You are on page 1of 10

European Journal of Work and Organizational Psychology

ISSN: (Print) (Online) Journal homepage: https://www.tandfonline.com/loi/pewo20

Applicants’ pre-test reactions towards video


interviews: the role of expected chances to
demonstrate potential and to use nonverbal cues

Karin Proost, Filip Germeys & Arne Vanderstukken

To cite this article: Karin Proost, Filip Germeys & Arne Vanderstukken (2021) Applicants’ pre-
test reactions towards video interviews: the role of expected chances to demonstrate potential
and to use nonverbal cues, European Journal of Work and Organizational Psychology, 30:2,
265-273, DOI: 10.1080/1359432X.2020.1817975

To link to this article: https://doi.org/10.1080/1359432X.2020.1817975

© 2020 The Author(s). Published by Informa


UK Limited, trading as Taylor & Francis
Group.

Published online: 07 Sep 2020.

Submit your article to this journal

Article views: 4175

View related articles

View Crossmark data

Full Terms & Conditions of access and use can be found at


https://www.tandfonline.com/action/journalInformation?journalCode=pewo20
EUROPEAN JOURNAL OF WORK AND ORGANIZATIONAL PSYCHOLOGY
2021, VOL. 30, NO. 2, 265–273
https://doi.org/10.1080/1359432X.2020.1817975

Applicants’ pre-test reactions towards video interviews: the role of expected chances
to demonstrate potential and to use nonverbal cues
Karin Proosta,b, Filip Germeysb and Arne Vanderstukkenc
a
Faculty of Psychology, Open University of the Netherlands, Heerlen, The Netherlands; bFaculty of Economics and Business, KU Leuven, Leuven,
Belgium; cFaculty of Management, Open University of the Netherlands, Heerlen, The Netherlands

ABSTRACT ARTICLE HISTORY


Technological innovations, among which the use of video interviews in personnel selection, are wel­ Received 1 July 2019
comed by organizations for reasons such as reductions in cost and time and the ability to reach a more Accepted 28 August 2020
global labour market. The literature to date suggests that applicants do not share this enthusiasm and KEYWORDS
feel less attracted towards organizations that use video interviews versus face-to-face interviews. In this Selection expectations; video
study, we extended the literature by studying pre- instead of post-test reactions and testing two interview; impression
explanatory mechanisms, namely expected chances to demonstrate potential and to use nonverbal management; organizational
cues. In a first within-subjects experiment among 38 students, the results of two t-tests showed that attraction; recruitment
participants preferred the face-to-face interview above the video interview and that they expected lower
chances to demonstrate potential and to use nonverbal cues in the video interview. In a second between-
subjects experiment among 121 potential job seekers, these results were partially replicated. The results
of a t-test and a mediation bootstrap analysis with PROCESS showed a significant indirect effect of
interview medium on organizational attraction via expected chances to demonstrate potential but not
via expected opportunities to use nonverbal communication. This study has important practical implica­
tions for organizations in the current context of talent shortages.

The personnel selection interview is one of the most popular recommendation and litigation intentions (Geenen et al.,
methods for selecting the right applicant for the job (Macan, 2012) and organizational attraction (Schreurs et al., 2009;
2009), for several reasons. First, the interview, if correctly struc­ Wanous et al., 1983). Moreover, pre-test expectations have
tured, is quite accurate in predicting the future performance of been shown to be key in understanding post-test reactions
applicants (Schmidt & Hunter, 1998). Second, applicants tend to (Bell et al., 2004). An important contribution of this study to
favour the selection interview above other methods such as the literature is thus that we investigate whether the use of
personality tests and cognitive ability tests (Hausknecht et al., video interviews, as part of a selection procedure, influences
2004). applicants’ reactions as measured prior to taking the interview.
The interview has known several trends over its many years Second, the literature to date is limited in the sense that it
of use, among which the introduction of videoconferencing suggests that applicants react more negatively towards video
software to meet with potential employees (Lievens et al., interviews than face-to-face interviews but fails to answer the
2002; Sears et al., 2013). This technological innovation is “Why” (Anderson, 2003). Chapman et al. (2003) and later
embraced by organizations for reasons such as cost and time Blacksmith et al. (2016) suggested that video interviews may
reduction, ability to reach a more global labour market (Bauer make it more difficult for applicants to create a favourable
et al., 2004), and keeping the procedure more green (Andrews impression but did not empirically test this idea. Other studies
et al., 2013). tested mechanisms such as procedural fairness and interviewer
The literature, however, shows that applicants do not share characteristics (see, e.g., Sears et al., 2013). In this study, we
this enthusiasm (see Blacksmith et al., 2016, for a meta-analysis) acknowledge the fact that communication occurs through two
. For example, Horn and Behrend (2017) showed that 89% of important channels, namely both verbal and nonverbal and
applicants preferred face-to-face interviews while only 9% pre­ that both channels may on the one hand be hampered by the
ferred video interviews. The current literature on reactions use of video’s in hiring interviews and on the other hand may
towards the use of technology in selection procedures, how­ influence interview performance (Burnett & Motowidlo, 1998).
ever, focuses on post-test reactions (e.g., Kroeck & Magnusen, Specifically and based on media richness theory (Daft & Lengel,
1997). Although this has its added value, it overlooks a second 1986), we suggest that applicants will expect lower chances to
crucial type of response towards a selection procedure, i.e., pre- demonstrate potential and to communicate through nonverbal
test reactions. Prior to applying and test-taking, applicants signals in video interviews versus face-to-face interviews. We
typically receive information about the selection procedure further expect that this will lead to a lower attractiveness of
that moulds their expectations. These expectations have been organizations that use video interviews versus face-to-face
shown to influence pre-test motivation (Bell et al., 2006), interviews. We empirically test this idea in two scenario studies,

CONTACT Karin Proost karin.proost@ou.nl


© 2020 The Author(s). Published by Informa UK Limited, trading as Taylor & Francis Group.
This is an Open Access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial-NoDerivatives License (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-nd/4.0/),
which permits non-commercial re-use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited, and is not altered, transformed, or built upon in any way.
266 K. PROOST ET AL.

manipulating whether applicants apply for a company that 1986, p. 560) and depends on the availability of immediate
uses video versus face-to-face interviews. feedback (i.e., possibility to ask questions and to make correc­
tions), multiple cues (i.e., number of social cues available),
language variety (i.e., the range of meaning that can be con­
From post-test to pre-test reactions towards video
veyed), and personal focus (i.e., personal feelings and emotions
interviews
can be expressed, Daft et al., 1987). According to Media
We know from the literature that, although applicants, in general, Richness Theory, despite giant leaps in the development of
react positively to the use of technology in personnel selection, video conferencing technologies, video conferencing is still
they are more hesitant with respect to video interviews (see behind on face-to-face interactions in terms of media richness
Blacksmith et al., 2016 for a meta-analysis). For example, in a semi- (Dennis & Kinney, 1998; Ishii et al., 2019). For example, in video
experimental field study, Kroeck and Magnusen (1997) showed conferencing, verbal communication may be hindered by time
a weak preference for face-to-face interviews versus video inter­ lags and other connection failures leading to less fluent com­
views, although this preference was stronger for recruiters than for munication exchanges, which may prevent fluent turn taking
applicants. Later studies showed more pronounced negative reac­ between communicating parties (Powers et al., 2011; Wegge,
tions of applicants. For example, Straus et al. (2001) showed that 2006). Also, nonverbal communication may be hindered in
students reacted more negatively to previously conducted mock different ways. For example, it may be more difficult to keep
selection interviews via video versus face-to-face in terms of inter­ good eye contact, especially since that would mean that both
viewer likeability, ease of regulating and understanding the con­ parties would have to look directly in the camera, mostly posi­
versation, and feeling comfortable during the interview. Also, tioned on top of the computer screen (see Bohannon et al.,
Chapman et al. (2003) showed in a field study among students 2013 for a review). Also, the opportunity to communicate via
who participated in a campus recruitment procedure, that appli­ body language and gestures (e.g., hand gestures that put
cants perceived the interviewer, the interview procedure, and the emphasis on important parts of the story) is restricted as people
interview medium as less procedurally fair when the interview was are typically displayed from their mid-chest up (Chapman &
conducted via video versus face-to-face. Finally, Sears et al. (2013) Rowe, 2002). Barry and Fulmer (2004) speak of a lower “social
showed that applicants rated the video interview lower on proce­ bandwidth”, as the video interview may limit the exchange of
dural fairness compared to the face-to-face interview on dimen­ social (i.e., verbal, nonverbal and paralinguistic) cues.
sions such as chance to perform, offering selection information Media Richness Theory further posits that the extent to
and job-relatedness, as well as on interviewer characteristics such which these limitations hinder social exchanges and interpre­
as personableness, trustworthiness, competence and overall tations depends on the goal of the communication (Daft et al.,
appearance. 1987). When the goal of the communication is to exchange
These studies measured the outcomes under study after factual information, lean communication media such as email
conducting a (mock) interview. In this study, we direct our and shared online file storage are effective. When, on the
attention to pre-test reactions or expectations, which can be contrary, communication is meant to reduce equivocality or
defined as “probabilistic beliefs about the characteristics of the ambiguity, a rich communication medium is important. This is
forthcoming selection procedure” (Schreurs et al., 2008, p. 170). particularly so for the hiring interview, which can be defined
As described by Derous et al. (2004), pre-test reactions may be as a social exchange process: While the interviewer tries to sell
the “stepping-stones of future behavior” (p. 100), and thus may the organization and thus to attract the most qualified candi­
offer valuable insights into applicants’ post-test reactions (see date (Wilhelmy et al., 2016), the applicant tries to convince the
also Howardson & Behrend, 2014). recruiter that he/she is the most qualified candidate for the
Before participating in the selection procedure, applicants job (Gilmore & Ferris, 1989; Proost et al., 2010). In this context,
form expectations about it, based on the information they both verbal and nonverbal communication plays a dominant
receive about the selection procedure. For example, organiza­ role (Peeters & Lievens, 2006) and applicants may fear that this
tions may inform applicants about the tests (and the technol­ is hindered in the video interview. For example, the technical
ogy) that will be used in the selection procedure, motivated by difficulties and lack of nonverbal cues may make it more
procedural fairness concerns (Bauer et al., 1998; Gilliland, 1995). difficult to get a sense of what the interviewer is thinking
Based on media richness theory (Daft & Lengel, 1986), we expect and thus adequately judge how their performance is evalu­
that informing applicants that the interview will be held via ated, which consequently hinders the choice for the most
video may elicit more negative pre-test reactions compared to effective way to create a desired impression (see DeGroot &
face-to-face interviews. We assume this because applicants may Gooty, 2009; Doherty-Sneddon et al., 1997). Also, restrictions
expect that video interviews may offer fewer opportunities to in terms of use of smiles, nods, hand gestures and body
demonstrate potential as well as to use nonverbal communica­ movements can lead to lower performance as they make it
tion compared to face-to-face interviews. This pre-test expecta­ more difficult to impress the recruiter (Peeters & Lievens,
tion may, in turn, lead to lower organizational attraction. 2006) and to reach a shared understanding about the appli­
cant’s performance (Chapman & Rowe, 2002). Finally, the lack
of eye contact has been shown to lead to lower trust between
Media richness theory and chances for (non)verbal
the parties (Bekkering, 2004) as well as lower likeability and
communication
lower ratings on intelligence (Fullwood, 2007), aspects which
Media richness is defined as “the ability of information to have an important influence on interview outcomes (Fox &
change understanding within a time interval” (Daft & Lengel, Spector, 2000).
EUROPEAN JOURNAL OF WORK AND ORGANIZATIONAL PSYCHOLOGY 267

When a medium is used that hinders this verbal and non­ 18.9% had already participated in a employment interview via
verbal communication, it may be expected that applicants react videoconferencing software.
less positively. For example, Schreurs et al. (2009) showed that After being informed about the purpose of the study (“short
organizations are perceived as more attractive when applicants study into the use of technology in the selection interview and
expect higher chances to show their potential. The same may applicants’ reactions towards interviews via Skype”) and com­
apply to nonverbal communication. Based on this literature, we pleting the informed consent, participants were asked to ima­
formulate the following hypotheses: gine that they had applied for a job, passed the first screening
and were kindly invited to participate in an employment inter­
Hypothesis 1: Applicants are more attracted to organizations view. On the next page, the same message appeared, for
conducting face-to-face interviews than to organizations con­ a different company. One of the invitations (randomized across
ducting video interviews. questionnaires) specified that the interview would take place
face-to-face. The other invitation indicated that the interview
Hypothesis 2: Applicants expect more chances to demonstrate would take place via Skype (i.e., the video interview). Other
potential (H2a) and to use nonverbal cues (H2b) in face-to-face details such as the date of the interview and location of the
interviews, compared to video interviews. company were kept similar (although not identical, as to safe­
guard the realism of the vignettes). Finally, all participants com­
Hypothesis 3: Organizational attraction will be lower for video pleted a questionnaire on chance to demonstrate potential and
interviews versus face-to-face interviews because applicants use nonverbal cues, attractiveness of the organization and some
expect lower chances to demonstrate potential (H3a), and to demographic variables and were thanked for their participation.
use nonverbal cues (H3b) in video interviews compared to face-
to-face interviews.
Measures
Chance to demonstrate potential
Pre-Test expectations about chances to demonstrate potential
Overview of the studies were assessed with five items taken from Schreurs et al. (2008).
We conducted two experimental studies to test the hypoth­ A sample item is: “During this interview, I expect to get suffi­
eses. In the first study, in line with the study of Kroeck and cient opportunity to show what I’m capable of”. Items were
Magnusen (1997), we asked applicants to directly contrast the answered on a five-point scale (1 = a lot more for face-to-face
face-to-face interview with the video interview in a within- interviews, 5 = a lot more for video interviews). Internal consis­
subjects experimental design. We assessed pre-test expecta­ tency of the scale was.84.
tions about chances to demonstrate potential and to use non­
verbal cues as well as organizational attraction. In the second The use of nonverbal cues
study, we followed a between-subjects experimental design Based on the literature, we developed three items to measure
where each participant was assigned to only one condition pre-test expectations about the use of nonverbal cues (i.e.,
(i.e., face-to-face interview versus video). Only the latter design “During this interview, I expect that I will be able to make eye
allowed a direct test of the proposed mediations (hypothesis 3). contact with the interviewer/to smile to the interviewer/to use
We chose to combine these two types of experiments as hand gestures”), using the same five-point scale. Internal con­
they each have their own strengths and weaknesses (see sistency of this scale was .82.
Charness et al., 2012). For example, a within-subjects design is
prone to demand effects (i.e., the participants interpret the Organizational attraction
intentions of the experimenter and answer accordingly) but Organizational attraction was assessed with seven items,
offers more statistical power. On the contrary, a between- adopted from Highhouse et al. (2003). Three items measured
subjects design offers no answering anchors but is more con­ general attractiveness (e.g., “A job at this organization is very
servative in nature. By combining both experiments, weak­ appealing to me.”) and four items measured pursuit intentions
nesses can be compensated and strengths can be reinforced. (e.g., “I would make this organization one of my first choices as
an employer.”). All items were measured on a five-point Likert
scale (1 = strongly disagree, 5 = strongly agree). These items
Study 1 were presented twice: once about the organization using the
face-to-face interview (α = .92) and once about the organization
Sample and procedure
using the video interview (α = .85).
Study 1 was a within-subjects, vignette study on paper, distrib­
uted among 40 Business Administration students. Two stu­
Analyses
dents did not answer all the questions for the focal variables
and were thus not included in the analyses, resulting in a final Because the measure of pre-test expectations was a relative
sample of 38 students. one in this study, we conducted a one-sample t-test to study
Seventy-eight per cent of the participants were women and whether applicants perceived significantly more opportunities
the participants were on average 22.27 years (SDage = 1.26). to demonstrate potential and to use nonverbal cues in a face-to
51.3% had participated in an employment interview, 94.6% had -face interview (i.e., when the mean is significantly lower than 3,
experience using videoconferencing software (i.e., Skype), and the midpoint of the scale) or significantly more opportunities in
268 K. PROOST ET AL.

a video interview (i.e., when the mean is significantly higher could not be tested in this study. Therefore, we set up a second
than 3). We used a paired-samples t-test to study the effect of study that allowed to test hypothesis 3, following a between-
interview medium on organizational attraction. subjects design.

Results Study 2
Descriptive statistics and intercorrelations Sample and procedure
Table 1 shows the descriptive statistics and correlations between Study 2 was an online between-subjects vignette study. An invita­
the variables in this study. Only the correlation between expecta­ tion was sent to 212 potential job seekers who belonged to the
tions about chances to demonstrate potential and to use non­ (social) network of the researcher. One hundred and twenty-one
verbal cues was significantly positive. participants decided to partake in the study (58% response rate).
Assuming a medium effect size of .50 (based on the meta-analysis
Hypotheses testing of Blacksmith et al., 2016 and the first study, that even showed
The results supported hypotheses 1, suggesting an effect of large effect sizes) and an alpha of .05, the achieved power of this
interview medium on organizational attraction. Specifically, the sample is .82 for a directional hypothesis and .73 for a non-
results showed that applicants found organizations using face- directional hypothesis (Gpower). One hundred and fourteen par­
to-face interviews significantly more attractive compared to ticipants completed the sociodemographic questionnaire at the
organizations using video interviews (MFTF = 3.73, MVideo = 2.95, end of the survey (58% women, Mage = 32.41, SDage = 11.57). The
MDiff = 0.79, SD = 0.64, t(37) = 7.56, p < .01, d = 1.22). majority of participants were employed (69%), the others were
H2 suggested that applicants’ expectations about the chance either student (29%) or fulltime job seeker (3%). Of the students,
to demonstrate potential H2a) and to use nonverbal cues (H2b) 47% was seeking a job and applied to one or more organizations.
would be lower for video interviews compared to face-to-face Most participants knew Skype (99%) and had used it before to
interviews. The results indeed showed that applicants expected make calls (77%) or to organize a video chat (66%). Only one
a significantly higher chance to demonstrate potential in face-to- participant had used Skype to participate in a selection interview.
face interviews vs. the video interview (as they scored signifi­ This study was similar to Study 1 but featured a between-
cantly lower than 3, the midpoint of the scale; M = 1.91, SD = .51, t subjects design with two conditions (i.e., the face-to-face condi­
(37) = 13.20, p < .01, d = 2.14), which supports hypothesis 2a. tion and the video condition) to which the participants were
Second and in line with hypothesis 2b, more nonverbal commu­ randomly assigned. Specifically, participants were asked to pre­
nication opportunities were expected in the face-to-face inter­ tend that they had applied for multiple jobs a couple of weeks
view vs. the video interview (again scoring significantly lower earlier, but were then presented with only one of the emails (i.e.,
than 3; M = 1.83, SD = .75, t(37) = 9.58, p < .01, d = 1.56). either the face-to-face interview email or the video interview
email). As a conservative test of the effect, we informed the
participants in this study that the interview (either face-to-face or
Discussion video) would take place in the offices of the potential employer, in
The results of the first study supported the first two hypotheses order to exclude the alternative explanation that the face-to-face
and showed that applicants prefer face-to-face interviews over interview was favoured because of personal contact with the
video interviews and expect lower chances to demonstrate recruiter and first acquaintance with the company. To justify the
potential and to use nonverbal cues in video interviews versus request for the video interview to be held at the office, the emails
face-to-face interviews. A post hoc power analysis (Gpower) mentioned that participants had to be physically present to com­
showed that this study had an adequate power of 1.00 to con­ plete psychological tests. Other details were kept constant (e.g.,
duct a one-sample t-test, with alpha set to .05 and an effect size date of the interview). After reading the email, all participants
of 1.22 (set to the lowest value that was found in this study, see completed a questionnaire on chances to demonstrate potential
H1). Although this study focused on pre-test expectations, the and use nonverbal cues as well as organizational attraction.
lower attractiveness of organizations that use video interviews is Although participants were randomized across conditions,
completely in line with studies on post-test perceptions about we checked whether the two groups were similar in terms of
video versus face-to-face interviews (Chapman et al., 2003; Sears demographic variables (i.e., age and gender) and experience
et al., 2013; Straus et al., 2001). This study adds to this literature with skype. The results of a chi-square test did not show
by focusing on pre-test expectations and by testing two possible a significant relationship between condition and gender
explanations. Whether these expectations really mediate (0 = male, 1 = female), χ2 = 1.88, p = .17, nor between condition
between the interview medium and organizational attraction, and experience (0 = no, 1 = yes), χ2 = 1.66, p = .20. An inde­
pendent sample t-test also did not show a significant difference
in mean age between the two groups, t(111) = .35, p = .72.
Table 1. Descriptives and intercorrelations of study 1.
M SD 2 3 4
1. Chances to demonstrate potential 1.91 .51 .39* −.25 .27 Measures
2. Use of nonverbal cues 1.83 .75 −.28+ −.05
3. Organizational attraction ftf 3.73 .41 .30+ Chance to demonstrate potential
4 Organizational attraction video 2.95 .63 Pre-Test expectations about chances to demonstrate potential
ftf = face-to-face; +: p < .10; *: p < .05. were assessed with the same five items as in study 1. Instead of
EUROPEAN JOURNAL OF WORK AND ORGANIZATIONAL PSYCHOLOGY 269

making a direct comparison as in study 1, participants were = 3.32, SD = .96, t(112) = .87, p = .39, d = .16). Hypothesis 1 was
asked to answer on a 5-point Likert-type scale (1 = strongly not supported by the data.
disagree; 5 = strongly agree). Cronbach’s alpha of this scale Second and line with H2a, the results showed that appli­
was .85. cants in the face-to-face interview condition expected signifi­
cantly more chances to demonstrate potential compared to
The use of nonverbal cues applicants in the video interview condition (MFTF = 3.73,
Expectations about the use of nonverbal cues were also SD = .69, MVideo = 3.43, SD = .82, t(112) = 2.11, p = .04,
assessed in the same way but with one additional item, i.e., d = .40). However, we did not find any significant differences
“During this interview, I expect that I will be able to nod my between applicants in the face-to-face interview condition and
head to show agreement with the opinion of the interviewer”. applicants in the video interview condition regarding expecta­
Internal consistency of this four-item scale was .73. All items tions about nonverbal communication (MFTF = 3.71, SD = .66,
were rated on a Likert scale (1 = strongly disagree, 5 = strongly MVideo = 3.73, SD = .78, t(112) = −.14, p = .89, d = .03). In other
agree). words, the data did not support hypotheses 2b.
Finally, the results of the PROCESS analysis showed
a significant indirect effect of interview medium on organiza­
Organizational attraction
tional attraction via expectations about chances to demonstrate
Organizational attraction was measured with a more elaborate
potential (B = −.05, SE = .03, 95% CI: −.13 – −.01). This supports
scale of ten items, with five items measuring general attractive­
H3a by showing that participants in the video interview condi­
ness and five items measuring pursuit intentions (Highhouse
tion found the organization less attractive compared with parti­
et al., 2003). All items were scored on a Likert-scale from 1 (=
cipants in the face-to-face interview condition because they saw
totally disagree) to 5 (= totally agree). This scale had an internal
fewer opportunities to show their potential. The effect of
consistency of .94.
chances to demonstrate potential on organizational attraction
was significantly positive (B = .33, SE = .11, p < .01). We did not
Analyses find support for H3b, since the indirect effect via expectations
about nonverbal communication was not significant (B = .00,
We conducted three independent sample t-tests to compare SE = .01, 95% CI: −.03 – .03). Also, the effect of expectations about
the expectations about chances to demonstrate potential, to nonverbal communication on organizational attraction was not
use nonverbal cues and organizational attraction across condi­ significant (B = .18, SE = .11, p = .12).
tions (i.e., one t-test for H1 and H2a and H2b, respectively). We
further tested the indirect effects of face-to-face versus video
interviews on organizational attraction via chances to demon­ Discussion
strate potential and the use of nonverbal cues (H3a and H3b) by The results of the second experiment show that applicants
generating 50 000 bootstrap samples using Hayes’ PROCESS expect that face-to-face interviews offer more opportunities
macro model 4, which allows to test for mediation (Hayes, to demonstrate potential than video interviews and that this
2017). We included both mediators in the same analysis. pre-test expectation is positively related to organizational
attraction. These results are in line with previous research on
post-test reactions (Sears et al., 2013). Contrary to expectations
Results
based on media richness theory (Daft & Lengel, 1986), the
Descriptive statistics and intercorrelations results did not show an effect of interview medium on expecta­
Table 2 shows the descriptive statistics and correlations tions about nonverbal communication.
between the variables in this study. Both expectations about
chances to demonstrate potential and to use nonverbal cues
General discussion
related positively to organizational attraction. Also, interview
medium correlated negatively which chances to demonstrate Organizations increasingly use (video) technology in the con­
potential, meaning that participants perceived lower chances text of personnel selection as it offers many benefits such as
in video interviews than in face-to-face interviews. savings in time and costs while being able to reach a much
larger pool of applicants (Bauer et al., 2004). This increased
Hypotheses testing mobility across geographic borders may benefit organizations
First, we tested the difference in organizational attraction in the that are having a difficult time to attract talented candidates
face-to-face interview condition and in the video interview con­ (Beechler & Woodward, 2009).
dition and this was not significant (MFTF = 3.46, SD = .77, MVideo Organizations, however, may also want to take into account
the reactions of applicants towards this new technology (König
et al., 2010). Whereas applicants generally react positively
Table 2. Descriptives and intercorrelations of study 2. towards the use of computer-assisted testing, the literature
M SD 2 3 4 suggests that they are much more reserved with respect to
1. Interview medium −.20* .01 −.08 the use of (video) technology in the selection interview
2. Chances to demonstrate potential 3.62 .75 .37** .36**
3. Use of nonverbal cues 3.71 .71 .26** (Blacksmith et al., 2016). This study expands our understanding
4. Organizational attraction 3.40 .84 of applicants’ pre-test reactions towards the use of face-to-face
**p < .01. versus video interviews in the selection context and tests two
270 K. PROOST ET AL.

possible explanatory mechanism namely expectations about Since applicants may expect that showing potential may be
chances to demonstrate potential and to use nonverbal com­ hindered in video interviews, this may explain stronger effects
munication. These pre-test reactions are important predictors in the first study. An interesting avenue for future research is to
of applicants’ willingness to participate in the selection proce­ look more into the effects of age and work experience on
dure and thus have an important impact on the selection ratio attitudes towards video interviews.
and the success of the organization to attract a talented pool of Finally, participants in the second study, contrary to the first
applicants. study, were all invited at the company. This may also explain
the weaker effects in the second study for two reasons. First, as
the video interview still allowed site visits and as these site visits
Theoretical implications and suggestions for future
are important sources of information for applicants and form
research
the basis for perceptions of person-organization fit (Parsons
The results of the first study showed that applicants are less et al., 1999) and applicant attraction (Uggerslev et al., 2012),
attracted towards organizations that use video interviews ver­ this may explain why applicants did not report lower attraction
sus face-to-face interviews, measured before test-taking. This levels in the video condition. Also, Boswell et al. (2003) showed
result is in line with the stream of literature on this topic, that applicants appreciate site visits, offering opportunities to
conducted at the end of the 20th and beginning of the 21st meet possible future colleagues but also other high ranking
century and focusing on post-test reactions (Chapman et al., persons. When this is prohibited by conducting video inter­
2003; Sears et al., 2013; Straus et al., 2001, see also Blacksmith views, this may offer an alternative explanation for negative
et al., 2016 for a meta-analysis). This study adds to this literature applicant reactions towards video interviews. Second, the fact
by showing that not only post-test reactions but also pre-test that the video interview was held on site, may have eliminated
expectations of applicants are more negative towards video possible negative signals that are sent by the use of video
interviews. These pre-test reactions may be important determi­ interviews in terms of not being taking seriously by the orga­
nants of the observed post-test reactions (Bell et al., 2004) and nization or not wanting to make time to assess the candidate in
organizations may thus benefit from trying to bend these pre- a real-life setting. Future research could therefore focus more
test expectations in a more positive way. on possible signals that are sent by the organization when
The results further showed that applicants expect lower using video interviews and on how these signals affect organi­
chances to demonstrate potential and be hindered in the use zational attraction.
of nonverbal cues in video interviews. These results are in line On the other hand, the fact that the interview was in the
with media richness theory (Daft & Lengel, 1986; Daft et al., company but still via video may have created a rather artificial
1987) and studies showing that video interviews may hinder situation. We did not clarify why the interview was via video.
fluent turn taking, eye contact and communicating via body A plausible explanation could be that, in international compa­
language and gestures (Bohannon et al., 2013; Powers et al., nies, the HR manager does not work in the country where the
2011; Wegge, 2006). applicant resides. We did not assess how applicants interpreted
The results were somewhat different in the second study in this situation. Taking this into account or offering a justification
the sense that the direct effect of interview medium on orga­ for interviews via video on site could be important avenues for
nizational attraction was not significant. Although Hayes (2009) future research.
argues that an indirect effect can exist and be meaningful
interpreted in the absence of a direct effect, the results of
Limitations
study 2 should be interpreted with caution. Only an indirect
effect was found of interview medium on organizational attrac­ The generalizability of our results may be restricted by several
tion via chances to demonstrate potential. Also, the effect of factors. First, we conducted two lab studies with vignettes.
interview medium on the use of nonverbal cues was not These designs are criticized for a lack of realism and thus
significant. limited external validity. Specifically, participants in our studies
This difference in results between the two studies may be may not feel the pressure and motivation that comes with
explained in different ways. First, the fact that we asked for a high-stake selection interview and attach less importance to
a direct comparison between the two conditions in the first being offered the chance to demonstrate potential. In a fictive
study and not in the second study may have strengthened the world, they may see the video interview as novel and challen­
effect in the first study. On the other hand, the scenario in the ging, while in a high-stake context these positive elements may
first study may be more realistic as applicants, when looking for be overshadowed by concerns about opportunities to show
a job, most often apply to different organizations and receive their potential. As such, the results of our studies may be an
invitations for interviews from multiple organizations that have underestimation of the effect in a real-life context. However, we
different selection procedures. In practice, applicants thus made sure to make the vignette realistic: All participants were
directly compare organizations and selection procedures. potential job seekers and getting an invitation for an interview
Second, the participants in the first study were last year stu­ from one or two companies is a realistic scenario in that case.
dents at the university while participants in the second study Moreover, experimental vignette studies have one clear advan­
were older and mostly employed. This younger group of parti­ tage, namely they allow to draw causal conclusions since it is
cipants may be more concerned with showing their potential. possible to control for all possible confounds (Antonakis et al.,
As suggested by Fletcher (2013), older and more experienced 2010). Especially the latter is more difficult in more complex,
applicants may “expect to be taken for what they are” (p. 273). realistic settings such as mock interviews as they produce more
EUROPEAN JOURNAL OF WORK AND ORGANIZATIONAL PSYCHOLOGY 271

“natural noise” (Aguinis & Bradley, 2014). The optimal alterna­ intentions to apply, to pursue employment or to accept a job
tive would be to experimentally manipulate in interview invita­ offer (Bell et al., 2006; Schreurs et al., 2009).
tions from real companies whether the interview will be held This study suggests that one possible reason for this lower
via video versus face-to-face. It is questionable, however, to attractiveness is lower expectations about chances to demon­
what extent this is practically feasible and desirable, seen the strate potential. It may therefore be important for organizations
possible negative recruitment outcomes. that want to use video interviews to pay attention to this con­
Second, we did not define the extent to which the interview cern. One possible way is by communicating clearly to applicants
would be structured. Participants in this study may have intui­ that they will get the opportunity to show their knowledge, skills
tively expected an unstructured interview, as this type of inter­ and abilities (Arvey & Sackett, 1993), and be offered the explicit
view is preferred by companies (Highhouse, 2008; Lievens & De opportunity to point out possible competences that might have
Paepe, 2004). As such, the observed effect in this study may be been overlooked (Bies & Shapiro, 1988). Bies and Shapiro also
typical for an unstructured interview and could have been showed that clearly explaining the underlying motives for certain
different in a structured interview. For example, Chapman and decisions may help. If organizations clearly explain to applicants
Rowe (2002) showed that applicants are more positive towards why video interviews are used instead of face-to-face interviews,
video interviews when the interview is structured vs. unstruc­ this may increase pre-test reactions towards video interviews.
tured or semi-structured. More research is needed to test the The results of the first study also showed that applicants
generalizability of the results across different interview types. expect that video interviews hinder nonverbal communication.
We focused on organizational attraction as the focal variable Bohannon and colleagues offer some interesting suggestions in
of interest and based on the literature, suggested that lower their review with respect to eye contact, such as embedding
organizational attraction may lead to lower job pursuit/accep­ a camera in the display (Tapia et al., 2003) or using software to
tance intentions (McCarthy et al., 2017). Chapman et al. (2003), create the illusion of eye contact (Gemmell et al., 2000).
however, showed that it may be important to differentiate The more negative reactions towards video interviews in
between outcomes. Specifically, they found that video inter­ this study do not have to generalize to other selection prac­
views were perceived as less procedurally fair than face-to-face tices. For example, Internet-based technology can well be used
interviews. However, interview medium did not affect the to attract candidates (Lievens & Harris, 2003; Thompson et al.,
intentions to accept a job offer from the company. Future 2009), to offer realistic job previews (Highhouse et al., 2004) or
research should therefore include other attitudinal outcomes even to give online feedback (Dineen et al., 2002).
and even move beyond attitudinal outcomes and also include
more behavioural outcomes.
This study is limited in the sense that it focuses on two Conclusion
explanatory mechanisms for possible differences in applicant
This study scrutinized applicants’ pre-test reactions towards the
pre-test reactions towards video versus face-to-face interviews.
use of video interviews in selection procedures. The results
We thereby focused on possible shortcomings of video inter­
suggest that applicants rate organizations that use video inter­
views, that can be derived from media richness theory (Daft &
views as less attractive compared to those that use traditional
Lengel, 1986). On the other hand, video interviews have
face-to-face interviews. This effect is in part explained by the
a number of assets that may elicit positive applicant reactions
fact that applicants expected fewer chances to demonstrate
and remain underexplored such as remote access to colleagues
their potential in video interviews (vs. face-to-face interviews).
and managers across the world, less discrimination of less
mobile applicants, etc. Further research could take applicant
reactions towards these aspects more into account.
Disclosure statement
Finally, we compared face-to-face interviews with video
interviews in this study but did not consider different versions No potential conflict of interest was reported by the authors.
of video interviews. For example, a promising avenue for future
research could be to compare synchronous with asynchronous
video interviews in which applicants’ answers are recorded and References
evaluated at a later point in time. Suen et al. (2019) provided Aguinis, H., & Bradley, K. J. (2014). Best practice recommendations for
first evidence that asynchronous interviews are more disliked designing and implementing experimental vignette methodology
by applicants compared to synchronous video interviews as studies. Organizational Research Methods, 17(4), 351–371. https://doi.
they “dehumanize” the interview. org/10.1177/1094428114547952
Anderson, N. (2003). Applicant and recruiter reactions to new technology in
selection: A critical review and agenda for future research. International
Practical implications Journal of Selection and Assessment, 11(2–3), 121–136. https://doi.org/10.
1111/1468-2389.00235
The results of this study suggest that applicants may feel less Andrews, L., Klein, S. R., Forsman, J., & Sachau, D. (2013). It’s easy being
attracted to organizations that use video interviews versus green: Benefits of technology-enabled work. In A. H. Huffman &
S. R. Klein (Eds.), Green organizations: Driving change with
face-to-face interviews in their selection procedure. In the
I-O psychology (pp. 300–322). Routledge.
“war for talent”, it may be important for organizations to take Antonakis, J., Bendahan, S., Jacquart, P., & Lalive, R. (2010). On making causal
these pre-test reactions into account as they may not only claims: A review and recommendations. The Leadership Quarterly, 21(6),
lower attraction but lead to lower pre-test motivation, 1086–1120. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.leaqua.2010.10.010
272 K. PROOST ET AL.

Arvey, R. D., & Sackett, P. R. (1993). Fairness in selection: Current develop­ Dennis, A. R., & Kinney, S. T. (1998). Testing media richness theory in the
ments and perspectives. In N. Schmitt & W. Borman (Eds.), Personnel new media: The effects of cues, feedback, and task equivocality.
selection (pp. 171–202). Jossey-Bass. Information Systems Research, 9(3), 256–274. https://doi.org/10.1287/
Barry, B., & Fulmer, I. S. (2004). The medium and the message: The adaptive isre.9.3.256
use of communication media in dyadic influence. Academy of Derous, E., Born, M., & De Witte, K. (2004). How applicants want and expect
Management Review, 29(2), 272–292. https://doi.org/10.5465/amr.2004. to be treated: Applicants’ selection treatment beliefs and the develop­
12736093 ment of the social process questionnaire on selection. International
Bauer, T. N., Maertz, C. P., Dolen, M. R., & Campion, M. A. (1998). Longitudinal Journal of Selection and Assessment, 12(1–2), 99–119. https://doi.org/10.
assessment of applicant reactions to employment testing and test out­ 1111/j.0965.075X.2004.00267.x
come feedback. Journal of Applied Psychology, 83(6), 892–903. https:// Dineen, B. R., Ash, S. R., & Noe, R. A. (2002). A web of applicant attraction:
doi.org/10.1037/0021-9010.83.6.892 Person-organization fit in the context of web-based recruitment. Journal
Bauer, T. N., Truxillo, D. M., Paronto, M. E., & Weekley, J. A. (2004). Applicant of Applied Psychology, 87(4), 723–734. https://doi.org/10.1037//0021-
reactions to different selection technology: Face-to-face, interactive 9010.87.4.723
voice response, and computer-assisted telephone screening interviews. Doherty-Sneddon, G., Anderson, A., O’Malley, C., Langton, S., Garrod, S., &
International Journal of Selection and Assessment, 12(1–2), 135–148. Bruce, V. (1997). Face-to-face and video-mediated communication:
https://doi.org/10.1111/j.0965-075X.2004.00269.x A comparison of dialogue structure and task performance. Journal of
Beechler, S., & Woodward, I. C. (2009). The global “war for talent”. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 3(2), 105–125. https://doi.org/10.1037/
International Management, 15(3), 273–285. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.int 1076-898X.3.2.105
man.2009.01.002 Fletcher, C. (2013). Impression management in the selection interview. In
Bekkering, T. J. E. (2004). Visual angle in videoconferencing: The issue of trust R. A. Giacalone & P. Rosenfeld (Eds.), Impression management in the
[Doctoral dissertation]. Mississippi State University, ProQuest organization (pp. 269–281). Lawrence Erlbaum.
Dissertations Publishing. (Accession no. 3120803). Fox, S., & Spector, P. E. (2000). Relations of emotional intelligence, practical
Bell, B. S., Ryan, A. M., & Wiechmann, D. (2004). Justice expectations and intelligence, general intelligence, and trait affectivity with interview
applicant perceptions. International Journal of Selection and Assessment, outcomes: It’s not all just ‘G’. Journal of Organizational Behavior, 21(2),
12(1–2), 24–38. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.0965-075X.2004.00261.x 203–220. https://doi.org/10.1002/(SICI)1099-1379(200003)21:2<203::
Bell, B. S., Wiechmann, D., & Ryan, A. M. (2006). Consequences of organiza­ AID-JOB38>3.0.CO;2-Z
tional justice expectations in a selection system. Journal of Applied Fullwood, C. (2007). The effect of mediation on impression formation:
Psychology, 91(2), 455–466. https://doi.org/10.1037/0021-9010.91.2.455 A comparison of face-to-face and video-mediated conditions. Applied
Bies, R. J., & Shapiro, D. L. (1988). Voice and justification: Their influence on Ergonomics, 38(3), 267–273. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.apergo.2006.06.002
procedural fairness judgments. The Academy of Management Journal, 31 Geenen, B., Proost, K., van Dijke, M., De Witte, K., & von Grumbkow, J. (2012).
(3), 676–685. https://www.jstor.org/stable/256465 The role of affect in the relationship between distributive justice expec­
Blacksmith, N., Wilford, J. C., & Behrend, T. S. (2016). Technology in the tations and applicants’ recommendation and litigation intentions.
employment interview: A meta-analysis and future research agenda. International Journal of Selection and Assessment, 20(4), 404–413.
Personnel Assessment and Decisions, 2(1), 12–20. https://doi.org/10. https://doi.org/10.1111/ijsa.12003
25035/pad.2016.002 Gilliland, S. W. (1995). Fairness from the applicant’s perspective: Reactions
Bohannon, L. S., Herbert, A. M., Pelz, J. B., & Rantanen, E. M. (2013). Eye to employee selection procedures. International Journal of Selection and
contact and video-mediated communication: A review. Displays, 34(2), Assessment, 3(1), 11–19. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1468-2389.1995.
177–185. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.displa.2012.10.009 tb0002.x
Boswell, W. R., Roehling, M. V., LePine, M. A., & Moynihan, L. M. (2003). Gilmore, D. C., & Ferris, G. R. (1989). The effects of applicant impression
Individual job-choice decisions and the impact of job attributes and management tactics on interviewer judgments. Journal of Management,
recruitment practices: A longitudinal field study. Human Resource 15(4), 557–564. https://doi.org/10.1177/014920638901500405
Management, 42(1), 23–37. https://doi.org/10.1002/hrm.10062 Hausknecht, J. P., Day, D. V., & Thomas, S. C. (2004). Applicant reactions to
Burnett, J. R., & Motowidlo, S. J. (1998). Relations between different sources selection procedures: An updated model and meta-analysis. Personnel
of information in the structured selection interview. Personnel Psychology, 57(3), 639–683. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1744-6570.2004.
Psychology, 51(4), 963–983. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1744-6570.1998. 00003.x
tb00747.x Hayes, A. F. (2009). Beyond Baron and Kenny: Statistical mediation analysis
Chapman, D. S., & Rowe, P. M. (2002). The influence of videoconference in the new millennium. Communication Monographs, 76(4), 408–420.
technology and interview structure on the recruiting function of the https://doi.org/10.1080/03637750903310360
employment interview: A field experiment. International Journal of Hayes, A. F. (2017). Introduction to mediation, moderation, and conditional
Selection and Assessment, 10(3), 185–197. https://doi.org/10.1111/1468- process analysis: A regression-based approach. Guilford Publications.
2389.00208 Highhouse, S. (2008). Stubborn reliance on intuition and subjectivity in
Chapman, D. S., Uggerslev, K. L., & Webster, J. (2003). Applicant reactions to employee selection. Industrial and Organizational Psychology, 1(3),
face-to-face and technology-mediated interviews: A field investigation. 333–342. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1754-9434.2008.00058.x
The Journal of Applied Psychology, 88(5), 944–953. https://doi.org/10. Highhouse, S., Lievens, F., & Sinar, E. F. (2003). Measuring attraction to
1037/0021-9010.88.5.944 organizations. Educational and Psychological Measurement, 63(6),
Charness, G., Gneezy, U., & Kuhn, M. A. (2012). Experimental methods: 986–1001. https://doi.org/10.1177/0013164403258403
Between-subject and within-subject design. Journal of Economic Highhouse, S., Stanton, J. M., & Reeve, C. L. (2004). Examining reactions to
Behavior & Organization, 81(1), 1–8. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jebo.2011. employer information using a simulated web-based job fair. Journal of
08.009 Career Assessment, 12(1), 85–96. https://doi.org/10.1177/
Daft, R. L., & Lengel, R. H. (1986). Organizational information requirements, 1069072703257738
media richness and structural design. Management Science, 32(5), Horn, R. G., & Behrend, T. S. (2017). Video killed the interview star: Does
554–571. https://doi.org/10.1287/mnsc.32.5.554 picture-in-picture affect interview performance? Personnel Assessment
Daft, R. L., Lengel, R. H., & Trevino, L. K. (1987). Message equivocality, media and Decisions, 3(1), 51–59. https://doi.org/https://dx.doi.10.25035/pad.
selection and manager performance: Implications for information 2017.005
systems. MIS Quarterly, 11(3), 355–366. https://doi.org/10.2307/248682 Howardson, G. N., & Behrend, T. S. (2014). Using the Internet to recruit
DeGroot, T., & Gooty, J. (2009). Can nonverbal cues be used to make employees: Comparing the effects of usability expectations and objec­
meaningful personality attributes in employment interviews? Journal tive technological characteristics on Internet recruitment outcomes.
of Business and Psychology, 24(2), 179–192. https://doi.org/10.1007/ Computers in Human Behavior, 31, 334–342. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.
s10869-009-9098-0 chb.2013.10.057
EUROPEAN JOURNAL OF WORK AND ORGANIZATIONAL PSYCHOLOGY 273

Ishii, K., Lyons, M. M., & Carr, S. A. (2019). Revisiting media richness theory of 85 years of research findings. Psychological Bulletin, 124(2), 262–274.
for today and future. Human Behavior and Emerging Technologies, 1(2), https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.124.2.262
124–131. https://doi.org/10.1002/hbe2.138 Schreurs, B., Derous, E., Proost, K., Notelaers, G., & De Witte, K. (2008).
König, C. J., Klehe, U. C., Berchtold, M., & Kleinmann, M. (2010). Reasons for Applicant selection expectations: Validating a multidimensional mea­
being selective when choosing personnel selection procedures. sure in the military. International Journal of Selection and Assessment, 16
International Journal of Selection and Assessment, 18(1), 17–27. https:// (2), 170–176. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1468-2389.2008.00421.x
doi.org/10.1111/j.1468-2389.2010.00485.x Schreurs, B., Derous, E., Van Hooft, E., Proost, K., & De Witte, K. (2009).
Kroeck, K. G., & Magnusen, K. O. (1997). Employer and job candidate reac­ Predicting applicants’ job pursuit behavior from their selection expecta­
tions to videoconference job interviewing. International Journal of tions: The mediating role of the theory of planned behavior. Journal of
Selection and Assessment, 5(2), 137–142. https://doi.org/10.1111/1468- Organizational Behavior, 30(6), 761–783. https://doi.org/10.1002/job.570
2389.00053 Sears, G. J., Zhang, H., Wiesner, W. H., Hackett, R. D., & Yuan, Y. (2013).
Lievens, F., & De Paepe, A. (2004). An empirical investigation of A comparative assessment of videoconference and face-to-face employ­
interviewer-related factors that discourage the use of high structure ment interviews. Management Decision, 51(8), 1733–1752. https://doi.
interviews. Journal of Organizational Behavior, 25(1), 29–46. https://doi. org/10.1108/MD-09-2012-0642
org/10.1002/job.246 Gemmell, J, Toyama, K, Zitnick, C. L., Kang, T., & Seitz, S. (2000). Gaze
Lievens, F., & Harris, M. M. (2003). Research on Internet recruiting and awareness for video-conferencing: A software approach. IEEE
testing: Current status and future directions. In C. L. Cooper & Multimedia, 7(4), 26–35. https://doi.org/10.1109/93.895152
I. T. Robertson (Eds.), International review of industrial and organizational Straus, S. G., Miles, J. A., & Levesque, L. L. (2001). The effects of videocon­
psychology (Vol. 18, pp. 131–165). John Wiley. ference, telephone, and face-to-face media on interviewer and applicant
Lievens, F., Van Dam, K., & Anderson, N. (2002). Recent trends and chal­ judgements in employment interviews. Journal of Management, 27(3),
lenges in personnel selection. Personnel Review, 31(5), 580–601. https:// 363–381. https://doi.org/10.1177/014920630102700308
doi.org/10.1108/00483480210438771 Suen, H.-Y., Chen, M. Y.-C., & Lu, S.-H. (2019). Does the use of synchrony and
Macan, T. H. (2009). The employment interview: A review of current studies artificial intelligence in video interviews affect interview ratings and
and directions for future research. Human Resource Management Review, applicant attitudes? Computers in Human Behavior, 98, 93–101. https://
19(3), 203–218. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.hrmr.2009.03.006 doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2019.04.012
McCarthy, J. M., Bauer, T. N., Truxillo, D. M., Anderson, N., Costa, A., & Tapia, E. M., Intille, S. S., Rebula, J., & Stoddard, S. (2003). Concept and partial
Ahmed, S. M. (2017). Applicant perspectives during selection: A review prototype video: Ubiquitous video communication with the perception
addressing “so what?”, “what’s new”, and “where to go?”. Journal of of eye contact. Proceedings of UBICOMP (pp. 1–2). Massachusetts
Management, 43(6), 1693–1725. https://doi.org/10.1177/ Institute of Technology.
0149206316681846 Thompson, J., Braddy, P. W., & Wuensch, K. L. (2009). E-recruitment and the
Parsons, C. K., Cable, D. M., & Liden, R. C. (1999). Establishing person- benefits of organizational web appeal. Computers in Human Behavior, 24
organization fit. In R. W. Eder & M. M. Harris (Eds.), The employment (5), 2384–2398. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2008.02.014
interview handbook (pp. 125–141). Sage Publications. Uggerslev, K. L., Fassina, N. E., & Kraichy, D. (2012). Recruiting through the
Peeters, H., & Lievens, F. (2006). Verbal and nonverbal impression manage­ stages: A meta-analytic test of predictors of applicant attraction at
ment tactics in behavior description and situational interviews. different stages of the recruiting process. Personnel Psychology, 65(3),
International Journal of Selection and Assessment, 14(3), 206–222. 597–660. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1744-6570.2012.01254.x
https://doi.org/10.111/j.1468-2389.2006.00348.x Wanous, P., Keon, T. L., & Latack, J. C. (1983). Expectancy theory and
Powers, S. R., Rauh, C., Henning, R. A., Buck, R. W., & West, T. V. (2011). The occupational/organizational choice: A review and test. Organizational
effect of video feedback delay on frustration and emotion communica­ Behavior and Human Performance, 32(1), 66–86. https://doi.org/10.1016/
tion accuracy. Computers in Human Behavior, 27(5), 1651–1657. https:// 0030-5073(83)90140-X
doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2011.02.003 Wegge, J. (2006). Communication via videoconference: Emotional and
Proost, K., Schreurs, B., De Witte, K., & Derous, E. (2010). Ingratiation and cognitive consequences of affective personality dispositions, seeing
self-promotion in the selection interview: The effects of using single one’s own picture, and disturbing events. Human-Computer Interaction,
tactics or a combination of tactics on interviewer judgments. Journal of 21(3), 273–318. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15327051hci2103_1
Applied Social Psychology, 40(9), 2155–2169. https://doi.org/10.1111/j. Wilhelmy, A., Kleinmann, M., König, C. J., Melchers, K. G., & Truxillo, D. M.
1559-1816.2010.00654.x (2016). How and why do interviewers try to make impressions on appli­
Schmidt, F. L., & Hunter, J. E. (1998). The validity and utility of selection cants? A qualitative study. Journal of Applied Psychology, 101(3),
methods in personnel psychology: Practical and theoretical implications 313–332. https://doi.org/10.1037/ap10000046

You might also like