You are on page 1of 14

TRAUMA, VIOLENCE, & ABUSE

14(1) 54-66
Assessment and Treatment of Distorted ª The Author(s) 2013
Reprints and permission:
sagepub.com/journalsPermissions.nav
Schemas in Sexual Offenders DOI: 10.1177/1524838012463970
http://tva.sagepub.com

Anthony R. Beech1, Ross M. Bartels1, and Louise Dixon1

Abstract
The aim of this review is to examine the literature related to the assessment and treatment of sex offenders’ distorted schemas.
Where appropriate, the review draws upon current insights from the field of social cognition to aid in the critical evaluation of the
findings. First, the review considers the various different methodologies for assessing distorted schemas, discussing their strengths
and limitations. Second, the review examines the work related to the treatment of sex offenders’ schemas. Suggestions for future
research, and the implications for clinical practice, are highlighted in the article.

Keywords
schemas, sex offenders, distorted cognitions, schema therapy, indirect measures

Introduction Schemas Defined


Researchers and clinicians have frequently noted that some sex A schema is a cognitive structure defined as a network of
offenders show evidence of distorted, offense-supportive learned associations (Bem, 1981) that guide attention, inform
thinking patterns. For example, a child molester may state that perceptions, and save mental energy by providing shortcuts
sexual activity causes no harm to children, or a rapist may to interpreting incoming stimuli (Fiske & Taylor, 1984). The
describe how women exist to meet the needs of men. Such processes that give rise to such capabilities are said to occur
cognitions are regarded as an important dynamic (changeable) automatically (Kahneman, 2011). That is, when a certain sti-
risk factor for sexual offending (Thornton, 2002), and are com- mulus is encountered, it automatically activates (or primes) a
monly referred to as cognitive distortions (Abel, Becker, & particular association in one’s cognitive network (i.e., schema).
Cunningham-Rathner, 1984), and have been defined as postof- The pattern of activation will depend largely on the cognitive
fense rationalizations and justifications to account for sexually availability of the schema (Tversky & Kahneman, 1973).
deviant interests and behavior (Maruna & Mann, 2006). This pop- Importantly, automatic reactions resulting from the activation
ular conceptualization has guided treatment, in that, offenders, of schematic associations provide the basis for people’s beliefs
historically, have been encouraged to take responsibility for their (Gawronski & Bodenhausen, 2006; Gilbert, 1991; Kahneman,
offenses (Salter, 1988). Based on insights from the field of social 2011). For example, a response elicited by the activation of a
cognition, the topic of sex offender cognition may be viewed as child–sex association may be transformed into the propositional
being somewhat more complex in nature. For example, Ward, belief ‘‘Children want sex.’’ Such beliefs may be unbelieved if
Hudson, Johnston, and Marshall (1997) note that cognitive struc- they are rejected on the basis of deliberate evaluation—a more
tures (i.e., schemas) give rise to cognitive products (i.e., conscious effortful process that involves consciously evaluating whether
thoughts and self-statements) via cognitive processing. There is a the initial belief is true or false (Gawronski & Bodenhausen,
growing consensus that (distorted) cognitive structures (i.e., 2006; Gilbert, 1991). Thus, according to this work, schematic
schemas should be the primary target for clinicians (Dean, Mann, associations are not synonymous with beliefs but instead
Milner, & Maruna, 2007; Ó Ciardha & Gannon, 2011). This is provide the basis for beliefs. This difference indicates that
because schemas are regarded as more psychologically meaning- while schemas may exist outside of conscious awareness,
ful than postoffense rationalizations (Mann, Hanson, & Thornton, beliefs may not (Gilbert, 1991). The important point here is that
2010). Schemas are thought to give rise to distorted interpreta-
tions and thoughts (Mann & Beech, 2003), contribute to the onset
1
of sexual offending (Ó Ciardha & Gannon, 2011), and influence University of Birmingham, Birmingham, UK
sexual recidivism (Thornton, 2002). Therefore, in this review, we
Corresponding Author:
will focus specifically on the assessment and treatment of sex Anthony R. Beech, Centre for Forensic and Criminological Psychology, School
offenders’ underlying schemas. We will now discuss the schema of Psychology, University of Birmingham, Edgbaston, Birmingham B15 2TT, UK
concept in more detail. Email: a.r.beech@bham.ac.uk
Beech et al. 55

an automatic, schema-driven belief can differ from a proposi- (Polaschek & Ward 2002), an important factor predictive of
tional belief based on deliberate evaluation (see Moran & sexual reoffending in some offenders (Hanson & Morton-
Bar-Anan, 2012). However, this does not mean that automatic, Bourgon, 2005). Conversely, the schemas that generate
schema-driven cognition never influences behavior, because middle-level beliefs in child molesters (e.g., that children enjoy
under time pressure or during times of mental effort (i.e., when sex) and rapists (e.g., that women are constantly sexually
cognitive resources are too busy or low to engage in controlled receptive) are related more to their respective type of offending
thought), people rely more on automatic beliefs (Shiv & (Beech, Ward, & Fisher, 2006). We will now examine the
Fedorikhin, 1999). This distinction will be referred to where literature related to their assessment.
relevant when discussing the assessment and treatment of
schemas.
Assessment of Distorted Schemas
Schemas and Sexual Offending The accurate assessment of a schema is difficult as individuals
are thought to lack introspective access to their content (Mann
In relation to sexual offending, distorted schemas produce & Beech, 2003). Nevertheless, attempts have been made using
distorted beliefs or evaluations when a relevant stimulus is a number of strategies. For example, there have been some
encountered (Mann & Beech, 2003). Ward (2000) proposed attempts at using case file information to identify offense-
that sex offender schemas should be thought of as implicit supportive schemas, with some success (Bennett, 2011; Smid,
theories1; constructs that, like scientific theories, are used to 2010). Also, ‘‘life-maps’’ are reported as being useful as they
explain, predict, and interpret interpersonal phenomena. can highlight recurring events that may be schema-driven or
According to Ward (2000), sex offenders’ schemas can give that may have created distorted schemas (Mann & Shingler,
rise to beliefs at the general level (i.e., general beliefs about the 2006; Milner & Webster, 2005). However, the most common
nature of people and world), the middle level (i.e., beliefs about forms of assessment within the literature are interviews, psy-
categories of people, such as women or children), and the chometrics, and indirect measures. These three main methodol-
specific level (i.e., beliefs about a particular victim). Ward ogies will be discussed.
notes that general and middle-level beliefs are most important
as they form the foundation of offenders’ interpretation of
victims’ actions and mental state. Interviews
By examining the items of cognitive distortion question- Interviews are a potentially useful way of assessing offenders’
naires, Ward and Keenan (1999) and Polaschek and Ward distorted schemas, as they elicit rich data. For example, by
(2002) proposed five core schemas held by some child asking broad, open-ended questions about an offender’s his-
molesters and rapists, respectively. For child molesters, these tory, offense chain, and general daily functioning, information
include: (1) Dangerous world—generating beliefs that adults can be gained that may suggest the influence of a schema.
are hostile and threatening compared to children; (2) Entitle- Questions can also be framed to directly address core schemas,
ment—generating beliefs that one is entitled to do what they such as asking about their views on adult relationships, whether
want, due to feeling superior and more important than others; they feel in control of their lives, and so forth. Further, asking
(3) Uncontrollability—generating beliefs that the individual about such topics are likely to activate the relevant schemas
has no control over their life circumstances; (4) Children as (Keown, Gannon, & Ward, 2010), suggesting that that offen-
sexual beings—generating beliefs that children both need and ders’ answers may be schema-driven. Indeed, interviews using
desire sexual pleasure, and are able to make informed decisions these types of questions have been conducted with sex offen-
related to sexual behavior; and (5) Nature of harm—generating ders, the results of which provide corroboration for the schemas
beliefs that sexual activity with children is essentially harmless previously described. For example, Marziano, Ward, Beech,
(Ward & Keenan, 1999). and Pattison (2006) interviewed 22 child molesters and found
For rapists, Polaschek and Ward (2002) proposed a set of evidence for all five of Ward and Keenan’s schemas, with the
schemas that have clear parallels with those proposed for child most prevalent being Children as Sexual Beings. More
molesters, these include (1) Dangerous World; (2) Entitlement; recently, Keown et al. (2010) interviewed 33 child molesters
and (3) Male Sex Drive is Uncontrollable. The other two refer and found evidence for all five schemas, with Children as
specifically to women; namely, (4) Women as Sex Objects— Sexual Beings, Nature of Harm, and Uncontrollability most
generating beliefs that women constantly desire sex, even if prevalent. Furthermore, Beech, Parrett, Ward, and Fisher
it is coerced or violent; and (5) Women are Unknowable—gen- (2009) and Gannon, Hoare, Rose, and Parrett (2012) found
erating beliefs that women are inherently different from men, evidence for these five schemas in female child molesters.
and that these differences cannot be readily understood by men. As for interviews with rapists, the schemas proposed by
Child molesters and rapists are seen to both hold schemas Polaschek and Ward (2002) have been corroborated. For exam-
underpinning general-level beliefs about the world, a sense of ple, Polaschek and Gannon (2004) found evidence for all five
entitlement, and a lack of control. These three schemas are non- schemas, although they renamed Women are Unknowable as
sexual (Gannon, Keown, & Rose, 2009a) and are thought to Women are Dangerous, as women were often described as mal-
play a role in promoting a general antisocial orientation evolent as well as unpredictable. The most prevalent schemas
56 TRAUMA, VIOLENCE, & ABUSE 14(1)

were Women are Dangerous, Women as Sex Objects, and Enti- (Check, Malamuth, Elias, & Barton, 1985); the Attitudes
tlement. In another study, Beech et al. (2006) interviewed 41 Towards Women scale (Spence, Helmreich, & Stapp, 1973);
rapists and found evidence for all 5 schemas, with Dangerous and Bumby’s RAPE scale (Bumby, 1996). There is some lim-
World and Women as Sex Objects most common. They also ited research showing that rapists endorse the statements on
found that the presence/absence of certain schemas was related these measures more than nonrapists (Scott & Tetreault,
to certain rapist subtypes, demonstrating how cognition may 1987). However, the majority of the research indicates that
differ due to offender heterogeneity. For example, violently rapists do not significantly differ from sexual murderers,
motivated rapists predominantly held Dangerous World; sexu- violent offenders, or child molesters on these scales (Beech,
ally motivated rapists predominantly held Women as Sex Oliver, Fisher, & Beckett, 2005; Bumby, 1996; Pervan &
Objects; and sadistically motivated rapists held both Dangerous Hunter, 2007; Scully, 1990). However, a meta-analysis of 39
World and Women as Sex Objects. In addition, these five rape- studies found that rape-related cognition, as measured by some
related schemas have been identified, via interviews, in sexual of the measures outlined above, was a strong predictor of
murderers (Beech, Fisher, & Ward, 2005). sexual aggression (Murnen, Wright, & Kaluzny, 2002).
To be taken as a whole, these studies suggest that interviews The issue is that for these measures to be useful tools for
are useful for identifying schemas. However, there is variation assessing schemas, the items must reflect the schemas that sex
regarding which schemas are most prevalent. Also, the data offenders are thought to hold. To investigate this, Gannon et al.
gained from interviews are based on offenders’ self-statements (2009a) examined the extent to which the 167 items from the
and so could also reflect a deliberate, explicit attitude; socially six distortion scales mentioned above (excluding the SWCH
desirable responding; excuse-making (particularly when speak- scale) reflected Ward and Keenan’s five schemas. They found
ing about their offense); an error in memory; or an effect of treat- that the highest percentage of items were classified as relating
ment. As there is no way to differentiate between these to Children as Sexual Beings (32%), followed by Nature of
possibilities, researchers and clinicians should be cautious when Harm and ‘‘Unclassified’’ (each 23%). The nonsexual schemas
interpreting interview data. of Entitlement, Dangerous world, and Uncontrollability were
greatly underrepresented (9%, 8%, and 5%, respectively).
Similarly, a factor analysis of the SWCH scale produced two
Psychometric Assessment factors consistent with the Children as Sexual Beings and
Psychometric measures are probably the most common method Nature of Harm schemas (Mann et al., 2007), further demon-
of assessing distorted cognition. Most of these measures strating the underrepresentation of nonsexual schemas. Unfor-
involve a list of items thought to reflect a distorted belief. tunately, researchers have yet to group the items of rape-related
Respondents are required to state how strongly they agree or scales into the five rape-related schemas. However, by examin-
disagree with each item, usually on a Likert-type scale. A num- ing the questionnaire items more closely and referring to the
ber of different measures have been constructed to assess the relevant literature, some of the measures do appear to reflect
distorted cognition of both child molesters and rapists. Some factors consistent with rape-related schemas. For example, a
of the most common measures for child molester cognitions factor analysis of Bumby’s RAPE scale produced two factors
include: the MOLEST scale (Bumby, 1996); Abel and Becker’s that both comprise items consistent with Women as Sex
Cognition scale (Abel et al., 1989); the Hanson Sex Attitude Objects (Hermann, Babchishin, Nunes, Leth-Steensen, &
Questionnaire (Hanson, Gizzarelli, & Scott, 1994); the Cogni- Cortoni, 2012).
tive Distortions and Immaturity scale from the Multiphasic From this research, commonly used psychometrics appear to
Sex Inventory (Nichols & Molinder, 1984); Offenses Against primarily reflect middle-level beliefs (i.e., those related to chil-
Children scale, from the Questionnaire on Attitudes Consistent dren/women and sex). This suggests that clinicians may be
with Sexual Offending (Lindsay, Whitefield, & Carson, 2007); unintentionally ignoring offenders’ nonsexual distorted cogni-
and the Cognitive Distortions scale of the Children and Sex tion during dynamic risk assessments. A more recent psycho-
Questionnaire/Beliefs About Children scale (Beckett, 1987). metric—the My Life questionnaire (Mann & Hollin, 2010)—
Studies using these measures indicate that child molesters pro- may be useful here. This scale is based upon offenders’ self-
duce higher scores relative to comparison groups (Beech, reported explanations for their offending and is thought to
Fisher, & Beckett, 1999; Craig, Thornton, Beech, & Browne, measure distorted schemas. Factor analysis shows the scale is
2007; Hanson, Gizzarelli, & Scott, 1994; Keown et al., comprised of two factors: Dominance (related to a need for
2010). More recently, Mann, Webster, Wakeling, and Marshall respect and desire for revenge) and Disadvantaged (related to
(2007) developed a brief, 18-item measure named the Sex with beliefs about being damaged and used by others). According
Children (SWCH) scale. Compared to rapists and nonoffen- to the authors, the latter factor is consistent with the Dangerous
ders, child molesters scored more highly, as did high-risk world schema, and sex offenders and nonsex offenders were
molesters compared to low-risk molesters. found to score higher on this factor than nonoffenders. How-
Questionnaires commonly used to assess offense-supportive ever, the Dominance factor also shares some parallels with
cognitions in rapists include Burt’s (1980) Rape Myth Accep- Dangerous World, as people with this schema find it necessary
tance, Adversarial Sexual Beliefs, and Acceptance of Interper- to fight back and achieve dominance over people (Ward, 2000).
sonal Violence scales; the Hostility Towards Women scale Thus, with further validation, this measure may be a useful
Beech et al. 57

addition to the assessment of distorted cognition, as it provides (Gawronski & Bodenhausen (2007, p. 696). Thus, an offen-
a measure of nonsexual, general-level beliefs (such as those der’s positive reaction to the statement ‘‘Children are willing
related to ‘‘grievance thinking’’—see Barnett, 2011). to have sexual activity with adults’’ may be rejected if it is
There have also been some attempts made to construct ques- inconsistent with other considered propositions (i.e., those learnt
tionnaires designed specifically to measure all five of Ward and through treatment). On the surface, therefore, it may seem as
Keenan’s schemas. For example, Goddard (2006) developed though an offender does not harbor a distorted schema when
the Implicit Theories Questionnaire (ITQ), a 204-item scale in fact they do. The issue for clinicians is that there is no way
that measures how strongly respondents endorse statements to distinguish this kind of responding from that provided by
that relate to one of the five schemas. Jones and Vess (2010) someone who has no distorted schemas, or from someone who
tested the ITQ on a sample of 30 child molesters and found has lied. In light of this, it may be more beneficial to use indirect
Dangerous World to be most strongly endorsed. However, as measures that are able to bypass deliberated responding.
there were no comparison samples, it is unknown how these
scores compare to nonoffenders or other sex offenders. Howitt
and Sheldon (2007) devised a similar questionnaire comprised
Indirect Measures
of 39 items and tested it on a sample of Internet offenders, con- Indirect measures provide an outcome (i.e., response latency)
tact child molesters, and mixed offenders (i.e., contact child that a researcher may infer as being indicative of an underlying
molesters and internet offenders). They found that, across all schema (De Houwer & Moors, 2010). There are a wide range of
offender groups, items related to Dangerous World and Uncon- indirect measures that have been used to assess cognition in sex
trollability were the most strongly endorsed. They also found offenders (for a review, see Snowden, Craig, & Gray, 2011).
that Internet offenders endorsed more statements related to Some of these measures have been adapted to specifically
Children as Sexual Beings than contact offenders. A consistent assess distorted schemas. For example, the Lexical Decision
finding across both studies is that items related to Dangerous Task (LDT; Meyer & Schvaneveldt, 1971) has been adapted
World are strongly endorsed. This demonstrates an advantage to specifically measure Ward and Keenan’s five schemas in
of using measures designed specifically to assess all five sche- child molesters (Keown, Gannon, & Ward, 2008). For this task,
mas; that is, they address nonsexual schemas that are underre- participants must quickly decide whether a string of letters
presented in other distortion scales. forms a word or not after having read an incomplete sentence
Despite these observations, there are a number of important (i.e., prime). Keown et al. (2008) hypothesized that once a
issues regarding the use of psychometrics. First, despite the schema was primed by an incomplete sentence, (i.e., Having
number of psychometrics that exists, few have properly been sex with children won’t do them any), child molesters would
empirically tested for their predictive abilities and psycho- respond faster to words that completed the sentence in a
metric properties. For a psychological test to be useful, it must schema-consistent manner (e.g., harm) relative to nonconsis-
have been standardized on the population it will be used with tent words (e.g., good). However, the authors found evidence
and provide normative data to which an individual can be com- for only one of the schemas, namely, Uncontrollability. This
pared (Craig & Beech, 2009). However, many tests that assess suggests that the LDT may not be the best tool for identifying
distorted beliefs in sex offenders frequently lack standardiza- distorted schemas. However, more research is warranted before
tion with appropriate norms, making any comparisons ques- any solid conclusions are made.
tionable. Second, self-report measures are highly susceptible A recently developed indirect measure called the Implicit
to social desirable responding. Gannon, Keown, and Polaschek Relational Assessment Procedure (IRAP; Barnes-Holmes
(2007) showed that extrafamilial child molesters’ responses on et al., 2006) was adapted to assess Children as Sexual Beings
a distortion questionnaire increased after they were made to schema (Dawson, Barnes-Holmes, Gresswell, Hart, & Gore,
believe they were being monitored for faking. This suggests 2009). In this task, offenders are presented with stimuli related
that offenders were initially being dishonest. Note, however, to one of the four category pairings (i.e., child–sexual, child–
that the change in offenders’ responses chiefly involved nonsexual, adult–sexual, and adults–nonsexual) and are
disagreeing less to items, as opposed to agreeing more (see also required to respond with ‘‘true’’ or ‘‘false.’’ These relational
Keown et al., 2010). responses incite participants to deliberately evaluate the sti-
This leads on to the final problem, which is that using muli. For example, when Sexually Aware is presented during
self-report measures to assess schemas is intrinsically proble- a trial with ‘‘child–sexual’’ as the category label (Dawson
matic. While schemas are largely inaccessible to conscious et al., 2009), participants will evaluate whether they consider
awareness (Mann & Shingler, 2006), the reactions (i.e., cogni- children to be sexually aware. Child–sexual stimuli are pre-
tive products) elicited by the activation of schemas tend to sumed to produce an immediate response in child molesters
provide the basis for evaluative judgments, such as those asked that will provide the basis for their evaluation. Thus, under time
on psychometrics (Gawronski & Bodenhausen, 2007). Thus, it pressure, offenders are likely to agree with the belief. Rather
is possible for questionnaires to tap schema content. However, than freely responding, however, participants are instructed
‘‘people also sometimes reject their affective reactions as a to respond in a manner that is either consistent or inconsistent
valid basis for an evaluative judgment when these reactions are with societal norms. Thus, using the example above, blocks
inconsistent with other momentarily considered propositions’’ involving trials that are inconsistent with societal norms would
58 TRAUMA, VIOLENCE, & ABUSE 14(1)

require participants to press ‘‘true,’’ whereas blocks involving nonoffending males who report sexual arousal toward low-
trials consistent with social norms would require participants force child abuse (Gannon & O’Connor, 2011).
to press ‘‘false.’’ Here, nonoffenders would be expected to There is also evidence that the IAT is sensitive to the hetero-
respond slower during inconsistent blocks compared to consis- geneity among sex offenders. For example, using Brown
tent blocks relative to child molesters. et al.’s (2009) IAT design, Bartels, Harkins, and Beech (in pre-
In their study, Dawson et al. (2009) found that both child paration) found a marginally significant difference between
molesters and nonoffenders showed a bias toward adults as sex- exclusively extrafamilial child molesters and a group of nonex-
ual and children as not sexual. However, the bias was signifi- trafamilial sex offenders, with the former demonstrating a
cantly weaker in child molesters. Also, child molesters did stronger child–sex association. In addition, hebephiles (i.e.,
not show any bias for confirming or denying child-sexual those with child victims above 12 years) do not appear to show
beliefs. It is important to highlight that, because the IRAP stronger child–sex associations (Brown et al., 2009) nor do
involves confirming and denying beliefs, it is targeted more female child molesters (Gannon, Rose, & Williams, 2009b).
at the deliberate (explicit) evaluation of a belief rather than its This research indicates that the IAT is a promising tool for
underlying associations (Dawson et al., 2009; Gawronski & De assessing offense-supportive schemas. However, there are a
Houwer, in press). Thus, although child molesters neither number of important issues to address. The first is that IAT
affirmed nor rejected child-sexual beliefs, it does not rule out effects may be due to strong adult–not sex associations, rather
the possibility that they hold child–sex schematic associations. than child–sex associations (Snowden et al., 2011). Due to the
Therefore, tasks that target ‘‘raw’’ associations may prove more IAT’s relative nature, researchers are unable to rule out this
useful in trying to assess offense-supportive schemas. possibility. However, other measures have been devised to
The most popular indirect measure of this kind is the Impli- allow for the assessment of all four possible associations, such
cit Association Task (IAT; Greenwald, McGhee, & Schwartz, as the Sorting Paired Features task (SPF; Bar-Anan, Nosek, &
1998). Thus, on this basis, we will discuss the IAT in more Vianello, 2009). This task allows all four associations to be
depth. In brief, the IAT is a categorization task that uses assessed in a single block. Stimuli are presented in pairs
response latencies to infer how strongly two concepts are (e.g., School and Orgasm) in the middle of the screen and are
associated in long-term memory (e.g., flowers and pleasant) sorted into their appropriate category pairing (i.e., child–sex,
relative to an opposing association (i.e., insects and pleasant). child–not sex, adult–sex, and adults–not sex) located in the four
Since schemas are conceptualized as networks of associations corners. Thus, in the example above, the correct category
held in memory, it can be argued that a sex offender holding would be ‘‘child–sex.’’ In a recent study, Bartels et al. (in pre-
the Children as Sexual Beings schema will harbor strong asso- paration) used the SPF with child molesters to assess the four
ciations between children and sex. Arguably, the IAT would be possible associations described above. The results showed that
able to assess this schema by measuring the strength of child– exclusively extrafamilial child molesters associated children
sex associations held in child molesters’ memory. and sex more strongly than the other three associations,
Mihailides, Devilly, and Ward (2004) were among the first whereas nonoffenders and a group of nonextrafamilial abusers
to use the IAT to investigate the associative nature of schemas associated adults and sex most strongly. Furthermore, the
related to child molesters: Children as Sexual Beings, Uncon- strength of the child–sex association in extrafamilial child
trollability, and Entitlement. Relative to nonsex offenders and molesters was significantly different to that of nonoffenders
nonoffenders, child molesters were found to show stronger (p ¼ .009), and nonextrafamilial abusers (p ¼ .024). The non-
‘‘children-sex’’ and ‘‘losing control-sex’’ associations. In addi- offenders and nonextrafamilial abusers did not differ. This
tion, child molesters demonstrated stronger ‘‘mine–sex’’ asso- implies that some child molesters’ may harbor less cognitively
ciations than nonoffenders. These three associations can be accessible schemas or no distorted schemas.
seen as being indicative of the presence of their respective The second issue with the IAT is that all studies have found
underlying schema. Since then, a number of researchers have differences at the group level, when, to be useful as an assess-
used the IAT to find evidence for strong child–sex associations ment tool, the IAT needs to be meaningful at the individual
in child molesters (Banse, Schmidt, & Clarbour, 2010; Brown, level (Brown et al., 2009). A problem with trying to detect dif-
Gray, & Snowden, 2009; Gray, Brown, MacCulloch, Smith, & ferences at the individual level comes from counterbalancing
Snowden, 2005; Nunes, Firestone, & Baldwin, 2007; Steffens, the blocks of an IAT. This is common practice to cancel out
Yundina, & Panning, 2008). Each of these IAT studies differ on order effects at the group level. However, such effects still
some methodological point, such as stimuli type (words, pic- remain at the individual level. Thus, while some have
tures), category labels (sex, sexy, erotic, sexually exciting), and attempted to discriminate offenders at the individual level in
number of blocks (two, five, or seven). However, Babchishin, studies using between-subjects counterbalancing (e.g., Gray
Nunes, and Hermann (2012) conducted a meta-analysis on et al., 2005), this should be avoided as order effects are present
these six published studies, as well as five other unpublished at the level of the individual (Messner & Vosgerau, 2010).
IAT studies, and found that child–sex IATs (regardless of their Brown et al. (2009) attempted to avoid this by holding blocks
methodological differences) can efficiently distinguish child constant for all participants; a strategy that Messner and
abusers from nonabusers. There is also some support for Vosgerau (2010) advise against because switching between
child-se– associations (albeit using a pen-and-paper IAT) in fixed blocks will simply inflate or diminish IAT scores. They
Beech et al. 59

suggest counterbalancing blocks within-subjects up to 3–4 lead to dishonest responding or a response that reflects a con-
times, as they found that this eliminates order effects at the sciously derived belief. Indirect measures, such as the IAT,
individual level. It is worth noting that in Brown et al.’s study, offer a promising means for assessing distorted schemas as they
the adult-sex block was always presented first, suggesting that target the associations that underlie schemas; are less suscepti-
an association between adults and sex would have been ble to faking; and do not allow for deliberated thought. So far,
inflated. However, they still found pedophilic child molesters IAT research has focused on male child molesters, with results
evidenced stronger child–sex associations. Thus, this is an demonstrating that they associate children and sex. However,
important area to research further as it has direct implications more research is still needed to determine the construct validity
for the clinical assessment of offender cognition. of the child–sex IAT; whether it can discriminate offenders at
Third, there are issues with regard to what the child–sex IAT the individual level; whether the IAT can assess nonsexual
is actually measuring. This has major implications because if schemas; and whether it is useful with other sex offenders.
the child–sex IAT becomes capable of discriminating offenders Additionally, there are other indirect measures that need to
at the individual level, assessors will need to know on what be tested and developed further (e.g., LDT, SPF), as well as
basis this occurs. Traditionally, the IAT was designed to assess some promising new measures that are yet to be tested with sex
associations that may underlie implicit attitudes (Greenwald offenders (e.g., the Function Acquisition Speed Test [FAST];
et al., 1998). From this perspective, the child–sex IAT arguably O’Reilly, Roche, Ruiz, Tyndall, & Gavin, 2012). In the next
measures associations that underpin attitudes about children section, we will briefly review the literature pertaining to the
and sex. Despite this, many researchers view the child–sex IAT treatment of distorted schemas.
as measuring deviant sexual interests (e.g., Banse et al., 2010).
Acknowledging the overlap, Ó Ciardha (2011) argues that the
IAT most likely assesses the schemas that underlie deviant
Treatment of Distorted Schemas
interests. Thus, more research is needed to determine the con- Most sex offender treatment programs (SOTPs) in North Amer-
struct validity of child–sex IATs. ica and the United Kingdom use a cognitive–behavioral
Fourth, it is clear that most IAT studies have focused on approach (Beech et al., 1999; McGrath, Cumming, Burchard,
assessing cognition related to sex. Even in Mihailides et al.’s Zeoli, & Ellerby, 2010), as there is strong empirical support for
(2004) study, where two nonsexual schemas were assessed its efficacy compared to other approaches (Hanson et al., 2002;
(Entitlement and Uncontrollability), the authors operationa- Lösel & Schmucker, 2005; Woodrow & Bright, 2010). The
lized each schema in a sexual manner (i.e., entitlement to sex main aim of the cognitive–behavioral approach is to change
and uncontrollability of sexuality). Therefore, future IAT stud- problematic behavior by addressing the cognitions that under-
ies should aim to assess nonsexual schemas. For example, an lie it. Thus, addressing distorted schemas, and the beliefs and
IAT designed to assess how strongly trust is associated with cognitions they generate, falls central to a cognitive–behavioral
children relative to adults may be indicative of a Dangerous approach. Surprisingly, there is a paucity of research examin-
world schema (see Gervais, 2011, e.g., trust-IAT). Further- ing the effects of cognitive–behavioral treatment on distorted
more, the focus on child–sex associations has also meant that cognition and schemas. However, some empirical evidence
schemas related to other sex offenders have been ignored. The does exist. For example, sex offenders who completed a cogni-
IAT has been used to show that rapists hold a less negative tive–behavioral program in Australia showed posttreatment
attitude toward rape than nonrapists (Hermann, McPhail, reductions on Bumby’s RAPE and MOLEST scales as well
Nunes, & Sewell, 2010), although it is unclear whether this as the subscales of Burt’s Rape Myth scale (Mamone, Keeling,
is indicative of an underlying schema (e.g., Women as sex Sleeman, & McElhone, 2002). Also, in the United Kingdom,
objects). Similarly, more attention should be paid to offender Beech et al. (2005) found that rapists and sexual murderers
heterogeneity, given that only a few IAT studies have undergoing SOTP demonstrated reduced scores on Bumby’s
addressed this issue. This is important because not all sex RAPE scale.
offenders hold distorted schemas and there is heterogeneity A common procedure for treating sex offender cognition is
within different offender groups. For example, Beech et al. ‘‘cognitive restructuring’’ (Bumby, 1996). For example, in the
(2006) found that different rapist subtypes held certain sche- United States, 90% of community programs (n ¼ 329) and 91%
mas more strongly than others. Thus, researchers should of residential programs (n ¼ 79) use cognitive restructuring
endeavor to use the IAT to assess the schemas of other sex (McGrath et al., 2010). This procedure typically involves help-
offenders and of sex offender subtypes. ing offenders identify and challenge their distorted beliefs so
To summarize, there are an array of methods that can poten- that new ways of thinking can develop. A few studies have
tially be used to identify distorted schemas. The use of inter- shown this method to be effective. For example, Bumby
views allows a range of topics to be explored, providing (1996) found that, in combined sample of child molesters and
possible clues to schematic content. Questionnaires, particu- rapists that underwent 9 months of cognitive restructuring,
larly those designed to assess all of the proposed schemas, also scores on both the RAPE and MOLEST scales decreased in the
provide some indication of distorted schemas. However, the first 3 months and again in the period between 3 and 6 months.
data from both of these approaches have to be interpreted cau- Similarly, in a sample of child molesters that underwent cogni-
tiously as they require deliberate thought processes, which can tive restructuring, Bickley and Beech (2003) found that
60 TRAUMA, VIOLENCE, & ABUSE 14(1)

approach-goal offenders showed a reduction in distorted beliefs Sex Objects, all of which manifested in their attitudes toward
about sex and children. Also, the efficacy of an SOTP designed female therapists. These schemas and the cognitions they eli-
for sex offenders with deficits in cognitive and social function- cited were described as ‘‘intractable and extremely resistant
ing—which includes a component on modifying offense- to change’’ (p. 148). However, this may have been largely due
supportive thinking—was recently evaluated (Williams, to process issues, such as offender collusion and a lack of moti-
Wakeling, & Webster, 2007). Results indicated that child vation (Eccleston & Own, 2007).
molesters showed a marked reduction in their beliefs about It should be noted, however, that schemas have been
children and sex. acknowledged as being difficult to change, particularly within
Drake, Ward, Nathan, and Lee (2001) proposed a four-step structured, time-limited programs (Mann & Shingler, 2006).
framework for cognitive restructuring designed to address Thus, while attempts are made to help offenders create new
Ward and Keenan’s five schemas that underlie distorted think- schemas, such as via role-plays, there is more emphasis on
ing patterns. They argued that distorted cognitions should first schema recognition and management (Mann & Shingler,
be elicited, for example, by having offenders describe their 2006). In other words, offenders are instilled with the knowledge
offense chain. Second, the identified cognitions should be and skills necessary to recognize occasions when a distorted
reframed as reflecting a particular schema (e.g., Dangerous schema may be activated so as to interrupt or resist its influence.
World). Third, once identified, the schemas should be Based on the limited empirical research, schema-therapy
addressed by helping offenders develop more realistic interpre- appears to be useful for treating distorted schemas. The major
tations of certain events and apply these interpretations to advantages are that it aims to directly tackle underlying sche-
future situations. The final step is to review whether any mas, as opposed to post hoc rationalizations and that it offers
change has taken place. Thus, instead of focusing solely on a more tailored approach to the treatment of offender cognition
surface-level cognitions, this form of cognitive restructuring (Beech et al., 2005, 2006). However, there are some issues that
aims to address their underlying cause. As Drake et al. should also be addressed. First, there is a clear shortage of
(2001) note, this framework is analogous to schema-focused empirical research investigating the treatment of sex offenders’
therapy (e.g., McGinn & Young, 1996). distorted schemas. Second, the results that do exist are all based
Schema therapy is form of cognitive treatment that is now upon psychometric assessment. As discussed earlier, the use of
strongly advocated for use with sex offenders, given the psychometric measures to assess schemas is highly proble-
emphasis placed on addressing distorted schemas (Mann & matic. Treatment evaluators may want to consider using indi-
Shingler, 2006). It usually involves four stages of therapy: Cog- rect measures to assess schema changes. Third, it appears as
nitive (teaching basic cognitive techniques to identify and con- though process variables can have an effect on the treatment
tradict schema-driven thoughts); Interpersonal (where the of distorted schemas (Eccleston & Owen, 2007). Some notable
clinician and group further challenge identified schema- examples include therapeutic style, group composition, thera-
driven thoughts); Experiential (e.g., having offenders role- peutic climate, and motivation. Regarding therapeutic style,
play past experiences to recognize how their schemas have Thornton, Mann, and Williams (2000) found, while hostile/
developed and guided their subsequent processing of social and confrontational therapists helped reduce offense-related
environmental information); and Behavioral (practicing new beliefs, warm/supportive therapists achieved this as well as
adaptive schemas in real life using behavioral experiments). changes in more general distorted beliefs, such as those relating
In the United Kingdom, Her Majesty’s Prison Service runs an to the mistrust of women (c.f. Women are Dangerous) and sex-
Extended SOTP that employs this form of schema-focused ual entitlement (c.f. Entitlement). In terms of group composi-
therapy (Mann & Beech, 2003) and there is some, albeit tion, it has been suggested that mixing rapists and child
limited, support for its efficacy. For example, Thornton and molesters decreases the risk that offenders will collude with
Shingler (2001) found that program completers demonstrated one another regarding their distorted beliefs (Harkins & Beech,
reductions in schema-related cognitions related to entitlement 2008). Indeed, in their rapist-only program, Eccleston and
and a view that women are deceitful. Owen (2007) observed that rapists colluded with each other
More recently, in a mixed sample of rapists and child moles- in terms of supporting their distorted beliefs about women.
ters, Barnett (2011) found that the Extended SOTP reduced However, Eccleston and Owen also note that mixing rapists
self-reported grievance thinking, particularly for those who with child molesters may lead to certain beliefs not being effec-
scored highly on the measures. This is important because tively addressed. Regarding therapeutic climate, Beech and
grievance thinking reflects a Dangerous World schema (Mann Hamilton-Giachritsis (2005) found that a reduction in
et al., 2007; Ward, 2000). For example, Beech et al. (2006) pro-offending beliefs (which included distorted beliefs about
found that grievance-motivated rapists hold Dangerous World children and sex) was correlated with higher levels of group
most strongly. In addition to these few positive findings, some cohesiveness and expressiveness (i.e., the freedom to act and
have reported less successful results. For example, Eccleston express feelings in the group). Finally, motivation has been
and Owen (2007) describe a treatment program that was devel- linked to successful changes in distorted cognition. For exam-
oped just for rapists, which involved identifying and modifying ple, Terry and Mitchell (2001) found that child molesters who
underlying schemas. In their initial group, they found evidence were not motivated to participate in treatment were less likely
for Dangerous World, Women as Dangerous and, Women as to show changes in distorted cognition.
Beech et al. 61

Fourth, there are external elements to distorted cognition relationship with a child), are blocked, or are prioritized over
that also need to be addressed. For example, Ward (2009) others. According to the GLM, treatment should therefore
argued that the internal elements (e.g., schemas) of human involve assisting offenders in devising a ‘‘good lives plan’’ and
cognition form a functional relationship with certain elements equipping them with the necessary skills to appropriately
of the external world, resulting in a hybrid cognitive system. acquire their goods. Ward and Marshall (2007) state that an
For sex offenders, external elements include offenders’ social offender’s good lives plan helps them construct a more adap-
and cultural environment; technologies such as deviant porno- tive ‘‘narrative identity’’ that involves desisting from offend-
graphy; and other people (e.g., consider the rapists’ collusion ing. Ward and Marshall note how this demonstrates peoples’
and support of one another’s distorted beliefs reported by ability to be reflective and an active agent in shaping their lives.
Eccleston and Owen, 2007). Thus, Ward advises that external Ward et al. (2007) note that many existing treatment mod-
elements should also be identified and addressed when treating ules already address an overarching good, which in the case
offenders’ distorted cognition, so that the therapeutic question of distorted schemas is the good of knowledge. This is because
turns from ‘‘How can I think differently’’ to ‘‘How can I live offenders are gaining an insight and understanding of their
differently’’ (p. 255). thinking patterns. Ward et al. also state that changing schemas
Fifth, there are some aspects of schema therapy that differ to that generate hostile beliefs (i.e., Dangerous World) will help
the recent work on cognitive structures. For example, drawing offenders acquire the good of relatedness and inner peace, as
upon early work on schema therapy, Dean et al., (2007) state they will experience more harmonious relationships and so
that therapists should help offenders weaken old schemas and experience less anger, respectively. They also argue that
strengthen new ones. However, recent research in social treating schemas such as Uncontrollability will result in the
cognition may require this to be reconceptualized. As discussed acquisition of agency, as offenders will feel more in control
earlier, activated schemas give rise to automatic beliefs that are of their lives. Importantly, agency has recently been shown
endorsed unless rejected on the basis of being false. Rejection to be a factor associated with ‘‘desistance’’ (Farmer, Beech,
of an automatic belief requires an individual to momentarily & Ward, 2012). It should be noted that Harkins, Flak, Beech,
(and consciously) reflect on other propositions (Gawronski & and Woodhams (2012) compared relapse prevention and GLM
Bodenhausen, 2006). Indeed, it has been found that schema approaches within a community-based SOTP and found no dif-
content (i.e., stereotypes) is more likely to change if individuals ferences in the proportion of individuals who demonstrated
are trained to consciously confirm counterbeliefs as opposed to treatment change for pro-offending attitudes. However, offen-
reject schema-driven beliefs (Gawronski, Deutsch, Mbirkou, ders who received GLM-based treatment did report that their
Seibt, & Strack, 2008). Thus, it may be better to think of attitudes about themselves and the future were more positive,
schema treatment as providing offenders with new, counter- which appeared to influence their motivation for future per-
schema propositions that (1) can help in rejecting schema- sonal work.
related beliefs; and (2) if frequently confirmed, will lead to Some final considerations relate to addressing schemas in an
changes in the original, distorted schema. indirect manner by treating different, yet related, factors. For
Finally, despite many advocating the targeting of schemas example, Wood and Riggs (2009) found that child molesters
(Dean et al., 2007; Drake et al., 2001; Maruna & Mann, with an insecure attachment style (both preoccupied and fear-
2006; Ó Ciardha & Gannon, 2011), it would appear that very ful) showed more distorted cognitions than those with a secure
few programs employ schema therapy in North America attachment. Thus, addressing attachment issues in therapy may
(McGrath et al., 2010). In McGrath et al.’s report, only 11% bring about positive changes in schemas. Similarly, Marziano
of 329 community programs and 9% of 79 residential pro- et al. (2006) found that child molesters who had been sexually
grams in the United States use schema therapy with adult sex abused during childhood evidenced Dangerous World more
offenders. Curiously, McGrath et al. found that 90% of the than those who had not been abused. This suggests that
community and 74% of the residential programs use schema effectively addressing early abuse may help ameliorate these
therapy with adolescents and children who have sexually schemas (see Ricci, Clayton, & Shapiro, 2006). Also, addres-
offended. Thus, more programs need to start incorporating sing self-regulatory problems that increase the probability of
schema therapy for adults. acting on schema-driven beliefs/thoughts (e.g., impulsivity;
There are also some other relevant considerations to note. Mann & Beech, 2003) is likely to be a useful strategy for
For example, there has been increasing recognition that sex schema management.
offender treatment should be delivered in a manner that
promotes the values and rights of the offender, such as that
outlined by the Good Lives model (GLM; Ward, Mann, &
Conclusions
Gannon, 2007; Ward & Stewart, 2003). The GLM states that It has been proposed that when assessing and treating sex
human beings all seek a set of primary ‘‘goods,’’ which Ward offenders’ cognition, therapists should target the underlying
and Maruna (2007) define as states of mind, personal character- schemas that generate distorted beliefs rather than focus on
istics, activities, or experiences that are sought for their own postoffense rationalizations. Thus, the aim of this review was
sake. For sex offenders, certain goods are sought in an inap- to examine what is currently known about the assessment and
propriate manner (e.g., seeking relatedness through a sexual treatment of sex offenders’ schemas. Current work from
62 TRAUMA, VIOLENCE, & ABUSE 14(1)

social–cognitive psychology was drawn upon and referred to in assessment tools and more effective treatment strategies can be
order to facilitate informative discussions on the two topics. developed.
With regard to schema assessment, there are a number of avail-
able methodologies, with psychometrics being the most widely
used. However, an important observation is that general, nonsex- Key Points of the Research Review
ual schemas are greatly underrepresented on many self-report
measures. This is an important point because such schemas pro-  Schemas are argued to be the most important factor when
mote general antisociality, which is a strong predictor of sexual dealing with sex offenders distorted cognition in treatment.
recidivism. Thus, by not being sufficiently assessed, offenders  In general, questionnaires are the most common strategy for
who harbor nonsexual distorted schemas may leave therapy with assessing distorted cognition. However, they may be less
important criminogenic factors unaddressed. Recent measures effective at specifically assessing schemas because respon-
such as the ‘‘My Life Questionnaire’’ offer some promise for dents have time to deliberate and reject their schema-driven
assessing nonsexual schemas. Also, while psychometrics offer beliefs.
some indication of schema content, it is difficult to differentiate  Indirect measures may offer a better indication of schema
low endorsements based on propositional (conscious) processing content as they reduce the time needed to engage in
from low endorsements based dishonesty. A more promising deliberated thought. The IAT is the most commonly
route may be to use indirect measures, as they are less affected used indirect measure. Results from IAT studies suggest
by these two factors. Thus, researchers should continue in to that many child abusers hold associations indicative of
design, test, and develop a reliable and standardized indirect ‘‘child as sexual beings’’ schema.
measure.  There is limited empirical research related to the treatment
The empirical literature on treating distorted schemas is a lot of schemas. Cognitive restructuring and schema therapy
less than that on schema assessment. However, there is some both show some success as reducing scores on question-
indication that schema-focused therapy can reduce distorted, naires, suggesting that a change in schema content or an
schema-driven beliefs that are measured psychometrically. increase in schema management has occurred.
However, as to whether these findings reflect a change in  Efforts have been made to better the treatment of distorted
schema content is difficult to tell, given the issues surrounding cognition/schemas. These include attending to the external
the use of psychometrics to assess schemas. Some recent sug- world in which offenders reside; adopting a strengths-based
gestions have been made that may increase the efficacy of approach to treatment; addressing relevant process issues;
schema-focused therapy. These include addressing external and treating factors associated with distorted schemas
elements that are integrated into offenders’ cognitive system, (e.g., attachment issues, early sexual abuse).
and delivering cognitive treatment in an approach-goal manner
that focuses on promoting clients’ strengths (i.e., acquiring pri-
mary goods). The idea of helping offenders become more
reflective in order for them to construct an adaptive narrative Implications for Practice, Policy, and
identity may be a particularly useful treatment strategy for Research
treating distorted cognition. This is because it shares some par-
Practice and policy
allels with the social–cognitive idea of overriding schema-
driven beliefs by considering alternate propositions (a reflective  Clinicians should remain mindful of schema-driven beliefs
process). For instance, a person who has constructed a ‘‘healthy and beliefs based upon conscious evaluation.
narrative identity’’ may reject/resist positive, schema-driven  Indirect measures should be more commonly used to assess
beliefs toward chocolate using new propositions related to their distorted schemas.
new identity (e.g., I want to get healthy). Arguably, sex offenders  To gain a fuller understanding of an offender’s cognition,
with a new ‘‘redemptive narrative identity’’ will be able to do the clinicians should assess both sexual and nonsexual
same in relation to their schema-driven beliefs about children/ schemas.
women.  Cognitive restructuring procedures should involve target-
Other research indicates that certain process issues (e.g., ing schemas as opposed to surface-level cognition. Also,
motivation, therapeutic style) are important to consider when more treatment programs should strive to incorporate
treating distorted beliefs, as are factors empirically associated schema therapy.
with distorted cognition (e.g., attachment, sexual abuse).  To potentially increase the efficacy of schema therapy,
Finally, it should also be noted that not all sex offenders show clinicians should attend to external elements related to dis-
evidence of distorted cognition. Thus, schema therapy should torted cognition; emphasize the attainment of ‘‘goods’’; and
only really be offered to those who demonstrate schema- address important process issues.
driven beliefs. In sum, the treatment of distorted schemas is
an important and promising therapeutic goal. However, a lot
Research
of more work in this area needs to be done to better understand  More work is needed to understand, develop, and validate a
the nature of sex offender cognition so that more sophisticated standardized indirect measure.
Beech et al. 63

 Researchers should aim to develop direct and indirect mea- Beech, A. R., Fisher, D., & Beckett, R. C. (1999). Step 3: An evalua-
sures that assess nonsexual schemas, such as dangerous tion of the prison sex offender treatment programme. Home office
world. occasional report. London, England: Home Office Publications
 A lot more empirical research is needed regarding the eva- Unit. Retrieved from www.homeoffice.gov.uk/rds/pdfs/occ-step3.
luation of treatment strategies, such as cognitive restructur- pdf
ing and schema therapy. Beech, A. R., Fisher, D., & Ward, T. (2005). Sexual murderers’ impli-
 Researchers should draw more upon the wealth of relevant cit theories. Journal of Interpersonal Violence, 20, 1366-1389.
research in cognitive and social–cognitive psychology, as it Beech, A. R., & Hamilton-Giachritsis, C. E. (2005). Relationship
is likely to open new ideas and avenues related to schema between therapeutic climate and treatment outcome in a group-
assessment and treatment. based sexual offender program. Sexual Abuse: A Journal of
Research and Treatment, 17, 127.
Beech, A. R., Oliver, C., Fisher, D., & Beckett, R. C. (2005). Step 4:
Declaration of Conflicting Interests
The sex offendertreatment programme in prison: Addressing the
The authors declared no potential conflicts of interest with respect to needs of rapists and sexual murderers. Birmingham, UK: Univer-
the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article. sity of Birmingham.
Beech, A. R., Parrett, N., Fisher, D., & Ward, T. (2009). Assessing
Funding female offenders’ motivations and cognitions: An exploratory
The authors received no financial support for the research, authorship, study. Psychology, Crime and Law, 15, 201-217.
and/or publication of this article. Beech, A. R., Ward, T., & Fisher, D. (2006). The identification of sex-
ual and violent motivations in men who assault women: Implica-
Note tions for treatment. Journal of Interpersonal Violence, 21,
1635-1653.
1. To remain consistent, we will continue to use the term schema
Bem, S. L. (1981). Gender schema theory: A cognitive account of sex
throughout the article.
typing. Psychological Review, 88, 354-364.
Bennett, A. H. (2011). Assessing the implicit theories and motivations
References of rapists, pedophilic child molesters, and mixed sexual offenders.
Abel, G. G., Becker, J. V., & Cunningham-Rathner, J. (1984). Compli- Unpublished master’s thesis. The University of British Columbia,
cations, consent, and cognitions in sex between children and Okanagan.
adults. International Journal of Law and Psychiatry, 7, 89-103. Bickley, J. A., & Beech, A. R. (2003). Implications for treatment of
Abel, G. G., Gore, D. K., Holland, C. L., Camp, N., Becker, J. V., & sexual offenders of the Ward and Hudson model of relapse. Sexual
Rathner, J. (1989). The measurement of the cognitive distortions of Abuse: A Journal of Research and Treatment, 15, 121-134.
child molesters. Annals of Sex Research, 2, 135-153. Brown, A. S., Gray, N. S., & Snowden, R. J. (2009). Implicit measure-
Babchishin, K. M., Nunes, K. L., & Hermann, C. A. (2012). The ment of associations in child sex abusers: Role of victim type and
validity of implicit association test (IAT) measures of sexual denial. Sexual Abuse: A Journal of Research and Treatment, 21,
attraction to children: A meta-analysis. Archives of Sexual 166-180.
Behavior. doi: 10.1007/s10508-012-0022-8 Bumby, K. M. (1996). Assessing the cognitive distortions of child
Banse, R., Schmidt, A. F., & Clarbour, J. (2010). Indirect measures of molesters and rapists: Developments and validation of the molest
sexual interest in child sex offenders: A multi-method approach. and rape scales. Sexual Abuse: A Journal of Research and Treat-
Criminal Justice and Behavior, 37, 319-335. ment, 8, 3754.
Bar-Anan, Y., Nosek, B. A., & Vianello, M. (2009). The sorting paired Burt, M. R. (1980). Cultural myths and supports for rape. Journal of
features task: A measure of association strengths. Experimental Personality and Social Psychology, 38, 217-230.
Psychology, 56, 329-343. Check, J., Malamuth, N., Elias, B., & Barton, S. (1985). On hostile
Barnes-Holmes, D., Barnes-Holmes, Y., Power, P., Hayden, E., Milne, ground. Psychology Today, 19, 56-61.
R., & Stewart, I. (2006). Do you know what you believe? Develop- Craig, L. A., & Beech, A. R. (2009). Psychometric assessment of
ing the implicit relational assessment procedure as a direct measure sexual deviance. In: A. R. Beech, L. A. Craig, & K. D. Browne
of implicit beliefs. Irish Psychologist, 32, 169-177. (Eds.), Assessment and treatment of sex offenders: A handbook
Barnett, G. D. (2011). What is grievance thinking and how can we (pp. 89-107). Chichester, England: Wiley.
measure this in sexual offenders? Legal and Criminological Psy- Craig, L. A., Thornton, D., Beech, A. R., & Browne, K. D. (2007).
chology, 16, 37-61. The relationship of statistical and psychological risk markers to
Bartels, R. M, Harkins, L., & Beech, A. R. (in preparation). Applying sexual reconviction. Criminal Justice and Behavior, 34,
the sorting paired features task to investigate child abuser’ 314-329.
cognition. Dawson, D. L., Barnes-Holmes, D., Gresswell, D. M., Hart, A. J. P., &
Beckett, R. C. (1987). The Children and sex questionnaire. Available Gore, N. J. (2009). Assessing the implicit beliefs of sexual offen-
from Richard Beckett, Centre for Forensic and Criminological ders using the implicit relational assessment procedure: A first
Psychology, School of Psychology, University of Birmingham, study. Sexual Abuse: A Journal of Research and Treatment, 21,
Edgbaston, Birmingham, B15 2TT, UK. 57-75.
64 TRAUMA, VIOLENCE, & ABUSE 14(1)

Dean, C., Mann, R. E., Milner, R., & Maruna, S. (2007). Changing Gervais, W. M. (2011). Finding the faithless: Perceived atheist
child molesters cognitions. In T. A. Gannon, T. Ward, A. R. Beech, prevalence reduces anti-atheist prejudice. Personality and Social
& D. Fisher (Eds.), Aggressive offender’ cognitions: Theory, Psychology Bulletin, 37, 543-556.
research and practice (pp. 117-134). Chichester, England: Wiley. Gilbert, D. T. (1991). How mental systems believe. American
De Houwer, J., & Moors, A. (2010). Implicit measures: Similarities Psychologist, 46, 107-119.
and differences. In B. Gawronski & B. K. Payne (Eds.), Handbook Goddard, H. L. (2006). Assessing the implicit theories of child moles-
of social cognition: Measurement, theory, and applications (pp. ters: Development and validation of the sex offenders’ implicit the-
176-193). New York, NY: Guilford. ories scale. Unpublished undergraduate thesis. University of
Drake, C. R., Ward, T., Nathan, P., & Lee, J. K. P. (2001). Challenging Birmingham, UK.
the cognitive distortions of child molesters: An implicit theory Gray, N. S., Brown, A. S., MacCulloch, M. J., Smith, J., & Snowden, R.
approach. Journal of Sexual Aggression, 7, 25-40. J. (2005). An implicit test of the association between children and
Eccleston, L., & Owen, K. (2007). Cognitive treatment just for rapists: sex in pedophiles. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 114, 304308.
Recent developments. In T. A. Gannon, T. Ward, A. R. Beech, & Greenwald, A. G., McGhee, J. L., & Schwartz, J. L. (1998). Measuring
D. Fisher (Eds.), Aggressive offenders’ cognition: Theory, individual difference in implicit cognition: The implicit associa-
research, and treatment (pp. 135-153). Chichester, England: tion test. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74,
Wiley. 1464-1480.
Farmer, M., Beech, A. R., & Ward, T. (2012). Assessing desistance in Hanson, R. K., Gizzarelli, R., & Scott, H. (1994). The attitudes of
child molesters: A qualitative analysis. Journal of Interpersonal incest offenders: Sexual entitlement and acceptance of sex with
Violence, 25, 930-950. children. Criminal Justice and Behavior, 21, 187-202.
Fiske, S. T., & Taylor, S. E. (1984). Social cognition New York, NY: Hanson, R. K., Gordon, A., Harris, A. J. R., Marques, J. K., Murphy,
McGraw-Hill. W., Quinsey, V. L., & Seto, M. C. (2002). First report of the Col-
Gannon, T. A., Hoare Rose, M. R., & Parrett, N. (2012). Re- laborative Outcome Project on the effectiveness of psychological
examination of female child molesters’ implicit theories: Evidence treatment for sex offenders. Sexual Abuse: A Journal of Research
of female specificity? Psychology Crime and Law, 18, 209-224. and Treatment, 14, 159-194.
Gannon, T. A., Keown, K., & Polaschek, D. L. L. (2007). Increasing Hanson, R. K., & Morton-Bourgon, K. E. (2005). The characteristics
honest responding on cognitive distortions in child molesters: The of persistent sexual offenders: A meta-analysis of recidivism stud-
bogus pipeline revisited. Sexual Abuse: A Journal of Research and ies. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 73,
Treatment, 19, 5-22. 1154-1163.
Gannon, T. A., Keown, K., & Rose, M. R. (2009a). An examination of Harkins, L., & Beech, A. R. (2008). Examining the impact of mixing
current psychometric assessments of child molesters‘ offense- child molesters and rapists in group-based cognitive-behavioral
supportive beliefs using ward‘s implicit theories. International treatment for sexual offenders. International Journal of Offender
Journal of Offender Therapy and Comparative Criminology, 53, Therapy and Comparative Criminology, 52, 31-45.
316-333. Harkins, L., Flak, V. E., Beech, A. R., & Woodhams, J. (2012). Eva-
Gannon, T. A., & O’Connor, A. (2011). The development of the inter- luation of a community-based sex offender treatment program
est in child molestation scale. Sexual Abuse: A Journal of Research using a good lives model approach. Sexual Abuse: A Journal of
and Treatment, 23, 474-493. Research and Treatment, 1-25. [Available online].
Gannon, T. A., Rose, M. R., & Williams, S. E. (2009b). Do female Hermann, C. A., Babchishin, K. M., Nunes, K. L., Leth-Steensen, C.,
child molesters hold implicit associations between children and & Cortoni, F. (2012). Factor structure of the bumby rape scale: A
sex? A preliminary investigation. Journal of Sexual Aggression, two-factor model. Psychology, Crime, and Law. Advanced online
15, 55-61. publication. doi:10.1177/0093854812436802
Gawronski, B., & Bodenhausen, G. V. (2006) Associative and propo- Hermann, C. A., McPhail, I. V., Nunes, K. L., & Sewell, R. (2010).
sitional processes in evaluation: An integrative review of implicit The effect of sexual arousal on rapists’ implicit and explicit atti-
and explicit attitude change. Psychological Bulletin, 132, tudes toward rape. Paper presented at the 29th Annual Research
692-731. and Treatment Conference of the Association for the Treatment
Gawronski, B., & Bodenhausen, G. V. (2007). Unraveling the pro- of Sexual Abusers, Phoenix, Arizona, October 2010.
cesses underlying evaluation: Attitudes from the perspective of the Howitt, D., & Sheldon, K. (2007). The role of cognitive distortions in
APE model. Social Cognition, 25, 687-717. paedophilic offending; Internet and contact offenders compared.
Gawronski, B., & De Houwer, J. (in press). Implicit measures in social Psychology, Crime and Law, 13, 469486.
and personality psychology. In H. T. Reis & C. M. Judd (Eds.), Jones, J., & Vess, J. (2010). Implicit theories and personality patterns
Handbook of research methods in social and personality psychol- in child-victim sex offenders. Sexual Abuse in Australia and New
ogy (2nd ed.). New York, NY: Cambridge University Press. Zealand, 2, 58-65.
Gawronski, B., Deutsch, R., Mbirkou, S., Seibt, B., & Strack, F. Kahneman, D. (2011). Thinking, fast and slow. New York, NY: Farrar,
(2008). When ‘‘just say no’’ is not enough: Affirmation versus Straus and Giroux.
negation training and the reduction of automatic stereotype Keown, K., Gannon, T. A., & Ward, T. (2008). What were they think-
activation. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 44, ing? An exploration of child sexual offenders’ beliefs using a lex-
370-377. ical decision task. Psychology, Crime and Law, 14,317-337.
Beech et al. 65

Keown, K., Gannon, T. A., & Ward, T. (2010). What’s in a measure? Milner, R., & Webster, S. (2005). Identifying schemas in child moles-
A multi-method study of child sexual offenders’ beliefs. Psychol- ters, rapists, and violent offenders. Sexual Abuse: A Journal of
ogy, Crime and Law, 16, 125-143. Research and Treatment, 17, 425-438.
Lindsay, W. R., Whitefield, E., & Carson, D. (2007). An assessment Moran, Y., & Bar-Anan, S. K. (2012). The effect of object-valence
for attitudes consistent with sexual offending for use with relations on automatic evaluation. Cognition and Emotion. doi:
offenders with intellectual disabilities. Legal and Criminological 10.1080/02699931.2012.732040
Psychology, 12, 55-68. Murnen, C., Wright, G., & Kaluzny, H. R. (2002). If ‘‘boys will be
Lösel, F., & Schmucker, M. (2005). The effectiveness of treatment for boys,’’ then girls will be victims? A meta-analytic review of the
sexual offenders: A comprehensive meta-analysis. Journal of research that relates masculine ideology to sexual aggression. Sex
Experimental Criminology, 1, 117-146. Roles: A Journal of Research, 46, 359-375.
Mamone, N., Keeling, J., Sleeman, V., & McElhone, M. (2002). A Nichols, I., & Molinder, K. L. (1984). Multiphasic Sex Inventory
preliminary evaluation of the impact of an intensive treatment pro- Manual. Available from Nichols and Molinder, 437 Bowes Drive,
gramme on the cognitive distortions of sexual offenders. Paper pre- Tacoma, WA, USA.
sented at the Second Biennial Conference of the Australian and Nunes, P., Firestone, M. W., & Baldwin, C. (2007). Indirect
New Zealand Association for the Treatment of Sexual Abusers, assessment of cognitions of child sexual abusers with the
Sydney, Australia. Implicit Association Test. Criminal Justice and Behavior, 34,
Mann, R. E., & Beech, A. R. (2003). Cognitive distortions, schemas, 454-475.
and implicit theories. In T. Ward, D. R. Laws, & S. M. Hudson Ó Ciardha, C. (2011). A theoretical framework for understanding
(Eds.), Sexual deviance: Issues and controversies (pp. 135-153). deviant sexual interest and cognitive distortions as overlapping
Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. constructs contributing to sexual offending against children.
Mann, R. E., Hanson, R. K., & Thornton, D. (2010). Assessing risk for Aggression and Violent Behavior, 16, 493-502.
sexual recidivism: Some proposals on the nature of psychologi- Ó Ciardha, T. A., & Gannon, A. (2011). The cognitive distortions of
cally meaningful risk factors. Sexual Abuse: A Journal of Research child molesters are in need of treatment. Journal of Sexual Aggres-
and Treatment, 22, 191-217. sion, 17, 130-141.
Mann, R. E., & Hollin, C. (2010). Self-reported schemas in sexual O’Reilly, B., Roche, M. R., Ruiz, I., Tyndall, A., & Gavin, S. (2012).
offenders. Journal of Psychiatry and Psychology, 21, 834-851. The function acquisition speed test (FAST): A behavior-analytic
Mann, R. E., & Shingler, J. (2006). Schema-driven cognition in sexual implicit test for assessing stimulus relations. The Psychological
offenders: Theory, assessment and treatment. In W. L. Marshall, Y. Record, 62, 507-528.
M. Fernandez, L. E. Marshall, & G. A. Serran (Eds.), Sexual offen- Pervan, M., & Hunter, D. L. L. (2007). Cognitive distortions and
der treatment: Controversial issues (pp. 173-185). Chichester, UK: social self-esteem in sexual offenders. Applied Psychology in
Wiley. Criminal Justice, 3, 75-91.
Mann, R., Webster, S. D., Wakeling, H. C., & Marshall, W. L. (2007). Polaschek, T. A., & Gannon, D. L. L. (2004). The implicit theories of
The measurement and influence of child sexual abuse supportive rapists: What convicted offenders tell us. Sexual Abuse: A Journal
beliefs. Psychology, Crime and Law, 13, 443-458. of Research and Treatment, 16, 299-315.
Maruna, S., & Mann, R. E. (2006). A fundamental attribution error? Polaschek, T., & Ward, R. J. (2002). The implicit theories of potential
Rethinking cognitive distortions. Legal and Criminological Psy- rapists: What our questionnaires tell us. Aggression and Violent
chology, 11, 155-177. Behavior, 7, 385-406.
Marziano, V., Ward, T., Beech, A. R., & Pattison, P. (2006). Identifi- Ricci, C. A., Clayton, F., & Shapiro, A. (2006). Some effects of
cation of five fundamental implicit theories underlying cognitive EMDR treatment with previously abused child molesters:
distortions in child molesters: A preliminary study. Crime, Theoretical reviews and preliminary findings. Journal of Forensic
Psychology and Law, 12, 97-105. Psychiatry and Psychology, 17, 538-562.
McGinn, L. K., & Young, J. E. (1996). Schema-focused therapy. Salter, R. L. (1988). Treating child sex offenders and victims: A prac-
Frontiers of Cognitive Therapy, 35, 63-70. tical guide. Newbury Park, CA: Sage.
McGrath, R. J., Cumming, G. F., Burchard, B. L., Zeoli, S., & Ellerby, Scott, L. A., & Tetreault, D. (1987). Attitudes of rapists and other
L. (2010). Current practices and emerging trends in sexual abuser violent offenders toward women. Journal of Social Psychology,
management: The safer society 2009 North American Survey. 127, 375-380.
Brandon, VT: Safer Society Press. Scully, B. (1990). Understanding sexual violence: A study of
Messner, C., & Vosgerau, J. (2010). Cognitive inertia and the implicit convicted rapists. Boston, MA: Unwin Hyman.
association test. Journal of Marketing Research, 47, 374-386. Shiv, A., & Fedorikhin, W. (1999). Heart and mind in conflict: The
Meyer, D. E., & Schvaneveldt, R. W. (1971). Facilitation in recog- interplay of affect and cognition in consumer decision making.
nizing pairs of words: Evidence of a dependence between Journal of Consumer Research, 26, 278-292.
retrieval operations. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 90, Smid, R. J. (2010). Deriving implicit theories from file
227-234. information and establishing relationships to recidivism.
Mihailides, S., Devilly, G. J., & Ward, T. (2004). Implicit cognitive Paper presented at the 11th conference of the International
distortions and sexual offending. Sexual Abuse: A Journal of Association for the Treatment of Sexual Offenders, Oslo,
Research and Treatment, 16, 333-350. Norway.
66 TRAUMA, VIOLENCE, & ABUSE 14(1)

Snowden, R. L., Craig, N. S., & Gray, J. T. (2011). Indirect behavioral Ward, C. A., & Stewart, F. (2003). Criminogenic needs and human
measures of cognition among sexual offenders. Journal of Sex needs: A theoretical model. Psychology, Crime and Law, 9, 125-143.
Research, 48, 192-217. Williams, H., Wakeling, S., & Webster, E. (2007). A psychometric
Spence, R., Helmreich, J., & Stapp, M. C. (1973). A short version of study of six self-report measures for use with sexual offender with
the attitudes toward women scale (AWS). Bulletin of the Psycho- cognitive and social functioning deficits. Psychology Crime and
nomic Society, 2, 219-220. Law, 13, 505-522.
Steffens, E., Yundina, M., & Panning, K. J. (2008). Automatic Wood, S., & Riggs, A. (2009). Adult attachment, cognitive distor-
associations with ‘erotic’ in child sexual offenders: Identifying tions, and views of self, others, and the future among child moles-
those in danger of reoffense. Sexual Offender Treatment, 3, 1-9. ters. Sexual Abuse: A Journal of Research and Treatment, 21,
Terry, E. W., & Mitchell, D. (2001). Motivation and sex offender 375-390.
treatment efficacy: Leading a horse to water and making it drink? Woodrow, D., & Bright (2010). Effectiveness of a sex offender treat-
International Journal of Offender Therapy and Comparative ment program: A risk band analysis. International Journal of
Criminology, 45, 663-672. Offender Therapy and Comparative Criminology, 55, 43-55.
Thornton, D. (2002). Constructing and testing a framework for
dynamic risk assessment. Sexual Abuse: A Journal of Research and
Treatment, 14, 139-154. Bios
Thornton, R. E., Mann, F., & Williams, D. (2000). Therapeutic style Anthony R. Beech is the Head of the Centre for Forensic and Crim-
in sex offender treatment. Unpublished paper available from: HM inological Psychology at the University of Birmingham, UK. He has
Prison Service, Room 725, Abel House, London, SW1P 4LH, authored over 150 peer-reviewed articles, 40 book chapters, and 6
UK. books in the area of forensic science/criminal justice. His particular
Thornton, J., & Shingler, A. (2001). Impact of schema level work on research interests include risk assessment, investigating the neurobio-
sexual offenders’ cognitive distortions. Paper presented at the logical bases of offending, and increasing the effectiveness of the
20th Annual Research and Treatment Conference for the Treat- treatment given to offenders.
ment of Sexual Abusers, San Antonio, TX, October 2001. Ross M. Bartels is a doctoral student in the Centre for Forensic and
Tversky, D., & Kahneman, T. (1973). Availability: A heuristic for Criminological Psychology at the University of Birmingham, UK.
judging frequency and probability. Cognitive Psychology, 5, He earned his master’s degree in Forensic Psychology at the Univer-
207-232. sity of Kent, where he also worked as a research assistant. His research
Ward, T. (2000). Sexual offenders’ cognitive distortions as implicit interests focus on distorted cognition and interests in sex offenders,
theories. Aggression and Violent Behavior, 5, 491-507. with a particular emphasis on sexual fantasies. Drawing upon concepts
Ward, T. (2009). The extended mind theory of cognitive distortions in and research from within sociocognitive psychology, his research
aims to understand the processes that underlie sex offenders’ implicit
sex offenders. Journal of Sexual Aggression, 15, 247-259.
and explicit cognition, with the hope of informing future research, the-
Ward, S. M., Hudson, L., Johnston, W. L., & Marshall, T. (1997).
ory, and practice.
Cognitive distortions in sex offenders: An integrative review.
Clinical Psychology Review, 17, 479-507. Louise Dixon is a senior lecturer at the Centre for Forensic and Crim-
Ward, T., & Keenan, T. (1999). Child molesters’ implicit theories. inological Psychology, University of Birmingham, UK. She is also a
Journal of Interpersonal Violence, 14, 821-838. registered Forensic Psychologist with expertise in working with vio-
Ward, R. E., Mann, T. A., & Gannon, T. (2007). The good lives model lent and sexually violent offenders. Her research interests primarily
center on the theory, assessment, and prevention of family violence.
of offender rehabilitation: Clinical implications. Aggression and
Recent work has focused on furthering theoretical debate into the
Violent Behavior, 12, 87-107.
etiology of intimate partner violence, understanding patterns of family
Ward, W. L., & Marshall, T. (2007). Narrative identity and offender violence perpetration, and the co-occurrence of partner and child mal-
rehabilitation. International Journal of Offender Therapy and treatment in the family. She has published many articles and book
Comparative Criminology, 51, 279-297. chapters in these domains and is awaiting the publication of her coe-
Ward, S., & Maruna, T. (2007). Rehabilitation: Beyond the risk dited book What Works in Offender Rehabilitation: An Evidenced
assessment paradigm. London, England: Routledge. Based Approach to Assessment and Treatment published by Wiley.
Copyright of Trauma, Violence & Abuse is the property of Sage Publications Inc. and its content may not be
copied or emailed to multiple sites or posted to a listserv without the copyright holder's express written
permission. However, users may print, download, or email articles for individual use.

You might also like