You are on page 1of 7

Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology Copyright 1988 by the American Psychological Association, Inc.

1988, Vol. 56, No. 5, 696-702 0022-006X/88/$00.75

Training in Experiential Therapy

Leslie S. Greenberg and Rhonda L. Goldman


\brk University
Toronto, Ontario, Canada

Literature on training in the core facilitative conditions is reviewed to highlight issues that have
emerged from this research tradition. A reconceptualization of the way in which empathic respond-
ing is conceputalized is called for, and suggestions are made about features that need to be addressed
in empathy training. Four methods of training therapists (didactic, skill training, experiential, and
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

personal growth) are examined, and a combination of these methods is proposed as the best means
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

of training experiential therapists. Current developments in manualization and adherence measure-


ment in experiential approaches are reviewed. The advantages of context-sensitive, process diagnos-
tic manuals are discussed. These manuals specify not only what the therapist does but also when and
in what sequence interventions are made.

Experiential therapy refers to a broad class of humanistic and deterioration in patient functioning (Truax & Carkhuff, 1967).
phenomenological therapies that emerged in the forties and fif- This claim spawned a great body of research on the training
ties, largely as an alternative to behavioral and psychoanalytic of therapists in the skills, attitudes, and behaviors necessary to
perspectives. Experiential therapy in this tradition is best repre- create highly facilitative conditions. Such research began with
sented by the work of Rogers (1957) and Perls (Perls, Heiferline, Rogers's (1957) graded training experiences and was continued
& Goodman, 1951), the founders of client-centered therapy and by Truax and Carkhuffs (1967) didactic-experiential training.
Gestalt therapy, respectively. Systematic training programs in Finally, Ivey (1971) designed his microcounseling training pro-
these approaches did not develop until the sixties, when re- gram.
search was conducted largely on training in the client-centered In Rogers's (1957) graded experiences, the students listened
tradition. to tape-recorded interviews, experienced live demonstrations
Experiential therapy focuses on increasing the client's aware- by a supervisor, partook in group and personal therapy, con-
ness of his or her current feelings, perceptions, and physical ducted individual psychotherapy, and recorded their own inter-
state and emphasizes the formation of an accepting, I—Thou views for discussion with a facilitative supervisor. Rogers
relationship between the client and the therapist. The therapist stressed attitudinal and relationship aspects over training in be-
works actively with each client, often using special techniques havior and was the first to emphasize that the most effective
to enhance awareness and to promote the experience and ex- learning occurs experientially in the same type of facilitative
pression of emotionally laden material. The therapist views cli- environment as the patient-therapist relationship. In addition,
ents as possessing the potential to grow and as being experts on Rogers implemented the method of recording interviews for the
their own experience and does not interpret the clients' reasons purpose of facilitative supervision. Truax and Carkhuff (1967),
for their experience or advise them on how to solve problems. in their didactic-experiential training program, argued that
Rather, the therapist is reflective or experimental in style, guid- most psychotherapy training programs had stressed theory and
ing the client's attentional focus and making suggestions to patient psychodynamics instead of how to create a facilitative
stimulate new experience. In a reflective mode, the therapist relationship and emphasized that the skills of relationship
symbolizes aspects of the client's experience and feeds this back building were of primary importance in training good thera-
to the client. In an experimental mode, the therapist asks clients pists. Thus, they developed a program that trained students in
to participate in an in-therapy activity in order to discover interpersonal skills. They implemented a training procedure
something new about how they function. that began with the trainee's own experience as a learning base.
The three central elements of their training program (Truax &
Review of Training in the Facilitative Conditions Carkhuff, 1967; Carkhuff, 1969a, 1969b) included a therapeu-
tic context in which the supervisor provided high levels of thera-
Initially, research from the client-centered group suggested
peutic conditions, didactic training in the implementation of
that high levels of accurate empathy, nonpossessive warmth,
therapeutic conditions, and a quasi-group therapy experience
and genuineness were associated with constructive patient
in which trainees engaged in personal exploration to foster the
change and that an absence of these conditions could lead to a
emergence of their therapeutic selves. Didactic training began
with students listening to and rating psychotherapy tapes on
scales that assessed accurate empathy, nonpossessive warmth,
Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Leslie and genuineness. Students were thus initially trained in percep-
S. Greenberg, Department of Psychology, York University, 4700 Keele tual discrimination of the conditions.
Street, North York, Ontario, Canada M3J 1P3. Studies were conducted to establish the effectiveness of the

696
SPECIAL SERIES: EXPERIENTIAL THERAPY TRAINING 697

didactic-experiential training program in teaching counselors derlining the "state" nature of empathy. His interactive model
the facilitative conditions. For example, Truax and Carkhuff conceptualized empathy as a process in which the client re-
(1967) concluded that, during 100 hr of training that were spe- counts the experience and the therapist first attends and receives
cifically directed toward teaching the facilitative conditions, the experience, then resonates to it, and finally expresses this
they could bring the performance of students and lay personnel awareness to the client. Next, the client receives this awareness
to a level similar to that of an experienced therapist. and evaluates the therapist's understanding. This view of empa-
Ivey (1971) developed a training program that attempted to thy as an interactive state that is created within the therapeutic
teach specific counseling skills in a brief period of time. This context is far more valid than a trait conception and suggests
brief microcounseling training program focused initially on that empathy can be learned.
teaching three basic skills: (a) attentiveness and related con-
cepts, (b) accurate reflection, and (c) summarization of feeling.
Reconceptualization of Empathy
From 1 to 2 hr were allotted to the learning of each skill. In a
variety of studies (Ivey, 197 l;Moreland, Ivey, & Phillips, 1973), Barrett-Lennard's (1981) conception of empathic communi-
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

Ivey and colleagues demonstrated that skills could be taught cation as a process in which the therapist resonates to the cli-
effectively through teaching, modeling, and practice. ent's experience takes an important step toward a more accu-
rate definition of empathy. Empathy involves a form of affective
Empathy as a Unitary Variable? resonance in which the therapist is able to sense what it is like
to be the client in the situation and communicates this to the
After a fruitful beginning, research in the training programs client.
faded out during the seventies, partly because the concept of Once the therapist is in the client's frame of reference, how-
empathy was neither clearly defined nor clearly measured. The ever, it is important for her or him to differentiate what is most
theoretical assumption underlying the training programs was alive for the client so that the core emotional experience can be
that empathy was a unitary and therefore measurable con- symbolized and attended to. Greenberg and Safran (1987), in
struct. To the contrary, studies were published showing that their work on emotion in psychotherapy, suggested that the ex-
different ratings of "empathic conditions" did not correlate perience of certain emotions provides the organism with biolog-
(Kurtz & Grummon, 1972) and that empathy was related to ically adaptive information that enhances orientation and aids
a number of other variables (Matarazzo & Weins, 1977). For problem solving. Thus, when entering the client's frame of ref-
example, Shapiro (1968) found that many of the empathy cues erence, the therapist must attempt to respond not just to any
were visual, although many of the related studies used audio- expression of emotion but to those emotions that provide bio-
tapes as data. In addition, he found that empathy was correlated logically adaptive information. Thus, the therapist is selectively
with demonstrations of warmth and genuineness and with a reflecting particular feelings at particular times. Empathy thus
global good/bad therapist evaluation. Empathy ratings have involves selectively responding both to what is most alive in the
also been related to the proportion of therapist responses relat- client's experience and to primary adaptive emotional re-
ing to emotion (Mintz & Luborsky, 1971; Wenegrat, 1974). In sponses.
addition, Rennie, Burke, and Toukmanian (1978) found that In addition, effective empathy requires picking up the ongo-
empathy raters focused more on the linguistic style of the coun- ing, growing edge of the client's experience, the aspect that is
selor's response than on the relation between the response and developmentally moving the client forward to a greater sense of
the client utterance preceding it. competence and mastery. The consistent recognition and vali-
One of the strongest debates in the training arena has focused dation of the growth capacity encourages the client to move to
on whether or not empathic conditions are a function of the higher levels of self-organization and to move toward therapeu-
particular character traits of the therapist. The notion of a tic change. For these reasons, it is appropriate to reconceptual-
"high-functioning" therapist was initially proposed by Piaget, ize empathic communication as a highly selective as well as re-
Berenson, and Carkhuff (1967) to describe a person who pos- flective process. The principles of empathic selection need to
sesses the necessary qualities to display warmth, genuineness, be clearly defined, and therapists need to be trained in these
and positive regard. principles.
The following decade, however, has produced a wealth of re- A third and related requirement for developing a more
search refuting the trait conception of empathy and showing differentiated view of empathic responding is a clear definition
that empathy is dependent on the specific counseling context of when this type of responding is most appropriate and of what
rather than on the characteristics of the therapist (Beutler, John- effects it has at these times (Lambert, DeJulio, & Stein, 1978).
son, Neville, & Workman, 1973; Chinsky & Rappaport, 1970; This follows the more context-sensitive view of process research
Gladstein, 1977; Mitchell, Bozarth, & Krauft, 1977; Rappa- (Rice & Greenberg, 1984; Greenberg, 1986a, 1986b). In this
port & Chinsky, 1972). In a review of the literature, Lambert, view, not all therapist or client processes serve the same func-
DeJulio, and Stein (1978) pointed out that both client-per- tion at all points in therapy. Rather, the meanings and effects of
ceived empathy and tape-judged empathy were superior to trait different processes, such as empathy, depend on their particular
measures of empathy in predicting therapy outcome. The au- contexts. For example, an empathic response to a client's state-
thors stated explicitly that the evidence revealed the assessment ment of underlying vulnerability will have a different purpose
of actual in-therapy interactions as most beneficial. Barrett- and effect than an empathic response to a client's voiced reac-
Lennard (1981) proposed a more refined model of empathic tive frustration. Alternately, an empathic response in a context
communication that laid out stages of the empathic process un- of consistent empathic responding by the therapist is different
698 LESLIE S. GREENBERG AND RHONDA L. GOLDMAN

from an empathic response following a confrontation. The con- ing has primarily been taught in Gestalt institutes through the
text of an empathic response thus requires specification if we medium of personal therapeutic work, emphasizing individual
are truly to describe this type of therapist operation. awareness, personal growth, and the attendant attitude change.
What is needed, then, is a more accurate and specific defini- Greenberg (1980), however, noted the need for training in ad-
tion of therapeutic empathy that goes beyond the view of empa- vanced experiential therapy skills (Greenberg & Kahn, 1979),
thy as a global reflection of feeling. Initial attempts have been suggested that some of the skills of Gestalt therapy could be
made to accomplish this. Gendlin (1981), for example, pro- taught in a systematic fashion, and designed a combined didac-
posed the notion of experiential listening, which involves re- tic-experiential and skill training program. This program fo-
sponding to particular delineated experiential referents, and cused on training in the use of the Gestalt two-chair experi-
Klein, Mathieu-Coughlin, and Kiesler (1986) attempted to ment, a method used in Gestalt therapy to help clients resolve
measure aspects of experiential listening with their therapist ex- conflict by engaging in a dialogue between the opposing aspects
periencing scale. A more comprehensive reconceptualization, of the conflict.
however, must measure empathy as a form of interactive com- Greenberg and Sarkissian (1984) evaluated the effectiveness
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

munication in which the therapist uses a denned responsive of this program. Results indicated that, after training, counsel-
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

style while selectively focusing on particular client processes at ors responded to a client statement of conflict more often with
particular points in specific contexts. an appropriate two-chair dialogue intervention and provided
more direct guidance and more attention to nonverbal refer-
Effective Modes of Training ents. The training program thus succeeded in training students
in some of the active intervention skills of Gestalt therapy.
The issue of how best to train therapists, through experiential This training program used four basic training processes by
learning or behavioral skill training, has received a great deal combining behavioral skill training, conceptual learning, expe-
of attention in the training literature. The didactic experiential riential learning, and personal therapy into a comprehensive
program (Truax & Carkhuff, 1967) relied primarily on experi- training approach. It went beyond earlier interpersonal skills
ential learning as a basis for training. The contention was that training programs by encouraging trainees to work on personal
if trainees did not experience high levels of warmth, regard, issues in a therapeutic fashion as an aspect of training. This
understanding, and genuineness from the trainers, they would encouragement resulted from the rationale that increased
not function at a high therapeutic level themselves (Truax & awareness of personal issues would facilitate a trainee's thera-
Carkhuff, 1967). Ivey (1971), on the other hand, proposed the peutic sensitivity and firsthand knowledge of the process of
teaching of counseling skills in a brief period (1-2 hr) and sug- change. In our view, training is most effective when it includes
gested that experiential learning is not necessary. Investigators conceptual instruction, experiential learning, skill training that
evaluated these claims and compared the efficacy of the two involves modeling and practice, and personal therapeutic work.
training methods (Matarazzo, 1978; Perry, 1975; Toukmanian Further research is necessary to evaluate this claim.
&Rennie, 1975; Toukmanian, Capelle, & Rennie, 1978;Quart-
aro&Rennie, 1983). Current Developments
It appears from this research that skills such as attending and
summarization of feeling can be learned didactically through Rice and Greenberg (1984) suggested that the therapeutic
brief tnicrocounseling training. The ability to create and main- process in experiential therapy is most accurately construed as
tain an empathic relationship, however, is learned in a more a series of "when—then" events in which specific client perfor-
experiential fashion (Pagell, Carkhuff, & Berenson, 1967). This mance patterns are viewed as markers of underlying informa-
ability requires the perceptual skill to hear the subtle feelings tion processing difficulties that repeatedly present themselves
and complex meanings expressed by the client, which is a skill in therapy as opportunities for different types of interventions.
that takes time and personal development to acquire. Once one Thus, when the client presents a particular marker, such as the
hears feelings in a differentiated fashion, the skill of reflecting statement of a conflict, then the therapist intervenes in a partic-
them is much easier to acquire. Empathic reflection itself, how- ular fashion, such as by suggesting a dialogue between aspects
ever, is not just a reflexive parroting act. As mentioned, em- of the conflict. In this view, the therapist must be adept both
pathic communication is a complex interactive process that oc- at making process diagnoses of client in-session states that are
curs through many channels of communication. The ability to amenable to intervention and at implementing appropriate in-
be accurately empathic is thus acquired through experience, terventions. The perceptual skills needed for recognizing client
which requires time and personal growth. Rennie, Brewster, and markers and states thus become as important as conceptual
Toukmanian (1985), for example, found that the ability of (theoretical) or executive (intervention) skills because they de-
trainees to deeply process their own experience as clients, as termine the appropriate focus and timing of the intervention.
assessed by the Experiencing Scale (Klein et al., 1986), was cor- Greenberg and Rice (1987) thus suggested an approach to
related with the extent to which they exhibited high levels of experiential therapy in which intervention is both marker
empathy as therapists. driven (i.e., particular interventions are implemented when
particular markers emerge in therapy) and task focused (i.e.,
each intervention is designed to engage the client in a specific
Research on Training in Gestalt Therapy
therapeutic task to help resolve the problem indicated by the
Research on Gestalt training has also examined the relative marker).
merits of skill training and experiential learning. Gestalt train- Manuals that adequately train therapists and provide proper
SPECIAL SERIES: EXPERIENTIAL THERAPY TRAINING 699

adherence measures for evaluating the adequacy of treatment sponding skills in addition to other Rogerian relationship skills
implementation will need to specify not only what the therapist and must also learn skills designed to help clients resolve partic-
does but at which time a particular intervention should be used. ular information-processing problems. In addition, process di-
Without this degree of specification, manuals are just a list of agnostic skills are taught to trainees, implicitly if not explicitly,
ingredients. The sequencing and proper mixture of the ingredi- to facilitate the identification of client in-session markers of par-
ents, however, determine the true potency of any treatment. Ex- ticular underlying information-processing difficulty.
periential manuals that have been recently designed will next
be reviewed briefly to provide both a summary of current devel-
Experiential Manuals
opments and a discussion of issues in the development of pro-
cess diagnostic manuals. Mahrer (1983, 1986) has developed his own type of experien-
tial therapy. He divided this therapy into five classes of therapeu-

Manuals From the Client-Centered Tradition tic experiencing. The classes follow a sequence in which each
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

class of experiencing, while leading to change in its own right,


This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

Both Gendlin (1981) and Rice (1984) have described techni- also prepares the person for the next class. The first class of
cal therapist operations that fall within but go beyond the provi- experiencing involves focusing on current bodily sensations.
sion of the Rogerian core conditions. Rice denned a client The second class of experiencing involves the differentiation
marker, the problematic reaction, as an in-therapy state in and elaboration of the bodily felt experience occurring at the
which the client describes a stimulus situation and a reaction moment. This leads to the emergence of an underlying state that
to the situation that is experienced as problematic. Rice then was not previously in awareness and then to the owning of this
detailed the characteristics and steps of a systematic evocative experience as belonging to the self. Finally, the client is encour-
unfolding procedure to facilitate resolution of the problem aged to risk changed ways of being and behaving in the extra-
(Rice & Saperia, 1984). The client resolution process has been therapy world based on this newly experienced aspect of self.
shown to follow a specific path, and this problem-resolution Mahrer has described both the general procedures followed in
model has been used as a process diagnostic guide for therapist this therapy and a host of specific interventions.
intervention (Rice & Saperia, 1984).
Gendlin (1981) provided a step-by-step phenomenological
Can Experiential Therapy Be Discriminated?
description of a focusing procedure. Focusing involves the con-
centration of attention to create a bodily felt sense of a person's A number of experiential manuals have attempted to detail
experience. In the focusing manual, Gendlin described an im- and measure the skills necessary to conduct experiential ther-
portant experiential therapeutic process and the steps through apy. These are reviewed next.
which the therapist should guide the client to maximize the
gains from experiencing. Gendlin also indicated markers for the Experiential Therapy of Depression Manual
best times to use focusing by suggesting that focusing should be
used when the client appears to be on the surface of something An experiential therapy of depression is currently being eval-
important, feels confused, or has said all that he or she can say uated by Elliott (1988). The therapy is based on a synthesis of
about the problem and feels stuck (Gendlin, 1974). Thus, al- client-centered and Gestalt approaches that was developed by
though Gendlin did not formally design a therapist manual, he Greenberg and Rice (1987), who integrated a set of therapeutic
clearly specified what the therapist and client should do and tasks into a client-centered style of relating (Elliott, Greenberg,
when the technique of focusing is most useful. Rice, & Clark, 1987). The therapy is explicitly marker-driven
Toukmanian (1984) developed a manual for training thera- and task-focused, meaning that the therapist's interventions are
pists in a neo-Rogerian perceptual-cognitive approach that fo- guided by process markers that indicate when a client is ready to
cuses on changing the manner in which clients construe them- work on a particular task that is designed to resolve a particular
selves and the world. Markers for overly constricted information problematic experience.
processing, which impede problem solving, are specified, and This experiential therapy requires that therapists be em-
phases of intervention and specific therapist behaviors that are pathic and prizing, foster immediate experiencing, and facili-
used to modify this constricted schematic processing are de- tate completion of therapeutic tasks. The basic mode in this
scribed. In another neo-Rogerian manual, Rennie (1987) incor- therapy is reflective listening and imaginative entry into the cli-
porated the teaching of some of the basic client-centered inter- ent's world, whereas the specialized techniques are Gendlin's
personal skills such as attending and empathic responding but (1981) focusing, which is used to symbolize inner meaning and
emphasized training in both interventions that promote imme- deepen experience, and systematic evocative unfolding (Rice,
diacy of experiencing and metacommunication about the inter- 1984), which is used when clients present puzzling personal re-
action between client and therapist. Metacommunication is a actions to situations. The Gestalt technique of two-chair work
key process in Rennie's view because it allows therapists to is used when the client displays splits (presently felt conflicts)
know how they are affecting their clients. (Greenberg, 1984). Empty-chair work, defined as engaging in a
These four manuals all specify approaches that have grown dialogue with an imagined other in an empty chair, is imple-
out of a client-centered tradition. Each approach focuses on mented to facilitate completion of an incomplete experience
particular types of information processing difficulties and when the client expresses unfinished business with a significant
guides the manner of exploration to improve processing. To other (Greenberg &Safran, 1987; Perls etal., 1951).
train in these approaches, one must learn basic empathic re- Elliott has devised a number of context-sensitive measures of
700 LESLIE S. GREENBERG AND RHONDA L. GOLDMAN

therapist interventions. The therapists rate their own intentions third phase of the therapy, experiments are implemented to fa-
and their adherence to the manual at the end of each session to cilitate emotional arousal. Daldrup et al. (in press) identified
record whether they have identified markers and implemented five types of experiments. These are dialogue experiments, mo-
appropriate interventions. Therapist skillfulness in implement- toric expressions useful to release emotion, verbal experiments
ing the interventions is also assessed. In addition, therapists rate to help reorganize cognitive systems, nonverbal experiments,
whether they used nonexperiential interventions such as inter- and experiments of closure to facilitate continued movement.
pretation or advice giving. The amount of training needed to Different types of interruptions of emotional expression as well
reach adherence has not yet been specified, although it is clear as the appropriate interventions for these have been specified.
that considerable training is needed to learn the specialized The FEP training program comprises three phases: theory
techniques. development, skill training, and experiential learning. In the
first phase, a theoretical understanding is achieved and skills are
Gestalt Experiential Therapy developed through lectures, demonstrations, and group work.
The second phase involves intensive skill training through a di-
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

Greenberg and Goldman (1987) developed a manual to de- dactic base combined with experiential work in both personal
scribe how a Gestalt experiential therapist creates and main- and group settings involving supervision. The final phase in-
tains a therapeutic relationship. The manual delineates both volves intense experiential learning with patients. Supervision
perceptual and action modes. Within each of the modes, the is conducted in person or through the use of audiotapes or vid-
manual specifies actions engaged in by the therapist. In addi- eotapes.
tion, the manual describes the intentions that lie behind each of An extensive set of adherence measures has been developed
the actions. to determine the teachableness of this manual (Daldrup et al.,
An adherence measure was constructed to test whether the in press). Process diagnostic measures of particular client states
manual adequately described their brand of Gestalt experiential were developed, and the appropriateness of interventions for
therapy. Based on the manual, a 17-item instrument was de- these states was assessed. Data suggested that FEP is a teachable
signed to describe the therapist's conduct in general relation- method that can be applied in a reliable fashion. In a compari-
ship skills (e.g., attitude of acceptance) and specific interven- son of FEP trainees with trainees of two educational groups,
tions (e.g., focusing on present experience). therapists in the two treatment conditions were rated as per-
In a preliminary study, minimally trained raters agreed on forming differently, which confirmed the discriminative valid-
the degree to which a relationship skill was being displayed 77% ity of the compliance measure. Finally, compliance scores of
of the time and agreed on which technique was being imple- FEP therapists were compared with those of therapists who
mented 73% of the time. The raters were given a set of 2-min were conducting cognitive therapy. The FEP therapists consis-
tape segments taken from Gestalt therapy, cognitive therapy, tently performed significantly higher on a FEP compliance
and eclectic therapy. Ninety-two percent of the therapist actions measure at the end of training than they did either before train-
on the Gestalt experiential therapy segments were judged as ac- ing or compared with cognitive therapists.
tions represented on the specific intervention items, whereas Thus, an extensive manual describing the perceptual and
only 14% of the non-Gestalt therapist actions were judged as conceptual skills used in FEP has been completed, and an ad-
Gestalt style interventions. Thus, preliminary indications of the herence measure has verified that the FEP intervention manual
reliability and validity of this instrument were established. can be taught and implemented.
In addition to this general manual, four specific Gestalt ex-
periments have been manualized (Greenberg & Minden, 1987).
Intentions and therapist actions have been described for each Future Directions
experiment. A context-sensitive adherence measure has been
devised and is currently being tested. Clearly, research has only begun on second-generation expe-
riential therapy manuals, but it does appear that the actions of
experiential therapists are specifiable and that they are discrim-
Focused Expressive Therapy
inably different from the actions of cognitive and nonspecific
Beutler and his co-workers (Beutler, 1986) have evaluated an general practice therapists.
expressive treatment, focused expressive therapy (FEP; Dal- A number of trends are apparent from this literature review.
drup, Beutler, Engle, & Greenberg, in press) for problems of The first is the tendency to integrate different experiential meth-
constricted emotion. This treatment focuses primarily on emo- ods into a combined treatment package. The second is the use
tions that relate to anger. of process diagnosis to identify in-session problems in affective
The treatment has been manualized in detail (Daldrup et al., information processing, which is coupled with specific inter-
in press). The five basic tasks in this treatment require (a) the ventions designed to guide the client to process information in
establishment of a focus for work, (b) the elicitation of a com- the way regarded as most therapeutic at that particular mo-
mitment to work, (c) the development of the work process itself ment.
to a point at which affective intensity peaks and declines, (d) an An important contribution from the experiential manuals is
assessment of the work conducted, and (e) the development of their attempt to specify when an intervention should be used. In
plans and homework for the future. Within each of these phases, addition, the specification of therapist intentions that lie behind
the specific actions are described and markers are specified to actions, as done in both experiential manuals and other studies
indicate when interventions should be implemented. In the (Hill&O'Grady, 1985; Elliott, 1985; Stiles, 1986), is important
SPECIAL SERIES: EXPERIENTIAL THERAPY TRAINING 701

in helping to define and discriminate interventions in the Greenberg, L. S. (1986a). Change process research. Journal of Consult-
different treatments. ing and Clinical Psychology, 54, 4-9.
An important ongoing issue in the training of experiential Greenberg, L. S. (1986b). Research strategies. In L. S. Greenberg & W.
therapists is the tension between training the whole person and Pinsof (Eds.), The psychotherapeutic process: A research handbook
(pp. 707-734). New York: Guilford Press.
training in specific skills. Training that leads to attitude change
Greenberg, L. S., & Goldman, R. (1987). The relationship in gestalt
and personal growth is of great importance in experiential ther-
experiential therapy: A manual and adherence measure. Unpublished
apy because it allows trainees to know in a personal fashion how
manuscript, York University, Toronto.
the experiential change process works. Trainees need to feel Greenberg, L. S., & Kahn, S. E. (1979). The stimulation phase in coun-
comfortable with exploring their clients' inner worlds and with seling. Counselor Education and Supervision, 19, 137-145.
the possible emotional expression that might occur. The best Greenberg, L. S., & Minden, R. (1987). Gestalt therapy experiments:
training for this is the exploration of one's own inner world and A manual and adherence measure. Unpublished manuscript, York
one's own emotional experience. University, Toronto.
In addition to acquiring the appropriate therapeutic atti- Greenberg, L. S., &Rice, L. (1987). Experiential therapy: An integrative
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

approach. Unpublished manuscript, York University, Toronto.


This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

tudes, trainees can leam specific intervention skills through ex-


periential learning and personal work. However, more work is Greenbeig, L. S., & Safran, J. (1987). Emotion in psychotherapy: Affect
needed to explicate the perceptual and process-diagnostic skills and cognition in the process of change. New "Ybrk: Guilford Press.
Greenberg, L. S., & Sarkissian, M. (1984). Evaluation of counselor
that are required to be an experiential therapist. Ultimately, it
training in gestalt methods. Counselor Education and Supervision,
is the experiential therapist's perceptual discriminations and
23, 328-340.
his or her ability to hear, see, and understand the subtleties of
Hill, C. E., & O'Grady, K. E. (1985). List of therapist intentions illus-
the client's experience and to identify markers of underlying in- trated in a single case study and with therapists of varying theoretical
formation-processing difficulties that determines the quality of orientations. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 32, 3-22.
the therapy. Ivey, A. (1971). Microcounseling: Innovations in interviewing training.
Springfield, IL: Charles C Thomas.
Klein, M., Mathieu-Coughlin, P., & Kiesler, D. (1986). The experienc-
References ing scales. In L. Greenberg & W. Pinsof (Eds.), The psychotherapeutic
process: A research handbook (pp. 21 -71). New York: Guilford Press.
Barrett-Lennard, G. T. (1981). The empathy cycle: Refinement of a
Kurtz, R. R., & Grummon, D. L. (1972). Different approaches to the
nuclear concept. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 28, 91-100.
measurement of therapist empathy and their relationship to therapy
Beutler, L. (1986). An efficacy study of experiential psychotherapy
outcomes. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 39, 106-
among depressed patients. Unpublished manuscript, University of
115.
Arizona, Tuscon.
Lambert, M., DeJulio, S., & Stein, D. (1978). Therapist interpersonal
Beutler, L. E., Johnson, D. X, Neville, C. W., Jr., & Workman, S. N.
skills: Process, outcome, method. Considerations and recommenda-
(1973). Some sources of variance in "accurate empathy" ratings.
tions for future research. Psychological Bulletin, 85, 467-489.
Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 40, 167-169.
Mahrer, A. (1983). Experiential psychotherapy: Basic practices. New
Carkhuff, R. R. (1969a). Helping and human relations: Vol. 1. Selection
York: Brunner/Mazel.
and training. New \brk: Holt, Rinehart & Winston.
Carkhuff, R. R. (I969b). Helping and human relations: Vol. 2. Practice Mahrer, A. (1986). Therapeutic experiencing: The process of change.
and research. New \ork: Holt, Rinehart & Winston. New York: Norton.
Chinsky, J. M., & Rappaport, J. (1970). Brief critique of the meaning Matarazzo, R. (1978). Methods of teaching therapeutic skill. In S. Gar-
and reliability of "accurate empathy" ratings. Psychological Bulletin, field & A. Bergin (Eds.), Handbook of psychotherapy and behavior
73, 379-382. change (pp. 941-966). New York: Wiley.
Daldrup, R., Beutler, L., Engle, D., & Greenberg, L. S. (in press). Fo- Matarazzo, R., & Weins, A. (1977). Speech behavior as an objective
cused expressive therapy. New \ork: Guilford Press. correlate of empathy and outcome in interview and psychotherapy
Elliott, R. (1985). Helpful and nonhelpful events in brief counseling research. Behavior Modification, 1, 453-480.
interviews: An empirical taxonomy. Journal of Counseling Psychol- Mintz, J., &. Luborsky, L. (1971). Segments versus whole sessions:
ogy, 32, 307-322. Which is the better unit for psychotherapy process research? Journal
Elliott, R. (1988). Change processes in experiential therapy of depres- of Abnormal Psychology, 78, 180-191.
sion. Unpublished manuscript, University of Toledo, Ohio. Mitchell, K. M., Bozarth, S. S., & Krauft, C. C. (1977). A reappraisal
Elliott, R., Greenberg, L. S., Rice, L., & Clark, C. (1987). Experiential of the therapeutic effectiveness of accurate empathy, non-possessive-
psychotherapy of depression. Unpublished manuscript, University of ness, and genuineness. In A. S. Gurman & A. M. Razin (Eds.), Effec-
Toledo, Ohio. tive psychotherapy: A handbook of research (pp. 482-502). New York:
Gendlin, E. T. (1974). Client-centered and experiential psychotherapy. Pergamon Press.
In D. A. Wexler & L. Rice (Eds.), Innovations in client-centered ther- Moreland, J. R., Ivey, A. E., & Phillips, J. S. (1973). An evaluation of
apy (pp. 211 -246). New York: Wiley. microcounseling as an interviewer training tool. Journal of Consult-
Gendlin, E. T. (1981). Focusing. New York: Bantam. ing and Clinical Psychology, 41, 294-300.
Gladstein, G. A. (1977). Empathy and counseling outcome: An empiri- Pagell, W, Carkhuff, R., & Berenson, B. (1967). The predicted differen-
cal and conceptual review. Counseling Psychologist, 6,70-79. tial effects of the level of counselor training on the level of functioning
Greenberg, L. S. (1980). Training counsellors in gestalt methods. Cana- of outpatients. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 23, 510-512.
dian Counsellor, 14, 174-180. Perls, E, Hefferline, R., & Goodman, P. (1951). Gestalt therapy. New
Greenberg, L. S. (1984). A task analysis of intrapersonal conflict resolu- York: Dell.
tion. In L. Rice & L. S. Greenberg (Eds.), Patterns of change: Intensive Perry, M. (1975). Modelling and instructions in training for counselor
analysis of psychotherapeutic process (pp. 67-123). New York: Guil- empathy. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 22, 173-179.
ford Press. Piaget, G. N., Berenson, B. G., & Carkhuff, R. R. (1967). Differential
702 LESLIE S. GREENBERG AND RHONDA L. GOLDMAN

effects of the manipulation of therapeutic conditions by high and Rogers, C. R. (1957). The necessary and sufficient conditions of thera-
moderate-functioning upon high- and low-functioning clients. Jour- peutic personality change. Journal of Consulting Psychology, 21, 95-
nal of Consulting Psychology, 31, 481-486. 103.
Quartaro, G. K., & Rennie, D. L. (1983). Effects of trainee expectancies Shapiro, J. G. (1968). Relationship between expert and neophyte ratings
and specific instructions on counseling skill acquisition. Canadian of therapeutic conditions. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychol-
Journal of Behavioral Science, 15, 174-185. ogy, 32, 87-89.
Rappaport, J., & Chinsky, J. M. (1972). Accurate empathy: Confusion Stiles, W. (1986). Levels of intended meaning of utterances. British
of a construct. Psychological Bulletin, 77, 400-404. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 25, 213-222.
Rennie, D. L. (1987). Psychotherapy inside out: A training guide to sec- Toukmanian, S. G. (1984). Therapist manual: Perceptual-cognitive
ond generation person-centered therapy. Unpublished manual, 'York method of therapy. Unpublished manuscript, York University, To-
University, Toronto. ronto.
Rennie, D. L., Brewster, L. J., & Toukmanian, S. G. (1985). The coun- Toukmanian, S. G., Capelle, R. G., & Rennie, D. L. (1978). Counsellor
sellor trainee as client: Client process as a predictor of counseling skill trainee awareness of evaluated criteria: A neglected variable. Cana-
acquisition. Canadian Journal of Behavior Science, 17, 16-28. dian Counsellor, 12, 177-183.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

Rennie, D. L., Burke, H. L., & Toukmanian, S. G. (1978). Counsellor Toukmanian, S. G., & Rennie, D. L. (1975). Microcounseling versus
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

communication style as a determinant of rater-perceived empathy. human relations training: Relative effectiveness with undergraduate
Canadian Counsellor. 12,235-241. trainees. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 22, 345-352.
Rice, L. (1984). Client tasks in client centered therapy. In R. Levant Truax, C., & Carkhuff, R. R. (1967). Toward effective counseling and
& J. Shlien (Eds.), Client-centered therapy and the person centered psychotherapy: Training and practice. Chicago: Aldine.
approach: New directions in theory, research and practice (pp. 182- Wenegrat, A. (1974). A factor analytic study of the Truax accurate em-
202). New York: Wiley. pathy scale. Psychotherapy: Theory, Research and Practice, 11, 48-
Rice, L., AGreenberg, L. S. (Eds.). (1984). Patterns of change: Intensive 51.
analysis of psychotherapeutic process. New York: Guilford Press.
Rice, L., & Saperia, E. (1984). A task analysis of the resolution of prob-
lematic reactions. In L. Rice & L. S. Greenberg (Eds.), Patterns of Received February 10,1988
change: Intensive analysis of psychotherapeutic process (pp. 29-66). Revision received March 16, 1988
New York: Guilford Press. Accepted March 16, 1988 •

1989 APA Convention "Call for Programs"

The "Call for Programs" for the 1989 annual APA convention will be included in the October
issue of the APA Monitor. The 1989 convention will be in New Orleans, Louisiana, from August
11 through 15. Deadline for submission of program and presentation proposals is December
15, 1988. This earlier deadline is required because many university and college campuses will
close for the holidays in mid-December and because the convention is in mid-August. Addi-
tional copies of the "Call" will be available from the APA Convention Office in October.

You might also like