You are on page 1of 15

Emotion Regulation in Two-Year-Olds: Strategies and Emotional Expression in Four

Contexts
Author(s): Wendy S. Grolnick, Lisa J. Bridges and James P. Connell
Source: Child Development, Vol. 67, No. 3 (Jun., 1996), pp. 928-941
Published by: Wiley on behalf of the Society for Research in Child Development
Stable URL: http://www.jstor.org/stable/1131871
Accessed: 09-05-2016 06:03 UTC

REFERENCES
Linked references are available on JSTOR for this article:
http://www.jstor.org/stable/1131871?seq=1&cid=pdf-reference#references_tab_contents
You may need to log in to JSTOR to access the linked references.

Your use of the JSTOR archive indicates your acceptance of the Terms & Conditions of Use, available at
http://about.jstor.org/terms

JSTOR is a not-for-profit service that helps scholars, researchers, and students discover, use, and build upon a wide range of content in a trusted
digital archive. We use information technology and tools to increase productivity and facilitate new forms of scholarship. For more information about
JSTOR, please contact support@jstor.org.

Society for Research in Child Development, Wiley are collaborating with JSTOR to digitize,
preserve and extend access to Child Development

This content downloaded from 155.69.24.171 on Mon, 09 May 2016 06:03:01 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
Emotion Regulation in Two-Year-Olds:
Strategies and Emotional Expression
in Four Contexts

Wendy S. Grolnick
Clark University

Lisa J. Bridges
University of California, Riverside

James P. Connell
Institute for Research and Reform in Education

GROLNICK, WENDY S.; BRIDGES, LISA J.; and CONNELL, JAMES P. Emotion Regulation in Two-Year-
Olds: Strategies and Emotional Expression in Four Contexts. CHILD DEVELOPMENT, 1996, 67,
928-941. A descriptive study of the modulation of negative emotion in the toddler period was
conducted by examining expressions of negative emotion and the strategies used to reduce or
change these expressions. 6 strategies were identified and evaluated in terms of their frequency
of use in different situations, relations with emotional expressiveness, and cross-situational con-
sistency. 37 2-year-olds were seen in 2 laboratory contexts (delay and separation) each with 2
variants. Emotion regulation strategies and emotional expressiveness were coded from video-
tapes of children's behavior in these 4 situations. Findings suggest that active engagement was
most commonly used and most negatively associated with child distress. Use of strategies varied
by context, and there was more cross-situational consistency in use of strategies that were more
negatively or positively associated with distress within a given context than in use of particular
strategies without consideration of their within-context significance.

The development of emotion regulation scribe behaviors and thoughts that help
has been recognized as a central component individuals deal with stressful or distressing
of socioemotional development during the situations (Bridges & Grolnick, 1995). Such
infant-toddler period (Kopp, 1989; Tronick, capacities have been indexed in various
1989). Tronick (1989) noted that among the ways such as time to delay behavior during
goals that direct infant behaviors are those waiting (Mischel, 1974) and responses to
for regulating emotions-experiencing posi- fear-inducing stimuli (Rothbart, 1981) and
tive ones and controlling negative ones. Fur- have been conceptualized as an aspect of the
ther, early experiences of emotion regula- situation (e.g., Mischel, 1973) as well as a
tion are important precursors of later dimension of temperament (Rothbart &
affective (Field, 1987; Kopp, 1989; Tronick, Derryberry, 1981). We define emotion reg-
1989) and personality development (Cole ulation as the set of processes involved
& Kaslow, 1989; Thompson, Connell, & in initiating, maintaining, and modulating
Bridges, 1988). Despite current interest in emotional responsiveness, both negative
early emotion regulation, empirical work and positive. Kopp (1989), Rothbart (e.g.,
with infants and toddlers remains scant. The Rothbart & Posner, 1985), and Thompson
major goal of this study was to present de- (1994) have offered similar perspectives. In
scriptive data on emotion regulation in 2- this study, we focus on the modulation of
year-olds. negative emotion by examining expressions
of negative emotion and the strategies used
Constructs relevant to emotion regula-
to reduce or change these expressions.
tion such as self-regulation, behavior regula-
tion, and self-control have been used to de- As indicated earlier, our examination fo-

This research was supported by a grant from the National Institute of Mental Health (ROI
MH 44449-03) to the three authors. Reprint requests should be sent to Wendy S. Grolnick,
Frances L. Hiatt School of Psychology, Clark University, 950 Main Street, Worcester, MA 01610.
[Child Development, 1996, 67, 928-941. @ 1996 by the Society for Research in Child Development, Inc.
All rights reserved. 0009-3920/96/6703-0029$01.00]

This content downloaded from 155.69.24.171 on Mon, 09 May 2016 06:03:01 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
Grolnick, Bridges, and Connell 929

cuses on expressions of negative emotion, less active and more or less sustained, rang-
including both facial and vocal expressions. ing from looking away (Fox, 1989) to sus-
Expressions of and experiences of emotion tained toy play (Braungart & Stifter, 1991).
are not synonymous, and experienced emo- Mischel (1974) demonstrated that children
tion that is not expressed might also require who are most able to delay are those who
regulation and be relevant to a study of emo- use a variety of self-distraction techniques
tion regulation. We expect, however, consis- (Mischel & Ebbeson, 1970; Mischel, Ebbe-
tent with differential emotions theory (Izard son, & Zeiss, 1972). In the one study that
& Malatesta, 1987) that, at age 2, agreement looked directly at emotion-regulating behav-
between expressed and experienced emo- iors within the infant-toddler period,
tion would be quite high and focus only on Braungart and Stifter (1991) differentiated
expressed emotion in this study. between toy play (engaged and sustained
behavior) and looking at objects and people
Emotion regulation strategies are be-
(less engaged behavior) and found that
haviors used to regulate emotional experi-
greater distress was associated with less toy
ence. Rudimentary strategies to modulate
play. In our study, we examine use of several
arousal, such as looking away, are available
attention-reorienting strategies, including
early in infancy (Fox, 1989; Gianino & Tron-
visual and motor exploration, passive use of
ick, 1988). Strategies presumably become
objects, and active engagement with substi-
more complex with age as attentional, mem-
tute objects.
ory, and other cognitive processes become
more sophisticated (Kopp, 1982). A second set of behaviors focused on
comfort or reassurance. Comfort behaviors
Two-year-olds were chosen for this
study because they were likely to exhibit can be self-directed, including resting one's
multiple emotion regulation strategies. By head on the floor, stroking one's hair, thumb
the end of the second year, the beginnings of sucking and using familiar or special objects
effortful control emerge (Posner & Rothbart, to obtain comfort (Klackenberg, 1949; Pass-
1980) with the onset of representational and man & Weisberg, 1975). Comfort behavior
symbolic capacity (Piaget, 1952, 1954) and can also be other directed, such as seeking
with the new awareness of social expecta- proximity to and contact with caretakers
tions to keep control (Kopp, 1989). These (e.g., Ainsworth & Wittig, 1969). These
other-directed behaviors have been linked
emerging capacities make it possible for the
child to use several different kinds of strate- to distress by Connell and Thompson (1986),
gies for regulating emotion (e.g., attentional, Gaensbauer et al. (1983), and Thompson et
symbolic), though the child may be limited al. (1988). Their findings support the catego-
by the concreteness of his or her representa- rization of these behaviors as potential emo-
tional capacity (Kopp, 1982) and the limits tion regulation strategies.
of sustained attention (Posner & Rothbart, Comfort behaviors can also be symbolic
1980).
or representational. In a delay situation,
To date, few researchers have attempted such behavior might involve gamelike activ-
to examine specific behaviors hypothesized ity such as pretending to retrieve the desired
to regulate emotion, though several studies object (Piaget, 1962) or the use of language
have examined relations between emotion to bring action under control and to inhibit
and attachment behaviors (e.g., Connell & impulsive behavior (Vygotsky, 1986). To
Thompson, 1986; Gaensbauer, Connell, & date, use of verbalization to facilitate behav-
Schultz, 1983,) and Mischel and his col- ior has mainly been examined in task situa-
leagues (e.g., Mischel, 1974) have examined tions and with older children (Berk, in press;
several behaviors that may be relevant to Flavell, 1966). In this study, we examine use
emotion regulation, specifically those that of verbal and representational strategies, for
influence preschoolers' abilities to delay example, within our separation situation,
gratification. In delineating emotion regula- verbal strategies might include repeating
tion strategies in this study, we draw on "She'll be right back" or "I'm a big girl."
these bodies of work as well as that of re-
searchers specifically examining strategies Finally, the distressed child can main-
to modulate distress. tain or increase focus on the distressing stim-
ulus and attempt to alter a temporarily unre-
The first set of strategies are behaviors sponsive environment by, for example,
that shift attention from arousing stimuli searching for the mother during separation.
(Derryberry & Rothbart, 1988; Fox, 1989). Search for mother was correlated positively
Such attentional processes can be more or with distress (Gaensbauer, Connell, &

This content downloaded from 155.69.24.171 on Mon, 09 May 2016 06:03:01 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
930 Child Development

Schultz, 1983), and Mischel (1974) demon- A second set of research questions con-
strated that, when attention was directed to- cerned the influence of context on emotional
ward the goal, voluntary delay was dimin- regulatory processes. A first question was,
ished. How do strategies children tend to use vary
by context? For example, in this sample of
Based on this literature, we hypothe- 2-year olds, the presence of a participatory
sized different relations between these strat-
adult may make more active, reorienting
egies and emotional distress. We thus con- strategies more likely. Second, how do asso-
structed a continuum from the most
ciations between strategy use and emotional
stimulus-bound strategies to the more ac- expression differ in different contexts? For
tively reorienting strategies. The most stim- example, attempting to gain physical com-
ulus-bound strategies include maintaining fort from the mother in a stressful situation
or increasing focus on a source of frustration in which she is available may be negatively
or distress. Given a temporarily unrespon- associated with distress, but this same strat-
sive environment, these strategies should be egy might be positively associated with dis-
positively associated with distress. On this tress when she is not participatory. Finally,
same continuum, comforting strategies, be- we asked whether there may also be
cause they do not involve focus on the dis- cross-contextual consistency of individuals
tressing stimulus, but also do not involve in their use of regulatory strategies. Such
strong, active reengagement with another as-
consistency was addressed by assessing
pect of the environment, should show inter- whether children used the same strategies
mediate relations with distress. Finally, across different situations and whether cer-
strategies involving active reorientation of tain children tended to consistently use
attention and behavior are hypothesized to strategies with similar associations with dis-
be more negatively associated with distress. tress across the situations.
It is important to clarify our view of In summary, this descriptive study
emotion regulation with respect to the child should illuminate associations between par-
and his or her caretakers. Similar to others
ticular emotion regulation strategies and dis-
(Kopp, 1989; Rothbart & Posner, 1985; tress within and across different situations,
Thompson, 1994), we view the growth of as well as suggest contextual and individual
emotion regulation as both an individually sources of variation in the use of these strat-
based and a relational phenomenon. No egies.
doubt, especially when the adult is not
asked to take a more passive role, the parent Method
aids the child in emotion regulation. Rudi-
mentary emotion regulation may involve be- Subjects
ing able to distract oneself only when such Subjects were 37 toddlers (20 boys, 17
opportunities are presented. Consistent with girls) and their mothers. Subjects were first
our view, we included situations that did seen within 2 weeks (+ or -) of their sec-
and did not include an adult. Thus, we could ond birthdays. A second visit took place ap-
compare use of strategies in these different proximately 1 month later. The mean age
situations. was 24.2 months (SD = 1.4 weeks) at the
The strategies and their relations with first visit and 25.2 months (SD = 1.7 weeks)
at the second visit. Mothers were recruited
emotional expressiveness are examined in
through newspaper advertisements and
two contexts, separation and delay, each
with two variants. Each of the four situations through records from prenatal exercise
classes including the due dates of the moth-
are mildly stressful for children, yet they
present different challenges and different ers who attended. Two-thirds (67.6%) of the
children were firstborn. The mean age of the
levels of adult support. In separation, the
child's emotion regulation cannot be sup- mothers was 34.5 years. The sample was pre-
dominantly Caucasian and upper middle
ported by the presence of his or her primary
caregiver. Indeed, the absence of the mother class; 53% came from social class I on Hol-
is the primary source of distress within this lingshead's Two Factor Index (1975), 30%
setting. Two delay situations were used that
from social class II, 13% from social class
III, and 3% from social class IV.
varied in the extent to which the mother was
asked to remain relatively passive or active Overview of Design
in interaction with the child, thus varying All subject visits took place in the Child
the level of direct maternal involvement in
Development Laboratory. In one session,
the child's emotion regulation.
mothers and children were given a 10-min

This content downloaded from 155.69.24.171 on Mon, 09 May 2016 06:03:01 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
Grolnick, Bridges, and Connell 931
free play warm-up and then participated in Procedure for Food Delay.-This proce-
two delay procedures. One of the proce- dure is a modification of that used by
dures for each subject was conducted with Golden, Montare, and Bridger (1977). The
an instructional set to the mother to be fairly experimenter showed the child a plate of
passive and the other without limitations on goldfish crackers and raisins (the mother was
her behavior. The two delay procedures asked before coming whether her child liked
were separated by a 10-min play period. or could eat these foods; all children liked
at least one of the foods). The experimenter
In the other laboratory session, subjects
then ate a cracker in front of the child and
were given a free play warm-up period fol-
lowed by the separation procedure which in- said, "Mmm, that tastes good. These are for
cluded a period of separation from mother later. I need to do some things outside. I'm
going to put them away for a little while and
(child alone), followed by separation from
when I get back, you can have them. I'll be
mother with a slightly familiar experimenter
(experimenter present). The order of the ses- right back." She then placed them on the
shelf and left for 3 min, after which she re-
sions (delay and separation) was counterbal-
turned, gave the child one raisin or cracker,
anced with subjects randomly assigned to
one of the two orders. and then left for a second 3 min. After the
second 3 min, she returned and gave the
Delay Procedure child the plate of snacks. Thus, this delay
In each delay, the stimulus was placed situation also lasted for 6 min.
on a shelf within view but out of the child's
reach. The child was unable to obtain it be- Instructions to parents: Passive and ac-
fore the delay period was over. tive conditions.-In order to assess the
At the end of the free play, the experi-
child's emotional expressiveness and emo-
menter came into the room and gathered all tion regulation strategies under varying lev-
els of external support and assistance, the
but a few moderately interesting toys (e.g.,
a shape sorter, blocks, a soft ball, a small food and gift delays were conducted under
doll). After these toys were placed in the two different conditions: parent-active or
middle of the room, the experimenter asked parent-passive. In the parent-passive condi-
the mother to sit on a chair. The mother was tion, the mother was asked to remain in the
chair and to not initiate interaction with the
told that her child was going to be presented
with a situation in which he or she must wait child during the waiting period. She was,
to have something, and she was asked not to however, told that she should feel free to
respond to the child's initiations when nec-
retrieve the object for the child. Following
this, the mother received one of the two sets essary. In the parent-active condition, the
mother was asked to interact with the child
of guidelines according to assignment to the
parent-passive or -active condition. The ex- in any way she wanted. The only restriction
perimenter then brought in one of two ob- was that she should not retrieve the object.
jects: an attractive gift or a plate of food. The
Separation Procedure
orders of the two episodes (parent-active,
parent-passive) and two desirable objects Prior to coming to the laboratory, the
(food, gift) were all counterbalanced yield- mother was asked to bring a "special object"
ing four conditions (gift/passive, food/ac- belonging to the child, such as a "security"
tive; food/active, gift/passive; gift/active, blanket, and one other favorite toy so that
the child would have it with him or her dur-
food/passive; and food/passive, gift/active).
ing the separations. Upon coming to the lab-
Procedure for Attractive Gift.-The gift oratory, the mother and child engaged in 10
task was adapted from a procedure devel- min of free play in the lab playroom. After
oped by Block and Block (1980; see also 2 min, the mother brought out the "special
Vaughn, Kopp, & Krakow, 1984). The experi- object" which remained in the playroom for
menter brought a brightly wrapped present the rest of the procedure. After another 2
into the room and let the child look closely min, a familiar experimenter joined the
at it. After doing so, the experimenter said, mother and toddler for 3 min of play, after
"Oh, I forgot something outside. I'm going which she left and the mother and toddler
to put the present away until I get back. continued playing. A tap on the one-way
When I get back you can have your present. mirror then signaled the parent to leave, tell-
I'll be right back." The experimenter then ing her child that she would return shortly.
placed the gift on the shelf and left the room Upon exiting, the parent was shown to the
for 6 min, after which she returned and gave observation room where she could watch her
the gift to the child. child. The child was alone for up to 6 min. If

This content downloaded from 155.69.24.171 on Mon, 09 May 2016 06:03:01 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
932 Child Development
the child became extremely distressed, the ence of six categories of strategy was coded
procedure was terminated early. After 6 min by two independent raters. Multiple strate-
(or earlier), the experimenter entered. This gies within each interval were possible.
period lasted up to 3 min. If the child be- With one exception (other-directed comfort
came upset, the experimenter was instructed seeking was not possible in the separation
to engage the child in activity or try to soothe alone) all categories were possible in all sit-
the child. Otherwise, the experimenter took uations. The categories were as follows: Ac-
a more responsive role. Finally, the mother tive engagement with a substitute toy in-
returned and the experimenter left. volved sustained use of or interest in toys.
This category included active play alone or
Coding of Emotional Expression with the parent (in the parent-active delay)
Emotional expression in the four situa-
or the experimenter (experimenter-present
tions was coded in 5-sec intervals by two
separation). It should be noted that, in order
independent raters. All strategy ratings were
for behaviors to be included in this category,
done before emotional expression ratings so
children must demonstrate engagement
that the ratings of the two would not be con-
with the toys. For example, if the mother
founded. To further insure independence of
offers a game but the child does not engage
ratings, it was required that at least one of
the raters of emotion had not rated the sub-
in it, the behavior is not coded in this cate-
gory. Passive use of objects and exploration
ject for strategy use. Emotional expression involved behaviors oriented to the environ-
was coded using scales developed by
ment without active task engagement. This
Thompson (Thompson & Lamb, 1984).
included looking at or fingering toys and ex-
These scales assessed expressed affect from
ploring the room. These behaviors could
positive to negative using two measures; fa-
also be parent- or experimenter-related. Be-
cial expression, which ranged from 1 (bright
cause of the pervasiveness of behaviors in
smile) to 4 (neutral attentive, receptive ex-
this category and subsequent problems in
pression) to 8 (cryface), and vocalization,
obtaining reliability,' this code was only
which ranged from 1 (intense delight) to 4
given when it was the only behavior evident
(neutral vocalization) to 14 (hyperventilated
cry). During intervals in which the infant did
within a segment. Symbolic self-soothing
behaviors were statements or activities indi-
not present his or her face to the camera or
cating the use of cognitive/symbolic ways to
did not vocalize, affect was not coded. The deal with frustration. This included self-
facial and vocal scales were then averaged
directed statements such as "I'm a big girl"
to form one score. Summary scores designed
or talking about the experimenter (e.g.,
to describe the intensity and topography of
children's emotional responses were then "She'll be right back"). Physical self-
created. These included the mean emotion soothing included bodily directed behaviors
(e.g., thumb sucking, hair twisting) and us-
rating for each situation, the most negative
ing soft or familiar objects (e.g., hugging or
(highest) emotion reached and the most pos-
stroking a teddy bear or blanket) presumed
itive (lowest) emotion score. Latency to dis-
to be for comfort or security. Other-directed
tress onset was computed as the number of
included comfort-seeking behaviors such as
5-sec intervals before the first coding in the
wanting to be held, touching parent's or ex-
negative range (greater than 4). Finally, la-
perimenter's hair or clothing, reclining on
tency to peak distress was the number of in-
tervals before the child reached his or her the parent's or experimenter's lap. These be-
haviors could be distal or proximal. Focus on
peak (or most negative) level of distress in
the situation. the delay object/search for parent involved
behaviors indicating that the child was fo-
Coding of Emotion Regulation Strategies cused on the delay object or, in separation,
The child's emotion regulation strate- the parent, without symbolic self-soothing
gies were coded in the same 5-sec intervals attempts. This may include asking about the
as was emotional expression for each of the object (e.g., "When do I get my present?")
four situations. In each interval, the pres- or attempts to retrieve the object or the par-

1 Initially the passive strategy was coded even if another strategy was coded in the interval.
We found that virtually every interval ended up including this code since the child was likely
to be engaging in this type of behavior when doing other things. For example, the child might
be holding a toy when searching for his mother or might glance up during active toy play.
Obtaining reliability in this case was difficult since pauses in action were perceived by some
raters as a passive strategy but not others. Given this, we elected to code passive use of objects
and exploration only when no other strategy was coded in the interval.

This content downloaded from 155.69.24.171 on Mon, 09 May 2016 06:03:01 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
Grolnick, Bridges, and Connell 933
ent (e.g., trying to climb up to get the object, and 82% in the parent-active delay. Thus,
calling for assistance, trying to leave the in all situations the majority of children
room). showed some negative affect. Given the
level of negative affect displayed in our situ-
Results ations, they seem reasonably characterized
as mildly stressful.
Reliability
Interrater reliability was computed as A third piece of data concerns the per-
Cohen's kappas (Cohen, 1960) for both strat- centage of children who indicated some in-
egy and emotional expression ratings. In terest in or preoccupation with the frustrat-
each case, reliabilities are based on random ing situation (either the mother's absence in
samples of 20 subjects. Statistics reflect the separations or the forbidden object in the
agreement between raters prior to discus- delays). This could be determined by com-
sion. For emotion ratings, the kappa statistic puting the percentage of children in each
provides a conservative estimate of reliabil- situation who at some point focused on the
ity since exact agreement (controlling for object (coding of "focus on delay object/
base rates) is required while the emotion search"). For the child-alone separation, all
scale represents at least an ordinal scale. 37 children focused on mother (i.e., engaged
Kappas for facial and vocal emotion, respec- in some search behavior) at least once, while
tively, were: parent-passive delay = .94, .85, 69% of children did so when with the experi-
parent-active delay = .92, .77, child-alone menter. The figures for focus on delay object
separation = .94, .89, and experimenter- were 100% for the parent-passive delay and
present separation = .95, .92. Kappas for 94% for the parent-active delay.
strategy codings were computed for each
As indicated in the method section, epi-
strategy separately and across strategies for
sodes were cut short if the child showed ex-
the four situations. Mean kappas (across the
cessive distress. The number of children
four situations) were: active engagement =
whose episodes were shortened were as fol-
.74, passive use of objects = .69, symbolic
lows: 0 for parent-active delay, 2 for parent-
self-soothing = .70, physical self-soothing =
passive delay, 19 for child-alone separation,
.85, other-directed = .86, and focus on delay
and 8 for experimenter-present separation.
object/search = .81. Overall kappas were for
Of the child-alone separations that were
the four situations: parent-passive delay =
shortened, most were in the 3-4-min range.
.81, parent-active delay = .78, child-alone
When evaluating strategy use, we utilized
separation = .82, experimenter-present sep-
aration = .80. the percentage of intervals in which various
strategies occurred, thus prorating these epi-
Evaluating situations for arousal.-The sodes.
four situations were expected to be mildly
stressful for 2-year-olds and to require emo- Emotional expression.-The intensity
tion regulation. In order to address whether and topography indices of emotion were cor-
related, and, thus, principal component fac-
this was true, three pieces of data were ex-
tor analyses were computed for the five emo-
amined. First, means for emotional expres-
tion indices (average emotion, most negative
sion ratings (see Table 1) were in the nega-
tive range, though they varied from 4.08 [highest] emotion, latency to distress onset,
latency to peak distress onset, and most posi-
(parent-active delay) to 5.75 (child-alone
tive [lowest] emotion) for each situation to
separation). There were, as expected, large determine whether these indices could be
individual differences as indicated by stan-
dard deviations varying from .83 to 1.9. combined. Factor analysis with emotion in-
There were higher standard deviations for dices such as those used in the present study
the separations than for the delays. has been used in several previous studies
(e.g., Connell & Thompson, 1986; Thomp-
While the mean emotion ratings provide son et al., 1988). Results indicated one-factor
some information about distress, children solutions (using the Scree test [Cattell,
fluctuated from interval to interval in their 1966]) for each of the four situations. Eigen-
affect. The percentage of children in each values for the emotion factor for the four sit-
situation who expressed negative emotion uations were: Parent-passive delay = 3.56,
was thus also examined. Eighty-two percent parent-active delay = 3.34, child-alone sep-
of children showed some negative affect (af- aration = 3.88, experiment-present separa-
fect ratings greater than 4) in the child-alone tion = 3.95. For each situation, factor load-
separation, 56% in the experimenter-present ings for all variables were above .53. Based
separation, 83% in the parent-passive delay, on these results, emotion variables were

This content downloaded from 155.69.24.171 on Mon, 09 May 2016 06:03:01 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
934 Child Development
TABLE 1

MEANS AND F VALUES FOR REPEATED-MEASURES ANOVAS WITH SITUATION AS GROUPING VARIABLE

Experimenter-
Child-Alone Present Parent-Active Parent-Passive
Separation Separation Delay Delay F(3, 33)
Emotion:

Emotion composite ... .73a .26ab -.82c -.23b 6.62**


SD ........................... 1.74 1.85 .79 1.04
Range ................... - 1.0-4.6 - 1.06-3.93 -2.05-1.68 - 1.16-3.53
Average emotion ...... 5.75a 5.24ab 4.08C 4.72b 8.34***
SD ........................... 1.84 1.9 .83 1.12
Range ................... 3.9-9.9 3.83-9.24 2.75-6.57 3.72-8.70
Strategies:
Active engagement ... 30.39a* 44.82b 44.20b 26.86a 6.03***
SD ........................... 26.92 35.71 24.38 20.20
Range ................... 0-91.99 0-94.59 0-89.04 0-76.54
Passive use of ob-
jects .................. 12.89 16.57 16.16 11.79 1.88
SD ........................... 15.22 16.65 13.14 8.90
Range ................... 0-50 0-57.14 0-53.42 0-31.42
Symbolic self-
soothing ............... 1.15b .09a 1.15b 1.87c 3.69*
SD ........................... 3.42 .46 2.19 4.10
Range ................... 0-17.65 0-2.56 0-6.49 0-19.40
Physical self-
soothing ............... 19.41 14.62 9.51 12.49 .78
SD ........................... 29.53 24.03 12.67 20.74
Range ................... 0-94 0-90.91 0-52.63 0-95.89

Other directed ........... ?11.08


SD ........................... 2.65a 11.81
7.79b 23.79C
19.11 17.00***

Range ...................... ? 0-57.14 0-46.74 0-91.67


Focus on delay ob-
ject/search ........... 33.73a 14.98b 20.51b 22.42b 2.70+
SD ........................... 34.67 24.90 19.37 20.53
Range ................... 0-100 0-77.77 0-82.43 0-74.16

NOTE.-Means not sharing the same subscript are significantly different at p < .05 level.
+ p < .10.
* p < .05.
** p < .01.
*** p < .001.

standardized and combined to create four 23.79), and focus on delay object/search,
composite emotion scores. Alphas for the 22.93 (14.98-33.73). The results indicate
four composite variables were as follows: that all strategies were used in all situations,
parent-passive delay = .90, parent-active though their use differed by situation (situa-
delay = .87, child-alone separation = .92, tional differences are discussed later).
and experimenter-present separation = .93.
Correlations among strategies within
Emotion regulation strategies.-In or- each of the four situations indicated, for all
der to determine whether the strategies situations, negative relations between active
were used in at least some of the situations, engagement and focus on delay object/
average percentages of intervals including search, ranging from r = -.38, p < .05 to r
each of the six strategies were computed. = -.64, p < .001. Other relations varied by
The average percent usage across situation situation. For the parent-passive delay, use
and ranges (in parentheses) for the four sepa- of active engagement was negatively associ-
rate situations were: active engagement, ated with other-directed behavior, r = -.45,
36.54 (26.86-44.82), passive use of objects, p < .01, and physical self-soothing was nega-
14.53, (11.79-16.57), symbolic self-soothing, tively related to focus on delay object, r =
1.06 (.09-1.87), physical self-soothing, 14.01 -.40, p < .01. For the parent-active delay
(9.51-19.41), other-directed, 8.56 (2.65- and experimenter-present separation situa-

This content downloaded from 155.69.24.171 on Mon, 09 May 2016 06:03:01 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
Grolnick, Bridges, and Connell 935
TABLE 2

WITHIN-SITUATION CORRELATIONS BETWEEN STRATEGY USE AND EMOTIONAL EXPRESSIVENESS

SEPARATION DELAY

Experimenter Experimenter Parent Parent


Absent Present Active Passive

1. Active engagement ..................................... -.56*** -.84*** -.54*** -.27


2. Passive use of objects .................................... -.49** -.35* -.09 -.39*
3. Symbolic self-soothing ....................... -.10 -.19 -.02 -.22
4. Physical self-soothing ..................... -.004 .48** .25 .30*
5. Other directed ............................................... .31* .00 .15
6. Focus on frustration object/search for parent ... .63*** .63*** .47** .02

* p < .05.
** p < .01.
*** p < .001.

tions, active engagement was negatively as- object/search lowest (most positively associ-
sociated with physical self-soothing, r = ated with distress), though the possibility
-.49, p < .01 and r = -.65, p < .001, re- that variations in the continuum might be
spectively. Finally, for the child-alone sepa- apparent in different contexts was consid-
ration, there were negative relations be- ered. In order to address this hypothesis, re-
tween search for parent and both passive use lations between strategies and emotional ex-
of objects, r = -.35, p < .05, and physical pressiveness within each situation were
self-soothing, r = -.48, p < .01. examined.

Correlations between emotional expres-


Order and object effects.-To check for
siveness and strategy use within the four sit-
possible session order effects (separation vs.
uations are presented in Table 2. For three
delay first), ANOVAs were conducted for
situations (the two separations and parent-
each of the six strategies and the emotion
active delay), significant negative relations
composite for each of the four situations with
between active engagement and emotional
order as the grouping variable. Of the 28
expression indicate that children who were
analyses, only two F values were significant.
less upset were more likely to actively inter-
Results indicated that there was more sym-
act with objects than were those who were
bolic self-soothing, F = 4.79, p < .04, in the
more upset. Similarly, passive use of objects
child-alone separation when the separation
was first (M = .21) relative to when the delay
was negatively associated with distress
(though not as strongly as for active engage-
was first (M = .13). Second, there was a
ment) in three of the four situations. Level
higher proportion of active engagement with
of distress was positively correlated with fo-
objects in the parent-passive delay, F =
cus on the delay object/search in all but the
6.19, p < .02, when the delay was first (M =
parent-passive delay. Use of physical self-
51.1) relative to the separation (M = 30.3).
soothing was positively related to distress in
These results are likely to have occurred by
the experimenter-present separation and
chance so are not interpretable. Similar
ANOVAs conducted for order of parent con-
parent-passive delay situations. Finally,
other-directed behavior was positively asso-
dition (passive or active first) within the de-
ciated with distress only in the experi-
lay session revealed no significant effects on
menter-present separation.
strategies or emotion. Finally, ANOVAs to
determine whether delay object (gift vs.
Contextual Influences on Strategy Use and
food) elicited different emotion regulation
Emotional Expressiveness
strategies or greater distress indicated no To examine contextual differences in
significant effects.
emotional expression, a repeated-measures
Relations between Strategy Use and ANOVA was conducted for the emotion
Emotional Expressiveness composite. The grouping variables were sex
It was hypothesized that the six strate- of child (between subjects) and situation
gies could be placed along a continuum with (four situations; within subjects) and the in-
active engagement highest (most negatively teraction between sex of child and situation.
associated with distress) and focus on delay Sex of child was included because there is

This content downloaded from 155.69.24.171 on Mon, 09 May 2016 06:03:01 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
936 Child Development

some evidence that males and females may gagement was also used more in the experi-
differ in responses to frustration (Goldberg menter-present separation than the parent-
& Lewis, 1969) and emotional expression passive delay, F(1, 35) = 7.59, p < .01, and
(Malatesta & Haviland, 1982). There was a in the parent-active delay relative to the
strong effect for situation, F(3, 33) = 6.62, p child-alone separation, F(1, 35) = 7.25, p <
< .002, but no effects for sex of child or the .01. Symbolic self-soothing was used less in
interaction. Pairwise repeated-measure the experimenter-present separation than in
ANOVAs indicated that children displayed the parent-passive delay, F(1, 35) = 7.14, p
greater distress in the child-alone separation < .01, or the parent-active delay, F(1, 35) =
than either the parent-active, F(1, 35) = 8.59, p < .01, and more in the parent-passive
41.56, p < .001, or parent-passive, F(1, 35) delay than in the child-alone separation, F(1,
= 17.30, p < .001, delays. There was no sig- 35) = 4.08, p < .05, or the parent-active de-
nificant difference between distress in the lay, F(1, 35) = 4.68, p < .05. Other-directed
two separations. Children did exhibit behavior was used more in the parent-
greater distress in the parent-passive rela- passive delay than in the parent-active de-
tive to the parent-active delay, F(1, 35) = lay, F(1, 35) = 14.27, p < .001, or the experi-
4.04, p < .05, and in the experimenter- menter-present separation, F(1, 35) = 33.08,
present separation relative to the parent- p < .001, and more in the parent-active delay
active delay, F(1, 35) = 16.40, p < .001. Be- than in the experimenter-present separation,
cause the means for the emotion composite F(1, 35) = 4.87, p < .05. Finally, focus on
are not interpretable along the Thompson delay object/search was used more in the
scale, the means and statistics for mean emo- child-alone than in the experimenter-
tion (unstandardized) are also presented. present situation, F(1, 35) = 8.06, p < .01,
Statistics for mean emotion parallel those for the parent-active delay, F(1, 35) = 4.79, p <
the emotion composite. .05, or the parent-passive delay, F(1, 35) =
To examine contextual differences in
4.05, p < .05.
strategy use, a repeated-measures multivari- Cross-Situational Consistency of Strategies
ate analysis of variance (MANOVA) was per- and Emotional Expressiveness
formed. The MANOVA examined five out of We next examined whether individual
the six strategies-other-directed was ex- differences in strategy use and emotional ex-
cluded from the analysis because it was only pression were consistent across situation.
a potential strategy in three of the situations. First, correlations were computed between
Results of the MANOVA revealed a strong the proportions of intervals in which particu-
main effect for situation, F(1, 35) = 31.77, p lar strategies were used in each of four situa-
< .001, but no significant sex of child or sex tions. Second we created a strategy compos-
of child x situation effects. ite by weighting individual strategy scores
To follow up on the significant situation by their correlations with emotional ex-
effect uncovered in the MANOVA and to as- pression within each situation and then
sess situational differences in other-directed summing these weighted scores. These
behavior, repeated-measures ANOVAs were weighted composite scores for each situation
were then correlated with each other.
conducted for each of the six strategies. Sig-
nificant situation effects were obtained for Correlations between the same strate-
three strategies (active engagement, other- gies across situation (see Table 3) indicated
directed, and symbolic self-soothing) and a that there were significant correlations be-
marginally significant effect for one (focus tween strategies used in the two delays and
on delay object/search). Pairwise repeated- strategies used in the two separations, but
measures ANOVAs for situation were con-
there was little consistency in strategy use
ducted for these four strategies (e.g., use of across the delay and separation situations.
active engagement in experimenter-present Active engagement and other-directed be-
separation and parent-active delay-six pos- havior were, however, significantly corre-
sible pairs of situations) (see Table 1). Re- lated across the experimenter-present sepa-
sults indicated that active engagement was ration and parent-passive delay situations.
used more in the experimenter-present than
the child-alone separation, F(1, 35) = 4.27, Relations between the weighted scores
p < .05, and more in the parent-active than composite indicated a similar pattern of as-
the parent-passive delay, F(1, 35) = 11.29, p sociations. There were significant correla-
< .01, suggesting that there was most active tions between use of more active, reorient-
engagement in situations where an adult ing strategies in the two separation
was available and participatory. Active en- situations, r = .48, p < .01, and a positive

This content downloaded from 155.69.24.171 on Mon, 09 May 2016 06:03:01 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
. 0
? ?

I0I

? ~~4.4 ?,

oo c3
co

? * 0

C9a

z
CI I0
CY) 04

C a
0 ?9
++ + Q

W .4
z z -.
Z

z I

- - I 01 i

cd ?4 >1

on :>, .g I
Cd

S0 It-000
cd

H 3
. cd

0.
L01flJ~
o ,VOC
-o o

z
I I N c
* *
o - ")
0,10hI.J)
-'-- CC
cn -c 3 ~ ~ V3 U

* 0I + *
: iI t" if 0 1

0 l)
Cd

This content downloaded from 155.69.24.171 on Mon, 09 May 2016 06:03:01 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
938 Child Development

but nonsignificant correlation for the two de- the presence of an adult (either mother or
lay situations, r = .28, p < .11. There was experimenter), and thus the adult may have
also a significant correlation between use of contributed to children being able to use
more reorienting strategies in the parent- this strategy. The importance of adults for
passive delay and the experimenter-present active engagement in 2-year-olds is sup-
separation situations, r = .35, p < .05. ported by that fact that it was used most
when an adult was available and participa-
Correlations were also computed to ex-
amine individual differences in distress tory (i.e., more in the parent-active delay
across situation. Results indicated consis-
than the parent-passive delay and more in
the experimenter-present separation than in
tency in distress expressed across the two
the child-alone separation). It is likely that,
separation situations, r = .58, p < .001, and
at age 2, the child is most able to become
across the two delay situations, r = .39, p <
engaged with alternate activities and sustain
.05, but there were no significant correla- self-distraction with the assistance of an
tions across the separation and delay situa-
tions.
adult. Further studies might begin to disen-
tangle what the caregiver contributes to use
Discussion of particular strategies.

In this study, we present descriptive In contrast to active engagement, the


data on emotional expression and emotion low use of symbolic self-soothing suggests
regulation strategies, two components of that, while children of this age may be capa-
emotion regulation, in 2-year-olds. Descrip- ble of representing their experience, they
tive data are presented on (1) levels of emo- may not be able to use such capacities in
tional expressiveness and strategy use in times of stress. Use of other strategies varied
four situations, (2) relations between strat- by context. For example, children used the
egy use and emotional expressiveness, and most other-directed behavior in the parent-
(3) the consistency of emotional expressive- passive delay. This may have been in part
ness and strategy use across situations. due to the strangeness of a situation where
mother was present but inactive. However,
First, with regard to emotional expres-
this situation is not atypical in the home in
sion, the data suggest strong individual dif-
that mothers often engage in activities while
ferences in responses to two separation and
their child is present but in which their child
two delay procedures. While overall the sit-
cannot participate (such as cooking on the
uations could be described as mildly stress-
stove). In fact, several mothers noted that
ful given that the majority of children this "clingy" behavior was typical of such
showed some distress in each of the four sit-
situations at home. This result again high-
uations, some children became extremely lights the relative dependence of 2-year-olds
upset and others less so. Interestingly, cross-
on caretakers for emotion regulation.
situational consistency was greatest for the
two separations which also elicited the
Obtained relations between strategies
greatest distress. Such differential reactions
and emotional expression were as predicted
could be a function of temperament (Roth-
for the most part-children who used active
bart & Derryberry, 1981) or attachment rela-
engagement were least distressed while
tions reflecting the history of caretaker-child
those who used focus on delay object/search
interactions (Frodi & Thompson, 1985).
tended to be more highly distressed. We
For emotion regulation strategies, re- caution that this study does not elucidate the
sults indicated that all six strategies ap- causal relations between strategy use and
peared in all situations. Across situations, emotional expressiveness. While our results
the most frequently used strategy was active are consistent with the interpretation that
engagement with substitute objects. This particular strategies, those that involve the
finding indicates that, by 2 years, children most reorientation away from the stressful
are capable of reorienting their attention to- situation, are most effective in reducing dis-
ward other objects in their environments. tress, it is quite possible that, within a given
Such abilities may be facilitated by in- assessment, the intensity of the child's dis-
creases in effortful control and representa- tress may promote or inhibit the deployment
tional capacities occurring around this age of particular strategies. Additional studies
(Piaget, 1962; Posner & Rothbart, 1980). It will be required to evaluate the causal links
should be emphasized that, for three of the between these aspects of emotion regulation
situations, active engagement occurred in using sequential and lag analyses.

This content downloaded from 155.69.24.171 on Mon, 09 May 2016 06:03:01 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
Grolnick, Bridges, and Connell 939
Our results also speak to the consistency may also suggest that some children have
of individual differences in use of particular more contextual support for the use of these
strategies. In general, there was consistency strategies.
across the two delays and across the two sep-
Although the results add to the litera-
arations in use of specific strategies but few
relations across the different contexts. There ture in describing emotion regulation pro-
cesses in 2-year-olds, some key issues still
were also positive relations between emo- remain to be resolved. What are the causal
tional expression in the two delays and the
relations between emotional expressiveness
two separations but little consistency across
and strategy use? What about emotional reg-
context. One interpretation of these findings
ulation develops with age? Are certain strat-
is that the delay situations may be more
egies being used more or less or do the effec-
likely to elicit anger whereas the separations
tiveness and flexible use of the same
might be more likely to elicit sadness and/
strategies change with the new demands and
or fear. It is possible that the regulation of
supports that many children face? What are
these emotions might be specific. In future
the contextual supports that increase the fre-
research, it would be interesting to consider
quency of use of more active, attentional
the regulation of conceptually discrete emo-
strategies? Finally, what is the role of tem-
tions separately.
perament in individual differences in emo-
It is also to be expected, of course, that tion regulation? Our descriptive findings
two measures taken within the same assess- may provide a start in beginning to tackle
ment on the same day would be more highly these and other key issues in the develop-
associated with each other than would two ment of emotion regulation.
measures taken on different days. While it
would be optimal to include delays and sep-
arations within the same assessment, the References
strain this would impose on the child makes
Ainsworth, M. D. S., & Wittig, M. (1969). Attach-
this infeasible. In addition, following sepa-
ment and exploratory behavior of one-year-
ration with delay would not only impose un-
olds in a strange situation. In B. M. Foss (Ed.),
due stress but would likely affect the child's
Determinants of infant behavior (Vol. 4, pp.
response. Due to these issues, we chose the
111-136). London: Methuen.
procedure that would result in the most
Berk, L. E. (in press). Children's private speech:
valid assessment and least unnecessary
An overview of theory and the status of re-
stress. It is the case, however, that previous
search. In R. M. Diaz & L. E. Berk (Eds.),
research examining consistency in infant
Private speech: Interaction to self-regulation.
emotional responsiveness using the Strange
Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Situation (i.e., the same context across multi-
Block, J. H., & Block, J. (1980). The role of ego-
ple assessments) has demonstrated consider-
control and ego-resiliency in the organization
able cross-time and cross-parent consistency
of behavior. In W. A. Collins (Ed.), Minnesota
in emotional expression and social interac-
Symposia on Child Psychology (Vol. 13).
tive behaviors (e.g., Bridges & Connell,
Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
1991; Connell & Thompson, 1986). Thus,
lack of correlation across context in this Braungart, J. M., & Stifter, C. A. (1991). Regulation
of negative reactivity during the strange situa-
study is likely to be due to contextual differ-
tion: Temperament and attachment in 12-
ences rather than lack of proximity in time
alone. month-old infants. Infant Behavior and De-
velopment, 14, 349-364.
When considering the weighted scores Bridges, L. J., & Connell, J. P. (1991). Consistency
composites, consistency was somewhat and inconsistency in infant emotional and so-
more evident across the two contextually dif- cial interactive behavior across contexts and
ferent types of situation and less evident be- caregivers. Infant Behavior and Develop-
tween the two delay situations. Specifically, ment, 14, 471-487.
children who used strategies more nega- Bridges, L. J., & Grolnick, W. S. (1995). The devel-
tively related to distress in the delay where opment of emotional self-regulation in in-
the mother was passive also tended to use fancy and early childhood. In N. Eisenberg
such strategies in the separation where the (Ed.), Review of personality and social psy-
experimenter was available. Such cross- chology (Vol. 15). New York: Sage.
contextual relations in use of more actively Cattell, R. B. (1966). The scree test for the number
reorienting strategies might represent of factors. Multivariate Behavioral Research,
emerging personality characteristics. They 1, 245-276.

This content downloaded from 155.69.24.171 on Mon, 09 May 2016 06:03:01 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
940 Child Development
Cohen, J. (1960). A coefficient of agreement for A developmental perspective. Develop-
nominal scales. Educational and Psychologi- mental Psychology, 18, 199-214.
cal Measurement, 20, 37-46. Kopp, C. B. (1989). Regulation of distress and neg-
Cole, P. M., & Kaslow, N. J. (1989). Interactional ative emotions: A developmental view. De-
and cognitive strategies for affect regulation: velopmental Psychology, 25, 343-354.
Developmental perspectives on childhood Malatesta, C. Z., & Haviland, J. M. (1982). Learn-
depression. In L. Alloy (Ed.), Cognitive pro- ing display rules: The socialization of emo-
cesses in depression (pp. 310-343). New tion expression in infancy. Child Develop-
York: Guilford. ment, 53, 991-1003.
Connell, J. P., & Thompson, R. A. (1986). Emotion Mischel, W. (1973). Toward a cognitive social
and social interaction in the Strange Situa- learning reconceptualization of personality.
tion: Consistencies and asymmetric influ- Psychology Review, 80, 252-283.
ences in the second year. Child Development, Mischel, W. (1974). Processes in delay of gratifi-
57, 733-745. cation. In L. Berkowitz (Ed.), Progress in ex-
Derryberry, D., & Rothbart, M. K. (1988). Arousal, perimental personality research (Vol. 3, pp.
affect, and attention as components of temper- 249-292). New York: Academic.
ament. Journal of Personality and Social Psy- Mischel, W., & Ebbesen, E. B. (1970). Attention
chology, 55, 958-966. in delay of gratification. Journal of Personal-
Field, T. (1987). Interaction and attachment in ity and Social Psychology, 16, 329-337.
normal and atypical infants. Journal of Con- Mischel, W., Ebbesen, E. B., & Zeiss, A. (1972).
sulting and Clinical Psychology, 55, 853-859. Cognitive and attentional mechanisms in de-
Flavell, J. (1966). Le langage priv6 (Private lan- lay of gratification. Journal of Personality and
guage). Bulletin de Psychologie, 19, 698-701. Social Psychology, 21, 204-218.
Fox, N. (1989). Psychophysiological correlates of Passman, R. H., & Weisberg, P. (1975). Mothers
emotional reactivity in the first year of life. and blankets as agents for promoting play and
Developmental Psychology, 25, 364-372. exploration by young children in a novel en-
Frodi, A., & Thompson, R. (1985). Infants' af- vironment: The effects of social and nonsocial
fective responses in the Strange Situation: Ef- attachment objects. Developmental Psychol-
fects of prematurity and quality of attachment. ogy, 11, 170-177.
Child Development, 56, 1280-1290. Piaget, J. (1952). The origins of intelligence in
Gaensbauer, T. J., Connell, J. P., & Schultz, L. A. children. New York: International Universi-
(1983). Emotion and attachment: Interrela- ties Press.
tionships in a structured laboratory paradigm. Piaget, J. (1954). The construction of reality in the
Developmental Psychology, 19, 815-831. child. New York: Basic.
Gianino, A., & Tronick, E. Z. (1988). The mutual Piaget, J. (1962). Play, dreams and imitation in
regulation model: The infant's self and inter- childhood. New York: Norton.
active regulation coping and defense. In Posner, M. I., & Rothbart, M. K. (1980). The devel-
T. Field, P. McCabe, & N. Schneiderman opment of attentional mechanisms. Nebraska
(Eds.), Stress and coping. Hillsdale, NJ: Erl- Symposium on Motivation, 28, 1-52.
baum. Rothbart, M. K. (1981). Measurement of tempera-
Goldberg, S., & Lewis, M. (1969). Play behavior ment in infancy. Child Development, 52,
in the year-old infant: Early sex differences. 569-578.
Child Development, 41, 21-31. Rothbart, M. K., & Derryberry, D. (1981). Devel-
Golden, M., Montare, A., & Bridger, W. H. (1977). opment of individual differences in tempera-
Verbal control of delay behavior in two-year- ment. In M. E. Lamb & A. L. Brown (Eds.),
old boys as a function of social class. Child Advances in developmental psychology (Vol.
Development, 48, 1107-1111. 1, 37-86). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Hollingshead, A. B. (1975). Four-Factor Index of Rothbart, M. K., & Posner, M. I. (1985). Tempera-
Social Status. Unpublished manuscript, Yale ment and the development of self-regulation.
University. In L. C. Hartlage & C. F. Telzrow (Eds.), The
Izard, C. E., & Malatesta, C. Z. (1987). Perspec- neuropsychology of individual differences
tives on emotional development I: Differen- (pp. 93-123). New York: Plenum.
tial emotions theory of early emotional devel- Thompson, R. A. (1994). Emotion regulation: A
opment. In J. D. Osofsky (Ed.), Handbook of theme in search of definition. In N. Fox (Ed.),
infant development (2d ed.). New York: The development of emotion regulation.
Wiley. Monographs of the Society for Research in
Klackenberg, G. (1949). Thumbsucking: Fre- Child Development, 59(2-3, Serial No. 240).
quency and etiology. Pediatrics, 4, 418-424. Thompson, R. A., Connell, J. P., & Bridges, L. J.
Kopp, C. B. (1982). Antecedents of self-regulation: (1988). Temperament, emotion, and social in-

This content downloaded from 155.69.24.171 on Mon, 09 May 2016 06:03:01 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
Grolnick, Bridges, and Connell 941

teractive behavior in the Strange Situation: A communication in infants. American Psychol-


component process analysis of attachment ogist, 44, 112-119.
system functioning. Child Development, 59, Vaughn, B. E., Kopp, C. G., & Krakow, J. B. (1984).
1102-1110. The emergence and consolidation of self-
Thompson, R. A., & Lamb, M. E. (1984). Assessing control from 18 to 30 months of age: Norma-
qualitative dimensions of emotional respon- tive trends and individual differences. Child
siveness in infants: Separation reactions in Development, 55, 990-1004.
the strange situation. Infant Behavior and Vygotsky, L. S. (1986). Thought and language. (2d
Development, 7, 423-445. ed.). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. (Originally
Tronick, E. Z. (1989). Emotions and emotional published 1934)

This content downloaded from 155.69.24.171 on Mon, 09 May 2016 06:03:01 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms

You might also like