You are on page 1of 6

INVITED REVIEW

Review: The Quiet Eye in Sports Performance—Is the Quiet Eye the
Ultimate Explanation or Only the Beginning?
Downloaded from http://journals.lww.com/optvissci by BhDMf5ePHKav1zEoum1tQfN4a+kJLhEZgbsIHo4XMi0hCyw

Kristine Dalton, OD, PhD, FAAO1*


CX1AWnYQp/IlQrHD3i3D0OdRyi7TvSFl4Cf3VC1y0abggQZXdgGj2MwlZLeI= on 10/17/2023

SIGNIFICANCE: The quiet eye is a gaze behavior that seems to differentiate both expert-novice performance and
successful and unsuccessful performance in experts; however, the quiet eye may only represent one aspect of the
perceptual-cognitive expertise differences that exist between athletes and nonathletes.
Research suggests that expert-novice differences in vision and visual-motor coordination skills exist between ath-
letes and nonathletes, although the underlying mechanisms driving these differences are not well understood. The
quiet eye is the final fixation or tracking gaze made before the initiation of the action of importance in a motor co-
ordination task and is quite possibly the most well-studied vision strategy variable in sport. The quiet eye has been
shown to be a key component of both expert ability and successful performance. However, the quiet eye is not without
its criticisms, and the perceptual mechanisms underlying this unique gaze behavior are not yet well understood. The
question that remains to be answered is whether the differences in vision and visual-motor coordination skills that
exist between athletes and nonathletes can be explained by the quiet eye alone or if the explanation is more
complicated.

Author Affiliations:
1
School of Optometry and Vision
Science, University of Waterloo,
Optom Vis Sci 2021;98:732–737. doi:10.1097/OPX.0000000000001728 Waterloo, Ontario, Canada
Copyright © 2021 American Academy of Optometry *kndalton@uwaterloo.ca

Curiosity about what makes athletes so exceptionally good at On the other hand, Starkes and Deakin,6 Rothstein,7 Abernethy,8
what they do, especially when compared with the average human, and Hughes et al.9 argued that there was no difference between ath-
dates back to the early 21st century or, more specifically, 1921, letes and nonathletes, at least when their vision was compared with
when researchers attempted to determine what made Babe Ruth standard, often static, clinical tests. Abernethy10 went on to suggest
the greatest home-run hitter of all time.1 In the century since, many that enhancement of the visual hardware or refractive, binocular vi-
researchers have attempted to answer those very questions: do ath- sion, and ocular health status of athletes beyond normal levels was
letes (experts) have better vision than nonathletes (novices)? If unlikely to improve sports performance. Instead, Abernethy10 rec-
expert-novice differences exist, are they physiological or learned? ommended improving athletes' visual perceptual strategies or soft-
If expert-novice differences are learned, are they trainable and does ware as a means to enhancing sports performance instead. By the
training improve on-field performance? time Erickson11 published his book in 2007, the question of
Sherman2 conducted one of the first extensive literature reviews whether athletes did indeed have better vision than nonathletes
examining the role of vision in sport and concluded that vision was was still hotly contested.
the “signal” driving the muscle response in sports activities. Sherman2 Most recently, work by Laby et al.,12,13 Burris et al.,14 and Liu
suggested that visual tasks, such as dynamic visual acuity, depth et al.15 have consistently demonstrated that athletes have a combi-
perception, eye movement accuracy and speed, peripheral vision, nation of superior visual and visual-motor coordination abilities
and visualization, could be of greater importance in sports. Sherman2 compared with nonathletes. Moreover, these research groups have
went on further to suggest that these visual tasks could be en- demonstrated that, within athlete populations, there are differ-
hanced through training. Stine et al.3 expanded upon the work of ences, as professionals have also been shown to have superior vi-
Sherman2 and concluded that various athletic groups had superior sual abilities compared with amateurs.12–15
visual abilities to nonathletic groups, particularly on tasks that were Considering the evidence that has been collected to date, it
more representative of the demands of sport such as peripheral vi- seems likely that expert-novice visual performance differences ex-
sion and dynamic visual acuity. Hitzeman and Beckerman4 also ist. The questions that remain to be answered now are whether
found that there were certain visual skills that were important to these differences are physiological or learned and whether they
sports performance and that visual skills of athletes and nonath- are trainable. An underlying theme across all the literature investi-
letes differ. Research from Laby et al.,5 demonstrating that profes- gating expert-novice visual performance differences in sport is that
sional baseball players had superior vision compared with the the differences are more often found in aspects of vision that have
general population, further supports this conclusion. stronger functional relationships to performance tasks at hand.

www.optvissci.com Optom Vis Sci 2021; Vol 98(7) 732

Copyright © American Academy of Optometry. Unauthorized reproduction of this article is prohibited.


The Quiet Eye in Sport Performance — Dalton

Differences in visual functions that are more likely enhanced action of importance (movement of the hands in the shooting action
through learning, such as dynamic visual acuity, oculomotor control, in basketball). The quiet eye was found to be longer in experts
and visuomotor reaction times, are frequently reported.2,3,12–15 (972 milliseconds on hits, 806 milliseconds on misses) compared
Differences in physiological aspects of vision such as static visual with near-experts (357 milliseconds on hits, 393 milliseconds on mis-
acuity and contrast sensitivity have also been noted, albeit less ses) and longer on successful shots compared with misses in experts.
frequently.2,3,5 Thus, it seems that the recommendation of The concept of the quiet eye has evolved significantly since it
Abernethy10 to improve athlete's software as a means of enhancing was originally defined in 1996. The evolution of the quiet eye def-
Downloaded from http://journals.lww.com/optvissci by BhDMf5ePHKav1zEoum1tQfN4a+kJLhEZgbsIHo4XMi0hCyw

sports performance was well founded. inition has been driven by several factors including technology. Ini-
Perhaps the most prominent sports performance software com- tially, the quiet eye was defined as the portion of the final fixation
ponent that has been studied to date is the quiet eye. Presently, the that occurred before the onset of the critical action within 3° visual
CX1AWnYQp/IlQrHD3i3D0OdRyi7TvSFl4Cf3VC1y0abggQZXdgGj2MwlZLeI= on 10/17/2023

quiet eye is defined as the “the final fixation or tracking gaze prior angle. However, improvements in eye tracking technology made it
to the onset of the critical action, that is located on a specific loca- possible to measure gaze behaviors in sport with more preci-
tion or object in the visuomotor workspace within [1° to] 3° of visual sion and allowed for the quiet eye to be measured within 1°
angle (or less) for a minimum of 100 ms.”16–21 The quiet eye has rather than 3°. Furthermore, when sports such as ice hockey
been proposed as the period when task-relevant environmental were examined, researchers found that the quiet eye was pres-
cues are processed and motor plans are coordinated for successful ent, but it was consistently directed toward moving targets (i.e.,
completion of the upcoming task. skaters).24 Because the quiet eye gaze behavior was moving with
The earliest published work investigating the vision strategy of the skaters, it was not a fixation, but it was still a stable gaze behav-
golfers and what would later be termed the quiet eye was con- ior on a target.
ducted in the early 1990s at the University of Calgary by Dr. Joan The quiet eye, as we now know it, has traditionally been
Vickers. Vickers22 recorded the eye movements of both lower- thought of as a single isolated fixation that occurs only once dur-
and higher-skilled golfers while they performed consecutive flat ing a period of action, immediately before the onset of a critical
3-m putts until 10 hits (successful putts) and 10 misses were ob- action. The singularity of the quiet eye has been one of its
tained. The lower-skilled golfers would have been considered greatest criticisms, as there is a growing body of evidence sug-
very good amateurs or average club-level golfers, whereas the gesting that multiple different gaze behaviors could be important
higher-skilled golfers would have all been very good club-level for sports performance.20,25–34 Thus, it seems that the definition
players. Golfers wore a mobile eye tracker, and various gaze be- of the quiet eye is again on the verge of change. Even Vickers21 re-
haviors were examined throughout the duration of the putt, in- cently stated that multiple quiet eye periods could exist in an ac-
cluding the length and location of specific fixations and the tion. The evolving definition of the quiet eye clearly speaks to the
pattern of fixations and saccades used. complexity of this concept.
Higher-skilled golfers were found to use a vision strategy that in- As a singular gaze behavior, the quiet eye has been shown to
cluded longer fixations on the ball and the target and fewer fixations differentiate expert and nonexpert performances, as well as suc-
on the club compared with the lower-skilled golfers. Higher-skilled cessful and unsuccessful performances in a number of different
golfers also used more express saccades to shift their gaze between sports, including shooting, archery, hockey, volleyball, badmin-
targets faster and maintained their gaze on the putting surface lon- ton, dart throwing, billiards, soccer, and tennis,17,19,21,24,35–57
ger after ball contact. Finally, the last fixation before the initiation in three large multisport meta-analyses,58–60 and in other areas
of the backswing was found to be longer (1788 vs. 911 milliseconds) such as motor coordination in children,61 surgery,62–65 and law
in golfers with higher skill levels. The last fixation before backswing enforcement.66 The presence of the quiet eye across so many
initiation was also found to be longer on successful putts, regardless sports and disciplines is a strong argument in support of its im-
of the golfer's skill level. Based on these results, Vickers22 proposed portance for sports performance.
that an ideal vision strategy in putting would be one in which express Intervention studies have demonstrated that quiet eye training
saccades to the putter were used in the preparation phase, along leads to increased fixation durations and translates to improved
with a single fixation of greater than 1700 milliseconds directed to performance in various sports, including golf, basketball, soccer,
the ball during the back/forward swing phase and a stable fixation shooting, and darts44,67–80; motor coordination skills in children81–83;
on the green for more than 200 milliseconds after ball contact. This and laparoscopic surgery.84 The recent multisport meta-analysis con-
strategy was hypothesized to improve golfer's performance by reduc- ducted by Lebeau et al.59 extracted two effect sizes for quiet eye train-
ing the amount of distracting information collected throughout the ing. First, quiet eye training was found to have a large effect on quiet
swing and increasing the precision of the visual-motor coordination eye duration, whereby trained groups demonstrated significantly lon-
of the hands when the putter contacted the ball. ger quiet eye durations than did untrained groups, as would have been
In 1996, Vickers16,23 explored the vision strategies of expert expected because of the targeted intervention. The second effect size
and near-expert basketball players while they completed 10 suc- Lebeau et al. found was that quiet eye training resulted in improved
cessful and 10 unsuccessful free throws. Vickers looked at several performance in the trained groups compared with the untrained
different aspects of the players' free throws, including the duration groups. Lebeau et al. proposed that the enhanced performance after
of each phase of the shot, the number of gaze behaviors made dur- quiet eye training was a by-product of the heightened focus of atten-
ing each shot, the type and location of gaze behaviors made during tion on an external cue resulting in better coordination of the motor
the shot, and head movements during the different shot phases. task. The findings of Lebeau et al. and the independent intervention
Vickers reported that the most significant findings of this study studies again present a strong argument in support of the importance
were that the expert group had a longer final fixation and a longer of the quiet eye in sports performance. However, the smaller effect
quiet eye, combined with an earlier fixation offset than the size noted in the translation of quiet eye training to sports performance
near-expert group. The quiet eye, as it was then defined, was the by Lebeau et al. also hints at the possibility that sports performance is
portion of the final fixation that started with the onset of the final more complicated than the quiet eye alone would suggest.

www.optvissci.com Optom Vis Sci 2021; Vol 98(7) 733

Copyright © American Academy of Optometry. Unauthorized reproduction of this article is prohibited.


The Quiet Eye in Sport Performance — Dalton

It has been hypothesized that the quiet eye plays a role in ensur- fixation was important for golf putting performance. More specifi-
ing that the brain receives the visual information it needs to plan, cally, Vine et al. found that, when larger proportions of the quite
organize, and initiate the extensive neural networks involved in mo- eye occurred after putter-ball contact (“quiet eye dwell” time), ath-
tor control tasks.21 The quiet eye has been suggested to represent letes performed better. The debate about optimal timing of the
optimal visual attentional control,70,71 whereby longer quiet eye quiet eye is perhaps most pronounced in the sport of basketball.
periods are thought to give performers more time to program their Vickers et al.,21,79 Vickers,23,96 Wilson et al.,46 and Harle and
movements while minimizing distractions from other environmen- Vickers68 have demonstrated that an earlier quiet eye period is im-
Downloaded from http://journals.lww.com/optvissci by BhDMf5ePHKav1zEoum1tQfN4a+kJLhEZgbsIHo4XMi0hCyw

tal or emotional triggers.17,18,23 Electroencephalographic studies, portant for successful performance, whereas Oudejans et al.69,97
demonstrating a harmonization of brain activity during the quiet and de Oliveira et al.98–100 have argued that late looking is important
eye period, support the theory that the quiet eye represents optimal for sports performance, and Moeinirad et al.101 have suggested that
CX1AWnYQp/IlQrHD3i3D0OdRyi7TvSFl4Cf3VC1y0abggQZXdgGj2MwlZLeI= on 10/17/2023

visual attention control.37,38,47,85–87 However, several other theo- both early and late quiet eye durations are longer in expert basketball
ries, including attentional control theory,36,46,88 dorsal and ventral players compared with nonexperts.
stream processing,89,90 and the inhibition hypothesis,56,91 have Oudejans et al.69,97 and de Oliveira et al.98–100 have approached
also been proposed to explain how the quiet eye could enhance the study of gaze behaviors in basketball from an ecological ap-
visual-motor skills and sports performance. Although the quiet proach. The ecological approach is based on the theory that athletes
eye's underlying mechanism is not yet understood, there certainly rely on optic flow fields to identify and perceive the affordances in
seems to be an overwhelming amount of evidence supporting its their environments in real time and then use this information to orga-
importance in performance. nize their movements. Although less commonly used to study gaze
Despite the seemingly impressive support for the importance of behaviors in sport, research conducted using the ecological ap-
the quiet eye in sports performance, the quiet eye is not without its proach has demonstrated that time-to-contact information obtained
limitations. The definition of quite eye itself, to some extent at through optic flow fields was sufficient to guide motor behaviors.102,103
least, has been constrained by the technological capabilities of These results would suggest that, perhaps, the relationships be-
commercially available gaze trackers and gaze analysis programs. tween gaze behaviors in sport are more complex than they might
Furthermore, there is a significant amount of research demonstrat- initially seem.
ing other aspects of athletes' vision strategies are important for per- More recently, there has been an increase in research suggest-
formance and should be taken into consideration. ing that quiet eye duration does not always correlate with better
Historically, gaze behavior analysis has been done manually, task performance and task complexity influences quiet eye dura-
whereby videos are analyzed frame by frame and gaze behaviors tion in both novice and expert performers.20,25,104–107 This re-
are coded by an observer.92 This method is both time-consuming search further supports the possibility that the quiet eye may play
and complicated by the fact that individuals coding the data rely a different role in sports performance than originally thought.
on the accuracy of the fixation point recorded in the video produced Van Lier et al.25 conducted a study to examine the effects of
by the gaze tracking software, which can be as large as 1° in size. slope on gaze in golf putting with 12 high-skilled professionals di-
Furthermore, fixations that are between 1 and 3° in size may con- vided into two groups (high and low skills) based on their success
sist of fixations and other types of gaze behaviors, such as pursuits, on a putting task. Both groups of golfers completed putts on greens
small saccades, or microsacccades.20,26 The combination of gaze of variable slopes while wearing a gaze tracker. No differences in
behaviors that are possible, within the region of interest that is the final ball fixation duration (equivalent to the quiet eye) or the fi-
the quiet eye fixation, may be strategically important, yet are pres- nal hole fixation were found between the skill groups on successful
ently not characterized.20 and missed putts or the different slope conditions. Differences
Moreover, most of the commercially available gaze trackers are were found between successful and missed putts overall in the dif-
only monocular, whereas most sports are played binocularly. Pre- ferent slope conditions, though. On the most challenging slope,
liminary work with a binocular eye tracker, done by Dalton26 and golfers had a shorter quiet eye, spending significantly less time
Dalton et al.,93 found that professional golfers with a dominant viewing the ball and more time viewing the slope, suggesting that
eye had longer fixations and higher putting success than did profes- the golfers modified their visual search behavior according to the
sional golfers without a dominant eye. Interestingly, no difference in task conditions.
the distribution of handedness or ocular dominance between elite Horn et al.104 studied the impact of training dart throwing with
and amateur golfers was found.93 The findings of Dalton and Dalton variable and predictable target presentation patterns and found
et al. suggest that there may be a relationship between ocular dom- that individuals who trained using variable targets had significantly
inance and the duration of the quiet eye, but more research in this longer quiet eyes than individuals who trained using predictable
area is needed. Binocular visual functions such as depth percep- targets. Interestingly though, performance accuracy was not differ-
tion94 and ocular dominance95 are thought to be important for per- ent between the two training groups despite the variable target
formance in many sports, although the role of ocular dominance in training group having a longer quiet eye duration. Based on these
sports performance is more heavily contested. If relationships be- findings, Horn et al. suggested that the quiet eye may be related
tween binocular vision functions and gaze behaviors do exist, they to the programming demands of a specific task rather than simply
could provide additional insight into the role of gaze behaviors and a factor mediating accuracy by minimizing distractions. More re-
vision strategy in sports performance. cently, Horn and Marchetto105 demonstrated that quiet eye du-
There is a substantial amount of recent research demonstrating ration was shorter when targets were precisely precued, again
that the optimal timing of the quiet eye (early or late) varies across suggesting that quiet eye duration may be related to program-
tasks. For example, Klostermann et al.57 studied intermediate and ming demands on specific tasks.
highly skilled basketball players in defended and undefended In a novel study conducted by Klostermann et al., the duration of
game situations and found that an earlier quiet eye fixation onset the quiet eye was experimentally manipulated during a throwing
was linked to success. Conversely, Vine et al.45 found that a later task. During the task, the target was presented with different timings

www.optvissci.com Optom Vis Sci 2021; Vol 98(7) 734

Copyright © American Academy of Optometry. Unauthorized reproduction of this article is prohibited.


The Quiet Eye in Sport Performance — Dalton

(short and long) at different locations (random and predictable) from the target earlier in the final shot phase compared with nov-
while the throwing movement was occurring. This design allowed re- ices. Most recently, Vickers21 examined five different quiet eye pe-
searchers to modify the duration of the last fixation and the amount riods in defended and undefended basketball 3-point shots and
of information available to be processed by the participants on each found that shooting accuracy was enhanced when an early quiet
trial. Klostermann et al.106 found that longer quiet eye durations eye offset occurred before receiving the pass, an early saccade
were associated with performance but only on tasks with the highest was made to the target (the hoop), and a longer quiet eye fixation
information processing load. On tasks that were more predictable, directed toward the center of the hoop occurred during arm flexion.
Downloaded from http://journals.lww.com/optvissci by BhDMf5ePHKav1zEoum1tQfN4a+kJLhEZgbsIHo4XMi0hCyw

the effect of quiet eye duration was no longer apparent. Using a Vickers21 went on to state that it was never intended for just one
similar experimental paradigm, Klostermann et al.107 again dem- quiet eye period to be considered during a movement; rather,
onstrated that the quiet eye was only associated with performance movements could contain multiple quiet eye periods, and each
CX1AWnYQp/IlQrHD3i3D0OdRyi7TvSFl4Cf3VC1y0abggQZXdgGj2MwlZLeI= on 10/17/2023

enhancement in the conditions that required the most visual one of those periods should be isolated and examined in more de-
processing. tail to determine which quiet eye periods were associated with
As mentioned earlier, much of the traditional quiet eye research higher performance.
conducted to date has focused on a singular gaze behavior, despite When we consider of all the research on the quiet eye that has
a growing body of research suggesting that other gaze behaviors been published over the past four decades, where does that leave
may equally play critical roles in performance.20 However, the idea us? The quiet eye certainly seems to be important for sports perfor-
of multiple quiet eyes or multiple gaze behavior differences is not mance, and yet, equally, other aspects of athlete's vision strategies
new. In her original research article on golf, Vickers22 actually ex- also seem to be important for sports performance. Klostermann
amined a number of different gaze behaviors throughout the putt and Moeinirad60 recently completed a third meta-analysis investi-
and found that, in addition to having a longer quiet eye period, ex- gating gaze behavior differences, including fixation durations and
pert golfers spent more time looking at the ball than the hole, and locations, the total number of fixations, and quiet eye duration in
they more express saccades to shift their gaze between the ball expert and novice performers. Gaze location and quiet eye duration
and the hole faster than amateurs. Van Lier et al.,25 Dalton,26 were again found to be different in expert performers compared
and Naito et al.27 also found that the gaze behaviors associated with novice performers. However, in the analysis of fixation dura-
with putting success were much more complicated than the quiet tions and fixation locations, a very different pattern emerged; there
eye alone could explain. were more studies that found nonsignificant differences between
Likewise, research in both soccer and tennis has shown that ex- experts and novices compared with studies that found significant
pert and novices demonstrate multiple different gaze parameters, positive or negative results. Klostermann and Moeinirad also con-
including relatively more or less fixations, which are located at dif- ducted a publication analysis that demonstrated that the empirical
ferent places in the visual scene, at different time points in the evidence for general differences in gaze behavior between experts
action,28–32 and in some instances, no differences in expert-novice and novices has declined in recent years. Based on their findings,
gaze behaviors were found, suggesting a possible difference in visual Klostermann and Moeinirad cautioned against assuming that a dis-
perception between these groups instead.33,34 tinct difference is capable of differentiating performance expertise;
In her first basketball article, Vickers16 examined several differ- instead, perceptual-cognitive expertise differences should be as-
ent gaze and head movement behaviors as well. Experts were found sumed to be the result of expert perceptual performance. There-
to have a longer final fixation, a longer quiet eye fixation, and a lon- fore, although there certainly seem to be differences in athlete's
ger quiet eye offset. The quiet eye offset was defined as the dura- visual performance that appear to be related to their vision strate-
tion of time from the end of the quiet eye to the release of the gies, we still have much to learn. It seems the quiet eye is only
ball, and a longer quiet eye offset meant that experts looked away the beginning.

ARTICLE INFORMATION 3. Stine CD, Arterburn MR, Stern NS. Vision and Sports: 9. Hughes PK, Blundell NL, Walters JM. Visual and Psy-
A Review of the Literature. J Am Optom Assoc 1982;53: chomotor Performance of Elite, Intermediate and Nov-
Submitted: November 8, 2020 627–33. ice Table Tennis Competitors. Clin Exp Optom 1993;
Accepted: March 7, 2021 4. Hitzeman SA, Beckerman SA. What the Literature 76:51–60.
Says About Sports Vision. Optom Clin 1993;3: 10. Abernethy B. Enhancing Sports Performance
Funding/Support: The author has reported no funding/support.
145–69. through Clinical and Experimental Optometry. Clin Exp
Conflict of Interest Disclosure: The author does not have Optom 1986;69:189–96.
5. Laby DM, Rosenbaum AL, Kirschen DG, et al. The Vi-
reported a financial conflict of interest.
sual Function of Professional Baseball Players. Am J 11. Erickson GB. Sports Vision: Vision Care for the
Author Contributions: The author confirms sole responsibility Ophthalmol 1996;122:476–85. Enhancment of Sports Performance. St. Louis, MO:
for this work. Butterworth-Heinemann Elsevier; 2007.
6. Starkes JL, Deakin J. Perception in Sport: A Cognitive
Approach to Skilled Performance. In: Straub WF, 12. Laby DM, Kirschen DG, Govindarajulu U, et al. The
Williams JM, eds. Cognitive Sport Psychology. Lansing, Hand-eye Coordination of Professional Baseball Players:
REFERENCES NY: Sports Science Associates; 1984:115–28. The Relationship to Batting. Optom Vis Sci 2018;95:
7. Rothstein AL. The Perceptual Process, Vision and Motor 557–67.
1. Fullerton HS. Why Babe Ruth Is the Greatest Home Skills. In: Zichkowsky LD, Fuchs CZ, eds. The Psychology of 13. Laby DM, Kirschen DG, Govindarajulu U, et al. The
Run Hitter: Popular Science Monthly Tests in the Labo- Motor Behavior: Development, Control, Learning and Effect of Visual Function on the Batting Performance of
ratory His Brain, Eye, Ear and Muscle—and Gets His Se- Performance. Ithaca, NY: Movment Publications; Professional Baseball Players. Sci Rep 2019;9:16847.
cret. Pop Sci Monthly 1921;99:19. 1986:191–214. 14. Burris K, Liu S, Appelbaum L. Visual-motor Exper-
2. Sherman A. Overview of Research Information Re- 8. Abernethy B. Selective Attention in Fast Ball Sports: tise in Athletes: Insights from Semiparametric Modelling
garding Vision and Sports. J Am Optom Assoc 1980; II: Expert-novice Differences. Aust J Sci Med Sport of 2317 Athletes Tested on the Nike SPARQ Sensory
51:661–6. 1987;19:7–16. Station. J Sports Sci 2020;38:320–9.

www.optvissci.com Optom Vis Sci 2021; Vol 98(7) 735

Copyright © American Academy of Optometry. Unauthorized reproduction of this article is prohibited.


The Quiet Eye in Sport Performance — Dalton

15. Liu S, Edmunds FR, Burris K, et al. Visual and 35. Behan M, Wilson M. State Anxiety and Visual Atten- 53. van Lier WV, van der Kamp J, Savelsbergh G. Gaze in
Oculomotor Abilities Predict Professional Baseball tion: The Role of the Quiet Eye Period in Aiming to a Far Golf Putting: Effects of Slope. Int J Sport Psychol 2008;
Batting Performance. Int J Perform Anal Sport 2020; Target. J Sports Sci 2008;26:207–15. 41:160–76.
20:683–700. 36. Causer J, Holmes PS, Smith NC, et al. Anxiety, Move- 54. Williams AM, Singer RN, Frehlich SG. Quiet Eye Du-
16. Vickers JN. Visual Control when Aiming at a Far Tar- ment Kinematics, and Visual Attention in Elite-level Per- ration, Expertise, and Task Complexity in Near and Far
get. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 1996;22: formers. Emotion 2011;11:595–602. Aiming Tasks. J Mot Behav 2002;34:197–207.
342–54. 37. Janelle CM, Hillman CH, Apparies RJ, et al. Exper- 55. Panchuk D, Vickers JN, Hopkins WG. Quiet Eye Pre-
17. Vickers JN. Perception, Cognition and Decision tise Differences in Cortical Activation and Gaze Behavior dicts Goaltender Success in Deflected Ice Hockey
Downloaded from http://journals.lww.com/optvissci by BhDMf5ePHKav1zEoum1tQfN4a+kJLhEZgbsIHo4XMi0hCyw

Training: The Quiet Eye in Action. Champagn, IL: Hu- during Rifle Shooting. J Sport Exerc Psychol 2000;22: Shots. Eur J Sport Sci 2017;17:93–9.
man Kinetics Publishers; 2007. 167–82. 56. Klostermann A. Especial Skill vs. Quiet Eye Duration
18. Vickers JN. Advances in Coupling Perception and 38. Mann DT, Coombes SA, Mousseau MB, et al. Quiet in Basketball Free Throw: Evidence for the Inhibition of
CX1AWnYQp/IlQrHD3i3D0OdRyi7TvSFl4Cf3VC1y0abggQZXdgGj2MwlZLeI= on 10/17/2023

Action: The Quiet Eye as a Bidirectional Link between Eye and the Bereitschaftspotential: Visuomotor Mecha- Competing Task Solutions. Eur J Sport Sci 2019;19:
Gaze, Attention, and Action. Prog Brain Res 2009; nisms of Expert Motor Performance. Cogn Process 964–71.
174:279–88. 2011;12:223–34. 57. Klostermann A, Panchuk D, Farrow D. Perception-
19. Wilson MR, Pearcy RC. Visuomotor Control of 39. McPherson SL, Vickers JN. Cognitive Control in action Coupling in Complex Game Play: Exploring the
Straight and Breaking Golf Putts. Percept Mot Skills Motor Expertise. Int J Sport Exerc Psychol 2004;2: Quiet Eye in Contested Basketball Jump Shots. J Sports
2009;109:555–62. 274–300. Sci 2018;36:1054–60.

20. Gonzalez CC, Causer J, Miall RC, et al. Identifying 40. Panchuk D, Vickers JN. Gaze Behaviors of Goal- 58. Mann DT, Williams AM, Ward P, et al. Perceptual-
the Causal Mechanisms of the Quiet Eye. Eur J Sport tenders under Spatial-temporal Constraints. Hum Mov cognitive Expertise in Sport: A Meta-analysis. J Sport
Sci 2017;17:74–84. Sci 2006;25:733–52. Exerc Psychol 2007;29:457–78.

21. Vickers JN, Causer J, Vanhooren D. The Role of 41. Panchuk D, Vickers JN. Using Spatial Occlusion to 59. Lebeau JC, Liu S, Saenz-Moncaleano C, et al. Quiet
Quiet Eye Timing and Location in the Basketball Explore the Control Strategies Used in Rapid Interceptive Eye and Performance in Sport: A Meta-analysis. J Sport
Three-point Shot: A New Research Paradigm. Front Actions: Predictive or Prospective Control? J Sports Sci Exerc Psychol 2016;38:441–57.
Psychol 2019;10:2424. 2009;27:1249–60. 60. Klostermann A, Moeinirad S. Fewer Fixations of Lon-
42. Piras A, Vickers JN. The Effect of Fixation Transi- ger Duration? Expert Gaze Behavior Revisited. Ger J Exerc
22. Vickers JN. Gaze Control in Putting. Perception
tions on Quiet Eye Duration and Performance in the Soc- Sport Res 2020;50:146–61.
1992;21:117–32.
cer Penalty Kick: Instep versus Inside Kicks. Cogn 61. Wilson MR, Miles CA, Vine SJ, et al. Quiet Eye Dis-
23. Vickers JN. Control of Visual Attention during the
Process 2011;12:245–55. tinguishes Children of High and Low Motor Coordination
Basketball Free Throw. Am J Sports Med 1996;24:
43. Vickers JN, Adolphe RM. Gaze Behaviour during a Abilities. Med Sci Sports Exerc 2013;45:1144–51.
S93–7.
Ball Tracking and Aiming Skill. Int J Sports Vis 1997; 62. Wilson MR, McGrath JS, Vine SJ, et al. Perceptual
24. Martell SG, Vickers JN. Gaze Characteristics of Elite
4:19–28. Impairment and Psychomotor Control in Virtual Laparo-
and Near-elite Athletes in Ice Hockey Defensive Tactics.
44. Vickers JN, Williams AM. Performing under Pres- scopic Surgery. Surg Endosc 2011;25:2268–74.
Hum Mov Sci 2004;22:689–712.
sure: The Effects of Physiological Arousal, Cognitive 63. Wilson M, McGrath J, Vine S, et al. Psychomotor
25. Van Lier W, Van der Kamp J, Savelsbergh GJ. Gaze
Anxiety, and Gaze Control in Biathlon. J Mot Behav Control in a Virtual Laparoscopic Surgery Training Envi-
in Golf Putting: Effects of Task Complexity. Int J Sport
2007;39:381–94. ronment: Gaze Control Parameters Differentiate Novices
Psychol 2010;41:160–76.
45. Vine SJ, Lee D, Moore LJ, et al. Quiet Eye and Chok- from Experts. Surg Endosc 2010;24:2458–64.
26. Dalton K. The Vision Strategy of Golf Putting [PhD 64. Causer J, Vickers JN, Snelgrove R, et al. Perform-
ing: Online Control Breaks Down at the Point of Perfor-
Thesis]. Birmingham, UK: Aston University; 2013. ing under Pressure: Quiet Eye Training Improves Sur-
mance Failure. Med Sci Sports Exerc 2013;45:
Available at: https://research.aston.ac.uk/en/ gical Knot-tying Performance. Surgery 2014;156:
1988–94.
studentTheses/the-vision-strategy-of-golf-putting. 1089–96.
Accesed May 11, 2021. 46. Wilson MR, Vine SJ, Wood G. The Influence of
Anxiety on Visual Attentional Control in Basketball 65. Causer J, Harvey A, Snelgrove R, et al. Quiet Eye
27. Naito K, Kato T, Fukuda T. Expertise and Position of Free Throw Shooting. J Sport Exerc Psychol 2009; Training Improves Surgical Knot Tying More Than Tradi-
Line of Sight in Golf Putting. Percept Mot Skills 2004; 31:152–68. tional Technical Training: A Randomized Controlled
99:163–70. Study. Am J Surg 2014;208:171–7.
47. Janelle CM, Hillman CH. Concurrent Measurement
28. Williams AM, Davids K, Burwitz L, et al. Visual of Electroencephalographic and Ocular Indices of Atten- 66. Vickers JN, Lewinski W. Performing under Pressure:
Search Strategies in Experienced and Inexperienced tion during Rifle Shooting: An Exploratory Case Study. Gaze Control, Decision Making and Shooting Perfor-
Soccer Players. Res Q Exerc Sport 1994;65:127–35. Int J Sports Vis 2000;6:21–9. mance of Elite and Rookie Police Officers. Hum Mov
29. Singer RN, Williams AM, Frehlich SG, et al. New 48. Kim S, Lee S, Ryu D, et al. How Do National Bad-
Sci 2012;31:101–17.
Frontiers in Visual Search: An Exploratory Study in minton Players Utilize Advanced Visual Cues to Antici- 67. Adolphe R, Vickers JN, LaPlante G. The Effects of
Live Tennis Situations. Res Q Exerc Sport 1998; pate an Attacker's Intent? J Sport Exerc Psychol 2007; Training Visual Attention on Gaze Behaviour and Accu-
69:290–6. 29:S97. racy: A Pilot Study. Int J Sports Vis 1997;4:28–33.
30. Williams AM, Davids K. Visual Search Strategy, Se- 49. Lee SM, Kim S, Park SH. Self-paced Sport Events 68. Harle SK, Vickers JN. Training Quiet Eye Improves
lective Attention, and Expertise in Soccer. Res Q Exerc under Temporal Constraints: Visual Search, Quiet Eye, Accuracy in the Basketball Free Throw. Sport Psychol
Sport 1998;69:111–28. Expertise and Constrained Performance Time in Farm 2001;15:289–305.
31. Ward P, Williams AM, Bennett SJ. Visual Search and Aiming Tasks. Int J Appl Sports Sci 2009;21:146–61. 69. Oudejans RR, Koedijker JM, Bleijendaal I, et al. The
Biological Motion Perception in Tennis. Res Q Exerc 50. Martell SG. The Optimal Quiet Eye Period and the Education of Attention in Aiming at a Far Target: Train-
Sport 2002;73:107–12. Regulation of Visual Information in Compex Aiming ing Visual Control in Basketball Jump Shooting. Int J
32. Savelsbergh GJ, Van der Kamp J, Williams AM, et al. [Doctoral Thesis]. Calgary, Canada: University of Cal- Sport Psychol 2005;3:197–221.
Anticipation and Visual Search Behaviour in Expert Soc- gary; 2010. 70. Vine SJ, Moore LJ, Wilson MR. Quiet Eye Training
cer Goalkeepers. Ergonomics 2005;48:1686–97. 51. Nibbeling N, Oudejans RR, Daanen HA. Effects of Facilitates Competitive Putting Performance in Elite
33. Savelsbergh GJ, Williams AM, Van der Kamp J, et al. Anxiety, a Cognitive Secondary Task, and Expertise on Golfers. Front Psychol 2011;2:8.
Visual Search, Anticipation and Expertise in Soccer Gaze Behaviour and Performance in a Far Aiming Task. 71. Vine SJ, Wilson MR. Quiet Eye Training: Effects on
Goalkeepers. J Sports Sci 2002;20:279–87. Psychol Sport Exerc 2012;13:427–35. Learning and Performance under Pressure. J Appl Sport
34. Savelsbergh GJ, Onrust M, Rouwenhorst A, et al. Vi- 52. Park SH. The Change of Gaze Behaviour, Eye-head Psychol 2010;22:361–76.
sual Search and Locomotion Behaviour in a Four-to-four Coordination, and Temporal Characteristics of Swing 72. Causer J, Holmes PS, Williams AM. Quiet Eye Train-
Football Tactical Position Game. Int J Sport Psychol by Task Constraints in Tennis Volley Strokes. Int J Appl ing in a Visuomotor Control Task. Med Sci Sports Exerc
2006;37:248–64. Sports Sci 2005;17:51–70. 2011;43:1042–9.

www.optvissci.com Optom Vis Sci 2021; Vol 98(7) 736

Copyright © American Academy of Optometry. Unauthorized reproduction of this article is prohibited.


The Quiet Eye in Sport Performance — Dalton

73. Vine SJ, Moore LJ, Wilson MR. Quiet Eye Training: 84. Wilson M, Coleman M, McGrath J. Developing Basic 96. Vickers JN. Location of Fixation, Landing Position of
The Acquisition, Refinement and Resilient Performance Hand-eye Coordination Skills for Laparoscopic Surgery the Ball and Spatial Visual Attention during Free Throw
of Targeting Skills. Eur J Sport Sci 2012;1–8. Using Gaze Training. BJU Int 2010;105:1356–8. Shooting. Int J Sports Vis 1996;3:54–60.
74. Wood G, Wilson MR. Quiet-eye Training for Soccer 85. Vickers JN, Crews D. Short Term Memory Character- 97. Oudejans RR, van de Langenberg RW, Hutter RI.
Penalty Kicks. Cogn Process 2011;12:257–66. istics of Golfers: Concurrent Measures of Gaze and EEG. Aiming at a Far Target under Different Viewing Condi-
In: Science and Golf IV: Proceedings of the World Con- tions: Visual Control in Basketball Jump Shooting.
75. Vine SJ, Wilson MR. The Influence of Quiet Eye
gress of Science of Golf, St. Andrews, Scotland, July Hum Mov Sci 2002;21:457–80.
Training and Pressure on Attention and Visuo-motor
Control. Acta Psychol (Amst) 2011;136:340–6. 22–26, 2002. St. Andrews, Scotland; 2002. 98. de Oliveira RF, Oudejans RR, Beek PJ. Late Infor-
Downloaded from http://journals.lww.com/optvissci by BhDMf5ePHKav1zEoum1tQfN4a+kJLhEZgbsIHo4XMi0hCyw

86. Muraskin J, Dodhia S, Lieberman G, et al. Brain Dy- mation Pick-up Is Preferred in Basketball Jump Shoot-
76. Moore LJ, Vine SJ, Cooke A, et al. Quiet Eye Training
namics of Post-task Resting State Are Influenced by Ex- ing. J Sports Sci 2006;24:933–40.
Expedites Motor Learning and Aids Performance under
Heightened Anxiety: The Roles of Response Program- pertise: Insights from Baseball Players. Hum Brain 99. de Oliveira RF, Huys R, Oudejans RR, et al. Basket-
CX1AWnYQp/IlQrHD3i3D0OdRyi7TvSFl4Cf3VC1y0abggQZXdgGj2MwlZLeI= on 10/17/2023

ming and External Attention. Psychophysiology 2012; Mapp 2016;37:4454–71. ball Jump Shooting Is Controlled Online by Vision.
49:1005–15. 87. Gallicchio G, Cooke A, Ring C. Assessing Ocular Ac- Exp Psychol 2007;54:180–6.
77. Vine SJ, Moore LJ, Wilson MR. Quiet Eye Training: tivity during Performance of Motor Skills Using Electro- 100. de Oliveira RF, Oudejans RR, Beek PJ. Gaze Behav-
The Acquisition, Refinement and Resilient Performance oculography. Psychophysiology 2018;55:e13070. ior in Basketball Shooting: Further Evidence for Online Vi-
of Targeting Skills. Eur J Sport Sci 2014;14(Suppl. 1): 88. Eysenck MW, Derakshan N, Santos R, et al. Anxiety sual Control. Res Q Exerc Sport 2008;79:399–404.
S235–42. and Cognitive Performance: Attentional Control Theory. 101. Moeinirad S, Abdoli B, Farsi A, et al. The Role of
78. Wood G, Wilson MR. Quiet Eye Training, Perceived Emotion 2007;7:336–53. Quiet Eye Duration and Its Components in a Complex
Control and Performing under Pressure. Psychol Sport Far-aiming Task. J Motor Learn Dev 2020;8:516–27.
89. Vickers JN. Neuroscience of the Quiet Eye in Golf
Exerc 2012;13:721–8. Putting. Int J Golf Sci 2012;1:2–9. 102. Lee DN. A Theory of Visual Control of Braking
79. Vickers JN, Vandervies B, Kohut C, et al. Quiet Eye Based on Information About Time-to-collision. Percep-
90. Vickers JN, Williams AM. The Role of Mental Pro-
Training Improves Accuracy in Basketball Field Goal tion 1976;5:437–59.
cesses in Elite Sports Performance. Cogn Psychol
Shooting. Prog Brain Res 2017;234:1–12. 2017;1–25. 103. Lee DN. General Tau Theory: Evolution to Date.
80. Norouzi E, Hosseini FS, Vaezmosavi M, et al. Effect Perception 2009;38:837–50.
91. Klostermann A, Kredel R, Hossner EJ. On the Inter-
of Quiet Eye and Quiet Mind Training on Motor Learning
action of Attentional Focus and Gaze: The Quiet Eye In- 104. Horn RR, Okumura MS, Alexander MG, et al. Quiet
among Novice Dart Players. Motor Control 2019;24: Eye Duration Is Responsive to Variability of Practice and
hibits Focus-related Performance Decrements. J Sport
204–21. to the Axis of Target Changes. Res Q Exerc Sport 2012;
Exerc Psychol 2014;36:392–400.
81. Miles CA, Wood G, Vine SJ, et al. Quiet Eye Train- 83:204–11.
92. Software for Determing the Quiet Eye. Quiet Eye So-
ing Aids the Long-term Learning of Throwing and 105. Horn RR, Marchetto JD. Approximate Target
lutions. Available at: http://www.quieteyesolutions.
Catching in Children: Preliminary Evidence for a Pre- Pre-cueing Reduces Programming Quiet Eye and Move-
com/. Accessed October 31, 2020.
dictive Control Strategy. Eur J Sport Sci 2017;17: ment Preparation Time: Evidence for Parameter Pre-
100–8. 93. Dalton K, Guillon M, Naroo S. Ocular Dominance programming? Res Q Exerc Sport 2020;1–9.
and Handedness in Golf Putting. Optom Vis Sci 2015;
82. Miles CA, Wood G, Vine SJ, et al. Quiet Eye Training 106. Klostermann A, Kredel R, Hossner EJ. The “Quiet
92:968–75.
Facilitates Visuomotor Coordination in Children with De- Eye” and Motor Performance: Task Demands Matter!
velopmental Coordination Disorder. Res Dev Disabil 94. Kirschen D, Laby D. The Role of Sports Vision in Eye J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 2013;39:1270–8.
2015;40:31–41. Care Today. Eye Contact Lens 2011;37:127–30.
107. Klostermann A, Kredel R, Hossner EJ. The Quiet
83. Miles CAL, Vine SJ, Wood G, et al. Quiet Eye Train- 95. Laby DM, Kirschen DG. Thoughts on Ocular Eye without a Target: The Primacy of Visual Information
ing Improves Throw and Catch Performance in Children. Dominance—Is It Actually a Preference? Eye Contact Processing. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 2014;
Psychol Sport Exerc 2014;15:511–5. Lens 2011;37:140–4. 40:2167–78.

www.optvissci.com Optom Vis Sci 2021; Vol 98(7) 737

Copyright © American Academy of Optometry. Unauthorized reproduction of this article is prohibited.

You might also like