You are on page 1of 10

Research Article

British Journal of Occupational Therapy


2020, Vol. 83(2) 78–87
Participation patterns of adolescents with ! The Author(s) 2019
Article reuse guidelines:
autism spectrum disorder compared to their sagepub.com/journals-permissions
DOI: 10.1177/0308022619853518
peers: Parents’ perspectives journals.sagepub.com/home/bjot

Liron Lamash1,2 , Gary Bedell1, Naomi Josman2

Abstract
Introduction: Although participation is an important outcome of rehabilitation interventions and a critical indicator of quality of
life, few studies have focused on the participation patterns of adolescents with autism spectrum disorder in their daily activities or
the environmental factors that influence such participation. This study aims to describe the participation characteristics of
adolescents with autism spectrum disorder compared with typically developing peers in home, school and community and to
identify supporting or hindering environmental features.
Method: The Participation and Environment Measure for Children and Youth was administered to 59 parents of adolescents
(M ¼ 14.51 years) with autism spectrum disorder and a control group of 188 parents of typically developing adolescents in Israel,
and the results were compared.
Results: Adolescents with autism spectrum disorder showed lower participation levels at home and school, and much lower
levels in the community, preferring solitary activities that involve technology. Their typically developing peers participated in more
activities that involved social interaction skill. In addition, parents of adolescents with autism spectrum disorder rated more
environmental factors as barriers in all environments.
Conclusion: Participation gaps between adolescents with autism spectrum disorder and their typically developing peers should be
considered when developing interventions, allocating services and creating policies to meet the unique needs of adolescents with
autism spectrum disorder.

Keywords
Autism spectrum disorder, adolescence, participation, environment, occupational therapy
Received 27 December 2018; accepted 8 May 2019

Introduction
ability to execute tasks or actions, and involvement in
Autism spectrum disorder (ASD) is a neurodevelopmen- life situations (World Health Organization (WHO),
tal disorder characterized by deficits in social communi- 2007). Its environmental factors include aspects of the
cation and social interaction across multiple contexts physical, sensory, social and attitudinal worlds, and it
(for example resulting from deficits in social-emotional defines participation as ‘involvement in a life situation’
reciprocity, verbal and nonverbal communicative behav- or as ‘the lived experience’ of people in the context in
iours used for social interaction, and maintaining and which they live, linked to activity and environmental
understanding relationships), as well as restricted, repet- demands, and adequacy of resources (Bedell, 2012).
itive patterns of behaviour, interests or activities. The Participation has become an important outcome of reha-
daily functioning and social participation of individuals bilitation interventions and a critical indicator of quality
with ASD are critical features when assessing impair- of life (WHO, 2001).
ment severity and indicate overall health and wellbeing
(American Psychiatric Association (APA), 2013).
Adolescence challenges individuals with ASD as they Literature review
transition to independent adult life in everyday contexts The majority of published studies that refer to partici-
(Seltzer et al., 2003). Several studies showed that youth pation among individuals with ASD address children
with ASD tend to be increasingly isolated, with more
restricted independence, employment and higher educa-
1
tion than their typically developing (TD) peers (Liptak Occupational Therapy Department, Tufts University, MA, USA
2
Faculty of Social Welfare & Health Sciences, University of Haifa, Israel
et al., 2011; Shattuck et al., 2011).
Corresponding author:
The International Classification of Functioning, Liron Lamash, University of Haifa, Abba Khoushy Ave 199, Haifa, 3498838,
Disability and Health: Children and Youth Version is Israel.
designed to document development characteristics, Email: Lironlamash@gmail.com
Lamash et al. 79

under the age of 12 years or mixed ages. Those that Given the increasing demand for improved transition
focused on adolescents with ASD examined mainly programming for adolescents with disabilities, this
social participation and recreational activities. For study’s aims were to compare participation patterns of
example, Potvin et al. (2013) compared the recreational adolescents with ASD to their TD peers in home, school
engagement of children with high functional autism and and community settings using the PEM-CY. An addi-
their TD peers and indicated that children with autism tional study goal was to identify environmental features
showed less diversity of participation than did their TD and resources that support or hinder participation of
peers and participated in significantly fewer physical adolescents with ASD in those settings.
activities and pretend play. However, the authors
found no difference in the adolescents’ personal intensi-
ty, enjoyment or recreation preferences. Solish et al. Method
(2010) compared activity participation and friendship
in TD children, children with autism and children with
Participants
an intellectual disability between the ages of 5 and In Israel, most educational and clinical services (such as
17 years and indicated that children with autism partic- occupational therapy and speech therapy) for children
ipated in significantly fewer social and recreational activ- and adolescents with special needs are provided in their
ities than did the TD and intellectual disability groups. educational environment, under the supervision of the
However, because it would be appropriate to expect Ministry of Education. As such, we recruited a conve-
participation patterns among adolescents to differ from nience sample of participants through the Israeli educa-
participation among younger children, developing tional system. It consisted of 245 adolescents between
knowledge of the adolescence period is very important. the ages of 11 and 19 years (M ¼ 14.51, SD ¼ 1.54).
The existing literature lacks reference to extensive par- The ASD group consisted of 59 adolescents (M ¼ 14.51
ticipation of adolescents with ASD in different environ- years, SD ¼ 1.54; 49 boys and 10 girls). The group’s
ments or to the environmental factors that influence
gender demographic generally parallels the male-to-
participation, as the International Classification of
female ratio in high functioning ASD adolescents,
Functioning, Disability and Health for Children and
which typically converges at approximately 4:1 or 5:1
Youth (ICF-CY) (World Health Organization, 2007)
(Bishop et al., 2016). All participants attended seventh-
model recommends. Askari et al.’s (2015) scoping
to twelfth-grade classes at six mainstream secondary
review on participation of youth with ASD used exten-
schools. The group with ASD attended classes designed
sive systematic strategies to review evidence but found
few relevant articles, indicating that the existing knowl- specifically for students with ASD but within the main-
edge is limited and that few studies used objective meas- stream system. Their primary teachers provided a file
ures of participation. Askari et al. recommended that with an updated diagnosis of ASD and IQ scores.
future research with adolescents with ASD use standard- Only participants with recorded average or above-
ized measures. average IQs were included in the ASD group. The TD
The Participation and Environment Measure for group consisted of 188 adolescents (M ¼ 14.76 years,
Children and Youth (PEM-CY) is a unique standardized SD ¼ 1.88; 79 boys and 109 girls). We excluded partic-
assessment of participation in home, school and commu- ipants with significant physical or mental health condi-
nity activities and of environmental factors that influ- tions that might limit their movements, senses or
ence such participation (Coster et al., 2011). The activities (and therefore, participation) during the
PEM-CY’s strength – presentation of those patterns research period.
and factors in various contexts – adds important infor-
mation to studies with children and youth with develop- Measures
mental and acquired disabilities (Bedell et al., 2013;
Data were collected using a demographic questionnaire
Coster et al., 2013; Khetani et al., 2014). The value of
and the PEM-CY, which the parents completed. The
using the PEM-CY to characterize the participation pat-
terns of adolescents with ASD is that it adds reference to demographic questionnaire provided information, such
the environmental determinants of participation impor- as age, gender, socioeconomic status (relative to the
tant for developing effective interventions. Israeli national average wage), parent education (high
Studies that have used the PEM-CY to compare par- school or less, professional training, academic gradua-
ticipation of children and youth with ASD to their TD tion) and residence type (rural or urban), about the ado-
peers included exclusively younger children or mixed-age lescents and their families.
samples, or focused on only one setting among home, According to the manual, the PEM-CY was devel-
school or community (see for example Bedell et al., 2013; oped and tested as a parental report measure, and
Coster et al., 2013; Egilson et al., 2017; Law et al., 2013; there is no self-reported version for adolescents. The
Simpson et al., 2018). Thus, they did not fully utilize the PEM-CY is a parental reporting questionnaire that
PEM-CY to capture adolescent age-specific characteris- measures participation patterns of children and adoles-
tics or the overall picture of participation of adolescents cents aged 5 to 17 years in home, school and community
with ASD across settings. settings, and the extent to which they perceive
80 British Journal of Occupational Therapy 83(2)

environmental features as supporting or impeding the Data analyses were conducted using SPSS version
children’s participation. 21.0. Descriptive statistics included frequencies, means
The PEM-CY participation section includes 25 ques- and standard deviations for demographics and for
tions (10 for home, 10 for community and 5 for school), item- and summary-level PEM-CY scores. One-way
and the environment section includes 45 questions (12 ANCOVA with Bonferroni correction (p < .001) was
for home, 17 for school and 16 for community). For conducted to examine the PEM-CY summary scores dif-
each participation item, the parent was asked to rate ferences between groups, controlling for gender.
the frequency with which the adolescent participated in Independent-sample t-tests with Bonferroni correction
the activities (never ¼ 0; daily ¼ 7), how involved the (p < .01) and chi-squared tests were conducted to exam-
adolescent is when doing these activities in terms of ine the PEM-CY item differences between groups.
the level of attention, concentration, emotional engage- Levene’s test for equality of variances was conducted,
ment or satisfaction (minimally involved ¼ 1; very and degrees of freedom were adjusted for unequal var-
involved ¼ 5), and whether the parent desired some iances. In addition, the weighted effect size was calculat-
type of change (yes/no, and the type of change desired: ed by Cramer’s phi (u) for chi-squared tests. An u of
frequency, involvement or variety). However, for the .1 was considered a small effect, .3 a medium effect and
purposes of this study, we examined only participation .5 or higher a large effect. The Hedges’ g effect size was
frequency and extent of involvement. calculated for t-test comparisons of groups with differ-
Within each environment (home, school and commu- ent sample sizes (Ellis, 2010; Hedges and Olkin, 1985).
nity), the parents reported whether various environmen- A Hedges’ g of .2 was considered a small effect, .5 a
tal features affected their children’s participation. Some medium effect and .8 or higher a large effect.
items asked about the extent of the feature’s helpfulness
(usually makes harder ¼ 1, sometimes helps, sometimes
Results
makes harder ¼ 2 and usually helps or not an issue ¼ 3).
Others asked about the availability of environmental Participant patterns
resources (usually no ¼ 1, sometimes yes, sometimes
no ¼ 2 and usually yes or not needed ¼ 3). The environ- No significant differences were found between groups
mental summary score for each setting was the sum of all for age, socioeconomic status or residence type.
item ratings based on PEM-CY scoring guidelines However, a significant difference was found in gender
(Coster et al., 2011). composition (v2(1) ¼ 30.28, p < .001, g ¼ .35), as
The PEM-CY’s internal consistency is moderate to expected, given that the percentage of girls in the
good for participation frequency (a ¼ 0.59–0.70), partic- group of TD adolescents was significantly higher than
ipation involvement (a ¼ 0.72–0.83) and environmental in the ASD group (58% vs. 16.9%). The TD group also
supportiveness (a ¼ 0.67–0.91). Test–retest reliability has included a higher percentage of participants who
been reported as moderate to good for all participation reported practising religion (v2(1) ¼ 160.86, p < .001,
and environment summary scores with interclass corre- g ¼ .74) (Table 1).
lations (ICC) from 0.58 to 0.95, and across items within
the home, school and community sections of the instru- Differences between adolescents with ASD and
ment (ICC ¼ 0.68–0.96) (Khetani et al., 2014). For the TD adolescents
purposes of this study, the PEM-CY was cross-culturally
Descriptive statistics for the PEM-CY summary scores
translated into Hebrew using forward and backward
highlight differences between the adolescents with ASD
translation and reviewed by an expert committee. No
and their TD peers in each setting (home, school and
changes were made to the PEM-CY items’ content or
community), including participation frequency, involve-
form except for a few cultural adjustments of the sam-
ment levels and environmental impacts. As the PEM-CY
ples approved by the authors. All participants completed
questionnaire authors recommended, we used radar
the Hebrew version. Cronbach’s alpha results exhibited
charts to present the percentage of parents who defined
an alpha coefficient of .75 in the total frequency score,
items as inhibiting participation in each setting (Coster
.89 in the total involvement score and .87 in the total
et al., 2011). Following examination of differences in
environment score of the PEM-CY. Those results indi-
ratios between boys and girls in both groups, we exam-
cate the high internal consistency and reliability of the
ined gender differences in participation patterns within
PEM-CY Hebrew version.
each group. There was no difference between boys and
girls in participation frequency or involvement level in
Data collection and analysis either group. Therefore, the data analysis continued to
The ethics committee of the academic institution and the relate to the two groups regardless of the gender
Chief Science Office, Israeli Ministry of Education, composition.
approved this study. All participants who met the inclu-
sion criteria and their parents signed informed consent Home environment: participation frequency. No significant
forms and completed the demographic questionnaire difference was found in participation-frequency summa-
and PEM-CY. ry scores between adolescents with ASD and TD
Lamash et al. 81

Table 1. Demographic characteristics of groups. Home environment: environmental impacts. Significant


Adolescents with ASD TD adolescents
differences were found between the groups in the home
(N ¼ 59) (N ¼ 188) environmental summary score after controlling for
gender (F(1,244) ¼ 19.06, p < .001, g2p ¼ .07). Parents of
Variable n % n % adolescents with ASD perceived the total home environ-
Gender mental patterns as less supportive of participation than
Male 49 83.1 79 42.0 did parents of the TD adolescents. Figure 1 presents
Female 10 16.9 109 58.0 group differences by percentage of parents who identi-
Grade fied home environment features as inhibiting home
Middle school 41 74.6 111 59.0
High school 18 25.4 77 41.0
participation.
Religiosity
Secular 57 96.6 20 10.7 School environment: participation frequency. No signifi-
Religious 2 3.4 168 89.3 cant difference was found between the group with
Socioeconomic status ASD and the TD group in school participation-
High 27 45.8 79 42.0
Average 24 40.7 94 46.4
frequency summary scores. However, compared to the
Low 6 10.1 15 8.0 TD group, the adolescents with ASD participated signif-
Missing data 2 3.4 - - icantly less often in getting together with peers outside of
Type of residence class (t(230,53) ¼ 2.79, p < .01, 95% CI [0.24, 0.91],
Urban area 47 79.6 158 84.0 g ¼ .57) and school-sponsored teams, clubs and organiza-
Suburban/rural 12 20.4 30 16.0
Father education
tions (t(230,72) ¼ 1.98, p < .05, 95% CI [0.00,
High school or less 11 18.6 70 37.2 0.66], g ¼ .33).
Professional training 9 15.2 36 19.1
Academic education 34 57.6 82 43.6 School environment: involvement level. A significant dif-
Missing data 4 8.6 - - ference was found between groups in the involvement
Mother education
High school or less 7 11.9 17 9.0
summary score after controlling for gender (F(1,229) ¼
Professional training 11 18.6 60 31.9 6.36, p < .01, g2p ¼ .03). Item-level analysis indicated ado-
Academic education 39 66.1 111 59.0 lescents with ASD were significantly less involved in get-
Missing data 2 3.4 - - ting together with peers outside of class (t(230,62) ¼
ASD: autism spectrum disorder; TD: typically developing. 4.36, p < .001, 95% CI [0.43, 1.10], g ¼ .76), activities
of field trips and school events (t(230,74) ¼ 2.56, p < .05,
95% CI [0.12, 0.79], g ¼ .45) and school-sponsored teams,
adolescents. However, significant differences were found clubs and organizations (t(230,76) ¼ 2.87, p < .01, 95%
between groups in five participation-frequency items in CI [0.10, 0.76], g ¼ .43) than were their TD peers.
the home environment. Compared to the TD adoles-
cents, those with ASD participated significantly more School environment: environmental impacts. Significant
often in playing computer and video games (t(244,211) ¼ differences were found between groups in school envi-
–11.68, p < .001, 95% CI [0.92, 1.54], g ¼ 1.23) and watch- ronmental summary scores after controlling for gender
ing TV, videos and DVDs (t(245,203) ¼ –10.68, p < .001, (F(1,229) ¼ 6.33, p < .01, g2p ¼ .03). School environment
95% CI [0.82, 1.44], g ¼ 1.12), and significantly less often was perceived as less supportive for adolescents with
in taking part in household chores (t(245,87) ¼ 2.96, p < .01, ASD participation than for TD adolescents. Figure 2
95% CI [0.18, 0.78], g ¼ .48), doing homework (t(245,76) ¼ presents the group differences by percentage of school
4.01, p < .001, 95% CI [0.42, 1.02], g ¼ .72) and getting environmental features that were identified as inhibiting
together with other people (t(245,103) ¼ 2.95, p < .01, 95% school participation.
CI [0.14, 0.74], g ¼ .44).
Community environment: participation frequency. A signif-
icant difference was found between groups in the com-
Home environment: involvement level. No significant dif-
munity participation-frequency summary score after
ference was found in the extent of involvement summary
controlling for gender (F(1,244) ¼ 54.00, p < .001,
scores, but significant differences were found in four
g2p ¼ .18). The adolescents with ASD participated signif-
home participation-involvement items. The adolescents icantly less often in eight community activities. Medium
with ASD were significantly more involved in playing and high effect sizes were found in religious or spiritual
computer and video games (t(245,179) ¼ –7.82, p < .001, gatherings (t(245,91) ¼ 10.29, p < .001, 95% CI [0.93,
95% CI [0.58, 1.18], g ¼ .88) and watching TV, videos and 1.55], g ¼ 1.24), working for pay (t(245,91) ¼ 13.35,
DVDs (t(244,183) ¼ –6.02, p < .001, 95% CI [0.38, 0.97], p < .001, 95% CI [0.90, 1.52], g ¼ 1.21), getting together
g ¼ .67), and significantly less in getting together with with other children (t(245,91) ¼ 7.2, p < .001, 95% CI
other people (t(245,104) ¼ –3.67, p < .001, 95% CI [0.76, 1.38], g ¼ 1.07), neighbourhood outings (t
[0.23, 0.83], g ¼ .53) and doing homework (t(245,88) ¼ (245,91) ¼ 4.63, p < .001, 95% CI [0.39, 0.99], g ¼ .69)
2.12, p < .05, 95% CI [0.04, 0.62], g ¼ .33) than their and overnight visits or trips (t(245,91) ¼ 3.53, p < .001,
TD peers. 95% CI [0.23, 0.82], g ¼ .53).
82 British Journal of Occupational Therapy 83(2)

Figure 1. Group differences in home environment items.


ASD: autism spectrum disorder; TD: typically developing.

Figure 2. Group differences in school environment items.


ASD: autism spectrum disorder; TD: typically developing.
Lamash et al. 83

Community environment: involvement level. A significant Discussion and implications


difference was found in the community involvement
summary score between adolescents with and without Participation patterns
ASD after controlling for gender (F(1,244) ¼ 38.42, This study’s findings identify participation patterns of
p < .001, g2p ¼ .14). Significant differences were noted in adolescents with ASD across home, school and com-
the same items the adolescents with ASD performed less munity settings compared to their TD peers. Overall,
frequently. Of these, items with the highest effect sizes adolescents with ASD had total participation levels
were working for pay (t(245,155) ¼ 12.54, p < .001, 95% similar to their TD peers at home (with differences in
CI [0.97, 1.59], g ¼ 1.28), religious or spiritual gatherings the type of activities performed) but relatively lower
(t(245,91) ¼ 7.58, p < .001, 95% CI [0.82, 1.44], g ¼ 1.12), levels in some school activities and much lower levels
getting together with other children (t(245,91) ¼ 6.76, in the community in terms of frequency and
p < .001, 95% CI [0.70, 1.31], g ¼ 1.00), overnight visits involvement.
or trips (t(245,90) ¼ 5.20, p < .001, 95% CI [0.51, 1.11], Major differences between the groups in home par-
g ¼ .81) and neighbourhood outings (t(245,89) ¼ 3.47, ticipation were in activity preferences. Adolescents with
p < .010, 95% CI [0.22, 0.81], g ¼ .51). ASD reported spending more time using technology (TV
and videos), whereas TD adolescents spent more time
Community environment: environmental impacts. involved in social and academic activities. The literature
Significant differences were found between groups in has consistently reported the tendency of individuals
community environmental summary scores after con- with ASD to engage in discretionary solitary activities
trolling for gender (F(1,241) ¼ 36.36, p < .001, g2p ¼ .13). involving technology (Potvin et al., 2013; Shattuck et al.,
Parents of the adolescents with ASD perceived the com- 2011). In the home environment, adolescents with ASD
munity environment as less supportive of participation participated less frequently and with lower involvement
than did parents of TD adolescents. Figure 3 presents levels in getting together with people, household chores
differences between the groups’ percentage of parents and school preparation, similar to other studies that
who reported community environmental features as reported PEM-CY scores for children with ASD in
inhibiting their children’s community participation. those activities (see for example Egilson et al., 2017).

Figure 3. Group differences in community environment items.


ASD: autism spectrum disorder; TD: typically developing.
84 British Journal of Occupational Therapy 83(2)

They suggested that, unlike TD children, meeting friends Environment


requires great effort from family members of children
Overall, parents of adolescents with ASD rated environ-
with ASD to collaborate and prepare the child in
mental factors as barriers more often than did parents of
advance, causing the family to be more selective of
TD adolescents, similar to findings in studies with pop-
social activities, which may lead to lower participation
ulations with other disabilities (see for example Anaby
frequency. In addition, according to Hampshire et al.
et al., 2014; Bedell et al., 2013; Law et al., 2013).
(2016), homework challenges are common for students
Cognitive demands (such as concentration, attention,
with autism spectrum disorders who demonstrate diffi-
problem-solving), social demands (such as communica-
culty developing effective study habits and frequently
tion, interacting with others), sensory demands (such as
need adult involvement to handle academic skills. This
noise, crowds, lighting) and physical activity demands
reinforces the findings of the low frequency of in-home
(such as strength, endurance, coordination) were the
academic activity among the adolescents with ASD in
most-reported environmental barriers for adolescents
the current study. At school, the participation frequency
with ASD, similar to Bedell et al.’s (2013) study, which
of adolescents with ASD did not significantly differ from
examined community participation of school-age chil-
their TD peers in classroom activities, despite the lower
dren with a wide range of disabling conditions.
involvement levels. However, it was notable that in
We expected social demands and peer relationships
school-wide activities outside their usual classrooms across settings to be participation barriers for adoles-
(such as field trips or school events), adolescents with cents with ASD, given that the extant literature identi-
ASD participated significantly less often and were less fied social and communication deficits as core
involved. Outside the familiar ASD-targeted classes impairments in ASD (see for example Shattuck et al.,
adapted to their needs, less familiar and less predictable 2011; Taheri et al., 2016). We also expected sensory fea-
activities were likely more challenging to navigate and tures to be reported as major participation barriers
thus harder to adapt to individual needs and preferences because different sensory experiences are common in
(Bogdashina, 2016). Another explanation may relate to ASD (APA, 2013). For example, higher overall rates
the sensory diversity or unusual interest in environmental of atypical sensory experience were consistently reported
sensory aspects frequently noted in this population (APA, in children with autism compared to TD children
2013) and presented in the literature as a significant factor (Ben-Sasson et al., 2009; McCormick et al., 2016).
influencing their capacity to participate in daily activities Moreover, the APA’s (2013) most recent diagnostic revi-
(Ben-Sasson et al., 2009; Little et al., 2014; O’Donnell sion includes sensory processing challenges as a key fea-
et al., 2012). ture of ASD and a significant barrier to social
These findings highlight that adolescents with ASD interactions and relationships, given the range and
had higher participation and involvement when activities unpredictability of sensory experiences in these activi-
involved them with peers with ASD (for example class- ties. However, despite the extensive literature on cogni-
room activities) than when activities involved them with tive and metacognitive impairments among individuals
TD adolescents (for example getting together with peers with ASD characterized by deficient planning, flexibility
outside the classroom), perhaps related to their social and behaviour regulation, such as Panerai et al.’s (2016)
preferences. These results support studies that indicated research, surprisingly few studies indicated similar find-
children with ASD usually prefer spending time with ings to ours, linking cognitive factors to participation in
other children with ASD or with other disabilities over home, school or community life (Hill, 2004; Lamash
spending time with TD children (see for example Locke et al., 2017; Pugliese et al., 2015). In our study, cognitive
et al., 2010). activity demands were rated as major participation bar-
In the community environment, the groups differed riers for adolescents with ASD across all three settings,
significantly in all activities except community events and adolescents with ASD, all reportedly of normal
(less prevalent for both groups), classes and lessons. intelligence, had significantly greater problems with cog-
Other studies have reported similar low levels of commu- nitive activity demands compared to TD peers.
nity and social participation of adolescents with ASD Furthermore, few preliminary studies focused on
compared to adolescents with other disabilities, such as improving executive functions to improve daily activities
learning disabilities or speech and language impairments, performance and increase participation in individuals
and to TD adolescents (see for example Levine et al., with ASD (see for example Goodman et al., 2017;
2007; Shattuck et al., 2011). The National Longitudinal Lamash et al., 2017). Our results underscore the need
Transition Study 2, an in-depth look at life in non-school for further research into the effects of cognitive and
hours for youth (13–17 years) with disabilities (for exam- metacognitive functions to inform strategies and inter-
ple deafness, blindness, mental retardation and traumatic ventions to promote participation.
brain injury) based on parent reports, also presented sim- In this study, most parents did not describe inade-
ilar findings. For example, youth with ASD contacted quate or lack of financial resources as a participation
friends, spent time outdoors, played sports or worked barrier. Of the few who did, most were parents of ado-
for pay less frequently than did youth with other disabil- lescents with ASD. Myers et al. (2015) found adequate
ities (Wagner et al., 2003). resources (such as services, transportation and supplies)
Lamash et al. 85

to be significantly associated with the social participa- What the study has added
tion of individuals with ASD, and family household
This study describes the participation patterns of
income significantly associated with community partici-
adolescents with ASD and the environmental factors
pation. Raising a child with ASD usually requires addi-
that hinder their participation. Social, sensory and
tional costs for clinical and intervention services,
cognitive factors were rated as the main barriers.
specialized services, treatments and lost family wages
(Grasu, 2015). Although a majority of the parents in
this sample did not describe inadequate financial resour-
Acknowledgements
ces as participation barriers, these factors must be con-
We thank Hadas Shahaf for being part of this research group
sidered in intervention planning and policy-making for during her M.Sc. studies in occupational therapy, University of
adolescents with ASD and their families. Haifa. We also thank the graduates of the Occupational Therapy
Mivchar Program of the University of Haifa who helped with
Limitations data collection.

This study focused on adolescents with ASD who attend


Research ethics
ASD-designated classes in mainstream schools. Further
This study was approved by the Faculty of Social Welfare and
research should assess participation patterns of adoles-
Health Sciences, University of Haifa ethics committee (approval
cents with ASD who attend mainstream classes. Also, numbers 104/14, 308/15) and the Israeli Chief Science Office in
the PEM-CY was developed and designed as a parental the Ministry of Education (2014).
report questionnaire; therefore, the findings of the cur-
rent study are based on parent-only perspectives. Future Consent
research that includes adolescent perspectives and solic- All potential participants who met the inclusion criteria and their
its feedback from other stakeholders would broaden parents signed consent forms and were informed of the study’s risks
understanding of the adolescents’ needs and preferences and benefits, that their participation was voluntary and that their
identity would not be disclosed. All data were handled anonymous-
and inform services, interventions and policies to pro-
ly by serial numbers and stored in a secured closet accessible to only
mote their participation. Finally, collecting data via con- the principal investigator.
venience sampling led to differences in
sociodemographic variables. Results cannot be general- Declaration of conflicting interests
ized beyond this sample, and we recommend further
The authors declared no potential conflicts of interest with respect
research with a larger, more representative sample and to the research, authorship and/or publication of this article.
matched control group.
Funding
Conclusion and implications The authors received no financial support for the research, author-
ship and/or publication of this article.
Most of our results are consistent with previous studies
and indicate that adolescents with ASD have significant-
Contributorship
ly lower participation (involvement and frequency) than
Liron Lamash researched literature, applied for ethical approval,
do their TD peers. However, this study uniquely high-
collected the data and carried out the statistical analysis, and wrote
lights the importance of specific activity demands on the manuscript.
participation. The most frequently cited participation All authors contributed to the methodology of the project,
barriers – sensory, cognitive and social-activity demands interpreted the data, and reviewed and approved the final version.
– suggest interventions could effectively combine cogni-
tive and social problem-solving methods to enhance ORCID iD
school and community participation. Finally, these find- Liron Lamash https://orcid.org/0000-0001-8241-348X
ings extend knowledge important to allocating services
and creating policies to meet the unique needs of ado- References
lescents with ASD. American Psychiatric Association [APA] (2013) Diagnostic and
Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, 5th ed. Arlington,
VA: American Psychiatric Publishing.
Key findings
Anaby D, Law M, Coster W, et al. (2014) The mediating role of
• Adolescents with autism spectrum disorder (ASD) the environment in explaining participation of children and
participate in more solitary activities that involve youth with and without disabilities across home, school,
technology, whereas typically developing adolescents and community. Archives of Physical Medicine and
participate in more social and academic activities. Rehabilitation 95(5): 908–917.
• Parents of adolescents with ASD rated cognitive, Askari S, Anaby D, Bergthorson M, et al. (2015) Participation
social, sensory and physical activity demands as the of children and youth with autism spectrum disorder: A
most-reported environmental barriers for adolescents scoping review. Review Journal of Autism and
with ASD. Developmental Disorders 2(1): 103–114.
86 British Journal of Occupational Therapy 83(2)

Bedell G, Coster W, Law M, et al. (2013) Community partic- International Conference on Virtual Rehabilitation (ICVR),
ipation, supports, and barriers of school-age children with IEEE, pp.1–7.
and without disabilities. Archives of Physical Medicine and Law M, Anaby D, Teplicky R, et al. (2013) Participation in the
Rehabilitation 94(2): 315–323. home environment among children and youth with and
Bedell G (2012) Measurement of social participation. In: without disabilities. British Journal of Occupational
Anderson V and Beauchamp MH (eds.) Developmental Therapy 76(2): 58–66.
social neuroscience and childhood brain insult: Theory and Levine P, Marder C and Wagner M (2007) Services and sup-
Practice. New York: The Guildford Press, pp.184–206. ports for secondary school students with disabilities: A spe-
Ben-Sasson A, Hen L, Fluss R, et al. (2009) A meta-analysis of cial topic report of findings from the national longitudinal
sensory modulation symptoms in individuals with autism transition study-2 (NLTS2). Report for the National Center
spectrum disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental for Special Education Research, ERIC (ED496552).
Disorders 39(1): 1–11. Liptak GS, Kennedy JA and Dosa NP (2011) Social participa-
Bishop L, Veenstra-Vander Weele J and Sanders J (2016) tion in a nationally representative sample of older youth and
Attention finally being paid to girls at risk of autism. young adults with autism. Journal of Developmental &
Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Behavioral Pediatrics 32(4): 277–283.
Psychiatry 55(3): 159–160. Little LM, Sideris J, Ausderau K, et al. (2014) Activity partic-
Bogdashina O (2016) Sensory Perceptual Issues in Autism and ipation among children with autism spectrum disorder.
Asperger Syndrome: Different Sensory Experiences – Different American Journal of Occupational Therapy 68(2): 177–185.
Perceptual Worlds, 2nd ed. London: Jessica Kingsley. Locke J, Ishijima EH, Kasari C, et al. (2010) Loneliness, friend-
Coster W, Law M, Bedell G, et al. (2011) Participation and ship quality and the social networks of adolescents with
environment measure for children and youth (PEM-CY). high-functioning autism in an inclusive school setting.
Available at: www.canchild.ca/system/tenon/assets/attach Journal of Research in Special Educational Needs
ments/000/000/677/original/Khetani2011Colorado_PDF. 10(2): 74–81.
pdf (accessed 12 March 2012). McCormick C, Hepburn S, Young GS, et al. (2016) Sensory
Coster W, Law M, Bedell G, et al. (2013) School participation, symptoms in children with autism spectrum disorder, other
supports and barriers of students with and without disabil- developmental disorders and typical development: A longi-
ities. Child: Care, Health and Development 39(4): 535–543. tudinal study. Autism 20(5): 572–579.
Egilson ST, Jakobsd ottir G, Ólafsson K, et al. (2017) Myers E, Davis BE, Stobbe G, et al. (2015) Community and
Community participation and environment of children social participation among individuals with autism spec-
with and without autism spectrum disorder: Parent perspec- trum disorder transitioning to adulthood. Journal of
tives. Scandinavian Journal of Occupational Therapy Autism and Developmental Disorders 45(8): 2373–2381.
24(3): 187–196. O’Donnell S, Deitz J, Kartin D, et al. (2012) Sensory process-
Ellis PD (2010) The Essential Guide to Effect Sizes: Statistical ing, problem behaviour, adaptive behaviour, and cognition
Power, Meta-analysis, and the Interpretation of Research in preschool children with autism spectrum disorders.
Results. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press. American Journal of Occupational Therapy 66(5): 586–594.
Goodman LR, Corkum P and Johnson SA (2017) A metacog- Panerai S, Tasca D, Ferri R, et al. (2016) Metacognitive and
nitive training pilot study for adolescents with autism spec- emotional/motivational executive functions in individuals
trum disorder: Lessons learned from the preliminary stages with autism spectrum disorder and attention deficit hyper-
of intervention development. Journal of Intellectual & activity disorder: Preliminary results. Rivista Di Psichiatria
Developmental Disability 42(2): 204–210. 51(3): 104–109.
Grasu M (2015) Challenges in families with a child with autism Potvin M, Snider L, Prelock P, et al. (2013) Recreational par-
spectrum disorder. Bulletin of the Transilvania University of ticipation of children with high functioning autism. Journal
Brasov 7(1): 87–94. of Autism and Developmental Disorders 43(2): 445–457.
Hampshire PK, Butera GD and Bellini S (2016) Self-manage- Pugliese CE, Anthony L, Strang JF, et al. (2015) Increasing
ment and parents as interventionists to improve homework adaptive behaviour skill deficits from childhood to adoles-
independence in students with autism spectrum disorders. cence in autism spectrum disorder: Role of executive func-
Preventing School Failure: Alternative Education for tion. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders
Children and Youth 60(1): 22–34. 45(6): 1579–1587.
Hedges LV and Olkin I (1985) Statistical Methods for Meta- Seltzer MM, Krauss MW, Shattuck PT, et al. (2003) The symp-
analysis. Orlando, FL: Academic Press. toms of autism spectrum disorders in adolescence and adult-
Hill EL (2004) Evaluating the theory of executive dysfunction hood. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders
in autism. Developmental Review 24(2): 189–233. 33(6): 565–581.
Khetani M, Marley J, Baker M, et al. (2014) Validity of the Shattuck PT, Orsmond GI, Wagner M, et al. (2011)
participation and environment measure for children and Participation in social activities among adolescents with
youth (PEM-CY) for health impact assessment (HIA) in an autism spectrum disorder. PloS One 6(11): e27176.
sustainable development projects. Disability and Health Simpson K, Keen D, Adams D, et al. (2018) Participation of
Journal 7(2): 226–235. children on the autism spectrum in home, school, and com-
Lamash L, Klinger E and Josman N (2017) Using a virtual munity. Child: Care, Health and Development 44(1): 99–107.
supermarket to promote independent functioning among Solish A, Perry A and Minnes P (2010) Participation of chil-
adolescents with autism spectrum disorder. 2017 dren with and without disabilities in social, recreational and
Lamash et al. 87

leisure activities. Journal of Applied Research in Intellectual Wagner M, Cadwallader T, Marder C, et al. (2003) Life outside
Disabilities 23(3): 226–236. the classroom for youth with disabilities. A report from the
Taheri A, Perry A and Minnes P (2016) Examining the social National Longitudinal Transition Study-2 (NLTS2). Menlo
participation of children and adolescents with intellectual Park, CA: SRI International.
disabilities and autism spectrum disorder in relation World Health Organization [WHO] (2007) International
to peers. Journal of Intellectual Disability Research Classification of Functioning, Disability and Health:
60(5): 435–443. Children and Youth Version. Geneva: Author.

You might also like