You are on page 1of 10

Social Science & Medicine 53 (2001) 1621–1630

Health behaviour, risk awareness and emotional well-being in


students from Eastern Europe and Western Europe
Andrew Steptoea,*, Jane Wardleb
a
Psychobiology Group, Department of Epidemiology and Public Health, University College London, 1-19 Torrington Place,
London WC1E 6BT, UK
b
Health Behaviour Unit, Department of Epidemiology and Public Health, University College London, 1-19 Torrington Place,
London WC1E 6BT, UK

Abstract

Life expectancy and other indices of health have deteriorated markedly in the former socialist countries of Eastern
Europe over recent decades. The possible roles of lifestyles, knowledge about health and behaviour, emotional well-
being and perceptions of control were assessed in a cross-sectional survey of young adults of similar educational status
in Eastern and Western Europe. As part of the European Health and Behaviour Survey, data were collected in 1989–
1991 from 4170 university students aged 18–30 years from Austria, Belgium, the Federal Republic of Germany, the
Netherlands and Switzerland, and from 2293 students from the German Democratic Republic, Hungary and Poland.
Measures were obtained of health behaviours, awareness of the role of lifestyle factors in health, depression, social
support, health locus of control, and the value placed on health. After adjustment for age and sex, East European
students had less healthy lifestyles than Western Europeans according to a composite index of 11 health behaviours,
with significant differences for seven activities: regular exercise, drinking alcohol, avoiding dietary fat, eating fibre,
adding salt to food, wearing a seat-belt, and using sunscreen protection. East European students were less likely to be
aware of the relationship between lifestyle factors (smoking, exercise, fat and salt consumption) and cardiovascular
disease risk. In addition, they were more depressed (adjusted odds of elevated scores on the Beck Depression Inventory
of 2.46, 95% C.I. 1.95–3.09), reported lower social support, and had higher beliefs in the ‘‘chance’’ and ‘‘powerful
others’’ locus of control. Internal locus of control levels did not differ across regions, and Eastern Europeans placed a
higher valuation on their health. Unhealthy lifestyles associated with lack of information about health and behaviour,
greater beliefs in uncontrollable influences, and diminished emotional well-being, may contribute to poor health status
in Eastern Europe. # 2001 Elsevier Science Ltd. All rights reserved.

Keywords: UK; Health behaviour; Emotional well-being; Risk perception; Eastern Europe; Western Europe

Introduction Costeool, & Mitchell, 1997; Hertzman, Kelly, & Bobak,


1996). Over the past 30 years, there has been a
The differences in health and rates of premature progressive increase in differences in life expectancy
mortality between Western Europe and Central and between the countries of the European Union and the
Eastern Europe are of major concern (Bobadilla, countries of Central and Eastern Europe (Bobak &
Marmot, 1996). For example, between 1970 and 1991,
*Correspondence address: Health Behaviour Unit, Depart- there were decreases in male life expectancy at age 15 in
ment of Epidemiology and Public Health, University College Hungary, Poland, Romania, and Bulgaria, compared
London, 1-19 Torrington Place, London WC1E 6BT, UK. Tel.: with increases of more than three years in the European
+44-20-7679-5628; fax: +44-20-7916-8542. Union. Life expectancy at age 15 increased over the
E-mail address: a.steptoe@ucl.ac.uk (A. Steptoe). same period among women in Eastern Europe, but to a

0277-9536/01/$ - see front matter # 2001 Elsevier Science Ltd. All rights reserved.
PII: S 0 2 7 7 - 9 5 3 6 ( 0 0 ) 0 0 4 4 6 - 9
1622 A. Steptoe, J. Wardle / Social Science & Medicine 53 (2001) 1621–1630

much lesser extent than among Western Europeans. then we predict that they will be less aware than their
Premature death from cardiovascular diseases and Western counterparts of the role played by habitual
external causes are major contributors to this pattern. behaviours in disease risk. We tested this hypothesis by
A number of explanations for these trends have been comparing the levels of awareness of the involvement of
put forward (Bobak & Marmot, 1996; Cockerham, lifestyle factors (fat intake, salt intake, smoking, physical
1997; 1999). Poor medical care in Central and Eastern activity, etc.) in cardiovascular disease (heart disease and
Europe may make a limited contribution, though this is high blood pressure).
unlikely to account for more than 10% of the difference The hypothesised sense of powerlessness in East
in all-cause mortality (Boys, Forster, & Jozan, 1991). European citizens was indexed in terms of health locus
Environmental pollution may also play a significant of control. Health locus of control is a domain-specific
role, particularly in Poland, the former East Germany control construct that indexes beliefs in different types of
and the Czech Republic. However, it has been argued means-ends relationship (Skinner, 1996). Since it is
that health behaviours and psychosocial stress are the specific to the domain of health, it may not reflect
most important factors. Smoking, patterns of alcohol general perceptions of control. However, it is particu-
intake, and dietary factors such as fat and antioxidant larly relevant to the actions people carry out to maintain
consumption may all contribute, although there is health. We have previously shown that health locus of
uncertainty about the magnitude of differences between control is associated with the likelihood of healthy
Western Europe and the former Communist states of behavioural choices (Steptoe & Wardle, in press). The
Eastern Europe (Watson, 1995; Uitenbroek, Kerekovs- association between low sense of control over life in
ka, & Festchieva, 1996; Bobak et al., 1999). general (and health in particular) and self-rated health
The health behaviour explanation implies that Wes- has previously been described in a Russian sample
tern and Eastern Europeans may differ not only in (Bobak, Pikhart, Hertzman, Rose, & Marmot, 1998).
individual habits, but also in health-related lifestyles. A However, the measures of control had poor internal
healthy lifestyle implies healthy practices across a range consistency, and no comparison with other countries
of personal behaviours and activities. Comparisons of a was possible. In the present analysis, we used the well-
broad range of health behaviours between regions of established Multidimensional Health Locus of Control
Europe have been limited thus far. In the present study, scales that distinguish beliefs in internal control from
we therefore investigated the prevalence of unhealthy beliefs in chance, and beliefs in powerful others
options in twelve health behaviours, so as to obtain (specifically health professionals). We predicted that
evidence concerning broad lifestyle differences between respondents from Eastern Europe would have lower
Western and Eastern Europe. internal control beliefs and greater beliefs in chance than
We also investigated factors that might be associated Western Europeans. We also hypothesised that Eastern
with differences in health behaviour. Cockerham (1997) Europeans would have stronger beliefs in the influence
has argued that unhealthy lifestyles in Eastern Europe of powerful others over their health, since the Commu-
are determined less by individual choice than by nist political system might have engendered greater
structural constraints embodied in reduced life chances. dependence on authority than was apparent in Western
People may lack information about health and beha- Europe.
viour, or have little control over provision (limiting If differences in healthy lifestyles between Western
dietary and exercise choices), while living in societies in and Eastern Europeans do exist, they might also be
which unhealthy lifestyles are normative. These factors related to differences in valuations of health. People who
may be associated with a sense of powerlessness do not place a high priority on health within their value
and fatalism engendered by the political system systems are less likely to be concerned about carrying
(Nagorski, 1993). Watson (1995) has emphasised out activities that are hazardous to health in the long
that Eastern Europeans may experience cumulative term (Norman & Bennett, 1996). We therefore adminis-
frustration due to a mismatch between personal tered a standard measure of the value of health, to assess
aspirations and the means of achieving them, and an whether respondents from Western and Eastern Europe
inability to improve the material situation through differed in the importance they placed on health.
exclusion by the state from activity in the public sphere. Finally, we tested psychosocial explanations for
Chronic social stress resulting from feelings of dis- differences in health between Eastern and Western
advantage and uncertainty over fundamental shifts in Europe by assessing depressive symptoms and social
economic and political organisation may also contri- support. Differences in social stress might be expected to
bute, leading to a pervasive sense of lack of control. be translated into higher levels of depressive symptoms
We address these factors by assessing risk awareness, in Eastern Europeans, while greater social fragmenta-
locus of control, the valuation of health, social support tion could lead to reduced social support. The pre-
and depressive symptoms. If people living in Eastern valence in depressive symptoms assessed in national
Europe lack information about health and behaviour, representative surveys in Hungary increased markedly
A. Steptoe, J. Wardle / Social Science & Medicine 53 (2001) 1621–1630 1623

between 1988 and 1995, particularly in lower social Secondly, university students are relatively healthy. It is
status groups (Kopp, Szedmak, & Skrabski, 1998). known that health behaviours are influenced by illness
Direct evidence between East and West are limited thus and disability, so the inclusion of more diverse samples
far. However, it was found in a study of middle-aged would necessitate the collection of complex data
men that Lithuanians reported lower social support at concerning physical health and disability, complicating
work, lower job control, and high levels of depressive international comparisons (Gottlieb & Green, 1984).
symptoms than did Swedish men of the same age Thirdly, university students occupy a significant position
(Kristenson, et al., 1998). in public life. The politicians and policy makers of the
These issues were investigated in the present analysis future have typically been university students, and
using data from the European Health and Behaviour students themselves have played a major role in public
Survey (EHBS), a study of health behaviour, attitudes life and social change in many countries over the past
and knowledge in University students from 21 countries century. The attitudes and behaviours of university
carried out between 1989 and 1991 (Wardle & Steptoe, students are not only important in themselves, but are
1991; Steptoe & Wardle, 1996). The comparison in this also relevant to policies concerning health and preven-
analysis is between students in three East European tion in the future. A fourth advantage of studying
countries (former East Germany, Hungary and Poland) students is that data were typically collected in classes.
and West European student samples. There are im- Although participation was voluntary, response rates
portant variations between regions such as central exceeded 90% in most countries, far higher than is
Europe, Scandinavia and Mediterranean Europe in common when participants are invited individually to
dietary habits and other health-related behaviours complete surveys.
(Steptoe & Wardle, 1992). In order to provide an
appropriate comparison for the three East European
country samples, Western Europe was therefore repre-
sented by five geographically close countries (Austria, Method
Belgium, Netherlands, Switzerland and former West
Germany). The period of data collection was one of Participants
great political and economic change in the three Eastern
European countries. In November 1989, the Berlin wall The methodology and sampling in the EHBS have
was breached, and German reunification officially took been detailed elsewhere (Steptoe & Wardle, 1996). A
place 11 months later. In Hungary, the demise of the target sample of 700–800 male and female university
socialist state led to the election of a non-communist students of non-health subjects aged 18–25 were
government in the spring of 1990, while Poland saw the administered a self-completed structured questionnaire,
collapse of the communist regime at the end of 1989. using a uniform format in all languages. The ques-
This survey of personal health behaviour was not tionnaire was given to students in classes, and response
intended to provide a comparison of representative rates were generally greater than 90%. The test–retest
samples of young adults from different countries. reliability of the survey is satisfactory (Wardle &
University students are generally better educated than Steptoe, 1991). Table 1 summarises the number of
other young adults, and tend to originate from more students in the five Western and three Eastern European
privileged family backgrounds. Educational attainment countries included in this comparison. Separate samples
and other markers of social status are associated with
differences in healthy practices and with knowledge of
the role of behaviour in disease risk (Wardle et al., 1999; Table 1
Wardle, Parmenter, & Waller, 2000). The respondents in Gender and age distribution of participants
the study may therefore have carried out more healthy Total Men Women Age
behaviour and have been better informed about risks sample (mean  SD)
than young adults who had not entered tertiary
education. Nevertheless, there are several advantages Western Europe
to surveying university students. Firstly, it is imperative Austria 810 416 394 21.5  2.6
Belgium 1223 543 680 19.0  1.2
in international studies to compare like with like,
Netherlands 749 247 502 22.2  2.4
otherwise, no conclusions can be drawn about the Switzerland 597 242 355 22.2  2.7
pattern of results in different regions of the world. West Germany 791 400 391 23.5  5.6
University students are an easily identifiable, homo-
genous and accessible sector of the population. By Eastern Europe
restricting the range of respondents in terms of East Germany 738 351 387 22.3  2.2
Hungary 756 377 379 20.9  2.4
education, we were able to reduce differences between
Poland 799 399 400 21.8  2.1
country samples that were due to extraneous factors.
1624 A. Steptoe, J. Wardle / Social Science & Medicine 53 (2001) 1621–1630

were obtained from West and East Germany, since data Salt intake was assessed by asking respondents
were collected at the time of unification. Within each whether they added salt to their meals. They were
country, data were collected in the following centres: classified according to whether they usually added salt,
Vienna (Austria), Diepenbeek (Belgium), Leipzig (for- or added salt only sometimes, occasionally or never.
mer German Democratic Republic), Hamburg and Seat-belt use was assessed by asking respondents
Wuppertal (Federal Republic of Germany), Budapest whether they wore a seat-belt when riding or driving in
(Hungary), Groningen (the Netherlands), Bytom and the front seat of a car, all of the time, some of the time,
Krakow (Poland), and Lausanne (Switzerland). The or never. Those who responded with all of the time were
varying numbers of respondents from participating regarded as regular seat-belt users. Respondents who
countries reflects differences in the extent of the survey stated that they did not ride in cars were excluded from
in each centre, and not variations in response rate. this analysis.
Data concerning health behaviours and risk aware- Sunscreen use was assessed by asking participants if
ness were available from all countries. Data related to they used sun protection, cream or lotion when
depression, social support, health locus of control and sunbathing. Participants who stated that they never
value of health were collected in two of the three East sunbathed were excluded from these analyses.
European countries (East Germany, Poland) and three Tooth brushing was categorised according to whether
West European countries (Netherlands, Switzerland, or not participants brushed their teeth at least daily.
West Germany). Breast self-examination for women and testicle self-
examination for men were assessed by asking respon-
Health behaviours dents if they knew how to carry out such examinations,
and if so how frequently they carried them out (for
Twelve health behaviours were investigated in this details, see Wardle et al., 1994, 1995). Respondents were
analysis. Data were analysed using logistic regression, so considered to be carrying out these self-examinations
that each behaviour rating was transformed into binary regularly if they examined themselves ten or more times
outcomes, with the ‘‘healthy’’ option being coded 0, and per year.
the ‘‘unhealthy’’ option coded 1. A healthy lifestyle index was constructed by summing
Cigarette smoking was assessed with standard mea- the number of healthier options endorsed by each
sures (Steptoe, et al., 1995). Participants were defined as individual. This could range from 0 (no healthy
regular smokers if they reported smoking more than one behaviours) to 11 (all healthy behaviours). Respondents
cigarette per day. who scored positively on the majority of items (i.e. six or
Exercise was assessed by asking participants if they more) were deemed to have a healthy lifestyle. A second
had taken any exercise, or carried out any sport or 6-item index was compiled of behaviours that might be
physically active pastime over the last two weeks. They considered most important for health (smoking, alcohol
were also asked how many times they had exercised over intake, exercise, fat consumption, fibre consumption and
the past two weeks. Lack of regular exercise was defined use of a seat-belt). In this case, a score of four or more
as exercising four or fewer times over the past two was taken to indicate a healthy lifestyle.
weeks.
Alcohol consumption was assessed by asking partici- Risk awareness
pants if they were non-drinkers, occasional or regular
drinkers. Those who drank alcohol were also asked on Awareness of risks associated with different aspects of
how many days over the past two weeks they had drunk, lifestyle were assessed by asking participants with simple
and how many drinks they had consumed on average on yes/no responses whether they were of the opinion that
each occasion. The proportion of regular alcohol high blood pressure was influenced (either positively or
drinkers was relatively low in this survey, with only negatively) by salt intake, alcohol consumption, and
18.3% drinking an average of one or more drinks per physical exercise, and whether heart disease was
day. Respondents were therefore categorised according influenced by smoking, physical exercise and fat
to whether or not they drank alcohol. consumption. These items were extracted from a larger
Fat consumption was assessed by asking whether or array of risk awareness measures on the grounds that
not the respondent made a conscious effort to avoid they had been endorsed in a survey of experienced public
eating foods that contain fat and cholesterol. health physicians and epidemiologists in European
Fibre consumption was assessed by asking whether or universities (Steptoe & Wardle, 1994).
not the respondent made a conscious effort to eat foods
that are high in fibre. Other measures
Fruit consumption was assessed in terms of frequency.
Participants were divided according to whether or not Depressive symptoms were assessed with the shortened
they ate fruit daily. Beck depression inventory (BDI, Beck & Beck, 1972).
A. Steptoe, J. Wardle / Social Science & Medicine 53 (2001) 1621–1630 1625

This consists of 14 items from the full 21 item BDI, and and age were entered simultaneously into the regression
is known to correlate well with other measures of models. Western Europe was taken as the reference
depression (Beck, Steer, & Garbin, 1988). Scores of five group. Odds, adjusted for age and sex, for Eastern
or more were taken to define moderate depressive European students engaging in ‘‘unhealthy’’ behavioural
symptoms, as recommended by Beck and Beck (1972). options, being aware of links between behaviours and
Social Support was assessed using the six-item version cardiovascular disease risk, and having depressive
of the Social Support Questionnaire (Sarason, Sarason, symptoms or low social supports are presented. The
Shearin, & Pierce, 1987). Measures of social support 95% confidence intervals for odds ratios and p values
availability and social support satisfaction were derived obtained by logistic regression were obtained using Stata
from the Social Support Questionnaire. The social (Stata Corporation, College Station, TX).
support availability measure was the average number
of people who could be called upon to support the
individual in the six difficult situations specified. Results
Participants were classified according to whether or
not they could call upon more than two people for social Table 1 summarises the study sample. There was a
support. Satisfaction with support was assessed with a total of 6463 men and women in the survey, with 4170
six point scale ranging from 1=very dissatisfied to from West European and 2293 from East European
6=very satisfied. A threshold of 4.5 divided participants countries. There were a greater number of women than
with high and low satisfaction. men in the samples from Belgium, Netherlands and
Health locus of control was assessed using the Multi- Switzerland, with broadly equal numbers in other
dimensional Health Locus of Control scales devised by countries.
Wallston, Wallston, and De Vellis (1978). This consists
of 18 items, each of which is rated on a six-point scale Health behaviours
from strongly disagree to strongly agree. Six items
contribute to the internal locus of control scale (e.g. ‘‘I The raw frequencies of unhealthy behaviours and the
can pretty much stay healthy by taking good care of adjusted odds of unhealthy behaviours in East and West
myself’’), six to the powerful others scale (e.g. ‘‘following European samples are shown in Table 2. There were
doctor’s orders to the letter is the best way for me to stay more regular smokers in West than in East European
healthy’’), and six to the chance locus of control scale samples. For seven of the remaining twelve behaviours,
(e.g. ‘‘when I become ill, it’s a matter of fate’’). Ratings the odds of unhealthy behaviour were greater in Eastern
were summed, so scores on each scale could range from than Western Europeans. After adjustment for age and
6 to 36, with higher scores indicating greater beliefs in sex, students from Eastern Europe were significantly
internal, powerful others and chance control over more likely than Western Europeans not to take regular
health. The multidimensional health locus of control exercise, to drink alcohol, to make no efforts to avoid
scales have been used extensively in health research, and dietary fat and eat fibre, to add salt to their food, not to
have adequate internal consistency (Wallston, Wallston wear seat-belts, and not to use sunscreen. There were no
& DeVellis, 1978; Norman & Bennett, 1996). Health differences for fruit consumption. Regular breast self-
locus of control scales were analysed by dividing the examination and testicle self-examination was infre-
sample by median split into low and high scoring quent in the entire cohort, and were unrelated to
groups. European region. Eastern Europeans were more likely
Value of Health. The value that individuals place on to brush their teeth daily, but the absolute numbers of
health as a priority in their lives was assessed with the respondents not brushing their teeth regularly was very
Health as a Value scale (Lau, Hartman, & Ware, 1986). small (2.8% versus 1.2%). These analyses indicate that
This consists of four items (e.g. ‘‘If you don’t have your with the exception of smoking, tooth brushing and self-
health, you don’t have anything’’), each of which was examination, fewer East than West European students
rated on the same six point scale as health locus of engaged in protective health behaviours.
control. Ratings were summed, so could range from 4 to Analysis of the combined healthy lifestyle index
24, with higher scores indicating greater value placed on indicated that 47.1% of Eastern Europeans had a
health, and analysed after dividing by median split into healthy lifestyle, compared with 63.4% of Western
low and high groups. Europeans. Adjusted odds of having a healthy lifestyle
for East European students were 0.52 (C.I. 0.47–0.58,
Statistical analysis p50.001). On the more restricted six-item index, 36.1%
of Western Europeans scored four or more, compared
Data were analysed using multiple logistic regression with 23.9% of Eastern Europeans. The adjusted odds of
on each behaviour, risk awareness measure, and well- having a lifestyle score of four or more for East
being and attitudinal measure. Regions (West, East), sex European students were 0.57 (C.I. 0.50–0.63, p50.001).
1626 A. Steptoe, J. Wardle / Social Science & Medicine 53 (2001) 1621–1630

The regional comparisons were adjusted for age and in the West European countries were more likely to be
sex, as the latter two factors were also related to health aware of links between high blood pressure and salt
behaviours. Although not the topic of this report, it intake, high blood pressure and alcohol consumption,
should be noted that overall, women were less likely and high blood pressure and exercise. They were also
than men to be regular smokers or alcohol drinkers, and more likely to be aware of the associations between
were more likely to avoid fat, eat fibre, eat fruit, use a heart disease and smoking, heart disease and exercise,
seat-belt, use sunscreen and brush their teeth daily. Men and heart disease and fat consumption. All these effects
on the other hand, were more likely to exercise regularly were significant after adjusting for age and sex; they
and to avoid adding salt to their food. Older participants indicate lower risk awareness among East European
were more likely than younger individuals to be students.
smokers, alcohol drinkers, not to eat fruit daily, and
to add salt to their food. But on the positive side, older
respondents were more likely to avoid fat, eat fibre, use Emotional well-being, social support, and locus of control
seat-belts, sunscreen, brush their teeth regularly, and to
carry out regular breast and testicle self-examination. A rather consistent pattern of differences between
West and East European students emerged from these
Risk awareness measures. Eastern Europeans were substantially more
likely to have depressive symptoms and to report low
Table 3 summarises the proportion of students from social support than Western Europeans. The unadjusted
Eastern and Western Europe who were aware of links proportion of respondents with depressive symptoms
between lifestyle factors and high blood pressure or above threshold was 43.2% in East and 23.5% in West
heart disease. Odds ratios above one indicate a greater European country samples. East European students
awareness of the links between lifestyle and health in were more than twice as likely to have few sources of
East compared with West European students. Students social support, and to be dissatisfied with social support.

Table 2
Odds of unhealthy behaviours in students from Eastern and Western Europe

Western Eastern Odds adjusted for age Significant


Europe (%) Europe (%) and sex [95% CI]

Regular smoking (>1/day) 22.9 19.8 0.79 [0.69–0.90] 0.001


Lack of regular exercise 63.8 70.3 1.37 [1.22–1.53] 0.001
Alcohol consumption 86.6 89.4 1.22 [1.04–1.44] 0.018
No avoidance of dietary fat 58.3 63.1 1.20 [1.07–1.34] 0.001
No effort to eat fibre 56.8 69.3 1.74 [1.55–1.94] 0.001
Eating fruit less than daily 45.7 47.3 1.01 [0.91–1.12] ns
Usually adding salt to food 31.0 45.9 1.88 [1.69–2.08] 0.001
Not always using a seat-belt 18.4 42.4 3.29 [2.93–3.80] 0.001
Not using sunscreen when sunbathing 21.9 32.6 1.68 [1.48–1.90] 0.001
Not brushing teeth daily 2.8 1.2 0.41 [0.27–0.62] 0.001
Not carrying out breast self-examination regularly 90.5 89.0 0.91 [0.71–1.16] ns
Not carrying out testicle self-examination regularly 96.2 97.0 1.26 [0.82–1.94] ns

Table 3
Awareness of risk of links between lifestyle and health: proportions of Eastern and Western students, and adjusted odds

Western Eastern Odds adjusted for age Significance


Europe (%) Europe (%) and sex [95% CI]

High blood pressure and salt intake 65.5 56.2 0.66 [0.59 to 0.73] 0.001
High blood pressure and alcohol consumption 58.6 52.7 0.77 [0.70 to 0.86] 0.001
High blood pressure and exercise 45.4 42.8 0.88 [0.79 to 0.98] 0.017
Heart disease and smoking 65.2 57.1 0.69 [0.62 to 0.76] 0.001
Heart disease and exercise 53.0 42.7 0.70 [0.63 to 0.77] 0.001
Heart disease and fat consumption 73.8 66.4 0.68 [0.61 to 0.76] 0.001
A. Steptoe, J. Wardle / Social Science & Medicine 53 (2001) 1621–1630 1627

There was no difference in internal health locus of students in Hungary (Piko, 1996). It is likely that the
control. However, Eastern Europeans reported signifi- diffusion of healthier smoking habits would commence
cantly higher beliefs in powerful others and higher in young, educated sectors of the population. The recent
beliefs in chance locus of control. They therefore had a WHO cross-national survey of health-related behaviour
greater external health locus of control orientation than in national representative samples of 15-year olds
Western Europeans. Contrary to our prediction, East showed that daily smoking averaged 21% in the Western
European students placed a higher value on their health European and 21.3% in the Eastern European countries
than West European students. studied here (Currie, Hurrelmann, Settertobulte, Smith,
These analyses were carried out using logistic & Todd, 2000).
methods, dividing scores according to somewhat arbi- The differences between West and East European
trary thresholds. However, a similar pattern of results samples in other health behaviours were similar to those
emerged from analyses of variance of the depression, that have been described elsewhere. For example, the
social support, locus of control and health value scores, higher level of regular exercise in West European
covarying for age and sex. students is similar to the pattern observed in 15-year
olds, where 65.2% of Western Europeans exercised more
than 2 h a week, compared with 52.3% of Eastern
Discussion Europeans (Currie et al., 2000). The East European
students in the present study were less likely to make
The results of these analyses indicate that the sample efforts to avoid fat and to eat fibre than their Western
of university students from Eastern Europe report that counterparts. In the WHO cross-national study, 11% of
they have relatively less healthy lifestyles than Western 15-year olds from Eastern Europe ate chips or fried
European students do. They were also less knowledge- potatoes every day, compared with 3% of Western
able about the health risks associated with various Europeans, and daily consumption of chocolate and
aspects of lifestyle, more depressed, reported lower other sweets was reported in 52.3% versus 39%. At the
social support, and had higher external health locus national level, the estimated number of calories per day
control beliefs. Because the study was restricted to derived from animal fat in 1990 averaged 309 in the five
university students, the prevalence of health behaviours West European countries studied here, compared with
and other factors are not representative of the countries 423 in Hungary and Poland (Food and Agriculture
involved. The profiles come from comparatively healthy, Organization, 1999). By contrast, the proportion of total
well-informed and educated sectors of the young energy derived from fat, fruit and vegetables was 6% in
population. Nevertheless, there is no reason to think West European and 4% in East European countries.
that the kinds of differences between West and East The higher prevalence of alcohol consumption in East
recorded here would not be observed in the entire European students replicates findings in other surveys
population. (Cockerham, 1999).
Direct comparisons with patterns of health behaviour Our observation that East European students were
recorded in wider samples are not possible, since we are less likely to use seat-belts when driving in the front seat
not aware of other studies that have collected data using of a car is particularly interesting in the light of the
uniform coding systems in similar sectors of the excess mortality from external causes such as accidents
population in these countries. One striking discrepancy that has been described in former socialist states (Bobak
is between the analyses of regular smoking and & Marmot, 1996). The lower levels of sunscreen use
consumption of tobacco in West and East European might place the East European sample at raised risk for
countries. As noted in Table 2, the prevalence of regular skin cancers as well. There were no differences between
smoking was lower among East than West European European regions in the two behaviours carried out
student samples. East European countries have among exclusively for detecting cancer: breast self-examination
the highest rates in the world of premature death and testicle self-examination. However, the frequency of
attributable to smoking in both men and women (Peto, these behaviours was very low, so variance was
Lopez, Boreham, Thun, & Health, 1994). The World restricted (Wardle et al., 1994, 1995).
Health Organization (1997) estimated that in 1990–1992, We would argue that the pattern of less healthy
Poland ranked the highest of 111 countries in terms of lifestyles observed among Eastern European students is
annual per capita consumption of cigarettes. Hungary not simply a product of individual choice and disregard
was third, with Switzerland ranked fifth, the Nether- for health. Indeed, the data indicate that the East
lands eighth, and the three other countries in this study European sample placed a higher value on their health
lower down. However, these figures are averages across than did Western Europeans (Table 4). Rather, the
the entire adult population of all ages and levels of pattern arises from structural factors leading to lower
education. A similar prevalence of smoking to the one provision of health information and greater dependence
we observed has been reported in a survey of medical on external forces for health maintenance. The analyses
1628 A. Steptoe, J. Wardle / Social Science & Medicine 53 (2001) 1621–1630

Table 4
Emotional well-being, social support and locus of control in students from Eastern and Western Europe

Western Eastern Odds adjusted for age Significance


Europe (%) Europe (%) and sex [95% C.I.]

Depression 23.5 43.2 2.46 [1.95–3.09] 0.001


Social support: low number of supports 13.9 36.9 3.44 [2.61–4.52] 0.001
Social support: low satisfaction 15.7 31.6 2.31 [1.76–3.04] 0.001
Low internal health locus of control 35.3 31.4 0.86 [0.71–1.04] ns
High powerful others health locus of control 44.4 63.1 2.19 [1.83–2.64] 0.001
High chance health locus of control 40.0 50.2 1.53 [1.28–1.83] 0.001
Low value of health 52.0 42.1 0.68 [0.56–0.83] 0.001

of risk awareness indicated that East European students external health control beliefs in Eastern Europeans. It
were less likely than Western students to know that has previously been suggested that internal and chance
heart disease was related to smoking, exercise or fat health locus of control beliefs are predictive of health
intake, and were also less aware of links between high behaviour in healthy populations, while powerful other
blood pressure and salt intake, exercise and alcohol ratings are more relevant to people suffering from
consumption. Data from other comparative studies are chronic illness (Furnham & Steele, 1993). Against
not available, but there is evidence that only a small prediction, there were no differences in internal health
minority of Polish adults with high blood pressure locus of control beliefs, but both aspects of external
attempt to limit salt intake and dietary fat (Krupa- control (beliefs in chance and beliefs in powerful others)
Wojciechowska, Narkiewicz, & Rynkiewicz, 1996). If were higher in the East European sample. The greater
people are less knowledgeable about the role of lifestyle belief in powerful others may be a residue of dependence
factors in the avoidance of disease, then health motives on a centralised state and reliance on authority for
for lifestyle change may be diminished. health advice and health maintenance. However, the
There were striking differences between West and East higher ratings of belief in the role of chance in health
European samples in depressed mood and perceptions of suggest that fatalistic attitudes and a sense of pessimism
social support. Depression has been recognised as a may prevail among these young adults.
major health problem among students (Allgöwer, 2000). A number of limitations to these analyses should be
Nearly twice as many Eastern than Western Europeans recognised. The survey was cross-sectional and based on
had scores above the criterion for moderate depression, self-report, so causal conclusions linking health beha-
suggesting that the problem was widespread. There is viours with risk awareness, mood and beliefs cannot be
limited evidence for similar differences between Eastern drawn. Data were self-reported, and patterns were not
and Western Europe in working populations (Kristen- verified objectively. Perceptions of control were indexed
son et al., 1998). The difference in social support is also by health locus of control, and not by a general measure
important in the light of the protective role that social of sense of control or powerlessness. Differences
relationships play in coping with stress and maintaining between countries in access to higher education could
health and well-being (Cohen & Syme, 1985). have influenced the comparisons made here. The sample
It is not possible from this cross-sectional survey to collected was not large enough to allow reliable
conclude that differences in depression or social support comparisons to be made between individual countries.
contribute to the less healthy patterns of personal At the time of data collection, the East European
behaviour reported by respondents from Eastern Eur- countries were experiencing considerable turmoil result-
ope. We have previously shown in the larger survey, ing from political change, and the dismantling of the
from which the present data set was derived, that collective welfare system which included (however
depression is associated with a range of less healthy ineffectively) free health and education services, sub-
behaviours, while social support has the reverse effect sidised accommodation, and job security for many
(Allgöwer, Wardle, & Steptoe, in press). It is plausible (Millard, 1992; Szalai & Oronsz, 1992). In the period
both that depressed mood leads to patterns such as immediately following data collection, the economic
sedentary behaviour, alcohol consumption and lack of output fell substantially in Poland and Hungary, while
care about seat-belt use, and that behaviours such as unemployment increased (Monee Project, 1999). The
physical exercise contribute to improved mood states. results may therefore reflect the specific conditions
The results of this analysis suggest that negative prevailing in 1989 and 1990. On the other hand, recent
moods and low social support are accompanied by more surveys suggest that health practices may continue to
A. Steptoe, J. Wardle / Social Science & Medicine 53 (2001) 1621–1630 1629

deteriorate in Eastern Europe. For example, the Junker, R., Fobker, M., Schulte, H., Assmann, G., &
prevalence of cigarette smoking among secondary Marmot, M. (1999). An ecological study of determinants of
school students aged 15–18 years in Budapest increased coronary heart disease rates: A comparison of Czech,
from 36% in 1995 to 46% in 1999 (CDC, 2000). We are Bavarian and Israeli men. International Journal of Epide-
currently collecting further data from similar samples in miology, 28, 437–444.
Bobak, M., & Marmot, M. (1996). East–west mortality divide
Western and Eastern Europe, in order to establish
and its potential explanations: proposed research agenda.
whether differences are stable or have changed over the British Medical Journal, 312, 421–425.
past decade in this sector of the population. Bobak, M., Pikhart, H., Hertzman, C., Rose, R., & Marmot,
Our results suggest that lifestyle factors may con- M. (1998). Socioeconomic factors, perceived control and
tribute to differences in health and well-being between self-reported health in Russia. A cross-sectional survey.
Western and Eastern Europe, and that these differences Social Science and Medicine, 47, 269–279.
are associated with poorer health knowledge, a greater Boys, R. J., Forster, D. P., & Jozan, P. (1991). Mortality from
sense of powerlessness over health, social isolation and causes amenable and non-amenable to medical care: The
depressed mood in educated young adults in East experience of Eastern Europe. British Medical Journal, 303,
European countries. 879–883.
CDC. (2000). Prevalence of cigarette smoking among secondary
school students } Budapest, Hungary, 1995 and 1999.
MMWR, 49, 438-441.
Acknowledgements Cockerham, W. C. (1997). The social determinants of the
decline of life expectancy in Russia and Eastern Europe: A
The authors are most grateful to Martin Bobak for lifestyle explanation. Journal of Health and Social Behavior,
his comments on these analyses and earlier drafts of 38, 117–130.
the manuscript. The EHBS was supported in part Cockerham, W. C. (1999). Health and social change in Russia
by the European Commission BIOMED program, and Eastern Europe. New York: Routledge.
and by the Economic and Social Research Council, Cohen, S., Syme, S. L. (Eds.), (1985). Social support and health.
UK. New York: Academic Press.
Currie, C., Hurrelmann, K., Settertobulte, W., Smith, R.,
The following colleagues participated in data collec-
Todd, J. (Eds.), (2000). Health and health behaviour among
tion: Austria: Prof. Margit Koller and Prof. Elisabeth young people. Copenhagen: World Health Organization.
Groll-Knapp (Vienna); Belgium: Prof. Jan Vinck Food and Agriculture Organization (1999). Food balance
(Diepenbeek); East Germany: Dr. Konrad Reschke sheets. Food and Agriculture Organization of the United
(Leipzig); West Germany: Dr. Claus Vögele (Marburg) Nations. Available: http://www.fao.org.
and Prof. Gudrun Sartory (Wuppertal); Hungary: Prof. Furnham, A., & Steele, H. (1993). Measuring locus of control:
Maria Kopp and Dr. Árpad Skrabskı́ (Budapest); The A critique of general, children’s, health- and work-related
Netherlands: Prof. Robbert Sanderman (Groningen); locus of control questionnaires. British Journal of Psychol-
Poland: Dr. Zbigniew Zarczynski (Krakow) and Prof. ogy, 84, 443–479.
Andrzej Brodziak (Bytom); Switzerland: Prof. Ruth Gottlieb, N. H., & Green, L. W. (1984). Life events, social
networks, life-style and health: An analysis of the 1979
Burckhardt and Dr. Laurent Rossier (Lausanne).
national survey of personal Health practices and conse-
quences. Health Education Quarterly, 11, 91–105.
Hertzman, C., Kelly, S., & Bobak, M. (Eds.), (1996). East–west
References life expectancy gap in Europe: Environmental and non-
environmental determinants. Dordrecht, NL: Kluwer Aca-
Allgöwer, A. (2000). Gesundheitsförderung an der Universität: demic.
Zur gesundheitlichen Lage von Studierenden. Opladen: Kopp, M. S., Szedmak, S., & Skrabski, A. (1998). Socio-
Leske+Budrich. economic differences and psychosocial aspects of stress in a
Allgöwer, A., Wardle, J., & Steptoe, A. (in press). Depressive changing society. Annals of the New York Academy of
symptoms, social support and personal health behaviors in Sciences, 851, 538–543.
young men and women. Health Psychology, in press. Kristenson, M., Kucinskiene, Z., Bergdahl, B., Calkauskas, H.,
Beck, A. T., & Beck, R. W. (1972). Screening depressed patients Urmonas, V., & Orth-Gomer, K. (1998). Increased
in family practice. Postgraduate Medicine, 52, 81–85. psychosocial strain in Lithuanian versus Swedish men:
Beck, A. T., Steer, R. A., & Garbin, M. G. (1988). the LiVicordia study. Psychosomatic Medicine, 60,
Psychometric properties of the Beck Depression inventory: 277–282.
Twenty-five years of evaluation. Clinical Psychology Review, Krupa-Wojciechowska, B., Narkiewicz, K., & Rynkiewicz, A.
8, 77–100. (1996). Awareness and treatment of arterial hypertension in
Bobadilla, J. L., Costeool, C. A., & Mitchell, F. (Eds.), (1997). Poland. Journal of Human Hypertension, 10 (Suppl. 3),
Premature death in the new independent states. Washington, 25–27.
DC: National Academy Press. Lau, R. R., Hartman, K. A., & Ware, J. E. (1986). Health as a
Bobak, M., Hense, H. W., Kark, J., Kuch, B., Vojtisek, P., value: methodological and theoretical considerations.
Sinnreich, R., Gostomzyk, J., Bui, M., von Eckardstein, A., Health Psychology, 5, 25–43.
1630 A. Steptoe, J. Wardle / Social Science & Medicine 53 (2001) 1621–1630

Millard, F. (1992). Social policy in Poland. In B. Deacon (Ed.), Steptoe, A., Wardle, J., Smith, H., Kopp, M., Skrabski, A.,
The new Eastern Europe: Social policy past, present and Vinck, J., & Zotti, A. M. (1995). Tobacco smoking in young
future (pp. 118–143). London: Sage. adults from 21 European countries: Associations with
Monee Project. (1999). After the fall: The human impact of ten attitudes and risk awareness. Addiction, 90, 571–582.
years of transition. Florence: UNICEF. Szalai, J., & Oronsz, E. (1992). Social policy in Hungary. In B.
Nagorski, A. (1993). The birth of freedom: Shaping lives and Deacon (Ed.), The New Eastern Europe: Social policy past,
societies in the new Eastern Europe. New York: Simon & present and future(pp. (pp. 144–166). London: Sage.
Schuster. Uitenbroek, D. G., Kerekovska, A., & Festchieva, N. (1996).
Norman, P., & Bennett, P. (1996). Health locus of control. In Health lifestyle behaviour and socio-demographic charac-
M. Conner, & P. Norman (Eds.), Predicting health behaviour teristics. A study of Varna, Glasgow and Edinburgh. Social
(pp. 62–94). Buckingham & Philadelphia: Open University Science and Medicine, 43, 367–377.
Press. Wallston, K. A., Wallston, B. S., & DeVellis, R. F. (1978).
Peto, R., Lopez, A., Boreham, J., Thun, M., & Health, C. Development of the multidimensional health locus of
(1994). Mortality from smoking in developed countries 1950– control (MHLC) scale. Health Education Monographs, 6,
2000. Oxford: Oxford University Press. 160–170.
Piko, B., Barabas, K., & Markos, J. (1996). Health risk Wardle, J., Farrell, M., Hillsdon, M., Jarvis, M., Sutton, S., &
behaviour of a medical student population: report on a Thorogood, M. (1999). Smoking, drinking, physical activity
pilot study. Journal of the Royal Society of Health, 116, and screening uptake and health inequalities. In D. Gordon,
97–100. M. Shaw, D. Dorling, & G. Davey Smith (Eds.), Inequalities
Sarason, I. G., Sarason, B. R., Shearin, E. N., & Pierce, G. in health (pp. 213–239). Bristol: University of Bristol/Policy
(1987). A brief measure of social support: Practical and Press.
theoretical implications. Journal of Social and Personal Wardle, J., Parmenter, K., & Waller, J. (2000). Nutrition
Relationships, 4, 497–510. knowledge and food intake. Appetite, 34, 269–273.
Skinner, E. A. (1996). A guide to constructs of control. Journal Wardle, J., & Steptoe, A. (1991). The European health and
of Personality and Social Psychology, 71, 549–570. behaviour survey: Rationale, methods and initial results
Steptoe, A., & Wardle, J. (1992). Cognitive predictors of health from the United Kingdom. Social Science and Medicine, 33,
behaviours in contrasting regions of Europe. British Journal 925–936.
of Clinical Psychology, 31, 485–502. Wardle, J., Steptoe, A., Burckhardt, R., Vögele, C., Vila, J., &
Steptoe, A., & Wardle, J. (1994). What the experts think: A Zarczynski, Z. (1994). Testicular self-examination: Attitudes
European survey of expert opinion about the influence of and practices among young men in Europe. Preventive
lifestyle on health. European Journal of Epidemiology, 10, Medicine, 23, 206–210.
195–203. Wardle, J., Steptoe, A., Smith, H., Groll-Knapp, E., Koller, M.,
Steptoe, A., & Wardle, J. (1996). The European health and Smith, D., & Brodziak, A. (1995). Breast self-examination:
behaviour survey: The development of an international Attitudes and practices among young women in Europe.
study in health psychology. Psychology and Health, 11, European Journal of Cancer Prevention, 4, 61–68.
49–73. Watson, P. (1995). Explaining rising mortality among men in
Steptoe, A., & Wardle, J. Locus of control and health Eastern Europe. Social Science and Medicine, 41, 923–934.
behaviour revisited: a multivariate analysis of young adults World Health Organization. (1997). Tobacco or health: First
from 18 countries. British Journal of Psychology, in press. global status report. Geneva: World Health Organization.

You might also like