 2015). Moreover, single tasks can be very evident for meditation studies; all studies examined mindfulness
sensitive to similarities with training regimens and impacted by a training, but studies employed different mindfulness-based in­
component of the intervention despite no change in the underlying terventions. This was also true for physical activity interventions
ability (Moreau, 2021). A more convincing demonstration of interven­ studies, which investigated a range of interventions such as target
tion effects is achieved by using multiple different measures of the target shooting, dance and yoga. Interventions varied on many other di­
construct, which can be used to create a composite score or latent var­ mensions (computerised vs in-person, type of instructions, training du­
iable for analysis (Moreau et al., 2016). However, while this may be rations, settings, etc.). In relation to training duration, studies used a
aspirational, it is important to recognise that the use of multiple mea­ highly disparate amount of training durations. For example, cognitive
sures may not always be possible to achieve in a given study or may attention training studies varied from 105 to 1920min. Some training
prove problematic if, for example, differences in the psychometric durations were spaced across time (e.g., 6 months), while others took
properties of the measures makes their combination dubious. It is also place within a short time frame (e.g., 4 weeks). Thus, many factors may
important to note that when interventions include multiple outcome influence whether one benefits from an intervention; however, these
measures of an underlying construct, and report only one significant characteristics are often not examined. Moreover, very few researchers
effect, claims of improvement are much more ambiguous (Moreau et al., endeavour to replicate a training study. To really understand the
2021). effectiveness of an intervention, replications must be done.

3.2.4. Statistical issues 3.2.6. Sample characteristics


Studies typically included multiple outcomes and tested multiple There was some variability in study populations within and across
comparisons. A considerable implication of multiple statistical tests in intervention types. It is possible that intervention effects may vary
one study is that the significance level of the study may need to be across populations, for example, due to differences in baseline sustained
adjusted to control for the family-wise error rate (Bender and Lange, attention performance. That is, some research suggests those with poor
2001). For example, in a study when two outcomes are analysed inde­ baseline performance may benefit the most from training (compensation
pendently at α = .05, the probability of finding at least one false positive effects), while other research suggests that those with high baseline
significant result increases to 0.098 (Vickerstaff et al., 2019). Many performance benefit the most (magnification effects; for review see, Katz
studies did not correct for multiple comparisons / correct p values for et al., 2021). In the cognitive attention training studies reviewed, seven
multiple outcomes (e.g., 8, 13, 14). Reporting results without correcting studies (1, 3, 5, 7, 9, 13, 14) examined the impact of training in child­
for multiple comparisons can inflate the rate of false-positive results, ren/adolescents with ADHD, five studies (2, 4, 10, 11, 12) had typically
introducing erroneous results into the literature (Simons et al., 2016). It developing participants and two other studies looked at child­
is likely that some of these intervention effects would not have been ren/adolescents with other neurodevelopmental conditions (6, 8). The
statistically significant had p-values been corrected for multiple com­ two most promising cognitive attention training studies (3, 6) included
parisons. However, there is a lack of consensus in the literature about neurodevelopmental samples. Study 3 examined the impact of training
when it is appropriate to correct for multiple comparisons (e.g., Li et al., in participants with ADHD and Study 6 examined the impact of training
2017; Parker and Weir, 2020). The decision to correct for multiple in participants with various neurodevelopmental disorders. Notably,
comparisons may be influenced by the type of study being conducted (e. Study 3 specifically included a sample of participants with deficits in
g., exploratory vs confirmatory studies). That is, the cost of a false cognitive attention. All meditation studies included participants that
positive result in a confirmatory study is much greater than an explor­ could be categorised as typically developing (note, Study 16 included
atory study, which occurs at the initial phase of development and will be participants with a history of childhood neglect). Most physical activity
tested in further studies (Li et al., 2017; Parker and Weir, 2020). A studies included typically developing participants (23, 25, 26, 29, 33,
second weakness identified within the group of studies reviewed related 34, 35, 36); four studies had ADHD samples (28, 30, 31, 37), while three
to the inferential statistics used. Some studies (e.g., 19) reported sig­ studies had other samples (i.e., Charcot-Marie-Tooth disease, hearing
nificant improvements in the intervention group but failed to adequately deficits and childhood brain tumour survivors). The studies reporting
compare improvement in the intervention group to improvement in the the positive effects of physical activity on sustained attention included
control group. A difference in significance between the intervention and typically developing samples (25, 29, 33, 34, 36), ADHD samples (28,
control group (i.e., the training group shows significant improvement 30, 31, 37) and samples of children with Charcot-Marie-Tooth disease
with p < .05 and the control group does not with p > .05) is not the same (24) and hearing deficits (27). However, in the current review, it is
as a significant difference in the extent of the improvement (Vater et al., difficult to ascertain the potential effects of study populations on
2021). Therefore, studies that report statistically significant improve­ meditation and physical activity intervention effects due to methodo­
ments within the intervention group in this manner provide very weak logical limitations evident across studies.
evidence for transfer effects. Lastly, in some instances it was not clear Age is another important factor that may influence the impact of
what analyses were performed (e.g., 22). each intervention (Katz et al., 2021). This is because childhood and
adolescence is a critical period for sustained attention development and
3.2.5. Different interventions and durations cognitive maturation. Many studies included in this review had a wide
Many different types of interventions were employed across studies age range (16 out of 37 studies had an age range ≥ 5 years), which
within each attention training category. In the case of cognitive atten­ spanned more than one period of development across childhood and
tion training studies, 10 different interventions were evaluated across 14 adolescence. Note, 13 studies did not report an age range. Across all
studies, with only two interventions being evaluated twice; Tali Train intervention types, most studies examined participants in middle
and Pay Attention!. Training programmes trained an array of attentional/ childhood and early adolescence (7–14 years; cognitive attention
cognitive functions (though all aimed to train sustained attention). Some training 8 out of 14, meditation training 4 out of 8, and physical activity
primarily targeted attention functions (e.g., Tali Train, AixTent, Pay 13 out of 15). Intervention effects may differ across and within studies
Attention!), while others targeted attention and other cognitive func­ due to age, but this is difficult to examine here due to the wide and
tions (e.g., ACTIVATE™, Scientific Brain Training). This variability was overlapping age ranges. Taken together, it is possible that attention

16
E.J. Slattery et al. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 137 (2022) 104633

training interventions targeting sustained attention may work differ­ produce potential long-lasting effects. Given that these interventions
ently at different ages or with different populations; however, these largely work upon the systems which influence sustained attention (i.e.,
factors were too variable among the studies included in this review to be arousal & salience networks) rather than the sustained attention system
analysed systematically. itself (i.e., frontoparietal network), and given that they likely typically
function to modulate sustained attention, it is possible that enduring
3.2.7. Pre-registration and data analysis plans effects are unlikely. Frequent engagement in these interventions may be
In recent years, there has been an increasing emphasis on the pre- necessary to modulate sustained attention when needed or as a matter of
registration of study protocols and data analysis plans in psychology. routine. However, this is an open research question which needs to be
However, very few recent studies were prospectively registered. Of the determined by future research.
29 studies published since 2015, 13 studies were registered but only 5 of This is a timely review with many strengths (e.g., inclusion of mul­
these studies were registered prior to data collection. Prospective pre­ tiple interventions to enhance sustained attention, critical analysis and
registration is essential for research transparency and credibility. recommendations to improve the evidence base). However, it is
important to acknowledge its weaknesses. Only 10% of title and ab­
4. Overall discussion stracts / full text articles were screened by two reviewers. This was done
due to limited time and resources. While single screening has been used
The present review provides a fine-grained critical analysis of pop­ in other recent systematic reviews in the health and medical research
ular attention training interventions specifically targeting sustained field (e.g., Gram et al., 2019; Sato et al., 2021), this approach can miss
attention in children and adolescents. Findings varied within and across more eligible studies than double screening (Waffenschmidt et al.,
the different intervention approaches. The current evidence indicates 2019). Best practice is for the screening process to be conducted by two
that cognitive attention training does not reliably improve the capacity independent reviewers (Gartlehner et al., 2020). Moreover, only one
to sustain attention in children and adolescents. In contrast, the evi­ reviewer completed data extraction and quality assessment. It is rec­
dence for mindfulness training and physical activity (with a cognitive ommended that more than one reviewer extract data and complete
component) is somewhat more promising though these effects should be quality assessment to minimise errors and reduce potential bias (Bou­
considered preliminary and need to be replicated with greater meth­ tron et al., 2022; Li et al., 2022). In the current review, dual screening,
odological rigour (i.e., pre-registration, larger samples, randomised data extraction and quality assessment would have strengthened our
designs, active control groups and multiple measures of sustained results and conclusions.
attention).
For other measures of attention, cognitive attention training 5. Conclusion
demonstrated very limited transfer. Physical activity studies rarely
assessed such measures. Notably, mindfulness training had rather We found limited evidence that it is possible to train children’s
consistent positive effects on selective attention. Across all three inter­ ability to sustain attention. Cognitive attention training did not reliably
vention types, there was weak evidence for transfer to other aspects of improve sustained attention capacity. The evidence for mindfulness
cognition, behaviour, and academic achievement. A number of positive training and physical activity was somewhat more promising though
effects of cognitive attention training on far transfer measures were re­ these effects should be considered preliminary. Moving forward, medi­
ported (e.g., ADHD symptoms); however, some of these effects were tation training and physical activity intervention studies should adhere
found in the absence of near transfer, which is theoretically challenging to best methodological practices in order to determine the true effect of
to explain. In the case of mindfulness and physical activity, it is difficult these interventions on sustained attention. These practices include use
to ascertain transfer to other cognitive, academic and behavioural of prospective pre-registration and data analysis plans, more precise
measures due to the small number of such measures employed in these terminology around the type of intervention study being conducted,
studies and the measures employed varied considerably across studies. randomised designs, adequately powered sample sizes, matched groups
The primary focus of research in this area has been whether these at baseline, an appropriate control group aligned to the aims of the
interventions work rather than why they might work. The mechanisms study, multiple measures of sustained attention and long-term follow up.
of improvement are hypothesised to be different across each interven­
tion type reviewed here. Cognitive attention training focusses on Declarations of interest
improving the cognitive control aspect of sustained attention using the
repetitive practice of a cognitive task designed to exercise the fronto­ None.
parietal network. Physical activity likely targets sustained attention via
physiological arousal factors but may also target cognitive control when Data availability
the activity requires focus to be continuously maintained on goal-
directed stimuli. Finally, mindfulness training likely targets arousal Data will be made available on request.
factors (cortical and physiological arousal), emotional factors (calming
the emotions and enabling a reappraisal of what should be attended to), Appendix A
and cognitive factors (maintaining continuous focus on certain stimuli
over time) implicated in sustained attention. Thus, in theory, physical See Table A1.
activity and mindfulness training offer the most potential for enhancing
sustained attention because they target multiple factors underlying
one’s capacity to sustain attention. However, notably, none of the
reviewed physical activity or mindfulness studies included a follow up
testing occasion. As a result, it is unclear whether these interventions

17
E.J. Slattery et al.
Table A1
PEDro scores for each intervention study.
Rias of biasing according to the PEDro scale

Eligibility Random Concealed Baseline Blind Blind Blind Adequate Intention-to- Between-group Point estimates / Score
Criteria Allocation Allocation comparability Subjects Therapists Assessors Follow up treat comparison variability

Cognitive Attention Training


Nejati (2021) 1 1 0 1 0 0 0 0 0 1 1 4
Slattery et al. (2021) 1 1 0 1 1 0 0 1 1 1 1 7
Kollins et al. (2020) 1 1 0 1 1 0 1 1 1 1 1 8
Kirk et al. (2019) 1 1 1 1 0 0 1 1 1 1 1 8
Bikic et al. (2018) 1 1 1 1 0 0 1 1 1 1 1 8
Tullo et al. (2018) 1 1 0 1 0 0 0 1 1 1 1 6
Bikic et al. (2017) 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 0 1 1 9
(Kirk et al., 2016) 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 10
Mishra et al. (2016) 1 1 1 0 1 1 1 0 0 1 1 7
Sarzyńska et al. (2017) 0 0 0 0 1 0 0 1 0 1 1 4
Schrieff-Elson et al. (2017) 1 1 1 1 0 1 0 1 0 1 1 7
Rueda et al. (2012) 1 1 0 1 0 0 0 1 1 1 1 6
Tucha et al. (2011) 0 1 0 1 1 0 0 0 0 1 1 5
Kerns et al. (1999) 1 0 0 1 1 0 0 1 0 1 0 4
Meditation Training
Bauer et al. (2020) 0 1 0 1 1 0 1 0 0 1 1 6
Mishra et al. (2020) 1 1 0 1 1 1 1 1 0 1 1 8
18

Tarrasch (2018) 1 0 0 1 0 0 0 1 0 1 1 4
Kiani et al. (2017) 1 1 0 1 0 0 0 1 1 1 1 6
Wimmer et al. (2016) 0 1 0 1 0 0 0 0 0 1 1 4
Ricarte et al. (2015) 0 1 0 1 0 0 0 1 1 1 1 6
Leonard et al. (2013) 0 1 0 1 1 0 0 0 0 1 1 5
Napoli et al. (2005) 0 1 0 0 0 0 0 1 0 1 1 4
Physical Activity
Meijer et al., 2021 0 1 1 1 0 0 0 1 0 1 1 6
Cherriere et al. (2020) 1 0 0 1 0 0 0 1 1 1 1 5
Duarte et al. (2020) 1 1 0 1 0 0 0 0 0 1 0 3
García-Hermoso et al. (2020) 1 1 0 1 0 0 0 1 1 1 1 6
Hedayatjoo et al. (2020) 1 1 0 0 0 0 2 0 0 1 1 4

Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 137 (2022) 104633


Månsson et al. (2019) 1 0 0 0 0 0 0 1 1 1 1 4
Buchele Harris et al. (2018) 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 1 0 1 1 3
Rezaei et al. (2018) 0 1 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 1 1 3
Chou and Huang (2017) 1 0 0 1 0 0 0 1 0 1 1 4
Sabel et al. (2017) 1 0 0 1 0 0 1 1 1 0 1 5
Gallotta et al. (2015) 1 1 0 0 0 0 0 1 0 1 1 5
Zach et al. (2015) 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 1 1 2
Verret et al. (2012) 1 0 0 0 0 0 0 1 0 1 1 3
Spitzer and Hollmann (2013) 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 1 0 1 0 2
Study 1
Spitzer and Hollmann (2013) 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 1 1 1 0 3
Study 2
E.J. Slattery et al. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 137 (2022) 104633

Appendix B. Summary of Each Study (n = 39), active control (n = 35), or passive control (n = 33). The
training group completed adaptive game-based exercises from the Tali
Study 1: Nejati (2021) Train programme designed to train sustained, selective, and executive
attention. The active control completed non-adaptive game-based ex­
Children with ADHD aged 8–14 years were randomly assigned to a ercises that required minimal attention skills. Sessions were held in
training (n = 15) or passive control group (n = 15). The training group school over a 5-week period. All groups were tested on near transfer
received a programme for attention rehabilitation and strengthening measures (visual sustained attention, visual/auditory selective atten­
(PARS) over 4–5 weeks. The programme targeted sustained attention, tion, executive attention) and far transfer measures (verbal/visuospatial
selective attention and attention shifting using non-computerised ac­ working memory, numeracy, and inattention/hyperactivity) at pre-test,
tivities. Both groups completed assessments of sustained attention post-test and 6-month follow up. The results indicated the training group
(Persian Attention Registration Test; 3 measures), selective attention made significantly greater improvements in visual sustained attention
(Stroop Test; 4 measures), attention shifting (Trail Making Test; 2 from baseline to follow up compared to the passive control. In contrast,
measures), inhibitory control (Go/No-Go Test; 3 measures) and working the passive control group made greater gains in auditory selective
memory (1- Back Task; 2 measures) before and after the intervention. attention from baseline to post-training compared to the intervention
The results showed significant group × time interaction effects for some group. No other effects were found for objective measures. A number of
measures including omission errors (p = .001, ηp2 =.424) and commis­ significant intervention effects were reported for rating scale measures.
sion errors (p = .014, ηp2 =.218) on the Persian Attention Registration Specifically, the training group demonstrated significantly greater im­
Test, incongruent stage speed (p = .041, ηp2 =.163) and the selective provements in teacher-rated inattention and hyperactivity from baseline
attention index (p = .036, ηp2 =.170) on the Stroop Test, go reaction to post-training compared to the active and passive control groups.
time (p = .013, ηp2 =.097) and no-go accuracy (p = .021, ηp2 =.196) on Parents reported significantly larger improvements in hyperactivity
the Go/No-Go Test, and accuracy (p = .016, ηp2 =.211) on the 1- Back from baseline to post-training compared to the passive control. Im­
Test. This study had several methodological limitations including no provements were maintained for teacher ratings of hyperactivity at
correction for multiple comparisons (at least 14 different variables follow up compared to the passive control. The study concluded that
analysed), a passive control and a relatively small sample size. classroom-based attention training has select benefits in reducing inat­
tention and hyperactivity but may not promote gains in cognitive or
Study 2: Slattery et al. (2021) academic skills in primary school children. However, this conclusion is
at odds with the underlying rationale of cognitive attention training, as
In this cluster-randomised preliminary study, children aged 9–11 effects of training on behaviour are typically seen as dependent on in­
years were assigned to the Keeping Score! attention training group creases in cognitive capacity. Study strengths include the inclusion of
(n = 18) or an active control group (n = 18). Training was based on both an active and passive control group, fairly large sample size, long-
sustained updating and required participants to silently keep score term follow up and prospective pre-registration.
during an interactive game of table tennis. Both groups completed
cognitive assessments of sustained attention (Vigil, Cerberus and Sus­ Study 5: Bikic et al. (2018)
tained Attention to Response Task subtests of the TEA-Ch2; 3 measures),
short-term memory (Forward Span task WISC-V; 1 measure) and Children with ADHD aged 6–13 years were randomly assigned a
working memory (Digit Span Backward subtest of the WISC-5 and the training group (n = 35) or passive control (n = 35). The training group
Operation Span and Symmetry Span subtests of the Adaptive Composite completed the computer programme ACTIVATE™ at home. Participants
Complex Span; 3 measures) at baseline, post-test and 6-week follow up. completed 40-minute sessions six days a week for 8 weeks. The pro­
Parents also completed ratings of executive function behaviour (BRIEF; gramme trained multiple cognitive functions simultaneously, including
4 measures). There were no significant time by group interaction effects sustained attention, working memory, speed of information processing,
for any objective or subjective outcome measure. response inhibition, cognitive flexibility, category formation, and
pattern recognition. All exercises were adaptive, and the programme
Study 3: Kollins et al. (2020) included an in-built reward system. The results showed no significant
improvements in the primary outcome, sustained attention, or second­
In this large randomised controlled trial, children with ADHD aged ary outcomes, executive function/attentional behaviour after 8, 12 or 24
8–12 years were assigned to a digital intervention group (n = 180) or an weeks. However, the training group showed significant improvements in
active control group (n = 168). Participants in the intervention group one exploratory measure, planning ability, from pre- to post-training
received the AKL-T01 programme designed to train attention and compared to the control (p = .006). This effect was maintained at both
cognitive control using video game-based activities at home over 4 the 12-week (p = .035) and 24-week (p = .017) follow up.
weeks. The active control group completed activities that required
participants to spell as many words as possible, by connecting letters in a Study 6: Tullo et al. (2018)
game-like grid. The results showed a statistically significant improve­
ment in the primary outcome, sustained attention, assessed using TOVA In this randomised-controlled trial, students with neuro­
Attention Performance Index in the intervention group compared to the developmental conditions (6–18 years) were assigned to a training
control. No intervention effects were found for secondary outcomes, group (n = 43), active control (n = 43) or passive control (n = 43). The
including the Impairment Rating Scale, ADHD-RS-IV (3 measures), training group received NeuroTracker training designed to trained vi­
Clinical Global Impressions-Improvement and the BRIEF (3 measures). sual attention including sustained and selective attention. Training was
No intervention effects were found for minor secondary efficacy end­ adaptive. Participants completed a total of 15 sessions over 5 weeks at
points, working memory (CANTAB-spatial working memory; 8 mea­ school. Sessions lasted 7 min. The active control completed the com­
sures) or inhibitory function (TOVA; 1 measure). Study strengths include puter game 2048. Groups were matched on age and two WASI subscales.
a large sample size, a sample of participants with deficits in cognitive Sustained attention was assessed using d prime scores from the Conners’
attention and prospective pre-registration. Continuous Performance Test at pre- and post-test. The results showed a
significant difference in standardised change CPT-3 d prime scores be­
Study 4: Kirk et al. (2019) tween the training group and active control (p = .033, d = 0.524) and
between the training group and passive control (p = .048, d = 0.497).
Children (5–9 years) were cluster-randomised to the training group There was no significant difference between the active and passive

19
E.J. Slattery et al. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 137 (2022) 104633

controls. Though this study did not assess far transfer or long-term ef­ post-test. Parents rated ADHD symptom severity and clinicians
fects, it had a number of strengths including a large sample size, rand­ completed the Clinical Global Impression (CGI) of Severity of Illness
omised design, and active/passive control groups. Scale at baseline, mid-intervention, post-intervention and at a 6-month
follow up. The results indicated that change in ADHD symptom severity
Study 7: Bikic et al. (2017) for the intervention group vs the control group from baseline to mid-/
post-intervention did not reach between-group significance but was
This double-blind randomised pilot allocated participants to an significant at follow-up (p = 0.04, d = 0.36). Within-group ANOVAs
intervention group (n = 9) or active control (n = 9). The intervention applied over the four assessment time points (baseline, mid-, post-,
group completed exercises from the Scientific Brain Training pro­ follow up) showed that symptom severity ratings changed significantly
gramme for 30-minutes five days a week for seven weeks at home. The in the intervention group (p = 0.003) but not in the control group.
exercises were adaptive and trained numerous cognitive functions, Nonparametric ANOVA over the four assessed time points did not find a
including sustained attention and working memory. The control group significant effect in the intervention group for CGI but did find a sig­
played a non-adaptive version of Tetris. The Cambridge Neuropsycho­ nificant effect in the control group (p < 0.007). The training group
logical Test Automated Battery and ADHD rating scale were completed demonstrated statistically significant improvements in response inhibi­
at pre- and post-training. The test battery included measures of sus­ tion and Stroop interference compared to the control group. No other
tained attention, processing speed, visual memory, short-term memory, effects reached statistical significance.
spatial working memory, planning and rule acquisition/visual discrim­
ination/attention shifting. There were no significant differences be­ Study 10: Sarzyńska et al. (2017)
tween the groups on sustained attention (assessed using a Rapid Visual
Information Processing task) or any other cognitive / behavioural Sarzyńska et al. (2017) reported near transfer to sustained attention
measure. However, there were some within-group differences from pre- but no far transfer to fluid intelligence in a sample of 8-year-old chil­
to post-training. In the training group, scores on two sustained attention dren. In this non-randomised study, participants were assigned to an
variables improved, A prime (the ability to detect the target sequence; intervention (n = 30) or active control (n = 24) group. The training
p = .003, d = 1.5) and probability of hit (p = .005, d = 1.3). In contrast, group completed computerised tasks designed to train multiple atten­
the control group improved on one spatial working memory variable tional functions. The difficulty of the tasks was adjusted according to the
(between errors; p = 0.042, d = 0.88). child’s performance and a token system encouraged motivation. The
control group completed computerised tasks that targeted problem
Study 8: Kirk et al. (2016) solving and perceptual skills. Both groups completed a total of 10 ses­
sions 2 or 3 times a week. Each session lasted 30 min. Outcome measures
Results of this single intervention are reported separately in Kirk of attention (d2 Attention test; 7 different variables) and intelligence
et al. (2016; attention outcomes; 7 different variables) and Kirk et al. (Raven’s Progressive Matrices Test) were completed at baseline,
(2017; academic, executive functioning and behavioural/emotional post-test and 3-month follow up (training group only). The results
problems outcomes; 20 different variables). In this study, children with a showed significant group × time interaction effects for some d2 vari­
diagnosis of intellectual disability and attentional difficulties (aged 4–11 ables, including false alarms (p = .0003, ηp2 = 0.06), discriminability
years) were randomly assigned to receive Tali Train (n = 38) or an (p = 0.001, ηp2 =.05) and bias (p = .02, ηp2 = 0.03).
active control (n = 38). Outcome measures were completed at baseline,
post-training and 3-month follow up. Participants in the training group Study 11: Schrieff-Elson et al. (2017)
showed significantly greater improvements in some aspects of selective
attention from baseline to post-training (d = 0.24) and baseline to 3- Fifteen low socioeconomic status children aged 7–13 years were
month follow up (d = 0.26) compared to participants in the control assigned to an intervention group (n = 5), active control (n = 5) or
group. Training did not have an immediate effect on academic outcomes passive control (n = 5). The intervention group received the Pay
but participants in the training group showed significantly greater im­ Attention! training programme, which consisted of four non-
provements in numeracy skills from baseline to follow up compared to computerised tasks designed to train sustained, selective, alternating,
participants in the control (d = 0.15). The training was not effective in and divided attention. The active control played physical games like
improving sustained attention, attention control, cardinality, vocabu­ hopscotch. Participants received two 45-minute sessions per week for 10
lary, phonological abilities, inattentive/hyperactive behaviour, working weeks. Tests of attention, working memory and inhibition were
memory behaviour, executive function behaviour or behavioural prob­ completed before and after the intervention. Training was not effective
lems. Although this study had a fairly large sample size and included an in improving sustained attention (assessed using the Score! subtest from
active control group, it did not correct for multiple comparisons. the Test of Everyday Attention for Children and Numbers Forward from
Correction for multiple comparisons would have been appropriate as the Children’s Memory Scale) or any other outcome. The active control
over 20 different variables were investigated. Therefore, the results displayed higher inhibition scores compared to the passive control at
should be interpreted cautiously. post-test (p = .036). Within-group analyses indicated that the inter­
vention group improved in aspects of selective attention, attentional
Study 9: Mishra et al. (2016) control and inhibition from pre- to post-test. In contrast, both controls
improved in aspects of selective attention. Given the very small sample
In this study, children with ADHD were randomly assigned to an size, all findings should be interpreted cautiously.
intervention group (n = 21) or active control (n = 10). The intervention
group completed adaptive online cognitive exercises that focused on Study 12: Rueda et al. (2012)
attended-signal training or distractor-suppression training over a 6-
month period. The exercises targeted sustained attention, working In this study, 5-year-old children were pseudo-randomly assigned to
memory and response inhibition. The active control played the online a training group (n = 19) or control group (n = 18) matched for gender,
Hoyle puzzle game. Both groups completed assessments of sustained IQ and flanker inference scores on the Attention Network Test (ANT).
attention (first 10 min of the TOVA; 1 measure), response inhibition The training group completed computerised exercises designed to train
(second half of the TOVA; 1 measure), short-term memory (spatial ca­ tracking, discrimination, conflict resolution, inhibition, and sustained
pacity task and verbal capacity task; 2 measures) and executive function attention. All exercises were adaptive. Training took place twice weekly
(Stroop Colour and Word Interference Test; 1 measure) at baseline and for 45 min over 5 weeks. The active control watched cartoons. The

20
E.J. Slattery et al. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 137 (2022) 104633

following measures were completed at pre- and post-training: ANT, Disorder Evaluation). The authors concluded that attention training
delay gratification task, gambling task and Kaufman Brief Intelligence interventions “may be a valuable treatment option for improving
Test. There were no significant differences between the training and cognitive efficiency in children with ADHD”. However, given the many
control group in sustained attention (alerting scores from the ANT) or methodological concerns (e.g., small sample size, adequacy of the active
any other cognitive outcome over time (i.e., no significant time × group control, no correction for multiple comparisons (at least 14 between
interaction effects). However, a number of significant differences were group comparisons conducted), no follow up assessment and no ran­
found in planned contrast analysis. In the training group, there was a domisation), this conclusion seems far-reaching.
significant improvement in IQ matrices from baseline to post-training
and significant improvements in a gambling task from baseline to Study 15: Bauer et al. (2020)
follow up. Both groups showed significant improvements in some
outcome measures from a delay of gratification task from baseline to Bauer et al. (2020) found that a school-based mindfulness pro­
post-training and baseline to follow up. However, to show a training gramme improves aspects of sustained attention in 6th grade students.
benefit relative to the control, improvements in the training group must The study reports a small subset of data (n = 31) from participants who
be compared directly to improvements in the control group (time × completed an fMRI protocol as part of a larger RCT (N = 96). The pro­
group intervention effects were not statistically significant). gramme was designed to train focused attention and change students’
mindsets about their stress over 8 weeks. The control group received
Study 13: Tucha et al. (2011) coding training which matched the time commitment and engagement
of the mindfulness intervention. Sustained attention was assessed using
The results from this study are reported in both Tucha et al. (2011) a SART pre- and post-training. The results indicated that the mindfulness
and Lange et al. (2012). Children with ADHD were randomly assigned to group displayed significantly better Go-accuracy scores after the inter­
an AixTent attention training group (n = 16) or active control (n = 16). vention compared to the coding group (p = .01, Cohen f2 =.47). There
AixTent is an adaptive computerised programme designed to trained were no significant differences between the groups in RT variability or
sustained, selective, and divided attention. Participants completed two No-Go accuracy after the intervention. Given the small sample size and
sessions a week for four weeks. Each session lasted 1 h. The control study design (i.e., participants self-selected to take part in the fMRI
group completed visual perception training. The intervention and con­ protocol), the results should be interpreted cautiously.
trol groups underwent tests of tonic and phasic alertness, vigilance, se­
lective attention, divided attention, and flexibility before and after Study 16: Mishra et al. (2020)
training. The training group demonstrated improvements in some as­
pects of vigilance (commission errors; p = .011, d = 0.59), divided Adolescents with childhood neglect (aged 10–18 years) were cluster
attention (commission errors; p = .002, d = 1.28), and flexibility randomised to either an internal attention intervention (IAI; n = 15),
(commission errors; p = .050, d = 0.84) from baseline to post-training external attention intervention (EAI; n = 15) or a passive control
(15 paired t-tests). In contrast, the control group show no changes (15 (treatment-as-usual; n = 15). The internal attention group used the app
paired t-tests). At post-training, there was one statistically significance Meditrain to practice attending to the sensations of their breath. The
difference between the training and active control in divided attention external attention group completed exercises from brainhq.com
(commission errors; p = .003) out of 15 comparisons. Differences in designed to train focused attention, divided attention, selective atten­
ipsative scores (change from pre- to post-assessment) were also calcu­ tion and working memory. Participants in both groups completed 30
lated for both groups. The training group demonstrated significantly sessions over 6 weeks. Sessions lasted 30 min. Participants in the
greater ipsative change compared to the control in aspects of vigilance intervention and control groups underwent tests of sustained attention
(errors of commission; p = .047) and divided attention (errors of com­ (test modelled after the Test of Variables of Attention; response time
mission; p = .014; 15 comparisons). However, no correction for multiple variability) and interference control (Flanker Test; RT on incongruent
comparisons was applied despite the large number of pairwise trials minus RT on congruent trials) before and after the intervention.
comparisons. Caregivers rated ADHD symptoms at pre-test, post-test and 1-year follow
up and teachers rated students’ academic performance after the inter­
Study 14: Kerns et al. (1999) vention and at 1-year follow up. There was a significant time × group
interaction effect following the intervention for both outcomes, sus­
In this early influential study, Kerns et al. (1999) reported im­ tained attention (p = .01, η2 = 0.17) and interference resolution
provements in sustained attention following training with the Pay (p = .03, η2 = 0.16). Post hoc within group tests showed that these
Attention! programme in children with ADHD. Participants (aged 7–11 outcomes improved from baseline to post-training in only the internal
years) were assigned to the intervention (n = 7) group or active control attention group (p = .01 and p = .015). There were no significant
(n = 7). Pay Attention! trained sustained, selective, alternating and changes in the external attention group. The passive control showed a
divided attention using various non-computerised tasks. All tasks were significant decline in sustained attention at post-test (p = .03). There
adaptive, and participants received feedback on their performance. The was no significant difference between groups in ratings of inattention/
control group played computer games. Both groups completed 16 hyperactivity at post-training but significant improvements in hyper­
30-minute sessions over 8 weeks. Participants in the intervention and activity were found from baseline to 1-year follow up in the internal
control groups underwent cognitive tests (attention, executive function attention group (p = .005) and from post-test to follow up (p = .038).
and academic efficacy; at least 13 different variables) and paren­ There was a significant difference between the groups after the inter­
t/teachers completed ratings of ADHD symptoms. The training group vention on teacher-rated academic performance (p = .04, η2 = 0.15),
demonstrated improvements in auditory sustained attention (p < .001; with the paper reporting that the IAI group had high ratings relative to
Attention Capacity Test), planning (p = .010; Mazes subtest WISC-III), the EAI and treatment-as-usual groups though the results of these pair­
inhibition (p = .005; Day Night Stroop), selective attention (p = 0.15; wise comparisons are not reported.
Underlying Task) and academic efficiency (p = .015) when compared to
the control group. No intervention effects were found for sustained Study 17: Tarrasch (2018)
attention (Children’s Continuous Performance Test), impulsivity
(Matching Familiar Figures Test), freedom from distractibility (Coding Tarrasch et al. (2018) reported that mindfulness training was effec­
and Digit Span subtests WISC-III), spatial ability (Hooper Visual Orga­ tive in improving sustained attention in children aged 8–11 years. In this
nisation Test), or inattentive/hyperactive behaviours (Attention Deficit non-randomised study, participants were assigned to mindfulness

21
E.J. Slattery et al. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 137 (2022) 104633

training (n = 58) or a passive control (education as usual; n = 43). The Study 20: Ricarte et al. (2015)
training group received 10 sessions of mindfulness-based stress reduc­
tion (MBSR) in a group setting. Both groups were tested in a pre- and Ricarte et al. (2015) found a school-based adaption of the Mindful­
post-test using a CPT (sustained attention) and Conjunctive Visual ness Emotional Intelligence Training Programme improved concentra­
Search Task (selective attention). At pre-test, the intervention group tion and immediate auditory-verbal memory in children aged 6–13
demonstrated a significantly higher CPT commissions (p < .05) and years. The intervention group (n = 45) received mindfulness training
Conjunctive Search accuracy (for trials including 8 displays with and every day for 6 weeks and was compared to a passive control group
without targets, ps < .05) compared to the control. A significant time (n = 45). Sustained attention (Trail Making Test Part A), focused
× group interaction effect for CPT commissions (p < .01, ηp2 = 0.124) attention (Perception of Differences Test), mental flexibility (Trial
was found with post hoc Tukey’s HSD indicating the mindfulness group Making Test Part B), concentration (Digit Span subtest WISC-III total
demonstrated improvements from baseline to post-training (p < .001, score), auditory-verbal memory (Digit Span Forward) and working
d = 0.89). No effects were found for CPT omissions or RTSD. Some memory (Digit Span Backward) were compared between the two groups.
significant improvements were reported for aspects of selective atten­ Immediately after training, the intervention but not the control group
tion from pre- to post-test in both the mindfulness and control groups showed improvements in concentration (p < .001, ηp2 =.12) and
(depended on whether the target was included and the number of items auditory-verbal memory (p < .001, ηp2 =.13). No training effects were
in the display). This was a non-randomised study and groups were not found for any other cognitive measure. Study strengths include its fairly
equivalent before training on some outcome variables (which were not large sample size and randomised design, but the intervention group
accounted for in the analyses), which makes the findings difficult to likely had a greater expectation of improvement than the passive
interpret. control.

Study 18: Kiani et al. (2017) Study 21: Leonard et al. (2013)

In this study, female adolescents with elevated ADHD symptoms This cluster-randomised controlled trial assigned two hundred and
were randomly assigned to a mindfulness training group (n = 15) or a sixty-four incarcerated youths (aged 16–18 years) to a cognitive
passive control (n = 15). The intervention group received mindfulness behavioural/mindfulness intervention group or active control. The
training with sessions focusing on attention and the five senses. Sessions intervention combined aspects of cognitive behavioural therapy with
were held weekly for 8 weeks. Both groups completed assessments of the attentional and response modification elements of mindfulness
sustained attention (CPT; 3 measures), working memory (Digit Span meditation. The active control completed cognitive perception training.
subtest and Letter-Number Sequencing subtest of the WISC-IV; 5 mea­ Both conditions completed 750 min of training in small groups over 3–5
sures), inhibition (Stroop Word-Colour Interference Test; 1 measure) weeks. The ANT was administered pre- and post-training (9 different
and planning (Tower of London Test; 1 measure) at baseline and one- variables). There was a significant time × group interaction effect for
month after the intervention. The results indicated that the mindful­ overall ANT performance (% correct; p < .001, ηp2 =.06). At post-
ness group demonstrated statistically significant improvements in inhi­ training, scores were significantly higher in the intervention group
bition (p = .01, ηp2 =.22) and planning (p = .04, ηp2 =.15) compared to compared to the control (p = .04, d = 0.30). There was a time × group
the control group. No other between group differences were found. interaction effect for response variability (p = .02, ηp2 =.03). At T2,
Within group analyses indicated that the training group improved on response variability was lower (responses were more stable) in the
working memory from pre- to post-test (p = .004). intervention group compared to the control (p = .02, d = 0.34). No
significant effects were found for alerting, orienting or conflict moni­
Study 19: Wimmer et al. (2016) toring (RT or % correct).

Children were assigned to an intervention group (n = 16), active Study 22: Napoli et al. (2005)
control (n = 8) or passive control (n = 10). The intervention group
received MBSR training 25 times over 18 weeks. The active control Primary school children were randomly assigned to a mindfulness
received concentration skills training. Groups completed a vigilance intervention (n = 114) group or control (n = 114). The intervention
task, reversible figure task, Stroop Colour-Word Interference task, group completed the Attention Academy Programme designed to help
recognition task, visual search task and the WSCT-64 at pre- and post- students improve their quality of life through practicing mindfulness.
test. In the paper, results are reported at p < .10 due to the “small The control group completed “reading and other quiet activities”. Both
sample size and pilot character of the study”. A number of intervention groups had twelve 45-min sessions over 24 weeks. Sustained attention
effects are reported at this significance level; however, only two of these (TEA-Ch Score!, Walk, Don’t Walk and Code Transmission subtests),
effects are significant at p < .05 (vigilance task accuracy in no-responses selective attention (TEA-Ch Sky Search and Map Mission subtests) and
and visual search RT in blank trials). The passive control group accuracy ADHD symptoms (ACTeRS) were assessed at baseline and post-training.
in no-responses scores significantly worsened from pre- to post-test A series of t tests were used to examine pre-to-post-test differences be­
(p = .05), whereas the interaction and active control remained un­ tween groups. No intervention effects were found for sustained atten­
changed. This finding is interpreted as “partly” confirming the hypoth­ tion. The mindfulness group demonstrated significant improvements in
esis that mindfulness and concentration training are associated with selective attention (total score from the TEA-Ch; p < .001, d = 0.60) and
improvements in sustained attention. However, it provides very weak attentional behaviour (p = .001, d = 0.49). Limitations include the lack
evidence of an intervention effect as there was no direct comparison of clarity and/or consistency of control activities, no long-term follow up
between groups and a decline in performance in the control does not and unclear what statistical analyses were conducted (e.g., “paired t-
represent a sensible transfer pattern. Regarding RT in blank trials on the tests (…) between groups”).
visual search task, the mindfulness improved compared to the passive
control (p = .02) and the passive control improved compared to the Study 23: Meijer et al. (2021)
active control (p = .03). Note, children were not randomly assigned to
the passive control. In this cluster-randomised controlled trial, children were assigned to
an aerobic exercise group (n = 221), cognitively demanding exercise
group (n = 240) or a passive control group (n = 430). The aerobic ex­
ercise group consisted of activities designed to target moderate-to-

22
E.J. Slattery et al. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 137 (2022) 104633

vigorous intensity while avoiding high cognitive demands. The focus to-vigorous. The d2 Test of Attention (2 measures) and academic per­
was on highly repetitive and automated exercises (e.g., running, squats formance (numeric grade scores ranging from 7 excellent to 1 not suf­
etc.). The cognitively demanding exercise group consisted of team ficient) were administered. No statistically significant intervention
games or exercises that required complex movement, strategic play, effects were found for concentration (d =.08, p = .535) or selective
cooperation between children, anticipating behaviour and dealing with attention (d =.04, p = .124). However, significant changes were found
changing task demands. Both groups received four sessions per week for language performance (d =.83, p < .001) and mathematics perfor­
over 14 weeks. Participants completed the ANT (5 measures), Digit Span mance (d =.60, p < .001).
Task (2 measures), Grid Task (2 measures), Stop Signal Task (1 measure)
at baseline and post-test. Principal components analysis was used to Study 27: Hedayatjoo et al. (2020)
derive composite scores. Six components were extracted: 1) information
processing and control (information processing speed, lapses of atten­ Thirty-six children with hearing deficits (7–12 years) were randomly
tion and motor inhibition), 2) interference control (speed and accuracy assigned to an intervention group (n = 18) or passive control group
of interference control), 3) attention accuracy (accuracy of alerting (n = 18). The intervention group completed balance exercises over a 4-
attention and spatial orientation), 4) visuospatial working memory week period. Both groups completed a CPT to assess sustained attention
(visuospatial working memory and visuospatial short-term memory), 5) before and after the intervention. The results indicated that the inter­
verbal working memory (verbal working memory and verbal short-term vention group improved significantly from pre- to post-test on CPT
memory), and 6) attention efficiency (speed of alerting attention and performance (p = .002) though it was not clear what CPT outcome
spatial attention to information). The results indicated no statistically measure was used in the analysis. In contrast, the control group did not
significant differences between the two intervention groups and the significantly improve from pre- to post-test (p = .906). In addition, the
control group for any of the components. Study strengths include its pre- to post-test difference was compared between groups and indicated
large sample size and extensive cognitive test battery. a significant difference between the groups (p = .017).

Study 24: Cherriere et al. (2020) Study 28: Månsson et al. (2019)

In this non-randomised study, children with Charcot-Marie-Tooth In this non-randomised study, children (those with ADHD and those
disease aged 7–12 years were assigned to a dance group (n = 5) or a with ADHD symptoms) aged 10–14 years were assigned to a physical
passive control (n = 4). Participants in the dance group received activity group (n = 64) or passive control (n = 66). The physical activity
training in various complex dance forms (e.g., jazz, break dance and tap) group participated in target shooting practice for a period of 6-months
twice weekly for 10 weeks. Both groups completed measures of sus­ (1 h session each week). Participants in both groups completed the
tained attention (Score! subtest and Score DT subtest of the TEA-Ch) cognitive tests of attention QbTest™ (2 sustained attention variables
short-term memory (Forward Span subtest of the WISC-IV) and work­ and 2 hyperactivity variables) and parents/teachers completed a rating
ing memory (Backward Span subtest of the WISC-IV) at baseline and scale measures of ADHD symptoms (ADHD Rating Scale; 1 variable),
post-test. The intervention group demonstrated greater improvements in behaviour problems (Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire; 1 vari­
sustained attention (Score! subtest; p = .03) compared to the control able), and quality of life (KIDSCREEN-27; 1 variable) before and after
group. No other between group effects were statistically significant. the intervention. The physical activity group demonstrated greater im­
Serious methodological limitations include the small sample size and provements in the two measures of sustained attention, Qb reaction time
non-randomised design. variance (p = .013) and Qb omission errors (p = .019), parent-rated
ADHD symptoms (p = .024) and parent-rated behavioural problems
Study 25: Duarte et al. (2020) (p = .027). However, there was a significant difference between the
groups at baseline on QB omissions, which complicates the interpreta­
Adolescents aged 12–14 years were cluster-randomised to a Qigong tion of intervention-related improvements in this outcome. Further­
group (n = 22), active control group (n = 22) or passive control group more, it is likely the parents of children in the intervention group likely
(n = 22). Qigong integrates movement and breathing exercises that had a greater expectation of improvement than parents of children in the
require and induce a special kind of mental state of ‘awareness’ or control group.
‘attention’. The Qigong group performed the White Ball Qigong exercise,
according to the Heidelberg Model of Traditional Chinese Medicine. The Study 29: Buchele Harris et al. (2018)
active control group performed a sham Qigong. Both groups performed
their respective activities for 5 min, twice a week, over a 4-week period. Buchele Harris et al. (2018) concluded that daily brief
They were also encouraged to repeat the exercises at home on a daily coordinated-bilateral physical activities (CBPA) can improve attention
basis. Participants completed the d2 test of attention at baseline, after and concentration in fifth grade students. In this study, participants were
two weeks and four weeks of training. The following measures were assigned to a CBPA training + Fitbit group (n = 31), Fitbit only group
extracted: total number of items processed (TN), total hits (TH), total (n = 29), or passive control (n = 56). The CBPA group received CBPA
number of items minus error scores (TN-E), concentration performance breaks (6 min × 5 days) during the school day and were given fitness
(CP) and fluctuation rate (FR). The Qigong group demonstrated signif­ trackers to wear Monday to Friday over 4 weeks. Sustained attention
icant improvements in the total number of items processed (TN), total was assessed pre- and post-training using outcome measures from the d2
hits (TH), total number of items minus error scores (TN-E) and con­ Test of Attention (processing speed, focused attention, concentration
centration performance (CP) compared to both the active and passive performance and attention span). At pre-test, the control group had
control groups after four weeks of practice. significantly higher processing speed, focused attention, and concen­
tration performance scores than the CBPA and Fitbit only group. Post
Study 26: García-Hermoso et al. (2020) hoc comparisons revealed significant improvements in the CBPA group
in speed (p = .010), focused attention (p = .02), concentration perfor­
In this cluster-randomised controlled trial, children from schools mance (p < .001) and attention span (p < .001) compared to the passive
with low socioeconomic status were assigned to the Active-Start pro­ control. There were also significant improvements in the CBPA group in
gramme (n = 100) or a passive control (n = 70). Participants in the concentration performance (p < .001) and attention span (p = .011)
Active-Start programme completed physical games focused on group compared to the Fitbit only group. These findings suggest the
cooperation. The intensity of the main part of the sessions was moderate- coordinated-bilateral physical activities may improve sustained

23
E.J. Slattery et al. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 137 (2022) 104633

attention though results should be interpreted cautiously given the Study 33: Gallotta et al. (2015)
many study limitations. Limitations include no randomisation and sig­
nificant differences between groups at pre-test which were not In this cluster-randomised trial, normal and overweight children
accounted for in the analyses. aged 8–11 years were assigned to a traditional physical activity group
(n = 78), a coordinative physical activity group (n = 83) or a passive
Study 30: Rezaei et al. (2018) control (school as usual; n = 69). The traditional physical activity group
primarily focused on endurance, strength, flexibility exercises and cir­
In this study, 21 children aged 7–11 years were assigned to a yoga cuit training for cardiovascular health. The coordinative physical ac­
group (n = 7), neurofeedback group (n = 7) or passive control (n = 7). tivity intervention was focused on improving the coordination and
The yoga group consisted of yoga and relaxation exercises. The neuro­ dexterity of participants. Interventions were administered in school over
feedback training consisted of theta/beta training. Both groups received 5 months. Both groups completed two 1-hour sessions per week over a
24 training sessions over 8 weeks. Various measures of attention (7 five-month period. The d2 Test of Attention was administered at pre-
different variables) were administered before and after the intervention. and post-test. Three outcome measures were extracted: items processed,
The authors first compared each intervention group to the control group concentration and % errors. Within group analyses indicated that the
and then compared the two intervention groups. For the purposes of this traditional physical activity group improved in all measures from pre- to
review, we only report yoga-control comparisons. In relation to the posttest and the coordinative physical activity group improved in con­
yoga-control comparison, an ANCOVA (not clear what was controlled centration and % errors. No improvements were found in the control
for) indicated a significant effect of group on CPT correct detection from pre- to post-test. Analyses were also conducted with the absolute
(p = .001, ηp2 =.878), CPT errors of omission (p = .008, ηp2 = .722), CPT variation and the percentage variation (i.e., post-intervention score –
errors of commission (p = .001, ηp2 =.888), WISC number sequencing pre-intervention score). The traditional physical activity group led to a
(p = .004, ηp2 =.772) and WISC Coding (p = .015, ηp2 =.652). The yoga higher improvement of total number of items processed compared to the
group seemed to demonstrate improvements in all these variables except coordinative group, while coordinative physical activity led to higher
for CPT errors of commissions, which were lower in the control group. improvements in concentration and % error scores than the traditional
Given the small sample used, the results should be interpreted physical activity and control group (exact p values not reported).
cautiously.
Study 34: Zach et al. (2015)

Study 31: Chou and Huang (2017) In this study, class groups (aged 4–5 years) were assigned to a dance
group (n = 40), orienteering group (n = 44) or passive control (n = 39).
In this non-randomised study, the intervention group received 8 Both intervention groups received 9 weekly sessions. Participants
weeks of yoga with two sessions per week and was compared to a passive completed MOXO Continuous Performance Test (8 variables) and a
control group. Participants were assigned to a yoga exercise intervention spatial perception task (at least 4 variables). Note, two versions of the
(n = 25) or treatment as usual (n = 25). Each yoga session consisted of MOXO CPT task were used in the study. The orienteering group
10-minutes of stretching and warming up followed by a 20-min yoga completed an interactive version, while the dance group and control
activity, which included concentration and balance, improved attention group completed a non-interactive version. A series of ANOVAs
and breath and body awareness, and a 10-min cool down. Both groups comparing the dance to the control group indicated there was a signif­
completed a sustained attention task (Visual Pursuit Test) and a icant time by group interaction effect on CPT attention (p < .05, η2
discrimination task at baseline and within one week of completing the =.062) and CPT impulsivity (p < .01, η2 =.126) when the stimuli were
intervention. At post-test, the yoga group showed improvements in accompanied by disturbances (no follow up tests reported). A paired
sustained attention, Visual Pursuit Accuracy Rate (p = .010, d = 0.78) samples t-test indicated the orienteering group improved its perfor­
and RT (p < .001, d = − 1.20) compared to the control. Improvements in mance on the interactive version of the MOXO (Timing; p = .046). A
the Discrimination task Response Accuracy (p < .001, d = 1.09) and RT number of significant time × group interaction effects were found for
(p < .001, d = − 1.25) were also found in the yoga group compared to the spatial perception task but again no post hoc tests were reported.
the control group at post-test. Overall, these findings suggest that a yoga Strengths of the study included a fairly large sample size though lack of
exercise intervention may improve sustained attention in children with randomisation, no correction for multiple comparisons and no active
ADHD; however, the study has many methodological limitations (e.g., control group limit conclusions.
non-randomised design, passive control and no long-term follow up for
cognitive measures), which restrict the interpretation of results. Study 35: Spitzer and Hollmann (2013; Study 1)

Study 32: Sabel et al. (2017) In this non-randomised study, two sixth grade classes were assigned
to an intervention group (n = 24) or passive control (n = 20). The
Sabel et al. (2017) found that active video gaming improved motor intervention group received three extra exercise lessons (e.g., basketball,
and process skills but not sustained attention in thirteen childhood brain soccer, dance) for 30-min during the first hour of the school day over a 4-
tumour survivors aged 7–17 years. Participants were randomised to month period (36 sessions). The control group received education as
either an intervention or waitlist control group. After 10–12 weeks the usual. Participants completed the d2 Test of Attention (4 different sus­
groups crossed over. The intervention involved active video gaming tained attention variables) and teachers completed ratings of social and
using Nintendo Wii (5 days per week) and weekly online coaching ses­ studying behaviour (10 subscales). Academic grades in maths, German
sions for 10–12 weeks. Training had no effect on the primary cognitive and English were also used as study outcomes. No intervention effects
outcome measures which included (among others) sustained attention, were reported for sustained attention. However, there was a significant
working memory, long-term memory, information processing speed and interaction effect found for teacher ratings of accurateness while
executive function. Parallel group analysis indicated that the interven­ studying (p = .01). The authors reported that scores in the intervention
tion group demonstrated significantly improved Process scores on the improved after the intervention while scores in the control group
Activities of Daily Living assessment (p = .030), suggesting they became decreased (post hoc tests and descriptive results were not reported).
more independent in their daily lives (secondary outcome). However, There was also a significant interaction effect on German grades. Grades
we cannot draw conclusions about the impact of active video gaming on in the intervention group improved at post-test whereas grades in the
attention due to the small sample size and cross over design. control group worsened (again post hoc tests and descriptive results

24
E.J. Slattery et al. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 137 (2022) 104633

were not reported). Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 27 (12), 1563–1574. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00787-


018-1151-y.
Bishop, S.R., Lau, M., Shapiro, S., Carlson, L., Anderson, N.D., Carmody, J., Segal, Z.V.,
Study 36: Spitzer and Hollmann (2013; Study 2) Abbey, S., Speca, M., Velting, D., 2004. Mindfulness: a proposed operational
definition. Clin. Psychol. Sci. Pract. 11 (3), 230–241. https://doi.org/10.1093/
clipsy.bph077.
One-hundred and forty-eight sixth grade children were initially Boutron, I., Page, M.J., Higgins, J.P.T., Altman, D.G., Lundh, A., Hróbjartsson, A., 2022.
assigned to an intervention or passive control according to class groups. Chapter 7: Considering bias and conflicts of interest among the included studies. In:
However, one school subsequently dropped out, which resulted in a final Higgins, J.P.T., Thomas, J., Chandler, J., Cumpston, M., Li, T., Page, M.J., Welch, V.
A. (Eds.), Cochrane Handbook for Systematic Reviews of Interventions (Version 6.3).
sample of 88 children. The intervention group received 30 extra exercise Cochrane. www.training.cochrane.org/handbook.
lessons for 30-minutes at the start of the school day. Participants took Bowie, C.R., Harvey, P.D., 2006. Administration and interpretation of the Trail Making
part in various games that could be played with 3–4 members on each Test. Nat. Protoc. 1 (5), 2277. https://doi.org/10.1038/nprot.2006.390.
Breslau, J., Miller, E., Breslau, N., Bohnert, K., Lucia, V., Schweitzer, J., 2009. The impact
team. This study used the same outcome measures as Spitzer and Holl­
of early behavior disturbances on academic achievement in high school. Pediatrics
mann (2013; Study 1). The results indicated a group by time interaction 123 (6), 1472–1476. https://doi.org/10.1542/peds.2008-1406.
effect on concentration performance (d2 Test of Attention; p = .01). The (*) Buchele Harris, H., Cortina, K.S., Templin, T., Colabianchi, N., Chen, W., 2018.
authors reported that pre-to-post improvement was greater in the Impact of coordinated-bilateral physical activities on attention and concentration in
school-aged children. Biomed. Res. Int. 2018, 2539748. https://doi.org/10.1249/01.
physical activity group than in the control (tests and descriptive results mss.0000538293.60019.1f.
were not reported). There was also a significant time by group interac­ Buckner, R.L., DiNicola, L.M., 2019. The brain’s default network: updated anatomy,
tion effect on maths grades. The authors reported that the control physiology and evolving insights. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 20 (10), 593–608. https://doi.
org/10.1038/s41583-019-0212-7.
group’s grades worsened while the physical activity group’s grades Button, K.S., Ioannidis, J.P.A., Mokrysz, C., Nosek, B.A., Flint, J., Robinson, E.S.J.,
improved (again tests were not reported). In relation to teacher ratings, Munafò, M.R., 2013. Power failure: why small sample size undermines the reliability
the authors reported that the “prosocial behaviour of the intervention of neuroscience. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 14 (5), 365–376. https://doi.org/10.1038/
nrn3475.
group changed positively” though again these results were not reported. Cashin, A.G., McAuley, J.H., 2019. Clinimetrics: physiotherapy evidence database
(PEDro) scale. J. Physiother. 66 (1) https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jphys.2019.08.005.
Study 37: Verret et al. (2012) (*) Cherriere, C., Martel, M., Fortin, S., Raymond, M.J., Veilleux, L.N., Lemay, M., 2020.
An adapted dance program for children with Charcot-Marie-Tooth disease: an
exploratory study. J. Bodyw. Mov. Ther. 24 (2), 85–91. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.
In this study, children with ADHD (7–12 years) were assigned to the jbmt.2019.09.009.
intervention (n = 10, 9 boys) or control group (n = 11, 10 boys). The (*) Chou, C.C., Huang, C.J., 2017. Effects of an 8-week yoga program on sustained
attention and discrimination function in children with attention deficit hyperactivity
intervention group received 30 physical activity training sessions over disorder. Peerj 5. https://doi.org/10.7717/peerj.2883.
10 weeks. Various physical activities were used such as basketball, Cotman, C.W., Berchtold, N.C., Christie, L.-A., 2007. Exercise builds brain health: key
soccer, and tag. The main objective of each session was to maintain roles of growth factor cascades and inflammation. Trends Neurosci. 30 (9), 464–472.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tins.2007.06.011.
moderate to vigorous intensity. Participants in both groups completed Coull, J.T., Frith, C.D., Frackowiak, R.S.J., Grasby, P.M., 1996. A fronto-parietal network
cognitive tests of attention and response inhibition (assessed using the for rapid visual information processing: a PET study of sustained attention and
Test of Everyday Attention for Children; 5 different variables) and working memory. Neuropsychologia 34 (11), 1085–1095. https://doi.org/10.1016/
0028-3932(96)00029-2.
parent/teachers completed a measure of behaviour problems (Child
Crowe, S.F., 1998. The differential contribution of mental tracking, cognitive flexibility,
Behaviour Checklist; 11 different variables) before and after the inter­ visual search, and motor speed to performance on parts A and B of the trail making
vention. The intervention group demonstrated better sustained attention test. J. Clin. Psychol. 54 (5), 585–591. https://doi.org/10.1002/(SICI)1097-4679
(199808)54:5<585::AID-JCLP4>3.0.CO;2-K.
(Score; p = .03) and visual search (Sky Search Time Target; p = .05) at
Diamond, A., Ling, D.S., 2016. Conclusions about interventions, programs, and
post-test compared to the control. Participants in the physical activity approaches for improving executive functions that appear justified and those that,
group had significantly lower parent ratings for Child Behaviour despite much hype, do not. Dev. Cogn. Neurosci. 18, 34–48. https://doi.org/
Checklist total problems (p = .038), social problems (p = .033), thought 10.1016/j.dcn.2015.11.005.
Döpfner, M., Breuer, D., Wille, N., Erhart, M., Ravens-Sieberer, U., 2008. How often do
problems (p = 0.19) and attention problems (p = .01) after the inter­ children meet ICD-10/DSM-IV criteria of attention deficit-/hyperactivity disorder
vention compared to the control. For teacher ratings, the physical ac­ and hyperkinetic disorder? Parent-based prevalence rates in a national
tivity group had a significantly lower anxiety-depression score (p = .01) sample–results of the BELLA study. Eur. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 17 (1), 59–70.
https://doi.org/10.1007/s00787-008-1007-y.
and fewer social problems (p = .05) after the intervention compared to (*) Duarte, L., Gonçalves, M., Mendes, P., Matos, L.C., Greten, H.J., Machado, J., 2020.
the control. As noted by the authors, a key limitation of the study was Can Qigong improve attention in adolescents? a prospective randomised controlled
that both parents and teachers were aware of the treatment and prob­ trial. J. Bodyw. Mov. Ther. 24 (1), 175–181. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.
jbmt.2019.05.005.
ably had expectations for changes. Duncan, G.J., Dowsett, C.J., Claessens, A., Magnuson, K., Huston, A.C., Klebanov, P.,
Pagani, L.S., Feinstein, L., Engel, M., Brooks-Gunn, J., 2007. School readiness and
References later achievement. Dev. Psychol. 43 (6), 1428. https://doi.org/10.1037/0012-
1649.43.6.1428.
Esterman, M., Rothlein, D., 2019. Models of sustained attention. Curr. Opin. Psychol. 29,
*References marked with an asterisk denote studies included in the review.
174–180. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.copsyc.2019.03.005.
(*) Bauer, C.C., Rozenkrantz, L., Caballero, C., Nieto-Castanon, A., Scherer, E., West, M.
Esterman, M., Noonan, S.K., Rosenberg, M., DeGutis, J., 2013. In the zone or zoning out?
R., Mrazek, M., Phillips, D.T., Gabrieli, J.D., Whitfield-Gabrieli, S., 2020.
Tracking behavioral and neural fluctuations during sustained attention. Cereb.
Mindfulness training preserves sustained attention and resting state anticorrelation
Cortex 23 (11), 2712–2723. https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhs261.
between default-mode network and dorsolateral prefrontal cortex: a randomized
Esterman, M., Rosenberg, M.D., Noonan, S.K., 2014. Intrinsic fluctuations in sustained
controlled trial. Hum. Brain Mapp. 41 (18), 5356–5369. https://doi.org/10.1002/
attention and distractor processing. J. Neurosci. 34 (5), 1724–1730. https://doi.org/
hbm.25197.
10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2658-13.2014.
Bender, R., Lange, S., 2001. Adjusting for multiple testing—when and how? J. Clin.
(*) Gallotta, M.C., Emerenziani, G.P., Iazzoni, S., Meucci, M., Baldari, C., Guidetti, L.,
Epidemiol. 54 (4), 343–349. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0895-4356(00)00314-0.
2015. Impacts of coordinative training on normal weight and overweight/obese
Best, J.R., 2010. Effects of physical activity on children’s executive function:
children’s attentional performance. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 9. https://doi.org/
contributions of experimental research on aerobic exercise. Dev. Rev. 30 (4),
10.3389/fnhum.2015.00577.
331–351. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.dr.2010.08.001.
(*) García-Hermoso, A., Hormazábal-Aguayo, I., Fernández-Vergara, O., González-
Betts, J., McKay, J., Maruff, P., Anderson, V., 2006. The development of sustained
Calderón, N., Russell-Guzmán, J., Vicencio-Rojas, F., Chacana-Cañas, C., Ramírez-
attention in children: the effect of age and task load. Child Neuropsychol. 12 (3),
Vélez, R., 2020. A before-school physical activity intervention to improve cognitive
205–221. https://doi.org/10.1080/09297040500488522.
parameters in children: the Active-Start study. Scand. J. Med. Sci. Sports 30 (1),
(*) Bikic, A., Christensen, T.O., Leckman, J.F., Bilenberg, N., Dalsgaard, S., 2017.
108–116. https://doi.org/10.1111/sms.13537.
A double-blind randomized pilot trial comparing computerized cognitive exercises to
Gartlehner, G., Hansen, R.A., Nissman, D., Lohr, K.N., Carey, T.S., 2006. A simple and
Tetris in adolescents with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Nord. J.
valid tool distinguished efficacy from effectiveness studies. J. Clin. Epidemiol. 59
Psychiatry 71 (6), 455–464. https://doi.org/10.1080/08039488.2017.1328070.
(10), 1040–1048. Available from. 〈https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/sites/books/NB
(*) Bikic, A., Leckman, J.F., Christensen, T.O., Bilenberg, N., Dalsgaard, S., 2018.
K44029/〉.
Attention and executive functions computer training for attention-deficit/
hyperactivity disorder (ADHD): results from a randomized, controlled trial. Eur.

25
E.J. Slattery et al. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 137 (2022) 104633

Gartlehner, G., Affengruber, L., Titscher, V., Noel-Storr, A., Dooley, G., Ballarini, N., Lundervold, A.J., Bøe, T., Lundervold, A., 2017. Inattention in primary school is not good
König, F., 2020. Single-reviewer abstract screening missed 13 percent of relevant for your future school achievement—A pattern classification study. PLoS One 12
studies: a crowd-based, randomized controlled trial. J. Clin. Epidemiol. 121, 20–28. (11), e0188310. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0188310.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jclinepi.2020.01.005. Mackworth, N.H., 1948. The breakdown of vigilance during prolonged visual search.
Gillberg, M., Åkerstedt, T., 1998. Sleep loss and performance: no “safe” duration of a Q. J. Exp. Psychol. 1 (1), 6–21. https://doi.org/10.1080/17470214808416738.
monotonous task. Physiol. Behav. 64 (5), 599–604. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0031- Mak, C., Whittingham, K., Cunnington, R., Boyd, R.N., 2018. Efficacy of mindfulness-
9384(98)00063-8. based interventions for attention and executive function in children and
Gram, L., Fitchett, A., Ashraf, A., Daruwalla, N., Osrin, D., 2019. Promoting women’s and adolescents—A systematic review. Mindfulness 9 (1), 59–78. https://doi.org/
children’s health through community groups in low-income and middle-income 10.1007/s12671-017-0770-6.
countries: a mixed-methods systematic review of mechanisms, enablers and barriers. (*) Månsson, A.G., Elmose, M., Mejldal, A., Dalsgaard, S., Roessler, K.K., 2019. The
BMJ Glob. Health 4 (6), e001972. https://doi.org/10.1136/bmjgh-2019-001972. effects of practicing target-shooting sport on the severity of inattentive, hyperactive,
de Greeff, J.W., Bosker, R.J., Oosterlaan, J., Visscher, C., Hartman, E., 2018. Effects of and impulsive symptoms in children: a non-randomised controlled open-label study
physical activity on executive functions, attention and academic performance in in Denmark. Nord. J. Psychiatry 73 (4–5), 233–243. https://doi.org/10.1080/
preadolescent children: a meta-analysis. J. Sci. Med. Sport 21 (5), 501–507. https:// 08039488.2019.1612467.
doi.org/10.1016/j.jsams.2017.09.595. McAvinue, L.P., Habekost, T., Johnson, K.A., Kyllingsbæk, S., Vangkilde, S.,
Green, C.S., Bavelier, D., Kramer, A.F., Vinogradov, S., Ansorge, U., Ball, K.K., Bingel, U., Bundesen, C., Robertson, I.H., 2012. Sustained attention, attentional selectivity, and
Chein, J.M., Colzato, L.S., Edwards, J.D., 2019. Improving methodological standards attentional capacity across the lifespan. Atten. Percept. Psychophys. 74 (8),
in behavioral interventions for cognitive enhancement. J. Cogn. Enhanc. 3 (1), 2–29. 1570–1582. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13414-012-0352-6.
https://doi.org/10.1007/s41465-018-0115-y. McAvinue, L.P., Vangkilde, S., Johnson, K.A., Habekost, T., Kyllingsbæk, S.,
(*) Hedayatjoo, M., Rezaee, M., Zarei, M.A., Mirzakhany, N., Nazeri, A., Baghban, A.A., Bundesen, C., Robertson, I.H., 2015. A componential analysis of visual attention in
Hedayatjoo, Z., Dezfoly, R.M., 2020. Effect of balance training on balance children with ADHD. J. Atten. Disord. 19 (10), 882–894. https://doi.org/10.1177/
performance, motor coordination, and attention in children with hearing deficits. 1087054712461935.
Arch. Neurosci. 7 (1). https://doi.org/10.5812/ans.84869. (*) Meijer, A., Königs, M., van der Fels, I.M.J., Visscher, C., Bosker, R.J., Hartman, E.,
Hillman, C.H., Erickson, K.I., Kramer, A.F., 2008. Be smart, exercise your heart: exercise Oosterlaan, J., 2021. The effects of aerobic versus cognitively demanding exercise
effects on brain and cognition. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 9 (1), 58–65. https://doi.org/ interventions on executive functioning in school-aged children: a cluster-randomized
10.1038/nrn2298. controlled trial. J. Sport Exerc. Psychol. 1–13. https://doi.org/10.1123/jsep.2020-
Kabat-Zinn, J., 2003. Mindfulness-based interventions in context: past, present, and 0034.
future. Clin. Psychol. Sci. Pract. 10 (2), 144–156. https://doi.org/10.1093/clipsy. (*) Mishra, J., Sagar, R., Joseph, A., Gazzaley, A., Merzenich, M., 2016. Training sensory
bpg016. signal-to-noise resolution in children with ADHD in a global mental health setting.
Katz, B., Shah, P., Meyer, D.E., 2018. How to play 20 questions with nature and lose: Transl. Psychiatry 6 (4). https://doi.org/10.1038/tp.2016.45.
reflections on 100 years of brain-training research. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 115 (*) Mishra, J., Sagar, R., Parveen, S., Kumaran, S., Modi, K., Maric, V., Ziegler, D.,
(40), 9897–9904. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1617102114. Gazzaley, A., 2020. Closed-loop digital meditation for neurocognitive and behavioral
Katz, B., Jones, M.R., Shah, P., Buschkuehl, M., Jaeggi, S.M., 2021. Individual differences development in adolescents with childhood neglect. Transl. Psychiatry 10 (1).
in cognitive training research. In: Strobach, T., Karbach, J. (Eds.), Cognitive https://doi.org/10.1038/s41398-020-0820-z.
Training: An Overview of Features and Applications, 2nd ed. Springer, pp. 107–123. Moreau, D., 2021. How malleable are cognitive abilities? A critical perspective on
(*) Kerns, K.A., Eso, K., Thomson, J., 1999. Investigation of a direct intervention for popular brief interventions. PsyArXiv. https://doi.org/10.31234/osf.io/sxwah.
improving attention in young children with ADHD. Dev. Neuropsychol. 16 (2), Moreau, D., Conway, A.R., 2013. Cognitive enhancement: a comparative review of
273–295. https://doi.org/10.1207/S15326942DN1602_9. computerized and athletic training programs. Int. Rev. Sport Exerc. Psychol. 6 (1),
(*) Kiani, B., Hadianfard, H., Mitchell, J.T., 2017. The impact of mindfulness meditation 155–183. https://doi.org/10.1080/1750984X.2012.758763.
training on executive functions and emotion dysregulation in an Iranian sample of Moreau, D., Kirk, I.J., Waldie, K.E., 2016. Seven pervasive statistical flaws in cognitive
female adolescents with elevated attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder symptoms. training interventions. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 10, 153. https://doi.org/10.3389/
Aust. J. Psychol. 69 (4), 273–282. https://doi.org/10.1111/ajpy.12148. fnhum.2016.00153.
(*) Kirk, H., Gray, K., Ellis, K., Taffe, J., Cornish, K., 2017. Impact of attention training on (*) Napoli, M., Krech, P.R., Holley, L.C., 2005. Mindfulness training for elementary
academic achievement, executive functioning, and behavior: a randomized school students: the attention academy. J. Appl. Sch. Psychol. 21 (1), 99–125.
controlled trial. Am. J. Intellect. Dev. Disabil. 122 (2), 97–117. https://doi.org/ https://doi.org/10.1300/J370v21n01_05.
10.1352/1944-7558-122.2.97. (*) Nejati, V., 2021. Program for attention rehabilitation and strengthening (PARS)
(*) Kirk, H.E., Gray, K.M., Ellis, K., Taffe, J., Cornish, K.M., 2016. Computerised attention improves executive functions in children with attention deficit- hyperactivity
training for children with intellectual and developmental disabilities: a randomised disorder (ADHD). Res. Dev. Disabil. 113, 103937. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.
controlled trial. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 57 (12), 1380–1389. https://doi.org/ ridd.2021.103937.
10.1111/jcpp.12615. Parasuraman, R., 1979. Memory load and event rate control sensitivity decrements in
(*) Kirk, H.E., Spencer-Smith, M., Wiley, J.F., Cornish, K.M., 2019. Gamified attention sustained attention. Science 205 (4409), 924–927. https://doi.org/10.1126/
training in the primary school classroom: a cluster-randomized controlled trial. science.472714.
J. Atten. Disord. 25 (8), 1146–1159. https://doi.org/10.1177/1087054719887435. Parker, R.A., Weir, C.J., 2020. Non-adjustment for multiple testing in multi-arm trials of
Kirk, H.E., Raber, A., Richmond, S., Cornish, K.M., 2020. Examining potential predictors distinct treatments: rationale and justification. Clin. Trials 17 (5), 562–566. https://
of attention training outcomes in children with intellectual and developmental doi.org/10.1177/1740774520941419.
disorders. J. Intellect. Dev. Disabil. 1–7. https://doi.org/10.3109/ Paus, T., Zatorre, R.J., Hofle, N., Caramanos, Z., Gotman, J., Petrides, M., Evans, A.C.,
13668250.2020.1821939. 1997. Time-related changes in neural systems underlying attention and arousal
(*) Kollins, S.H., DeLoss, D.J., Cañadas, E., Lutz, J., Findling, R.L., Keefe, R.S., Epstein, J. during the performance of an auditory vigilance task. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 9 (3),
N., Cutler, A.J., Faraone, S.V., 2020. A novel digital intervention for actively 392–408. https://doi.org/10.1162/jocn.1997.9.3.392.
reducing severity of paediatric ADHD (STARS-ADHD): a randomised controlled trial. Peng, P., Miller, A.C., 2016. Does attention training work? A selective meta-analysis to
Lancet Digit. Health 2 (4), e168–e178. https://doi.org/10.1016/S2589-7500(20) explore the effects of attention training and moderators. Learn. Individ. Differ. 45,
30017-0. 77–87. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.lindif.2015.11.012.
Lange, K.W., Tucha, L., Hauser, A., Hauser, J., Lange, K.M., Stasik, D., Tucha, O., 2012. Petersen, S.E., Posner, M.I., 2012. The attention system of the human brain: 20 years
Attention training in attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Aula Abiert. 40 (3), after. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 35 (1), 73–89. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-neuro-
55–60. 062111-150525.
Langner, R., Eickhoff, S.B., 2013. Sustaining attention to simple tasks: a meta-analytic Pingault, J.-B., Tremblay, R.E., Vitaro, F., Carbonneau, R., Genolini, C., Falissard, B.,
review of the neural mechanisms of vigilant attention. Psychol. Bull. 139 (4), 870. Côté, S.M., 2011. Childhood trajectories of inattention and hyperactivity and
https://doi.org/10.1037/a0030694. prediction of educational attainment in early adulthood: a 16-year longitudinal
(*) Leonard, N.R., Jha, A.P., Casarjian, B., Goolsarran, M., Garcia, C., Clel, C.M., population-based study. Am. J. Psychiatry 168 (11), 1164–1170. https://doi.org/
Gwadz, M.V., Massey, Z., 2013. Mindfulness training improves attentional task 10.1176/appi.ajp.2011.10121732.
performance in incarcerated youth: a group randomized controlled intervention Posner, M.I., Rothbart, M.K., Tang, Y.-Y., 2015. Enhancing attention through training.
trial. Front. Psychol. 4. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00792. Curr. Opin. Behav. Sci. 4, 1–5. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cobeha.2014.12.008.
Lewis, F.C., Reeve, R.A., Kelly, S.P., Johnson, K.A., 2017. Sustained attention to a Rabiner, D.L., Malone, P.S., 2004. The impact of tutoring on early reading achievement
predictable, unengaging Go/No-Go task shows ongoing development between 6 and for children with and without attention problems. J. Abnorm. Child Psychol. 32 (3),
11 years. Atten. Percept. Psychophys. 79 (6), 1726–1741. https://doi.org/10.3758/ 273–284. https://doi.org/10.1023/B:JACP.0000026141.20174.17.
s13414-017-1351-4. Rapport, M.D., Orban, S.A., Kofler, M.J., Friedman, L.M., 2013. Do programs designed to
Li, G., Taljaard, M., Van den Heuvel, E.R., Levine, M.A., Cook, D.J., Wells, G.A., train working memory, other executive functions, and attention benefit children
Thabane, L., 2017. An introduction to multiplicity issues in clinical trials: the what, with ADHD? A meta-analytic review of cognitive, academic, and behavioral
why, when and how. Int. J. Epidemiol. https://doi.org/10.1093/ije/dyw320. outcomes. Clin. Psychol. Rev. 33 (8), 1237–1252. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.
Li, T., Higgins, J.P.T., Deeks, J.J., 2022. Chapter 5: Collecting data. In: Higgins, J.P.T., cpr.2013.08.005.
Thomas, J., Chandler, J., Cumpston, M., Li, T., Page, M.J., Welch, V.A. (Eds.), Raz, A., Buhle, J., 2006. Typologies of attentional networks. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 7 (5),
Cochrane Handbook for Systematic Reviews of Interventions (Version 6.3). 367–379. https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn1903.
Cochrane. Available from. www.training.cochrane.org/handbook. Redick, T.S., Shipstead, Z., Wiemers, E.A., Melby-Lervåg, M., Hulme, C., 2015. What’s
Lin, C.C., Hsiao, C.K., Chen, W.J., 1999. Development of sustained attention assessed working in working memory training? An educational perspective. Educ. Psychol.
using the continuous performance test among children 6–15 years of age. J. Abnorm. Rev. 27 (4), 617–633. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10648-015-9314-6.
Child Psychol. 27 (5), 403–412. https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1021932119311.

26
E.J. Slattery et al. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 137 (2022) 104633

(*) Rezaei, M., Kamarzard, T.S., Razavi, M.N., 2018. The effects of neurofeedback, yoga Steinmayr, R., Ziegler, M., Träuble, B., 2010. Do intelligence and sustained attention
interventions on memory and cognitive activity in children with attention deficit/ interact in predicting academic achievement? Learn. Individ. Differ. 20 (1), 14–18.
hyperactivity disorder: a randomized controlled trial. Ann. Appl. Sport Sci. 6 (4), https://doi.org/10.1016/j.lindif.2009.10.009.
17–27. https://doi.org/10.29252/aassjournal.6.4.17. Sturm, W., Willmes, K., 2001. On the functional neuroanatomy of intrinsic and phasic
(*) Ricarte, J.J., Ros, L., Latorre, J.M., Beltrán, M.T., 2015. Mindfulness-based alertness. Neuroimage 14 (1), S76–S84. https://doi.org/10.1006/nimg.2001.0839.
intervention in a rural primary school: effects on attention, concentration and mood. Tamm, L., Epstein, J.N., Peugh, J.L., Nakonezny, P.A., Hughes, C.W., 2013. Preliminary
Int. J. Cogn. Ther. 8 (3), 258–270. https://doi.org/10.1521/ijct_2015_8_03. data suggesting the efficacy of attention training for school-aged children with
Robertson, I.H., Manly, T., Andrade, J., Baddeley, B.T., Yiend, J., 1997. Oops!’: ADHD. Dev. Cogn. Neurosci. 4, 16–28. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.dcn.2012.11.004.
performance correlates of everyday attentional failures in traumatic brain injured Tang, Y.-Y., Posner, M.I., 2009. Attention training and attention state training. Trends
and normal subjects. Neuropsychologia 35 (6), 747–758. https://doi.org/10.1016/ Cogn. Sci. 13 (5), 222–227. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2009.01.009.
S0028-3932(97)00015-8. (*) Tarrasch, R., 2018. The effects of mindfulness practice on attentional functions
(*) Rueda, M.R., Checa, P., Cómbita, L.M., 2012. Enhanced efficiency of the executive among primary school children. J. Child Fam. Stud. 27 (8), 2632–2642. https://doi.
attention network after training in preschool children: immediate changes and org/10.1007/s10826-018-1073-9.
effects after two months. Dev. Cogn. Neurosci. 2, S192–S204. https://doi.org/ (*) Tucha, O., Tucha, L., Kaumann, G., König, S., Lange, K.M., Stasik, D., Streather, Z.,
10.1016/j.dcn.2011.09.004. Engelschalk, T., Lange, K.W., 2011. Training of attention functions in children with
(*) Sabel, M., Sjolund, A., Broeren, J., Arvidsson, D., Saury, J.M., Gillenstr, J., attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Atten. Defic. Hyperact Disord. 3 (3),
Emanuelson, I., Blomgren, K., Lannering, B., 2017. Effects of physically active video 271–283. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12402-011-0059-x.
gaming on cognition and activities of daily living in childhood brain tumor (*) Tullo, D., Guy, J., Faubert, J., Bertone, A., 2018. Training with a three-dimensional
survivors: A randomized pilot study. Neuro-Oncol. Pract. 4 (2), 98–110. https://doi. multiple object-tracking (3D-MOT) paradigm improves attention in students with a
org/10.1093/nop/npw020. neurodevelopmental condition: a randomized controlled trial. Dev. Sci. 21 (6), 1–11.
Sarter, M., Givens, B., Bruno, J.P., 2001. The cognitive neuroscience of sustained https://doi.org/10.1111/desc.12670.
attention: Where top-down meets bottom-up. Brain Res. Rev. 35 (2), 146–160. Unsworth, N., Robison, M.K., 2017. A locus coeruleus-norepinephrine account of
https://doi.org/10.1016/S0165-0173(01)00044-3. individual differences in working memory capacity and attention control. Psychon.
(*) Sarzyńska, J., Żelechowska, D., Falkiewicz, M., Nęcka, E., 2017. Attention training in Bull. Rev. 24 (4), 1282–1311. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13423-016-1220-5.
schoolchildren improves attention but fails to enhance fluid intelligence. Stud. Unsworth, N., Robison, M.K., 2020. Working memory capacity and sustained attention: a
Psychol. 59 (1), 50–65. https://doi.org/10.21909/sp.2017.01.730. cognitive-energetic perspective. J. Exp. Psychol. Learn. Mem. Cogn. 46 (1), 77.
Sato, Y., Saijo, Y., Yoshioka, E., 2021. Work stress and oral conditions: a systematic https://doi.org/10.1037/xlm0000712.
review of observational studies. BMJ Open 11 (5), e046532. https://doi.org/ Vater, C., Gray, R., Holcombe, A.O., 2021. A critical systematic review of the
10.1136/bmjopen-2020-046532. Neurotracker perceptual-cognitive training tool. Psychon. Bull. Rev. https://doi.org/
(*) Schrieff-Elson, L.E., Ockhuizen, J.H., During, G., Thomas, K.G.F., 2017. Attention- 10.3758/s13423-021-01892-2.
training with children from socioeconomically disadvantaged backgrounds in Cape Vazou, S., Pesce, C., Lakes, K., Smiley-Oyen, A., 2019. More than one road leads to Rome:
Town. J. Child Adolesc. Ment. Health 29 (2), 147–167. https://doi.org/10.2989/ a narrative review and meta-analysis of physical activity intervention effects on
17280583.2017.1372285. cognition in youth. Int. J. Sport Exerc. Psychol. 17 (2), 153–178. https://doi.org/
Seeley, W.W., Menon, V., Schatzberg, A.F., Keller, J., Glover, G.H., Kenna, H., Reiss, A.L., 10.1080/1612197X.2016.1223423.
Greicius, M.D., 2007. Dissociable intrinsic connectivity networks for salience (*) Verret, C., Guay, M.-C., Berthiaume, C., Gardiner, P., Béliveau, L., 2012. A physical
processing and executive control. J. Neurosci. 27 (9), 2349. https://doi.org/ activity program improves behavior and cognitive functions in children with ADHD:
10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5587-06.2007. an exploratory study. J. Atten. Disord. 16 (1), 71–80. https://doi.org/10.1177/
Shipstead, Z., Redick, T.S., Engle, R.W., 2012. Is working memory training effective. 1087054710379735.
Psychol. Bull. 138 (4), 628–654. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0027473. Vickerstaff, V., Omar, R.Z., Ambler, G., 2019. Methods to adjust for multiple comparisons
Simmons, J.P., Nelson, L.D., Simonsohn, U., 2011. False-positive psychology: undisclosed in the analysis and sample size calculation of randomised controlled trials with
flexibility in data collection and analysis allows presenting anything as significant. multiple primary outcomes. BMC Med. Res. Methodol. 19 (1), 1–13. https://doi.org/
Psychol. Sci. 22 (11), 1359–1366. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797611417632. 10.1186/s12874-019-0754-4.
Simons, D.J., Boot, W.R., Charness, N., Gathercole, S.E., Chabris, C.F., Hambrick, D.Z., Waffenschmidt, S., Knelangen, M., Sieben, W., Bühn, S., Pieper, D., 2019. Single
Stine-Morrow, E.A., 2016. Do “brain-training” programs work? Psychol. Sci. Public screening versus conventional double screening for study selection in systematic
Interest 17 (3), 103–186. https://doi.org/10.1177/1529100616661983. reviews: a methodological systematic review. BMC Med. Res. Methodol. 19 (1), 1–9.
(*) Slattery, E.J., Ryan, P., Fortune, D., McAvinue, L.P., 2021. Evaluation of a School- https://doi.org/10.1186/s12874-019-0782-0.
based Attention Training Programme for Improving Sustained Attention. (*) Wimmer, L., Bellingrath, S., von Stockhausen, L., 2016. Cognitive effects of
Department of Psychology, University of Limerick, Ireland [Manuscript submitted mindfulness training: results of a pilot study based on a theory driven approach.
for publication]. Front. Psychol. 7. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2016.01037.
Slattery, E.J., Ryan, P., Fortune, D.G., McAvinue, L.P., 2022. Unique and overlapping Yan, C., Zhou, H., Wei, W., Wang, Y.-j, Cui, L., Chan, R.C., Deng, C., 2018.
contributions of sustained attention and working memory to parent and teacher Developmental trajectories of attention in typically developing Chinese children: a
ratings of inattentive behavior. Child Neuropsychol. 1–23. https://doi.org/10.1080/ four-wave longitudinal study. Dev. Neuropsychol. 43 (6), 479–496. https://doi.org/
09297049.2021.2022112. 10.1080/87565641.2018.1487442.
(*) Spitzer, U.S., Hollmann, W., 2013. Experimental observations of the effects of Yerkes, R.M., Dodson, J.D., 1908. The relation of strength of stimulus to rapidity of habit
physical exercise on attention, academic and prosocial performance in school formation. J. Comp. Neurol. Psychol. 18.
settings. Trends Neurosci. Educ. 2 (1), 1–6. https://doi.org/10.1016/j. (*) Zach, S., Inglis, V., Fox, O., Berger, I., Stahl, A., 2015. The effect of physical activity
tine.2013.03.002. on spatial perception and attention in early childhood. Cogn. Dev. 36, 31–39.
Sridharan, D., Levitin, D.J., Menon, V., 2008. A critical role for the right fronto-insular https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cogdev.2015.08.003.
cortex in switching between central-executive and default-mode networks. Proc. Zelazo, P.D., Lyons, K.E., 2012. The potential benefits of mindfulness training in early
Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 105 (34), 12569–12574. https://doi.org/10.1073/ childhood: a developmental social cognitive neuroscience perspective. Child Dev.
pnas.0800005105. Perspect. 6 (2), 154–160. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1750-8606.2012.0024.

27

You might also like