You are on page 1of 8

Journal of Applied Psychology In the public domain

1992, Vol. 77, No. 6, 910-917

Gender and the Relationship Between Perceived Fairness


of Pay or Promotion and Job Satisfaction
L. Alan Witt and Lendell G. Nye
Federal Aviation Administration Civil Aeromedical Institute
Oklahoma City, Oklahoma

Brockner and Adsit (1986) found that satisfaction with an exchange relationship was more strongly
related to perceptions of equity among men than women. Kahn (1972) reported that men were
more likely than women to distribute outcomes to individuals in direct proportion to their input.
We evaluated potential gender differences among 12,979 personnel in 30 different organizational
systems in (a) correlations between fairness and job satisfaction scores and (b) standardized group
differences in the perceived amounts of pay and promotion fairness and expressed levels of facet
and global job satisfaction. The fairness-satisfaction relationship was not higher for men, and there
were no practical differences in fairness perceptions and job satisfaction between men and women.

Much of the research on employee job satisfaction has been Mottaz, 1986). Some researchers have reported no gender dif-
based on the explicit assumptions that (a) job satisfaction is a ferences in overall job satisfaction (Fry & Greenfeld, 1980;
potential determinant of absenteeism, turnover, in-role job per- Sauser & York, 1978; D. B. Smith & Plant, 1982; Weaver, 1980),
formance, and extra-role behaviors and (b) the primary anteced- whereas others have reported relationships between job satisfac-
ents of job attitudes are within management's ability to influ- tion and gender (DArcy, Syrotiuk, & Siddique, 1984; Forgionne
ence. Accompanying the latter assumption has been a notion & Peelers, 1982; Hulin & Smith, 1965; Mannheim, 1983;
that managers should perhaps employ different strategies to Murray & Atkinson, 1981; Shapiro & Stern, 1975; P. C. Smith,
promote job satisfaction among women and men because orga- Kendall, & Hulin, 1969; Stockford & Kunze, 1950; Varca,
nizational issues are supposedly differentially salient to men Shaffer, & McCauley, 1983; Weaver, 1974).
and women in the development of their job satisfaction. Brun- Why would individuals receiving less pay and differential
ing and Snyder (1983) pointed out that different management opportunities not be less satisfied with their jobs? This question
strategies may be useful when gender differences exist but may is at the heart of what is called the "paradox of the contented
lead to discrimination when they do not. In the present study, working woman" (Jackson, 1989). In their review of the litera-
we investigated possible gender differences in the levels of, and ture on gender and job values, Nieva and Gutek (1981) con-
relationships between, perceptions of fairness of pay and pro- cluded that women value pay and promotions less and interper-
motion and feelings of job satisfaction among men and women sonal relationships more than men do. However, subsequent
working in several different organizations. As did Deaux research conducted among men and women in similar occupa-
(1985), we discuss non-biologically based differences between tions indicated no gender differences in valuing money
men and women in terms of gender rather than sex. (Crosby, 1982; Golding, Resnick, & Crosby, 1983; Harris &
After reviewing 21 studies of gender differences in job satis- Earle, 1986). This issue has practical significance; namely, if
faction, Herzberg, Mausner, Peterson, and Capwell (1957, p. 13) women value money less than men do, then money-related
suggested that "studies comparing men and women in job satis- issues may have less salience for women. For example, per-
faction do not lead to any simple conclusions about such differ- ceived fairness of pay and promotion may be of less importance
ences." Little has changed over the years. Despite differential in the development of job satisfaction for women.
opportunities and pay inequity (Crosby, 1982), relationships be- Experimental research testing equity theory predictions
tween gender and job satisfaction have been inconsistent (cf. (Adams, 1963) has primarily focused on two areas of distribu-
tive justice behaviors—reward allocation and reactions to in-
justice (Major & Deaux, 1982). Studies of reward allocation as a
An earlier version of this article was presented as a poster at the function of effort generally support equity theory predictions
Sixth Annual Conference of the Society for Industrial and Organiza- that individuals make allocations on the basis of perceptions of
tional Psychology, April 26,1991, St. Louis. others' efforts (Greenberg & Cohen, 1982). Studies examining
We gratefully acknowledge (a) the significant suggestions made by reactions to injustice indicate that, consistent with equity
Larry Roth on an earlier draft of this article, (b) the helpful comments
theory predictions, people are less satisfied with, and are more
made by two anonymous reviewers, Carolyn Dollar, Jennifer Myers,
and Dave Schroeder on earlier drafts of this article, and (c) the produc-
motivated to change, inequitable conditions than equitable
tion assistance of Chan M. Hellman. conditions (Walster, Walster, & Berscheid, 1978). However, em-
Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to L. pirical research (cf. Major & Deaux, 1982) suggests that gender
Alan Witt, Federal Aviation Administration Civil Aeromedical Insti- may moderate reactions to inequity (unfairness). Men are more
tute (AAM-522), P. Q Box 25082, Oklahoma City, Oklahoma 73125. likely than women to distribute outcomes to individuals in
910
PERCEIVED FAIRNESS AND JOB SATISFACTION 911

direct proportion to their input (Kahn, 1972; Lane & Messe, Method
1971; Leventhal & Lane, 1970), whereas women are more likely
to use the equality norm. Some research suggests that men and
Subjects
women may differ in their interaction goals, with women striv- A number of populations were sampled. AH subjects voluntarily re-
ing for interpersonal or social success and men striving for ex- sponded by mail to organizational surveys. The sample sizes and per-
ploitive, competitive success (e.g., Kahn, O'Leary, Krulewitz, & centages reflecting the response rates for each organization are pre-
Lamm, 1980). However, more recent studies have found that sented in Table 1. Sample 1 comprised employees (357 staff, 244 fac-
women allocate more generously than men even when they are ulty, and 128 administrators) of Western Illinois University, measured
in 1987. Sample 2 consisted of workers at a military research and devel-
matched in terms of valuing interpersonal success (Major &
opment laboratory in the southwestern United States, measured in
Adams, 1983; Swap & Rubin, 1983). It also has been argued that 1988. Sample 3 comprised employees at a small printing plant in the
whereas men may tend to identify effort and skill as relevant midwestern United States, measured in 1989. Sample 4 consisted of the
inputs, women may tend to identify participation as more rele- nonmedical employees of a small hospital in the midwestern United
vant (Walster & Walster, 1975). States, measured in 1989. Sample 5 comprised school teachers in a
Kahn, Nelson, and Gaeddert (1980) argued that gender dif- rural school district in the midwestern United States, measured in
ferences most likely occur when situational demands are ambig- 1987. Sample 6 consisted of workers at a tool factory in the Serbian
uous or weak. In such circumstances, men and women may region of Yugoslavia, measured in 1988.
AH remaining samples were taken from populations of employees of
attempt to modify such situations according to their interper- the Federal Aviation Administration (FAA). Sample 7 comprised
sonal orientation. Such a gender difference would have impli- flight service specialists participating in an FAA training program and
cations in work groups and in supervisor-subordinate interac- was measured in 1989 (Broach, 1989). Sample 8 consisted of air traffic
tions. For example, female managers may be using the equality controllers employed in airport towers who were participating in the
norm when assigning work tasks and appraising performance, same FAA training program. Sample 9 comprised controllers em-
and male managers may be using the equity norm. ployed in en route air traffic control centers who were also participat-
Brockner and Adsit (1986) noted that an important but un- ing in the same FAA training program. Samples 10-19 and 20-30 con-
tested implication of a greater salience of the equity norm for sisted of FAA employees who participated in the 1988 (Myers,
Schroeder, Van Deventer, & Collins, 1988) and 1990 (Myers, Witt, &
men is that men's satisfaction with an exchange relationship Schroeder, 1991) FA A Job Satisfaction Survey administrations, respec-
may be influenced more by the presence or absence of equity tively. Employees participating in these surveys were placed in their
than is women's satisfaction. This is consistent with Nieva and respective FAA organizations, which are listed in Table 1. The division
Gutek's (1981) literature review suggesting that women value of FAA personnel into separate organizations reflects not only the
pay and promotions less than men do. Testing this notion, structure of the FAA and its separate management systems but also the
Brockner and Adsit (1986) conducted a study of 51 persons in very different tasks performed and different cultures of these various
three clubs, one of which comprised 14 women, another of organizations under the FAA umbrella.
which mainly comprised men (16 men and 4 women), and the
third of which comprised 10 men and 7 women. Brockner and Measures
Adsit reported that the equity-satisfaction relationship was con-
siderably stronger among men than among women. The rela- Job satisfaction. Responses to multiple facet items of job satisfac-
tion and to one global item were assessed in all but two of the samples.
tionship was pronounced among men in the mostly male group
The rationale for this strategy stemmed from evidence suggesting that
and especially weak for the group composed only of women. (a) specific satisfaction measures "better reflect changes in relevant
Brockner and Adsit's (1986) finding has an important impli- situational factors because of the more precise referent" (Gerhart,
cation for organizational theory, namely, that fairness percep- 1987, p. 371); (b) responses to a global item (i.e., an item that does not
tions may be more salient in the development of job satisfaction specify with regard to what referent the rater is to express his or her job
among men than among women. The conceptual link between satisfaction) may more likely reflect individual differences than re-
satisfaction with an exchange relationship (as studied by sponses to specific items (Ajzen & Fishbein, 1977); and (c) single items
Brockner and Adsit) and job satisfaction (as studied here) is that may not be as unreliable as previously thought (Scarpello & Campbell,
job satisfaction may reflect the individual's affective response 1983). Thus, the use of total facet and global measures provided the
opportunity to examine gender differences operating in response to
to the exchange relationship between the individual and the
both context-specific and context-free cues.
organization. The purpose of the present study was to evaluate Three items were presented on a 7-point Likert-type scale (1 =
Brockner and Adsit's (1986) findings across a variety of work strongly disagree, 7 = strongly agree) to subjects in Sample 1: (a) "I am
organizations and different measures of job satisfaction and satisfied with my work," (b) "I am satisfied with my pay" and (c) "I am
perceived fairness. We tested the hypotheses that (a) gender satisfied with my job overall." The sum of these items comprised the
would moderate the relationship between job satisfaction and total facet score, and the third item constituted the global score.
the perceived fairness of pay and promotion and (b) the levels of Subjects in Sample 2 indicated on 16 items the extent to which they
positive perceptions of fairness and expressions of job satisfac- were satisfied (1 = very dissatisfied, 7 = very satisfied) with (a) "the
tion would be higher for men. Confirmation would provide fringe benefits you receive" (Schnake, 1983), (b) "the amount of free-
some support for the argument that differential management dom you have on your job," (c) "the chances you have to accomplish
something worthwhile" (Schnake, 1983), (d) "your family's attitude to-
strategies might be necessary and justifiable. However, a failure ward your job" (Hendrix, 1979), (e) "the chances you have to take part
to find gender differences would be consistent with research in making decisions" (Schnake, 1983), (f) "the first work assignment
indicating no gender differences in such areas as valuing pay you received upon arrival here," (g) "the physical layout of your work
(e.g., Harris & Earle, 1986). station," (h) "the quality of technical equipment that you use" (Keller,
912 L. ALAN WITT AND LENDELL G. NYE

Table 1
Sample Sizes and Response Rates
Response
No. of rate
Sample respondents (%)

1: 1987, university employees 729 40.95


2: 1988, military research workers 76 83.33
3: 1989, printers 55 52.38
4: 1989, nonmedical hospital employees 75 48.88
5: 1987, schoolteachers 65 60.19
6: 1988, Serbian tool factory workers 134 67.00
7: 1989, FAA flight service specialists 159 —
8: 1989, FAA tower and terminal air traffic controllers 797 —
9: 1989, FAA en route air traffic controllers 903 —
10: 1988, FAA accounting staff 53 63.86
11: 1988, FAA air traffic controllers 2,290 69.16
12: 1988, FAA aircraft certification staff 110 57.56
13: 1988, FAA airport workers 49 71.73
14: 1988, FAA airway facilities workers 1,204 71.04
15: 1988, FAA flight standards staff 522 81.92
16: 1988, FAA human resources management staff 76 69.14
17: 1988, FAA logistics staff 203 60.47
18: 1988, FAA computer operations staff 95 52.20
19: 1988, FAA secruity staff 49 64.94
20: 1990, FAA accounting staff 53 84.71
21: 1990, FAA air traffic controllers 2,868 67.00
22: 1990, FAA aircraft certification staff 94 43.00
23: 1990, FAA airport workers 55 48.00
24: 1 990, FAA airway facilities workers 1,270 73.50
25: 1990, FAA aviation standards staff 95 54.60
26: 1990, FAA flight standards staff 521 69.30
27: 1990, FAA human resources management staff 94 48.87
28: 1990, FAA logistics staff 178 64.07
29: 1990, FAA computer operations staff 58 33.47
30: 1990, FAA security staff 58 73.08
Note. FAA = Federal Aviation Administration. Response rates for Samples 7,8, and 9 were not determined.

1986), (i) "the friendliness of the people you work with" (Schnake, Manley, 1978) version of Hoppock's (1935) four-item job satisfaction
1983), (j) "the chances you have to learn new things" (Schnake, 1983), scale. No item was used as a global measure.
(k) "the amount of pay you get" (Schnake, 1983), (1) "the way you are Subjects in Sample 6 completed five items presented with 7-point
treated by the people you work with" (Schnake, 1983), (m) "the amount Likert-type scales (-3 = strongly disagree, 3 = strongly agree): (a) "I am
of job security you have" (Schnake, 1983), (n) "the work itself (what you very satisfied with my pay," (b) "I am very satisfied with my supervi-
do)," (o) "the local area (weather, things to do, community size, etc.)" sor," (c) "I am dissatisfied with my co-workers" (reverse scored), (d) "I
(Hendrix, 1979), and (p) "your job as a whole" (Hendrix, 1979). The am satisfied with the opportunities for promotion at this factory" and
sum of these items comprised the total facet score, and the last item (e) "I am very happy doing the work that I do." No item was used as a
constituted the global score. global measure. Items were printed in Serbian.
Subjects in Samples 3 and 4 completed the following items presented Subjects in Samples 7,8, and 9 completed five items used in previous
on a 7-point, Likert-type scale (1 = extremely dissatisfied, 1 = extremely federal government surveys (Office of Personnel Management, 1979),2
satisfied) and taken from the Job Content Satisfaction section of Hack- presented on a 5-point Likert-type scale (1 = strongly disagree, 5 =
man and Oldham's (1974,1980) Job Diagnostic Survey: (a) "the amount strongly agree) on which the employees indicated their agreement with
of job security I have," (b) "the amount of pay and fringe benefits I (a) "All in all, I am satisfied with my pay," (b) "I am satisfied with the
receive," (c) "the amount of personal growth and development I get in chances of getting a promotion," (c) "I am satisfied with the amount of
doing my job," (d) "the people I talk to and work with on my job," (e) job security I have," and (d) "All in all, I am satisfied with my work
"the feeing of worthwhile accomplishment I get from doing my job," group." The sum of these items comprised the total facet score. An
(f) "the chance to get to know other people while on the job," (g) "the additional item, "In general, I am satisfied with my job" comprised
amount of support and guidance I receive from my supervisor," (h) the global score.
"the amount of independent thought and action I can exercise in my
job," (i) "how secure things look for me in the future here," (j) "the ' From The Job Diagnostic Survey: An Instrument for the Diagnosis
chance to help other people while at work," (k) "the amount of chal- of Jobs and Evaluation of Job Redesign Projects (Tech. Rep. 4) by J. R.
lenge in my job" and (1) "the overall quality of supervision I receive in Hackman and G. R. Oldham, 1974, Yale University, New Haven, CT.
my work."' The total score on these items comprised the total facet Copyright held by authors. Reprinted with permission.
2
score. An additional item from Hackman and Oldham's scale, "my job From Federal Employee Attitudes: Phase 1. Baseline Survey 1979
overall," constituted the global score. (pp. 12-19) by the Office of Personnel Management, 1979, Washing-
Subjects in Sample 5 completed the validated (McNichols, Stahl, & ton, DC: US. Government Printing Office. In the public domain.
PERCEIVED FAIRNESS AND JOB SATISFACTION 913

In Samples 10-30, subjects were asked to complete the following by sample size and averaged (r). Second, the observed variances of the
items presented on a 5-point, Likert-type scale (1 = very dissatisfied, 5 = correlations were calculated and corrected for the variance expected
very satisfied): (a) "Overall, how satisfied are you with your pay?" (b) due to sampling error. A 90% lower credibility value (LCV), the point
"Overall, how satisfied are you with your benefits?" (c) "Overall, how above which 90% of correlations were found, was determined from the
satisfied are you with your retirement system?" (d) "In general, how corrected standard deviation (SDC). Third, standard errors for the
satisfied are you with your job—the kind of work you do?" (e) "How mean of the sample-size-weighted correlations were calculated with
satisfied are you with your physical working conditions (e.g., space, Schmidt, Hunter, and Raju's (1988) formulas and used to create confi-
noise, light, heat, privacy)?" (f) "Overall, how satisfied are you with dence intervals (CI) as estimates of the accuracy and magnitude of each
your work group?" (g) "How satisfied are you with the quality of feed- mean correlation. An LCV that does not include zero has been inter-
back you receive from your supervisor?" (h) "In general, how satisfied preted (Whitener, 1990) as indicating that an effect was generalizable
are you with your supervisor?" (i) "Overall, how satisfied are you with across studies, in which case there were no important moderator influ-
your organization as a place to work?" and (j) "Overall, how satisfied ences and the mean correlation represented the population effect size
are you with the management of your organization?". The sum of these (the homogeneous case). With homogeneous studies, the formula for
items comprised the total facet score. An additional item, "Overall, calculating the standard error, which was used to form the confidence
how satisfied are you with your job?" comprised the global score. interval, accounts only for sampling error (Schmidt et al., 1988). In
Means, standard deviations, and Cronbach's alphas for the facet scales contrast, for analyses in which moderators are indicated (the heteroge-
are available on request. neous case), the formula for the standard error reflects both sampling
Fairness. Subjects in Sample 1 completed Scholl, Cooper, and error and residual variance after sampling error has been removed. For
McKenna's (1987) pay equity scale <pt = .92). Subjects in Sample 2 com- this study, confidence intervals around the mean of the sample-size-
pleted an item presented on a 7-point Likert-type scale (1 = strongly weighted correlations were calculated with both formulas because the
disagree, 7 = strongly agree), "My pay is fair." Subjects in Samples 3 and observed variances of correlations were corrected only for the artifact
4 completed one item taken from the Job Content Satisfaction section of sampling error. Thus, the corrected standard deviations were overes-
of Hackman and Oldham's (1974,1980) Job Diagnostic Survey, "the timates of the true variances because we did not correct for other
degree to which I am fairly paid for what I contribute to this organiza- artifacts, such as measurement error in the job satisfaction and fairness
tion." Subjects in Sample 5 completed two items presented on a 5- constructs.
point, Likert-type scale (1 = strongly disagree, 5 = strongly agree), "The The value of d for each of the organizations was calculated by sub-
pay increases and promotions that teachers receive here are based on tracting the mean of a measure (e.g., perceived promotion fairness) for
what the teacher puts into his/her work," and "Pay raises are based on women from the mean of that measure for men in the same organiza-
whom you know" (reverse scored). Subjects in Sample 6 completed two tion and then dividing the difference by the within-group standard
items printed in Serbian and presented on a 7-point Likert-type scale deviation. Thus, positive d values represent higher perceived fairness
(—3 = strongly disagree, 3 = strongly agree): (a) "The promotions that or job satisfaction for men in an organization. The mean of the gender
workers receive here are based on what the worker puts into his/her group differences across all organizations (d), based on sample-size-
work," and (b) "Promotions are based on whom you know" (reverse weighted effects, was calculated for each of the fairness and job satisfac-
scored). FAA subjects completed items assessing both pay and promo- tion measures. The observed variance of each J was corrected for sam-
tion fairness. Subjects in Samples 7,8, and 9 completed two items used pling error. A 95% credibility interval was constructed around each
in previous federal government surveys (Office of Personnel Manage- effect size as an indication of the generalizability of gender group
ment, 1979) presented on a 5-point Likert-type scale (1 = strongly dis- differences across the organizations in this study. The practical signifi-
agree, 5 = strongly agree) on which the employees indicated their agree- cance of the magnitude of each effect size was evaluated with the "bino-
ment with (a) "Considering my skills and the effort I put into my work, I mial effect size display" (BESD; Rosenthal & Rubin, 1982) and Cohen's
am satisfied with my pay" and (b) "Promotions or unscheduled pay (1977) categories of effect sizes: .20 = small, .50 = medium, and .80 =
increases here depend on how well a person performs his or her job." large.
Subjects in Samples 10-30 completed two items on a 5-point Likert-
type scale (1 = not at all, 5 = to a very great extent): (a) "To what extent Results
does the FAA pay well, compared to other places?" and (b) "To what
extent are promotions in your organization given to those who are best Inconsistent with the first hypothesis, for each type of fair-
qualified?" Means and standard deviations are available on request. ness and job satisfaction relationship, the observed mean corre-
lations (Table 2) across work organizations were very similar for
men and women: pay fairness with facet satisfaction (for men,
Procedure r= .44 and for women, r = .39), pay fairness with global job
We used meta-analysis procedures (Hunter & Schmidt, 1990) to eval- satisfaction (for men, r = .29 and for women, r = .23), promotion
uate gender differences in (a) correlation coefficients between job satis- fairness with facet satisfaction (for men, r = .54 and for women,
faction and fairness scores and (b) levels of fairness and job satisfaction r = .59), and promotion fairness with global job satisfaction (for
as operationalized in standardized group differences (^statistic) in the men, r = .42 and for women, r = .45). Although more variance
perceived amounts of pay fairness and promotion fairness and the was indicated across sets of correlation coefficients than was
expressed levels of both facet and global job satisfaction. We used the accounted for by sampling error variance, the LCVs for all rela-
meta-analytic approach because of the many different measures tionships were well above zero, which indicated considerable
within our study. Our primary objectives were to estimate the magni-
tudes of the effect sizes, in terms of both rand d for men and women,
consistency across organizations. The confidence intervals for
and to examine how generalizable the results were across the organiza- the accuracy of the correlations between each of the fairness-
tions. job satisfaction measures were significantly different from zero
Initially, analyses were performed for each of the four sets of fair- and overlapped for the subsets based on gender. This last result
ness-job satisfaction associations, and then the correlation coeffi- was found both when the standard error was calculated with
cients were grouped into subsets based on gender. The procedures the formula for the homogeneous case and when the formula
comprised three steps: First, the observed correlations were weighted for the heterogeneous case was applied.
914 L. ALAN WITT AND LENDELL G. NYE

Table 2
Meta-Analysis of Relationships Between Fairness Perceptions and Job Satisfaction Measures
Relationship/ Observed
group K n r Range variance 5£>c LCV CI, CI2
Pay fairness and
facet satisfaction
Total 56 12,780 .45 -.14 to .74 .004 .04 .37 .41 -.45 .40-.46
Men 28 10,179 .44 .10 to .74 .003 .03 .40 .42-.46 .41 -.47
Women 28 2,601 .39 -.14 to .68 .009 .03 .35 .3S-.43 .34-.44
Pay fairness and
global job satisfaction
Total 56 12,780 .28 -.16 to .74 .007 .06 .20 .26-.30 .27-3 1
Men 28 10,179 .29 -.04 to .58 .003 .02 .26 .27-.31 .26-.32
Women 28 2,601 .23 -.16 to .74 .002 .10 .10 .19-.27 .15-.3I
Promotion fairness and
facet satisfaction
Total 52 12,053 .55 .20 to .81 .021 .14 .37 .53-.S7 .50-.60
Men 26 9,784 .54 .20 to .70 .022 .14 .36 .52-.S6 .47-.61
Women 26 2,269 .59 .29 to .81 .014 .09 .47 .5S-.62 .S3-.63
Promotion fairness and
global job satisfaction
Total 48 11,854 .43 .15 to .83 .011 .09 .31 .41 -.45 J9-.47
Men 24 9,658 .42 .15 to .66 .012 .10 .29 .40-.44 .36-.4S
Women 24 2,196 .45 .17 to .83 .008 .03 .41 .42-.4S .40-. 50
Note. K = number of correlations; n = sample size; r = mean of correlations weighted by sample size; SDC
= standard deviation of correlation coefficients corrected for sampling error; LCV = 90% lower credibility
value (the point above which 90% of corrected correlations are found); CI, = confidence interval calculated
with the formula for the standard error given the lack of important moderators (the homogeneous case);
CI2 = confidence interval calculated with the formula for the standard error based on both sampling error
and residual variance (the heterogeneous case).

Within gender subsets, some differences in variability were the relationships between fairness perceptions and job satisfac-
found across organizations for three of the fairness-job satisfac- tion for men and women.
tion associations. For pay fairness with global job satisfaction, As shown in Table 3, the results of the analysis of gender
more variability was found for women (SDC = .10) than for men group differences in perceptions of fairness and levels of ex-
(SDC = .02) across 28 organizations. However, for promotion pressed job satisfaction indicate that the men had lower posi-
fairness with facet and global job satisfaction, more variability tive perceptions of both pay and promotion fairness. However,
was indicated for men (SDC = .14 and .10, respectively) than for the gender group differences involving pay fairness (d= -.18)
women (SDC = .09 and .03, respectively). The exception was the and promotion fairness (d = -.17) were lower than Cohen's
association between pay fairness and facet satisfaction: SDC (1977) criterion for a small effect size, namely, an absolute value
equaled .03 for both men and women. These findings of nontri- of .20. Furthermore, the lack of practical gender group differ-
vial corrected variance in the subsets of correlations suggests ences on the two fairness measures was indicated by BESD
the possibility that unknown moderators differentially affected values, which estimated that only about 54% of the women had

Table 3
Gender Group Differences for Fairness Perceptions and Satisfaction
95%
Range BESD credibility
Measure K d ofd (Women) interval of d
Pay fairness 28 -.18 -.48 to .28 54.4% -.32 to -.04
Promotion fairness 26 -.17 -.49 to .55 54.1% -.31 to -.02
Facet job satisfaction 30 -.12 -.81 to .56 53.5% -.26 to .03
Global job satisfaction 28 -.03 -.53 to .50 50.9% -.11 to. 13
Note. The credibility interval is calculated from the observed variance of the effect sizes corrected for
sampling error. The negative effect sizes reflect less perceived fairness and job satisfaction reported by
men. The BESD is the estimated percentage ofwomen above the estimated population mean on a given
measure. K = number of group comparisons; d = mean effect size across organizations; BESD = bino-
mial effect size display.
PERCEIVED FAIRNESS AND JOB SATISFACTION 915

positive perceptions at a level above the population means. The tations (Sauser & York, 1978) than men, perhaps because of
gender group differences on the facet and global measures of past wage discrimination. Second, others (Berger, Zelditch,
job satisfaction were even smaller (d= —.12 and d= —.03, re- Anderson, & Cohen, 1972; Chesler & Goodman, 1976; Major,
spectively). More variability was found for the d effect sizes McFarlin, & Gagnon, 1984) have argued that women's lower
across organizations than would be expected due to sampling standards (which reduce the salience of fairness considerations)
error. Also, the corrected standard deviations were large com- result from a tendency for women to compare their outcomes
pared with the d values, as indicated by the credibility intervals, with those of other women rather than to those of men, who are
which included zero for the job satisfaction measures and ap- typically paid more (Treiman & Hartmann, 1981). Alterna-
proached zero for fairness perceptions. This finding is incon- tively, the focus may not be on other women per se but rather on
gruent with the second hypothesis and suggests a lack of consis- others whose situations are more relevant; this notion is consis-
tency in the direction of the gender group differences and the tent with studies indicating that people select comparison refer-
presence of potential moderating factors in fairness and job ents that permit assessment of outcomes (e.g., pay or status)
satisfaction perceptions across the organizations in this study. most salient to themselves (Miller, 1984; Wheeler & Koestner,
1984). A third argument suggests that gender differences in
internal pay standards stem from women's valuing money
Discussion
(Crosby, 1982; Jackson, 1989) and interpersonal relationships
Consistent with previous research, perceived fairness ac- differently (Kahn, O'Leary, et al., 1980). Major and Deaux
counted for a significant amount of the variance in job satisfac- (1982) offered a fourth perspective: Because women devalue
tion, and the data suggest no practical gender differences in the their inputs relative to men, they tend to underreward their
relationships. Indeed, the results of the meta-analysis suggest work. Fifth, Callahan-Levy and Messe (1979) argued that
that the magnitude of the differences observed in the present women perceive less of a connection between their work and
study were within what might be expected due to sampling pay as the result of sex role socialization. The utility of some of
error. Thus, extending the literature, these data suggest no these arguments may be somewhat less than in the past because
gender differences in the fairness-job satisfaction relationship. of a narrowing of gender differences at work in recent years
Similarly, there were no practical gender differences in the lev- (e.g., Beutell & Brenner, 1986).
els of perceived fairness of promotions and pay or in feelings of Alternatively, in line with Lerner's (1977) argument that the
job satisfaction (single-item global and total facet scores). choice of equity or equality is based on whether the individuals
Why are the present findings inconsistent with the results see others as unique individuals or as role players, it is possible
reported by Brockner and Adsit (1986)? It is possible that the that co-workers in the present study were seen by both genders
significance, and particularly the magnitude, of the gender dif- as role players or occupants of their formal positions. Thus,
ferences reported by Brockner and Adsit reflect Type I error. fairness issues would have been salient in the development of
Indeed, laboratory studies using artificial organizational sys- job satisfaction for both men and women. Moreover, Bern's
tems or field studies using nonessential social organizations (1985) gender schema model suggests that persons with well-
(i.e., clubs or social groups that do not directly affect one's abil- developed sex-typed schemata are more likely to assess their
ity to make a living) may have limited external validity. Alterna- social environment in gender-related ways; it is possible that the
tively, answers to the question may come from the identifica- populations tapped in the present samples may have had less
tion of conceptual differences between the present and pre- focused sex-typed schemata than the college-age subject popula-
vious research, prior explanations of gender differences in tions often tapped in previous equity studies.
allocations to the self, or possible differences between the pres- A decade ago, Kahn, Nelson, and Gaeddert (1980, p. 748)
ent and previously measured samples. argued that "much of the research on reward allocations has
Conceptual differences between the present study and overestimated the extent of sex differences." In studies of allo-
Brockner and Adsit's (1986) study may explain differences in cation behavior, men and women have responded differently to
the results. In the latter study, satisfaction with an exchange the same situation (Kidder, Bellettirie, & Cohn, 1977) and simi-
relationship was the criterion of interest, not job satisfaction. In larly to different situations (Kahn, Nelson, & Gaeddert, 1980).
addition, Brockner and Adsit (1986) operationalized fairness in Such results suggest a lack of clarity on this issue and that not
equity terms: (a) the difference between the other's perceived all relevant variables have been identified. As noted by Freed-
outcome and inputs, and (b) the difference between one's own man and Phillips (1988), studies on gender differences in moti-
outcome and inputs. In the present study, fairness was opera- vation and work values have failed to demonstrate that gender
tionalized in ratings of the fairness of pay and promotions, and is the actual factor underlying observed differences. Indeed,
the comparison others on which fairness formulations were to Beutell and Brenner (1986) noted that the trend is toward simi-
be based were not explicitly identified. Thus, the people with larity rather than dissimilarity in work values. Feingold's (1988)
whom the individuals in this study were comparing their situa- study of standardized aptitude tests given between 1947 and
tions may have varied significantly, as may have internal stan- 1980, which revealed a decline in gender differences, suggests
dards. that gender differences in cognitive abilities may now be vir-
Another answer may lie in the utility of previous and perhaps tually nonexistent. Perhaps gender differences in organiza-
outdated explanations of gender differences in allocations to tional behavior also may now be less significant than previously
the self, of which there have been several. First, some re- observed or thought. On the other hand, the issue may not be
searchers have suggested that women have lower adaptation gender but rather some other structural or biographical vari-
levels for their pay (P. C. Smith et al., 1969) and lower pay expec- able, such as "breadwinner" status (Witt, 1988). More generally,
916 L. ALAN WITT AND LENDELL G. NYE

one reviewer suggested that potential moderators of the rela- Brockner, J., & Adsit, L. (1986). The moderating effect of sex on the
tionship between fairness perceptions and job satisfaction equity-satisfaction relationship. Journal of Applied Psychology, 71,
might include job status and organizational characteristics, 585-590.
such as size or public versus private sector. In future studies, as Bruning, N. S., & Snyder, R. A. (1983). Sex and position as predictors of
moderators are identified, it would be appropriate to deter- organizational commitment. Academy of Management Journal, 26,
mine whether gender creates a higher order interaction, that is, 485-491.
Callahan-Levy, C. M., & Messe, L. A. (1979). Sex differences in the
whether a variable could significantly affect a fairness-job satis-
allocation of pay. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 37,
faction relationship for only one gender. Given the importance 433-446.
of nondiscriminatory management practices and the concerns Chesler, P., & Goodman, E. J. (1976). Women, money, and power. New
underlying comparable worth (cf. Major, 1987, 1989), addi- York: Morrow.
tional research is needed to carefully examine these issues. Cohen, J. (1977). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences.
New York: Academic Press.
Practical Implications Crosby, E (1982). Relative deprivation and working women. New York:
Oxford University Press.
The results suggest that fairness is an important component DArcy, C., Syrotiuk, J., & Siddique, C. M. (1984). Perceived job attri-
of job satisfaction among the personnel sampled in the present butes, job satisfaction, and psychological distress: A comparison of
study. In other words, employees perceiving fairness in pay and working men and women. Human Relations, 37, 603-611.
promotions were more likely to feel satisfied with their jobs Deaux, K. (1985). Sex and gender. Annual Review of Psychology, 36,
than employees perceiving less fairness or unfairness. This sug- 49-81.
gests the utility of supervisors' and managers' explaining and Feingold, A. (1988). Cognitive gender differences are disappearing.
American Psychologist, 43, 95-103.
discussing personnel outcomes and, when appropriate, empha-
Forgionne, G. A., & Peelers, V E. (1982). Differences in job motivation
sizing the fairness of those outcomes and the procedures lead- and satisfaction among women and men managers. Human Rela-
ing to those outcomes. Supervisor articulation of how person- tions, 35,101-118.
nel decisions are made is important (Greenberg, Bies, & Eskew, Freedman, S. M., & Phillips, J. S. (1988). The changing nature of re-
1991), although its eifects are influenced by the predisposi- search on women at work. Journal of Management, 14, 231 -251.
tional filters of employees. Because perceptions of fairness are Fry, L. W, & Greenfeld, S. (1980). An examination of attitudinal differ-
one component of job satisfaction, employees who have access ences between policewomen and policemen. JournalofApplied Psy-
to information about personnel decisions and how they are chology, 65,123-126.
made are more likely to be satisfied with their jobs, if the infor- Gerhart, B. (1987). How important are dispositional factors as determi-
mation reveals fair practices. nants of job satisfaction? Implications for job design and other per-
In addition, the results reported here suggest that caution sonnel programs. Journalof Applied Psychology, 72, 366-373.
should be used when assuming gender differences in the fair- Golding, J., Resnick, A., & Crosby, F (1983). Work satisfaction as a
function of gender and job status. Psychology ofWomen Quarterly, 7,
ness-job satisfaction relationship. In other words, the data do
286-290.
not support the notion that different management strategies Greenberg, J., Bies, R. J., & Eskew, D. E. (1991). Establishing fairness in
should be used for men and women when the objective is to the eye of the beholder. In R. Giacalone (Ed.), Applied impression
increase job satisfaction through fairness perceptions among management (pp. 111-132). Newbury Park, CA: Sage.
the personnel. Extrapolating from the present data, the use of Greenberg, J., &Cohen, R. L. (1982). Equity and justice in social behav-
such strategies very likely would lead to inappropriate manage- ior. New York: Academic Press.
ment practices. Hackman, J. R., & Oldham, G. R. (1974). The Job Diagnostic Survey:
An instrument for the diagnosis of jobs and evaluation of job redesign
projects (Tech. Rep. 4). New Haven, CT: Yale University.
References Hackman, J. R., & Oldham, G. R. (1980). Work redesign. Reading, MA:
Adams, J. S. (1963). Toward an understanding of equity. Journal of Addison-Wesley.
Abnormal and Social Psychology, 67, 422-436. Harris, C. T, & Earle, J. R. (1986). Gender and work values: Survey
Ajzen, I., & Fishbein, M. (1977). Attitude-behavior relationships: A findings from a working-class sample. Sex Roles, 15, 487-494.
theoretical analysis and review of empirical research. Psychological Hendrix, W H. (1979). Organizational assessment indices of effective-
Bulletin, 84, 888-918. ness (AFHRL Tech. Rep. 79-49). Washington, DC: U.S. Govern-
Bern, S. J. (1985). Androgyny and gender schema theory: A conceptual ment Printing Office.
and empirical integration. In T. B. Sonderegga (Ed.), Nebraska Sym- Herzberg, F., Mausner, B., Peterson, R. A., & Capwell, D. F. (1957). Job
posium on Motivation: Psychology and gender (pp. 179-226). Lin- attitudes: Review of research and opinion. Pittsburgh, PA: Psychologi-
coln: University of Nebraska. cal Services of Pittsburgh.
Berger, J., Zelditch, M., Jr., Anderson, B., & Cohen, B. P. (1972). Struc- Hoppock, R. (1935). Job satisfaction. New York: Harper & Row.
tural aspects of distributive justice: A status value reformulation. In Hulin, C., & Smith, P. C. (1965). Sex differences in job satisfaction.
J. Berger, M. Zelditch, Jr., & B. Anderson (Eds.), Sociological theories Journalof Applied Psychology, 49, 209-216.
in progress (Vol. 2, pp. 119-146). Boston: Houghton-Mifflin. Hunter, E. E., & Schmidt, F. L. (1990). Methods of meta-analysis:
Beutell, N. J., & Brenner, O. C. (1986). Sex differences in work values. Correcting error and bias in research findings. Newbury Park, CA:
Journal of Vocational Behavior, 28, 29-41. Sage.
Broach, D. (1989). Airway Science Curriculum Demonstration Project: Jackson, L. A. (1989). Relative deprivation and the gender wage gap.
Second summative evaluation report (Final report to the Office of Journal of Social Issues, 45, 117-133.
Training and Higher Education). Oklahoma City, OK: Civil Aero- Kahn, A. (1972). Reactions to the generosity or stinginess of an intelli-
medica) Institute, Human Resources Research Division. gent or stupid work partner: A test of equity theory in a direct ex-
PERCEIVED FAIRNESS AND JOB SATISFACTION 917

change relationship. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, Rosenthal, R. & Rubin, D. B. (1982). A simple, general purpose display
27,116-123. of magnitude of experimental effect. Journal of Educational Psychol-
Kahn, A., Nelson, R. E., & Gaeddert, W P. (1980). Sex of subject and ogy, 74,166-169.
sex composition of the group as determinants of reward allocations. Sauser, W I, & York, M. (1978). Sex differences in job satisfaction: A
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 38, 737-750. re-examination. Personnel Psychology, 31, 537-547.
Kahn, A., O'Leary, V, Krulewitz, J. E., & Lamm, H. (1980). Equity and Scarpello, V, & Campbell, J. P. (1983). Job satisfaction: Are all the parts
equality: Men and women means to a just end. Basic and Applied there? Personnel Psychology, 36, 577-600.
Social Psychology, 1,173-197. Schmidt, K. L., Hunter, J. E., & Raju, N. S. (1988). Validity generaliza-
Keller, R. T. (1986). Predictors of the performance of project groups in tion and situational specificity: A second look at the 75% rule and
R&D organizations. Academy of Management Journal, 29,715-725. Fisher's z transformation. Journal of Applied Psychology, 73, 665-
Kidder, L. G., Bellettirie, G, & Cohn, E. S. (1977). Secret ambitions and 672.
public performances: The effects of anonymity on reward alloca- Schnake, M. E. (1983). An empirical assessment of the effects of affec-
tions made by men and women. Journal of Experimental Social Psy- tive response in the measurement of organizational climate. Person-
chology, 13, 70-80. nel Psychology, 36, 791-807.
Lane, I. M., & Messe, L. A. (1971). Equity and the distribution of Scholl, R. W, Cooper, E. A., & McKenna, J. F. (1987). Referent selection
rewards. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 20,1-17. in determining equity perceptions: Differential effects on behavior
Lerner, M. J. (1977). The justice motive: Some hypothesis as to its and attitudinal outcomes. Personnel Psychology, 40,113-124.
origins and forms. Journal of Personality, 45,1-52. Shapiro, H. J., & Stern, L. W (1975). Job satisfaction—men and
Leventhal, G. S., & Lane, D. W (1970). Sex, age, and equity behavior. women, professional and nonprofessional workers. Personnel Jour-
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 15, 312-316. nal, 54, 388-389.
Major, B. (1987). Gender and the psychology of entitlement. In P. Smith, D. B., & Plant, W T. (1982). Sex differences in the job satisfac-
Shaver&C. Hendrick(Eds),Reviewofpersonality and social psychol- tion of university professors. Journal of Applied Psychology, 6 7,249-
ogy: Sex and gender (Vol. 7, pp. 124-148). Beverly Hills, CA: Sage. 251.
Major, B. (1989). Gender differences in comparisons and entitlement: Smith, P. C., Kendall, L., & Hulin, C. (1969). The measurement of satis-
Implications for comparable worth. Journal of Social Issues, 45, 99- faction in work and retirement: A strategy for the study of attitudes.
115. Chicago: Rand-McNally.
Major, B., & Adams, J. B. (1983). The role of gender, interpersonal
Stockford, L. Q, & Kunze, K. R. (1950). Psychology and the pay check.
orientation, and self-presentation in distributive justice behavior.
Personnel, 27,129-143.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 45, 598-608.
Major, B., & Deaux, K. (1982). Individual differences in justice behav- Swap, W, & Rubin, J. Z. (1983). Measurement of interpersonal orienta-
ior. In J. Greenberg & R. Cohen (Eds.), Equity and justice in social tion. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 44, 208-219.
behavior (pp. 43-76). New York: Academic Press. Treiman, D. J., & Hartmann, H. I. (1981). Women, work, and wages:
Major, B., McFarlin, D. B., & Gagnon, D. (1984). Overworked and un- Equal pay for jobs of equal value. Washington, DC: National Acad-
derpaid: On the nature of gender differences in personal entitle- emy Press.
ment. JournalofPersonality and Social Psychology, 47,1399-1412. Varca, P. E., Shaffer, G. S., & McCauley, C. D. (1983). Sex differences in
Mannheim, B. (1983). Men and women industrial workers: Job satisfac- job satisfaction reconsidered. Academy of Management Journal, 26,
tion, work role centrality, and work place preference. WorkandOccu- 348-353.
pations, 10, 413-436. Walster, E. H., & Walster, G. W (1975). Equity and social justice. Jour-
McNichols, C. W, Stahl, M. J., & Manley, T. R. (1978). A validation of nal of Social Issues, 31, 21-43.
Hoppock's job satisfaction measure. Academy of Management Jour- Walster, E. H., Walster, G. W, & Berscheid, E. (1978). Equity: Theory
nal, 21, 737-742. and research. Boston: Allyn & Bacon.
Miller, C. T. (1984). Self-schema, gender, and social comparison: A Weaver, C. N. (1974). Correlates of job satisfaction: Some evidence
clarification of the related attributes hypothesis. Journalof Personal- from the national surveys. Academy of Management Journal, 17,
ity and Social Psychology, 46,1222-1229. 373-375.
Mottaz, C. (1986). Gender differences in work satisfaction, work-re- Weaver, C. N. (1980). Job satisfaction in the United States in the 1970's.
lated rewards and values, and the determinants of work satisfaction. Journal of Applied Psychology, 65, 364-367.
Human Relations, 39, 359-378. Wheeler, L., & Koestner, R. (1984). Performance evaluation: On
Murray, M. A., & Atkinson, T. (1981). Gender differences in correlates choosing to know the related attributes of others when we know their
of job satisfaction. Canadian Journalof Behavioral Sciences, 13,44- performance. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 20, 263-
52. 271.
Myers, J. G, Schroeder, D. J., Van Deventer, A., & Collins, W E. (1988). Whitener, E. M. (1990). Confusion of confidence intervals and credibil-
1988 FA A job satisfaction survey: National report. Washington, DC: ity intervals in meta-analysis. Journal of Applied Psychology, 75,315-
Federal Aviation Administration, Office of Aviation Medicine. 321.
Myers, J. G, Witt, L. A., & Schroeder, D. J. (1991). 1990FAA job satis- Witt, L. A. (1988). Breadwinner vs. non-breadwinner differences in
faction survey: National report. Washington, DC: Federal Aviation married women's job satisfaction and perceptions of organizational
Administration, Office of Aviation Medicine. climate. Human Relations, 41, 483-491.
Nieva, V F, & Gutek, B. A. (1981). Women and work: A psychological
perspective. New York: Praeger.
Office of Personnel Management. (1979). Federal employee attitudes: Received January 2,1992
Phase 1. Baseline survey 1979. Washington, DC: U. S. Government Revision received March 9,1992
Printing Office. Accepted March 11,1992 •

You might also like