You are on page 1of 13

Journal of Health and Social Behavior

Social Conditions as 51(S) S28­–S40


© American Sociological Association 2010
Fundamental Causes of Health DOI: 10.1177/0022146510383498
http://jhsb.sagepub.com

Inequalities:Theory, Evidence,
and Policy Implications

Jo C. Phelan1, Bruce G. Link1,2, and


Parisa Tehranifar1

Abstract
Link and Phelan (1995) developed the theory of fundamental causes to explain why the association between
socioeconomic status (SES) and mortality has persisted despite radical changes in the diseases and risk
factors that are presumed to explain it. They proposed that the enduring association results because
SES embodies an array of resources, such as money, knowledge, prestige, power, and beneficial social
connections that protect health no matter what mechanisms are relevant at any given time. In this article,
we explicate the theory, review key findings, discuss refinements and limits to the theory, and discuss
implications for health policies that might reduce health inequalities. We advocate policies that encourage
medical and other health-promoting advances while at the same time breaking or weakening the link
between these advances and socioeconomic resources. This can be accomplished either by reducing
disparities in socioeconomic resources themselves or by developing interventions that, by their nature,
are more equally distributed across SES groups.

Keywords:
health disparities, social stratification, fundamental causes, health, mortality

As we mark the fiftieth anniversary of the Medical years, 472; at 12 years, 352; and at >12 years, 165)
Sociology Section of the American Sociological (National Center for Health Statistics 2008). Simi-
Association, one of the most basic and critical lar levels of inequality are observed between
problems addressed by medical sociologists is a income groups.
very old one: the fact that society’s poorer and less These inequalities in overall health and mortal-
privileged members live in worse health and die ity are not only very common in modern times, but
much younger than the rich and more privileged they have persisted at similar levels at least since
ones. Socioeconomic inequalities in health and the early nineteenth century (Antonovsky 1967).
mortality are very large, very robust, and very well This persistence is puzzling because major ­diseases
documented. Typically, age-adjusted risk of death and risk factors that appeared to account for the
for those in the lowest socioeconomic level is
double to triple that for the highest level
1
(Antonovsky 1967; Sorlie, Backlund, and Keller Columbia University
2
New York State Psychiatric Institute
1995; Kunst, Feikje, and Mackenbach 1998). To
illustrate, in 2005, all-cause, age-adjusted death Corresponding Author:
rates for individuals between the ages of 25 and 64 Jo C. Phelan, Columbia University, Department of
were strongly related to education level for both Sociomedical Sciences, 722 W. 168th Street, 16th floor,
men (at < 12 years, 821 per 100,000; at 12 years, New York, NY 10032
605; and at > 12 years, 249) and women (at < 12 E-mail: jcp13@columbia.edu
Phelan et al. S29

inequalities seen in earlier periods (i.e., deadly it influences multiple disease outcomes, meaning
infectious diseases such as diphtheria, measles, that it is not limited to only one or a few diseases
typhoid fever, and tuberculosis fueled by over- or health problems. Second, it affects these disease
crowding and poor sanitation in low socioeco- outcomes through multiple risk factors. Third, it
nomic status homes and communities) have been involves access to resources that can be used to
virtually eradicated in the developed world. Rather avoid risks or to minimize the consequences of
than disappearing, socioeconomic status (SES) disease once it occurs. Finally, the association
inequalities in mortality have persisted and now between a fundamental cause and health is repro-
reflect new major causes of death including can- duced over time via the replacement of intervening
cers and cardiovascular illness, fueled by risk fac- mechanisms (Link and Phelan 1995). It is the per-
tors such as poor diet, inadequate exercise, and sistent association of SES with overall health in the
smoking that are more common in lower SES face of dramatic changes in mechanisms linking
groups. Socioeconomic inequalities in health and SES and health that led Link and Phelan to call
mortality have even survived concerted efforts to SES a “fundamental” cause of health inequalities.
eliminate them, such as institution of the United
Kingdom’s National Health Service, their vast
publicly-funded health care system (Black et al. The Central Role of Flexible Resources
1982). for SES Inequalities in Health
It is this persistence across time that Link and
Phelan (1995) aimed to explain with their theory of According to the theory of fundamental causes, an
fundamental causes. They reasoned that we cannot important reason that SES is related to multiple
claim to understand why health inequalities exist if disease outcomes through multiple pathways that
we cannot explain why they persist under condi- change over time is that individuals and groups
tions that should eliminate or reduce them, and if deploy resources to avoid risks and adopt protec-
we can understand why they persist, this may pro- tive strategies. Key resources such as knowledge,
vide clues to the more general problem of the money, power, prestige, and beneficial social con-
causes of health inequalities. That is, the remarka- nections can be used no matter what the risk and
ble persistence of inequalities may provide a lever protective factors are in a given circumstance.
for understanding the more general fact of their Consequently, fundamental causes affect health
existence. even when the profile of risk and protective factors
In this article, we will explicate the theory as it and diseases changes radically. If the problem is
has developed over the past 15 years, review key cholera, for example, a person with greater
empirical findings, develop some refinements of resources is better able to avoid areas where the
the theory, address potential limits of the theory, disease is rampant, and highly resourced commu-
and discuss implications for health policies that nities are better able to prohibit entry of infected
might reduce health inequalities. persons. If the problem is heart disease, a person
with greater resources is better able to maintain a
heart-healthy lifestyle and get the best medical
The Theory treatment available. Because these resources can
be used in different ways in different situations, we
The theory of fundamental causes is rooted in call them flexible resources.
Lieberson’s (1985) concept of basic causes, which It is their capacity to be used flexibly by indi-
was first applied to the association between SES viduals and groups that places resources of knowl-
and mortality by House and colleagues (House edge, money, power, prestige, and beneficial social
et al. 1990, 1994). The theory has been developed connections at the center of fundamental cause
primarily by Link and Phelan (Link and Phelan theory. Their flexible use tells us why SES gradi-
1995; Phelan et al. 2004; Link and Phelan, forth- ents tend to reproduce themselves over time. This
coming), with significant elaboration and exten- focus on resources and their deployment does not
sion by Lutfey and Freese (2005). deny the importance of antecedent causes of the
The primary statement of the theory appeared resources themselves that lie in the social, eco-
in 1995 in a previous special issue of the Journal nomic, and political structures of society. In fact,
of Health and Social Behavior. According to Link fundamental cause theory is deeply connected to
and Phelan (1995), a fundamental social cause of the sociological study of stratification in this
health inequalities has four essential features. First, way—the resources highlighted in fundamental
S30 Journal of Health and Social Behavior 51(S)

cause theory must come from somewhere, and In these circumstances, the person benefits in
theories of the origins of inequalities are the best numerous ways that do not depend on his or her
source for understanding these processes. To own initiative or ability to personally construct a
understand how flexible resources might facilitate healthy situation; it is an “add on” benefit opera-
the creation of new mechanisms linking SES and tive at the contextual level. These contexts may be
health, consider the following example. Screening meso (families) or macro levels (a congressional
for several cancers has become possible over the block that opposes changes in health care policy
past few decades, making it feasible to detect can- that would shift the distribution of health care
cer or its precursors earlier, thereby helping to away from high SES groups to the uninsured),
prevent mortality from these cancers. Since the formal (employer or trade union) or informal
screening procedures represent relatively recent (social networks). The clearest example of funda-
technological advances, one can imagine a time mental cause theory occurs when groups explicitly
before the procedures existed, when resources had push for better health conditions for their mem-
no bearing on access to cancer screening because bers. But the health-enhancing use of group
the procedures did not exist. There was no mecha- resources can operate at a less explicit level. Con-
nism linking SES to screening access to health. sider Cockerham’s (2005) ideas about the influ-
But after the screening procedures were devel- ence of status groups on health lifestyles. According
oped, people with more resources could use those to Cockerham, social norms and other social sup-
resources to gain access to the life-saving screens. ports, such as the health-product industry, rein-
Link et al. (1998) presented evidence from the force distinctive health lifestyles in different status
Behavioral Risk Factor Survey showing that groups, and the lifestyles of high SES groups are
screening rates for cervical and breast cancer are particularly healthy ones. In these instances sta-
indeed associated with education and income.1 A tus groups do not explicitly advocate for health-
new mechanism had emerged to link social condi- enhancing conditions, but rather members form
tions to health outcomes. The idea is that this proc- cultural practices around food, exercise, and
ess extends beyond this example to many, many other health-related circumstances that influence
others. the behavior of status-group members. These
The flexible resources that are central to funda- lifestyles are shaped by the extant stock of
mental cause theory operate at both individual and health knowledge and pecuniary resources gen-
contextual levels. At the individual level, flexible erally available in particular status groups—a
resources can be conceptualized as the “cause of circumstance that generally leads to healthier
causes” or “risk of risks” that shape individual lifestyles in higher status groups. For example, it
health behaviors by influencing whether people is almost unheard of for snacks offered at meet-
know about, have access to, can afford, and receive ings held at the Mailman School of Public
social support for their efforts to engage in health- Health at Columbia University to not include
enhancing or health-protective behaviors. In addi- multiple varieties of fruits; Dunkin Donuts, in
tion, resources shape access to broad contexts that contrast, are rare indeed. It is not as if the people
vary dramatically in associated risk profiles and who order these snacks explicitly consider the
protective factors. For example, a person with health impact of their choices each time a deci-
many resources can afford to live in a high SES sion is made. Instead, cultural practices shaped
neighborhood where neighbors are also of high over time lead them to order the conventional,
status and where, collectively, enormous clout is and the conventional in this context is generally
exerted to ensure that crime, noise, violence, pol- healthy fare.
lution, traffic, and vermin are minimized, and that
the best health-care facilities, parks, playgrounds,
and food stores are located nearby. Once a person Key Empirical Findings
has used SES-related resources to locate in an
advantaged neighborhood, a host of health-­ Empirical tests of the theory are not obvious or
enhancing circumstances comes along as a pack- straightforward. A demonstration of socioeco-
age deal. Similarly, a person who uses educational nomic inequalities in health or mortality, even ones
credentials to procure a high-status occupation that persists over time, does not in itself constitute
inherits a package deal that is more likely to support for the theory. It is precisely the nearly
include excellent health benefits and less likely to ubiquitous inverse association between SES and
involve ­dangerous conditions and toxic exposures. mortality that the theory attempts to explain.
Phelan et al. S31

­ emonstrating this association in any particular


D e­ thnographic analysis, they use the example of
circumstances cannot adjudicate between funda- routine diabetes care in two socioeconomically
mental causes and other possible explanations of contrasting clinics to articulate several concrete
those facts. ways in which differential health outcomes emerge
Empirical support for the theory relies on eval- in the two clinics. For example, the clinic serving
uating the four essential features of a fundamental higher SES patients provided better continuity of
cause of health inequalities (Link and Phelan care, and the higher SES patients encountered
1995). In the following sections, we present key fewer costs of complying with treatment regimens
findings bearing on each of these components: and had more knowledge about diabetes. Similar
(1) evidence that SES influences multiple disease analyses conducted in a variety of contexts relating
outcomes; (2) evidence that SES is related to multi- to treatment or prevention of a variety of diseases
ple risk factors for disease and death; (3) evidence would enrich our understanding of the pathways
that the deployment of resources plays a critical role through which SES influences health and longevity.
in the association between SES and health/mortality;
and (4) evidence that the association between SES
and health/mortality is reproduced over time via the Evidence that the Deployment of Resources
replacement of intervening mechanisms. Plays a Critical Role in the Association
between SES and Health
Evidence That SES Is Related to Multiple Central to fundamental cause theory is the idea that
Disease Outcomes via Multiple Risk Factors resources of money, knowledge, power, prestige,
and beneficial social connections are critical to
The first two propositions are strongly supported maintaining a health advantage. Empirically test-
by empirical data. Low SES is related to a multi- ing the importance of resources per se is difficult,
plicity of diseases and other causes of death. The because it requires the identification of situations
broad generality of this association can be sum- in which the ability to use socioeconomic resources
marized with two sets of facts: (1) Low SES is can be analytically separated from SES itself (e.g.,
related to mortality from each of the broad catego- situations in which high SES persons are prevented
ries of chronic diseases, communicable diseases, from using their resources to gain a health advan-
and injuries (Pamuk et al. 1998; National Center tage). If the utilization of resources is critical in
for Health Statistics 2008), and (2) low SES is maintaining health or prolonging life, then in situ-
related to mortality from each of the 14 major ations in which the resources associated with
causes of death in the International Classification higher status are of no use, high SES should confer
of Diseases (Illsley and Mullen 1985). no advantage, and the usually robust association
There is also clear evidence that SES is associ- between SES and health or mortality should be
ated with numerous risk and protective factors for greatly reduced.
disease and other causes of death, both currently One such situation occurs when the causes and
and in the past. These include smoking, sedentari- cures of fatal diseases are unknown. In these cir-
ness, and being overweight (Lantz et al. 1998; cumstances, socioeconomic resources cannot be
Link 2008); stressful life conditions (Turner, used to avoid death due to these diseases, because
Wheaton, and Lloyd 1995; House and Williams it is not known how the resources should be
2000); social isolation (House and Williams 2000; deployed. Thus, to the extent that the ability to use
Ruberman et al. 1984); preventive health care socioeconomic resources is critical in maintaining
(Dutton 1978; Link et al. 1998); and crowded and SES inequalities in mortality, there should be
unsanitary living conditions, unsanitary water sup- strong SES gradients in mortality for causes of
plies, and malnutrition (Rosen 1979). death that are highly preventable —for which we
Lutfey and Freese (2005) describe this compo- have good knowledge and effective measures for
nent of the theory as involving a “massive multi- prevention or treatment. However, for causes of
plicity of mechanisms.” They suggest that, because death about which we know little regarding pre-
fundamental cause processes are “holographic,” vention or treatment, SES gradients in mortality
such a multiplicity of mechanisms should be found should be much weaker. Consistent with this pre-
in all or most particular instances in which SES diction, Phelan et al. (2004) found that socioeco-
and health outcomes are connected. Using an nomic inequalities in mortality were significantly
S32 Journal of Health and Social Behavior 51(S)

more pronounced for causes of death that were which prevailing medical recommendations are
reliably rated by two physician-epidemiologists as subsequently discovered to be harmful (Carpiano
being highly preventable (such as lung cancer and and Kelly 2007) and the case of old age, when the
ischemic heart disease), and thus more amenable growing frailty of the body may place limits on the
to the application of flexible resources than for effectiveness of interventions (Phelan et al. 2004).
causes that were rated as not very preventable (such
as brain cancer and arrhythmias). Although they do
not address or explicitly test fundamental cause Evidence That the Association between SES
theory, three other studies that reported evidence and Health/Mortality Is Reproduced over
on this issue also found that the SES-mortality Time via the Replacement of Intervening
association was stronger for preventable causes of Mechanisms
death (Dahl, Hofoss, and Elstad 2007; Marshall
et al. 1993; Song and Byeon 2000). The fourth essential feature of SES as a fundamental
Evidence for the validity and generality of cause of health inequalities is that the association
these findings is strengthened by another study that between SES and health/mortality is reproduced
employed a similar research strategy but (1) exam- over time via the replacement of intervening mech-
ined a different set of causes of death, (2) confined anisms. This key element of the theory arose from
attention to treatment rather than including preven- two sets of observations: (1) The SES-mortality
tion, (3) used a different and more objective meas- association persisted over time despite the decline
ure of amenability to treatment, and (4) examined of mechanisms (e.g., poor sanitation and wide-
racial and ethnic differences as opposed to socio- spread death from infectious disease) that formerly
economic ones.2 Tehranifar et al. (2009) identified, provided important links between SES and mor-
prior to hypothesis testing, cancers that are more or tality; and (2) new, previously weak or absent
less amenable to treatment and examined whether mechanisms currently link SES and mortality (e.g.,
racial-ethnic differences in disease-specific mor- smoking, exercise, diet, and cardiovascular dis-
tality varied according to the degree to which that ease). These observations are consistent with the
disease is amenable to available medical interven- idea that socioeconomic inequalities in health are
tion. This study used five-year survival rates for 53 reproduced via the replacement of intervening
different cancer sites as a measure of effectiveness mechanisms. To more fully evaluate this compo-
of treatment and/or early detection methods. Con- nent of the theory, however, more direct evidence
sistent with fundamental cause theory, survival was needed showing the emergence of new mecha-
disparities comparing disadvantaged minority nisms. In particular, the theory predicts that new
groups (African Americans, American Indians, and mechanisms arise following the development of
Hispanics) to whites were substantially greater for new knowledge or medical intervention related to
cancers that were more amenable to treatment some disease, because higher SES individuals and
(e.g., cancers with five-year relative survival rates groups are better equipped to take advantage of the
≥ 70%, such as bladder, breast, and prostate can- new knowledge. Therefore, a key empirical ques-
cers) than they were for cancers that were less so tion is whether the SES-health gradient shifts in
(e.g., cancers with five-year relative survival rates favor of high SES individuals following the devel-
< 40%, such as liver, pancreatic, and esophageal opment of new knowledge. This evidence is par-
cancers). ticularly persuasive if the health outcome for
These studies show that, somewhat ironically, which a shifted gradient is observed is directly
one way in which fundamental cause theory can be related to the emergent knowledge, for example, if
tested is by looking for exceptions to the strong an advance in heart disease treatment furthers the
SES gradient in health or mortality that is almost advantage of high SES individuals in terms of
always observed—exceptions in which the ability heart disease mortality. Just as important is evi-
to use resources to gain a health advantage is dence that, in the absence of advances in knowl-
blocked. In these examples, the use of socioeco- edge, the SES gradient in relevant health outcomes
nomic resources to improve health is blocked remains fairly steady.
because risk factors are unknown and treatments Several such analyses have now been con-
do not exist (Phelan et al. 2004; Tehranifar et al. ducted. Phelan and Link (2005) examined selected
2009). Other situations in which resources may be causes of death for which great strides in preven-
unhelpful or even harmful may be exploitable for tion or treatment were made over the last half of
testing of the theory. Examples are situations in the twentieth century (heart disease, lung cancer,
Phelan et al. S33

and colon cancer), and for which much less socioeconomic gradients in smoking. Scientific
progress had been made over the same period evidence strongly linking smoking to lung cancer
(brain cancer, ovarian cancer, and pancreatic can- emerged in the early 1950s. To assess changes that
cer). Looking at age-adjusted death rates by race may have occurred in the decades following the
and by county-level SES, they reported that, for production of this new knowledge, Link (2008)
the causes of death where little had been learned analyzed multiple public opinion polls assessing
about treatment or prevention, mortality rates smoking beliefs and behaviors. Evidence from the
stayed fairly steady, and the degree of inequality first surveys conducted just as the scientific evi-
based on race and SES stayed fairly steady as well. dence was emerging in 1954 showed that, while
By contrast, for the causes of death where gains in most people had heard about the findings, only a
treatment and prevention had been significant, minority believed that smoking was a cause of
overall mortality rates declined while race and SES lung cancer, and no educational gradient in this
gradients shifted in the direction of relatively belief was evident. Nor was smoking behavior
higher mortality for the less advantaged group. strongly linked to educational attainment in 1954.
Subsequent studies have gone much further in Over the subsequent 45 years, as people began to
drawing specific connections between gains in adopt the belief that smoking is a cause of lung
knowledge and subsequent changes in relevant cancer, sharp educational gradients opened up in
disease outcomes. Carpiano and Kelly (2007) ana- this belief. Additionally, people of higher educa-
lyzed changes in breast cancer incidence following tion were less likely to start smoking and more
the widely publicized findings from the Women’s likely to quit, thereby generating a strong SES
Health Initiative (WHI) that linked hormone gradient in smoking behavior (Link 2008). A new
replacement therapy to increased breast cancer risk and powerful mechanism linking SES to an impor-
(Haas et al. 2004). In the following two years, tant health behavior had emerged.
consistent with the racial pattern in the use of hor- The studies just described are particularly valu-
mone replacement therapy (Haas 2004; Hulley able for their ability to pinpoint temporal connec-
et al. 1998), breast cancer incidence among white tions between particular developments in
women age 50 and older, the age group most likely knowledge and technology surrounding specific
to have been using hormone therapy before the WHI diseases, on the one hand, and changes in SES-
study results were publicized, dropped precipi- related health gradients predicted by the theory, on
tously, while incidence among black women in that the other. Moreover, these studies address major
age group stayed fairly steady (Carpiano and Kelly diseases that are important causes of death. How-
2007). These findings were confirmed by another ever, there is always the possibility that these cases
study that also considered county-level median are not representative of the situation that holds
household income and breast tumor estrogen (ER) more generally when new health knowledge or
receptor status (Krieger, Chen and Waterman 2010). technology develops. For this reason, the more
That study found the decline in breast cancer inci- systematic and comprehensive analysis of Glied
dence after the WHI study publication to be lim- and Lleras-Muney (2008) is particularly valuable.
ited to white women, aged 50 and older, who were This study provides evidence that the results of the
residents of high income counties and had estro- case studies reported above are indeed generaliza-
gen-positive breast tumors (the type of tumor most ble. Like Phelan et al. (2004) and Tehranifar et al.
likely to be affected by hormone replacement). (2009), Glied and Lleras-Muney conducted a sys-
Chang and Lauderdale (2009) studied the tematic test based on a comprehensive set of dis-
impact of statins (an effective and expensive med- eases. In fact, Glied and Lleras-Muney repeated
ication to lower cholesterol) on socioeconomic their analysis with two separate data sets: the Mor-
gradients in cholesterol levels. Using nationally tality Detail Files from the National Center for
representative data from 1976 to 2004, they found Health Statistics, and the Surveillance Epidemiology
that those with higher income initially had higher and End Results cancer registry. They operational-
cholesterol levels, but that the SES-cholesterol ized the development of life-saving knowledge and
association then reversed and became negative in technology, or “innovation,” in two ways. In the
the era of widespread statin use. first they used the rate of change in mortality over
Link (2008) traced changes in knowledge, time to indicate progress in addressing mortality
beliefs, and behavior that followed the discovery due to particular diseases, the assumption being
of a causal link between cigarette smoking and that the greater the decline in mortality, the greater
lung cancer, and that eventually led to strong the progress that has been made. In the second,
S34 Journal of Health and Social Behavior 51(S)

they used the number of active drugs approved strengthen the theory but to understand how it may
to treat particular diseases, with the assumption be possible to weaken new mechanisms connect-
that more progress has been made where more ing SES and disease/mortality, and how old ones
new drugs have been developed to treat disease. may be undermined.
They found, consistent with the theory of funda-
mental causes, that educational gradients became
larger for diseases where greater innovation had Specifying Conditions that Modify the
occurred. Impact of New Knowledge on Health
In summary, evidence has accumulated that is Inequalities
consistent with each of the four components of
fundamental cause theory. Empirical testing of the The situation that most clearly exemplifies funda-
theory is accelerating, and studies are now being mental cause processes is one in which we initially
conducted by researchers other than the theory’s know nothing about how to prevent or cure a dis-
originators. This is a desirable development, as it ease, and there is no association between SES and
raises confidence that the theory is being subjected morbidity or mortality due to that disease. Then,
to scientific scrutiny. upon discovery of modifiable risk or protective
factors, an inverse association between SES and
the disease in question emerges. But other situa-
Returning To The Theory: tions that differ from this prototype are not only
Refinements And Limitations possible but to be expected.
One factor that should modify the impact of
Refinements to Fundamental Cause Theory emergent knowledge is the pre-existing SES distri-
The theory has two sets of implications for conti- bution of the disease at the time of a new advance
nuity and change in health inequalities over time. in prevention or treatment.3 The pre-existing asso-
The theory’s basic principle—that a superior col- ciation between the disease and SES is unlikely to
lection of flexible resources held by higher SES be null for two reasons. First, when new knowl-
individuals and the collectivities to which they edge and technology emerge, it is often the case
belong allow those of higher SES to avoid disease that prior knowledge and technology have already
and death in widely divergent circumstances— shaped the association between SES and disease;
leads to the prediction that, at any given time, the new knowledge will further shape this associa-
greater resources will produce better health, and tion. Second, even in the absence of previous
consequently inequalities in health and mortality knowledge about its risk and protective factors, a
will persist as long as resource inequalities do. disease may be influenced by factors that are asso-
At the same time, this long-term stability in the ciated with SES, either directly or inversely. For
association between SES and health/mortality example, before cholesterol was identified as a risk
results from the amalgamation of effects across factor for cardiovascular disease its levels were
many specific processes and conditions. New likely higher in higher SES populations because
knowledge and technology relating to innumerable such populations had greater access to relatively
diseases emerges constantly. The nature of the new expensive fatty foods.
knowledge varies, and the social conditions in The reason that prior associations between risk
which this knowledge emerges also vary. As a factors or diseases and SES are important for fun-
result, while in general new knowledge and medi- damental cause theory is that the new knowledge
cal development about a disease will lead to a shift has greater utility for those who have the disease or
in the disease gradient in favor of higher SES indi- risk factor. Notably, if the initial association
viduals and groups, they will not all have an identi- between SES and the disease is inverse such that
cal impact on this gradient. Another reason for the people of lower SES are at greater risk, an effec-
long-term stability in the SES-mortality associa- tive intervention can reduce inequalities in that
tion is that old mechanisms wane to be replaced by disease. This is because more people of low SES
new ones. Again, the demise of mechanisms is not a are likely to benefit from the intervention, because
uniform process: Some mechanisms have long more of them have the disease initially. This can be
lives, others short ones. In this section, we take steps true even if persons of higher SES who have the
toward understanding some of the conditions that disease are more likely to gain access to and ben-
lead to variations in the processes of mechanism efit from the intervention than lower SES persons
generation and demise. Our aim is not only to who have the disease. We call this a “give back
Phelan et al. S35

effect” (Link and Phelan, forthcoming), because linking resources to health existed, but only for a
the initial inverse SES-to-disease association pro- short time. The vaccine was quickly approved for
vides a starting point that allows the new knowl- widespread distribution to the U.S. population, and
edge about the disease to “give back” some polio was virtually eradicated here. Other mecha-
equality even though it may also exemplify a fun- nisms remain potent for a very long time. For,
damental cause process in which the knowledge is example the discovery of the pap test for the early
not distributed equally across socioeconomic detection and prevention of cervical cancer has
groups. For example, smoking is a risk factor that existed since the 1940s. Early on, access to the test
has been influenced by knowledge of its harmful was shaped by flexible resources creating an ine-
effects such that what was once a direct SES-to- quality in the use of this life-saving screen that
smoking association has become a sharply graded remains prominent today. As these examples sug-
inverse association, and one reason that SES is gest, some mechanisms become long-lasting while
related to smoking-related diseases. others have short lives. If we can understand what
In this context a “give back” effect would arise leads to the demise of mechanisms, and especially
if a new intervention blocked the effect of smoking how that decline is related to flexible resources, we
on heart disease or lung cancer mortality. Even if may open avenues to speed such a demise and
this new intervention was itself maldistributed by reduce health inequalities. Indeed, much of the
SES, a “give back” effect might arise because public health significance of fundamental cause
smoking is so much more common in low SES theory may reside in understanding how the link
populations; in other words, there are more people between flexible resources and health-relevant risk
at the low end who can benefit from the new inter- and protective factors has been broken.
vention. Importantly, from a fundamental cause
perspective, if the intervention had been discov-
ered earlier, before an SES-to-disease association Limits on Fundamental Cause Theory:
in smoking emerged, and if the intervention had Countervailing Mechanisms
been maldistributed by SES at that time, the inter-
vention would have created an inverse association Whereas the previous sections elaborated funda-
between SES and lung cancer or heart disease. mental cause theory, here we consider conditions
that place limits on the theory.
We believe readers will agree that health and
Mechanism Demise and Death longevity are desirable, but they are not all that a
Whereas it is understandable that empirical tests person may want. Other things being equal, those
have focused on the creation of mechanisms that with more resources can be expected to deploy
produce health inequalities, fundamental cause the- those resources to increase health. But there are
ory is predicated on the idea that mechanisms are undoubtedly situations in which the goals of health
replaced. Replacement requires that old mecha- and long life compete with and may cede domi-
nisms wane in importance over time. In fact, the nance to other important life goals. Perhaps desid-
theory emerged in part because prominent risk- erata such as power, manliness, or beauty are
factor mechanisms associated with vicious infectious sometimes more powerful motivators than health,
diseases declined in significance as germ theory, and are pursued to the detriment of health. Lutfey
improved sanitation, and vaccination came into and Freese (2005) refer to these competing goals
existence. Thus, understanding the demise and death as “countervailing mechanisms.” The potential for
of mechanisms linking flexible resources to disease countervailing mechanisms does not threaten the
is an important area that needs more development truth-value of fundamental cause theory, because
and testing. We offer two examples that may help “fundamental relationships do not require that all
others develop this area of inquiry more fully. of the pathways between X and Y support the rela-
Salk’s discovery of the polio vaccine is an tionship. The only requirement is that the effects of
example of a mechanism that was very short-lived. [countervailing] mechanisms are cumulatively
Before his discovery, people of all resource levels smaller than the effects of mechanisms producing
could be afflicted, including, for example, Presi- the fundamental relationship” (Lutfey and Freese
dent Franklin Roosevelt. After the discovery, 2005:1365). However, to the extent that counter-
resource-rich individuals were more likely to vailing mechanisms are called upon post hoc to
receive the vaccine and be protected. A mechanism explain results that do not support the theory,
S36 Journal of Health and Social Behavior 51(S)

countervailing mechanisms pose a challenge to the status pursuit is one potential countervailing mech-
falsifiability of the theory. For this reason, as well anism to the SES-health association. In the context
as for the fuller understanding of health inequali- of particular empirical studies, researchers may be
ties, it is desirable to attend to countervailing able to consider a priori whether the situation
mechanisms systematically, as Lutfey and Freese under study is one in which the goals of health and
argue, and attempt to move the consideration of social status are likely to collide. Additional
countervailing mechanisms from post hoc to a ­motivations that might potentially be powerful
priori. enough to operate as countervailing mechanisms to
We first note that the connection between SES SES include power, affiliation, self-esteem, iden-
and health is an extremely powerful one and that tity, freedom, creation, and leisure (Maslow 1943;
goals that successfully compete with those of Max-Neef, Elizalde, and Hopenhayn 1989).
health and long life must surely be quite potent. Note that, in most circumstances, we would
For example, the goal of health attainment has expect the goal of good health to be compatible
been powerful enough to override or socially with goals of power, self-esteem and so on, and we
reconstruct many aspects of pleasure and pain— would expect higher SES individuals to use their
which would seem to be basic and powerful forces resources to achieve more of all these desiderata
in their own right—among the socioeconomically than lower SES persons would be able to. Still, the
privileged. Erstwhile pleasures such as well mar- example of status pursuit as a countervailing
bled steaks are eschewed by higher SES groups in mechanism suggests that there will be instances
favor of sushi-grade tuna. Similarly, in the past, when other powerful motivations that are more
exhausting physical activity was considered some- readily attained by high SES persons work to the
thing that high SES people were fortunate enough detriment of health. In those situations, the usual
to be able to avoid. Now, “no pain no gain” pre- association between resources and health should
vails in the most expensive health clubs. Cigarette be attenuated. Also note that these countervailing
smoking, although highly addictive, as well as mechanisms may create conditions when SES will
sexual practices that increase the risk of HIV/ not operate as a fundamental cause of health and
AIDS, have also been significantly altered by high mortality, but they do not negate the power of SES
SES groups in the name of health attainment. We as a fundamental cause of unequal life chances
also note that the goals of health and longevity are more generally.
strongly supported by social norms and other
forms of social support among high status groups
as part of the beneficial health lifestyle associated Implications For Health
with high SES (Cockerham 2005). We suggest that Policy
the power of health attainment to shape the behav-
ior of high SES individuals is largely due to these The fundamental cause approach leads to very
social forces, and we propose that successful coun- different policies for addressing health inequali-
tervailing mechanisms are also likely to be embed- ties than does an individually oriented risk-factor
ded in strong social norms and support. approach. The latter asks us to locate modifiable
One such motivation that may meet these con- risk factors that lie between distal cause (such as
ditions is status attainment. In Lutfey and Freese’s SES) and disease, and to intervene in those risk
(2005) ethnographic analysis of diabetes treat- factors. By addressing intervening factors, the
ment, the pursuit of status, for example, occupa- logic goes, we will eliminate health disparities.
tional success or staying thin, sometimes led higher Our approach points to the pitfalls of this logic
SES diabetic patients to behave in ways detrimen- and suggests that developing new interventions,
tal to the management of their disease. Similarly, even when beneficial to health, is very likely to
Courtenay (2000) suggests that signifiers of mas- increase social inequalities in health outcomes. The
culinity such as the denial of weakness and engage- idea that medical progress often leads to increased
ment in risky or aggressive behavior often health inequality leads to an obvious conundrum:
undermine men’s health. Thus, the pursuit of mas- Must we choose between improving overall levels
culine status may help explain the fact that women, of health and reducing inequalities in health? Some
who are generally lower resourced than men, live argue that continued inequalities in health out-
longer than men, a fact that would not be predicted comes are acceptable as long as overall health
by fundamental cause theory.4 It seems, then, that improves or that some improvement is achieved for
Phelan et al. S37

most social groups. We, on the other hand, are com- Prioritize the Development of Interventions
mitted to reducing health inequalities, but it seems that Do Not Entail the Use of Resources or
wrong-headed to oppose advances in health knowl- that Minimize the Relevance of Resources
edge and technology because those may increase
inequalities. We see no reason not to make both As we seek to create interventions to improve
outcomes important goals, simultaneously pursu- health, we need to ask if an intervention is some-
ing better overall health and reduced inequalities. thing that anyone can potentially adopt, or whether
We suggest some general strategies that we the benefit will only be available to people with the
believe will lead to improved overall population necessary resources. Fundamental cause theory
health without further widening social inequalities suggests that health inequalities based on SES can
in health. Our approach points to policies that be reduced by instituting health interventions that
encourage advances while breaking or weakening automatically benefit individuals irrespective of
the link between these advances and socioeco- their own resources or behaviors. Examples are the
nomic resources, either by reducing disparities in manufacture of automobiles with air bags as
socioeconomic resources themselves, or by devel- opposed to relying on the use of seatbelts; provid-
oping interventions that, by their nature, are more ing health screenings in schools, workplaces, and
equally distributed across SES groups. other community settings rather than only through
private physicians; providing health care to all
citizens rather than only to those with the requisite
Reduce Resource Inequalities resources; requiring window guards in all high-rise
The first recommendation falls outside the explicit apartments rather than advising parents to watch
domain of health policy, but according to funda- their children carefully; thoroughly inspecting
mental cause theory is intimately tied to it. The meat rather than advising consumers to wash cut-
theory stipulates that people and collectivities use ting boards and cook meat thoroughly; adding folic
their knowledge, money, power, prestige, and acid to grains rather than recommending that sup-
social connections to gain a health advantage, and plements be taken by pregnant women to prevent
thereby reproduce the SES gradient in health. The neural tube defects in developing embryos; requir-
most direct policy implication of the theory is that, ing landlords to keep homes free of lead paint
if we redistribute resources in the population so as hazards rather than warning parents to protect their
to reduce the degree of resource inequality, toddlers from chipped paint. In some cases, such as
inequalities in health should also decrease. Policies this last example, existing risks will be greater in
relevant to fundamental causes of disease form a low-income neighborhoods and contexts, and spe-
major part of the national agenda, whether this cial enforcement of these policies may be required
involves the minimum wage, housing for homeless in those contexts. In each example, the former
and low-income people, capital-gains and estate solution does not give an advantage to those with
taxes, parenting leave, social security, head-start greater resources, because individual resources are
programs and college-admission policies, regula- unrelated and irrelevant to benefiting from the
tion of lending practices, or other initiatives of this intervention.
type. We argue that all these policies are health- However, even if we become far more creative
relevant policies and that understanding how they in developing contextually based interventions
are relevant should be claimed as an essential part that blanket an entire population with health ben-
of the domain of medical sociology. efit, addressing many health problems will still
require individual resources and action. In these
cases, resource-rich persons are likely to fare bet-
Contextualize Risk Factors ter. Even in these cases, however, we can influence
Potential interventions that seek to change individ- the trajectory of inequalities by attending to the
ual risk profiles should first identify factors that put type of interventions we adopt. When we create
people at risk of risks, for example, power disadvan- interventions that are expensive, complicated and
tages that prevent some people from adopting safe time-consuming to carry out, and difficult to dis-
sex strategies or neighborhood environments that tribute broadly, we are likely to create health
make healthful foods unavailable. This will avoid ­disparities (Chang and Lauderdale 2009). Con-
the enactment of interventions aimed at changing versely, to the extent that we develop interventions
behaviors that are powerfully influenced by factors that are relatively affordable and easy to dissemi-
left untouched by the ­intervention. nate and use, we should be able to reduce the
S38 Journal of Health and Social Behavior 51(S)

degree to which new interventions give advantage the magnitude of inequalities in socioeconomic
to high SES persons. Goldman and Lakdawalla resources themselves and/or by minimizing the
(2005) analyzed two case studies supporting the extent to which socioeconomic resources buy a
idea that the introduction of difficult-to-implement health advantage. By attending to these principles,
treatments (in their analysis, HAART treatment for we believe we can move toward the important
HIV/AIDS) lead to increased SES inequalities in dual goals of continuing to improve overall popu-
health outcomes, whereas treatments that are sim- lation health while distributing that health more
pler and require less effort (in their analysis, beta- equally.
blockers to reduce hypertension) reduce such
inequalities. As Chang and Lauderdale (2009) sug- Funding
gest, this principle should also apply to cost: New
interventions that are less expensive should result This work was supported by a Young Investigator Award
in smaller SES-based health inequalities than granted to Professor Tehranifar by the Lance Armstrong
those that are more expensive. Chang and Lauder- Foundation.
dale also point out, importantly, that, “technolo-
gies that have the potential to contract disparities
will not do so unless they also diffuse broadly”
Notes
(Chang and Lauderdale 2009:257). We add that a 1. We acknowledge recent debate and changes in guide-
necessary ingredient of successful diffusion will lines with regard to screening interval and age at
be broadly disseminated and clearly stated infor- initiation of screening mammography and pap tests.
mation about how an intervention can help one’s However, convincing evidence supports the effective-
health, where that intervention is available, ness of these screens in reducing cancer mortality and
whether and how much of it is covered by health morbidity (U.S. Preventive Services Task Force
insurance plans, and, if not, how much it will cost 2009; ACOG Committee on Practice Bulletins—
individuals. Gynecology 2009).
2. Fundamental cause theory was developed to explain
the enduring effects of SES on health and mortality. It
Conclusion is possible that other social statuses, such as race, eth-
nicity, or gender, also have enduring associations with
The theory of fundamental causes attempts to resources of money, knowledge, power, prestige, and
explain why the association of SES to health and beneficial social connections, and with health and
mortality has persisted despite the demise of risk mortality, and that they may also operate as funda-
factors and diseases that appeared to explain the mental causes. Even if not, however, race and
association. Mounting evidence in support of the ethnicity are currently strongly related to resources
theory of fundamental causes begins to suggest and consequently would be expected to behave simi-
that the theory is not just an interesting idea but larly to SES in analyses such as Tehranifar’s
very possibly a valid explanation of persistent SES (Tehranifar et al. 2009), which focus on the current
inequalities in health and mortality. We believe health context.
this empirical support warrants the investment of 3. We thank David Mechanic for this insight.
medical sociologists in (1) further empirical analy- 4. Recent research suggests that, when health behaviors
ses using a variety of methodologies to give of women come to resemble those of men more
greater weight to the body of research, to specify closely, the female mortality advantage declines
and elaborate the processes at work, and to find (Preston and Wang 2006).
conditions that may block these processes and
(2) developing elaborations, extensions, and mod-
ifications of the theory itself. We also believe the References
accumulated evidence warrants serious attention ACOG Committee on Practice Bulletins—Gynecology.
to the implications of the theory for health policy. 2009. “ACOG Practice Bulletin No. 109: Cervical
Those implications are that, to achieve greater Cytology Screening.” Obstetrics and Gynecology
equality in matters of life, death, and health, the 114:1409–20.
connection between socioeconomic resources and Antonovsky, A. 1967. “Social Class, Life Expectancy,
health-beneficial preventive measures and treat- and Overall Mortality.” Milbank Memorial Quarterly
ments must be broken or diminished, by reducing 45:31–73.
Phelan et al. S39

Black, Douglas, J. N. Morris, Cyril Smith, and Peter S. L. Syme. Washington, DC: National Academy
Townsend. 1982. Inequalities in Health: The Black Press.
Report. Middlesex, England: Penguin. Hulley, S., D. Grady, T. Bush, C. Furberg, D. Her-
Carpiano, Richard M. and Brian C. Kelly. 2007. “Scien- rington, B. Riggs, and E. Vittinghoff. 1998. “Ran-
tific Knowledge as Resource and Risk: What Does domized Trial of Estrogen Plus Progestin for
Hormone Replacement Tell Us About Health Dispari- Secondary Prevention of Coronary Heart Disease in
ties.” Presented at the annual meeting of the American Postmenopausal Women: Heart and Estrogen/Pro-
Sociological Association, August 2007, New York. gestin Replacement Study (HERS) Research Group.”
Chang, Virginia W. and Diane S. Lauderdale. 2009. Journal of the American Medical Association 280:
“Fundamental Cause Theory, Technological Innova- 605–13.
tion, and Health Disparities: The Case of Cholesterol Illsley, Raymond and Ken Mullen. 1985. “The Health
in the Era of Statins.” Journal of Health and Social Needs of Disadvantaged Client Groups.” Pp. 389–
Behavior 50:245–60. 402 in Oxford Textbook of Public Health, edited by
Cockerham, William. 2005. “Health Lifestyle Theory and W. W. Holland, R. Detels, and G. Know. Oxford, Eng-
the Convergence of Agency and Structure.” Journal land: Oxford University Press.
of Health and Social Behavior 46:51–67. Krieger, N., J. T. Chen, and P. D. Waterman. 2010.
Courtenay, Will H. 2000. “Constructions of Masculinity “Decline in U.S. Breast Cancer Rates after the Women’s
and their Influence on Men’s Well-Being: A Theory Health Initiative: Socioeconomic and Racial/Ethnic
of Gender and Health.” Social Science and Medicine Differentials.” American Journal of Public Health
50:1385–1401. 100:S132–S139.
Dahl, Espen, Dag Hofoss, and Jon I. Elstad. 2007. “Edu- Kunst, A. E., G. Feikje, and J. P. Mackenbach [EU Work-
cational Inequalities in Avoidable Deaths in Norway: ing Group on Socioeconomic Inequalities in Health].
A Population Based Study.” Health Sociology Review 1998. “Occupational Class and Cause Specific Mor-
16:146–59. tality in Middle Aged Men in 11 European Countries:
Dutton, Diana H. 1978. “Explaining the Low Use of Comparison of Population Based Studies.” British
Health Services by the Poor: Costs, Attitude, or Medical Journal 316:1636–42.
Delivery Systems.” American Sociological Review Lantz, P. M., J. S. House, J. M. Lepkowski, D. R. Wil-
43:348–68. liams, R. P. Mero, and J. Chen. 1998. “Results from a
Glied, Sherry and Adriana Lleras-Muney. 2008. “Techno- Nationally Representative Prospective Study of U.S.
logical Innovation and Inequality in Health.” Demog- Adults.” Journal of The American Medical Associa-
raphy 45:741–61. tion 279:1703–08.
Goldman, D. P. and D. N. Lakdawalla. 2005. “A Theory Lieberson, Stanley. 1985. Making It Count: The Improve-
of Health Disparities and Medical Technology.” Con- ment of Social Research and Theory. Berkeley: Uni-
tributions to Economic Analysis and Policy 4:1–30. versity of California Press.
Haas, J. S., C. P. Kaplan, E. P. Gerstenberger, and K. Ker- Link, Bruce G. 2008. “Epidemiological Sociology and
likowske. 2004. “Changes in the Use of Postmeno- the Social Shaping of Population Health.” Journal of
pausal Hormone Therapy after the Publication of Health and Social Behavior 49:367–84.
Clinical Trial Results.” Annals of Internal Medicine Link, Bruce G., M. Northridge, J. Phelan, and M. Ganz.
140:184–88. 1998. “Social Epidemiology and the Fundamental
House, James S., Ronald C. Kessler, A. R. Herzog, R. P. Cause Concept: On the Structuring of Effective Can-
Mero, A. M. Kinney, and M. J. Breslow. 1990. “Age, cer Screens by Socioeconomic Status.” The Milbank
Socioeconomic Status, and Health.” The Milbank Quarterly 76:375–402.
Quarterly 68:383–411. Link, Bruce G. and Jo C. Phelan. 1995. “Social Condi-
House, James S., James M. Lepkowski, Ann M. Kinney, tions as Fundamental Causes of Disease.” Journal of
Richard P. Mero, Ronald C. Kessler, and A. Regula Health and Social Behavior 35(Extra Issue):80–94.
Herzog. 1994. “The Social Stratification of Aging ———. Forthcoming. “Social Conditions as Funda-
and Health.” Journal of Health and Social Behavior mental Causes of Health Inequalities.” In Handbook
35:213–34. of Medical Sociology, 6th ed., edited by C. Bird,
House, James S. and David R. Williams. 2000. “Under- P. Conrad, and A. Fremont. Nashville, TN: Vanderbilt
standing and Reducing Socioeconomic and Racial/ University Press.
Ethnic Disparities in Health.” Pp. 81–124 in Promot- Lutfey, Karen and Jeremy Freese. 2005. “Toward Some
ing Health: Intervention Strategies from Social and Fundamentals of Fundamental Causality: Socio-
Behavioral Research, edited by B. D. Smedley and economic Status and Health in the Routine Clinic
S40 Journal of Health and Social Behavior 51(S)

Visit for Diabetes.” American Journal of Sociology Sorlie, P., M. Backlund, and J. Keller. 1995. “U.S. Mor-
110:1326–72. tality by Economic, Demographic, and Social Charac-
Marshall, Stephen W., Ichiro Kawachi, Neil Pearce, and teristics: The National Longitudinal Mortality Study.”
Barry Borman. 1993. “Social Class Differences in American Journal of Public Health 85:949–56.
Mortality from Diseases Amenable to Medical Inter- Tehranifar, P., A. I. Neugut, J. C. Phelan, B. G. Link,
vention in New Zealand.” International Journal of Y. Liao, M. Desai, M. B. Terry. 2009. “Medical
Epidemiology 22:255–61. Advances and Racial/Ethnic Disparities in Cancer
Maslow, A. H. 1943. “A Theory of Human Motivation.” Survival.” Cancer Epidemiology, Biomarkers and
Psychological Review 50:370–96. Prevention 18:2701–08.
Max-Neef, Manfred, Antonio Elizalde, and Martín Turner, R. Jay., Blair Wheaton, and Donald A. Lloyd.
Hopenhayn. 1989. “Human Scale Development: An 1995. “The Epidemiology of Social Stress.” Ameri-
Option for the Future.” Development Dialogue: A Journal can Sociological Review 60:104–25.
of International Development Cooperation 1:7–80. U.S. Preventive Services Task Force. 2009. “Screening
National Center for Health Statistics. 2008. Health for Breast Cancer: U.S. Preventive Services Task
United States 2008: With Chartbook on Trends in the Force Recommendation Statement.” Annals of Inter-
Health of Americans. Hyattsville, MD. nal Medicine 151:716–26.
Pamuk, E. R., D. Makuc, K. Heck, C. Reuben, and K.
Lochner. 1998. Socioeconomic Status and Health
Bios
Chartbook: Health, United States, 1998. Hyattsville,
MD: National Center for Health Statistics. Jo C. Phelan is professor of sociomedical sciences at the
Phelan, Jo C. and Bruce G. Link. 2005. “Controlling Mailman School of Public Health of Columbia University.
Disease and Creating Disparities: A Fundamental Her research interests include social stigma, conceptions
Cause Perspective.” The Journals of Gerontology of mental illness, the impact of the “genetics revolution”
60B(special issue II):27–33. on the stigma of mental illness, attitudes and beliefs relat-
Phelan, Jo C., Bruce G. Link, Ana Diez-Roux, Ichiro ing to social inequality and its legitimation, and social
Kawachi, and Bruce Levin. 2004. “Fundamental Causes inequalities in health and mortality. In collaboration with
of Social Inequalities in Mortality: A Test of the Theory.” Bruce Link, she developed the argument that frames social
Journal of Health and Social Behavior 45:265–85. conditions as fundamental causes of disease.
Preston, S. H. and H. Wang. 2006. “Sex Mortality Differ-
ences in the United States: The Role of Cohort Smok- Bruce G. Link is professor of epidemiology and socio-
ing Patterns.” Demography 43:631–46. medical sciences at the Mailman School of Public Health
Rosen, G. 1979. “The Evolution of Social Medicine.” Pp. of Columbia University, and a Research Scientist at New
23–50 in The Handbook of Medical Sociology, 3rd York State Psychiatric Institute. His interests include the
ed., edited by H. Freeman, S. Levine, and L. Reeder. nature and consequences of stigma for people with mental
Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall. illnesses, the connection between mental illnesses and
Ruberman, William, Eve Weinblatt, Judith D. Goldberg, violent behaviors, and explanations for associations
and Banvir S. Chaudhary. 1984. “Psychological between social conditions and morbidity and mortality.
Influences on Mortality after Myocardial Infarction.”
New England Journal of Medicine 311:552–59. Parisa Tehranifar is assistant professor of epidemiology
Song, Yun-Mi and Jai Jun Byeon. 2000. “Excess Mor- at the Mailman School of Public Health of Columbia Uni-
tality from Avoidable and Non-Avoidable Causes in versity. Her research combines social science and
Men of Low Socioeconomic Status: A Prospective epidemiologic perspectives and methods to studies of
Study in Korea.” Journal of Epidemiology and Com- social inequalities in cancer and other chronic disease
munity Health 54:166–72. risk.

You might also like