You are on page 1of 10

ACADEMIC EMPLOYEES WORKPLACE

DECEPTION DETECTION KNOWLEDGE

PSYCHOLOGY AND EDUCATION: A MULTIDISCIPLINARY JOURNAL


2023
Volume: 6
Pages: 703-711
Document ID: 2022PEMJ477
DOI: 10.5281/zenodo.7502766
Manuscript Accepted: 2023-3-1
Psych Educ,2023, 6: 703-711, Document ID: PEMJ477, doi:10.5281/zenodo.7502766, ISSN 2822-4353
Research Article

Academic Employees’ Workplace Deception Detection Knowledge


Apple Grace Bonhoc*, Maico Demi Aperocho
For affiliations and correspondence, see the last page.

Abstract
Humans are known as fallible lie detectors, often considered inept and nescient in unmasking deceit. To
unravel the truth behind this speculation, this organizational communication study assessed the academic
employees' workplace deception (WD) detection knowledge when using Facial micro-expressions (FME),
context, verbal, vocal, and non-verbal cues. It determined the significant difference in their WD knowledge
after exposure to the treatments, investigated their ways in cognitive and psychological processing of cues
when detecting WD, and examined how their WD detection knowledge influenced their communication
behavior in the organization. Non-equivalent Control Group pretest-posttest quasi-experimental design was
employed for this purpose. Results indicate that both control and treatment groups were much knowledgeable
when using FME before and after exposure to Facial Micro-Expression Training (FMET). While findings
reveal no significant difference in the treatment group's WD detection knowledge after FMET, the said group
shows an increase from less to moderate knowledge after Online Deception Detection Training (OODT). The
researchers believe that much is yet to be discovered in the field of detecting deceptive communication
involving academic employees. The outbreak of the Corona Virus Disease 19 (COVID-19) in the Philippines
might have caused disruptions and disadvantages, but these limitations may be turned into a light that brings
in fresh perspectives. Hence, it is recommended that more studies in deception detection be conducted in the
Philippines.

Keywords: organizational communication, workplace deception detection, academic employees,


facial micro-expressions, employee communication behavior.

Introduction within educational environments to detect lies


(Aamodt & Custer, 2006; Vrij et al., 2006). Little is
W o r k p l a c e d e c e p t i o n ( WD) is an act of known about academic employees' cognitive and
communicating a false message(s) to a coworker (e.g., psychological ways of processing cues to detect WD.
superior, subordinate, peer) to achieve personal over Limited research is also available today on how WD
organizational goals. It breaks trust and wanes detection knowledge influences the academic
workplace relationships. If left undetected or employees' communication behavior in their
unaddressed, it will not take long until it creates a organization.
community filled with cynicism and contempt. WD
leads to highly stressful interactions, often more Out of this very minimal study, some researchers
stressful than actual job requirements (Waldron, 2000, claim that teachers rely on stereotypical beliefs or
cited by Bryant and Sias, 2011). It also catalyzes a invalid deception cues and have a stronger truth bias
toxic work environment and blocks potential which hinder them from having accurate detection
workforce collaboration. It is deemed illegal and (Marksteiner et al., 2011, Ulatowska, 2017). Their bias
hinders profitability (Lee & Mckenzie, 1995 as cited in likely stems from their expectation of being honest
Bryant and Sias, 2011). WD, as a form of social interactants and the demands ascribed to their
organizational conflict, may be present in profession.
conversations and discourses of employees and may
eventually result in the breakdown of organizational In the Philippines, teachers must adhere to its code of
communication (Miller, 2015). With the string of lies ethics (e.g., Article 5, Section 1 of the Code of Ethics
and strained workplace relationships, building a for Professional Teachers: “Teacher shall, at all times,
learning organization will be impossible (Senge, be imbued with the spirit of professional loyalty,
2004). mutual confidence, and faith in one another, self-
sacrifice for the common good, and full cooperation
For the past years, the field of deception detection
with colleagues.”) The standards, mandates, directives,
research has attracted p sy cho log ists and
and missions given to teachers may greatly influence
communication scholars alike. While many of them
their beliefs and biases on deception detection. To our
focused on deception in general human interactions
knowledge, the Filipino teachers’ beliefs about
(Nortje & Tredoux, 2019; Stel et al., 2020; Izotovas et
al., 2020), few have approached the issue in academic deception cues are still unknown as of this writing,
workplaces and have investigated the ability of people making it imperative to have academic employees as
the population for this study. Although this research is
Bonhoc & Aperocho 703/711
Psych Educ,2023, 6: 703-711, Document ID: PEMJ477, doi:10.5281/zenodo.7502766, ISSN 2822-4353
Research Article

limited to academic employees from one institution


and only concentrated on their WD detection
knowledge and its influence on their communication
behavior in the organization, this may still serve as a Methodology
good study context and starting point for deception
detection research in the Philippines.
Research Design
More than addressing the shortage of deception
detection studies in the academic domain and the The researchers used a quantitative approach,
Philippines, this study was conducted with academic specifically a non-equivalent control group pretest-
employees as the respondents because their profession posttest quasi-experimental design. Two groups were
exposes them to varied deceptive interactions such as formed and named Section Undercover or Treatment
Group and Section Sherlock or Control Group. Both
lies committed by students (Ulatowska, 2016). The
groups took the Workplace Deception Detection
biennial report prepared by Josephson Institute of
Knowledge Test (WDDKT), Facial Micro-Expressions
Ethics (JIoE, 2012) shows that 55 percent of the
Assessment (FMEA) pretest, and posttest, but only
students had lied to a teacher about something.
Section Undercover underwent the 5-day online
Similarly, Nemenzo’s (2018) study shows that in a
training. For Online Training Day (OTD) 1, the topics
particular Philippine-based school, teachers highly
were Introduction to Deception Detection, and
problematize students who lie to them. Falsification
Introduction to Workplace Deception (WD) Detection
and concealment are the students’ common methods of
while OTD 2 was about WD Detection through verbal
deception as they make excuses for late work and cues, and OTD 3 for WD Detection through vocal
attendance issues (Griffin et al., 2015). Alarmingly, cues. OTD 4 focused on WD Detection through non-
several students, except first years, perceived their verbal cues, and finally, OTD 5 was about WD
professors as less honest (Tamayo, 2014). This finding Detection through Facial Micro Expressions Training.
may apparently suggest that workplace deception
indeed exists in academic institutions. Table 1. Diagramming Notation, Application of Non-
equivalent Control Group pretest-posttest Quasi-
To help address these concerns, Driskell (2012), as Experimental Design
cited by Ulatowska (2016), emphasized the importance
of increasing teachers’ ability to detect deceit through
professional training. Ability and knowledge in
detecting deception is germane in uncovering the true
intention, motivation, and emotions of students and co-
academic employees. Given these contexts, the
researchers were prompted to conduct this quantitative
investigation as a steppingstone for further research
that will recommend an appropriate training for
workplace deception detection.

For the purpose of this study, the researchers sought


answers to the following: (1) What is the level of the
academic employees’ Workplace Deception (WD)
detection knowledge when using the following cues Respondents of the Study
before the quasi-experiment: (a) facial micro-
expressions; and (b) context, verbal, vocal, and non- The respondents of this study were administrators,
verbal cues?; and (2) Is there a significant difference in faculty and non-teaching personnel from a junior high
the level of the treatment group’s WD detection school in Davao City. To ensure the equal distribution
knowledge after the conduct of the quasi-experiment of subjects in the Treatment and Control Group, four
and exposure to the treatments? Generally, this study (4) subjects must be deducted from the categories with
assessed the academic employees’ level of knowledge odd numbers (e.g. Faculty and AA/NTP), making 116
in using communication cues (e.g., facial micro- total number of respondents with 58 males and 58
expressions, context, verbal, vocal, and non-verbal
cues) to detect WD before the quasi-experiment and
after the treatment group’s exposure to the treatments.

Bonhoc & Aperocho 704/711


Psych Educ,2023, 6: 703-711, Document ID: PEMJ477, doi:10.5281/zenodo.7502766, ISSN 2822-4353
Research Article

females. However, because of the subjects’ tight Research Instruments


schedule and heavy workloads caused by the transition There were two (2) research instruments in this study.
from traditional to online teaching, there was a very These were the Workplace Deception Detection
slow response from the targeted subjects. To make it Knowledge Test (WDDKT) and the Facial Micro-
easier for them and to expedite the process, they were Expression Assessment (FMEA) or the Pretest-
asked to fill-out an online informed consent form. Posttest. The treatment group was also exposed to a
Despite several follow-ups and requests, only few had treatment, the Facial micro-expressions Training
expressed their willingness to participate in the study. (FMET). The Workplace Deception Detection
The tabular presentation below shows the total number Knowledge Test (WDDKT) was a 40-item validated
of academic employees who accomplished the multiple-choice test. The questions from items 1-20
Informed Consent Form (ICF) and actually were mostly about vocal cues to deception and
participated in the study. The study’s subjects also deceptive statements, while 21-40 were generally
satisfied the selection criteria: to have at least one (1) about WD situations performed by/towards a superior,
year of work experience/length of service in the subordinate, or peer. Items 30, 32, 33, and 35 were
selected locale to qualify for this study and they are with digital illustrations to help visualize the facial
natural-born Filipino citizens to observe better how micro-expressions. The rubric and interpretation guide
Filipinos conceal facial micro-expressions and detect in assessing the respondents’ WD detection knowledge
WD through it. level can be found below.

Table 2. Actual distribution for the pretest-posttest Table 3. Rubric for the Workplace Deception
Quasi-Experimental Design Detection Knowledge Test (WDDKT)

Sampling Procedure

This study employed two random sampling


procedures, namely stratified random sampling, and
convenience sampling. The researchers divided the
population (academic employees with signed ICF) into
strata based on their shared characteristics such as age
and length of service. The codes for each stratum were
A1Y1, A1Y2, A2Y1, A2Y2. The samples were then
taken from each coded stratum and distributed to the
control and treatment groups. Later in the middle of
the study, the volunteer subjects encountered varied The Facial Micro Expression Assessment (FMEA) was
personal and work-related concerns due to the the second research instrument for this study. This was
struggles of online teaching and stress caused by the based on the Micro-Expressions Assessment and
pandemic. Consequently, some of them were only able Training of the Paul Ekman Group (PEG). With the
to take the pre-test and unable to continue the rest of permission of the instruments’ proponent, the
the assessments. This scenario prompted the researchers made their version of the FMEA for the
researchers to pursue convenience sampling: those pretest and posttest using storyboards. The storyboards
who were only willing and available participated in the in digital sketch and smudge paint forms showed the
study. faces of the talents and labels that described the
specific facial movement present for a particular
emotion. For example, to reveal the surprise facial
expression, the eyebrows must be raised but not drawn
together, eyes are widened, and the jaw dropped down.

Bonhoc & Aperocho 705/711


Psych Educ,2023, 6: 703-711, Document ID: PEMJ477, doi:10.5281/zenodo.7502766, ISSN 2822-4353
Research Article

The videos, as stimuli, have three frames, each with 1


second total running time. The first frame showed the
neutral or blank facial expression characterized by the
lack of emotion; the second frame depicted a particular
emotion's facial micro-expression and was
immediately concealed and morphed to the neutral or
blank facial expression. The subjects were asked to Results and Discussion
identify the suppressed emotion in the video by
observing the facial micro-expression. Moreover, the
FMEA contained the baseline facial micro-expression This organizational communication study assessed the
video and two videos for each universal emotion (e.g., academic employees' knowledge in detecting
happy, anger, sad, fear, disgust, contempt, surprise), Workplace Deception (WD) before the quasi-
thus producing 14 videos/items in the first part. The experiment and after their exposure to the treatments
second part of the FMEA required the subjects to such as Facial Micro-Expressions Training (FMET)
watch a YouTube video clip found in this link and Online Deception Detection Training (ODDT).
https://www.youtube.com/watch?v=YaB7wr27gag&t= This research also tested two null hypotheses. It was
1s from GMA News' which was published last 4 May postulated that there was no difference in the level of
2020 with the caption "24 Oras: Woman evicted from the treatment group's WD detection knowledge in
rented home, claims barangay officials threaten to take using facial micro-expressions as cues after having
her baby." The subjects viewed the video clip from experienced FMET. It was also speculated that if the
4:13 to 5:04 and after that, identified whether the treatment group underwent the ODDT, there would
accused is lying based on facial micro-expressions. still be no difference in their level of WD detection
The third and last part showed the interview with a knowledge using context verbal, vocal, and non-verbal
Filipina government worker. The interview questions cues. To evaluate the academic employees' WD
were work-related and contextualized to the current detection knowledge, the control group and treatment
pandemic scenarios. The subjects were asked to group took the 19-item Facial Micro-Expression
identify her emotions using facial micro-expressions as Assessment (FMEA) and 40-item Workplace
cues. There is a total of 19 items for FMEA. The table Deception Detection Knowledge Test (WDDKT).
below contains the rubric and interpretation guide for
the Facial Micro-Expressions Assessment. Level of the Academic Employees’ WD Detection
Knowledge when Using Facial Micro-Expressions as
Table 4. Rubric for the Facial Micro-Expressions Cues before the Quasi-Experiment
Assessment (FMEA)
Frequency, percentage counts, weighted mean, and t-
test were used to test the hypotheses. The result on the
level of academic employees’ Workplace Deception
detection knowledge when using facial micro-
expressions before the quasi-experiment was reflected
in Table 5. The table consists of five columns. The
first column contained the descriptive level and range
of scores from 0-4 points (less knowledgeable) to 15-
19 points (very much knowledgeable). The second and
third column contained the data for the control group,
while the fourth and fifth columns were for the
treatment group. The mean was indicated at the bottom
part of the table.

Bonhoc & Aperocho 706/711


Psych Educ,2023, 6: 703-711, Document ID: PEMJ477, doi:10.5281/zenodo.7502766, ISSN 2822-4353
Research Article

Table 5. Level of academic employees’ Workplace The statistical analysis outcome showed that in the
Deception (WD) detection knowledge when using pre-test, the control group has a mean score of 13. The
facial micro expressions before the quasi- experiment treatment group also took the same WDDKT prior to
undergoing the Online Deception Detection Training
(ODDT) and they got a mean score of 12. This
indicated that both groups fell under the “Less
Knowledgeable” level during the pre-test. In this
study's rubric, the score fell under the "Less
Knowledgeable" level. This also implied that the
respondents had prior context-relevant WD detection
knowledge, but it was not in-depth. It also suggested that
although they were assumed to be familiar with the
o rg a n i z a t io n 's con text and their co-
workers/conversation partners, their misconceptions
about detecting deception hinder them from
uncovering the truth.
Table 5 shows that in the pre-test, the 27 respondents
Overall, the results revealed that the respondents were
under the control group have a mean score of 10,
“Much Knowledgeable” in using FME as cues to
which implied that based on FMEA and this study's
detect deception before the experiment and the
rubric, they were much knowledgeable in detecting
treatments. They had, however, recognized the
WD using facial micro-expressions as cues. The
difficulty in spotting the specific leakages and facial
treatment group with 20 respondents also fell on the
muscle movement. Their determination of emotions
same level with 12 as the cohort's mean score, two
using FME cues was often done unknowingly,
points higher than the control group's mean. In this
unconsciously, or through peripheral routes. On the
study, the level "Much knowledgeable" suggested that
other hand, their WDDKT results show that they are
the respondents from both groups know how to detect
under the “Less Knowledgeable” level when using
concealed emotions by spotting the person's facial
context, verbal, vocal, and non-verbal cues in detecting
expression leakage.
deception. From their narratives, it can be inferred that
their low mean scores may be attributed to
Level of the Academic Employees’ WD Detection
misconceptions, lack of experience in using cues, low
Knowledge when Using Context, Verbal, Vocal,
motivation to detect deception, and partial analysis of
and Non-verbal Cues before the Quasi-Experiment manipulated information.

Aside from measuring the respondents’ WD detection Significant Difference in the Level of Treatment
knowledge using FME, their knowledge when using Group’s WD Detection Knowledge when Using
context, vocal, and non-verbal cues as means of
FME after the Conduct of the Quasi-Experiment
deception detection was also examined using the
and Exposure to FMET
Workplace Deception Detection Knowledge Test
(WDDKT). The same statistical tools were utilized to
The Treatment Group was exposed to Facial Micro-
analyze their scores, and the results are displayed in
Expression Training (FMET), one of the treatments in
Table 6.
this study. The training and the stimulus video
Table 6. Level of academic employees’ Workplace
materials that it contained were inspired by Paul
Deception (WD) detection knowledge when using Ekman Group’s Micro-Expressions Training Tool
vocal, verbal and non-verbal cues before the quasi- (METT). Jordan et al. (2019) critiqued that METT was
experiment not effective in increasing their respondents’ deception
detection accuracy. Previous research also suggested
that frequent training did not lead to better deception
detection (Kassin & Fong, 1999). However, more
recent studies took an optimistic stance that training
enhances the participants' accuracy in interpreting
behaviors through facial micro-expressions
(Matsumoto & Hwang, Hyi Sung (2011) Crews et al.,
2007; Reinhard et al., 2011). Given these pieces of
literature, the researchers hypothesized that after the
treatment group underwent the FMET, there will be no

Bonhoc & Aperocho 707/711


Psych Educ,2023, 6: 703-711, Document ID: PEMJ477, doi:10.5281/zenodo.7502766, ISSN 2822-4353
Research Article

significant difference in the group’s WD detection The outcome of the testing also prompted not to reject
knowledge level when using FME as cues to detect the null hypothesis, instead to reserve judgment
deception. This null hypothesis was tested, and its considering that there was insufficient evidence and
results are displayed in Table 7. limited sample to conclude that FMET could increase
the WD detection knowledge of academic employees
Table 7. Significant difference in the level of treatment when using facial micro-expressions. Their WD
group’s WD Detection knowledge after the conduct of detection knowledge level may not have differed
the quasi-experiment when using Facial Micro significantly because they opted for the peripheral
Expressions (FME) route when decoding facial expressions. Their
exposure to the treatment (FMET) and the stimulus
materials (e.g., training videos) might not have created
an impact as they were unable to thoroughly scrutinize
the facial muscles' movement that hides the talent's
real emotion in the video. It is also possible to infer
As shown in Table 7, there was no remarkable that the respondents were unable to raise their
difference between the pretest and posttest mean performance to the “Very Much Knowledgeable” level
scores of the treatment group respondents despite the as they were used to clustering the facial micro-
one-point increase. Clearly, this improvement was not expressions with other cues (e.g., context) and
statistically significant because the p-value is higher maximizing the baseline information during actual
than .05 (p-value = .110 > .05). The results indicate conversations.
that the Facial Micro-Expression Training (FMET) has
little or no effect on the respondents’ level of WD Lastly, the treatment group was asynchronously
detection knowledge when using facial micro- exposed to the treatments for two months. The time
expressions as cues to detect deception. Their score they had identified and spent may not be sufficient for
remained in “Much Knowledgeable” level failing to FMET to elicit a significant difference. The
reach the “Very Much Knowledgeable” level. The respondents' experiences during the surge of
hypothesis was, therefore, supported by the findings. COVID-19 in Davao City, Philippines, might have
also affected the efficacy of their asynchronous online
It appeared that the result of the hypothesis testing was training and the respondents' engagement towards it.
also consistent with the findings and literature from the
study of Jordan et al. (2019), as the training/treatment Taken together, the result suggests us that after having
did not enhance the Facial micro-expressions been exposed to the treatment (FMET), the treatment
Assessment (FMEA) scores of the respondents. The group’s mean scores did not differ significantly; and
outcome of this testing also prompted the re- the treatment had not caused a notable difference. It
examination of the variables contained in the was argued that their current level of WD knowledge
conceptual framework. It led to the cognizance that when using FME cues was retained because, in their
although the dependent variable (WD detection prior experiences, they used to cluster it with other
knowledge) was rationally related to the independent cues and did not use it as a sole basis for deception
variable (training/treatment), the statistical value detection. It was also revealed that while they claim to
shows that the magnitude of difference caused by the be attentive, their observation of the micro-expression
independ en t variable and observed in the appears to be done inadvertently. As such, they could
sample/treatment group was weak and insignificant. not specify the facial muscle movement that may point
Thus, if exceeding the treatment group’s pre-test out the leakage of the hidden emotion. The time that
FMEA scores and eventually reaching “Very Much the respondents allocated for the asynchronous online
Knowledgeable” level in the post test was the indicator training and the COVID-19 situation in the city might
of the FMET/treatment’s effectiveness, then the result have also indirectly affected their engagement and
reveals that FMET was unable to satisfy that receptiveness towards the FMET.
requirement. This showed that FMET would also be
more likely ineffective in the population of academic Significant Difference in the Level of Treatment
employees. In addition, the statistical difference Group’s WD Detection Knowledge when Using
generated through the sample/subjects could not also Context, Verbal, Vocal, and Non-verbal Cues after
be made as a basis for making an inference of FMET’s the Conduct of the Quasi-Experiment and
effectiveness towards the population. Exposure to ODDT

In this research, the treatment group's pretest and

Bonhoc & Aperocho 708/711


Psych Educ,2023, 6: 703-711, Document ID: PEMJ477, doi:10.5281/zenodo.7502766, ISSN 2822-4353
Research Article

posttest mean scores for the Workplace Deception treatment may have caused the increase in the
Detection Knowledge Test (WDDKT) were compared. treatment group’s WD detection knowledge level.
This was done to ascertain the likelihood that the
treatment or that the Online Deception Detection Given the positive result and rise in the treatment
Training (ODDT) could cause a difference and group's WD detection knowledge, this study rejected
effectively increase the treatment group's WD the null hypothesis. The findings showed a difference
detection knowledge when using context, verbal, in the academic employees' WD detection knowledge
vocal, and non-verbal cues. after exposure to the treatment or ODDT.

Considering the previously mentioned contrasting In essence, this quasi-experimental study was
literature about the effectiveness of training to enhance conducted using the 19-item Facial Micro-Expressions
deception detection accuracy, the researchers decided Assessment (FMEA) and the 40-item Workplace
to test the null hypothesis that there was no difference Deception Detection Knowledge Test (WDDKT). It
in the treatment group's WD detection knowledge level also exposed the treatment group to the treatments
after undergoing the treatment or ODDT. The result of (e.g., Facial Micro-Expression Training (FMET) and
the hypothesis testing is presented in Table 8. Online Deception Detection Training (ODDT). The
quasi-experiment was done to ascertain a significant
Table 8. Significant difference in the level of treatment difference in the level of the treatment group’s WD
group’s WD Detection knowledge after the conduct of detection knowledge when using facial micro-
the quasi-experiment when using vocal, verbal, and expressions (FME) and context, verbal, vocal, and
non-verbal cues non-verbal cues after their exposure to the treatments.

The results revealed no significant difference in the


treatment group's WD detection knowledge level when
using FME after their exposure to Facial Micro-
Expression Training (FMET). It remained in the
It can be gleaned from Table 8 that after the treatment "Much Knowledgeable" level and was unable to
group’s exposure to the treatment/Online Deception increase. In a way, this finding supported Jordan et al.
Detection Training (ODDT), the group gained a (2019) that the micro-expressions training tool
statistically significant increase in their performance, (METT) from the Paul Ekman Group (PEG), the basis
from their pretest mean score of 12 to a posttest mean of Facial Micro-Expression Training (FMET), was
score of 18, as supported by a p-value lower than .05 ineffective as it was unable to enhance the
(p-value = .000 < .05). participants’ performance. It also showed that
participants often cluster the FME with e.g., context,
This implied that the treatment group’s (N = 20) WD non-verbal cues, and maximize the baseline
detection knowledge level had increased from “Less information when detecting deception during actual
Knowledgeable” to “Moderately Knowledgeable” conversations.
unlike the control group (N = 27), who did not
undergo the training. Moreover, the treatment group’s WD detection
knowledge when using context, verbal, vocal, and non-
This also conveyed that the treatment or ODDT may verbal cues improved after the Online Deception
have enhanced the respondents’ context-relevant WD Detection Training (ODDT). The result showed a
detection knowledge and caused their mean scores to significant difference given the increase of their level
improve from 12 to 18. Opposite to the FMET, which from the “Less Knowledgeable” to “Moderately
was inspired by METT, the ODDT was created by this Knowledgeable” level. The participants confirmed that
study’s researcher based on Paul Ekman Group, the ODDT influenced them to be more conscious and
Goman (2013), Vrij (2008), and Rolls (2011)). Thus, aware of communicative cues to deception. Given this
there was no available published literature that concurs finding, the hypothesis was, therefore, rejected.
or refutes its effectiveness. It can only be deduced that
there was a possible existing relationship between the
Conclusion
dependent variable (WD detection knowledge) and
ODDT (independent variable). The statistical
difference also indicated the likelihood that the said This researchers believe that much is yet to be
discovered in the field of detecting deceptive
communication involving academic employees. The

Bonhoc & Aperocho 709/711


Psych Educ,2023, 6: 703-711, Document ID: PEMJ477, doi:10.5281/zenodo.7502766, ISSN 2822-4353
Research Article

outbreak of the Corona Virus Disease 19 (COVID-19) and that captures online interaction dynamics. This
in the Philippines might have caused disruptions and may be done to ascertain if there is an improvement in
disadvantages, but these limitations may be turned into the receivers’ attentiveness, ability to adjust
a light that brings in fresh perspectives. communication strategies, recognition of facial
expressions, and generation of meaning through
To this study’s locale, the academic employees may be feedback when there are more available contexts and
given online training on Facial Micro-Expressions information that are also typically present in real-life
using FMET, OODT, and another material that deceptive episodes.
captures online interaction dynamics. Considering that
the new normal necessitates adjustments for having a Researchers who want to conduct a deception
lean medium of communication, it is best if they are detection study in the Philippines may delve deeper
trained to be mindful and conscious in decoding FME into Filipinos’ verbal communication, expressions, and
and other communicative cues in an online setting. linguistic markers of deception behavior. This study
The knowledge and skill that they can gain from it may enable communicators to effectively decode
may help them detect possible WD, at the same time, deceptive messages and protect themselves from being
interpret their colleagues’ thoughts, motivations, and lied to.
emotions. In effect, this may stimulate good
conversations, nurture friendship and work References
relationships.
Aamodt, M. & Custer, H. (2006). Who can best catch a liar? A
Interesting future research may also stem from the meta-analysis of individual differences in detecting deception.
limitations encountered in the making of this study. https://www.researchgate.net/publication/232424344_Who_can_bes
Considering the “new normal” of organizations, t _ c a t c h _ a _ l i a r _ A _ m e t a -
analysis_of_individual_differences_in_detecting_deception
res earch t ack l in g ac ad em i c em p lo y ee s’
communication style, organization’s communication Bryant, E. & Sias, P. (2011). Sensemaking and relational
flow of direction in a virtual setting, academic consequences of peer coworker deception. Communication
monographs, 1,
employees’ work-from-home positive and negative
115-137.https://www.academia.edu/18024001/Sensemaking_and_R
communication behavior (e.g., virtual workplace elational_Consequences_of_Peer_Coworker_Deception
deception) may also be explored.
Driskell, J. E. (2012). Effectiveness of deception detection training:
A meta-analysis. Psychology, Crime, & Law, 18, 713–731.
This present study may also be further extended in Ekman, P. (1970). Universal facial expressions of emotion.
many ways by communication scholars and other California Mental Health Research Digest, 8(4), 151-158.
interested researchers. Because this was conducted https://psycnet.apa.org/record/1972-06605-001%20Ekman,%20P.%
during the Corona Virus Disease 19 (COVID-19) 20(1985).%20Telling%20lies,%20clues%20to%20deceit%20in%20t
he marketplace, politics, and marriage. New York, NY: W.W.
outbreak in the Philippines, the researchers adjusted Norton
the methodologies to observe social distancing
restrictions and safety protocols. Consequently, the Ekman, P. (1992). Telling lies: Clues to deceit in the marketplace,
politics and marriage.
limitations adversely affected the researchers’ capacity http://communicationcache.com/uploads/1/0/8/8/telling_lies_clues_t
to gather data from different locations and a larger o_deceit_in_the_marketplace_politics_and_marriage.pdf
population. The data for this study were collected in
Ekman, P. (2006). Darwin, deception, and facial expression. Annals
one academic institution only. This sampling
of the New York Academy of Sciences,1000(1), 205-221. doi:
drawback may limit the ability to generalize the results 10.1196/annals.1280.010
to the larger population. Additional data taken from
the same population and several other populations Ekman, P. & O'Sullivan, M. (1991). Who can catch a liar? The
American Psychological Association, 46(9), 913-920.
would be helpful to corroborate the findings. https://pdfs.semanticscholar.org/4387/dcd6339f9f070ea9915f5b62a0
5c77ebd5a8.pdf
This study employed Facial Micro-Expression
Assessment (FMEA) and Facial Micro-Expression Ekman, P., O'Sullivan, M. & Frank, M. (1999). A few can catch a
liar. The American Psychological Association, Inc. Vol. 10, No.(3),
Tool (FMET) to test and train the respondents. The https://www.paulekman.com/wpcontent/uploads/2013/07/A-Few-Ca
academic employees had a chance to spot the leakages n-Catch-A-Liar.pdf
and determine the real emotion through the model’s
Griffin, D. J., San Bolkan, & Goodboy, A. K. (2015). Academic
facial expression as depicted in the video. Although dishonesty beyond cheating and plagiarism: Students’ interpersonal
this was logical and plausible, it would be interesting deception in the college classroom. ResearchGate; Taylor & Francis
to examine and craft an assessment that evokes
communication, participants’ emotion, and motivation

Bonhoc & Aperocho 710/711


Psych Educ,2023, 6: 703-711, Document ID: PEMJ477, doi:10.5281/zenodo.7502766, ISSN 2822-4353
Research Article

(Routledge). deception? A review of current techniques and theories. South


https://www.researchgate.net/publication/283793358_Academic_Di Afri c a n Jou rn al of P s y c h ol o g y , 49 (4 ), 49 1– 504 .
shonesty_Beyond_Cheating_and_Plagiarism_Students%E2%80%99 https://doi.org/10.1177/0081246318822953
_Interpersonal_Deception_in_the_College_Classroom
R o l l s , D. ( 2 0 1 1 ) H o w t o s p o t a
Goman, C. (2013). The truth about lies in the workplace. Berret- liar.https://www.slideshare.net/danrolls1/how-to-spot-a-liar
Koehler Publishers, Inc.
https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/abs/10.1111/j.1751- Stel, M., Schwarz, A., Dijk, E., Knippenberg, A. (2020) The limits
9020.2010.00344.x of conscious deception detection: When reliance on false deception
cues contributes to inaccurate judgments.
Izotovas, A., Vrij, A., Hope, L., Strömwall, L. A., Granhag, P. A., https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fpsyg.2020.01331/full
& Mann, S. (2020). Deception detection in repeated interviews: The
effects of immediate type of questioning on the delayed accounts. Tamayo, A. (2014). Virtues of honesty in a higher education
Journal of Investigative Psychology and Offender Profiling, 17(3), institution. American Journal of Social Sciences, 2(1),1-6.
224–237. https://doi.org/10.1002/jip.1561
Ulatowska, J. (2017) Teachers’ beliefs about cues to deception and
Jordan, S., Brimbal, L., Wallace, D. B., Kassin, S. M., Hartwig, M., the ability to detect deceit. Educational Psychology.
& Street, C. N. H. (2019). A test of the micro‐expressions training https://www.researchgate.net/publication/308051468_Teachers'_beli
tool: Does it improve lie detection? Journal of Investigative efs_about_cues_to_deception_and_the_ability_to_detect_deceit
Psychology and Offender Profiling, 16(3), 222–235.
https://doi.org/10.1002/jip.1532
Affiliations and Corresponding Information
Josephson Institute of Ethics. (2012). Report card on American
Apple Grace L. Bonhoc, DComm
youth. http://www.josephsoninstitute.org/reportcard
• Ateneo De Davao University, Philippines
Miller, K. (2015) Organizational communication: Approaches and • University of the Philippines Open University
processes. Arizona State University: Cengage Learning (UPOU)
Nemenzo, N. B. (2018, April 20). Problems encountered by teachers
in the teaching-learning process: A basis of an action plan. Maico Demi B. Aperocho, Ph.D.
https://www.researchgate.net/publication/324606765_Problems_Enc • College of Arts and Sciences Education University
ountered_by_Teachers_in_the_TeachingLearning_Process_A_Basis of Mindanao, Davao City, Philippines
_of_an_Action_Plan
• Department of Modern Language and Literature
Nortje, A. & Tredoux, C. (2019). How good are we at detecting Gonzaga University Spokane, WA, USA

Bonhoc & Aperocho 711/711

You might also like