You are on page 1of 8

Received: 16 February 2020 | Revised: 27 October 2020 | Accepted: 1 November 2020

DOI: 10.1111/eth.13112

RESEARCH ARTICLE

“Where ignorance is bliss, 'tis folly to be wise”: Indiscriminate


male care in a neotropical spider

Rafael Rios Moura1,3 | Isabella Dias Oliveira3 | João Vasconcellos-Neto2 |


Marcelo Oliveira Gonzaga3

1
Departamento de Ciências Biológicas,
Núcleo de Extensão e Pesquisa em Ecologia Abstract
e Evolução (NEPEE), Universidade do Estado Certainty of paternity is considered an important condition for the evolution and
de Minas Gerais, Ituiutaba, Brazil
2 maintenance of extended male care. In some species, males may cannibalize unrelated
Departamento de Biologia Animal,
Universidade Estadual de Campinas, offspring or abandon the progeny when the uncertainty of paternity is high, or when
Campinas, Brazil
they take over nest sites or females from other males. However, male protection of
3
Instituto de Biologia, Universidade Federal
de Uberlândia, Uberlândia, Brazil
offspring can also evolve in situations of uncertainty of paternity, especially when
males cannot recognize offspring relatedness. In such cases, males may take care of
Correspondence
Departamento de Ciências Biológicas,
all their mate's offspring, regardless of paternity. In Manogea porracea (Araneidae),
Universidade do Estado de Minas Gerais – the only known spider species where males care for offspring, males repel competi-
UEMG, R. Ver. Geraldo Moisés da Silva, s/n
- Universitário, CEP 38302-192, Ituiutaba,
tors by assuming and defending specific positions within a female's web, but females
MG, SP, Brazil. accept multiple partners during the reproductive season. Consequently, males may
Email: rafael.moura@uemg.br
care for some offspring produced with the sperm of their mate's previous partners.
Funding information If males cannot detect offspring relatedness, we expect that they will not cannibalize
Conselho Nacional de Desenvolvimento
Científico e Tecnológico, Grant/Award
progeny and will actively protect all offspring against predation. The main goal of this
Number: 306157/2014-4, 403733/2012- study was to investigate whether the extended male care depends on offspring relat-
0, 441225/2016-0, 445832/2014-2 and
465562/2014-0; Duratex S.A.; Fundação
edness recognition. Therefore, we experimentally manipulated offspring relatedness
de A mparo à Pesquisa do Estado de Minas and the presence of foster males and two egg predators usually found invading M.
Gerais, Grant/Award Number: APQ-02104-
14, APQ-02984-17, APQ-03202-13 and
porracea webs. We also compared our results with data from an experiment per-
CRA-30058/12; Fundação de Amparo à formed by Moura, Vasconcellos-Neto, & Gonzaga (2017) using the same laboratory
Pesquisa do Estado de São Paulo, Grant/
Award Number: 2017/14196-5 and
procedures, but introducing egg sacs fathered exclusively by the males. Males did
465562/2014-0 not cannibalize offspring and protected the progeny against predation regardless of

Editor: Wolfgang Goymann


offspring relatedness. In addition, all males moved the egg sacs to the center of the
web, remaining close to the progeny. We conclude that M. porracea males protect all
progeny present in their partner's web and increase offspring survivorship regardless
of relatedness. We discuss the behavioral and evolutionary implications of our find-
ings, and potential triggers of male care in M. porracea.

KEYWORDS

araneophagy, biparental care, egg predators, kin recognition, paternity assurance

[Correction added on 1 December 2020, after first online publication: the affiliation numbering has been updated.]

Ethology. 2021;127:223–230. wileyonlinelibrary.com/journal/eth© 2020 Wiley-VCH GmbH | 223


14390310, 2021, 3, Downloaded from https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/eth.13112 by UNEB - Universidade do Estado da Bahia, Wiley Online Library on [30/10/2023]. See the Terms and Conditions (https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/terms-and-conditions) on Wiley Online Library for rules of use; OA articles are governed by the applicable Creative Commons License
224 | MOURA et al.

1 | I NTRO D U C TI O N protection. To minimize the risk of cuckoldry, males can use cues (e.g.,
sexual pheromones) to monitor female mating status (virgin or mated,
Females typically invest more in parental activities than males Gaskett, 2007) or increase fertilization success by, for example,
(Clutton-Brock, 1991; Royle et al., 2012). For males, the costs of pro- using mating plugs (Schulte et al., 2010) or guarding mates (Moura
viding parental care (e.g., reduction in mating opportunities with new & Gonzaga, 2017). Furthermore, an absence of parental recognition
partners; increased exposure to predation; and reduction in food in- may evolve in species with high risk of cuckoldry if the cost for future
take) usually outweigh the benefits of providing care to offspring reproductive success of males and for paternal care are low (Griffin
(Fromhage & Jennions, 2016; Queller, 1997; Requena & Alonzo, 2017). et al., 2013). Therefore, parental care can still evolve in polyandrous
Therefore, males usually take care of offspring when they share the mating system when males cannot discriminate offspring relatedness.
costs of parental activities with females, that is, biparental care (ver- Maternal care is often observed in solitary spiders (Yip &
tebrates: Balshine, 2012; invertebrates: Machado & Trumbo, 2018). In Rayor, 2014), while paternal care has been described in only one spe-
some cases, males can assume the parental care role in the absence of cies to date. Manogea porracea (C. L. Koch, 1838) (Araneidae) males
their partners, but the care is usually provided by the female, that is, repair damage to nursery webs and actively protect egg sacs and
amphisexual care (Beal & Tallamy, 2006; Buzatto & Machado, 2009). spiderlings against araneophagic spiders (Moura et al., 2017). Males
Exclusive paternal care is rarely observed (Tallamy, 2001), except for a mature earlier than females in the breeding season and guard sub-
few taxa (amphibians: Furness & Capellini, 2019; harvestmen: Requena adult partners (Moura et al., 2020). During this period, they build a
et al., 2014; fish: Reynolds et al., 2002). These cases offer good op- small dome-shaped web above female's web and fight with other
portunities to investigate whether particular costs of paternal activi- males to secure a prime position. After female maturation and copu-
ties can be outweighed by the benefits of enhanced offspring fitness, lation, males can continue guarding their partner or move to another
providing important insights into mechanisms that affect the evolution web to seek new mating opportunities (Moura & Gonzaga, 2017;
and maintenance of parental behaviors. Moura et al., 2020). So, some males can copulate and guard a mated
Certainty of paternity is usually considered in both experimen- female, even when she has already produced egg sacs and probably
tal (Safari & Goymann, 2018) and theoretical studies (Fromhage & holds sperm from another male in her spermathecae. In this scenario,
Jennions, 2016) as an important factor influencing the occurrence males may incur the risk of sperm competition during egg fertiliza-
and/or intensity of male care. Most studies have found that pater- tion and may ultimately take care of some proportion of unrelated
nal care is more likely to evolve in scenarios where certainty of pa- offspring.
ternity is high (Fromhage & Jennions, 2016; Kokko & Jennions, 2008; The costs of protecting an unrelated progeny are high, as males
Kvarnemo, 2006; Requena & Alonzo, 2017; Wright, 1998). In taxa with may be killed by potential egg predators and lose additional mating
external fertilization, males can often directly assess paternity, and pa- opportunities, especially when several females are still available (i.e.,
ternal behaviors are relatively common (Smith & Wootton, 1995). On in the middle of the breeding season; Moura & Gonzaga, 2017; Moura
the other hand, it is much more difficult for males to assess paternity et al., 2020). In this situation, there are two possible hypotheses: (a)
when fertilization is internal, and females control genetic parentage. if paternity can be detected, males may practice filial cannibalism or
Under high female promiscuity, for example, males may increase fer- reduce their parental protection of unrelated offspring, increasing
tilization efforts by sperm competition and copulatory courtship be- the exposure of some egg sacs or juveniles to predators, or (b) if
haviors to stimulate cryptic female choice (Eberhard, 1996; Requena paternity cannot be detected, males may take care of all offspring.
& Alonzo, 2017) and leave females just after mating (Gilbert & In this study, we tested these competing hypotheses by adopting
Manica, 2015; Yip & Rayor, 2014). Paternity detection is particularly the same laboratory procedures proposed by Moura et al. (2017),
complicated in many arthropods because promiscuous females can but introducing egg sacs collected from distinct webs to compare
store sperm from multiple mates in their spermathecae (Herberstein offspring survival in the presence and in the absence of foster fa-
et al., 2011; Pascini & Martins, 2017). Consequently, males have a high thers. We then compared the results of Moura et al. (2017) using bi-
chance of caring for some proportion of unrelated offspring when they ological fathers and those from this study to evaluate whether males
copulate with mated females. Thus, internal fertilization and high un- adjust their paternal protection depending on offspring relatedness.
certainty of paternity are thought to be barriers to the evolution of We also evaluated whether foster males cannibalized unrelated off-
paternal care in most arthropods (spiders: Yip & Rayor, 2014; insects: spring, and whether male are willing to assume a protective position
Gilbert & Manica, 2015). below the egg sac.
Cuckoldry is expected to reduce paternal effort when the un-
certainty of paternity is high (Griffin et al., 2013). Therefore, when
males can recognize unrelated progeny, they may cannibalize eggs 2 | M ATE R I A L A N D M E TH O DS
or juveniles before mating (Gonzaga & Leiner, 2013; Kondoh &
Okuda, 2002). Alternatively, males may adjust the parental care de- 2.1 | Study area
pending on the presence of indicators of cuckoldry and their capacity
to detect sperm already deposited in the female spermathecae (e.g., We performed data collection at Fazenda Nova Monte Carmelo
Neff, 2003). Thus, offspring recognition by males may drive paternal (18°49′27ʺS, 47°51ʹ47ʺW), Estrela do Sul, Minas Gerais, Brazil. The
14390310, 2021, 3, Downloaded from https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/eth.13112 by UNEB - Universidade do Estado da Bahia, Wiley Online Library on [30/10/2023]. See the Terms and Conditions (https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/terms-and-conditions) on Wiley Online Library for rules of use; OA articles are governed by the applicable Creative Commons License
MOURA et al. | 225

study area is a Eucalyptus plantation, surrounded by areas of na- abilities to repel predators related to male size. Egg sac mass is a
tive Cerrado. Individuals of M. porracea usually build webs close to good proxy for the number of eggs because M. porracea eggs vary
the ground, using Eucalyptus branches and leaf litter as attachment little in size (see Moura et al., 2017).
points for web-supporting silk threads (Moura et al., 2017). The re- Males were left in the containers with their adoptive egg sacs
gion has a tropical savannah climate defined as Aw according to the for 24 hr before initiating the experiment. We next carefully added
Köppen-Geiger classification (Peel et al., 2007) and can be separated predators at the edges of the web in the containers to establish the
into two seasons, the dry (from April to October) and the rainy (from following treatments: only the adoptive male (n = 20); adoptive male
November to March) (Aquino et al., 2007). and F. caudatus (n = 10); only F. caudatus (n = 10); adoptive male
and A. elevatus (n = 10); and only A. elevatus (n = 8). We built a con-
trol with only adoptive males and egg sacs with the largest sample
2.2 | Data collection and experimental design size aiming to increase our changes of detecting male aggressive be-
haviors toward unrelated offspring. Daily observation records were
We reproduced the same experimental protocols performed by made of males’ position in the webs, and any cannibalism of egg sacs
Moura et al. (2017) that investigated the protection of a progeny fa- or spiderlings. Since both M. porracea and egg predators are active
thered by the males. Individuals were collected from the same popu- during all day, we performed observations during the afternoon (be-
lation and in the same period of the reproductive season to make tween 2 and 6 p.m.). After 10 days, we removed both M. porracea
studies comparable. males and araneophagic predators from the experimental contain-
In October 2018, we collected 40 males, 58 females, and 87 egg ers. We then waited for 20 days and counted the number of spider-
sacs of M. porracea. All males were found in webs with egg sacs. lings that emerged from the egg sacs in each treatment.
We conducted data collection in this period because the sex ratio is We compared our results with data from Moura et al. (2017),
slight biased toward females; thus, males may be more likely to aban- which is another study conducted by our research team that per-
don the egg sacs and search for new mating opportunities (Moura & formed the same laboratory procedures to evaluate the protection
Gonzaga, 2017; Moura et al., 2020). To test whether a foster male of egg sacs fathered by the males. We performed the following
will protect an unrelated brood from natural enemies, we collected comparisons: (a) the treatments of this study with a control in which
females of the two araneophagic species from family Theridiidae, males remained with their own egg sacs during the experiment; (b)
Faidutus caudatus (n = 10) and Agyrodes elevatus (n = 8), which are the data on the presence of F. caudatus and (c) A. elevatus with the
usually found on M. porracea webs (Moura et al., 2017). All the indi- data on protection of egg sacs by the biological and foster males. In
viduals of the araneophagic species were adult females and were not all models, we tested differences in the number of spiderlings that
fed before the experiment. To reduce the chances of sampling egg hatched (response variable) between these treatments (predictor
sacs from adjacent webs that were fathered by the collected males, variable) using generalized linear models (GLM) with Poisson error
we did not sample males and eggs sacs found on webs within a 6 m distribution and planned comparison tests for subgroup differences.
radius from the focal web. We haphazardly collected males and egg We graphically inspected the residuals and evaluated overdisper-
sacs spread in an area of 1.29 km2 (4.3 × 0.3 km; length × width) sion using “AER” package (Kleiber & Zeileis, 2008). To run the mod-
close to the trail. We carefully stored each individual and egg sac in els, we used “lme4” (Bates et al., 2015) and “car” packages (Fox &
a separate Falcon tube and transported them to the Laboratório de Weisberg, 2019). We conducted all data analysis in R version 3.6.2
Aracnologia of the Universidade Federal de Uberlândia (Uberlândia, (R Development Core Team, 2020).
Minas Gerais, Brazil). Voucher specimens are deposited in the collec-
tion of Universidade Federal de Minas Gerais (curator A. J. Santos),
Minas Gerais, Brazil. 3 | R E S U LT S
In the laboratory, we put each female in a separate experimental
container (19 × 18 cm; depth × diameter) for at least 24 hr to build All males moved the egg sacs to the center of the web and assumed
webs. All containers had Eucalyptus branches that could be used as the protective position (Figure 1a,b). No cannibalization of egg sacs
attachment points during web building, and wet cotton balls to main- occurred. Two samples of the control group (with male and egg sac)
tain humidity. After web construction, we carefully removed the fe- were removed from analysis because spiderlings escaped from a
males and placed the males and egg sacs in the containers. Males small opening in the experimental containers, and an egg sac was
and egg sacs were first sorted into size categories, and then, indi- attacked by fungi. Both araneophagic predators were observed con-
viduals within each category were randomly assigned to treatments. suming the egg sacs after the second day of the experiment. In treat-
For example, the first set of five heaviest egg sacs and the first set ments with males, predators carefully approached the egg sacs, and
of five largest males were sorted and distributed between the five males reacted aggressively when they identified predation attempts.
treatments (described below). Then, the second set of five heaviest Manogea porracea males killed A. elevatus in three trials, and a male
egg sacs and second set of five largest males were distributed ac- M. porracea was killed by A. elevatus in one trial. No male or preda-
cording to the same procedure and so on. Using this approach, we tor was killed in treatments with F. caudatus. Both predator species
avoided bias arising from distinct clutch sizes or potentially different were observed consuming the eggs and remained close to the egg
14390310, 2021, 3, Downloaded from https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/eth.13112 by UNEB - Universidade do Estado da Bahia, Wiley Online Library on [30/10/2023]. See the Terms and Conditions (https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/terms-and-conditions) on Wiley Online Library for rules of use; OA articles are governed by the applicable Creative Commons License
226 | MOURA et al.

(a) (b) F I G U R E 1 Laboratory experiment with


Manogea porracea males, egg sacs with
unrelated eggs and spiderlings, and egg
predators. (a) A foster father assuming a
protective position, below the egg sac. (b)
A foster father protecting an egg sac with
recently hatched spiderlings. (c) Faiditus
caudatus and (d) Argyrodes elevatus close
to an egg sac in the treatments without
a male [Colour figure can be viewed at
wileyonlinelibrary.com]
(c) (d)

F I G U R E 2 Number of spiderlings
that emerged from Manogea
porracea egg sacs in each treatment:
(i) biological father + egg sac; (ii)
foster father + egg sac; (iii) foster
father + Argyrodes elevatus + egg sac;
(iv) Argyrodes elevatus + egg sac; (v)
foster father + Faiditus caudatus + egg
sac; (vi) Faiditus caudatus + egg sac. The
red points show means, and bars show
standard errors. The black dots represent
the number of spiderlings that survived
in each trial. Equal letters correspond to
p > .05 and different letters to p < .05
for planned comparison tests. Data from
the treatment of related offspring (i) were
provided by Moura et al. (2017) [Colour
figure can be viewed at wileyonlinelibrary.
com]

sacs during most experiment duration in the treatment without M. foster fathers were similarly effective in offspring protection against
porracea males (Figure 1c,d). predation (see post hoc test results in Figures 2 and 3).
The number of spiderlings that emerged from egg sacs differed
between treatments (χ2 = 143.7; df =5; p < .001). The number of
spiderlings that hatched from the egg sacs was higher in treatments 4 | D I S CU S S I O N
in which males were present than in treatment in which they were
absence, regardless of offspring relatedness (Figure 2). The pres- In this study, M. porracea males did not cannibalize unrelated off-
ence of males increased offspring survival by 2.5 folds, on average, spring and engaged in protecting the progeny in all trials. All males
in treatments with A. elevatus and F. caudatus, when compared with moved the egg sacs to the center of the web and assumed a pro-
2
treatments containing only these predators (A. elevatus: χ = 64.6; tective position below the egg sac. Foster fathers were as effec-
df = 3; p < .001; F. caudatus: χ2 = 11.5; df = 3; p < .001). Offspring tive as the biological fathers in the protection of eggs against the
relatedness did not increase the efficiency of offspring protection two araneophagic spider species observed invading their webs in
against both araneophagic species (Figure 3). Both biological and the field. Therefore, our results support the hypothesis that males
14390310, 2021, 3, Downloaded from https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/eth.13112 by UNEB - Universidade do Estado da Bahia, Wiley Online Library on [30/10/2023]. See the Terms and Conditions (https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/terms-and-conditions) on Wiley Online Library for rules of use; OA articles are governed by the applicable Creative Commons License
MOURA et al. | 227

F I G U R E 3 Number of spiderlings that emerged from Manogea porracea egg sacs in treatments containing predators, (a) Argyrodes elevatus
and (b) Faiditus caudatus, and distinct relatedness between males and egg sacs. The red points show means, and bars show standard errors.
The black dots represent the number of spiderlings that survived in each trial. Equal letters correspond to p > .05 and different letters to
p < .05 for planned comparison tests. Data from the treatments of related offspring were provided by Moura et al. (2017) [Colour figure can
be viewed at wileyonlinelibrary.com]

cannot detect offspring relatedness. In the following lines, we will costs associated with the incapacity of identifying unrelated prog-
briefly discuss the behavioral process that can influence male care eny would be low for males, and offspring recognition would not be
and trigger paternal behaviors in this species. We then discuss the expected to evolve. There are some evidences supporting this “mo-
possible factors determining the absence of offspring discrimination nogamy hypothesis.” Large males of M. porracea have advantages in
by males. intrasexual competition (Moura & Gonzaga, 2017). In a promiscuous
The rarity of paternal care in Araneae may be explained by short population, they would increase their fitness by deserting fathered
male life spans and high uncertainty of paternity (Yip & Rayor, 2014). offspring and searching for new mating opportunities. However, the
The dome-shaped webs built by M. porracea males, directly above probability of caring for offspring is similar between males with dif-
the web of a female, may overcome these constraints. While spider ferent sizes in the studied population (Moura et al., 2019), mean-
males usually do not feed during adulthood (Buchli, 1969), the con- ing that strong large competitors were not more prone to abandon
struction of these webs by M. porracea males may provide resources offspring compared to weak small competitors. Therefore, after
during all the reproductive season, potentially increasing their lifes- copulation, males are likely to remain in the web guarding the mate,
pan. In addition, the interception of prey in their own webs reduces regardless their chances of obtaining new mating opportunities.
potential conflicts and competition for prey with their partners In another possible scenario, males may often abandon their
(Moura et al., 2017). Finally, the web building behavior also provides initial partners after copulation, searching for additional mating op-
a long lasting connection between the webs of males and females, portunities. This behavior would possibly generate a reproductive
which facilitates pre- and post-copulatory mate guarding by males advantage during the beginning of the breeding season, when sev-
(Moura & Gonzaga, 2017; Moura et al., 2017). Therefore, the emer- eral receptive females are still available. In addition, as females are
gence of longstanding male webs may have played a significant role able to protect the eggs without male assistance (Moura et al., 2017),
in determining a relatively high certainty of paternity in this species. the impact of male desertion on offspring survival would be low in
In M. porracea, early male maturation (protandry) and the behav- this period due to maternal care. Late in the breeding season, how-
ioral strategy of guarding subadult females before and after mating ever, males would acquire at least some mating opportunities with
may reduce sperm competition, minimize cuckoldry, and increase previously mated females, which carry sperm produced by other
male reproductive success (Moura et al., 2020). There is no informa- males or that already laid some egg sacs before their arrival. Thus,
tion on the natural frequency of male replacement in female's webs, the paternal care observed after female's death will often involve the
but it is possible that the mating system of M. porracea has a higher protection of some proportion of unrelated eggs and/or juveniles.
proportion of monogamous partners than previously thought. If this Our results indicate that males of M. porracea accept the significant
is true and male desertion is a relatively uncommon occurrence, the risks of agonistic interactions with araneophagic invaders to protect
14390310, 2021, 3, Downloaded from https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/eth.13112 by UNEB - Universidade do Estado da Bahia, Wiley Online Library on [30/10/2023]. See the Terms and Conditions (https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/terms-and-conditions) on Wiley Online Library for rules of use; OA articles are governed by the applicable Creative Commons License
228 | MOURA et al.

the egg sacs even in this situation of paternity uncertainty. The especially if the copulation duration of the first partner is low or he
maintenance of these protective behaviors by males in the absence produces less sperm than the second one (Assis & Foellmer, 2016).
of females may be assured by the relative advantage of increasing Females may also dump the sperm from their spermathecae, such
the survivorship of at least some of their offspring when receptive as observed in other spider species (Eberhard, 2009). In both cases,
females are unavailable, rare, or widely dispersed. The proportion females should be receptive to additional mating opportunities, and
of eggs under alloparental care would be influenced by patterns of the new partners may not confront intense sperm competition. To
sperm precedence. evaluate these hypotheses, future studies may investigate whether
We have no information on the possibility of sperm mixing within (a) the amount of sperm deposited within female spermathecae after
the spermathecae or on the occurrence of cryptic female choice, the first copulation is sufficient to fertilize all eggs produce during
but first male sperm precedence is usually predicted for polyan- her lifespan; (b) the copulation with an additional partner largely
drous Entelegynae spider species, such as M. porracea (Snow & increases the number of fertilized eggs laid by females; and (c) the
Andrade, 2005; Watson, 1991). However, it is important to evaluate female can remove the sperm from its spermathecae.
whether first sperm precedence is a pattern of egg fertilization in In conclusion, we found consistent evidence that males of
M. porracea, because the spermathecae morphology may be a poor M. porracea can efficiently protect egg sacs, regardless of parent-
predictor of sperm precedence for Araneae (Uhl, 2002), especially hood, against the two most common araneophagic spider invaders
when the female mate with multiple partners (Zeh & Zeh, 1994). found in their webs. We have suggested some possible causes for
Considering a possible greater advantage for the first male in sperm the investment and risk assumed by males to protect the egg sacs,
competition determined by sperm precedence, indiscriminate pa- but further studies, especially on patterns of sperm precedence,
ternal care may represent a large cost for males pairing late in the percentage of offspring fathered by the protective male, mating
reproductive season. This would be true even for those males that system, and the frequency of male desertion, are necessary to
copulated before the first event of egg laying by the females. In the fully understand this exceptional evolutionary strategy adopted by
situation established in our experiment, however, the costs may be M. porracea.
even higher. In this case, males experienced a situation in which the
egg sacs attached on the web contained only the progeny of other AC K N OW L E D G M E N T S
males. The results indicating the protective role assumed and effi- The pre-title “Where ignorance is bliss, 'tis folly to be wise” is
ciently performed by males in this situation lead to the question of a reference to the poem “Ode on a Distant Prospect of Eton
why they do not act to prevent the investment in alloparental care College” (1742) by Thomas Gray. This project was supported
by consuming the egg sacs previously laid by females or by avoiding by Fundação de Amparo à Pesquisa do Estado de São Paulo
agonistic interactions with the araneophagic invader. (São Paulo Research Foundation, FAPESP: Proc. 2017/14196-5;
The answer to that question requires further investigation, but 465562/2014-0), Fundação de Amparo à Pesquisa do Estado de
there are some testable possibilities to explain the protection of un- Minas Gerais (FAPEMIG: Proc. APQ-02984-17, APQ-02104-14,
related progeny by foster males. The first possibility is that males CRA-30058/12, APQ-03202-13), Duratex S.A., Instituto Nacional
simply cannot discriminate offspring relatedness. We are assuming de Ciência e Tecnologia dos Hymenoptera Parasitoides da Região
that males in our experiment are aware that they are not the fathers Sudeste (HYMPAR/Sudeste—CNPq/CAPES/FAPESP), and Conselho
of the progeny because the experiment simulate the entrance in a Nacional de Desenvolvimento Científico e Tecnológico (CNPq: Proc.
new web. However, it is possible that males are unable to recognize 306157/2014-4; 403733/2012-0; 445832/2014-2; 465562/2014-
any chemical or structural sign to differentiate the egg sacs pro- 0; 441225/2016-0).
duced by their original partners from those inserted in the vials. In
this case, they would be just extending the care provided in a situa- C O N FL I C T O F I N T E R E S T
tion of relative certainty of paternity to the experimental condition The authors declare that they have no conflict of interest.
with a different egg sac. Females of at least three Anelosimus species
(Theridiidae) cannot discriminate between their progeny and foreign ORCID
egg sacs, which may be a step in the evolution of alloparental care Rafael Rios Moura https://orcid.org/0000-0002-7911-4734
and sociality in this group (Samuk & Avilés, 2013). More recently,
however, there has been an accumulation of evidence for parental REFERENCES
discrimination in spiders, but it was investigated only from the fe- Anthony, C. D. (2003). Kinship influences cannibalism in the wolf spider,
male perspective (Anthony, 2003; Clark & Jackson, 1994; Japyassú Pardosa milvina. Journal of Insect Behavior, 16(1), 23–36. https://doi.
org/10.1023/A:10228​93127216
et al., 2003; Ruch et al., 2014; Yip & Rayor, 2014). There is no evi-
Aquino, F. D. G., Walter, B. M. T., & Ribeiro, J. F. (2007). Woody com-
dence, by now, that males of M. porracea can evaluate offspring re- munity dynamics in two fragments of “cerrado” stricto sensu over
latedness (Moura et al., 2017, 2019), and our study is an additional a seven-year period (1995–2002), MA, Brazil. Revista Brasileira de
evidence against male parental recognition hypothesis. Botânica, 30(1), 113–121. https://doi.org/10.1590/S0100-84042​
00700​0100011
A second possibility is that females may use the entire sperm stor-
age from the first male to fertilize the eggs laid in the initial egg sacs,
14390310, 2021, 3, Downloaded from https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/eth.13112 by UNEB - Universidade do Estado da Bahia, Wiley Online Library on [30/10/2023]. See the Terms and Conditions (https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/terms-and-conditions) on Wiley Online Library for rules of use; OA articles are governed by the applicable Creative Commons License
MOURA et al. | 229

Assis, B. A., & Foellmer, M. W. (2016). One size fits all? Determinants Kvarnemo, C. (2006). Evolution and maintenance of male care: Is in-
of sperm transfer in a highly dimorphic orb-web spider. Journal of creased paternity a neglected benefit of care? Behavioral Ecology,
Evolutionary Biology, 29(6), 1106–1120. https://doi.org/10.1111/ 17(1), 144–148. https://doi.org/10.1093/behec​o/ari097
jeb.12848 Machado, G., & Trumbo, S. T. (2018). Parental care. In A. Córdoba-
Balshine, S. (2012). Patterns of parental care in vertebrates. In N. J. Aguilar, D. González-Tokman, & I. González-Santoyo (Eds.), Insect be-
Royle, P. T. Smiseth, & M. Kölliker (Eds.), Parental care (pp. 62–75). havior: From mechanisms to ecological and evolutionary consequences
Oxford University Press. (pp. 203–218). Oxford University Press.
Bates, D., Mächler, M., Bolker, B., & Walker, S. (2015). Fitting linear Moura, R. R., & Gonzaga, M. O. (2017). Temporal variation in size-as-
mixed-effects models using lme4. Journal of Statistical Software, sortative mating and male mate choice in a spider with amphisex-
67(1), 1–48. https://doi.org/10.18637/​jss.v067.i01 ual care. The Science of Nature, 104, 3–4. https://doi.org/10.1007/
Beal, C. A., & Tallamy, D. W. (2006). A new record of amphisexual care in s00114-017-1448-6
an insect with exclusive paternal care: Rhynocoris tristis (Heteroptera: Moura, R. R., Pinto, N. S., Vasconcellos-Neto, J., & Gonzaga, M. O. (2019).
Reduviidae). Journal of Ethology, 24(3), 308. https://doi.org/10.1007/ Does body size influence male care? Investigating an alternative re-
s10164-006-0212-8 productive tactic in Manogea porracea (Araneae: Araneidae), a spider
Buchli, H. H. R. (1969). Hunting behavior in the Ctenizidae. American with amphisexual care. Journal of Insect Behavior, 32(3), 201–206.
Zoologist, 9(1), 175–193. https://doi.org/10.1093/icb/9.1.175 https://doi.org/10.1007/s10905-019-09725-w
Buzatto, B. A., & Machado, G. (2009). Amphisexual care in Acutisoma Moura, R. R., Vasconcellos-Neto, J., & Gonzaga, M. O. (2017). Extended
proximum (Arachnida, Opiliones), a neotropical harvestman with ex- male care in Manogea porracea (Araneae: Araneidae): The exceptional
clusive maternal care. Insectes Sociaux, 56(1), 106–108. https://doi. case of a spider with amphisexual care. Animal Behaviour, 123, 1–9.
org/10.1007/s00040-008-1044-z https://doi.org/10.1016/j.anbeh​av.2016.09.018
Clark, R. J., & Jackson, R. R. (1994). Portia labiata, a cannibalistic jumping Moura, R. R., Vasconcellos-Neto, J., & Gonzaga, M. O. (2020). Female
spider, discriminates between own and foreign eggsacs. International sexual maturity as a determining factor of size-assortative pairing
Journal of Comparative Psychology, 7(1), 38–43. in the protandrous spider Manogea porracea (Araneae, Araneidae).
Clutton-Brock, T. H. (1991). The evolution of parental care. Princeton Zoologischer Anzeiger, 284, 1–6. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.
University Press. jcz.2019.11.003
Eberhard, W. (1996). Female control: Sexual selection by cryptic female Neff, B. D. (2003). Decisions about parental care in response to perceived
choice. Princeton University Press. paternity. Nature, 422(6933), 716–719. https://doi.org/10.1038/
Eberhard, W. (2009). Why study spider sex: Special traits of spiders facil- natur​e 01528
itate studies of sperm competition and cryptic female choice. Journal Pascini, T. V., & Martins, G. F. (2017). The insect spermatheca: An overview.
of Arachnology, 32, 545–556. Zoology, 121, 56–71. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.zool.2016.12.001
Fox, J., & Weisberg, S. (2019). An R companion to applied regression, 2nd Peel, M. C., Finlayson, B. L., & McMahon, T. A. (2007). Updated world
ed: Sage. http://socse​r v.socsci.mcmas​ter.ca/jfox/Books/​Compa​nion map of the Köppen-Geiger climate classification. Hydrology and
Fromhage, L., & Jennions, M. D. (2016). Coevolution of parental invest- Earth System Sciences, 11(5), 1633–1644. https://doi.org/10.5194/
ment and sexually selected traits drives sex-role divergence. Nature hess-11-1633-2007
Communications, 7(1), e12517. https://doi.org/10.1038/ncomm​s12517 Queller, D. C. (1997). Why do females care more than males? Proceedings
Furness, A. I., & Capellini, I. (2019). The evolution of parental care diver- of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 264(1388), 1555–1557.
sity in amphibians. Nature Communications, 10(1), 4709. https://doi. https://doi.org/10.1098/rspb.1997.0216
org/10.1038/s41467-019-12608-5 R Development Core Team (2020). R: A language and environment for sta-
Gaskett, A. C. (2007). Spider sex pheromones: Emission, reception, tistical computing (p. 3.6.2). https://www.r-proje​c t.org/ .
structures, and functions. Biological Reviews, 82(1), 27–48. https:// Requena, G. S., & Alonzo, S. H. (2017). Sperm competition games when
doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-185X.2006.00002.x males invest in paternal care. Proceedings of the Royal Society B:
Gilbert, J. D. J., & Manica, A. (2015). The evolution of parental care in Biological Sciences, 284(1860), e20171266. https://doi.org/10.1098/
insects: A test of current hypotheses. Evolution, 69(5), 1255–1270. rspb.2017.1266
https://doi.org/10.1111/evo.12656 Requena, G. S., Munguía-Steyer, R., & Machado, G. (2014). Paternal care
Gonzaga, M. O., & Leiner, N. O. (2013). Maternal care and infanticide by and sexual selection in arthropods. In Sexual Selection: Perspectives
males in Helvibis longicauda (Araneae: Theridiidae). Ethology, 119(1), and models from the Neotropics (pp. 201–233). Elsevier. https://doi.
20–28. https://doi.org/10.1111/eth.12032 org/10.1016/B978-0-12-416028-6.00008-6
Griffin, A. S., Alonzo, S. H., & Cornwallis, C. K. (2013). Why do cuckolded Reynolds, J. D., Goodwin, N. B., & Freckleton, R. P. (2002). Evolutionary
males provide paternal care? PLoS Biology, 11(3), e1001520. https:// transitions in parental care and live bearing in vertebrates.
doi.org/10.1371/journ​al.pbio.1001520 Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B:
Herberstein, M. E., Schneider, J. M., Uhl, G., & Michalik, P. (2011). Sperm Biological Sciences, 357(1419), 269–281. https://doi.org/10.1098/
dynamics in spiders. Behavioral Ecology, 22(4), 692–695. https://doi. rstb.2001.0930
org/10.1093/behec​o/arr053 Royle, N. J., Smiseth, P. T., & Kölliker, M. (2012). The evolution of parental
Japyassú, H. F., Macagnan, C. R., & Knysak, I. (2003). Eggsac recognition care. Oxford University Press.
in Loxosceles gaucho (Araneae, Sicariidae) and the evolution of mater- Ruch, J., Herberstein, M. E., & Schneider, J. M. (2014). Offspring dynamics
nal care in spiders. Journal of Arachnology, 31(1), 90–104. affect food provisioning, growth and mortality in a brood-caring spi-
Kleiber, C., & Zeileis, A. (2008). Applied econometrics with R: der. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 281(1779),
Springer-Verlag. e20132180. https://doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2013.2180
Kokko, H., & Jennions, M. D. (2008). Parental investment, sexual selec- Safari, I., & Goymann, W. (2018). Certainty of paternity in two coucal
tion and sex ratios. Journal of Evolutionary Biology, 21(4), 919–948. species with divergent sex roles: The devil takes the hindmost.
https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1420-9101.2008.01540.x BMC Evolutionary Biology, 18(1), 110. https://doi.org/10.1186/
Kondoh, M., & Okuda, N. (2002). Mate availability influences filial can- s12862-018-1225-y
nibalism in fish with paternal care. Animal Behaviour, 63(2), 227–233. Samuk, K., & Avilés, L. (2013). Indiscriminate care of offspring predates
https://doi.org/10.1006/anbe.2001.1915 the evolution of sociality in alloparenting social spiders. Behavioral
14390310, 2021, 3, Downloaded from https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/eth.13112 by UNEB - Universidade do Estado da Bahia, Wiley Online Library on [30/10/2023]. See the Terms and Conditions (https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/terms-and-conditions) on Wiley Online Library for rules of use; OA articles are governed by the applicable Creative Commons License
230 | MOURA et al.

Ecology and Sociobiology, 67(8), 1275–1284. https://doi.org/10.1007/ Animal Behaviour, 41(1), 135–148. https://doi.org/10.1016/
s00265-013-1555-0 S0003-3472(05)80509-3
Schulte, K. F., Uhl, G., & Schneider, J. M. (2010). Mate choice in males Wright, J. (1998). Paternity and paternal care. In T. R. Birkhead, & A. P.
with one-shot genitalia: Limited importance of female fecundity. Moller (Eds.), Sperm Competition and Sexual Selection (pp. 117–145).
Animal Behaviour, 80(4), 699–706. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.anbeh​ Academic Press.
av.2010.07.005 Yip, E. C., & Rayor, L. S. (2014). Maternal care and subsocial behaviour in
Smith, C., & Wootton, R. J. (1995). The costs of parental care in teleost spiders. Biological Reviews, 89(2), 427–449. https://doi.org/10.1111/
fishes. Reviews in Fish Biology and Fisheries, 5(1), 7–22. https://doi. brv.12060
org/10.1007/BF011​03363 Zeh, J. A., & Zeh, D. W. (1994). Last-male sperm precedence breaks down
Snow, L. S., & Andrade, M. C. (2005). Multiple sperm storage organs fa- when females mate with three males. Proceedings of the Royal Society
cilitate female control of paternity. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: of London. Series B: Biological Sciences, 257(1350), 287–292. https://
Biological Sciences, 272(1568), 1139–1144. https://doi.org/10.1098/ doi.org/10.1098/rspb.1994.0127
rspb.2005.3088
Tallamy, D. W. (2001). Evolution of exclusive paternal care in arthro-
pods. Annual Review of Entomology, 46(1), 139–165. https://doi.
How to cite this article: Moura RR, Oliveira ID, Vasconcellos-
org/10.1146/annur​ev.ento.46.1.139
Neto J, Gonzaga MO. “Where ignorance is bliss, 'tis folly to be
Uhl, G. (2002). Female genital morphology and sperm priority patterns in
spiders (Araneae). In S. Toft, & N. Scharff (Eds.), European Arachnology wise”: Indiscriminate male care in a neotropical spider. Ethology.
2000 (pp. 145–156). Aarhus University Press. 2021;127:223–230. https://doi.org/10.1111/eth.13112
Watson, P. J. (1991). Multiple paternity and first mate sperm precedence
in the sierra dome spider, Linyphia litigiosa Keyserling (Linyphiidae).

You might also like