You are on page 1of 8

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/ajcn/advance-article-abstract/doi/10.

1093/ajcn/nqz241/5573003 by University of California, Los Angeles user on 09 December 2019


Original Research Communications

Randomized placebo-controlled study of the memory effects of


pomegranate juice in middle-aged and older adults
Prabha Siddarth,1 Zhaoping Li,2 Karen J Miller,1 Linda M Ercoli,1 David A Merril,1,3 Susanne M Henning,2 David Heber,2
and Gary W Small1
1 Department of Psychiatry and Biobehavioral Sciences, Semel Institute for Neuroscience and Human Behavior, David Geffen School of Medicine at the
University of California, Los Angeles, CA, USA; 2 Center for Human Nutrition, David Geffen School of Medicine at the University of California, Los Angeles,
CA, USA; and 3 Pacific Brain Health Center at Pacific Neuroscience Institute, Santa Monica, CA, USA

ABSTRACT Introduction
Background: Antioxidant nutrients such as the polyphenols in Forgetfulness can be a normal part of aging as changes
pomegranate juice may prevent neuronal damage from the free
including oxidative damage and inflammation occur in the brain.
radicals produced during normal metabolism. Previous research in
Mild forgetfulness and loss of memory are among the top health
animals and a short-term clinical trial in middle-aged and older
concerns of middle-aged and older adults. Memory loss can
adults support the potential memory benefits of pomegranate juice;
begin as early as age 45 y (1) and as these deficits progress
however, the long-term effects of pomegranate juice consumption on
with advancing age, they can interfere with daily functioning
cognition have not been studied.
and quality of life (2). Memory training programs are used to
Objective: In this study, we investigated the long-term effect of
maintain cognitive performance in older adults with normal aging
pomegranate juice on memory in nondemented middle-aged and
older adults.
or mild memory challenges (3). However, there are few well-
Methods: We performed a 12-month, randomized, double-blind, designed prospective studies of nutritional interventions intended
placebo-controlled trial of pomegranate juice in middle-aged and to improve or stabilize memory performance during aging.
older adults. Two hundred and sixty-one subjects (aged 50–75 y) Healthy lifestyle behaviors such as engaging in aerobic
were randomly assigned to consume pomegranate juice [8 oz (236.5 exercise, increasing mental activity, staying socially active,
mL) per day] or a placebo drink (8 oz, matched constituents of avoiding tobacco and excess alcohol, and consuming a healthy
pomegranate juice except for pomegranate polyphenols). Memory diet have also been recommended to benefit memory performance
measures [Brief Visuospatial Memory Test-Revised (BVMT-R) and and brain function (4–7). A promising line of research has
Buschke Selective Reminding Test (SRT)] were assessed at 6 focused on foods that contain properties beneficial to healthy
and 12 mo and analyzed using a mixed-effects general linear brain functioning. Investigators have found that various nutrients
model. such as curcumin, sage, vitamin D, ω-3 fatty acids, and
Results: Twenty-eight subjects in the pomegranate juice group and
33 subjects in the placebo group dropped out before completing
the study. Baseline variables in the 98 pomegranate juice and 102
This study was supported by The Wonderful Company, LLC, Fran and Ray
placebo group subjects who completed the study did not differ
Stark Foundation Fund for Alzheimer’s Disease Research, John and Barbara
significantly. Group by time interaction was statistically significant
McLoughlin Gift Fundand the Parlow-Solomon Professorship (GWS).
for BVMT-R Learning (F[2, 257]= 5.90, P = 0.003; between-group The Wonderful Company, LLC, provided a gift for research in this area and
effect size [ES] = 0.45): the change within the pomegranate group provided the juice and placebo drinks for the participants. The company had
was not significant (ES = 0.15), whereas the placebo group showed no role in the design, implementation, analysis, and interpretation of the data.
a significant decline (ES = −0.35). Changes in the other BVMT-R Supplemental Tables 1 and 2 are available from the “Supplementary data”
scores as well as the SRT measures were not significantly different link in the online posting of the article and from the same link in the online
between groups. table of contents at https://academic.oup.com/ajcn/.
Conclusions: Daily consumption of pomegranate juice may stabilize Data described in the article will be made available in a de-identified form
the ability to learn visual information over a 12-mo period. This trial without restriction.
Address correspondence to PS (e-mail: psiddarth@mednet.ucla.edu).
was registered at clinicaltrials.gov as NCT02093130. Am J Clin
Abbreviations used: BVMT-R, Brief Visuospatial Memory Test-Revised;
Nutr 2019;00:1–8.
CLTR, Consistent Long-Term Retrieval; ES, effect size; MCI, mild cognitive
impairment; SRT, Selective Reminding Test.
Keywords: pomegranate juice, antioxidant polyphenol, normal Received March 7, 2019. Accepted for publication August 29, 2019.
aging, memory, cognition First published online 0, 2019; doi: https://doi.org/10.1093/ajcn/nqz241.

Am J Clin Nutr 2019;00:1–8. Printed in USA. Copyright © American Society for Nutrition 2019. All rights reserved. 1
2 Siddarth et al.

antioxidant foods may help to maintain cognitive function during ratio to each group. Group assignments were blinded to all
aging (8–11). participants and study personnel, including care providers and
Some phytonutrients including pomegranate ellagitannins those assessing outcomes. Baseline assessments consisted of vital

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/ajcn/advance-article-abstract/doi/10.1093/ajcn/nqz241/5573003 by University of California, Los Angeles user on 09 December 2019
may reduce oxidative stress and inflammation, and thus could signs (blood pressure, respiratory rate, pulse, and temperature),
maintain brain health during aging (12). Large-scale, placebo- electrocardiogram, serum electrolytes, thyroid function, and
controlled, randomized studies with antioxidant supplements in complete blood counts. Potential adverse events (including but
nondemented adults have demonstrated better cognitive function not limited to gastrointestinal difficulties, headaches, fatigue, skin
including 39% enhanced long-term memory performance in one rash) and compliance with the pomegranate juice or placebo drink
study (13). Adherence to a Mediterranean diet pattern, which consumption were assessed every 3 mo.
emphasizes colorful fruits and vegetables high in antioxidants,
has been linked to improved cognitive functioning (5) and
reduced rates of cognitive decline (14) in elderly subjects. Subject selection and assessment
Numerous natural antioxidant compounds found in fruits and We performed baseline cognitive assessments on 261 subjects
vegetables have also been associated with neuroprotective effects who met study entry criteria from a pool of 995 potential
(15). volunteers recruited through advertisements, media, and referrals
In the present study, we investigated the effect of pomegranate from physicians and families. Our study protocol detailed the
juice on cognition in nondemented middle-aged and older adults. methods and procedures, as well as prespecified inclusion and
Pomegranate juice is rich in ellagitannin polyphenols, which exclusion criteria (this trial was registered at clinicaltrials.gov
have both antioxidant and anti-inflammatory properties. Animal as NCT02093130). We determined a priori that a sample size
studies have shown that pomegranate juice and other polyphenol- of ≥100 completers per arm was needed to detect an effect size
rich fruit juices protect against oxidant stress and amyloid- (difference between group means as a multiple of the SD) of ≥0.4
induced brain injury in transgenic mouse models (16, 17). SDs, given an alpha level of 0.05 and power of 0.8.
Although some human studies on the use of polyphenols, such To be included, volunteers were required to have objective
as resveratrol (18–20) and flavanol (21), have provided initial cognitive performance scores and clinical histories consistent
evidence that these supplements are associated with improved with normal aging or mild cognitive impairment (MCI). Subjects
memory performance, these studies were relatively short in with evidence of dementia or a major neurocognitive disorder
duration, ranging from acute interventions to 26 wk. The most were excluded from the study. All subjects were aged between
recent study (22) with 60 healthy elderly participants failed to 50 and 75 y and agreed to participate for the entire 12 mo
show significant improvements in verbal memory after 6 mo of of the trial. Subjects had adequate visual and auditory acuity
resveratrol use. In a pilot study of 23 subjects (aged 50–75 y) who for neuropsychological testing; and had screening laboratory
had mild memory complaints associated with aging, we found tests and electrocardiograms without evidence of significant
that drinking 8 oz. of pomegranate juice daily for 1 mo both medical conditions that might interfere with the study. Exclusion
improved memory performance and altered brain neural activity, criteria included: significant cerebrovascular disease (i.e., strokes
measured using functional MRI, in comparison to a control limiting communication abilities and interfering with study
group drinking taste-matched placebo juice without polyphenols procedures); probable Alzheimer’s disease, other dementia;
(23). The present study was designed to determine whether neurological or physical illnesses associated with cognitive
pomegranate juice has long-term benefits over 1 y on cognitive deterioration; inability to undergo MRI or with any significant
abilities in middle-aged and older nondemented volunteers. abnormality on brain MRI (e.g., tumor, stroke, vasogenic edema);
a history of myocardial infarction within the previous year or un-
stable cardiac disease; uncontrolled hypertension (systolic blood
Methods pressure >170 or diastolic blood pressure >100); significant
liver or pulmonary disease, diabetes, or cancer; major depression
Study design or any major psychiatric disorder; history of alcoholism or
Utilizing a randomized, double-blind, 2-group parallel design, substance addiction; use of vitamins other than a standard
we tested memory performance in subjects consuming 8 oz. multivitamin supplement; or use of any cognitive enhancing
(236.5 mL) of pomegranate juice (The Wonderful Company, supplements (e.g., gingko biloba, phosphatidyl serine), cognitive
LLC) compared with those consuming a placebo drink. Eight enhancing drugs (e.g., donepezil, rivastigmine, memantine),
ounces of pomegranate juice used for the intervention contain any juice containing pomegranate or pomegranate juice, or
the following polyphenols: 368 mg punicalagins, 93 mg antho- investigational drugs within the previous month or longer,
cyanins, 29 mg ellagic acid, and 98 mg other tannins, and we depending on drug half-life. None of the participants from the
have previously shown that plasma concentrations are similar previous pilot study (23) were included in the present study.
when consuming 8 oz. of juice or 1 g of pomegranate extract At baseline, all subjects completed the Montreal Cognitive
(24, 25). The placebo drink contained the same constituents Assessment (26) and the Beck Depression Inventory (27). A
of pomegranate juice (flavor, color, sugar, and acidity level) neuropsychological test battery (28) was also performed to
except for pomegranate polyphenols. Identical packaging was assist in excluding subjects with dementia, and a standardized
used for both the pomegranate juice and the placebo drink. measure of memory self-awareness, the Memory Functioning
Subjects were sent weekly supplies of the study drink and Questionnaire (29), confirmed the presence of mild memory
were instructed to drink 8 oz. daily for 12 mo. The statistician complaints.
prepared the randomization schedule using a computer-generated Compliance was determined based on participant diary
random assignment scheme, which assigned subjects in a 1:1 records. Participants were instructed to keep diaries every week
Memory effects of pomegranate juice 3
and indicate whether they followed the protocol every day. Then, (number of words recalled during the 30-min delayed trial) scores
during each of their visits, the research staff inspected these as outcome measures.
diaries and tabulated, for each participant, the number of days To minimize practice effects, alternate versions of the Buschke

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/ajcn/advance-article-abstract/doi/10.1093/ajcn/nqz241/5573003 by University of California, Los Angeles user on 09 December 2019
elapsed and the number of days marked by the participant as not SRT and the BVMT-R were administered and counterbalanced.
compliant. At the end of the study, the percentage of compliance All tests were administered by trained psychometrists.
was calculated as the percentage of the total number of days
compliant over the total number of days elapsed. If the percentage
of compliance was ≥80%, then the participant was deemed to Statistical analysis
have followed the protocol. Before statistical analyses, all data were inspected for outliers,
Blood samples were drawn for DNA analysis for apoE skewness, kurtosis, and homogeneity of variance to ensure their
genotyping using standard techniques (30). Genetic data were appropriateness for parametric statistical tests. The pomegranate
available for 241 subjects. Written informed consent was juice and placebo groups were compared on baseline demo-
obtained from all subjects in accordance with the University of graphic and clinical characteristics with chi-square statistics for
California, Los Angeles Human Subjects Protection Committee categorical measures and t-tests for continuous measures. All
procedures. The trial began in January 2014 and was completed subjects with baseline data were included in the examination of
in May 2018 and all data were collected at the Semel Institute all outcomes in intent-to-treat analyses. Our outcome measures
for Neuroscience and Human Behavior and Center for Human are continuous and were analyzed using a mixed-effects general
Nutrition, David Geffen School of Medicine at the University of linear model, as implemented in PROC MIXED in SAS v 9.4
California, Los Angeles, CA. (SAS Institute, Cary, NC). Mixed models allow the use of all
available data from all subjects, thereby minimizing the effects
of loss to follow-up (33). We included treatment group as the
Primary outcome measures
between-subject factor, time as the within-subject factor, and the
Because our previous research indicated memory effects of interaction term between time and treatment group as predictors.
pomegranates, we chose 2 memory tests, namely, the Brief Age, sex, and education level were used as covariates. We also
Visuospatial Memory Test-Revised (BVMT-R) (31) and the estimated separate moderation models for apoE-4 genetic risk,
Buschke Selective Reminding Test (SRT) (32) as the primary overweight/obesity, and MCI status by including the interaction
outcome instruments after 6 and 12 mo of treatment; thus, term of apoE-4 or overweight/obesity or MCI by treatment group
these tests were given at each time point. Performance on the by time, all respective lower-order interactions and main effects,
BVMT-R was the primary outcome for visual memory. The and the same covariates as the basic model. Post-hoc analyses
BVMT-R is a standardized measure of visual learning and recall determined the significance of between-group differences and
that presents 6 geometric designs to the subject for 10 s. The within-group changes. We present test scores and statistics as
subject is subsequently asked to draw on a separate page as well as effect sizes (Cohen’s d) for group differences and within-
many of the figures as possible in their correct location. This group changes. Because we had 6 outcome measures (3 scores
is repeated for a total of 3 learning trials. The Delayed Recall for BVMT-R and 3 scores for Buschke SRT), the significance
trial is administered after a 25-min delay. A learning score threshold for each of the outcome measures was set at 0.008
(indicating how the participant benefited from repetition) is also (0.05/6; 2-tailed).
calculated by subtracting the total correct items of Trial 1 from
the highest number of correct items recalled on Trial 2 or Trial 3.
Outcome measures from BVMT-R included the following: Total Results
Recall (number of designs recalled over the 3 learning trials;
measures Immediate Recall [i.e., short-term memory]); Delayed Participant flow and follow-up
Recall (number of designs recalled during the 25-min delayed Of the 261 subjects who met the study inclusion criteria for
trial; reflects delayed recall for new information); and Learning randomization, 130 were assigned to the pomegranate group and
scores. 131 to the placebo condition. Nearly a quarter (23%) of the
The Buschke SRT (32) performance was the primary outcome randomized participants dropped out before completion. Neither
for verbal memory. The SRT is a standardized measure of verbal the drop-out rate (pomegranate: 22%; placebo: 25%) nor the
learning that presents 12 unrelated words to the subject, who reasons for dropping out differed between the pomegranate and
is then asked to immediately recall as many words as possible. the placebo groups (Figure 1). Subjects who withdrew did not
The examiner then selectively re-presents words (i.e., reminds) differ significantly from those who completed the study in mean
that the subject was unable to recall on all subsequent trials, age or baseline cognitive measures. Of the 200 subjects who
although subjects must attempt to recall the entire word list. completed the study, 98 belonged to the pomegranate group and
This procedure is repeated until either the respondent attempts 102 to the placebo group. For the pomegranate group, mean (SD)
all 12 learning trials, or until the respondent recites the entire list follow-up was 12.4 (1.3) mo; for the placebo group it was 12.6
on 2 consecutive trials without reminders (32). The Consistent (1.7) mo. Based on participant diaries, compliance rates for both
Long-Term Retrieval (CLTR) score is the number of words that treatment groups were equal (89%).
the subject recalls consecutively over trials without receiving Treatment groups did not differ significantly in baseline demo-
reminders and indicates how well the subject consolidates graphic variables, including age, sex, family history of dementia,
the new information during the learning phase (encoding). In apoE-4 risk status, or in scores on the Montreal Cognitive
addition to CLTR, we used the Buschke SRT Total Recall Assessment, Beck Depression Inventory, and Wechsler Test of
(number of words recalled over the 12 trials) and Delayed Recall Adult Reading (Table 1). Educational level was significantly
4 Siddarth et al.

995 Screened

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/ajcn/advance-article-abstract/doi/10.1093/ajcn/nqz241/5573003 by University of California, Los Angeles user on 09 December 2019
670 Excluded
Medical condition; excluded medication/supplement; lives too far; time
commitment; claustrophobic; MRI contraindication; age; BMI; not interested;
participating in another study; lost contact; too little compensation, etc.

325 Consented to participate

64 Dropped before randomization


Unable to get medical clearance (abnormal labs, metal in MRI) – 15; too busy
– 12; concerned about calories – 7; excluded health condition – 7; low
cognitive screening assessment – 6; lost contact – 5; no longer interested –
4; high BMI – 2; new health condition (unknown - per subject request) – 3;
will travel too much – 1; excluded medication – 1; did not want MRI – 1

261 Randomized to pom juice or placebo

61 (28 pom; 33 placebo) Dropped after randomization


Adverse event (upset stomach, heartburn, gastric irritation, diarrhea, etc.) – 8
Pom, 10 placebo; other adverse event (rash, congestion, bladder irritation,
headache, yeast infection) – 4 pom, 5 placebo; did not like taste – 3 pom, 5
placebo; new medical condition (cancer diabetes, prediabetes, etc.) – 2 pom,
4 placebo; compliance issues/ too busy – 5 pom, 3 placebo; concern about
sugar/ calories – 3 pom, 2 placebo; did not want possibility of placebo – 2
placebo; new medication use – 2 pom; worsening clinical condition – 1
placebo; moved – 1 pom, 1 placebo

200 Completed

FIGURE 1 Flow diagram of participants. pom, pomegranate.

different between groups (pomegranate group: 16.7 [2.1] y; P = 0.4). For Delayed Recall, the pomegranate group improved
placebo group: 16.2 [2.0] y, t(259) = 2.0, P = 0.05; Table 1). (t(257) = 2.31, P = 0.02), but not significant after correction,
Treatment groups also did not differ in any of the memory whereas the placebo group showed no change (t(257) = 0.31,
measures at baseline, both the subjective memory measures and P = 0.8).
the primary outcome measures of verbal and visual memory For the primary verbal memory outcome (SRT), the group
(Tables 1 and 2). In addition, the number of adverse events by time interaction term was not statistically significant for any
for both groups was comparable, the most common being of the 3 measures. Both the pomegranate and placebo groups
gastrointestinal issues (19% in the pomegranate group;16% in the improved in Total (pomegranate: t(257) = 3.25, P = 0.001;
placebo group). A list of all adverse events reported are tabulated placebo: t(257) = 4.01, P = 0.0001; Table 2) and CLTR
in Supplemental Table 1. (pomegranate: t(257) = 2.27, P = 0.02; placebo: t(257) = 3.50,
P = 0.001) scores, whereas neither group showed change in
Delayed Recall (pomegranate: t(257) = −0.51, P = 0.6; placebo:
Memory outcomes t(257) = 1.46, P = 0.15).

Mean memory scores from both BVMT-R and Buschke SRT


at baseline, 6 mo, and 12 mo by treatment group are presented Moderation models
in Table 2 for all participants. In addition, we present these scores
for completers only in each treatment group in Supplemental There was no significant group by time by apoE-4 status
Table 2. For the primary visual memory outcome (BVMT-R), interaction for any of the outcome measures (P = 0.3–0.9). Thus,
the group by time interaction was statistically significant for apoE moderation of treatment effect was not indicated. Similarly,
the Learning score: the change in the pomegranate group was being overweight/obese or MCI status did not moderate treatment
not significant (t(257) = 1.67, P = 0.1), whereas the placebo effect (P = 0.2–0.8).
group showed significant decline (t(257) = −3.19, P = 0.002)
(Figure 2 and Table 2). Changes in the BVMT-R Total and
Delayed Recall scores were not significantly different between Discussion
groups. Neither group showed change in BVMT-R Total Recall In this 12-mo randomized controlled trial, visual memory
(pomegranate: t(257) = 1.11, P = 0.2; placebo: t(257) = 0.86, performance, specifically the ability to learn visual information
Memory effects of pomegranate juice 5
TABLE 1 Baseline demographic characteristics of participants1

Pomegranate juice Placebo drink


(n = 130) (n = 131)

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/ajcn/advance-article-abstract/doi/10.1093/ajcn/nqz241/5573003 by University of California, Los Angeles user on 09 December 2019
Age, y 60.8 (6.5) 59.9 (6.4)
Education,2 y 16.7 (2.1) 16.2 (2.0)
Sex, female, n (%) 85 (65) 89 (68)
BMI 25.3 (4.6) 25.6 (4.5)
Overweight/obese, n (%) 35 (27)/21 (16) 44 (33)/21 (16)
Montreal Cognitive Assessment 27.3 (2.0) 27.4 (2.0)
Wechsler Test of Adult Reading 42.0 (7.1) 40.3 (7.8)
Mild cognitive impairment, n (%) 67 (52) 59 (45)
Beck Depression Inventory 4.5 (4.3) 5.2 (5.4)
Family history of dementia, n (%) 55 (42) 55 (42)
apoE-4 carriers,3 n (%) 35 (29) 28 (23)
Memory Functioning Questionnaire
Frequency of forgetting 166.7 (29.1) 168.9 (35.1)
Seriousness of forgetting 83.7 (26.2) 85.4 (25.3)
Retrospective functioning 15.7 (4.5) 16.0 (5.3)
Mnemonics usage 23.1 (9.5) 23.3 (8.1)
1 Values are means with SDs in parentheses, unless otherwise noted.
2 t(259)= 2.0, P = 0.05.
3 Not available for 11 pomegranate juice and 9 placebo subjects.

over repeated learning trials, was shown to be maintained of dietary constituents on the central nervous system, the role
in individuals who drank pomegranate juice compared with of dietary modification on gut bacterial populations and the
a placebo drink. This finding is consistent with our earlier possibility of affecting the production of biologically active
pilot study showing greater levels of activity in brain regions compounds such as urolithins, and relating the microbial shifts
controlling visual memory functions in subjects consuming to memory and learning have been an active area of study. For
pomegranate juice over 1 mo compared with subjects consuming example, Li and associates (36) reported a correlation between
a placebo drink (23). In the 1-mo pilot study, a significant dietary-induced shifts in bacterial diversity and animal learning
between-group improvement in a verbal memory measure was that may indicate a role for gut bacterial diversity in memory and
found, which this larger study did not replicate. The briefer learning.
duration of the intervention in the pilot study—4 wk compared The currently available data are insufficient to postulate an
with 12 mo in the current investigation—could explain the exact mechanism of action for pomegranate juice on memory;
discrepancy in these results. It is thus possible that pomegranate however, several possibilities exist. Yuan and colleagues (37)
juice consumption has short-term effects on verbal memory found that although polyphenols are unable to cross the
that are not sustained long term. We also emphasize that only blood–brain barrier, urolithins, produced by gut bacteria from
1 cognitive measure (of the 6 that were examined) showed pomegranate polyphenols, are able to cross the blood–brain bar-
a significant between-group difference, and thus the effect of rier, and such pomegranate metabolites could have an impact on
pomegranate juice is relatively narrow. However, pomegranate memory neural circuits. Further, in a previous study, Olajide and
juice is a safe, easy-to-consume antioxidant nutrient beverage, co-workers (38) demonstrated that freeze-dried pomegranates
and any memory benefits that it may offer to an aging population inhibited inflammation, as well as amyloidogenesis in IL-1β-
are worth noting. stimulated SK-N-SH cells (39), and such anti-inflammatory
Other investigations have documented memory benefits of effects may support cognitive health. Other investigators have
pomegranate polyphenol consumption. A 2013 study (34) found (40, 41) that in patients with atherosclerosis, a year
reported that in patients undergoing heart surgery, a pomegranate of pomegranate juice supplementation reduced clogging in the
extract administered 1 wk before and 6 wk after the surgery carotid arteries compared with a control group, where the
protected against memory loss. In a mouse model of Alzheimer’s blockage increased. It is thus possible that regular pomegranate
disease (35), mice consuming pomegranate juice not only learned consumption could result in improved blood flow to brain regions
water maze tasks more quickly and swam faster, but also had controlling memory.
50% less accumulation of soluble amyloid-β42 and amyloid Several limitations of the present study deserve comment.
deposition in the hippocampus compared with control mice. Only 26% of the screened volunteers were included in the study,
These studies demonstrate improvements in a spatial learning and it is possible that our recruitment method yielded a sample of
and memory task as the result of pomegranate juice consumption motivated, educated, physically healthy subjects concerned about
consistent with our finding of visual memory benefits in humans age-related memory problems, which is not representative of
consuming pomegranate juice. the general population. Also, ∼23% of the enrolled participants
The ability of dietary manipulation to influence memory and dropped out before study completion. Although such completion
learning is well recognized. In addition to possible direct effects rates are comparable to those of other similar trials, this too
6 Siddarth et al.

Between-group statistics 2 ; effect size (95%

F(2, 257) = 5.90, P = 0.003; 0.45 (0.17, 0.73)


F(2, 257) = 1.12, P = 0.3; 0.01 (−0.26, 0.29)
F(2, 257) = 1.47, P = 0.2; 0.15 (−0.13, 0.43)

F(2, 257) = 0.14, P = 0.9; 0.09 (−0.19, 0.37)


F(2, 257) = 1.02, P = 0.4; 0.20 (−0.07, 0.48)
F(2, 257) = 0.38, P = 0.7; 0.07 (−0.21, 0.35)

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/ajcn/advance-article-abstract/doi/10.1093/ajcn/nqz241/5573003 by University of California, Los Angeles user on 09 December 2019
CI)3

FIGURE 2 Mean BVMT-R Visual Memory Learning Score compared


statistics from general linear models, with treatment group, time, and time × treatment group as predictors and age, sex, and education level as covariates.
with time in the pomegranate (baseline N = 130; 6 mo N = 103; 12 mo
% Change

N = 98) and placebo (baseline N = 131; 6 mo N = 104; 12 mo N = 102)


0.5
− 1.2
− 26.3
2.6

groups. Horizontal bar lines indicate 95% CIs of the means. Group by time
1
8

interaction is statistically significant, F(2, 257) = 5.90, P = 0.003; between-


group ES = 0.45. Change in the pomegranate group was not significant,
t(257) = 1.67, P = 0.1, whereas the placebo group showed significant decline,
t(257) = −3.19, P = 0.002. BVMT-R, Brief Visuospatial Memory Test-
119.3 (14.3)

86.0 (33.7)
Brief Visuospatial Memory Test-Revised; CLTR, Consistent Long-Term Retrieval; SRT, Buschke Selective Reminding Test.
(n = 102)

2.9 (1.6)
21.3 (6.0)

10.1 (2.3)
8.0 (2.3)

Revised; ES, effect size; Pom, pomegranate.


12-mo
Placebo drink

could have had an impact on the results. While we corrected


for multiple comparisons in our analyses, it is important to note
119.8 (14.5)

87.1 (34.5)
(n = 104)

3.5 (1.9)
20.4 (5.5)

10.2 (2.0)
8.1 (2.0)

that we detected a significant between-group difference in only


6-mo

one of the memory measures that we examined. In addition, we


controlled for education in all the analyses, but we cannot rule out
the possibility that the relatively small but significant difference
116.3 (14.2)

79.6 (33.2)

in educational level between the 2 treatment groups may have had


(n = 131)

3.8 (1.8)
21.2 (5.7)

10.0 (2.2)
8.1 (2.2)
Baseline

an influence on the results. Finally, measures such as cholesterol,


blood, or imaging biomarkers, which may have shed light on
mechanistic aspects, were not collected during this trial.
Despite such limitations, the study had several strengths,
% Change

including the double-blind placebo-controlled design, large


1.4

14.3
3.3
0
5

10

sample size, the relatively long (12 mo) treatment duration, and
the high compliance rates in both arms of the study. Further, we
included subjects with a range of cognitive abilities, thus allowing
119.2 (14.4)

88.1 (34.4)

for generalizability of results to all nondemented individuals aged


21.4 (5.0)

4.0 (2.2)

10.0 (2.1)
8.4 (1.9)
(n = 98)
12-mo

50–75 y.
Pomegranate juice
TABLE 2 Baseline, 6-mo, and 12-mo memory scores, and % changes1

Forgetfulness and memory loss are common experiences of


aging. Most people develop mild age-related cognitive challenges
that remain relatively stable, whereas others develop MCI, a more
120.1 (14.5)

90.8 (35.5)
(n = 103)
21.4 (5.1)

10.2 (2.0)
8.5 (1.9)
3.7 (2.0)

serious form of cognitive decline that is a risk factor for dementia.


6-mo

Even intense memory training interventions in nondemented


people have yielded only modest effect sizes in terms of
maintaining memory performance and have demonstrated only
115.4 (15.4)

80.1 (34.6)

limited transfer of training (42). Further, there is a significant


(n = 130)
21.1 (5.4)
8.0 (2.2)
3.5 (1.8)

10.0 (2.3)
Baseline

gap between the development of effective cognitive intervention


programs and convenient availability to the general population,
and a simple nutritional supplementation, such as pomegranate
juice, may be more accessible to many individuals. Thus, these
findings that pomegranate juice maintains visual memory skills
BVMT-R Delayed Recall

in middle-aged and older adults could have a potential impact on


BVMT-R Total Recall

visual memory issues commonly associated with aging.


SRT Delayed Recall
BVMT-R Learning
SRT Total Recall

3 Cohen’s d.
1 BVMT-R,

The authors’ contributions were as follows—PS, ZL, DH, and GWS:


Memory tests

SRT CLTR

designed the research; KJM, LME, DAM, and SMH: conducted the research;
PS: analyzed data; PS, ZL, DH, and GWS: wrote the manuscript; PS
2F

and GWS: had primary responsibility for final content; and all authors
read and approved the final manuscript. The University of California, Los
Memory effects of pomegranate juice 7
Angeles, owns a US patent (6,274,119) entitled “Methods for Labeling ß- 16. Schevey CT, Brewer MS. Effect of natural antioxidants and lipid model
Amyloid Plaques and Neurofibrillary Tangles;” which has been licensed to system on lipid oxidation. J Food Qual 2015;38(1):40–52.
Ceremark Pharma, LLC. GWS is among the inventors and has financial 17. West T, Atzeva M, Holtzman DM. Pomegranate polyphenols and

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/ajcn/advance-article-abstract/doi/10.1093/ajcn/nqz241/5573003 by University of California, Los Angeles user on 09 December 2019
interest in Ceremark Pharma, LLC. GWS also reports having served as an resveratrol protect the neonatal brain against hypoxic–ischemic injury.
Dev Neurosci 2007;29(4–5):363–72.
advisor to and/or having received lecture fees from AARP, Acadia, Allergan,
18. Evans HM, Howe PRC, Wong RHX. Effects of resveratrol on cognitive
Avanir, Axovant, Handok, Herbalife, Lundbeck, Lilly, Genentech, Novartis, performance, mood and cerebrovascular function in post-menopausal
Otsuka, Recruitment Partners, Roche, and Theravalues. DH reports receiving women; a 14-week randomised placebo-controlled intervention trial.
consulting fees from Herbalife and the McCormick Science Institute. All Nutrients 2017;9(1):E27.
other authors declare no conflicts of interest. 19. Witte AV, Kerti L, Margulies DS, Floel A. Effects of resveratrol
on memory performance, hippocampal functional connectivity, and
glucose metabolism in healthy older adults. J Neurosci 2014;34(23):
References 7862–70.
20. Wong RHX, Raederstorff D, Howe PRC. Acute resveratrol
1. Rusmaully J, Dugravot A, Moatti J-P, Marmot MG, Elbaz A, Kivimaki consumption improves neurovascular coupling capacity in adults
M, Sabia S, Singh-Manoux A. Contribution of cognitive performance with type 2 diabetes mellitus. Nutrients 2016;8(7):E425.
and cognitive decline to associations between socioeconomic factors 21. Brickman AM, Khan UA, Provenzano FA, Yeung LK, Suzuki W,
and dementia: a cohort study. PLoS Med 2017;14(6):e1002334. Schroeter H, Wall M, Sloan RP, Small SA. Enhancing dentate gyrus
2. Handels RLH, Wolfs CAG, Aalten P, Verhey FRJ, Severens JL. function with dietary flavanols improves cognition in older adults. Nat
Determinants of care costs of patients with dementia or cognitive Neurosci 2014;17(12):1798–803.
impairment. Alzheimer Dis Assoc Disord 2013;27(1):30–6. 22. Huhn S, Beyer F, Zhang R, Lampe L, Grothe J, Kratzsch J,
3. Tennstedt SL, Unverzagt FW. The ACTIVE study: study overview and Willenberg A, Breitfeld J, Kovacs P, Stumvoll M, et al. Effects
major findings. J Aging Health 2013;25(8 Suppl):3S–20S. of resveratrol on memory performance, hippocampus connectivity
4. Erickson KI, Voss MW, Prakash RS, Basak C, Szabo A, Chaddock L, and microstructure in older adults—a randomized controlled trial.
Kim JS, Heo S, Alves H, White SM, et al. Exercise training increases Neuroimage 2018;174:177–90.
size of hippocampus and improves memory. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 23. Bookheimer SY, Renner BA, Ekstrom A, Li Z, Henning SM, Brown JA,
2011;108(7):3017–22. Jones M, Moody T, Small GW. Pomegranate juice augments memory
5. Valls-Pedret C, Sala-Vila A, Serra-Mir M, Corella D, de la Torre R, and fMRI activity in middle-aged and older adults with mild memory
Martínez-González MÁ, Martínez-Lapiscina EH, Fitó M, Pérez-Heras complaints. Evidence-Based Complementary and Alternative Medicine
A, Salas-Salvadó J, et al. Mediterranean diet and age-related cognitive 2013. https://doi.org/10.1155/2013/946298.
decline. JAMA Intern Med 2015;175(7):1094–103. 24. Seeram NP, Henning SM, Zhang Y, Suchard M, Li Z, Heber D.
6. Miller KJ, Dye RV, Kim J, Jennings JL, O’Toole E, Wong J, Siddarth Pomegranate juice ellagitannin metabolites are present in human plasma
P. Effect of a computerized brain exercise program on cognitive and some persist in urine for up to 48 hours. J Nutr 2006;136(10):
performance in older adults. Am J Geriatr Psychiatry 2013;21(7): 2481–5.
655–63. 25. Seeram NP, Zhang Y, McKeever R, Henning SM, Lee RP, Suchard MA,
7. Park DC, Lodi-Smith J, Drew L, Haber S, Hebrank A, Bischof Li Z, Chen S, Thames G, Zerlin A, et al. Pomegranate juice and extracts
GN, Aamodt W. The impact of sustained engagement on cognitive provide similar levels of plasma and urinary ellagitannin metabolites in
function in older adults: the synapse project. Psychol Sci 2014;25(1): human subjects. J Med Food 2008;11(2):390–4.
103–12. 26. Rossetti HC, Lacritz LH, Munro Cullum C, Weiner MF. Normative data
8. Small GW, Siddarth P, Li Z, Miller KJ, Ercoli L, Emerson ND, Martinez for the Montreal Cognitive Assessment (MoCA) in a population-based
J, Wong KP, Liu J, Merrill DA, et al. Memory and brain amyloid sample. Neurology 2011;77(13):1272–5.
and tau effects of a bioavailable form of curcumin in non-demented 27. Beck AT, Ward CH, Mendelson M, Mock J, Erbaugh J. An inventory
adults: a double-blind, placebo-controlled 18-month trial. Am J Geriatr for measuring depression. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1961;4:561–71.
Psychiatry 2018;26(3):266–77. 28. Lezak MD, Howieson DB, Loring DW, Hannay HJ, Fischer JS.
9. Scholey AB, Tildesley NTJ, Ballard CG, Wesnes KA, Tasker A, Perry Neuropsychological Assessment(4th ed.). New York, NY Oxford
EK, Kennedy DO. An extract of Salvia (sage) with anticholinesterase University Press.
properties improves memory and attention in healthy older volunteers. 29. Gilewski MJ, Zelinski EM, Schaie KW. “The Memory Functioning
Psychopharmacology (Berl) 2008;198(1):127–39. Questionnaire for assessment of memory complaints in adulthood and
10. Miller JW, Harvey DJ, Beckett LA, Green R, Farias ST, Reed old age”: Correction to Gilewski et al. Psychol Aging 1992;7(2):298.
BR, Olichney JM, De Carli C. Vitamin D status and rates of https://doi.org/10.1037/h0090381.
cognitive decline in a multiethnic cohort of older adults. JAMA Neurol 30. Small GW, Siddarth P, Burggren AC, Kepe V, Ercoli LM, Miller KJ,
2015;72(11):1295–303. Lavretsky H, Thompson PM, Cole GM, Huang SC, et al. Influence
11. Kulzow N, Witte AV, Kerti L, Grittner U, Schuchardt JP, Hahn A, Floel of cognitive status, age, and APOE-4 genetic risk on brain FDDNP
A. Impact of omega-3 fatty acid supplementation on memory functions positron-emission tomography imaging in persons without dementia.
in healthy older adults. J Alzheimer’s Dis 2016;51(3):713–25. Arch Gen Psychiatry 2009;66(1):81–87.
12. Gelain DP, Antonio Behr G, Birnfeld De Oliveira R, Trujillo M. 31. Benedict RHB, Schretlen D, Groninger L, Dobraski M, Shpritz
Antioxidant therapies for neurodegenerative diseases: mechanisms, B. Revision of the Brief Visuospatial Memory Test: studies of
current trends, and perspectives. Oxid Med Cell Longev 2012;2012: normal performance, reliability, and validity. Psychol Assess 1996;8(2):
895153. 145–53.
13. Kesse-Guyot E, Fezeu L, Jeandel C, Ferry M, Andreeva V, Amieva 32. Buschke H. Selective reminding for analysis of memory and learning.
H, Hercberg S, Galan P. French adults’ cognitive performance after J Verbal Learn Verbal Behav 1973;12(5):543–50.
daily supplementation with antioxidant vitamins and minerals at 33. Chakraborty H. A Mixed Model Approach for Intent-to-treat Analysis
nutritional doses: a post hoc analysis of the supplementation in in Longitudinal Clinical Trials with Missing Values. Research Triangle
vitamins and mineral antioxidants (SU.VI.MAX) trial. Am J Clin Nutr Park (NC): RTI Press; 2009.
2011;94(3):892–9. 34. Ropacki SA, Patel SM, Hartman RE. Pomegranate supplementation
14. Tangney CC, Kwasny MJ, Li H, Wilson RS, Evans DA, Morris protects against memory dysfunction after heart surgery: a
MC. Adherence to a Mediterranean-type dietary pattern and cognitive pilot study. Evid Based Complement Alternat Med 2013.
decline in a community population. Am J Clin Nutr 2011;93(3): https://doi.org/10.1155/2013/932401.
601–7. 35. Hartman RE, Shah A, Fagan AM, Schwetye KE, Parsadanian M,
15. Subash S, Essa MM, Al-Adawi S, Memon MA, Manivasagam T, Schulman RN, Finn MB, Holtzman DM. Pomegranate juice decreases
Akbar M. Neuroprotective effects of berry fruits on neurodegenerative amyloid load and improves behavior in a mouse model of Alzheimer’s
diseases. Neural Regen Res 2014;9(16):1557–66. disease. Neurobiol Dis 2006;24(3):506–15.
8 Siddarth et al.

36. Li W, Dowd SE, Scurlock B, Acosta-Martinez V, Lyte M. Memory 40. Aviram M, Dornfeld L, Rosenblat M, Volkova N, Kaplan M, Coleman
and learning behavior in mice is temporally associated with diet- R, Hayek T, Presser D, Fuhrman B. Pomegranate juice consumption
induced alterations in gut bacteria. Physiol Behav 2009;96(4–5): reduces oxidative stress, atherogenic modifications to LDL, and platelet

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/ajcn/advance-article-abstract/doi/10.1093/ajcn/nqz241/5573003 by University of California, Los Angeles user on 09 December 2019
557–67. aggregation: studies in humans and in atherosclerotic apolipoprotein E-
37. Yuan T, Ma H, Liu W, Niesen DB, Shah N, Crews R, Rose K, deficient mice. Am J Clin Nutr 2000;71(5):1062–76.
Vattem DA, Seeram NP. Pomegranate’s neuroprotective effects against 41. Aviram M, Rosenblat M, Gaitini D, Nitecki S, Hoffman A, Dornfeld
Alzheimer’s disease are mediated by urolithins, its ellagitannin-gut L, Volkova N, Presser D, Attias J, Liker H, et al. Pomegranate juice
microbial derived metabolites. ACS Chem Neurosci 2016;7(1):26–33. consumption for 3 years by patients with carotid artery stenosis reduces
38. Olajide OA, Kumar A, Velagapudi R, Okorji UP, Fiebich BL. common carotid intima-media thickness, blood pressure and LDL
Punicalagin inhibits neuroinflammation in LPS-activated rat primary oxidation. Clin Nutr 2004;23(3):423–33.
microglia. Mol Nutr Food Res 2014;58(9):1843–51. 42. Rebok GW, Carlson MC, Langbaum JBS. Training and maintaining
39. Velagapudi R, Baco G, Khela S, Okorji U, Olajide O. Pomegranate memory abilities in healthy older adults: traditional and novel
inhibits neuroinflammation and amyloidogenesis in IL-1β-stimulated approaches. J Gerontol B Psychol Sci Soc Sci 2007;62 Spec No 1:
SK-N-SH cells. Eur J Nutr 2016;55(4):1653–60. 53–61.

You might also like