You are on page 1of 16

Body Image 46 (2023) 356–371

Contents lists available at ScienceDirect

Body Image
journal homepage: www.journals.elsevier.com/body-image

Are humorous or distractor images more effective than self-compassion


messages for combatting the negative body image consequences of social
media? An experimental test of possible micro-intervention stimuli
Bryony Davies *, 1, Mark Turner 2, Julie Udell 3
Department of Psychology, King Henry Building, University of Portsmouth, Portsmouth PO1 2DY, United Kingdom

A R T I C L E I N F O A B S T R A C T

Keywords: Appearance-focused images on social media are thought to be particularly detrimental for body image. However,
Instagram social media sites can also be used to encourage positive health behaviours. Three linked experiments with 620
Self-compassion Instagram users explored the protective capabilities of appearance-related self-compassion and appearance-
Humour
related humorous messages for women’s body image during Instagram use. Using simulated Instagram
Micro-intervention
Appearance Comparison
browsing tasks, participants were exposed to a set of fitspiration Instagram posts mixed with either self-
Body Dissatisfaction compassion or humorous body image messages, or appearance-neutral images. Results indicated that
appearance-related self-compassion and humorous messages were not more effective at protecting against
negative appearance and life satisfaction outcomes than appearance-neutral images, and did not influence
appearance comparison (Experiment 1), even when the personal relevance to participants’ health was reinforced
through experimental manipulation (Experiment 2). Rather, the presence of any image which did not contain
pictures of women, regardless of image content, led to improved body image outcomes compared to exposure to
fitspiration images alone (Experiment 3). Interpersonal factors such as the similarity of a female target’s
appearance also influenced the nature of comparisons made. The study highlights the importance of diluting
appearance-focused content with other social media images in ongoing research practice and for user well-being.

1. Introduction poorer psychological well-being (e.g. Burnell et al., 2020), whilst tem­
porary periods of abstinence from social media can improve mood and
Social media platforms are a popular, ubiquitous and accessible form life satisfaction (Fioravanti et al., 2020). Despite the negative conse­
of communication which have been widely embraced by the public, quences of use, some authors have identified the ‘immense potential’ of
providing opportunities for users to seek out information, share opin­ social media for disseminating health-related information and encour­
ions, and learn from others across a broad range of formats. However, aging positive health behaviours (e.g. Maher et al., 2014, p. 1). How­
the provision of reliable health communication in this context is often ever, more research is needed to understand how social media site
varied (Moorhead et al., 2013) and an effective strategy is yet to exist, usage, in its many forms, might specifically be adapted to better protect
which exploits the influence of social media in a positive way, to body image online.
counteract the impact of pervasive appearance ideals (Tiggemann, Instagram is a social media platform where users primarily share
2022). Social media use has been linked to body image disturbance, photos and videos. Due to its emphasis on visual image sharing, Insta­
particularly among younger users (e.g. Saiphoo & Vahedi, 2019), with gram is thought to be one of the most harmful platforms and has been
the viewing of images depicting idealised appearance thought to be associated with greater rates of increased appearance comparison and
particularly detrimental to body image (de Valle et al., 2021). Browsing decreased body satisfaction compared to Facebook (Engeln et al., 2020).
the social media profiles of other users has also been associated with Engagement with appearance-focused content on Instagram in

* Corresponding author.
E-mail address: Bryony.Davies@port.ac.uk (B. Davies).
1
Orcid: 0000-0002-3225-4940
2
Orcid: 0000-0001-9072-9708
3
Orcid: 0000-0003-0427-9216

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bodyim.2023.07.003
Received 1 November 2022; Received in revised form 3 July 2023; Accepted 7 July 2023
Available online 18 July 2023
1740-1445/© 2023 The Authors. Published by Elsevier Ltd. This is an open access article under the CC BY license (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
B. Davies et al. Body Image 46 (2023) 356–371

particular, rather than more general use has been associated with body image outcomes online, although it was not clear why this might
negative body image outcomes (Cohen et al., 2017), with exposure to be the case. More recently, the benefit of interacting with body positive
images of attractive peers and celebrities being detrimental to mood and and appearance-neutral content has been replicated on Facebook. Far­
body satisfaction (Brown & Tiggemann, 2016). Instagram has also douly et al. (2023) exposed participants to either body positive or
become associated with the spread of different appearance trends. One interesting science/culture content via Facebook groups, over a
such trend, known as fitspiration, consists of idealised images of thin and two-week period. They found that both body positive and
toned women, which aim to encourage exercise and healthy lifestyles, appearance-neutral content was associated with decreased body
often for appearance-focused reasons (Tiggemann & Zaccardo, 2018). dissatisfaction, but that only body positive content was associated with
Exposure to fitspiration content is known to increase body dissatisfac­ decreased reports of appearance comparison. The authors speculated
tion and negative mood among women (Prichard et al., 2020; Tigge­ that the impact of appearance-neutral content might encourage users to
mann & Zaccardo, 2015). In previous research, the negative effects of focus on things not related to appearance or reduce the salience of
exposure to appearance-focused Instagram content have often been appearance-focused content on Facebook.
identified through experimental comparison with exposure to An alternative theoretical explanation for the efficacy of different
appearance-neutral images that demonstrate no negative impact on types of content for protecting against the negative impacts of social
body image outcomes (de Valle et al., 2021). Such appearance-neutral media, are their links to different mechanisms of coping. Both self-
images have included travel images (e.g. Brown & Tiggemann, 2016), compassion and humour are known emotion-focused and avoidant
architecture or landscapes (e.g. Nelson et al., 2022), and interior design strategies, respectively, for coping with negative experiences (Allen &
images (e.g. Slater et al., 2017). However, such studies are not able to Leary, 2010; Plester, 2009) and have been linked to coping via social
show how body image outcomes might be improved. media (Cauberghe et al., 2021; Keyte et al., 2021). Slater et al. (2017)
were amongst the first to note the popularity of self-compassion themes
1.1. Countering negative body image effects on social media on Instagram, where in 2017 the search ‘#selfcompassion’ yielded 60,
000 results. In the intervening years, this trend has continued with, as of
One approach that has been tested for protecting women’s body 2023, ‘#selfcompassion’ yielding over 1.5 million results. By compari­
image from exposure to ideal images is the use of digital disclaimer son, humour has also come to hold an important place on social media,
messages. These seek to indicate the edited or unrealistic nature of the for example through the use of memes which provide a foundation for
physical appearance of media models as a means of highlighting their circulating ideas, and influencing and reflecting discourse within society
inappropriateness as a comparison target. However, based on results (Miltner, 2018). Memes are images, videos or text which are shared
from their systematic review, McComb and Mills (2020) suggest that peer-to-peer, circulated widely and transformed online by multiple users
overall, disclaimers had no positive effect on body image outcomes. (Shifman, 2013), with the humorous nature of their content being the
Moreover, disclaimers which detail how a model’s appearance has been key driver behind their circulation. Such humour is now widespread on
digitally altered, or included warnings of potential harm, can actually Instagram where, to date, a simple search for ‘#funnymemes’ yields over
lead to increases in body dissatisfaction. This may be because dis­ 76 million results. While researchers are beginning to gain insight into
claimers do not reduce the tendency to make appearance comparisons how such trends may be linked to coping in some areas (e.g. Akram &
(Bury et al., 2016), whilst evidence from eye tracking studies suggests Drabble, 2022; Myrick et al., 2022), further research is needed to un­
that the presence of disclaimers may even act to direct participants’ derstand how memes may be used to protect women against negative
attention to the model’s body to a greater extent (Tiggemann et al., body image outcomes experienced through social media.
2019a).
Some studies have explored whether messages, in the form of ‘self- 1.1.1. Self-compassion and body image
disclaimers’ communicated via image captions (e.g. ‘not real life – took Self-compassion involves replacing self-critical tendencies with un­
over 100 images trying to look good’) are more persuasive when origi­ derstanding, and the acceptance of flaws as being part of human nature
nating from the individual in the image (Fardouly & Holland, 2018). (Neff, 2011). Evidence has suggested that interventions designed to
However, exposure to idealised Instagram images was found to increase encourage self-compassion can be effective at protecting body image,
body dissatisfaction regardless of the presence or absence of a and can help women cope with the negative impact of
self-disclaimer label. Self-disclaimers also do not lead to reduced appearance-focused images. Moffitt et al. (2018) found that the negative
appearance comparisons with the person in the image or lead to images impact of exposure to thin-ideal images could be lessened by women
being seen as less realistic (Livingston et al., 2020). Disclaimer messages engaging in writing tasks involving expressing kindness towards their
in the form of ‘reality check’ comments made by other users which call own appearances. The benefit of self-compassion on social media has
out idealised Instagram posts for being fake or unrealistic have been been demonstrated in studies where self-compassion messages posted
shown to reduce body dissatisfaction compared to positive appearance alongside idealised appearance images have been found to improve
appraisal comments, but do not effect appearance comparisons (Tigge­ body image outcomes for women (Barron et al., 2021; Slater et al.,
mann & Velissaris, 2020). Taken together, current research suggests that 2017), although it is less clear whether such messages also reduce the
the effectiveness of disclaimer labels is sensitive to who authors the tendency to make appearance comparisons.
message; disclaimers are less effective and seen as more insincere when Content alleging to promote body positivity has become increasingly
produced by the same person who is believed to have edited the image. popular on social media, with several different forms of expression un­
However, peer-messages which identify images as inauthentic may be derlying the concept (Darwin & Miller, 2021). Themes that have been
more effective at protecting body image. identified in body positive content on Instagram include the promotion
Other studies have examined the impact of different types of social of self-love, the acceptance of different body types, and an appreciation
media post. For example, Fioravanti et al. (2021) investigated the effects for health and gratitude for the body’s capabilities rather than focusing
of following either fitspiration, body positive or appearance-neutral on appearance (Cohen et al., 2019). However, evidence of the impact of
content over 28 days. They found that daily exposure to body positive body positivity on body image outcomes appears mixed. Davies et al.
content led to increased body satisfaction and positive mood, compared (2020) found that body positive captions encouraging self-acceptance of
to exposure to fitspiration content. However, exposure to both types of appearance were associated with improved weight-esteem compared to
content led to increased appearance comparison. They also found that captions encouraging the improvement of personal fitness, when posted
participants who viewed appearance-neutral content (e.g. travel/nature alongside images of thin and toned women. In contrast, Tiggemann et al.
images) experienced increases in body satisfaction and positive mood, (2020) found body positive captions had no overall effect on body
suggesting that such posts could hold some value for reducing negative dissatisfaction after viewing images of average-sized and thin models,

357
B. Davies et al. Body Image 46 (2023) 356–371

compared to the same images with no captions. More recently, Legault appearance-focused social comparison is thought to be very difficult to
and Sago (2022) have demonstrated that messages which pressured change, partly because they tend to occur spontaneously, and partly
women to feel body positive (e.g. ‘you must accept your body’) may be because women knowingly choose to compare themselves to other
more detrimental to body image outcomes, than messages which attractive women, since they represent the standards by which they
emphasised personal agency (e.g. ‘you are the author of you own expect themselves to be judged (Tiggemann, 2022). The tendency of
happiness’) or acceptance from others in the evaluation of appearance women to assess their level of attractiveness relative to others has long
(e.g. ‘there are lots of people out there who appreciate you just the way been recognised (Leahey et al., 2007), with ‘upward’ comparisons to­
you are’). It follows that not all forms of ostensibly supportive body wards targets who are perceived to have superior attributes being the
image messages may help to reduce the negative effects of social media most common (Fardouly et al., 2017). The emotional outcome of up­
on body image, and that the manner through which communications ward comparisons is thought to depend on whether an individual as­
express positivity or specifically focus on appearance may be critical in similates or contrasts their own appearance with a target. For example, a
determining their effectiveness. perception of similarity between the individual and an attractive target
can lead the individual to develop more positive self-evaluations of their
1.1.2. Humour and body image own appearance through association (Mussweiler, 2001; Kang & Liu,
Some research has highlighted the possible utility of humour for 2019). If the upward comparison target’s status is also seen by the in­
counteracting the effects of appearance-focused images. Specifically, dividual as being personally achievable, then upward comparisons can
Slater et al. (2019) found that the negative impact of idealised celebrity create positive feelings of inspiration and hope (Lockwood & Kunda,
images was reduced when they were presented alongside parody images 1997). In this manner, it has been argued that ‘benign envy’ (Meier &
which mimicked the original source. The authors suggest that humorous Schäfer, 2018) towards a comparison target on social media who is
content designed to critique unrealistic appearance standards may hold believed to deserve their status, can motivate positive change in other
value for improving body image outcomes online. Despite this, the use of users. The nature of interpersonal appraisals including perceived simi­
parody for comic effect may be limited in its practical application, since larity to a target and the achievability of the target’s appearance may
this specific form of imitation requires user knowledge or reference to therefore be expected to influence how appearance comparisons impact
original source being parodied to be effective. women online, beyond the simple consideration of whether or not
The ability of other forms of humour to protect body image outcomes comparisons to a target are made. While there is some evidence of the
online is not yet known. However, the benefit of humour has been evi­ role of interpersonal appraisals on social media (e.g. Kang & Liu, 2019),
denced in broader well-being research, where humour interventions more experimental research is needed to explore how they impact body
have successfully led to improvements in emotional well-being (Craw­ image outcomes in response to appearance-focused images.
ford & Caltabiano, 2011). Humour can be used by women to reject
traditional female stereotypes and roles (Case & Lippard, 2009), and as a 1.3. The current study
way of allowing audiences to contest accepted social hierarchies and
behaviours (Amarasingam, 2010). Satirical humour can also be used to Currently, no clear intervention strategy exists for protecting female
challenge unrealistic beliefs by reflecting their absurdity (Barreca, body image during exposure to idealised appearance images on social
1988). It follows that humour may offer a possible strategy for pro­ media. However, popular online trends which link to mechanisms of
moting the notion that appearance ideals on social media are unrealistic, coping, for example self-compassion and humour, may provide insight
and for discouraging appearance comparison. As a popular form of hu­ into how digital messages could be used to protect body image online. At
morous online content, memes are known to spread effectively when present, there are a number of gaps in the literature. Firstly, previous
audiences emotionally resonate with them (Miltner, 2014), and the use studies have included an array of generalised self-compassion state­
of memes has been shown to increase interest in internet campaigns (e.g. ments (e.g. about the importance of kindness and understanding) with
Lenda et al., 2020). Memes could, therefore, provide a suitable vehicle more directive statements about the body, such that the specific utility
through which messages protective of body image could be shared. of appearance-related, self-compassion (ARSC) messages cannot be
Previous authors have emphasised the importance of health com­ easily determined. Secondly, while a prominent trend on social media
munications which can be targeted to the specific audiences, are tailored and an important mechanism for coping, the utility of humour for pro­
to suit specific means of dissemination (Klassen et al., 2018) and harness tecting body image online is relatively unexamined. Research is yet to
salient cultural practices to encourage engagement (Kostygina et al., fully consider the impact of humorous messages on body dissatisfaction,
2020). It may therefore be expected that health communications which and the application of satirical themes to critique accepted appearance
are congruent with the typical form of messages seen on social media norms. Finally, we believe the examination of social comparison in
platforms, such as Instagram, and offer some level of personal relevance relation to idealised images has generally overlooked the importance of
may be more effective at producing attitudinal or behavioural change interpersonal appraisals such as appearance achievability or the desire
(Schmid et al., 2008). Messages are known to be processed more care­ to look like a comparison target, beyond simple consideration of the
fully and thought to be more persuasive when they are perceived as direction of comparison.
being more personally relevant (Petty et al., 1983). It may therefore be The current research was designed to address the identified gaps in
expected that humorous messages about body image and appearance, the literature through a series of three linked experiments. Experiment 1
which are relatable to participants and delivered in a format that is explored the respective impact of ARSC and humorous body image
familiar and relevant, may provide an effective means of reducing body messages posted to Instagram on mood, appearance and life satisfaction
image concern. outcomes. To do this, participants completed a simulated Instagram
browsing task in one of three conditions. Each condition contained a mix
1.2. Social comparison and other forms of appraisal of fitspiration images with either images containing ARSC messages,
images containing humorous messages or appearance-neutral images.
A further factor that is important to consider when attempting to To further explore the results of Experiment 1, Experiment 2 used an
address the negative impact of social media on body image is that of induction task to influence the extent to which participants would feel
social comparison. The tendency to compare ourselves to others is well- that their body image is negatively impacted by Instagram, and thereby
established as a predictor of body dissatisfaction (Myers & Crowther, influence the perceived relevance of the ARSC or humorous messages to
2009). It follows that an intervention which works effectively to the participants’ personal situation. This was done before completing a
discourage the occurrence of social comparisons will be more effective simulated browsing task similar to that used in Experiment 1. Finally,
at protecting body image. However, the occurrence of Experiment 3 confirmed the impact of the ARSC and humorous messages

358
B. Davies et al. Body Image 46 (2023) 356–371

on Instagram, during a free browsing task on Instagram itself. This fitness. An initial pool of 50 images presented as Instagram posts were
browsing task exposed participants to one of 5 profiles which contained piloted with an independent sample of 30 raters to identify the best
fitspiration images on their own, or mixed with images containing ARSC examples of fitspiration (taking into account the combination of the
messages, humorous messages or one of two different types of image and its accompanying caption), based on the definition ‘physical
appearance-neutral images. In this final experiment, measures of inter­ fitness-based images of women in exercise clothes, designed to motivate and
personal appraisal were taken after the browsing task to explore how inspire others to engage in exercise and healthy lifestyles’. The final images
this was related to the impact of appearance comparisons made by included women from different ethnic backgrounds at a ratio of 18 white
participants. As a whole, the experiments provide a structured approach to 6 other ethnicities, to approximately reflect the ethnic balance of the
to evaluating and comparing the effectiveness of different body image UK population.
health communications on Instagram that are aimed at reducing body The 8 images containing ARSC messages encouraging body positivity
dissatisfaction and harmful appearance comparisons. were sourced through Instagram and internet searches using terms such
as ‘appearance self-compassion’, ‘body acceptance’, ‘body positivity’. ARSC
2. Experiment 1 images consisted of quotes with simple backgrounds (e.g. patterns), for
example ‘Speak to your body the way you would speak to your closest friend
A controlled browsing task was used to investigate the relative about hers… with kindness and compassion’. An initial pool of 16 examples
effectiveness of two different types of body image message conditions were piloted to identify the 8 which best fit the definition ‘messages
(ARSC or humorous) for combatting the negative impact of fitspiration encouraging kind appearance-related self-talk, the acceptance of physical
images, when compared to a third condition of appearance-neutral flaws as natural and commonplace, and the ability to recognise when to avoid
Instagram images which contained no depictions of, or reference to, overidentification with appearance concerns’.
physical appearance. All conditions contained a mix of fitspiration im­ The 8 images containing humorous body image messages were
ages with either images containing ARSC messages, images containing sourced through Instagram and internet searches using the terms ‘funny
humorous messages or appearance-neutral images. The inclusion of the body positivity’, ‘body image meme’. All images contained satirical quotes
appearance-neutral condition was to ensure that any benefit from the with a supporting cartoon-like picture, for example ‘How to get a bikini
exposure to the body image messages could be attributed to the content body: put a bikini on a body’. An initial pool of 16 were piloted to identify
of the message and was not simply a result of diluting the fitspiration the 8 which best fitted the definition ‘funny messages using humour, irony
themes with another type of Instagram content. or exaggeration to expose and criticise unrealistic appearance standards for
We hypothesised that body image messages depicting ARSC or hu­ women (e.g. preference for thin and toned bodies)’.
morous themes would mitigate some of the negative impacts of fitspi­ Appearance-neutral images were sourced via Instagram and focused
ration images on mood, appearance and life satisfaction outcome on ‘interior design’ following similar previous studies (e.g. Slater et al.,
measures, compared to appearance-neutral images. Specifically, it was 2017). These images depicted rooms or furniture only and contained no
expected that the outcome measures of negative mood, appearance people. The initial pool of 16 were piloted to identify the 8 which were
dissatisfaction and appearance comparison taken in the study would rated lowest for ‘containing themes relating to female body image or that
decrease, and life satisfaction increase, after viewing images in the ARSC may inspire positive or negative emotion’.
or humorous conditions, whereas the opposite effect would occur in the All pilot testing was completed by a sample of 30 independent raters
appearance-neutral image condition, resulting in a significant interac­ from the target demographic. All images were presented within Insta­
tion between image condition and time (Hypothesis 1). gram posts. For all posts, author captions were included which rein­
forced the theme definitions for each image type and were consistent
with the specific theme. The captions were either adapted from real
2.1. Method Instagram posts or created to match the image. For example, for the
fitspiration posts captions were reflective of real fitspiration captions
2.1.1. Participants online (e.g. ‘summer bodies are made in the winter’). For the ARSC and
Participants were 241 women aged between 18 and 25 years old (M humorous posts, captions were consistent with the specific theme (e.g.
= 22.04, SD = 2.13), who were purposively sampled based on being ‘embrace the flaws that make you human’ for ARSC). For appearance-
active Instagram users. Participants were not filtered by ethnicity, but neutral posts, captions were reflective of the interior design theme (e.
were all current residents of the UK. A priori power analysis using g. ‘homes with charm’). Identifying characteristics including profile
G*Power (Faul et al., 2007) revealed a minimum sample size of 120 names, likes and comments were removed from each post.
would be required for the proposed experimental design (assuming a
significance level, α = 0.05, effect size of Cohen’s f = 0.25 and assumed 2.1.3. Measures
power of 80%).
2.1.3.1. Sample characteristics. Participants self-reported their height
2.1.2. Materials and weight, which was used to calculate the average BMI for the sample.
Participants also self-rated their own level of Instagram use. This was
2.1.2.1. Stimuli. All participants viewed a total of 32 Instagram posts in done on a scale from 0 (Never use Instagram) to 100 (Constantly use
one of three conditions. All conditions contained the same 24 fitspira­ Instagram).
tion images and were mixed with 8 images containing either ARSC
messages (ARSC condition), humorous messages (humorous condition) 2.1.3.2. Pre- and post-exposure measures. Following previous studies (e.
or appearance-neutral images (appearance-neutral condition). This fol­ g. Heinberg & Thompson, 1995; Brown & Tiggemann, 2016) visual
lowed a 4:1 ratio as used in previous research (Slater et al., 2017). All analogue scales (VAS) were used to measure state changes in outcome
conditions contained the same fitspiration images to test whether the variables at pre- and post-exposure. Using 100-point sliding scales, from
ARSC or humorous messages could help to reduce the negative effects of ‘not at all’ (0) to ‘very much’ (100), participants rated how they felt right
exposure to appearance-focused images, compared to now, in that moment, in response to each question prompt. VAS scales
appearance-neutral images. are believed to be less prone to memory effects and therefore more
Fitspiration images were sourced from Instagram searches using the sensitive to brief experimental manipulations, and have been shown to
terms ‘fitspiration’ and ‘fitspo’. The images depicted young women with correlate well with more detailed psychological measurements of mood
thin and toned bodies either engaging in exercise, or posing in exercise and body dissatisfaction (Heinberg & Thompson, 1995).
clothes or in exercise settings, with captions encouraging exercise and

359
B. Davies et al. Body Image 46 (2023) 356–371

2.1.3.2.1. State mood. Five VAS mood indicators were used (angry, using Qualtrics XM. After providing basic demographic information
happy, anxious, depressed, confident). The two positive mood adjectives participants completed all pre-exposure state measures. Participants
were reverse scored and the five ratings were combined into one mea­ were then randomly assigned to one of the three experimental condi­
sure of negative mood (from 0 to 100). The internal reliability for tions: (i) ARSC condition, (ii) humorous condition or (iii) appearance-
negative mood in the current sample was acceptable at both pre- neutral condition. In each condition, the experimental images (ARSC,
exposure (α = 0.77) and post-exposure (α = 0.74). humorous or appearance-neutral) were combined with the 24 fitspira­
2.1.3.2.2. State body and facial dissatisfaction. VAS scales were used tion images, in a random order. All images were presented within
to measure 7 different aspects of physical appearance. Four items Instagram posts and were shown as a slideshow. To ensure engagement
assessed participants’ evaluation of their own bodies (body shape, body with the stimuli, each image was presented individually for a minimum
weight, body fat, physical appearance) and 3 items assessed partici­ of 15 s before participants were able to move on to the next slide. Par­
pants’ feelings regarding their facial appearance (facial attractiveness, ticipants were also instructed, prior to completing the browsing task,
skin complexion, hair condition) following similar attributes to those that they would be given a short memory test regarding the images they
used in the Body Esteem Scale BES-R (Frost et al., 2018). VAS scales had seen at the end of the study. The time participants spent viewing
were scored such that higher values indicated greater dissatisfaction each image was also recorded, with the average for each image ranging
with the body or face. Both measures overall showed acceptable internal from 19 to 25 s, and an average of 23 min being taken to complete the
reliability at pre-exposure (body dissatisfaction: α = 0.93; facial dissat­ full study. After the browsing task and memory test, participants then
isfaction α = 0.63) and post-exposure (body dissatisfaction: α = 0.95; completed all measures for a second time to capture post-exposure
facial dissatisfaction α = 0.77). scores.
2.1.3.2.3. State appearance comparison. State appearance compari­
son was measured using an adapted form of Tiggemann and McGill’s 2.2. Results
(2004) State Appearance Comparison Scale. Participants rated how
much they thought about their appearance and how much they 2.2.1. Preliminary analysis
compared their overall body, specific body parts and their facial The average BMI for the sample was 24.40 (SD = 6.44) and their self-
attractiveness to other women (from ‘not at all’ to ‘a lot’). At rated level of Instagram use was high (M = 80.90; 95% CI 78.01–83.79).
pre-exposure, participants were asked to imagine the extent to which Preliminary one-way ANOVAs confirmed that participants in the
they would compare themselves to others or think about appearance, if three conditions did not differ in terms of age (F(2, 238) = 0.26, p =
they were using Instagram right now. At post-exposure, participants .772, η2p = .002), BMI (F(2, 238) = 0.95, p = .390, η2p = .008), self-
answered based on the extent to which they had compared themselves to reported Instagram use (F(2, 238) = 0.22, p = .805, η2p = .002), or the
others, or thought about appearance, during the browsing task. VAS total time spent observing the images (F(2, 238) = 0.46, p = .630, η2p =
ratings were combined to provide a single measure of state appearance .004).
comparison which showed acceptable internal reliability at Further preliminary one-way ANOVAs confirmed that there were no
pre-exposure (α = 0.90) and post-exposure (α = 0.91). baseline differences for negative mood (F(2, 238) = 0.58, p = .563, η2p =
2.1.3.2.4. State body shame. State body shame was assessed using .005), body dissatisfaction (F(2, 238) = 0.43, p = .651, η2p = .004), facial
items adapted from the Body Image Shame Scale (BISS) (Duarte et al., dissatisfaction (F(2, 238) = 0.152, p = .859, η2p = .001), body shame (F
2015) and the Body Shame sub-scale of the Objectified Body Con­ (2, 238) = 0.12, p = .887, η2p = .001), appearance comparison (F(2, 238)
sciousness Scale (OBCS) (Dakanalis et al., 2017). The resulting scale = 0.37, p = .694, η2p = .003), or life satisfaction (F(2, 238) = 0.32, p =
consisted of 6 statements (e.g. ‘It bothers me to see my body undressed’) .723, η2p = .003) across the three conditions.
where participants indicated how true each statement was for them from
‘not true for me’ to ‘completely true for me’. This measure of state body 2.2.2. Effect of time and image condition
shame had acceptable internal reliability at pre-exposure (α = 0.88) and Pre- and post-exposure scores for each of the six dependent measures
post-exposure (α = 0.91). in each image condition are shown in Table 1. To test Hypothesis 1, a
2.1.3.2.5. State life satisfaction. State life satisfaction was assessed series of 3 (image condition) × 2 (time) mixed design factorial ANOVAs
using a modified version of the Satisfaction with Life Scale (SWLS; were conducted using the main dependent measures (negative mood,
Diener et al., 1984). The scale consists of 5 statements (e.g. ‘The con­ body dissatisfaction, facial dissatisfaction, body shame, appearance
ditions of my life are excellent.’) where participants indicate their level comparison and life satisfaction).
of agreement with each statement from ‘strongly disagree’ to ‘strongly These tests showed no significant two-way interaction to occur be­
agree’. While typically used as a trait measure, the SWLS has shown tween time and condition in the case of all six dependent measures:
sufficient sensitivity during clinical interventions to detect change negative mood (F(2, 238) = 2.97, p = .053, η2p = .024), body dissatis­
(Pavot, Diener (1993)), and items on the scale are thought to be sensitive faction (F(2, 238) = 2.51, p = .084, η2p = .021), facial dissatisfaction (F(2,
to situational factors (Joshanloo, 2022). To increase the ability of the 238) = 1.76, p = .175, η2p = .015), body shame (F(2, 238) = 0.87, p =
scale to detect acute changes, participants were instructed to answer .419, η2p = .007), appearance comparison (F(2, 238) = 1.96, p = .143, η2p
scale items by thinking about how they feel ‘right now’, with responses = .016), or life satisfaction (F(2, 238) = 2.80, p = .063, η2p = .023). This
being measured on a 100-point sliding scale. The measure had accept­ suggests that any changes in pre- and post-exposure scores were
able internal reliability at pre-exposure (α = 0.91) and post-exposure (α consistent across the different image conditions, for each measure. Hy­
= 0.94). pothesis 1 was therefore rejected.
With respect to the main effect of time, a significant increase in scores
2.1.4. Procedure was found from pre- to post-exposure with respect to: negative mood (F
Participants were recruited through the online participant service (2, 238) = 14.14, p < .001, η2p = .056), body dissatisfaction (F(2, 238) =
Prolific, where it was advertised as an ‘Instagram browsing study’. 38.12, p < .001, η2p = .138), facial dissatisfaction (F(2, 238) = 29.66, p <
Reference was not specifically made to body image, but potential par­ .001, η2p = .111) and body shame (F(2, 238) = 8.30, p = .004, η2p = .034),
ticipants were informed that they would see images of other women and although no significant difference in appearance comparisons was
be asked to answer questions about their appearance, prior to consent­ observed (F(2, 238) = 0.49, p = .487, η2p = .002). A significant decrease in
ing to take part. Any participants who felt they would not be comfortable life satisfaction scores was also found from pre- to post-exposure (F(2,
doing this were advised not to take part. Participants received a small 238) = 11.86, p < .001, η2p = .047). With respect to image condition, no
financial reward for taking part, commensurate with other comparable significant main effects were found for any of the six measures at the p <
studies on the Prolific platform. The experiment was conducted online .05 level.

360
B. Davies et al. Body Image 46 (2023) 356–371

Table 1 relevant to them and to their goals (Petty & Cacioppo, 1986). Recog­
Experiment 1. Pre- and Post-exposure means (M) and standard deviations (SD) nition that an issue is personally important and meaningful to an in­
for all outcome variables (n = 241). dividual’s life, has been found to positively influence their attention to a
Outcome Variable Condition topic and motivation to engage with message content (Kruglanski et al.,
ARSC (n Humorous (n Appearance-
2006). Previous research has suggested that individuals who perceive
= 79) = 80) Neutral (n = 82) themselves to have greater personal involvement with a health topic,
will be more likely to be persuaded by messages about the topic, as they
Negative mood Pre M 40.33 38.66 (18.81) 41.97 (22.12)
(SD) (17.52) will recognise that the message has relevance to their situation (Brav­
Post M 43.51 44.96 (19.17) 42.83 (21.54) erman, 2008). This bias towards information that holds personal rele­
(SD) (19.55) vance being less likely to be ignored or disregarded, has been used to
Body Pre M 62.63 66.26 (27.56) 65.77 (27.03) enhance the processing of health communication (Kreuter & Wray,
dissatisfaction (SD) (25.64)
Post M 70.50 69.61 (28.65) 70.31 (27.32)
2003), and improve the effectiveness of advertisements on social media
(SD) (26.08) (Jung, 2017). Previous qualitative research has similarly shown that
Facial Pre M 56.39 54.55 (21.78) 55.93 (22.25) women express the need to feel personally involved with a health topic
dissatisfaction (SD) (21.57) in order to pay attention to, and connect with, a specific message about
Post M 62.85 57.17 (25.65) 60.47 (26.43)
that topic (Aldoory, 2001). This has been corroborated in a more recent
(SD) (23.96)
Body shame Pre M 49.64 51.02 (27.00) 51.68 (27.65) study where recognition that an issue is of personal relevance was linked
(SD) (26.05) to more effortful processing of message content (Dhanesh & Nekmat,
Post M 51.86 54.20 (27.37) 52.54 (28.23) 2019). Previous surveys of young social media users (e.g. Anderson &
(SD) (27.47) Jiang, 2018) have indicated that they mostly do not consider themselves
Appearance Pre M 63.44 66.79 (27.12) 65.50 (26.27)
comparison (SD) (20.72)
to be adversely affected by the medium, with only 26% reporting social
Post M 65.56 62.12 (30.51) 65.10 (27.12) media use has negatively impacted their esteem, fewer than 4%
(SD) (25.23) reporting that it made them feel ‘a lot’ worse. Taken together, this might
Life satisfaction Pre M 47.90 46.57 (25.90) 44.72 (25.88) suggest that while there is awareness of the psychological impacts of
(SD) (23.73)
social media, young people may not perceive themselves to be at great
Post M 43.63 46.14 (28.70) 42.60 (26.86)
(SD) (25.47) personal risk, which may have limited the impact of the body image
messages in Experiment 1.

2.3. Brief discussion 3. Experiment 2

The aim of Experiment 1 was to demonstrate that embedding ARSC In Experiment 2, the level of personal involvement with body image
or humorous body image messages would be an effective means of off­ concern during Instagram use, and thus the relevance of body image
setting the negative impacts of exposure to fitspiration images, across a messages, was explored by introducing a manipulation to the same
range of mood, appearance and life satisfaction outcome measures. design used in Experiment 1. This manipulation was intended to either
Contrary to expectations, mean body dissatisfaction, facial dissatis­ emphasise or minimise participants’ perception of personal involvement
faction, body shame and negative mood scores increased from pre- to with negative body image outcomes during Instagram use, and thus
post-exposure, in all three image conditions, and no significant inter­ increase or reduce how relevant the messages (ARSC or humorous)
action was found between time and image condition. This suggests that would be to participants’ beliefs about their own body image.
the inclusion of either ARSC or humorous message types in the simulated We hypothesised that participants who were led to believe that their
browsing task failed to prevent the increases in appearance dissatisfac­ risk of negative outcomes from Instagram use was high would experi­
tion and negative mood typically found after viewing fitspiration im­ ence greater negative mood, appearance and life satisfaction outcomes
ages, nor did their impact differ markedly from the pattern of results after browsing fitspiration images, compared to participants who were
seen when appearance-neutral images were included. No significant led to believe that their risk was low (Hypothesis 2a).
change in appearance comparison scores was observed from pre- to post- We further hypothesised a three-way interaction between image
exposure when aggregated across the different image conditions, and no condition, message relevance and time, whereby browsing body image
interaction was observed between image condition and pre/post messages in the ARSC and humorous conditions would reduce the
appearance comparison scores. As speculated by Tiggemann (2022) in negative impact of the fitspiration images to a greater extent for par­
the case of digital disclaimers, it may be that the manipulations intro­ ticipants who were led to believe that their risk of negative outcomes
duced failed to combat the influence of fitspiration images because the from Instagram use was high, compared to participants who were led to
messages did not successfully reduce the number of appearance com­ believe that their risk was low (Hypothesis 2b).
parisons made by participants during the browsing tasks. It could be that
brief exposure to such messages is, therefore, not sufficient to reduce 3.1. Method
practiced tendencies for comparing oneself to other users online or to
disrupt the negative impact of appearance ideal images. 3.1.1. Participants
Since each condition ensured participants spent a minimum of 15 s Participants comprised a new sample of 189 females aged 18–25
viewing each post, it seems unlikely that the results could be attributed years (M = 20.29, SD = 2.37), who were active Instagram users.
to inattentiveness to the images. However, the findings might reflect a Approximately half of the participants were recruited through a
lack of depth to the processing of body image messages, or that partic­ departmental participant pool scheme at our institution, with the
ipants may not have recognised that the ARSC or humorous messages remaining participants being recruited through the participant recruit­
might be applicable to them. In other words, if participants did not ment service Prolific. Participants were not filtered by ethnicity but were
consider themselves to be negatively influenced by appearance-focused all current residents of the UK. A priori power analysis using G*Power
images online, they may be unlikely to heed messages attempting to (Faul et al., 2007) revealed a minimum sample size of 162 would be
protect women’s body image from such images. Previously, the role of required for the proposed experimental design (α = 0.05, effect size f =
personal involvement with the topic of a message has been identified to 0.25, assumed power of 80%).
influence its persuasive capability (Petty et al., 1983). Personal
involvement refers to when an individual recognises that a message is

361
B. Davies et al. Body Image 46 (2023) 356–371

3.1.2. Materials and Measures baseline differences for negative mood (F(2, 186) = 0.94, p = .393, η2p =
The Instagram images and outcome measures used in Experiment 2 .010), body dissatisfaction (F(2, 186) = 1.83, p = .163, η2p = .019), facial
exactly replicated those used in Experiment 1, with participants being dissatisfaction (F(2, 186) = 2.84, p = .061, η2p = .030), body shame (F(2,
randomly allocated to one of the same three experimental conditions 186) = 1.17, p = .323, η2p = .012), appearance comparison (F(2, 186) =
(ARSC, humorous or appearance-neutral). Experimental images were 1.15, p = .318, η2p = .012), or life satisfaction (F(2, 186) = 0.20, p = .816,
mixed with the same 24 fitspiration images in each condition at the η2p = .002) across the three image conditions.
identical ratio (4:1) used in Experiment 1, with all images being dis­ A one-way ANOVA confirmed that the induction task successfully
played using the same simulated browsing task procedure. manipulated perceived personal relevance, whereby participants in the
high relevance condition (M = 56.73; SD = 26.13) considered them­
3.1.3. Induction task selves significantly more susceptible to body image harm on Instagram
To influence the degree to which participants recognised themselves (F(1, 187) = 8.63, p = .004, η2p = .044) after reading the passage
to be at personal risk of negative body image outcomes when using compared to participants in the low relevance condition (M = 45.86; SD
Instagram, and thus influence whether they considered the messages to = 24.64).
be relevant to their situation, an experimental manipulation was intro­ Further preliminary checks confirmed that there were no baseline
duced. Participants read one of two 250-word passages describing the differences between participants allocated to the high and low relevance
impact of Instagram use on young women in their age group in the UK. conditions with respect to negative mood (F(1, 187) = 0.20, p = .650, η2p
The aim of the passage was to invoke participants’ awareness of the = .010), body dissatisfaction (F(1, 187) = 0.66, p = .420, η2p = .003),
negative consequences of social media and their own level of personal facial dissatisfaction (F(1, 187) = 0.13, p = .716, η2p = .001), body shame
risk, prior to completing the browsing task. Both passages provided (F(1, 187) = 0.04, p = .843, η2p = .000), appearance comparison (F(1,
factual information about the impact Instagram can have on body 187) = 0.47, p = .492, η2p = .003), or life satisfaction (F(1, 187) = 1.73, p
image. However, the specific wording of each paragraph was manipu­ = .191, η2p = .009).
lated either to emphasise (e.g. higher personal relevance, greater
severity of consequences) or minimise (e.g. lower personal relevance, 3.2.2. Effect of time, image condition and perceived personal relevance
lower severity of consequences) participants’ personal risk of negative Pre- and post-exposure scores for each of the six dependent measures
body image from Instagram use. For example, one sentence read “9/10 in each image condition as a function of the personal relevance
women in your age group who use Instagram are unhappy with their bodies” manipulation are shown in Table 2. In order to test whether the effects of
(high relevance) compared to “Some young women who use Instagram are the image condition on exposure outcome measures would be greater
unhappy with their bodies” (low relevance). The information used in the when participants perceived their risk of negative outcomes from
passages was informed by the #StatusofMind report (Royal Society for Instagram use to be greater, a series of three-way mixed ANOVAs 3
Public Health, 2017). (image condition) x 2 (relevance condition) x 2 (time) were conducted
A manipulation check was conducted to ensure the induction task on the six dependent measures.
successfully manipulated the relevance of the messages. This assessed With respect to Hypothesis 2a, no significant two-way interactions
participants’ perceived level of personal risk of body image harm from were observed between time and the message relevance induction with
Instagram use. Participants were asked to rate how susceptible to body respect to all six dependent measures: negative mood (F(1, 183) = 2.32,
image harm they felt personally, answering on a 100-point sliding VAS p = .129, η2p = .013), body dissatisfaction (F(1, 183) = 1.15, p = .286, η2p
from ‘not at all susceptible’ to ‘extremely susceptible’. Time spent = .006), facial dissatisfaction (F(1, 183) = 0.11, p = .736, η2p = .001),
completing the induction task was also measured to ensure that partic­ body shame (F(1, 183) = 0.04, p = .840, η2p = .000), appearance com­
ipants paid adequate attention to the paragraph (average reading time, parison (F(1, 183) = 3.52, p = .062, η2p = .019), or life satisfaction (F(1,
around 1 m 30 s). 183) = 1.38, p = .241, η2p = .007). This suggests that leading participants
to perceive their risk of negative outcomes from Instagram use to be
3.1.4. Procedure greater, did not influence changes in mood, appearance dissatisfaction,
The experiment was advertised using similar recruitment informa­ appearance comparison or life satisfaction measures, any differently
tion to Experiment 1. Those who participated via Prolific received a from those who perceived their risk to be low. Therefore, Hypothesis 2a
small financial reward for taking part, whereas those who participated was rejected.
via the institutional participant pool scheme received course credit With respect to Hypothesis 2b, no significant three-way interactions
instead of a monetary reward. The experimental procedure replicated were observed between image condition, message relevance and time,
that used in Experiment 1 exactly, with the exception that after with respect to all six dependent measures: negative mood (F(2, 183) =
completing the pre-exposure measures, participants were randomly 1.24, p = .292, η2p = .013), body dissatisfaction (F(2, 183) = 0.65, p =
allocated to the high (n = 95) or low (n = 94) relevance condition, and .525, η2p = .007), facial dissatisfaction (F(2, 183) = 0.05, p = .949, η2p =
read the appropriate paragraph of text relevant to their condition. .001), body shame (F(2, 183) = 0.58, p = .56, η2p = .006), appearance
Following this, participants were then randomly allocated into one of comparison (F(2, 183) = 0.47, p = .626, η2p = .005), or life satisfaction (F
the three experimental conditions to complete the Instagram browsing (2, 183) = 0.32, p = .728, η2p = .003). This suggests the impact of
task, followed by the post-exposure measures, as in Experiment 1. browsing body image messages in the ARSC and humorous conditions
on pre- and post-exposure measures was not affected by the manipula­
3.2. Results tion of perceived risk of negative outcomes from Instagram use. There­
fore, Hypothesis 2b was rejected.
3.2.1. Preliminary analysis In addition, with respect to the three-way ANOVAs conducted, no
The mean BMI of the sample was 24.17 (SD = 5.25) and their self- significant two-way interaction effects were observed between image
rated level of Instagram use was high (M = 74.47; 95% CI 71.22–77.72). condition and time, or between image condition and message relevance,
Preliminary one-way ANOVAs confirmed that participants did not at the p < .05 significance level. Furthermore, no significant main effects
differ in terms of age (F(5, 183) = 0.57, p = .723, η2p = .015), BMI (F(5, of image condition or message relevance condition were observed at p <
183) = 0.93, p = .462, η2p = .025), self-rated Instagram use (F(5, 183) = .05.
1.16, p = .329, η2p = .031), or total time spent viewing the Instagram With respect to the main effect of time, significant differences were
posts (F(5, 183) = 0.93, p = .466, η2p = .025) as a function of their observed for 3 of the 6 outcome measures. Facial dissatisfaction signif­
allocation to the three image conditions or two relevance conditions. icantly increased (F(1, 183) = 7.00, p = .009, η2p = .037) from pre- to
Further preliminary one-way ANOVAs confirmed that there were no post-exposure, while appearance comparison significantly decreased (F

362
B. Davies et al. Body Image 46 (2023) 356–371

Table 2
Experiment 2. Pre- and Post-exposure means (M) and standard deviations (SD) for all outcome variables by image condition and relevance induction (n = 189).
Outcome Variable Condition

High relevance Low relevance

ARSC (n = Humorous (n = Appearance-Neutral (n = ARSC (n = Humorous (n = Appearance-Neutral (n =


33) 20) 42) 30) 41) 23)

Negative mood Pre M(SD) 46.87 (22.24) 39.49 (17.30) 38.13 (17.17) 38.15 (20.26) 42.68 (16.65) 38.30 (24.44)
Post M 46.22 (22.32) 42.75 (18.17) 42.55 (18.89) 38.30 (19.91) 42.85 (19.18) 36.62 (25.03)
(SD)

Body dissatisfaction Pre M(SD) 68.82 (26.63) 67.25 (25.77) 58.90 (25.95) 67.92 (27.22) 68.21 (21.36) 64.07 (27.43)
Post M 70.30 (27.07) 67.10 (24.73) 63.80 (24.27) 67.77 (27.92) 68.45 (23.46) 63.73 (29.93)
(SD)

Facial dissatisfaction Pre M(SD) 52.94 (22.63) 56.30 (21.56) 53.12 (23.04) 49.97 (20.27) 61.71 (23.39) 49.30 (22.72)
Post M 56.68 (24.69) 57.20 (26.12) 57.85 (24.78) 53.07 (25.36) 62.71 (23.23) 52.45 (23.42)
(SD)

Body shame Pre M(SD) 54.18 (24.25) 52.15 (30.18) 46.17 (26.24) 53.62 (29.87) 46.23 (23.94) 49.74 (27.28)
Post M 54.35 (24.18) 52.65 (29.51) 47.32 (27.65) 53.04 (30.17) 49.43 (24.14) 49.86 (27.42)
(SD)

Appearance Pre M(SD) 61.98 (25.18) 55.78 (24.92) 58.45 (27.80) 59.38 (25.45) 53.05 (24.35) 59.09 (25.87)
comparison Post M 60.20 (26.66) 56.41 (28.54) 57.59 (29.08) 52.75 (30.98) 50.90 (28.29) 48.12 (31.54)
(SD)

Life satisfaction Pre M(SD) 46.48 (22.16) 41.93 (21.42) 44.36 (22.37) 49.91 (20.45) 48.15 (22.18) 48.45 (24.56)
Post M 43.58 (23.42) 40.97 (23.33) 41.39 (22.87) 46.15 (23.36) 43.96 (23.13) 44.67 (30.13)
(SD)

(1, 183) = 5.28, p = .023, η2p = .028), and life satisfaction significantly This was contrary to previous research which has suggested that state
decreased (F(1, 183) = 19.92, p < .001, η2p = .098); from pre- to post- appearance comparison increases after viewing fitspiration images (e.g.
exposure. No significant differences in negative mood (F(1, 183) = Jerónimo & Carraça, 2022). Since in Experiment 2, baseline ratings of
1.06, p = .304, η2p = .006), body dissatisfaction (F(1, 183) = 0.97, p = the main outcome measures were collected prior to reading the rele­
.325, η2p = .005) or body shame (F(1, 183) = 0.98, p = .325, η2p = .005) vance manipulation, it is likely that this text, in addition to the images,
were observed from pre- to post-exposure. combined to influence state appearance comparisons. More specifically,
it could be argued that reading any information drawing attention to the
likelihood of body image harm when viewing fitspiration images,
3.3. Brief discussion whether suggestive of high or low personal relevance, may serve to
reduce appearance comparisons. Whilst a ‘no information’ control group
The aim of Experiment 2 was to determine whether differences in the was not used in Experiment 2, that would support a direct test of this
6 mood, appearance and life satisfaction outcome variables would occur assumption, it should be noted that the observed trend in Experiment 2
between the three image conditions when participants felt body image was different to that seen in Experiment 1 where no relevance manip­
messages were more personally relevant to them. The perceived per­ ulation was applied and no change in appearance comparisons was
sonal relevance of a health topic to an observer is thought to increase the found after viewing images. Subsequent research could therefore further
persuasiveness of health-related communication (Braverman, 2008; consider whether reading simple factual information about how Insta­
Kreuter & Wray, 2003). However, contrary to our hypothesis, manipu­ gram and appearance comparisons can be negative for young women,
lating the personal relevance of the messages did not impact on the might help to mitigate potential harmful body image effects.
observed effectiveness of ARSC or humorous messages. This suggests Whereas Slater et al. (2017) found that self-compassion quotes about
that, regardless of the apparent level of personal relevance or risk, self-love or kindness to the person could be effective at protecting ob­
neither of these two body image message types appeared to influence the servers from some negative impacts of fitspiration, the findings of Ex­
impact of exposure to fitspiration images any differently than periments 1 and 2 suggest mood, appearance and life satisfaction
appearance-neutral images. It can be concluded that ARSC and humor­ outcome measures either worsened or remained unchanged after
ous messages were, therefore, no more effective at counteracting the exposure to fitspiration images mixed with other message types used in
negative influence of fitspiration images than appearance-neutral im­ the current study, including ARSC messages. Whilst this should not be
ages, even when participants felt the potential severity of body image taken to imply that viewing ARSC or humorous messages are harmful, it
harm from social media use was greater to them. does suggest that overall they had no positive effect on body
A further important observation was the absence of any interaction image-related outcomes. The difference in findings between these
between message relevance and time. This indicates that, contrary to our studies might be attributed to the appearance-related nature of the
hypothesis, participants who were led to believe that their risk of self-compassion messages used in the present study. As has been spec­
negative outcomes from Instagram use was higher did not experience ulated by other authors (e.g. Tiggemann, 2022), body image messages,
greater negative outcomes after browsing fitspiration images, compared no matter how well-intentioned, may still draw attention to different
to those who were led to believe their risk was lower. This could be aspects of appearance, and reinforce participants’ existing beliefs about
interpreted to suggest that with respect to body image, the experience of their own body image or the physical attractiveness of the women in the
dissatisfaction or shame regarding one’s appearance is not strongly fitspiration images. In this sense, it might be argued that any
linked to the perception of being at risk. Interestingly, state appearance appearance-related message (whether framed positively through
comparison was found to decrease after image exposure compared to self-compassion or humour) which causes self-reflection on one’s own
before, regardless of the perceived personal relevance induction used.

363
B. Davies et al. Body Image 46 (2023) 356–371

appearance may not have the expected effect of protecting body image. Participants were aged 18–25 years (M = 22.16, SD = 2.07). A priori
Rather, it may be the distraction value of images which do not directly power analysis using G*Power (Faul et al., 2007) suggested a minimum
show images of female physical appearance which may have greater sample size of 150 would be required for the proposed experimental
protective value. For example, Maddox (2021) reports that Instagram design (α = 0.05, effect size f = 0.25, assumed power of 80%).
users described seeking images of cute animals as a form of resistance
and rejection of typical social media appearance-focused content and as 4.1.2. Materials
a way of retaining some autonomy over dominant image trends. Pet
images were considered to make the platform more ‘habitable’ as a form 4.1.2.1. Stimuli. Five Instagram profiles were created for the purpose of
of opposition to the “faux-perfection, influencers, politics, and toxicity” the experiment. Each profile contained the same 24 fitspiration images
(p. 3338). Other research has also linked ‘cute’ animal pictures to pos­ from Experiment 1. One profile, the fitspiration only condition, con­
itive emotional responses and carefulness (Sherman et al., 2009). Such tained only these 24 images, while the remaining 4 profiles included 8
images present an interesting contradiction to fitspiration images that additional images which were dispersed amongst the fitspiration images
focus on objectifying appearance features, or the guilt that may be and served to dilute the fitspiration themes. The images were uploaded
induced from messages surrounding issues of weight, exercise and diet as posts to each of the Instagram profiles in a random order. The profiles
restraint (Tiggemann & Zaccardo, 2018). were given generic names to associate them to the study, and partici­
pants were asked to browse the images as they would do while browsing
4. Experiment 3 other Instagram feeds. Participants were informed that that all images
shown on the profile were real posts from the Instagram accounts of
Experiments 1 and 2 concluded that ARSC and humorous messages different users, but that the profile names, likes and comments had been
were no more effective at protecting against the negative impacts of removed. Participants were asked not to like or comment on any of the
fitspiration images than appearance-neutral images, during controlled posts.
browsing tasks. In Experiment 3, exposure to fitspiration images only Each of the five Instagram accounts defined a different experimental
was compared to ‘mixed’ browsing of fitspiration images which were condition. The 8 posts used for the ARSC condition, humorous condition
combined with either ARSC, humorous, appearance-neutral or cute and appearance-neutral condition were identical to those used in
animal images. The aim here was to explore whether diluting fitspira­ Experiment 1 (e.g. including identical author captions). A new condition
tion images with other types of image that do not contain images of was introduced consisting of 8 images of cute animals which were
women, rather than the inclusion of body image messages per se, would sourced through Instagram searches using terms such as ‘cute pets’. The 8
lead to reduced negative outcomes. The experiment also addresses images were selected during pilot testing of 16 images based on the
whether images which have been typically associated with positive perceived ‘cuteness’ of the animal in each image.
affect (i.e. images of cute animals) may serve to positively distract
participants from negative appearance themes. The impact of interper­ 4.1.3. Measures
sonal appraisals concerning how participants position their own goals All pre- and post-exposure measures used in Experiment 1 were
and motivations with respect to the attainability of fitspiration is also replicated in the current experiment, with the inclusion of an additional
explored. In this experiment, participants were exposed to Instagram measure to explore appearance comparisons made during the browsing
images using a more naturalistic browsing task which involved access­ tasks.
ing their own Instagram accounts to complete the browsing task within
Instagram itself (rather than via a slideshow of serially presented images 4.1.3.1. Appearance comparison: interpersonal appraisal. At post-
within Qualtrics as in Experiments 1 and 2). This was done to better exposure, 6 additional questions about appearance comparison were
replicate everyday use of the platform. asked to explore the specific characteristics of comparisons made by
We hypothesised that participants who browsed fitspiration images participants during the browsing task. The measures recorded how
only (mixed with no other images) would experience greater negative similar participants perceived themselves to be to the female targets
mood, appearance and life satisfaction outcomes compared to partici­ they had seen in the fitspiration images, how achievable the target’s
pants who viewed fitspiration images embedded alongside other images appearance was and how much participants desired to look like the
on Instagram (Hypothesis 3a). targets. Each item was measured on a 100-point VAS from ‘not at all’ to
We further hypothesised that participants who browsed fitspiration ‘very much’. The direction of the appearance comparisons made with the
images alongside positive distractor images depicting animal ‘cuteness’ target’s body, face, and general lifestyle were also measured. Partici­
would experience fewer negative outcomes, compared to participants pants were asked to rate whether they considered their own appearance
who viewed fitspiration images alongside body image (ARSC or hu­ and lifestyle to be better or worse than the target’s they had seen. These
morous) messages or appearance-neutral interior design images (Hy­ ratings were taken on a 100-point scale from ‘worse’ (0) to ‘better’ (100),
pothesis 3b). with the mid-point labelled ‘about the same’ (50) and were made
Finally, we hypothesised that the specific nature of appraisals, made collectively in response to all images seen.
by participants towards the women in the fitspiration images, would
influence how participants felt about their own appearance and their 4.1.3.2. Memory test. A post-exposure memory test was used to ensure
appearance comparison tendency after completing the browsing task participants paid attention to the browsing task. Twelve sets of images
(Hypothesis 3c). were created containing one image from the browsing task and another
image showing a similar theme, but that had not been included in the
4.1. Method browsing task. Participants were asked to identify which of the image
pairs (A or B) had been included in the browsing task. Following the
4.1.1. Participants same ratio of images used in the browsing task (1:4), 8 image-pairs were
A further sample of 204 females, who were active Instagram users, fitspiration images and 4 image-pairs were of non-fitspiration images (e.
were recruited. Participants were not filtered by ethnicity but were all g. ARSC), as relevant to the condition to which the participant had been
current residents of the UK. A total of 14 participants were excluded assigned.
from the analysis based on the time taken to complete the browsing task
(< 1 min n = 6; > 10 min n = 4) or for failing the attention checks 4.1.4. Procedure
(failing to correctly identify ≥75% of the images used in the experiment This experiment was advertised using the same procedure as
during the memory test n = 4), leaving a total of 190 participants.

364
B. Davies et al. Body Image 46 (2023) 356–371

Experiment 1, with all participants being recruited through the Prolific body dissatisfaction (F(1, 185) = 8.41, p = .004, η2p = .043), facial
online service and receiving a small financial payment for taking part. dissatisfaction (F(1, 185) = 5.02, p = .026, η2p = .026), body shame (F(1,
The image browsing task took place within each participant’s own 185) = 12.63, p < .001, η2p = .064) and life satisfaction (F(1, 185) = 4.50,
Instagram account with their responses being recorded through the p = .035, η2p = .024). These results provide support for Hypothesis 3a
Qualtrics XM online research platform. Participants first completed since participants in the fitspiration only condition experienced signif­
consent, demographic and pre-exposure measures within the online icantly greater increases in body dissatisfaction, facial dissatisfaction
platform, before being randomly allocated to one of the five conditions and body shame and a significantly greater decrease in life satisfaction,
to complete their browsing task. Once allocated to a condition, partici­ compared to participants in the mixed conditions. Table 3 shows the
pants were provided with instructions directing them to use their own mean change scores for each of the 6 dependant variables.
Instagram accounts to search for the named Instagram profile that was In order to test the prediction that positive images of cute animals
unique to their condition. Participants were instructed to ‘browse all of would better mitigate the negative effects of the fitspiration images,
the images posted to this profile making sure to look at all of the posts. This compared to the other body image (ARSC and humorous) messages or
can be done in any order, spending as much or as little time on each image appearance-neutral interior design images, a one-way independent
and browsing up and down as you wish’. Participants spent on average 3 MANOVA was conducted. This compared the 4 mixed conditions
min and 34 s completing the browsing task. To ensure participants (excluding the fitspiration only condition) using the change scores for
engaged actively with the task, they were informed prior to taking part the 6 outcome measures as dependent variables. This revealed no sig­
that they would later be asked to complete a memory test regarding the nificant multivariate effect of condition (F(18, 433.24) = 0.69, p = .821,
images they had seen. In addition, as part of the browsing task partici­ η2p = .026; Wilks’ λ = 0.923).
pants were asked to select their three favourite images they had seen on In addition, 6 planned orthogonal comparisons confirmed there were
the profile during the task and to send the links for these images, via no significant differences in the change scores between the cute animal
Instagram, to the Experimenter. Participants spent an average of 2 min condition and the other three mixed conditions (ARSC, humorous or
and 55 s completing this element of the task. After completing the appearance-neutral interior design) for all 6 outcome measures: nega­
Instagram browsing task, participants were directed to return to the tive mood (F(1, 158) = 0.01, p = .980, η2p = .000), body dissatisfaction (F
Qualtrics platform in order to complete the remaining post-exposure (1, 158) = 0.78, p = .380, η2p = .005), facial dissatisfaction (F(1, 158) =
measures and memory test. 0.07, p = .78, η2p = .000), body shame (F(1, 158) = 0.62, p = .432, η2p =
.004), appearance comparison (F(1, 158) = 0.79, p = .375, η2p = .005) or
4.2. Results life satisfaction (F(1, 158) = 0.18, p = .673, η2p = .001). These results
indicated that the mixed condition containing images of cute animals
4.2.1. Sample characteristics did not lead to significantly better outcomes than any of the other three
Participants had a mean BMI of 24.54 (SD = 6.15) and self-rated their mixed conditions. Therefore, Hypothesis 3b was rejected.
use of Instagram to be high (M = 85.04; 95% CI 82.73–87.36). Pre­
liminary one-way ANOVAs confirmed that participants in the five con­ 4.2.3. The role of appearance comparison: interpersonal appraisals
ditions did not differ in terms of their age (F(4, 185) = 1.32, p = .265, η2p To assess the direction of comparisons relating to how participants
= .028), BMI (F(4, 185) = 1.47, p = .213, η2p = .031), self-reported evaluated their own body and facial appearance, and overall lifestyle,
Instagram use (F(4, 185) = 0.42, p = .792, η2p = .009), or time spent compared to the female images they had seen during the Instagram
browsing the Instagram profile (F(4, 185) = 1.10, p = .357, η2p = .023). browsing task, comparison ratings were categorised into three groups.
Ratings of ≤ 44 were classified as upward comparisons (participants
4.2.2. The impact of diluting fitspiration images rated themselves to be ‘worse’ than the female targets), scores of 56 ≥
To explore whether diluting the fitspiration images with other image were classified as downward comparisons (participants rated them­
types during Instagram browsing would reduce the negative impact on selves to be ‘better’ than the female targets) and scores ≥ 45 to ≤ 55
the measures taken, regardless of whether dilution came from body were considered as lateral comparisons (participants considered them­
image relevant messages (i.e. ARSC or humorous conditions) or other selves as being no different from the targets). Across all conditions,
distractor images (i.e. appearance-neutral interior design images or upward comparisons (body n = 164; face n = 128; lifestyle n = 147)
images of cute animals), a series of one-way independent groups were more common than downward (body n = 12; face n = 22; lifestyle
ANOVAs were performed. For this experiment, change scores were n = 23) and lateral (body n = 14; face n = 40; lifestyle n = 20) com­
calculated for each of the 6 outcome variables, by subtracting the post- parisons. This suggested that the majority of participants were engaging
exposure scores from the pre-exposure scores, and planned comparisons in upward comparisons to both the physical appearance and lifestyle of
were used to compare the change scores in the fitspiration only condi­ the women in the fitspiration images shown during the browsing tasks.
tion, to change scores observed in the remaining mixed conditions Participants were also asked to appraise their similarity to the female
(ARSC, humorous, appearance-neutral interior design, cute animals). targets, how much they desired to look like them and how achievable
Since Experiment 1 and 2 had already demonstrated there was no they thought the women’s bodies were. The sample averages were
overall difference in the impact of ARSC, humorous or appearance- compared to the mid-point (50) of each 100-point measure. Fig. 1 de­
neutral images on the outcome measures, relative to each other, the picts how each of the measures, including the comparison direction
rationale for this analysis was to test specifically whether diluting fit­ measures, related to the midpoint of the scale.
spiration images would lead to a different pattern of results compared to Statistical comparison to the mid-point of each rating revealed that
exposure to fitspiration imagery alone. For the change scores, larger, participants considered themselves to be more dissimilar than similar to
positive values indicated that the scores on the outcome measure had the fitspiration women (t(189) = − 17.06, p < .001, d = − 1.24). Par­
reduced following exposure to the images. ticipants desired to look like the fitspiration women (t(189) = 10.48,
Six planned orthogonal comparisons were made using the SPSS p < .001, d = 0.76) and considered the targets bodies to be achievable (t
LMatrix subcommand. For two of the planned comparisons, no signifi­ (189) = − 2.28, p = .024, d = 0.17). For all three comparison attributes,
cant difference was observed between the fitspiration only condition participants considered their bodies (t(189) = − 16.46, p < .001,
and the mixed conditions with respect to negative mood (F(1, 185) = d = − 1.19), faces (t(189) = − 11.09, p < .001, d = − 0.80) and lifestyles
1.26, p = .263, η2p = .007) or appearance comparison (F(1, 185) = 3.77, (t(189) = − 12.69, p < .001, d = − 0.92) to be worse than those of the
p = .054, η2p = .020). females in the fitspiration images.
Four planned comparisons revealed significant differences between To investigate the specific role played in the mood, appearance and
the fitspiration only condition and the mixed conditions with respect to life satisfaction outcomes by the manner in which fitspiration images

365
B. Davies et al. Body Image 46 (2023) 356–371

Table 3
Experiment 3. Planned orthogonal comparisons between fitspiration-only condition and fitspiration-plus-distractor conditions. Data shown are mean change scores (
± 95% CI) for each VAS measure from pre- to post- exposure (n = 190).
Measure Mean change in VAS rating [ ± 95% CI] for each Browsing Task Condition Planned
Comparison

Fitspiration Only (n = 38) ARSC (n = 39) Humorous (n = 37) Appearance-Neutral (n = 38) Cute animals (n = 38) F p

Negative mood -1.11 [− 5.05, 2.84] 3.81 [− 0.09, 7.71] 0.01 [− 4.00, 4.01] 0.32 [− 3.63, 4.27] 1.50 [− 2.45, 5.44] 1.26 .263 ns
Body dissatisfaction -5.87 [− 10.33, − 1.41] 3.81 [− 0.59, 8.22] 1.64 [− 2.88, 6.16] 0.91 [− 3.56, 5.37] -0.51 [− 4.97, 3.96] 8.41 .004
Facial Dissatisfaction -4.19 [− 8.07, − 0.32] 2.32 [− 1.51, 6.14] 0.51 [− 3.41, 4.44] -0.84 [− 4.71, 3.03] 0.92 [− 2.95, 4.80] 5.02 .026
Body shame -7.12 [− 10.80, − 3.45] 0.79 [− 2.84, 4.42] 0.06 [− 3.67, 3.78] 0.72 [− 2.96, 4.39] -0.46 [− 4.14, 3.21] 12.63 < .001
Appearance -2.51 [− 8.87, 3.85] 7.23 [0.95, 13.51] 0.39 [− 6.05, 6.84] 3.86 [− 2.50, 10.22] 6.45 [− 0.09, 12.81] 3.77 .054 ns
comparison
Life Satisfaction 3.09 [0.05, 6.13] 0.45 [− 2.56, 3.45] -0.13 [− 3.21, 2.95] -1.50 [− 4.54, 1.54] -1.10 [− 4.14, 1.95] 4.50 .035

Note. Negative mean values indicate an increase in the measure from pre-exposure to post-exposure.

Fig. 1. Experiment 3. Mean participant-target comparison ratings ( ± 95% CI) for interpersonal appraisal measures using 100-point visual analogue scales. Statistical
data indicate single mean t-test probabilities (2-tailed) and effect size (Cohen’s d) values between sample mean and scale mid-point for each comparison rat­
ing (n = 190).

were evaluated, the relationships between the six interpersonal suggest that the different interpersonal appraisal scores of participants
appraisal factors, with the post-exposure measures were examined were significantly related to the outcome measures taken following the
across all conditions. Table 4 shows the zero-order correlations between browsing tasks. However, not all the relationships remained significant
the post-exposure mood, appearance and life satisfaction measures and after controlling for baseline levels of the relevant measure. The partial
interpersonal appraisal factors, and partial correlations for the same correlations support the idea that, when participants considered their
variables when controlling for pre-exposure scores. Generally, the re­ own appearance to have greater similarity to the comparison target’s
lationships observed provide support for Hypothesis 3c since the data appearance, outcomes across the body image measures were more

Table 4
Experiment 3. Pearson zero-order correlations (r, df=188) between VAS participant-target comparison ratings (interpersonal appraisal factors) and post-exposure
measures of mood, appearance dissatisfaction and comparison and life satisfaction, and partial correlations (rp, df = 187) controlling for baseline levels of the rele­
vant measure.
Comparison Ratings Correlation Type Post-Exposure Scores

Negative Mood Body Dissatisfaction Facial Dissatisfaction Body Shame Appearance Comparison Life Satisfaction

Similarity to target r -.41** -.64** -.33** -.57** -.38** .43**


rp -.22* -.36** -.25** -.28** -.25** .20
Body Comparison r -.36** -.65** -.38** -.52** -.43** .35**
rp -.18 -.42** -.30** -.25** -.31** .21*
Facial Comparison r -.36** -.53** -.56** -.39** -.43** .42**
rp -.17 -.34** -.35** -.14 -.37** .28**
Lifestyle Comparison r -.39** -.52** -.30** -.46** -.38** .42**
rp -.17 -.33** -.26** -.20 -.20 .24**
Target achievability r -.27** -.19** -.13 -.29** -.21** .25**
rp -.09 -.09 -.03 -.17 -.16 .03
Desire to look like target r .26** .44** .34** .39** -.48** .17
rp .15 .26** .25** .20 -.32** -.10
*
p < .005 (Bonferroni corrected p value);
**
p < .001.

366
B. Davies et al. Body Image 46 (2023) 356–371

positive. Upward comparisons, where participants considered their likely that the dynamic range of scores on these variables was artificially
body and facial appearance to be inferior to the comparison target’s restricted in the current study, by the use of fitspiration images. Future
appearance, led to poorer outcomes for appearance dissatisfaction, research could further explore the interaction between similarity, ach­
appearance comparison and life satisfaction, but not negative mood. ievability and desirability, across a broader range of body types to better
Only upward body comparison, and not facial comparison, led to poorer understand their combined effect on body image outcomes.
outcomes for body shame when baseline levels were controlled. Upward
lifestyle comparisons led to poorer outcomes for body and facial 5. General discussion
appearance dissatisfaction and life satisfaction only. Where participants
desired to look like the comparison target to a greater extent, they The current research builds on previous literature by exploring the
experienced poorer outcomes for appearance dissatisfaction and protective impact of ARSC and humorous body image messages when
appearance comparison, but not negative mood, body shame or life posted alongside fitspiration content. The results show that images
satisfaction. How achievable the targets’ physical appearance was for containing ARSC or humorous messages did not influence the negative
the observer was not related to any of the outcome measures, after effects of fitspiration images on users any differently than appearance-
controlling for baseline levels. Therefore, Hypothesis 3c was partially neutral images (Hypothesis 1), irrespective of whether participants rec­
supported. ognised that the messages were relevant to their personal body image
(Hypothesis 2b). Both ARSC and humorous messages also failed to impact
4.3. Brief discussion state appearance comparison. In Experiment 3, the addition of a fitspi­
ration only condition demonstrated that combining fitspiration images
The aim of Experiment 3 was to explore whether diluting fitspiration with any other image used in the study, regardless of type, negated the
images with other image types, rather than the inclusion of body posi­ impact observed in the fitspiration only condition (Hypothesis 3a and
tive communication (ARSC or humorous messages) per se would lead to Hypothesis 3b). This suggests that mixing fitspiration posts with other
less negative outcomes for participants, and whether the use of other types of Instagram post that does not contain appearance-related im­
distraction (appearance-neutral interior design or cute animal images) ages, may be enough to counter some of the short-term impacts of
would further improve this effect. In partial support of Hypothesis 3a, the exposure to fitspiration themes. This finding has important methodo­
results indicated that participants in the fitspiration only condition logical implications as it draws into question the appropriateness of the
experienced significant increases in body dissatisfaction, facial dissat­ use of fitspiration-only control conditions when making claims about the
isfaction and body shame and significant decreases in life satisfaction, protective influence of different image types. It can be argued that there
compared to participants in any of the four mixed conditions. This is a benefit to the inclusion of other image types during appearance-
suggests that combining fitspiration posts with any of the other posts focused Instagram use, which results not from the content of the im­
included in this study negated some of the negative effects of viewing ages per se, but from the diluting effect they create on appearance
the fitspiration images on their own. However, contrary to Hypothesis 3b themes. Future studies could therefore usefully consider the balance of
no differences in outcome measures were observed between the mixed appearance-neutral to appearance-focused images used in experimental
conditions. This suggests that positive distraction by using cute animal research, in order to more carefully evaluate and make recommenda­
images was not more protective for body image than appearance-neutral tions regarding the level of image dilution needed to create a beneficial
interior design images or images containing body image-related effect on body image-related outcomes. This conclusion is consistent
messages. with the recent analysis of body positive and science/culture Facebook
Experiment 3 also aimed to explore how interpersonal appraisals pages (Fardouly et al., 2023) which resulted in no differences in body
with the target women would impact on the outcome measures. To this dissatisfaction outcomes being found between the two page types.
extent, Hypothesis 3c, that type of appraisal made would influence mood, While in general self-compassion interventions have shown positive
appearance and life satisfaction outcomes after completing the browsing effects for body image concern and eating behaviour (Turk & Waller,
tasks, was partially supported by correlational evidence. These findings 2020), the findings of the current study suggest that ARSC messages are
were consistent with the idea that contrasting or assimilating with a not effective at promoting positive body image during acute exposure to
comparison target influences the outcome of upward comparisons (e.g. fitspiration images. This is in contrast to previous research, which has
Mussweiler, 2001). The results add to the literature by evidencing that shown benefits from self-compassion quotes about the whole self (Slater
participants who consider themselves to be dissimilar from a compari­ et al., 2017). The current findings may have implications for the use of
son target, who they desire to look like, experience the greatest negative social media messages about body image alongside appearance-focused
outcomes when browsing fitspiration images, particularly with respect social media content, since humorous messages were also found to be
to appearance dissatisfaction and level of state appearance comparison. ineffective. Taken together, with the findings of previous research that
Previous research has suggested that where the status or attributes of an neither disclaimer or body positive captions were able to reduce the
upward comparison target are considered personally achievable (e.g. negative impact of exposure to appearance-focused images, it might be
their attractive appearance could be matched by the observer), then suggested that the influence of images is too great to be countered by
upward comparisons may result in more positive feelings and inspira­ short messages alone (Brown & Tiggemann, 2020). This notion might
tion (Lockwood & Kunda, 1997). It might therefore, have been expected also be consistent with the disparity in the effectiveness of using hu­
that perceiving the appearance of the fitspiration women to be more morous parody images (Slater et al., 2019), versus the humorous mes­
achievable would be associated with more positive outcomes in the sages in the current experiments which also aimed to critique the
current study. However, this was not the case for any of the mood, common sense of beauty standards. Combined with the lack of support
appearance and lifestyle outcomes, when baseline levels were for disclaimer-type labels, it may be that rather than employing
controlled. This implies that the connection between these outcomes message-based techniques, efforts to protect body image online should
and a fitspiration-style body type being considered achievable, was look to create strategies which are focused on image-based in­
better accounted for by the observer’s pre-existing level of dissatisfac­ terventions. For example, one strategy could be to utilised efforts that
tion or shame with their appearance, rather than as a direct result of seek to increase the representation of larger body sizes, where exposure
their experience when viewing images. It was found in the present study to body positive posts including images of women with a range of body
that participants overall tended to rate themselves as being dissimilar to sizes can lead to increased body satisfaction and appreciation, compared
the females in the images seen, with a strong desire to look like them, but to exposure to the thin-ideal and appearance-neutral images (Nelson
generally indicating an average possibility of achieving this appearance et al., 2022). Furthermore, unlike exposure to fitspiration images of thin
(with respect to absolute levels of the measures taken). It is therefore women, exposure to ‘curvy fitspiration’ (exercise images of women with

367
B. Davies et al. Body Image 46 (2023) 356–371

larger body sizes) is known to lead to improvements in body satisfaction intervention strategies intended to protect body image online, the
and reduced weight bias among women (Cha et al., 2022). This suggests findings of this study imply that a wealth of different possibilities may
that even within appearance-focused domains the impact of more in­ exist, since intervention designs do not need to be tied to the topic of
clusive images may be beneficial. body image in order to promote potential beneficial effects. On the
State appearance comparison was measured in the current research contrary, body image-related messages embedded within social media
to explore the potential of the ARSC and humorous messages for pages that are intended to be protective may have no additional benefits
reducing appearance-based comparison, since this is a known mediator over other forms of non-appearance focused content.
of the relationship between appearance-focused social media use and
body dissatisfaction (Hendrickse et al., 2017). However, evidence from 5.1. Study limitations
all three experiments suggested that neither ARSC or humorous message
types had any impact on levels of appearance comparisons made by The findings of the current experiments should be taken with the
participants, nor did fitspiration images shown in isolation have a sig­ following limitations in mind. Firstly, the experiments sampled from a
nificant impact on appearance comparison in Experiment 3. It may be demographic of young women in the UK, therefore the results may not
that acute exposure to body image messages intended to dissuade people generalise to other groups of women. In the present study, no data
from making comparisons, are not sufficient on their own to combat a pertaining to the cultural background of participants were collected
practiced tendency to make upward comparisons on social media which may limit the applicability of the current findings to populations
(Verduyn et al., 2020). In this way, combatting the frequency of with a similar ethnic mix to that found within the UK. Thin-ideal
appearance comparisons may only be feasible through more compre­ internalisation and weight concern are known to differ between
hensive intervention strategies. different ethnic or cultural groups (Rakhkovskaya & Warren, 2014). For
Tiggemann et al. (2019b) have suggested that women may actively example, some authors have noted the current trend of the desirability of
choose to compare themselves with attractive women on social media as ‘slim-thick’ figures which are newer to white-centred media, but have
they set the standard for which they expect themselves to be judged. been long established among other cultures (McComb & Mills, 2022).
This idea is linked to comparison research that has argued for the Negative body image outcomes have also been shown to be greater
motivational quality of some types of upward comparison (Lockwood & among women from different ethnic backgrounds in response to
Kunda, 1997). The current study adds to this evidence base by showing white-focused images in mainstream media (Thomas, Kleyman, 2020).
that there are relationships between how participants feel after browsing It follows that both comparisons made towards, and the desirability of
Instagram and some of the interpersonal appraisals they make with the fitspiration figures included in the present study to some degree will
comparison targets during browsing (Hypothesis 3c). Taken together, be influenced by both the ethnic background of the observer and the
rather than trying to reduce instances of upward comparison, which are target. Whilst a range of ethnicities were included within the fitspiration
thought to be largely automatic and therefore inevitable images used in the present study, in an attempt to compensate for these
(Bocage-Barthélémy et al., 2018), alternative methods should look to differences, more detailed and routine measurement of observer-target
influence the nature of interpersonal appraisals made with upward differences in ethnicity may be required in future similar studies to
comparison targets as a means of making comparisons less harmful. In more fully consider these effects.
other words, rather than trying to prevent comparisons, research should Secondly, in all three experiments, exposure to the different types of
look to explore how similar women perceive themselves to be to others image was relatively brief. Consequently, the conclusions drawn
online, as a means of manipulating the nature of comparisons from being regarding the efficacy of different images to protect body image are
disconcerting to be more inspirational. As previous research has based on, and may only apply to, single exposures that are short in
demonstrated, upward comparisons can result in benign envy which duration. Whilst it may be argued that this is perhaps consistent with the
encourages motivation for self-improvement (Meier & Schäfer, 2018), nature of images to which people are exposed during typical social
rather than harmful dejection and body dissatisfaction. Some longitu­ media use, it remains unclear what the impact of exposure to different
dinal evidence has suggested that increased exposure to attractive message types would be over longer periods of exposure, both with
appearance-focused images was associated with both social comparison, respect to fitspiration images and ARSC or humorous images.
and inspiration, via social media up to four months later (Schreurs et al., Thirdly, since all three experiments took place online, it is not
2022). However, exercising for appearance reasons has also been asso­ possible to guarantee that participants were fully attending to the image
ciated with feelings of guilt and negative body image (Hurst et al., 2017) browsing tasks. Whilst safeguards were used, including timing partici­
and disengagement with goals (Mailey et al., 2018) over time. Further pants’ responses and conducting memory tests to check for engagement,
research is therefore needed to better understand the longer term out­ it remains possible that participants did not interact with the content of
comes, and conditions under which, positive benefits of inspiration images as they would do typically in their own time. Controlled
derived from social media images may occur. browsing tasks were used in Experiments 1 and 2 which may increase
The current research has also demonstrated the potential impact of confidence that the findings are likely to reflect the efficacy of the
exposure to fitspiration images on life satisfaction. Whilst some previous messages themselves, rather than a failure of the images to capture user
research has demonstrated the negative impact on one’s own life satis­ attention which could have been the case in Experiment 3. The use of
faction levels of viewing positive posts made by strangers (de Vries et al., more naturalistic approaches or observation of participants during real-
2018), the current study further shows that fitspiration content may world browsing may therefore be warranted to further reduce the gap
pose a threat to life satisfaction. This finding makes sense when between experimental findings and everyday engagement with social
considering that fitspiration content often promotes body ideals through media.
the portrayal of a healthy and active lifestyle (Boepple et al., 2016).
Future research could usefully further explore this specific effect. 5.2. Conclusions
Overall, the implications of this study suggest that encouraging
women to avoid browsing Instagram feeds of images that are exclusively The current study provides evidence that images containing ARSC
appearance-focused (e.g. feeds associated with appearance-focused and humorous body image messages are no more effective at protecting
hashtags such as ‘#fitspiration’) will help to protect their body image social media users from negative appearance and life satisfaction effects
online, by diversifying the range of content to which they are exposed. than appearance-neutral images, when displayed alongside fitspiration
Similarly, encouraging women to follow some Instagram accounts un­ images. Neither type of message had an impact on state appearance
related to appearance, may help protect users online, due to the dilution comparison. However, unexpectedly the results evidence that the
effect this creates on their own feed. With respect to future digital negative impact of fitspiration imagery is sensitive to the presence of

368
B. Davies et al. Body Image 46 (2023) 356–371

other posts which do not contain images of women, regardless of Case, C. E., & Lippard, C. D. (2009). Humorous assaults on patriarchal ideology.
Sociological Inquiry, 79(2), 240–255. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1475-
whether the posts include messages related to body image or images
682X.2009.00282.x
unconnected to physical appearance. This is an important consideration Cauberghe, V., Van Wesenbeeck, I., De Jans, S., Hudders, L., & Ponnet, K. (2021). How
for the design of future studies in this field. Given the range of content adolescents use social media to cope with feelings of loneliness and anxiety during
types that participants are simultaneously exposed to online, in order to COVID-19 lockdown. Cyberpsychology, Behavior, and Social Networking, 24(4),
250–257. https://doi.org/10.1089/cyber.2020.0478
generalise claims about the effects of fitspiration to typical Instagram Cha, H. S., Mayers, J. A., & Stutts, L. A. (2022). The impact of curvy fitspiration and
use, future research should develop methodologies which better imitate fitspiration on body dissatisfaction, negative mood, and weight bias in women.
the range of content consumed online. The current research has also Stigma and Health, 7(2), 226–233. https://doi.org/10.1037/sah0000367
Cohen, R., Irwin, L., Newton-John, T., & Slater, A. (2019). #bodypositivity: A content
demonstrated the importance of interpersonal appraisals, made in analysis of body positive accounts on Instagram. Body Image, 29, 47–57. https://doi.
relation to comparison targets, which may have potential implications org/10.1016/j.bodyim.2019.02.007
for alternative methods for combatting the harmful body image effects Cohen, R., Newton-John, T., & Slater, A. (2017). The relationship between Facebook and
Instagram appearance-focused activities and body image concerns in young women.
of social media images. Body Image, 23, 183–187. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bodyim.2017.10.002
Crawford, S. A., & Caltabiano, N. J. (2011). Promoting emotional well-being through the
CRediT authorship contribution statement use of humour. Journal of Positive Psychology, 6(3), 237–252. https://doi.org/
10.1080/17439760.2011.577087
Dakanalis, A., Timko, A. C., Clerici, M., Riva, G., & Carrà, G. (2017). Objectified body
Bryony Davies: Conceptualization, Methodology, Investigation, consciousness (OBC) in eating psychopathology: Construct validity, reliability, and
Formal analysis, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing. measurement invariance of the 24-Item OBC Scale in clinical and nonclinical
adolescent samples. Assessment, 24(2), 252–274. https://doi.org/10.1177/
Mark Turner: Supervision, Conceptualization, Methodology, Formal
1073191115602553
analysis, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing. Julie Darwin, H., & Miller, A. (2021). Factions, frames, and postfeminism(s) in the body
Udell: Supervision, Conceptualization, Methodology. positive movement. Feminist Media Studies, 21(6), 873–890. https://doi.org/
10.1080/14680777.2020.1736118
Davies, B., Turner, M., & Udell, J. (2020). Add a comment … how fitspiration and body
Declaration of Competing Interest positive captions attached to social media images influence the mood and body
esteem of young female Instagram users. Body Image, 33, 101–105. https://doi.org/
This research was supported by an Economic and Social Research 10.1016/j.bodyim.2020.02.009
de Valle, M. K., Gallego-García, M., Williamson, P., & Wade, T. D. (2021). Social media,
Council, South Coast Doctoral Training Partnership studentship award. body image, and the question of causation: Meta-analyses of experimental and
longitudinal evidence. Body Image, 39, 276–292. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.
Data Availability bodyim.2021.10.001
de Vries, D. A., Möller, A. M., Wieringa, M. S., Eigenraam, A. W., & Hamelink, K. (2018).
Social comparison as the thief of joy: Emotional consequences of viewing strangers’
Data will be made available on request. Instagram posts. Media Psychology, 21(2), 222–245. https://doi.org/10.1080/
15213269.2016.1267647
Dhanesh, G. S., & Nekmat, E. (2019). Facts over stories for involved publics: Framing
References effects in CSR messaging and the roles of issue involvement, message elaboration,
affect, and skepticism. Management Communication Quarterly, 33(1), 7–38. https://
Aldoory, L. (2001). Making health communications meaningful for women: Factors that doi.org/10.1177/0893318918793941
influence involvement. Journal of Public Relations Research, 13(2), 163–185. https:// Diener, E., Emmons, R. A., Larsen, R. J., & Griffin, S. (1984). The satisfaction with life
doi.org/10.1207/S1532754XJPRR1302_3 scale. Journal of Personality Assessment, 49(1), 71–75. https://doi.org/10.1207/
Allen, A. B., & Leary, M. R. (2010). Self-Compassion, stress, and coping. Social and s15327752jpa4901_13
Personality Psychology Compass, 4(2), 107–118. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1751- Duarte, C., Pinto-Gouveia, J., Ferreira, C., & Batista, D. (2015). Body image as a source of
9004.2009.00246.x shame: A new measure for the assessment of the multifaceted nature of body image
Akram, U., & Drabble, J. (2022). Mental health memes: beneficial or aversive in relation shame. Clinical Psychology and Psychotherapy, 22(6), 656–666. https://doi.org/
to psychiatric symptoms. Humanities and Social Sciences Communications, 9(1), 1–6. 10.1002/cpp.1925
https://doi.org/10.1057/s41599-022-01381-4 Engeln, R., Loach, R., Imundo, M. N., & Zola, A. (2020). Compared to Facebook,
Amarasingam, A. (2010). Laughter the best medicine: Muslim comedians and social Instagram use causes more appearance comparison and lower body satisfaction in
criticism in post-9 / 11 America. Journal of Muslim Minority Affairs, 30(4), 463–477. college women. Body Image, 34, 38–45. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.
https://doi.org/10.1080/13602004.2010.533444 bodyim.2020.04.007
Anderson, M., & Jiang, J. (2018). Teen’s social media habits and experiences. PEW Fardouly, J., & Holland, E. (2018). Social media is not real life: The effect of attaching
Research Center, (November), 33. Retrieved from 〈https://www.pewinternet.or disclaimer-type labels to idealized social media images on women’s body image and
g/2018/11/28/teens-social-media-habits-and-experiences/〉. mood. New Media and Society, 20(11), 4311–4328. https://doi.org/10.1177/
Barreca, R. (1988). Last-laughs-perspectives on women and comedy-introduction. 1461444818771083
Women’s Studies: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 15, 1–3. Fardouly, J., Pinkus, R. T., & Vartanian, L. R. (2017). The impact of appearance
Barron, A. M., Krumrei-Mancuso, E. J., & Harriger, J. A. (2021). The effects of fitspiration comparisons made through social media, traditional media, and in person in
and self-compassion Instagram posts on body image and self-compassion in men and women’s everyday lives. Body Image, 20, 31–39. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.
women. Body Image, 37, 14–27. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bodyim.2021.01.003 bodyim.2016.11.002
Bocage-Barthélémy, Y., Chatard, A., Jaafari, N., Tello, N., Billieux, J., Daveau, E., & Fardouly, J., Slater, A., Parnell, J., & Diedrichs, P. C. (2023). Can following body positive
Selimbegović, L. (2018). Automatic social comparison: Cognitive load facilitates an or appearance neutral Facebook pages improve young women’s body image and
increase in negative thought accessibility after thin ideal exposure among women. mood? Testing novel social media micro-interventions. Body Image, 44, 136–147.
PLoS One, 13(3), Article e0193200. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0193200 https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bodyim.2022.12.008
Boepple, L., Ata, R. N., Rum, R., & Thompson, J. K. (2016). Strong is the new skinny: A Faul, F., Erdfelder, E., Lang, A.-G., & Buchner, A. (2007). G*Power 3: A flexible statistical
content analysis of fitspiration websites. Body Image, 17, 132–135. https://doi.org/ power analysis program for the social, behavioral, and biomedical sciences. Behavior
10.1016/j.bodyim.2016.03.001 Research Methods, 39(2), 175–191. https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03193146
Braverman, J. (2008). Testimonials versus informational persuasive messages: The Fioravanti, G., Prostamo, A., & Casale, S. (2020). Taking a short break from Instagram:
moderating effect of delivery mode and personal involvement. Communication The effects on subjective well-being. Cyberpsychology, Behavior, and Social
Research, 35(5), 666–694. https://doi.org/10.1177/0093650208321785 Networking, 23(2), 107–112. https://doi.org/10.1089/cyber.2019.0400
Brown, Z., & Tiggemann, M. (2016). Attractive celebrity and peer images on Instagram: Frost, K. A., Franzoi, S. L., Oswald, D. L., & Shields, S. A. (2018). Revising the body
Effect on women’s mood and body image. Body Image, 19, 37–43. https://doi.org/ esteem scale with a U.S. college student sample: Evaluation, validation, and uses for
10.1016/j.bodyim.2016.08.007 the BES-R. Sex Roles, 78(1–2), 1–17. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11199-017-0776-5
Brown, Z., & Tiggemann, M. (2020). A picture is worth a thousand words: The effect of Heinberg, L. J., & Thompson, J. K. (1995). Body image and televised images of thinness
viewing celebrity Instagram images with disclaimer and body positive captions on and attractiveness: A controlled laboratory investigation. Journal of Social and
women’s. Body Image, 33, 190–198. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bodyim.2020.03.003 Clinical Psychology, 14(4), 325–338. https://doi.org/10.1521/jscp.1995.14.4.325
Burnell, K., George, M. J., & Underwood, M. K. (2020). Browsing different Instagram Hendrickse, J., Arpan, L. M., Clayton, R. B., & Ridgway, J. L. (2017). Instagram and
profiles and associations with psychological well-being. Frontiers in Human Dynamics, college women’s body image: Investigating the roles of appearance-related
2, 1–14. https://doi.org/10.3389/fhumd.2020.585518 comparisons and intrasexual competition. Computers in Human Behavior, 74, 92–100.
Bury, B., Tiggemann, M., & Slater, A. (2016). The effect of digital alteration disclaimer https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2017.04.027
labels on social comparison and body image: Instructions and individual differences. Hurst, M., Dittmar, H., Banerjee, R., & Bond, R. (2017). “I just feel so guilty”: The role of
Body Image, 17, 136–142. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bodyim.2016.03.005 introjected regulation in linking appearance goals for exercise with women’s. Body
Image, 20, 120–129. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bodyim.2016.12.002

369
B. Davies et al. Body Image 46 (2023) 356–371

Jerónimo, F., & Carraça, E. V. (2022). Effects of fitspiration content on body image: a Mussweiler, T. (2001). Focus of comparison as a determinant of assimilation versus
systematic review. Eating and Weight Disorders-Studies on Anorexia, Bulimia and contrast in social comparison. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 27(1),
Obesity, 27(8), 3017–3035. https://doi.org/10.1007/s40519-022-01505-4 38–47. https://doi.org/10.1177/0146167201271004
Jung, A. R. (2017). The influence of perceived ad relevance on social media advertising: Myers, T. A., & Crowther, J. H. (2009). Social comparison as a predictor of body
An empirical examination of a mediating role of privacy concern. Computers in dissatisfaction: A Meta-Analytic review. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 118(4),
Human Behavior, 70, 303–309. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2017.01.008 683–698. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0016763
Joshanloo, M. (2022). Latent state-trait modeling of satisfaction with life scale: An item- Myrick, J. G., Nabi, R. L., & Eng, N. J. (2022). Consuming memes during the COVID
level analysis using Dutch panel data. Journal of Happiness Studies, 23(7), pandemic: Effects of memes and meme type on COVID-related stress and coping
3429–3440. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10902-022-00550-1 efficacy. Psychology of Popular Media, 11(3), 316–323. https://doi.org/10.1037/
Kang, J., & Liu, B. (2019). A similarity mindset matters on social media: Using algorithm- ppm0000371
generated similarity metrics to foster assimilation in upward social comparison. Neff, K. D. (2011). Self-compassion, self-esteem, and well-being. Social and Personality
Social Media and Society, 5(4). https://doi.org/10.1177/2056305119890884 Psychology Compass, 5(1), 1–12. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1751-9004.2010.00330.x
Keyte, R., Mullis, L., Egan, H., Hussain, M., Cook, A., & Mantzios, M. (2021). Self- Nelson, S. L., Harriger, J. A., Miller-Perrin, C., & Rouse, S. V. (2022). The effects of body-
compassion and Instagram use is explained by the relation to anxiety, depression, positive Instagram posts on body image in adult women. Body Image, 42, 338–346.
and stress. Journal of Technology in Behavioral Science, 6, 436–441. https://doi.org/ https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bodyim.2022.07.013
10.1007/s41347-020-00186-z Pavot, W., & Diener, E. (1993). Review of the satisfaction with life scale. Psychological
Klassen, K. M., Borleis, E. S., Brennan, L., Reid, M., McCaffrey, T. A., & Lim, M. S. C. Assessment, 5, 164–172.
(2018). What people “like”: Analysis of social media strategies used by food industry Petty, R. E., & Cacioppo, J. T. (1986). The elaboration likelihood model of persuasion.
brands, lifestyle brands, and health promotion organizations on Facebook and Communication and Persuasion (pp. 1–24). New York: Springer,. https://doi.org/
Instagram. Journal of Medical Internet Research, 20(6), 1–9. https://doi.org/10.2196/ 10.1007/978-1-4612-4964-1_1
10227 Petty, R. E., Cacioppo, J. T., & Schumann, D. (1983). Central and peripheral routes to
Kostygina, G., Tran, H., Binns, S., Szczypka, G., Emery, S., Vallone, D., & Hair, E. (2020). advertising effectiveness: The moderating role of involvement. Journal of Consumer
Boosting health campaign reach and engagement through use of social media Research, 10(2), 135. https://doi.org/10.1086/208954
influencers and memes. Social Media and Society, 6(2). https://doi.org/10.1177/ Plester, B. (2009). Healthy humour: Using humour to cope at work. Kōtuitui: New Zealand
2056305120912475 Journal of Social Sciences Online, 4(1), 89–102. https://doi.org/10.1080/
Kreuter, M. W., & Wray, R. J. (2003). Tailored and targeted health communication: 1177083X.2009.9522446
Strategies for enhancing information relevance. American Journal of Health Behavior, Prichard, I., Kavanagh, E., Mulgrew, K. E., Lim, M. S. C., & Tiggemann, M. (2020). The
27(3). https://doi.org/10.5993/ajhb.27.1.s3.6 effect of Instagram #fitspiration images on young women’s mood, body image, and
Kruglanski, A. W., Chen, X., Pierro, A., Mannetti, L., Erb, H. P., & Spiegel, S. (2006). exercise behaviour. Body Image, 33, 1–6. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.
Persuasion according to the unimodel: Implications for cancer communication. bodyim.2020.02.002
Journal of Communication, 56, S105–S122. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1460- Rakhkovskaya, L. M., & Warren, C. S. (2014). Ethnic identity, thin-ideal internalization,
2466.2006.00285.x and eating pathology in ethnically diverse college women. Body Image, 11(4),
Leahey, T. M., Crowther, J. H., & Mickelson, K. D. (2007). The frequency, nature, and 438–445. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bodyim.2014.07.003
effects of naturally occurring appearance-focused social comparisons. Behavior Royal Society for Public Health. (2017). Social media and young people’ s mental health
Therapy, 38(2), 132–143. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.beth.2006.06.004 and wellbeing. Royal Society for Public Health, 32. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.
Legault, L., & Sago, A. (2022). When body positivity falls flat: Divergent effects of body pone.0055745
acceptance messages that support vs. undermine basic psychological needs. Body Saiphoo, A. N., & Vahedi, Z. (2019). A meta-analytic review of the relationship between
Image, 41, 225–238. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bodyim.2022.02.013 social media use and body image disturbance. Computers in Human Behavior, 101,
Lenda, M., Skórka, P., Mazur, B., Sutherland, W., Tryjanowski, P., Moroń, D., 259–275. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2019.07.028
Meijaard, E., Hugh, P., & Wilson, K. A. (2020). Effects of amusing memes on concern Schmid, K. L., Rivers, S. E., Latimer, A. E., & Salovey, P. (2008). Targeting or tailoring?
for unappealing species. Conservation Biology, 34(5), 1200–1209. https://doi.org/ Marketing Health Services, 28(1), 32–37.
10.1111/cobi.13523 Schreurs, L., Meier, A., & Vandenbosch, L. (2022). Exposure to the positivity bias and
Livingston, J., Holland, E., & Fardouly, J. (2020). Exposing digital posing: The effect of adolescents’ differential longitudinal links with social comparison, inspiration and
social media self-disclaimer captions on women’s body dissatisfaction, mood, and envy depending on social media literacy. Current Psychology, 1–21. https://doi.org/
impressions of the user. Body Image, 32, 150–154. https://doi.org/10.1016/j. 10.1007/s12144-022-03893-3
bodyim.2019.12.006 Sherman, G. D., Haidt, J., & Coan, J. A. (2009). Viewing cute images increases behavioral
Lockwood, P., & Kunda, Z. (1997). Superstars and me: Predicting the impact of role carefulness. Emotion, 9(2), 282–286. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0014904
models on the self. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 73(1), 91–103. Shifman, L. (2013). Memes in digital culture. MIT Press. https://doi.org/10.7551/
https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.73.1.91 mitpress/9429.001.0001
Maddox, J. (2021). The secret life of pet Instagram accounts: Joy, resistance, and Slater, A., Cole, N., & Fardouly, J. (2019). The effect of exposure to parodies of thin-ideal
commodification in the Internet’s cute economy. New Media and Society, 23(11), images on young women’s body image and mood. Body Image, 29, 82–89. https://
3332–3348. https://doi.org/10.1177/1461444820956345 doi.org/10.1016/j.bodyim.2019.03.001
Maher, C. A., Lewis, L. K., Ferrar, K., Marshall, S., De Bourdeaudhuij, I., & Slater, A., Varsani, N., & Diedrichs, P. C. (2017). #fitspo or #loveyourself? The impact of
Vandelanotte, C. (2014). Are health behavior change interventions that use online fitspiration and self-compassion Instagram images on women’s body image, self-
social networks effective? A systematic review. Journal of Medical Internet Research, compassion, and mood. Body Image, 22, 87–96. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.
16(2), 1–13. https://doi.org/10.2196/jmir.2952 bodyim.2017.06.004
Mailey, E. L., Dlugonski, D., Hsu, W., & Segar, M. (2018). Goals matter: Exercising for Thomas, F. N., & Kleyman, K. S. (2020). The influence of Western body ideals on Kenyan,
well-being but not health or appearance predicts future exercise among parents. Kenyan American, and African Americans’ body image. Journal of Prevention &
Journal of Physical Activity and Health, 15(11), 857–865. https://doi.org/10.1123/ Intervention in the Community, 48(4), 312–328. https://doi.org/10.1080/
jpah.2017-0469 10852352.2019.1627084
McComb, S. E., & Mills, J. S. (2020). A systematic review on the effects of media Tiggemann, M. (2022). Digital modification and body image on social media: Disclaimer
disclaimers on young women’s body image and mood. Body Image, 32, 34–52. labels, captions, hashtags, and comments. Body Image, 41, 172–180. https://doi.org/
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bodyim.2019.10.010 10.1016/j.bodyim.2022.02.012
McComb, S. E., & Mills, J. S. (2022). The effect of physical appearance perfectionism and Tiggemann, M., Anderberg, I., & Brown, Z. (2020). #Loveyourbody: The effect of body
social comparison to thin-, slim-thick-, and fit-ideal Instagram imagery on young positive Instagram captions on women’s body image. Body Image. 33, 129–136.
women’s body image. Body Image, 40, 165–175. https://doi.org/10.1016/j. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bodyim.2020.02.015
bodyim.2021.12.003 Tiggemann, M., Brown, Z., & Anderberg, I. (2019b). Effect of digital alteration
Meier, A., & Schäfer, S. (2018). Positive side of social comparison on social network sites: information and disclaimer labels attached to fashion magazine advertisements on
How envy can drive inspiration on Instagram. Cyberpsychology, Behavior, and Social women’s body dissatisfaction. Body Image, 30, 221–227. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.
Networking, 21(7), 411–417. https://doi.org/10.1089/cyber.2017.0708 bodyim.2019.07.008
Miltner, K. M. (2014). “There’s no place for lulz on LOLCats”: The role of genre, gender, Tiggemann, M., Brown, Z., & Thomas, N. (2019a). (Don’t) look here!: The effect of
and group identity in the interpretation and enjoyment of an Internet meme. First different forms of label added to fashion advertisements on women’s visual
Monday. https://doi.org/10.5210/fm.v19i8.5391 attention. Body Image, 31, 88–95. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bodyim.2019.08.011
Miltner, K. M. (2018). Memes. In J. Burgess, T. Poell, & A. E. Marwick (Eds.), The SAGE Tiggemann, M., & McGill, B. (2004). The role of social comparison in the effect of
handbook of social media (pp. 412–428). SAGE Publications Ltd.. 〈https://us.sagepub. magazine advertisements on women’s mood and body dissatisfaction. Journal of
com/en-us/nam/the-sage-handbook-of-social-media/book245739#description〉 Social and Clinical Psychology, 23(1), 23–44. https://doi.org/10.1521/
Moffitt, R. L., Neumann, D. L., & Williamson, S. P. (2018). Comparing the efficacy of a jscp.23.1.23.26991
brief self-esteem and self-compassion intervention for state body dissatisfaction and Tiggemann, M., & Velissaris, V. G. (2020). The effect of viewing challenging “reality
self-improvement motivation. Body Image, 27, 67–76. https://doi.org/10.1016/j. check” Instagram comments on women’s body image. Body Image, 33, 257–263.
bodyim.2018.08.008 https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bodyim.2020.04.004
Moorhead, S. A., Hazlett, D. E., Harrison, L., Carroll, J. K., Irwin, A., & Hoving, C. (2013). Tiggemann, M., & Zaccardo, M. (2015). Exercise to be fit, not skinny”: The effect of
A new dimension of health care: Systematic review of the uses, benefits, and fitspiration imagery on women’s body image. Body Image, 15, 61–67. https://doi.
limitations of social media for health communication. Journal of Medical Internet org/10.1016/j.bodyim.2015.06.003
Research, 15(4), 1–17. https://doi.org/10.2196/jmir.1933

370
B. Davies et al. Body Image 46 (2023) 356–371

Tiggemann, M., & Zaccardo, M. (2018). ‘Strong is the new skinny’: A content analysis of Verduyn, P., Gugushvili, N., Massar, K., Täht, K., & Kross, E. (2020). Social comparison
#fitspiration images on Instagram. Journal of Health Psychology, 23(8), 1003–1011. on social networking sites. Current Opinion in Psychology, 36, 32–37. https://doi.org/
https://doi.org/10.1177/1359105316639436 10.1016/j.copsyc.2020.04.002
Turk, F., & Waller, G. (2020). Is self-compassion relevant to the pathology and treatment
of eating and body image concerns? A systematic review and meta-analysis. Clinical
Psychology Review, 79, Article 101856. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cpr.2020.101856

371

You might also like