You are on page 1of 13

See discussions, stats, and author profiles for this publication at: https://www.researchgate.

net/publication/232555483

Biased Assimilation and Attitude Polarization: The Effects of


Prior Theories on Subsequently Considered Evidence

Article in Journal of Personality and Social Psychology · November 1979


DOI: 10.1037/0022-3514.37.11.2098

CITATIONS READS

3,619 15,106

3 authors, including:

Lee Ross Mark Lepper


Stanford University Stanford University
102 PUBLICATIONS 31,030 CITATIONS 105 PUBLICATIONS 27,764 CITATIONS

SEE PROFILE SEE PROFILE

All content following this page was uploaded by Lee Ross on 03 June 2014.

The user has requested enhancement of the downloaded file.


Journal of Personality and Social Psychology
1979, Vol. 37, No. 11, 2098-2109

Biased Assimilation and Attitude Polarization: The Effects of


Prior Theories on Subsequently Considered Evidence
Charles G. Lord, Lee Ross, and Mark R. Lepper
Stanford University
People who hold strong opinions on complex social issues are likely to examine
relevant empirical evidence in a biased manner. They are apt to accept "con-
firming" evidence at face value while subjecting "discontinuing" evidence to
critical evaluation, and as a result to draw undue support for their initial posi-
tions from mixed or random empirical findings. Thus, the result of exposing
contending factions in a social dispute to an identical body of relevant em-
pirical evidence may be not a narrowing of disagreement but rather an in-
crease in polarization. To test these assumptions and predictions, subjects
supporting and opposing capital punishment were exposed to two purported
studies, one seemingly confirming and one seemingly disconfirming their exist-
ing beliefs about the deterrent efficacy of the death penalty. As predicted, both
proponents and opponents of capital punishment rated those results and
procedures that confirmed their own beliefs to be the more convincing and
probative ones, and they reported corresponding shifts in their beliefs as the
various results and procedures were presented. The net effect of such evalua-
tions and opinion shifts was the postulated increase in attitude polarization.

The human understanding when it has once adopted vation in terms of the symbols or metaphors
an opinion draws all things else to support and agree they evoke (Abelson, 1976; Kinder &
with it. And though there be a greater number and
weight of instances to be found on the other side, Kiewiet, Note 1) or in conformity with views
yet these it either neglects and despises, or else by expressed by opinion leaders we like or re-
some distinction sets aside and rejects, in order that spect (Katz, 19S7). When "evidence" is
by this great and pernicious predetermination the brought to bear it is apt to be incomplete,
authority of its former conclusion may remain biased, and of marginal probative value—
inviolate. (Bacon, 1620/1960)
typically, no more than a couple of vivid,
Often, more often than we care to admit, concrete, but dubiously representative in-
our attitudes on important social issues reflect stances or cases (cf. Abelson, 1972; Nisbett
only our preconceptions, vague impressions, & Ross, in press). It is unsurprising, therefore,
and untested assumptions. We respond to that important social issues and policies
social policies concerning compensatory edu- generally prompt sharp disagreements, even
cation, water fluoridation, or energy conser- among highly concerned and intelligent citi-
zens, and that such disagreements often sur-
This research was supported in part by Research vive strenous attempts at resolution through
Grants MH-26736 from the National Institute of discussion and persuasion.
Mental Health and BNS-78-01211 from the Na- An interesting question, and one that
tional Science Foundation to Mark R. Lepper and prompts the present research, involves the
Lee Ross. The authors would like to express their
appreciation to Daryl Bern, Richard Nisbett, Robert
consequences of introducing the opposing
Zajonc, and Mark Zanna for their thoughtful com- factions to relevant and objective data. This
ments and suggestions on earlier drafts of this question seems particularly pertinent for con-
article, and to Lisa Burns for acting as the experi- temporary social scientists, who have fre-
menter.
Requests for reprints should be sent to any of
quently called for "more empirically based"
the authors, Department of Psychology, Stanford social decision making (e.g., Campbell, 1969).
University, Stanford, California 94305. Very likely, data providing consistent and

Copyright 1979 by the American Psychological Association, Inc. 0022-3514/79/3711-2098$00.75

2098
BIASED ASSIMILATION 2099

unequivocal support for one or another posi- ation of subsequent evidence, including work
tion on a given issue can influence decision on classic Einstellung effects (Luchins, 1942,
makers and, with sufficiently energetic dis- 1957), social influence processes (Asch,
semination, public opinion at large. But what 1946), impression formation (e.g., Jones &
effects can be expected for more mixed or Goethals, 1971), recognition of degraded
inconclusive evidence of the sort that is stimuli (Bruner & Potter, 1964), resistance
bound to arise for most complex social issues, to change of social attitudes and stereotypes
especially where full-fledged experiments (Abelson, 1959; Allport, 1954), self-fulfilling
yielding decisive and easy-to-generalize results prophecies (Merton, 1948; Rosenhan, 1973;
are a rarity? Logically, one might expect Snyder, Tanke, & Berscheid, 1977), and the
mixed evidence to produce some moderation persistence of "illusory correlations" (Chap-
in the views expressed by opposing factions. man & Chapman, 1967, 1969). In a partic-
At worst, one might expect such inconclusive ularly relevant recent demonstration, Ma-
evidence to be ignored. honey (1977) has shown that trained social
The present study examines a rather differ- scientists are not immune to theory-based
ent thesis—one born in an analysis of the evaluations. In this study, professional re-
layperson's general shortcomings as an intui- viewers' judgments about experimental pro-
tive scientist (cf. Nisbett & Ross, in press; cedures and resultant publication recom-
Ross, 1977) and his more specific short- mendations varied dramatically with the
comings in adjusting unwarranted beliefs in degree to which the findings of a study under
the light of empirical challenges (cf. Ross, review agreed or disagreed with the reviewers'
Lepper, & Hubbard, 1975). Our thesis is that own theoretical predilections.
belief polarization will increase, rather than Thus, there is considerable evidence that
decrease or remain unchanged, when mixed people tend to interpret subsequent evidence
or inconclusive findings are assimilated by so as to maintain their initial beliefs. The
proponents of opposite viewpoints. This biased assimilation processes underlying this
"polarization hypothesis" can be derived from effect may include a propensity to remember
the simple assumption that data relevant the strengths of confirming evidence but the
to a belief are not processed impartially. In- weaknesses of disconfirming evidence, to judge
stead, judgments about the validity, reliability, confirming evidence as relevant and reliable
relevance, and sometimes even the meaning but disconfirming evidence as irrelevant and
of proffered evidence are biased by the ap- unreliable, and to accept confirming evidence
parent consistency of that evidence with the at face value while scrutinizing disconfirming
perceiver's theories and expectations. Thus evidence hypercritically. With confirming evi-
individuals will dismiss and discount empirical dence, we suspect that both lay and profes-
evidence that contradicts their initial views sional scientists rapidly reduce the complexity
but will derive support from evidence, of no of the information and remember only a few
greater probativeness, that seems consistent well-chosen supportive impressions. With dis-
with their views. Through such biased assim- confirming evidence, they continue to reflect
ilation even a random set of outcomes or upon any information that suggests less dam-
events can appear to lend support for an aging "alternative interpretations." Indeed,
entrenched position, and both sides in a given they may even come to regard the ambiguities
debate can have their positions bolstered by and conceptual flaws in the data opposing
the same set of data. their hypotheses as somehow suggestive of
As the introductory quotation suggests, the the fundamental correctness of those hypoth-
notions of biased assimilation and resulting eses. Thus, completely inconsistent or even
belief perseverance have a long history. random data—when "processed" in a suitably
Beyond philosophical speculations and a biased fashion—can maintain or even rein-
wealth of anecdotal evidence, considerable force one's preconceptions.
research attests to the capacity of preconcep- The present study was designed to examine
tions and initial theories to bias the consider- both the biased assimilation processes that
2100 C. LORD, L. ROSS, AND M. LEPPER

may occur when subjects with strong initial students were recruited to participate in a related
attitudes are confronted with empirical data experiment as partial fulfillment of a course require-
ment. Twenty-four were "proponents" who favored
concerning a controversial social issue and the capital punishment, believed it to have a deterrent
consequent polarization of attitudes hypoth- effect, and thought most of the relevant research
esized to result when subjects with differing supported their own beliefs. Twenty-four were "op-
initial attitudes are exposed to a common set ponents" who opposed capital punishment, doubted
of "mixed" experimental results. The social its deterrent effect, and thought that the relevant
research supported their views.
controversy chosen for our investigation was
the issue of capital punishment and its effec-
tiveness as a deterrent to murder. This choice Procedure
was made primarily because the issue is the
Upon entering the experiment, mixed groups of
subject of strongly held views that frequently proponents and opponents were seated at a large
do become the target of public education and table. The experimenter, blind to subjects' attitudes,
media persuasion attempts, and has been the told them that they would each be asked to read
focus of considerable social science research 2 of 20 randomly selected studies on the deterrent
efficacy of the death penalty and asked them to use
in the last twenty years. Indeed, as our basic their own "evaluative powers" in thinking about
hypothesis suggests, contending factions in what the author(s) of the study did, what the critics
this debate often cite and derive encourage- had to say, and whether the research provided sup-
ment from the same body of inconclusive port for one side or the other of this issue.
correlational research (Furman v. Georgia, The experimenter next showed subjects a set of
10 index cards, each containing a brief statement of
1972; Sarat & Vidmar, 1976; Sellin, 1967). the results of a single study. Each subject was asked
In the present experiment, we presented to choose one card and read it silently. In reality,
both proponents and opponents of capital all 10 cards in any one session were identical, pro-
punishment first with the results and then viding either prodeterrent information, for example:
with procedural details, critiques, and rebut- Kroner and Phillips (1977) compared murder rates
tals for two studies dealing with the deterrent for the year before and the year after adoption of
efficacy of the death penalty—one study con- capital punishment in 14 states. In 11 of the 14
firming their initial beliefs and one study dis- states, murder rates were lower after adoption of
the death penalty. This research supports the
confirming their initial beliefs. We anticipated deterrent effect of the death penalty.
biased assimilation at every stage of this pro-
cedure. First, we expected subjects to rate or antideterrent information, for example:
the quality and probative value of studies
Palmer and Crandall (1977) compared murder
confirming their beliefs on deterrent efficacy rates in 10 pairs of neighboring states with differ-
more highly than studies challenging their ent capital punishment laws. In 8 of the 10 pairs,
beliefs. Second, we anticipated corresponding murder rates were higher in the state with capital
effects on subjects' attitudes and beliefs such punishment. This research opposes the deterrent
effect of the death penalty.
that studies confirming subjects' views would
exert a greater impact than studies discon- To control for order effects, half of the proponents
firming those views. Finally, as a function of and half of the opponents saw a "prodeterrence"
these assimilative biases, we hypothesized that result first, and half saw an "antideterrence" result
the net result of exposure to the conflicting first. The studies cited, although invented specifically
for the present study, were characteristic of research
results of these two studies would be an found in the current literature cited in judicial
increased polarization of subjects' beliefs on decisions.
deterrent efficacy and attitudes towards After reading one of these "result cards," subjects
capital punishment. answered two sets of questions, on 16-point scales,
about changes in their attitudes toward capital
punishment (from —8 = more opposed, to 8 = more
Method in favor) and their beliefs about the deterrent
efficacy of the death penalty (from —8 = less belief
Subjects that capital punishment has a deterrent effect, to 8 =
more belief in the deterrent effect). One set of ques-
A total of 1S1 undergraduates completed an in-class tions examined change occasioned by the single piece
questionnaire that included three items on capital of information they had just finished reading; a
punishment. Two to four weeks later, 48 of these second set of questions assessed the cumulative change
BIASED ASSIMILATION 2101

produced by all of the materials read since the start entially evaluate the quality and "convincing-
of the experiment.1 ness" of the same empirical studies and find-
Next the experimenter distributed detailed research
descriptions bearing code letters corresponding to ings. The relevant evaluations, presented in
those on the result cards. The descriptions gave Table 1, revealed strong support for the
details of the researchers' procedure, reiterated the hypothesized bias in favor of the study that
results, mentioned several prominent criticisms of confirmed subjects' initial attitudes.
the study "in the literature," listed the authors'
rebuttals to some of the criticisms and depicted the A two-way analysis of variance (Initial
data both in table form and graphically. After read- Attitude X Order of Presentation) on the
ing this more detailed description and critique of the differences between ratings of convincingness
first study, subjects were asked to judge how well of the prodeterrence and antideterrence stud-
or poorly the study had been conducted (from —8 = ies yielded only a main effect of initial atti-
very poorly done, to 8 = very well done), and how
convincing the study seemed as evidence on the tude, F(l, 44) = 32.07, p < .001. Proponents
deterrent efficacy of capital punishment (from —8 =
completely unconvincing, to 8 = completely convinc- 1
ing)." Following this evaluation, subjects were asked Since most of our subjects had reported initial
to write why they thought the study they had just positions at, or very close to, the ends of the
read did or did not support the argument that capital attitude and belief scales used for selection pur-
punishment is a deterrent to murder, and then to poses, our initial plan to assess attitude polariza-
answer a second set of attitude and belief change tion—in terms of difference scores assessing changes
questions on the effects of the description alone and in subjects' attitudes and beliefs on these same
the effects of all experimental materials (i.e., the scales from these initial measures to the completion
results and subsequent description and critique) up of the experiment—proved impossible. As a substi-
to that point in time. tute, we employed three sorts of measures to assess
Following completion of these questions, the entire attitude change. First, we asked subjects, after each
new piece of information, to indicate any changes
procedure was repeated, with a second fictitious study
reporting results opposite to those of the first. Again, in their attitudes and beliefs occasioned by that
subjects initially received only a brief description of single piece of information. Second, we asked sub-
jects, at these same points, to report on "cumula-
the results of this second study but were then pro-
vided with a detailed presentation of the procedure, tive" changes in their attitudes and beliefs since
the start of the experiment. Third, subjects were
results, and critiques. As before, subjects were asked
asked to keep "running records" of their attitudes
to evaluate both the impact of each single piece of
and beliefs on enlarged versions of the scales ini-
evidence and the impact of all experimental materials
tially used for selection purposes. Although all of
up to that point in the experiment on their attitudes
these measures individually raise some problems,
toward capital punishment and their beliefs concern-
the congruence of data across these different mea-
ing its deterrent efficacy. surement devices gives us some confidence con-
To control for possible differences in the inherent
cerning the results reported. Indeed, because the
plausibility of the two studies, two sets of materials
results obtained on the "running record" measure
were employed that interchanged the ostensible
so completely parallel the findings obtained on the
results of the two invented experiments. The overall
cumulative change question depicted in Figures 1
design was thus completely counterbalanced with and 2, in terms of both the array of means and the
respect to subjects' initial attitudes, order of confirm-
obtained significance levels, the data from this
ing vs. discontinuing evidence, and the association of measure will not be reported separately.
the "before-after" vs. "adjacent states" designs with 2
Subjects were also asked, at this point, whether
positive or negative results.3 At the end of the pro- they thought the researchers had favored or op-
cedure, subjects were carefully debriefed concerning posed the death penalty and whether they thought
the fictitious nature of the studies and were asked an unbiased consideration should lead one to treat
not to reveal this deception to others. In addition the study as evidence for or against capital punish-
to insuring that subjects understood the fictional ment. Analyses on the first question showed only
nature of the experimental materials, this debriefing that subjects believed the researchers' attitudes to
included discussion of the processes underlying the coincide with their stated results. Analyses on the
assimilation of evidence to previous theories and second question proved wholly redundant with
reassurance that a skeptical reaction to poorly those presented for the "convincingness" and "well
designed research is often a praiseworthy cognitive done" questions.
response. 3
Preliminary analyses were conducted to see if
the particular association of positive versus nega-
Results tive results with either the before-after or ad-
Evaluations oj the Two Studies jacent-states designs would affect the results ob-
tained. There were no significant effects or inter-
Our first hypothesis was that subjects hold- actions involving this variation in stimulus materials;
ing different initial positions would differ- hence, the data were collapsed across this factor.
2102 C. LORD, L. ROSS, AND M. LEPPER

Table 1 selected for dramatic effect but not unrepre-


Evaluations of Prodeterrence and Antideterrence sentative in content—in Table 2. As these
Studies by Proponents and Opponents of comments make clear, the same study can
Capital Punishment
elicit entirely opposite evaluations from
people who hold different initial beliefs about
Study Proponents Opponents
a complex social issue. This evidence of bias
Mean ratings of how well the two in subjects' evaluations of the quality and
studies had been conducted convincingness of the two studies is consistent
Prodeterrence 1.5 -2.1 with the biased assimilation hypothesis and
Antideterrence -1.6 - .3 sets the stage for testing our further predic-
Difference 3.1 -1.8 tions concerning attitude and belief polariza-
tion.
Mean ratings of how convincing the two studies were
as evidence on the deterrent efficacy of
capital punishment Overall Attitude Polarization
Prodeterrence 1.4 -2.1
Antideterrence -1.8 .1 Given such biased evaluations, our primary
Difference 3.2 -2.2 hypothesis was that exposure to the "mixed" "
data set comprised by the two studies would
Note. Positive numbers indicate a positive evalua- result in a further polarization of subjects'
tion of the study's convincingness or procedure.
Negative numbers indicate a negative evaluation of attitudes and beliefs rather than the con-
the study's convincingness or procedure. vergence that an impartial consideration of
these inconclusive data might warrant. To
test this hypothesis requires a consideration
regarded the prodeterrence study as signifi-
cantly more convincing than the antideter-
rence study, i(23) = S.I8, p < .001,4 regard- 4
All p values reported in this article are based on
less of whether it was the "before-after" two-tailed tests of significance.
5
design that suggested the efficacy of capital In order to examine possible main effects of
either study direction or initial attitude on sub-
punishment and the "adjacent states" design jects' ratings of how convincing and how well done
that refuted it, or vice versa. Opponents, by the studies were—findings that would not be por-
contrast, regarded the prodeterrence study as trayed in the difference score analysis reported—a
significantly less convincing than the anti- three-way analysis of variance (Initial Attitude X
deterrence study, <(23) = -3.02, p < .01, Order of Presentation X Direction of Study) was
also performed. There were no main effects of study
again irrespective of which research design direction on either measure. A main effect of initial
was purported to have produced which type attitude—indicating that opponents evaluated the
of results. The same was true of the difference total set of evidence more negatively than did pro-
between ratings of how well done the two ponents—proved significant for the "well done"
question, F( 1,44) =4.69, p < .05, but not for the
studies had been, F(l, 44) = 33.52, p < "convincing" question, F(l,44) = 1.53, ns.
.001.6 As above, proponents found the pro- 6
The term mixed, we should emphasize, refers to
deterrence study to have been better con- the fact that one study yielded evidence confirming
ducted than the antideterrence study, £ ( 2 3 ) the deterrent efficacy of the death penalty, whereas
the other study yielded evidence disconfirming such
= S.37, p < .001, whereas opponents found efficacy (with appropriate counterbalancing of pur-
the prodeterrence study to have been less well ported procedures and purported results). Subjects,
conducted, /(23) = —2.80, p < .05. As one regardless of initial position, clearly recognized this
might expect, the correlation between the discrepancy between results, as will be apparent in
our analyses of their responses to the simple state-
"convincingness" and "well done" questions ments of the study's main findings. We do not
was substantial, r — .67, p < .001. mean to imply that the subjects "phenomenologi-
These differing opinions of the quality of cally" judged the two studies to be of equal pro-
the two studies were also reflected in subjects' bative value; indeed, as indicated in the preceding
discussion, identical procedures were clearly judged
written comments. At the risk of opening our- to differ in their probativeness depending on the
selves to a charge of "biased assimilation," congruity between the study's outcomes and the
we present a set of subjects' comments— subject's initial beliefs.
BIASED ASSIMILATION 2103

Table 2
Selected Comments on Prodeterrence and Antideterrence Studies by Proponents
and Opponents of Capital Punishment

Comments on
Subject Prodeterrence study Antideterrence study

Set 1 materials
S8 Proponent "It does support capital punishment "The evidence given is relatively
in that it presents facts showing meaningless without data about
that there is a deterrent effect how the overall crime rate went
and seems to have gathered data up in those years."
properly."
S24 Proponent "The experiment was well thought "There were too many flaws in the
out, the data collected was valid, picking of the states and too many
and they were able to come up variables involved in the experi-
with responses to all criticisms." ment as a whole to change my
opinion."
535 Opponent "The study was taken only 1 year "The states were chosen at random,
before and 1 year after capital so the results show the average
punishment was reinstated. To be effect capital punishment has
a more effective study they should across the nation. The fact that 8
have taken data from at least 10 out of 10 states show a rise in
years before and as many years as murders stands as good evidence."
possible after."
536 Opponent "I don't feel such a straightforward "There aren't as many uncontrolled
conclusion can be made from the variables in this experiment as in
data collected." the other one, so I'm still willing
to believe the conclusion made."

Set 2 materials
514 Proponent "It shows a good direct comparison "I don't think they have complete
between contrasting death penalty enough collection of data. Also,
effectiveness. Using neighboring as suggested, the murder rates
states helps to make the experi- should be expressed as percentages,
ment more accurate by using not as straight figures."
similar locations."
515 Proponent "It seems that the researchers "The research didn't cover a long
studied a carefully selected group enough period of time to prove
of states and that they were care- that capital punishment is not a
ful in interpreting their results." deterrent to murder."
S25 Opponent "The data presented are a randomly "The murder rates climbed in all but
drawn set of 10. This fact seems two of the states after new laws
to be the study's biggest problem. were passed and no strong evidence
Also many other factors are not to contradict the researchers has
accounted for which are very been presented."
important to the nature of the
results."
S38 Opponent "There might be very different "These tests were comparing the
circumstances between the sets of same state to itself, so I feel it
two states, even though they were could be a fairly good measure."
sharing a border."

of subjects' final attitudes, after exposure to The relevant data provide strong support
both studies and related critiques and rebut- for the polarization hypothesis. Asked for
tals, relative to the start of the experiment. their final attitudes relative to the experi-
2104 C. LORD, L. ROSS, AND M. LEPPER

Table 3 sive or mixed data will lead to increased


Mean Attitude and Belief Changes for a polarization rather than to uncertainty and
Single Piece of Information moderation. Moreover, the degree of polar-
ization shown by individual subjects was pre-
Initial attitudes
dicted by differences in subjects' willingness
Issue and study Proponents Opponents to be less critical of procedures yielding sup-
portive evidence than of procedures yielding
Results only nonsupportive evidence. Significant correla-
Capital punishment tions were found between overall attitude
Prodeterrence 1.3 0.4 change regarding capital punishment and dif-
Antideterrence -0.7 -0.9 ferences in ratings of both how convincing,
Combined 0.6 -0.5
Deterrent efficacy r = .56, p < .001, and how well done, r -
Prodeterrence 1.9 0.7 .56, p < .001, the studies were. Overall
Antideterrence -0.9 -1.6 changes in beliefs in deterrent efficacy pro-
Combined 1.0 -0.9 duced comparable correlations of .53 and .57,
Details, data, critiques, rebuttals
both ps < .001.
Capital punishment
Prodeterrence 0.8 -0.9 Components 0} Attitude Polarization
Antideterrence 0.7 -0.8
Combined 1.5 —1.7 In view of this strong evidence of overall
Deterrent efficacy attitude polarization, it is worth examining
Prodeterrence 0.7 -1.0 the course of attitude polarization as subjects'
Antideterrence 0.7 -0.8
Combined 1.4 -1.8 opinions were successively assessed after
exposure to the first study, the details and
Note. Positive numbers indicate a more positive critiques of the first study, the results of the
attitude or belief about capital punishment and its second study, and the details and critiques of
deterrent effect. Negative numbers indicate a more the second study. At each stage, it will be
negative attitude or belief about capital punishment
and its deterrent effect. recalled, subjects were asked about the
impact of the single piece of information they
had just considered and the cumulative
ment's start, proponents reported that they impact of all information presented to that
were more in favor of capital punishment, point. Let us first examine the reported
t ( 2 3 ) = S.07, p < .001, whereas opponents effects of single segments of evidence and
reported that they were less in favor of then the effects of accumulated evidence over
capital punishment, t(23) = -3.34, p < .01. time.
In a two-way analysis of variance (Initial
Attitude X Order of Presentation), the effect Effect oj Exposure to the Results of
of initial attitude was highly significant, Each Study
F (1, 44) = 30.06, p < .001, and neither the
order effect nor the interaction approached Considering the result cards as single pieces
significance. Similar results characterized of evidence, both proponents and opponents
subjects' beliefs about deterrent efficacy. reported shifting their attitudes in the direc-
Proponents reported greater belief in the tion of the stated results for both the pro-
deterrent effect of capital punishment, i(23) deterrence, <(47) = 4.67, p < .001, and anti-
= 4.26, p < .001, whereas opponents reported deterrence, < ( 4 7 ) = -5.15, p < .001, studies.
less belief in this deterrent effect, <(23) = As shown in the top half of Table 3, however,
-3.79, p<.OQl. Final attitudes toward subjects' responses to the two studies alfo
capital punishment and beliefs concerning varied with initial attitude. Proponents tended
deterrent efficacy were highly correlated, to be influenced more by the prodeterrence
r= .88, p < .001. study and opponents more by the anti-
Such results provide strong support for the deterrence study. Thus a two-way analysis
main experimental hypothesis that inconclu- of variance (Initial Attitude X Order of Pre-
BIASED ASSIMILATION 2105

sentation) on combined change from the two first either the prodeterrence study or the
result cards considered individually yielded antideterrence study, provide a more detailed
only a main effect of initial attitude for both view of the attitude polarization process.
attitudes toward the death penalty, F(\, 44) They allow, as well, an examination of the
= 6.3S, p < .02,7 and beliefs about its deter- hypothesized "rebound effect," that the pro-
rent effect, F(l, 44) = 10.37, p < .01. Inter- vision of any plausible reason for discounting
estingly, the analysis of beliefs regarding data that contradict one's preconceptions will
deterrent efficacy also showed an unantici- eliminate the effects that mere knowledge of
pated interaction effect, F ( l , 44) = 7.48, p those data may have produced.
< .01, with proponents showing a differential The existence of such a "rebound effect" is
response to results alone regardless of order obvious from examination of these figures.
of presentation but opponents showing a dif- Whether they encountered the discontinuing
ferential response to results alone only when result first or second, both proponents and
the confirming study was presented first. opponents seemed to be swayed momentarily
by this evidence, only to revert to their
Effect oj Exposure to Procedures and Data, former attitudes and beliefs (and in 23% of
Critiques and Rebuttals the individual cases, to even more extreme
positions) after inspecting the procedural
When provided with a more detailed details and data, and the critiques and rebut-
description of the procedures and data, to- tals found in the literature. Across all sub-
gether with relevant critiques and authors' jects, this rebound in opinions proved signifi-
rebuttals, subjects seemed to ignore the stated cant for both the capital punishment, £(47)
results of the study. As shown in the bottom = 4.43, p < .001, and deterrent efficacy,
half of Table 3, both proponents and oppo- / ( 4 7 ) = 4.58, p < .001, issues. By contrast,
nents interpreted the additional information, no compensating rebound effects resulted from
relative to the results alone, as strongly sup- reading the descriptions and critiques of stud-
porting their own initial attitudes. Detailed ies supporting subjects' initial attitudes, for
descriptions of either the prodeterrence or either capital punishment, < ( 4 7 ) = .60, ns,
the antideterrence study, with accompanying or deterrent efficacy, £(47) — .23, ns.
critiques, caused proponents to favor capital
punishment more and believe in its deterrent
efficacy more, but caused opponents to oppose Discussion
capital punishment more and believe in its The results of the present experiment pro-
deterrent efficacy less. A two-way analysis of vide strong and consistent support for the
variance (Initial Attitude x Order of Pre- attitude polarization hypothesis and for the
sentation) on attitude change for the two biased assimilation mechanisms postulated to
descriptions combined yielded only a signifi- underlie such polarization. The net effect of
cant main effect of initial attitude for both exposing proponents and opponents of capital
the capital punishment issue, F ( l , 44) = punishment to identical evidence—studies
28.10, p < .001, and the deterrent efficacy ostensibly offering equivalent levels of sup-
question, F ( l , 44) = 26.93, p < .001. port and disconfirmation—was to increase
further the gap between their views. The
Changes in Attitudes Across Time mechanisms responsible for this polarization
of subjects' attitudes and beliefs were clearly
Subjects' reported changes in attitudes and
beliefs, relative to the start of the experiment,
following exposure to each of the four sep- 7
In order to rule out the possibility that direc-
arate pieces of information are depicted in tion of study interacted with initial attitude, a
Figure 1 for attitudes concerning capital three-way analysis of variance (Initial Attitude X
Order of Presentation X Direction of Study) Was
punishment and in Figure 2 for beliefs con- also performed on these data. The relevant interac-
cerning deterrent efficacy. These data, por- tion term did not approach significance, F(l,44) =
trayed separately for subjects who received 1.62, ns.
2106 C. LORD, L. ROSS, AND M. LEPPER

1 1 1 1
H PROPONENTS
MORE IN FAVOR OF Q OPPONENTS
CAPITAL PUNISHMENT

-ll i . \
u u\
NO CHANGE % %
L_J

-1
PRO- DETERRENCE ANTI-DETERRENCE
MORE OPPOSED TO RESULTS DETAILS RESULTS DET AILS
CAPITAL PUNISHMENT 1 1 1

MORE IN FAVOR OF
CAPITAL PUNISHMENT
1 1 1 +j
- +2

- - +1

B B
M

NO CHANGE

71 -
0

-1

—2
AN n- DETERREf JCE PRO-DETERRENCE
MORE OPPOSED TO RESULTS DETAILS RESULTS DETAILS
CAPITAL PUNISHMENT

TIME

Figure J . Top panel: Attitude changes on capital punishment relative to start of experiment as
reported across time by subjects who received prodeterrence study first. Bottom panel: Attitude
changes on capital punishment relative to start of experiment as reported across time by subjects
who received antideterrence study first.

suggested by correlational analyses. Subjects' strongly assumed, then studies whose out-
decisions about whether to accept a study's comes reflect that truth may reasonably be
findings at face value or to search for flaws given greater credence than studies whose
and entertain alternative interpretations outcomes fail to reflect that truth. Hence the
seemed to depend far less on the particular physicist would be "biased," but appropri-
procedure employed than on whether the ately so, if a new procedure for evaluating the
study's results coincided with their existing speed of light were accepted if it gave the
beliefs. "right answer" but rejected if it gave the
"wrong answer." The same bias leads most
The Normative Issue of us to be skeptical about reports of mirac-
ulous virgin births or herbal cures for cancer,
It is worth commenting explicitly about the and despite the risk that such theory-based
normative status of our subjects' apparent and experience-based skepticism may render
biases. First, there can be no real quarrel with us unable to recognize a miraculous event
a willingness to infer that studies supporting when it occurs, overall we are surely well
one's theory-based expectations are more served by our bias. Our subjects' willingness
probative than, or methodologically superior to impugn or defend findings as a function of
to, studies that contradict one's expectations. their conformity to expectations can, in part,
When an "objective truth" is known or be similarly defended. Only the strength of
BIASED ASSIMILATION 2107

their initial convictions in the face of the selves to be encouraged by patterns of data
existing inconclusive social data and argu- that they ought to have found troubling.
ments can be regarded as "suspect." Through such processes laypeople and pro-
Our subjects' main inferential shortcoming, fessional scientists alike find it all too easy
in other words, did not lie in their inclination to cling to impressions, beliefs, and theories
to process evidence in a biased manner. Will- that have ceased to be compatible with the
ingness to interpret new evidence in the light latest and best evidence available (Mahoney,
of past knowledge and experience is essential 1976, 1977).
for any organism to make sense of, and
respond adaptively to, its environment. Polarization: Real or Merely Reported?
Rather, their sin lay in their readiness to use
evidence already processed in a biased man- Before further pursuing the broader impli-
ner to bolster the very theory or belief that cations of the present demonstration, it is
initially "justified" the processing bias. In necessary to consider an important question
so doing, subjects exposed themselves to the raised by our procedure: Did our subjects
familiar risk of making their hypotheses un- really show change (i.e., polarization) in their
falsifiable—a serious risk in a domain where private beliefs about the desirability and
it is clear that at least one party in a dispute deterrent efficacy of capital punishment? Cer-
holds a false hypothesis—and allowing them- tainly they told us, explicitly, that their

1 1 1
HI PROPONENTS
MORE BELIEF THAT I] OPPONENTS —
+3
CAPITAL PUNISHMENT
HAS DETERRENT EFFECT
+2

- +1

11 In . I
NO CHANGE

MORE DISBELIEF THAT


CAPITAL PUNISHMENT
_

PRO-DETERRENCE
RESULTS DETAILS
L_J u
ANTI-DETERRENCE
RESULTS DETAILS
« -1

-2
HAS DETERRENT EFFECT

MORE BELIEF THAT 1 1 I +3


CAPITAL PUNISHMENT
HAS DETERRENT EFFECT
- - +2

m I - +1
!! f!
NO CHANGE

n E^j 1

- _1

- 1—' - -2
MORE DISBELIEF THAT ANTI-DETERRENCE PRO-DETERRENCE
CAPITAL PUNISHMENT RESULTS DETAILS RESULTS DETAILS -3
HAS DETERRENT EFFECT 1 1 1

TIME

Figure 2. Top panel: Belief changes on capital punishment's deterrent efficacy relative to start of
experiment as reported across time by subjects who received prodeterrence study first. Bottom
panel: Belief changes on capital punishment's deterrent efficacy relative to start of experiment
as reported across time by subjects who received antideterrence study first.
2108 C. LORD, L. ROSS, AND M. LEPPER

attitudes and beliefs did change after each the evidence that first gave rise to such
new piece of evidence was presented, and beliefs. In essence, these prior studies demon-
from the beginning to the end of the experi- strate that beliefs can survive the complete
ment. Moreover, they did show a willingness subtraction of the critical formative evidence
to report a shift in their attitudes in the direc- on which they were initially based. In a com-
tion of findings that were contrary to their plementary fashion, the present study shows
beliefs, at least until those findings were that strongly entrenched beliefs can also sur-
exposed to methodological scrutiny and pos- vive the addition of nonsupportive evidence.
sible alternative interpretations. Nevertheless, These findings pose some fundamental ques-
it could be argued that subjects were not tions for traditional attribution models. To
reporting real shifts in attitudes but instead the extent that beliefs and impressions can be
were merely reporting what they believed to shown to persevere in the face of subsequent
be a rational or appropriate response to each challenging data, we need a "top down"
increment in the available evidence. Although rather than-—or perhaps in conjunction with
we believe that it remains an impressive —a "bottom up" approach (cf. Bobrow &
demonstration of assimilation biases to show Norman, 1975) to the question of how indi-
that contending factions both believe the same viduals extract meaning from their social
data to justify their position "objectively," environment. Instead of viewing people as
the potential limitations of the present mea- impartial, data-driven processors, the present
sures should be kept in mind in evaluating the research suggests our models must take into
relationship of this study to prior polarization account the ways in which intuitive scientists
research. As noted earlier (see Footnote 1) assess the relevance, reliability, representative-
our intended strategy of assessing direct ness, and implications of any given sample of
changes from our initial selection measures of data or behavior within the framework of the
attitudes and beliefs, rather than asking sub- hypotheses or implicit theories they bring
jects to report such changes within the experi- to the situation (Lepper, 1977). In everyday
ment, was neither feasible nor appropriate, life, as well as in the course of scientific
given the necessity of selecting subjects with controversies (cf. Kuhn, 1970), the mere
strong and consistent initial views on this availability of contradictory evidence rarely
issue. Potentially such methodological prob- seems sufficient to cause us to abandon our
lems could be overcome in subsequent re- prior beliefs or theories.
search through the use of less extreme samples
or, perhaps more convincingly, by seeing Social Science Research and Social Policy
whether biased assimilation of mixed evidence
will make subjects more willing to act on We conclude this article, as we began it,
their already extreme beliefs. by considering the important links between
social policy, public attitudes and beliefs
about such policy, and the role of the social
Belief Perseverance and Attribution Processes scientist. If our study demonstrates anything,
it surely demonstrates that social scientists
The present results importantly extend can not expect rationality, enlightenment,
the growing body of research on the persever- and consensus about policy to emerge from
ance of impressions and beliefs. Two of the their attempts to furnish "objective" data
present authors and their colleagues have now about burning social issues. If people of oppos-
amassed a number of studies showing that, ing views can each find support for those
once formed, impressions about the self views in the same body of evidence, it is small
(Ross et al., 1975; Jennings, Lepper, & Ross, wonder that social science research, dealing
Note 2; Lepper, Ross, & Lau, Note 3), beliefs with complex and emotional social issues and
about other people (Ross et al., 1975), or forced to rely upon inconclusive designs,
theories about functional relationships be- measures, and modes of analysis, will fre-
tween variables (Anderson, Lepper, & Ross, quently fuel rather than calm the fires of
Note 4) can survive the total discrediting of debate.
BIASED ASSIMILATION 2109

Reference Notes signs. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 1969, 74,


271-280.
1. Kinder, D. R., & Kiewiet, D. R. Economic griev- Furman v. Georgia, 408 U.S. 238, 1972.
ances and political behavior: The role of personal Jones, E. E., & Goethals, G. R. Order effects in
discontents and symbolic judgments. Paper pre- impression formation. Attribution context and the
sented at the meeting of the American Psycho- nature of the entity. In E. E. Jones et al. (Eds.),
logical Association, San Francisco, August 1977. Attribution: Perceiving the causes of behavior,
Morristown, N.J.: General Learning Press, 1971.
2. Jennings, D. L., Lepper, M. R., & Ross, L. Per-
Katz, E. The two-step flow of communication: An
sistence of impressions of personal persuasiveness:
up-to-date report on an hypothesis. Public Opinion
Perseverance of erroneous self assessments outside
Quarterly, 1957, 21, 61-68.
the debriefing paradigm. Unpublished manuscript,
Kuhn, T. S. The structure of scientific revolutions
Stanford University, 1978. (2nd ed.). Chicago: University of Chicago Press,
3. Lepper, M. R., Ross, L., & Lau, R. R. Persistence 1970.
of inaccurate and discredited personal impres- Lepper, M. R. Cognition and social behavior. Science,
sions: A field demonstration of attributional 1977, 196, 765-766.
perseverance. Unpublished manuscript, Stanford Luchins, A. S. Mechanization in problem solving:
University, 1979. The effect of Einstellung. Psychological Mono-
4. Anderson, C. A., Lepper, M. R., & Ross, L. Theory graphs, 1942, 54, 1-95.
perseverance: The persistent influence of dis- Luchins, A. S. Experimental attempts to minimize
credited information. Unpublished manuscript, the impact of first impressions. In C. I. Ho viand
Stanford University, 1979. (Ed.), The order of presentation in persuasion.
New Haven, Conn.: Yale University Press, 1957.
Mahoney, M. J. Scientist as subject: The psycho-
References logical imperative. Cambridge, Mass.: Ballinger,
1976.
Abelson, R. P. Modes of resolution of belief dilemmas. Mahoney, M. J. Publication prejudices: An experi-
Conflict Resolution, 1959, 3, 343-352. mental study of confirmatory bias in the peer
Abelson, R. P. Are attitudes necessary? In B. T. review system. Cognitive Therapy and Research,
King & E. McGuinnies (Eds.), Attitudes, conflict, 1977, 7, 161-17S.
and social change. New York: Academic Press, Merton, R. K. The self-fulfilling prophecy. Antioch
1972. Review, 1948, 8, 193-210.
Abelson, R. P. Script processing in attitude formation Nisbett, R., & Ross, L. Human inference: Strategies
and decision making. In J. S. Carroll and J. W. and shortcomings of social judgment. Englewood
Payne (Eds.), Cognition and social behavior. Cliffs, N.J.: Prentice-Hall, in press.
Hillsdale, N.J.: Erlbaum, 1976. Rosenhan, D. L. On being sane in insane places.
Allport, G. W. The nature of prejudice. Reading, Science, 1973, 179, 250-258.
Mass.: Addison-Wesley, 1954. Ross, L. The intuitive psychologist and his short-
Asch, S. Forming impressions of personality. Journal comings: Distortions in the attribution process. In
of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 1946, 41, 258- L. Berkowitz (Ed.), Advances in experimental
290. social psychology (Vol. 10). New York: Academic
Bacon, F. The new organon and related writings. Press, 1977.
New York: Liberal Arts Press, 1960. (Originally Ross, L., Lepper, M. R., & Hubbard, M. Perseverance
published, 1620.) in self-perception and social perception: Biased
Bobrow, D. G., & Norman, D. A. Some principles attributional processes in the debriefing paradigm.
of memory schemata. In D. G. Bobrow & A. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 1975,
Collins (Eds.), Representation and understanding: 32, 880-892.
Studies in cognitive science. New York: Academic Sarat, A., & Vidmar, N. Public opinion, the death
Press, 1975. penalty, and the Eighth Amendment: Testing the
Bruner, J. S., & Potter, M. C. Interference in visual Marshall hypothesis. Wisconsin Law Review, 1976,
recognition. Science, 1964, 144, 424-425. 171-197.
Campbell, D. T. On reforms as experiments. Amer- Sellin, T. (Ed.). The death penalty. New York:
ican Psychologist, 1969, 24, 409-429. Harper & Row, 1967.
Snyder, M., Tanke, E. D., & Berscheid, E. Social
Chapman, L., & Chapman, J. The genesis of popular
perception and interpersonal behavior: On the self-
but erroneous psychodiagnostic observations.
fulfilling nature of social stereotypes. Journal of
Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 1967, 72, 193-
Personality and Social Psychology, 1977, 35, 656-
204.
666.
Chapman, L., & Chapman, J. Illusory correlation as
an obstacle to the use of valid psychodiagnostic Received February 16, 1979 •

View publication stats

You might also like