You are on page 1of 20

HHS Public Access

Author manuscript
Clin Psychol (New York). Author manuscript; available in PMC 2018 June 01.
Author Manuscript

Published in final edited form as:


Clin Psychol (New York). 2017 June ; 24(2): 203–217. doi:10.1111/cpsp.12192.

From the Cradle to the Grave: The Effect of Adverse Caregiving


Environments on Attachment and Relationships Throughout the
Lifespan
Colleen Doyle and Dante Cicchetti
Institute of Child Development, University of Minnesota
Author Manuscript

Abstract
This paper reviews research examining the effects of adverse early caregiving on relationships
throughout the lifespan. Central attachment constructs are summarized and integrated into a
review of research on the long-term effects of institutional rearing and child maltreatment.
Findings are interpreted within the organizational perspective on development, which
conceptualizes attachment as a stage-salient task of infancy that influences the reorganization of
adaptive/maladaptive functioning around subsequent stage-salient tasks. Children who experience
adverse early caregiving are more likely to exhibit aberrant attachment behaviors, deficits in
social-emotional competencies, and persisting difficulties in social functioning and relationship
outcomes. Disorganized attachment behavior stemming from adverse early caregiving has been a
major focus of this work. Intervention efforts that target mental representations related to
attachment relationships can facilitate improved social functioning. Clinical implications of this
Author Manuscript

work are discussed.

Keywords
attachment; caregiving; institutional care; maltreatment; relationships; Randomized Control Trial
(RCTs) interventions

Relationships provide a fundamental context for development across the lifespan. These
relationships include those with parents, early care and education providers, peers, and
romantic partners. Because early caregiving experiences critically shape brain and behavior
development, children who experience absent or abusive caregiving are denied opportunities
for healthy development. Instead, adverse caregiving environments can set into motion a
Author Manuscript

probabilistic developmental pathway that is characterized by increased risk for maladaptive


behavior and relationship disadvantage across the lifespan.

This paper reviews a growing literature examining the association among childhood
adversity, attachment disruptions, and relationships in childhood, adolescence, and
adulthood. We focus on two types of childhood adversity: institutional rearing and child
maltreatment. First, we review attachment theory, the central conceptual framework guiding

Please address correspondence and reprint requests to: Colleen Doyle or Dante Cicchetti, Institute of Child Development, University
of Minnesota, 51 E. River Road, Minneapolis, MN 55455 contact: doyle291@umn.edu or cicchett@umn.edu.
Doyle and Cicchetti Page 2

work in this area. Next, we summarize recent empirical research and intervention efforts
Author Manuscript

applying attachment constructs to the study of developmental pathways and phenomena


linking extreme, pathogenic caregiving environments to social functioning and relationship
outcomes later in life. Finally, we discuss the clinical implications of these findings, as well
as promising directions for future work. Our approach is informed by an organizational
perspective on development (Cicchetti & Toth, 2015; Sroufe, Egeland, Carlson, & Collins,
2005), which conceptualizes development as a series of functional reorganizations around
stage-salient tasks. Importantly, from this perspective, the meaning of social behaviors, and
not merely their occurrence, is informative. Additionally, an organizational perspective
considers how stage-salient tasks may provide an opportunity for growth, resilience, and
recovery, in addition to vulnerability. For these reasons, this perspective has important
clinical value for researchers examining processes and pathways linking adverse early
caregiving to long-term social functioning. Assessing what has been learned from this
Author Manuscript

research is important, as an understanding of how childhood adversity influences attachment


and relationships across the lifespan has broad implications for clinicians, researchers,
policy makers, and the public at large.

Central Tenets of Attachment Theory


According to attachment theory, early caregiving experiences influence adaptation and
maladaptation across the lifespan by organizing individual and relational developmental
processes “from the cradle to the grave” (Bowlby, 1969/1982; p. 208). This conceptual
framework has advanced our understanding of normal and abnormal development by
generating testable hypotheses about the developmental processes and pathways linking
early caregiving experiences to long-term outcomes. The central assumption of attachment
theory is that relationships function to increase survival. Attachment theory contends that
Author Manuscript

infants are biologically predisposed to bond with caregivers, signal for their protection and
care, and seek proximity to those who can provide safety and support. From early interaction
patterns with caregivers, infants are thought to develop an internal working model of
attachment, or a set of mental representations of the self, the caregiver, and the world
(Sherman, Rice, & Cassidy, 2015). These internal working models operate across the
lifespan to promote survival by guiding the individual's expectations and explorations of the
social and physical environment.

In addition to promoting survival, attachment relationships can propel infants along


developmental pathways of adaptation or maladaptation. Ainsworth and colleagues (1978)
generated significant interest in this area by bringing attention to the individual differences
in the infant-caregiver attachment relationship (Ainsworth, Blehar, Waters, & Wall, 1978).
Author Manuscript

Considerable research shows that a caregiver's behavior toward the child is the key
determinant of the attachment relationship and the child's internal working model (de Wolff
& van IJzendoorn, 1997), and that both the caregiver's behavior and the child's internal
working model subsequently influence the child's social and emotional development (e.g.,
Sroufe, 2005). Specifically, caregivers can promote a secure attachment relationship when
they are available, sensitive to a child's needs, and responsive to a child's cues. Children who
are securely attached learn they can rely on the caregiver for protection, and consequently
these children perceive themselves as worthy of receiving care and love, and relationships as

Clin Psychol (New York). Author manuscript; available in PMC 2018 June 01.
Doyle and Cicchetti Page 3

safe and worth pursuing. In contrast, caregivers who are unavailable, low in sensitivity, or
Author Manuscript

inconsistently or inappropriately responsive to a child's needs promote an insecure


attachment relationship. These children may develop ambivalent expectations about their
own self-worth and view relationships as hurtful, rejecting, or unsafe. Thus, early caregiving
experiences can initiate cascading and reciprocal effects among attachment relationships,
social development, and social adaptation over the lifespan. Importantly, research shows that
secure vs. insecure attachment relationships cannot be explained by genetic factors,
implying a process of environmental transmission via parent-child social interactions.

The profound and persisting effects of early caregiving on social functioning can be
understood from the organizational perspective on development (see Figure 1). This
perspective conceptualizes development as a series of functional reorganizations around
stage-salient tasks. Importantly, the organizational perspective on development holds that the
meaning of a behavior, not merely its occurrence, must be considered. For example,
Author Manuscript

disengaging from a social conflict may be adaptive or maladaptive, depending on the social
situation. Accordingly, this perspective can offer clinical insights on relationship outcomes
linked to adverse caregiving, rather than only reporting incidence rates. Additionally, by
focusing on the meaning of behavior, this perspective brings attention to the functions and
context sensitivity of underlying behavioral and biological systems that organize attachment
and relational behavior at each stage of development. Thus, this perspective considers
mechanisms and mediators of early child adversity at multiple levels of analyses: biological,
cognitive, behavioral, and social. Finally, the organizational perspective on development
considers how each stage-salient task involves opportunities for growth, recovery, resilience,
the consolidation of functional skills, as well as challenges that may open windows of
vulnerability. However, overall adaptation across the lifespan is depicted as cumulative and
hierarchical, such that successful resolution of an early stage-salient task increases the
Author Manuscript

likelihood of subsequent successful task resolution. Thus, an organizational perspective on


development is needed to comprehend the continuity and discontinuity of attachment and
relational behaviors within the context of developmental changes, as well as the opportunity
for resilience following early adverse experiences.

The literature provides strong support for the cumulative and hierarchical depiction of social
adaption/maladaption across development following differences in early caregiving
experiences (see Groh et al., 2014). Specifically, research shows that stage-salient
attachment behaviors of infancy, such as smiling, clinging, and checking back with a
caregiver, are associated with the successful resolution of subsequent relational stage-salient
tasks, such as exploring peer interactions, interpreting social cues of peers, and establishing
secure peer relationships. Longitudinal studies, more frequently existing with study samples
Author Manuscript

experiencing typically occurring variations in early caregiving experiences, show that social
and emotional competency in childhood feed forward over time to facilitate successful
resolution of stage-salient tasks related to adolescent and adult peer and romantic
relationships, such as establishing positive self-esteem and developing conflict resolution
skills (see Grossmann, Grossmann, & Waters, 2006). It should be noted that the
organizational perspective on development is inherent to the approach of many attachment
researchers, though it may not be explicitly acknowledged.

Clin Psychol (New York). Author manuscript; available in PMC 2018 June 01.
Doyle and Cicchetti Page 4

Early Adversity and Attachment Disturbances in Childhood


Author Manuscript

Adverse caregiving environments, such as those characterized by neglect, deprivation, or


abuse, can be viewed as extreme, pathogenic forms of insensitive caregiving within the
continuum of caretaking casualty (see Sameroff & Chandler, 1975). These adverse
environments deviate from typical caregiving environments such that infants experience the
absence of necessary input, as well as exposure to adverse input. In institutional settings,
caregiving generally is non-individualized, routine, and cursory, rather than affectionate,
sensitive and responsive (van IJzendoorn et al., 2011). Similarly, children who experience
maltreatment are exposed to social-emotional deprivation and/or abuse. As a result, children
who experience institutional care or maltreatment are denied vital opportunities to form
healthy attachments, resulting in behavior presenting as attachment disturbances and
disorders, and difficulties establishing peer relationships.
Author Manuscript

Disorganized attachment, comprised of fearful, conflicted, and disoriented behaviors, has


been a primary focus of empirical and clinical work on attachment disturbances related to
these two forms of adverse early caregiving. Numerous independent studies have shown that
frightening or unpredictable caregiver behavior (e.g., verbally or physically intrusive or
hostile behavior), as opposed to insensitive behavior (e.g., missing or ignoring child's cues or
emotions) promotes disorganized child attachment behaviors (e.g., freezing, hitting the
parent, or contradictory behavior like running up and then pulling away) (see Lyons-Ruth &
Jacobvitz, 2008). Accordingly, disorganized behaviors are thought to reflect the absence or
breakdown of children's relational strategies for dealing with threat, and suggest that
children do not have the opportunity to learn effective self-soothing or self-regulation skills.
From an organizational perspective of development, the proposed meaning of these
behaviors has important implications for the sequelae of early adverse caregiving. Meta-
Author Manuscript

analyses suggest that approximately 15% of infants in normative samples exhibit


disorganized attachment behavior; in contrast, the prevalence rate is two to three times
higher among infants who have experienced institutional rearing (Lionetti, Pastore, &
Barone, 2015; van den Dries, Juffer, van IJzendoorn, & Bakermans-Kranenburg, 2009) and
up to 80-90% in child maltreatment samples (see Cicchetti & Toth, 2015; Cyr, Euser,
Bakermans-Kranenburg, & van IJzendoorn, 2010). These analyses suggest that children who
experience institutional care or maltreatment have a relatively equivalent chance of
developing an insecure or disorganized attachment, and a relatively minor chance of
developing a secure attachment.

Extreme, pathogenic caregiving environments also are associated with two attachment
disorders: reactive attachment disorder (RAD), an emotionally withdrawn/inhibited pattern
Author Manuscript

of behavior, and disinhibited social engagement disorder (DSED), an indiscriminately


social/disinhibited pattern of behavior (see Zeanah & Gleason, 2015). Importantly,
disorganized attachment is conceptualized as reflecting a state of distorted attachment, while
attachment disorders are conceptualized as reflecting “non-attachment.” While the majority
of children who experience institutional rearing and maltreatment exhibit insecure or
disorganized attachments, and may show symptoms of RAD or DSED, most do not meet
criteria for these disorders (Zeanah et al., 2015). Recent research shows RAD and DSED

Clin Psychol (New York). Author manuscript; available in PMC 2018 June 01.
Doyle and Cicchetti Page 5

both arise from inadequate caregiving environments characterized by social neglect, and
Author Manuscript

have distinct presentations, correlates, and response to intervention.

The core features of RAD include a general absence of attachment behaviors, including
failure to seek and respond to comfort from a caregiver when distressed, as well as reduced
social and emotional reciprocity with a caregiver. RAD is uncommon, and is estimated to
occur in approximately 10% of children who experience inadequate caregiving (American
Psychiatric Association [APA], 2013). Notably, RAD symptoms are greater in children who
have experienced more severe social neglect, and dissipate rapidly after intervention efforts
provide children the opportunity to form selective attachments with nurturing foster or
adoptive parents (Zeanah et al., 2015). In contrast, the core features of DSED include a
general absence of developmentally appropriate reticence around unfamiliar adults, and a
failure to check in and maintain proximity with caregivers. Importantly, children who meet
criteria for DSED may exhibit disinhibited behavior with strangers and show typical
Author Manuscript

attachment behaviors with primary caregivers. Approximately 20% of children who have
experienced social neglect meet criteria for DSED (APA, 2013). Numerous adoption studies
show DSED symptoms are associated with length of time in institutional care, persist after
intervention, and are not associated with the quality of caregiving in foster or adoptive
homes (Zeanah et al., 2015).

Although post-institutionalized (PI) children show remarkable recovery after they are placed
in nurturing families, there is considerable variability in developmental outcomes. As a
group, PI children exhibit delays and deficits in identifying and interpreting emotions,
perspective taking, understanding and observing social boundaries, and regulating emotions
(Gunnar & Quevedo, 2007). From an organization perspective of development, failure to
resolve these stage-salient tasks put PI children at increased risk for persisting social
Author Manuscript

competency deficits and relationship difficulties. Overall, research shows social problems
are positively correlated with age at adoption, with variability related to type of social
outcome and severity of early deprivation (Zeanah, Gunnar, McCall, Kreppner, & Fox,
2011). Interestingly, children adopted before turning one years old exhibit attachment
security with adoptive parents that is comparable to non-adopted peers; in contrast, adoption
after 12 months is associated with increased risk for insecure, disorganized, and atypical
attachment patterns (van den Dries et al., 2009). Recent research shows that nearly all PI
infants, regardless of age at adoption, form some kind of attachment with adoptive parents
by 9 months post-placement (Carlson, Hostinar, Mliner, & Gunnar, 2014). These findings
speak to the robust capacity for human attachment.

Consistent with an organizational perspective on development, research shows the negative


Author Manuscript

relational patterns acquired in a maltreating early caregiving environment become


incorporated into the child's mental representations of self, others, and the social world,
which are relevant for later peer relations (Cicchetti & Toth, 2015). Accordingly,
maltreatment may lead to negative expectations of the self and others, as well as the concept
of relationships as involving victimization and coercion. Through these disturbances in
representational models, the negative effects of early maltreatment can be carried forward to
later stage-salient developmental tasks, such as establishing peer relationships. This
conceptualization of continuity in relational functioning is supported by a large body of

Clin Psychol (New York). Author manuscript; available in PMC 2018 June 01.
Doyle and Cicchetti Page 6

research showing maltreatment is associated with later peer difficulties in childhood and
Author Manuscript

adolescence (see Riggs, 2010).

In comparison to their peers, children who have experienced maltreatment are more likely to
actively withdraw from social interactions and display more aggressive behavior when they
do interact (Cicchetti & Toth, 2015). This conflicted pattern of both “flight” and “fight”
interaction with peers is consistent with research showing that maltreatment is associated
with disorganized attachment behavior. It suggests children who have experienced
maltreatment may operate from a disorganized representational model that includes elements
of both victim and victimizer (Cicchetti & Toth, 2015). Accordingly, research shows that
maltreated children are more likely than nonmaltreated children to bully other children and
more likely to be victimized by their peers (Cicchetti & Toth, 2015). Additionally, child
maltreatment, particularly child sexual abuse, has been associated with age-inappropriate
sexualized behavior in children (Trickett, Noll, & Putnam, 2011). Maltreatment also places
Author Manuscript

children at risk for being rejected and victimized by their peers. Research shows that the
continuation of maltreatment is associated with repeated (as opposed to transitory) peer
rejection, and that aggression largely accounts for the association between maltreatment and
peer rejection (Bolger & Patterson, 2001). Moreover, genetically informed twin designs
show that early experiences of maltreatment play a causal role in the development of
antisocial behavior like aggression, independent of genetic effects (Jaffee, Caspi, Moffitt, &
Taylor, 2004). Importantly, these results suggest that preventing maltreatment experiences
can prevent its later impact on peer relationships.

Additionally, research shows that the effects of maltreatment on disrupted peer functioning
may be accounted for by perturbations in cognitive and emotional processes (Cicchetti &
Toth, 2015). In response to a research paradigm that involves two adult experimenters acting
Author Manuscript

out a social conflict scenario that elicits simulated anger in both the actors, children who
have experienced maltreatment demonstrate patterns of both overcontrolled (i.e., low
reactivity, high avoidance) and undercontrolled (i.e., both positive and negative affect,
disorganized behavior, and mismatched social cognitions) physiological and behavioral
regulatory profiles (Maughan & Cicchetti, 2002). This conflicted pattern of regulation is
again akin to a disorganized attachment profile. It suggests children who have experienced
maltreatment may fail to develop health regulatory strategies, and instead may learn different
strategies of coping with social adversity that are adaptive in early environments, and
maladaptive in later peer settings. Children who have experienced physical abuse display
deficits in social information processing, such as making errors in encoding social cues,
exhibiting a bias toward attributing hostile intent, generating more aggressive responses, and
positively evaluating aggression as an appropriate response (Cicchetti et al., 2015). These
Author Manuscript

deficits mediate the association between physical abuse and aggression towards peers.

Early Adversity and Attachment Disturbances in Adolescence


Although several longitudinal studies have shown that PI youth have higher social problems
in adolescence compared to non-adopted youth, some studies have found no group
differences (Hawk & McCall, 2010). Numerous studies have examined parent and teacher
report of peer difficulties, and shown that PI adolescents have higher rates compared to non-

Clin Psychol (New York). Author manuscript; available in PMC 2018 June 01.
Doyle and Cicchetti Page 7

adopted youth peer problems (e.g., Gunnar & Quevedo, 2007). Longitudinal data from the
Author Manuscript

English and Romanian Adoptees Study show that adoption after six months is associated
with symptoms of DSED persisting to age 15; in contrast, children whose exposure to
institutional care was limited to six months did not show persisting effects (Rutter et al.,
2010). These findings suggest a sensitive period of early neurobehavioral development that
is significantly influenced by the quality of caregiving may open at six months old. Since
DSED behaviors transgress social boundaries and cues, it is not surprising that youth with
these problems report difficulty establishing meaningful peer relationships (Kay & Green,
2013).

Longitudinal research shows that disruption to attachment behaviors in infancy, stemming


from childhood adversity, confer risk for social maladaptation in adolescence. In social
contexts, such as peer interaction, aberrant or absent attachment behaviors are likely to
negatively affect the “readability” of the youth's social signals, as well as the reactions the
Author Manuscript

teen can elicit from other relationship partners. During adolescence, age-appropriate
behavior related to emerging sexual identify is a stage-salient task. Longitudinal research
shows that child maltreatment, particularly sexual abuse, is associated with maladaptive
sexual behaviors, including sexual revictimization, risky sexual behaviors, and teen
parenthood (Trickett, Noll, & Putnam, 2011). Underlying these behavioral alterations are
alterations to reproductive biology including early onset of puberty and dysregulated stress
reactivity, as well as alterations to cognitive and affective functioning, such as dissociative
symptoms, self-harm, and depression (Trickett et al., 2011).

Several longitudinal studies, including the Family Pathways Project, have shown that
disorganized attachment behaviors in infancy are associated with elevated risk for low self-
esteem, emotional instability, and internalizing and externalizing behaviors that cause
Author Manuscript

relational problems with parents, peers, and romantic partners in middle childhood and
adolescence (see Lyons-Ruth et al., 2008; Obsuth, Henninghausen, Brumariu, & Lyons-
Ruth, 2014). Similar to the odd or contradictory presentation of disorganized behaviors in
infancy, during adolescence these children show erratic behavior in the context of social
interactions that is sometimes withdrawn, sometimes solicitous, sometimes controlling, and
sometimes hostile. Moreover, punitive adolescent interactions were associated with poor
quality romantic relationships that include abuse to and from the partner (Obsuth et al.,
2014). Furthermore, research suggests that even when disorganized youth develop consistent
social strategies, they struggle to adapt these strategies to different social contexts (see
Cassidy, 2000). These findings suggest that disrupted attachment behaviors mediate the
relationship between early adverse caregiving and lack of social-emotional competence in
childhood.
Author Manuscript

Early Adversity and Attachment Disturbances in Adulthood


In accordance with an organizational perspective on development, research also suggests
that individuals with aberrant or absent attachment behaviors experience difficulties in adult
social and romantic relationships. There are few published reports on adult relationship
outcomes in PI individuals, and many studies include individuals who entered institutional
care later than the infant-toddler period. As a result, it is unclear if these outcomes are

Clin Psychol (New York). Author manuscript; available in PMC 2018 June 01.
Doyle and Cicchetti Page 8

representative of adults who experienced adverse caregiving only during a discrete period of
Author Manuscript

early development. Beyond this confound, the findings are mixed. For example, some
research suggests that PI adults do not differ from non-adopted adults in peer problems or
number of close confidents (Westhues & Cohen, 1997; Sigal, Perry, Rossignol, & Ouimet,
2003), whereas other studies suggest PI adults have fewer social supports and close
confidants (Sigal et al., 2003; Weiner & Kupermintz, 2001). However, several investigators
have shown that PI adults are less likely to be married than their non-adopted peers (e.g.,
Tieman, van der Ende, & Verhulst, 2005). Currently, to our knowledge there are no
longitudinal data available on quality of adult romantic relationships for individuals who
experienced early institutional caregiving. However, these data are expected to be
forthcoming from the English and Romanian Adoptees Study.

The bulk of this research comes from studies using the Adult Attachment Interview (AAI;
George, Kaplan, & Main, 1996). This 60 minute semi-structured interview with adults
Author Manuscript

assesses caregiving experiences during childhood. Participants are asked to report general
reflections on their relationship with caregivers as well as specific memories that illustrate
how they feel. Additionally, adults are asked to state how these relationships may have
influenced their development and current functioning. Together, these features are designed
to assess adult internal working models of attachment. Adults are classified as secure/
autonomous, insecure/dismissing, insecure/preoccupied, or unresolved. Research shows
these classifications are stable across short-term test-re-test studies and are significantly
associated with social functioning in adulthood (see Riggs, 2010). Adults who are secure/
autonomous exhibit a balanced perspective of early relationships and value relationships.
This classification is associated with greater capacity for intimacy, and better marital
functioning and satisfaction. In contrast, adults who are insecure/dismissing deny or devalue
the impact of early relationships, and often have difficulty recalling specific events.
Author Manuscript

Additionally, they are more likely to both describe an early history of rejection and idealize
early experiences. In relation to adult social functioning, the insecure/dismissing
classification is associated with maladaptive social strategies, such as interpersonal
avoidance, hostility, and holding perfectionistic expectations of social partners. Adults who
are insecure/preoccupied report confusion or ambivalence about early experiences, and often
describe current relationships that are characterized by active anger or passivity. This
classification is associated with rumination about close relationships and interpersonal
distancing. Finally, unsolved adults exhibit a mixture of dismissing and preoccupied
behaviors in the context of social relationships. This classification is associated with general
distrust of others, hostility, and low levels of empathy.

Studies that have empirically examined the association between infant attachment
Author Manuscript

classifications and AAI classifications in samples with typically occurring variations in early
caregiving experiences have found moderate to strong correspondence between secure/
insecure classifications, yet suggest that experiences beyond infancy play a role in adult
attachment (e.g., Waters, Merrick, Treboux, Crowell, & Albersheim, 2000). These findings
provide support for an organizational perspective on development, suggesting that although
social-emotional competence may initially be consolidated in infancy, there are
opportunities to reorganize functioning in childhood, adolescence, and adulthood.

Clin Psychol (New York). Author manuscript; available in PMC 2018 June 01.
Doyle and Cicchetti Page 9

Finally, research that retrospectively assess childhood maltreatment and current adult social
Author Manuscript

functioning show that adults who acknowledge maltreatment report elevated interpersonal
sensitivity, hostility, and aggression, as well as low self-esteem, a poor or disorganized sense
of self, and symptoms of dissociative disorders (e.g., see Riggs, 2010). Moreover,
experiencing child maltreatment is associated with poor physical and mental health in
adulthood, as well as risky sexual behavior, and unintendend pregnancy (Felitti et al., 1998).
Additionally, lower quality relationships in adulthood with an individual's partner/spouse
and with children (Umberson, Williams, Thomas, Liu, & Thomeer, 2015).

Intervention Research
Given strong evidence that early social interactions between infants and their caregivers
provide an important foundation for subsequent development, there is a growing interest in
interventions that target these interactions as a means of influencing developmental
Author Manuscript

outcomes. Although correlational studies can shed light on whether measures of parent-child
attachment are associated with later outcomes, this research cannot determine whether
associations are causal. In contrast, intervention-oriented research can offer an effective test
for causal hypotheses. Accordingly, intervention studies provide an important complement
to experimental and observational work by helping clinicians and researchers discover
causal mechanisms that can facilitate change in quality of infant attachment behaviors. This
literature shows that early intervention can set the stage for positive developmental
pathways. Intervention programs that focus on promoting attachment and relational process
have also shown profound and persisting benefits on child development. Among the most
well-studied of these interventions are the Bucharest Early Intervention Project (BEIP;
Nelson, Fox, & Zeanah, 2014), Child-Parent Psychotherapy (CPP; Lieberman & Van Horn,
2005) the Interpersonal Therapy for Adolescents (IPT-A; Mufson, Dorta, Moreau, &
Author Manuscript

Weissman, 2004), and the Skills Training in Affective and Interpersonal Regulation for
Adolescents (STAIR-A; Gudiño, Leonard, & Cloitre, 2016). Many of these interventions are
informed by developmental theory and guided by the organizational perspective on
development.

BEIP is a preventative intervention randomized controlled trial (RCT) of a foster care


intervention for children who were placed in an institution for young children in Bucharest,
Romania at or shortly after birth. Children (N=136) were randomly assigned to remain in
institutional care (ICG; care-as-usual group), or to be removed from institutional care and
placed in a high-quality foster care group (FCG) designed specifically for BEIP. On average,
FCG entered foster care at 22 months. A group of never-institutionalized children (NIG)
served as community controls. Children were assessed at multiple stage-salient
Author Manuscript

developmental periods, including 30, 42, 54, and 96 months. Results showed that FCG
children showed improved cognitive, emotional, behavioral, and social functioning. In
contrast, children who remained in institutional care showed ongoing developmental deficits
across all domains. Moreover, social communication problems for ICG children increased as
a function of the percentage of a child's life spent in an institution. For multiple domains
relevant to social functioning (e.g., on measures of attachment security and emotional
responsiveness and reactivity) FCG children performed between ICG and NIG children.
Altogether, these findings demonstrate that children who experience adverse early

Clin Psychol (New York). Author manuscript; available in PMC 2018 June 01.
Doyle and Cicchetti Page 10

caregiving environments can show developmental plasticity and recovery following


Author Manuscript

intervention, including in domains that are relevant to long-term social functioning and
relational outcomes. However, the fact that the FCG continued to perform above the ICG but
below the NIG suggests that early caregiving experience can establish probabilistic
parameters on development. Importantly, the inclusion of a normative community control
group in the BEIP design provided a stringent test for evaluating the efficacy of the foster
care intervention and the organizational perspective on development.

CPP is designed to support child development by fostering a positive, healthy parent-child


attachment with at-risk children and their caregivers who have experienced some form of
trauma (Lieberman & Van Horn, 2005). CPP is informed by attachment theory and the
parent-child attachment relationship is conceptualized as the “client” and main recipient of
therapeutic services, rather than the parent or the child independently. In addition to
improving the parent-child relationship, the CPP intervention focuses on safety, affect
Author Manuscript

regulation, and the joint construction of a trauma narrative. Several RCT studies have shown
that CPP is effective at promoting adaptive social functioning and successful resolution of
stage-salient tasks at various points in development. This includes improving attachment
security in maltreated toddlers (Cicchetti, Rogosch, & Toth, 2006), and positive
representations of self and caregiver in maltreated preschoolers (Toth, Maughan, Manly,
Spagnola, & Cicchetti, 2002), as well as reducing emotional-behavioral problems in
preschoolers exposed to domestic violence (e.g., Lieberman, Van Horn & Ghosh Ippen,
2006).

In an RCT study examining the malleability of insecure and disorganized attachment


following CPP, one-year-old infants from maltreating families (N=137) and their mothers
were randomly assigned to infant-parent psychotherapy (IPP), psychoeducational parenting
Author Manuscript

intervention (PPI), or a community standard (CS) control group (Cicchetti et al., 2006). A
fourth group of infants (N=52) and their mothers from nonmaltreating families served as an
additional low-income normative comparison (NC) group. At baseline, mothers in the
maltreatment group reported greater abuse and neglect in their own childhoods, more
insecure relationships with their own mothers, more maladaptive parenting attitudes, and
they were observed to evince lower maternal sensitivity relative to nonmaltreatment group
mothers. Infants in the maltreatment groups had significantly higher rates of disorganized
attachment than infants in the NC group. These findings suggest that mental representations
developed from early relationship experiences may have the potential to influence parent-
child interactions with the next generation. At postintervention follow-up at age 26 months,
only children in the IPP and PPI groups demonstrated substantial increases in secure
attachment. Moreover, disorganized attachment continued to predominate in the CS group.
Author Manuscript

Interestingly, both CPP and the nonrelational intervention (PPI) were found to be equally
efficacious in fostering secure attachment and in reducing disorganized attachment in infants
from maltreating families (Cicchetti et al., 2006). However, when attachment was assessed
again one year after the conclusion of treatment, only CPP was found to be efficacious in
promoting sustained security of attachment over time (Pickreign, Stronach, Toth, Rogosch,
& Cicchetti, 2013). These results suggest that, in cases of extremely maladaptive parenting,
intervention models that go beyond parent skills training may be necessary.

Clin Psychol (New York). Author manuscript; available in PMC 2018 June 01.
Doyle and Cicchetti Page 11

Another RCT study of maltreated preschoolers (N=112) showed the efficacy of CPP to alter
Author Manuscript

preschoolers' internal working models of attachment related to representations of themselves


and their caregivers (Toth, Maughan, Manly, Spagnola, & Cicchetti, 2002). The investigators
targeted these internal working models as an important outcome since they are thought to
form the basis of children's future relationship expectations, as reviewed above. Importantly,
results showed CPP's clinical focus on the parent-child relationship as the mechanism of
change resulted in greater duration of treatment effects in behavior problems compared to
standard community treatment supplemented by case management.

Findings from the Family Pathways Project, one of the few longitudinal studies
prospectively examining the persisting effects of adverse early caregiving, have been
important in advancing a growing understanding of how the consequences of child adversity
may carry forward to influence the next generation. This 30-year study of infants of low-
income mothers at-risk for maltreatment has shown that insecure and disorganized
Author Manuscript

attachment is associated with maladaptive social development in childhood, adolescence,


and adulthood. During the study, mother-infant dyads received weekly home visiting
services. These home-visit services targeted four clinical goals: (1) building a trusting
parent-child relationship; (2) increasing the family's ability to access community resources
in order to meet basic needs; (3) modeling and reinforcing positive, reciprocal parent-child
behavior; and (4) decreasing maternal social isolation by facilitating participation in
parenting group activities. Infants (N=64) were enrolled before they turned 9 months and
weekly visits continued until the child reached age 2. By 18 months of age, when attachment
behaviors were measured, families had completed an average of 47 home visits. A group of
nonmaltreating, low-income mothers (N=35) and infants who had never sought or received
social services directed at parenting skills, or undergone extensive psychiatric treatment,
provided a community comparison group. Mothers in this comparison group were individual
Author Manuscript

matched to high-risk mothers based on family income, maternal education level, age, race,
as well as child age, gender, and birth order. Results show that at 18 months, children in the
intervention group were significantly less likely to show disorders of attachment (Lyons-
Ruth, Connell, Grunebaum, & Botein, 1990). Moreover, at age five, teacher and parent
ratings of child behavior problems were assessed. Results showed children who received
home visits had lower teacher-rated hostile-aggressive behavior problems in kindergarten,
particularly when services were delivered weekly for at least one year (Lyons-Ruth &
Melnick, 2007).

IPT is an intervention for adolescents (IPT-A, Mufson, et al., 2004) and adults who have
experienced early adversity and are dealing with depression and interpersonal conflict. IPT
has roots in the Interpersonal Theory of Psychiatry (Sullivan, 1953), as well as attachment
Author Manuscript

theory. As interpreted from an IPT framework, as well as the organizational perspective on


development, relationship problems can give rise to emotional distress, and a lack of positive
relationships also can have a negative effect on mental health and functioning. Informed by
the organizational perspective on development, IPT-A tailors intervention efforts to facilitate
the successful resolution of youth stage-salient tasks, such as establishing close peer and
romantic relationships, and dealing with bereavement. Like CPP, the target of IPT
intervention efforts is a primary problematic relationship. Psychoeducation and interpersonal
skills-building efforts are targeted to address issues within the primary problematic

Clin Psychol (New York). Author manuscript; available in PMC 2018 June 01.
Doyle and Cicchetti Page 12

relationship, with the goal of generalizing to other relational contexts. IPT theorizes that
Author Manuscript

reducing relational conflict and facilitating the development of adaptive social skills will
simultaneously reduce current mental health symptoms and enhance future resilience.

Although IPT is increasingly used for treatment of adolescents and adults who have
experienced maltreatment (see Toth, Gravener-Davis, Guild,& Cicchetti, 2013), no study to
date has explicitly adapted IPT for individuals with histories of institutional care or child
maltreatment; thus, it is in need of further research attention. Existing research comparing
IPT to care as usual has shown IPT to be more efficacious in reducing depressive symptoms
as well as improving relationships with immediate family members for women with histories
of childhood sexual abuse (Gamble et al., 2011). The provision of intervention during the
adolescent period may be critical in preventing further entrenchment of maladaptive
interpersonal patterns resulting from childhood maltreatment. Intervention during this period
is particularly important because adolescence is a transitional turning point in biological,
Author Manuscript

psychological, and social development that presents opportunities and challenges for
adolescents as they embark on resolving stage-salient tasks initiating from close same-sex
friends, developing an identity, and forming romantic relationships.

STAIR-A is a skill-based intervention tailored for adolescents who have experienced chronic
exposure to trauma, including maltreatment, which targets emotion management and
interpersonal skills (e.g., Gudiño, et al., 2016). Results of a recent pilot study shows that
STAIR-A reduces depression symptoms and severity, and increases emotional and
interpersonal coping competencies. Specifically, adolescent girls who participated in STAIR-
A reported significantly improved perceptions of social engagement and locus of control
post-treatment. This intervention approach, along with others, suggests that interpersonal
functioning can operate both as a vulnerability and protective factor for adolescents.
Author Manuscript

In addition to the growing number of intervention studies, two meta-analyses provide further
support that attachment-based interventions focused on promoting maternal sensitivity
and/or attachment security are moderately successful (Bakermans-Kranenburg, van
IJzendoorn, & Juffer, 2003 Bakermans-Kranenburg, van IJzendoorn, & Juffer, 2005). Of
these, interventions focusing on maternal sensitivity and starting after the infant is six
months demonstrate the best success. Interestingly, this finding is in-line with those
previously discussed showing that only PI children adopted after six months old showed
persistent DSED symptoms in adolescence (Rutter et al., 2010). Additionally, findings from
these meta-analyses suggest that intervention studies do not need to explicitly target the
reduction of frightening maternal behavior, such as negative or intrusive verbal or physical
behavior (e.g., mocking or teasing infant, pulling infant by wrist), to succeed in reducing
Author Manuscript

infant disorganization. Notably, Juffer and colleagues (2005) have showed that targeted
changes in maternal sensitivity do not mediate the reduction of disorganized child behavior.
Instead, the authors speculate that the intervention may have indirectly reduced frightening
parenting behavior, perhaps by increasing parents' attention to their child's behavior as well
as the impact of their own behavior on their child.

Clin Psychol (New York). Author manuscript; available in PMC 2018 June 01.
Doyle and Cicchetti Page 13

Discussion: Clinical Implications, Ongoing Debates, and Future Directions


Author Manuscript

A collective, cumulative vision is emerging regarding the processes by which childhood


adversity contributes to relationship outcomes across the lifespan. First, research
demonstrates that relationships with early caregivers don't just support later behavioral
development, they actively shape the architecture of the brain. Accordingly, there is a
growing interest in identifying the neural circuitry that support attachment and social
functioning in close relationships, and characterizing neural differences associated with
typical and inadequate caregiving environments. One open question this research can help
answer is whether the neural circuitry that support attachment behaviors retain plasticity
throughout life (Cicchetti, 2015). Although intervention studies provide some evidence of
this in the form of observed changes in attachment behavior, future research is needed to
provide important empirical support.
Author Manuscript

A second, associated theme is related to how early adverse caregiving environments and
ongoing social difficulties may influence mental and physical health via alterations to
neurobiological regulatory systems. The emerging weathering hypothesis (Geronimus, 2013)
suggests that epigenetic mechanisms, which help regulate gene expression, may play a
significant role in the developmental cascade initiated by early adverse caregiving. This
model predicts that social adversity leads to chronic activation of stress regulatory systems
that in turn increases disease vulnerability over the lifespan via epigenetic mechanisms. This
hypothesis is supported by animal models (Meaney & Szyf, 2005). Additionally, recent
work shows that racial discrimination, an extreme form of social adversity, is associated with
a greater rate of epigenetic wear and tear on the body in young adults, and that supportive
family environments measured in adolescence buffer the effect of discrimination (Brody,
Miller, Yu, Beach, & Chen, 2016). Ehrlich and colleagues (2016) have shown that
Author Manuscript

relationship trajectories characterized by chronic stress are associated with a chronic


proinflammatory response in Caucasian adolescent girls. (Ehrclich, Miller, Rohleder, &
Adam, 2016). Together, these findings emphasize the importance of healthy, supportive
relationships for outcomes across development, and the need for further research examining
the biological mechanisms associated with social adversity.

A third emerging theme is the continuity and discontinuity in attachment constructs.


Relationships are intrinsically longitudinal and complex phenomena, calling for
sophisticated statistical models and methods. As a result, the growing body of attachment
work continues to motivate theoretical approaches and empirical methods that integrate
interpersonal and intrapersonal analyses. For example, Fraley, Roisman, and Haltigan (2013)
recently have used structural model comparisons to demonstrate that the quality of early
Author Manuscript

caregiving has predictive significance that extends from infancy into adulthood in normative
samples. This finding has also been replicated in a higher risk cohort (Raby, Roisman,
Fraley, and Simpson, 2015), which provides strong evidence for central attachment tenants
by demonstrating that caregiving shapes developmental processes that consolidate early in
life and continue to influence developmental pathways over the long-term. Although there is
evidence that these attachment disturbances show modest short- and long-term stability, and
are risk factors for later disruptive behavior and psychopathology (van IJzendoorn et al.,

Clin Psychol (New York). Author manuscript; available in PMC 2018 June 01.
Doyle and Cicchetti Page 14

1999), this modeling technique has yet to be tested with longitudinal data of children who
Author Manuscript

experienced inadequate, extreme, or pathogenic early caregiving environments.

Finally, although neglect, deprivation, violence and maltreatment increase the risk of
maladjustment, many children who experience these forms of adversity go on to have strong,
thriving relationships. This work strongly suggests that a clearer understanding of the
protective factors and processes that predict positive relationship outcomes, despite exposure
to childhood adversity, has significant implications for prevention, intervention, and policy.
Additionally, the clinical implications of this work are that research on resilient functioning
in children who experience early adversity can shed light on the “self-righting” abilities
inherent to development that clinicians and researchers can attempt to foster and facilitate
(Cicchetti et al., 2015).

Conclusion
Author Manuscript

For better or for worse, early childcare experiences can have a pervasive and persistent
influence on a child's development and relationships throughout the lifespan. According to
attachment theory, infants are predisposed to form relationships with caregivers and learn
complex social-emotional information from caregiver interactions. Research shows that
early parent-child experiences shape the child's internal working models that guide the
formation of future relationships. Thus, the nature of a child's early relationships has long-
lasting implications for learning social competence, relationships, and development over
time. Children who experience extreme or pathogenic forms of early caregiving, in the form
of institutional rearing, exposure to domestic or community violence, or maltreatment, are
denied opportunities for normal development normal development of social and emotional
skills needed to cope with future challenges and build future social relationships. These
Author Manuscript

forms of childhood adversity may set in motion a probabilistic developmental pathway,


which may have detrimental effects on relationships throughout the lifespan. Research
shows that these experiences are associated with negative mental representations of self and
other and impaired behavioral, social, and emotional competence. In turn, these deficits can
make it more difficult for children and adolescents to form peer relationships, friendships,
and later adult romantic relationships. In particular, the literature examining long-term
effects of inadequate and pathogenic caregiving environments have identified disorganized
attachment behaviors as significant to sequelae of adverse early caregiving. Disorganized
attachment behaviors stemming from adverse early caregiving and observable in infancy
have been associated with persisting deficits in social competencies and social maladaptation
in childhood, adolescence, and adulthood.

Additionally, early caregiving adversity disrupts a child's rapidly developing brain, with
Author Manuscript

significant implications for biological and behavioral functioning (Boyce, Sokolowski, &
Robinson, 2012). This theme sets the stage for questions related to remediation and recovery
that are beginning to be addressed by intervention efforts. RCT studies shows that children's
emotional self-regulatory and social competency skills are amenable to environmental
support. For example, RAD symptoms exhibited by PI children prior to adoption decrease
once they are removed from conditions of social deprivation and have an opportunity to form
an attachment relationship with adoptive parents (Zeanah et al., 2015). Additionally,

Clin Psychol (New York). Author manuscript; available in PMC 2018 June 01.
Doyle and Cicchetti Page 15

research has shown that CPP, an intervention that targets the dyadic relationship, decreases
Author Manuscript

problematic infant behavior, even at the one-year follow-up point (Toth, Sturge-Apple,
Rogosch, & Cicchetti, 2015). The fact that CPP effects strengthen and grow over time is
testament to the importance of the internal working model as a developmental mechanism
for carrying forward early childcare experiences across development.

Importantly, an organizational perspective on development provides a valuable framework


for research considering how early adverse caregiving experiences may set up probabilistic
parameters for social adaptation/maladaptation across the lifespan. By examining how stage-
salient tasks at each point of development influence subsequent developmental processes
and stage-salient tasks, and attending to the meaning and not merely the occurrence of a
behavior, the organizational perspective on development can provide valuable insights on
mechanisms that may transmit or perpetuate risk, as well as opportunities for prevention and
intervention.
Author Manuscript

Relationships are foundational to the development, learning, and overall well-being of


children, adolescents, and adults. Research examining relationships in at-risk populations
provides an informative and complementary perspective to the examination of social
relationships in a normative development (Cicchetti & Toth, 2015). By increasing the risk
for profound and persisting relationship disadvantages over the lifespan, adverse early
childcare experiences may have cumulative effects on mental and physical health over time.
This body of work demonstrates the importance of safe, stable, and secure early
relationships for children in establishing a foundation of health and well-being across the
lifespan.

Acknowledgments
Author Manuscript

The preparation of this manuscript was supported by grants from the Jacobs Foundation, the National Institute of
Health (grant MH091070) and the Spunk Fund, Inc.

References
Ainsworth, MDS., Blehar, MC., Waters, E., Wall, S. Patterns of attachment: Observations in the
Strange Situation and at home. Hillsdale, NH: Erlbaum; 1978.
American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders: DSM-5.
Washington, D.C.: American Psychiatric Publications; 2013.
Bakermans-Kranenburg MJ, Van IJzendoorn MH, Juffer F. Less is more: meta-analyses of sensitivity
and attachment interventions in early childhood. Psychological Bulletin. 2003; 129(2):195.doi:
10.1037/0033-3909.129.2.195 [PubMed: 12696839]
Bakermans-Kranenburg MJ, Van IJzendoorn MH, Juffer F. Disorganized infant attachment and
preventive interventions: A review and meta-analysis. Infant Mental Health Journal. 2005; 26(3):
Author Manuscript

191–216. DOI: 10.10002/imhj.20046 [PubMed: 28682504]


Bolger KE, Patterson CJ. Developmental pathways from child maltreatment to peer rejection. Child
Development. 2001; 72(2):549–568. DOI: 10.1111/1467-8624.00296 [PubMed: 11333084]
Bowlby, J. Attachment and Loss: Vol 1 Attachment. New York: Basic Books; 1969/1982.
Boyce WT, Sokolowski MB, Robinson GE. Toward a new biology of social adversity. Proceedings of
the National Academy of Sciences. 2012; 109(Supplement 2):17143–17148. DOI: 10.1073/pnas.
1121264109
Brody GH, Miller GE, Yu T, Beach SR, Chen E. Supportive Family Environments Ameliorate the Link
Between Racial Discrimination and Epigenetic Aging A Replication Across Two Longitudinal

Clin Psychol (New York). Author manuscript; available in PMC 2018 June 01.
Doyle and Cicchetti Page 16

Cohorts. Psychological Science. 2016; 27(4):530–541. DOI: 10.1177/0956797615626703 [PubMed:


26917213]
Author Manuscript

Carlson EA, Hostinar CE, Mliner SB, Gunnar MR. The emergence of attachment following early
social deprivation. Development and Psychopathology. 2014; 26(2):479–489. DOI: 10.1017/
S095455794104000078 [PubMed: 24621881]
Cassidy J. Adult romantic attachments: A developmental perspective on individual differences. Review
of General Psychology. 2000; 4(2):111.doi: 10.1037/1089-2680.4.2.111
Cicchetti D. Preventive Intervention Efficacy, Development, and Neural Plasticity. Journal of the
American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry. 2015; 54(2):83–85. DOI: 10.1016/j.jaac.
2014.11.006 [PubMed: 25617248]
Cicchetti D, Rogosch FA, Toth SL. Fostering secure attachment in infants in maltreating families
through preventive interventions. Development and Psychopathology. 2006; 18(03):623–649. DOI:
10.1017/S0954579406060329 [PubMed: 17152394]
Cicchetti, D., Toth, SL. Child Maltreatment. In: Lamb, M., Garcia Coll, C., editors. Handbook of child
psychology and developmental science. New York: Wiley; 2015.
Cyr C, Euser EM, Bakermans-Kranenburg MJ, Van IJzendoorn MH. Attachment security and
Author Manuscript

disorganization in maltreating and high-risk families: A series of meta-analyses. Development and


Psychopathology. 2010; 22(01):87–108. DOI: 10.1017/S0954579409990289 [PubMed: 20102649]
de Wolff MS, IJzendoorn MH. Sensitivity and attachment: A meta - analysis on parental antecedents of
infant attachment. Child Development. 1997; 68(4):571–591. DOI: 10.2307/1132107 [PubMed:
9306636]
Ehrlich KB, Miller GE, Rohleder N, Adam EK. Trajectories of relationship stress and inflammatory
processes in adolescence. Development and Psychopathology. 2016; 28(01):127–138. DOI:
10.1017/S0954579415000334 [PubMed: 25851449]
Felitti VJ, Anda RF, Nordenberg D, Williamson DF, Spitz AM, Edwards V, et al. Marks JS.
Relationship of childhood abuse and household dysfunction to many of the leading causes of death
in adults. The Adverse Childhood Experiences (ACE) Study. American Journal of Preventive
Medicine. 1998; 14:245–258. DOI: 10.1016/S0749-3797(98)00017-8 [PubMed: 9635069]
Fraley RC, Roisman GI, Haltigan JD. The legacy of early experiences in development: Formalizing
alternative models of how early experiences are carried forward over time. Developmental
Psychology. 2013; 49(1):109.doi: 10.1037/a0027852 [PubMed: 22448982]
Author Manuscript

Gamble SA, Smith PN, Poleshuck EL, He H, Talbot NL. Domain-specific social functioning
improvements during treatment of depressed women with histories of childhood sexual abuse.
Journal of Affective Disorders. 2011; 130:478–482. DOI: 10.1016/j.jad.2010.10.016 [PubMed:
21051087]
Geronimus AT. Deep integration: letting the epigenome out of the bottle without losing sight of the
structural origins of population health. American Journal of Public Health. 2013; 103(S1):S56–
S63. DOI: 10.2105/AJPH.2013.301380 [PubMed: 23927509]
George, C., Kaplan, N., Main, M. Unpublished manuscript. 3rd. University of California; Berkeley:
1996. Adult Attachment Interview.
Groh AM, Fearon RP, Bakermans-Kranenburg MJ, Van IJzendoorn MH, Steele RD, Roisman GI. The
significance of attachment security for children's social competence with peers: A meta-analytic
study. Attachment & Human Development. 2014; 16(2):103–136. DOI:
10.1080/14616734.2014.883535 [PubMed: 24547936]
Grossmann, KE.Grossmann, K., Waters, E., editors. Attachment from infancy to adulthood: The major
Author Manuscript

longitudinal studies. New York: Guilford Press; 2006.


Gudiño OG, Leonard S, Cloitre M. STAIR-A for girls: a pilot study of a skills-based group for
traumatized youth in an urban school setting. Journal of Child & Adolescent Trauma. 2016; 9(1):
67–79. DOI: 10.1007/s40653-015-0061-0
Gunnar MR, Quevedo KM. Early care experiences and HPA axis regulation in children: a mechanism
for later trauma vulnerability. Progress in brain research. 2007; 167:137–149. DOI: 10.1016/
S0079-6123(07)67010-1

Clin Psychol (New York). Author manuscript; available in PMC 2018 June 01.
Doyle and Cicchetti Page 17

Hawk B, McCall RB. CBCL behavior problems of post-institutionalized international adoptees.


Clinical Child and Family Psychology Review. 2010; 13(2):199–211. DOI: 10.1007/
Author Manuscript

s105670-010-0068-x [PubMed: 20514520]


Jaffee SR, Caspi A, Moffitt TE, Taylor A. Physical maltreatment victim to antisocial child: Evidence
of an environmentally mediated process. Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 2004; 113(1):44–55.
DOI: 10.1037/0021-843X.113.1.44 [PubMed: 14992656]
Julian MM. Age at adoption from institutional care as a window into the lasting effects of early
experiences. Clinical child and family psychology review. 2013; 16(2):101–145. DOI: 10.1007/
s10567-013-0130-6 [PubMed: 23576122]
Kay C, Green J. Reactive attachment disorder following early maltreatment: Systematic evidence
beyond the institution. Journal of abnormal child psychology. 2013; 41(4):571–581. DOI: 10.1007/
s10802-012-9705-9 [PubMed: 23250477]
Lieberman AF, Ippen CG, Van Horn P. Child-parent psychotherapy: 6-month follow-up of a
randomized controlled trial. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry.
2006; 45(8):913–918. DOI: 10.1097/01.chi.0000222784.03735.92 [PubMed: 16865033]
Lieberman, AF., Van Horn, P. Don't hit my mommy!: A manual for child-parent psychotherapy with
Author Manuscript

young witnesses of family violence. Washington D.C.: Zero to Three Press; 2005.
Mufson, L., Dorta, KP., Moreau, D., Weissman, MM. Interpersonal psychotherapy for depressed
adolescents. New York: Guilford Press; 2011.
Lionetti F, Pastore M, Barone L. Attachment in institutionalized children: A review and meta-analysis.
Child abuse & neglect. 2015; 42:135–145. DOI: 10.1016/j.chiabu.2015.02.013 [PubMed:
25747874]
Lyons-Ruth K, Connell DB, Grunebaum HU, Botein S. Infants at social risk: Maternal depression and
family support services as mediators of infant development and security of attachment. Child
development. 1990; 61(1):85–98. DOI: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.1990.tb02762.x [PubMed: 2307048]
Lyons-Ruth, K., Jacobvitz, D. Attachment disorganization: Genetic factors, parenting contexts, and
developmental transformation from infancy to adulthood. In: Cassidy, J., Shaver, PR., editors.
Handbook of Attachment Theory and Research. 2nd. New York: Guilford Press; 2008. p. 666-697.
Lyons-Ruth K, Melnick S. Dose–response effect of mother–infant clinical home visiting on aggressive
behavior problems in kindergarten. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent
Psychiatry. 2004; 43(6):699–707. DOI: 10.1097/01.chi.0000122730.72597.07 [PubMed:
Author Manuscript

15167086]
MacLean K. The impact of institutionalization on child development. Development and
Psychopathology. 2003; 15(04):853–884. DOI: 10.1017/S0954579403000415 [PubMed:
14984130]
Maughan A, Cicchetti D. Impact of child maltreatment and interadult violence on children's emotion
regulation abilities and socioemotional adjustment. Child Development. 2002; 73(5):1525–1542.
DOI: 10.1111/1467-8624.00488 [PubMed: 12361317]
Meaney MJ, Szyf M. Maternal care as a model for experience-dependent chromatin plasticity? Trends
in neurosciences. 2005; 28(9):456–463. DOI: 10.1016/j.tins.2005/07.006 [PubMed: 16054244]
Mufson, L., Dorta, KP., Moreau, D., Weissman, MM. Interpersonal psychotherapy for depressed
adolescents. New York: Guilford Press; 2011.
Nelson, CA., Fox, NA., Zeanah, CH. Romania's abandoned children: Deprivation, brain development,
and the struggle for recovery. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press; 2014.
Obsuth I, Hennighausen K, Brumariu LE, Lyons-Ruth K. Disorganized behavior in adolescent-parent
Author Manuscript

interaction: relations to attachment state of mind, partner abuse, and psychopathology. Child
Development. 2014; 85(1):370–387. DOI: 10.1111/cdev.12113 [PubMed: 23621826]
Pickreign Stronach E, Toth SL, Rogosch FA, Cicchetti D. Preventive interventions and sustained
attachment security in maltreated children: A 12-month follow-up of a randomized controlled trial.
Development and Psychopathology. 2013; 25(4pt1):919–930. DOI: 10.1017/s0954579413000278
[PubMed: 24229539]
Raby KL, Roisman GI, Fraley RC, Simpson JA. The enduring predictive significance of early maternal
sensitivity: Social and academic competence through age 32 years. Child Development. 2015;
86(3):695–708. DOI: 10.1111/cdev.12325 [PubMed: 25521785]

Clin Psychol (New York). Author manuscript; available in PMC 2018 June 01.
Doyle and Cicchetti Page 18

Riggs SA. Childhood emotional abuse and the attachment system across the life cycle: What theory
and research tell us. Journal of Aggression, Maltreatment & Trauma. 2010; 19(1):5–51. DOI:
Author Manuscript

10.1080/10926770903475968
Rutter M, SonugaBarke EJ, Beckett C, Castle J, Kreppner J, Kumsta R, et al. Gunnar MR.
Deprivation-specific psychological patterns: Effects of institutional deprivation. Monographs of
the Society for Research in Child Development. 2010; 75:1–252. [PubMed: 20500631]
Sameroff AJ, Chandler MJ. Review of child development research. Review of Child Development
Research. 1975; 4
Sherman LJ, Rice K, Cassidy J. Infant capacities related to building internal working models of
attachment figures: A theoretical and empirical review. Developmental Review. 2015; 37:109–141.
DOI: 10.1016/j.dr.2015.06.001
Sigal JJ, Perry JC, Rossignol M, Ouimet MC. Unwanted infants: Psychological and physical
consequences of inadequate orphanage care 50 years later. American Journal of Orthopsychiatry.
2003; 73(1):3–12. DOI: 10.1037/0002-9432.73.1.3 [PubMed: 12674514]
Sroufe, LA., Egeland, B., Carlson, EA., Collins, WA. The development of the person: The Minnesota
study of risk and adaptation from birth to adulthood. New York, NY: Guilford Press; 2005.
Author Manuscript

Sullivan, HS. The interpersonal theory of psychiatry. New York: W W Norton & Co; 1953.
Tieman W, van der Ende J, Verhulst FC. Psychiatric disorders in young adult intercountry adoptees:
An epidemiological study. American Journal of Psychiatry. 2005; 162(3):592–598. DOI: 10.1176/
appi.ajp.162.3.592 [PubMed: 15741478]
Toth SL, Maughan A, Manly JT, Spagnola M, Cicchetti D. The relative efficacy of two interventions in
altering maltreated preschool children's representational models: Implications for attachment
theory. Development and Psychopathology. 2002; 14(4):877–908. DOI: 10.1017/
S095457940200411X [PubMed: 12549708]
Toth SL, Rogosch FA, Manly JT, Cicchetti D. The efficacy of toddler-parent psychotherapy to
reorganize attachment in the young offspring of mothers with major depressive disorder: A
randomized preventive trial. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology. 2006; 74(6):1006–
1016. DOI: 10.1037/0022-006X.74.6.1006 [PubMed: 17154731]
Trickett PK, Noll JG, Putnam FW. The impact of sexual abuse on female development: Lessons from a
multigenerational, longitudinal research study. Development and Psychopathology. 2011; 23:453–
476. doi:org/10.1017/S0954579411000174. [PubMed: 23786689]
Author Manuscript

Umberson D, Thomeer MB, Williams K, Thomas PA, Liu H. Childhood adversity and men's
relationships in adulthood: Life course processes and racial disadvantage. The Journals of
Gerontology Series B: Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences. 2015; doi: 10.1093/geronb/
gbv091
Van den Dries L, Juffer F, van IJzendoorn MH, Bakermans-Kranenburg MJ. Fostering security? A
meta-analysis of attachment in adopted children. Children and Youth Services Review. 2009;
31(3):410–421. doi:10/1016/j.childyouth.2008.09.008.
van IJzendoorn MH, Palacios J, Sonuga-Barke EJ, Gunnar MR, Vorria P, McCall RB, Juffer F.
Children in institutional care: Delayed development and resilience. Monographs of the Society for
Research in Child Development. 2011; 76(4):8–30. DOI: 10.111/j.1540-5834.2011.00626.x
[PubMed: 25125707]
Van IJzendoorn MH, Schuengel C, Bakermans-Kranenburg MJ. Disorganized attachment in early
childhood: Meta-analysis of precursors, concomitants, and sequelae. Development and
Psychopathology. 1999; 11(02):225–250. doi:0.1017/S0954579499002035. [PubMed: 16506532]
Author Manuscript

Waters E, Merrick S, Treboux D, Crowell J, Albersheim L. Attachment security in infancy and early
adulthood: A twenty-year longitudinal study. Child development. 2000; 71(3):684–689. DOI:
10.1111/1467-8624.00176 [PubMed: 10953934]
Weiner A, Kupermintz H. Facing adulthood alone: The long-term impact of family break-up and infant
institutions, a longitudinal study. British Journal of Social Work. 2001; 31(2):213–234. DOI:
10.1093/bjsw/31.2.213
Westhues A, Cohen JS. A comparison of the adjustment of adolescent and young adult inter-country
adoptees and their siblings. International Journal of Behavioral Development. 1997; 20(1):47–65.
DOI: 10.1080/016502597385432

Clin Psychol (New York). Author manuscript; available in PMC 2018 June 01.
Doyle and Cicchetti Page 19

Zeanah CH, Gleason MM. Annual Research Review: Attachment disorders in early childhood–clinical
presentation, causes, correlates, and treatment. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry. 2015;
Author Manuscript

56(3):207–222. DOI: 10.1111/jcpp.12347 [PubMed: 25359236]


Zeanah CH, Gunnar MR, McCall RB, Kreppner JM, Fox NA. Sensitive periods. Monographs of the
Society for Research in Child Development. 2011; 76(4):147–162. DOI: 10.1111/j.
1540-5834.2011.00631.x [PubMed: 25125708]
Author Manuscript
Author Manuscript
Author Manuscript

Clin Psychol (New York). Author manuscript; available in PMC 2018 June 01.
Doyle and Cicchetti Page 20
Author Manuscript
Author Manuscript

Figure 1.
Conceptual model depicting the organizational perspective on social development in the
context of adverse early caregiving. This perspective suggests that failure of the caregiving
environment to support healthy development around stage-salient attachment tasks may lead
to deficits in social-emotional competence in early childhood that increase the likelihood for
difficulties and deficits in subsequent stage-salient tasks across the lifespan. Although
longitudinal studies provide support for this cumulative, hierarchical depiction of social
maladaptation following early adversity, intervention studies targeting child attachment
behaviors or mental representations directly or indirectly also suggest that social-emotional
Author Manuscript

development can be reorganized at different stages in development, allowing for


opportunities of recovery.
Author Manuscript

Clin Psychol (New York). Author manuscript; available in PMC 2018 June 01.

You might also like