You are on page 1of 6

Applied Animal Behaviour Science 223 (2020) 104916

Contents lists available at ScienceDirect

Applied Animal Behaviour Science


journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/applanim

The effect of oxytocin on yawning by dogs (Canis familiaris) exposed to T


human yawns
Anna Kis*, Katinka Tóth, Orsolya Kanizsár, József Topál
Research Centre for Natural Sciences, Institute of Cognitive Neuroscience and Psychology, Hungarian Academy of Sciences, Magyar Tudósok krt. 2, 1117, Budapest,
Hungary

A R T I C LE I N FO A B S T R A C T

Keywords: Previous studies have found that human yawning is contagious to dogs, but the results are still controversial. It is
Dog also debated whether contagious yawning is a sign of empathy, and the physiological mechanisms behind this
Contagious yawning phenomenon are also unknown. Our goal was to further investigate if human yawning is contagious to dogs, and
Empathy how it is affected by intranasally administered oxytocin. We also tested if contagious yawning was related to
Oxytocin
dogsʼ empathetic skills measured via an owner-completed questionnaire. Dogs (N = 33) were presented with
human yawns (yawning condition – YC) or mouth openings (gaping condition – GC) on two test occasions after
intranasal administration of 12 IU oxytocin or placebo. Contagious yawning (defined as a significant increase in
the number of yawns in YC as compared to GC) was not found in either of the treatment groups and the number
of yawns was not related to the owner-reported emotion contagion of the dogs. However, oxytocin pre-treatment
significantly decreased the number of yawns in dogs (in general, but more particularly during the phase when
human yawns and gaping were presented). Furthermore, there was a positive relation between signs of stress and
anxiety (mouth licking) and the number of yawns (in general, but more particularly during the post phase when
human yawns and gaping were not any more presented). We conclude that dogs’ yawning observed in this
situation may largely be due to situational stress. Thus the difference between the oxytocin and the placebo
group is possibly caused by the decreased social stress and not by oxytocin’s hypothesised effect on empathy.

1. Introduction response to the experimenterʼs yawning in contrast to the control con-


dition when none of them did. This performance is comparable to or
Due to their special domestication history (Hare et al., 2002) as well even exceeds the level of yawn contagion in adult humans (Provine,
as their prolonged and intensive contact with humans (Wynne et al., 1986). In contrast, Harr et al. (2009) reported that human yawns pre-
2008) dogs have been shown to be an ideal model species for studying sented on videos were not contagious to dogs, while others (O’Hara and
different aspects of human social behaviour (Hare and Tomasello, 2005; Reeve, 2011) found that if contagious yawning does exist in dogs at all,
Miklósi and Topál, 2013) as well as its neural (Bunford et al., 2017), it is only present to a much lesser degree than originally proposed.
hormonal (Kis et al., 2017a) and genetic (Parker and Ostrander, 2005; Another study (Silva et al., 2012), however, has confirmed that human
Hejjas et al., 2007) background. Although even such easily measurable yawning is contagious to dogs, moreover according to their results the
markers of social competence as dogs’ infant-like ability to follow mere sound of yawning was enough to elicit the phenomenon. This
human pointing (Lakatos et al., 2009) are surrounded by vivid debates inconsistency of findings may largely be due to methodological differ-
(Riedel et al., 2008; Udell and Wynne, 2010; Elgier et al., 2012; ences. The experimental arrangement of the “original” study (Joly-
Pongrácz et al., 2013), it is increasingly assumed that dogs develop a Mascheroni et al., 2008), for example, has received lot of criticism
relatively deep understanding of nuances of interspecific (dog-human) because it was done in an unnatural setting; dogs had to sit facing the
social interactions. One of the presumed hallmarks of dogs’ social experimenter during the presentation of the yawns, thus the behaviour
competence is empathy, that makes it possible for human yawning to be labelled contagious yawning may not be related to ‘empathic response’
contagious to dogs (Joly-Mascheroni et al., 2008), although research in but might stem, at least partly, from situational stress (Silva and de
this field is still controversial (O’Hara and Reeve, 2011). Joly- Sousa, 2011).
Mascheroni et al. (2008) found that 72 % of the dogs yawned in While yawn contagion is often labelled as a complex capacity


Corresponding author.
E-mail address: vargane.kis.anna@ttk.mta.hu (A. Kis).

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.applanim.2019.104916
Received 3 June 2019; Received in revised form 15 November 2019; Accepted 17 November 2019
Available online 26 November 2019
0168-1591/ © 2019 Elsevier B.V. All rights reserved.
A. Kis, et al. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 223 (2020) 104916

related to empathy, yawning in itself is a wide-spread and common social behaviour including affiliative versus agonistic responses
behavioural event that consists of a long inspiratory phase, a brief acme (Romero et al., 2014; Hernádi et al., 2015), the use of human com-
and a rapid expiration (Daquin et al., 2001). The function of yawning, municative signals (Oliva et al., 2015; Oláh et al., 2017) and gaze in-
however, is not yet well understood (Guggisberg et al., 2007). In hu- itiation towards humans (Nagasawa et al., 2015; Kis et al., 2017b).
mans it is known that this semi-voluntary event increases vigilance and These results are generally and mostly in line with findings from human
aims to alert when sleepiness occurs, as well as probably has an im- studies, although important null-results have also been reported e.g. on
portant role for social communication, e.g. to synchronize group be- dogs’ attachment to humans (Thielke et al., 2017, but see: Kovács et al.,
haviour. Yawning has been found in all the major vertebrate classes, 2018). Moreover, similarly to humans (e.g. Bartz et al., 2011), con-
including several mammalian orders (e.g., primates, carnivores, and siderable individual as well as group-level differences have been re-
rodents), birds, reptiles, amphibians and fish (Baenninger and Greco, vealed in terms of dogs’ oxytocin-responsiveness (e.g. breed: Kis et al.,
1991). Despite the ubiquitous nature of yawning, several evolutionary 2014a; Kovács et al., 2016b; sex: Cimarelli et al., 2016; Kovács et al.,
hypotheses have been proposed suggesting that yawning has a specific 2016a). The effect of oxytocin on empathy and contagious yawning,
function in humans, mostly because of its contagiousness (Smith, however, has not yet been investigated in dogs.
1999). The purpose of the present study is threefold: 1) to assess the
Another widely studied and highly controversial topic is the role yawning-inducing effect of human yawns and the association between
oxytocin plays in the regulation of human-specific socio-cognitive yawning and overt signs of stress such as mouth licking in dogs; 2) to
capabilities. It is well known that in humans oxytocin, among others, test if yawn contagion is related to dogsʼ baseline empathy (measured
plays a pivotal role in social bonding (Donaldson and Young, 2008), as via an owner-completed questionnaire); and 3) to investigate the effect
well as in socio-cognitive skills (Savaskan et al., 2008). The oxytocin of intranasally administered oxytocin on the contagiousness of human
system is also related to psychiatric conditions such as autism, de- yawning to dogs. One possible prediction is that contagious yawning, as
pression and social anxiety disorders (Hollander et al., 2007). Fur- a form of empathetic behaviour, would increase after oxytocin treat-
thermore polymorphisms in the oxytocin receptor (OXTR) gene influ- ment, and would be unrelated to stress indicators. On the contrary, it
ence such complex human social behaviours as attachment (Gillath can also be predicted that dogs’ yawning in response to human yawns is
et al., 2008; Chen et al., 2011) or empathy (Rodrigues et al., 2009). The related to stress signs (unrelated to empathy), and would thus decrease
latter result ‒ the relationship between the oxytocin system and human after oxytocin treatment.
empathy ‒ is especially important for the present study. We have to
note, however, that empathy is a complex umbrella term, and its re- 2. Methods
lation to the oxytocin system is not straightforward. It has been shown
for example that intranasally administered oxytocin does not affect the 2.1. Ethical statement
activity of brain regions (anterior insula) responsible for empathic
distress (self-centred feelings of discomfort and anxiety in response to Research was done in accordance with the Hungarian regulations on
the perceived physical or emotional pain of another person – Singer animal experimentation and the Guidelines for the use of animals in
et al., 2008). On the other hand empathic concern (other-oriented research described by the Association for the Study Animal Behaviour
feelings of sympathy and compassion) are related to the oxytocin (ASAB). Ethical approval was obtained from the National Animal
system as it has been shown (Barraza and Zak, 2009) that after viewing Experimentation Ethics Committee (Ref No. XIV-I-001/531-4-2012).
a video of a young boy suffering from cancer there was a positive re-
lationship between the increase in peripheral oxytocin levels and feel- 2.2. Subjects
ings of empathic concern, but not empathic distress. Furthermore the
magnitude of the change in participants’ blood oxytocin level was re- 33 adult pet dogs (mean age ± SD: 4.68 ± 2.58 years) and their
lated to their baseline empathy measured via questionnaire. However, owners were recruited on a voluntary basis. All dogs were older than
there are some more recent results (Tabak et al., 2015) arguing that 1 year of age, as similarly to humans a developmental effect has been
empathic concern is related to the vasopressin, but not to the oxytocin observed in the contagiousness of yawning in dogs (Harr et al., 2009;
system. Results regarding the relationship between the oxytocin system O’Hara and Reeve, 2011). Subjects in our study were both mixed
and the third component of empathy, cognitive empathy (the capacity (n = 14) and purebred (n = 19), intact (n = 11) and neutered
to understand an otherʼs mental state without feeling in the same way) (n = 22), male (n = 19), and female (n = 14), single dog (n = 16) and
are also controversial. Some (Domes et al., 2007) have shown that in- from multiple-dog households (n = 17). Subjects were randomly as-
tranasally administered oxytocin increases adult participant’s perfor- signed to test conditions and the two conditions did not differ in subject
mance in a “mind-reading” task where they have to attribute emotions properties.
to photographs representing a pair of eyes. Another study (Rodrigues
et al., 2009) has found an association between performance in the same 2.3. Procedure
task and single nucleotide polymorphisms in the OXTR gene. But there
are results (Hurlemann et al., 2010) suggesting that intranasally ad- The experiment took place in a room (3.9 m × 4.1 m) at the Family
ministered oxytocin is only related to empathic concern and not to Dog Project laboratory, Eötvös University, Budapest. Subjects partici-
cognitive empathy. Although the effect of oxytocin on contagious pated in two test occasions with a minimum of 1 and a maximum of 13
yawning has not yet been investigated in humans either, there is an days in-between (mean ± SD: 5.43 ± 3.60 days) receiving oxytocin
indirect relationship as autistic children have lower plasma oxytocin and placebo treatment (see later) in a counterbalanced order. Both the
levels (Modahl et al., 1998) and show diminished susceptibility to owner and the experimenter were blind to the treatment and owners
contagious yawning (Senju et al., 2007; Helt et al., 2010). were unable to guess if their dogs had received oxytocin or placebo on
In case of dogs it has already been shown that the oxytocin system is either the first (Binomial test; p = 0.608) or the second (p = 1.000)
related to human-directed social behaviour (for a summary of the most occasions. Subjects were randomly assigned to either the Yawning
recent results see Kis et al., 2019). The classic findings include evidence (N = 15) or the Gaping (control; N = 18) condition (see later), parti-
for an increase in peripheral oxytocin levels not only after positive cipating in this condition after both the oxytocin and the placebo pre-
social interactions (Odendaal and Meintjes, 2003; Handlin et al., 2011; treatment. Test occasions in both conditions (and for both pre-treat-
Mitsui et al., 2011; Hritcu et al., 2019), but also following intranasal ments) consisted of first a test phase (during which the human ex-
oxytocin treatment (Temesi et al., 2017). Such intranasal oxytocin perimenter presented yawns or gaping) and then a post phase of quiet
treatments have also been found to influence various forms of dogs’ waiting without any yawns or gaping presented.

2
A. Kis, et al. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 223 (2020) 104916

2.3.1. Oxytocin and placebo treatment, waiting period Gaping. The test phase of the gaping condition differed from the
The pretreatment was performed following a protocol previously yawning in that in this case instead of yawning the experimenter re-
validated by showing the effect of oxytocin on heart rate and heart rate peatedly opened the mouth wide saying a prolonged [a:] sound of
variability (Kis et al., 2014b). Dogs were administered 3 puffs of pla- 5–10 s duration, and then closed her mouth. The test phase was also
cebo (0.7 % NaCl solution), or 3 puffs of oxytocin (12 IU, Syntocinon, followed by a 5-minute-long post phase without gaping.
Novartis) intranasally to alternating nostrils. This was followed by a Coding of the video recordings after the test revealed that dogs in
40 min waiting period necessary for the central oxytocin levels to reach the gaping condition witnessed (defined as orienting towards the ex-
plateau (Born et al., 2002). During this time dogs spent the first 30 min perimenter) a similar number of demonstrations as in the yawning
with an on-leash walk with their owner and the experimenter at the condition: average ( ± SD) 19 ± 3.6 gapings accounting for the
university campus. The experimenter ensured that neither the owner 60.3 ± 7.3 % of the gapings performed in the placebo groups and an
nor other people or dogs made any social contact (e.g. eye-contact, dog- average ( ± SD) 18 ± 2 gapings accounting for the 61 ± 9.2 % of the
directed talk) with the dog as this could have affected oxytocin levels gapings performed in the oxytocin group.
(Nagasawa et al., 2015).
For the remaining 10 min dogs and owners were left alone in the 2.4. Data analysis
behavioural laboratory. Dogs were off the leash and free to move in the
room, while owners were sitting on a chair and instructed not to look at The number of elicited yawns was coded from the video recordings
or interact with their dogs. Owners were informed that they would be during both the test and the post phases as the main outcome measure.
video recorded from that moment on and they were asked to fill in a The number of mouth licks was also coded from the videos. Coding was
canine empathy questionnaire ‒ a child empathy questionnaire (Rieffe blind to subject details and oxytocin / placebo treatment. From the
et al., 2010) adapted for dogs ‒ during the waiting period. Owners were questionnaire data the Emotion contagion factor was extracted ac-
instructed to answer all questions to their best knowledge, even if some cording to the guidelines of Rieffe et al. (2010). Data analysis was
of the items are anthropomorphic and/or they cannot recall the exact carried out with Generalized Linear Mixed Model (with dog ID as
same situation. The questionnaire consisted of 20 items and the owners random factor) using the number of yawns as the target variable and
had to rate how often the given behaviour occurred in their dog (1: condition (yawning versus gaping), phase (test versus post), pre-treat-
never ‒ 5: always). The items belonged to three scales: Emotion Con- ment (oxytocin versus placebo) as well as the number of mouth licks
tagion, Attention to Others’ Feelings, and Prosocial Actions. For the and questionnaire measured emotion contagion as predicting variables.
current analysis solely the Emotion Contagion scale was used, this Main effects and all possible two-way interactions were examined,
consisted of the following seven items: “when another dog cries, my dog using backward elimination to step-by-step eliminate the least sig-
gets upset too” “when other dogs fight, my dog gets upset”, “my dog nificant factors from the model. All analyses were carried out with SPSS
also needs to be comforted when another dog is in pain” “when another version 22.
dog makes a bad fall, shortly after my dog pretends to fall too” “when
another dog gets frightened, my dog freezes or starts to cry” “when 3. Results
another dog cries, my dog looks away” “when another dog is upset, my
dog needs to be comforted too”. Contagious yawning was not convincingly observable in our sub-
jects, as there was only a non-significant tendency in the placebo group
2.3.2. Behavioural test for more yawns in the yawning compared to the gaping condition
The experimental procedure was based on that used by Madsen and (condition × pre-treatment: F(1,121) = 2.815, p = 0.096; condition
Persson (2013) thus in an effort to make the situation as natural as main effect: F(1,121) = 0.144, p = 0.705; Fig. 1). It was found that
possible, dogs were free to move in the experimental room while both oxytocin pre-treatment significantly decreased the number of yawns,
the owner and a female experimenter were seated on the floor. Subjects especially during the test phase (pre-treatment × phase:
on both occasions (once after oxytocin and once after placebo pre- F(1,121) = 4.499, p = 0.036; pre-treatment main effect: F(1,121) = 4.459,
treatment, in a random order) participated in the same test condition: p = 0.037).
they could see the experimenter either yawning or gaping. The tests In addition the number of yawns was strongly related to the number
were carried out by three female experimenters, with an even dis- of mouth licks, especially during the post phase (mouth licks × phase:
tribution among the two conditions. Each subject had the same ex-
perimenter on both occasions.
Yawning. During the 5 min of the test phase the experimenter re-
peatedly yawned for at least 15 times. Experimenters yawned as
naturally as possible, with yawning defined as opening the mouth fully,
drawing in air, tilting the head and body backwards, closing the eyes,
and producing a vocalisation during exhalation, for a total duration of
5–10 s. As the mere sound of human yawning has also been shown to be
contagious to dogs (Silva et al., 2012), subjects were not forced to look
at the experimenter and make eye-contact during the demonstrations, it
was sufficient for the experimenter to be in the dogsʼ peripheral vision.
The test phase was followed by a 5-min-long post phase during which
no yawns were produced, the experimenter and the owner were just
talking to each other.
Coding of the video recordings after the test revealed that dogs in
the placebo group witnessed (defined as orienting towards the experi-
menter) on average ( ± SD) 18 ± 7 yawns during the demonstration
accounting for the 59 ± 17 % of the yawns performed. Subjects in the
oxytocin group witnessed on average ( ± SD) 15 ± 5 yawns during the
demonstration accounting for the 53 ± 18 % of the yawns performed.
This number was similar to those in previous experiments (e.g. 16 ± 2 Fig. 1. The number of elicited yawns (median, quartiles, whiskers, outliers) for
in Madsen and Persson, 2013). the different phases, conditions and pre-treatments.

3
A. Kis, et al. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 223 (2020) 104916

realistic to evoke empathic-like behaviour (Szánthó et al., 2017).


It has also been raised (Silva et al., 2011) that yawning in the canine
contagious yawning paradigms might be an artefact resulting from the
stress of the experimental situation. Others (Romero et al., 2013),
however, found no indication of elevated stress during the contagious
yawning test based on heart rate measures. Future studies might
overcome these controversies by directly comparing subtly different
paradigms for both contagious yawning as well as stress indicators. In
the present study we also found that, especially during the post phase of
both conditions, the number of yawns was positively related to the
number of mouth licks, a commonly observed sign of stress and anxiety
(e.g. Pastore et al., 2011). Since mouth licks were not presented by the
experimenter in either of the conditions (and as their number was re-
lated to the number of dogs’ yawns) it is plausible to assume that
yawning was mostly also a manifestation of stress. Our results about the
effect of oxytocin point to the same direction, as it was found that,
especially during the test phase, oxytocin pre-treated dogs yawned
Fig. 2. Relationship between the number of elicited yawns and mouth licks for considerably less, a finding consistent with oxytocin’s stress relieving
the differentphases, conditions and pre-treatments. effect (Neumann, 2002). If, on the other hand, oxytocin would have
induced a more empathetic response as hypothesised, the opposite ef-
F(1,121) = 8.234, p = 0.005; mouth licks main effects: F(1,121) = 14.100, fect should have been found (an increase in the number of yawns,
p < 0.001; Fig. 2). The main effect of phase was not significant specifically in the yawning condition).
(F(1,121) = 1.038, p = 0.310). In sum our results provide evidence for the stress relieving effect of
The effect of questionnaire measured emotion contagion (empathy) oxytocin in a social situation, as well as shed further light on the con-
was not significant neither as a main effect, nor in interaction with any troversies of the commonly used contagious yawning paradigm.
other variables (all p > 0.1).
Declaration of Competing Interest

4. Discussion We wish to confirm that there are no known conflicts of interest


associated with this publication and there has been no significant fi-
The present study provides mixed results regarding the effect of nancial support for this work that could have influenced its outcome.
oxytocin on dogs’ yawn contagion. The lack of contagious yawning in We confirm that the manuscript has been read and approved by all
the placebo group contradicts previous findings (Joly-Mascheroni et al., named authors and that there are no other persons who satisfied the
2008), although some (Harr et al., 2009) have found similar negative criteria for authorship but are not listed. We further confirm that the
results. Our findings are also in line with the fact that yawn contagion order of authors listed in the manuscript has been approved by all of us.
in humans emerges only in preschool-age children reaching adult levels We confirm that we have given due consideration to the protection
at around the age of 12 years (Anderson and Meno, 2003; Millen and of intellectual property associated with this work and that there are no
Anderson, 2011), while dogs’ socio-cognitive capacities are generally impediments to publication, including the timing of publication, with
comparable to that of considerably younger children (Lakatos et al., respect to intellectual property. In so doing we confirm that we have
2009; Péter et al., 2014). Emotion contagion, however, has been found followed the regulations of our institutions concerning intellectual
in dogs both in response to positively and negatively valenced sound property.
playbacks (Huber et al., 2017) as well as in the form of rapid mimicry We further confirm that any aspect of the work covered in this
(Palagi et al., 2015). Thus we would not argue for the lack of emotion manuscript that has involved either experimental animals or human
contagion in dogs, in fact the tendency we found in the placebo groups patients has been conducted with the ethical approval of all relevant
hints for a weak yawn contagion in our subjects as well. However, bodies and that such approvals are acknowledged within the manu-
based on several of our current findings we might argue that yawning in script.
this contagious yawning test situation is not a form of empathic re-
sponse. Acknowledgements
The lack of association between dogsʼ owner-reported emotion
contagion and the number of elicited yawns suggests dissociation be- We thank the Family Dog Project (led by Ádám Miklósi) for pro-
tween these two measures. The relationship between empathy and viding laboratory space and materials. Financial support was provided
contagious yawning is also debated in case of humans. Some (Chartrand by the Hungarian Scientific Research Fund (OTKA FK128242 &
and Bargh, 1999) argue that more empathetic people are more prone to K128448), the BIAL Foundation (grant no 169/16) and the János Bolyai
all form of social mimicry and thus might be more susceptible to yawn Research Scholarship of the Hungarian Academy of Sciences.
contagion (Platek et al., 2003). Others (Deputte, 1994) differentiate two
types of yawns in primates, the so-called rest yawns, and the social References
yawns; with only this latter belonging to social mimicries. Similar
theories ‒ yawning as social mimicry (Yoon and Tennie, 2010) ‒ have Anderson, J.R., Meno, P., 2003. Psychological influences on yawning in children. Curr.
also been proposed for dogs, as it has been found that dogs living in a Psychol. Lett. 11. http://cpl.revues.org/index390.html.
Baenninger, R., Greco, M., 1991. Some antecedents and consequences of yawning.
rescue shelter yawned more at mostly unfamiliar human models com- Psychol. Rec. 41, 453–460.
pared to pet dogs (O’Hara and Reeve, 2011). This might be explained by Barraza, Ja, Zak, P.J., 2009. Empathy toward strangers triggers oxytocin release and
contact-deprived dogs recognizing the potential for new social affilia- subsequent generosity. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 1167, 182–189. https://doi.org/10.
1111/j.1749-6632.2009.04504.x.
tion and thus responding by returning non-consciously mimicking ac- Bartz, J.A., Zaki, J., Bolger, N., Ochsner, K.N., 2011. Social effects of oxytocin in humans:
tions. Measuring dogs’ empathy with an owner-completed ques- context and person matter. Trends Cogn. Sci. 15, 301–309.
tionnaire is also a controversial issue, although such surveys still seem Born, J., Lange, T., Kern, W., et al., 2002. Sniffing neuropeptides: a transnasal approach to
the human brain. Nat. Neurosci. 5, 514–516. https://doi.org/10.1038/nn849.
to be the most reliable, as behavioural tests are usually not sufficiently

4
A. Kis, et al. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 223 (2020) 104916

Bunford, N., Andics, A., Kis, A., et al., 2017. Canis familiaris as model for non-invasive gestures. Anim. Cogn. 12, 621–631. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10071-009-0221-4.
comparative neuroscience. Trends Neurosci. 40, 438–452. https://doi.org/10.1016/j. Madsen, E.A., Persson, T., 2013. Contagious yawning in domestic dog puppies (Canis
tins.2017.05.003. lupus familiaris): the effect of ontogeny and emotional closeness on low-level imi-
Chartrand, T.L., Bargh, J.A., 1999. The chameleon effect: the perception-behavior link tation in dogs. Anim. Cogn. 16, 233–240. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10071-012-
and social interaction. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 76, 893–910. 0568-9.
Chen, F.S., Barth, M.E., Johnson, S.L., et al., 2011. Oxytocin receptor (OXTR) poly- Miklósi, Á, Topál, J., 2013. What does it take to become “best friends”? Evolutionary
morphisms and attachment in human infants. Front. Psychol. 2 (200). changes in canine social competence. Trends Cogn. Sci. 17, 287–294. https://doi.org/
Cimarelli, G., Turcsán, B., Bánlaki, Z., et al., 2016. Dog owners’ interaction styles : their 10.1016/j.tics.2013.04.005.
components and associations with reactions of pet dogs to a social. Front. Psychol. 7, Millen, A., Anderson, J.R., 2011. Neither infants nor toddlers catch yawns from their
1979. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2016.01979. mothers. Biol. Lett. 7, 440–442.
Daquin, G., Micallef, J., Blin, O., 2001. Yawning. Sleep Med. Rev. 5, 299–312. Mitsui, S., Yamamoto, M., Nagasawa, M., et al., 2011. Urinary oxytocin as a noninvasive
Deputte, B.L., 1994. Ethological study of yawning in primates. I. Quantitative analysis biomarker of positive emotion in dogs. Horm. Behav. 60, 239–243. https://doi.org/
and study of causation in two species of old world monkeys (Cercocebus albigena and 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2011.05.012.
Macaca fascicularis). Ethology 98, 221–245. Modahl, C., Green, L., Fein, D., et al., 1998. Plasma oxytocin levels in autistic children.
Domes, G., Heinrichs, M., Michel, A., et al., 2007. Oxytocin improves “mind-reading” in Biol. Psychiatry 43, 270–277.
humans. Biol. Psychiatry 61, 731–733. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2006.07. Nagasawa, M., Mitsui, S., En, S., et al., 2015. Oxytocin-gaze positive loop and the coe-
015. volution of human-dog bonds. Science 348 (80), 333–336.
Donaldson, Z.R., Young, L.J., 2008. Oxytocin, vasopressin, and the neurogenetics of so- Neumann, I.D., 2002. Involvement of the brain oxytocin system in stress coping: inter-
ciality. Science 322 (80), 900–904. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1158668. actions with the hypothalamo-pituitary-adrenal axis. In: Poulain, D., Oliet, S.,
Elgier, A.M., Jakovcevic, A., Mustaca, A.E., Bentosela, M., 2012. Pointing following in Theodosis, D. (Eds.), Progress in Brain Research.
dogs: are simple or complex cognitive mechanisms involved? Anim. Cogn. 15, O’Hara, S.J., Reeve, A.V., 2011. A test of the yawning contagion and emotional con-
1111–1119. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10071-012-0534-6. nectedness hypothesis in dogs, Canis familiaris. Anim. Behav. 81, 335–340. https://
Gillath, O., Shaver, P.R., Baek, J.-M., Chun, D.S., 2008. Genetic correlates of adult at- doi.org/10.1016/j.anbehav.2010.11.005.
tachment style. Pers. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 34, 1396–1405. Odendaal, J.S.J., Meintjes, R.A., 2003. Neurophysiological correlates of affiliative beha-
Guggisberg, A.G., Mathis, J., Herrmann, U.S., Hess, C.W., 2007. The functional re- viour between humans and dogs. Vet. J. 165, 296–301. https://doi.org/10.1016/
lationship between yawning and vigilance. Behav. Brain Res. 179, 159–166. S1090-0233(02)00237-X.
Handlin, L., Hydbring-sandberg, E., Nilsson, A., et al., 2011. Short-term interaction be- Oláh, K., Topál, J., Kovács, K., et al., 2017. Gaze-following and reaction to an aversive
tween dogs and their owners : effects on oxytocin, cortisol, insulin and heart rate — social interaction have corresponding associations with variation in the OXTR gene in
an exploratory study. Anthrozoos 24, 301–315. dogs but not in human infants. Front. Psychol. 8, 1–13. https://doi.org/10.3389/
Hare, B., Brown, M., Williamson, C., Tomasello, M., 2002. The domestication of social fpsyg.2017.02156.
cognition in dogs. Science 298 (80), 1634–1636. https://doi.org/10.1126/science. Oliva, J.L., Rault, J.-L., Appleton, B., Lill, A., 2015. Oxytocin enhances the appropriate use
1072702. of human social cues by the domestic dog (Canis familiaris) in an object choice task.
Hare, B., Tomasello, M., 2005. Human-like social skills in dogs? Trends Cogn. Sci. 9, Anim. Cogn. 18, 767–775. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10071-015-0843-7.
439–444. Palagi, E., Nicotra, V., Cordoni, G., 2015. Rapid Mimicry and Emotional Contagion in
Harr, A.L., Gilbert, V.R., Phillips, Ka, 2009. Do dogs (Canis familiaris) show contagious Domestic Dogs.
yawning? Anim. Cogn. 12, 833–837. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10071-009-0233-0. Parker, H.G., Ostrander, Ea, 2005. Canine genomics and genetics: running with the pack.
Hejjas, K., Vas, J., Topál, J., et al., 2007. Association of polymorphisms in the dopamine PLoS Genet. 1, e58.
D4 receptor gene and the activity-impulsivity endophenotype in dogs. Anim. Genet. Pastore, C., Pirrone, F., Balzarotti, F., et al., 2011. Evaluation of physiological and be-
38, 629–633. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1365-2052.2007.01657.x. havioral stress-dependent parameters in agility dogs. J. Vet. Behav. 6, 188–194.
Helt, M.S., Eigsti, I., Snyder, P.J., Fein, D.A., 2010. Contagious yawning in autistic and https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jveb.2011.01.001.
typical development. Child Dev. 81, 1620–1631. Péter, A., Gergely, A., Topál, J., et al., 2014. A simple but powerful test of perseverative
Hernádi, A., Kis, A., Kanizsár, O., et al., 2015. Intranasally administered oxytocin affects search in dogs and toddlers. Q. J. Exp. Psychol. 1–12. https://doi.org/10.1080/
how dogs (Canis familiaris) react to the threatening approach of their owner and an 17470218.2014.970206.
unfamiliar experimenter. Behav. Processes 119, 1–5. https://doi.org/10.1016/j. Platek, S.M., Critton, S.R., Myers, T.E., Jr, G.G.G., 2003. Contagious yawning: the role of
beproc.2015.07.001. self-awareness and mental state attribution. Cognit. Brain Res. 17, 223–227.
Hollander, E., Bartz, J.A., Chaplin, W., et al., 2007. Oxytocin increases retention of social Pongrácz, P., Gácsi, M., Hegedüs, D., et al., 2013. Test sensitivity is important for de-
cognition in autism. Biol. Psychiatry 61, 498–503. tecting variability in pointing comprehension in canines. Anim. Cogn. 16, 721–735.
Hritcu, L.D., Horhogea, C., Ciobica, A., Spataru, M.C., Spataru, C., Kis, A., 2019. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10071-013-0607-1.
Conceptual replication of canine serum oxytocin increase following a positive dog- Provine, R.R., 1986. Yawning as a stereotyped action pattern and releasing stimulus.
human interaction. Rev. Chim. 70 (5), 1579–1581. Ethology 72, 109–122.
Huber, A., Barber, A.L.A., Faragó, T., et al., 2017. Investigating emotional contagion in Riedel, J., Schumann, K., Kaminski, J., et al., 2008. The early ontogeny of human–dog
dogs (Canis familiaris) to emotional sounds of humans and conspecifics. Anim. Cogn. communication. Anim. Behav. 75, 1003–1014. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.anbehav.
20, 703–715. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10071-017-1092-8. 2007.08.010.
Hurlemann, R., Patin, A., Onur, Oa, et al., 2010. Oxytocin enhances amygdala-dependent, Rieffe, C., Ketelaar, L., Wiefferink, C.H., 2010. Assessing empathy in young children:
socially reinforced learning and emotional empathy in humans. J. Neurosci. 30, construction and validation of an Empathy Questionnaire (EmQue). Pers. Individ. Dif.
4999–5007. https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5538-09.2010. 49, 362–367. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2010.03.046.
Joly-Mascheroni, R.M., Senju, A., Shepherd, A.J., 2008. Dogs catch human yawns. Biol. Rodrigues, S.M., Saslow, L.R., Garcia, N., et al., 2009. Oxytocin receptor genetic variation
Lett. 4, 446–448. https://doi.org/10.1098/rsbl.2008.0333. relates to empathy and stress reactivity in humans. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U. S. A. 106,
Kis, A., Bence, M., Lakatos, G., et al., 2014a. Oxytocin receptor gene polymorphisms are 21437–21441.
associated with human directed social behavior in dogs (Canis familiaris). PLoS One Romero, T., Konno, A., Hasegawa, T., 2013. Familiarity Bias and physiological responses
9, e83993. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0083993. in contagious yawning by dogs support link to empathy. PLoS One 8, e71365.
Kis, A., Ciobica, A., Topál, J., 2017a. The effect of oxytocin on human-directed social https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0071365.
behaviour in dogs (Canis familiaris). Horm. Behav. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.yhbeh. Romero, T., Nagasawa, M., Mogi, K., et al., 2014. Oxytocin promotes social bonding in
2017.06.001. dogs. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. 111, 9085–9090. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.
Kis, A., Hernádi, A., Miklósi, B., Kanizsár, O., Topál, J., 2017b. The way dogs (Canis 1322868111.
familiaris) look at human emotional faces is modulated by oxytocin. an eye-tracking Savaskan, E., Ehrhardt, R., Schulz, A., et al., 2008. Post-learning intranasal oxytocin
study. Front. Behav. Neurosci. 11, 210. modulates human memory for facial identity. Psychoneuroendocrinology 33,
Kis, A., Kanizsár, O., Gácsi, M., Topál, J., 2014b. Intranasally administered oxytocin 368–374.
decreases heart rate and increases heart rate variability in dogs. J. Vet. Behav. 9, e15. Senju, A., Maeda, M., Kikuchi, Y., et al., 2007. Absence of contagious yawning in children
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jveb.2014.09.050. with autism spectrum disorder. Biol. Lett. 3, 706–708. https://doi.org/10.1098/rsbl.
Kis, A., Oliva, J.L., Virányi, Z., Topál, J., 2019. Editorial: oxytocin and social behaviour in 2007.0337.
dogs and other (self-) domesticated species: methodological caveats and promising Silva, K., Bessa, J., de Sousa, L., 2012. Auditory contagious yawning in domestic dogs
perspectives. Front. Psychol. 10, 732. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2019.00732. (Canis familiaris): first evidence for social modulation. Anim. Cogn. 2–5. https://doi.
Kovács, K., Kis, A., Kanizsár, O., et al., 2016a. The effect of oxytocin on biological motion org/10.1007/s10071-012-0473-2.
perception in dogs (Canis familiaris). Anim. Cogn. 19, 513–522. https://doi.org/10. Silva, K., de Sousa, L., 2011. “Canis empathicus”? A proposal on dogs’ capacity to em-
1007/s10071-015-0951-4. pathize with humans. Biol. Lett. 7, 489–492. https://doi.org/10.1098/rsbl.2011.
Kovács, K., Kis, A., Pogány, Á, et al., 2016b. Differential effects of oxytocin on social 0083.
sensitivity in two distinct breeds of dogs (Canis familiaris). Silva, K., De Sousa, L., de Sousa, L., 2011. “Canis empathicus”? A proposal on dogs’ ca-
Psychoneuroendocrinology 74, 212–220. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psyneuen.2016. pacity to empathize with humans. Biol. Lett. 7, 489–492. https://doi.org/10.1098/
09.010. rsbl.2011.0083.
Kovács, K., Virányi, Z., Kis, A., Turcsán, B., Hudecz, Á., Marmota, M.T., Topál, J., 2018. Singer, T., Snozzi, R., Bird, G., et al., 2008. Effects of oxytocin and prosocial behavior on
Dog-owner attachment is associated with oxytocin receptor gene polymorphisms in brain responses to direct and vicariously experienced pain. Emotion 8, 781–791.
both parties. a comparative study on austrian and hungarian border collies. Front. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0014195.
Psychol. 9, 435. Smith, E.O., 1999. Yawning: an evolutionary perspective. Hum. Evol. 14, 191–198.
Lakatos, G., Soproni, K., Dóka, A., Miklósi, Á, 2009. A comparative approach to dogs’ Szánthó, F., Miklósi, Á, Kubinyi, E., 2017. Is your dog empathic? Developing a dog
(Canis familiaris) and human infants’ comprehension of various forms of pointing emotional reactivity survey. PLoS One 12, e0170397. https://doi.org/10.1371/

5
A. Kis, et al. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 223 (2020) 104916

journal.pone.0170397. 2017.01699.
Tabak, B.A., Meyer, M.L., Castle, E., et al., 2015. Vasopressin, but not oxytocin, increases Udell, M.A.R., Wynne, C.D.L., 2010. Ontogeny and phylogeny: both are essential to
empathic concern among individuals who received higher levels of paternal warmth: human-sensitive behaviour in the genus Canis. Anim. Behav. 79, e9–e14. https://doi.
a randomized controlled trial. Psychoneuroendocrinology 51, 253–261. https://doi. org/10.1016/j.anbehav.2009.11.033.
org/10.1016/j.psyneuen.2014.10.006. Wynne, C.D.L., Udell, M.A.R., Lord, Ka., 2008. Ontogeny’s impacts on human–dog com-
Temesi, A., Thuróczy, J., Balogh, L., Mikósi, Á, 2017. Increased serum and urinary oxy- munication. Anim. Behav. 76, e1–e4. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.anbehav.2008.03.
tocin concentrations after nasal administration in beagle dogs. Front. Vet. Sci. 4, 147. 010.
Thielke, L.E., Rosenlicht, G., Saturn, S.R., Udell, M.A.R., 2017. Nasally-administered Yoon, J.M.D., Tennie, C., 2010. Contagious yawning: a reflection of empathy, mimicry, or
oxytocin has limited effects on owner-directed attachment behavior in pet dogs contagion? Anim. Behav. 79, e1–e3. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.anbehav.2010.02.
(Canis lupus familiaris). Front. Psychol. 8, 1–12. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg. 011.

You might also like