You are on page 1of 15

Developmental Psychology Copyright 2008 by the American Psychological Association

2008, Vol. 44, No. 3, 840 – 854 0012-1649/08/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/0012-1649.44.3.840

Discerning Direct and Mediated Effects of Ecological Structures and


Processes on Adolescents’ Educational Outcomes

Aprile D. Benner Sandra Graham and Rashmita S. Mistry


University of Texas at Austin University of California, Los Angeles

This short-term longitudinal study examined the relations among family and school characteristics,
family-level processes (youth perceptions of parent–adolescent interactions), school-level processes
(youth perceptions of school belonging, school climate), adolescents’ school engagement, and later
academic performance. Participants were an ethnically diverse, urban sample of 1,120 9th-grade students
(M age ⫽ 14.6 years). The structural characteristics of families and schools influenced the proximal
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

processes that occurred therein, and these proximal processes, in turn, influenced students’ proximal (i.e.,
engagement) and distal educational outcomes (i.e., grades in school). Moreover, the structural charac-
teristics of families and schools influenced proximal and distal outcomes indirectly through their
influence on the proximal processes. The multimediated ecological model suggested that intervening at
the process level may be a successful means of improving both adolescents’ engagement in school and
their subsequent school performance.

Keywords: adolescence, family influences, school influences, academic performance, ecological theory

There is a growing recognition among developmentalists that of the current study is to determine whether structures (i.e., char-
environmental contexts, such as families, schools, and neighbor- acteristics of families and schools) influence adolescents’ educa-
hoods, affect numerous developmental domains, including mental tional outcomes either directly or indirectly through their effects
health, socioemotional functioning, academic performance, and on proximal processes (i.e., the interactions that occur within
risk behaviors (Chung & Steinberg, 2006; Cook, Herman, Phillips, families and schools). Examination of the differential effects of
& Settersen, 2002; Leventhal & Brooks-Gunn, 2000). One theo- structure and process may provide insights into how best to inter-
retical lens for understanding these contextual effects is ecological vene to support adolescents’ academic success. In particular, if we
theory, which posits that human development is driven by proxi- identify more consistent effects of family and school processes,
mal processes (Bronfenbrenner, 1979; Bronfenbrenner & Morris, findings would have important implications for intervention design
1998). Proximal processes are increasingly complex interactions and implementation, particularly given that processes within sys-
between the individual and the environment that occur throughout tems are generally more amenable to change than structural char-
the numerous ecological systems in which individuals are embed- acteristics.
ded. For example, within the family microsystem, parental moni- In the current study, we link survey data from a multiethnic,
toring of adolescents’ activities would be a proximal process, urban sample with school records data and school-level data drawn
while within schools, one proximal process could be the extent to from the California Department of Education (CDE). These com-
which teachers and other school personnel treat students fairly.
bined data sources enable us to examine the direct influences of
While ecological theory poses as fundamental the interplay
family and school structural characteristics on family-level pro-
between the structural characteristics of ecological contexts and
cesses (youth perceptions of parent–adolescent interactions) and
the processes that occur therein, researchers sometimes conflate
school-level processes (youth perceptions of school belonging,
the two, developing models that do not differentiate between
fairness, academic climate, and interracial climate). We also ex-
structure and process. This results in two unfortunate conse-
amine the direct influence of structural characteristics on adoles-
quences: First, structure and process are conceptualized as coequal
cents’ academic characteristics (school engagement). More impor-
rather than interrelational; and second, possible interactions that
may have implications for intervention are ignored. A primary goal tantly, we explore the indirect effects of these structural
characteristics through the proximal processes that occur within
each context. While numerous studies have examined direct links
between either family or school characteristics and academic char-
Aprile D. Benner, Department of Human Ecology, University of Texas acteristics, few investigate multiple microsystems simultaneously.
at Austin; Sandra Graham and Rashmita S. Mistry, Department of Educa- By including both systems in a single model, we are able to
tion, University of California, Los Angeles. account for one system when examining influences within the
This work was supported by grants from the National Science Founda-
other. Relatedly, few studies have examined mesosystemic influ-
tion and the Linguistic Minority Research Institute awarded to Sandra
Graham. ences on academic outcomes, linking the effects of structural
Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Aprile D. characteristics in one microsystem to the processes occurring in
Benner, Department of Human Ecology, University of Texas at Austin, another, and how this relationship might affect both proximal and
Austin, TX 78712. E-mail: abenner@prc.utexas.edu distal educational performance. Identification of mesosystemic

840
ECOLOGICAL CONTEXTS AND EDUCATIONAL OUTCOMES 841

influences would suggest that future interventions should target & Dufur, 2001). Parental monitoring and regulation are associated
not only the individual microsystems of families and schools but with better grades in school for boys (Stolz et al., 2004), stronger
also the intersection of these systems. For example, encouraging reading achievement (Parcel & Dufur, 2001), and positive growth
family involvement in schools can promote proximal processes in mathematics achievement across high school (Gregory & Wein-
within families (e.g., parental monitoring of youth homework) and stein, 2004). Less parental psychological control (i.e., more auton-
within schools (e.g., improving school climate). omy granting) predicts better grades in school (Stolz et al., 2004).
A second goal of the current study is to examine whether Few studies, however, have attempted to parse apart the effects
proximal processes within families and schools have a direct of the structural characteristics of families from the processes that
influence on later educational outcomes or whether these processes occur therein. One exception is Felner and colleagues (1995), who
influence distal outcomes through their effects on adolescents’ adopted an ecological–mediational model to examine the effects of
academic characteristics (i.e., engagement in school). Ecological structural characteristics (e.g., parent education) on proximal en-
theory posits that both individual characteristics and contextual vironmental processes (e.g., family climate, support) and the pos-
processes influence developmental outcomes, yet few studies have sible mediating influence of these processes on youth adjustment.
simultaneously explored the effects of these on adolescents’
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

Their findings suggest that parent education influences the proxi-


This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

achievement. To this end, we examine how family and school mal processes that occur within families— households in which
proximal processes and adolescents’ school engagement influence neither parent graduated from high school exhibited poorer family
later academic performance. Moreover, although there are a grow- climate and social support, as reported by youth. While parent
ing number of studies examining the effects of school climate on education also exerted a significant influence on youth’s academic
youth outcomes (e.g., Kuperminc, Leadbeater, & Blatt, 2001; adjustment, as measured by grades in school and academic com-
Loukas, Suzuki, & Horton, 2006), most do not use an ecological petence, proximal family processes partially mediated this rela-
approach. Additionally, there continues to be a large number of tionship. The current study further builds on this parsing apart of
studies examining effects of family processes on youth outcomes the influences of family structure and family process. More spe-
(see Parke & Buriel, 2006); however, few studies examine the cifically, the current study uses a larger and more diverse urban
effects of both ecological contexts simultaneously in a single sample to explore multiple family structural characteristics and
model, a contribution of this current work. Finally, given the family processes and the influence of these, both direct and indi-
importance that ecological theory places on individual character- rect, on adolescents’ academic performance.
istics, this research focuses particular attention on how modeled
relationships may differ for boys and girls or for students of
different races– ethnicities. School Structure and Process and Adolescent
We explored these relationships with a multiethnic, urban sam- Achievement
ple of adolescents who had recently experienced the transition to
high school (i.e., 9th grade). The selection of 9th grade was The relationship between school structural characteristics and
purposeful and driven by ecological theory, which posits that life adolescents’ performance is well established. Students in high-
transitions, such as moving from middle to high school, affect the poverty schools (generally measured by the percentage of students
processes that occur within microsystems such as families and qualifying for the federal school lunch program) and schools with
schools (Bronfenbrenner, 1986; Elder, 1985). By choosing the high percentages of minority students generally experience more
9th-grade transition, we were able to examine how students engage academic difficulties than their peers attending more affluent
in processes within a new microsystem (i.e., students as active schools and schools with fewer racial– ethnic minority students
participants who react to and interact within their new environ- (Caldas, 1993; Lee, 2000; Lee & Croninger, 1994; McNeal, 1997;
ment) and how these processes, in turn, affected their later perfor- Roscigno, 2000). Students at large schools tend to perform more
mance in school. poorly on standardized tests and exhibit less growth in achieve-
ment across time (Lee, Smith, & Croninger, 1997) and have higher
Influences of Families on Adolescent Academic dropout rates (Baker et al., 2001). Similar academic difficulties
emerge for students in schools with high student-to-teacher ratios
Achievement
(Baker et al., 2001; McNeal, 1997). Just as school structural
The influence of structural characteristics of families on youth characteristics influence youth outcomes, so too do the processes
achievement is well documented in the extant literature. Youth in that occur within schools. Adolescents who express more positive
two-parent families tend to perform better on standardized perceptions of their school climate tend to earn higher grades in
achievement tests and earn higher grades, and they are more likely school (Stone & Han, 2005) and perform better on standardized
to graduate high school (Lee & Croninger, 1994; Roscigno, 2000). achievement tests (Eamon, 2005). Positive evaluations of schools’
Similarly, parental education is also associated with achievement interracial climates also positively affect adolescents’ grades in
test results, grades in school, and high school completion (Felner school (Stone & Han, 2005). More positive perceptions of rela-
et al., 1995; Roscigno, 2000). In addition to the influence of tionships with teachers are associated with higher grades in school
familial structural characteristics, the processes that occur within (Crosnoe, 2004; Gutman & Midgley, 2000; Stolz et al., 2004;
families also influence adolescent outcomes. Adolescents whose Woolley & Grogan-Kaylor, 2006) and stronger achievement test
parents are more involved in their education, both at home and at performance (Gregory & Weinstein, 2004). While researchers
school, earn higher grades in school (Gutman & Midgley, 2000; know a fair amount about the link between schools’ structural
Woolley & Grogan-Kaylor, 2006) and have higher reading and characteristics and adolescents’ academic performance and the
math achievement (Eamon, 2005; Lee & Croninger, 1994; Parcel link between school processes and performance, little is known
842 BENNER, GRAHAM, AND MISTRY

about the mechanisms by which school structural characteristics 5. Does the strength of association between model con-
and processes affect adolescents’ outcomes. structs differ by adolescent gender or race– ethnicity?

The Current Study The placement of constructs in our model and hypotheses re-
lated to our model were both research driven and theory driven.
Using an ecological framework, this study seeks to examine the We hypothesized that direct relationships would emerge between
links between structural characteristics, processes, and academic microsystem structural characteristics and processes, between pro-
outcomes in two microsystems—schools and families. Through cesses and proximal academic outcomes, and between proximal
this examination, we seek to highlight the mechanisms by which and distal academic outcomes. We also expected mesosystemic
structural characteristics and microsystem processes exert their influences, with school structural characteristics predicting family
influence on adolescents’ academic performance. Figure 1 presents academic involvement, and family structural characteristics pre-
the conceptual model that guided our research. Using longitudinal dicting youth’s perceptions of their school climate. Further, we
data on a sample of multiethnic, urban adolescents, we addressed expected that microsystem processes would mediate the relation-
five primary research questions: ship between structural characteristics and proximal academic
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

outcomes. Finally, we anticipated that proximal outcomes would


This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

1. How do the structural characteristics of families and mediate the relationship between microsystem processes and distal
schools affect the processes that occur therein, and how academic outcomes. In the absence of a strong empirical basis for
do these microsystem structures and processes affect making predictions about whether the model would function dif-
adolescents’ proximal and distal outcomes? So, for ex- ferently for boys or girls or for adolescents from different ethnic
ample, might living in a two-parent household affect the groups, we treated the gender and race– ethnicity analyses as
extent of family involvement in academics, and might exploratory.
this family involvement, in turn, affect adolescents’
school engagement and their later grades in school?
Method
2. Is the relationship between structural characteristics of
microsystems and proximal academic outcomes medi- Participants
ated by the processes that occur within microsystems?
Thus, for example, might family involvement in academ- Participants were 1,120 9th-grade students (484 boys and 636
ics mediate the relationship between a two-parent house- girls, M age ⫽ 14.6 year) who had participated in a larger longi-
hold and school engagement? tudinal study of social adjustment from 6th to 10th grade (Bell-
more, Witkow, Graham, & Juvonen, 2004). Students were initially
3. Is the relationship between microsystem processes and recruited at the beginning of 6th grade from 11 middle schools in
distal academic outcomes mediated by proximal aca- metropolitan Los Angeles, with schools chosen from among those
demic outcomes? So, might school engagement mediate of comparable size in demographically similar low-income/
the relationship between family academic involvement working-class neighborhoods. All schools were eligible for Title I
and later grades in school? compensatory funding and were carefully selected to represent a
continuum of ethnic diversity (e.g., from primarily Latino or
4. Are there cross-system interactions (i.e., mesosystem in- African American to ethnically diverse). Students were re-
fluences)? In other words, might school size, a school- recruited the fall of 9th grade to take part in a new study of the
level structural characteristic, influence family involve- transition to high school. We were able to successfully follow
ment in academics, a family-level process? more than 80% of the 8th-grade sample, enrolled in 140 high

Family Family
Structure Processes

Student
GPA
Beliefs/Behaviors

School School
Structure Processes

9th Grade Fall 9th Grade Spring

Figure 1. Hypothesized model of family and school effects on adolescents’ grades in school.
ECOLOGICAL CONTEXTS AND EDUCATIONAL OUTCOMES 843

schools in 9th grade. In total, 57 students were excluded from the identified their mothers’ highest educational attainment using the
high school portion of the study because either they or their parents following categories: (a) did not finish high school, (b) high school
refused to provide consent or assent. Attrition from the study was graduate, (c) vocational school or community college, or (d) 4-year
generally due to student mobility and school drop out or chronic college graduate or beyond. Students also identified, using a
absence. checklist, the adults who lived in their homes—the checklist in-
According to students’ self-reported ethnic affiliation, the sam- cluded biological parents, stepparents, grandparents, other rela-
ple for the current study was 48% Latino (n ⫽ 532), 15% African tives, and foster parents. Additionally, students could indicate
American (n ⫽ 167), 13% Asian (n ⫽ 142), 11% Caucasian (n ⫽ living in a group home or another living arrangement. We created
124), and 14% biracial–multiethnic (n ⫽ 153). More than 90% of two household structure variables based on their responses to these
Latino and Asian youth were at least second generation (U.S.-born items. First, if students lived with both a mother (biological or
children of immigrants), and all were sufficiently proficient in step) and a father (biological or step), they were identified as living
English to complete written questionnaires. in a two-parent home (coded as 1). All other living arrangements
were coded as 0. Additionally, we created a dichotomous variable
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

Procedure indicating whether nonparental family members resided in the


This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

home. Students who reported living with grandparents or other


Each school year, students with both parental consent and stu- relatives received a score of 1, while students who lived with
dent assent completed confidential questionnaires during the mid- neither grandparents nor other relatives received a score of 0.
dle of both the fall and the spring semesters. This study specifically School structural characteristics. We used five individual
draws on student survey data from the fall of 9th grade. Trained measures of school structure from the CDE that have been linked
research assistants (graduate and undergraduate students) visited to adolescents’ educational beliefs and academic outcomes: aver-
high schools to administer student surveys. During each question- age school socioeconomic status (SES), average school achieve-
naire administration, students were generally surveyed during elec- ment, school size, student–teacher ratio, and school diversity.
tive classes; administration typically occurred in empty rooms on These school structural characteristics were analyzed at the indi-
the school campus (e.g., library, cafeteria, classroom). Research vidual level because we were interested in relationships between
assistants read all items aloud as small groups of students provided
structural characteristics and students’ perceptions of their schools.
their responses on a computerized survey. When high schools
Our analytical methods account for the nested nature of school
enrolled fewer than 5 study participants, research assistants con-
structural characteristics (see Overview of Analysis). Average
ducted phone surveys. Students were mailed the questionnaire
school SES was calculated by subtracting the percentage of stu-
materials, and research assistants contacted each student by phone,
dents qualifying for the federal free and reduced-price lunch pro-
reading all items aloud as the student provided individual re-
gram from 100%, such that higher scores indicated that fewer
sponses on their questionnaires. Students then returned the ques-
students qualified for the program. School achievement data were
tionnaires in a postage-paid envelope. Questionnaires took approx-
drawn from schools’ Academic Performance Index (API) base
imately 40 min to complete, and students received small monetary
scores. Schools’ API base scores are determined by school-level
compensation for their participation.
performance on California’s statewide assessment program, in-
In addition to student surveys, each student participant’s English
teacher was asked to complete a student engagement questionnaire cluding results on the California Standards Tests, the California
during the fall and spring semester of each year. In the rare case in Alternate Performance Assessment, and the California High
which an English teacher was unable to participate in the teacher School Exit Examination. API scores range from 200 to 1,000,
survey, a teacher in one of the student’s other core-content classes with the state’s target API performance score set at 800 for all
(e.g., mathematics, science, social studies) was asked to partici- schools. School enrollment was converted from a continuous vari-
pate. Students participating in phone surveys were mailed a sealed able into an ordinal, seven-category variable due to the high
teacher questionnaire packet and were asked to give the packet to variance of the continuous variable, with the following school size
their English teachers. The packet included a cover letter describ- groupings: (a) less than 2,000 students, (b) 2,001 to 2,500 students,
ing the study, the teacher questionnaire, and a postage-paid return (c) 2,501 to 3,000 students, (d) 3,001 to 3,500 students, (e) 3,501
envelope. The teacher questionnaire took approximately 5 min to to 4,000 students, (f) 4,001 to 4,500 students, and (g) more than
complete, and teachers received a small monetary compensation 4,500 students. Student–teacher ratio was computed by dividing
for their participation, with rates varying based on the number of total school enrollment by the total number of full-time equivalent
surveys completed. Teacher survey data were available for 793 teachers at the school.
students (71% of the sample).1 School ethnic diversity calculations were based on school-level
School record data (i.e., grades) were collected for all partici- race– ethnicity data. The CDE data include school racial– ethnic
pating students each semester. School records data for this study
were drawn specifically from spring of 9th grade. School demo- 1
Cross-tabulation results indicated a difference in missing teacher data
graphic data were downloaded from the CDE website (www.cde
by gender and race– ethnicity. Independent-samples t tests revealed that
.ca.gov) for each student’s high school. students with teacher survey data tended to attend more affluent schools
with fewer students and higher academic performance than those without
Measures these data. These students also rated their climates as more fair and
stronger academically, and they had mothers with more education. The
Family structural characteristics. We used three measures of study’s analysis methods, detailed in the Overview of Analysis to follow,
family structure, all procured through the student survey. Students are able to account and adjust for these missing data.
844 BENNER, GRAHAM, AND MISTRY

breakdown by the following categories: American Indian– tions of their classmates’ academic behaviors. A sample item is
Alaskan, Asian, Pacific Islander, Filipino, Hispanic, African “Most of the kids in my classes. . .work hard in school.” Items
American (non-Hispanic), White, and multiethnic–missing. CDE were rated on a 5-point scale, ranging from 1 (hardly true at all)
data were aggregated into four primary racial– ethnic categories— to 5 (always true). Higher mean scores indicate a stronger aca-
Latina/o, African American, Asian American, and White—for the demic climate (␣ ⫽ .75).
purpose of calculating ethnic diversity for the students’ high Finally, to assess perceptions of the school interracial climate,
schools.2 School-level ethnic diversity was computed using Simp- students completed a 14-item abridged and slightly modified ver-
son’s (1949) index of diversity, which has been used in other sion of the School Interracial Climate Scale (Green, Adams, &
studies drawing on the larger longitudinal data for the current Turner, 1988). We purposefully selected the 11 items that were
study (Bellmore et al., 2004; Juvonen, Nishina, & Graham, 2006): specific to the school itself or teachers at the school (as opposed to


g those directly related to students’ interracial interactions). A sam-
DC ⫽ 1 ⫺ p 21 . ple item is “Teachers here like students of different ethnic groups
i⫽1 to get along.” Students rated items on a 5-point scale ranging from
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

1 (for sure yes) to 5 (no way). Items were reverse-coded such that
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

In this formula, a school’s ethnic diversity (Dc) depends on the


proportion (p) of students in the school who are in each ethnic higher scores indicate a more positive interracial climate (␣ ⫽
group (i). The proportions are squared and summed across the total .85).
number of ethnic groups in the school (g). The index of diversity Proximal adolescent outcomes—student engagement. Two
accounts for both the relative proportion of each ethnic group in measures from different informants were used to assess students’
the school and the number of ethnic groups represented within the engagement in school in the fall of 9th grade. Teachers completed
school, providing the probability (ranging from 0 to approximately six items from the Short Form of the Teacher Report of Engage-
1) that 2 students randomly selected from the same school will ment Questionnaire (Wellborn & Connell, 1991) to rate the degree
belong to different ethnic groups. Higher scores on the diversity to which they perceived the student as engaged versus disaffected
index reflect greater ethnic diversity within the school. For the from schoolwork. A sample item is “In my class, this student pays
high schools in this sample, scores ranged from 0.04 to 0.74 (M ⫽ attention.” Teacher ratings range from 1 (never) to 4 (always), with
0.53, SD ⫽ 0.18), indicating good spread along a diversity con- higher scores reflecting greater student engagement (␣ ⫽ .89).
tinuum. Students self-reported their school engagement using the Per-
Family process variables. Students completed a 15-item ceived Social Norms for Schoolwork and Achievement During
global family climate measure adopted from the Family Environ- Adolescence (Witkow, 2006). Students rated six items related to
ment Scale (Moos & Moos, 1981) and the Parental Bonding perceptions of their academic engagement. A sample item is “I pay
Instrument (Parker, Tupling, & Brown, 1979). Students rated all attention in class.” Items were rated on a 5-point scale, ranging
items on a 5-point scale, ranging from 1 (for sure yes) to 5 (no from 1 (hardly true at all) to 5 (always true). Higher mean scores
way). The current study draws specifically from two subscales. indicate greater academic engagement (␣ ⫽ .78). Although the
The Family Support for Academics subscale comprises three
teacher-rated and student-rated engagement measures were mod-
items, including “I get help from my family when I have to make
erately correlated (r ⫽ .62), we retained the separate assessments
decisions about school.” The Family Monitoring subscale included
due to the unique perspective of each informant and our expecta-
four items. A sample item is “I must call home if I’m going to be
tions that informant differences might have different effects on
late.” Item responses were reverse coded, so that higher ratings
reflected greater family support for academics and family moni- other modeled constructs.
toring. We used individual items to construct latent family vari- Distal adolescent outcome— grade point average (GPA). In-
ables, so alphas are not presented. formation on students’ grades for spring of 9th grade was collected
School process variables. Students reported on four dimen- from their report cards at the end of the school year. Grades for all
sions of school processes—school belonging, fairness, academic courses were coded on a 5-point scale (A ⫽ 4 and F ⫽ 0) and then
climate, and interracial climate. School belonging and fairness were averaged to create a composite GPA for each student.
were assessed using items adapted from Gottfredson’s (1984) Covariates. Analyses included six control variables. Five co-
Effective School Battery. The School Belonging subscale (␣ ⫽ variates related to adolescents’ sociodemographic characteristics:
.74) included five items that assessed students’ sense of belonging ethnicity (separate dummy variables for African American, Latino,
to their high schools. A sample item is “I feel like I’m a part of this Asian, biracial–multiethnic; White was the omitted reference
school.” Ratings ranged from 1 (no way) to 5 (for sure yes), with group) and gender (0 ⫽ male, 1 ⫽ female). A final covariate, GPA
higher mean scores reflecting greater school belonging. The at the end of 8th grade, controlled for adolescents’ prior academic
School Fairness subscale comprised 3 items (␣ ⫽ .73) that asked achievement.
about the extent to which students felt that the rules were equitable
and all students were treated the same (e.g., “The principal is
fair”). All items were rated on a 5-point scale, from 1 (always) to
5 (never), with higher scores reflecting greater feelings of school
fairness.
School academic climate was assessed using the Perceived 2
Because the CDE aggregates biracial–multiethnic students with miss-
Social Norms for Schoolwork and Achievement During Adoles- ing students, we could not use the proportion of biracial–multiethnic
cence (Witkow, 2006). Students rated six items related to percep- students in calculations of the diversity index.
ECOLOGICAL CONTEXTS AND EDUCATIONAL OUTCOMES 845

Results Formation of Latent Variables

Overview of Analysis In order to investigate processes within families and schools, we


created latent factors representing two domains within each mi-
Structural equation modeling (SEM) was used to test relations crosystem: (a) academic support and monitoring within families
among the study constructs. Using SEM, we assessed the magni- and (b) academic climate and general school climate within
tude and significance of relations among the model’s exogenous schools. Latent variable analysis, akin to factor analysis, is a more
(i.e., predictors) and endogenous (i.e., mediators and outcomes) parsimonious representation of the processes under study, allowing
variables. One benefit of using SEM is the ability to simulta- us to better account for the individual contribution of each item
neously test for direct and indirect (i.e., mediated) effects, which is and also to more precisely model measurement error (Bollen,
1989). Adolescents’ responses to the three items on family aca-
not possible with an ordinary least squares regression approach
demic support composed the family academics latent variable. The
(Stage, Carter, & Norma, 2004). The model was estimated using
family monitoring factor comprised the four items from the same
Mplus 4.2 (Muthén & Muthén, 1998 –2006). Mplus was chosen, in
scale. For the school microsystem, the six items related to percep-
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

part, because the current data set includes some missing data, and
tions of academic climate composed the school academics latent
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

the Mplus estimation procedure handles missing data through


variable. And, finally, the mean scores from the three measures of
full-information maximum likelihood imputation, enabling us to school climate—school belonging, school fairness, and interracial
include all available data. Full-information maximum likelihood climate—were used to create a latent factor of general school
does not estimate the missing data, as is the case with mean- or climate. For all four latent factors, individual loadings were gen-
regression-based imputation techniques. Rather, it fits the covari- erally comparable, and all were statistically significant at a mini-
ance structure model directly to the observed (and available) raw mum probability level of .001 (see Table 1).
data for each participant. Full-information maximum likelihood Intercorrelations, means, and standard deviations for all exoge-
assumes that the missing data are either missing completely at nous and endogenous variables within our model are presented in
random or missing at random (MAR). For data MAR, missingness Table 2. Figure 2 presents the standardized path coefficients for the
is a function of the observed variables in the model. The current final model, with latent variable path coefficients presented in
data had approximately 50 different missing data patterns, sug- Table 1; only significant paths are shown. The overall model fit
gesting that there was not one systematic pattern of missingness, was good, ␹2(253, N ⫽ 1,120) ⫽ 727.7, p ⬍ .001 (comparative fit
an assumption of MAR. Moreover, as recommended by Widaman index ⫽ 0.92, root-mean-square error of approximation ⫽ 0.041),
(2006), we also address the assumptions of MAR through our and the model accounted for a large portion of the variance in the
inclusion of covariates (i.e., gender, race– ethnicity, prior grades in proximal (R2 ⫽ 0.47 and 0.36 for adolescent- and teacher-reported
school), which are related to missing data for our study constructs. engagement, respectively) and distal student (R2 ⫽ 0.60 for GPA)
The current data set also contains data from students clustered outcomes. All path coefficients are net all other relationships
within schools, and Mplus is able to account for such clustering within the model, net the influence of covariates, and net the
and produce correctly adjusted standard errors in the model esti- effects of within-level, within microsystem (e.g., school structures,
mations. Additionally, the maximum likelihood-robust estimator family processes, etc.) residual correlations.3
in Mplus calculates robust standard errors, which, in turn, allowed For ease of interpretation, the results to follow are presented
separately for the family and school microsystems; however, it is
us to determine whether the mediated effects within the model
important to note that the final model included both microsystems
were significant (see Enders, 2002). We tested two paths for
in a single analysis, as depicted in Figure 2, and results take into
possible mediation—microsystem processes as mediators of the
account influences from both microsystems and the set of model
relationship between structural characteristics and adolescents’
covariates. As such, the results to follow take into account all other
proximal outcomes, and adolescents’ proximal outcomes as medi-
modeled relationships (predictors, mediators, proximal and distal
ators of the relationships between microsystem processes and later outcomes, covariates). For each microsystem, we first describe the
academic achievement. We conducted our mediation analyses in direct paths within the model. In these analyses, we examined
this way so that we could both establish the relationship between whether each system’s structural characteristics predicted the pro-
structure and process on proximal adolescent outcomes and, at the cesses that occurred therein, whether these processes predicted
same, examine the effects of processes on proximal and distal adolescents’ proximal outcomes (engagement in school), and
adolescent outcomes. Inferences for the indirect effects were based whether these, in turn, predicted the distal outcome of GPA. As
on the Mplus estimation of indirect effects, which estimates indi- noted earlier, all variables except GPA were assessed in the fall of
rect effects with delta method standard errors (Muthén & Muthén, 9th grade; GPA was assessed at the end of 9th grade.
1998 –2006) and Sobel’s (1982) asymptotic z test. Recently, there After describing direct relationships, we then present findings
has been some critique of Baron and Kenny’s (1986) causal steps from mediation analyses. We then highlight findings of analyses
approach for testing mediation (see Dearing & Hamilton, 2006, for
a review), and use of the Sobel test is generally recommended for 3
Prior GPA was one of the covariates included in the model, and as a
studies with larger sample sizes (see Bollen & Stine, 1990; Dear-
covariate, we adjusted for its influence throughout all modeled parameters.
ing & Hamilton, 2006; MacKinnon, Lockwood, Hoffman, West, & We also ran models in which prior GPA was a predictor variable (adjusting
Sheets, 2002). All of the mediated relationships we observed for its influence only on 9th-grade GPA) and in which prior GPA was not
indicated partial mediation with one exception, which is noted in included in the model. Results for these models were not substantially
the Results section. different from the model in which prior GPA was modeled as a covariate.
846 BENNER, GRAHAM, AND MISTRY

Table 1 two-parent home and adolescent engagement was also mediated by


Unstandardized and Standardized Factor Loadings for Final perceived family academic support. Living with both parents pre-
Model dicted more school engagement to the degree that adolescents
perceived that their parents helped them with their school work.
Factor loadings Family monitoring did not mediate the relationship between family
Family and school latent factor Unstandardized Standardized
structure and adolescents’ engagement.
Concurrent with examining how family processes served as
Family support for academics mediators, we also explored how proximal adolescent outcomes
Help with school decisions 1.00 0.64 might mediate the link between family processes and the distal
Understand classes should take 0.96 0.62
adolescent outcome of spring semester GPA. As shown in Table 3,
Important to do well in school 0.70 0.57
Family monitoring adolescent-reported school engagement mediated the relationship
Rules are strict 1.00 0.30 between family support for academics and adolescents’ GPA, but
Go out as often as want (r) 0.71 0.20 not that between family monitoring and GPA.
Call home if late 2.29 0.72 To summarize, within the family microsystem, higher maternal
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

Wants to know who friends are 2.03 0.61


This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

Academic climate education, residence in a two-parent family, and having other


Get good grades 1.00 0.75 relatives in the home predicted the extent to which families were
Work hard in school 1.09 0.81 involved in their adolescents’ schooling and were monitoring
Do homework 1.10 0.83 adolescents’ activities. These structural characteristics influenced
Pay attention in class 1.09 0.82
adolescents’ engagement in school through their impact on family
School climatea
School belonging 1.00 0.66 processes (specifically, family involvement in adolescents’ school-
School fairness 1.00 0.69 ing). Engagement in school predicted later school performance.
Interracial climate 0.83 0.62

Note. All parameter estimates significant at p ⬍ .001. r ⫽ reverse coded. The School Microsystem
a
Based on composite (mean) scores rather than measured items.
Testing direct paths. The structural characteristics of schools,
particularly diversity, average achievement level, and school size,
that examined cross-system effects (i.e., school structural charac- predicted the processes that occurred within schools (see lower left
teristics’ effects on family processes and vice versa) and finally panel of Figure 2). Specifically, greater school diversity was
discuss moderation analyses that explored possible differences in related to poorer perceptions of the academic climate and the
modeled relationships by adolescent gender and race– ethnicity. general school climate. School achievement predicted these same
process variables, although in the opposite direction, such that
students in higher performing schools perceived a more positive
The Family Microsystem
academic climate and had greater feelings of belonging and more
Testing direct paths. As seen in the upper left portion of Fig- positive perceptions about their schools’ fairness and interracial
ure 2, the structural characteristics of families predicted the processes climate. Like diversity, increasing school size was associated with
that occurred therein. Living in a two-parent family was associated more negative perceptions about academic climate and general
with greater family support for academics and family monitoring. school climate. School SES was found to be related only to general
Higher maternal education predicted greater family support for aca- perceptions of school climate, and student-to-teacher ratios did not
demics. Having other relatives in the home was related to family predict any of the proximal processes under study.
monitoring. Family processes, in turn, were related to adolescents’ School processes, in turn, were related to adolescents’ school
proximal outcomes (upper middle portion of Figure 2). Greater family engagement. As shown in the lower middle panels of Figure 2,
support for academics was related to greater self-reported school academic climate and general school climate positively predicted
engagement. Higher perceived family monitoring was negatively re- adolescent-reported academic engagement. General school climate
lated to adolescent-reported school engagement, suggesting that ado- also was positively related to teacher-rated school engagement.
lescents who felt that their parents kept close tabs on their where- Unexpectedly, however, students’ ratings of the academic climate
abouts were less invested in school work. Both adolescent- and as positive were negatively related to teacher-reported school
teacher-reported engagement were predictive of adolescents’ later engagement. Finally, as noted earlier, greater adolescent- and
grades in school (right portion of Figure 2); the residuals for the two teacher-reported school engagement in the fall of 9th grade were
engagement measures were correlated (r ⫽ .13, p ⬍ .001). associated with higher grades in the spring of 9th grade.
Testing mediated relations. Results for tests of mediation are Testing mediated relations. Mediational analyses examined
presented in Table 3. One set of mediation analyses examined the the extent to which school processes mediated the relationship
extent to which family processes mediated the relationship be- between school structural characteristics and adolescents’ school
tween structural characteristics and adolescents’ school engage- engagement and whether school engagement mediated the rela-
ment. Family support for academics fully mediated the relationship tionship between school processes and later grades in school. The
between maternal education and adolescent-reported engagement. results of these tests of mediation are displayed in Table 4. In
That is, as mothers’ level of education increased, adolescents’ examining school processes as mediators (see top panel of Table
engagement in school also increased, and this effect was explained 4), we found that general school climate and academic climate
by the extent to which adolescents felt that their families supported mediated the relationships among structural characteristics and
their academic endeavors. The relationship between living in a engagement in school. In particular, some of the effects of school
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

Table 2
Means, Standard Deviations, and Intercorrelations Among Modeled Variables

Variable 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14 15 16 17

1. Mother education —
2. Two-parent home ⫺.06 —
3. Relatives in home ⫺.08* ⫺.13*** —
4. School diversity .26*** ⫺.01 ⫺.11*** —
5. Student–teacher ratio ⫺.08* .09** .02 ⫺.39*** —
6. School size ⫺.18*** .02 .03 ⫺.61*** .38*** —
7. School SES .25*** .00 ⫺.07* .69*** ⫺.30*** ⫺.60*** —
8. School achievement .28*** .04 ⫺.08** .60*** ⫺.11*** ⫺.51*** .63*** —
9. Family academic support .11*** .09** .02 ⫺.01 .01 .01 ⫺.02 .02 —
10. Family monitoring ⫺.02 .10** .05 ⫺.01 .04 .04 ⫺.02 .04 .27*** —
11. School belonging .04 .07* ⫺.03 ⫺.02 ⫺.00 ⫺.03 .03 .14*** .25*** .10*** —
12. Academic climate ⫺.02 .01 .07* ⫺.06* ⫺.01 ⫺.05 ⫺.01 .07* .12*** .07* .24*** —
13. Racial climate .03 .03 .03 ⫺.10** .03 ⫺.01 ⫺.04 .04 .31*** .12*** .42*** .22*** —
14. School fairness ⫺.06 .08 .05 ⫺.09** .08** ⫺.04 ⫺.04 .09** .25*** .08** .44*** .19*** .44*** —
15. School engagement (A) .13*** .08* ⫺.05 .01 ⫺.01 ⫺.03 .05 .11*** .41*** .16*** .38*** .26*** .32*** .17*** —
16. School engagement (T) .13*** .09* ⫺.01 .05 ⫺.01 .04 ⫺.02 .19*** .13*** .12*** .16*** ⫺.03 .13*** .09* .43*** —
17. GPA .20*** .17*** ⫺.10** .08* ⫺.02 ⫺.05 .09** .25*** .20*** .15*** .17*** .00 .13*** .12*** .44*** .62*** —
M 2.47 0.72 0.23 0.53 24.70 3,152 48.10 598.7 4.04 3.52 3.51 3.02 3.56 3.60 3.68 2.63 2.49
SD 1.13 0.45 0.42 0.18 1.89 939 21.16 85.6 0.70 0.68 0.76 0.62 0.67 0.73 0.68 0.76 1.02
N 905 1,110 1,110 1,105 1,111 1,115 1,111 1,110 1,118 1,118 1,119 1,115 1,115 1,119 1,116 793 1,017
ECOLOGICAL CONTEXTS AND EDUCATIONAL OUTCOMES

Note. Total possible N ⫽ 1,120 for student-reported data and 793 for teacher-reported data. All process variables are calculated means and not latent constructs. A ⫽ adolescent-reported engagement;
T ⫽ teacher-reported engagement.
*
p ⬍ .05. ** p ⬍ .01. *** p ⬍ .001.
847
848 BENNER, GRAHAM, AND MISTRY

Structures Processes Proximal Outcomes Distal Outcome

Mother 0.12*
Education Family
FAMILY SYSTEM

Support for 0.40***


Academics
0.11**
Two-Parent
Family
0.13** School
Family -0.13*
Engagement (A)
Other Relatives Monitoring
in Home 0.09* R2 = 0.47
0.08*

School
Diversity -0.16**
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

Grades
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

-0.23*** 0.14***
R2 = 0.60
Student- Academic
Teacher Ratio Climate
SCHOOL SYSTEM

-0.12*** 0.31***
0.20***
School
School Engagement (T)
Achievement

0.25*** R2 = 0.36
0.37***

School School
-0.11*
SES Climate 0.17***

-0.10*

School -0.10*
Size

Figure 2. Standardized coefficients for model of family and school effects on adolescents’ grades in school.
N ⫽ 1,120. Structural and process factors within each microsystem (i.e., family, school) were allowed to
correlate with each other. Only significant microsystem paths shown. Comparative fit index ⫽ 0.92, root-mean-
square error of approximation ⫽ 0.041. T ⫽ teacher-reported engagement; A ⫽ adolescent-reported engage-
ment. * p ⬍ .05. ** p ⬍ .01. *** p ⬍ .001.

achievement, diversity, size, and SES on students’ engagement In summary, school diversity, school-wide achievement, school
were explained by students’ perceptions of the general school enrollment, and to a lesser extent school SES predicted school
climate. Additionally, although academic climate also helped to processes, including perceptions of the academic and general
explicate the relationship between school-level achievement and school climate. These school processes predicted school engage-
school diversity and student engagement, the relationships were ment as rated by both adolescents and their teachers, although the
somewhat more complex because of the differential associations relationships between school processes and engagement were dif-
between academic climate and teacher- versus adolescent-rated ferent for the two informant sources. Both student and teacher
engagement. engagement, in turn, predicted adolescents’ later GPA.
Concurrent to these analyses, we also explored how school
engagement might mediate the link between school processes and Testing Cross-System Effects Within the Model
the distal adolescent outcome of spring semester GPA (see bottom
panel of Table 4). Adolescent-reported engagement mediated the Consistent with the tenets of ecological theory, in addition to
relationship between academic climate and GPA as well as the examining how structural characteristics influenced processes
association between general school climate and GPA. The effects within each microsystem, we also explored the extent to which
of a positive school climate on students’ academic performance structural characteristics within one microsystem might affect pro-
was in part explained by the degree to which students perceived cesses occurring within the other microsystem. For example, does
themselves as engaged in academically relevant behaviors (e.g., school-wide average achievement influence parents’ academic in-
paying attention in class). Teacher-reported engagement mediated volvement in their adolescents’ education? Or might maternal
the climate–GPA relationship as well, although in the opposite education influence an adolescent’s sense of the academic climate
direction. of the school? While ecological theory suggested that these cross-
ECOLOGICAL CONTEXTS AND EDUCATIONAL OUTCOMES 849

Table 3
Sobel Tests of Mediation for the Family Microsystem

Mediated pathway Z p

Structure 3 Process 3 Proximal Outcomes


Mother education 3 family academic support 3 engagement (A) 2.21 .027
2-parent home 3 family academic support 3 engagement (A) 2.50 .013
2-parent home 3 family monitoring 3 engagement (A) ⫺1.62 .106
Relatives in home 3 family monitoring 3 engagement (A) ⫺1.51 .130
Process 3 Proximal Outcomes 3 Distal Outcome
Family academic support 3 engagement (A) 3 GPA 2.12 .034
Family monitoring 3 engagement (A) 3 GPA ⫺1.50 .133

Note. A ⫽ adolescent-reported engagement.


This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

system influences (i.e., mesosystem influences) would exist, re- due to their smaller size, White and Asian students were combined
sults from our analyses suggest that school structural characteris- into a single (academically more successful) group, and we ex-
tics did not influence family processes, nor did familial cluded biracial–multiethnic students.
characteristics generally influence processes within schools. The Using methods recommended by Bollen (1989) and Kenny
only significant cross-system paths were the direct effects of other (2005), we used a stepwise process for the multigroup compari-
relatives in the home on perceived academic climate (␤ ⫽ .07, p ⬍ sons, whereby we included a series of increasingly restrictive
.05) and school enrollment on family monitoring (␤ ⫽ .07, p ⬍ constraints on the model parameters and observed whether doing
.05). so led to a significant decrease in the overall model fit. Omnibus
tests (e.g., chi-square difference tests and comparisons of compar-
Testing Moderation Within the Model ative fit index and root-mean-square error of approximation val-
We conducted two sets of moderation analyses to determine ues) were relied on to determine whether introduction of an addi-
whether the strength of association between model constructs tional set of parameter constraints resulted in a significant decrease
differed by adolescent gender or race– ethnicity—that is, whether in the model fit. The sequencing of the nested models was as
the model functioned differently for boys versus girls or for follows. We first estimated a base model in which all model
adolescents from different ethnic groups. For moderation analyses parameters were freely estimated across groups. Successive mod-
involving ethnicity, we examined differences across three groups: els represented ever more restrictive and nested models within this
White and Asian adolescents, African American adolescents, and larger base model—that is, forcing an increasing number of rela-
Latino adolescents. Due to the complexity of the model and tions to be equivalent across groups. Should such impositions
limitations in subgroup sample sizes, we could not retain all of the result in a significant decrease in overall model fit, it would
separate ethnic groups in these analyses. As historically margin- suggest that the more restrictive model does not fit the data as well
alized groups in American public schools, African American and as the less restrictive model and, as such, that there are meaningful
Latino students were analyzed as separate ethnic groups. However, differences across the groups in the pattern of associations among

Table 4
Sobel Tests of Mediation for the School Microsystem

Mediated pathway Z p

Structure 3 Process 3 Proximal Outcomes


School achievement 3 academic climate 3 engagement (A) 2.76 .006
School achievement 3 academic climate 3 engagement (T) ⫺2.63 .008
School diversity 3 academic climate 3 engagement (A) ⫺2.35 .019
School diversity 3 academic climate 3 engagement (T) 2.27 .023
School achievement 3 school climate 3 engagement (A) 3.89 .001
School achievement 3 school climate 3 engagement (T) 2.80 .005
School diversity 3 school climate 3 engagement (A) ⫺3.78 .001
School diversity 3 school climate 3 engagement (T) ⫺2.75 .006
School size 3 school climate 3 engagement (A) ⫺2.02 .043
School SES 3 school climate 3 engagement (A) ⫺2.08 .037
Process 3 Proximal Outcomes 3 Distal Outcome
Academic climate 3 engagement (A) 3 GPA 1.99 .047
Academic climate 3 engagement (T) 3 GPA ⫺3.31 .001
School climate 3 engagement (A) 3 GPA 2.25 .024
School climate 3 engagement (T) 3 GPA 3.43 .001

Note. A ⫽ adolescent-reported engagement; T ⫽ teacher-reported engagement. Only significant ( p ⬍ .05)


mediated relationships included.
850 BENNER, GRAHAM, AND MISTRY

the sets of covariance matrices. For each microsystem these suc- coefficient failed to reach significance (␤ ⫽ ⫺.14, ns). No group
cessive models involved constraining paths from (a) the measure- differences by race– ethnicity emerged in any other model rela-
ment model (i.e., loadings of the individual indicators on the latent tionships.
variable), (b) structural characteristics to processes, (c) structural
characteristics to proximal outcomes, (d) processes to proximal Discussion
outcomes, (e) processes to the distal outcome, and (f) proximal to
distal outcomes. This process allowed us to test where precisely in This study disentangles the effects of structural characteristics
our complex model group-related differences existed. of families and schools from the processes that occur therein and
For the multigroup analysis involving gender, our attempts to explores their effects on adolescents’ school engagement and later
estimate the complete model (depicted in Figure 2), which in- school performance. While previous studies have identified links
cluded the measurement model within the larger path analysis between either microsystem structural characteristics or processes,
model for both microsystem and mesosystem influences simulta- our study’s contribution to this existing research is the inclusion of
neously, failed to converge. Moderation analyses by race– structural characteristics and processes concurrently, as well as
ethnicity proved more fruitful. As shown in Table 5, for multi- examination of structural characteristics and processes in two
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

group analyses involving race– ethnicity, results indicated separate microsystems—families and schools. The conceptual
significant differences across groups in the relationships between framework for the proposed model was ecological theory, and our
school structures and school processes. Further analyses revealed results are consistent with its theoretical tenets. Specifically, we
that the group differences were specific to the relationship of found that the structural characteristics of families and schools
school structure to school academic climate. For White and Asian influenced the proximal processes that occur therein, and these
adolescents, school achievement was strongly associated with per- proximal processes, in turn, influenced students’ proximal and
ceptions of academic climate, such that higher levels of school- distal outcomes. Moreover, we found that structural characteristics
wide academic achievement were related to more positive percep- of families and schools tended to exert their influence on outcomes
tions of school academic climate (␤ ⫽ .36, p ⬍ .001). The indirectly through their influence on the proximal processes.
magnitude of this relationship was substantially smaller for Latino
students (␤ ⫽ .20, p ⬍ .001) and was nonsignificant for African The Moderating Role of Ethnicity
American students (␤ ⫽ .09, ns). A different pattern of results
emerged for the relationship between school size and perceptions The multigroup analyses examining the moderating effects of
of academic climate. For White and Asian students, increasing ethnicity were largely nonsignificant and suggest the generality of
school size was associated with more positive perceptions of the the overall model across adolescents from different ethnic groups
academic climate (␤ ⫽ .18, p ⬍ .05). In contrast, for Latino (the same, however, cannot be said for adolescent gender due to
students, increasing school size was associated with more negative that model’s failure to converge). The one set of analyses that
perceptions of the academic climate (␤ ⫽ ⫺.19, p ⬍ .001); the documented significant ethnicity effects suggested that the rela-
same trend was observed for African American youth, although the tionship of two school structural variables (achievement level and

Table 5
Moderation Results: Effects of Race–Ethnicity on Model Relationships

Constrained path ␹2 (df) p (for ⌬␹2)

1. None 1,276.2 (677) —


2. Family process measurement model (family 1,287.6 (687) ns
processes)
3. Family structures 3 family processes 1,296.3 (699) ns
4. Family structures 3 proximal outcomes 1,311.1 (711) ns
5. Family processes 3 proximal outcomes 1,326.5 (719) ns
6. Family processes 3 distal outcome 1,328.2 (723) ns
7. School process measurement model (school processes) 1,337.1 (733) ns
8. School structures 3 school processes 1,374.3 (753) .05
School achievement 3 school processes 1,347.9 (737) .05
School achievement 3 general school climate 1,338.8 (735) ns
School achievement 3 academic climate 1,347.9 (737) .05
School size 3 school processes 1,354.8 (739) .01
School size 3 general school climate 1,342.3 (737) ns
School size 3 academic climate 1,354.8 (739) .01
School diversity, ratio, SES 3 school processes 1,357.9 (749) ns
9. School structures 3 proximal outcomes 1,381.2 (769) ns
10. School processes 3 proximal outcomes 1,388.2 (777) ns
11. School processes 3 distal outcome 1,393.1 (781) ns
12. Proximal outcomes 3 distal outcome 1,398.6 (785) ns

Note. Models are nested step-wise such that a given step includes current constraints as well as constraints
imposed in previous steps. Bolded pathways denote differences across racial– ethnic groups. ns ⫽ nonsignificant
difference.
ECOLOGICAL CONTEXTS AND EDUCATIONAL OUTCOMES 851

size) to perceptions of academic climate (engagement of one’s methods, integrating data from adolescent and teacher reports,
classmates) operated differently across racial– ethnic groups. For school records, and data from the CDE. Such modeling decisions
White and Asian students, greater school-wide achievement and were deliberate, driven by concerns with respondent bias as well as
larger school size predicted perceptions that classmates were more by an interest in taking a multifaceted approach to examining
academically engaged. For Latino and African American youth, in family and school influences on urban adolescents.
contrast, there was no association between school achievement Relatedly, our study examines the links among structural char-
level and academic climate, and increasing school size predicted acteristics, contextual processes, and adolescent outcomes in two
decreases in the climate variable. distinct microsystems simultaneously and also models mesosys-
We suspect that these findings are due, in part, to the fact that temic influences on outcomes. Our results find that, in both sys-
the academic climate engagement variable tapped participants’ tems, structural characteristics are exerting much of their effects on
inferences about classmates’ achievement behaviors, and there was adolescent outcomes through their influence on processes that
systematic variance by ethnicity in the achievement levels of their occur within each system. Moreover, we find that families and
reference group. For example, if White and Asian students used schools have unique contributions to adolescents’ achievement
same-ethnicity peers as the reference group, then it is not surpris- outcomes—these microsystem influences are not interchangeable,
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

ing that higher overall school achievement predicted more per- nor are there any cross-system effects (i.e., mesosystemic of school
ceived classmate engagement since White and Asians students in structural characteristics influencing family processes or vice
this sample were doing better in school than their Latino and versa). Students in the current study attended large, urban schools
African American counterparts. Academic tracking at the high with high percentages of disadvantaged youth. Such schools face
school level can segregate high- and low-achieving ethnic groups, continual challenges to provide students and families with quality
which further promotes in-group members as the reference group educational opportunities and positive, safe school climates (Ko-
(e.g., Oakes, 1995). Latino and African American students in our zol, 1991, 2005; Rothstein, 2004). These challenging climates, in
sample attended schools with lower average school-wide achieve- turn, can negatively affect parents’ involvement in their children’s
ment (and a more limited achievement range), and their exposure education (Hoover-Dempsey et al., 2005), thus limiting the well-
to highly engaged classmates may have been especially limited in documented benefits of parent involvement (Gutman & Midgley,
the largest schools. It is evident that academic tracking, which can 2000; Lee & Croninger, 1994). In addition to these school-related
promote or inhibit the mixing opportunities of students of different issues, low-income and minority families face other barriers to
ethnic groups, is an important school structural variable that should educational involvement as well. These families often feel less
be included in process models such as those tested here, especially efficacious in relation to their children’s education, particularly
when those models involve making inferences about the peers in linguistic-minority families, which negatively affects their in-
one’s ecology. volvement in schooling (see Fuligni & Yoshikawa, 2003; Hoover-
Dempsey et al., 2005, for reviews). This is particularly relevant to
Student-Versus Teacher-Rated Academic Engagement our sample, as most of our Latino students are children of immi-
grants and speak primarily Spanish at home. Additionally, these
We identified an unexpected relationship between adolescents’ families face structural barriers that limit their involvement—for
perceptions of their academic climate and their teachers’ ratings of example, low-income parents are more likely to work at jobs with
their school engagement. The more academically involved stu- inflexible or unpredictable hours, have multiple jobs, or face
dents perceive their classmates to be, the less engaged their teacher financial constraints that create challenges with transportation
perceived them to be. These differential relationships between (Cooper & Crosnoe, 2007; Garcı́a Coll et al., 2002; Lareau, 2004).
academic climate and teacher- versus adolescent-rated engagement Finding ways to better connect families to schools is imperative for
were also evidenced in the directionality of the mediational anal- the educational success of these youth.
yses. While schools and families represent two microsystems in
It is not entirely clear why the relationship between academic which adolescents are embedded, future studies should examine
climate and self-rated versus teacher-rated engagement was differ- whether the findings reported here can be replicated within other
ent. Students may have rated their classmates as unrealistically pertinent microsystems, such as that involving peers. During ad-
highly engaged, in part to be self-enhancing, since the correlation olescence, individuals experience differentiated social environ-
between peer-rated and self-rated engagement was high. Also, ments that shift primary socialization responsibility from the fam-
even low-achieving classmates can be perceived as relatively en- ily to peers and the school (Harris, 1998). Given the pivotal role
gaged if the perceived classroom norm for achievement is low, as that peers play during adolescence, an examination of the effects of
in low-ability classrooms in schools with extreme tracking. Dif- the structural characteristics of peer groups (e.g., ethnic composi-
ferences between students’ perception of being engaged in school, tion, age and gender distribution) and the processes that occur
which is in part determined by subjective comparison to others, within peer groups (e.g., conflict, support) is warranted. Studies of
and their teachers’ presumably more objective perceptions, which the effects of the peer microsystem on academic outcomes are
elicits a different form of social comparison, is also a topic that particularly needed during school transitions when stable peer
merits further study. affiliations may be disrupted (Reyes, Gillock, & Kobus, 1994;
Roeser, Eccles, & Freedman-Doan, 1999).
Strengths, Caveats, and Limitations While the research reported here contributes to our understand-
ing about how structural characteristics and processes within fam-
Several design features contributed to the strength of the current ilies and schools influence adolescents’ educational outcomes,
study. First, our measurement model included multi-informant some limitations and caveats should be noted. First, this is a
852 BENNER, GRAHAM, AND MISTRY

short-term longitudinal study, and thus some of the data are cross- to test for moderating effects in each ethnic group separately,
sectional in nature. Therefore, only limited conclusions about including biracial participants, as well as for Gender ⫻ Ethnicity
causality can be made. The contribution lies in our ability to statistical interactions. Moreover, results from our moderation
inform understanding of the pathways by which structural charac- analyses are somewhat qualified by the fact that the ethnic groups
teristics and microsystem processes influence adolescents’ aca- were not drawn from independent clusters; this would require that
demic performance. Future studies should build on the findings we match schools on the structural characteristics under study and
presented here, examining the more distal effects of these micro- then select different ethnic groups from each school, which is
system structure–process relationships. Use of cross-lag, autore- beyond the scope of the current study.
gressive models would also be an important next step in determin- And finally, while we identify mediational pathways throughout
ing whether changes in microsystem processes occur across time our model, we acknowledge that, at times, bivariate correlations
and how these changes might affect changes in adolescent out- between the structural characteristics of families and schools are
comes. Cross-lagged models allow for inferences about the tem- only marginally correlated with student engagement and later
poral relationships between constructs to be made, and auto- grades in school. The causal steps method for testing mediation
regression purges much of the intraindividual or omitted error bias, proposed by Baron and Kenny (1986) requires establishing statis-
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

inherent in observational data (Mayer & Carroll, 1988; Singer & tically significant, direct relationships between predictors and out-
Willett, 2003). These techniques allow researchers to better deal comes. Other researchers (see Holmbeck, 1997; MacKinnon et al.,
with issues of selection bias, a significant improvement over more 2002) have suggested that models that lack the relationship be-
traditional ordinary least squares regression-based analyses. tween predictor and outcome but establish significant links be-
Second, related to our creation of latent constructs, we recognize tween predictor and mediator and mediator and outcomes can be
that family monitoring in particular exhibited uneven factor load- interpreted but suggest a more conservative label of indirect effect
ings across the four items. All loadings, however, were significant or intervening effect rather than mediated effect. Recent work,
at p ⬍ .001, and we made the decision to retain the four factors however, has suggested that the Baron and Kenny approach can be
based on past work. Examination of the items and loadings indi- problematic, both in terms of limited statistical power and limited
cates that specific parental monitoring behaviors (requiring ado- adaptation to multimediated models (see Dearing & Hamilton,
lescent to call home, wanting to know who the adolescent’s friends 2006, for a review). Moreover, Shrout and Bolger (2002) argued
are) exhibit higher loadings than items reflecting general parental that studies should not necessarily require distal variables to di-
laxity or oversight (overall strictness of family rules, permitting rectly predict outcomes (before taking into account possible prox-
adolescent to go out as often as he or she wants). The differential imal mediators), particularly when researchers believe that the
loading pattern suggests that concrete parental behaviors around direct effects will be more nuanced (i.e., that effect sizes will be
monitoring drive adolescents’ perceptions of family monitoring small). The analyses and results presented here are consistent with
more so than perceptions of the overall family monitoring climate. the arguments of this recent scholarship.
Those perceptions of concrete behaviors also appear to relate
differently to academic engagement than what we anticipated on Implications for Intervention
the basis of family monitoring research. Our latent variable was
negatively related to student reported engagement, and it did not The multimediated ecological model documented in this re-
mediate any of the relations between structural family variables search provides insights into directions for intervention. Findings
and academic engagement. Thus, adolescents who are doing less suggest that intervening at the process level may be a successful
well in school may perceive their parents as especially vigilant means of improving both adolescents’ engagement in school and
about where they are and who their friends are. Future studies their subsequent school performance. For example, in the school
should further explore the complexity of perceived family moni- microsystem, educators may be able to increase students’ feelings
toring, determining whether assessments of the construct would be of belonging and connectedness to schools through interventions
strengthened through an increased focus on concrete monitoring that foster greater involvement in extracurricular activities, includ-
behaviors. ing participation in sports and school clubs. Research has shown
Third, while we interpret direct effects findings, we recognize that participation in extracurricular activities and after-school pro-
that ecological theory (Bronfenbrenner, 1979; Bronfenbrenner & grams improves not only student achievement (Mahoney, Cairns,
Morris, 1998) suggests that the influence of some constructs in our & Farmer, 2003; Mahoney, Lord, & Carryl, 2005) but intergroup
model may be bidirectional. That is, it may be that adolescents’ relations as well (Clotfelter, 2002). Intervening to improve stu-
engagement in school influences their feelings of school belonging dents’ feelings of belonging and connectedness to school may be
or their families’ involvement in academics. Future studies should more amenable (and more cost effective) than trying to change the
consider examining these relationships longitudinally to determine structural characteristics of schools that influence academic en-
whether this bidirectionality is indeed influencing model results. gagement.
Fourth, we acknowledge that in the family microsystem, structure At the family microsystem level, our findings highlight the
and process variables were based on student self-report. A stronger importance of intervening to promote greater parental involve-
design, not possible with these data, would have included parents ment. Previous research has shown that higher levels of parental
and other family members as informants about family structure involvement are associated with better academic performance,
and process, as well as additional family characteristics (i.e., from grades and achievement test scores to teacher ratings of
family income) shown to be powerful predictors of adolescent students’ academic attitudes (see Fan & Chen, 2001; Henderson,
well-being (see review by McLoyd, 1998). Fifth, although our 1987; Jeynes, 2007, for reviews). A number of methods have been
large and multiethnic sample is noteworthy, we lacked the power found to improve parental involvement in their children’s educa-
ECOLOGICAL CONTEXTS AND EDUCATIONAL OUTCOMES 853

tion, including having teachers simply encourage greater involve- associations between and within families. Journal of Educational Psy-
ment (Deslandes & Bertrand, 2005), reducing barriers to parental chology, 98, 653– 664.
involvement (see Eccles & Harold, 1996, for a review), and Deslandes, R., & Bertrand, R. (2005). Motivation of parent involvement in
instituting more formal parental involvement intervention pro- secondary-level schooling. Journal of Education Research, 98, 164 –
gramming (Terrion, 2006). Improving parental involvement, in 175.
Eamon, M. K. (2005). Social-demographic, school, neighborhood, and
turn, has been linked to improvements in students’ academic
parenting influences on the academic achievement of Latino young
achievement (Dearing, Kreider, Simpkins, & Weiss, 2006). As in adolescents. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 34, 163–174.
the school microsystem, in the long run it may be more beneficial Eccles, J. S., & Harold, R. D. (1996). Family involvement in children’s and
to focus on changing parent behavior rather than (or in addition to) adolescents’ schooling. In A. Booth & J. F. Dunn (Eds.), Family–school
the factors that lead to particular family structural characteristics. links: How do they affect educational outcomes? Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Mediational models such as those documented here, therefore, Elder, G. H. (1985). Perspectives on the life course. In G. H. Elder (Ed.),
illuminate the need for distinguishing structure from process and Life course dynamics: Trajectories and transitions, 1968 –1980 (pp.
for better understanding both the proximal and distal antecedents 23– 49). Ithaca, NY: Cornell University Press.
of healthy adjustment during adolescence. Enders, C. K. (2002). Applying the Bollen-Stine bootstrap for goodness-
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

of-fit measures to structural equation models with missing data. Multi-


This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

variate Behavioral Research, 37, 359 –377.


References Fan, X., & Chen, M. (2001). Parental involvement and students’ academic
achievement: A meta-analysis. Educational Psychology Review, 13,
Baker, J. A., Derrer, R. D., Davis, S. M., Dinklage-Travis, H. E., Linder,
1–22.
D. S., & Nicholson, M. D. (2001). The flip side of the coin: Under-
Felner, R. D., Brand, S., DuBois, D. L., Adan, A. M., Mulhall, P. F., &
standing the school’s contribution to dropout and completion. School
Evans, E. G. (1995). Socioeconomic disadvantage, proximal environ-
Psychology Quarterly, 16, 406 – 426.
mental experiences, and socioemotional and academic adjustment in
Baron, R. M., & Kenny, D. A. (1986). The moderator–mediator variable
early adolescence: Investigation of a mediated effects model. Child
distinction in social psychological research: Conceptual, strategic, and
Development, 66, 774 –792.
statistical considerations. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
Fuligni, A. J., & Yoshikawa, H. (2003). Socioeconomic resources, parent-
51, 1173–1182.
ing, and child development among immigrant families. In M. H. Born-
Bellmore, A. D., Witkow, M. R., Graham, S., & Juvonen, J. (2004).
stein & R. H. Bradley (Eds.), Socioeconomic status, parenting, and child
Beyond the individual: The impact of ethnic context and classroom
development (pp. 107–124). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
behavioral norms on victims’ adjustment. Developmental Psychology,
Garc´ia Coll, C., Akiba, D., Palacios, N., Bailey, B., Silver, R., DiMartino,
40, 1159 –1172.
L., et al. (2002). Parental involvement in children’s education: Lessons
Bollen, K. A. (1989). Structural equations with latent variables. New
from three immigrant groups. Parenting: Science and Practice, 2, 303–
York: Wiley.
324.
Bollen, K. A., & Stine, R. (1990). Direct and indirect effects: Classical and
Gottfredson, G. (1984). Effective School Battery. Odessa, FL: Psycholog-
bootstrap estimates of variability. Sociological Methods, 20, 115–140.
Bronfenbrenner, U. (1979). The ecology of human development: Experi- ical Assessment Resources.
ments by nature and design. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. Green, C. W., Adams, A. M., & Turner, C. W. (1988). Development and
Bronfenbrenner, U. (1986). Ecology of the family as a context for human validation of the school interracial climate scale. American Journal of
development: Research perspectives. Developmental Psychology, 22, Community Psychology, 16, 241–259.
723–742. Gregory, A., & Weinstein, R. S. (2004). Connection and regulation at home
Bronfenbrenner, U., & Morris, P. (1998). The ecology of developmental and in school: Predicting growth in achievement for adolescents. Journal
processes. In W. Damon & R. M. Lerner (Eds.), Handbook of child of Adolescent Research, 19, 405– 427.
psychology (pp. 993–1028). New York: Wiley. Gutman, L. M., & Midgley, C. (2000). The role of protective factors in
Caldas, S. J. (1993). Reexamination of input and process factor effects on supporting the academic achievement of poor African American stu-
public school achievement. Journal of Education Research, 86, 206 – dents during the middle school transition. Journal of Youth and Adoles-
214. cence, 29, 223–248.
Chung, H. L., & Steinberg, L. (2006). Relations between neighborhood Harris, J. (1998). The nurture assumption: Why children turn out the way
factors, parenting behaviors, peer deviance, and delinquency among they do. New York: Free Press.
serious juvenile offenders. Developmental Psychology, 42, 319 –331. Henderson, A. T. (1987). The evidence continues to grow: Parent involve-
Clotfelter, C. T. (2002). Interracial contact in high school extracurricular ment improves student achievement. Columbia, MD: National Commit-
activities. The Urban Review, 34, 25– 46. tee for Citizens in Education.
Cook, T. D., Herman, M. R., Phillips, M., & Settersen, R. A., Jr. (2002). Holmbeck, G. N. (1997). Toward terminological, conceptual, and statisti-
Some ways in which neighborhoods, nuclear families, friendship groups, cal clarity in the study of mediators and moderators: Examples from the
and schools jointly affect changes in early adolescent development. child-clinical and pediatric psychology literatures. Journal of Consulting
Child Development, 73, 1283–1309. and Clinical Psychology, 65, 599 – 610.
Cooper, C. E., & Crosnoe, R. (2007). The engagement in schooling of Hoover-Dempsey, K. V., Walker, J. M. T., Sandler, H. M., Whetsel, D.,
economically disadvantaged parents and children. Youth and Society, 38, Green, C. L., Wilkins, A. S., et al. (2005). Why do parents become
372–391. involved? Research findings and implications. The Elementary School
Crosnoe, R. (2004). Social capital and the interplay of families and schools. Journal, 106, 105–130.
Journal of Marriage and Family, 66, 267–280. Jeynes, W. H. (2007). The relationship between parental involvement and
Dearing, E., & Hamilton, L. C. (2006). Contemporary advances and classic urban secondary school student academic achievement: A meta-analysis.
advice for analyzing mediating and moderating variables. Monographs Urban Education, 42, 82–110.
of the Society for Research in Child Development, 71(3, Serial No. 285). Juvonen, J., Nishina, A., & Graham, S. (2006). Ethnic diversity and
Dearing, E., Kreider, H., Simpkins, S., & Weiss, H. B. (2006). Family perceptions of safety in urban middle schools. Psychological Science,
involvement in school and low-income children’s literacy: Longitudinal 17, 393– 400.
854 BENNER, GRAHAM, AND MISTRY

Kenny, D. A. (2005). Multiple group models. Retrieved April 20, 2006, 3. Social, emotional, and personality development (6th ed., pp. 429 –
from http://davidakenny.net/cm/mgroups.htm 504). Hoboken, NJ: Wiley.
Kozol, J. (1991). Savage inequalities: Children in America’s schools. New Parker, G., Tupling, H., & Brown, L. B. (1979). A parental bonding
York: Harper Perennial. instrument. British Journal of Psychology, 52, 1–10.
Kozol, J. (2005). The shame of the nation: The restoration of apartheid Reyes, O., Gillock, K. L., & Kobus, K. (1994). A longitudinal study of
schooling in America. New York: Crown Trade Paperbacks/Crown. school adjustment in urban, minority adolescents: Effects of a high
Kuperminc, G. P., Leadbeater, B. J., & Blatt, S. J. (2001). School social school transition program. American Journal of Community Psychology,
climate and individual differences in vulnerability to psychopathology 22, 341–369.
among middle school students. Journal of School Psychology, 39, 141– Roeser, R. W., Eccles, J. S., & Freedman-Doan, C. (1999). Academic
159. functioning and mental health in adolescence: Patterns, progressions,
Lareau, A. (2004). Unequal childhoods: Class, race, and family life. and routes from childhood. Journal of Adolescent Research, 14, 135–
Berkeley, CA: University of California Press. 174.
Lee, V. E. (2000). Using hierarchical linear modeling to study social Roscigno, V. J. (2000). Family/school inequality and African-American/
contexts: The case of school effects. Educational Psychologist, 35, Hispanic achievement. Social Problems, 47, 266 –290.
125–141. Rothstein, R. (2004). Class and schools: Using social, economic, and
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

Lee, V. E., & Croninger, R. G. (1994). The relative importance of home educational reform to close the Black–White achievement gap. Wash-
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

and school in the development of literacy skills for middle-grade stu- ington DC: Economic Policy Institute.
dents. American Journal of Education, 102, 286 –329. Shrout, P. E., & Bolger, N. (2002). Mediation in experimental and non-
Lee, V. E., Smith, J. B., & Croninger, R. G. (1997). How high school experimental studies: New procedures and recommendations. Psycho-
organization influences the equitable distribution of learning in mathe- logical Methods, 7, 422– 445.
matics and science. Sociology of Education, 70, 128 –150. Simpson, E. H. (1949, April 30). Measurement of diversity. Nature, 163,
Leventhal, T., & Brooks-Gunn, J. (2000). The neighborhoods they live in: 688.
The effects of neighborhood residence on child and adolescent out- Singer, J. D., & Willett, J. B. (2003). Applied longitudinal data analysis.
comes. Psychological Bulletin, 126, 309 –337. New York: Oxford University Press.
Loukas, A., Suzuki, R., & Horton, K. (2006). Examining school connect- Sobel, M. E. (1982). Asymptotic confidence intervals for indirect effects in
edness a mediator of school climate effects. Journal of Research on structural equation models. Sociological Methods, 13, 290 –312.
Adolescence, 16, 491–502. Stage, F. K., Carter, H. C., & Norma, A. (2004). Path analysis: An
MacKinnon, D. P., Lockwood, C. M., Hoffman, J. M., West, S. G., & introduction and analysis of a decade of research. Journal of Education
Sheets, V. (2002). A comparison of methods to test mediation and other Research, 98, 5–12.
intervening variable effects. Psychological Methods, 7, 83–104. Stolz, H. E., Barber, B. K., Olsen, J. A., Erickson, L. D., Bradford, K. P.,
Mahoney, J. L., Cairns, B. D., & Farmer, T. W. (2003). Promoting Maughan, S. L., et al. (2004). Family and school socialization and
interpersonal competence and educational success through extracurric- adolescent academic achievement: A cross-national dominance analysis
ular activity participation. Journal of Educational Psychology, 95, 409 – of achievement predictors. Marriage and Family Review, 36, 7–33.
418. Stone, S., & Han, M. (2005). Perceived school environments, perceived
Mahoney, J. L., Lord, H., & Carryl, E. (2005). An ecological analysis of discrimination, and school performance among children of Mexican
after-school program participation and the development of academic immigrants. Children and Youth Services Review, 27, 51– 66.
performance and motivational attributes for disadvantaged children. Terrion, J. L. (2006). Building social capital in vulnerable families: Suc-
Child Development, 76, 811– 825. cess markers of a school-based intervention program. Youth and Society,
Mayer, L. S., & Carroll, S. S. (1988). Measures of dependence for cross- 38, 155–176.
lagged panel models. Sociological Methods and Research, 17, 93–120. Wellborn, J. G., & Connell, J. P. (1991). Engagement versus disaffection:
McLoyd, V. C. (1998). Socioeconomic disadvantage and child develop- Motivated patterns of action in the academic domain. Rochester, NY:
ment. American Psychologist, 53, 185–204. University of Rochester.
McNeal, R. B., Jr. (1997). High school dropouts: A closer examination of Widaman, K. (2006). Missing data: What to do with or without them.
school effects. Social Science Quarterly, 78, 209 –222. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 71(3,
Moos, R. H., & Moos, B. S. (1981). Family Environment Scale. Palo Alto, Serial No. 285).
CA: Consulting Psychology Press. Witkow, M. R. (2006). Perceived social norms for schoolwork and
Muthén, L. K., & Muthén, B. O. (1998 –2006). Mplus user’s guide (4th ed.) achievement during adolescence. Ypsilanti: Eastern Michigan Univer-
[Computer software manual]. Los Angeles: Author. sity.
Oakes, J. (1995). Two cities’ tracking and within-school segregation. Woolley, M. E., & Grogan-Kaylor, A. (2006). Protective family factors in
Teachers College Record, 96, 681– 690. the context of neighborhood: Promoting positive school outcomes. Fam-
Parcel, T. L., & Dufur, M. J. (2001). Capital at home and at school: Effects ily Relations, 55, 93–104.
on student achievement. Social Forces, 79, 881–911.
Parke, R. D., & Buriel, R. (2006). Socialization in the family: Ethnic and Received February 26, 2007
ecological perspectives. In N. Eisenberg (Vol. Ed.), W. Damon (Series Revision received February 22, 2008
Ed.), & R. M. Lerner (Series Ed.), Handbook of child psychology: Vol. Accepted February 26, 2008 䡲

You might also like