You are on page 1of 16

Journal of Business Research 148 (2022) 174–189

Contents lists available at ScienceDirect

Journal of Business Research


journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/jbusres

The more the better? Data disclosure between the conflicting priorities of
privacy concerns, information sensitivity and personalization in
e-commerce
Matthias H.J. Gouthier *, Carina Nennstiel , Nora Kern , Lars Wendel
Institute for Management, University of Koblenz-Landau, Universitaetsstrasse 1, 56070 Koblenz, Germany

A R T I C L E I N F O A B S T R A C T

Keywords: E-commerce companies are beginning to use data analytics to individually address their customers, as such firms
Data privacy are strongly dependent on customers’ willingness to disclose (WTD) personal information. Conceptually known
Situational privacy calculus as privacy calculus, a cognitive comparison of the predicted benefits and risks of such a disclosure results in
Personalization
subjective expected utility (SEU). As this construct has not been elucidated in the context of situational factors,
Information sensitivity
Subjective expected utility (SEU)
namely personalization and information sensitivity, this research utilizes a quantitative experimental study
Willingness to disclose personal information design to analyze these factors and evaluate the impact of SEU on customers’ WTD. Based on an online survey,
(WTD) the study reveals a positive but decreasing effect of personalization on SEU, while identifying information
sensitivity as a negative influential factor. In general, the results emphasize the importance of a fair and
transparent exchange relationship to foster a data disclosure setting without acute data privacy concerns.

1. Introduction Xia, & Dhillon, 2021; Wieringa et al., 2019). Such enhanced customer
privacy concerns are considered to influence online activities in e-
The e-commerce sector has seen significant growth in recent years, commerce (Bansal, Mariam, & Gefen, 2010; Eastin, Brinson, Doorey, &
emphasizing a clear shift from offline to online activities. Worldwide, Wilcox, 2016; Martin, 2016; Tucker, 2019; Wieringa et al., 2019). A
retail e-commerce sales have increased from USD 2.382 trillion in 2017 study from Accenture (2020) revealed that approximately 70% of con­
to an estimated USD 4.927 trillion by 2021 (Sabanoglu, 2020). With the sumers are concerned about data privacy and commercial tracking
onset of the COVID-19 pandemic in 2020 and the associated changes in associated with their online activities, behaviors, locations, and in­
general purchasing behavior, e-commerce trade has been experiencing a terests. However, another study conducted by them found that about
surge in development (Monitor Deloitte, 2020). 83% of consumers are willing to share their data in exchange for a
While shopping online, customers leave traces in the form of sharing, personalized customer experience (Accenture Interactive, 2018).
browsing, and behavioral data that companies can use to learn more This observed tension between data privacy concerns and the desire
about customers and their needs. Furthermore, firms can exploit this to receive more relevant and personalized offers is highlighted by a
resource to design innovative digital marketing activities that create paradox in the literature describing the dichotomy between customers’
new business value (Martin, 2016; Spiekermann, Acquisti, Böhme, & general privacy concerns and actual disclosure behavior (Barth & de
Hui, 2015; Tucker, 2019). Jong, 2017). Customers can reduce or solve this disjunction by applying
Because of such lucrative opportunities, the collection, storage, and a cost–benefit evaluation to the prospect of information disclosure. The
usage of personal data in e-commerce have expanded, fueling a growing underlying intrapsychological decision-making process by which one
area of conflict between companies and customers. Although an assesses these uncertain future advantages (benefits) and disadvantages
increasing number of companies recognize the possession of usage- (costs) is known as situational privacy calculus (Culnan & Armstrong,
based and personal customer information as a critical success factor, 1999; Laufer & Wolfe, 1977a; Wilson & Valacich, 2012). The resulting
customers are concerned about data security and their right to infor­ evaluation of this trade-off between the expected benefits and costs of a
mational self-determination (e.g., Kokolakis, 2017; Kolotylo-Kulkarni, potential decision is referred to as the subjective expected utility (SEU).

* Corresponding author.
E-mail addresses: gouthier@uni-koblenz.de (M.H.J. Gouthier), cnennstiel@uni-koblenz.de (C. Nennstiel), noke@uni-koblenz.de (N. Kern), lars.wendel@web.de
(L. Wendel).

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jbusres.2022.04.034
Received 31 March 2021; Received in revised form 14 April 2022; Accepted 18 April 2022
Available online 6 May 2022
0148-2963/© 2022 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved.
M.H.J. Gouthier et al. Journal of Business Research 148 (2022) 174–189

In the context of subjective decision-making, one compares the different Ioannou, Tussyadiah, & Lu, 2020; Morando, Iemma, & Raiteri, 2014;
courses of action in terms of SEU, aiming to identify the alternative with Nissenbaum, 2010), but also contributes to the evolving literature on the
the highest expected value. Typically, a customer’s objective is not privacy paradox by exploring customer privacy issues.
solely to maximize the benefits, but also to attain the most attractive
trade-off between the two components that presents an acceptable level 2. Conceptual foundations and hypotheses
of risk (Zhu, Ou, van den Heuvel, & Liu, 2017). In e-commerce, SEU is
characterized by a customer’s perception of the attractiveness of a 2.1. Privacy concerns in the context of e-commerce
company-promised personalized communication measure, considering
the high level of uncertainty present in the form of data privacy risk (Li Companies collect an increasing amount of data to better understand
& Sarathy, 2007). Based on this uncertainty and the evaluation of future their customers; information on elements such as behaviors and atti­
benefits, customers determine their willingness to disclose personal in­ tudes allows them to optimize digital customer experiences in e-com­
formation (WTD) (e.g., Culnan & Bies, 2003). Although WTD has merce accordingly (Spiekermann et al., 2015; Tucker, 2019). While
already been the focus of several studies (e.g., Aiello et al., 2020; Kim & some data are publicly available, such as those found on social media
Kim, 2018; Trang & Weiger, 2020), research has yet to consider how the channels, other types of digital interactions between a company and its
level of personalization applied in communication and offers affects customers require that customers consent to the storage and use of their
one’s WTD. As a result, there is a limited understanding of the extent to data. However, customers are increasingly concerned about disclosing
which different levels of personalized content in marketing communi­ their data online as this entails an invasion of privacy (e.g., Kokolakis,
cations justify the collection of personal data in the customers’ eyes. An 2017).
overview of previous research and its key findings is given in Table 1, In the digital age, the term ‘privacy’ is primarily applied in the
showing great interest in the topic across disciplines and leaving space context of data use and associated data protection. Acquisti, Taylor, and
for further investigation, especially regarding the relationship between Wagman (2016) described privacy as a customer’s control over the
personalization, information sensitivity, and WTD. sharing of data, while Waks (1989) asserted that privacy means being
Personalization is considered not only a general benefit but also a able to protect personal and sensitive information against misuse.
situational incentive to influence the customer’s perception of data Protecting personal information becomes increasingly difficult in an
disclosure. This categorization of personalization as a situational factor online environment—mainly because of the growing possibilities
is based on the assumption that it is re-evaluated individually depending offered by data collection, analytics, and storage—as customers develop
on the situation at hand (Li, Sarathy, & Xu, 2010; Wilson & Valacich, deepening concerns about their privacy. Privacy concerns in the context
2012). In addition, the sensitivity of the personal information requested of communication measures focus on so-called “informational privacy”
plays a central role in the decision-making process: the more sensitive (Holvast, 1993; Kokolakis, 2017; Rosenberg, 1992). This level of privacy
the data, the greater the perceived risk and the lower the expected value is based on the right to informational self-determination. It considers the
customers associate with its disclosure (Mothersbaugh, Foxx, Beatty, & prerogative of individuals to determine whether and how their data are
Wang, 2012). Higher information sensitivity creates a disparity between collected, stored, and processed, which is particularly important in e-
the expected costs and benefits of information disclosure, which is commerce (Holvast, 1993; Rosenberg, 1992). Customers perceive per­
considered again in the context of situational privacy calculus. The main sonal information to differ in their intimacy depending on the sensitivity
issue is that current research on situational privacy calculus does not of the collected data (Mothersbaugh et al., 2012). Although control over
differentiate personal information in terms of information sensitivity, all kinds of information is required (Fried, 1984), researchers agree that
meaning that the actual weight of this situational influence remains to the more intimate the information, the deeper the intrusion into per­
be adequately analyzed. sonal rights and the more critical it is to protect them (Lwin, Wirtz, &
Customers also draw on prior experiences to validate their decisions Williams, 2007). Generally, privacy concerns can be regarded as atti­
regarding WTD (Campbell, 1997). The perceived fairness of similar tudes toward informational privacy. As in theory, attitudes serve as
exchanges in the past plays a role in Internet users’ information privacy reliable predictors of behavior, and it can be inferred that individuals
concerns (IUIPC). As concerns based on previous experiences are with positive attitudes toward informational privacy (minor privacy
generally considered an appropriate addition to situational privacy concerns) are more likely to disclose data than those with negative at­
calculus, IUIPC can be seen as a contextual factor that may influence a titudes (serious privacy concerns).
customer’s decision (Campbell, 1997; Xu, Dinev, Smith, & Hart, 2008).
Going beyond the purely cognitive comparison of the expected 2.2. Situational privacy calculus
benefits and costs embodied in SEU, this study evaluates how the
measure is affected by situational factors (i.e., personalization and in­ Despite harboring general privacy concerns and being aware of the
formation sensitivity) and IUIPC as a context-based factor. Thus far, associated risks, customers often disclose personal data online and make
there has been a lack of empirical evidence regarding a corresponding little active effort to protect it (Norberg, Horne, & Horne, 2007). This
privacy calculus that considers the degree of information sensitivity and discrepancy between attitudes and behaviors concerning data protection
level of personalization promised in a specific situation (e.g., Barth & de is known as the (information) privacy paradox (Belanger & Crossler,
Jong, 2017; Kokolakis, 2017). Therefore, we employed a scenario-based 2011; Kokolakis, 2017; Norberg et al., 2007; Smith, Dinev, & Xu, 2011).
experimental design to fill these research gaps, simulate a newsletter When consumers are asked directly about sharing data, their risk
subscription, and apply a structural equation modeling (SEM) approach perception will, for the most part, lead them to reject the prospect.
supplemented by a group comparison. By analyzing these situational Nevertheless, when involved in a digital marketing exchange that de­
factors, we aim to provide companies with innovative insights into how pends on sharing personal data, individual behavior shows no clear
to increase customers’ WTD, thus allowing personalized and individu­ consistency with the expressed attitude, which means that personal data
alized communication in e-commerce. are often actively disclosed (Norberg et al., 2007). Wilson and Valacich
Considering the aforementioned gaps in the literature, we set four (2012) employed privacy calculus as a theoretical basis to explain this
research goals: (1) to understand how personalization as a situational phenomenon and solve the paradox.
factor affects SEU in e-commerce, (2) to analyze how information To understand privacy calculus, privacy is viewed as an economic
sensitivity as a second situational factor influences SEU, (3) to test the good that can be traded for certain benefits, such as personalization or
impact of IUIPC on SEU, and (4) to investigate the effect of SEU on WTD. financial or social rewards (Bennett, 1995; Wilson & Valacich, 2012).
To fill these gaps, this research not only answers previous calls to learn Customers consider these positive outcomes along with any adverse
more about situational factors of privacy calculus (i.e., Kokolakis, 2017; consequences of disclosing personal data in terms of their privacy, such

175
M.H.J. Gouthier et al. Journal of Business Research 148 (2022) 174–189

Table 1
Literature review.
References Methodology Topic/Context Focus and main findings

Aiello et al. (2020) Mixed-method design (focus group, • Privacy concerns as a feeling of - Different levels of perceived warmth alleviate privacy concerns
online experiment, laboratory vulnerability - Asking for personal information at the end of a customer journey
experiment) • Role of perceived warmth leads to higher perceived warmth and lower privacy concerns
Bansal et al. (2010) Lab experiment • Personal health information - Role of personal dispositions in information disclosure (e.g.
health state, experience, culture)
- A strong significant impact of personal dispositions on factors
determining privacy concerns, trust, and disclosure behavior
Barth and de Jong Systematic literature review • Privacy paradox - Discrepancy between attitude and behavior
(2017) - User’s decision-making process is generally driven by two con­
siderations: (1) risk–benefit evaluation and (2) risk assessment
deemed to be none or negligible
- Irrational decision-making: act on intuition rather than a precise
trade-off
- Context-dependency of the privacy paradox
Eastin et al. (2016) Quantitative (Online survey) • Mobile commerce activities - Theoretical foundation of communication privacy management
• Privacy concerns of mobile users (CPM)
- Privacy concerns are formed by collection, control, awareness,
unauthorized secondary use, improper access, location tracking,
trust, and attitude
- Control, unauthorized access, trust, and attitude significantly
predicted 73% of the variance in mobile activity
- Concerns about perceived control and unauthorized access to
personal information as a negative influence on mobile activity
Ioannou et al. Model testing by online survey • Travelers’ online privacy concerns (TOPC) - Testing of a proposed theoretical model explaining antecedents
(2020) and willingness to share information and outcomes of TOPC
- Results extend the Antecedents – Privacy Concerns – Outcomes
(APCO) framework by individual factors that shape TOPC
- Context-dependence of privacy preferences
- Travelers have privacy concerns, but they are still willing to
share their behavioral data
- Case of biometric information: the disclosure decision is
dependent upon expected benefits rather than privacy concerns
Kim and Kim (2018) Quantitative (Online survey with a • Recommendation systems - Perceived benefits, convenience, and vulnerability have no
YouTube experience) • Five distinct types of information significant effects on WTD
- Perceived severity → WTD: positive significant effect
- Coping efficacy → WTD: positive effect (except for feedback
information)
- Self-efficacy: positive effect only on disclosing feedback
information
Kokolakis (2017) Literature review (interpretations, • General privacy concerns - Inconsistency of privacy attitude and behavior: paradox
different contexts, • Sharing information for relatively small - Need for further research
conceptualizations) rewards - Call of synthetic studies: diversity of personal information and
concerns
- Evidence of actual behavior (not self-reported)
Li et al. (2010) Experimental study design • Effects of competing situational benefits - Information disclosure as the trade-off of exchange benefits and
and risk factors on information disclosure two types of privacy beliefs (privacy protection belief and pri­
• E-commerce transaction vacy risk belief)
- The monetary reward effect is dependent upon the fairness of
information exchange
Morando et al. Literature review • Users’ valuation of personal data - Valuation of personal data dependent on contextual aspects, e.g.
(2014) creating conflicts between declared and revealed preferences
- Data portability as an enabler to increase consumer
empowerment
Mothersbaugh et al. Online experiment (fictional online • Antecedents of information disclosure, - Usage of prospect theory
(2012) TV program) including information sensitivity - Analysis of WTD in an online service context
- Higher information sensitivity → weaker effects on
customization benefits, but stronger effects of information
control and privacy concerns
- Customization benefits can overcome the negative effects of
sensitive information requests when concern is lower or control
is higher
- Suggestion to include information sensitivity as a moderator
- Privacy paradox may result from a failure to account for
information sensitivity
Trang and Weiger Questionnaire-based field study • Use of gamification to increase users’ WTD - Engaging with gamified apps influences the situational
(2020) processing or privacy issues and related disposition
- State of deep cognitive absorption: disclosure of more
information when there is a higher perception of privacy benefits
Wieringa et al. Literature review • Data analytics and privacy concerns - Specification of different functions in the context of data
(2019) analytics and privacy and their comparison on different levels
(customer, intermediary, firm)
- Identification of five responsibilities for personal data and
analytics (data collection, data verification, data storage, and
control; deriving insights; and disseminating insights)
(continued on next page)

176
M.H.J. Gouthier et al. Journal of Business Research 148 (2022) 174–189

Table 1 (continued )
References Methodology Topic/Context Focus and main findings

- Increasing the role of customers relative to responsibilities can


alleviate privacy concerns
- Consumers take on more personal data responsibilities: need to
be better equipped to investigate data veracity
- Customers should be more involved in data storage and control
Wilson and Valacich Development of a theoretical model • Disclosure behavior and potential - Actual behavior might be overridden by situational factors
(2012) and application of experimental irrational behavior - Decision-making is not fully rational
design
Xu et al. (2008) Development of an integrative model • Fragmented understanding of privacy - Discipline-dependent understanding of privacy
and its verification using a survey • Model testing with four types of websites - Integration of information boundary theory and cognitive
(e-commerce, social network, financial, processing
and healthcare) - Individual disposition to privacy and situational cues
- Importance of perceived privacy control
- Disposition to value privacy as an important predictor of privacy
risk
- Social norm as a predictor to value privacy
Xu, Luo, Carroll, Experimental design • Location-aware marketing (LAM) - Privacy concerns in the context of location-aware marketing
Rosson (2011) - Including personal characteristics and two types of
personalization approaches: overt & covert
- Variation in types show different influences of personalization on
trade-off, mediated through personal characteristics
Zhu et al. (2017) Application of utility model • Personalization of services and - Relative value and utility of private/personal information
information disclosure - Contradiction between privacy and personalization
- Introduction of a utility model based on multi-attribute utility
theory (MAUT)
- Significant distinctions of calculating benefits and costs among
three groups of consumers

as the risk of data misuse (Dinev & Hart, 2006; Hann, Hui, Lee, & Png, (Wilson & Valacich, 2012). This term assumes that the trade-off between
2007; Laufer & Wolfe, 1977b). Researchers classify this relationship as a anticipated gains and losses depends on situational circumstances and
cost–benefit calculation or cognitive risk–benefit comparison (Culnan & influences that ultimately form behavioral intentions. Furthermore,
Armstrong, 1999; Laufer & Wolfe, 1977a). dispositional factors such as social norms and attitudes can influence the
According to the Homo economicus theory, individual customers outcome of situational privacy calculus (Acquisti & Grossklags, 2005;
strive to maximize the benefits of their behavioral decisions (Culnan & Wilson & Valacich, 2012).
Bies, 2003). A precise understanding of the trade-off between future
costs and benefits is crucial for achieving this objective. The result of this
evaluation is seen as the rational foundation for decision-making in the 2.3. Subjective expected utility
context of protecting one’s data. When a customer evaluates an oppor­
tunity, the anticipated benefits of the resulting decision should be higher In e-commerce, following the theory of situational privacy calculus,
than the feared costs; extended to several alternatives for action, the an interactive negotiation between customers and companies about
decision would always fall on the option expected to have the highest disclosing certain information occurs. In general, customers have two
value (Culnan & Bies, 2003; Gerber, Gerber, & Volkamer, 2018). This distinct alternatives for action: accepting the disclosure of their data to
assumption is in line with SEU theory, which aims to maximize the receive certain benefits, such as personalized offers and communication,
positive difference between the expected benefits and costs of an alter­ or rejecting it, and thus abstaining from said benefits. A further sub-form
native (Zhu et al., 2017). of data disclosure can be seen in the concrete adjustment of data-sharing
Research has applied the evaluation of privacy calculus to several preferences, which typically occur when setting cookie preferences on
digital contexts and identified various benefits (e.g., Culnan & Bies, websites. However, because this sub-form is irrelevant to our specific
2003; Dinev & Hart, 2006; Hann et al., 2007; Li et al., 2010) that can application field of newsletters, it will not be considered further.
generally be classified as financial, functional, psychological, or social The choice of action alternative depends on the customer’s subjec­
(e.g., Bennett, 1995; Schiffman, Kanuk, & Hansen, 2008; Wilson & tively expected costs and desired benefits, whether the alternative is
Valacich, 2012). Some of the most important benefits of e-commerce are associated with a positive or negative value. The information disclosure
personalization, convenience (via shortened search time), and discounts agreement (action alternative 1) assumes, as a consequence, that various
(Zhu et al., 2017). These benefits are also seen as opportunity costs for types of information will be disclosed in exchange for expected benefits
data non-disclosure (Acquisti et al., 2016). In exchange for these bene­ provided by the company. With the rejection of data disclosure (action
fits, there are costs to be paid, some of which are difficult to grasp. The alternative 2), the consequence is that the data remain protected, but the
costs often entail a certain perceived risk and include negative conse­ customer receives no benefits. In this study, situational privacy calculus
quences in the case of data disclosure, such as losing possession of one’s is operationalized by the SEU. Following SEU theory, the action alter­
personal information, facing social criticism or denigration, or having natives identified are assessed based on their action consequences (Zhu
one’s data used for other unspecified purposes, such as data crime (Barth et al., 2017). Consequences of action are defined as all states that can
& de Jong, 2017; Gerber et al., 2018; Wilson & Valacich, 2012). result from the choice of an option (based on March, 1994). For
Based on the fundamental ideas of privacy calculus, recent literature example, the action consequences can be represented and assessed by
has emphasized the importance of situational privacy concerns (e.g., the disclosure of different pieces of information that result from agreeing
Acquisti, 2004; Li et al., 2010). For example, Wilson and Valacich (2012) to an action alternative. However, because the action consequences are
highlighted the relevance of situational factors for decisions in the never expected with absolute certainty, it is insufficient to judge the
context of data privacy as such details can exert a significant influence action alternatives using their consequences alone. Therefore, the sub­
on expected benefits or costs and thus the cost–benefit evaluation. jective probability of consequences occurring is also considered. Thus,
Accordingly, they proposed the term “situational privacy calculus” the consequences of action do not carry as much weight—positive or
negative—if they are assessed to have a low probability of occurring.

177
M.H.J. Gouthier et al. Journal of Business Research 148 (2022) 174–189

Accordingly, utility and uncertainty represent two central SEU reduce the subjective expected utility (Malhotra, Kim, & Agarwal,
variables. 2004). Accordingly, we propose the following hypothesis:
Because situational privacy calculus is investigated, it is crucial to
consider the various factors influencing the situation itself, and thereby H2: The higher the degree of information sensitivity, the lower the sub­
the cost–benefit calculation and resulting SEU. jective expected utility (SEU).

2.4. Factors influencing the situational privacy calculus Another aspect that should be considered when examining situa­
tional privacy calculus is IUIPC, which arises from a consumer’s general
In creating positive customer experiences in e-commerce, the attitude toward informational privacy. IUIPC consists of three di­
perception of benefits often depends on the expected level of personal­ mensions: the user’s concerns about collecting personal information
ization. By contrast, the perception of costs is largely determined by the from companies in the online context, user’s control over the informa­
sensitivity of the information to be disclosed (Kim & Kim, 2018). tion collected, and user’s awareness of how companies use that infor­
Therefore, companies tend to use personalization of content as an mation (Malhotra et al., 2004). As soon as customers find themselves in
incentive to increase the perception of benefits and justify the collection a situation in which they must weigh the risk of data disclosure, they
of sensitive data (Culnan & Armstrong, 1999; Morosan & DeFranco, draw on prior experience to compare the current situation with a similar
2015; Wirtz & Lwin, 2009). Both personalization (e.g., Xu, Luo, Carroll, decision-making situation in the past. In informational privacy, attitude
& Rosson, 2011) and information sensitivity (e.g., Li et al., 2010; depends on previous experiences regarding perceived fairness in ex­
Sheehan & Hoy, 2000) are essential situation-specific factors that in­ changes of this kind (Campbell, 1997). As more significant concerns
fluence privacy calculus. about information privacy can be expected to negatively affect cost-
The level of personalization can be expressed in terms of differenti­ benefit evaluations (Kim & Kim, 2018; Martin & Murphy, 2017), we
ation in online communication with the customer and categorized into derive the following hypothesis:
three forms (Fan & Poole, 2006; Vesanen, 2007). First, mass marketing
represents the zero level of personalization, as this strategy involves no H3: The greater the Internet users’ information privacy concerns (IUIPC),
differentiation in customer communication. This is known as standard­ the lower the subjective expected utility (SEU).
ized communication. Second, personalized customer communication
involves a superficial understanding of the customer, which makes it 2.5. Willingness to disclose personal information
possible to address the customer by name. Third, an even higher level of
personalization involves individualized communication. The content Overall, the outcome value of the SEU indicates whether one de­
can be adapted to the customer’s needs because the company has a termines the situation-specific cost-benefit trade-off to be positive or
deeper data-driven understanding of the customer (e.g., Fan & Poole, negative and how likely it is to consider the potential consequences of
2006). With any form of personalization, customers’ perceived benefits the evaluated action (Campbell, 1997). According to privacy calculus
from the incentive are based on subjective evaluations gleaned by theory, an individual opts for disclosure if the exchange is perceived as
comparing the benefits with their motives and goals (Xu et al., 2011; positive, with the expected benefits exceeding the costs, and against
Wilson & Valacich, 2012). In addition, potential benefits can only be disclosure if the anticipated costs of disclosure exceed the benefits
anticipated, as there is a time-lagged causality between disclosure and (Culnan & Bies, 2003). It can also be assumed that the situational pri­
the resulting benefits. Personalization can thus be understood as an vacy calculus operationalized by the SEU influences customers’ WTD
experience good (Ford, Smith, & Swasy, 1988) as its benefits can only be (Trang & Weiger, 2020). Hence, we propose the following hypothesis:
evaluated while consuming personalized communication measures. This
means that the situational evaluation of benefits and costs underlies H4: The higher the subjective expected utility (SEU), the greater the
customer subjectivity and uncertainty. Because personalization in the willingness to disclose personal information (WTD).
context of this study is assumed to be a situational factor influencing
SEU as a potential benefit (Chellappa & Sin, 2005; Wieringa et al., The research model depicted in Fig. 1 summarizes the hypothesized
2019), we propose the following hypothesis: relationships and serves as the conceptual basis of the empirical study.

H1: The higher the level of personalization, the higher the subjective ex­ 3. Methodology
pected utility (SEU).
This study employed a scenario-based research design to test the
However, the level of personalization depends on the breadth and hypotheses. To empirically investigate the effects of situational factors
depth of the information available to companies (Awad & Krishnan, and IUIPC as a contextual factor on SEU, as well as the impact of SEU on
2006). This means that online communication can only achieve a high customers’ WTD in the e-commerce context, the given scenarios utilized
level of personalization if the customer provides the company with newsletter subscriptions as a concrete measure for companies to
required information. However, information disclosure and the associ­ communicate with customers in a personalized manner.
ated loss of control over one’s data are perceived as risks owing to
general privacy concerns. These risks are reflected in the cost of the 3.1. Research design
situation-specific cost–benefit calculation, with the expected costs
depending on the sensitivity of the information required. Prior studies We chose to conduct our study in the application field of a newsletter
have repeatedly addressed and operationalized the relevance of infor­ subscription because it allows for realistic testing of different levels of
mation sensitivity (e.g., Bansal et al., 2010; Kim & Kim, 2018; Mother­ personalization and different degrees of information sensitivity.
sbaugh et al., 2012; Phelps, Nowak, & Ferrel, 2000; Xie, Teo, & Wan, Furthermore, it offers practical applicability and relevance, as e-mail
2006). In general, customers tend to attribute greater sensitivity to marketing is critical in e-commerce (e.g., Hudák, Kianičková, &
financial or health information than to general or lifestyle-related in­ Madleňák, 2017). We employed a 3 × 2 between-subjects design,
formation or information related to shopping habits and preferences manipulating the level of personalization (standardized, personalized,
(Gabisch & Milne, 2014; Smith et al., 2011). The perceived sensitivity of or individualized) and degree of information sensitivity (low vs. high).
information depends mainly on its specificity and whether an intrusion The study was conducted as an online survey in which participants were
into personal rights accompanies its disclosure (Nowak & Phelps, 1992). randomly assigned to one of six groups and confronted with a text
More sensitive data are then associated with higher costs and thus might describing a privacy-compliant newsletter subscription.

178
M.H.J. Gouthier et al. Journal of Business Research 148 (2022) 174–189

Fig. 1. Conceptual model.

We applied a scenario-based evaluation to determine how re­ approximately 50 participants each. During the survey period, we
spondents perceived the promised newsletter content regarding the collected 306 data sets using an online questionnaire distributed in
personal data to be disclosed. The introduction of the scenario-based Germany via the university network and other online portals (e.g., social
survey presented participants with specifics regarding the intended networks). We offered no incentives for participation. Owing to the
data collection to put themselves in the respective situation and un­ application field of a newsletter subscription and, more generally, the e-
derstand what was at stake (Shearer, 2004). The level of personalization commerce context, participants had to be at least 16 years of age.
was reflected in three different scenario descriptions reporting the However, as we set no further restrictions on participation or specific
benefits that participants could expect from disclosing information in selection procedures for the sample, we assumed random selection. This
the course of a newsletter subscription. The benefits increased according random selection was maintained in the assignment of participants to
to the level of personalization (standardized, personalized, and indi­ the survey scenarios, which will be specified in greater detail in the data
vidualized). Whereas standardization means sending out a newsletter in analysis.
the sense of a mass marketing approach without any personalized in­ At the beginning of the survey, we checked whether participants
formation, a personalization approach entails sending customer news­ displayed an adequate level of personal involvement in online data
letters with segment-specific communication. It offers targeted protection. Examining the data records revealed that six had an
communication to the customer group to which they have been assigned involvement score below the threshold of 3.0, which was deemed to be
based on their shared information. Individualization goes a step further, sufficient; these six records were then eliminated to ensure validity.
with communication and offers tailored to an individual’s interests and Another record was removed because of its very short processing time,
needs. In general, personalization constructs a superficial understanding leaving an effective sample of 299 data records. Of these, 45.2% were
of customers (e.g., knowing their names), while individualization is female, 54.5% were male, and 0.3% declined to indicate their gender.
more in-depth in incorporating individual needs and behavior based on Furthermore, there was a balanced distribution of female and male
the data collected (Fan & Poole, 2006; Vesanen, 2007). participants among the six study groups. In terms of age, the respondents
The two groups assigned to each of the three personalization sce­ were between 16 and 72 years, with an average age of 33.13 years.
narios differed regarding the second manipulation of information
sensitivity, namely, the type of personal information to be disclosed. The 3.3. Measures
use of information sensitivity in our experimental study design was
based on the categorization of potential information to be disclosed, When operationalizing the latent variables, we primarily used
given in Mothersbaugh et al.’s (2012) previously validated findings. The existing validated scales. To check whether the participants generally
information items were divided into eight categories, which were engaged in online data privacy, we used involvement as a control var­
mapped to two to five items each. The category “website perception” iable, operationalized according to a revised version of Zaichkowsky’s
was not applicable in our context because no simulation of a company’s (1985) personal involvement inventory. The measurement of this vari­
website was presented to the participants. Therefore, this category was able entails the use of a semantic differential on a seven-point Likert
eliminated. The items of the remaining seven categories were then split scale.
into two roughly equal sets according to the factorial design for this The central construct of the study is expressed in the SEU, as it
study, such that one study group shared information with lower sensi­ represents the result of a cognitive evaluation of situation-specific costs
tivity. Simultaneously, the other group was requested to share more and benefits, thus operationalizing situational privacy calculus. The
sensitive personal information. Contact, financial, and demographic consequences include the trade-off between personalization as a positive
information about the family circle represented sensitive data. On the consequence to be expected and the potential risk or costs one perceives
other hand, less sensitive data included demographic information about as being associated with disclosing personal data.
oneself, media usage, and information concerning one’s lifestyle and Generally, SEU is calculated using a product sum model, including
leisure time in both the online and offline contexts. Participants the product value of the subjective assessment of the alternative eval­
responded to questions regarding their willingness to disclose each piece uation and its probability of occurrence for each consequence of an
of information. action alternative. The sum of these results is the SEU of the action
alternative Hi (Kunz, 2004; Nau, 2006):
3.2. Sample ∑
SEU(Hi ) = Uij *pij
Based on the manipulation of personalization and information
j

sensitivity, we created a 3 × 2 factorial design consisting of six groups of Uij is mapped as the subjective evaluation of action consequence j for

179
M.H.J. Gouthier et al. Journal of Business Research 148 (2022) 174–189

action alternative i. Low evaluations would mean high costs or negative 4. Results
consequences for revealing information, while high evaluations indicate
expected benefits or positive consequences from the decision-maker’s 4.1. Manipulation check
point of view. The other element, pij, represents the subjectively assessed
probability of occurrence for action consequence j of action alternative i. Because of the study’s experimental design, it was essential to check
For each scenario in our study, we assessed customers’ perceptions of whether the manipulation of the two situational factors—information
the sensibility to disclose specific information in exchange for the ex­ sensitivity and personalization—was successful and led to the corre­
pected action consequence. Uij is metrically operationalized as rational sponding effects in the respective study groups. First, the manipulation
and monopolar on a scale of 0 to 10. This scale allows for not only of information sensitivity was implemented in such a way that half of the
statements about equal distances of ratios but also the setting of a nat­ groups had to evaluate the disclosure of a set of personal information
ural zero point; a score of 0 means that the respondent evaluates the with a lower degree of sensitivity, while the other groups had to do the
action consequence of disclosure to be entirely negative, and 10 in­ same with more sensitive data. We expected the SEU evaluation to be
dicates an entirely positive evaluation, going hand in hand with the more positive for the former than for the latter. The manipulation can be
benefits that are pledged as an incentive in the survey. The scale was proven via the boxplot representation of the corresponding effects in
normalized to an interval of 0 to 1. Fig. 2, which serves as a graphical overview of the distribution of the
The uncertainty measure (pij) tracks the subject’s probabilistic variable to be able to assess, for example, the dispersion and corre­
assessment of the extent to which the disclosure will be used for the sponding outliers.
purpose described in the scenario, and ranges from 0 (not certain at all; These results reveal a lower dispersion of the measured values and
0%) to 10 (absolutely certain; 100%). Therefore, the subjective proba­ higher SEU of the quartiles with low information sensitivity than those
bility of occurrence of the assessed action consequences was surveyed. with high sensitivity.
To calculate the SEU, the scale was normalized to an interval of 0 to 1. Second, the manipulation of personalization was implemented in a
Based on the arithmetic mean for Uij and pij determined by metric way that there were three groups, each evaluating a scenario of a
scaling, the overall net value of disclosure was calculated for all infor­ newsletter description with different levels of personalization (stan­
mation items given. dardized, personalized, individualized) as an expected benefit of infor­
In addition, context-specific dispositions of informational privacy mation disclosure. As highlighted by the boxplot depicting the effects of
can be represented as IUIPC, which, as a second-order construct, inte­ personalization, greater dispersion can be observed with increasing
grate the dimensions of control, awareness, and collection (Malhotra levels of personalization, each of which has higher utility values.
et al., 2004). We measured the dimensions on discrete, seven-point Simultaneously, the groups with standardized communication exhibited
Likert scales, with three items each to measure control and awareness a more homogeneous distribution, with few exceptions. This finding is
and four items for collection. WTD items were recorded on a four-point supported by the results shown in Fig. 3.
Likert scale (never willing, not very willing, somewhat willing, always Grouped by personalization level, with both low and high informa­
willing) adopted from Phelps et al. (2000). Unlike the other constructs in tion sensitivity combined, the situation measure increases overall as
this study, for WTD, it seemed reasonable to omit a middle category and personalization increases, allowing us to assume a generally successful
thereby elicit an expression of at least a tendency toward the (non) manipulation.
disclosure of personal information. Furthermore, as there were only two
possibilities in each scenario (disclosure or non-disclosure), the ten­
dencies given could help better classify actual behavior.

Fig. 2. Boxplot of information sensitivity (manipulation check).

180
M.H.J. Gouthier et al. Journal of Business Research 148 (2022) 174–189

Fig. 3. Boxplot of personalization levels (manipulation check).

Table 2
Latent variables, items, and indicator loadings.
Second-order First-order construct Items Indicator α
construct loadings

Level of personalization (own Scenarios of subscription to [1] standardized, [2] personalized, and [3] 1.000
operationalization) individualized newsletters
Information sensitivity (based on Low information sensitivity: marital status, first/last name, date of birth, gender, 1.000
Mothersbaugh et al., 2012) education level, monthly online purchases, weekly internet usage, hobbies,
brands/products, newspapers/magazines/books, favorite shows
High information sensitivity: private phone number, work phone number, mobile
phone number, postal address, fax number, credit card number, credit card
expiration date, yearly income, number of employed household members, number
of family members
IUIPC (Malhotra Control Consumer online privacy is really a matter of consumers’ right to exercise control 0.870 0.703
et al., 2004) and autonomy over decisions about how their information is collected, used, and
shared.
Consumer control of personal information lies at the heart of consumer privacy. 0.790
I believe that online privacy is invaded when control is lost and unwillingly 0.712
reduced as a result of a marketing transaction.
Awareness Companies seeking information online should disclose the way the data are 0.842 0.745
collected, processed, and used.
A good consumer online privacy policy should have a clear and conspicuous 0.767
disclosure.
It is very important to me that I am aware of and knowledgeable about how my 0.830
personal information will be used.
Collection It usually bothers me when online companies ask me for personal information. 0.817 0.852
When online companies ask me for personal information, I sometimes think twice 0.787
before providing it.
It bothers me to give personal information to so many online companies. 0.908
I’m concerned that online companies are collecting too much personal information 0.817
about me.
Subjective expected utility (own Consequence of action: How would you rate the disclosure of the following 1.000
operationalization based on Kunz (2004); Nau information in relation to the described scenario? (0 = entirely negative
(2006)) consequence; 10 = entirely positive consequence)
Probability of occurrence/uncertainty: How likely do you think it is that the
disclosure of the respective information will be used for the purpose of the above-
mentioned scenario? (0 = not certain at all; 10 = quite certain)
Willingness to disclose personal Please indicate your willingness to provide the following information to the online 1.000
information (Phelps et al., 2000) company (four-point scale: always willing, somewhat willing, not very willing, never
willing)

181
M.H.J. Gouthier et al. Journal of Business Research 148 (2022) 174–189

4.2. Data analysis and results 1.679 for the variance inflation factor (VIF) of the second-order
construct, indicating that no multicollinearity was present (Henseler
We applied partial least squares structural equation modelling (PLS- et al., 2009). The overall measurement model resulting for the first- and
SEM) to analyze the data collected and test our hypotheses. This method second-order constructs prove that the measurement model is suitable
has been gaining popularity in experimental research because it has for the analysis at the structural model level.
several advantages over other statistical techniques, such as the analysis Second, we estimated the structural model, which shows that the
of variances (Breitsohl, 2019; Henseler, Ringle, & Sinkovics, 2009). PLS- model fits the data well. Fig. 4 illustrates these results, showing all path
SEM provides a holistic approach for analyzing the effects and testing coefficients and the proportions of variances of the dependent variables.
hypotheses in experimental settings (e.g., Andrei, Zait, Vatamanescu, & The R2 values of SEU (0.484) and WTD (0.652) indicate that the up­
Pînzaru, 2017). Accordingly, we evaluated both manipu­ stream variables adequately explain the endogenous variables, whereas
lations—personalization and information sensitivity— as ordinal vari­ Q2 values greater than zero for all endogenous variables indicate pre­
ables (level of personalization with [1] standardized, [2] personalized, dictive relevance. We used the blindfolding procedure to verify the
and [3] individualized communication; information sensitivity with [1] predictive relevance, resulting in cross-validated redundancy values of
low sensitivity and [2] high sensitivity). Furthermore, we modeled 0.477 (SEU) and 0.647 (WTD).
IUIPC as a type two (reflective–formative) second-order construct Hypothesis testing revealed support for H1 as the level of personal­
(Jarvis, MacKenzie, & Podsakoff, 2003) consisting of three dimensions ization showed a significant positive effect on SEU (β = 0.419, p <
(awareness, control, and collection). The underlying first-order con­ 0.001). The results also confirmed H2, which predicted the disclosure of
structs and their manifest variables are shown in Table 2, which also information with a higher sensitivity to harm SEU (β = − 0.504, p <
depicts the associated items and respective indicator loadings. 0.001). Supporting H3, IUIPC had a significant negative effect on SEU (β
We modeled and calculated the construct using the repeated indi­ = − 0.233, p < 0.001). Finally, concerning the outcome variable, WDT,
cator approach with mode A measurements (Noonan & Wold, 1983; we found a strong positive effect of SEU (β = 0.807, p < 0.001). Hence,
Wold, 1982). Because of its lower constraints, we chose PLS-SEM over H4 was also supported by the underlying data. The respective results are
covariance-based SEM approaches to estimate the model (Afthanorhan, underlined by the illustrations in Fig. 4 and Table 4, showing the SEM
2013). We conducted the estimation using SmartPLS 3 by applying the and its respective key statistical figures.
PLS algorithm and nonparametric bootstrapping (with 5,000 replica­
tions). First, we evaluated the quality of the measurement model by 4.2.1. Additional analysis
verifying all required quality criteria (Hair, Sarstedt, Ringle, & Mena, Although all the proposed hypotheses could be tested through SEM
2012). The indicator reliability of the reflective scales could be ensured analysis, the findings failed to consider the different experimental con­
as all standardized loadings exceeded the threshold of 0.7 (Hair et al., ditions that were present. Therefore, we conducted an additional anal­
2012). ysis of the group differences, which revealed further findings relevant to
Furthermore, the composite reliability of all first-order constructs the SEU. Consolidated across all groups, the SEU displayed a mean of
was greater than 0.7, indicating internal consistency. As SEU and WTD 0.237 after normalization and a standard deviation of 0.275, repre­
were calculated separately, the variables consisted of only one indicator, senting a relatively low average of the calculated value. However,
resulting in a composite reliability of 1.0. Convergent validity can be looking at the position and scatter parameters of individual groups can
confirmed as the average variance extracted (AVE) exceeds 0.5 in all provide deeper information. To this end, Table 5 provides an overview
cases (Fornell & Larcker, 1981). To assess discriminant validity, we used of each group and highlights their differences.
the heterotrait-monotrait (HTMT) approach. The highest HTMT value Groups 1 (n = 51) and 2 (n = 48) had the lowest mean SEU values
obtained was 0.807, which lies below the conservative threshold of 0.85, and standard deviations below 0.1 (G1: M = 0.087, SD = 0.093; G2: M =
thus indicating no lack of discriminant validity (Henseler et al., 2009; 0.048, SD = 0.087). These groups received a standardized newsletter
Voorhees, Brady, Calantone, & Ramirez, 2016). Table 3 displays the with the lowest level of personalization as a benefit. A significant dif­
construct-level correlations and psychometric properties to present all ference was observed for Groups 3 (n = 50) and 4 (n = 50), who were
results in detail. offered a medium level of personalization. Group 3 was confronted with
To assess the psychometric properties of IUIPC modeled as formative the disclosure of less sensitive information and presented a mean value
at the second-order measurement level, we tested the significance of and dispersion that were notably higher (G3: M = 0.486, SD = 0.282).
indicators and multicollinearity to evaluate the measures’ goodness Group 4 faced the disclosure of more sensitive information and showed
(Chin, 2010; Goetz, Liehr-Gobbers, & Krafft, 2010). In terms of indicator significantly lower SEU values (G4: M = 0.103, SD = 0.104). The results
significance, the weights of the indicators forming the second-order for Groups 5 (n = 50) and 6 (n = 50) were similar to those of Groups 3
construct, represented by the path coefficients, exceeded the threshold and 4 (G5: M = 0.554, SD = 0.270; G6: M = 0.142, SD = 0.167), indi­
of t > 1.98, and thus signal significance on the 1% level (Chin & cating only a minimal difference between individualized and personal­
Newsted, 1999). To evaluate the second-order hierarchical measure­ ized communication as an incentive.
ment model, we assessed the degree of multicollinearity among the To test whether there were significant differences between the mean
formative indicators (Diamantopoulos & Winklhofer, 2001; Grewal, values of the six groups and their effects on situational privacy calculus
Cote, & Baumgartner, 2004). The results revealed the highest value of (reflected in the SEU), we also carried out variance analytical

Table 3
Psychometric properties of variables.
Variable M SD CR AVE 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8

1. IUIPC 5.497 0.823 0.879 n/a 1.000


2. Control 5.336 0.957 0.835 0.630 1.000
3. Awareness 6.124 0.739 0.854 0.662 0.741 1.000
4. Collection 5.488 1.027 0.901 0.695 0.417 0.608 1.000
5. Information sensitivity 0.046 0.019 0.032 0.056 1.000
6. Personalization 0.057 0.090 0.033 0.029 0.012 1.000
7. SEU 0.237 0.275 0.264 0.300 0.208 0.175 0.502 0.416 1.000
8. WTD 2.179 0.831 0.299 0.215 0.221 0.289 0.541 0.371 0.807 1.000

Note. M = mean; SD = standard deviation; CR = composite reliability; AVE = average variance extracted.

182
M.H.J. Gouthier et al. Journal of Business Research 148 (2022) 174–189

Fig. 4. Results of the SEM.

between the groups.


Table 4
To visualize the differences between groups, we performed an
Study results of SEM analysis.
additional pairwise comparison and tested the null hypothesis again for
Hypothesized path Hypothesis β SE t-value Supported each pair. The effect sizes were then calculated. The pairwise compari­
Personalization → H1 0.419 0.037 11.315*** Yes son confirmed a significant difference between standardization and
SEU personalization, as well as between standardization and individualiza­
Information H2 − 0.504 0.033 15.327*** Yes tion. However, the difference between personalization and individuali­
sensitivity → SEU
IUIPC → SEU H3 − 0.233 0.043 5.425*** Yes
zation was not significant, with only a marginal increase. The
SEU → WTD H4 0.807 0.033 24.289*** Yes calculation of the effect strength using the correlation coefficient r
*** clarified the finding: we found a moderately strong effect between
Note: p < 0.001; SE = standard error.
standardization and personalization (r = 0.455), a strong effect between
standardization and individualization (r = 0.545), and no sufficiently
procedures to compare mean values. The implementation of parametric strong effect for the non-significant relationship between personaliza­
procedures, such as two-factor ANOVA, requires a normal distribution of tion and individualization (r = 0.090). Although the test showed that the
data within the groups and homogeneity of the variances between the subjectively expected benefit was expressed differently based on the
groups (Ostertagová, Ostertag, & Kovác, 2014). As these preconditions level of personalization, there seemed to be a degree of saturation at
could not be confirmed through appropriate tests, our analysis called for which an additional enhancement in personalization did not lead to a
a nonparametric test procedure that presupposes nothing about distri­ significant perceived additional benefit for a customer. Whereas effect
butions: the Kruskal–Wallis test is recommended in such cases differences in the subjectively expected benefit could be identified from
(Ostertagová et al., 2014; Kruskal & Wallis, 1952). fully standardized to segment-specific communication, in the next step
to individualization, they were present but too marginal to formulate a
4.2.2. Level of personalization distinct statement.
A comparison of the mean ranks revealed a positive relationship
between the level of personalization and resulting SEU. However, while 4.2.3. Degree of information sensitivity
there was a massive leap in SEU from offering a personalized newsletter To test group differences in terms of information sensitivity, we
as an incentive (mean rank = 170.86) instead of a standardized version
(mean rank = 92.15), further improvement in individualizing a news­
letter (mean rank = 186.42) led to only a small increase in SEU. This
Table 6
result is described by the comparison of the mean ranks according to the
Comparison of mean ranks according to Kruskal–Wallis.
Kruskal-Wallis test in Table 6.
Level of personalization n Mean rank
The null hypothesis, H0, assumed that there is no difference between
the mean ranks of groups. We used asymptotic significance testing SEU Total Standardized 99 92.15
because each sample had more than 30 respondents (Malhotra, 2007). Personalized 100 170.86
Individualized 100 186.42
As the value of asymptotic significance was 0.00, and thus below the N = 299
threshold of 0.05, we rejected H0 and assumed significant differences

Table 5
Results of group comparison of SEU.
Group n Minimum Maximum Median M SD

SEU Total 1 (standardized, low) 51 0.00 0.34 0.045 0.087 0.093


2 (standardized, high) 48 0.00 0.41 0.010 0.048 0.087
3 (personalized, low) 50 0.00 0.90 0.496 0.486 0.282
4 (personalized, high) 50 0.00 0.53 0.078 0.103 0.104
5 (individualized, low) 50 0.00 0.97 0.606 0.554 0.270
6 (individualized, high) 50 0.00 0.84 0.094 0.142 0.167

183
M.H.J. Gouthier et al. Journal of Business Research 148 (2022) 174–189

carried out a Mann–Whitney U test, which is applicable for cases with level of personal communication. Moreover, an extensive degree of
two independent samples (low and high). In addition to creating the individualization could evoke negative feelings among customers; they
ranks, the respective rank sums are given in the first step, resulting from might suspect a loss of control over the disclosed data, which could be
the product of the number of observations and mean rank. The goal is accompanied by fears of espionage or a sense of paternalism (e.g.,
again to reject the null hypothesis, which assumes that there are no Gironda & Korgaonkar, 2018). Such risks influence the perception of
significant differences between groups with low and high information SEU and lead to a comparatively smaller increase in it.
sensitivity in terms of their effects on the dependent variable SEU. A theory applicable here is the marginal utility theory, which can be
Table 7 depicts the results of the underlying Mann-Whitney U test. adapted from the classical theory of goods. It is assumed that as the
Regarding SEU, a comparison of the two groups showed that those consumption of a good increases, its marginal utility decreases (Hor­
with low information sensitivity had a higher rank sum than those with owitz, List, & McConnell, 2007). Following other studies (e.g., Greene &
high information sensitivity (rank sum = 28,703 vs. 16,147). The Baron, 2001) to adapt this idea to our own context, marginal utility also
Mann–Whitney U test also confirmed a significant difference between decreases as the level of personalization increases. This diminishing
the groups, allowing us to reject the null hypothesis. In addition, the marginal utility is then reflected in the evaluation of the expected
calculation of the effect size showed that the independent samples had a benefit, explaining the comparatively small difference in SEU from
moderately strong effect on the dependent variable SEU (r = 0.469). personalization to individualization.
Fig. 5 illustrates the difference in the evaluation of values by group by Within H2, the situational factor of information sensitivity is seen as
showing the SEU for each group in connection with the respective in­ the negative counterpart to the benefit of personalization, and we hy­
formation sensitivity. pothesized a correspondingly negative effect on SEU. Our empirical
The drawn line shows the SEU across the three levels of personali­ analysis supports this assumption, finding that the more sensitive the
zation at low information sensitivity. Here, the increase in SEU from information requested, the lower the associated SEU. This is in line with
standardized to personalized communication is the most pronounced, other results showing that the level of sensitivity intensifies privacy
with only a marginal increase in the next step of individualization. The concerns and, therefore, risk perception (e.g., Bandara, Fernando, &
dashed line marks the progression at high information sensitivity, rising Akter, 2020; Malhotra et al., 2004; Sheehan & Hoy, 2000). In the case of
more slowly and steadily with increased personalization than with low- highly sensitive information, risk assumptions outweigh potential
sensitivity information. benefit appraisals such that an increase in personalization leads to only
minor effects in SEU.
5. Discussion Thus, it can be indirectly deduced that the perception of a fair ex­
change can only arise if a company sufficiently justifies its collection of
Based on the above data analysis, several aspects must be discussed sensitive data; receiving a standardized, personalized, or individualized
in the context of situational privacy calculus. newsletter is on its own insufficient grounds. Essentially, it can be
confirmed that the type of information requested plays a major role in
SEU evaluation, and that companies can better justify the demand for
5.1. Situational effects on SEU
less sensitive data on lifestyle, shopping habits, and simple demographic
information than highly sensitive data, such as contact information,
Situational privacy calculus assumes that situational circumstances
financial figures, or family demographics. Further studies have
in the decision-making process lead to a cost–benefit calculation in
confirmed that justification is particularly useful when people initially
which decision-makers weigh the anticipated risks and benefits of
see little benefit from personalizing a service (e.g., Aiello et al., 2020;
disclosing personal information. SEU is then composed of perceived cost
White, Zahay, Thorbjørnsen, & Shavitt, 2008). Moreover, highly
and benefit elements weighted by the subjective probability of the
personalized messages, which, in principle, require a tremendous
consequences of disclosure behavior. Following the assumption that the
amount of personal information, can quickly lead to customer reactance
data disclosure decision depends on the situation at hand, our study
because of a feeling of intrusiveness (White et al., 2008). Therefore,
manipulated two situational factors: the level of personalization as a
transparent and meaningful justification for collecting sensitive infor­
valuable benefit and degree of information sensitivity as perceivable
mation is crucial.
costs (Acquisti, Brandimarte, & Loewenstein, 2015).
Aiello et al. (2020) also pointed out that a customer’s assessment of
H1 predicted a positive relationship between the level of personali­
data collection depends on the customer journey stage. As increased
zation and SEU, which is confirmed by the SEM results. This finding
data queries are considered even more critical at the beginning of a
verifies the assumption that the level of personalization is seen as a
customer journey, it is vital to request data at the appropriate time.
primary benefit in the trade-off process as it increases the relevance of
the marketing content for the individual (Lee & Cranage, 2011; Morosan
& DeFranco, 2015; Sundar & Marathe, 2010). This expected value and
the motivation to process the respective information (Dijkstra, 2014) are 5.2. IUIPC as a disposition affecting SEU
reflected in the corresponding SEU.
It is also valuable to discuss the additional results of the group In addition to situational factors and based on the neobehavioristic
comparisons. We see that SEU increases substantially between stan­ approach, we also theoretically described context-specific dispositions
dardized and personalized communication, but only slightly with the from past experiences (Campbell, 1997; Caudill & Murphy, 2000; Mal­
step-up to individualized newsletters. This could be because the leap in hotra et al., 2004). H3 proposed a negative influence of IUIPC on SEU
SEU from standardized to personalized was already so high that re­ and showed a comparatively significant low effect size based on the
spondents found the difference between a personalized or individualized SEM, which nevertheless allows for its acceptance. This result empha­
newsletter to be less valuable; the important thing was having some sizes that part of the evaluation of utility in a decision-making situation
depends on past processing and learning processes from similar situa­
tions (Campbell, 1997). It was noted that only a fraction of the total
Table 7
variance of the SEU could be explained by the independent latent var­
Results of Mann–Whitney U test.
iables included. It can be stated that IUIPC contributes to the evaluation
Information sensitivity n Mean rank Rank sum
of SEU but does not seem to be the decisive factor, and past experiences
SEU Total Low 151 190.09 28,703 are only one of several aspects that explain the trade-off between ben­
High 148 109.10 16,147 efits and costs. Here, it might be more critical for the individual to assess
N = 299
the situation at hand rather than rely on past experiences.

184
M.H.J. Gouthier et al. Journal of Business Research 148 (2022) 174–189

Fig. 5. Graphical summary of group differences on SEU.

5.3. SEU as a central predictor of information disclosure become increasingly relevant. According to Hudák et al. (2017), news­
letter marketing must actively support this. This highlights the impor­
Finally, SEU represents the central assumption of situational privacy tance of selecting appropriate communication measures and designing
calculus that results in customer (non)disclosure behavior. Unlike suitable exchange processes to provide customers with the best indi­
context-specific dispositions, future positive and negative consequences vidual solutions. To be willing to disclose personal information, a
are weighed and form the basis for the net value presented by the SEU. customer must perceive a subjective benefit. Our results emphasize that
H4 assumed a positive impact of SEU on respondents’ WTD, which was perceived incentives can help provide such benefits and justify the
supported by the data analysis. For customers, anticipated utility can be collection of personal data. However, companies should exercise caution
compared with a subjective expected value, which allows for statements when requesting more sensitive information, as incentives such as
about whether they position themselves positively or negatively toward personalization are not necessarily sufficient to justify substantial data-
a decision (Adjerid, Peer, & Acquisti, 2018). This considers the pro­ collection efforts. Based on the confirmation of situational privacy cal­
portional (un)certainty in decisions regarding information disclosure. culus, companies should seek benefits or incentives of comparable value
Confirmation of the hypothesis by the described path coefficient in­ that can be offered in return for data disclosure and the customer per­
dicates that situational privacy calculus is expressed in the SEU, which ceives as fair.
appears to be a reliable predictor of willingness to disclose. The strong Research has confirmed that situational influences tend to overwrite
influence of SEU is also reflected in the explained variance. Therefore, existing dispositions such as general privacy concerns (Kehr, Kowatsch,
this substantiates the results of previous studies that focused on the Wentzel, & Fleisch, 2015b; Wilson & Valacich, 2012). This intensifies
positive relationship between expected value and willingness to share the need to create a fair and transparent exchange in which customers
information, based on the personalization privacy calculus approach (e. show a certain degree of appreciation for incentives, such as personal­
g., Kim, Park, Park, & Ahn, 2019; Xu et al., 2011). ization. To ensure a profitable and longer-lasting relationship, it would
In summary, we can confirm the results of previous studies that have be interesting to examine medium- to long-term changes in personal
operationalized privacy calculus as a meaningful explanatory approach dispositions and their impact on customers’ perceptions of utility. As
to better explain online privacy behavior (e.g., Dinev & Hart, 2006; reduced utility can be caused by attitudes that have already been
Hallam & Zanella, 2017; Kehr, Kowatsch, Wentzel, & Fleisch, 2015a; Li established, incentives must also be followed by correct executions to
et al., 2010; Wilson & Valacich, 2012) but leave space for the integration avoid negative user experiences that harm the customer’s positioning in
of additional factors that were not included in this investigation. the future. It has already been shown that the presence of increased data
protection concerns about informational privacy can be traced back in
6. Implications and limitations part to the three dimensions of collection, control, and awareness
(Malhotra et al., 2004). To improve the public image of data privacy
6.1. Practical implications practices, the European Union has recently adopted several data pro­
tection measures that are increasingly enforced by fines in the case of
Owing to the nature of this study, the results provide e-commerce noncompliance. As small- and medium-sized companies are responsible
companies with information on the prerequisites that should be met for for their own data privacy–compliant implementation, they face sig­
fair data exchange processes to ensure a mutually profitable relationship nificant hurdles in conforming to regulations. Therefore, legislators and
with customers. In general, the service and support of all relevant prominent companies should cooperate to support them in their en­
customer touchpoints in the pre-sales, sales, and after-sales phases have deavors. This would help close the gap between ideas regarding data

185
M.H.J. Gouthier et al. Journal of Business Research 148 (2022) 174–189

privacy compliance and concrete security implementation. In addition, utility. Simultaneously, prospect theory provides another research
it can generate further confidence-building measures concerning the perspective with the establishment of a central reference point for
security and reliability of the use of personal data in e-commerce. An further theoretical and empirical investigations.
existing example of this can be found in so-called “permission market­
ing,” whereby recipients are informed about collecting and using their 6.3. Limitations and avenues for future research
data and authorizing the company to contact them (based on Culnan &
Armstrong, 1999). Ultimately, a shift from mass marketing to one-to-one In addition to our study’s practical and theoretical implications, it is
marketing can only take place with the support of customers, as they are important to note its certain limitations. First, one may criticize the use
the ones who will appreciate being contacted by individualized and of two different empirical approaches to test our hypotheses. However,
need-based communication on the one hand and being treated lawfully group comparisons are crucial for evaluating the different effects of
in their privacy on the other hand (Gironda & Korgaonkar, 2018). situational factors. The lack of a normal distribution of variables made
the corresponding analysis more difficult; the ANOVA had to be
6.2. Theoretical implications replaced, although it would have been helpful to investigate the
respective interaction effects of the variables. Second, because the study
On a theoretical level, the results of this study contribute to the was based on an exploratory design using a scenario technique, we could
elucidation of situational privacy calculus literature. From a neo­ only evaluate anticipated behavior and not actual behavior, an aspect
behavioristic perspective, Belk (1975) stimulus–organism–response that also finds criticism in Norberg et al. (2007). Nevertheless, this
model can be assumed to be a reasonable conceptual basis for explaining problem is generally countered by formulating scenarios as realistically
the digital privacy behavior of individuals. Owing to the influence of as possible.
situational stimuli, individuals undertake an unobservable intra­ Experimental research approaches would be particularly suitable for
psychological evaluation process, which can be made measurable by further investigations in which the purpose of data collection is
indicators under certain conditions; this process can be found in the explained transparently to customers to analyze the respective group
situational privacy calculus as a weighing of the benefits and risks. effects with further influential factors. Third, in terms of further influ­
Whereas benefits embody the evaluation of current information pro­ ential factors, our study is limited to a specific geographical setting with
cessing, attitude constructs are the result of past processing and learning associated data privacy regulations, namely in the context of European
processes that can be retrieved in the decision process. This study regulations. This makes the findings less generalizable. There are also
demonstrated that the resulting SEU can serve as a reliable indicator of areas with more lenient regulations worldwide, and customers in those
behavioral intention. While past research has neglected any meaningful countries might have other expectations when thinking about informa­
distinction according to the degree of sensitivity to personal data, we can tion disclosure. This limitation highlights an avenue for further research
confirm that the perception of utility and willingness to disclose also to include different privacy regulations or cross-cultural effects as a
depends on the type of personal information requested and the extent to means to analyze potential differences in perceptions of situational
which this request is justified by the company’s improved offer or privacy calculus (e.g., Robinson, 2017; Trepte et al., 2017), even though
communication. the basic mechanism of the approach remains unaffected. Another
The present work refers to digital marketing, which should serve as a limitation in terms of the generalizability of the results is the study
concrete example of direct communication and service provision in e- context of a newsletter subscription. Researchers in this area could
commerce, depending on experiences that have already been made in consider testing our findings in other contexts. Finally, it might be
similar contexts. Although practitioners and researchers should not helpful for future research to consider situational privacy calculus with
neglect individual dispositions such as IUIPC when considering data more than two action alternatives. Because of the application fields of
protection behavior, the assumption that situational factors can almost newsletters, the choices were deliberately limited here to “disclosure”
completely override general or categorical attitudes cannot be refuted and “nondisclosure.” Nevertheless, integrating a possible third alterna­
(Barth & de Jong, 2017; Wilson & Valacich, 2012). Emphasizing the tive (e.g., with an adaptation of preference settings) is valuable because
importance of subjective and situation-specific decision-making, the it provides potential insights into a more differentiated evaluation pro­
SEU theory is considered a good complement to the operationalization cess related to the SEU.
of privacy calculus. Although it is based on rational or utility-
maximizing assumptions, the consideration of subjective uncertainty 7. Conclusion
allows room for irrational customer behavior. Thus, it can be assumed
that situational privacy calculus is also subject to mental reduction The overall aim of this study was to explain situational privacy cal­
techniques such as heuristics, which can undoubtedly simplify the culus in e-commerce. To this end, we identified personalization and
weighing process based on existing information. information sensitivity as two essential situation-specific factors to
Finally, linking our underlying research approach to Kahneman and manipulate a customer’s cost–benefit evaluation.
Tversky (1979) prospect theory may also be helpful. This theory is not Referring to the SEU theory, personalization has a positive influence
explicitly addressed in our study. While weighing decisions under the on SEU, although there is only a slight difference between the perception
occurrence of risk is considered, we assume no general risk aversion of personalized and individualized incentives. Specifically, the shift
with SEU, as required in prospect theory. Overall, prospect theory is from standardized to personalized communication has a strong positive
seen as an extension of the SEU approach. It divides the decision-making effect on SEU. By contrast, a further increase in personalization can be
process into two phases and guides the evaluation of alternatives seen as too intrusive, ultimately leading to the potential costs of
accordingly. This theory was developed to explain violations of the disclosure nearly outweighing the expected additional benefits.
principles of rational decision-making based on a value function. In terms of information sensitivity, we confirmed that a request for
Accordingly, the outcomes are defined as negative or positive deviations highly sensitive information negatively impacts SEU and that a corre­
from a neutral baseline. Losses are weighted more heavily than gains, sponding justification for collecting personal data becomes increasingly
and it turns out that aversion to losses is generally much greater than the relevant with greater information sensitivity. Because we could only
desire for gains (Kahneman & Tversky, 1979). There is a further possi­ prove a weak effect of IUIPC on SEU, general privacy concerns cannot be
bility of investigating situational privacy calculus with this assumption. acknowledged as a central predictor. Future research should investigate
This was not part of the underlying research questions by calculating the additional factors that might influence SEU. Nevertheless, the positive
general trade-off between the benefits and costs of a decision alternative effect of SEU on customers’ WTD is indicated by our data analysis, thus
with equal weight. Thus, the present research design measures absolute supporting situational privacy calculus.

186
M.H.J. Gouthier et al. Journal of Business Research 148 (2022) 174–189

In summary, this study contributes to a better understanding of Barth, S., & de Jong, M. D. T. (2017). The privacy paradox – Investigating discrepancies
between expressed privacy concerns and actual online behavior – A systematic
customer behavior regarding privacy decisions. Situational privacy
literature review. Telematics and Informatics, 34(7), 1038–1058.
calculus represents a promising solution to the privacy paradox. Com­ Belanger, F., & Crossler, R. E. (2011). Privacy in the digital age: A review of information
panies should actively promote fair data-exchange processes instead of privacy research in information systems. MIS Quarterly, 35(4), 1017–1041.
relying on customers’ free disclosure of their personal data in e-com­ Belk, R. W. (1975). Situational Variables and Consumer Behavior. Journal of Consumer
Research, 2(3), 157–164.
merce. To close the gap between theory and practice, there is a need for Bennett, C. J. (1995). The political economy of privacy: A review of the literature. Center
further research in this area as the technological tools for creating for social and legal research, DOE genome project (Final draft), University of Victoria,
information-rich customer profiles, and visions for one-to-one commu­ Department of Political Science. Victoria. Hackensack, NJ: Center for Social and Legal
Research.
nication continue to grow. Companies need to be aware that the more Breitsohl, H. (2019). Beyond ANOVA: An Introduction to Structural Equation Models for
information customers share, the more likely companies are to be Experimental Designs. Organizational Research Methods, 22(3), 649–677.
required to improve their products and services (Hritzuk, 2018). Campbell, A. J. (1997). Relationship marketing in consumer markets: A comparison of
managerial and consumer attitudes about information privacy. Journal of Direct
Moreover, their collection of personal data must be justified by in­ Marketing, 11(3), 44–57.
centives to win customers as supporters of a one-to-one marketing Caudill, E. M., & Murphy, P. E. (2000). Consumer Online Privacy: Legal and Ethical
approach (Peppers & Rogers, 2000). To enable transparent and fair Issues. Journal of Public Policy & Marketing, 19(1), 7–19.
Chellappa, R. K., & Sin, R. G. (2005). Personalization versus privacy: An empirical
exchange, it is important to better understand customer behavior in examination of the online consumer’s dilemma. Information Technology and
digital environments and the technologies used to collect and process Management, 6(2), 181–202.
data in e-commerce, such as machine learning and AI. Therefore, com­ Chin, W. (2010). How to write up and report PLS analyses. In: Esposito Vinzi, V., Chin,
W. W., Henseler, J., Wang, H. (eds). Handbook of partial least squares: concepts,
panies and legislators urgently need to agree on meaningful and uni­
methods and applications, 655–690.
formly applicable legislation and implementation so that customers’ Chin, W. W., & Newsted, P. R. (1999). Structural equation modeling: Analysis with small
informational privacy is protected without putting small- and medium- samples using partial least squares. In R. Hoyle (Ed.), Statistical strategies for small
sized enterprises at a disadvantage. sample research (pp. 307–341). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
Culnan, M. J., & Armstrong, P. K. (1999). Information privacy concerns, procedural
fairness, and impersonal trust: An empirical investigation. Organization Science, 10
CRediT authorship contribution statement (1), 104–115.
Culnan, M. J., & Bies, R. J. (2003). Consumer privacy: Balancing economic and justice
considerations. Journal of Social Issues, 59(2), 323–342.
Matthias H.J. Gouthier: Writing – review & editing, Writing – Diamantopoulos, A., & Winklhofer, H. (2001). Index Construction Formative Indicators:
original draft, Methodology, Supervision, Conceptualization. Carina An Alternative to Scale Development. Journal of Marketing Research, 38(2), 269–277.
Dijkstra, A. (2014). The persuasive effects of personalization through: Name mentioning
Nennstiel: Writing – review & editing, Writing – original draft, Formal
in a smoking cessation message. User Model-Adapted Interaction, 24, 393–411.
analysis, Validation. Nora Kern: Conceptualization, Methodology, Dinev, T., & Hart, P. (2006). An Extended Privacy Calculus Model for E-Commerce
Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing. Lars Wendel: Transactions. Information Systems Research, 17(1), 61–80.
Eastin, M., Brinson, N. H., Doorey, A., & Wilcox, G. B. (2016). Living in a big data world:
Methodology, Investigation.
Predicting mobile commerce activity through privacy concerns. Computers in Human
Behavior, 58, 214–220.
Declaration of Competing Interest Fan, H., & Poole, M. S. (2006). What Is Personalization? Perspectives in the Design and
Implementation of Personalization in Information Systems. Journal of Organizational
Computing and Electronic Commerce, 16(3/4), 179–202.
The authors declare that they have no known competing financial Ford, G. T., Smith, D. B., & Swasy, J. L. (1988). An Empirical Test of the Search,
interests or personal relationships that could have appeared to influence Experience and Credence Attributes Framework. Advances in Consumer Research,
15. In M. J. Houston (Ed.), Provo (pp. 239–244). UT: Association for Consumer
the work reported in this paper. Research.
Fornell, C., & Larcker, D. F. (1981). Evaluations structural equation models with
References unobservable variables and measurement error. Journal of Marketing Research, 18(1),
39–50.
Fried, C. (1984). Privacy. In F. Schoeman (Ed.), Philosophical dimensions of privacy: An
Accenture (2020). We, the post-digital people. Can your enterprise survive the tech-
anthology (pp. 203–222). Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press.
clash?. Technology Vision 2020. Provocative thinking, transformative insights,
Gabisch, J., & Milne, G. R. (2014). The impact of compensation on information
tangible outcomes.
ownership and privacy control. Journal of Consumer Marketing, 31(1), 13–16.
Accenture Interactive (2018). Making it personal. Why brands must move from
Gerber, N., Gerber, P., & Volkamer, M. (2018). Explaining the privacy paradox: A
communication to conversation for greater personalization. Pulse Check 2018.
systematic review of literature investigating privacy attitude and behavior.
Acquisti, A. (2004). Privacy in electronic commerce and the economics of immediate
Computers and Security, 77, 226–261.
gratification. Proceedings of the ACM Conference on Electronic Commerce, 5, 21–29.
Gironda, J., & Korgaonkar, P. (2018). iSpy? Tailored Versus Invasive Ads and
Acquisti, A., Brandimarte, L., Loewenstein, G. (2015). Privacy and human behavior in the
Consumers’ Perceptions of Personalized Advertising. Electronic Commerce Research
age of information. Science, 347(6221), 509–514.
and Applications, 29, 64–77.
Acquisti, A., & Grossklags, J. (2005). Privacy and rationality in individual decision
Goetz, O., Liehr-Gobbers, K., Krafft, M. (2010). Evaluation of Structural Equation Models
making. IEEE Security and Privacy, 3(1), 26–33.
Using the Partial Least Squares (PLS) Approach. In: Esposito Vinzi, V., Chin, W. W.,
Acquisti, A., Taylor, C., & Wagman, L. (2016). The economics of privacy. Journal of
Henseler, J., Wang, H. (eds). Handbook of partial least squares: concepts, methods
Economic Literature, 54(2), 442–492.
and applications, 411–427.
Adjerid, I., Peer, E., & Acquisti, A. (2018). Beyond the Privacy Paradox: Objective Versus
Greene, J., & Baron, J. (2001). Intuitions about Declining Marginal Utility. Journal of
Relative Risk in Privacy Decision Making. MIS Quarterly, 42(1), 465–488.
Behavioral Decision Making, 14(3), 243–255.
Afthanorhan, W. M. A. B. W. (2013). A Comparison of Partial Least Square Structural
Grewal, R., Cote, J. A., & Baumgartner, H. (2004). Multicollinearity and Measurement
Equation Modeling (PLS-SEM) and Covariance Based SEM (CB-SEM) for
Error in Structural Equation Models: Implications for Theory Testing. Marketing
Confirmatory Factor Analysis. International Journal of Engineering Science and
Science, 23(4), 519–529.
Innovative Technology, 2(5), 198–205.
Hair, J. F., Sarstedt, M., Ringle, C. M., & Mena, J. A. (2012). An assessment of the use of
Aiello, G., Donvito, R., Acuti, D., Grazzini, L., Mazzoli, V., Vannucci, V., & Viglia, G.
partial least squares structural equation modeling in marketing research. Journal of
(2020). Customers’ Willingness to Disclose Personal Information throughout the
the Academy of Marketing Science, 40(3), 414–433.
Customer Purchase Journey in Retailing: The Role of Perceived Warmth. Journal of
Hallam, C., & Zanella, G. (2017). Online self-disclosure: The privacy paradox explained
Retailing, 96(4), 490–506.
as a temporally discounted balance between concerns and rewards. Computers in
Andrei, A. G., Zait, A., Vatamanescu, E. M., & Pînzaru, F. (2017). Word-of-mouth
Human Behavior, 68, 217–227.
generation and brand communication strategy – Findings from an experimental
Hann, I. H., Hui, K. L., Lee, S. Y. T., & Png, I. P. L. (2007). Overcoming online information
study explored with PLS-SEM. Industrial Management & Data Systems, 117(3),
privacy concerns: An information-processing theory approach. Journal of
478–495.
Management Information Systems, 24(2), 13–42.
Awad, N. F., & Krishnan, M. S. (2006). The personalization privacy paradox: An
Henseler, J., Ringle, C. M., & Sinkovics, R. R. (2009). The Use of Partial Least Squares
empirical evaluation of information transparency and the willingness to be profiled
Path Modeling in International Marketing. Advances in International Marketing, 20,
online for personalization. MIS Quarterly, 30(1), 13–28.
277–319.
Bandara, R., Fernando, M., & Akter, S. (2020). Privacy concerns in E-commerce: A
Holvast, J. (1993). Vulnerability and privacy: are we on the way to a riskfree society?
taxonomy and a future research agenda. Electronic Markets, 30, 629–647.
Proceedings of the IFIP-WG 9.2 conference, May 20–22, 1993, Namur, Belgium.
Bansal, G., Mariam, F., & Gefen, D. (2010). The impact of personal dispositions on
Horowitz, J., List, L., & McConnell, K. E. (2007). A Test of Diminishing Marginal Value.
information sensitivity, privacy concern and trust in disclosing health information
Economica, 74, 650–663.
online. Decision Support Systems, 49(2), 138–150.

187
M.H.J. Gouthier et al. Journal of Business Research 148 (2022) 174–189

Hritzuk, N. (2018). Why companies risk losing customers by not reciprocating on shared Peppers, D., & Rogers, M. (2000). Papers: Build a one-to-one learning relationship with
data: Rebuilding the data-sharing economy in a consumer-driven world. Journal of your customers. Interactive Marketing, 1(3), 243–250.
Advertising Research, 58(4), 394–398. Phelps, J., Nowak, G., & Ferrel, E. (2000). Privacy concerns and consumer willingness to
Hudák, M., Kianičková, E., & Madleňák, R. (2017). The Importance of E-mail Marketing provide personal information. Journal of Public Policy & Marketing, 19(1), 27–41.
in E-commerce. Procedia Engineering, 192, 342–347. Robinson, C. (2017). Disclosure of personal data in ecommerce: A cross-national
Ioannou, A., Tussyadiah, I., & Lu, Y. (2020). Privacy concerns and disclosure of biometric comparison of Estonia and the United States. Telematics and Informatics, 34(2),
and behavioral data for travel. International Journal of Information Management, 54, 569–582.
102–122. Rosenberg, R. (1992). The social impact of computers. Academic Press Inc.
Jarvis, C. B., MacKenzie, S. B., & Podsakoff, P. M. (2003). A Critical Review of Construct Sabanoglu, T. (2020). Retail e-commerce sales worldwide from 2014 to 2023. Statista.
Indicators and Measurement Model Misspecification in Marketing and Consumer https://www.statista.com/statistics/379046/worldwide-retail-e-commerce-sales/,
Research. Journal of Consumer Research, 30(2), 199–218. accessed: March 05, 2021.
Kahneman, D., & Tversky, A. (1979). Prospect Theory: An Analysis of Decision under Schiffman, L. G., Kanuk, L. L., Hansen, H. (2008). Consumer Behaviour: A European
Risk. Econometrica, 47(2), 263–292. Outlook. In: Pearson Education (1. ed.). Pearson Education Limited.
Kehr, F., Kowatsch, T., Wentzel, D., Fleisch, E. (2015a). Thinking Styles and Privacy Shearer, A. W. (2004). Approaching scenario-based studies: Three perceptions about the
Decisions: Need for Cognition, Faith into Intuition, and the Privacy Calculus. 12th future and considerations for landscape planning. Environment and Planning B:
International Conference on Wirtschaftsinformatik, 1–14. Planning and Design, 32, 67–87.
Kehr, F., Kowatsch, T., Wentzel, D., & Fleisch, E. (2015b). Blissfully ignorant: The effects Sheehan, K., & Hoy, M. (2000). Dimensions of Privacy Concerns Among Online
of general privacy concerns, general institutional trust, and affect in the privacy Consumers. Journal of Public Policy & Marketing, 19(1), 62–73.
calculus. Information Systems Journal, 25(6), 607–635. Smith, H. J., Dinev, T., & Xu, H. (2011). Information privacy research: An
Kim, D., Park, K., Park, Y., & Ahn, J. H. (2019). Willingness to provide personal interdisciplinary review. MIS Quarterly, 35(4), 989–1015.
information: Perspective of privacy calculus in IoT services. Computers in Human Spiekermann, S., Acquisti, A., Böhme, R., & Hui, K.-L. (2015). The challenges of personal
Behavior, 92, 273–281. data markets and privacy. Electronic Markets, 25(2), 161–167.
Kim, M. S., & Kim, S. (2018). Factors influencing willingness to provide personal Sundar, S. S., & Marathe, S. S. (2010). Personalization versus Customization: The
information for personalized recommendations. Computers in Human Behavior, 91, Importance of Agency, Privacy and Power Usage. Human Communication Research,
143–152. 36, 298–322.
Kokolakis, S. (2017). Privacy attitudes and privacy behaviour: A review of current Trang, S., & Weiger, W. H. (2020). The perils of gamification: Does engaging with
research on the privacy paradox phenomenon. Computers & Security, 64, 122–134. gamified services increase users’ willingness to disclose personal information?
Kolotylo-Kulkarni, M., Xia, W., & Dhillon, G. (2021). Information disclosure in e- Computers in Human Behavior, 116(2), 490–506.
commerce: A systematic review and agenda for future research. Journal of Business Trepte, S., Reinecke, L., Ellison, N. B., Quiring, O., Yao, M. Z., & Ziegele, M. (2017).
Research, 126, 221–238. A Cross-Cultural Perspective on the Privacy Calculus. Social Media + Society.
Kruskal, W. H., & Wallis, W. A. (1952). Use of Ranks in One-Criterion Variance Analysis. Tucker, C. (2019). Privacy, algorithms, and artificial intelligence. In A. Agrawal, J. Gans,
Journal of the American Statistical Association, 47(260), 583–621. & A. Goldfarb (Eds.), The economics of artificial intelligence: An agenda (pp. 423–437).
Kunz, V. (2004). Rational choice, Frankfurt. London: University of Chicago Press.
Laufer, R. S., & Wolfe, M. (1977a). Privacy as a Social Issue and Behavioural Concept. Vesanen, J. (2007). What is Personalization? A Conceptual Framework. European Journal
Journal of Social Issues, 33(3), 22–41. of Marketing, 41(5/6), 409–418.
Laufer, R. S., & Wolfe, M. (1977b). Privacy as a Concept and a Social Issue: A Voorhees, C. M., Brady, M. K., Calantone, R., & Ramirez, E. (2016). Discriminant validity
Multidimensional Developmental Theory. Journal of Social Issues, 33(3), 22–42. testing in marketing: An analysis, causes for concern, and proposed remedies.
Lee, C. H., & Cranage, D. A. (2011). Personalisation-privacy paradox: The effects of Journal of the Academy of Marketing Science, 44, 119–134.
personalization and privacy assurance on customer responses to travel Web Sites. Waks, R. (1989). Personal information: Privacy and the law; Oxford, England: Clarendon.
Tourism Management, 32(5), 987–994. White, T. B., Zahay, D. L., Thorbjørnsen, H., & Shavitt, S. (2008). Getting too personal:
Li, H., Sarathy, R. (2007). Understanding online information disclosure as a privacy Reactance to highly personalized email solicitations. Marketing Letters, 19(1), 39–50.
calculus adjusted by exchange fairness. ICIS 2007 Proceedings - Twenty Eighth Wieringa, J., Kannan, P. K., Ma, X., Reutterer, T., Risselada, H., & Skiera, B. (2019). Data
International Conference on Information Systems, 1–14. analytics in a privacy-concerned world. Journal of Business Research, 122, 915–925.
Li, H., Sarathy, R., & Xu, H. (2010). Understanding situational online information Wilson, D. W., & Valacich, J. S. (2012). Unpacking the privacy paradox: Irrational
disclosure as a privacy calculus. Journal of Computer Information Systems, 51(1), decision-making within the privacy calculus. International Conference on Information
62–71. Systems, ICIS, 2012(5), 4152–4162.
Lwin, M., Wirtz, J., & Williams, J. D. (2007). Consumer online privacy concerns and Wirtz, J., & Lwin, M. O. (2009). Regulatory focus theory, trust, and privacy concern.
responses: A power–responsibility equilibrium perspective. Journal of the Academy of Journal of Service Research, 12(2), 190–207.
Marketing Science, 35(4), 572–585. Wold, H. (1982). Soft modeling: the basic design and some extensions. In: Jöreskog, K.
Malhotra, N. K. (2007). Marketing Research (5th ed.). Pearson. G., Wold, H. (eds.). Systems under Indirect Observation: Causality, Structure,
Malhotra, N. K., Kim, S. S., & Agarwal, J. (2004). Internet users’ information privacy Prediction, 2, North Holland, Amsterdam, 1–54.
concerns (IUIPC): The construct, the scale, and a causal model. Information Systems Xie, E., Teo, H. H., & Wan, W. (2006). Volunteering Personal Information on the Internet:
Research, 15(4), 336–355. Effects of Reputation, Privacy Notices, and Rewards on Online Consumer Behavior.
March, J. G. (1994). Primer on Decision Making: How Decisions Happen, New York. Marketing Letters, 17(1), 61–74.
Martin, K. (2016). Data aggregators, consumer data, and responsibility online: Who is Xu, H., Dinev, T., Smith, H. J., Hart, P. (2008). Examining the formation of individual’s
tracking consumers online and should they stop? The Information Society, 32(1), privacy concerns: Toward an integrative view. International Conference on
51–63. Information Systems (ICIS) 2008 Proceedings, 6, 1–16.
Martin, K. D., & Murphy, P. E. (2017). The role of data privacy in marketing. Journal of Xu, H., Luo, X., Carroll, J. M., & Rosson, M. B. (2011). The personalization privacy
the Academy of Marketing Science, 45, 135–155. paradox: An exploratory study of decision making process for location-aware
Monitor Deloitte (2020). Impact of the COVID-19 crisis on short- and medium-term marketing. Decision Support Systems, 51(1), 42–52.
consumer behaviour? Will the COVID-19 crisis have a lasting effect on Zaichkowsky, J. L. (1985). Measuring the Involvement Construct. Journal of Consumer
consumption?, Issue 06/2020. Research, 12(3), 341–352.
Morando, F., Iemma, R., & Raiteri, E. (2014). Privacy evaluation: What empirical Zhu, H., Ou, C. X. J., van den Heuvel, W. J. A. M., & Liu, H. (2017). Privacy calculus and
research on users’ valuation of personal data tells us. Internet Policy Review, 3(2), its utility for personalization services in e-commerce: An analysis of consumer
1–11. decision-making. Information and Management, 54(4), 427–437.
Morosan, C., & DeFranco, A. (2015). Disclosing personal information via hotel apps: A
privacy calculus perspective. International Journal of Hospitality Management, 47,
Matthias H. J. Gouthier is a professor of marketing and digital services and director of the
120–130.
Center for Service Excellence at the University of Koblenz-Landau, Germany. His research
Mothersbaugh, D. L., Foxx, W. K., Beatty, S. E., & Wang, S. (2012). Disclosure
focuses on digital services, service excellence, customer delight, service-oriented human
Antecedents in an Online Service Context: The Role of Sensitivity of Information.
resource management, and customer experience management. He has published articles in
Journal of Service Research, 15(1), 76–98.
International Business Review, International Journal of Services and Operations Man­
Nau, R. (2006). Extensions of the Subjective Expected Utility Model. In: Edwards, W.,
agement, Journal of Marketing Theory and Practice, Journal of Service Management,
Miles, R. jr., von Winterfeldt, D. (eds.). Advances in Decision Analysis: From
Journal of Service Research, Journal of Service Theory and Practice (formerly: Managing
Foundations to Application, Cambridge University Press.
Service Quality), Journal of the Academy of Marketing Science, Marketing Theory, Total
Nissenbaum, H. (2010). Privacy in context: Technology, policy, and the integrity of social life.
Quality Management & Business Excellence, and others.
Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press.
Noonan, R., & Wold, H. (1983). Evaluating school systems using partial least squares.
Evaluation in Education, 7, 219–364. Carina Nennstiel is a research associate at the Chair of Marketing and Digital Services at
Norberg, P. A., Horne, D. R., & Horne, D. A. (2007). The Privacy Paradox: Personal the University of Koblenz-Landau, Germany. Her research interests include customer
Information Disclosure Intentions versus Behaviors. Journal of Consumer Affairs, 41 inspiration in marketing, customer experience management, digitalization of services as
(1), 100–126. well as service excellence.
Nowak, G. J., & Phelps, J. (1992). Understanding privacy concerns. An assessment of
consumers’ information-related knowledge and beliefs. Journal of Direct Marketing, 6
Nora Kern is a research associate at the Chair of Marketing and Digital Services at the
(4), 28–39.
University of Koblenz-Landau, Germany. Her research interests are personalization stra­
Ostertagová, E., Ostertag, O., & Kovác, J. (2014). Methodology and Application of the
tegies in marketing, hyper-personalization, customer experience management, and service
Kruskal-Wallis Test. Applied Mechanics and Materials, 611, 115–120.
excellence.

188
M.H.J. Gouthier et al. Journal of Business Research 148 (2022) 174–189

Lars Wendel is a graduate of the master’s program in Information Management at the


University of Koblenz-Landau, Germany. Now he is working as a Junior SAP consultant at
the IT company “Bechtle” in Bonn, Germany.

189

You might also like