You are on page 1of 12

Journal of Psychotherapy Integration © 2017 American Psychological Association

2017, Vol. 27, No. 1, 47–58 1053-0479/17/$12.00 http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/int0000061

Adaptation of the Motive-Oriented Therapeutic Relationship Scale


to Group Setting in Dialectical-Behaviour Therapy for Borderline
Personality Disorder

Sabine Keller, Dominique Page, Franz Caspar


Yves de Roten, University of Bern
and Jean-Nicolas Despland
University of Lausanne
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

Ueli Kramer
University of Lausanne

The therapeutic relationship as a process is usually studied in individual therapy, and


less in group therapy. One reason for this paucity of research may be the complex
methodology necessary to do process research on group therapy. One of the therapeutic
approaches using the group as part of the therapy is dialectical behavior therapy (DBT)
for borderline personality disorder (BPD). The purpose of the present study is to
develop a group version of a process measure that has been successfully used in
individual therapy, the Motive-Oriented Therapeutic Relationship (MOTR) scale,
based on individualized case conceptualizations using the Plan Analysis approach. To
do this, 10 sessions of a DBT skills group therapy were analyzed from a comprehensive
dataset within a randomized controlled trial. Included were therapy completers: 3
patients and 2 therapists. The therapists were unaware of MOTR. The results revealed
that the adaptation of the MOTR to DBT skills group was feasible. Its adaptation
showed differences of the therapists in their use of MOTR when comparing the
different patients: Therapist presented with higher degrees of MOTR toward 1 patient,
compared to another. Overall results suggest that effective therapists in DBT skills
training intervene with rather low mean levels of MOTR, and great intrasession
variability of MOTR. We conclude that the adaptation of the MOTR-instrument to
group therapy is feasible and yields meaningful results. Therefore, this scale may be
used in process research in group therapy, in particular when 1 wishes to have an
individualized measure of the therapeutic relationship.

Keywords: group therapy, Motive-Oriented Therapeutic Relationship, dialectical


behavior therapy, borderline personality disorder, process

The process of the therapeutic relationship is therapist(s)—makes such an endeavor highly diffi-
more often studied in a setting of one therapist cult. Therefore, it is useful to develop a method of
facing one patient; it is less analyzed in group measuring the therapeutic process that is adapted to
therapy (Norcross, 2011). The complexity of pos- the specific requirements of group therapy.
sible interactions and connections between several Group therapy in clinical settings is defined
individuals—the members of the group and the by the number of participants, by the fact that

This article was published Online First February 2, 2017. We thank Joya Raha for her commitment in editing the
Sabine Keller, Dominique Page, Yves de Roten, and present article.
Jean-Nicolas Despland, Department of Psychiatry- Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to
CHUV, University of Lausanne; Franz Caspar, Depart- Sabine Keller, CHUV Département de Psychiatrie, Institut Uni-
ment of Clinical Psychology and Psychotherapy, versitaire de Psychothérapie—Centre de Recherche en Psycho-
University of Bern; Ueli Kramer, Department of Psychi- thérapie, Site de Cery, Bâtiment Les Cèdres, 1008 Prilly, Swit-
atry-CHUV, University of Lausanne. zerland. E-mail: sabine.keller.psy@outlook.com

47
48 KELLER ET AL.

there are one or more mental health profession- predicting variations in outcome (Caspar &
als to facilitate the group process, by the fact Grosse-Holtforth, 2009; Kramer & Stiles, 2015;
that participants suffer from a diagnosed psy- McMain et al., 2015; Stiles et al., 1998). In this
chiatric disorder and also by negotiated and context, a group therapist who is appropriately
predetermined goals and tasks. Beyond symp- responsive to the group may give particular
tom reduction associated to group therapy, in attention to emerging productive relationship
the context of a cost-effective treatment (Fals- processes in the group and its members. So far,
Stewart, Marks, & Schafer, 1993; Kashner, very little research has been conducted to un-
Rost, Cohen, Anderson, & Smith, 1995; Weiss derstand group processes on such a detailed
et al., 2000), the benefits of group therapy may level of responsiveness.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

“include a reduced sense of isolation and One of the therapeutic approaches using the
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

uniqueness, mutual support, exposure to posi- group as part of the therapy is dialectical be-
tive models, and the opportunity to develop havior therapy (DBT) for borderline personality
coping skills by interacting with others” disorder (BPD). Dialectical behavior therapy
(Levine & Hogg, 2010, p. 922). Norcross was developed by Linehan (1993a, 1993b) for
(2011) reviewed the process variables related to patients presenting with BPD. Its effectiveness
group therapy which include, among others, the has been demonstrated by a number of studies
therapeutic alliance, empathy, goal consensus, (e.g., Decker & Naugle, 2008; Linehan, Arm-
collaboration, and group cohesion. Group cohe- strong, Suarez, Allmon, & Heard, 1991; Line-
sion, or cohesiveness, is the most prominent han et al., 2006; Linehan, Heard, & Armstrong,
relationship variable in group therapy (Burlin- 1993). DBT has five components: individual
game, Fuhriman, & Johnson, 2002; Norcross, therapy, skills training group therapy, therapists
2011) and is an important relational aspect in consultation among themselves and with
within the therapy group (Piper, Marrache, La- other DBT therapists, phone consultation be-
croix, Richardsen, & Jones, 1983). Piper et al. tween sessions and assistance in the structure of
(1983) reached this conclusion from studying the patient’s environment. DBT has a specific
nine groups composed of a total of 45 partici- vision of the therapeutic relationship, which is
pants with the purpose of learning about pro- understood as the core of change in therapy.
cesses in small groups. They measured the Indeed, the collaboration created between the
concept of cohesion using a self-report ques- therapist and the patient will allow the patient to
tionnaire, and it appeared that patient commit- oscillate between acceptance and change (Line-
ment to the group—a subscale of cohesion— han, 1993a). Given this importance of the rela-
was a crucial factor for individual members of tionship in DBT group therapy, it might be
the group to stay in treatment. In this study, particularly fruitful to deepen its understanding
patient commitment was also connected to the by using the responsiveness concept and ana-
perception of the physical distance between the lyze group sessions of DBT skills therapy in
members of the group. Toren and Shechtman fine-grained manner. Such detailed description
(2010) used Structural Equation Models to ex- might help to understand the actual therapeutic
plain the interactional nature of relationship interactions in DBT and might enable to deter-
variables in groups. Finally, Tikkanen and mine appropriate responsiveness in DBT. This
Leiman (2014) used a fine-grained dialogical- understanding may also help to refine therapy
sequence analysis to study relationship pro- interventions, starting from the observation of
cesses in groups, on the level of actual therapist- actual in-session behaviors.
patient speech turns. The latter method is In addition, moment-by-moment relationship
particularly prolific, as it becomes possible to processes are particularly important to track in
take into account the therapist’s responsive- treatments with patients presenting with BPD.
ness—the fact that the intervention choice is According to the Diagnostic and Statistical
codetermined by emerging context variables, Manual for Mental Disorder-Fourth Edition–
such as patient interactional features (Stiles, Text Revised (DSM–IV–TR), BPD has an im-
Honos-Webb, & Surko, 1998). Such features pact on cognitive, emotional, and relational as-
may emerge on a moment-by-moment basis. pects. These features may affect the patient’s
Therapist responsiveness is discussed as a par- interaction style in group therapy but suppos-
ticularly central variable in understanding and edly, in a different way for each patient; there-
ADAPTATION OF MOTR TO GROUP THERAPY 49

fore, a particular attention to the idiosyncratic Higher-order Plans may or may not directly
relationship variables at stake for each patient in relate to fundamental human needs, which are
the group process is necessary. understood to be limited in number and are
A method which takes into account the im- generally the same for everyone. Human funda-
pact of emerging—idiosyncratic—patient inter- mental needs may include, but are not limited
action features is, based on Plan Analysis, to, the need for control, for maintaining self-
the Motive-Oriented Therapeutic Relationship esteem, the need to be close (or, the avoidance
(MOTR; Caspar, 2007; Grawe, 1980). This con- of being alone), to maintain one’s psychic
cept is therefore consistent with the responsive- and/or physical integrity, and the need for pro-
ness concept previously described. The founda- tection and healing (Grawe, 2004).
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

tions of Plan Analysis were laid by Grawe As an example, consider the following ob-
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

(1980) with the Vertical Behavior Analysis, served in-session behavior: a patient who does
which was based on a reflection of “difficult” not remove his jacket during the session, despite
patients in a group therapy for social anxiety. the warm room temperature. We can infer for
Despite the accurate implementation of a group this particular behavior the Plan “get ready to
intervention, according to a prescription of be- leave,” which may stem from “keep your dis-
havior techniques, some patients did not opti- tance” and by the basic needs “protect yourself”
mally benefit from the treatment. How can we and “keep control.” Alternatively, it may also be
understand the individual patient’s underlying possible to infer the Plan “keep control of your-
motives of these cases? By referring to Miller, self,” which may imply “keep control of your
Galanter, and Pribram (1960), Caspar and life,” and “keep control”; also the therapist or
Grawe developed Plan Analysis, an individual- rater can infer “show the therapist that you can
ized procedure of case conceptualisation aiming go at any time,” and then “control the therapy
at understanding the instrumental structure of situation,” to arrive at “maintain your auton-
Plans mediating between concrete behaviors as omy” (see Figure 1). Despite the individualized
means, and motives; patient behaviors, as well methodology, there is a qualitative study that
as experiencing are seen from the perspective of established a prototypical Plan Analysis with
instrumental Plans. The latter word is capital- patients presenting with BPD (Berthoud,
ized following a suggestion by Miller and col- Kramer, de Roten, Despland, & Caspar, 2013).
leagues (1960) to highlight the difference in The MOTR (Caspar, 2007; Caspar & Grosse-
meaning as compared with the everyday lan- Holtforth, 2009; Grawe, 1992) is a type of re-
guage use of the word, above all presuming that lationship in which the therapist, using such a
most Plans are not rational or conscious. The Plan Analysis case formulation makes interven-
theoretical basis of this approach is large, in- tions responding to the patient’s acceptable mo-
cluding interpersonal approaches as well as so- tives in a proactive fashion. The aim is to assure
cial and developmental psychology, and cogni- a patient’s underlying acceptable motives,
tive science (i.e., information processing, thereby preventing the need for specific instru-
schemas; Caspar, 2007; Grawe, 1980, 1992). mental behaviors used by the patient. An ac-
Starting from observable behaviors and with ceptable motive is one that does not threaten the
a particular focus on the patient’s nonverbals, therapeutic relationship or unduly restrict the
the assessor infers behavior-related motives and therapeutic possibilities. According to this con-
goals (Caspar, 2007). To do this, the therapist cept (Caspar, 2007), nonverbal aspects are just
asks the question: “Which conscious or uncon- as important as verbal aspects. Indeed, if the
scious purpose could underlie a particular as- therapist’s intervention aims to address an ac-
pect of an individual’s behavior or experience?” ceptable Plan at a verbal level, but does this
(Caspar, 2007, p. 251). Here, the motives are with contradictory nonverbal attitude and para-
understood as a possible instrumental explana- verbal markers, it will not have the same im-
tion for the observed behaviors and experiences. pact. Correlated to this notion, process and con-
There may be several motives underlying the tent can be distinguished (Caspar et al., 2000).
same behavior and several behaviors that serve Referring to the example of the jacket, a
the same motive. Accordingly, the therapist or therapist using a complementary— or motive-
rater establishes a hierarchy of Plans, each Plan oriented—intervention, could, for example, ad-
being a purpose for the one (or ones) below. dress the acceptable Plan “keep control” by
50 KELLER ET AL.

Protect yourself Keep control Maintain your autonomy

Keep your distance Control Therapy

Keep control of your life


This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

Keep control of you


Get ready to leave
Show the therapist that you

can go at any time

Does not remove her jacket

Figure 1. An example of a part of a Plan Analysis.

saying to the patient “it is you who makes the ploratory data with regard to the specific applica-
decisions,” in a soft voice. A noncomplemen- tion of such a scale to the role of MOTR in DBT
tary intervention may be: “take off your jacket skills groups therapy.
immediately” in an authoritative tone. For a
more detailed clinical example of intervention,
Method
the reader may refer to Kramer, Berthoud,
Keller, and Caspar (2014).
Context
MOTR has shown links with outcome. Var-
ious studies have focused on MOTR, some with The sample of patients studied here is taken
patients suffering from depression (Caspar, from a larger randomized controlled trial on DBT
Grossmann, Unmüssig, & Schramm, 2005; skills group training for BPD (for more informa-
Schmutz, 2012), and others with patients pre- tion see Kramer, Pascual-Leone, et al., 2016; Page
senting with personality disorders (Kramer, & Kramer, 2012). In addition to the group ther-
Kolly, et al., 2014, 2011). apy, all patients had individual therapy.
MOTR, currently used for measuring process in
individual therapy, had originally emerged from Sample
elaborations related to group therapy (Grawe,
1980). We think it is time to take it back to its Patients. The selected therapy group is
roots by applying this concept to relationship pro- constituted of three patient completers who are
cesses in group therapy. Therefore, the purpose of all diagnosed with BPD (American Psychiatric
this study is to adapt a method of measuring Association, 2000). The group is composed of
process, the MOTR scale, to a group setting. In the two women and one man, with a mean age of
present study, we also aim at presenting first ex- 39.34 (range between 29 and 48).
ADAPTATION OF MOTR TO GROUP THERAPY 51

One of the three patients, Anna, is 41 years 18). Therapists followed a specific manual (for
old and remains stable regarding her symptoms more details: Page, 2010).
over the course of DBT skills training. Betty,
another patient, is 29 years old and shows great Instruments
improvement in her symptoms. The third pa-
tient is Gary, a 48-year-old man, whose symp- All questionnaires were given at intake, mid-
toms also decline over the course of the therapy way through the group treatment, and at dis-
(see Table 1). The group was originally com- charge. The Outcome Questionnaire (OQ-45;
posed of five patients but two patients had Lambert et al., 1996) is a self-report question-
dropped out of the therapy group during the two naire and includes 45 items to assess results of
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

first sessions (and were, therefore, excluded psychotherapy. It describes general symptoms
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

from this particular process analysis). This par- with a global score and has three subscales:
ticular group was selected for analysis in terms symptom distress, interpersonal role, and social
of feasibility of the process analysis in question: role. The French version was translated and
such a small group might present with a man- validated by Emond et al. (2004). Cronbach’s ␣
ageable degree of complexity and at the same for this small sample was .92.
time, is able to accurately address our research The Borderline Symptom List (BSL-23; Bo-
question on the adaptation of the MOTR to hus et al., 2009) is a short version, which in-
group therapy. cludes 23 items to assess borderline symptoms.
Therapists. Two therapists, a woman and a The French version was approved by the au-
man, facilitated the therapy group. They were thors. It is a self-rating instrument and uses a
trained in DBT techniques and were supervised general score. Cronbach’s ␣ for this small sam-
weekly. One of the therapists is a psychologist ple was .93.
and psychotherapist, and the other is a nurse. The Working Alliance Inventory-short version
They both are between 35 and 45 years old. (WAI-short version; Horvath & Greenberg, 1989)
They followed a DBT basic training course for assesses the therapeutic alliance with 12 items.
several months, as well as a specific 3-day in- The patient answers the questions by himself or
tensive training to be able to facilitate this par- herself. The questions evaluate the therapeutic
ticular group. They were unaware of the alliance, the link between the patient and the
MOTR-concept. therapist and the degree of agreement about
the tasks and aims of the therapy. The French
Treatment translation and validation was presented by
Corbière, Bisson, Lauzon, and Ricard (2006).
Therapy sessions were scheduled once a Cronbach’s ␣ for this small sample was .95.
week and each one lasted 1 hr and 30 min.
According to Linehan’s method (Linehan, Plan Analysis and the MOTR Scale
1993a, 2003b), patients were given the oppor-
tunity to train their skills in managing emotions The original method of the MOTR scale
through mindfulness (Sessions 2 to 6), improv- (Caspar et al., 2005) was conceived for an in-
ing their distress tolerance (Sessions 8 to 10) dividual setting, meaning one therapist facing
and by learning to regulate their emotions one patient. For a detailed description of the
through different techniques (Sessions 11 to procedure of application in an individual set-

Table 1
Descriptives of the Patients
Evolution
OQ-45 BSL-23 WAI
Patient Age Intake Middle End Intake Middle End Intake Middle End
Anna 41 95 96 101 2.05 2.10 2.26 61 31 63
Betty 29 87 70 15 .91 .57 .09 63 70 80
Gary 48 122 120 30 3.57 3.78 .43 41 22 74
52 KELLER ET AL.

ting, one may refer to Caspar and Grosse-

3
Complementary

Complementary
Nonverbal

Nonverbal
Holtforth (2009).
First of all, in this research, a rater constructed

2
the Plan Analysis of each of the three patients

MOTR scale (7-point scale)

MOTR scale (7-point scale)


using the first session, based on videotape. The

1
rater noted all instrumental behavior, both on a

MOTR (Motive-Oriented Therapeutic Relationship) Rating Scale: Individual Versus Group Coding Sheet (According to Caspar et al., 2005)
nonverbal and verbal level. The rater then inferred

0
underlying Plans. Consistent with the method, be-
haviors were written in the third person singular of

⫺1

⫺1
the indicative present, and the Plans were written

Anticomplementary

Anticomplementary
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

in the second person singular of the imperative


This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

present (see Figure 1).

Verbal

Verbal
⫺2

⫺2
To rate the MOTR, the rater analyzed video
sessions that were different from the session used
to establish the Plan Analysis. The sessions to be

⫺3

⫺3
rated with MOTR were divided into time seg-
ments, based on an initial step of analysis using
the identification of themes (Caspar, 2007). Each

Brief description
of the situation
Brief description of the situation (verbal and nonverbal
time unit lasted a maximum of 10 min, but may
have been as short as a few words. To code
MOTR for each time segment, the rater initially
focused on the patient to determine which Plan
behavior of the patient and the therapist
Individual MOTR rating scale

was being activated. Based on that, the rater fo-

Group MOTR rating scale


cused on the therapist’s speech and behavior pat-
terns during this time segment. He or she then

Central Plans
described the interventions of the therapist. The
rater assessed whether the therapist addressed the
acceptable Plan connected to the patient’s acti-
vated Plan; the rater coded the intervention. The
seven-point MOTR-scale ranges from ⫺3 (anti-
Plans activated

complementary: meaning therapist behaviors con-


Adaptations required for MOTR-group highlighted using bold font.
tradict central patient Plans) to 3 (complementary:
patient
in the

meaning therapist behaviors favor central patient


Plans) through zero (neutral), both on verbal and
para- and nonverbal levels. An acceptable Plan, or
“central Plan,” is the one that does not threaten the
therapeutic relationship. If the therapist happened
Anna, Betty,
(e.g., group,
Central Plans

to address a different Plan in the patient, the rater


and Gary)
speaks to
Therapist

coded this intervention less positively on the


7-point Likert scale (either verbally or nonver-
bally). The therapist could address several Plans
within a given sequence, up to a maximum of
three. One code was given for the verbal aspects
Plans activated

and one for the nonverbal aspects of each Plan; as


such, a maximum of nine codes could be given.
Therapist
patient
in the

All these elements were rated on a coding sheet


(see Table 2).

Procedure
Time during

Time during
session

session

This research is based on data taken from a


Table 2

pool used for a main study, a randomized con-


the

the

Note.

trolled trial that aimed at assessing the effec-


ADAPTATION OF MOTR TO GROUP THERAPY 53

tiveness of a 20-session short version of DBT code and another one subcolumn for the non-
group therapy (Kramer et al., 2016). The re- verbal code.
search was accepted by the ethics committee in No adaptation was required concerning the
2011. Every patient was informed of the aim of fact of noting the time and cutting sequences
the study and what would be required of him or during a session. That means that like for an
her throughout the study. Each patient signed a individual session, the sequence changes at the
written consent. The 20 sessions were video- point when the theme of conversation changes
taped. For reasons of coding feasibility, the or, the latest, after 10 min.
authors of the present article chose to analyze Concerning the first additional column, the
only half of the sessions, that is, a total of 10 initials of the therapist(s) speaking were noted
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

group sessions lasting 90 min each. To increase for each sequence. With two therapists, as in the
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

intersession comparability, only sessions in- present case, there are four ways to record
volving the same two therapists and the same which therapist speaks. In one sequence, it may
three patients were analyzed. Accordingly, the be only the (1) first or (2) second therapist who
following 10 sessions were evaluated with speaks; alternatively, (3 and 4) two therapists
MOTR: sessions number 4, 5, 11, 12, 13, 14, speak in the same sequence, complementing
15, 16, 17, and 18. each other, but therapist 1 may be the “main
Pseudonyms were used for the three patients: speaker” and therapist 2 the “second speaker”
Anna, Betty, and Gary. The MOTR (Caspar et (Option 3), or vice versa (Option 4), as a func-
al., 2005; Caspar & Grosse-Holtforth, 2009) tion of time of actual speech per therapist per
was rated and the reliability computed. sequence. The other added column refers to the
speech addressee, we noted to whom the thera-
Results pist(s) speak(s): (1–3) to one or another of the
three individual patients, or (4) to the group as
Adaptation to Group Setting of the a whole.
MOTR Scale Normally, one code is used for the verbal
aspects and one for the nonverbal aspects to rate
The adaptation of the MOTR rating scale to a a therapist’s degree of using the MOTR for each
group format yielded the following results. In patient. In the case of a group with two thera-
what follows, we will use the term of MOTR- pists, the authors chose to have one code for the
group to specify the group version. The actual two therapists concerning verbal aspects (taking
adaptation involved several operational steps. both therapist as “one” person) and have two
The original version of the MOTR coding differentiated codes for both therapists regard-
scheme has five columns, to which two columns ing nonverbal aspects of MOTR.
for the group version were added to retain ad- It might be counterintuitive why we chose a
ditional information. One column is used to single code for the verbal level for the two
record to whom the therapist is speaking (an therapists for each segment, whereas both ther-
individual patient or the group as a whole; all apists were rated separately on the nonverbal
segments where the two therapists were talking level. First, the nonverbal component of MOTR
among each other were excluded), and one col- has shown to relate to various aspects of pathol-
umn is used to record which therapist is speak- ogy and intervention outcome across studies,
ing during the sequence (a specific therapist or which was less the case for the verbal aspects of
both therapists together). The resulting seven MOTR (Caspar et al., 2005; Kramer et al.,
columns of the MOTR-group rating scheme are 2011). Second, for the context of group therapy,
the following: (a) the time of occurrence in the it seemed particularly promising to code the
session (sequence to be rated), (b) the initials of nonverbal aspects for each therapist for each
the therapists, (c) to whom the therapists is sequence. However, it did not seem useful to
speaking, (d) Plans activated in the patient, (e) code the strict verbal aspects in such a differ-
central Plans (acceptable Plans in the patient’s entiated way. The only situation that was ne-
Plan Analysis; as explained above), (f) a brief glected as a result of this method was one in
description of the behaviors (verbal and nonver- which both therapists spoke at the same time;
bal behavior of patients and therapists), and (g) this may have merited different verbal codes of
MOTR-scale, one subcolumn for the verbal MOTR. For this particular case, which was ex-
54 KELLER ET AL.

tremely rare in the dataset used, it was agreed ment on choice of Plans relevant for a specific
that therapist talking at the same time would be sequence (mean across three patients and two
coded as a nonverbal aspect of communication. therapists) was 65%; the Spearman rank corre-
To respect parsimony, only one Plan was lations (mean across three patients and two ther-
selected by patient and by sequence of time. apists) was 0.78 for verbal, 0.70 for nonverbal,
Given that there were three patients and two and 0.74 overall.
therapists, a maximum of six codes per se-
quence and for all patients was possible. This is Level of MOTR
a restriction with regard to the individual coding
of MOTR. The verbal and nonverbal average of MOTR
was obtained by adding each score and by di-
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

In some sequences, the therapists talked to


viding this sum by the number of codes in a
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

the group as a whole and only one patient re-


sponded, whereas the other two were silent. To session. The overall verbal score for all sessions
code such a therapist intervention in reference for all three patients and two therapists was 0.14
to an activated Plan in the patient (who remains (SD ⫽ 0.15) and the general nonverbal score
silent and, thus, does not give any indicator as to was 0.33 (SD ⫽ 0.20). Therapists did not differ
which Plan may be activated), a specific Plan, in their overall nonverbal score, since they re-
called a “lead-Plan,” was identified a priori for ceived a score of 0.32 (SD ⫽ 0.25) and 0.34
each patient. This concept can also be under- (SD ⫽ 0.20), respectively (see Table 3). The
stood as a “default Plan.” This lead-Plan is one verbal mean scores were 0.31 (SD ⫽ 0.23) for
that is used when no other Plan can be ad- Anna, 0.10 (SD ⫽ 0.15) for Betty and 0.05
dressed and when the rater assesses that the (SD ⫽ 0.27) for Gary (see Table 4). A between-
utilization of this “lead-Plan” is coherent. The patient t test (t(13) ⫽ .021, p ⫽ .05) showed a
lead-Plan was determined on the basis of one small, but significant difference between verbal
session in which all Plans were considered but interventions for Anna relative to Gary: on av-
only one Plan was selected as being the most erage, Anna received a little more complemen-
representative of the patient. In selecting this tary interventions than Gary. It may be interest-
lead-Plan, the following question was asked: “if ing to relate this result to the specific Plans in
we had to keep only one Plan of the full Plan both patients. Regarding the nonverbal level,
Analysis, which one would it be?” For Anna, the therapists had comparable scores relative to
the lead-Plan was “be normal”; for Betty “show each patient.
that you make an effort” and for Gary “avoid
showing your feelings.” Thus, acceptable lead- Discussion
Plans related to each were, respectively, “keep
up your image,” “show that you have the re- In the present study, a scale for measuring the
sources,” and “avoid being hurt.” By identify- relationship process in great details was adapted
ing these acceptable lead-Plans, the eventual for a group therapy setting. The MOTR scale
impact of a therapist’s motive-oriented behavior analyses verbal, non- and para-verbal levels of
on one specific patient, as the session unfolded, patient-focused idiosyncratic therapist interven-
was able to be measured, even if the latter tions.
remained silent in the first place.

Reliability Table 3
Mean, SD, and Range of Verbal and Nonverbal
Reliability was established for one (out of MOTR (Motive-Oriented Therapeutic
three; 33% reliability sample) Plan Analysis by Relationship) Group
two raters, as per the Caspar and colleagues’ Range
(2005) procedure, and was sufficient (60%).
Concerning the reliability of the MOTR-group Group Mean SD Min. Max.
scale, the Caspar and colleagues’ (2005) proce- Verbal .14 .15 ⫺.06 .48
dure was used. Acceptable reliability for the Nonverbal .33 .20 .00 .71
MOTR-group was found. The agreement on Nonverbal Therapist1 .32 .25 .00 .79
Nonverbal Therapist2 .34 .20 .00 .62
identification of sequences was 80%; the agree-
ADAPTATION OF MOTR TO GROUP THERAPY 55

Table 4 It was noted, as part of our results, that some


Mean and SD Per Session and Patient for the patients do not speak during a specific sequence,
Verbal Aspect of MOTR (Motive-Oriented which led to the introduction of the notion of a
Therapeutic Relationship) Group lead (or default) Plan for each patient, for coding
Anna Betty Gary Mean integrity. Even though only one patient was ad-
dressed by a particular therapist intervention, and
Session Mean SD Mean SD Mean SD Mean SD the other patients remained silent, it was necessary
Session 4 .60 .84 ⫺.21 .80 .00 .74 .08 .84 to have a patient Plan to consider, to which the
Session 5 .23 .44 .21 .80 ⫺.17 .58 .10 .64 therapist was actually addressing his or her inter-
Session 11 .22 .67 .11 .33 ⫺.22 .67 .04 .59 vention. This was based on the assumption that
Session 12 .33 .82 .20 .42 ⫺.13 .83 .08 .70
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

Session 13 .00 .45 .00 1.00 ⫺.10 .88 ⫺.06 .80


each intervention in a group has an impact on all
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

Session 14 .55 .69 .00 .89 .00 .47 .19 .74 patients, which may differ according to their indi-
Session 15 .30 .48 .00 .00 .30 .48 .21 .41 vidual Plan structure.
Session 16 ⫺.11 .78 .22 .83 .00 .53 .04 .72 Consider, for example, Session 14 at Minute
Session 17 .44 .53 .18 .40 .09 .54 .23 .50 19, when one of the patients speaks about a
Session 18 .50 .53 .27 .47 .70 .48 .48 .51 friend who is losing her brother to cancer. One
Total .31 .23 .10 .15 .05 .27 .14 .15
of the therapists responds, “We are all deeply
touched by what you are saying.” This quite
simple disclosure of affection may have influ-
Adapting the MOTR Scale to enced each patient in a different way. One may
Group Therapy feel sad, but understood by the therapist’s inter-
vention; one may feel empathy for the other
To take into account the complexity of a group patient, and also for the therapist as he or she
setting, the main changes to the MOTR scale are discloses affect; one may also be helped toward
actually very simple: we added two additional becoming potentially aware of all of his or her
columns on the coding sheet. In the case of several emotions. This therapist intervention could also
therapists, it is meaningful to (a) specify which encourage other patients—the “listeners”—to
therapist is speaking and (b) to whom the therapist open up more about other individual difficulties,
is addressing his or her message (i.e., the group or by the creation of an emotionally validating and
a particular patient). Our adaptation proposes that welcoming group atmosphere. This example
verbal aspects of the interventions made by more shows that even if the patient is silent in such a
than one therapist form a single code, whereas it group process, each therapist intervention needs
remains highly meaningful to measure moment- to be rated for each “listening” patient, as well.
by-moment differences between the two therapists The adaptation of this research tool opens the
for the nonverbal aspect of the interventions, de- field of possible components to examine in a
lineated therefore, in two separate codes. The con- group setting. It allows one to systematize the
cept of lead-Plan is created and integrated into the observation of certain elements, such as the
coding. Finally, we demonstrated that the reliabili- frequency of therapists’—patient-focused—
ties for all coding steps are satisfying. interventions compared with given sequences,
Concerning the adaptation of the actual coding, as well as to note which patient the therapist
it seems meaningful to add a column to specify addresses. It also allows one to observe the
which therapist is speaking and at which times. therapeutic relationship in a new way, radically
This may also help in performing more advanced taking into account the idiosyncrasy of the ther-
analyses on speech turns and duration of speech apeutic dialogue (Stiles, Honos-Webb, &
per therapist (Tikkanen & Leiman, 2014). Al- Surko, 1998), as applied to group processes.
though this was not the objective of the present
research, it would nonetheless be interesting to see Responsiveness in DBT Skills Training
whether this variable would have an impact on the
therapeutic process: is there a measurable differ- On a descriptive level, when applying this cod-
ence between sessions in which therapists are in- ing scheme to DBT skills training group, the lev-
volved unequally and those in which the therapists els of MOTR (both verbal and nonverbal) were
are involved similarly in terms of frequency of around 0, with a trend toward overall positive
verbal expression? MOTR. This means that, on average, DBT group
56 KELLER ET AL.

therapy does not seem to rely much on this par- nor limited, to DBT skills group, but to any group
ticular process variable, despite good outcome. therapy facing patients with BPD, and any patient
This is consistent with findings in another study population where the interpersonal problems
(Kramer, Kolly, et al., 2014) where the authors might impede on the therapeutic cooperation in
found similar levels of MOTR in a comparison the here and now of the group therapy.
group that included therapists who were not in-
tended to use MOTR, but who applied an effective Limitations and Perspectives
psychiatric treatment.
This result could be interpreted as DBT poten- The present research is important, as it widens
tially presenting with low levels of MOTR as a the scope of study and ultimately allows for a
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

feature of responsive therapy cooperation and re- greater understanding of the therapeutic process in
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

lationship: we argue that average session-levels of therapy groups. However, it has a number of lim-
MOTR close to zero hide a great variability in the itations, among others, the small number of pa-
use of MOTR, in the present case varying between tients and therapists included, the fact that the
⫺2 and 2. An increased level of MOTR at only results are based on only one type of group, which
one point in therapy might be sufficient for a cannot be generalized to all types of groups. Fur-
particular patient, as it is understood that such thermore, we only analyzed 10 sessions. How-
interventions have a particularly precious subjec- ever, the methodological adaptation described
tive value for that individual patient. The average here may be applied to all group contexts. It would
level of MOTR might, therefore, not indicate the be interesting to use this adapted process measure
actual quality of the therapist responsiveness to to less structured group therapy formats, to exam-
the patient or to the patient group, but the peak ine therapist responsiveness in these therapies.
may. Individual noncomplementary interventions, This type of therapeutic relationship can con-
in the form of specific therapist confrontations, for ceptualize the patient’s problems without relying
example, might also be beneficial if conducted on a therapeutic approach, which allows for an
within a complementary relationship context. The agreement between therapist approaches, and
latter explanation particularly applies to our con- therefore, remains close to the patient’s behavior.
text, as the relationship theory of the DBT inter- Now that this study has demonstrated that the
vention proposes to balance out challenging with MOTR scale is indeed applicable in a group set-
acceptance interventions in the relationship be- ting, it seems propitious to test the application of
tween the therapist(s) and each participant en- this type of relationship with research in which
rolled in the skills group training (Linehan, therapists apply this kind of individualized case
1993a). To test these assumptions on a microbasis, formulation and intervention facing several pa-
the in-session fluctuation (i.e., the therapist’s adap- tients at the same time in a group setting. This is
tation to the patient’s Plans) of the responsive inter- a particularly promising area of development of
vention—or its in-session peak (i.e., here 2)—may integrative clinical models, as we know now that
be linked with session outcome in DBT skills. the adding of the motive-oriented therapeutic re-
Alternatively, a stimulating integrative fol- lationship, based on the Plan Analysis, in individ-
low-up question may be the added value of the use ual therapy has small to medium short-term ef-
by the therapists of MOTR in DBT skills groups. fects (Kramer et al., 2014) on symptom change,
Would the process and outcome of the skills train- over a generalized psychiatric treatment, for pa-
ing be enhanced when the therapists is trained in tients with borderline personality disorder.
formulating a case according to the Plan Analysis
and when he applies the motive-oriented therapeu-
tic relationship? Independent of the answer to this References
important question, we would say that there might
always be difficult interactional situations facing American Psychiatric Association. (2000). Diagnos-
tic and statistical manual of mental disorders,
patients with BPD in the DBT group therapy, fourth edition text revision. Washington, DC:
therefore, a fine-grained patient-focused under- American Psychiatric Association.
standing of the underlying interpersonal stakes Berthoud, L., Kramer, U., de Roten, Y., Despland,
(i.e., the motives) might be beneficial. As such, we J.-N., & Caspar, F. (2013). Using plan analysis in
would hypothesize that these assumed beneficial psychotherapeutic case formulation of borderline
effects of MOTR would not necessarily be related, personality disorder. Clinical Psychology & Psy-
ADAPTATION OF MOTR TO GROUP THERAPY 57

chotherapy, 20, 1–9. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/cpp tal Disease, 181, 189 –193. http://dx.doi.org/10
.784 .1097/00005053-199303000-00007
Bohus, M., Kleindienst, N., Limberger, M. F., Stieg- Grawe, K. (1980). Die diagnostisch-therapeutische
litz, R. D., Domsalla, M., Chapman, A. L., . . . funktion der gruppeninteraktion in verhalten-
Wolf, M. (2009). The short version of the Border- stherapeutischen gruppen. [The diagnostic-
line Symptom List (BSL-23): Development and therapeutic function of group interaction in behav-
initial data on psychometric properties. Psychopa- ioral treatment groups] In K. Grawe (Ed.),
thology, 42, 32–39. http://dx.doi.org/10.1159/ Verhaltenstherapie in gruppen (pp. 88 –223).
000173701 München, Germany: Urban & Schwarzenberg.
Burlingame, G. M., Fuhriman, A., & Johnson, J. Grawe, K. (1992). Schema theory and heuristic psy-
(2002). Cohesion in group psychotherapy. In J. C. chotherapy. Research report N°1–1992. Bern,
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

Norcross (Ed.), Psychotherapy relationships that Switzerland: Psychology Institute of Bern Univer-
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

work (pp. 71– 87). New York, NY: Oxford Uni- sity.
versity Press. Grawe, K. (2004). Psychological therapy. Seattle,
Caspar, F. (2007). Plan analysis. In T. D. Eells (Ed.), Washington: Hogrefe & Huber Publishers.
Handbook of psychotherapy case formulations Horvath, A. O., & Greenberg, L. S. (1989). Devel-
(2nd ed., pp. 221–289). New York, NY: Guilford opment and validation of the Working Alliance
Press. Inventory. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 36,
Caspar, F., & Grosse-Holtforth, M. (2009). Respon- 223–233. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-0167.36
siveness—Eine entscheidene Prozessvariable in .2.223
der Psychotherapie [Responsiveness—A pivotal Kashner, T. M., Rost, K., Cohen, B., Anderson,
process variable in psychotherapy]. Zeitschrift für M., & Smith, G. R., Jr. (1995). Enhancing the
Klinische Psychologie und Psychotherapie: For- health of somatization disorder patients. Effec-
schung und Praxis, 38, 61– 69. http://dx.doi.org/10 tiveness of short-term group therapy. Psychoso-
.1026/1616-3443.38.1.61 matics, 36, 462– 470. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/
Caspar, F., Grossmann, C., Unmüssig, C., & Sch-
S0033-3182(95)71627-9
ramm, E. (2005). Complementary therapeutic re-
Kramer, U., Berthoud, L., Keller, S., & Caspar, F.
lationship: Therapist behavior, interpersonal pat-
(2014). Motive-oriented psychotherapeutic rela-
terns, and therapeutic effects. Psychotherapy
tionship facing a patient presenting with narcissis-
Research, 15, 91–102. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/
tic personality disorder: A case study. Journal of
10503300512331327074
Contemporary Psychotherapy, 44, 71– 82. http://
Caspar, F., Pessier, J., Stuart, J., Safran, J., Wallner
Samstag, L., & Guiguis, M. (2000). One step fur- dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10879-013-9249-5
ther in assessing how interpretations influence the Kramer, U., Kolly, S., Berthoud, L., Keller, S., Pre-
process of psychotherapy. Psychotherapy Re- isig, M., Caspar, F., . . . Despland, J.-N. (2014).
search, 10, 309 –320. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/ Effects of motive-oriented therapeutic relationship
713663734 in a ten-session general psychiatric treatment of
Corbière, M., Bisson, J., Lauzon, S., & Ricard, N. borderline personality disorder: A randomized
(2006). Factorial validation of a French short-form controlled trial. Psychotherapy and Psychosomat-
of the Working Alliance Inventory. International ics, 83, 176 –186. http://dx.doi.org/10.1159/
Journal of Methods in Psychiatric Research, 15, 000358528
36 – 45. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/mpr.27 Kramer, U., Pascual-Leone, A., Berthoud, L., de Ro-
Decker, S. E., & Naugle, A. E. (2008). DBT for ten, Y., Marquet, P., Kolly, S., . . . Page, D. (2016).
sexual abuse survivors: Current status and future Assertive anger mediates effects of Dialectical be-
directions. Journal of behavior Analysis of Of- havior skills training in borderline personality dis-
fender and Victim: Treatment and Prevention, 1, order: A randomized controlled trial. Clinical Psy-
52– 69. chology & Psychotherapy, 23, 189 –202. http://dx
Emond, C., Savard, K., Lalande, G., Boisvert, N., .doi.org/10.1002/cpp.1956
Boutin, M., & Simard, V. (2004). Propriétés psy- Kramer, U., Rosciano, A., Pavlovic, M., Berthoud,
chométriques de la mesure d’impact (MI-45), ver- L., Despland, J.-N., de Roten, Y., & Caspar, F.
sion francophone du outcome questionnaire-45 (2011). Motive-oriented therapeutic relationship in
(OQ-45.2) [Psychometric Characteristics of the brief psychodynamic intervention for patients with
OQ-45, French Version]. Presentation at the AC- depression and personality disorders. Journal of
FAS Conference, Montréal, Québec, Canada. Clinical Psychology, 67, 1017–1027. http://dx.doi
Fals-Stewart, W., Marks, A. P., & Schafer, J. (1993). .org/10.1002/jclp.20806
A comparison of behavioral group therapy and Kramer, U., & Stiles, W. B. (2015). The responsive-
individual behavior therapy in treating obsessive- ness problem in psychotherapy: A review of pro-
compulsive disorder. Journal of Nervous and Men- posed solutions. Clinical Psychology: Science and
58 KELLER ET AL.

Practice, 22, 277–295. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/ versity Press. http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/acprof:oso/


cpsp.12107 9780199737208.001.0001
Lambert, M., Burlingame, G. M., Umphress, V., Page, D. (2010). Manuel d’animation du groupe de
Hansen, N. B., Vermeersch, D. A., Clouse, G. C., gestion des émotions, séance après séance [Ani-
& Yanchar, S. C. (1996). The reliability and va- mation’s manual of managing emotion group, ses-
lidity of the outcome questionnaire. Clinical Psy- sion after session]. Unpublished document.
chology & Psychotherapy, 3, 249 –258. http://dx Page, D., & Kramer, U. (2012). Groupe thérapeu-
.doi.org/10.1002/(SICI)1099-0879(199612)3: tique de gestion des émotions: Un territoire
4⬍249::AID-CPP106⬎3.0.CO;2-S d’acquisition des compétences chez les patients
Levine, J. M., & Hogg, M. A. (2010). Therapy avec trouble de la personnalité borderline [Group
groups. Encyclopedia of Group Processes & In- therapy for emotion skills training: An area of
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

tergroup Relations (Vol. 2, pp. 922–924). Thou- skills development in patients with borderline per-
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

sand Oaks, California: SAGE Reference. http://dx sonality disorder]. Psychiatrie française, 42, 142–
.doi.org/10.4135/9781412972017 161.
Linehan, M. M. (1993a). Cognitive-behavioral treat- Piper, W. E., Marrache, M., Lacroix, R., Richardsen,
ment of borderline personality disorder. New A. M., & Jones, B. D. (1983). Cohesion as a basic
York, NY: Guilford Press. bond in groups. Human Relations, 36, 93–108.
Linehan, M. M. (1993b). Skills training manual for http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/001872678303600201
treating borderline personality disorder. New Schmutz, I. (2012). Motivorientierte Beziehungsge-
York, NY: Guilford Press. staltung: Zusammenhang mit dem Therapieerfolg
Linehan, M. M., Armstrong, H. E., Suarez, A., und differenzielle Wirkung nach interpersonalen,
Allmon, D., & Heard, H. L. (1991). Cognitive- störungsdiagnostischen und geschlechtsspezifis-
behavioral treatment of chronically parasuicidal chen Merkmalen [Motive-oriented therapeutic re-
borderline patients. Archives of General Psychia- lationship: Relation to treatment outcome and dif-
try, 48, 1060 –1064. http://dx.doi.org/10.1001/ ferential effects by interpersonal, diagnostic and
archpsyc.1991.01810360024003 gender characteristics]. Unpublished thesis. Bern,
Linehan, M. M., Comtois, K. A., Murray, A. M., Switzerland.
Brown, M. Z., Gallop, R. J., Heard, H. L., . . . Stiles, W. B., Honos-Webb, L., & Surko, M. (1998).
Lindenboim, N. (2006). Two-year randomized Responsiveness in psychotherapy. Clinical Psy-
controlled trial and follow-up of dialectical behav- chology: Science and Practice, 5, 439 – 458. http://
ior therapy vs therapy by experts for suicidal be- dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1468-2850.1998.tb00166.x
haviors and borderline personality disorder. Ar- Tikkanen, S., & Leiman, M. (2014). Resolution of an
chives of General Psychiatry, 63, 757–766. http:// impasse at a network meeting: Dialogical sequence
dx.doi.org/10.1001/archpsyc.63.7.757 analysis of the use of a shared formulation. Coun-
Linehan, M. M., Heard, H. L., & Armstrong, H. E. selling Psychology Quarterly, 27, 154 –173. http://
(1993). Naturalistic follow-up of a behavioral dx.doi.org/10.1080/09515070.2013.873859
treatment for chronically parasuicidal borderline Toren, Z., & Shechtman, Z. (2010). Association of
patients. Archives of General Psychiatry, 50, 971– personal, process, and outcome variables in group
974. http://dx.doi.org/10.1001/archpsyc.1993 counselling: Testing an exploratory model. Group
.01820240055007 Dynamics: Theory, Research, and Practice, 14,
McMain, S. F., Boritz, T. Z., & Leybman, M. J. 292–303. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0018872
(2015). Common strategies for cultivating a posi- Weiss, R. D., Griffin, M. L., Greenfield, S. F., Na-
tive therapy relationship in the treatment of bor- javits, L. M., Wyner, D., Soto, J. A., & Hennen,
derline personality disorder. Journal of Psycho- J. A. (2000). Group therapy for patients with bi-
therapy Integration, 25, 20 –29. http://dx.doi.org/ polar disorder and substance dependence: Results
10.1037/a0038768 of a pilot study. The Journal of Clinical Psychia-
Miller, G. A., Galanter, E., & Pribram, K. H. (1960). try, 61, 361–367. http://dx.doi.org/10.4088/JCP
Plans and the structure of behavior. New York, .v61n0507
NY: Holt. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/10039-000
Norcross, J. (2011). Cohesion in group therapy. In
J. Norcross (Ed.), Psychotherapy relationships Received August 4, 2015
that work: Evidence-based responsiveness (2nd Revision received September 8, 2016
ed., pp. 110 –131). New York, NY: Oxford Uni- Accepted September 9, 2016 䡲

You might also like