You are on page 1of 12

The Journal of Pain, Vol 17, No 9 (September), 2016: pp 1001-1012

Available online at www.jpain.org and www.sciencedirect.com

Disrupted Self-Perception in People With Chronic Low


Back Pain. Further Evaluation of the Fremantle Back
Awareness Questionnaire

Benedict Martin Wand,* Mark Jon Catley,y Martin Ian Rabey,z,x Peter Bruce O’Sullivan,z
Neil Edward O’Connell,{ and Anne Julia Smithz
*The School of Physiotherapy, The University of Notre Dame Australia, Fremantle, Western Australia, Australia.
y
Sansom Institute for Health Research, University of South Australia, Adelaide, South Australia, Australia.
z
School of Physiotherapy and Exercise Science, Curtin University, Perth, Western Australia, Australia.
x
Neuroscience Research Australia, University of New South Wales, Randwick, New South Wales, Australia.
{
Department of Clinical Sciences, Health Economics Research Group, Institute of Environment, Health and Societies,
Brunel University, London, United Kingdom.

Abstract: Several lines of evidence suggest that body perception is altered in people with chronic
back pain. Maladaptive perceptual awareness of the back might contribute to the pain experience as
well as serve as a target for treatment. The Fremantle Back Awareness Questionnaire (FreBAQ) is a
simple questionnaire recently developed to assess back-specific altered self-perception. The aims of
this study were to present the outcomes of a comprehensive evaluation of the questionnaire’s
psychometric properties and explore the potential relationships between body perception, nocicep-
tive sensitivity, distress, and beliefs about back pain and the contribution these factors might play in
explaining pain and disability. Two hundred fifty-one people with chronic back pain completed the
questionnaire as well as a battery of clinical tests. The Rasch model was used to explore the question-
naires’ psychometric properties and correlation and multiple linear regression analyses were used to
explore the relationship between altered body perception and clinical status. The FreBAQ appears
unidimensional with no redundant items, has minimal ceiling and floor effects, acceptable internal
consistency, was functional on the category rating scale, and was not biased by demographic or clin-
ical variables. FreBAQ scores were correlated with sensitivity, distress, and beliefs and were uniquely
associated with pain and disability.
Perspective: Several lines of evidence suggest that body perception might be disturbed in people
with chronic low back pain, possibly contributing to the condition and offering a potential target for
treatment. The FreBAQ was developed as a quick and simple way of measuring back-specific body
perception in people with chronic low back pain. The questionnaire appears to be a psychometrically
sound way of assessing altered self-perception. The level of altered self-perception is positively
correlated with pain intensity and disability as well as showing associations with psychological
distress, pain catastrophization, fear avoidance beliefs, and lumbar pressure pain threshold. In this
sample, it appears that altered self-perception might be a more important determinant of clinical
severity than psychological distress, pain catastrophization, fear avoidance beliefs, or lumbar pres-
sure pain threshold.
ª 2016 The Authors. Published by Elsevier Inc. on behalf of the American Pain Society. This is an open
access article under the CC BY-NC-ND license (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-nd/4.0/).
Key words: Chronic low back pain, psychometrics, Rasch analysis, body image.

Received January 29, 2016; Revised May 30, 2016; Accepted June 1, 2016. Address reprint requests to Benedict Martin Wand, PhD, School of
Physiotherapy, The University of Notre Dame Australia, 19 Mouat St, Fre-
M.I.R. was supported by an Australian Postgraduate Award, Curtin Uni- mantle, WA 6959, Australia. E-mail: benedict.wand@nd.edu.au
versity Postgraduate Scholarship, Musculoskeletal Association of Char-
tered Physiotherapists Doctoral Award and the Chartered Society of 1526-5900
Physiotherapy Charitable Trust. The funding sources had no role in study ª 2016 The Authors. Published by Elsevier Inc. on behalf of the Amer-
design; the collection, analysis and interpretation of data; in the writing ican Pain Society. This is an open access article under the CC BY-NC-
of the report; or in the decision to submit the article for publication. ND license (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-nd/4.0/).
The authors have no conflicts of interest to declare. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jpain.2016.06.003
Supplementary data accompanying this article are available online at
www.jpain.org and www.sciencedirect.com.

1001
1002 The Journal of Pain Disrupted Self-Perception in CLBP

L
45
ow back pain (LBP) is currently the leading cause of the persistence of LBP and may be targets for treat-
disability worldwide41 and its management con- ment.49 There is considerable evidence available to clini-
sumes substantial health care resources.21 Clinical cians on ways to evaluate the beliefs of people with LBP,
trial data indicate that most current interventions for although few data on how to assess body perception in
LBP have limited efficacy20 and epidemiological evidence this population. We recently presented information on
suggests that outcomes are worsening despite increased the development of the Fremantle Back Awareness Ques-
health care expenditure.22,23 The failure of current tionnaire (FreBAQ), a self-report questionnaire designed
treatment approaches to significantly affect the to assess back-specific body perception.47 Data collected
problem has prompted numerous authors to suggest a from a small, homogeneous sample of people with CLBP
reappraisal of how the problem is considered and confirmed the feasibility of using the questionnaire in
managed.9,29,48 clinical practice and classical test theory approaches sup-
We have previously proposed a model for LBP persis- ported aspects of the reliability and validity of the Fre-
tence underpinned by data on the cognitive and BAQ, although with potential misfitting of 1 item.47
behavioral contributors to the LBP experience as well Some minor changes were also made to the wording of
as recent evidence of significant alteration in central ner- the questionnaire on the basis of feedback from partici-
vous system structure and function in people with pants in this preliminary study.47 The aim of this article is
chronic LBP (CLBP; Fig 1). The model suggests that mal- to report on the initial testing of the updated question-
adaptive beliefs about the nature of the back problem naire in a large heterogeneous sample of people with
and future consequences drive behaviors that might CLBP, particularly to present the outcomes of a compre-
bring about maladaptive neuroplastic changes.50 These hensive evaluation of the scale’s psychometric properties
central nervous system changes might contribute to using a Rasch analysis, and the modifications to the scale
ongoing LBP and disability by enhancing nociceptive ef- that these data might suggest. We also aimed to explore
ficiency, influencing normal attentional processing, and the potential relationships between body perception,
potentially creating a state of maladaptive perceptual nociceptive sensitivity, distress and beliefs about back
awareness of the back—that is a disruption of the pain, and the combined and unique contribution these
consciously felt body.17 This may be conceptualized in factors might play in explaining pain and disability in
terms of how the back feels to the individual, the sense this population.
of control and ownership they feel they have over their
back, and the meaning and precision of sensory informa-
tion from the back.45 Because pain is viewed as a the Methods
conscious correlate of the perception that the body is
in danger and in need of protection18,24 the integrity Design
of the consciously felt body should be seen as This cross-sectional cohort study was approved by the
fundamental to the emergence of pain. Human Research Ethics Committees of Curtin University,
In this model maladaptive beliefs and maladaptive body Royal Perth Hospital, and Sir Charles Gairdner Hospital in
image are seen as mutually reinforcing, contributing to Perth, Western Australia. The data presented were

Figure 1. The maladaptive perceptions model. Abbreviation: Dx, diagnosis.


Wand et al The Journal of Pain 1003
collected as part of a larger study undertaking extensive episode, pain distribution, current pain medications,
biopsychosocial profiling of people with persistent LBP, and the presence of any comorbidities. In addition, the
the results of which have been reported elsewhere.33 participants completed a set of standardized surveys
All participants provided informed consent and all pro- that assessed disability, pain, and psychological func-
cedures conformed to the Declaration of Helsinki. tioning. LBP-related disability was measured using the
RMDQ.35 Average back pain intensity over the past
Participants week was measured using the NRS described previously
and pain-related fear was estimated using the Fear
People with axial CLBP were recruited from 2 metro-
Avoidance Beliefs Questionnaire.43 Because only 76.2%
politan hospitals in Perth, Western Australia (1.4%), pri-
of the sample was currently working, only the physical
vate metropolitan physiotherapy clinics (20.1%), pain
activity subscale of the Fear Avoidance Beliefs Question-
management and general practice clinics (1.0%), and
naire was used. The level of pain-related catastrophiza-
via multimedia advertisements circulated throughout
tion was measured using the Pain Catastrophizing
the general community in metropolitan and regional
Scale.39 Symptoms of psychological distress (depression,
Western Australia (77.6%). Willing volunteers were
anxiety, and stress) were assessed with the Depression
asked to contact one of the researchers (M.I.R.) directly
Anxiety Stress Scales 21,19 with the average score for
by telephone or e-mail, and were then sent a screening
the 3 subscales used for analysis. Finally, participants
questionnaire. All questionnaire responses were
completed the FreBAQ (Supplemental Appendix 1).47
screened and ambiguous responses clarified in tele-
The original study involved an extensive sensory
phone communication.
profiling of the participants using a combination of
Volunteers were included if they were aged between
clinical bedside tests and laboratory tests.33 Only the
18 and 70 years, were fluent in written and spoken En-
assessment of lumbar spine nociceptive sensitivity is re-
glish, had experienced LBP for >3 months, scored $2
ported. Participants were positioned comfortably in a
on a numeric rating scale (NRS) for average pain inten-
prone position and testing was undertaken at the area
sity in the past week anchored with 0 = ‘‘no pain,’’ and
of maximal pain in the following order. Pressure pain
10 = ‘‘worst pain imaginable,’’ and $5 on the Roland
threshold was tested using an algometer with a probe
Morris Disability Questionnaire (RMDQ).35 In addition
size of 1 cm2 (Somedic AB, Ho € rby, Sweden) and was
participants needed a score of at least 60% LBP on
defined as the point at which the sensation of pressure
the following question44: ‘‘Which situation describes
changed to a sensation of pressure and pain.36 Pressure
your pain over the past 4 weeks the best? 100% of
was increased at a rate of 50 kPa/s until the participant
the pain in the low back; 80% of the pain in the low
indicated their pressure pain threshold by pressing a but-
back and 20% in the leg(s); 60% of the pain in the
ton. Thirty-second interstimulus intervals were adopted
low back and 40% in the leg(s); 50% of the pain in
to reduce the possibility of temporal summation. The
the low back and 50% in the leg(s); 40% of the pain
mean of 3 threshold recordings was used for analysis.
in the low back and 60% in the leg(s); or 20% of the
Heat pain threshold (HPT), the temperature at which a
pain in the low back and 80% in the leg(s).’’ The latter
sensation of warmth becomes the first sensation of heat
question reliably differentiates participants with domi-
and pain,36 was tested using the Thermotest (Somedic
nant leg pain or dominant LBP,44 minimizing the likeli-
AB). Testing began at 32 C and increased by 1 C/s until
hood of participants with primarily radicular pain
the participant indicated their HPT by pressing a button,
being entered into the study.
or the device’s upper temperature limit was reached
Volunteers were excluded if they reported any previ-
(50 C). Thirty-second interstimulus intervals were adop-
ous extensive spinal surgery (greater than single-level
ted and the mean of 3 threshold recordings was used
fusion or discectomy) or any type of spinal surgery within
for the analysis.
the past 6 months, were diagnosed with serious spinal
Cold pain threshold was recorded as the point at which
pathology (cancer, inflammatory arthropathy, or acute
the sensation of cold became the first sensation of cold
vertebral fracture), had been diagnosed with a neuro-
and pain.36 The same equipment for testing HPT was
logical disease, experienced bilateral pain at the dorsum
used for testing cold pain threshold. Testing began at
of the wrist/hand, or were currently pregnant.
32 C and the temperature of the thermode decreased
by 1 C/s until the participant detected their threshold
Procedure and pressed a button, or the device’s lower temperature
Only procedures relevant to this study are presented. limit was reached (4 C). Thirty-second interstimulus in-
For a fuller description of all testing undertaken see Ra- tervals were adopted and the mean of 3 threshold re-
bey et al.33 On initial presentation, all participants were cordings was used for analysis.
screened for eligibility—including the presence of red
flag conditions, given information about the project,
and invited to sign a consent form. Participants then pro- Sample Size
vided basic demographic information and had their The sample size requirement for this study was not
height and weight measured, from which their body determined a priori because the sample was recruited
mass index (BMI) was calculated. as part of an extensive study exploring multidimensional
All participants next completed a questionnaire that subgrouping in a CLBP population. The sample size of
solicited information about the length of the current 251 provided .8 power to detect potentially meaningful
1004 The Journal of Pain Disrupted Self-Perception in CLBP
independent associations of FreBAQ with pain and impairment,’ thus each item should assess a component
disability (ie, R2 of .03 or more in regression models after of this construct. That is, each item should share in com-
adjusting for covariates) at a < .05 (G*Power version mon an aspect of perceptual impairment yet be suffi-
3.1.9), was well over the minimum requirements for the ciently different so as not to be redundant. Assessment
number of subjects per variable for unbiased regression of unidimensionality seeks to identify clusters of items
coefficients and model R2 estimates in linear regression that together may be assessing a secondary dimension,
analyses,2 and was in excess of the 243 persons recom- thus threatening measurement of the primary dimen-
mended to ensure item calibration stability within 6 .5 sion. Unidimensionality was assessed through analysis
logits with 99% confidence.16 of item fit statistics and through principal components
analysis (PCA) of residuals.37 The c2 based fit statistics, re-
ported as mean-squares (in logits), have an expected
Data Analysis value of 1 logit. Fit was considered excessive if >1.4 or
<.6 logits53 and information-weighted fit statistic and
Sample Description outlier-sensitive fit statistic (outfit) were analyzed. The
Descriptive statistics were used to describe the demo- item characteristic curves of misfitting items were visu-
graphic and clinical characteristics of the sample. The Fre- ally inspected to assess item performance across the per-
BAQ was summarized with range, median, mean, and son agreeability range. The PCA residual correlation
standard deviation measures reported for the total score. matrix was inspected visually to identify the presence
The frequencies in each response category were also re- of secondary dimensions. Item clusters with substantial
ported. positive or negative loadings equivalent to an eigen-
value >2 were reviewed to ascertain whether a second
Psychometrics dimension was present.34
We used Rasch analysis (Winsteps v3.73.0 software; PCA also allows for a test of local independence of
Winsteps, Beaverton, Oregon) to assess the psychometric items40 and is used to identify redundant items. Large
properties of the FreBAQ (see Bond and Fox5 for a positive correlations >.5 were considered indicative of
comprehensive overview of Rasch analysis). The Andrich local dependence where the response to 1 item relies
Rating Scale model was chosen because the FreBAQ on the response to the other.
items all share the same rating scale.15 The following Assessment of person fit identifies people who re-
components were assessed: item hierarchy, category or- sponded in an unexpected manner. Person fit was consid-
der, targeting, unidimensionality, person fit, internal ered excessive if the outfit statistics were >2 logits.40
consistency, and differential item functioning.40 Misfitting persons were compared across variables with
Item hierarchy allows for the assessment of construct those who fit the model using a c2 test of significance
validity. The FreBAQ was developed to assess body (gender) or an independent samples t-test (FreBAQ total
perceptual impairments in people with back pain. We score, age, pain intensity [NRS], disability [RMDQ], and
compared the item hierarchy to ensure the items were BMI). Response strings of those misfitting persons were
ordered in a logical manner, from comparatively mild visually analyzed to identify patterns in their responses.
perceptual impairments to more severe impairments. The Winsteps software provides 2 measures of internal
Item reliability >.9 was considered sufficient to confirm consistency; the Rasch-specific ‘person reliability index’
the item hierarchy.14 and the more widely recognized Cronbach a.10 Accept-
Category ordering was assessed to determine how the able internal consistency is considered to be >.7 in both
sample used the rating scale. The FreBAQ has 5 response instances.32,40
categories (0–4) and thus 4 step-calibrations, thresholds The FreBAQ items should function similarly for all per-
at which the likelihood of endorsing 1 category is equal sons of the same level of agreeability. Differential item
to that of endorsing the next were assessed. Respondents functioning identifies whether characteristics other
with high overall scores are expected to endorse higher than the latent construct alters the functioning of the
categories on any given item. We assessed whether item (eg, male and female respondents with the same
each of the 5 categories were used and whether the re- level of perceptual impairment endorse an item differ-
spondents used each category in the expected manner. ently). We assessed whether age, gender, pain intensity,
Targeting refers to how well the FreBAQ items tar- or disability biased the functioning of the scale by split-
geted the sample. It was assessed by visual inspection ting the sample, according to median, and comparing
of the distribution of persons and item threshold aver- the 2 subgroups. BMI was split according to under-
ages and through comparison of the summary statistics. weight/healthy weight (<25) and overweight/obese
The average item endorsability was anchored at 0 logits; ($25). Items with statistically significant (P < .05) con-
therefore positive average person agreeability would trasts > .5 logits were further explored.1
suggest the sample experienced perceptual impairments
more frequently than the average of the scale. A nega- Relationship to Clinical Status
tive average person value would suggest the opposite.40 The association between FreBAQ scores and 1) demo-
For questionnaire items to be validly summated to pro- graphic characteristics (age, gender, BMI), 2) clinical sta-
vide an overall measure of a construct, the questionnaire tus (pain and disability), 3) cognitive/psychological
items must collectively assess 1 construct at a time. In this characteristics, and 4) nociceptive sensitivity measures,
case, the FreBAQ intends to measure ‘perceptual were assessed using correlation statistics (Pearson R,
Wand et al The Journal of Pain 1005
Spearman r, or point-biserial coefficient as appropriate). Participants Demographic and Clinical
Table 1.
Multivariable linear regression analysis was used to esti- Information (N = 251)
mate the unique association of FreBAQ scores with pain
and disability adjusted for demographic, cognitive/psy- MEAN (SD), MEDIAN
CHARACTERISTIC (IQR) OR N (%)
chological factors, and sensitivity measures. A 3-step pro-
cess was used, by first evaluating the univariate Demographic information
association of each independent variable with the Gender (female) 148 (59.0%)
dependent variable, then estimating a multivariable Age, y 48.8 (13.4)
Height, cm 170.9 (9.8)
model retaining those variables associated with the
Weight, kg 80.6 (16.7)
dependent variable at P < .1 (model 1), then estimating
Body mass index 27.6 (5.2)
a final model (model 2) retaining only those indepen- Work status
dent variables statistically significant at P < .1 from model At work (or studying) 188 (74.9)
1. Forward and backward stepwise variable selection was Off work 63 (25.1)
also performed and confirmed the stability of the final Clinical status
models (probability of entry/removal P = .05). For the Duration of LBP, mo* 120 (42–240)
disability model the log-transformed RMDQ was used Pain area
as the dependent variable because of the skewed distri- Back pain only 121 (48.2%)
bution of the measure. For the pain model the NRS for Back pain and leg pain 130 (51.8%)
Taking opioid medication 40 (15.9%)
average pain in the past week was used as the dependent
Average back pain intensity (scale of 0–10) 5.8 (1.9)
variable. Models were examined for absence of influen-
Disability (RMDQ; scale of 0–24) 9 (6–13)
tial observations and multicollinearity, linearity of associ- Pain catastrophization (PCS; scale of 0–52)y 18.8 (12.0)
ations, and normality and homoscedasticity of residuals. Fear avoidance (FABQ-PA; scale of 0–24) 14.1 (6.0)
Standardized b coefficients with 95% confidence inter- Psychological distress (DASS-21, scale of 0–42) 8.0 (4.0–12.7)
vals are reported to allow comparison of strength of as-
sociations. The total variance in disability and pain Abbreviations: IQR, interquartile range; RMDQ, Roland Morris Disability Ques-
tionnaire; PCS, Pain Catastrophizing Scale; FABQ-PA, Fear Avoidance Beliefs
explained by each final model (R2) was partitioned into
Questionnaire, physical activity subscale; DASS-21, Depression Anxiety Stress
unique variance attributable to FreBAQ and other vari- Scale 21.
ables in the model, and shared variance, by examination *Data missing for 4 cases.
of squared semipartial correlations between variables yData missing for 1 case.
and outcome.

suggesting ceiling and floor effects of the scale are


Results negligible.
Table 3 shows the average item endorsability thresh-
Sample Characteristics olds in hierarchal order, where higher thresholds indi-
Five hundred eighty-five volunteers were screened for cate items that are harder to endorse. Item 9 (My back
eligibility. Two hundred ninety-two were excluded for feels lopsided) was the easiest to endorse and item 8
the following reasons: low RMDQ score (n = 130), age (My back feels like it has shrunk) was the most difficult
>70 years (n = 42), dominant leg pain (n = 28), bilateral to endorse. The item order appeared to progress in a
wrist pain (n = 23), suspected serious spinal pathology largely coherent fashion, from the comparatively lesser
(n = 8), low pain intensity (n = 6), and failed to complete perceptual impairments (eg, item 9, My back feels
baseline assessment (n = 55). Of the 293 eligible partici- lopsided) to the more severe impairments (eg, item 1,
pants the first 42 completed a pilot version of the Fre- My back feels as though it is not part of the rest of my
BAQ47 and their data were not used in this analysis. body), suggesting the FreBAQ has construct validity. An
The remaining 251 participants completed the updated item reliability of .97 suggested the sample size was suf-
version of the questionnaire.47 There were no differ- ficient to confirm the item hierarchy is reproducible.
ences in gender (P = .127), age (P = .107), disability Interestingly, item 8 (My back feels like it has shrunk)
(RMDQ, P = .424), or pain (NRS, P = .608) between those was significantly more difficult to endorse than the other
completing the pilot versus updated version. Demo- items and did not fit the predicted hierarchal order.
graphic and clinical characteristics of the 251 included Visual inspection of the category structure suggested
participants are shown in Table 1. the respondents used the categories in the expected
The average total FreBAQ score was 9.8 (SD = 6.6) with manner although category 1 (rarely) was underused
a median score of 9.0 (interquartile range = 4.0–14.0). and did not have an interval on the latent variable (Fig 2).
Table 2 shows a full description of the frequency of The person-item distribution map shown in Fig 3 high-
response for each questionnaire item. lights the targeting of the FreBAQ to the sample. The
sample was loaded toward less frequent experiences of
perceptual impairment compared with the average
Psychometrics item endorsability. The average (SD) person agreeability
Rasch analysis was performed on the data from 251 was .96 (.84) logits (range = 2.92 to 1.85 logits),
participants. Fifteen (6%) persons registered a mini- compared with the default average (SD) item endorsabil-
mum score and no persons registered a maximum score, ity of 0 (.46) logits (range = .73 to .82 logits).
1006 The Journal of Pain Disrupted Self-Perception in CLBP
Table 2. Frequency of Responses to Each Item of the FreBAQ (N = 251)
NEVER, RARELY, OCCASIONALLY, OFTEN, ALWAYS,
ITEM N (%) N (%) N (%) N (%) N (%) MEDIAN MEAN
1. My back feels as though it is not part of the rest of my body 143 (57.0) 47 (18.7) 29 (11.5) 25 (10.0) 7 (2.8) 0 .8
2. I need to focus all my attention on my back to make it move the 58 (23.1) 46 (18.3) 81 (32.3) 51 (20.3) 15 (6.0) 2 1.7
way I want it to
3. I feel as if my back sometimes moves involuntarily, without my 144 (57.4) 52 (20.7) 33 (13.2) 19 (7.6) 3 (1.2) 0 .7
control
4. When performing everyday tasks, I don’t know how much my 104 (41.4) 75 (29.9) 39 (15.5) 29 (11.6) 4 (1.6) 1 1.0
back is moving
5. When performing everyday tasks, I am not sure exactly what 99 (39.4) 67 (26.7) 45 (17.9) 31 (12.4) 9 (3.6) 1 1.1
position my back is in
6. I can’t perceive the exact outline of my back 125 (49.8) 61 (24.3) 29 (11.6) 25 (10.0) 11 (4.4) 1 .9
7. My back feels like it is enlarged (swollen) 123 (49.0) 29 (11.6) 47 (18.7) 35 (13.9) 17 (6.8) 1 1.2
8. My back feels like it has shrunk 184 (73.3) 32 (12.8) 20 (8.0) 10 (4.0) 5 (2.0) 0 .5
9. My back feels lopsided (asymmetrical) 84 (33.5) 25 (10.0) 48 (19.1) 59 (23.5) 35 (13.9) 2 1.7

Abbreviation: FreBAQ, Fremantle Back Awareness Questionnaire.

The FreBAQ items constituted a unidimensional scale. (P = .02) and in more pain (P = .002). Visual analysis of the
Table 3 shows a summary of the fit statistics for the 9 items. response strings of the misfitting persons revealed no
Item 8 showed excessive positive outfit (1.7 logits) and meaningful patterns. Typically, persons with higher scores
analysis of the item characteristic curves suggested the unexpectedly ranked an item low or, less commonly, per-
misfit was due to respondents with higher scores overall sons with low scores overall scored an item high.
scoring this item low. Visual inspection of the PCA correla- A person reliability index of .74 and Cronbach a value
tion matrix suggested items 4 (When performing everyday of .80 indicated that the internal consistency of the Fre-
tasks, I don’t know how much my back is moving), 5 (When BAQ was adequate.40
performing everyday tasks, I am not sure exactly what po- Analysis of differential item functioning suggested
sition my back is in), and 6 (I can’t perceive the exact age may influence responses to item 8. Older persons
outline of my back) could plausibly constitute a second (n = 128) reported item 8 (.61 logits) easier to endorse
dimension. However, an eigenvalue of 2.0 suggested the than younger persons (n = 123), however, this difference
scale could be considered unidimensional.34 Assessment was not statistically significant (P = .054) and should be
of local dependence revealed no meaningful relationships viewed with caution because of the number of compari-
between the FreBAQ item residuals, suggesting none of sons. No other statistically significant contrasts >.5 logits
the questions are redundant. were observed, suggesting the items were not otherwise
Twenty-three persons (9%) showed excessive outfit. biased by the respondents’ age, gender, pain, disability,
Comparatively, misfitting persons were significantly older or BMI.

Relationship to Clinical Status


Table 3.Average Item Endorsability Thresholds, The FreBAQ showed significant initial bivariate associ-
Shown in Hierarchal Order, and Fit Statistics, ation with BMI, disability, pain intensity, pain catastroph-
for the FreBAQ Scores of Respondents With ization, fear avoidance, psychological distress, and
Back Pain (N = 251) lumbar pressure pain threshold (Table 4).
FREBAQ

ITEM MEASURE (LOGITS) SCORE* INFIT (MNSQ) OUTFIT (MNSQ)


8 .82 122 1.4 1.7
3 .39 187 1.2 1.2
1 .27 208 1.2 1.4
6 .13 235 .9 .8
4 .03 256 .7 .7
5 .1 286 .8 .8
7 .15 296 1.0 1.0
2 .66 421 .9 1.0
9 .73 438 1.2 1.2

Abbreviations: FreBAQ, Fremantle Back Awareness Questionnaire; Infit,


information-weighted fit statistic; Outfit, outlier-sensitive fit statistic; mnsq,
mean-squares. Figure 2. Probability curves for the 5-category Fremantle Back
NOTE. Higher measures indicate harder to endorse items and lower measures Awareness Questionnaire (FreBAQ; C0 = Never, C1 = Rarely, C2 =
indicate easier to endorse items. Occasionally, C3 = Sometimes, C4 = Always). Note the disordered
*Raw score of 1004 (possible score of 4  251 participants). threshold for C1.
Wand et al The Journal of Pain 1007

Figure 3. Item-person threshold map for the Fremantle Back Awareness Questionnaire (FreBAQ). Persons who rarely experience
perceptual impairments and items easier to endorse are located on the left side of the logit scale (ie, <0 logits); Persons who regularly
experience perceptual impairments and items harder to endorse are located to the right of the logit scale (ie, >0 logits). Average item
endorsability is set at 0 logits by default.

Table 5 shows the standardized b coefficients for linear BAQ explained 7.0% of the variance in pain intensity.
regression models for disability (logRMDQ). Alone, the The final model retaining FreBAQ and pain catastroph-
FreBAQ score explained 12.4% of the variance in ization explained 9.9% of the variance in pain intensity,
disability. The final model retaining FreBAQ score, psy- of which FreBAQ uniquely contributed 3.6%, pain cata-
chological distress, BMI, and pain intensity, explained strophization uniquely contributed 2.7%, and 3.6%
29.5% of the variance in disability. Of this, FreBAQ was shared between both variables. In the final model,
uniquely contributed 1.3%, whereas 13.4% was shared an increase of 1 SD in FreBAQ was estimated to be asso-
between all 4 variables. Psychological distress, BMI, and ciated with an increase in .20 SD of pain (NRS; 95% con-
pain intensity uniquely contributed 6.1%, 2.6%, and fidence interval, .07–.33; P = .007).
6.1%, respectively. In the final model, an increase of 1
SD in the FreBAQ score was estimated to be associated
with an increase in .13 SD of logRMDQ (95% confidence
Discussion
interval, .01–.25; P = .032). One key aim of this study was to report on the initial
Table 6 shows the standardized b coefficients for linear testing of the updated FreBAQ in a large heterogeneous
regression models for pain intensity (NRS). Alone, Fre- sample of people with CLBP and present the outcomes of
a comprehensive evaluation of the scales’ psychometric
properties using the Rasch model. The results of this
Correlations of Demographic
Table 4. analysis suggest that the scale functions well. The ques-
Characteristics, Clinical Status, Cognitive/ tionnaire appears unidimensional with no redundant
Psychological Characteristics and items, has minimal ceiling and floor effects, and accept-
able internal consistency, with a Cronbach a very close
Psychophysical Measures Using the FreBAQ
to that reported in the original development report.47
CHARACTERISTIC CORRELATION COEFFICIENT P The item hierarchy appeared to progress in a theoreti-
Gender (female) .023 .714 cally plausible fashion supporting the construct validity
Age, y .087 .166 of the questionnaire. Furthermore, the differential
BMI .161 .011 item functioning analysis showed that none of the items
Duration of LBP, mo .084 .188 were biased by demographic or clinical variables. Howev-
Disability (RMDQ) .319 <.001 er, the FreBAQ items were relatively difficult to endorse
Average back pain intensity .265 <.001 and are thus better suited to assessing those with
Pain catastrophization (PCS) .358 <.001
comparatively more frequent episodes of perceptual
Fear avoidance (PABQ-PA) .263 <.001
impairment.
Psychological distress (DASS-21) .376 <.001
Lumbar pressure pain threshold .139 .028
Item 8 (My back feels like it has shrunk) functioned
Lumbar cold pain threshold .112 .078 poorly in that it was significantly more difficult to
Lumbar heat pain threshold .077 .222 endorse than the other items, did not fit the predicted hi-
erarchal order, and showed misfit. This, however, was not
Abbreviations: FreBAQ, Fremantle Back Awareness Questionnaire; BMI, body unexpected because items 8 and 7 (My back feels like it
mass index; LBP, low back pain; RMDQ, Roland Morris Disability Questionnaire;
has shrunk, My back feels like it is enlarged) relate to
PCS, Pain Catastrophizing Scale; PABQ-PA, Fear Avoidance Beliefs Question-
naire, physical activity subscale; DASS-21, Depression Anxiety Stress Scale 21. the perceived size of the back. Although it is plausible
NOTE: Significant associations in bold. a respondent could experience either impairment at
1008 The Journal of Pain Disrupted Self-Perception in CLBP
Table 5. Linear Regression Models for Disability (logRMDQ)
UNIVARIABLE MULTIVARIABLE 1 MULTIVARIABLE 2

b (95% CI)* P b (95% CI)* P b (95% CI)* P


Back perception (FreBAQ) .35 (.23–.47) <.001 .10 (.02 to .23) .096 .13 (.01–.25) .032
Psychological distress (DASS-21) .41 (.29–.52) <.001 .22 (.09–.36) .001 .28 (.16–.40) <.001
Pain catastrophization (PCS) .33 (.21–.45) <.001 .06 (.07 to .20) .339
Fear avoidance (PABQ-PA) .22 (.10–.34) <.001 .09 (.02 to .21) .110
BMI .34 (.12–.36) <.001 .16 (.06 to .27) .003 .16 (.06–.27) .003
Lumbar pressure pain threshold .15 (.30 to .03) .021 .03 (.14 to .08) .594
Average back pain intensity (scale of 0–10) .37 (.25–.49) <.001 .26 (.15–.37) <.001 .26 (.15–.37) <.001

Abbreviations: RMDQ, Roland Morris Disability Questionnaire; CI, confidence interval; FreBAQ, Fremantle Back Awareness Questionnaire; DASS-21, Depression Anxiety
Stress Scale 21; PCS, Pain Catastrophizing Scale; PABQ-PA, Fear Avoidance Beliefs Questionnaire, physical activity subscale; BMI, body mass index.
*Standardized b coefficient represented expected change in SD units of logRMDQ for 1 SD change in independent variable.

differing times, it is more likely they will experience one are needed to explain these differences but it is plausible
and not the other. The data support this notion with that older people with relatively few perceptual impair-
most respondents reporting frequent feelings of ments experience occasional specific impairments that
enlargement but not shrinkage. The comparatively few are associated with age-related changes. Alternatively,
responses to item 8 accounted for the misfit and it being they may have not understood the question or answered
the most difficult item to endorse. That some respon- incorrectly. Nonetheless, that there were no overt pat-
dents do experience feelings of shrinkage, however, sug- terns in the response strings in general suggests the Fre-
gests the item might be important for some and the BAQ items are not problematic.
minor statistical anomalies it creates do not warrant its Another key aim was to explore the relationships be-
exclusion. tween body perception, nociceptive sensitivity, distress,
We noted that category 1 (Rarely) was underused by the and beliefs about back pain. As hypothesized, disturbed
sample, suggesting the respondents could not clearly perpetual awareness of the back correlated with distress,
discriminate between ‘rarely’ and ‘occasionally.’ Nonethe- fear avoidant beliefs, and catastrophizing cognitions
less, the scale behaved in the expected manner, with per- about pain. We also found that higher levels of disturbed
sons with more frequent perceptual impairments scoring self-perception were related to increased sensitivity to
higher on each item suggesting changes to the category pressure at the low back but not cold or heat. This may
structure of the scale are not necessary. Retaining the represent the different tissues that are involved in
original category structure also has the advantage of testing because thermal sensitivity likely assesses sensi-
enabling comparisons to be made with data already re- tivity to stimulus delivered to superficial tissues whereas
ported4,47 and ongoing studies which may use the scale. pressure sensitivity is thought to also assess sensitivity to
Overall, the sample used the FreBAQ as expected with stimuli delivered to deep tissue.11 This is consistent with
only 9% of respondents displaying misfit. That misfitting previous work, which has suggested that pressure pain
persons were significantly older can be explained, in thresholds are highly accurate in discriminating between
part, by their responses to item 8. Older persons found people with CLBP and healthy control participants,
item 8 somewhat easier to endorse compared with whereas the discriminative ability of heat and cold pain
younger persons suggesting older people experienced sensitivity is limited.26 The relationship found among
more frequent feeling of shrinkage, rather than expan- these variables offers some preliminary support for the
sion. Preferentially endorsing the rarely used item 8 model hypothesized in Fig 1, which suggest these factors
over item 7 would result in person misfit. Future studies are likely mutually reinforcing.

Table 6. Linear Regression Models for Pain Intensity (NRS, Scale of 0–10)
UNIVARIABLE MULTIVARIABLE 1 MULTIVARIABLE 2

b (95% CI)* P b (95% CI)* P b (95% CI)* P


Back perception (FreBAQ) .26 (.14–.38) <.001 .19 (.05–.32) .007 .20 (.07–.33) .002
Psychological distress (DASS-21) .22 (.09–.34) .001 .05 (.10 to .20) .486
Pain catastrophization (PCS) .25 (.13–.37) <.001 .15 (.01–.30) .034 .18 (.05–.30) .007
Fear avoidance (PABQ-PA) .04 (.07 to .16) .547
BMI .10 (.02 to .23) .111
Lumbar pressure pain threshold .10 (.22 to .03) .132

Abbreviations: NRS, numeric rating scale; CI, confidence interval; FreBAQ, Fremantle Back Awareness Questionnaire; DASS-21, Depression Anxiety Stress Scale 21; PCS,
Pain Catastrophizing Scale; PABQ-PA, Fear Avoidance Beliefs Questionnaire, physical activity subscale; BMI, body mass index.
*Standardized b coefficient represented expected change in SD units of NRS for 1 SD change in independent variable.
Wand et al The Journal of Pain 1009
We also provide some evidence that disrupted percep- belief that the back is fragile and not fit for function,
tual awareness of the back significantly and uniquely which may contribute to avoidant behavior. Actual pe-
contributes to pain intensity in this population. In our ripheral tissue health is also likely to contribute to the
sample, disturbed body perception appears to be more perception of fitness. Exploratory studies on healthy sub-
strongly associated with pain intensity than psychologi- jects have reported a body part-specific decrease in tem-
cal distress, fear avoidance beliefs, or an objective mea- perature25 and increased histamine reactivity,3 within
sure of lumbar spine sensitivity. It is plausible that minutes of experimental body awareness disruption,
changes in how the back feels to the individual can affect suggesting a link between self-perception and homeo-
the pain experience, as our data suggest. Planning and static control. It is not clear whether such changes do
coordination of movement requires an intact perception lead to meaningful changes in tissue health but the pos-
of the body and its position in space, and movement sibility that altered body perception could also nega-
quality may be compromised if body perception is dis- tively influence actual peripheral tissue health is
rupted. Suboptimal movement patterns might abnor- worthy of consideration.
mally load the back and contribute to nociceptive input The findings presented should be considered in light
and movement-related pain in those with CLBP.13,28 It of the limitations of the study. The sample is quite het-
has also been hypothesized that danger signals may erogeneous, being drawn from clinical and nonclinical
arise centrally as a result of incongruence between settings so likely represents participants with very
predicted and actual sensory feedback associated with different treatment histories and may partly explain
movement by virtue of disrupted body maps.12 This why the associations with clinical severity found in this
mechanism might also contribute to the pain experience study are weaker than we have previously noted with a
in people with CLBP whose perception of the back is sample drawn only from a clinical setting.47 Also,
degraded, although experimental support for this hy- although we attempted to only recruit participants
pothesis is inconsistent.27,51 It is also plausible that with nonspecific LBP, the tool used to exclude individuals
sensitivity might be enhanced by changes in body with radicular pain may not have successfully screened
perception.17 Pain emerges when we conclude our out all these individuals. Although altered
body to be under threat and in need of protection24 so self-perception appears to be a feature of CLBP its impor-
how the body is perceived should be seen as funda- tance in the development and persistence of CLBP re-
mental to the emergence of pain. In support of this mains uncertain. It should be considered that
idea are data that show that sensitivity to experimental self-perception changes may simply be epiphenomena.
pain is increased when perception of the body part is dis- We have taken a robust multivariate approach to assess-
torted by visual manipulation30,31 and is partly endorsed ing the unique relationships between self-perception
by the correlations noted between lumbar pressure pain and clinical features of CLBP. However, such approaches
threshold and FreBAQ scores. Finally loss of sensory can only control for known and measured variables
precision and decreased ability to accurately localize and it remains possible that observed relationships might
sensory input could enhance sensitivity by increasing be confounded by unknown variables. The cross-
the salience and threat value of any sensory sectional nature of the study also precludes us from
information, noxious or otherwise, received from the drawing any inferences of cause and effect. Finally, the
affected area. Importantly, preliminary data suggest contribution of self-perception to the variance seen in
that strategies that likely improve self-perception such pain and disability is relatively small. Small effect sizes in-
as mirror visual feedback52 and sensory discrimination crease the chance that relationships observed may not be
training46 may decrease activity-related pain in people causal in nature. Further longitudinal and experimental
with CLBP. studies are required to explore these issues.
We also found that FreBAQ scores were uniquely asso-
ciated with disability whereas measures of pain cata-
strophization, fear avoidance beliefs, and lumbar spine
sensitivity were not. It is plausible that how the back is Conclusions
perceived may uniquely influence disability. Although The findings presented provide further evidence that
numerous factors interact to determine the level of body perception is disturbed in people with CLBP. The
engagement in functional activities42 the perception of level of perceptual disturbance is positively correlated
the fitness, health, and robustness of the back might with pain intensity and disability. In this sample,
be factors that drive avoidance. Previous research has disturbed body perception seems to make a more impor-
shown that people with high levels of LBP-related tant contribution to severity of the clinical condition
disability have a more pathoanatomical perspective on than commonly considered factors such as pain cata-
the cause of their back pain than those with low levels strophization, psychological distress, fear avoidance be-
of disability,6 and qualitative research supports the liefs, and local tissue sensitivity. These findings suggest
notion that people with LBP perceive the back as fragile that assessment of body perception might be useful in
and easy to injure,8,38 particularly in those with high helping clinicians understand the complexity of the LBP
levels of pain-related fear.7 Features captured in the Fre- experience and could serve to guide management. The
BAQ such as feelings of disconnection from the back, data presented show that the FreBAQ is a simple,
finding the back difficult to control, and altered percep- feasible, and psychometrically sound method of assess-
tion in the size and shape of the back might add to the ing disruption of body image in people with CLBP.
1010 The Journal of Pain Disrupted Self-Perception in CLBP
Acknowledgments Supplementary Data
The authors thank Associate Professor Helen Slater and Supplementary data related to this article can be
Dr Darren Beales for their assistance with planning of the found online at http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jpain.2016.
study. 06.003.

References 17. Lotze M, Moseley GL: Role of distorted body image in


pain. Curr Rheumatol Rep 9:488-496, 2007

1. Alagumalai S, Curtis DD, Hungi N: Applied Rasch Mea- 18. Lotze M, Moseley GL: Theoretical considerations for
surement: A Book of Exemplars. Dordecht, Springer, 2005 chronic pain rehabilitation. Phys Ther 95:1316-1320, 2015

2. Austin PC, Steyerberg EW: The number of subjects per 19. Lovibond PF, Lovibond SH: The structure of negative
variable required in linear regression analyses. J Clin Epide- emotional states: comparison of the Depression Anxiety
miol 68:627-636, 2015 Stress Scales (DASS) with the Beck Depression and Anxiety
Inventories. Behav Res Ther 33:335-343, 1995
3. Barnsley N, McAuley JH, Mohan R, Dey A, Thomas P,
Moseley GL: The rubber hand illusion increases histamine 20. Machado LA, Kamper SJ, Herbert RD, Maher CG,
reactivity in the real arm. Curr Biol 21:R945-R946, 2011 McAuley JH: Analgesic effects of treatments for non-
specific low back pain: A meta-analysis of placebo-
4. Beales D, Lutz A, Thompson J, Wand BM, O’Sullivan P: controlled randomized trials. Rheumatology 48:520-527,
Disturbed body perception, reduced sleep, and kinesiopho- 2009
bia in subjects with pregnancy-related persistent lumbopel-
vic pain and moderate levels of disability: An exploratory 21. Manchikanti L, Singh V, Falco FJ, Benyamin RM,
study. Man Ther 21:69-75, 2015 Hirsch JA: Epidemiology of low back pain in adults. Neuro-
modulation 17:3-10, 2014
5. Bond TG, Fox CM: Applying the Rasch Model: Funda-
mental Measurement in the Human Sciences, 2nd ed. Mah- 22. Martin BI, Deyo RA, Mirza SK, Turner JA, Comstock BA,
wah, Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, 2007 Hollingworth W, Sullivan SD: Expenditures and health status
among adults with back and neck problems. JAMA 299:
6. Briggs AM, Jordan JE, Buchbinder R, Burnett AF, 656-664, 2008
O’Sullivan PB, Chua JY, Osborne RH, Straker LM: Health liter-
acy and beliefs among a community cohort with and 23. Martin BI, Turner JA, Mirza SK, Lee MJ, Comstock BA,
without chronic low back pain. Pain 150:275-283, 2010 Deyo RA: Trends in health care expenditures, utilization,
€ tze R, O’Sullivan P: and health status among US adults with spine problems,
7. Bunzli S, Smith A, Watkins R, Schu
1997–2006. Spine 34:2077-2084, 2009
What do people who score highly on the Tampa Scale of Ki-
nesiophobia really believe?: A mixed methods investigation 24. Moseley GL: Reconceptualising pain according to mod-
in people with chronic nonspecific low back pain. Clin J Pain ern pain science. Phys Ther Rev 12:169-178, 2007
31:621-632, 2015
25. Moseley GL, Olthof N, Venema A, Don S, Wijers M,
8. Darlow B, Dean S, Perry M, Mathieson F, Baxter GD, Gallace A, Spence C: Psychologically induced cooling of a
Dowell A: Easy to harm, hard to heal: Patient views about specific body part caused by the illusory ownership of an
the back. Spine 40:842-850, 2015 artificial counterpart. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 105:
9. Farmer MA, Baliki MN, Apkarian AV: A dynamic network 13169-13173, 2008
perspective of chronic pain. Neurosci Lett 520:197-203, 2012 26. Neziri AY, Curatolo M, Limacher A, Nu € esch E,
Radanov B, Andersen OK, Arendt-Nielsen L, Ju € ni P:
10. Fox CM, Jones JA: Uses of Rasch modeling in counseling
psychology research. J Counsel Psychol 45:30-45, 1998 Ranking of parameters of pain hypersensitivity according
to their discriminative ability in chronic low back pain.
11. Graven-Nielsen T, Mense S, Arendt-Nielsen L: Painful Pain 153:2083-2091, 2012
and non-painful pressure sensations from human skeletal
muscle. Exp Brain Res 159:273-283, 2004 27. Nishigami T, Nakano H, Osumi M, Tsujishita M, Mibu A,
Ushida T: Central neural mechanisms of interindividual dif-
12. Harris AJ: Cortical origin of pathological pain. Lancet ference in discomfort during sensorimotor incongruence
354:1464-1466, 1999 in healthy volunteers: An experimental study. Rheuma-
tology 53:1194-1199, 2014
13. Hodges PW, Smeets RJ: Interaction between pain, move-
ment, and physical activity: short-term benefits, long-term 28. O’Sullivan P: Diagnosis and classification of chronic low
consequences, and targets for treatment. Clin J Pain 31: back pain disorders: Maladaptive movement and motor con-
97-107, 2015 trol impairments as underlying mechanism. Man Ther 10:
242-255, 2005
14. Linacre J: A User’s Guide To Winsteps, Ministep Rasch-
Model Computer Programs, Program Manual 3.74.0. Beaver- 29. O’Sullivan P: It’s time for change with the management
ton, Winsteps.com, 2012 of non-specific chronic low back pain. Br J Sports Med 46:
224-227, 2012
15. Linacre J: Comparing ‘‘partial credit’’ and ‘‘rating scale’’
models. Rasch Measurement Transactions 14:768, 2000 30. Osumi M, Imai R, Ueta K, Nakano H, Nobusako S,
Morioka S: Factors associated with the modulation of pain
16. Linacre JM: Sample size and item calibration stability. by visual distortion of body size. Front Hum Neurosci 8:
Rasch Measurement Transactions 7:328, 1994 137, 2014
Wand et al The Journal of Pain 1011
31. Osumi M, Imai R, Ueta K, Nobusako S, Morioka S: Nega- Freeman MK, Gabbe BJ, Gabriel SE, Gakidou E,
tive body image associated with changes in the visual body Ganatra HA, Garcia B, Gaspari F, Gillum RF, Gmel G,
appearance increases pain perception. PLoS One 9:1-8, 2014 Gosselin R, Grainger R, Groeger J, Guillemin F, Gunnell D,
Gupta R, Haagsma J, Hagan H, Halasa YA, Hall W,
32. Portney LG, Watkins MP: Foundations of Clinical Haring D, Haro JM, Harrison JE, Havmoeller R, Hay RJ,
Research: Applications to Practice, 2nd ed. Upper Saddle Higashi H, Hill C, Hoen B, Hoffman H, Hotez PJ, Hoy D,
River, Prentice Hall Health, 2000 Huang JJ, Ibeanusi SE, Jacobsen KH, James SL, Jarvis D,
Jasrasaria R, Jayaraman S, Johns N, Jonas JB,
33. Rabey M, Slater H, O’Sullivan P, Beales D, Smith A: So- Karthikeyan G, Kassebaum N, Kawakami N, Keren A,
matosensory nociceptive characteristics differentiate sub- Khoo JP, King CH, Knowlton LM, Kobusingye O,
groups in people with chronic low back pain: A cluster Koranteng A, Krishnamurthi R, Lalloo R, Laslett LL,
analysis. Pain 156:1874-1884, 2015 Lathlean T, Leasher JL, Lee YY, Leigh J, Lim SS, Limb E,
34. Ra^ıche G: Critical eigenvalue sizes in standardized resid- Lin JK, Lipnick M, Lipshultz SE, Liu W, Loane M, Ohno SL,
ual principal components analysis. Rasch Measurement Lyons R, Ma J, Mabweijano J, MacIntyre MF,
Transactions 19:1012, 2005 Malekzadeh R, Mallinger L, Manivannan S, Marcenes W,
March L, Margolis DJ, Marks GB, Marks R, Matsumori A,
35. Roland M, Morris R: A study of the natural history of Matzopoulos R, Mayosi BM, McAnulty JH, McDermott MM,
back pain. Part I: Development of a reliable and sensitive McGill N, McGrath J, Medina-Mora ME, Meltzer M,
measure of disability in low-back pain. Spine 8:141-144, Mensah GA, Merriman TR, Meyer AC, Miglioli V, Miller M,
1983 Miller TR, Mitchell PB, Mocumbi AO, Moffitt TE,
Mokdad AA, Monasta L, Montico M, Moradi-Lakeh M,
€ lle TR, Treede RD, Beyer A,
36. Rolke R, Baron R, Maier C, To Moran A, Morawska L, Mori R, Murdoch ME, Mwaniki MK,
Binder A, Birbaumer N, Birklein F, Bo € tefu
€ r IC, Braune S, Naidoo K, Nair MN, Naldi L, Narayan KM, Nelson PK,
Flor H, Huge V, Klug R, Landwehrmeyer GB, Magerl W, Nelson RG, Nevitt MC, Newton CR, Nolte S, Norman P,
Maiho€ fner C, Rolko C, Schaub C, Scherens A: Quantitative Norman R, O’Donnell M, O’Hanlon S, Olives C, Omer SB,
sensory testing in the German Research Network on Neuro- Ortblad K, Osborne R, Ozgediz D, Page A, Pahari B,
pathic Pain (DFNS): Standardized protocol and reference Pandian JD, Rivero AP, Patten SB, Pearce N, Padilla RP,
values. Pain 123:231-243, 2006 Perez-Ruiz F, Perico N, Pesudovs K, Phillips D, Phillips MR,
Pierce K, Pion S, Polanczyk GV, Polinder S, Pope CA 3rd,
37. Smith EV: Detecting and evaluating the impact of multi- Popova S, Porrini E, Pourmalek F, Prince M, Pullan RL,
dimensionality using item fit statistics and principal compo- Ramaiah KD, Ranganathan D, Razavi H, Regan M, Rehm JT,
nent analysis of residuals. J Appl Meas 3:205-231, 2002 Rein DB, Remuzzi G, Richardson K, Rivara FP, Roberts T,
Robinson C, De Leo  n FR, Ronfani L, Room R, Rosenfeld LC,
38. Stenberg G, Fjellman-Wiklund A, Ahlgren C: ‘I am afraid
Rushton L, Sacco RL, Saha S, Sampson U, Sanchez-Riera L,
to make the damage worse’ - Fear of engaging in physical
Sanman E, Schwebel DC, Scott JG, Segui-Gomez M,
activity among patients with neck or back pain - A gender
Shahraz S, Shepard DS, Shin H, Shivakoti R, Singh D,
perspective. Scand J Caring Sci 28:146-154, 2014
Singh GM, Singh JA, Singleton J, Sleet DA, Sliwa K,
39. Sullivan MJ, Bishop SR, Pivik J: The Pain Catastrophizing Smith E, Smith JL, Stapelberg NJ, Steer A, Steiner T,
Scale: Development and validation. Psychol Assess 7: Stolk WA, Stovner LJ, Sudfeld C, Syed S, Tamburlini G,
524-532, 1995 Tavakkoli M, Taylor HR, Taylor JA, Taylor WJ, Thomas B,
Thomson WM, Thurston GD, Tleyjeh IM, Tonelli M,
40. Tennant A, Conaghan PG: The Rasch measurement Towbin JA, Truelsen T, Tsilimbaris MK, Ubeda C,
model in rheumatology: What is it and why use it? When Undurraga EA, van der Werf MJ, van Os J, Vavilala MS,
should it be applied, and what should one look for in a Rasch Venketasubramanian N, Wang M, Wang W, Watt K,
paper? Arthritis Rheum 57:1358-1362, 2007 Weatherall DJ, Weinstock MA, Weintraub R,
Weisskopf MG, Weissman MM, White RA, Whiteford H,
41. Vos T, Flaxman AD, Naghavi M, Lozano R, Michaud C, Wiersma ST, Wilkinson JD, Williams HC, Williams SR,
Ezzati M, Shibuya K, Salomon JA, Abdalla S, Aboyans V, Witt E, Wolfe F, Woolf AD, Wulf S, Yeh PH, Zaidi AK,
Abraham J, Ackerman I, Aggarwal R, Ahn SY, Ali MK, Zheng ZJ, Zonies D, Lopez AD, Murray CJ, AlMazroa MA,
Alvarado M, Anderson HR, Anderson LM, Andrews KG, Memish ZA: Years lived with disability (YLDs) for 1160
Atkinson C, Baddour LM, Bahalim AN, Barker-Collo S, sequelae of 289 diseases and injuries 1990-2010: A system-
Barrero LH, Bartels DH, Basa n
~ ez MG, Baxter A, Bell ML, atic analysis for the Global Burden of Disease Study 2010
Benjamin EJ, Bennett D, Bernabe  E, Bhalla K, Bhandari B, [erratum in: 381:628, 2013]. Lancet 380:2163-2196, 2013
Bikbov B, Bin Abdulhak A, Birbeck G, Black JA,
Blencowe H, Blore JD, Blyth F, Bolliger I, Bonaventure A, 42. Waddell G: The Back Pain Revolution. Edinburgh, Elsev-
Boufous S, Bourne R, Boussinesq M, Braithwaite T, ier Health Sciences, 2004
Brayne C, Bridgett L, Brooker S, Brooks P, Brugha TS,
Bryan-Hancock C, Bucello C, Buchbinder R, Buckle G, 43. Waddell G, Newton M, Henderson I, Somerville D,
Budke CM, Burch M, Burney P, Burstein R, Calabria B, Main CJ: A Fear-Avoidance Beliefs Questionnaire (FABQ)
Campbell B, Canter CE, Carabin H, Carapetis J, Carmona L, and the role of fear-avoidance beliefs in chronic low back
Cella C, Charlson F, Chen H, Cheng AT, Chou D, Chugh SS, pain and disability. Pain 52:157-168, 1993
Coffeng LE, Colan SD, Colquhoun S, Colson KE, Condon J,
44. Wai EK, Howse K, Pollock JW, Dornan H, Vexler L,
Connor MD, Cooper LT, Corriere M, Cortinovis M, de
Dagenais S: The reliability of determining ‘‘leg dominant
Vaccaro KC, Couser W, Cowie BC, Criqui MH, Cross M,
pain’’. Spine J 9:447-453, 2009
Dabhadkar KC, Dahiya M, Dahodwala N, Damsere-Derry J,
Danaei G, Davis A, De Leo D, Degenhardt L, Dellavalle R, 45. Wand BM: Chronic lower back pain: A maladaptive
Delossantos A, Denenberg J, Derrett S, Des Jarlais DC, perceptions model. Presented at the NOI 2012 Neurody-
Dharmaratne SD, Dherani M, Diaz-Torne C, Dolk H, namics and the Neuromatrix Conference, Adelaide,
Dorsey ER, Driscoll T, Duber H, Ebel B, Edmond K, Elbaz A, Australia, 2012
Ali SE, Erskine H, Erwin PJ, Espindola P, Ewoigbokhan SE,
Farzadfar F, Feigin V, Felson DT, Ferrari A, Ferri CP, 46. Wand BM, Abbaszadeh S, Smith AJ, Catley MJ,
vre EM, Finucane MM, Flaxman S, Flood L, Foreman K,
Fe Moseley GL: Acupuncture applied as a sensory discrimina-
Forouzanfar MH, Fowkes FG, Franklin R, Fransen M, tion training tool decreases movement-related pain in
1012 The Journal of Pain Disrupted Self-Perception in CLBP
patients with chronic low back pain more than acupuncture 50. Wand BM, Parkitny L, O’Connell NE, Luomajoki H,
alone: A randomised cross-over experiment. Br J Sports Med McAuley JH, Thacker M, Moseley GL: Cortical changes in
47:1085-1089, 2013 chronic low back pain: Current state of the art and implica-
tions for clinical practice. Man Ther 16:15-20, 2011
47. Wand BM, James M, Abbaszadeh S, George PJ,
Formby PM, Smith AJ, O’Connell NE: Assessing self- 51. Wand BM, Szpak L, George PJ, Bulsara MK,
perception in patients with chronic low back pain: Develop- O’Connell NE, Moseley GL: Moving in an environment of
ment of a back-specific body-perception questionnaire. J induced sensorimotor incongruence does not influence
Back Musculoskelet Rehabil 27:463-473, 2014 pain sensitivity in healthy volunteers: A randomised
within-subject experiment. PLoS One 9:e93701, 2014
48. Wand BM, O’Connell NE: Chronic non-specific low back
pain - Sub-groups or a single mechanism? BMC Musculoske- 52. Wand BM, Tulloch VM, George PJ, Smith AJ, Goucke R,
let Disord 9:11, 2008 O’Connell NE, Moseley GL: Seeing it helps: Movement-
related back pain is reduced by visualization of the back dur-
49. Wand BM, O’Connell NE, Di Pietro F, Bulsara M: ing movement. Clin J Pain 28:602-608, 2012
Managing chronic nonspecific low back pain with a
sensorimotor retraining approach: Exploratory multiple- 53. Wright BD, Linacre JM, Gustafson J, Martin-Lof P:
baseline study of 3 participants. Phys Ther 91: Reasonable mean-square fit values. Rasch Measurement
535-546, 2011 Transactions 8:370, 1994

You might also like