You are on page 1of 9

doi:10.

1093/brain/awl017 Brain (2006), 129, 944–952

Deciding how to decide: ventromedial frontal


lobe damage affects information acquisition in
multi-attribute decision making
Lesley K. Fellows

Montreal Neurological Institute, rue Université, Montréal, QC, Canada


Correspondence to: Lesley K. Fellows, Montreal Neurological Institute, Room 276, 3801 rue Université, Montréal,
QC H3A 2B4, Canada

Downloaded from http://brain.oxfordjournals.org/ at University College London on February 9, 2013


E-mail: lesley.fellows@mcgill.ca

Ventromedial frontal lobe (VMF) damage is associated with impaired decision making. Recent efforts to under-
stand the functions of this brain region have focused on its role in tracking reward, punishment and risk.
However, decision making is complex, and frontal lobe damage might be expected to affect it at other levels.
This study used process-tracing techniques to explore the effect of VMF damage on multi-attribute decision
making under certainty. Thirteen subjects with focal VMF damage were compared with 11 subjects with frontal
damage that spared the VMF and 21 demographically matched healthy control subjects. Participants chose
rental apartments in a standard information board task drawn from the literature on normal decision making.
VMF subjects performed the decision making task in a way that differed markedly from all other groups,
favouring an ‘alternative-based’ information acquisition strategy (i.e. they organized their information search
around individual apartments). In contrast, both healthy control subjects and subjects with damage predomi-
nantly involving dorsal and/or lateral prefrontal cortex pursued primarily ‘attribute-based’ search strategies
(in which information was acquired about categories such as rent and noise level across several apartments).
This difference in the pattern of information acquisition argues for systematic differences in the underlying
decision heuristics and strategies employed by subjects with VMF damage, which in turn may affect the quality
of their choices. These findings suggest that the processes supported by ventral and medial prefrontal cortex
need to be conceptualized more broadly, to account for changes in decision making under conditions of
certainty, as well as uncertainty, following damage to these areas.

Keywords: executive function; problem solving; prefrontal cortex; human, lesion

Abbreviations: D-CTL = dorsal/lateral control group; D/LF = dorsal and/or lateral frontal lobe;
V-CTL = ventromedial control group; VMF = ventromedial frontal lobe
Received September 28, 2005. Revised December 23, 2005. Accepted January 2, 2006. Advance Access publication February 2, 2006.

Introduction
Decision making, the process of choosing between options, 1997). Although this task does detect abnormal behaviour
is a fundamental human behaviour. In the last several years, in subjects with VMF damage, there is ongoing controversy
cognitive neuroscience studies have begun to investigate the about these findings and their interpretation (Manes et al.,
brain basis of decision making, with a particular focus on 2002; Tomb et al., 2002; Sanfey et al., 2003; Maia and
prefrontal cortex. This focus has been motivated, in part, McClelland, 2004, 2005; Bechara et al., 2005; Fellows and
by clinical reports that frontal damage [and particularly Farah, 2005).
ventromedial frontal (VMF) damage] can be associated with However, in some respects this controversy bypasses a
strikingly poor decision making (Eslinger and Damasio, larger issue. Decision making is a complex behaviour that
1985; Harlow, 1999; Ackerly, 2000). The most influential clearly involves multiple component processes (Krawczyk,
laboratory studies of decision making after VMF damage 2002; Fellows, 2004; Sugrue et al., 2005). In principle, the
have focused on risky decisions under uncertainty, as cap- poor real-life decisions of VMF-damaged subjects could be
tured by a card-based gambling task (Bechara et al., 1994, the result of deficits in any one or more than one of these
# The Author (2006). Published by Oxford University Press on behalf of the Guarantors of Brain. All rights reserved. For Permissions, please email: journals.permissions@oxfordjournals.org
Multi-attribute decision making following frontal damage Brain (2006), 129, 944–952 945

processes; there is no a priori reason to expect that the of decision making would seem to call upon several capacities
impaired decision making of such individuals rests solely, linked to prefrontal cortex, even in the absence of risk or
or even primarily, on a specific deficit in dealing with risk uncertainty. Further, the study of this aspect of decision mak-
or uncertainty. It is particularly important to clarify the ing may constitute a link between the literature examining
scope of decision-making impairments following frontal prefrontal contributions to planning and problem solving
lobe damage, because such evidence can be very helpful in in open-ended settings (Shallice and Burgess, 1991; Goel
interpreting the data being generated in the burgeoning func- et al., 1997; Goel and Grafman, 2000) and recent work on
tional imaging and electrophysiology literatures examining the role of the VMF region in representing the relative value of
the role of ventral and medial prefrontal cortex in various stimuli in simple associative learning paradigms (reviewed in
aspects of decision making (Critchley et al., 2001; Ernst et al., Rolls, 2000; Fellows, 2004; Sugrue et al., 2005).
2002; Gehring and Willoughby, 2002; Krawczyk, 2002; This study used a simple process-tracing methodology
McClure et al., 2004a; Tanaka et al., 2004). The present adapted from studies of decision making in normal subjects
study therefore represents an effort to move away from a to examine multi-attribute decision making in individuals
narrow focus on risky decision making, and instead examine with focal frontal lobe damage. The performance of those

Downloaded from http://brain.oxfordjournals.org/ at University College London on February 9, 2013


the effects of focal frontal lobe damage on a form of decision with VMF damage was contrasted with a lesioned control
making under certainty: multi-attribute decision making. group with frontal damage that spared the VMF, and with
Decision making does not have to involve risk to be hard. healthy age-matched control subjects. The author asked
Anyone who has purchased a car, chosen a school to attend whether damage to either frontal region is associated
or hired an employee from amongst several applicants knows with systematic differences in the strategic, information-
that weighing the pros and cons of options that differ across acquisition phase of such decision making.
multiple, sometimes incommensurate, dimensions can be very
difficult, even when all the information is known and the
potential outcomes are certain. This kind of decision making
Methods
has been examined in normal subjects, including studies of
the information acquisition strategies such subjects use, the Subjects
determinants of their choice of strategy and the quality of the The study involved 13 subjects with damage to the VMF and
11 subjects with damage to dorsal and/or lateral frontal lobe (D/LF)
decisions that result (Simon, 1955; Payne, 1976; Ford et al.,
(Fig. 1). These two groups differed in educational level, and so were
1989; Payne et al., 1993; Chu et al., 1999; Gigerenzer and
compared to separate demographically matched healthy control
Todd, 1999). This work has shown that individuals may groups [ventromedial control group (V-CTL), n = 11; dorsal/lateral
use a variety of strategies and heuristics to cope with the control group (D-CTL), n = 10]. Subjects with frontal damage
potential complexity of such decisions (Payne et al., 1992, were identified through the University of Pennsylvania Center for
1993; Gigerenzer and Todd, 1999). Indeed, in such settings Cognitive Neuroscience patient database. VMF damage was due to
‘deciding how to decide’ can become the central problem. rupture of anterior communicating aneurysm in 10 cases, and to
How many alternatives should be considered? How much ischaemic stroke in 3. D/LF damage followed ischaemic or haemor-
information should be learned about each? Should one rhagic stroke in eight cases, and resection of low-grade glioma with
search for the best alternative or settle for one that is ‘good local radiotherapy in three. Five VMF subjects and five D/LF sub-
enough’? Importantly, different solutions to these pre- jects were taking psychoactive medications. These were most com-
monly anticonvulsants and/or antidepressants. One VMF subject
decisional problems often result in different final choices
was taking an acetylcholinesterase inhibitor, another both an
(Payne et al., 1992), and in different levels of satisfaction
acetylcholinesterase inhibitor and methylphenidate, and a third
regarding these choices (Schwartz, 2004). modafenil. Subjects were tested at least 6 months (mean, 4.2 years,
One view emerging from process-oriented studies of range 0.5–12 years) after brain injury had occurred.
decision making is that these strategic pre-decisional pro- Lesions were traced from the most recent CT or MR imaging
cesses are flexible and adaptive, often constructed ‘on the fly’ available onto the standard Montreal Neurological Institute brain
as subjects engage in information acquisition. External con- using MRIcro software (Rorden and Brett, 2000). Clinical imaging
straints, such as instructions, information complexity and was used for this purpose and so pre-dated participation in this
time pressure, can influence such strategies. In addition, inter- study by months to years, depending on the recency of the brain
nal factors such as processing capacity and individual differ- injury, and the intensity of clinical follow-up of individual patients.
ences in decision goals (which may themselves be flexibly At the time of study participation, no subjects had clinical evidence
determined and updated as more information is acquired) (on history or neurological examination) of central nervous
system injury beyond that associated with the index lesion.
also contribute (Ford et al., 1989; Payne et al., 1992;
Subjects were assigned a priori to the VMF group if the lesion
Gigerenzer and Todd, 1999; Simon et al., 2004).
principally affected medial orbitofrontal and/or the ventral aspect
Thus, multi-attribute decision making involves a dynamic of the medial prefrontal cortex [following the boundaries laid out in
interplay between assessing the current relative value of Stuss and Levine (2002)]. Those with a lesion involving the frontal
alternatives, efficiently acquiring further information to opti- lobe anterior to the pre-central sulcus, but sparing VMF areas, were
mize this assessment of value and adjusting overall decision assigned to the D/LF group. As can be seen in Fig. 1, those in the D/LF
goals in light of the available information. As such, this form group most commonly had damage affecting the inferior and
946 Brain (2006), 129, 944–952 L. K. Fellows

Downloaded from http://brain.oxfordjournals.org/ at University College London on February 9, 2013


Fig. 1 Location and overlap of brain lesions. The top row shows the lesions of the 13 subjects with ventromedial frontal damage; the
bottom row shows those of the 11 D/LF subjects. Lesions are projected on the same seven axial slices of the standard Montreal
Neurological Institute brain for both groups, oriented according to radiological convention (right is left). Areas damaged in one subject are
shown in purple; warmer shades denote the degree to which lesions involve the same structures in two or more individuals, as indicated in
the key. Areas of damage common to both groups can be assessed by comparing each slice to the slice directly beneath it, and are described
in more detail in the text.

Table 1 Subject characteristics; see text for details [mean (SD)]


Group Age Education Estimated IQ Beck Frontal lesion
(years) (years) Depression volume (cc)
Inventory

VMF (n = 13) 56.1 (11.4) 13.0 (2.2) 115 (8) 12.4 (8.7) 22 (20)
V-CTL (n = 11) 60.4 (8.9) 13.1 (1.2) 119 (10) 6.0 (2.3)
D/LF (n = 11) 58.3 (11.2) 15.6 (2.6) 121 (11) 9.9 (4.7) 36 (36)
D-CTL (n = 10) 57.1 (12.7) 16.4 (1.8) 128 (6) 3.6 (4.0)

middle frontal gyrus, and none had damage affecting either medial volume of damaged tissue (all P > 0.05). The D/LF group scored
orbitofrontal cortex or that portion of medial prefrontal cortex significantly higher on the Beck Depression Inventory than the
ventral to the genu of the corpus callosum. Two VMF subjects D-CTL group (P < 0.01), as did the VMF compared with the
had damage that extended into D/LF areas: one with involvement V-CTL group (P < 0.05), but the two frontal groups did not differ
of the lateral aspect of the right inferior frontal gyrus and another significantly on this measure (P > 0.05).
with extensive bilateral frontopolar and dorsomedial prefrontal Subjects with frontal damage were administered a short neuro-
damage in addition to near-complete destruction of orbitofrontal psychological battery for screening purposes. Results from the tasks
and ventral medial prefrontal cortex. with potential sensitivity to frontal damage, as well as a verbal
Healthy control subjects were recruited by advertisement. Con- memory task (recall of a list of five words after a 1 min delay),
trols were not taking psychoactive medication and were free of are provided in Table 2 (not all subjects completed all tests). The
significant current or past psychiatric or neurological illness as groups differed significantly only in their performance on the
determined by history and screening neurological examination. Trails B task, with VMF subjects making more errors (Mann–
Controls were excluded if they scored less than 28 out of 30 on Whitney U-test, P < 0.01). The control groups also completed the
the mini-mental status examination (Folstein et al., 1983). IQ was animal and ‘F’ verbal fluency tasks. The D/LF group was significantly
estimated by means of the American version of the National Adult impaired on both fluency tasks compared with the D-CTL group
Reading Test. All subjects provided written, informed consent prior (P < 0.01), whereas the VMF group did not differ from the V-CTL
to participation in the study, in accordance with the Declaration of group on either measure.
Helsinki, and were paid a nominal fee for their time. The Institu-
tional Review Board of the University of Pennsylvania approved the
study protocol. Decision task
Demographic information is summarized in Table 1. The frontal The pattern of information search in multi-attribute decision
groups did not differ significantly from their respective control making was assessed using a standard ‘information board’ paradigm
groups in age, education or estimated IQ (t-test, all P > 0.05). (Payne, 1976). Subjects made hypothetical choices between one-
The D/LF group had significantly more education than the VMF bedroom apartments. Information about the apartments was dis-
group (P < 0.05), but did not differ significantly in estimated IQ or in played in table format on a computer screen. Initially, only the
Multi-attribute decision making following frontal damage Brain (2006), 129, 944–952 947

Table 2 Results of selected neuropsychological screening tests [mean (SD)]


Group Digit span forward Animal fluency ‘F’ fluency Trails B errors Verbal recall

VMF 5.0 (0) 15.4 (5.4) 10.7 (5.4) 3.6 (2.8) 3.4 (1.5)
V-CTL 19.6 (7.3) 11.9 (3.6)
D/LF 5.2 (1) 16.1 (4.6) 10.9 (3.9) 0.8 (0.9) 3.6 (1.0)
D-CTL 24.1 (4.6) 17.1 (5.6)

down)/(movements across + movements down). This results in a


score that ranges from 1 to +1, capturing the degree to which the
board was searched ‘down’ (by alternatives), or ‘across’ (by attri-
butes), regardless of how much information was acquired. An
entirely alternative (i.e. apartment)-based search strategy would

Downloaded from http://brain.oxfordjournals.org/ at University College London on February 9, 2013


yield a score of 1, whereas an entirely attribute-based strategy
would yield a score of +1. The percentage of the available information
examined and the final choice were also recorded.

Statistical analysis
Subjects with frontal lobe damage were classified a priori into two
groups: VMF and D/LF. These two groups were similar in age,
but differed significantly in level of education, necessitating two
healthy control groups matched with the respective frontal-damaged
Fig. 2 Example of information board layout, showing a partial view group on these two variables. Comparison of each frontal group with
of the 4 option-6 attribute (4 · 6) task, with some of the
the appropriate control group was carried out using t-tests where
information revealed.
the data conformed to a normal distribution, and Mann–Whitney
U-tests where they did not. Significance was set at P < 0.05,
apartment labels (e.g. ‘apartment A’, as column headings) and the two-tailed.
attributes for which information was available (e.g. ‘neighborhood’,
as row headings) were shown (Fig. 2). The information itself was
masked by white rectangles. Subjects indicated which information
Results
they wanted by clicking the mouse on the relevant white rectangle.
The rectangle was replaced by the information, which then remained Information search patterns
visible until a final decision was made, to eliminate any memory Subjects with VMF damage acquired information in a very
requirement. Subjects were instructed to seek as much information different way than did either those with D/LF damage or
as they felt they needed, and in the order that made sense to them, to either healthy control group. As shown in Fig. 3, VMF sub-
make a decision as efficiently as possible, proceeding ‘just as they jects predominantly pursued an alternative-based informa-
would in real life’. They were further told that it was not necessary to tion search strategy, acquiring information regarding
examine all the information and that they could decide on an apart-
multiple attributes of one alternative (e.g. apartment A),
ment whenever they felt ready. They were also told that there was no
then moving to another alternative. In contrast, the other
correct answer and that they should choose the apartment that was
right for them. There was no time limit. In cases where subjects were three groups of subjects pursued a primarily attribute-
uncomfortable using a mouse, they instead pointed to the rectangle based strategy, comparing several alternatives across one
they wished to open, and the experimenter moved the mouse for attribute (e.g. rent), then moving to another attribute. The
them. Subjects were also encouraged to ‘think aloud’, and their total score of VMF subjects differed significantly from that
comments were tape recorded, in keeping with the original of the V-CTL group [mean score (SD) summed across all
method of Payne (1976). However, participants with frontal lobe three tasks: VMF 1.3 (2.3), V-CTL 2.1 (1.2), Mann–
damage made few or no informative comments, so these data were Whitney U = 21, P < 0.01], whereas the scores of D/LF
not analysed further. and D-CTL groups did not (U = 49.5, P = 0.7). The same
All subjects first performed a practice decision problem, choosing pattern was evident in all three boards when analysed indi-
between two cars, provided with information about two attributes. vidually (Fig. 3).
They then selected an apartment from information boards of
Despite the differences in search strategies, all four
increasing complexity: 2 apartments-4 attributes (2 · 4), 4 apart-
ments-6 attributes (4 · 6), and 6 apartments-7 attributes (6 · 7). The
groups accessed a similar proportion of the available infor-
tasks were administered in the same order for all subjects. As is mation overall (Table 3). In keeping with the literature
typical for such tasks, there was no obviously best (or worst) choice. (Payne, 1976; Kerstholt, 1992), as the information content
The pattern of information acquisition was the main dependent of the decision boards increased, participants acquired
variable. This was summarized using the scoring system of Payne more information in absolute terms. However, they became
(1976), according to the formula: (movements across movements relatively more selective, examining a progressively smaller
948 Brain (2006), 129, 944–952 L. K. Fellows

proportion of the available information. This pattern did not others amongst the control subjects. Seventeen out of 21
differ across groups [ANOVA (analysis of variance), control subjects chose apartment B in the first problem, 12
effect of group F(3,41) = 1.7, P > 0.05, effect of task complexity out of 21 chose apartment B in the second problem, and 11
F(2,42) = 30.6, P < 0.0001, task complexity · group, F(6,82) = out of 21 chose apartment C in the third problem (Table 3).
1.2, P > 0.05)]. In contrast, the pattern of information acquisi- These were also the modal choices for the D/LF group. In
tion (search strategy) was not systematically affected by contrast, the VMF group tended to choose apartments that
increasing complexity. were not the modal choice overall more often than the other
Although there have been both anecdotal (Eslinger and groups. This tendency was only statistically significant in the 4
Damasio, 1985) and experimental (Rogers et al., 1999) reports · 6 task, where fewer VMF subjects chose the option that was
that VMF damage can lead to prolonged decision times, the modal choice of the group as a whole than did the V-CTL
this was not evident in the present experiment: The time subjects (Fisher’s exact test, P < 0.05). This suggests that the
to complete each task increased with task complexity markedly different search strategy of the VMF group influ-
(ANOVA, F(2,42) = 48.5, P < 0.0001), but there was no enced decision outcomes, as has been reported in normal
significant effect of group, nor interaction (all P > 0.5; subjects (Payne et al., 1992).

Downloaded from http://brain.oxfordjournals.org/ at University College London on February 9, 2013


Table 3). Thus, subjects with VMF damage acquired information in
The decision boards have no obvious right or wrong a systematically different way, and tended to make different
answers and therefore provide no absolute measure of final choices. Although there was no statistically significant
choice quality, as is typical for such process-oriented tasks. difference in the amount of information gathered across
Nevertheless, some apartments were more popular than all four groups, there was considerable variability in this
measure. Clinical lore [and some systematic experimental
work (e.g. Berlin et al., 2004)] indicates that VMF damage
can lead to heightened impulsivity. Can the choices of those
with VMF damage in the present experiment be considered
‘impulsive’, in some sense of this variously defined term
(Evenden, 1999)? The lack of any group-wise differences in
the amount of information examined or in the time taken to
complete the tasks argues against this possibility. However,
individual patterns of behaviour in the simplest decision task
provide some support for this view: in the 2 · 4 task, in which
subjects chose between just 2 apartments, all control and all
D/LF subjects accessed at least some information about both
alternatives, with 29 out of 32 examining all of the eight pieces
of information available. Strikingly, 6 of 13 VMF subjects did
not examine all the information, and 4 of 13 only examined
information about one of the alternatives before choosing it.
There were no evident differences in lesion characteristics or
demographic variables distinguishing these subjects from the
VMF group as a whole. This behaviour could be interpreted as
impulsive, in the sense that subjects are making a decision
based on information that is incomplete compared with that
Fig. 3 Median decision board scores for each group. Individual considered by the control group. An alternative explanation is
bars show the score for each decision board problem, as
indicated by the key (above). More positive scores indicate a more that VMF subjects had different decision goals: they were not
‘attribute-based’ search strategy, whereas more negative scores attempting to determine the relative merits of the different
indicate an ‘alternative-based’ search strategy. alternatives, but were instead comparing a given alternative to

Table 3 Amount of information acquired and number of attributes examined summed across all three decision boards and
expressed as a percentage [mean (SD)].
Group Information (%) Attributes (%) Decision time (s) Modal choice

2·4 4·6 6·7 2·4 4·6 6·7

VMF 71 (24) 93 (10) 65 (25) 113 (45) 174 (94) B A C/E


V-CTL 84 (19) 95 (10) 66 (31) 107 (67) 146 (72) B B C
D/LF 66 (22) 89 (14) 71 (48) 152 (88) 177 (112) B B C
D-CTL 76 (18) 94 (10) 58 (25) 110 (40) 138 (56) B B C

The modal choice in each decision board for each group is shown in the third column. Groups did not differ significantly in the amount
of information, nor in the number of attributes they examined.
Multi-attribute decision making following frontal damage Brain (2006), 129, 944–952 949

some absolute standard of acceptability, an often effective in managing information in minimally structured environ-
decision-making strategy known as ‘satisficing’ (Schwartz ments, similar to the impairments seen in ill-defined pro-
et al., 2002). blem-solving tasks following frontal injury. Second,
The majority of the VMF group had at least some degree of (perhaps relatedly) that it results in an impaired ability to
bi-hemispheric damage, precluding analysis of laterality determine the relative value of alternatives.
effects. The D/LF group included five individuals with left There is clearly an overlap between the ‘pre-decisional’,
hemisphere damage and six with right hemisphere damage. information acquisition phase of multi-attribute decision
There did not appear to be any systematic difference in search making and some forms of problem solving, both concep-
strategy or amount of information examined in these two tually and in their experimental instantiations. Indeed, the
subgroups. [Median total decision board score left D/LF process-oriented approach to studying decision making used
+2.1, right D/LF +1.8; mean (SD) proportion of information here was originally adapted from studies of problem solving
examined left D/LF 0.67 (0.22), right D/LF 0.66 (0.24)]. (Newell and Simon, 1972). Broadly similar methods have
Within the D/LF and VMF groups, lesion aetiology did not previously been used to study the effects of frontal lobe
appear to reliably predict search strategy, although such pat- damage on ill-structured problem solving. One such study

Downloaded from http://brain.oxfordjournals.org/ at University College London on February 9, 2013


terns would be difficult to detect given the small sample size. relied on a detailed analysis of ‘think aloud’ data generated
while subjects performed a hypothetical financial planning
task (Goel et al., 1997). Although frontal patients were com-
parable with healthy controls on a number of process mea-
Discussion sures, they had more difficulty structuring the multi-part
This was an exploratory study of the effects of frontal lobe problem, and made poor judgements about how satisfactory
damage on the processes underlying multi-attribute decision their proposed solutions were. Subjects with frontal damage
making. The intent was to more comprehensively delineate tended to stop before they had developed an objectively ade-
the stages of decision making that are affected by such quate plan. Similar difficulties have been noted following
damage. In contrast to other paradigms used in recent studies frontal lobe damage in another ill-structured, real-world
of decision making in patients with frontal damage, multi- task, the Multiple Errand test of Shallice and Burgess
attribute decision making does not involve risk, uncertainty (1991). Such studies have typically not examined D/LF and
or learning. VMF damage separately, although case reports suggest that
Subjects with VMF damage successfully performed the (relatively) isolated right D/LF/frontopolar (Goel and
decision tasks: they acquired a similar amount of information Grafman, 2000), or right orbitofrontal cortex damage
as the other groups and made decisions within a comparable (Satish et al., 1999), can be sufficient to cause difficulties in
period of time (although their choices were somewhat differ- ill-structured problem-solving tasks.
ent than the norm). However, the VMF group differed strik- It may be that the performance of VMF subjects in the
ingly from all other groups in how they acquired information. present study is simply an expression of similar difficulties
Importantly, this difference was systematic. Such patients did in the context of a decision-making task. However, there are
not acquire information in a disorganized way, but rather several reasons to think that there is a more specific basis for
consistently applied an information acquisition strategy these findings. VMF subjects pursued a consistent strategy
that differed from that of other participants. (albeit one that differed from the other groups), spent as
Process-oriented studies of multi-attribute decision mak- much time and gathered as much information as other par-
ing in normal subjects have argued that it is possible to draw ticipants. Further, the demands of the multi-attribute
inferences about a subject’s underlying decision strategy by decision tasks administered here differed in important ways
measuring how much information is acquired, and the pat- from those of the ill-defined problem-solving tasks reviewed
tern of this information search (e.g. Payne et al., 1993). VMF above: the decision tasks were brief, the possible behaviours
subjects acquired information following an alternative-based were simple and relatively constrained and information was
(sometimes called inter-dimensional) pattern, in contrast to supplied in a form intended to minimize working memory
the largely attribute-based (intra-dimensional) pattern fol- requirements. These factors may explain why subjects with
lowed by all other groups in the context of this task. That D/LF damage performed normally in the present experi-
is, VMF subjects tended to organize their information acqui- ment, in contrast to the reported effects of such damage
sition around individual apartments, whereas other groups on ill-structured problem solving. They also make it less likely
compared information about specific attributes, such as rent, that the performance of the VMF group can be explained by
across several apartments. This finding suggests that VMF poor planning or disorganization, at least in the most general
damage leads to systematic differences in the strategies and senses of these terms.
heuristics called upon in this form of decision making under Multi-attribute decision tasks share with ill-structured
certainty. problem solving the requirement to flexibly develop a strate-
How might VMF damage result in such strategy gic, orderly sequence of behaviours in a setting in which
differences? Two possibilities are suggested by separate the end-state is not made explicit. Participants were instructed
lines of evidence: first, that such damage leads to difficulties to make a choice, but had to specify for themselves their
950 Brain (2006), 129, 944–952 L. K. Fellows

decision-making goal, and judge when it had been met. There (Baylis and Gaffan, 1991), and in humans to inconsistent
is growing evidence that VMF damage impairs the ability to choices in a simple pairwise preference task (Fellows and
evaluate options. One possibility is that an underlying deficit Farah, 2004). In addition, functional imaging studies of
in the ability to compare the relative value of options led the preference judgements in healthy human subjects have impli-
subjects studied here to adopt qualitatively different decision- cated regions of OFC and medial prefrontal cortex in evalua-
making goals. tion (Zysset et al., 2002; Arana et al., 2003; Cunningham et al.,
Decision making in relatively open-ended contexts can be 2003; Paulus and Frank, 2003; McClure et al., 2004b).
viewed in terms of a fundamental dichotomy between two If VMF damage leads to difficulty in determining the rela-
possible decision-making goals: seeking the best alternative tive value of simple stimuli, comparing the relative value of
(maximizing) or seeking an acceptable alternative (satisfi- multi-attribute choices is presumably even more difficult.
cing). These two goals typically involve different information Maximizing requires determining the relative value of all
search strategies. Empirical data suggest that maximizing can options under consideration, whereas satisficing involves jud-
be carried out with a variety of search strategies, either alter- ging only whether an option’s value meets some minimum
native- or attribute-based, whereas satisficing typically standard. As such, the tendency of VMF subjects to acquire

Downloaded from http://brain.oxfordjournals.org/ at University College London on February 9, 2013


involves alternative-based information acquisition, the information in an alternative-based pattern in the present
pattern followed by the VMF group in the present study. experiment may reflect a more basic impairment in making
Interestingly, studies of normal individuals suggest that determinations of relative value, with a compensatory reliance
maximizing decision strategies are often adopted to avoid on a satisficing strategy.
the emotional state of regret. That is, individuals are moti- The tasks used here do not provide information about
vated to continue to search the decision space after accep- decision-making quality, so no conclusions can be drawn
table, or even excellent, options are identified, to avoid the about whether VMF subjects are effective in their alterna-
possibility of missing even better choices (Schwartz et al., tive-based decision making. Future work examining this
2002; Schwartz, 2004). question could shed light on whether VMF is implicated in
It is tempting to speculate that the alternative-based infor- judgements of both absolute (good–bad) and relative
mation acquisition of VMF subjects in the present study (better–worse) value, or is important only in the latter.
may reflect a tendency to satisfice, to judge an alternative In summary, this process-oriented study found that VMF
in absolute terms as ‘good enough’ rather than ‘seeking the damage systematically affected information acquisition in
best’. This possibility requires further study, but two existing multi-attribute decision-making problems. These findings
lines of evidence are at least consistent with this hypothesis. argue that such damage can fundamentally affect how deci-
First, recent work using a simple gambling paradigm found sions are made, even in the absence of uncertainty, risk or the
that subjects with VMF damage felt less regret after making need to consider future outcomes. Process-oriented
what turned out to be a sub-optimal choice (Camille et al., approaches to the study of decision making would seem to
2004). A diminished capacity to experience regret might make hold promise for understanding the intersection between stra-
such patients less inclined to adopt a maximizing strategy. tegic, ‘pre-decisional’ information acquisition and decision
Second, a variety of lines of evidence suggest that regions making per se. This study suggests that the VMFs play an
within VMF are involved in flexibly representing the context- important role at that intersection.
specific value of stimuli more generally. Lesions of OFC in
several species, including humans, have been shown to impair
performance on tasks that require the flexible updating of Acknowledgements
stimulus-reinforcement associations, such as reversal learn- The author wishes to thank Hilary Gerstein and Alisa Padon
ing (Jones and Mishkin, 1972; Dias et al., 1996; Schoenbaum for technical assistance, Marianna Stark for her help in patient
et al., 2002; Fellows and Farah, 2003; Clark et al., 2004; recruitment, and Martha Farah and Barry Schwartz for
Hornak et al., 2004; Izquierdo et al., 2004). Single unit record- helpful discussion. This work was supported by NIH R21
ings in monkeys have identified OFC neurons that respond NS045074, CIHR MOP-77583, and by a CIHR Clinician-
selectively to the most highly valued stimulus offered to the Scientist award.
animal, and that rapidly cease responding when circum-
stances change in a way that reduces the value of that stimulus
(Rolls, 1999, 2000; Tremblay and Schultz, 1999; Wallis and References
Ackerly S. Prefrontal lobes and social development. 1950. Yale J Biol Med
Miller, 2003). The somatic marker hypothesis, a prominent 2000; 73: 211–9.
theory of the role of VMF in risky decision making (Bechara Arana FS, Parkinson JA, Hinton E, Holland AJ, Owen AM, Roberts AC.
et al., 1997), is also fundamentally consistent with the view Dissociable contributions of the human amygdala and orbitofrontal
that this region is important in tracking the value of options, cortex to incentive motivation and goal selection. J Neurosci 2003; 23:
9632–8.
at least in certain circumstances.
Baylis LL, Gaffan D. Amygdalectomy and ventromedial prefrontal
There is some evidence that VMF is involved even in simple ablation produce similar deficits in food choice and in simple object
evaluative judgements under conditions of certainty: VMF discrimination learning for an unseen reward. Exp Brain Res 1991; 86:
lesions in macaques lead to abnormal food preferences 617–22.
Multi-attribute decision making following frontal damage Brain (2006), 129, 944–952 951

Bechara A, Damasio AR, Damasio H, Anderson SW. Insensitivity to future or dorsolateral prefrontal cortex in humans. J Cogn Neurosci 2004; 16:
consequences following damage to human prefrontal cortex. Cognition 463–78.
1994; 50: 7–15. Izquierdo A, Suda RK, Murray EA. Bilateral orbital prefrontal cortex lesions
Bechara A, Damasio H, Tranel D, Damasio AR. Deciding advantage- in rhesus monkeys disrupt choices guided by both reward value and
ously before knowing the advantageous strategy. Science 1997; 275: reward contingency. J Neurosci 2004; 24: 7540–8.
1293–5. Jones B, Mishkin M. Limbic lesions and the problem of stimulus—
Bechara A, Damasio H, Tranel D, Damasio AR. The Iowa Gambling Task and reinforcement associations. Exp Neurol 1972; 36: 362–77.
the somatic marker hypothesis: some questions and answers. Trends Cogn Kerstholt JH. Information search and choice accuracy as a function of task
Sci 2005; 9: 159–62; discussion 162–4. complexity and task structure. Acta Psychol 1992; 80: 185–97.
Berlin HA, Rolls ET, Kischka U. Impulsivity, time perception, emotion and Krawczyk DC. Contributions of the prefrontal cortex to the neural
reinforcement sensitivity in patients with orbitofrontal cortex lesions. basis of human decision making. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 2002; 26:
Brain 2004; 127: 1108–26. 631–64.
Camille N, Coricelli G, Sallet J, Pradat-Diehl P, Duhamel JR, Sirigu A. The Maia TV, McClelland JL. A reexamination of the evidence for the somatic
involvement of the orbitofrontal cortex in the experience of regret. Science marker hypothesis: what participants really know in the Iowa gambling
2004; 304: 1167–70. task. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 2004; 101: 16075–80.
Chu PC, Spires EE, Sueyoshi T. Cross-cultural differences in choice behavior Maia TV, McClelland JL. The somatic marker hypothesis: still many

Downloaded from http://brain.oxfordjournals.org/ at University College London on February 9, 2013


and use of decision aids: a comparison of Japan and the United States. questions but no answers. Trends Cogn Sci 2005; 9: 162–4.
Organ Behav Hum Decis Process 1999; 77: 147–70. Manes F, Sahakian B, Clark L, Rogers R, Antoun N, Aitken M, et al. Decision-
Clark L, Cools R, Robbins TW. The neuropsychology of ventral making processes following damage to the prefrontal cortex. Brain 2002;
prefrontal cortex: decision-making and reversal learning. Brain Cogn 2004; 125: 624–39.
55: 41–53. McClure SM, Laibson DI, Loewenstein G, Cohen JD. Separate neural
Critchley HD, Mathias CJ, Dolan RJ. Neural activity in the human brain systems value immediate and delayed monetary rewards. Science 2004a;
relating to uncertainty and arousal during anticipation. Neuron 2001; 306: 503–7.
29: 537–45. McClure SM, Li J, Tomlin D, Cypert KS, Montague LM, Montague PR.
Cunningham WA, Johnson MK, Gatenby JC, Gore JC, Banaji MR. Neural correlates of behavioral preference for culturally familiar drinks.
Neural components of social evaluation. J Pers Soc Psychol 2003; 85: Neuron 2004b; 44: 379–87.
639–49. Newell A, Simon HA. Human problem solving. Englewood Cliffs, NJ:
Dias R, Robbins TW, Roberts AC. Dissociation in prefrontal cortex of Prentice-Hall, 1972.
affective and attentional shifts. Nature 1996; 380: 69–72. Paulus MP, Frank LR. Ventromedial prefrontal cortex activation is critical
Ernst M, Bolla K, Mouratidis M, Contoreggi C, Matochik JA, Kurian V, et al. for preference judgments. Neuroreport 2003; 14: 1311–5.
Decision-making in a risk-taking task: a PET study. Neuropsychopharma- Payne JW. Task complexity and contingent processing in decision making: An
cology 2002; 26: 682–91. information search and protocol analysis. Organ Behav Hum Perform
Eslinger PJ, Damasio AR. Severe disturbance of higher cognition after 1976; 16: 366–87.
bilateral frontal lobe ablation: patient EVR. Neurology 1985; 35: Payne JW, Bettman JR, Johnson EJ. The adaptive decision maker. New York:
1731–41. Cambridge University Press, 1993.
Evenden JL. Varieties of impulsivity. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 1999; 146: Payne JW, Bettman JR, Coupey E, Johnson EJ. A constructive process view
348–61. of decision making: multiple strategies in judgment and choice. Acta
Fellows LK. The cognitive neuroscience of decision making: a review and Psychol 1992; 80: 107–41.
conceptual framework. Behav Cog Neurosci Rev 2004; 3: 159–72. Rogers RD, Everitt BJ, Baldacchino A, Blackshaw AJ, Swainson R, Wynne K,
Fellows LK, Farah MJ. Ventromedial frontal cortex mediates affective et al. Dissociable deficits in the decision-making cognition of chronic
shifting in humans: evidence from a reversal learning paradigm. Brain amphetamine abusers, opiate abusers, patients with focal damage to
2003; 126: 1830–7. prefrontal cortex, and tryptophan-depleted normal volunteers: evidence
Fellows LK, Farah MJ. Damage to ventromedial prefrontal cortex impairs for monoaminergic mechanisms. Neuropsychopharmacology 1999; 20:
simple preference judgments in humans. San Diego, CA: Society for 322–39.
Neuroscience;, 2004. Rolls ET. The brain and emotion. Oxford: Oxford University Press, 1999.
Fellows LK, Farah MJ. Different underlying impairments in decision-making Rolls ET. The orbitofrontal cortex and reward. Cereb Cortex 2000; 10:
following ventromedial and dorsolateral frontal lobe damage in humans. 284–94.
Cereb Cortex 2005; 15: 58–63. Rorden C, Brett M. Stereotaxic display of brain lesions. Behav Neurol 2000;
Folstein MF, Robins LN, Helzer JE. The mini-mental state examination. 12: 191–200.
Arch Gen Psychiatry 1983; 40: 812. Sanfey AG, Hastie R, Colvin MK, Grafman J. Phineas gauged: decision-
Ford JK, Schmitt N, Schechtman SL, Hults BM, Doherty ML. Process tracing making and the human prefrontal cortex. Neuropsychologia 2003; 41:
methods: contributions, problems, and neglected research questions. Org 1218–29.
Behav Hum Decision Proc 1989; 43: 75–117. Satish U, Streufert S, Eslinger PJ. Complex decision making after
Gehring WJ, Willoughby AR. The medial frontal cortex and the rapid orbitofrontal damage: neuropsychological and strategic management
processing of monetary gains and losses. Science 2002; 295: 2279–82. simulation assessment. Neurocase 1999; 5: 355–64.
Gigerenzer G, Todd PM. Simple heuristics that make us smart. New York: Schoenbaum G, Nugent SL, Saddoris MP, Setlow B. Orbitofrontal lesions
Oxford University Press, 1999. in rats impair reversal but not acquisition of go, no-go odor discrimin-
Goel V, Grafman J. Role of the right prefrontal cortex in ill-structured ations. Neuroreport 2002; 13: 885–90.
planning. Cogn Neuropsychol 2000; 17: 415–36. Schwartz B. The tyranny of choice. Sci Am 2004; 290: 70–5.
Goel V, Grafman J, Tajik J, Gana S, Danto D. A study of the performance of Schwartz B, Ward A, Monterosso J, Lyubomirsky S, White K, Lehman DR.
patients with frontal lobe lesions in a financial planning task. Brain 1997; Maximizing versus satisficing: happiness is a matter of choice. J Pers Soc
120: 1805–22. Psychol 2002; 83: 1178–97.
Harlow JM. Passage of an iron rod through the head. J Neuropsychiatry Shallice T, Burgess PW. Deficits in strategy application following frontal
Clin Neurosci 1999; 11: 281–3. lobe damage in man. Brain 1991; 114: 727–41.
Hornak J, O’Doherty J, Bramham J, Rolls ET, Morris RG, Bullock PR, et al. Simon D, Krawczyk DC, Holyoak KJ. Construction of preferences by
Reward-related reversal learning after surgical excisions in orbito-frontal constraint satisfaction. Psychol Sci 2004; 15: 331–6.
952 Brain (2006), 129, 944–952 L. K. Fellows

Simon HA. A behavioral model of rational choice. Q J Econ 1955; 69: Tomb I, Hauser M, Deldin P, Caramazza A. Do somatic markers mediate
99–118. decisions on the gambling task? Nat Neurosci 2002; 5: 1103–4; author reply
Stuss DT, Levine B. Adult clinical neuropsychology: lessons 1104.
from studies of the frontal lobes. Annu Rev Psychol 2002; 53: Tremblay L, Schultz W. Relative reward preference in primate orbitofrontal
401–33. cortex. Nature 1999; 398: 704–8.
Sugrue LP, Corrado GS, Newsome WT. Choosing the greater of two goods: Wallis JD, Miller EK. Neuronal activity in primate dorsolateral and orbital
neural currencies for valuation and decision making. Nat Rev Neurosci prefrontal cortex during performance of a reward preference task. Eur J
2005; 6: 363–75. Neurosci 2003; 18: 2069–81.
Tanaka SC, Doya K, Okada G, Ueda K, Okamoto Y, Yamawaki S. Prediction Zysset S, Huber O, Ferstl E, von Cramon DY. The anterior frontomedian
of immediate and future rewards differentially recruits cortico-basal cortex and evaluative judgment: an fMRI study. Neuroimage 2002; 15:
ganglia loops. Nat Neurosci 2004; 7: 887–93. 983–91.

Downloaded from http://brain.oxfordjournals.org/ at University College London on February 9, 2013

You might also like