You are on page 1of 25

Science Studies 2/2006

Animating Mechanism:
Animations and the Propagation of Affect in
the Lively Arts of Protein Modelling

Natasha Myers

In the scientific literature, proteins are frequently figured as molecular machines; that
is, as tiny mechanisms that operate in interlocking assemblages, and which act to
build and maintain the body as a higher-order machine. Mechanical models parse
living bodies in ways that seem, at first glance, to deaden lively processes. I build on
feminist contributions to the science studies literature to show how, rather than spell-
ing the “death of nature”, mechanistic reasoning in the life sciences can become a site
for feminist inquiry into modes of embodiment and the role of affect in the perform-
ance of scientific knowledge. I observe that researchers use their bodies kinaestheti-
cally to manipulate and learn protein structures. Such forms of body-work enable
modellers to animate their molecular mechanisms both onscreen and through elabo-
rate gestures and affects. In this way molecular mechanisms are enlivened as they
are propagated between researchers in pedagogical and professional contexts. I ar-
gue that this is not an extra-scientific phenomenon, but one intrinsic to the work of
mechanistic modelling.

Keywords: molecular models, animations, affect

Introduction cell surface proteins determined that


these molecules are long and straight.
During a series of interviews at a protein One part of the protein is embedded in
crystallography lab, a postdoctoral re- the cellular membrane, while the other
searcher demonstrated a molecular extends out into the extra-cellular envi-
mechanism he had worked out for inter- ronment where it is available to bind to
cellular adhesion. This is a mechanism similar molecules on adjacent cells. The
that operates between cells, and makes binding end of the protein has three
use of inter-locking proteins to maintain short protrusions that give it a ratcheted
the structural integrity of developing tis- structure. He hypothesized that this
sues. His structural study of a group of ratcheted structure provides a mecha-

Science Studies, Vol. 19(2006) No. 2, 6–30


Natasha Myers

nism to strengthen binding between structural biology. This crystallo-


adjacent cells. grapher’s body has become a key re-
We are in his lab, and I sit across from source for him to be able to make argu-
him as he tells me how his protein works. ments about molecular mechanisms.
I am busy scribbling notes in my note- His body is invested in his interpretation
book as he talks with barely enough time of protein structures, and the forms and
watch how he is demonstrating the potential functions of these proteins
structure. “Here, take my hand”, he says. animate his imagination. He in turn ani-
With this, I look up. “As if we were shak- mates his hypothesis by entangling us
ing hands.” I have to drop my notebook both in this lively demonstration of his
and pen in my lap, so that I can reach model. More than a pedagogical trick, I
out my hand, apologetic for having been argue that this bodily intuition has
so distracted by my note taking. He formed the foundation for his scientific
wants to convey the strength of the as- questions and committed him to several
sociations made between molecules years worth of research into these inter-
whose binding holds two adjacent cells molecular interactions. Despite little evi-
together. We clasp hands in a firm hand- dence to support his theory – that these
shake, but he leans back. I’m unprepared proteins bind to each other using all
for this, and our hands slip apart. “How three hooks – he still holds out hope that
would we make our grip stronger?” he he might one day find the crystal struc-
queries. “Suppose we are climbing a tures that can validate this feeling he has
mountain, what kind of grip would we for the strength of these molecular as-
need?” Still holding hands, he eases me sociations.
into an answer by gripping me at the His animation of the mechanism
wrist. I follow along, and clasp his wrist seems at first to contradict the tropes
in turn. We both lean away. Our grasp is and registers in which proteins are typi-
decidedly stronger. “Right”, I confirm. cally figured in the scientific literature.
Molecules binding at their first and sec- Proteins are frequently described as
ond hooks would form a stronger bond. “molecular machines”, “the machinery
“And how would we make it even of life”, “molecular devices and compo-
stronger?” He extends his grip further up nents” or even as “nature’s robots” (e.g.
my arm, clasping me at my elbow. I fol- Tanford and Reynolds, 2001). They are
low suit and we test the strength, mutu- figured as the inanimate mechanical le-
ally acknowledging the augmented sta- vers, hinges, locks, clamps, cogs, gears,
bility of this third hold. springs, pumps, and motors that assem-
Ratcheting up the grip, from binding ble, disassemble and reassemble in
at the hands, to the wrists, to the elbows, complex interlocking assemblages, and
he has sculpted a model for strong mo- which act to build and maintain the
lecular association by using the physi- body as a higher order machine (e.g. Hill
cal intuition of his body. By enlisting my and Rich, 1983; Bourne, 1986; Hoffman,
participation in this performance of his 1991; Kreisberg et al., 2002; Harrison,
model he interrupts my note taking and 2004; Chiu et al., 2005). Protein model-
redirects my ethnographic attention to- lers’ pervasive mechanistic rhetoric
wards the body-work of modelling in could be read as an attempt to eradicate

7
Science Studies 2/2006

vestiges of vitalism from biological ex- ety of media, including their own bod-
planations, to police rampant anthropo- ies, to animate chemical and physical
morphisms, and effectively reduce processes at the molecular scale. I have
messy systems to deterministic logic. conducted ethnographic interviews with
However, this crystallographer’s per- principal investigators, postdocs and
formance forces me to take a closer look graduate students in protein structure
at the nature of mechanistic reasoning laboratories, as well as with instructors,
and machine metaphors in biology. As teaching assistants and undergraduate
he performs them, mechanistic models students in teaching laboratories. In pro-
are more than deterministic abstrac- fessional contexts, I observe researchers
tions that explain away lively processes as they work with protein models at
by reducing them to their physical and computer interfaces, and as they relay
chemical properties. structural information to others in vari-
In this paper, I aim to show how, ous sites, including at the laboratory
rather than spelling the “death of nature” bench, in weekly lab meetings, and dur-
(see Merchant, 1983), mechanism in the ing conference talks and poster sessions.
life sciences might be an interesting site In pedagogical contexts, I have observed
for feminist analyses of scientific prac- semester-long graduate and under-
tice. Rather than deadening life, these graduate courses that teach concepts in
researchers’ expressive performances protein structure, including lecture
show up what Donna Haraway has courses on biomolecular kinetics, pro-
called the “unapologetic swerve of live- tein folding, practical macromolecular
liness” that animates both bodies and crystallography, as well as a hands-on
knowledge in-the-making (1997: 137). I laboratory course in biological engineer-
propose that with ethnographic atten- ing.
tion to the expressive body-work of mo- With the development of methods
lecular modelling, the roles of embodi- that can document the body-work of
ment, affect, and performance in scien- protein modelling and communication
tific knowledge production can be made among structural biologists, this study
visible, and a more lively account of aims for an innovation in STS analyses
mechanism in biology can emerge. of the performativity of scientific knowl-
The crystallographer described above edge. Erving Goffman (2001) has sug-
enacts his model in ways that are exem- gested that ethnographers must “tune”
plary of a kind of performativity that their bodies “in” to the daily activities
animates protein research more gener- and practices of those they study. This
ally. In this paper I draw on three years would require subjecting one’s own body
of ethnographic research among protein to the rhythms of another’s practices in
crystallographers, biological engineers order to gain a richer interpretation of
and other structural biologists who build the plays of affect, gesture and language
and use models of protein molecules. I among members in a particular group
examine modes of learning and commu- (Goffman, 2001: 154-155). In order to
nication among these modellers in re- tune myself in to the subtle body-work
search and teaching contexts, paying and tacit practices of structural biolo-
special attention to how they use a vari- gists, I draw on twenty-five years of train-

8
Natasha Myers

ing in classical ballet and contemporary modes of making, experiencing, and us-
dance, as well as three years of experi- ing models” (Griesemer, 2004: 435).
ence conducting research in molecular Modelling practices thus defy analyses
biology laboratories. These experiences that focus exclusively on scientists’
afford a kind of “situated knowledge” rhetoric, the technical production of
(Haraway, 1991) through which I observe models, or their representational status.
and interpret scientific practice. That is, The conceptualization and performance
they give me the skills to attend closely of models through researchers’ bodies
to others’ corporeal techniques, and en- calls, rather, for an ethnographic exami-
able me to draw on my own affinity for nation of the enactment of models, and
movement in order to detect, recall, and of modelling and theory-making in prac-
relay researchers’ subtle bodily affects, tice (on “enactment” see Mol, 2002;
including the tempos, rhythms, and Barad, 2003). I examine how protein
tones that propagate through their per- modelling practices can extend feminist
formances of scientific knowledge. As a theories of performativity in science, in
situated knowledge practice, my analy- particular the relations between modes
sis thus makes no attempt to mask the of embodiment, learning and commu-
ways in which I move with and am nication, and the role of affect in the
moved by life scientists’ lively practices. propagation of scientific knowledge.
Feminist scholars have made major
Producing and Performing Protein contributions to the literature on per-
Models formance and performativity in science.
This includes Judith Butler’s (1993)
As much as biologists have fetishized the analysis of the relationship between bio-
gene, historians and critics of the twen- logical sex and gender performance in
tieth century life sciences have fixed on her extension of Austinian theories of
the rhetoric of text, code and informa- performativity, and Donna Haraway’s
tion in genetics and genomics (see (1991; 1997) theory of “situated knowl-
Doyle, 1997; Keller, 1995; Kay, 2000). But edges”, which takes seriously the lively
life scientists do more than manipulate “material-semiotic” production and per-
words and DNA sequences. There is an- formance of scientific knowledge. Build-
other history to tell, one in which the ing on long-standing concerns in the
embodied nature of life science practice science studies literature with human
is perhaps more tangible. Here, I go be- and nonhuman agencies in scientific
yond an analysis of texts and inscrip- practice, Karen Barad (1996; 2003) draws
tions in molecular biology to examine on both Butler and Haraway to propose
the production, performance and a feminist theory of “agential realism”
propagation of multi-dimensional mod- that can account for the “enactment” of
els and animations of proteins in labo- scientific knowledge through the multi-
ratories and classrooms. As James ple material and conceptual agencies
Griesemer has noted, accounts of mod- involved in its production. Barad’s
els in the history of science require at- theory calls for an accounting of knowl-
tention to “gestural as well as symbolic edge production at the scale of the “phe-
knowledge and the variety of means and nomena” that are produced in experi-

9
Science Studies 2/2006

mental configurations, and so she pays nomena are the ontological inseparabil-
particular attention to the specific con- ity of agentially intra-acting ‘compo-
figurations that are set up between the nents’” (Barad, 2003: 815, emphasis as in
scientist, their apparatus for observa- the original). Barad shows how subjects
tion, and the things they observe. and objects precipitate out, as such,
In order to think through the dynamic from their experimental configurations.
relations between all agents in a labora- In other words, the “agencies” which
tory configuration, Barad distinguishes participate in experiments are them-
interaction from intra-action. For her, selves formed by each other in their in-
interaction “presumes the prior exist- tra-action. She is thus able to expand the
ence of independent entities”, and builds frame for analysis of scientific experi-
on a “Cartesian cut” that assumes an in- mentation to include the experimental
herent distinction and division between configurations of objects and appara-
subject and object in a given situation. tuses, as well as the material and discur-
Intra-action, on the other hand, “enacts sive agencies enacted by the scientist.1
an agential cut”, that is, “a local resolu- Barad’s notion of intra-action is par-
tion within the phenomenon” (Barad, ticularly illuminating for understanding
2003: 815). To elaborate her theory, the production of the visual facts of sci-
Barad extends Neils Bohr’s philosophy- ence. That is, visualizations, like protein
physics and his treatment of the wave- models, can be regarded as the products
particle duality of light, to account for of intra-actions between scientists, their
the impossibility of separating an experi- objects of analysis, and their visualiza-
mental object from the “agencies of ob- tion machinery – which includes the
servation” that draw it into view. Bohr material and semiotic technologies they
was concerned with how different labo- deploy to parse their data (on material-
ratory configurations could be used to semiosis, see Haraway, 1997). I extend
enact distinct properties of light. Light Barad’s work to understand how, in the
could be detected in the form of either entangled configurations of the life sci-
waves or particles, but never both forms ence laboratory and classroom, knowl-
at the same time. In Barad’s framework edge is enacted through affect and feel-
of intra-action, light becomes one of two ing as well as through instruments and
possible experimental objects – either a objects. So, while intra-actions can be
wave or a particle – through precise in- seen to morph the object in order to pro-
tra-actions between the scientist, their duce experimental data, one must not
agencies for observation, and the sub- assume that the human observer is left
stance subjected to experimentation. untouched. I aim to understand how, in
Thus, for her, laboratory observations their intra-actions with experimental
refer not so much to the object as such, objects and visualization media, scien-
but to the phenomenon performed at tists affect, and are affected by, scientific
the scale of the whole experimental con- knowledge as they produce and perform
figuration (Barad, 1996). For Barad, this it.
means that “phenomena do not merely I investigate the intra-actions that
mark the epistemological inseparability produce structural knowledge of protein
of ‘observer’ and ‘observed’; rather, phe- molecules. The primary objects, the

10
Natasha Myers

“epistemic things” in Hans-Jörg Rhein- how such temporally dynamic modes of


berger’s (1997) terminology, are the animation enable researchers to com-
proteinaceous substances being mod- municate more than just the form of a
elled. Yet, as invisible entities, molecules molecular mechanism. They also relay
as such are inextricably bound up with a range of affects and sensibilities that
the agencies of observation that draw enliven the model they perform. The
them into view. In this case, these agen- paper then turns to examine how such
cies include X-ray crystallographers’ ex- molecular affects are propagated
tensive assemblage of machines – in- through forms of mimetic communica-
cluding metaphors and interactive dig- tion between researchers, shaping how
ital visualization media – collectively knowledge of protein structures and
geared to produce and interpret atomic mechanisms is relayed in both profes-
resolution models of proteins as mol- sional and pedagogical settings.
ecules. Living substances are made mo-
lecular through these techniques and Sites of Intra-action in Protein
practices. The primary phenomena pro- Modelling
duced out of this intra-acting assem-
blage of human and nonhuman bodies Crystallographic modelling is a fine ex-
and machines are then interactive com- ample of intra-action in the production
puter graphic models of the atomic of visual facts in science. This visualiza-
structures of proteins. However, as I ex- tion practice involves active and pro-
amine below, these models are interac- longed handling and manipulation of
tive (they can be handled, manipulated experimental data throughout what is an
and modified), and so enable multiple often-arduous process of constructing
sites of intra-action, not only for those the model by hand (de Chadarevian,
who build them, but also for their ex- 2002) or onscreen (Myers, in press). Eric
tended users, including those who at- Francoeur and Jerome Segal (2004) have
tempt to pull these models off the screen shown how a series of computer hard-
and communicate the fine details of pro- ware and software innovations in the
tein structures to wider audiences. As 1960s and 1970s enabled protein mod-
this paper aims to show, such intra-ac- ellers to transition from building mo-
tions produce another range of phe- lecular models with physical materials
nomena, including modes of animation to using interactive computer graphics
that can bring mechanistic models to systems for the display and analysis of
life. structural data. Although modelling
After briefly describing the multiple materials have changed dramatically
sites of intra-action involved in produc- between the early days of physical mod-
ing and propagating protein models, this elling with mechanical ball and stick
paper turns to examine ways that pro- parts (Francoeur, 1997), early computer
tein modellers animate their models and graphics developers were able to pre-
mechanistic theories, with a focus on serve the materiality of physical models
situations where researchers use their by engineering workstations interactive
own bodies to communicate protein enough to give users the sensation that
structures and mechanisms. I explore they were directly manipulating the

11
Science Studies 2/2006

molecules onscreen with their hands navigate through the intricate folds of
(Langridge, 1974; 1981; Francoeur and the protein, zooming in on atomic de-
Segal, 2004; Myers, in press). tails, and rotating it through virtual
Today, the field of protein crystallo- space, it becomes a tangible object (see
graphy has been reinvigorated with aug- also Francoeur and Segal, 2004). This
mented computational power, faster in- practice constitutes a kind of body-work
teractive graphics capacity, and continu- that enables the researcher to learn the
ally improved software, facilitating the structure by incorporating the form of
production of atomic-resolution mod- the protein into their body as an “em-
els, animations and simulations that bodied model” (Myers, in press).
amplify protein structures to human The intra-actions that produce mo-
scale. Researchers also often use stere- lecular knowledge, however, do not end
oscopic visualization aids that make use at the computer interface: the details of
of 3D glasses and special graphics that a molecular structure, and hypotheses
make molecular models leap right off about how it functions, must be commu-
the screen. Through the time-consum- nicated among researchers and their
ing and physically engaging practice of students. I examine sites of social intra-
building and using protein models and action to understand how, once ac-
animations onscreen, otherwise invis- quired, structural knowledge of proteins
ible protein molecules are given body is propagated. My observations of the
and fleshed out in time. pedagogical lives of models show that
The human-computer interface that once a modeller learns the molecular
crystallographers use to build protein structure through body-work at the
models offers an exceptional site to ex- computer interface, he or she may then
amine the intra-actions that shape be able to perform the model for others
knowledge of protein structures and off-screen (see Myers, forthcoming). In-
mechanisms. Yet, once built, crystallo- deed, researchers frequently enact mo-
graphic protein models can travel: as lecular models through elaborate ges-
digital objects, they become available to tures and language in order to relay the
many other users. For example, once a specificities of molecular forms and
crystallographer builds a protein model, movements. In addition to teachers’ and
she uploads the structural data into the students’ lively performances of protein
Protein Data Bank (PDB), an online da- form in classrooms and teaching labo-
tabase. In so doing, she makes it avail- ratories, researchers also readily enact
able to a wider range of researchers, in- their embodied knowledge of molecular
cluding biological engineers, predictive structure. They do this in formal and in-
modellers, and drug designers who are formal research settings, including in
always on the lookout for new protein weekly lab meetings, at conferences, and
structures. A curious researcher will even as they chat with each other at the
download the coordinates of a protein laboratory bench. Additionally, ethno-
structure and manipulate it onscreen. As graphic interviews offer another site for
I describe elsewhere (Myers, in press), researchers to express molecular knowl-
these tools prosthetically couple the re- edge through their bodies. In each of
searcher to the model so that as they these sites, structural biologists may per-

12
Natasha Myers

form their knowledge of a protein along- “molecular biology” (Stent, 1968: 391).
side graphic renderings in order to Counter to the then nascent “informa-
elaborate a structure or its movements. tional school” that sought to reduce
In the absence of other visual media DNA to codes, the structural school ap-
their moving bodies can also become ef- proached biological molecules with the
fective stand-ins for the protein model. “idea that the physiological function of
I argue that these performative modes the cell” could be understood “only in
of body-work are also intra-active in the terms of the three-dimensional configu-
sense that they require others who can ration of its elements” (Stent, 1968: 391).
move with and be moved by these mo- This was a preoccupation that Stent saw
lecular gestures – in both the physical reflecting a “down-to-earth view of the
and affective senses of the verb “to relation of physics to biology”. In this
move” – in order for the details of the view, “all biological phenomena, no mat-
structure and hypotheses about molecu- ter what their complexity” could “ulti-
lar mechanisms to be relayed. mately be accounted for in terms of con-
ventional physical laws” (Stent, 1968:
Modelling Biological Mechanisms 391).
The contributions of structural biol-
Mechanism has held a prominent place ogy appeared to lose traction during the
in the history of biological modelling and sequencing craze of the molecular ge-
theory-making. This mode of reasoning netics and genomics revolutions, which
builds on a long history of theories and have dominated life science research
metaphors that inscribe living bodies as agendas over the past forty years (Doyle,
machines (see for example Gieson, 1969; 1997; Kay, 2000). Twenty-first-century
Hopwood, 1999; Keller, 1995; 2002; molecular biology is, however, in the
Lenoir, 1982; Pauly, 1996). Researchers midst of a protein structure revolution.2
working in the broad field of the life sci- As protein modellers ramp up the pace
ences have deployed mechanical theories of structure determination, making vis-
of biological function at many scales of ible the forms and movements of a vast
the organism, thus shaping the direction menagerie of proteins, they are produc-
of such fields as embryology and devel- ing a rich body of visual evidence that is
opment (see Hopwood, 1999) as well as fleshing out new understandings of bio-
cell biology (see Landecker, 2007). logical molecules, and enabling new
Mechanistic reasoning has also held sway lines of inquiry. An important feature of
in the field of biophysics, which includes this transition is that the nature of the
the crystallographic studies of protein substances that life science researchers
structures first initiated in the 1930s (see investigate, and the kinds of the data
de Chadarevian, 2002; Law, 1973). Indeed, they manipulate, are changing. Protein
mechanistic explanations formed the structure data defies the rhetoric of
foundation for what, in 1967, Gunter informatics, which, in the fields of genet-
Stent called the “structural school” of ics and genomics, has had the tendency
molecular biology. Until the late 1950s, to flatten life into code. By contrast, the
this school had dominated the field of practice of figuring life in structural bi-
what was then coming to be known as ology has forced researchers to confront

13
Science Studies 2/2006

the thickness and temporality of the sub- course is “measure, model, manipulate,
stances that give body to cells. and make”. He thus aims to build quan-
In the contemporary structural biol- titative models that will enable interven-
ogy literature, biological molecules are tion and re-engineering. In one sense,
frequently figured as determinate, pre- this biological engineer’s designs on life
dictable, regulate-able machines that are serve as a not so subtle reminder of the
reducible to their chemical and physi- ways that mechanistic thinking has his-
cal properties. Lecturing in a course on torically alarmed feminist theorists con-
biomolecular kinetics for biological en- cerned with the exploitation of nature
gineers-in-training, one protein model- (e.g. Merchant, 1983; Plumwood, 1993).
ler defines “mechanism” as the parsing I would, however, like to read his invo-
of a living entity, such as a cell, into dis- cation for mechanistic knowledge more
crete, interconnected units. For him, a generously.
mechanism is an abstraction that severs I want to draw attention to the kind
a larger entity into parts and orders them of understanding that he gestures to-
by their functionality, affording an effec- wards, even while he dismisses its merit.
tive means for manipulation. As a bio- Though he is not convinced it will get
logical engineer, he is invested in garner- you very far as an engineer, he does see
ing as much mechanistic knowledge that it is possible to “wrap some pretty
about his system as possible. He tells the words” around a model to aid in “de-
class: scribing” the mechanism, if only “after
the fact”. My fieldwork in his courses,
You have to get a mechanistic under-
standing of everything. Because that’s
and in group meetings among members
where the true power comes from. If of his laboratory show, however, that of-
you have a mechanistic understanding ten a protein is first modelled as a lively
you really know how it works and you body, before it becomes a mechanical
can change how it works. If you have object. Indeed, it is not only words, but
kind of a philosophical understanding
you can describe it after the fact. You bodies too, that get “wrapped” around
can wrap some pretty words around it, the model as the mechanism is concep-
but that understanding isn’t sufficient tualized and performed. My observa-
to empower you to make the system do tions suggest that modelling molecules
something different; that is, what you as complex molecular machines that
want it to do. So that’s our mantra. The
question is how deep into the mecha- take up space and move through time,
nism do you need to know? has enlisted life scientists’ own moving
bodies as resources to “give body”
The “true power” that he invokes is that
(Hopwood, 1999) to the mechanisms
ability to engineer new kinds of molecu-
they investigate, and to animate their
lar mechanisms that perform predict-
theories.3
able functions in living systems. He de-
The development and application of
sires a level of understanding that makes
a mechanical theory to a biological proc-
living processes tangible at the scale of
ess is a practice that requires postulat-
intra- and inter-molecular forces and
ing an internal teleology of things; that
energies. The “mantra” frequently re-
is, relationships between the part and
cited in this biological engineering
the whole, between structure and func-

14
Natasha Myers

tion, and between form and purpose. words and gestural forms are more than
Determining how molecules work, how aesthetic flourishes of expressive scien-
they perform their functions and inter- tists: they are integral to the very con-
act with each other in the cell (with the ception and development of mechani-
assumption that they in fact perform a cal models. Mechanistic reasoning is
kind of work), is an act of interpretation thus an intra-active, material-semiotic
– the researcher must form a hypothesis practice.
about an otherwise invisible process.
The framework that structural biologists Animating Mechanisms
draw on to make such interpretations is
clearly shaped by chemical and physi- Mechanisms, or things that operate me-
cal laws and theories. But it is also chanically, have three-dimensional,
shaped by their experience working with temporal structures, in ways similar to
models, and by analogies that produce living bodies. Mechanistic reasoning can
metonymic shifts between the scale of be understood as a practice of ascribing
human experience and that of molecu- form and teleology to an object, in such
lar life. For example, a long history of a way that accounts for how an object’s
“lock-and-key” metaphors, particularly shape changes and moves over time. A
prominent in research on antibody pro- biological mechanism, by definition, in-
teins (see Kay, 1993) has shaped the ways volves some kind of movement or
that structural biologists read mechani- change: biological substances are trans-
cal function out of structure. Currently, formed chemically and physically in the
the more pervasive analogies are those process of conducting work in the body.
of “molecular machine” which are fig- Animations or other moving images that
ured as the architectural and chemical pull entities into human time are very
“machinery” that “does work” in the cell useful visual aids for playing through the
and “drives” cellular life.4 temporal structures of mechanical ob-
I observe that in order to interpret the jects and theories. They are also excep-
functions of molecules, protein model- tional visualization tools in the life sci-
lers draw on their embodied experiences ences, as they have the capacity to con-
with human-scale mechanisms and ma- vey the pulsing rhythms, forms and
chines (both within and beyond the movements of living substances, cells
laboratory) as sources of practical logic and tissues.
and reasoning. This practical knowledge All kinds of animations have been de-
shapes how they produce and also how veloped in the history the life sciences.
they disseminate knowledge of protein For example, embryologist Wilhelm
structures and functions to others. Roux (1859-1924) employed everyday
Viewed from this perspective, mechanis- materials to create an animation that
tic modelling appears to rely on re- could defend mechanical theories of
searchers’ dexterity with theories and organismal development against Hans
language, as well as with their applica- Driesch’s vitalist theories. Roux ani-
tion of experiential knowledge. Thus, I mated the differential growth of cells in
propose that qualitative descriptions of embryogenesis by incubating balls of
protein mechanisms through both dough containing varying quantities of

15
Science Studies 2/2006

yeast, joining them together in cellular otherwise static abstractions or ways of


formations, and observing the patterns seeing that have already been systema-
they formed as they rose (see Hopwood, tized in scientific research. Thus, it is the
1999). Hannah Landecker (2005) identi- theories themselves that are animated
fies another form of animation, this one through time-based imaging technolo-
emerging in the early twentieth century. gies. Kelty and Landecker provide a cru-
In its joining of biological and filmic cial contribution to situating time-lapse
techniques, microcinematography was imaging and animation within the his-
a form of animation that brought cells tory of theories and models in life sci-
to life on film screens. Today, protein ence. To extend their work further, I fore-
crystallographers and protein folding re- ground the ways that animations not
searchers make use of the spatial and only embed ways of seeing, but also,
temporal possibilities of digital media to how, in pulling static models into time,
build and manipulate their protein mod- animations refigure these ways of see-
els as time-based renderings onscreen. In ing and the very theories they enact.
the process they animate the molecular Animations perform knowledge, and in
mechanisms they hypothesize and intuit. this, transform it.
Such animations are proudly displayed A wide array of protein animations
and available to be downloaded from has been developed, some with high-
laboratory websites, frequently projected end graphics, others with much simpler
to awed audiences in conference presen- imagery. One of the more elaborate mo-
tations and in undergraduate classrooms, lecular animations currently circulating
and they circulate widely through infor- among life science researchers and stu-
mal networks on the Internet. dents was developed for teaching core
Chris Kelty and Hannah Landecker biological concepts to Harvard under-
(2004) have proposed a “theory of anima- graduates. The project employed char-
tion” that examines the relations between acter animators and state of the art com-
moving image technologies and the pro- puter graphic animation systems in its
duction of knowledge in the life sciences. aim to offer a glimpse into the “inner life
For them, “media that represent the liv- of the cell”.5 Building directly on protein
ing organism over time, such as time- structure data, the creators see this as a
lapse microcinematography, not only “completely accurate rendering”
demonstrate the life of the organism in (Marchant, 2006). However, this 3D fly-
question, they also animate it in relation through set to ambient, orchestral mu-
to other, often dominant, modes of static sic does more than just pull mechanical
representation” (Kelty and Landecker, objects into time: these animations also
2004: 45). Kelty and Landecker are less provide glimpses into the scientists’ and
interested in the ways that animations animators’ molecular imaginations. As
simulate liveliness than their “status as one reviewer comments, the molecules
images in relation to knowledge” (Kelty and organelles “move with bug-like au-
and Landecker, 2004: 32, emphasis as in thority, slithering, gliding and twisting
the original). They read animations as through 3D space” (Marchant, 2006).
the playing of theories or models for- Such renderings make clear the etymo-
ward in time, that is, as the animation of logical relations among the terms ani-

16
Natasha Myers

mation, animal, and animism. A mecha- communicate molecular knowledge.


nism, like a protein, is not quite dead,
and it is not quite alive. However, by pull- Embodied Animations and
ing static molecular models into time, Molecular Affects
protein modellers enliven biological
mechanisms though elaborate narra- There are many viable media for animat-
tives, endowing them with animistic, ing life science data and hypotheses, in-
even wily behaviours. Modellers use cluding physical (e.g. rising dough), cel-
malleable media, and apply temporal luloid (e.g. film), and virtual (e.g. com-
structures and narrative forms such that puter graphic) media. These are all mal-
their animations produce a liveliness leable materials that can be used to pull
that inflects molecular knowledge with abstract concepts – like mechanical
a range of animistic affects. theories – into space and time. Research-
Animations like this could be de- ers bodies, it turns out, do just as well.
scribed fairly as “working conceptual With an interest in examining the role of
hallucinations”; that is, “hybrid combi- affect in the performance of knowledge
nations of schematic, iconic and even in science, I extend the study of anima-
fantastic features” (Gilbert and Mulkay tions beyond the visualization technolo-
quoted in Lynch, 1991: 209). I propose gies and renderings produced onscreen,
that such animations are renderings that to include modes of animation that en-
combine researchers’ practical knowl- liven the bodies and imaginations of sci-
edge with imagined forms: they are tem- entists. The body-work involved in pro-
porally dynamic tracings of researchers’ tein modelling and mechanistic reason-
physical intuitions – their feeling for – ing demonstrates vividly what protein
protein forms and movements. Animat- researchers must do with their bodies in
ing media thus afford protein modellers order to bring molecular models and
a medium through which they can ex- mechanisms to life. In many ways, they
press their molecular imaginations and rely on gestures and affects to commu-
intuitions in time. I see these animations nicate structural knowledge of proteins
as pulling their users’ and viewers’ bod- amongst themselves and their col-
ies into new understandings by entrain- leagues, and to students and their wider
ing them to molecular temporalities and publics. Through this ethnography, I
other ways of moving. In this sense, hope to expand the category of what
animations may be thought of as narra- counts among practices to be tracked in
tives that lure their users into new modes analyses of the visual cultures of science.
of embodiment through their play with In order to understand how embod-
time (see Stengers, 1999 on “lures”). I ied animations are enacted, I follow
propose that it is through moving images Kelty and Landecker (2004) to examine
and bodies that protein modellers are techniques in which lively substances
able to propagate their tacit knowledge are first fixed or frozen, and then re-ani-
of molecular structures and mecha- mated. In this way I can track how static
nisms. Entangled with this tacit knowl- renderings are pulled into time through
edge is a range of affects that turn out to the animating media of researchers’
be central to how researchers learn and bodies and imaginations. Protein

17
Science Studies 2/2006

crystallography offers an illuminating ing a second interview, we sit in front of


example. Fit squarely within a tradition his computer screen while he describes
of biological imaging and modelling that some of the challenges he faces using
fixes or freezes substances in order to automated programs to predict how two
bring them into view, crystallography proteins will bind to each other. Auto-
produces static models of protein struc- mated programs don’t work so well, be-
tures. To gather data on a protein struc- cause, as he reminds me, “proteins are
ture, a crystallographer first crystallizes breathing entities”. When I press him to
the protein, and then grows the crystals explain what he means by this, he re-
until they are large and organized sponds saying, “I don’t know, sounds a
enough to diffract X-rays. In order to bit romantic, doesn’t it?”
visualize the structure of the molecules The liveliness of the protein is a tan-
packed within each unit cell of the crys- gible concept for him: as he describes
tal, the crystal is dipped in liquid nitro- the protein he is currently working on,
gen just before it is subjected to X-ray he holds his hands out in front of his
diffraction. Cryofixation is widely used body, as if holding a pulsing substance.
to dampen the highly energetic protein The invisible object in his hands appears
molecules whose forms vibrate rapidly, to breathe like lungs.7 He knows how the
even within the ordered array of the crys- protein moves in part from his close
tal lattice. X-ray crystallography thus study of chemical laws and the physical
renders best-estimate models by averag- properties of proteins. But he also has a
ing out the dynamic movements and tacit, kinaesthetic knowledge of the form
subtle differences in conformation be- of molecule that he did not learn from
tween all the molecules arrayed in the books – a knowledge he has gained from
crystal. This technique produces a sin- having spent tremendous effort and ex-
gle structure, a static snapshot of the tended periods of time building and
calculated average of all the protein navigating through unique protein
molecules, frozen in time. structures onscreen. In the process of
Edward, a crystallographer conduct- building crystallographic models, work-
ing postdoctoral research in a protein ing with X-ray diffraction data, and
crystallography lab, tells me that this sculpting the model using interactive
snapshot can be challenging to interpret graphics, he has found a way to animate
for those not trained in the arts of pro- these static structures within his body
tein visualization.6 “Molecular biologists and imagination.
are notorious”, he tells me. “The main Diane, who heads the protein
criticism crystallographers have about crystallography lab where Edward works,
molecular biologists is that they don’t has quite a vivid molecular imagination.
think about the structure as a breathing She tells me that as she builds the model
entity. [For them] it’s just a rigid body.” onscreen, she simultaneously builds up
For non-experts who don’t have a feel for a detailed model of complex molecule
the physics and chemistry of protein in her “head”, to the extent that it be-
molecules, the structures available to comes available to her to rotate in her
download from the PDB don’t convey mind in three dimensions (see Myers, in
how dynamic proteins “really are”. Dur- press). She confesses, however, that this

18
Natasha Myers

is not easy to do: models, even through virtual technolo-


gies, enables her to learn the structure
And I try now as an advisor, I try to get
inside the structure and really try to un- kinaesthetically. In this way, she can get
derstand it at that level. And I have for “inside” the model. In many ways, how-
a few of them, but it is really time con- ever, the model also gets inside of her;
suming, I mean, to sort of have the and this is a key step if she is to acquire
structure in your head in three dimen- and use structural knowledge.
sions, which is how I felt about some
of the other structures that I actually Once a structure is determined, its
did build myself. mechanism of function must be inter-
preted, and a hypothesis formed. In or-
Her ability to construct an embodied
der to “think intelligently about struc-
model of the protein is facilitated by the
ture”, Diane must learn the structure in-
interface she has used to build the
timately. Once the details of the model
crystallographic model onscreen. Inter-
are embedded in her tissues, she has a
active computer graphics afford a kind
way to feel her way around inside the
of tangibility and manipulability that is
protein and figure out how it works. Like
similar in some ways to other modelling
Edward, Diane has a multi-sensory, ki-
materials, including physical media (see
naesthetic sense of how the protein
Myers, in press; Francoeur and Segal,
moves:
2004). It is through the laborious work
of modelling, manipulating the 3D And you know, it’s really this vision that
graphic space on her computer screen, you have of the active site, and sort of
this sense of how tightly packed it is and
that she able to sculpt a fleshed out twin
how much flexibility there might be
of the model, not just in her mind, but and where those regions of flexibility
in her body. In the process of modelling are. To have this sort of sense that you
the protein in silico, she cultivates an af- have. And you can think about it then
fective, kinaesthetic knowledge – a feel- moving in a way because you sort of
know something about what the den-
ing for – the possible forms and move-
sity was in each part, so that you know
ments of the protein in vivo. that that part is definitely mobile right
She can do this with models that she in there, but that this part would not be
has worked with and built herself, those mobile.
with which she is intimately familiar. She emphasizes how difficult it is to ex-
However, when presented with new press this multi-dimensional knowledge
structures and hypothetical mecha- to others: “It’s not something that is easy
nisms at conferences and in talks, she to communicate, because, you know you
often finds the data difficult to grasp. “I can’t explain something in three dimen-
would be at a meeting”, she tells me, “and sions to someone”. Where words fail, her
people would be discussing a mecha- body becomes increasingly articulate. In
nism, and I would kind of close my eyes interviews and while teaching Diane
and think about it and go, ‘No. Too far performs her embodied knowledge of
away’”. A mechanism is “too far away” the protein. Through expressive gestures
for Diane if she can’t interact with it or and affects she conveys her feeling for
manipulate it onscreen. The physicality the molecule’s intra-molecular tensions
of handling and building molecular and forces, its chemical attractions,

19
Science Studies 2/2006

repulsions, affinities, and the possible contort their bodies into sometimes-
ranges of motion across its chemical awkward configurations to demonstrate
bonds. These are forces that she feels in the conformational changes of the mol-
her own body, to the extent that when ecule and to show how it does its work
students present her with half-built pro- mechanically and chemically in the cell.
tein models whose configurations defy As much as they are inflected with af-
allowable bond angles and produce fect, I propose that embodied models,
clashes between the radii of atoms, she like those performed by Diane and
winces audibly. Demonstrating the mis- Edward, can also be thought of, in
shapen model by mimetically contort- Rheinberger’s terminology, as “technical
ing her body, she tells me: “I feel the pain objects” that are employed in the inves-
the molecule is in, because it just can’t tigation of “epistemic things”. I have
go like that!” Crystallographic models watched researchers fumble and correct
are thus inflected with affects, with mod- the models they perform through their
ellers’ feelings for the textures, tensions, bodies, sometimes realizing mid-gesture
forces and movements within and be- that they have the structure wrong. They
tween proteins. are quick to correct their own gestures
Some researchers are more reserved and forms as a means to correct the
in their performance of their embodied model. In the process, they learn new
models. At a scientific meeting, the same things while they play through possible
crystallographer who had enlisted my molecular configurations and move-
participation in performing his model of ments with their own bodies. Not so
a cell adhesion molecule confessed to much a kind of thought experiment, the
me that he had choreographed “a little body-work of reasoning in protein mod-
dance” for one of the other molecules elling could be considered as a kind of
that he had modelled. “I hate dancing”, body experiment. Thus, I observe em-
he emphasized, “but there was just no bodied models as tools readily available
other way to communicate the mecha- for researchers to use in their experi-
nism. I had to dance it”. He declined to mental practice. Like the crystallo-
show me his “secret” dance when I grapher who ratcheted up his grip to
asked, although he had performed it be- demonstrate the binding of cell adhe-
fore for a small group of colleagues. Still sion molecules, I see embodied models
other crystallographers, including as “vehicles for materializing questions”
Diane’s most advanced graduate stu- (Rheinberger, 1997: 28); that is, as means
dents and postdocs (those who have that can propel scientists into new kinds
successfully built crystallographic mod- of conceptual and corporeal under-
els), talk excitedly about molecular standings of their research objects.
movements they intuit, but can’t other-
wise see. They perform the vibrations of Molecular Gestures and Mimetic
molecules captured within growing pro- Modelling
tein crystals, and wave their arms about
to emulate the floppy ends of polypep- I have conducted ethnographic observa-
tide chains that come out blurred in tions in the weekly group meetings of
crystallographic snapshots. They also two laboratories. Members of one group

20
Natasha Myers

specialize in designing new protein and repeated the gesture over and over
structures. During one meeting, a PhD as he asked questions, inquiring and
student presenting her recent progress confirming with her that this was indeed
was interrupted by a constant stream of the form of the molecular interaction
questions from her colleagues who she was describing.
asked her to clarify the structure of the As this story suggests, researchers’
protein she is working on. Even with in- bodies become animating media, both
tricate computer graphic renderings of for figuring out how molecular mecha-
the molecule projected on the screen nisms work, and for relaying knowledge
behind her, she was compelled to per- about their structure. Scientists them-
form the structure. The protein she selves become lively models through
works on is complex: it forms a dimer, mimetic gestures that convey the form
which means that it is made up of two and movements of the molecule through
similar molecules bound together. It also the form and movements of their bod-
has intracellular and extra-cellular do- ies. This social intra-action shows how
mains, with parts of the protein that bodily movement plays a role in how re-
must traverse the lipid bi-layer of the cell searchers learn and communicate struc-
membrane. To communicate this intri- tural knowledge to others. In the back
cate structure to the group, she pro- and forth communication between the
ceeded to lift both her hands over her protein modeller and her interlocutors,
head and trace the winding backbones her embodied model of the protein (it-
of the twinned molecules, one with each self a mimetic model)9 is re-enacted in
hand, following them as they traversed an intra-active exchange until shared
extra-cellular and intra-cellular spaces. understanding is acknowledged.
Her gestures were large and sweeping, I see a kind of mimesis at play in pro-
as if she were painting an accurate Rib- tein modellers’ entrainment to molecu-
bon diagram of the molecule for all her lar movements, and in their teaching,
colleagues to see (see Figure 1).8 Her learning and communication. This relay
elaborate choreography brought her of forms and gestures can be seen as an
arms from high up, over her head, down intra-active process aimed at achieving
in front of her body, and all the way down mutual understanding. As the above ex-
to the ground. Her molecular dance amples show, such social intra-actions
ended with her fully bent over, hands enable researchers to propagate their
touching the floor. embodied models by communicating to
Questions still surfaced from the other researchers and students through
group, and she was asked to describe the a kind of iconic and indexical “gymnas-
mechanism that bound the molecules tics” (Bourdieu, 1977: 1). Pierre Bourdieu
together. “I like to think of it this way”, has likened such performances to a kind
she said, and repeatedly crossed her of “mimesis” that is similar to a “rite or
arms at the forearms, fists clenched, dance” in which there is “something in-
demonstrating with the tension in her effable”, something that “communicates,
musculature the binding energy be- so to speak, from body to body, i.e. on
tween the molecules. A visiting profes- the hither side of words or concepts”
sor, still confused, leaned over the table, (Bourdieu, 1977:1). It may be through

21
Science Studies 2/2006

Figure 1: Two views of a protein model rendered as a Ribbon diagram. Note the
arrows that indicate the direction of the folded polypeptide chain as it
winds through the stucture. Used with permission from an anonymous
ethnographic informant.

this mimetic, gestural language that has two layers for Taussig: it contains
biomolecules become intelligible, ma- both an element of copy or imitation, as
nipulable and workable as objects for well as the “palpable, sensuous connec-
the researcher, their colleagues and stu- tion between the body of the perceiver
dents. and the perceived” (Taussig, 1993: 21).
Michael Taussig’s (1993) multi-sensate This mimetic faculty nourishes and sus-
theory of mimesis captures some of this tains shared understanding and knowl-
movement and participation that I see at edge within a larger cultural milieu. On
play in the communication of molecular this, he suggests that the mimetic faculty
knowledge.10 Reading Benjamin, Taussig is “the nature that culture uses to create
develops the notion of an “optical tactil- second nature”. It is “the faculty to copy,
ity”, in which movement, sensation and imitate, make models, explore differ-
perception are woven together. Mimesis ence, yield into and become Other”

22
Natasha Myers

(Taussig, 1993: xiii). To mime is thus to bodies. The specificity of the media
intra-actively build up a model of an through which the signal moves, in this
other entity within one’s own body – a case, the physics and chemistry of the
model that can be shared with others. protein, morphs the signal into different
For Taussig, “[to] ponder mimesis is registers as it moves between molecules.
to become sooner or later caught in Transduction is also an evocative term
sticky webs of copy and contact, image to describe the propagation of move-
and bodily involvement of the perceiver ments and affects in social intra-actions
in the image” (Taussig, 1993: 21). Mime- between scientists. Through their intra-
sis involves perceptual and physical in- actions with each other and with their
tra-actions between participating bod- models, protein modellers can be seen
ies. It is this intimate contact between to transduce and so propagate the mo-
scientist and substance – a contact me- lecular affects and gestures they have
diated through prosthetic devices and cultivated in order to communicate their
visualizing machines – that is key for feeling for protein forms and mecha-
thinking through body-work of protein nisms.
modelling, reasoning and communica- While teaching a class on protein fold-
tion. Moreover, it is through a kind of ing, one researcher introduces Ribbon
mimesis that a researcher’s body and diagrams as representational conven-
their model come to move toward a kind tions that show the direction of the
of resemblance, which is not so much a polypeptide chain as it winds through
mirror-image reflection, but a kind of the folded protein (see Figure 1). Taking
resonance. Self and other, modeller and up some confusion around a homework
model are thus not so readily separable assignment, the professor goes over the
from their relation: models and bodies details of the wording in “Question 2”
become entangled in mimetic exchange. which asks students to “draw, copy, or
Below I investigate modes by which such trace” a figure from the textbook. Obvi-
resemblances are made to propagate. ously, some students had some trouble
interpreting the meaning of “copy.” He
Propagating Molecular Affects had to clarify: “This means hand copy! If
through Mimetic Transductions you Xerox it, you don’t assimilate it!” He
demands the students get involved in
Transduction is a term used widely in the the structures by tracing them: he tells
field of structural biology. Proteins are the class that they have to “signal ac-
figured as working machines that trans- tively” to “get the notion”. According to
duce force and energy within the cell him, “you can’t not learn something” if
(see for example Bourne, 1986; Harrison, you actively get involved. He shows the
2004).11 In this way, transduction is con- class how to “look”: “You have to trace
ceived as a mechanical process for mov- them”, he entreats, redirecting the stu-
ing and transforming signals between dents’ attention to a human-scale model
molecules; in a signalling network, mol- of a protein projected on the screen be-
ecules pass chemical energy and me- hind him. He reaches up and uses his
chanical forces between them in a kind hand to trace along the winding peptide
of contact-dance between molecular backbone. As he follows the peptide, his

23
Science Studies 2/2006

whole body gets swept up the fold. He mimetic exchange among a wide range
effectively insists that his students par- of protein researchers in a wide array of
ticipate bodily. In other words, they must settings. It is, of course, difficult to gauge
move with and be moved by the model precisely whether my sampling is “rep-
in order to learn the structure. In this resentative” of the larger field. However,
way, protein models can entangle their my aim is not to produce a portrait or
users in participatory intra-actions that caricature of these researchers, for the
are geared towards learning (see also very reason that structural biologists’
Myers, forthcoming). and biological engineers’ professional
To trace, that is, to hand-copy, is not identities are currently in formation: as
to photo-copy. Tracing is a means to these fields secure new footholds on the
transduce the form of the polypeptide rapidly expanding terrain of twenty-
chain through one’s body, not to delegate first-century life science, what is repre-
the task to a replicating machine. The sentative of their practices is yet to be
aim here is not to replicate, but to emu- determined. Rather, this study is better
late. Like the protein modeller who positioned to track how new modes of
danced the Ribbon diagram of her mol- embodiment can propagate among re-
ecule for her colleagues, the tracer’s searchers in training as professional
moving body, following the fold of the identities are being formed. My aim is to
chain, emulates the form of the protein identify the tacit processes through
with their body, without producing a which molecular gestures and affects are
replica or a copy. In this way, the notion transduced, and thus how such practices
of transduction forces me to account for can be made to propagate within and
the specificity of the modelling media, among communities of life scientists.
and the kinds of bodies involved in these
mimetic exchanges. Protein modellers Conclusion
communicate molecular forms within
the range of motions available to their As I have tried to show in this paper, pro-
bodies: their contortions never actually tein models are not the only phenom-
look like the graphic models they project enon (in Barad’s sense) produced in the
onscreen. Defying any simple theory of protein modelling laboratory. Protein
representation, embodied models and models are embodied and performed in
animations operate to emulate model- ways that propagate more than struc-
lers’ feeling for the tensions, forces, and tural information. Embodied animations
movements of molecules. Thus protein transduce affects, emotions, and feelings
modellers’ embodied animations extend that inflect knowledge about protein
and expand assumptions about what structure. Through their animated bod-
counts as a model or scientific visuali- ies, researchers perform their knowledge
zation in practice. Moreover, these in a register that both feeds into and ex-
animations require that theories of rep- ceeds the discourse of mechanism in
resentation and communication in sci- structural biology. In spite of their con-
ence account for the role of affect in tinuous attempts to police animistic lan-
propagating scientific knowledge. guage through mechanistic logic, I de-
I have observed this phenomenon of tect a surfacing of liveliness in structural

24
Natasha Myers

biologists’ performances of their mod- depend on this affective enlivening of


els. Molecular mechanisms are quick- mechanisms for the effective production
ened, that is, enlivened through these and deployment of mechanistic theories.
intra-actions, recasting the trope of “mo- Left to gather dust on the pages of el-
lecular machines” within which proteins ementary school textbooks or reduced
tend to be figured. I read structural bi- to dead metaphors that fail to hail bod-
ologists’ excited gestures as sympto- ily participation, mechanistic models of
matic irruptions of an otherwise disa- protein function may indeed be “de-
vowed liveliness, performances that ex- animations” of lively substances (see
press their affective entanglements in Haraway, 1998). If these models are dis-
knowledge making practices. entangled from the intra-actions that
Going beyond Barad’s call to account produce and sustain them, they may
for the multiple agencies through which appear deadening and inert. However, if
scientific knowledge is produced, this these visualizations can be drawn back
paper has aimed to document how such into Barad’s expanded framing of experi-
knowledge is inflected and transformed mental phenomena, and observed
in its very performance. I propose that within the assemblages through which
the intra-actions between participating they are enacted, then the “machinery
bodies (human, nonhuman and ma- of life” can be seen to take on much live-
chine) produce a second order phenom- lier form. Enlivened models animate
enon that could be called intra-animacy. imaginations, techniques, experimental
This is not some immaterial “animism” strategies, research questions and peda-
that imbues matter with some external gogical interactions. Embodied anima-
force, nor is it built up from a networked tions are thus more than aesthetic flour-
collection of individual agencies mod- ishes: such modes of body-work are a
elled on liberal notions of subjectivity. crucial step in luring scientists and their
This liveliness is a phenomenon that is students into new kinds of understand-
engendered through modellers’ intra- ing. I suggest that it is protein modellers’
actions with each other, and with their capacity and willingness to move with
objects and machines. In turn, it ani- and be moved by their models and
mates their imaginations and narratives animations – to mimetically transduce
about the substances of life. I observe the intricate details of proteinaceous
that this liveliness is performed as a forms – which enables this liveliness to
range of affects and gestures that make thrive.
visible structural biologists’ intimate
sensibilities with regards to molecular Acknowledgements
forms, their chemical affinities and
physical movements. As I have shown, Earlier versions of this paper have ben-
these performative affects are not an ex- efited from the contributions of many
tra-scientific phenomenona, but one in- generous readers including Donna
trinsic to the conceptual and material Haraway, Stefan Helmreich, Joseph
work of protein modelling. As such, the Dumit, Karen Barad, Kaushik Sunder
mechanical theories of protein function Rajan, Rebecca Herzig, members of the
that researchers produce can be seen to “Food for Thought” writing workshop at

25
Science Studies 2/2006

the University of California, Davis, and form of such entanglements – including


members of MIT’s “BioGroop”. This re- the modes of embodiment and forms of
knowledge performed – which remain
search was supported by a doctoral fel- within the purview of the scientist. This
lowship from the Social Sciences and inseparability of objects and agents di-
Humanities Research Council of Canada rects attention to issues raised in the femi-
(Award No. 752-2002-0301), and a pre- nist science studies literature of account-
doctoral fellowship from the National ability in the production scientific knowl-
edge.
Science Foundation (Grant No. 0220347).
I am grateful to the editors of this spe- For me, intra-action calls attention to in-
timate contact-dance between human
cial issue, the anonymous reviewers for
and nonhuman bodies and machines in
their careful reading and productive sug- scientific practice. In this essay, I aim to
gestions, and to the researchers whose show how such intra-actions depend on
lively work inspired this paper. This pa- the response-ability of participating bod-
per was awarded honourable mention ies. To move with and be moved by another
within an intra-active practice also calls
for the MIT Siegel Prize for best student
attention to the responsibilities involved
essay on science, technology and soci- in such entanglements. Thus, a theory of
ety. intra-action invests responsibility in the
scientist’s (and analyst’s) act of circum-
Notes scribing the phenomenon, locating sub-
jects and objects, and producing and per-
forming knowledge.
1 In a recent review of theories of agency in
the STS literature, Lucy Suchman (2007) 2 When the PDB was first founded in 1971,
grapples with the legacy of actor network fewer than a dozen protein structures had
theory (ANT) and “its aftermath”, includ- been determined and deposited. By 2000,
ing Barad’s theory of “intra-action”. 13,635 structures were available for view-
Suchman locates Barad within a lineage ers to download onto their computers and
of scholars concerned with the “mutual view through interactive visualization
constitution” of human and nonhuman software. As of November 2006, there were
agencies in scientific practice. Suchman 40,132 searchable structures in the data-
quotes Michel Callon to show that ANT’s base (see http://rscb.org/pdb).
“network” is not one “connecting entities
3 Here I take a cue from Nick Hopwood’s
which are already there, but a network
(1999) treatment of the history of
which configures ontologies. The agents,
embryological modelling practices. He
their dimensions, and what they are and
has documented how embodied knowl-
do, all depend on the morphology of the
edge contributed to late-nineteenth-cen-
relations in which they are involved”
tury formulations of mechanism in struc-
(Callon cited in Suchman, 2007). Yet, as
tural studies of developing organisms.
Suchman’s genealogy of theories of
Hopwood describes embryologist
agency in STS makes clear, Barad’s formu-
Wilhelm His’s (1831-1904) techniques for
lation is key for the development of a femi-
sculpting scale models of embryos in wax.
nist account of power, knowledge and re-
In defence of a mechanical theory of
sponsibility in science. Extending ANT to
embryological development, His devel-
the embodiment and performativity of
oped a method for precisely reconstruct-
the scientists, Barad’s agential realism
ing the form of embryos from the details
poses the question: Where do scientists’
derived from microscopic examination of
bodies end and experimental instruments
tissue slices. Using “projective” drafting
and objects begin? They do not simply
techniques and freehand wax sculpture,
interact, or mutually produce each other,
he worked the sectioned images into ex-
but are profoundly entangled. It is the

26
Natasha Myers

quisite three-dimensional form, a craft 6 All names of ethnographic research sub-


that demanded much artisanal skill. jects have been changed to maintain ano-
Hopwood sees His’s commitment to mod- nymity.
elling as “a passionate argument for dou-
7 Haemoglobin is one of the classical mo-
bly embodied knowledge” (Hopwood,
lecular structures that life science stu-
1999: 482). In Hopwood’s reading, His’s
dents learn, almost as a rite of passage into
insight into the mechanical processes of
studies of molecular form and protein
embryogenesis was gained through the
function. This is the molecule that carries
physicality of building models. His “had
oxygen in blood, and conveniently it is
first to make his problem, to use his fin-
often taught and remembered as a
gers”, and it was by this method that he
“breathing molecule”. In interviews, bio-
was able to “‘to give body’ to his [theoreti-
chemistry students often re-enact the
cal] views”. In giving material form to the
haemoglobin structure that they learn in
embryos he also “gave body” to his theory.
class by making a gesture similar to
His developed an embodied knowledge of
Edwards’, though the students’ renderings
the phenomenon in the practice of mak-
are less articulated and nuanced. In a
ing models, such that that the problem of
video interview accessible online, Max
development became familiar to his body.
Perutz, who determined this large, com-
As Hopwood argues, it was “the experi-
plex structure in the 1960s through inno-
ence of modelling” that was “the most
vations in crystallographic techniques,
compelling evidence of the importance of
can be seen animating the mechanism of
mechanical principles in development”
haemoglobin, demonstrating with delight
(Hopwood, 1999: 466). His thus learned
the form and movements of the protein
the mechanical forces of embryogenesis
as it captures and releases oxygen. See
by working with the physics of his body.
video interviews with Max Perutz online:
Hopwood’s study demonstrates that redi-
“Face to Face with Max Perutz,” Vega Sci-
recting attention to the enactment of
ence Trust. http://www.vega.org.uk/
models reveals lively bodies and imagina-
video/programme/1.
tions caught up in the work of theorizing
mechanism in the life sciences. 8 As Figure 1 demonstrates, and I show later
in the paper, Ribbon diagrams, one of the
4 One of the most pervasive metaphors cur-
most frequently used schema for render-
rently at play among those who use the
ing protein structures, have an instructive
trope of “molecular machines” is that of
quality about them. Arrows in the model
the car and its parts: the cell is likened to
indicate the direction of the polypeptide
a car, and looking into the cell compared
backbone (from the first amino acid in the
to peering “under the hood” of a car. In
chain to the last) as it winds through the
this language, cells are made up of pro-
structure. As this protein modeller’s per-
tein “components” and “devices”, and fig-
formance indicates, these are not just
uring out how they all fit together and
visual guides for the eye, but are also use-
keep the cell “running” is the job of the
ful for learning of forms kinaesthetically.
biologist. The close ties between struc-
Ribbon diagrams could be compared to
tural biology and mechanical engineering
the tracers and tailings of a rhythmic gym-
become very clear through these associa-
nast’s ribbon if she were to trace the wind-
tions. See for example a video lecture of
ing back bone of a protein as she dances.
biophysicist Paul Wiggins produced by the
One animation available online treats a
Whitehead Institute, titled “Under the
Ribbon diagram as if it were a roller-
hood: A beginner’s guide to the molecu-
coaster, by taking the viewer on a rather
lar motor” (see http://www.wi.mit.edu/
dizzying ride along the strands of the Rib-
news/video_gallery/index.html ).
bon backbone of a protein model. See
5 The animation is available to view online http://streaming.wi.mit.edu/?sub=
at http://www.studiodaily.com/main/ protein_rollercoaster&vid=X11_001_
technique/tprojects/6850.html. 220K_256x199.mov.

27
Science Studies 2/2006

9 In this sense “mimetic models” are those Butler, J. P.


that represent the form of the object in 1993 Bodies that matter: On the discursive
question. Galison (1997) develops this limits of “sex”. New York: Routledge.
term in the contexts of the history of cloud Chiu, W., Baker, M., Jiang, W., Dougherty, M.
chamber experiments in physics, while & Schmid, M.
Daston (2003) uses it to explore the “ex- 2005 “Electron cryomicroscopy of biological
treme mimesis” achieved in the use of machines at subnanometer resolu-
glass to craft highly accurate models of tion.” Structure 13: 363-372.
plant form. Daston, L.
10 Lucy Suchman (2007) also draws on both 2003 “The glass flowers.” Things that Talk,
Taussig and Barad for a theory of mime- Max Planck Institute for the History of
sis in artificial intelligence simulations. Science Preprint 233: 5-32.
Her insights resonate closely with my de Chadarevian, S.
reading here. 2002 Designs for life: Molecular biology af-
ter World War II. Cambridge: Cam-
11 The term transduction has at least three bridge University Press.
lineages in the history of science: one in Doyle, R.
acoustics and the other two in biology. It 1997 On beyond living: Rhetorical transfor-
simultaneously refers to the “action or mations of the life sciences. Stanford:
process of transducing a signal”, such as Stanford University Press.
sound through one medium to another, Francoeur, E.
and “the transfer of genetic material from 1997 “The forgotten tool: The design and use
one cell to another by a virus or virus-like of molecular models.” Social Studies of
particle” (Oxford English Dictionary). Ad- Science 27: 7-40.
ditionally, “signal transduction” is a con- Francoeur, E. & Segal, J.
cept frequently used in molecular biology 2004 “From model kits to interactive com-
to describe the transmission of extra-cel- puter graphics.” Pp. 402-429 in de
lular signals into the cell and the propa- Chadarevian and Hopwood (eds.),
gation this signal as biophysical molecu- Models: The third dimension of sci-
lar events (see also Mackenzie, 2002). I ence. Stanford: Stanford University
incorporate each of these aspects of the Press.
term in my use here. Galison, P.
1997 Image and logic: A material culture of
microphysics. Chicago: Chicago Uni-
References versity Press.
Gieson, G.
Barad, K. 1969 “The protoplasmic theory of life and
1996 “Meeting the universe halfway: Realism the vitalist-mechanist debate.” Isis 60
and social constructivism without con- (3): 272-292.
tradiction.” Pp. 161-194 in Nelson and Goffman, E.
Nelson (eds.), Feminism, science, and 2001 “On fieldwork.” Pp. 153-158 in Emerson
philosophy of science. Boston: Kluwer. (ed.), Contemporary field research.
2003 “Posthumanist performativity: Toward Prospect Heights, Ill.: Waveland Press.
an understanding of how matter comes Griesemer, J. R.
to matter.” Signs 28 (3): 801-831. 2004 “Three-dimensional models in philo-
Bourdieu, P. sophical perspective.” Pp. 433-442 in de
1977 Outline of a theory of practice. Cam- Chadarevian and Hopwood (eds.),
bridge: Cambridge University Press. Models: The third dimension of sci-
Bourne, H. ence. Stanford: Stanford University
1986 “One molecular machine can trans- Press.
duce diverse signals.” Nature 321: 814-
816.

28
Natasha Myers

Haraway, D. J. Kreisberg, J., Betts, S., Hasse-Pettingell, C. &


1991 Simians, cyborgs, and women: The King, J.
reinvention of nature. New York: 2002 “The interdigitated beta-helix domain
Routledge. of the p22 tailspike protein acts as a
1997 Modest_Witness@Second_Millennium. molecular clamp in trimer
FemaleMan_Meets_OncoMouse: stabilization.” Protein Science 11: 820-
Feminism and technoscience. New 830.
York: Routledge. Landecker, H.
1998 “Deanimations: Maps and portraits of 2005 “Cellular features: Microcinematogra-
life itself.” Pp. 181-207 in Jones and phy and early film theory.” Critical In-
Galison (eds.), Picturing science, pro- quiry 31: 903-937.
ducing art. New York: Routledge. 2007 Culturing life: How cells became tech-
Harrison, S. nologies. Cambridge, Mass.: Harvard
2004 “Whither structural biology?” Nature University Press.
Structural Biology and Molecular Biol- Langridge, R.
ogy 11: 12-15. 1974 “Interactive three-dimensional com-
Hill, R. & Rich, P. puter graphics in molecular biology.”
1983 “A physical interpretation for the natu- Federation Proceedings 33(12): 2332-
ral photosynthetic process.” Proceed- 2335.
ings of the National Academy of Sci- Langridge, R., Ferrin, T. E., Kuntz, I. D. &
ences 80: 978-982. Connolly, M. L.
Hoffman, M. 1981 “Real-time color graphics in studies of
1991 “An RNA first: It’s part of the gene-copy- molecular-interactions.” Science 211
ing machinery.” Science 252 (5005): (4483): 661-666.
506-507. Law, J.
Hopwood, N. 1973 “The development of specialties in sci-
1999 “‘Giving body’ to embryos: Modeling, ence: The case of x-ray protein
mechanism, and the microtome in late crystallography.” Science Studies 3 (3):
nineteenth-century anatomy.” Isis 90: 275-303.
462-496. Lenoir, T.
Kay, L. E. 1982 The strategy of life: Teleology and me-
1993 The molecular vision of life: Caltech, chanics in nineteenth century German
the Rockefeller Foundation, and the biology. Boston: D. Reidel Pub. Co.
rise of the new biology. New York: Ox- Lynch, M.
ford University Press. 1991 “Science in the age of mechanical re-
2000 Who wrote the book of life? A history production: Moral and epistemic rela-
of the genetic code. Stanford: Stanford tions between diagrams and photo-
University Press. graphs.” Biology and Philosophy 6: 205-
Keller, E. F. 226.
1995 Refiguring life: Metaphors of twentieth- Mackenzie, A.
century biology. New York: Columbia 2002 Transductions: Bodies and machines at
University Press. speed. New York: Continuum.
2002 Making sense of life: Explaining bio- Marchant, B.
logical development with models, 2006 “Cellular visions: The inner life of a
metaphors, and machines. Cambridge, cell.” http://www.studiodaily.com/
Mass.: Harvard University Press. main/technique/tprojects/6850.html
Kelty, C. & Landecker, H. Accessed November 20, 2006.
2004 “A theory of animation: Cells, L-sys- Merchant, C.
tems, and film.” Grey Room 1 (17): 30- 1983 The death of nature: Women, ecology,
63. and the scientific revolution. San Fran-
cisco: Harper & Row.

29
Science Studies 2/2006

Mol, A.
2002 The body multiple: Ontology in medi-
cal practice. Durham: Duke University
Press.
Myers, N.
forthcoming “Performing the protein fold:
The pedagogical lives of molecular
models.” in Turkle (ed.), The inner his-
tory of devices: Ethnography and the
first person. Cambridge, Mass.: MIT
Press.
in press “Molecular embodiments and the
body-work of modeling in protein
crystallography.” Social Studies of Sci-
ence.
Pauly, P. J.
1996 Controlling life: Jacques Loeb and the
engineering ideal in biology. Berkeley:
University of California Press.
Plumwood, V.
1993 Feminism and the mastery of nature.
New York: Routledge.
Rheinberger, H.-J.
1997 Toward a history of epistemic things:
Synthesizing proteins in the test tube.
Stanford: Stanford University Press.
Stengers, I.
1999 “Whitehead and the laws of nature.”
Salzburger Theologische Zeitschrift
(SaThZ) 2: 193-207.
Stent, G.
1968 “That was the molecular biology that
was.” Science 160 (3826): 390-391.
Suchman, L.
2007 Human-machine reconfigurations:
Plans and situated actions, 2nd ex-
panded edition. New York: Cambridge
University Press.
Tanford, C. and Reynolds, J.
2001 Nature’s robots: A history of proteins.
Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Taussig, M. T.
1993 Mimesis and alterity: A particular his-
tory of the senses. New York: Routledge.

Natasha Myers
Program in History, Anthropology, and
Science, Tehcnology, and Society
Massachusetts Institute of Technology,
USA
nmyers@mit.edu

30

You might also like