You are on page 1of 617

The SAGE Handbook of

Nonverbal
Communication

Valerie Manusov
The SAGE
Handbook of
Nonverbal
Communication
FM-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 5:25 PM Page ii
The SAGE
Handbook of
Nonverbal
Communication

Editors
Valerie Manusov R Miles L. Patterson
University of Washington University of Missouri, St. Louis
FM-Manusov.qxd 7/13/2006 4:44 PM Page iv

Copyright © 2006 by Sage Publications, Inc.

All rights reserved. No part of this book may be reproduced or utilized in any form or by any
means, electronic or mechanical, including photocopying, recording, or by any information storage
and retrieval system, without permission in writing from the publisher.

For information:

Sage Publications, Inc.


2455 Teller Road
Thousand Oaks, California 91320
E-mail: order@sagepub.com

Sage Publications Ltd.


1 Oliver’s Yard
55 City Road
London EC1Y 1SP
United Kingdom

Sage Publications India Pvt. Ltd.


B-42, Panchsheel Enclave
Post Box 4109
New Delhi 110 017 India

Printed in the United States of America

Library of Congress Cataloging-in-Publication Data

The SAGE handbook of nonverbal communication / [edited by] Valerie Manusov, Miles L. Patterson.
p. cm.
Includes bibliographical references and index.
ISBN 1-4129-0404-8 (cloth)
1. Nonverbal communication. I. Title: Handbook of nonverbal communication.
II. Manusov, Valerie Lynn. III. Patterson, Miles L.
BF637.N66S24 2006
302.2′22—dc22
2006004826

This book is printed on acid-free paper.

06 07 08 09 10 10 9 8 7 6 5 4 3 2 1

Acquiring Editor: Todd R. Armstrong


Editorial Assistant: Camille Herrera
Project Editor: Astrid Virding
Copyeditor: Quads/Linda Gray
Typesetter: C&M Digitals (P) Ltd.
Indexer: Juniee Oneida
Cover Designer: Edgar Abarca
FM-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 8:00 PM Page v

CONTENTS

Preface xi
Valerie Manusov and Miles L. Patterson

Acknowledgments ix

Introduction: The Ubiquity and Social Meaningfulness


of Nonverbal Communication xv
Howard Giles and Beth A. Le Poire

PART I: FOUNDATIONS
1. An Historical Overview of Nonverbal Research 3
Mark L. Knapp

2. The Evolution of Theories of Interactive Behavior 21


Miles L. Patterson

3. Methods for the Study of Nonverbal


Communication 41
Heather M. Gray and Nalini Ambady

4. Automatic Cognitive Processes and


Nonverbal Communication 59
Jessica L. Lakin

5. Nonverbal Skills and Abilities 79


Ronald E. Riggio
FM-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 5:25 PM Page vi

6. Nonverbal and Verbal Communication:


Hand Gestures and Facial Displays as Part of
Language Use in Face-to-face Dialogue 97
Janet Beavin Bavelas and Nicole Chovil

PART II: FACTORS OF INFLUENCE


7. The Biological Foundations of Social
Organization: The Dynamic Emergence of Social
Structure Through Nonverbal Communication 119
Ross Buck and Stacie Renfro Powers

8. An Evolutionary Approach to
Understanding Nonverbal Communication 139
Kory Floyd

9. Personality and Nonverbal Behavior:


A Complex Conundrum 159
Robert Gifford

10. Factoring in Age: Nonverbal


Communication Across the Life Span 181
Robert S. Feldman and James M. Tyler

11. Women’s and Men’s Nonverbal Communication:


Similarities, Differences, Stereotypes, and Origins 201
Judith A. Hall

12. Culture and Nonverbal Behavior 219


David Matsumoto

13. Casting Nonverbal Behavior in the Media:


Representations and Responses 237
Valerie Manusov and Adam Jaworski

PART III: FUNCTIONS


14. Nonverbal Behavior in Intimate Interactions
and Intimate Relationships 259
Peter A. Andersen, Laura K. Guerrero,
and Susanne M. Jones

15. Nonverbal Expressions of Dominance


and Power in Human Relationships 279
Judee K. Burgoon and Norah E. Dunbar

16. The Functions of Facial Expressions:


What’s in a Face? 299
Alan J. Fridlund and James A. Russell
FM-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 5:25 PM Page vii

17. Why and How the Silent Self Speaks


Volumes: Functional Approaches to
Nonverbal Impression Management 321
Caroline F. Keating

18. Nonverbal Communication and Deception 341


Aldert Vrij

19. The Interaction Management Function of


Nonverbal Cues: Theory and Research About
Mutual Behavioral Influence in Face-to-Face Settings 361
Joseph N. Cappella and Darren M. Schreiber

20. Nonverbal Behavior and Its Functions in


the Ecosystem of Rapport 381
Linda Tickle-Degnen

PART IV: CONTEXTS AND CONSEQUENCES


21. Nonverbal Communication in
Close Relationships 403
Patricia Noller

22. Nonverbal Communication in


Instructional Contexts 421
James C. McCroskey, Virginia P. Richmond,
and Linda L. McCroskey

23. Nonverbal Communication and Physician-


Patient Interaction: Review and New Directions 437
Jeffrey D. Robinson

24. Nonverbal Dynamics in Computer-Mediated


Communication or :( and the Net :( ’s with You,
:) and You :) Alone 461
Joseph B. Walther

25. Nonverbal Communication, Race,


and Intergroup Interaction 481
John F. Dovidio, Michelle Hebl, Jennifer A.
Richeson, and J. Nicole Shelton

26. Uses and Consequences of Nonverbal


Communication in the Context of Organizational Life 501
Martin S. Remland
FM-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 5:25 PM Page viii

PART V: FINAL THOUGHTS


Nonverbal Communication: Basic Issues and Future Prospects 522
Miles L. Patterson and Valerie Manusov

Author Index 533

Subject Index 559

About the Editors 575

About the Contributors 577


FM-Manusov.qxd 7/10/2006 10:43 AM Page ix

ACKNOWLEDGMENTS

T his volume would not exist if not for the encouragement and
direction of Todd Armstrong, Editor from Sage Publications.
Along with help from Deya Saoud and Camille Herrera, we had an easy
time bringing the The SAGE Handbook of Nonverbal Communication
into being. We are grateful for the careful copy editing work of Linda
Gray and to the always diligent Astrid Virding for guiding the manuscript
through production. We also thank our wonderful authors for their
extensive work on these chapters. Authors provided several drafts of
their chapters, responding wonderfully to our often demanding feed­
back. Because there is such demand for their expertise in contributing
to a wide variety of publications, we appreciate their efforts even more.
Valerie and Miles would like to thank our respective departments—
Communication at the University of Washington and Psychology at the
University of Missouri, St. Louis—for their support throughout this
process. More important, we would like to thank our respective fami­
lies for their constant support and patience: Chuck and Cameron
McSween and Dianne and Kevin Patterson.
SAGE Publications gratefully thanks the following reviewers: Mark
L. Knapp, Jesse H. Jones Centennial Professor in Communication and
UT Distinguished Teaching Professor, University of Texas at Austin;
Susanne M. Jones, University of Minnesota, Twin Cities; Ross Buck,
University of Connecticut; and Peter A. Andersen, San Diego State
University.

◆ ix
FM-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 5:25 PM Page x
FM-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 5:25 PM Page xi

PREFACE

� Valerie Manusov
University of Washington

� Miles L. Patterson
University of Missouri, St. Louis

P utting together any Handbook is daunting. The challenge of cre­


ating an edited volume is enhanced further when the research area
is interdisciplinary, as is the study of nonverbal cues. We decided that
the focus should be on communication and not the entire spectrum of
research concerned with nonverbal behavior. Over the years, scholars
have argued over where to draw the line between communicative and
noncommunicative events. In some cases, this led to a very restricted
view of communication, including only those behaviors that were sent
intentionally and had a consistent meaning, at least within a particular
culture. Our preference is to assume a broader definition of nonverbal
communication, encompassing the sending and receiving of informa­
tion through appearance, objects, the environment, and behavior in
social settings.
Choosing topics and authors to represent the breadth of the field
and, at the same time, provide a discriminating analysis of research and
theory, presented another challenge. There was a large range of exper­
tise and issues that merited consideration. Although not fully inclusive,
we were fortunate in enlisting scholars who have devoted much of

◆ xi
FM-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 5:25 PM Page xii

xii–––◆–––The SAGE Handbook of Nonverbal Communication

their academic life to understanding better communication works and how it has been
the processes involved in the give-and-take studied. Our second section, “factors of
of nonverbal communication. This helped influence,” brings together work on the
us in our goal for the Handbook to provide myriad forces that help shape our use of
a path to understanding the subtleties of nonverbal communication. This section
our social interactions and our relation­ emphasizes the importance of biology
ships with one another. The chapters in the (Buck & Renfro Powers), evolution
Handbook emphasize the primacy of non­ (Floyd), personality (Gifford), age (Feldman
verbal channels in facilitating interpersonal & Tyler), sex and gender (Hall), culture
contact and regulating our social worlds. (Matsumoto), and the media (Manusov &
Unlike other Handbooks, the current vol­ Jaworski). Each of these chapters argues for
ume’s chapters are not meant to be exhaus­ the ways in which the particular factors
tive of the research in the area. Rather, work to shape the practice and meaning of
authors were given the charge of making an nonverbal communication.
argument for what is important in their The third section of the handbook,
respective areas. Thus, for example, Fridlund “functions,” follows the premise that non­
and Russell call for a move away from think­ verbal communication serves a variety of
ing about emotions as the primary function different purposes. That is, nonverbal com­
of facial displays. Robinson, in his chapter munication facilitates short-term and long-
on physician-patient nonverbal interaction term ends in our social world. These
argues for the importance of a situated, functions include sending relational mes­
focused microanalytic assessment of the cues sages of intimacy (Andersen, Guerrero,
that occur in such interactions. Walther & Jones) and dominance (Burgoon &
argues that computer- mediated communica­ Dunbar), expressing intentions and, to a
tion is not devoid of nonverbal cues as is lesser degree, emotions (Fridlund &
often asserted but, rather, that chronemics Russell), creating and managing impressions
have always been a source of message value (Keating), deceiving others or helping us
for people communicating online. As readers detect deception (Vrij), regulating interac­
work through this Handbook, they will see a tion (Cappella & Schreiber), and building
range of expertise and perspective that and reflecting rapport (Tickle-Degnen).
reflects the amazing sophistication of current These chapters discuss the complexity of
scholarship on nonverbal communication. these communicative functions and suggest
To organize the large and diverse set the importance of nonverbal cues for the
of arguments about nonverbal communica­ communication of fundamental human
tion, we placed the chapters into four endeavors.
primary categories. This first section, An awareness of the importance of non­
“foundations,” provides an array of issues verbal cues is reflected again in our fourth
that underlie all conceptualizations of and section, “contexts and consequences.” In
research into nonverbal communication. this set of chapters, the authors work to
Specifically, chapters include the broad his­ reveal the ways in which particular contexts
tory of nonverbal communication (Knapp), shape and make salient certain nonverbal
parallel processes in nonverbal commu­ processes. They also discuss the very real
nication (Patterson), methods (Gray & implications of nonverbal behavior within
Ambady), cognitive bases (Lakin), skills these contexts. The contexts we have
(Riggio), and coordination with language focused on for this Handbook are close rela­
(Bavelas & Chovil). These issues are essen­ tionships (Noller), education (McCroskey,
tial for understanding how nonverbal Richmond, & McCroskey), physician-patient
FM-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 5:25 PM Page xiii

Preface–––◆–––xiii

interaction (Robinson), computer-mediated processes such as emotional expression over


communication (Walther), groups (Dovidio, others—even more apparent. Second, read­
Hebl, Richeson, & Shelton), and organiza­ ers will see that the chapters cross-reference
tions (Remland). The implications and one another, showing—sometimes despite
importance of nonverbal cues are also made areas of difference—the important connec­
clear in Giles and Le Poire’s engaging tion between many of the lines of research
Introduction. highlighted in this volume. They also reflect
Although we have worked to organize just how far research on nonverbal cues and
this Handbook into a larger frame, two processes has come.
caveats are important to note. First, reading Ours is an interdisciplinary field, creat­
across chapters shows a range of places ing opportunities to see the myriad factors
where debate exists in the research commu­ involved in nonverbal communication and
nity about the best ways to conceptualize sometimes adding blinders to what we
and measure certain nonverbal phenomena. choose to investigate. It is our hope that the
In our final chapter, Patterson and current Handbook encourages the former
Manusov work to make these debates— and discourages the latter, working to
about the role of learning and inheritance, develop a full and integrated set of future
about the nature and “privilege” of certain nonverbal scholarship.
FM-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 5:25 PM Page xiv
FM-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 5:25 PM Page xv

INTRODUCTION
The Ubiquity and Social Meaningfulness
of Nonverbal Communication

� Howard Giles
University of California, Santa Barbara

� Beth A. Le Poire
California Lutheran University

N onverbal communication—in all its impressive manifestations—


is central to the communication process by being “an inherent
and essential part of message creation (production) and interpretation
(processing)” (Burgoon, 1994, p. 239). It also generates enormous inter­
est among academics and the general public. Evidence of this interest
comes in myriad forms and practices that people find instrumental to
their daily lives. Our goal in this prologue is to excite readers’ under­
standings about the ubiquity of nonverbal communication and the ways
it is developed from our very earliest of days to its instrumental role in
maintaining lifelong partnerships (see Driver & Gottman, 2004, for a
larger discussion of nonverbal communication in long-term relation­
ships; Feldman & Tyler, this volume, for a discussion of nonverbal com­
munication across the lifespan). Moreover, as an introductory chapter
to a larger set of issues including the foundations, functions, contexts,
and consequences of nonverbal cues, we explore the particular influence

◆ xv
FM-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 5:25 PM Page xvi

xvi–––◆–––The SAGE Handbook of Nonverbal Communication

nonverbal communication has on impor­ outcomes significantly (van Swol, 2003).


tant health, developmental, and marital So important is the interaction of nonver­
outcomes. bal communication with verbal communi­
cation that numerous scholars caution
against separating nonverbal and verbal
♦ The Ubiquity of communication for analysis (e.g., Cappella
Nonverbal Communication & Street, 1985; see also Bavelas & Chovil,
this volume).
Furthermore, nonverbal communication
That nonverbal communication is integral is central to socially meaningful outcomes
to communicative interactions is indis­ of communication interactions across all
putable. Indeed, when verbal and nonverbal relationship types. In addition to the
cues are incongruent, Burgoon (1994) con­ processes mentioned previously, nonverbal
tends that individuals often accord greater cues can affect deception detection and its
credence to the latter than to the former outcomes (e.g., Forrest & Feldman, 2000;
features (see also Remland, this volume). Vrij, this volume), conflict management
Her argument is based on ample evidence. (e.g., Beaumont & Wagner, 2004), the
For example, Argyle, Salter, Nicholson, communication of stigma (e.g., Le Poire,
Williams, and Burgess (1970) created video­ 1994), information transmission (e.g.,
tapes of a performer reflecting superior, Frick-Horbury, 2002), and interactional
neutral, or inferior attitudes toward partici­ management (e.g., Jones, Gallois, Callan,
pants in their psychological experiments. & Barker, 1999; see Cappella & Schreiber,
This presentation of attitudes was accom­ this volume). Nonverbal communication is
plished in a design where verbal and non­ also central to the establishment, mainte­
verbal were congruent or incongruent with nance, and dissolution of relationships (see
each other. In one of the conditions, a supe­ Noller, this volume). Nonverbal communi­
rior statement was accompanied by nervous cation is valued as so important—and
smiling, a lowered head, and a tone of voice beyond the scope of our quest here—as to
expressing eagerness to please. The authors garner the attention of scholars in thou­
found the nonverbal cues were three to four sands of studies across communication, lin­
times more important in attributing superi­ guistics, sociology, psychology, psychiatry,
ority-inferiority to the actor than were the education, biology, physiology, and anthro­
verbal statements. pology. Many of these studies are refer­
Repeating the study, but this time using enced across this Handbook’s chapters.
a friend-hostile dimensions, nonverbal cues
were found to be six times more important
than what was said verbally (Argyle, ♦ The Social Impact of
Alkema, & Gilmour, 1971). This finding
Nonverbal Communication
may indicate that nonverbal communica­
tion is especially important when relational
messages are of paramount importance. Perhaps most important for the present
Even in interactions where verbal commu­ volume, nonverbal communication can be
nication accounts for more of the variance valued for the very real impacts it has in
in meaning acquisition (e.g., persuasion, day-to-day living. One area where this can
information transmission), nonverbal com­ be seen regards health. Nonverbal commu­
munication still contributes to communicative nication affects health outcomes in formal
FM-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 5:25 PM Page xvii

Introduction–––◆–––xvii

health care settings, and it can also be seen 2002). Additionally, because nonverbal
to have an effect in interpersonal relation­ behavior deficits are central to autism, non­
ships. Specifically, parents can affect mental verbal behaviors are often used as an early
and physical health outcomes in their diagnostic tool for detecting this condition
children through their nonverbal behavior in children (Bristol-Power & Spinella,
(e.g., Miller, Benson, & Galbraith, 2001). 1999). Partners’ use of nonverbal commu­
In addition, marital partners can also affect nication has also been linked to better
each other’s physical and mental health recidivism outcomes for substance abusers
through their nonverbal interaction behav­ (e.g., Le Poire, Dailey, & Duggan, 2002;
iors (e.g., Kiecolt-Glaser, McGuire, Robles, Le Poire, Duggan, & Dailey, 2001).
& Glaser, 2002). Nonverbal communication from care­
In addition to health, however, we focus givers toward their children has also been
in this section on nonverbal communication associated with better health outcomes for
and development, making the argument children. Specifically, as up to half of all
that nonverbal communication from parents are not likely to talk directly with
parents to children and teachers to children their children about sex (Jaccard, Dittus, &
also affects the development of children Gordon, 2000), it is reassuring to realize
physically, socially, and intellectually (see that parenting style can reduce the inci­
also, Feldman & Tyler, this volume). Also, dence of sexually risky behavior. A sum­
we look at some of the ample research mary of more than 20 studies indicates that
focusing on the role of nonverbal commu­ parent-child closeness (which is enhanced
nication in close relationships, centering on through nonverbal expressions of warmth)
the impact of nonverbal communication is associated with reduced adolescent preg­
with relational satisfaction. Finally, whereas nancy risk through sexual abstinence, post­
these important outcomes are all housed ponement of intercourse, having fewer
within important relationships, nonverbal sexual partners, and using contraception
communication can have important ramifi­ consistently (see Miller et al., 2001, for a
cations in nonpersonal relationships as well review). Thus, authoritative parents seem
(see also Dovidio, Hebl, Richeson, & to provide the right mix of nonverbal
Shelton, this volume; Remland, this vol­ warmth and parental control necessary to
ume). We focus here primarily on the ways provide their children with the tools neces­
in which nonverbal cues affect people’s sary to reduce their risk of pregnancy,
judgments of one another. regardless of whether or not they are talk­
ing directly with their adolescent children
about sex and the potential outcomes of
HEALTH OUTCOMES risky sex.
This relationship of greater warmth
The role of nonverbal communication in and better health outcomes holds for sub­
health care has been studied extensively stance use as well. For instance, adoles­
within physician-patient interactions (e.g., cents’ substance use is associated with both
Aruguete & Roberts, 2002; Rosenthal, family affection and parental control
2002; see Robinson, this volume), and (Hall, Henggeler, Ferreira, & East, 1992).
physical health outcomes of older patients Consistently, moderate amounts of parental
have been linked to the nonverbal com­ control and parental support are related to
munication of physical therapists (e.g., decreased illicit drug use, whereas higher
Ambady, Koo, Rosenthal, & Winograd, amounts of control and support were both
FM-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 5:25 PM Page xviii

xviii–––◆–––The SAGE Handbook of Nonverbal Communication

predictive of decreased alcohol use (Stice, on vocalics and touch as a basis for forming
Barrera, & Chassin, 1993). These results secure attachments to caregivers. Specifically,
provide evidence for the contention that the infants’ sensitivities to vocal cues appear to
nonverbal communication of warmth, in be rooted in the need for security in that
combination with higher amounts ofcon­ infants perceive up and down glides in pitch
trol, is associated with better health out­ as providing important information about
comes among adolescents. affect and security (Papousek, Bornstein,
Nonverbal expressions of negative emo­ Nuzzo, Papousek, & Symmes, 1990). Soon
tions within marriages may actually be thereafter, infants use vocalizations to estab­
related to mental and physical health out­ lish communication with their caregivers. By
comes as well. Supportive nonverbal and 8 to 12 weeks, they can coo, and by around
verbal communication is one of the resources 6 months, infants can babble (Oller, 1986).
associated with close personal relationships. These vocalizations seem to be aimed specif­
Such supportive communication diminishes ically at others. Infants actually vocalize
the expression of negative emotions and more when their parents are around than
enhances health in part through its positive when they are alone (Masataka, 1993).
impact on immune and endocrine regulation Moreover, when parents respond to these
(e.g., Kiecolt-Glaser et al., 2002). If spouses vocalizations, infants engage in even more
evoke greater amounts of negative emotional vocalizations (Legerstee, 1991).
expression, their actions can stimulate In addition, infants’ distress vocaliza­
immune dysregulation, which may be one of tions occur in the first 4 to 5 months (Stark,
the core mechanisms underlying conditions Rose, & McLagen, 1975) and are highly
such as cardiovascular disease, osteroporo­ potent and arousing signals for caregivers
sis, arthritis, Type 2 diabetes, and certain (Van Egeren & Barratt, 2004). In this way,
cancers (Kiecolt-Glaser et al., 2002). distress cries may be the most adaptive
Unhappy marriages, then, may be rife with form of communication that infants pos­
negative emotional expression, which can sess. Work comparing mothers with
explain the poorer mental and physical fathers, women with men, and parents with
health of unhappily married individuals. nonparents show that, across groups,
Supportive communication may facilitate people can interpret the earliest distress
greater physical health through its ability to cries correctly (Papousek, 1989). Mothers
ameliorate negative emotions. Alternatively, are better at distinguishing distress cries
less supportive communication and the for food as opposed to those of discom­
resulting negative emotions may actually be fort, however (Stallings, Fleming, Corter,
a detriment to physical health. Worthman, & Steiner, 2001). In general,
parents (regardless of culture) respond to
distress cries by holding, rocking/bouncing,
NONVERBAL COMMUNICATION singing, or talking in melodic rhythms
AND DEVELOPMENTAL (Keller et al., 1996; Papousek & Papousek,
OUTCOMES 1991). If the child is out of a parent’s reach
momentarily, the parent will begin rapid-
In addition to specific health outcomes, fire, high-pitched verbalizations with a
the influence of nonverbal communication pitch that falls by the end (Papousek,
between parents and children also reaches to Papousek, & Bornstein, 1985).
better developmental outcomes for children. Besides vocalizations, infants rely on
For instance, infants rely almost exclusively other forms of nonverbal communication.
FM-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 5:25 PM Page xix

Introduction–––◆–––xix

Increased gaze between mothers and infants Louiselle, Misukanis, & Mueller, 1988)
is related to more frequent vocalizations by and self-actualization (Dominguez &
both, but especially by mothers (Stevenson, Carton, 1997).
Ver Hoeve, Roach, & Leavitt, 1986). Children are likewise influenced by the
Because infants often avoid eye contact nonverbal communication of their teachers
when they are distressed, mothers of dis­ (see McCroskey, Richmond, & McCroskey,
tressed infants usually attempt to reestablish this volume). In a now famous study,
eye contact through increased touch, smil­ Rosenthal and Jacobsen (1968) compared
ing, and social play (Beebe & Stern, 1977). the expectancy effects of teachers who had
In addition, infants who use gestures more been given high expectations for students to
frequently may actually acquire language teachers who had not been afforded them.
more quickly (Van Egeren & Barratt, 2004). In this study, and the 400 or so follow-up
Likewise, children who use their hands to studies (for a review, see Rosenthal &
point relatively early (some as early as 3 Rubin, 1978), teachers communicated their
months) use more gestures overall and have expectancies to students in the classrooms
better speech comprehension (Butterworth through a whole host of nonverbal and ver­
& Morissette, 1996) than children who bal behaviors. Most relevant to nonverbal
point later in their development. communication, however, teachers commu­
Although we would caution against nicated in ways that foster a more positive
elevating nonverbal communication to pri­ climate with students for whom they have
mary or even causal status in what follows, high expectations. In a follow-up meta­
nonverbal cues are, arguably, central to analytic study examining interpersonal
the disciplinary styles that parents enact. expectancy effects in the classroom across
Specifically, parents who exhibit greater 31 meta-analyses, Harris and Rosenthal
warmth and responsivity toward their (1985) found significant effects for nonver­
children (in combination with moderate to bal behaviors communicating expectancies.
high control attempts) have children who Among them, eye contact, wait time, ges­
generally exhibit higher achievement (e.g., tures, distance, smiles, duration of interac­
authoritative parenting; Baumrind, 1996). tions, and speech rate were all significant in
This communicative manifestation of predicting positive outcomes. Of the non­
warmth is composed primarily of nonver­ verbal behaviors included in the analyses on
bal signals (see Andersen, Guerrero, & which Harris and Rosenthal’s analysis was
Jones, this volume). Furthermore, nonver­ based, only touch and lean were not related
bal warmth is related to better outcomes to outcomes.
in children. In one study, for example,
children of authoritative parents (i.e., those
showing high warmth and high demanding­ MARITAL NONVERBAL
ness) were the most cognitively motivated, PROCESSES AND OUTCOMES
competent, and achievement oriented
(Baumrind, 1991). In another study, they Nonverbal communication does not,
were also the most intrinsically motivated however, relate only to the successful devel­
(Ginsburg & Bronstein, 1993). Further, opment of children. The valence of nonver­
they attained the highest math and verbal bal communication has also been associated
achievement (Baumrind, 1991). Parents with marital satisfaction and marital stability.
with authoritative styles of parenting also Gottman and Levinson (1992) argue, based
had children with higher self-esteem (Buri, on numerous behavioral investigations, that
FM-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 5:25 PM Page xx

xx–––◆–––The SAGE Handbook of Nonverbal Communication

successful marriages have a 5:1 ratio in toward a partner and can often include psy­
terms of positive to negative behaviors. chological abuse and intentional insults.
They found that couples who displayed Nonverbal indicators of contempt include
more positivity than negativity when they eye rolling, disgust facial expressions,
spoke to each other were more satisfied, less sneers, and hostile humor. It is likely that
likely to have thought about divorce, and the intentional use of insults within conflict
less likely to have actually separated. This in marital relationships will result in dis­
ratio is especially important to marriages in tress as hurtful messages from romantic
which one or both partners are distressed partners and other family members elicit
(i.e., less satisfied). Individuals in distressed greater negative feelings than those from
relationships tend to display more negative other people (Vangelisti & Crumley, 1998).
and less positive affect, and they are more Such hurt, as part of a larger pattern of neg­
likely to reciprocate negative affect (e.g., ative communication, can be destructive to
Noller, 1984, this volume). Negative behav­ family relationships in that negative com­
iors may be the most predictive of marital munication (e.g., attacking the other, defen­
satisfaction (e.g., Gottman & Levinson, siveness, crying, ignoring the message),
1986; Huston & Vangelisti, 1991), with greater distancing behavior, and lower
negative behaviors being more predictive of relationship satisfaction can ensue (e.g.,
marital satisfaction than positive ones (e.g., Vangelisti, 1994, 2001; Vangelisti &
Broderick & O’Leary, 1986). This is the Crumley, 1998; Vangelisti & Young,
case, despite the fact that happier partners 2000). Stonewalling, on the other hand,
display more positive behaviors than do includes responding to an onslaught of neg­
their unhappier counterparts (e.g., Cutrona, ative affect with withdrawal including flat
1996). facial affect. Its use implies that the issue is
Negative nonverbal communication not worth addressing (strategically or not)
behaviors may also predict divorce. Gottman and is not worthy of an emotional response.
(1994) argues that the consistent use of Gottman (1994) notes that men are more
what he calls the “Four Horsemen of the likely to respond by stonewalling, because
Apocalypse” can portend the destruction of they tend to be more physiologically reac­
the marriage and tends to mark distressed tive during conflict and thus may feel more
couples (i.e., those with low amounts of of an intense pressure to withdraw from
marital satisfaction and high amounts of conflict situations (Gottman & Levinson,
marital instability). Gottman and his 1988). Stonewalling is one mechanism used
colleagues found that couples in distress to withdraw.
display greater expressions of criticism, In tandem with this work is research on
contempt, defensiveness, and stonewalling demand-withdrawal patterns during mari­
consistently. Whereas all these displays tal conflict (e.g., Caughlin & Vangelisti,
include combinations of verbal and non­ 1999). A large amount of research illustrates
verbal behavior, contempt and stonewalling, that when wives want changes in a relation­
in particular, are typically potent and nega­ ship, they are likely to make demands that
tive nonverbal messages. According to are followed by the husbands’ tendency to
Gottman, contempt and stonewalling, withdraw verbally and nonverbally (e.g.,
including their nonverbal manifestations, Baucom, Notarius, Burnett, & Haefner,
are particularly predictive of divorce. 1990; Christensen & Shenk, 1991;
To be more specific, contempt includes Gottman & Levinson, 1988). This pattern
expressions of extremely negative affect of conflict is destructive in that dissatisfied
FM-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 5:25 PM Page xxi

Introduction–––◆–––xxi

marriages often evidence the demand- responses to others who do not nonverbally
withdraw pattern of conflict, and these position themselves “correctly” when the
marriages frequently end in divorce (e.g., national anthem is being performed when
Heavey, Christensen, & Malamuth, 1995; pledging allegiance to the flag.
Noller, Feeney, Bonnell, & Callan, 1994; Immediacy displays may also reflect
Schaap, Buunk, & Kerkstra, 1988). One of social distance and avoidance (Burgoon &
the reasons why these marriages may end is Hale, 1984). For example, backward lean
that this particular destructive conflict pat­ and frowning, are inferentially rich to the
tern is not easily alterable (e.g., Jacobson, extent they are mined by us to attribute the
Follette, & Pagle, 1986). actor as likeable, respected, committed,
We have shown some means through credible, persuasive, dynamic, and so on
which nonverbal behaviors are an integral (e.g., see Zhou et al., 2002). So important is
component of the communication and the use of nonverbal immediacy to convey
meaning acquisition processes through the closeness (or distance) that expressions of
child-rearing process on to marital dynam­ intimacy and closeness differentiate between
ics and even divorce. These highly personal marriages that are simply enduring and
communication situations highlight some those that are enduring and the ideal happy
of the many meaningful ways in which non­ marriage (e.g., Cuber & Haroff, 1965;
verbal communication influences personal Gottman & Levinson, 1988). Attributional
relationship and developmental outcomes. work like this can be just as easily invoked
Nonverbal communication can also be watching soap operas or TV news inter­
highly influential across more nonpersonal views as it is in face-to-face interactions (see
relationships, however, and it is to this Manusov & Jaworski, this volume).
impact that we now turn our attention. Nonverbal cues also have their optimal
levels and latitudes of acceptance, however,
and, as such, can be overaccommodated as
THE NATURE OF NONVERBAL in the case where some older people become
COMMUNICATION IN NON­ the unwanted recipients of patronizing talk
INTERPERSONAL RELATIONSHIPS through exaggerated intonations, oversmil­
ing, and slowed-down speech rates (Ryan,
One of the most frequently studied para­ Hummert, & Boich, 1995). Being the recip­
meters of nonverbal communication (as we ient of such communications with patroniz­
have just discussed and as can be seen ers representing different people in different
throughout this Handbook) is immediacy, contexts over time is not easily discountable.
defined as those communication behaviors, It can eventually imply a debilitating mes­
some visual others vocal, that “enhance sage for the receiver that he or she is cogni­
closeness to and nonverbal interaction tively and communicatively now much older
with another” (Mehrabian, 1969, p. 213). and “past it.” In other words, not only can
Signaling (consciously or unconsciously) nonverbal cues engender social support and
feelings of, or the intent to become, rela­ relational harmony, they can also be part
tionally closer to another comes in various and parcel of the social construction of
guises and sizes, including gaze and eye aging and even demise (Giles, 1999).
contact, smiling, forward body, and vocal The social meanings of these and other
warmth (Andersen, 1979). Such stances can immediacy cues (e.g., the timing of events,
also be processed vis-à-vis inanimate touch, proximity, body movements) vary in
objects, as witnessed in some people’s their interpretive potential rather dramatically
FM-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 5:25 PM Page xxii

xxii–––◆–––The SAGE Handbook of Nonverbal Communication

between cultures (Burgoon, 1995; see also, The use of spectacles or even sunglasses in
Matsumoto, this volume) and often appear some contexts, watches, designer-labeled
as essential ingredients of cross-cultural clothing, jewelry, and men’s gold neck
training programs and etiquette books. chains; the plethora of possible hairstyles
Indeed, the absence of such cues, such as (buzz-cuts, braids, bob); and a ball-cap
a lack of nonverbal expressiveness or turned backward all have their unique social
neutrality, can influence important social meanings and, again, sometimes stimulate
decisions by teachers, medical personnel, law passionate reactions, deep-seated emotions,
enforcement, and so forth. In his discussion and intense comment from third parties.
of the performative nature of preaching, for Facial rings, lipstick colors, eye makeup, hair­
example, Robinson (1980) remarked that “a styles, body size, gang graffiti and gang signs,
pastor’s words may insist, ‘This is impor­ and cosmetics are but a few of the myriad
tant,’ but if his voice sounds flat and expres­ ways we can send messages out (again more
sionless and his body stands limp, the or less strategically) about our ideologies,
congregation will not believe him” (cited in aspirations, and group memberships, be they
Mikkelson & Floyd, 2005, p. 194). Often, ethnic, sexual orientation, political, or what­
reactions to the presence or the absence of ever (see Harwood & Giles, 2005).
nonverbal cues are stereotypic, leading to Given the recent fascination with physical
inaccurate presumptions and even irrespon­ beauty as evidenced by current American
sible actions. television programs focusing on cosmetic
There are, of course, many other non­ surgery (e.g., the Swan, Makeover), and the
verbal cues including accent that people use current emphasis on physical beauty and
to categorize others into social groups, youth evidenced by such trends as Botox and
often triggering allied trait attributions and breast augmentation, it is not that surprising
their associated affect (Giles & Billings, that nonverbal communication emphasizing
2004). Indeed, not a day goes by for one of physical attraction has received a great
us (HG) when his non-North American amount of research attention. Finally, the
accent is not commented upon by strangers. use of other possessions transported or in the
Speech rate, pitch, timing, rhythm, and workplace and home—certain kinds of cell
intonation are (paralinguistic) nonverbal phones; books, newspapers, or magazines
cues people can manipulate to manage read and or displayed; suitcase style; music
impressions or in forming impressions and movies played; new technological arti­
of others (Pittam, 2000; see also Keating, facts; cars; residence location and architec­
this volume). Paramount among these are ture; remodels; decorations and artwork;
historically sensitive dress styles and fash­ and so on—can communicate status and
ion, which sometimes elicit evaluative com­ trait attributions (cultured, pretentious, life­
ments among (and subsequent affirmation span crisis, macho, cheap, etc.). They can
from) older males about certain young also influence encounters, even those with
women’s dress modes, such as “I’m glad I strangers, through the type of messages they
don’t have a daughter these days!” Wearing convey.
a tie, jacket, or suit can signal formality and
professionalism in some work and leisure
contexts yet be construed as irreverently
♦ Conclusions
Western in some Muslim contexts. Further­
more, being under- or overdressed can Nonverbal communication is of consider­
cause observational concern or lament. able consequence in many aspects of
FM-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 5:25 PM Page xxiii

Introduction–––◆–––xxiii

social life. Numerous investigations place communicators, and the ways in which the
nonverbal communication as central to context influences nonverbal meaning and
meaning acquisition (Birdwhistell, 1970), process and creates relevant, and often
especially in communication interactions problematic, consequences, which a more
where relational communication (Burgoon complete understanding of nonverbal com­
& Le Poire, 1999), emotional expressions munication can help to address.
(Boone & Buck, 2003), and impression
management (Xin, 2004) are concerned.
Moreover, nonverbal communication can ♦ References
be influential in the relationships surround­
ing health care, and they can also have a
significant impact on important familial Ambady, N., Koo, J., Rosenthal, R., &
relationships. Children and their nurturers Winograd, C. H. (2002). Physical thera­
display nonverbal behavior that facilitates pists’ nonverbal communication predicts
the safety and development of infants, geriatric patients’ health outcomes.
Psychology and Aging, 17, 443–452.
children, and adolescents. The use of non­
Andersen, J. F. (1979). Teacher immediacy as a
verbal warmth (in combination with mod­
predictor of teaching effectiveness. In D.
erate to high control attempts), for Nimmo (Ed.), Communication yearbook 3
example, appears to ensure better health (pp. 543–559). New Brunswick, NJ:
outcomes for adolescents by reducing risky Prentice Hall.
sexual behavior and substance use and Argyle, M., Alkema, F., & Gilmour, R. (1971).
abuse. In addition, teachers’ use of nonver­ The communication of friendly and hostile
bal behavior communicates important attitudes by verbal and nonverbal signals.
expectancies to students and affects the European Journal of Social Psychology, 2,
student’s performance on a variety of acad­ 385–402.
emic indicators. Moreover, nonverbal com­ Argyle, M., Salter, V., Nicholson, H., Williams,
munication facilitates the “health” of M., & Burgess, P. (1970). The communica­
tion of inferior and superior attitudes by
marriages as well in that more positive
verbal and nonverbal signals. British
nonverbal communication and less negative
Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology,
destructive nonverbal communication 9, 222–231.
(e.g., contempt, stonewalling, withdrawal, Aruguete, M. S., & Roberts, C. A. (2002).
expression of negative emotions) predict Participants’ ratings of male physicians who
greater marital stability and satisfaction vary in race and communication style.
and even better physical health outcomes. Psychological Reports, 91, 793–806.
Clearly, nonverbal communication func­ Baucom, D. H., Notarius, C. I., Burnett, C. K.,
tions in important socially meaningful ways & Haefner, P. (1990). Gender differences
across a variety of relationships. With this and sex-role identity in marriage. In F. D.
backdrop accessed, we trust readers will Fincham & T. N. Bradbury (Eds.), The psy­
have a more informed springboard with chology of marriage (pp. 150–171). New
York: Guilford.
which to access specific chapters and the
Baumrind, D. (1991). The influence of parenting
various theoretical models discussed in
style on adolescent competence and sub­
them. Along with the Handbook’s editors, stance use. Journal of Early Adolescence,
we hope that readers will see the complexi­ 11, 56–95.
ties of this communication system, the many Baumrind, D. (1996). Parenting: The discipline
foundational issues into which it is embed­ controversy revisited. Family Relations, 45,
ded, the myriad functions that it serves for 405–414.
FM-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 5:25 PM Page xxiv

xxiv–––◆–––The SAGE Handbook of Nonverbal Communication

Beaumont, S. L., & Wagner, S. L. (2004). authoritarianism and authoritativeness


Adolescent-parent verbal conflict: The roles on self-esteem. Personality and Social
of conversational styles and disgust emo­ Psychology Bulletin, 14, 271–282.
tions. Journal of Language and Social Butterworth, G., & Morissette, P. (1996). Onset
Psychology, 23, 338–368. of pointing and the acquisition of language
Beebe, B., & Stern, D. N. (1977). Engagement- in infancy. Journal of Reproductive and
disengagement and early object experi­ Infant Psychology, 14, 219–231.
ences. In N. Freedman & S. Granel (Eds.), Cappella, J. N., & Street, R. L., Jr. (1985).
Communicative structures and psychic Introduction: A functional approach to the
structures (pp. 35–55). New York: Plenum structure of communication behavior. In
Press. R. L. Street Jr. & J. N. Cappella (Eds.),
Birdwhistell, R. L. (1970). Kinesics and context. Sequence and pattern in communicative
Philadelphia: University of Pennsylvania behavior (pp. 1–29). London: Edward
Press. Arnold.
Boone, R. T., & Buck, R. (2003). Emotional Caughlin, J. P., & Vangelisti, A. L. (1999).
expressivity and trustworthiness: The role Desire for change in one’s partner as pre­
of nonverbal behavior in the evolution dictor of the demand/withdraw pattern
of cooperation. Journal of Nonverbal of marital communication. Communication
Behavior, 27, 163–182. Monographs, 66, 66–89.
Bristol-Power, M. M., & Spinella, G. (1999). Christensen, A., & Shenk, J. L. (1991).
Research on screening and diagnosis in Communication, conflict, and psychologi­
autism: A work in progress. Journal of cal distance in nondistressed, clinic, and
Autism and Developmental Disorders, 29, divorcing couples. Journal of Consulting
435–438. and Clinical Psychology, 59, 458–463.
Broderick, J. E., & O’Leary, K. D. (1986). Cuber, J. F., & Haroff, P. (1965). Sex and the
Contributions to affect, attitudes, and significant Americans. Baltimore: Penguin.
behavior to marital satisfaction. Journal of Cutrona, C. E. (1996). Social support in couples.
Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 54, Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
514–517. Dominguez, M. M., & Carton, J. S. (1997). The
Burgoon, J. K. (1994). Nonverbal signals. In M. relationship between self-actualization and
L. Knapp & G. R. Miller (Eds.), Handbook parenting style. Journal of Social Behavior
of interpersonal communication (2nd ed., and Personality, 12, 1093–1100.
pp. 229–285). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. Driver, J. L., & Gottman, J. M. (2004). Daily
Burgoon, J. K. (1995). Cross-cultural and marital interactions and positive affect dur­
intercultural applications of expectancy ing marital conflict among newlywed cou­
violations theory. In R. L. Wiseman ples. Family Process, 43, 301–314.
(Ed.), Intercultural communication theory Forrest, J. A., & Feldman, R. S. (2000).
(pp. 194–215). Thousand Oaks: Sage. Detecting deception and judge’s involve­
Burgoon, J. K., & Hale, J. L. (1984). The fun­ ment: Lower task involvement leads to
damental topoi of relational communica­ better lie detection. Personality and Social
tion. Communication Monographs, 51, Psychology Bulletin, 26, 118–125.
193–214. Frick-Horbury, D. (2002). The effects of hand
Burgoon, J. K., & Le Poire, B. A. (1999). gestures on verbal recall as a function of
Nonverbal cues and interpersonal judg­ high- and low-verbal-skill levels. Journal of
ments: Participant and observer perceptions General Psychology, 129, 137–147.
of intimacy, dominance, composure, and Giles, H. (1999). Managing dilemmas in the
formality. Communication Monographs, “silent revolution”: A call to arms! Journal
66, 105–124. of Communication, 49, 170–182.
Buri, J. R., Louiselle, P. A., Misukanis, T. M., & Giles, H., & Billings, A. (2004). Language atti­
Mueller, R. A. (1988). Effects of parental tudes. In A. Davies & E. Elder (Eds.),
FM-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 5:25 PM Page xxv

Introduction–––◆–––xxv

Handbook of applied linguistics Jacobson, N. S., Follette, W. C., & Pagel, M.


(pp. 187–209). Oxford, UK: Blackwell. (1986). Predicting who will benefit from
Ginsburg, G., & Bronstein, P. (1993). Family behavioral marital therapy. Journal of
factors related to children’s intrinsic/extrin­ Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 54,
sic motivational orientation and academic 518–522.
performance. Child Development, 64, Jones, E., Gallois, C., Callan, V., & Barker, M.
1461–1471. (1999). Strategies of accommodation:
Gottman, J. M. (1994). What predicts divorce? Development of a coding system for conver­
The relationship between marital processes sational interaction. Journal of Language
and marital outcomes. Hillsdale, NJ: and Social Interaction, 18, 123–152.
Lawrence Erlbaum. Keller, H., Chasiotis, A., Risau Peters, J.,
Gottman, J. M., & Levinson, R. W. (1986). Voelker, S., Zach, U., & Restemeier, R.
Assessing the role of emotion in marriage. (1996). Psychobiological aspects of infant
Behavioral Assessment, 8, 31–48. crying. Early Development and Parenting,
Gottman, J. M., & Levinson, R. W. (1988). The 5, 1–13.
social psychophysiology of marriage. In Kiecolt-Glaser, J. K., McGuire, L., Robles, T. F., &
P. Noller & M. A. Fitzpatrick (Eds.), Glaser, R. (2002). Psychoneuroimmunology:
Perspectives on marital interaction (pp. Psychological influences on immune function
182–200). Philadelphia: Multilingual Matters. and health. Journal of Consulting and Clinical
Gottman, J. M., & Levinson, R. W. (1992). Psychology, 70, 537–547.
Marital processes predictive of later disso­ Legerstee, M. (1991). Changes in the quality of
lution: Behavior, physiology, and health. infant sounds as a function of social and
Journal of Personality and Social nonsocial stimulation. First Language, 11,
Psychology, 63, 221–233. 327–343.
Hall, J. A., Henggeler, S. W., Ferreira, D. K., & Le Poire, B. A. (1994). Attraction toward and
East, P. L. (1992). Sibling relations and sub­ nonverbal stigmatization of gays and
stance use in high-risk female adolescents. persons with AIDS: Evidence of instru­
Family Dynamics of Addiction Quarterly, mentally symbolic attitudinal structures.
2, 44–51. Human Communication Research, 21,
Harris, M. J., & Rosenthal, R. (1985). 241–279.
Mediation of interpersonal expectancy Le Poire, B. A., Dailey, R., & Duggan, A. (2002,
effects: 31 meta-analyses. Psychological November). Nonverbal reinforcement and
Bulletin, 97, 363–386. punishment of substance abuse: A conver­
Harwood, J., & Giles, H. (2005). Intergroup sational test of INC. Paper presented to the
communication: Multiple perspectives. National Communication Association,
Berlin: Peter Lang. New Orleans, LA.
Heavey, C. L., Christensen, A., & Malamuth, N. Le Poire, B. A., Duggan, A., & Dailey, R. (2001,
M. (1995). The longitudinal impact of November). The influence of nonverbal
demand and withdrawal during marital communication on continued substance
conflict. Journal of Consulting and Clinical abuse. Paper presented to the National
Psychology, 63, 797–801. Communication Association, Atlanta, GA.
Huston, T. L., & Vangelisti, A. L. (1991). Masataka, N. (1993). Effects of contingent and
Socioemotional behavior and satisfaction in noncontingent maternal stimulation on
marital relationships. Journal of Personality the vocal behaviour of three- to four­
and Social Psychology, 61, 721–733. month-old Japanese infants. Journal of
Jaccard, J., Dittus, P. J., & Gordon, V. V. Child Language, 20, 303–312.
(2000). Parent-teen communication about Mehrabian, A. (1969). Some referents and mea­
premarital sex: Factors associated with the sures of nonverbal behavior. Behavioral
extent of communication. Journal of Research Methods and Instruments, 1,
Adolescent Research, 15, 187–208. 213–217.
FM-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 5:25 PM Page xxvi

xxvi–––◆–––The SAGE Handbook of Nonverbal Communication

Mikkelson, A. C., & Floyd, K. (2005, February). Rosenthal, R., & Jacobsen, L. (1968).
Effective preaching: How nonverbal imme­ Pygmalion in the classroom. New York:
diacy influences motivation, affective learn­ Holt, Rinehart, & Winston.
ing, and perceptions of credibility. Paper Rosenthal, R., & Rubin, D. B. (1978). Interpersonal
presented at the Western States Com­ expectancy effects: The first 345 studies.
munication Association Annual Meeting, Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 3, 377–386.
San Francisco. Ryan, E. B., Hummert, M. L., & Boich, L.
Miller, B. C., Benson, B., & Galbraith, K. A. (1995). Communication predicaments of
(2001). Family relationships and adolescent aging: Patronizing behavior toward older
pregnancy risk: A research synthesis. adults. Journal of Language and Social
Developmental Review, 21, 1–38. Psychology, 14, 144–166.
Noller, P. (1984). Nonverbal communication and Schaap, C., Buunk, B., & Kerkstra, A. (1988).
marital interaction. Oxford, UK: Penguin. Marital conflict resolution. In P. Noller
Noller, P., Feeney, J. A., Bonnell, D., & Callan, & M. A. Fitzpatrick (Eds.), Perspectives
V. (1994). A longitudinal study of conflict on marital interaction (pp. 245–270).
in early marriage. Journal of Social and Clevedon, UK: Multilingual Matters.
Personal Relationships, 11, 233–253. Stallings, J., Fleming, A. S., Corter, C.,
Oller, D. K. (1986). Metaphonology and infant Worthman, C., & Steiner, M. (2001). The
vocalizations. In R. A. B. Lindblom (Ed.), effects of infant cries and odors on sympa­
Precursors of early speech (pp. 21–35). thy, cortisol, and autonomic responses in
New York: Stockton. new mothers and nonpostpartum women.
Papousek, M. (1989). Determinants of respon­ Parenting: Science and Practice, 1, 71–100.
siveness to infant vocal expression of Stark, R. E., Rose, S. N., & McLagen, M.
emotional state. Infant Behavior and (1975). Features of infant sounds: The first
Development, 12, 507–524. eight weeks of life. Journal of Child
Papousek, M., Bornstein, M. H., Nuzzo, C., Language, 2, 205–221.
Papousek, H., & Symmes, D. (1990). Infant Stevenson, M. B., Ver Hoeve, J. N., Roach, M.
responses to prototypical melodic contours A., & Leavitt, L. A. (1986). The beginning
in parental speech. Infant Behavior and of conversation: Early patterns of mother-
Development, 13, 539–545. infant vocal responsiveness. Infant Behavior
Papousek, M., & Papousek, H. (1991). Early ver­ and Development, 9, 423–440.
balizations as precursors of language devel­ Stice, E., Barrera, M., & Chassin, L. (1993).
opment. In M. E. Lamb & H. Keller (Eds.), Relation of parental support and control to
Infant development: Perspectives from adolescents’ externalizing symptomatology
German speaking countries (pp. 299–328). and substance use: A longitudinal examina­
Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum. tion of curvilinear effects. Journal of
Papousek, M., Papousek, H., & Bornstein, M. Abnormal Child Psychology, 21, 609–629.
H. (1985). The naturalistic vocal environ­ Van Egeren, L. A., & Barratt, M. S. (2004). The
ment of young infants: On the significance developmental origins of communication:
of homogeneity and variability in parent Interactional systems in infancy. In A.
speech. In T. Field & N. Fox (Eds.), Social Vangelisti (Ed.), Handbook of family com­
perception in infants (pp. 269–297). munication (pp. 287–310). Mahwah, NJ:
Norwood, NJ: Ablex. Lawrence Erlbaum.
Pittam, J. (2000). Voice and social identity. van Swol, L. M. (2003). The effects of nonverbal
Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. mirroring on perceived persuasiveness, agree­
Robinson, H. (1980). Biblical preaching: The ment with an imitator, and reciprocity in a
development and delivery of expository group discussion. Communication Research,
messages. Grand Rapids, MI: Baker Books. 30, 461–480.
Rosenthal, R. (2002). Covert communication in Vangelisti, A. L. (1994). Messages that hurt. In
classrooms, clinics, courtrooms, and cubi­ W. R Cupach & B. H. Spitzberg (Eds.), The
cles. American Psychologist, 57, 839–849. dark side of interpersonal communication
FM-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 5:25 PM Page xxvii

Introduction–––◆–––xxvii

(pp. 53–82). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Journal of Social and Personal Relation­
Erlbaum. ships, 17, 393–424.
Vangelisti, A. L. (2001). Making sense of hurtful Xin, K. R. (2004). Asian American managers:
interactions in close relationships: When An impression gap?: An investigation of
hurt feelings create distance. In V. Manusov impression management and supervisor-
& J. H. Harvey (Eds.), Attributions, com­ subordinate relationships. Journal of
munication behavior, and close relation­ Applied Behavioral Science, 40, 160–181.
ships (pp. 38–58). Cambridge, UK: Zhou, Q., Eisenberg, N., Losoya, S. H., Fabes,
Cambridge University Press. R. A., Reiser, M., Guthrie, I. K., Murphy,
Vangelisti, A. L., & Crumley, L. P. (1998). B. C., Cumberland, A. J., & Shepard, S. A.
Reactions to messages that hurt: The influ­ (2002). The relations of parental warmth
ence of relational contexts. Communication and positive expressiveness to children’s
Monographs, 65, 173–196. empathy-related responding and social
Vangelisti, A. L., & Young, S. L. (2000). When functioning: A longitudinal study. Child
words hurt: The effects of perceived Development, 73, 893–915.
intentionality on interpersonal relationships.
FM-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 5:25 PM Page xxviii
01-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 3:58 PM Page 1

PART I

FOUNDATIONS
01-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 3:58 PM Page 2
01-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 3:58 PM Page 3

1
AN HISTORICAL OVERVIEW
OF NONVERBAL RESEARCH

� Mark L. Knapp
University of Texas at Austin

One faces the future with one’s past.


—Pearl S. Buck

L ong before there were written records, painters, pottery makers,


sculptors, dancers, philosophers, leaders, and others, whose work
necessitated an understanding of how and why human beings use (or
should use) their bodies and with what effect, “researched” nonverbal
communication. Walking styles, gestures, forms of handshaking, and
other nonverbal behavior practiced centuries ago can be pieced together
by examining a variety of written documents, including diaries, plays,
histories, folklore, legal codes, and traveler’s accounts (Bremmer &
Roodenburg, 1991). In this chapter, I focus only on those written works
or scholarly movements that have had widespread influence and/or key
issues associated with nonverbal studies that serve as important links in
a chain leading to the modern study of nonverbal behavior and non­
verbal communication.

◆ 3
01-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 3:58 PM Page 4

4–––◆–––Foundations

♦ Rhetoric and the understanding of a speaker’s gestures,


Delivery Canon posture, and voice are central to an under­
standing of their effectiveness. Aristotle and
other Greek rhetoricians thought of rhetoric
The words of the philosopher Confucius as having five canons: invention, arrange­
(1951/ca. 500 BCE), who expounded on ment, style, memory, and delivery (Kennedy,
the ways to lead a good life, aesthetics, 1963). In offering these canons, Aristotle
politics, etiquette, and other topics more (1991/ca. 350 BCE) established the impor­
than 2,500 years ago, have been preserved. tance of the nonverbal behaviors involved
In these records, we find his commentary in delivering a speech, saying that some­
on communicating without words: times delivery exerts more influence than
the substance of the speech.
1. He said: I’d like to do without words. But it was the Roman orators and
2. Tze-Kung said: But, boss, if you don’t teachers who refined, clarified, categorized,
say it, how can we little guys pass it on? and expanded on the behaviors involved in
speech delivery (Kennedy, 1972). Cicero
3. He said: Sky, how does that talk? The
(1942/ca. 55 BCE) established a connection
four seasons go on, everything gets
between nonverbal behavior and emotion:
born. Sky, what words does the sky
“For nature has assigned to every emotion
use? (p. 87, Book 17, XIX).
a particular look and tone of voice and
Confucius also made observations repeat­ bearing of its own” (Section 216). He also
edly on what, today, we would refer to as addressed the relative influence of various
the coordination of verbal and nonverbal channels when he said, “In the matter of
signals: for example, “3. There are three delivery which we are now considering the
things a gentleman honors in his way of face is next in importance to the voice; and
life: . . . that his facial expression come near the eyes are the dominant feature of the
to corresponding with what he says” (p. 35, face” (Section 222). And like Confucius,
Book 8, IV), and Cicero underlined the need for congruence
of verbal and nonverbal signals: “For by
1. He said: elaborate phrases and action the body talks, so it is all the more
expression to fit [insinuating, pious necessary to make it agree with the
appearance] self-satisfied deference; Tso thought” (Section 222).
Ch’iuming was ashamed of; I also am Another Roman rhetorician, whose
ashamed of ’em. To conceal resentment influence on later rhetorical scholars
while shaking hands in a friendly manner, extended hundreds of years after his death,
Tso-Ch’iuming was ashamed to; I also am was Quintilian (1922/90 CE). Quintilian,
ashamed to. (p. 22–23, Book 5, XXIV) like those before him, argued for the “har­
mony” of speech, gesture, and face, but
At about the same time Confucius was he justified his recommendation by saying
proffering his advice in China, philosophers that incongruent signals will “not only lack
and scholars in Athens and Sicily initiated weight, but will fail to carry conviction”
the publication of the first handbooks on (chap. III, 67). He also discussed the role
oral rhetoric. The study of oral rhetoric or of an orator’s dress (chap. III, 137ff) and
persuasive speaking is an important tribu­ made an attempt to classify gestures into
tary of nonverbal knowledge because an two broad categories:
01-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 3:58 PM Page 5

An Historical Overview of Nonverbal Research–––◆–––5

The gestures of which I have thus far normally with important advances in rhetor­
spoken are such as naturally proceed ical thought. But the elocutionists were
from us simultaneously with our words. keenly interested in body movements and
But there are others which indicate things vocalizations, and they developed detailed
by means of mimicry. For example, you lists of the many ways a body and voice can
may suggest a sick man by mimicking be used to deliver written speeches and liter­
the gesture of a doctor feeling the pulse, ary works. In his effort to improve British
or a harpist by a movement of the hands education, Irish actor Thomas Sheridan
as though they were plucking the strings. sought to restore the stature of delivery in
(chap. III, 88) rhetorical study. His published lectures on
the subject from 1756 to 1762 discuss
An early attempt to ascribe meaning to
various nonverbal signals is also found in what is now standard speech-text
Quintilian’s Institutio Oratoria. In this text, material on oral interpretation, vocal
for example, throwing the head back was expressiveness, and gestures. Words,
a behavior that was believed to assist in Sheridan argues, are not the only con­
expressing arrogance and the eyebrows stituent of language. Expressions and
were depicted as showing “anger by con­ gestures also communicate. Indeed, they
traction . . . cheerfulness by expansion” are more primitive than words, more
(chap. III, 69, 80). natural where words are artificial, more
Attention to nonverbal behavior associ­ universal where words are national, and
ated typically with the delivery canon virtu­ more expressive of emotion than the
ally disappeared during the Middle Ages sophisticated language of words. (Bizzell
and the Renaissance when a concern for & Herzberg, 1990, p. 650)
style predominated. There was, however,
a brief treatment of delivery in Thomas Gilbert Austin (1753–1837), another
Wilson’s The Arte of Rhetorique in 1553. well-known elocutionist, developed an
In this book, Wilson discusses “how to elaborate notation system with symbols for
speak in a pleasing tone, how to gesture recording voice speed, force, variety, paus­
well, and how to pronounce correctly” ing, over 50 foot movements, over 100 arm
(cited in Bizzell & Herzberg, 1990, p. 587). positions, and thousands of hand positions,
In the mid-to-late 1600s, both Wilkins and not to mention arm elevation and motion,
Fenelon complained about the quality of among other behaviors (Bizzell & Herzberg,
pulpit oratory, whereas “others offered 1990). His illustrations from Chironomia
advice on delivery for preachers and (1806) are still reproduced widely (see
lawyers, with discussions of acting, facial Figure 1.1). The exceedingly detailed nota­
expression, posture, movement, gesture, tions represent a significant early contribu­
projection, tone, pace, and modulation” tion toward recording and analyzing
(Bizzell & Herzberg, 1990, p. 649). nonverbal behavior. But Austin’s approach
But it was the elocution movement was not widely accepted as an effective
that comprised the next major milestone method for learning how to perform while
linking nonverbal behavior and the rhetori­ delivering a speech because of the stilted and
cal canon of delivery. This movement, which unnatural behavior it effected.
began around 1750 and extended into Elocutionist François Delsarte (1811–
the early 20th century, is not associated 1871) also created a system of oratory that
01-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 3:58 PM Page 6

6–––◆–––Foundations

Figure 1.1 Illustrations from Austin’s Chironomia

detailed body movements (Shawn, 1954). ♦ The 19th Century:


Delsarte’s observation about shoulders is Two Influential Works
less noteworthy for its accuracy than it is
for the implication that nonverbal behavior
has a multimeaning potential: Contemporary scholarship focusing on
gestures and facial expressions owes a
Now the shoulder is limited, in its proper huge debt to two works published in the
domain, to proving, first, that the emo­ 19th century: de Jorio’s (1832) Gestural
tion expressed by the face is or is not Expression of the Ancients in the Light
true. Then, afterward, to marking, of Neapolitan Gesturing and Darwin’s
with mathematical rigor, the degree of (1872/1998) The Expression of the
intensity to which the emotion rises. Emotions in Man and Animals. Kendon
(Delaumosne, 1893, pp. 438–439) (1982b, 2004), who has researched thor­
oughly the development of gestural study
Gradually, however, the formal and throughout history, says this about de Jorio’s
detailed instruction of the elocutionists gave work: “It remains one of the most complex
way to a less formal and research-based treatises on the subject ever published and
approach to delivery in 20th-century public it is the first ever to present a study of ges­
speaking texts. Speech delivery in modern ture from what today would be called an
public speaking textbooks is girded with ethnographic point of view” (Kendon,
theories, findings, and citations from 2000, p. xx).
scholars whose work is viewed as central De Jorio believed that his analysis of the
to the study of nonverbal communica­ everyday gestural behavior of ordinary
tion (Gronbeck, McKerrow, Ehninger, & people in Naples would help archeologists
Monroe, 1990). and others to better understand the gestures
01-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 3:58 PM Page 7

An Historical Overview of Nonverbal Research–––◆–––7

depicted on Greco-Roman vases, frescos, deception at length, contemporary efforts


and sculptures that were being discovered. by scholars to study nonverbal expressive
In the process, however, he provided much behavior during deception are quite com­
more. Among other contributions, Kendon patible with his ideas (Ekman, 2001).
credits de Jorio for (1) establishing the
importance of context in understanding ges­
tures (e.g., the same gesture may take on a ♦ The Early 20th Century
different meaning with variations in accom­
panying facial and bodily activity); (2) iden­
tifying ways that gestures function with At least four key developments during the
words and as substitutes for words; (3) first half of the 20th century set the stage for
treating gesture for the first time “as if it is what was to be the blossoming of nonverbal
a culturally established communicative code studies at midcentury. First, there was a
analogous to language” (Kendon, 2000, growing interest in human interaction and
p. xx); and (4) an approach to making a communication by prominent scholars from
gesture dictionary that even today shows many disciplines (Delia, 1987). Symbolic
more promise than many extant efforts. interactionists (e.g., George Herbert Mead),
Charles Darwin, like de Jorio, was researchers interested in group dynamics
extremely skilled at amassing, describing, (e.g., Kurt Lewin, Elton Mayo), propa­
and interpreting a wealth of observations. ganda (e.g., Harold Lasswell), cybernetics
Ekman (1998a) said, “Almost everyone now (e.g., Norbert Wiener), and information
studying the facial expressions of emotion theory (e.g., Claude Shannon and Warren
acknowledges that the field began with Weaver) all took different approaches to
Darwin’s Expression” (p. xxviii). One hun­ communication, but together they made
dred years after Darwin’s observations from social interaction a key to understanding
largely anecdotal data, scholars using more social life at all levels. This not only opened
systematic and rigorous research methods the door for the examination of myriad
confirmed the validity of many of his obser­ behaviors affecting human transactions but
vations about the facial expressions of emo­ also made understanding of human interac­
tion in adult human beings, infants, and other tion an important and respectable area for
animals (Ekman, 1973; Ekman & Friesen, disciplines, such as sociology, psychology,
1971; Ekman, Friesen, & Ellsworth, 1972). anthropology, linguistics, psychiatry, ethol­
Darwin was one of the first to study ogy, and speech.
facial expressions in the context of evolu­ This focus on human interaction occurred
tionary principles and one of the first to use at a time when the concern for more scien­
photographs to illustrate expressions (see tific approaches to social issues was also
Floyd, this volume). His idea that there gaining strength. Thus, nonverbal research
was a pan-cultural morphology for certain during this period is characterized by the use
expressions stimulated controversy among of more sophisticated and precise proce­
scholars, but it also stimulated research that dures for studying and recording behavior,
enabled us to understand better what the use of film, and an increasingly innova­
aspects of an expression are common to our tive variety of methods. According to
species, the morphology of facial expres­ DePaulo and Friedman (1998), the experi­
sions, and how certain aspects of facial mental study of nonverbal behavior by psy­
expressions of emotion can be modified chologists grew rapidly during this period
by cultural teachings (Ekman, 1998b). And and by the 1920s “a very active group
whereas he did not discuss the issue of of researchers was studying spontaneous
01-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 3:58 PM Page 8

8–––◆–––Foundations

versus posed facial expression of emotion, people live and the cultural context with
vocal expression, and gestures” (p. 6). which they identify. This idea was credited
Anthropologist Eliot Chapple (1940, 1949), with rebutting Hitler and others who wanted
for example, began using a mechanical to persecute people for styles of body move­
device, called an interaction chronograph, ment and gesture that were believed to be
which produced an ongoing graphic record inferior and unchangeable because of their
of who talked, when, and for how long. race, culture, or ethnic group. It also served,
Among other uses, data from this device however, to delay the search for behaviors
provided an early glimpse at response that may be common to human beings
matching and interaction synchrony. throughout the world (Ekman, 1998b).
In 1930, anthropologist Franz Boas was Franz Boas, Efron’s advisor and mentor to
perhaps the first social scientist to use the many of the anthropologists who made sem­
motion picture camera to generate data in inal contributions to the study of nonverbal
natural settings with the goal of studying behavior in the 1950s, went so far as to say
human gestures, motor habits, and dance. this in the preface to Efron’s (1941) book:
Davis (1979) attributes the first microscopic “The environment has such a fundamental
frame-by-frame analysis of filmed movement influence that in larger groups, particularly
patterns to Halverson (1931), a child psy­ in sub-divisions of the White race, the genetic
chologist. During the 1920s, an extremely element may be ruled out entirely or almost
detailed system for annotating the move­ entirely as a determining factor” (p. 20).
ments of dancers quantitatively and qualita­ The belief that one might learn a lot
tively was devised by Rudolf Laban (1926; about a person’s internal states by observ­
Hutchinson, 1970). Although nonverbal ing his or her behavior was another early
researchers have not embraced Labanotation 20th-century conception that fits the
(or any other comprehensive whole-body study of nonverbal behavior well. Allport
coding system) as a way of recording ongo­ and Vernon (1933), for example, pro­
ing human behavior, coding and notational vided experimental evidence that certain
methods are common in nonverbal research. movement patterns were congruent with
Given the preceding trends, it is not certain personality traits. German expres­
surprising that David Efron’s (1941) land­ sion psychology during the 1930s was also
mark study of gestures employed a variety studying movement patterns and their link­
of innovative methods, including (1) direct age to personality as well as various aspects
observation of natural interaction; (2) of facial expressions of emotion (e.g.,
filmed interactions, which he studied and Asendorpf, 1982; Wallbott, 1982). But
from which he elicited perceptions of naive because this research was sometimes trans­
observers; (3) frequency counts, graphs, formed into Nazi racist ideology (Asendorpf
and charts; and (4) sketches of interactant & Wallbott, 1982) and was not associated
gesturing made by a professional painter. with rigorous research standards (Ellgring,
As Ekman noted in his preface to the 1981), few German emigrants to the United
reissue of Efron’s (1972) book, “Efron’s States pursued this work.
methods are unique for his time and exem­ The relationship of more static features
plary for ours. Rarely has such a diversity of body shape and appearance to personal­
of investigatory techniques been utilized in ity traits was also of interest during this
a single study of body movement” (p. 8). time. Kretschmer (1925), a psychiatrist,
Efron’s (1941) study is also significant coded the dimensions of many body parts
as an illustration of how nonverbal behavior in an effort to identify human form with
is a product of the environment in which certain abnormal personality characteristics
01-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 3:58 PM Page 9

An Historical Overview of Nonverbal Research–––◆–––9

and disorders. Sheldon (1940) also believed Trager, and Hall and they, like Goffman,
that body measurements would tell us a lot Scheflen, and Ruesch, believed an under­
about a person’s temperament, intelligence, standing of the structure and organization
moral worth, and future achievement. of interaction depended on observations of
Much of his work used three broad classifi­ co-occurring verbal and nonverbal behavior
cations of body types: the thin ectomorph, (see also Bavelas & Chovil, this volume;
the muscular mesomorph, and the fatty Robinson, this volume). The tendency of
endomorph. He gathered data for his cata­ researchers to develop a program of research
log of body types by photographing fresh­ around a single nonverbal behavior, which
man students in the nude at various colleges occurred with some frequency during the
and universities. Subsequent analysis of his 1960s and 1970s, was probably not what
personal notes showed that he drew racist these “structuralists” had in mind.
conclusions from his work (Rosenbaum, The study of nonverbal behavior as a
1995). Although studies of characteristics part of the overall structure and organiza­
associated with body shape stereotypes per­ tion of human interaction gained consider­
sisted in nonverbal research, the validity of able momentum as a result of a 1955
these perceptions has not been established. collaboration at the Center for Advanced
Behavioral Studies at Stanford University
(see reviews by Kendon, 1990; Leeds-
♦ The 1950s: Laying Hurwitz, 1987). Among others, this group
included psychiatrists (e.g., Fromm-
a Foundation
Reichman, Brosin), anthropological linguists
(e.g., McQuown, Hockett), and anthropolo­
For nonverbal behavior studies, the 1950s gists (e.g., G. Bateson, Birdwhistell). Using
was a decade of significant events and schol­ a brief film of “Doris,” one of Gregory
arship. The works of scholars like Trager Bateson’s patients in family therapy, efforts
(1958), Birdwhistell (1952, 1970), and were made to develop a detailed frame-by­
E. T. Hall (1959) are considered fundamen­ frame analysis of both verbal and nonverbal
tal to those who later specialized in para- behavior. Ironically, the film showed a lot of
language, body movement/posture, and Doris’ behavior and little of Bateson’s, and
space. Ironically, some of these pioneers the analysis of mutual influence, so critical
were not as much interested in understanding to the structural approach, was restricted
nonverbal behavior per se as they were in severely. The results of this collaboration
understanding the structure and organiza­ were never published, but the approach and
tion of interaction as a whole. Kendon and its application can be found in the work
Sigman (1996) point out, “Birdwhistell of Pittinger, Hockett, and Danehy (1960),
used to say that the study of nonverbal Kendon (1982a), Birdwhistell (1970,
communication would be like the study of pp. 227–250), and in what Scheflen (1973)
non-cardiac physiology” (p. 231). Lyons called “context analysis.”
(1972) is more direct: “The fact that there The use of the terms kinesics (Birdwhistell,
is such a complete and intimate interpene­ 1952) and proxemics (Hall, 1959) for body
tration of language and non-language movement/posture and spatial relations,
should always be borne in mind in consid­ respectively, illustrated the belief that non­
ering the relationship between verbal and verbal codes had a structure similar to a
non-verbal communication” (p. 54). linguistic code. About 1968, Birdwhistell
Language was very much a part of modified his approach, going beyond a
the academic background of Birdwhistell, purely linguistic analysis and maintaining
01-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 3:58 PM Page 10

10–––◆–––Foundations

that some, but not all, nonverbal behavior Goldman-Eisler (1968) were exploring
had an organization like language (Kendon & spontaneous speech, pauses, hesitations,
Sigman, 1996, p. 16). Birdwhistell’s place in and speech disturbances and their relation
the history of nonverbal studies lies not so to anxiety, emotion, and cognition.1 Frank
much with whether he was right or wrong (1957) published a seminal monograph on
about the way nonverbal behavior is coded, the role of touching in human interaction in
but in the fact that his work energized schol­ which he said, “in many interpersonal rela­
ars in several disciplines to examine body tions, tactile ‘language’ functions most effec­
movement and posture systematically from tively and communicates more fully than
various perspectives. vocal language” (p. 214). At about the same
Birdwhistell’s ideas also seem to have res­ time, Harlow’s (1958) work was receiving
onated with Erving Goffman, who took one widespread attention because it suggested
of his courses at the University of Toronto that touching was so powerful that it may
in the 1940s. Later, Goffman (1963, 1967, sometimes be preferred to nursing for young
1971) made it clear that his observations primates. A cornerstone for later studies of
associated with “interaction order” involved how environments affect human interaction
the dynamic interplay of the totality of was established in the “beautiful and ugly
behavior, including eye gaze, body move­ room” studies (Maslow & Mintz, 1956).
ment, gestures, positioning, verbalizations, It was also during the mid-1950s that
and the like. Goffman identified his approach Rosenthal (2002) felt he had unwittingly
to studying interaction with the way etholo­ directed the participants in his doctoral
gists studied animal behavior even though dissertation to behave in accord with his
he had little use for their Darwinian inter­ hypotheses. After establishing the existence
pretations (Kendon, 1988). of these expectancy effects in various arenas
Although the animal studies of etholo­ of human behavior, he then examined the
gists Konrad Lorenz and Niko Tinbergen way these expectancies were communica­
were well known in the 1950s, it was not ted by subtle (out of awareness) nonverbal
until the 1960s that human ethology and signals (Rosenthal, 1966; Rosenthal &
the work of scholars like Eibl-Eibesfeldt Jacobson, 1968). Ekman, whose later work
(1970, pp. 442–534) found their place exerted a powerful influence on the modern
within the nonverbal literature and reinvig­ study of nonverbal communication, pub­
orated the discussion of biological bases for lished his first article on the subject in 1957:
human behavior. For many years after World A proposal for a method of sampling and
War II, human behavior was commonly recording nonverbal behavior.
believed to be almost totally malleable by cul­ An early reference to the label, “nonver­
ture, and any data that suggested otherwise bal communication,” is also found in the
were considered “politically loaded” and work of interpersonal psychiatrist Jurgen
fodder for racist actions. For example, Ruesch (1953) when he refers to its multi-
Ekman (1998b) reports the stiff opposi­ meaning potential:
tion he received in the late 1960s from
Birdwhistell, Gregory Bateson, and Margaret Silent actions . . . always have a potentially
Mead when he reported research supporting twofold function: they are an implementa­
a pan-cultural morphology for certain facial tion in their own right and they may stand
expressions (Ekman & Friesen, 1971; for something else, or both. This double
Ekman, Sorenson, & Friesen, 1969). meaning of actions introduces great diffi­
The 1950s were also a fertile time for culties into the evaluation of nonverbal
psychological approaches. Mahl (1987) and communication inasmuch as a perceiver
01-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 3:58 PM Page 11

An Historical Overview of Nonverbal Research–––◆–––11

can never be quite sure when action is (Henley, 1977; and later, Hall, 1984 and
intended to convey a message and when it this volume; Mayo & Henley, 1981).
is intended for other purposes. (p. 233) In 1969, a thorough review of nonverbal
research (Duncan, 1969) and what was to
Ruesch’s later collaboration with pho­ become a highly influential theoretical trea­
tographer Weldon Kees may be the first tise on the origins, usage, coding, and cate­
book with “nonverbal communication” in gorization of nonverbal behavior (Ekman
the title (Ruesch & Kees, 1956). & Friesen, 1969; updated in Ekman, 1999)
appeared. This was a time when theoretical
issues like awareness, intent, coding, mean­
♦ The Tipping Point ing, classification, and units of measurement
were very much on the academic table. Even
questions about whether the term nonverbal
As an outgrowth of the pioneering work
described the emerging field of research ade­
put forth in the 1950s, scholars with inter­
quately (Harrison & Knapp, 1972; Kendon,
ests in paralanguage, kinesics, and prox­
1981) and questions about when nonverbal
emics continued their research. But the late
behavior should be considered “commu­
1960s and early 1970s was the period of
nicative” and when it should not were
emergence for a broadly visible, res­
pectable, and sustainable field of nonverbal debated (e.g., Wiener, Devoe, Rubinow, &
studies. One cannot fully understand the Geller, 1972; and later, Russell &
emergence of nonverbal studies during this Fernández-Dols, 1997).
period without considering the climate of In 1970, a freelance journalist, Julius
the times. It was an era that favored a con­ Fast, wrote a best-selling book that went a
cern for personal relationships and the long way toward making the subject of
kind of revelations about informal inte­ “body language” a topic of national interest
raction that arose out of “consciousness­ and recognition. Other popular books
raising” and “sensitivity” groups. Davis followed (Montagu, 1971; Morris, 1971;
(1971) said, 1977, 1985). Some of the less academic
popular accounts portrayed the “reading”
The enormous public interest in non­ of body language in misleading ways
verbal communications seems to be part (Koivumaki, 1975), but these books contin­
of the spirit of the times, the need that ued to arouse the public’s interest in the
many people feel to get back in touch subject. By 1970, courses in nonverbal
with their own emotions—the search for communication were being offered at
the emotional truth that perhaps gets Purdue, Michigan State, the University of
expressed nonverbally. (p. 2) California, and a few other universities (see
McCroskey, Richmond, & McCroskey,
Larger societal issues and trends also this volume). Textbooks for nonverbal
affected specific areas of nonverbal study. courses also appeared suddenly in social
For example, worries about world overpop­ psychology (e.g., Argyle, 1975; Mehrabian,
ulation (Erlich, 1971), and the fear that 1971; Weitz, 1974) and in communication
crowded humans might act like the aberrant (Eisenberg & Smith, 1971; Harrison, 1974;
rats in Calhoun’s (1962) widely publicized Knapp, 1972).
studies spurred the interest in space, density, On top of this growing interest in the
and territory. Soon after, the women’s study of nonverbal behavior was the
movement raised questions about nonverbal increasing availability of affordable video­
signals as subtle manifestations of power tape recorders. In 1972, Harrison, Cohen,
01-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 3:58 PM Page 12

12–––◆–––Foundations

Crouch, Genova, and Steinberg summa­ behavior were published during the last
rized this period by saying, four decades of the 20th century. The
breadth and depth of the literature today is
Sharp changes have taken place in the too massive for the kind of broad-based
nonverbal communication literature in literature reviews that characterized the
the past decade, and in particular, in the research done prior to 1980 (e.g., Burgoon,
last two years. A decade ago, few books 1980; Knapp, Wiemann, & Daly, 1978).2
existed; and the early works tended to be But two general approaches can be said to
speculative, anecdotal, and tentative. . . . have characterized the research during this
Major works are now emerging which, period: The structural and ethological
on the one hand, organize and synthesize researchers tended to emphasize descrip­
the existing data from a variety of fields. tions of how interactions are organized,
Research programs extending over a whereas others emphasized the manipula­
number of years are now culminating tion of psychological variables and/or
and the results are becoming available. nonverbal behavior to observe the effects.
Theoretical issues have become clarified, Within each of these two broad approaches,
and a range of active theories vie for sup­ some scholars focused on a particular body
port. Finally, methodological problems part, some examined multiple nonverbal
are being examined and, frequently, they signals, and some studies went beyond
are being solved. . . . The amount of body movements per se and focused on
knowledge has now reached a “critical “related non-word phenomena” like physi­
mass”—and a general availability—so cal appearance, environmental factors, and
that even more exciting things may be the use of space.
ahead. (pp. 473–474) In the tradition of those who laid the
foundation for scrutinizing the way interac­
More exciting things were ahead. In tants structure and organize their behavior,
1979, Davis said, “In the past five years Kendon (1977) showed how interactants
alone more books have been published on use their faces, eyes, and spatial formations
body language than in the preceding 95. It for various interaction functions, includ­
is an idea that has found its time” (p. 51). ing kissing and greeting rituals. Kendon’s
The first issue of the Journal of Environ­ (2004) analysis of gestures may be the most
mental Psychology and Nonverbal Behavior thorough work available on the subject. In
appeared in 1976, and in 1980 the name like manner, the detailed analyses of facial
was changed to the Journal of Nonverbal expressions of emotion by Ekman and
Behavior. As the output of research has his colleagues are unmatched (Ekman &
grown, articles and books devoted to the Friesen, 1975; Ekman, Friesen, & Hager,
special demands of conducting nonverbal 2002). Duncan and Fiske (1977) and
research and measuring nonverbal phenom­ Goodwin (1981) illustrated how speakers
ena have also been published (e.g., Kulp, and listeners coordinate their verbal and
Cornetto, & Knapp, 2005; Manusov, nonverbal behavior during the exchange
2005; Scherer & Ekman, 1982). of speaking turns, whereas Condon and
Ogston (1971) described a form of interde­
♦ An Outpouring of Research pendent behavior between interactants as
synchrony. Subsequent studies examined
a variety of ways interactants coordinate
Myriad studies bearing on the structure, their behavior through matching and syn­
organization, and effects of nonverbal chrony (Bernieri & Rosenthal, 1999; see
01-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 3:58 PM Page 13

An Historical Overview of Nonverbal Research–––◆–––13

Tickle-Degnen, this volume). Eibl-Eibesfeldt 1972; see also Andersen, Guerrero, &
(1970) compared the facial expressions of Jones, this volume). Multivariable studies
children born blind and deaf with those of are also common in studies that give special
others born with hearing and sight, whereas attention to context (Feldman, 1982;
other ethologists described patterns of eye Philippot, Feldman, & Coats, 1999), such
gaze, body movement, and paralinguistic as those detailed later in this volume.
phenomena (Hinde, 1972; von Cranach &
Vine, 1978).
In the “variable analytic” tradition, some ♦ Moving On
research programs have focused on specific
body areas, such as eye gaze and mutual gaze
(Argyle & Cook, 1976; Fehr & Exline, At different points in the 2,500-year history
1987), pupil dilation (Hess, 1975), vocal of nonverbal research, studies have varied
signals (Davitz, 1964; Scherer, 1986), and in terms of subject matter, methods, and
touch (Field, 1995). Four decades of work frequency. But the disciplinary breadth of
on the face by Ekman and his colleagues are interest and the number of studies conducted
the basis of a substantial body of literature during the past 40 years have provided
on the production and perception of expres­ unparalleled insights. Of course, not all sub­
sions of emotion (Ekman et al., 1972; Ekman areas of nonverbal research flourish at the
& Rosenberg, 1997; see Matsumoto, this same time. Areas of study like the human use
volume). Even though most studies have of space and territory, pupil dilation, and
examined the face and voice as spontaneous some others popular in the 1960s and 1970s,
emotional expressions, the work of some are currently at low ebb, whereas the study of
scholars (e.g., Fridlund, 1994) views these gestures, as one example, is currently at high
expressions as social displays that are pur­ tide. The recent interest in and proliferation
posefully enacted to communicate. As noted of gesture research necessitated the establish­
earlier, research in “related” areas like phys­ ment of a separate academic journal called
ical appearance (Berscheid & Walster, 1974; Gesture in 2001. The extent to which com­
Rhodes & Zebrowitz, 2002), space (Burgoon, municators can encode and decode nonver­
1978; Sommer, 1969, 2002), and the envi­ bal behavior accurately is another area of
ronment (Mehrabian, 1976) has been suffi­ considerable interest (Hall & Bernieri, 2001;
cient to establish an academic niche within see Riggio, this volume). Ambady, LaPlante,
the larger field of nonverbal studies. and Johnson (2001), for example, have
Typical of multivariable approaches shown that decoders often use very brief
are studies that focus on (1) interaction excerpts of another person’s ongoing nonver­
processes like the reciprocity and compen­ bal behavior to make judgments. These “thin
sation of nonverbal behavior (Argyle & slices” have also been shown to be reliable
Dean, 1965; Patterson, 1984) and incon­ predictors of behavior. Other scholars are
gruent verbal and nonverbal signals exploring the conditions under which non­
(Bavelas, Black, Chovil, & Mullett 1990; verbal behavior is “automatically” (with little
Volkmar & Siegel, 1982) and (2) interac­ or no awareness and/or intent) encoded and
tion outcomes or goals like deception decoded (Patterson, 1999; Spitz, 1997;
(Ekman, 2001; see also Vrij, this volume), Wegner & Bargh, 1998; see also Lakin, this
power and dominance (Ellyson & Dovidio, volume).
1985; see also Burgoon & Dunbar, this The application of nonverbal research
volume), expectancies (Blanck, 1993), and to particular settings like classrooms, court­
immediacy (Andersen, 1985; Mehrabian, rooms, marriages, political speeches, medical
01-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 3:58 PM Page 14

14–––◆–––Foundations

interviews, marketing, and cross-cultural that identifies a starting point for describing
encounters is also considered a worthy target the total interactive situation. Patterson
for current nonverbal research (Riggio & (1983), Knapp (1984), and others have
Feldman, 2005). argued for multisignal, multimeaning, inter­
Coexisting with this current tendency to active, and processual approaches to non­
value things on the basis of their utility in verbal research. These are all elements that
important arenas of everyday life is a wide­ characterize natural, human interaction. To
spread interest in the management of close the extent that future nonverbal research
relationships. This trend has set the stage assumes these features, the study of nonver­
for more studies of nonverbal communi­ bal signals may become what Birdwhistell,
cation among friends, family, and lovers Scheflen, Goffman, and others envisioned
(Feeney, Noller, Sheehan, & Peterson, in the mid-20th century.
1999; Knapp, 1983; Manusov, 1995;
Noller, 1984 and this volume), even though
the preponderance of nonverbal research to ♦ Notes
date has been conducted with interactants
who are strangers or acquaintances.
1. The Mahl and Goldman-Eisler volumes
Despite a current surge of interest in the
cited here summarize their research programs,
biological foundations of human behavior both of which were initiated in the early 1950s.
in all the behavioral sciences, we seem to be 2. This review is similarly constrained. Not
circumspect in attributing any given behav­ all important research contributions could be
ior exclusively to either culture or biology. noted. See Knapp and Hall (2005) for a more
Unlike the past, there appears to be a complete review of nonverbal research.
greater acceptance that both may play a
role in the manifestation of nonverbal
behavior (Segerstråle & Molnár, 1997). ♦ References
Paradoxically, nonverbal behavior seems to
be thought of and studied as both a subarea
that complements the study of human inter­ Allport, G. W., & Vernon, P. E. (1933). Studies in
expressive movement. New York: Macmillan.
action and a way of approaching the study
Ambady, N., LaPlante, D., & Johnson, E.
of human interaction per se. Those who
(2001). Thin-slice judgments as a measure
study gestures, for example, view the inter­ of interpersonal sensitivity. In J. A. Hall &
dependence of verbal behavior and gestures F. J. Bernieri (Eds.), Interpersonal sensitiv­
as natural and integral to their studies (e.g., ity: Theory and measurement (pp. 89–101).
Feyereisen & de Lannoy, 1991; Kendon, Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
2004; McNeill, 1992, 2000; see also, Andersen, P. A. (1985). Nonverbal immediacy
Bavelas & Chovil, this volume). Other in interpersonal communication. In A. W.
nonverbal scholars not only examine the Siegman & S. Feldstein (Eds.), Multichannel
interrelationship of verbal and nonverbal integrations of nonverbal behavior
behavior but also embrace the role of (pp. 1–36). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
appearance, environmental factors, space, Argyle, M. (1975). Bodily communication. New
York: International Universities Press.
and time as parts of the nonverbal domain.
Argyle, M., & Cook, M. (1976). Gaze and
In short, nonverbal studies leave no element
mutual gaze. New York: Cambridge
of social interaction untouched. University Press.
In one sense, therefore, the term nonver­ Argyle, M., & Dean, J. (1965). Eye contact,
bal suggests a separate piece of the interac­ distance and affiliation. Sociometry, 28,
tion puzzle, but in another sense it is a term 289–304.
01-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 3:58 PM Page 15

An Historical Overview of Nonverbal Research–––◆–––15

Aristotle. (1991/ca.350 BCE). On rhetoric In D. K. Nimmo (Ed.), Communication


(trans. G. A. Kennedy). New York: Oxford yearbook 4 (pp. 179–197). New Brunswick,
University Press. NJ: Transaction Books.
Asendorpf, J. (1982). Contributions of the Calhoun, J. B. (1962). Population density and
German “Expression Psychology” to non­ social pathology. Scientific American, 206,
verbal communication research: Part II. 139–148.
The face. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, Chapple, E. D. (1940). Measuring human rela­
6, 199–219. tions: An introduction to the study of inter­
Asendorpf, J., & Wallbott, H. G. (1982). action of individuals. Genetic Psychology
Contributions of the German “Expression Monographs, 22, 3–147.
Psychology” to nonverbal communication Chapple, E. D. (1949). The interaction chrono­
research. Part I: Theories and concepts. graph: Its evolution and present applica­
Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 6, 135–147. tion. Personnel, 25, 295–307.
Bavelas, J. B., Black, A., Chovil, N., & Mullett, Cicero, M. T. (1942/ca. 55 BCE). De oratore,
J. (1990). Equivocal communication. book III (H. Rackham, Trans.). Cambridge,
Newbury Park, CA: Sage. MA: Harvard University Press.
Bernieri, F. J., & Rosenthal, R. (1999). Condon, W. S., & Ogston, W. D. (1971).
Interpersonal coordination: Behavior Speech and body motion synchrony of
matching and interactional synchrony. the speaker-hearer. In D. L. Horton &
In R. S. Feldman & B. Rimé (Eds.), J. J. Jenkins (Eds.), Perception of language.
Fundamentals of nonverbal behavior Columbus, OH: Merrill.
(pp. 401–432). New York: Cambridge Confucius, K. (1951/ca. 500 BCE). Confucian
University Press. analects (E. Pound, Trans.). New York:
Berscheid, E., & Walster, E. H. (1974). Kasper & Horton.
Physical attractiveness. In L. Berkowitz Cranach, M. von, & Vine, I. (Eds.). (1978).
(Ed.), Advances in experimental social Social communication and movement.
psychology (Vol. 7, pp. 157–215). New New York: Academic Press.
York: Academic Press. Darwin, C. (1998). The expression of the emo­
Birdwhistell, R. L. (1952). Introduction to tions in man and animals. New York:
kinesics: An annotation system for analysis Oxford University Press. (Original work
of body motion and gesture. Washington, published 1872)
DC: U.S. Department of State Foreign Davis, F. (1971). Inside intuition. New York:
Service Institute. McGraw-Hill.
Birdwhistell, R. L. (1970). Kinesics and context. Davis, M. (1979). The state of the art: Past and
Philadelphia: University of Pennsylvania present trends in body movement research.
Press. In A. Wolfgang (Ed.), Nonverbal behavior:
Bizzell, P., & Herzberg, B. (Eds.). (1990). The Applications and cultural implications
rhetorical tradition: Readings from classical (pp. 51–66). New York: Academic Press.
times to the present. Boston: Bedford Books. Davitz, J. R. (1964). The communication
Blanck, P. D. (Ed.). (1993). Interpersonal expec­ of emotional meaning. New York:
tations: Theory, research, and applications. McGraw-Hill.
New York: Cambridge University Press. Delaumosne, A. (Ed.). (1893). Delsarte system
Bremmer, J., & Roodenburg, H. (Eds.). (1991). of oratory (4th ed.). New York: Edgar S.
A cultural history of gesture. Ithaca, NY: Werner.
Cornell University Press. Delia, J. G. (1987). Communication research:
Burgoon, J. K. (1978). A communication model A history. In C. R. Berger & S. H. Chaffee
of personal space violations: Explication (Eds.), Handbook of communication science
and an initial test. Human Communication (pp. 20–98). Newbury Park, CA: Sage.
Research, 4, 129–142. DePaulo, B. M., & Friedman, H. S. (1998).
Burgoon, J. K. (1980). Nonverbal communica­ Nonverbal communication. In D. T.
tion research in the 1970s: An overview. Gilbert, S. T. Fiske, & G. Lindzey (Eds.),
01-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 3:58 PM Page 16

16–––◆–––Foundations

The handbook of social psychology (Vol. 2, Ekman, P., & Friesen, W. V. (1975). Unmasking
4th ed.). New York: McGraw-Hill. the face. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall.
Duncan, S., Jr. (1969). Nonverbal communica­ Ekman, P., Friesen, W. V., & Ellsworth, P. (1972).
tion. Psychological Bulletin, 72, 118–137. Emotion in the human face. Elmsford, NY:
Duncan, S., Jr., & Fiske, D. W. (1977). Face-to­ Pergamon Press.
face interaction: Research, methods, and Ekman, P., Friesen, W. V., & Hager, J. C.
theory. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. (2002). The facial action coding system
Efron, D. (1941). Gesture and environment. New (2nd ed.). Salt Lake City, UT: Research
York: King’s Crown Press. (Republished as Nexus eBook.
Gesture, race and culture, 1972. The Hague: Ekman, P., & Rosenberg, E. (Eds.). (1997).
Mouton) What the face reveals: Basic and applied
Efron, D. (1972). Gesture, race and culture. The studies of spontaneous expression using the
Hague: Mouton. facial action coding system (FACS). New
Eibl-Eibesfeldt, I. (1970). Ethology: The biology York: Oxford University Press.
of behavior (2nd ed.). New York: Holt, Ekman, P., Sorenson, E. R., & Friesen, W. V.
Rinehart & Winston. (1969). Pan-cultural elements of facial dis­
Eisenberg, A. M., & Smith, R. R. (1971). Non­ plays of emotions. Science, 164, 86–88.
verbal communication. New York: Bobbs- Ellgring, H. (1981). Nonverbal communication:
Merrill. A review of research in Germany. German
Ekman, P. (1957). A methodological study of Journal of Psychology, 5, 59–84.
nonverbal behavior. Journal of Psychology, Ellyson, S. L., & Dovidio, J. F. (1985). Power,
43, 141–149. dominance and nonverbal behavior. New
Ekman, P. (Ed.). (1973). Darwin and facial York: Springer.
expression: A century of research in review. Erlich, P. R. (1971). The population bomb. New
New York: Academic Press. York: Ballantine Books.
Ekman, P. (1998a). Introduction to the third edi­ Fast, J. (1970). Body language. New York: Evans.
tion. In C. Darwin, The expression Feeney, J. A., Noller, P., Sheehan, G., & Peterson,
of the emotions of man and animals C. (1999). Conflict issues and conflict strate­
(3rd ed., pp. xxi–xxxvi). New York: Oxford gies as contexts for nonverbal behavior in
University Press. close relationships. In P. Philippot, R. S.
Ekman, P. (1998b). Afterword: Universality of Feldman, & E. J. Coats (Eds.), The social
emotional expression? A personal history of context of nonverbal behavior (pp. 348–
the dispute. In C. Darwin, The expression 371). New York: Cambridge University
of the emotions in man and animals (3rd Press.
ed., pp. 363–393). New York: Oxford Fehr, B. J., & Exline, R. V. (1987). Social visual
University Press. interaction: A conceptual and literature
Ekman, P. (1999). Emotional and conversational review. In A. W. Siegman & S. Feldstein
nonverbal signals. In L. Messing & (Eds.), Nonverbal behavior and communica­
R. Campbell (Eds.), Gesture, speech and sign tion (pp. 225–326). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
(pp. 45–55). New York: Oxford University Feldman, R. S. (Ed.). (1982). Development of
Press. nonverbal behavior in children. New York:
Ekman, P. (2001). Telling lies. New York: Springer.
Norton. Feyereisen, P., & de Lannoy, J-D. (1991).
Ekman, P., & Friesen, W. V. (1969). The reper­ Gestures and speech: Psychological investi­
toire of nonverbal behavior: Categories, gations. New York: Cambridge University
origins, usage, and coding. Semiotica, 1, Press.
49–98. Field, T. M. (Ed.). (1995). Touch in early devel­
Ekman, P., & Friesen, W. V. (1971). Constants opment. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
across cultures in the face and emotion. Frank, L. K. (1957). Tactile communication.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, Genetic Psychology Monographs, 56,
17, 124–129. 209–255.
01-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 3:58 PM Page 17

An Historical Overview of Nonverbal Research–––◆–––17

Fridlund, A. J. (1994). Human facial expression: Hutchinson, A. (1970). Labanotation: The system
An evolutionary view. San Diego, CA: of analyzing and recording movement. New
Academic Press. York: Theater Arts Books.
Goffman, E. (1963). Behavior in public places: de Jorio, A. (1832). La mimica degli antichi
Notes on the social organization of gather­ investigata nel gestire napolitano [Gestural
ings. New York: Free Press. expression of the ancients in the light
Goffman, E. (1967). Interaction ritual: Essays on of Neapolitan gesturing]. Naples, Italy:
face-to-face behavior. New York: Doubleday Stamperia del Fibreno.
Anchor. Kendon, A. (1977). Studies in the behavior of
Goffman, E. (1971). Relations in public. New social interaction. Bloomington: University
York: Basic Books. of Indiana Press.
Goldman-Eisler, F. (1968). Psycholinguistics: Kendon, A. (1981). Introduction: Current issues
Experiments in spontaneous speech. New in the study of “nonverbal communi­
York: Academic Press. cation.” In A. Kendon (Ed.), Nonverbal
Goodwin, C. (1981). Conversational organiza­ communication, interaction, and gesture
tion. New York: Academic Press. (pp. 1–53). New York: Mouton.
Gronbeck, B. E., McKerrow, R. E., Ehninger, D., Kendon, A. (1982a). The organization of behavior
& Monroe, A. H. (1990). Principles and in face-to-face interaction: Observations
types of speech communication. Glenview, on the development of a methodology. In
IL: Scott, Foresman/Little Brown. K. R. Scherer & P. Ekman (Eds.), Handbook
Hall, E. T. (1959). The silent language. Garden of methods in nonverbal research (pp. 440–
City, NY: Doubleday. 505). New York: Cambridge University Press.
Hall, J. A. (1984). Nonverbal sex differences. Kendon, A. (1982b). The study of gesture: Some
Baltimore: Johns Hopkins University Press. observations on its history. Recherches
Hall, J. A. & Bernieri, F. (Eds.). (2001). Sémiotiques/Semiotic Inquiry, 2, 45–62.
Interpersonal sensitivity: Theory and Kendon, A. (1988). Goffman’s approach to
measurement. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. face-to-face interaction. In P. Drew &
Halverson, H. M. (1931). An experimental study A. Wootton (Eds.), Erving Goffman:
of prehension of infants by means of sys­ Exploring the interaction order (pp. 14–40).
tematic cinema records. Genetic Psychology Cambridge, UK: Polity Press.
Monographs, 10, 107–286. Kendon, A. (1990). Some context for context
Harlow, H. F. (1958). The nature of love. analysis: A view of the origins of structural
American Psychologist, 13, 678–685. studies of face-to-face interaction. In
Harrison, R. P. (1974). Beyond words. Englewood A. Kendon (Ed.), Conducting interaction:
Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall. Patterns of focused encounters (pp.
Harrison, R. P., Cohen, A. A., Crouch, W. W., 15–49). New York: Cambridge University
Genova, K. L., & Steinberg, M. (1972). Press.
The nonverbal communication literature. Kendon, A. (2000). Introduction: Andrea de Jorio
Journal of Communication, 22, 460–476. and his work on gesture. In A. de Jorio
Harrison, R. P., & Knapp, M. L. (1972). Toward (Ed.), Gesture in Naples and gesture in clas­
an understanding of nonverbal communi­ sical antiquity (pp. xix–cvii). Bloomington:
cation systems. Journal of Communication, Indiana University Press.
22, 339–352. Kendon, A. (2004). Gesture: Visible action
Henley, N. M. (1977). Body politics: Power, sex, as utterance. New York: Cambridge
and nonverbal communication. Englewood University Press.
Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall. Kendon, A., & Sigman, S. J. (1996). Ray L.
Hess, E. H. (1975). The tell-tale eye. New York: Birdwhistell (1918–1994). Semiotica, 112,
Van Nostrand Reinhold. 231–261.
Hinde, R. A. (Ed.). (1972). Non-verbal commu­ Kennedy, G. (1963). The art of persuasion in
nication. New York: Cambridge University Greece. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University
Press. Press.
01-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 3:58 PM Page 18

18–––◆–––Foundations

Kennedy, G. (1972). The art of rhetoric in the Human Communication Research, 21,
Roman world. Princeton, NJ: Princeton 456–477.
University Press. Manusov, V. (Ed.). (2005). The sourcebook of
Knapp, M. L. (1972). Nonverbal communica­ nonverbal measures. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
tion in human interaction. New York: Holt, Maslow, A. H., & Mintz, N. L. (1956). Effects
Rinehart & Winston. of esthetic surroundings: I. Initial effects
Knapp, M. L. (1983). Dyadic relationship of three esthetic conditions upon perceiving
development. In J. M. Wiemann & “energy” and “well-being” in faces. Journal
R. P. Harrison (Eds.), Nonverbal interac­ of Psychology, 41, 247–254.
tion (pp. 179–207). Beverly Hills, CA: Sage. Mayo, C., & Henley, N. M. (Eds.). (1981). Gender
Knapp, M. L. (1984). The study of nonverbal and nonverbal behavior. New York: Springer.
behavior vis-á-vis human communication McNeill, D. (1992). Hand and mind: What
theory. In A. Wolfgang (Ed.), Nonverbal gestures reveal about thought. Chicago:
behavior: Perspectives, applications, inter­ University of Chicago Press.
cultural insights (pp. 15–40). New York: McNeill, D. (2000). Language and gesture. New
C. J. Hogrefe. York: Cambridge University Press.
Knapp, M. L., & Hall, J. A. (2005). Nonverbal Mehrabian, A. (1971). Silent messages. Belmont,
communication in human interaction (6th CA: Wadsworth.
ed.). Belmont, CA: Wadsworth. Mehrabian, A. (1972). Nonverbal communica­
Knapp, M. L., Wiemann, J. M., & Daly, J. A. tion. Chicago: Aldine-Atherton.
(1978). Nonverbal communication: Issues Mehrabian, A. (1976). Public places and private
and appraisal. Human Communication spaces. New York: Basic Books.
Research, 4, 271–280. Montagu, M. F. A. (1971). Touching: The
Koivumaki, J. H. (1975). Body language taught human significance of the skin. New York:
here. Journal of Communication, 25, 26–30. Columbia University Press.
Kretschmer, E. (1925). Physique and character: Morris, D. (1971). Intimate behavior. New York:
An investigation of the nature of constitu­ Random House.
tion and the theory of temperament. New Morris, D. (1977). Manwatching: A field guide
York: Harcourt Brace Jovanovich. to human behavior. New York: Abrams.
Kulp, C., Cornetto, K., & Knapp, M. L. (2005). Morris, D. (1985). Bodywatching. New York:
The description of nonverbal behavior. In Crown.
U. Ammon, N. Dittmar, K. J. Mattheier, & Noller, P. (1984). Nonverbal communication and
P. Trudgill (Eds.), An international handbook marital interaction. New York: Pergamon
of the science of language and society (Vol. 2, Press.
pp. xx–xx). New York: Walter De Gruyter. Patterson, M. L. (1983). Nonverbal behavior:
Laban, R. (1926). Choreographic. Jena, Germany: A functional perspective. New York:
Eugen Diederichs. Springer.
Leeds-Hurwitz, W. (1987). The social history Patterson, M. L. (1984). Nonverbal exchange:
of The natural history of an interview: A Past, present, and future. Journal of Non­
multidisciplinary investigation of social verbal Behavior, 8, 350–359.
communication. Research on Language Patterson, M. L. (1999). The evolution of a par­
and Social Interaction, 20, 1–51. allel process model of nonverbal communi­
Lyons, J. (1972). Human language. In R. A. Hinde cation. In P. Philippot, R. S. Feldman, &
(Ed.), Non-verbal communication (pp. 49– E. J. Coats (Eds.), The social context of
85). New York: Cambridge University Press. nonverbal behavior (pp. 317–347). New
Mahl, G. F. (1987). Explorations in nonverbal York: Cambridge University Press.
and vocal behavior. Hillside, NJ: Erlbaum. Philippot, P., Feldman, R. S., & Coats, E. J.
Manusov, V. (1995). Reacting to changes in (Eds.). (1999). The social context of non­
nonverbal behavior: Relational satisfaction verbal behavior. New York: Cambridge
and adaptation patterns in romantic dyads. University Press.
01-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 3:58 PM Page 19

An Historical Overview of Nonverbal Research–––◆–––19

Pittinger, R. E., Hockett, C. F., & Danehy, J. J. Segerstråle, U., & Molnár, P. (Eds.). (1997).
(1960). The first five minutes: A sample Nonverbal communication: Where nature
of microscopic interview analysis. Ithaca, meets culture. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
NY: Paul Martineau. Shawn, T. (1954). Every little movement: A
Quintilian, M. F. (1922/ca. 90 CE). The institu­ book about Francois Delsarte. Pittsfield,
tio oratoria, book XI (H. E. Butler, Trans.). MA: Eagle Print & Binding.
Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. Sheldon, W. H. (1940). The varieties of human
Rhodes, G., & Zebrowitz, L. A. (Eds.). (2002). physique: An introduction to constitutional
Facial attractiveness: Evolutionary, cognitive, psychology. New York: Harper & Row.
and social perspectives. Westport, CT: Ablex. Sommer, R. (1969). Personal space. Englewood
Riggio, R. E., & Feldman, R. S. (Eds.). (2005). Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall.
Applications of nonverbal communication. Sommer, R. (2002). Personal space in a digital
Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. age. In R. B. Bechtel & A. Churchman
Rosenbaum, R. (1995, January 15). The posture (Eds.), Handbook of environmental psy­
photo scandal. New York Times Magazine, chology (pp. 647–660). New York: Wiley.
pp. 26–31, 40, 46, 55–56. Spitz, H. H. (1997). Nonconscious move­
Rosenthal, R. (1966). Experimenter effects in ments: From mystical messages to facilitated
behavioral research. New York: Appleton­ communication. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Century-Crofts. Trager, G. L. (1958). Paralanguage: A first approx­
Rosenthal, R. (2002). Covert communication in imation. Studies in Linguistics, 13, 1–12.
classrooms, clinics, courtrooms, and cubi­ Volkmar, F. R., & Siegel, A. E. (1982).
cles. American Psychologist, 57, 839–849. Responses to consistent and discrepant
Rosenthal, R., & Jacobson, L. (1968). Pygmalion social communications. In R. S. Feldman
in the classroom. New York: Holt, Rinehart (Ed.), Development of nonverbal behavior
& Winston. in children (pp. 231–255). New York:
Ruesch, J. (1953). Synopsis of the theory of Springer.
human communication. Psychiatry, 16, Wallbott, H. G. (1982). Contributions of
215–243. the German “Expression Psychology” to
Ruesch, J., & Kees, W. (1956). Nonverbal com­ nonverbal communication research: Part
munication: Notes on the visual perception III. Gait, gestures, and body movement.
of human relations. Los Angeles: University Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 7, 20–32.
of California Press. Wegner, D. M., & Bargh, J. A. (1998).
Russell, J. A., & Fernández-Dols, J. M. (1997). Control and automaticity in social life. In
The psychology of facial expression. New D. T. Gilbert, S. T. Fiske, & G. Lindzey
York: Cambridge University Press. (Eds.), The handbook of social psychology
Scheflen, A. E. (1973). Communicational struc­ (Vol. 1, 4th ed., pp. 446–496). New York:
ture: Analysis of a psychotherapy transaction. McGraw-Hill.
Bloomington: University of Indiana Press. Weitz, S. (Ed.). (1974). Nonverbal communica­
Scherer, K. R. (1986). Vocal affect expression: tion: Readings with commentary. New
A review and a model for future research. York: Oxford University Press.
Psychological Bulletin, 99, 143–165. Wiener, M., Devoe, S., Rubinow, S., & Geller, J.
Scherer, K. R., & Ekman, P. (Eds.). (1982). (1972). Nonverbal behavior and nonverbal
Handbook of nonverbal behavior research. communication. Psychological Review, 79,
New York: Cambridge University Press. 185–214.
01-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 3:58 PM Page 20
02-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 4:00 PM Page 21

2
THE EVOLUTION OF THEORIES
OF INTERACTIVE BEHAVIOR
� Miles L. Patterson
University of Missouri, St. Louis

S peculation about the role and impact of nonverbal behavior in the


human condition has been present for centuries in philosophy,
science, and literature (see Knapp, this volume). Nevertheless, the devel­
opment of systematic and focused empirical research on nonverbal
behavior is a relatively recent phenomenon, growing rapidly from the
late 1950s through the present day. Although the vast majority of this
work consists of empirical research, theoretical scholarship has also been
important, not only in developing a broader understanding of nonverbal
communication but also in shaping subsequent empirical work. This
chapter focuses on some of this theoretical development. In particular, I
discuss theories of interactional nonverbal behavior (i.e., patterned cues
in face-to-face contexts).
Although nonverbal communication operates in a wide variety of con­
texts, it is especially important in face-to-face interactions. In such inter­
actions, patterns of behavior are “negotiated” subtly and move typically
toward some degree of stability. Partners’ behavior patterns might con­
verge and be similar in form, or they might diverge and be dissimilar.
Simultaneously, interactants make a wide variety of judgments about
their partners and the interaction. So, how do we explain the complex

◆ 21
02-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 4:00 PM Page 22

22–––◆–––Foundations

cognitive and behavioral adjustments in a Although these were all interesting issues,
wide range of face-to-face encounters? This this line of research provided little insight
is the central question underlying the theo­ into the dynamic relationships across non­
ries discussed in this chapter. The purposes verbal behaviors as people interacted with
of this chapter are (1) to trace the develop­ one another. The landscape changed dra­
ment of theories of interactive behavior and matically, however, with the publication of a
(2) to discuss, in some detail, a more com­ 1965 article by Argyle and Dean.
prehensive, parallel process model of non­
verbal communication (see also Patterson,
2001). Although this chapter focuses spe­ EQUILIBRIUM THEORY
cifically on the behavioral give-and-take
between people, these theories have impor­ In their equilibrium theory, Argyle and
tant implications for a wide variety of topics Dean (1965) focused on how individuals
in nonverbal communication, including maintain a comfortable or appropriate level
emotions, deception, influence, impression of behavioral intimacy or involvement in
management, and intimacy. interactions. They proposed that a small
Even though this chapter highlights the set of behaviors, including distance, gaze,
parallel process model of nonverbal com­ smiling, and verbal intimacy (self-disclosure)
munication, it is important to appreciate determines the overall level of involvement in
the how and why of the changing theoreti­ an interaction. As the underlying intimacy in
cal landscape over time. Because newer the­ a relationship increased, for example, from
ories build necessarily on earlier theories initial strangers to acquaintances to good
and on the research stimulated by them, it is friends or lovers, the comfortable level of
useful to consider the course of these devel­ involvement also increased. Over the course
opments. As a participant in these efforts of any specific interaction, there was pres­
over the last 30 years, I cannot claim an sure to maintain a balance, or equilibrium, in
absence of bias regarding the merits of par­ the level of involvement. For example, if a
ticular theories. Nevertheless, perhaps I can stranger approached too closely, one might
provide an insider’s perspective on the evo­ turn away and avoid eye contact. This kind
lution of theories of interactive behavior. of adjustment was termed compensation
because the reduction in gaze compensated
for the too close approach.
♦ Early Theories Equilibrium theory was especially impor­
tant because it was the first attempt to
explain the momentary behavioral adjust­
With few exceptions, the advent of systematic ments that people make over the course of
empirical research on nonverbal communica­ an interaction. Early empirical research not
tion was marked by a focus on one behavior only supported equilibrium theory, but it
or channel at a time (e.g., Exline, 1963; also expanded the range of relevant behav­
Sommer, 1959). For example, researchers iors (for a review, see Patterson, 1973. In
studying spatial behavior did not generally addition to the behaviors Argyle and Dean
concern themselves with the simultaneous (1965) proposed, body orientation, lean,
changes in gaze or facial expressions. touch, posture, and expressiveness also con­
Instead, investigators often examined how tributed to the overall involvement between
culture, gender, personality, or the specific interaction partners (see Andersen, Guerrero,
situation affected the behavior of interest. & Jones, this volume).
02-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 4:00 PM Page 23

The Evolution of Theories of Interactive Behavior–––◆–––23

Over time, however, two distinct limita­ explaining patterns of compensation and
tions to equilibrium theory became evident. reciprocation, important differences were
First, the results of a few studies directly also evident.
contradicted the predictions of equilibrium My own arousal-labeling theory pro­
theory (e.g., Breed, 1972; Chapman, 1975). posed that the experience of arousal in
Instead of compensating for increased response to a change in the partner’s non­
involvement, individuals in these studies verbal behavior precipitated a labeling or
increased, or reciprocated, the higher self-attribution process (Patterson, 1976).
involvement of a partner. Second, the This process was the mechanism at the core
majority of the empirical research support­ of Schachter and Singer’s (1962) two-factor
ing equilibrium theory involved confeder­ theory of emotions. The arousal-labeling
ates who acted in a relatively extreme theory predicted that if the partner’s change
fashion toward their unsuspecting partners in nonverbal involvement (e.g., a close
in settings where the research participants approach, touch, and a high level of gaze)
had little control over their immediate was sufficient to produce arousal, individu­
environments. Examples of this research als initiated the labeling process. Next,
included studies of spatial invasion, staring, if the resulting emotional state was posi­
or the initiation of unexpected touch. tive (e.g., liking, love, comfort), then the
Under these circumstances, it is not surpris­ individual would reciprocate the partner’s
ing that most people compensated. That is, increased involvement. Thus, a close
they left the setting, turned away, or approach, smile, and touch from a good
avoided gaze in response to the confeder­ friend would increase arousal, be labeled as
ate’s increased involvement. This kind of liking, and lead to reciprocating the friend’s
pattern might not be expected between high involvement. This reciprocation might
good friends interacting on their own terri­ take the form of smiling back at the friend
tories. In fact, reciprocation might be more and increasing gaze. If similar behavior
common in interactions between friends, was initiated unexpectedly by a stranger,
family member, or lovers. A different arousal would also be increased, but it
approach was needed to explain both com­ would be more often labeled as discomfort
pensation and reciprocation between and lead to compensation. Thus, the recipi­
strangers and intimate partners. ent might turn away and avoid gaze in
attempting to reestablish some degree of
comfort and control in the setting.
AFFECT-BASED THEORIES Around the same time, Burgoon pro­
posed an expectancy-violations model of
From the mid-1970s to the mid-1980s, a personal space (Burgoon, 1978; Burgoon &
number of different theories were advanced Jones, 1976). Although this theory focused
to explain compensation and reciproca­ originally on the effects of preferred inter­
tion across a wide range of relationships. action distances on communication out­
Because there was research linking arousal comes, such as communicator credibility
to increased levels of nonverbal involve­ and attractiveness (Hale & Burgoon,
ment (e.g., Gale, Lucas, Nissim, & Harpham, 1984), its extension to effects on nonverbal
1972; McBride, King, & James, 1965), involvement was fairly direct. Specifically,
arousal seemed a likely mediator of nonver­ when expectations about preferred levels
bal adjustments. Although several theories of involvement are violated, arousal is
shared a common emphasis on arousal in increased, and a labeling or interpretation
02-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 4:00 PM Page 24

24–––◆–––Foundations

of the arousal is made, as in the arousal- change in nonverbal involvement is the


labeling model. In general, the expectancy- proximate determinant of behavioral adjust­
violations model predicts a compensatory ments. In each case, the labeling or attribu­
adjustment to more extreme violations of tion of one’s arousal is critical. The fourth
expectancies and a reciprocal adjustment affect-based approach, discrepancy-arousal
to low-level violations of expectancies. This theory (Cappella & Greene, 1982), however,
pattern is qualified, however, by the reward proposed a very different explanation.
value of the partner. For example, the same Although Cappella and Greene suggested
moderate level violation of increased involve­ several distinguishing characteristics of their
ment initiated by a high- versus low-valued approach, the one that set it apart from
partner would produce very different other affect-based theories most clearly
adjustments. Specifically, the violation by the was its emphasis on arousal alone as the
high-valued partner would be labeled posi­ critical mediator of nonverbal adjustments.
tively and lead to reciprocation, whereas the Cappella and Greene argued that in the
same violation by the low-valued partner course of interaction, adjustments happen so
would be labeled negatively and lead to quickly that there is literally not enough time
compensation (Hale & Burgoon, 1984). for a labeling, or attribution, process to
Another theory focusing on the central mediate the behavioral changes. In other
role of arousal and how it is labeled was words, behavioral adjustments to a partner’s
the cognitive valence model (Andersen, change in involvement are more rapid than
1985). In this theory, reactions to a the presumed cognitive mediating processes.
partner’s change in nonverbal involvement Consequently, they proposed that arousal
were a product, first, of the intensity of the alone, which could be activated very rapidly,
arousal change and, second, of how moder­ was the critical mediator of nonverbal
ate levels of arousal change might be adjustments (Cappella & Greene, 1982).
labeled. Specifically, if a partner’s behavior According to discrepancy-arousal theory,
precipitated little or no change in one’s as an interaction starts, there is a more or
arousal, no behavioral adjustment (compen­ less automatic comparison between the
sation or reciprocation) was required. In actual and the expected levels of involve­
contrast, if the partner’s behavior precipi­ ment. In general, as the discrepancy between
tated a large increase or decrease in arousal, the expected and the actual level of involve­
then that would be experienced negatively ment increases, so does arousal. A critical
and result in compensation. It was only link in this theory is the relationship between
when the arousal change was in a moderate the intensity of arousal and a person’s result­
range that several valencing factors deter­ ing affect. Specifically, Cappella and Greene
mined the affective experience of an individ­ (1982) suggested that the valence and inten­
ual. Specifically, social norms, relationships, sity of affect follow a curvilinear relation­
perceptions of the partner, the environmen­ ship with arousal. That is, low to moderate
tal context, and other personal characteris­ levels of arousal produce increasingly posi­
tics of the individual shaped the experience tive affect, but as arousal increases beyond
of the arousal. Like the other theories, neg­ moderate levels, affect becomes less positive.
atively labeled or valenced arousal led to With increasingly high levels of arousal,
compensation, and positively labeled or affect becomes increasingly negative. At this
valenced arousal led to reciprocation. point in the sequence, one that happens
These three theories predict that an indi­ very rapidly, the predicted behavioral
vidual’s affective state following a partner’s adjustments parallel those in the other
02-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 4:00 PM Page 25

The Evolution of Theories of Interactive Behavior–––◆–––25

affect-based theories. That is, the greater but the few studies actually employing phys­
the positive affect that one experiences, the iological measures have not demonstrated
greater the reciprocation of the partner’s that arousal is a necessary mediator.
nonverbal involvement, and the greater the Assessing the role of cognitions in this
negative affect that one experiences, the kind of research is difficult because these
greater the compensation for the partner’s measures have to be taken after the interac­
nonverbal involvement. tions. Thus, it is only after the interaction is
Although these four theories propose completed that individuals might rate what
distinct processes mediating behavioral they think about their partners, themselves,
adjustments, it is very difficult to structure a and the interaction. Alternatively, research
critical test of their relative merits (but see participants might list the specific thoughts
Andersen, Guerrero, Buller, & Jorgensen, they recall from the interaction, sometimes
1998). In most cases, the theories make sim­ prompted by a videotape replay of the
ilar predictions for a specific set of circum­ interaction (Ickes, Bissonnette, Garcia, &
stances. For example, each of the theories Stinson, 1990; Patterson, 1983, p. 170).
predicts that substantially increased involve­ Such measures can provide some insight
ment (close approach, sustained gaze, a into what people might have been thinking
smile, and touch) from a disliked other pre­ during the interaction, but it is not the same
cipitates compensation (turning away and as being able to assess those cognitions as
gaze avoidance). Each of the theories also they happen.
predicts that similar increased involvement Assessment is further complicated by the
from a well-liked other precipitates recipro­ fact that reported cognitions and attribu­
cation (increased gaze, a smile, and touch). tions are often the product of the behavior,
Actually measuring the hypothesized not the cause of the behavior (Bem, 1972).
mediating processes (arousal change and Thus, positive ratings of a confederate after
cognitions) as they occur in interactions, an interaction do not necessarily mean that
however, is demanding. Although the mon­ positive cognitions mediated a reciproca­
itoring of physiological arousal in structured tion pattern. Rather, behavioral adjust­
interactions has been done occasionally ments could have happened for other
(e.g., Coutts, Schneider, & Montgomery, reasons and, in turn, precipitated the cogni­
1980; Whitcher & Fisher, 1979), most of tions. That is, the reported evaluations
the research on interactive behavior does might not be present at the time of the
not include physiological measures. In two actual behavior, but when participants are
studies in our own laboratory that did queried, they can provide such judgments
employ physiological measures, we found based on their behavior. Such a sequence
only very limited support for the predicted would be inconsistent with the predic­
increase in arousal following confederates’ tions of these early theories. Of course, the
increased nonverbal involvement toward sequencing issue would not apply to
a subject (Ickes, Patterson, Rajecki, & Cappella and Greene’s (1982) discrepancy-
Tanford, 1982, study 2; Patterson, Jordan, arousal theory, because it excludes the role
Hogan, & Frerker, 1981). In addition, the of cognitions in mediating adjustments
intrusive nature of physiological measures specifically.
also reduces the external validity of the Although these issues are important con­
results. It is probably fair to say that arousal cerns regarding these early theories, there
can mediate nonverbal adjustments, espe­ were more basic limitations to all the theo­
cially when a partner’s behavior is extreme, ries. First, all the theories were reactive in
02-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 4:00 PM Page 26

26–––◆–––Foundations

nature. That is, they provided a kind of ♦ A Functional Perspective


mechanistic explanation for predicting one on Nonverbal Exchange
person’s reaction, given a particular behav­
ior pattern from a partner. Even if the pro­
posed mechanisms captured adequately The basic assumption of the functional
the processes involved in nonverbal adjust­ model is that interactive behavior is prag­
ments to partner’s behavior, they were matic. That is, nonverbal behavior can serve
mute about the initiation of a particular a number of different functions in social set­
interaction. That is, what is the reason tings (Patterson, 1982, 1983). In the pursuit
behind the initial behavior in an interac­ of particular goals, we not only react to our
tion? Furthermore, once started, some partners; we also initiate behavioral patterns
sequences may not actually be reactive any­ to influence them. The pursuit of specific
way. That is, sometimes individuals are not goals may also require behavior inconsistent
simply responding to their partner’s imme­ with its underlying affect, contrary to the
diately preceding behavior; rather, both assumption of the theories reviewed in the
parties are sometimes acting out a common last section. Affect in the functional model
script. An example of this kind of occur­ still provides a critical role in the initiation
rence is the scripted routine in greetings. of, and reaction to, patterns of nonverbal
The second major limitation to all the behavior as a kind of “default” setting
early theories was that they were all affect in interactions. The initiation of particular
driven. Although the theories differed in goals, however, such as gaining compliance
just how individuals arrived at a particular from another person or deceiving someone,
affective state following a partner’s behav­ can override the role of affect in determining
ior, the common prediction across the the­ nonverbal behavior. Of course, this does not
ories was that negative affect (e.g., anxiety mean that these goal-oriented patterns are
or fear) precipitated compensation and pos­ necessarily well done or successful.
itive affect (e.g., liking or love) precipitated Interactive behavior is, however, con­
reciprocation. Common sense and empiri­ strained by several determinants (Patterson,
cal results (Ickes et al., 1982) indicate that 1991). Specifically, as emphasized in this
this is often not the case. For very practical handbook, biology, culture, gender, and per­
reasons, there are times when we cannot let sonality shape habitual patterns of interac­
our feelings determine our behavior. In a tion. The combination of genetic hardwiring,
similar fashion, it may be inappropriate, or the social and cultural environments, and
at least risky, to respond with a high level of experience over time determines our behav­
involvement to someone we like very much. ioral predispositions, physiological reactiv­
In both cases, we manage our behavior to ity to the social environment, and cognitive
create a desirable impression in spite of our expectancies about others. In effect, this is
underlying feelings (for more on impression the “baggage” that each of us brings to
management, see Keating, this volume). social settings and affects both the functions
That is, there is disconnect between what a directing the interaction and the modal
person feels and the person’s overt behav­ patterns of nonverbal involvement shown.
ior. These limitations in the early theories That is, some of what we are as individuals
prompted me to develop a different perspec­ is common in the hardwiring selected over
tive on interactive behavior, one grounded the course of evolution, but culture, gender,
in the functions served by particular behav­ and personality increase variability in the
ior patterns. way we view our social worlds and relate to
02-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 4:00 PM Page 27

The Evolution of Theories of Interactive Behavior–––◆–––27

others (see chapters by Floyd, Hall, Gifford, assessment, individuals may reevaluate the
and Matsumoto, this volume). The pro­ purpose (perceived function) of the interac­
posed linkages among the various determi­ tion as they are also making nonverbal
nants, mediating processes, and interaction adjustments. Over time, these covert and
outcomes can be seen in Figure 2.1. overt adjustments promote stability in the
Because this approach emphasizes the interaction; but if they do not work, an early
functions of interaction and recognizes that termination of the interaction is likely.
affect alone cannot determine particular The functional approach emphasizes the
patterns of nonverbal involvement, the utility of nonverbal communication in serv­
focus moves away from simply predict­ ing several general functions including
ing either compensation or reciprocation. (1) providing information, (2) regulating
Although people sometimes make reactive interaction, (3) expressing intimacy, (4) exer­
adjustments of compensation and recipro­ cising influence, and (5) managing impres­
cation, in the functional approach, individ­ sions (Patterson, 1991). Furthermore, similar
uals are portrayed as more proactive in patterns of behavior may be driven by differ­
initiating specific patterns of behavior in the ent functions. For example, the same close
service of different goals. Because it is inap­ approach, smile, and touch might reflect inti­
propriate to characterize such goal-driven macy or simply be an attempt to manipulate
patterns as simply compensation or recipro­ the partner. Although the functional per­
cation, a different kind of outcome metric spective captures the complex nature of non­
was proposed for the functional model: the verbal communication better than the
stability of nonverbal exchange. affect-based theories, it does come at a cost.
When the perceived function of a given Specifically, the functional model does not
interaction is shared by the partners, interac­ attempt straightforward, directional predic­
tions will tend to proceed in a relatively tions of behavioral adjustments, like those
stable and predictable manner. As partners’ made by the affect-based theories. As a
similarity in culture and personality increases, result, it falls short on an important quality
the probability that expectancies and behav­ of a good theory: being easily testable. On
ioral predispositions will be more compatible the other hand, because individuals can be
also increases. In turn, this increases the like­ proactive in meeting their goals and act inde­
lihood that nonverbal exchange will be more pendently of their underlying feelings, the
stable and predictable. Of course, there task of framing specific predictions will nec­
are exceptions to this generalization. For essarily be difficult. An interesting applica­
example, individuals who are complementary tion and extension of the functional model
on the dominance-submissiveness dimension can be seen in the area of social stigma and
will typically have more stable interac­ intergroup interactions (Hebl & Dovidio,
tions than those who are similar (see 2005; see also Dovidio, Hebl, Richeson, &
Burgoon & Dunbar, this volume, for more Shelton, this volume).
on the dominance-submissiveness dimen­
sion). When individuals have a sense of insta­
bility in the interaction, the model proposes ♦ Interaction Adaptation Theory
that they are likely to experience arousal
change and initiate a cognitive-affective
assessment of the situation (see Figure 2.1, In an ambitious attempt to resolve the incon­
right half). Depending on the level of sistencies between empirical results and
arousal change and the cognitive-affective various theoretical explanations, Burgoon
02-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 4:00 PM Page 28

28–––◆–––Foundations

Determinants Preinteraction
Remote Proximate Mediators Interaction Phase

Arousal
Change

Behavioral
Cognitive-
Predispositions
Affective
Biology Assessment
Culture
Arousal
Gender
Personality
Cognitive-
Affective
Perceived
Situation Expectancies No
Function

Relationship Stable?

Nonverbal Yes
Involvement

Continue to
Termination

Figure 2.1 An Illustration of the Functional Model of Nonverbal Communication

and her colleagues (Burgoon et al., 1998; determine a person’s interaction position
Burgoon, Stern, & Dillman, 1995) pro­ (IP)—that is, the dominant behavioral pre­
posed the interaction adaptation theory disposition likely for a given setting with a
(IAT). In this theory, several basic concepts particular partner. In other words, one’s IP
are proposed as the major determinants of is an estimate of the actor’s likely behavior
behavioral adjustments in interactions. The shaped by biology, experience, individual
first three are the required (R), expected characteristics, and expectancies about a
(E), and desired (D) levels of functionally partner.
driven behavior patterns. The R component The particular valence and level of
refers to biological needs and drives, often involvement represented in a person’s IP
operating outside of awareness, that influ­ are highly variable and dependent on the
ence interactive behavior. The E component weight of the contributing R-E-D compo­
refers to social factors, including knowl­ nents. For example, if a particular interac­
edge of the setting, social norms, and the tion has implications for a person’s safety
partner’s typical behavior in the setting, and welfare, R will influence the final IP. If
that combine to determine behavioral the setting and interaction are constrained
expectancies. The D component refers to a by social norms—for example, in a job
range of individual factors, including per­ interview—the effect of E will be primary in
sonality characteristics, attitudes, and determining IP. Likewise, if the situation is
moods unique to a particular individual. less structured and social norms are mini­
These three factors, in turn, combine to mized, the personality characteristics and
02-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 4:00 PM Page 29

The Evolution of Theories of Interactive Behavior–––◆–––29

momentary affect will result in D being form of matching and reciprocity, which
more important than R and E. Predictions promote coordination and similarity across
about the course of interaction adaptation interactants.
are possible only when the partner’s actual
interactive behavior (A) is known and
compared to the actor’s IP. ♦ Parallel Process Model of
In general, IAT predicts that when A Nonverbal Communication
matches or is only slightly discrepant from
IP, an actor should match or reciprocate
the partner’s behavior. As the discrepancy SETTING THE CONTEXT
between A and IP gets larger, actors are
more likely to engage in cognitive assess­ The theories discussed thus far have
ment and behavioral adjustment. According focused primarily on how individuals
to IAT, there is pressure to minimize the dis­ behave in interactions. Specifically, they
crepancy between A and IP to stabilize the address how we can explain, and poten­
interaction. The predicted behavioral adap­ tially predict, patterns of nonverbal involve­
tation is toward the factor (either A or IP) ment in social settings. Early theories were
that is more positively valenced. For primarily reactive in nature and stressed the
example, suppose the actor expects a high importance of affect in precipitating non­
level of involvement (IP) from a partner, but verbal adjustments. The functional model
the partner initiates a much lower level of and IAT recognized the necessity of trying
involvement (A). In this case, the actor to explain not only reactive adjustments but
should compensate by trying to enlist also behavior initiated by actors. Although
greater involvement from the partner and, individual actors engage necessarily in some
in the process, reduce the discrepancy cognitive activity in the process of manag­
between A and IP. If the actor expects a ing nonverbal involvement, the focus in
lower level of involvement (IP) from the both the earlier and the later theories was
partner, but the partner initiates a much clearly on behavior—that is, the encoding
higher level of involvement (A), then the or sending of nonverbal communication.
actor should converge with or reciprocate Whereas the decoding or receiving of
the partner’s high involvement. In the latter nonverbal behavior had been generally
case, as in the former, the discrepancy neglected in theories of interactive behavior,
between A and IP is reduced with the actor’s the opposite was the case in developing
behavioral adjustment. research and theory in social cognition (see,
In an experiment involving interactions e.g., Fiske & Taylor, 1995; Kunda, 1999).
among same- or cross-culture dyads, mixed This work, conducted primarily by social
support was found for the predictions of psychologists who were part of the “cogni­
interpersonal adaptation theory (Burgoon tive revolution” in psychology, provided a
et al., 1998). In general, partners adapted to new perspective on the old issues of person
one another as a function of their individual perception and social judgment. For
and cultural characteristics as predicted by example, information-processing theories
interpersonal adaptation theory and by (e.g., Brewer, 1988; Fiske & Neuberg, 1990)
other theories reviewed here. Perhaps the focused largely on how a perceiver might
most important contribution of interper­ attend to and process a person’s characteris­
sonal adaptation theory, however, is its tics, appearance, and category membership
emphasis on the pervasive pressure for in forming an impression. These theories
behavioral adjustments, typically in the recognized that these processes sometimes
02-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 4:00 PM Page 30

30–––◆–––Foundations

operate relatively automatically and some­ appearance) to influence others is adaptive,


times require effort (see Lakin, this volume). attending selectively to and processing non­
A similar view of combined automatic and verbal information from others is also
effortful (controlled) processes was pivotal adaptive. Although much of our sending
in a theory of how perceivers move from and receiving is relatively automatic, not
behavioral observations to attributions all of it is, and changing circumstances
(Gilbert & Krull, 1988; Gilbert, Pelham, & can require considerable cognitive effort in
Krull, 1988). Specifically, Gilbert and his col­ social settings. The parallel process model
leagues proposed that the first two stages of frames the encoding and decoding processes
judgment, categorizing the behavior (e.g., of nonverbal communication in a single
friendly behavior) and next drawing a dispo­ system, driven by a common set of determi­
sitional inference (e.g., friendly person), hap­ nants and mediating processes. The next
pen more or less automatically with little or section discusses the basic structure of the
no cognitive effort. Perceivers might, how­ model and Figure 2.2 provides an illustra­
ever, initiate an additional correction stage if tion of the linkages among the determinants
they are sufficiently motivated and have the and processes in the model.
cognitive resources necessary to do the cor­
rection (Gilbert & Krull; Gilbert et al.).
Around the same time, Bargh (1989, 1990) OVERVIEW OF THE PARALLEL
was making a strong case for automatic PROCESS MODEL
social judgments being the norm, not the
exception, in forming impressions. Such Determinants. The determinants in Figure
automatic judgments were not, however, 2.2 (left side) identify the most important,
without purpose. For example, Fiske (1992) though not the only, factors affecting
emphasized the pragmatic link of social cog­ the sending and receiving of nonverbal
nition to interactive behavior in a reprise of communication. These determinants con­
William James’s (1890/1983) observation strain our habitual ways of communicat­
from a century earlier that “thinking is for ing. That is, the effects of biology, culture,
doing” (pp. 959–960). In a similar fashion, gender, and personality predispose us to
the ecological theory of social perception communicate in a relatively consistent
(McArthur & Baron, 1983) proposed that fashion over time. Biology also reflects the
people are attuned particularly to perceiving role of evolutionary pressures in shaping
the social affordances of others (e.g., Is this adaptive, hardwired patterns of communi­
person good or bad for me?) quickly and cating with others (see Buck & Renfro
accurately. Powers, this volume, this volume; Floyd,
Although these researchers did not frame this volume). For example, the positive,
their mechanisms as part of a “communica­ nurturing response to the baby-face
tion” process, they were actually addressing appearance of infants is advantageous to
the receiving side of communication. The their survival (Zebrowitz, 1997, chap. 4).
parallel process model is an attempt to inte­ Special sensitivity to facial expressions as
grate the two sides of communication— signals of interpersonal intent may also be
social behavior and social judgment—into the product of natural selection (Fridlund,
a single framework. Communicators send 1994; Fridlund & Russell, this volume).
and receive nonverbal messages simultane­ Although natural selection has left us
ously in the service of specific goals. Just with some common, adaptive patterns
as changing our behavior (and often our of communication, culture, gender, and
02-Manusov.qxd
6/30/2006
Social Cognition
Social Cognitive-Affective and Behavioral
Determinants Environment Mediators Processes

4:00 PM
Partner Social

Page 31
Judgments
Interpersonal Attentional
Expectancies Focus

Affect
Biology Cognitive
Culture Resources
Gender
Personality
Goals

Cognitive
Effort
Dispositions

Action Actor
Setting Schemas Behavior

Figure 2.2 An Illustration of the Parallel Process Model of Nonverbal Communication



31
02-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 4:00 PM Page 32

32–––◆–––Foundations

personality introduce increased variability context for interaction, but the cognitive-
in communication. For example, even affective mediators are the processes that
though there is some degree of universality guide the course of communication. Inter­
in expressive reactions, differences across personal expectancies affect the social judg­
culture are also evident (Elfenbein & ment and behavioral processes in nonverbal
Ambady, 2002; Russell, 1994; see also communication simultaneously. For example,
Matsumoto, this volume). Next, the effect expectancies can create a self-fulfilling
of gender might be seen as the joint product prophecy in which actors’ own behavior
of biology (the hardwired patterns) and cul­ facilitates the behavior expected of a partner,
ture (societal norms) in shaping patterns of without the actors’ awareness of their role in
nonverbal communication (see Hall, this the process (Rosenthal, 1974). Nevertheless,
volume). Finally, individual differences in a partner’s subtle appearance cues or behav­
personality also contribute to contrasting ior can also signal underlying dispositions
styles of nonverbal communication (see (Jussim, 1991; Zebrowitz & Collins, 1997),
Gifford, this volume). Thus, the combined resulting in an accurate judgment and not a
effects of the determinants produce both self-fulfilling prophecy. Affect is a product of
basic commonalities and differences in non­ an individual’s momentary disposition and
verbal communication. goals, his or her relationship to the partner,
and the setting constraints. Affect can influ­
Social Environment. Because the determi­ ence both the formation of social judgments
nants also affect our choices of social envi­ (e.g., Alloy & Abramson, 1988) and the pat­
ronments, they have another, indirect terns of nonverbal involvement, as seen in
influence on nonverbal communication, as the early theories reviewed in this chapter.
seen in the second stage of the model. Dispositions refer to actor states precipitated
Interactions occur with specific partners in a specific social environment. The more
in particular social settings. Because we obvious dispositions are linked to actors’
interact differently with different people and personality characteristics (see Gifford, this
in different settings, the social environment volume). For example, the experience of
constrains our patterns of nonverbal social anxiety in a particular interaction can
communication. Just as we select settings, so lead to decreased involvement (larger inter­
do settings select us: I like to play golf, but personal distances and decreased gaze) and
the exclusive country clubs in St. Louis have can affect social judgments adversely
little interest in having me as a member, (Patterson & Ritts, 1997). Goals may be the
even if I could afford to join them. The most important of the mediators because
combined effect of self- and setting-selection they are the cognitive representations of
processes results in greater homogeneity desired states for which people strive (Berger,
among people in most settings (Barker, Knowlton, & Abrahams, 1996). Further­
1968; Wicker, 1979). In turn, this increased more, goal-directed behavior, and even the
similarity among people in specific settings goals themselves, can be activated automati­
not only facilitates greater accuracy in mak­ cally (Bargh & Chartrand, 1999).
ing social judgments of others (Funder, The final mediator in the model, cogni­
1987; Swann, 1984) but also facilitates tive resources, refers to the total cognitive
behavioral coordination in interactions. capacity available for managing our every­
day activities. In social interactions, cogni­
Cognitive-Affective Mediators. The determi­ tive resources may be focused on a wide
nants and the social environment set the variety of concerns. For example, people
02-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 4:00 PM Page 33

The Evolution of Theories of Interactive Behavior–––◆–––33

might be preoccupied with personal DYNAMICS OF PARALLEL


problems, financial difficulties, or looming PROCESSING
deadlines even as they are having a conver­
sation at work. Because the total pool of Goals. A basic assumption underlying the
cognitive resources is limited, the investment parallel process model, and one consistent
of substantial resources in matters outside of with the earlier functional model, is that
the interaction necessarily means that there communication is adaptive and goal ori­
is less that can be applied to the sending and ented. In social settings, people read their
receiving of nonverbal messages. In addi­ social environments (decoding) and send
tion, whatever resources are committed to nonverbal messages (encoding) simultane­
the immediate social situation can be vari­ ously to others around them in the pursuit
ously distributed toward the self, the part­ of particular goals. Although specific goals
ner, the setting, or the topic of conversation. guide the operation of the parallel encoding
and decoding processes, this does not mean
Social Judgment and Behavioral Processes. that people must be consciously aware of
The interaction processes are in the final the goals they are pursuing. Sometimes
stage of the model, with the social judg­ goals are triggered automatically and out­
ments represented in the top of Figure 2.2 side of awareness by the social environment
and behaviors in the bottom half. Consistent (Bargh, 1997).
with a functional approach, both social The relationship between the social judg­
judgments and behaviors operate in concert ment and behavioral processes in securing a
for a common goal. Although each “track” particular goal is a complex one. Although it
can engage substantial cognitive effort, is assumed commonly that an actor’s social
they typically operate on automatic. For judgments at Time 1 direct the actor’s behav­
example, on the social judgment “side,” ior at Time 2, specific goals can alter this
simply noticing an outgroup person may be sequence. Sometimes behavioral strategies
sufficient to activate a stereotypic judgment may be initiated to test social judgments.
(Bargh, 1989). On the behavioral “side,” That is, an individual might “float a trial bal­
the cognitive representation of a particular loon” to get a reading of a partner’s senti­
goal, such as trying to impress another ment on a particular issue without making
person, can be sufficient to trigger an auto­ a direct inquiry. For example, in dating
matic behavioral script. A particular goal, relationships, one person might escalate
however, not only directs an actor’s behav­ behavioral intimacy to determine the part­
ior but also directs the kinds of judgments ner’s readiness for a romantic relationship.
made about the partner. For example, an Another behavioral strategy might be play­
actor trying to make a positive impression ing hard to get as a means of testing a part­
is more focused on metaperspective judg­ ner’s interest and commitment. In addition,
ments (e.g., What does she think of me?) actors’ scripted behavioral routines may
than on direct perspective judgments (e.g., operate independently of their social judg­
What kind of person is she?). Thus, social ments of their partners (e.g., being pleasant
judgment and behavioral processes operate to the disliked boss), consistent with a func­
typically on automatic as they complement tional view of nonverbal communication.
each other in the pursuit of particular goals. Although the specific goals that drive
The next section takes a closer look at nonverbal communication can vary widely,
the conditional links among selected most people are also constrained by two
component processes in this model. broader metagoals (Berger, 1997, chap. 2).
02-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 4:00 PM Page 34

34–––◆–––Foundations

First, we tend to pursue our communication approach and avoidance that have undoubt­
goals in an efficient manner, minimizing edly been selected over the course of evolu­
effort. Fiske and Taylor (1995, chaps. 4–7) tion. These would include expressive
characterize perceivers as “cognitive misers” reactions that signal a person’s intended course
as they make judgments of others. Second, of action (Fridlund, 1994; see also Fridlund
most people employ the behavioral strate­ & Russell, this volume). Automatic patterns
gies that are appropriate and follow social of increased involvement (e.g., close approach,
norms and customs (Berger, 1997, chap. 2). gaze, and touch) might be activated in
Thus, people typically take the path of least response to increased attraction or a need for
resistance and avoid calling undue negative comforting and supporting another person.
attention to themselves. In contrast, decreased involvement or behav­
ioral avoidance may be precipitated by dis­
Social Judgments. In general, research indi­ like, fear, or embarrassment. Besides the
cates that most initial social judgments hap­ hardwired, affect-driven patterns, other
pen more or less automatically, often outside patterns become automatic over time as a
of conscious awareness (e.g., Bargh, 1994; function of experience and learning. For
Brewer, 1988; see also Lakin, this volume). example, most of us learn over time how to
When the information about others (i.e., “make a good impression” when there is a
appearance and behavior) is ambiguous or lot at stake. The cognitive representations of
inconsistent, considerable cognitive effort these automatic sequences may be described
might be engaged in resolving a final judg­ as action schemata (see Figure 2.2, bottom)
ment, but only if the perceiver is motivated and can be initiated with little or no cogni­
to do so (Gilbert et al., 1988). Nevertheless, tive effort (Abelson, 1981; Vallacher &
more is not always better when it comes to Wegner, 1987).
cognitive effort in making judgments. Rapid Another way to conceptualize the
judgments from “thin slices of behavior” dynamics of behavioral processes is in terms
are, more often than not, accurate (Ambady of potentially competing response systems—
& Rosenthal, 1992), and increased cogni­ that is, automatic versus controlled. Metcalfe
tive effort can even lead to more errors in and Mischel (1999) proposed such an
judgment (Patterson & Stockbridge, 1998; approach in their “hot/cool system analysis”
Wilson & Schooler, 1991). When additional of the conflict involved in the delay of grat­
reflection is needed in making a judgment, it ification. The “hot” response is the auto­
is possible only when there are sufficient cog­ matic approach to immediate gratification,
nitive resources available to the individual. which is initially under stimulus control
If a person is distracted, worried, tired, or (Metcalfe & Mischel, 1999), like Bargh’s
investing considerable effort in managing (1997) automatic actions. The hot system
behavior, then corrections to an initial judg­ develops early and is simple, reflexive, and
ment are unlikely. Thus, the initial automatic emotional in nature, like the affect-driven
judgment will dominate. reactions discussed in the early theories in
this chapter. In contrast, the “cool”
Social Behavior. Sending nonverbal commu­ response is a product of self-control. The
nication, like receiving it, engages a variety cool system develops later and is more com­
of processes (from relatively automatic to plex, reflective, and cognitive in nature.
more controlled). On the automatic end, Strack and Deutsch (2004) proposed a
our behavioral repertoire encompasses a model similar to the hot/cool system that
wide range of basic, hardwired patterns of engages both reflective and impulsive
02-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 4:00 PM Page 35

The Evolution of Theories of Interactive Behavior–––◆–––35

processes as determinants of social behavior. Although the application of cognitive


Specifically, the reflective system is based on resources in reconsidering a faulty judg­
knowledge about facts and values, whereas ment, or in monitoring and managing
the impulsive system is based on associative behavior, may be effective, there is no guar­
links and motivational orientations (Strack antee that this will be the case. Additional
& Deutsch, 2004). The predictions of both adjustments may be required or individuals
theories are consistent with the dynamics may simply terminate the interaction.
of the parallel process model—that is, stress Across interactions, the residual and cumu­
and the lack of cognitive resources increases lative effects of previous encounters shape
the probability of automatic actions and subsequent goals, expectancies, affect, and
decreases the probability of controlled or dispositions.
effortful actions.

Coordinating Parallel Processes. The ♦ Conclusion


dynamic relationship between the parallel
social judgment and behavior processes is
constrained first by the influence of the This chapter has traced the evolution of
determinants (biology, culture, gender, and interaction theories from Argyle and Dean’s
personality) and the social environment (see (1965) equilibrium theory to the parallel
Figure 2.2). Thus, we all come into particu­ process model of nonverbal communication
lar settings with some stable tendencies in (Patterson, 2001). As someone invested in
social judgments and social behavior. these developments for more than three
Automaticity in social judgments and behav­ decades, it seems to me that there are some
ior usually works well enough in navigating discernible trends over time. The early theo­
our social environments (Bargh, 1997), but it ries were reactive in nature, explaining and
also provides another advantage: cognitive predicting behavioral adjustments given a
efficiency. Automatic processes do not, how­ partner’s initial behavior. These theories
ever, always work. When more controlled differed in terms of the specific mediating
judgments and behavior are required, an processes, but they all emphasized a
individual needs to have the available cogni­ person’s affective reaction as the proximate
tive resources and be motivated to apply determinant of compensatory or reciprocal
those resources. If a person is stressed, cog­ adjustments to a partner’s nonverbal behav­
nitive resources are minimal, and controlled ior. Although the early theories dealt only
judgments and behaviors have low strength, with reactive adjustments and did not
then automatic judgments and behaviors will appreciate that strategic adjustments may
still tend to dominate. well be inconsistent with the underlying
What happens when we fail to achieve affect, they did offer specific, testable
our specific goals? If we are sufficiently predictions.
motivated, the feedback process can lead to In contrast, the functional model
adjustments in expectancies, affect, disposi­ (Patterson, 1982) and interpersonal adapta­
tions, and even the goals themselves (see tion theory (Burgoon et al., 1995) moved
Figure 2.2). Unless appropriate automatic away from simple reactive processes and
adjustments are accessible, the subsequent emphasized that nonverbal adjustments are
recycling through the parallel processes adaptive. That is, nonverbal patterns are
requires additional resources and effort shaped by different functions in different
to activate more controlled processes. settings. Thus, people are agents not only
02-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 4:00 PM Page 36

36–––◆–––Foundations

reacting to others but also initiating behav­ ♦ References


ior with specific ends in mind. Both theories
also recognized that determinants such as
Abelson, R. P. (1981). The psychological status
biology, culture, gender, and personality
of the script concept. American Psychologist,
shaped the course of interactive behavior. 36, 715–729.
Although these theories painted a more Alloy, L. B., & Abramson, L. Y. (1988).
representative picture of the complexity Depressive realism: Four theoretical per­
of interpersonal behavior, they did so at the spectives. In L. B. Alloy (Ed.), Cognitive
expense of specific, testable predictions, processes in depression (pp. 223–265).
characteristic of the earlier theories. New York: Guilford Press.
Around the same time—that is, during the Ambady, N., & Rosenthal, R. (1992). Thin
1980s and 1990s—the burgeoning research slices of behavior as predictors of inter­
in social cognition provided new and inter­ personal consequences: A meta-analysis.
esting insights into the dynamics of social Psychological Bulletin, 111, 256–274.
Andersen, P. A. (1985). Nonverbal immediacy
judgments. Although their perspective was
in interpersonal communication. In A. W.
different, social cognition researchers were
Siegman & S. Feldstein (Eds.), Multichannel
actually studying the decoding or receiving integrations of nonverbal behavior (pp. 1–
side of nonverbal communication. 36). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
I developed the parallel process model Andersen, P. A., Guerrero, L. K., Buller, D. B., &
as a means of integrating two separate Jorgensen, P. F. (1998). An empirical com­
research paradigms—one focusing primar­ parison of three theories of nonverbal imme­
ily on the behavioral processes and the diacy exchange. Human Communication
other on the social judgment processes of Research, 24, 501–535.
nonverbal communication—into a unified Argyle, M., & Dean, J. (1965). Eye-contact,
theoretical framework (Patterson, 2001). distance and affiliation. Sociometry, 28,
Consistent with the increased appreciation 289–304.
Bargh, J. A. (1989). Conditional automaticity:
of automatic behavioral and social judg­
Varieties of automatic influence in social
ment processes (Bargh, 1997), the parallel
perception and cognition. In J. S. Uleman
process model emphasizes the efficiency & J. A. Bargh (Eds.), Unintended thought
and utility of automatic processes in negoti­ (pp. 3–51). New York: Guilford Press.
ating our social environments. Some inter­ Bargh, J. A. (1990). Auto-motives: Preconscious
actions, however, require the initiation of determinants of thought and behavior. In
controlled processes, possible only when E. T. Higgins & R. M. Sorrentino (Eds.),
there are adequate cognitive resources Handbook of motivation and cognition
and the motivation to apply them. Thus, (2nd ed., pp. 93–130). New York: Guilford
the actor in the parallel process model Press.
maintains a delicate balance between Bargh, J. A. (1994). The four horsemen of auto­
behavioral and social judgment processes, maticity: Awareness, intention, efficiency,
and control in social cognition. In R. S. Wyer
typically operating on automatic, in the
& T. K. Srull (Eds.), Handbook of social
service of specific goals. I think that the
cognition (2nd ed., pp. 1–40). Hillsdale, NJ:
dynamics of the parallel process model are Erlbaum.
a closer approximation of the complexity of Bargh, J. A. (1997). The automaticity of every­
interactive behavior than those in previous day life. In R. S. Wyer (Ed.), Advances in
theories, but I have no illusion that this is social cognition (pp. 1–61). Mahwah, NJ:
the final word on the topic. Erlbaum.
02-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 4:00 PM Page 37

The Evolution of Theories of Interactive Behavior–––◆–––37

Bargh, J. A., & Chartrand, T. L. (1999). The Chapman, A. J. (1975). Eye contact, physical
unbearable automaticity of being. American proximity and laughter: A reexamination
Psychologist, 54, 462–479. of the equilibrium model of social intimacy.
Barker, R. G. (1968). Ecological psychology: Social Behavior and Personality, 3, 143–155.
Concepts and methods for studying the Coutts, L. M., Schneider, F. W., & Montgomery,
environment of human behavior. Stanford, S. (1980). An investigation of the arousal
CA: Stanford University Press. model of interpersonal intimacy. Journal
Bem, D. J. (1972). Self-perception theory. In of Experimental Social Psychology, 16,
L. Berkowitz (Ed.), Advances in experimen­ 545–561.
tal social psychology (Vol. 6, pp. 1–62). Elfenbein, H. A., & Ambady, N. (2002). On
New York: Academic Press. the universality and cultural specificity
Berger, C. B. (1997). Planning strategic interac­ of emotion recognition: A meta-analysis.
tion. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. Psychological Bulletin, 128, 203–235.
Berger, C. B., Knowlton, S. W., & Abrahams, Exline, R. V. (1963). Explorations in the process
M. F. (1996). The hierarchy principle in of person perception: Visual interaction in
strategic communication. Communication relation to competition, sex, and need for
Theory, 6, 111–142. affiliation. Journal of Personality, 31, 1–20.
Breed, G. (1972). The effect of intimacy: Fiske, S. T. (1992). Thinking is for doing:
Reciprocity or retreat? British Journal Portraits of social cognition from daguerreo­
of Social and Clinical Psychology, 11, type to laserphoto. Journal of Personality
135–142. and Social Psychology, 63, 877–889.
Brewer, M. B. (1988). A dual process model Fiske, S. T., & Neuberg, S. L. (1990). A contin­
of impression formation. In T. K. Srull & uum of impression formation, from cate­
R. S. Wyer Jr. (Eds.) Advances in social gory-based to individuating processes:
cognition (Vol. 1, pp. 1–36). Hillsdale, NJ: Influences of information and motiva­
Erlbaum. tion on attention and interpretation. In
Burgoon, J. K. (1978). A communication model M. Zanna (Ed.), Advances in experimental
of personal space violations: Explication social psychology (Vol. 23, pp. 1–74). New
and an initial test. Human Communication York: Academic Press.
Research, 4, 129–142. Fiske, S. T., & Taylor, S. E. (1995). Social cog­
Burgoon, J. K., Ebesu, A. S., White, C. H., Koch, nition (2nd ed.). New York: McGraw-Hill.
P., Alvaro, E. M., & Kikuchi, T. (1998). Fridlund, A. J. (1994). Human facial expression: An
The many faces of interaction adaptation. evolutionary view. San Diego: Academic Press.
In M. T. Palmer & G. A. Barnett (Eds.), Funder, D. C. (1987). Errors and mistakes:
Progress in communication sciences (Vol. Evaluating the accuracy of social judgment.
14, pp. 191–220). Stamford, CT: Ablex. Psychological Bulletin, 101, 75–90.
Burgoon, J. K., & Jones, S. B. (1976). Toward Gale, A., Lucas, B., Nissim, R., & Harpham, B.
a theory of personal space expectations (1972). Some EEG correlates of face-to­
and their violations. Human Communica­ face contact. British Journal of Social and
tion Research, 2, 131–146. Clinical Psychology, 11, 326–332.
Burgoon, J. K., Stern, L. A., & Dillman, L. Gilbert, D. T., & Krull, D. S. (1988). Seeing less
(1995). Interpersonal adaptation: Dyadic and knowing more: The benefits of percep­
interaction patterns. Cambridge, UK: tual ignorance. Journal of Personality and
Cambridge University Press. Social Psychology, 54, 193–202.
Cappella, J. N., & Greene, J. O. (1982). A Gilbert, D. T., Pelham, B. W., & Krull, D. S.
discrepancy-arousal explanation of mutual (1988). On cognitive busyness: When person
influence in expressive behavior for adult perceivers meet persons perceived. Journal
and infant-adult interaction. Communication of Personality and Social Psychology, 54,
Monographs, 49, 89–114. 733–740.
02-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 4:00 PM Page 38

38–––◆–––Foundations

Hale, J. L., & Burgoon, J. K. (1984). Models of Patterson, M. L. (1991). A functional approach
reactions to changes in nonverbal immediacy. to nonverbal exchange. In R. S. Feldman &
Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 8, 287–314. B. Rime (Eds.), Fundamentals of nonverbal
Hebl, M. R., & Dovidio, J. F. (2005). Promoting behavior (pp. 458–495). Cambridge, UK:
the “social” in the examination of social Cambridge University Press.
stigmas. Personality and Social Psychology Patterson, M. L. (2001). Toward a comprehen­
Review, 9, 156–182. sive model of nonverbal communication. In
Ickes, W., Bissonnette, V., Garcia, S., & Stinson, W. P. Robinson & H. Giles (Eds.), The new
L. L. (1990). Implementing and using the handbook of language and social psychol­
dyadic interaction paradigm. In C. Hendrick ogy (pp. 159–176). Chichester, UK: Wiley.
& M. S. Clark (Eds.), Research methods in Patterson, M. L., Jordan, A., Hogan, M. B., &
personality and social psychology (pp. Frerker, D. (1981). Effects of nonverbal
16–44). Newbury Park, CA: Sage. intimacy on arousal and behavioral adjust­
Ickes, W., Patterson, M. L., Rajecki, D. W., & ment. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 5,
Tanford, S. (1982). Behavioral and cogni­ 184–198.
tive consequences of reciprocal versus Patterson, M. L., & Ritts, V. (1997). Social and
compensatory responses to preinteraction communicative anxiety: A review and meta­
expectancies. Social Cognition, 1, 160–190. analysis. In B. R. Burleson (Ed.), Com­
James, W. (1983). The principles of psychology. munication yearbook 20 (pp. 262–303).
Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
(Original work published 1890) Patterson, M. L., & Stockbridge, E. (1998). Effects
Jussim, L. (1991). Social perception and social of cognitive demand and judgment strategy
reality: A reflection-construction model. on person perception accuracy. Journal of
Psychological Review, 98, 54–73. Nonverbal Behavior, 22, 253–263.
Kunda, Z. (1999). Social cognition: Making sense Rosenthal, R. (1974). On the social psychology of
of people. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. the self-fulfilling prophecy: Further evidence
McArthur, L. Z., & Baron, R. M. (1983). for Pygmalion effects and their mediating
Toward an ecological theory of social mechanisms. New York: M.S.S. Information
perception. Psychological Review, 90, Corporation Modular Publication.
215–238. Russell, J. A. (1994). Is there a universal recog­
McBride, G., King, M. C., & James, J. W. nition of emotion from facial expression?
(1965). Social proximity effects of galvanic A review of the cross-cultural studies.
skin responses in adult humans. Journal Psychological Bulletin, 115, 102–141.
of Psychology, 61, 153–157. Schachter, S., & Singer, J. E. (1962). Cognitive,
Metcalfe, J., & Mischel, W. (1999). A hot/cool social, and physiological determinants of
system analysis of delay of gratification: emotional state. Psychological Review,
Dynamics of willpower. Psychological 69, 379–399.
Review, 106, 3–19. Sommer, R. (1959). Studies in personal space.
Patterson, M. L. (1973). Compensation in non­ Sociometry, 22, 247–260.
verbal immediacy behaviors: A review. Strack, F., & Deutsch, R. (2004). Reflective and
Sociometry, 36, 237–252. impulsive determinants of social behavior.
Patterson, M. L. (1976). An arousal model Personality and Social Psychology Review,
of interpersonal intimacy. Psychological 8, 220–247.
Review, 83, 235–245. Swann, W. B., Jr. (1984). Quest for accuracy in
Patterson, M. L. (1982). A sequential functional person perception: A matter of pragmatics.
model of nonverbal exchange. Psycholo­ Psychological Review, 91, 457–477.
gical Review, 89, 231–249. Vallacher, R. R., & Wegner, D. M. (1987).
Patterson, M. L. (1983). Nonverbal behavior: What do people think they’re doing?
A functional perspective. New York: Action identification and human behavior.
Springer-Verlag. Psychological Review, 94, 3–15.
02-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 4:00 PM Page 39

The Evolution of Theories of Interactive Behavior–––◆–––39

Whitcher, S. J., & Fisher, J. D. (1979). decisions. Journal of Personality and Social
Mulitdimensional reactions to therapeutic Psychology, 60, 181–192.
touch in a hospital setting. Journal of Zebrowitz, L. A. (1997). Reading faces: Window
Personality and Social Psychology, 37, 87–96. to the soul? Boulder, CO: Westview Press.
Wicker, A. W. (1979). An introduction to Zebrowitz, L. A., & Collins, M. A. (1997).
ecological psychology. Monterey, CA: Accurate social perception at zero acquain­
Brooks/ Cole. tance: The affordances of a Gibsonian
Wilson, T. D., & Schooler, J. W. (1991). approach. Personality and Social Psychology
Thinking too much: Introspection can Review, 1, 204–223.
reduce the quality of preferences and
02-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 4:00 PM Page 40
03-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 4:01 PM Page 41

3
METHODS FOR THE STUDY OF
NONVERBAL COMMUNICATION

� Heather M. Gray
Harvard University

� Nalini Ambady
Tufts University

A glance at the chapters in this volume reveals the wide range of


questions researchers are asking about nonverbal communica­
tion. Whereas many are interested in how the nonverbal communica­
tion system has been shaped by biological, evolutionary, and cultural
forces, others focus on the influence of the modern environment.
Whereas some are curious about the social consequences of nonverbal
displays, others attempt to catalog the constructs conveyed through
facial expressions, bodily movements, vocal cues, and other ways of
communicating without words. We can begin to understand these issues
thanks in part to methods that capitalize on the widespread availability
of visual and auditory media. The purpose of this chapter is to provide
a survey of some of these methods.
The breadth of research questions about nonverbal communication
is due in large part to the fact that nonverbal displays can be conceptu­
alized as both independent and dependent variables. They are used as

◆ 41
03-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 4:01 PM Page 42

42–––◆–––Foundations

independent variables by researchers who questions that can be addressed and high­
want to understand the cognitive, affective, light some issues that await further explo­
or behavioral consequences of nonverbal ration. We then discuss major design
displays. For instance, to understand how considerations and the relative merits and
first impressions regulate interpersonal limitations of each approach.
processes, researchers can present nonverbal
cues and measure subsequent attitudes and
decisions (e.g., Ambady & Rosenthal, 1993; ♦ Nonverbal Behavior as an
Harris & Rosenthal, 2005; Zebrowitz & Independent Variable
Rhodes, 2004). Nonverbal cues are viewed
as dependent variables when the cue is
manipulated to infer subsequent cognitive We begin this section with a summary of
or affective changes by observing nonver­ the ways in which nonverbal behavior can
bal behavior (e.g., Dimberg, Thunberg, & be used as an independent variable, with
Elmehed, 2000; Richeson & Shelton, a focus on its widespread application of
2005). At other times, nonverbal displays photographs, vocal clips, video clips, and
are studied as both independent and depen­ interaction paradigms to the study of inter­
dent variables, such as when researchers personal communication.
assess emotional responses to emotional dis­
plays or investigate behaviors co-occurring PHOTOGRAPHS
during interaction between people (see, e.g.,
Bavelas & Chovil, this volume). The survey Some Appropriate Uses. Still photographs
of methods that follows is organized accord­ can be used to study a wide range of
ing to these two primary design considera­ processes, including the perception of fleet­
tions (i.e., nonverbal cues as independent ing states and enduring traits. One particu­
and dependent variables). larly active line of research asks whether
Although many researchers who focus emotions are recognized universally in
on interaction would not define their work static faces. The first studies to explore
according to these criteria but, rather, this question revealed that the six “basic”
define nonverbal communication within a emotions (anger, disgust, fear, happiness,
larger set of interactional processes (e.g., sadness, and surprise) are recognized at
adaptation), we believe that the current above-chance levels across cultures (e.g.,
approach can encapsulate some of what Ekman, 1972); in these studies, participants
occurs in those studies as well. Following viewed posed expressions and were asked to
this contention, we first review some ways identify the emotion being expressed from
in which nonverbal behavior is used as a list of predetermined labels. Recognition
an independent variable; such methods are of the intended category at accuracy rates
useful in uncovering the perceptual, affec­ greater than that expected by chance pro­
tive, and cognitive consequences of behav­ vided evidence for the universality of emo­
ior communicated in a range of nonverbal tion recognition.
channels. Next, we describe the ways in Current work extends this initial inquiry
which nonverbal behavior is assessed as a by focusing on a number of questions that
dependent variable, organized according to may have implications for our understand­
the channel of communication used to infer ing of the form and function of emotional
the underlying subjective experience. For displays: What are the social functions
each method within these larger areas, we of these displays (Marsh, Adams, & Kleck,
provide examples of the kinds of research 2004), and do more “self-conscious”
03-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 4:01 PM Page 43

Methods for the Study of Nonverbal Communication–––◆–––43

emotions—embarrassment, shame, and to study how aspects of identity, such as


pride—also have universally recognizable gender, age, and ethnicity, are perceived
displays (Tracy & Robins, 2004)? Although and interpreted. Social psychologists, for
even the more basic question of universality instance, have learned a great deal about
in recognition is hotly debated (Ekman, prejudice and stereotypes by measuring
1994; Russell, 1994), some new evidence cognitive and affective responses to still
suggests that although emotional displays photographs representing people of differ­
can be recognized across cultures, they may ent ethnic groups (e.g., Greenwald, Nosek,
be better understood by members of the & Banaji, 2003). Static facial cues also
encoder’s own national, ethnic, or regional form, albeit inaccurately, the basis for
group (Elfenbein & Ambady, 2002). impressions of enduring psychological
Other scholars attempt to understand traits. For instance, considerable research
how the ability to recognize emotions is demonstrates that people who have facial
related to long-term psychological adjust­ features that resemble those of infants (e.g.,
ment. Results demonstrate consistently that large eyes, round faces, small chins) are
the ability to recognize emotion from static perceived to have childlike traits, such as
faces is linked with both personal and social submissiveness, dependence, honesty, and
adjustment (e.g., Nowicki & Duke, 1994). weakness (for a review, see Zebrowitz, 1997).
Several standardized sets of emotional faces
are useful in addressing these questions, Design Considerations. Although the basic
particularly the Diagnostic Analysis of procedure for testing many of these phe­
Nonverbal Accuracy (DANVA; Nowicki nomena involves a relatively invariable set
& Duke, 1994). The DANVA contains 24 of steps, the researcher is faced with impor­
emotional expressions (six each of anger, tant decisions at nearly every stage. The
fear, happiness, and sadness), and it has been first decision to be made is whether to con­
validated extensively and is used widely. An struct a new set of stimuli or to use a pre­
updated version, the DANVA-2, includes existing set. Some sets have been developed
stimuli that vary in their intensity and rep­ primarily for the study of individual differ­
resent encoders of different racial groups ences in emotion recognition (e.g., Baum
(Baum & Nowicki, 1998). & Nowicki, 1998; Buck, 2005), and others
Still other researchers use photographs to are useful in exploring cultural factors in
gauge people’s ability to understand others’ this process (e.g., Ekman & Friesen, 1976;
mental lives. For example, Baron-Cohen Matsumoto & Ekman, 1988). Preexisting
and his colleagues have devised a task con­ sets involve less work because they have
sisting of black-and-white photographs of already been validated and include norma­
the eye region of faces (the “Reading the tive data against which special populations
Mind in the Eyes” test; Baron-Cohen, can be compared. On the other hand, these
Wheelwright, & Hill, 2001). These stimuli sets provide less control over important
have been used to investigate the extent to issues such as the states being expressed and
which people with autism or Asperger’s syn­ the selection of encoders and may not meet
drome show a selective impairment in the the needs of a particular research question.
ability to use nonverbal behavior to infer If the decision is made to construct a new
mental states (Baron-Cohen et al., 2001). set of stimuli, a number of additional issues
In addition to using photographs to study arise. For research on the recognition of
the communication of relatively short-lived emotions, as an example, should the expres­
characteristics, particularly emotional and sions be spontaneous, induced, or posed?
mental states, photography can also be used Spontaneous expressions can be sampled
03-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 4:01 PM Page 44

44–––◆–––Foundations

from a wide range of naturalistic situations; there are at least three ways to define accu­
however, they provide little control over the racy in this measurement context: (a) the
qualities being communicated, the encoders, degree of correspondence between a judg­
and the quality of the stimuli. Another, more ment and a criterion, (b) interpersonal con­
controllable, option involves inducing emo­ sensus, and (c) the degree of a judgment’s
tions in encoders. Like spontaneous displays, utility to the perceiver. Many researchers
induced displays have the advantage of being choose the first definition, using the display
naturalistic; their primary drawbacks include intended by the encoder (or the experi­
their diminished intensity and the ethical menter) as the criterion against which
issues associated with induced emotion. responses are compared. Because multiple
Given these issues, the preferred method criteria tend to overcome the inherent
involves obtaining posed displays from pro­ weaknesses of any single criterion, one
fessional or lay actors. Encoders can be option is to use more than one criterion in
asked to relax and imagine the scenario a single study.
vividly before performing it; this tends to There are several methods for validating
increase the intensity of expressions (Banse stimuli for recognizability and authenticity
& Scherer, 1996). For an extended discus­ before presenting them to study partici­
sion of these issues, see Archer, Akert, and pants. One commonly used procedure
Costanzo (1993) and Scherer (2003). involves presenting stimuli to an indepen­
Emotional expressions can also be either dent group of raters and computing the
“pure” or “blended” (consisting of a mix of “effective” reliability of their judgments (of
emotions, such as happiness and surprise). emotion or any other construct) using the
Although most preexisting sets contain rela­ Spearman-Brown formula with modified
tively pure expressions, perceivers appear to notations, as described by Rosenthal and
process blended expressions more efficiently, Rosnow (1991). The formula is R = nr/(1 +
possibly because they are more likely to (n − 1)r), where R = the effective reliability,
encounter them in real life (LaPlante & n = the number of raters, and r = the mean
Ambady, 2000). It is now possible to create reliability of all the raters (i.e., mean of the
blended expressions artificially using soft­ correlations).
ware such as Adobe Photoshop (e.g., Marsh, More pragmatic decisions must also
Elfenbein, & Ambady, 2003). New digital be made regarding the manner in which
editing techniques allowed Jones, Little, responses are provided. The majority of stud­
Burt, and Perrett (2004) to establish that ies in this area have used forced-choice or
the apparent health of the skin surface influ­ dimensional response formats. Alternatively,
ences perceived attractiveness independently participants can use a free-response format
of face shape. Digital editing software also and provide any label they choose. The rel­
permitted Adams and Kleck (2003) to ative merits of these response formats have
manipulate the direction of eye gaze in tar­ been hotly debated (for a review, see
gets expressing a range of emotions, which Elfenbein, Mandal, Ambady, Harizuka, &
was essential in documenting the joint con­ Kumar, 2002). Briefly, although many
tribution of eye gaze and emotional cues in scholars believe that emotions are categori­
the processing of facial displays. cal in nature, Russell (1993, 1994) and
These assessments are all meant to others argue that emotions should not
capture accuracy, but defining accuracy is be construed as mutually exclusive cate­
itself a challenging process (see Riggio, this gories and that forced-choice formats
volume). As outlined by Kruglanski (1989), inflate agreement artificially. Nevertheless,
03-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 4:01 PM Page 45

Methods for the Study of Nonverbal Communication–––◆–––45

forced-choice designs are more convenient, motion often plays a critically important
obviating the time-consuming and often role in social judgment (Ambady, Hallahan,
ambiguous task of analyzing open-ended & Connor, 1999; Heider & Simmel, 1944;
responses. In addition, modifying forced- Knight & Johnston, 1997). These limitations
choice designs by adding a “none-of-the­ should be taken into account when consider­
above” option appears to reduce artifactual ing how results may generalize to other kinds
agreement (Frank & Stennett, 2001; Haidt of viewing conditions.
& Keltner, 1999).
Although some scholars believe that all
humans are motivated naturally to decode VOCAL CLIPS
nonverbal cues (McArthur & Baron, 1983),
steps can still be taken to increase the partic­ Some Appropriate Uses. When we hear
ipants’ desire to take the perceptual tasks seri­ someone speak, we discern meaning not only
ously (such as limiting the length of the study from the words they choose but also from
and making it as interesting as possible). how those words are spoken. Prosodic cues
Also, it is important to consider the relative such as volume, pitch, and speech rate can
advantages and disadvantages of this method play a more important role than speech con­
of stimulus presentation. The hallmark tent in our social inferences, perhaps because
advantages of photographs are their ease we implicitly understand that it is difficult
of use and high degree of experimental con­ for a speaker to control these signals (Ekman
trol. Whereas control is achieved most & Friesen, 1969). As a result, vocal cues
prominently by the selection of a specific have powerful effects on social interaction
channel of nonverbal behavior to be dis­ (e.g., Hummert, Mazloff, & Henry, 1999;
played (e.g., the face, the torso, the entire Neumann & Strack, 2000; Noller, 2005).
body), it is also achieved in the choice of For instance, new research reveals that
encoders, the nature of the displays, and prosody (i.e., vocal qualities) may be as
the context in which the display arises. important as facial displays in communicat­
Researchers should also consider how ing emotion (for a review, see Johnstone &
the gender, age, socioeconomic status, and Scherer, 2000). Several related issues await
ethnicity of their encoders and perceivers further investigation. As in the case of facial
might affect the generalizability of results. expressions, there is some evidence for the
At each step, the investigator can make universal recognition of emotions based on
decisions necessary for addressing lingering language-free voice samples (see Scherer,
questions and advancing theory. With this 2003). At the same time, there appear to be
high level of control comes an inevitable culture-specific patterns in vocal emotion
trade-off in ecological validity. As discussed expression, similar to the nonverbal “accents”
by Elfenbein, Marsh, and Ambady (2002), in facial display of emotion across cultures
the nonverbal displays encountered in every­ (Marsh et al., 2003; Scherer, 2003).
day life “are subtle, embedded in a particular Some scholars in this area have focused
context, spontaneous, dynamic, fleeting, and on the link between psychological well-being
exist in combination with other expressions, and the ability to understand subtle vocal
words and behaviors” (p. 44). Standardized cues to emotion (e.g., Baum & Nowicki,
stimuli generally fail to meet at least some of 1998; Rosenthal, Hall, DiMatteo, Rogers, &
these criteria, but photographs often fail to Archer, 1979). This research has been aided
meet all of them. Only static stimuli can be by standardized sets of vocal cues, particu­
presented, a drawback given that perceived larly the Profile of Nonverbal Sensitivity
03-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 4:01 PM Page 46

46–––◆–––Foundations

(PONS; Rosenthal et al., 1979). The PONS communication, however, it is important to


assesses the ability to identify the meaning of isolate these features not only from their
nonverbal utterances presented in a range of verbal content but also from the visual
bodily channels including the tone of voice. displays and contextual background that
More recently, Baum and Nowicki (1998) accompany them in everyday life.
developed a subtest of the DANVA to mea­
sure the ability to infer emotion from the
voice (Rothman & Nowicki, 2004). VISUAL CLIPS

Design Considerations. When the decision Some Appropriate Uses. Visual clips com­
is made to employ auditory samples in bine the visual nature of still photographs
research, one option is to use a previously with the dynamic nature of vocal clips. In
developed set of stimuli, such as the PONS or doing so, visual clips have been vital in
the DANVA. If the decision is made to con­ documenting the remarkable accuracy with
struct a new set of stimuli, the resulting series which perceivers can infer enduring and
of pragmatic and theoretical decisions paral­ fleeting characteristics from brief glimpses
lel those used in photograph-based research. of behavior. In a fascinating line of early
A unique concern here involves remov­ research, for example, Johansson (1973)
ing the content of the spoken messages, affixed lights to the major joints of
which is necessary for isolating the proso­ actors and recorded high-contrast images of
dic features. One option involves editing the actors moving in space, such that per­
spoken samples by content filtering (which ceivers could only see a field of point lights.
removes the higher frequencies on which Perceivers could only identify the encoders
word recognition depends) or by randomized in these displays as human when exposed to
splicing (which rearranges segments of the moving point-light displays; in static form,
voice in a random manner), as is done in the they appeared as a random series of dots.
PONS test. Both can now be accomplished Subsequent work revealed that age and
quite easily using sound editing software; gender are also apparent in point-light
however, they produce sound patterns that movements (Koslowski & Cutting, 1977).
are not normally encountered in everyday Current work is exploring the extent to
life. which perceivers can recover other socially
The other option is to provide encoders relevant information, such as “Big Five”
with standardized content such as the alpha­ factors of personality (Heberlein, Adolphs,
bet (Berry, 1991) or a basic neutral sentence Tranel, & Damasio, 2004), from these
(Baum & Nowicki, 1998). This strategy highly impoverished displays.
retains the verbal content but standardizes Visual displays can be impoverished in
it so that only differences in nonverbal fea­ several other ways. One approach involves
tures remain (see also Noller, 2005). With sampling very brief samples, or “thin slices,”
the advent of analog and digital recording of ongoing movement. Even the “thinnest”
and playback devices, it is a relatively simple of slices capture the rich information on
matter to record, manipulate, and present which social judgments are made (for a
vocal cues to study participants (for more review, see Ambady, Bernieri, & Richeson,
details, see Scherer, 2003). Another advan­ 2000). Ambady and colleagues (1999), for
tage of this approach is that unlike still pho­ instance, asked perceivers to identify the
tographs, vocal cues are inherently dynamic. sexual orientation of encoders on the basis
To explore the role of nonverbal features in of silent, very brief (1 or 10 seconds) video
03-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 4:01 PM Page 47

Methods for the Study of Nonverbal Communication–––◆–––47

clips. Impressions of sexual orientation con­ useful for studying the effects of psycho­
formed to encoders’ self-reports at greater­ logical or physical trauma on nonverbal
than-chance levels, suggesting that sexual sensitivity. They can also be used to relate
orientation is conveyed reliably by dynamic individual differences in nonverbal sensitiv­
nonverbal behavior. ity to any number of skills, including work­
An exciting frontier of research is cata­ place performance. Constructing a new set
loguing the mental processes we are able of video clips “in-house” can also be advan­
to accomplish automatically, without inten­ tageous, particularly if one is interested in
tion, deliberation, and conscious aware­ defining accuracy as the ability to predict a
ness. In our own work, we studied the novel criterion, such as student evaluations
connection between induced mood and (Ambady & Rosenthal, 1993), therapeutic
the validity of social judgments based on outcomes (Ambady et al., 2002), or testos­
thin slices of behavior. Induced sadness terone level (Dabbs, Bernieri, & Strong,
impaired accuracy consistently, perhaps 2001). A more detailed summary of the
because it evoked a more reasoned, careful issues involved in such research can be
analysis of available information (Ambady found in Ambady et al. (2000).
& Gray, 2002). This finding is consistent A notable advantage of the visual-clip
with work by Patterson and Stockbridge approach is its flexibility. Depending on
(1998), who found that under some condi­ one’s theoretical orientation, behavior can
tions, perceivers made more accurate social be sampled from any channel of expressive
inferences when they were prevented from communication. The central trade-off in
deliberating carefully. Together, these results ecological validity concerns the extent to
suggest that first impressions may result which impressions formed on the basis of
from a relatively automatic form of cogni­ video clips differ from those made in every­
tive processing (Choi, Gray, & Ambady, day life. Because perceivers do not have con­
2004; Lakin, this volume). tact with the encoders they are viewing, they
do not experience the interaction demands
Design Considerations. Several important ubiquitous to real encounters (e.g., Gilbert,
construction and design decisions should Pelham, & Krull, 1988; Swann, 1984).
seem familiar at this point, because they are Additionally, relatively artificial settings
common to research using photographs, tend to heighten participants’ awareness
vocal clips, and video clips. Options for that their judgments are under scrutiny and
using preexisting sets of stimuli include therefore increase the extent to which infer­
the PONS, described earlier, and an easily ence processes are consciously monitored
administered measure called the interper­ (Forgas, 1999). These limitations should
sonal perception task (IPT; Costanzo & be kept in mind when considering how
Archer, 1989). The original IPT contains the results obtained in these studies would
30 short scenes and the shortened version generalize to other situations.
contains 15 of those scenes (Costanzo &
Archer, 1993). Both tap the ability to infer
INTERACTIONS
kinship, deception, competition, status, and
intimacy. These scenes are naturalistic Some Appropriate Uses. An alternative to
rather than posed, so there is an objectively presenting standardized nonverbal displays
correct answer to each question. Because is to allow them to emerge more naturally
the PONS and the IPT have already been by constructing brief interactions between
normed on healthy populations, they are study participants. Interaction paradigms
03-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 4:01 PM Page 48

48–––◆–––Foundations

have proved useful for furthering our variety of personality dimensions. These
understanding of complex interpersonal ratings are then averaged and correlated
processes, such as interpersonal adaptation with self-ratings of personality. Although
(i.e., the tendency for interaction partners the accuracy of such ratings improves with
to adjust spontaneously to or mutually acquaintanceship (e.g., Funder & Colvin,
influence one another; see Cappella & 1988), the convergence of strangers’ ratings
Schreiber, this volume). A long history of with self-ratings is surprisingly high (e.g.,
research in this area has demonstrated that Albright et al., 1988).
adaptation is a ubiquitous feature of human
social exchange (for a review, see Burgoon, Design Considerations. When photographs,
Stern, & Dillman, 1995). In some of this vocal clips, or visual clips are chosen, one
research, nonverbal signs of adaptation must accept a trade-off in realism in exchange
are manipulated to investigate their conse­ for high experimental control. With interac­
quences for relationship quality (Burgoon tions, the trade-off is reversed: Realism is
et al., 1995). Alternatively, as will be dis­ achieved in all the ways it is limited when
cussed in later sections, these signals can be standardized stimuli are presented. Limiting
observed naturalistically to draw inferences the extent to which participants feel they are
about preexisting relationships. being watched and studied, using hidden
Interaction paradigms can also be used cameras and microphones or one-way mir­
to better understand how people in rela­ rors, further heightens the realism of interac­
tionships come to understand one another. tion situations. Of course, less precision is
For instance, Ickes and his colleagues achieved over the interpersonal processes
devised an interaction paradigm for under investigation in the interaction para­
studying empathic accuracy, the ability to digm than occurs in controlled experiments.
read mental and emotional states (for a Precision concerns are especially salient in
review, see Ickes, 2001). In their unstruc­ research on social perception, given that
tured dyadic interaction paradigm, two forming impressions is a serial process; the
participants are left alone together for a encoder must display the relevant cues, these
brief time and their spontaneous conver­ cues must be made available to the perceiver,
sation is videotaped. Later, the partici­ the perceiver must detect these cues, and
pants view the videotapes independently finally, the perceiver must interpret these cues
and complete written reports detailing the in light of previously stored knowledge
thoughts and feelings they experienced (Funder, 1999). When standardized stimuli
during the interaction. They also attempt are presented, the experimenter can control
to infer the thoughts and feelings their the first three steps and study how informa­
partners experienced during the interac­ tion is processed once it has been displayed,
tion. Greater empathic accuracy is evi­ made available, and detected. When interac­
denced by greater correspondence between tion paradigms are used, however, all the
inferred and self-reported experience. stages are free to vary.
Even less exposure to another person can Interaction paradigms present some
be sufficient for drawing surprisingly accu­ idiosyncratic methodological concerns.
rate inferences of personality, however. For instance, should a confederate be used?
In the zero-acquaintance paradigm (e.g., Confederates are helpful in standardizing
Albright, Kenny, & Malloy, 1988), unac­ interaction patterns, although very tightly
quainted peers assemble in small groups controlling the conversation between a con­
and rate themselves and each other on a federate and a participant will likely result
03-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 4:01 PM Page 49

Methods for the Study of Nonverbal Communication–––◆–––49

in awkward, halting interactions. If confed­ for example, manipulate nonverbal displays


erates are to be used, care should be taken flexibly by presenting standardized stimuli
to ensure that they are unaware of the or constructing brief interactions and then
study’s hypothesis or at least the condition measure the reactions these displays evoke.
assignment of their partners. In addition, In the current section, we turn our attention
it is generally preferable to use more than to the use of nonverbal behavior as a depen­
one confederate to increase the generaliz­ dent variable. This strategy capitalizes on
ability of results. For an extended discus­ the connection between nonverbal behavior
sion of these issues, see Guerrero and Le and the underlying subjective experience to
Poire (2005). In addition to these method­ study the effects of manipulated variables
ological concerns, the investigation must on cognition, affect, and behavior.
also decide what the participants will do
during the interaction. Commonly, partici­
pants are asked to discuss a relatively neu­ FACIAL CUES
tral topic or to jointly complete a simple
task (Chartrand & Bargh, 1999). To study Some Appropriate Uses. When we want to
how people resolve conflict, one can ask understand and describe subjective experi­
participants to discuss a disagreement they ence, asking people directly can be unsatis­
have had recently (Roberts, 2005). factory for a number of reasons. Ongoing
It is usually necessary to have a video­ experiences may not be reportable because
taped or audiotaped record of the interac­ they are not consciously accessible, or
tion. In such cases, the researcher faces they may be skewed by self-presentation
another decision: whether to inform partic­ demands or retrospective biases (e.g.,
ipants that they will be recorded. In making Nisbett & Wilson, 1977). Nonverbal cues,
this decision, it is helpful to weigh the including facial behavior, are an excellent
potential effects of increased self-awareness alternative in many cases because their
against the ethical and practical concerns of emergence requires little or no conscious
secretly recording people. One compromise awareness and is less controllable than
is to inform participants that they will (or verbal reports (DePaulo, 1992; Ekman &
may) be recorded, and then minimize the Friesen, 1969).
salience of the recording devices in the hope These advantages are well illustrated by
that participants will forget they are being a study conducted by Bonanno and col­
recorded. In any case, during debriefing it is leagues (2002). These researchers hypothe­
essential to obtain informed consent for sized that facial expressions of positive
the future use of the recordings and to give and negative emotions may be informative
participants the right to have their tapes about the ways in which survivors of child­
erased. hood sexual assault respond to situations in
which they might disclose the abuse. To
that end, the researchers coded participants’
♦ Nonverbal Behavior as a facial cues to emotion during a narrative
interview. Results indicated that survivors
Dependent Variable
who did not disclose their abuse voluntarily
when given the opportunity to do so were
As was just seen, a range of methods is most likely to show facial displays of shame.
available for treating nonverbal behavior as This pattern suggests that facial displays
an independent variable. A researcher can, of emotion may be used indirectly to
03-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 4:01 PM Page 50

50–––◆–––Foundations

communicate aspects of experience that toward threatening cues. They measured


may be too painful to be disclosed directly. hypervigilance by assessing the direction
This study also demonstrates the usefulness and latency of eye movements toward emo­
of examining facial displays of emotion in tional faces. As predicted, anxious partici­
relation to a complex social problem, particu­ pants were more likely than healthy controls
larly one for which previous investigators have to quickly shift their gaze toward angry
relied almost solely on verbal self-report. faces. These results add to the growing body
Because facial cues to emotion can be of research on the role of social cognitive
too subtle to be recognized, researchers processes in some emotional disorders.
often turn to psychophysiological measures Observing eye behavior to infer mental
such as facial electromyography (EMG), processes is particularly useful when dealing
which involves the placement of small with participants who are unable to vocalize
surface electrodes over facial muscles. their thoughts. For example, a number of
Dimberg et al. (2000), for example, used experimental methods have been developed
EMG recording to test the hypothesis that that use infants’ eye movements as an index
humans are predisposed to react to emo­ of what they notice, perceive, and under­
tional facial stimuli spontaneously, quickly, stand. In the visual preference method, exper­
and independently of conscious cognitive imenters show infants two displays, either
processes. The researchers focused on two side by side or sequentially, and monitor their
major muscles: the zygomatic, which ele­ looking time at each display. Preferential
vates the lips to form a smile, and the cor­ looking toward one of the objects indicates
rugator, which knits the eyebrows to form that the infant is noticing a difference
a frown. These muscles have long been between the two stimuli (e.g., Wilcox &
associated with positive and negative affect, Clayton, 1968). The visual habituation pro­
respectively. While their muscle movements cedure can also be used to assess discrimina­
were recorded, participants were exposed tion (Caron, Caron, & MacLean, 1988).
to happy, neutral, or angry faces at expo­
sure times too short to be translated into Design Considerations. Several well-
conscious awareness. As predicted, happy developed methods have been employed for
faces evoked larger zygomatic activity and coding facial cues to emotion. The Emotion
lower corrugator activity, and angry faces Facial Affect Coding Scheme (EMFACS;
evoked larger corrugator activity and lower Ekman & Friesen, 1984) specifies the pat­
zygomatic activity, as early as half a second terns of facial movement believed to be
after the presentation of the faces, demon­ associated with the six basic emotions. This
strating that emotional reactions may be theoretically derived, anatomically based
evoked unconsciously. coding scheme describes facial activity in
Like emotions, cognitive operations terms of 44 unique action units (AUs).
can be inferred from behavior. Eye move­ Coders describe all visible muscular move­
ments are quite useful, given that they index ment in terms of AUs or AU combinations.
several aspects of cognition reliably. Shifts The Maximally Descriptive Facial Movement
in gaze direction, for example, can reflect Coding System (MAX; Izard, 1979) is a sim­
the allocation of attention toward stimuli ilar technique used primarily with infants
in the environment. Mogg, Millar, and and children. Such detailed coding schemes
Bradley (2000) took advantage of this con­ are not without their disadvantages. They
nection to test the hypothesis that clinical are relatively laborious and time-consuming,
anxiety is associated with hypervigilance and they require extensive training. In
03-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 4:01 PM Page 51

Methods for the Study of Nonverbal Communication–––◆–––51

addition, they necessitate that muscle move­ guilt, and increased mental load. In an
ments be visible to the human eye. This attempt to discern how these emotional and
problem is solved by EMG recording, in cognitive changes are reflected in the voice,
which detected muscle movements are fil­ Vrij, Edward, Roberts, and Bull (2000) ana­
tered, amplified, and smoothed before being lyzed vocal cues and verbal behavior during
subjected to traditional data-analysis proce­ deception and truth telling. Two reliable
dures (see Tassinary & Cacioppo, 2000, for vocal indicators of deception emerged: (1) a
more details). Like other psychophysiologi­ longer latency period (the time between a
cal techniques, EMG recording is relatively question and its answer) and (2) more speech
expensive and requires some additional train­ disturbances (instances of saying “ah” or
ing and equipment. An additional drawback “mm” between words; for a more detailed
is that the application of facial sensors is discussion, see Vrij, this volume).
relatively intrusive. Vocal cues also index interpersonal qual­
ities reliably. For instance, speech accom­
modation theory proposes that a speaker’s
VOCAL CUES need for affiliation is reflected in a tendency
to match a conversation partner’s speech
Some Appropriate Uses. Like facial dis­ cues (Giles & Smith, 1979). According to
plays, the voice can be an excellent index this perspective, people match their part­
of emotional experience. For instance, ner’s vocal cues spontaneously when they
faster speech rates are associated with more want to gain approval (for a review, see
pleasant emotions, whereas sadness is con­ Buller, 2005). Gregory and Webster (1996)
sistently associated with the tendency to tested this hypothesis using naturalistic
speak at a very slow rate and take longer samples obtained from conversations
pauses between words (see review by between a talk show host (Larry King) and
Siegman, 1987). Ellgring and Scherer (1996) several of his guests. Lower status guests
capitalized on this association in their tended to accommodate their voices to
attempt to obtain a behavior-based mea­ King’s, whereas King tended to accommo­
sure of progress in therapy for major date his voice to match those of higher sta­
depression. In the early course of therapy, tus guests (e.g., sitting presidents). Other
and again during remission, patients pro­ vocal forms of accommodation, such as
vided speech samples during standardized matching accent patterns, response laten­
interviews. Remission from depression was cies, and utterance durations, have been
marked by an increase in speech rate and identified (Buller, 2005).
a decrease in pause duration. On a broader
note, the authors speculate that observa­ Design Considerations. Quantifying vocal
tions of nonverbal behavior may be a useful cues involves a number of steps, beginning
tool in charting therapeutic progress, par­ with the recording of voice samples and the
ticularly given the limitations of patients’ isolation of specific segments for analysis.
ability and willingness to communicate some For speech rate analysis, trained coders
aspects of experience verbally. can be asked to listen to each segment and
Vocal cues can be used to index a broad count the number of syllables spoken.
range of emotional and cognitive states, a Alternatively, coders can simply rate speak­
point illustrated by their application to ers’ voices on a continuous scale from rela­
the study of deception. When telling a lie, tively slow to relatively fast. Perceived
deceivers often experience fear, excitement, similarity in speech rate can be measured by
03-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 4:01 PM Page 52

52–––◆–––Foundations

asking participants to rate whether their observed in relatively focused situations,


conversation partners spoke slower, faster, when people share a common focus of
or at about the same pace as them. These attention. In unfocused situations, to use
methods are reviewed in more detail by Goffman’s (1963) distinction, people are
Buller (2005). simply “mutually present”; common exam­
ples include riding in an elevator or standing
in line at the grocery store. Some scholars
BODILY MOVEMENTS have been interested in the patterns of non­
verbal adaptation that emerge in these
Some Appropriate Uses. Some bodily common situations. Patterson (2005), for
movements, when observed carefully, can instance, developed the passing encounters
paint a portrait of complex interpersonal paradigm to study how strangers mutually
processes that would otherwise lie beyond regulate their distance and contact when
awareness, not only to an experimenter but walking toward one another in public
also to the interactants. Gottman’s (1979) spaces. This nonreactive, structured para­
work on married couples illustrates this digm is being used to better understand
point. In his conflict interaction paradigm, not only general tendencies in the behavior
spouses are observed in the laboratory dis­ of strangers on the street but also the man­
cussing an area of disagreement in their ner in which these behaviors are influenced
lives. Systematic review of the videotapes by cultural and situational factors. Results
taken from these sessions reveals patterns from these studies may speak to larger
of verbal and nonverbal behavior that can issues about how strangers negotiate their
be surprisingly predictive of couples’ long- behavior so as to maintain comfort and
term outcomes, including the likelihood predictability.
and timing of divorce (e.g., Carrère &
Gottman, 1999). Design Considerations. The first step in
Subtle bodily movements also reveal rela­ operationalizing the outcome measures
tional qualities among nonintimates. For highlighted in this section is building on
instance, in the chameleon effect, people prior theory and research to decide which
unintentionally adopt the postures, gestures, bodily movements should theoretically
and mannerisms of their interaction part­ be related to the variables of interest. In
ners, particularly when they are more moti­ the case of marital communication, for
vated to affiliate (Chartrand & Bargh, instance, past research has indicated that
1999; Lakin & Chartrand, 2003). An alter­ marital satisfaction is reflected in spouses’
ative to such mimicry is complementarity: reciprocation of negative affect (Gottman,
the tendency to respond to nonverbal dis­ 1994; see Noller, this volume). Researchers
plays with contrasting behaviors. Tiedens interested in marital satisfaction, therefore,
and Fragale (2003) demonstrated recently might decide to look for escalations of neg­
that complementarity is particularly likely ative behavior (e.g., crying in response to
to emerge in displays of dominance and sub­ yelling). In the passing encounters para­
mission. In other words, we tend to display digm, observers are interested in how
submissive postures in response to a part­ participants respond to different types of
ner’s dominant postures, and vice versa. confederate behavior (Patterson, 2005).
Synchrony, mimicry, and complementarity Once the behavioral categories have
are all patterns of interpersonal adaptation been defined, the researcher must choose
03-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 4:01 PM Page 53

Methods for the Study of Nonverbal Communication–––◆–––53

a level of measurement. Behaviors can be by new advancements in methodology made


coded categorically (as present or absent) or possible primarily through interdisciplinary
on a more continuous scale (e.g., rating the collaborations. For instance, explorations
negativity of a given behavior on a 1–7 of the brain’s response to social stimuli are
scale). Behaviors can also be rated in terms becoming more common and refined,
of both frequency and duration. Bakeman thanks to the expanded use of techniques
(2005) and White and Sargent (2005) previously consigned to cognitive psychol­
provide detailed suggestions regarding the ogy and cognitive neuroscience (e.g., Chiao,
development and use of coding schemes in Bordeaux, & Ambady, 2004; Chiu,
general, and Cappella (2005) discusses cod­ Ambady, & Deldin, 2004; Heberlein,
ing specifically for adaptation in dyadic Adolphs, Tranel, & Damasio, 2004). Along
interactions. As in other coding schemes, the same lines, paradigms originally devel­
raters should be well trained and blind to oped to study social cognition are being
experimental conditions, and their reliabil­ used to expand our understanding of a
ity must be sufficiently high. Although it is range of psychological disorders, including
possible to code behavior in real time, video depression and social anxiety (e.g., Gotlib
recordings provide the opportunity for et al., 2004). Increased knowledge of infor­
multiple viewings and greater automation mation technology is making it possible
of some measurements (e.g., time). to study affective and cognitive responses
to nonverbal stimuli on a very large scale
using the World Wide Web (e.g., Nosek,
♦ Conclusion Banaji, & Greenwald, 2002).
The future of nonverbal communication
research may also be marked by advance­
A central goal of this review has been to ments in the application of research find­
survey the application of contemporary ings to a variety of important real-world
methods in nonverbal behavior research to settings, including the workplace and gov­
a variety of research questions. The exam­ ernment, courtrooms and police stations,
ples provided are not intended to convey hospitals and clinics, the classroom, and
the full range of issues that can be many others. Some of the findings discussed
addressed using nonverbal behavior meth­ in this review are being applied to these set­
ods; rather, they are meant to illustrate the tings. For instance, some are attempting to
diversity of topics that can benefit from improve law enforcement officers’ ability to
the use of these methods. In the interest detect lies by educating them on the non­
of brevity, we have limited our review to verbal indices of deception (Vrij & Mann,
methods that involve the presentation or 2005). Ambady and colleagues (2002)
sampling of nonverbal behavior. Self-report applied nonverbal research methods to
measures tapping nonverbal expressiveness study the interpersonal communication
(Riggio & Riggio, 2005), explicit knowl­ factors that predict the likelihood of mal­
edge of nonverbal cue meaning (e.g., Rosip practice claims, an important marker of
& Hall, 2004), and other constructs can be health care satisfaction. These and other
used to explore many of the issues we have approaches bring together a diverse group
discussed. of practitioners and researchers who can
In the coming years, the study of non­ benefit from expanded knowledge of non­
verbal communication will likely be marked verbal communication.
03-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 4:01 PM Page 54

54–––◆–––Foundations

♦ References of Personality and Social Psychology, 70,


614–636.
Baron-Cohen, S., Wheelwright, S., & Hill, J.
Adams, R. B., & Kleck, R. E. (2003). Perceived (2001). The “Reading the Mind in the Eyes”
gaze direction and the processing of facial test revised version: A study with normal
displays of emotion. Psychological Science, adults, and adults with Asperger syndrome
14, 644–647. or high-functioning autism. Journal of Child
Albright, L., Kenny, D. A., & Malloy, T. E. Psychology and Psychiatry, 42, 241–252.
(1988). Consensus in personality judgments Baum, K. M., & Nowicki, S. (1998). Perception
at zero acquaintance. Journal of Personality of emotion: Measuring decoding accuracy
and Social Psychology, 55, 387–395. of adult prosodic cues varying in intensity.
Ambady, N., Bernieri, F. J., & Richeson, J. A. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 22, 89–107.
(2000). Toward a histology of social Berry, D. S. (1991). Accuracy in social percep­
behavior: Judgmental accuracy from thin tion: Contribution of facial and vocal infor­
slices of the behavioral stream. Advances mation. Journal of Personality and Social
in Experimental Social Psychology, 32, Psychology, 61, 298–307.
201–271. Bonanno, G. A., Keltner, D., Noll, J. G., Putnam,
Ambady, N., & Gray, H. M. (2002). On being F. W., Trickett, P. K., LeJeune, J., et al.
sad and mistaken: Mood effects on the (2002). When the face reveals what words
accuracy of thin-slice judgment. Journal do not: Facial expressions of emotion, smil­
of Personality and Social Psychology, 83, ing, and the willingness to disclose child­
947–961. hood sexual abuse. Journal of Personality
Ambady, N., Hallahan, M., & Connor, B. and Social Psychology, 83, 94–110.
(1999). Accuracy of judgments of sexual ori­ Buck, R. (2005). Measuring emotional experi­
entation from thin slices of behavior. Journal ence, expression, and communication: The
of Personality and Social Psychology, 77, slide-viewing technique. In V. Manusov
538–547. (Ed.), The sourcebook of nonverbal mea­
Ambady, N., LaPlante, D., Nguyen, T., sures: Going beyond words (pp. 457–470).
Rosenthal, R., Chaumeton, N., & Levinson, Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
W. (2002). Surgeon’s tone of voice: A clue Buller, D. B. (2005). Methods for measuring
to malpractice history. Surgery, 132, 5–9. speech rate. In V. Manusov (Ed.), The
Ambady, N., & Rosenthal, R. (1993). Half a sourcebook of nonverbal measures: Going
minute: Predicting teacher evaluations from beyond words (pp. 317–324). Mahwah,
thin slices of nonverbal behavior and phys­ NJ: Erlbaum.
ical attractiveness. Journal of Personality Burgoon, J. K., Stern, L. A., & Dillman, L.
and Social Psychology, 64, 431–441. (1995). Interpersonal adaptation: Dyadic
Archer, D., Akert, R., & Costanzo, M. (1993). interaction patterns. New York, NY:
The accurate perception of nonverbal Cambridge University Press.
behavior: Questions of theory and research Cappella, J. N. (2005). Coding mutual adapta­
design. In P. D. Blank (Ed.), Inter­ tion in dyadic nonverbal interaction. In
personal expectations: Theory, research, V. Manusov (Ed.), The sourcebook of non­
and applications (pp. 242–260). New York: verbal measures: Going beyond words
Cambridge University Press. (pp. 383–392). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Bakeman, R. (2005). Analysis of coded nonver­ Caron, A. J., Caron, R. F., & MacLean, D.
bal behavior. In V. Manusov (Ed.), The J.(1988). Infant discrimination of natural­
sourcebook of nonverbal measures: Going istic emotional expressions: The role
beyond words (pp. 375–382). Mahwah, of face and voice. Child Development, 59,
NJ: Erlbaum. 604–616.
Banse, R., & Scherer, K. R. (1996). Acoustic pro­ Carrère, S., & Gottman, J. M. (1999). Predicting
files in vocal emotion expression. Journal divorce among newlyweds from the first
03-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 4:01 PM Page 55

Methods for the Study of Nonverbal Communication–––◆–––55

three minutes of a marital conflict discussion. Ekman, P., & Friesen, W. V. (1969). Nonverbal
Family Process, 38, 293–301. leakage and cues to deception. Psychiatry,
Chartrand, T. L., & Bargh, J. A. (1999). The 32, 88–106.
chameleon effect: The perception-behavior Ekman, P., & Friesen, W. V. (1976). Pictures
link and social interaction. Journal of of facial affect. Palo Alto, CA: Consulting
Personality and Social Psychology, 76, Psychologists Press.
893–910. Ekman, P., & Friesen, W. V. (1984). Emotion
Chiao, J. Y., Bordeaux, A. R., & Ambady, N. Facial Action Coding System (EMFACS).
(2004). Mental representations of social San Francisco: University of California
status. Cognition, 93, B49–B57. Press.
Chiu, P., Ambady, N., & Deldin, P. (2004). Elfenbein, H. A., & Ambady, N. (2002). On
Contingent negative variation to emotional the universality and cultural specificity
in- and out-group stimuli differentiates high- of emotion recognition: A meta-analysis.
and low-prejudiced individuals. Journal of Psychological Bulletin, 128, 203–235.
Cognitive Neuroscience, 6, 1830–1839. Elfenbein, H. A., Mandal, M. K., Ambady, N.,
Choi, Y. S., Gray, H. M., & Ambady, N. Harizuka, S., & Kumar, S. (2002). Cross-
(2004). The glimpsed world: Unintended cultural patterns in emotion recognition:
communication and unintended perception. Highlighting design and analytical tech­
In R. Hassin, J. Bargh, & J. Uleman (Eds.), niques. Emotion, 2, 75–84.
The new unconscious (pp. 309–333). New Elfenbein, H. A., Marsh, A. A., & Ambady, N.
York: Oxford University Press. (2002). Emotional intelligence and the
Costanzo, M., & Archer, D. (1989). Interpret­ recognition of emotion from facial expres­
ing the expressive behavior of others: The sions. In L. F. Barrett & P. Salovey (Eds.),
Interpersonal Perception Task. Journal of The wisdom in feeling (pp. 37–59). New
Nonverbal Behavior, 13, 225–245. York: Guilford.
Costanzo, M., & Archer, D. (Producers). Ellgring, H., & Scherer, K. R. (1996). Vocal
(1993). Interpersonal Perception Task-15 indicators of mood change in depression.
[Videotape]. (Available from the University Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 20,
of California, 2000 Center Street, Berkeley, 83–110.
CA 94704) Forgas, J. P. (1999). Feeling and speaking: Mood
Dabbs, J. M., Bernieri, F. J., & Strong, R. K. effects on verbal communication strategies.
(2001). Going on stage: Testosterone in Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin,
greetings and meetings. Journal of Research 25, 850–863.
in Personality, 35, 27–40. Frank, M. G., & Stennett, J. (2001). The forced-
DePaulo, B. M. (1992). Nonverbal behavior and choice paradigm and the perception of facial
self-presentation. Psychological Bulletin, expressions of emotion. Journal of Per­
11, 203–243. sonality and Social Psychology, 80, 75–85.
Dimberg, U., Thunberg, M., & Elmehed, K. Funder, D. C. (1999). Personality judgment:
(2000). Unconscious facial reactions to A realistic approach to person perception.
emotional facial expressions. Psychological San Diego, CA: Academic Press.
Science, 11, 86–89. Funder, D. C., & Colvin, C. R. (1988). Friends
Ekman, P. (1972). Universals and cultural and strangers: Acquaintanceship, agree­
differences in facial expressions of emotion. ment, and the accuracy of personality judg­
In J. Cole (Ed.), Nebraska Symposium on ment. Journal of Personality and Social
Motivation (pp. 207–283). Lincoln: Psychology, 55, 149–158.
University of Nebraska Press. Gilbert, D. T., Pelham, B. W., & Krull, D. S.
Ekman, P. (1994). Strong evidence for universals (1988). On cognitive busyness: When person
in facial expressions: A reply to Russell’s perceivers meet persons perceived. Journal
mistaken critique. Psychological Bulletin, of Personality and Social Psychology, 54,
115, 268–287. 733–740.
03-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 4:01 PM Page 56

56–––◆–––Foundations

Giles, H., & Smith, P. M. (1979). Accommodation Heider, F., & Simmel, M. (1944). An experimen­
theory: Optimal levels of convergence. In tal study of apparent behavior. American
H. Giles & R. St. Clair (Eds.), Language Journal of Psychology, 57, 43–259.
and social psychology (pp. 45–65). Hummert, M. L., Mazloff, D., & Henry, C.
Baltimore: University Park Press. (1999). Vocal characteristics of older adults
Goffman, E. (1963). Behavior in public places. and stereotyping. Journal of Nonverbal
New York: Free Press. Behavior, 23, 111–132.
Gotlib, I. H., Kasch, K. L., Trail, S., Joorman, J., Ickes, W. (2001). Measuring empathic accuracy.
Arnow, B., & Johnson, S. L. (2004). In J. A. Hall & F. J. Bernieri (Eds.), Inter­
Coherence and specificity of information- personal sensitivity: Theory and measure­
processing biases in depression and social ment (pp. 219–241). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
phobia. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, Izard, C. E. (1979). The Maximally Discriminative
113, 386–398. Facial Movement Coding System (MAX).
Gottman, J. M. (1979). Marital interaction: Newark: University of Delaware, Infor­
Empirical investigations. New York: mation Technologies and University Media
Academic Press. Services.
Gottman, J. M. (1994). What predicts divorce? Johansson, G. (1973). Visual perception of
Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. biological motion and a model for its analy­
Greenwald, A. G., Nosek, B. A., & Banaji, sis. Perception and Psychophysics, 14,
M. R. (2003). Understanding and using the 201–211.
Implicit Association Test: I. An improved Johnstone, T., & Scherer, K. R. (2000). Vocal
scoring algorithm. Journal of Personality communication of emotion. In M. Lewis
and Social Psychology, 85, 197–216. & J. Haviland (Eds.), Handbook of emo­
Gregory, S. W., & Webster, S. (1996). A non­ tion (2nd ed., pp. 220–235). New York:
verbal signal in voices of interview partners Guilford.
effectively predicts communication accom­ Jones, B. C., Little, A. C., Burt, D. M., &
modation and social status perceptions. Perrett, D. I. (2004). When facial attrac­
Journal of Personality and Social Psycho­ tiveness is only skin deep. Perception, 33,
logy, 70, 1231–1240. 569–576.
Guerrero, L. K., & Le Poire, B. A. (2005). Knight, B., & Johnston, A. (1997). The role
Nonverbal research involving experimen­ of movement in face recognition. Visual
tal manipulations by confederates. In Cognition, 4, 265–273.
V. Manusov (Ed.), The sourcebook of non­ Kozlowski, L. T., & Cutting, J. E. (1977).
verbal measures; Going beyond words Recognizing the sex of a walker from a
(pp. 507–522). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. dynamic point-light display. Perception and
Haidt, J., & Keltner, D. (1999). Culture and Psychophysics, 21, 575–580.
facial expression: Open-ended methods find Kruglanski, A. W. (1989). The psychology
more expressions and a gradient of recogni­ of being “right”: The problem of accuracy
tion. Cognition and Emotion, 13, 225–266. in social perception and cognition.
Harris, M., & Rosenthal, R. (2005). No more Psychological Bulletin, 106, 395–409.
teachers’ dirty looks: Effects of teacher Lakin, J. L., & Chartrand, T. L. (2003). Using
nonverbal behavior on student outcomes. nonconscious behavioral mimicry to create
In R. E. Riggio & R. S. Feldman (Eds.), affiliation and rapport. Psychological Science,
Applications of nonverbal communication 14, 334–339.
(pp. 157–192). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. LaPlante, D., & Ambady, N. (2000). Multiple
Heberlein, A. S, Adolphs, R., Tranel, D., & messages: Facial recognition advantage
Damasio, H. (2004). Cortical regions for for compound expressions. Journal of Non­
judgments of emotions and personality verbal Behavior, 24, 211–224.
traits from point-light walkers. Journal of Marsh, A. A., Adams, R. B., & Kleck, R. E.
Cognitive Neuroscience, 16, 1143–1158. (2004). Why do fear and anger look the
03-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 4:01 PM Page 57

Methods for the Study of Nonverbal Communication–––◆–––57

way they do? Form and social function in Patterson, M. L., & Stockbridge, E. (1998). Effects
facial expression. Personality and Social of cognitive demand and judgment strategy
Psychology Bulletin, 31, 73–86. on person perception accuracy. Journal of
Marsh, A. A., Elfenbein, H. A., & Ambady, N. Nonverbal Behavior, 22, 253–263.
(2003). Nonverbal “accents”: Cultural dif­ Richeson, J. A., & Shelton, J. N. (2005). Thin
ferences in facial expressions of emotion. slices of racial bias. Journal of Nonverbal
Psychological Science, 14, 373–376. Behavior, 29, 75–86.
Matsumoto, D., & Ekman, P. (1988). Japanese Riggio, R. E., & Riggio, H. R. (2005). Self-
and Caucasian facial expressions of emo­ report measures of emotional and nonver­
tion (JACFEE) [Slides]. San Francisco: San bal expressiveness. In V. Manusov (Ed.),
Francisco State University, Department of The sourcebook of nonverbal measures:
Psychology, Intercultural and Emotion Going beyond words (pp. 105–112).
Research Laboratory. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
McArthur, L. Z., & Baron, R. M. (1983). Roberts, L. J. (2005). Conflict, real life, and
Toward an ecological theory of social per­ videotape: Procedures for eliciting natural­
ception. Psychological Review, 90, 215–238. istic couple interactions. In V. Manusov
Mogg, K., Millar, N., & Bradley, B. P. (2000). (Ed.), The sourcebook of nonverbal mea­
Biases in eye movements to threatening sures: Going beyond words (pp. 471–481).
facial expressions in generalized anxiety Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
disorder and depressive disorder. Journal of Rosenthal, R., Hall, J. A., DiMatteo, M. R.,
Abnormal Psychology, 109, 695–704. Rogers, P. L., & Archer, D. (1979).
Neumann, R., & Strack, F. (2000). “Mood con­ Sensitivity to nonverbal communication:
tagion”: The automatic transfer of mood The PONS Test. Baltimore: Johns Hopkins
between persons. Journal of Personality University Press.
and Social Psychology, 79, 211–223. Rosenthal, R., & Rosnow, R. L. (1991).
Nisbett, R., & Wilson, T. (1977). Telling more Essentials of behavioral research: Methods
than we can know: Verbal reports on and data analysis (2nd ed.). Boston:
mental processes. Psychological Review, McGraw-Hill.
84, 231–259. Rosip, J. C., & Hall, J. A. (2004). Knowledge
Noller, P. (2005). Standard content methodol­ of nonverbal cues, gender, and nonverbal
ogy: Controlling the verbal channel. In decoding accuracy. Journal of Nonverbal
V. Manusov (Ed.), The sourcebook of Behavior, 28, 264–286.
nonverbal measures: Going beyond words Rothman, A. D., & Nowicki, S. (2004). A mea­
(pp. 417–430). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. sure of the ability to identify emotion
Nosek, B. A., Banaji, M. R., & Greenwald, in children’s tone of voice. Journal of
A. G. (2002). E-research: Ethics, security, Nonverbal Behavior, 29, 67–92.
design, and control in psychological research Russell, J. A. (1993). Forced-choice response
on the Internet. Journal of Social Issues, format in the study of facial expression.
58, 161–176. Motivation and Emotion, 17, 41–51.
Nowicki, S., & Duke, M. (1994). Individual Russell, J. A. (1994). Is there universal recogni­
differences in the nonverbal communica­ tion of emotion from facial expressions?
tion of affect: The Diagnostic Analysis A review of the cross-cultural studies.
of Nonverbal Accuracy Scale. Journal of Psychological Bulletin, 115, 102–141.
Nonverbal Behavior, 789–735. Scherer, K. R. (2003). Vocal communication of
Patterson, M. L. (2005). The passing encounters emotion: A review of research paradigms.
paradigm: Monitoring microinteractions Speech Communication, 40, 227–256.
between pedestrians. In V. Manusov (Ed.), Siegman, A. W. (1987). The telltale voice:
The sourcebook of nonverbal measures: Nonverbal messages of verbal communica­
Going beyond words (pp. 431–440). tion. In W. Siegman & S. Feldstein (Eds.),
Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. Nonverbal behavior and communication
03-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 4:01 PM Page 58

58–––◆–––Foundations

(2nd ed., pp. 351–433). Hillsdale, NJ: Vrij, A., & Mann, S. (2005). Police use of non­
Erlbaum. verbal behaviors as indicators of deception.
Swann, W. B., Jr. (1984). Quest for accuracy in In R. E. Riggio & R. S Feldman. (Eds.),
person perception: A matter of pragmatics. Applications of nonverbal communication
Psychological Review, 91, 457–477. (pp. 63–94). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Tassinary, L. G., & Cacioppo, J. T. (2000). The White, C. H., & Sargent, J. (2005). Researcher
skeletomotor system: Surface electromyog­ choices and practices in the study of non­
raphy. In J. T. Cacioppo, L. G. Tassinary, verbal communication. In V. Manusov
& G. Berntson (Eds.), Handbook of psy­ (Ed.), The sourcebook of nonverbal mea­
chophysiology (2nd ed., pp. 163–199). sures: Going beyond words (pp. 3–21).
New York: Cambridge University Press. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Tiedens, L. Z., & Fragale, A. R. (2003). Power Wilcox, B., & Clayton, F. (1968). Infant visual
moves: Complementarity in dominant and fixation on motion pictures of the human
submissive nonverbal behavior. Journal face. Journal of Experimental Child Psycho­
of Personality and Social Psychology, 84, logy, 6, 22–32.
558–568. Zebrowitz, L.A. (1997). Reading faces: Window
Tracy, J. L., & Robins, R. W. (2004). Show to the soul? Boulder, CO: Westview Press.
your pride: Evidence for a discrete emo­ Zebrowitz, L. A., & Rhodes, G. (2004).
tion expression. Psychological Science, 15, Sensitivity to “bad genes” and the anom­
194–197. alous face overgeneralization effect: Cue
Vrij, A., Edward, K., Roberts, K. P., & Bull, R. validity, cue utilization, and accuracy in
(2000). Detecting deceit via analysis of judging intelligence and health. Journal of
verbal and nonverbal behavior. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 28, 167–185.
Nonverbal Behavior, 24, 239–263.
04-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 7:23 PM Page 59

4
AUTOMATIC COGNITIVE
PROCESSES AND NONVERBAL
COMMUNICATION

� Jessica L. Lakin
Drew University

N onverbal behavior is arguably one of the most powerful meth­


ods of communication; it conveys important information about
a person’s likes and dislikes, emotions, personal characteristics, and
relationships (e.g., intimacy, dominance, trust, similarity). Whereas no
one would be surprised that verbal communication of this type of infor­
mation has a cognitive basis, messages conveyed and received nonver­
bally also have their basis in cognitive processes, although not always in
conscious, controlled ones. Thus, understanding nonverbal communi­
cation relies, to some extent, on appreciating its cognitive foundation.
This cognition refers to the mental activities and processes in which
humans (and other animals) engage. Cognitive activities include, but
are not limited to, learning, receiving, storing, processing, judging, and
using information (Neisser, 1967).
The cognitive processes associated with nonverbal communication
can occur with awareness, but they are also likely to, and in fact often
do, occur without conscious awareness (for a review, see Hassin,
Uleman, & Bargh, 2005). The purpose of this chapter is to review the

◆ 59
04-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 7:23 PM Page 60

60–––◆–––Foundations

automatic cognitive bases of nonverbal of will to start the process, whereas control­
behaviors. To accomplish this objective, lability refers to the fact that an act of
I first discuss what it means for a cognitive will can stop the process once it has been
process to be (relatively) automatic or (rela­ started (i.e., it does not run to conclusion
tively) controlled. This is followed by specific autonomously). Finally, and despite the lim­
examples of nonverbal communication that ited amount of cognitive resources that
appear to be relatively automatic. I then con­ people have and the already high demands
clude with a discussion of important issues on these resources, controlled processes
and future directions, including methodolog­ require a share of these limited attentional
ical considerations, the role of controlled resources (i.e., they require cognitive effort).
processes, and the importance of cognitive Automatic processes are more difficult
resources. to define. Because the four characteristics
associated with controlled processes do not
occur in an all-or-none fashion, it is not
♦ Automatic and correct to assume that automatic processes
are, by default, characterized by unaware­
Controlled Processes
ness, unintentionality, and uncontrollability
and require no cognitive effort. Although a
In 1975, Posner and Snyder considered a process that has these four characteristics
basic question of human existence: How would certainly be considered automatic,
much control do people have over their processes that are characterized by one,
thoughts, behaviors, and decisions? Since two, or even three of these features have
they posed their question, researchers have also been referred to historically as auto­
demonstrated that much of what we do cog­ matic (Bargh, 1994, 1996, 1997). It is also
nitively happens without intention, aware­ possible for various combinations of these
ness, or conscious control (Hassin et al., four basic characteristics to occur. For
2005). Although the history of automaticity example, experienced drivers intend to get
and automatic processes has been reviewed in a car and go somewhere, even if when
in extensive detail elsewhere (Bargh, 1994, they arrive, they have no conscious aware­
1996, 1997; Bargh & Chartrand, 1999), ness of anything that occurred during the
a basic discussion of what it means for a trip. This example demonstrates that there
process to be more controlled or more auto­ are several types of automatic processes,
matic seems necessary before turning atten­ resulting in a continuum of automaticity
tion to specific examples of nonverbal (ranging from completely automatic to
communication and their relationship to completely controlled) rather than a simple
automatic cognitive processes. dichotomy (Bargh, 1996, 1997; Bargh &
Controlled processes are characterized Chartrand, 1999).
by awareness, intentionality, controllability, Specifically, preconscious automaticity
and cognitive effort (Bargh, 1994, 1996; represents the completely automatic end of
Posner & Snyder, 1975; Shiffrin & Schneider, the continuum, because it corresponds to
1977). Aspects of awareness include recog­ the initial unconscious processing of incom­
nizing the cognitive process or stimulus ing environmental information. This analy­
consciously, but they also involve an sis occurs without intention, control, or
acknowledged recognition of the influence awareness, and it is largely effortless. Goal-
that the process or stimulus is having. directed automaticity, however, represents
Intentionality refers to the necessity of an act a point somewhere in the middle of the
04-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 7:23 PM Page 61

Automatic Cognitive Processes and Nonverbal Communication–––◆–––61

continuum, because it corresponds to literature. Some of these effects have been


intentional, controllable processes that argued explicitly to be a result of the
become automatic and effortless over time. perception-behavior link. As with the his­
Both preconscious and goal-dependent torical conception of automaticity, there are
automatic effects are autonomous: Once quite a few reviews of these effects (Bargh
the processes are started, they operate by & Chartrand, 1999; Wheeler & Petty,
themselves without awareness and con­ 2001), but I begin with a brief review here
scious guidance. Regardless of whether an to place automatic nonverbal communica­
automatic effect is preconscious or goal tion effects in context.
dependent, it is possible for it to become
controlled if a person becomes aware of
the process. Conscious processes that are IMPRESSIONS
effortful, intentional, and controllable
represent the completely controlled end of Impressions of other people can be
the continuum (Bargh, 1996, 1997; Bargh affected automatically by activation of rele­
& Chartrand, 1999). vant knowledge structures. Srull and Wyer
Recent research from the cognitive, (1979) demonstrated this when they made
social, comparative, and neuroscience liter­ traits related to “hostility” or “kindness”
atures has demonstrated a strong associa­ accessible to participants and then asked
tive link between perceptions from the them, in an ostensibly unrelated task, to
environment and the brain regions associ­ form an impression of a person whose
ated with producing the observed behavior behaviors were ambiguously hostile or
(i.e., a perception-behavior link; Bargh ambiguously kind. Their results indicated
& Chartrand, 1999; Bargh, Chen, & that participants were likely to interpret the
Burrows, 1996; see Dijksterhuis & Bargh, ambiguous behaviors in a manner consis­
2001, for a review). This memory-based tent with the traits that had been made
link results in relevant, associated behaviors accessible previously. Higgins, Rholes, and
becoming activated automatically on percep­ Jones (1977) demonstrated a similar effect
tion of a stimulus. Although the perception- with the activation of either positive
behavior link posits an unmediated (e.g., adventurous) or negative (e.g., reck­
relationship between perception and behav­ less) traits relevant to a person’s behaviors
ior, the existence of this link is dependent (e.g., crossing the Atlantic in a sailboat).
on the fact that ideas are represented men­ Participants evaluated the target person
tally (i.e., cognitively). Therefore, in a sense, more positively when the activated traits
some type of minimal cognitive mediation had positive connotations than when they
is involved (Dijksterhuis & Bargh, 2001). had negative connotations. People’s impres­
sions were affected by accessible constructs
without intention or awareness.
♦ Automatic Effects in Social From these two classic demonstrations,
an entire literature on spontaneous trait
Evaluation and Behavior
inferences grew (Uleman, 1999; Uleman,
Newman, & Moskowitz, 1996; Winter &
There is an impressive abundance of auto­ Uleman, 1984). It also became clear that
matic preconscious and goal-dependent impressions are based on traits that are
effects relevant to nonverbal communica­ chronically accessible (i.e., personally
tion demonstrated in the social cognition important; Higgins, 1996) as well as traits
04-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 7:23 PM Page 62

62–––◆–––Foundations

that are temporarily accessible (i.e., primed; activating stereotypes for African Americans
as in the work by Higgins et al., 1977; Srull causes hostility (Bargh et al., 1996) and
& Wyer, 1979) and that salient physical decreased intellectual performance (Steele &
cues (e.g., sex, race) can automatically acti­ Aronson, 1995; Wheeler, Jarvis, & Petty,
vate stereotypes associated with particular 2000). As evidenced by the social inappro­
groups of people (Brewer, 1988; Devine, priateness of many of these behavioral
1989; Fiske & Neuberg, 1990). Together, effects, participants are not aware that they
this work demonstrates that people make are occurring and are not being affected
inferences about the behaviors of others, intentionally, nor do they seem to be able to
without intention, control, effort, or aware­ control the effects that the activation of the
ness of having done so. Many of these various constructs is having.
behaviors are nonverbal cues.

ATTITUDES
BEHAVIOR
Just as impressions are formed automati­
Behavior is also affected automatically by cally when traits are accessible, either chroni­
external stimuli. In the first demonstration of cally or temporarily, evaluations of stimuli
this idea, Bargh et al. (1996) showed that are activated automatically when the stimuli
activating the trait “rude” caused people to are presented. This automatic evaluation
interrupt an experimenter who was ostensi­ work can be traced back to Zajonc (1980),
bly helping a confederate more quickly than who argued that the evaluation of a stimulus
did participants who had the trait “polite” is connected closely to the representation of
activated. This finding has been replicated the stimulus itself. That is, when the stimu­
in a number of behavioral domains: Activating lus is presented, a positive or negative eval­
“politicians” causes long-windedness (Dijk­ uation of that stimulus becomes activated
sterhuis & van Knippenberg, 2000), and acti­ without awareness, effort, or intention. In
vating “supermodels” causes poor performance support of this idea, research has demon­
on a trivia test, whereas activating “profes­ strated that attitudes are activated automat­
sors” causes better performance (Dijksterhuis ically when a stimulus is presented (Bargh,
& van Knippenberg, 1998). Chaiken, Govender, & Pratto, 1992; Fazio,
Likewise, activating traits associated Sanbonmatsu, Powell, & Kardes, 1986).
with conformity causes people to agree Using a procedure similar to the activation
more with a group of confederates (Epley & of impressions and behaviors, Kawakami,
Gilovich, 1999); activating helpfulness traits Dovidio, and Dijksterhuis (2003) have even
causes people to be more helpful (Macrae & found that when a stereotype is made acces­
Johnston, 1998); activating aggression- sible, people report attitudes consistent
related ideas causes people to give longer with the stereotypical attitudes of that
“shocks” to another participant (Carver, group. For example, when the category of
Ganellen, Froming, & Chambers, 1983); “elderly” is accessible, people report more
activating the elderly stereotype causes conservative attitudes, and when the cate­
people to walk more slowly (Bargh et al., gory “skinhead” is accessible, people report
1996), increases slowness on a lexical more prejudiced attitudes. These effects
decision task (Dijksterhuis, Spears, & occur even when the categories were made
Lepinasse, 2001), and promotes poor accessible to participants with a subliminal
memory (Dijksterhuis, Aarts, Bargh, & priming procedure, again suggesting that this
van Knippenberg, 2000; Levy, 1996); and effect occurs without conscious awareness.
04-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 7:23 PM Page 63

Automatic Cognitive Processes and Nonverbal Communication–––◆–––63

GOALS concepts. In other words, what people


think, do, and feel can be influenced with­
In addition to impressions, behaviors, out conscious awareness, intention, or
and attitudes, it has been proposed that goals control. In the next section of this chapter,
can become active automatically. Because I turn to evidence for this contention specif­
goal-related information and behaviors are ically within the domain of nonverbal
represented mentally, if a goal is pursued fre­ communication.
quently and consistently in a particular con­
text or with a particular person, the context
or person alone will eventually be able to ♦ Automatic Nonverbal
activate the goal without intention or aware­ Communication
ness. The goal is then pursued just as
it would be if it had been instigated con­
sciously (Bargh, 1990; Shah, 2005). In sup­ There are several general pieces of evidence
port of this argument, Chartrand and Bargh to suggest that at least some nonverbal com­
(1996) replicated two well-known informa­ munication is automatic. First, and as noted,
tion-processing goal studies, but they insti­ there are numerous demands on relatively
gated the goals in participants without their limited conscious cognitive resources; the
awareness; the results were identical to those sheer amount of these resources needed to
obtained when participants had been pursu­ process all nonverbal communication in a
ing the goals consciously. controlled manner makes this possibility
Other work has shown that partici­ exceedingly unlikely. Second, because the
pants who had achievement goals activated basis of verbal communication is largely
outside of awareness performed better on a conscious, humans have a tendency to direct
task, and persisted on the task even when a their focus to what is said and not to focus
more attractive alternative was introduced, consciously on the nonverbal cues that are,
than participants who did not have an by definition, unsaid (DePaulo & Friedman,
active achievement goal (Bargh, Gollwitzer, 1998). Yet it is clear that this information
Lee-Chai, Barndollar, & Trötschel, 2001). is still being processed cognitively because
Cooperation goals (Bargh et al., 2001) and it regulates interactions effectively (see
affiliation goals (Lakin & Chartrand, 2003) Cappella & Schreiber, this volume). Third,
can also be pursued without conscious some nonverbal cues cannot be controlled
awareness or guidance. Significant others easily, and when efforts to control those
can even be a source of automatic goal behaviors are exerted, they are not usually
activation. For example, Shah (2003) has successful (see later sections in this chapter).
found that performance on an experimental Finally, although some researchers have
task increases when close significant others argued that nonverbal communication is
who would want participants to do well are deliberate and strategic, there is recent evi­
made accessible to them. dence that even strategic behaviors can
occur automatically (Lakin & Chartrand,
2003; Lakin, Chartrand, & Arkin, 2005);
SUMMARY consciousness is not required for behavior to
be either strategic or adaptive.
The research reviewed in these sections The empirical work reviewed in this chap­
has demonstrated that people’s impressions, ter so far has focused on automaticity gener­
behaviors, attitudes, and goal pursuits ally. I turn attention now to the subcategory
can be affected by automatically activated of these effects that is relevant to nonverbal
04-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 7:23 PM Page 64

64–––◆–––Foundations

communication directly (for another review, interpersonal distance, and eye contact
see Choi, Gray, & Ambady, 2005). Very (Bernieri, 1988; Bernieri, Davis, Rosenthal,
little research on nonverbal communication, & Knee, 1994; Grahe & Bernieri, 1999; see
however, has been conducted in such a way Tickle-Degnen, this volume). Marital status
as to show definitive evidence that the effects can even be determined via nonverbal
are occurring automatically (i.e., by reduc­ means, given the surprising finding that
ing the likelihood of consciousness being the longer people have been married, the
involved, by using funneled-debriefing pro­ more similar they look (Zajonc, Adelmann,
cedures, by having awareness checks, etc.). Murphy, & Neidenthal, 1987).
Nevertheless, there are several programs of Is the encoding and decoding of this
research that demonstrate automatic non­ social relationship information occurring
verbal communication indirectly. A sensitive automatically? As the majority of this
reader will no doubt note that the subcate­ research has not been conducted with an
gories listed below correspond with some of eye toward answering this specific question,
the chapters in the Contexts and Functions the answer must be inferred from the
sections of this Handbook. A brief, non- methodologies that have been used, as well
comprehensive review of these topics is cov­ as some of the research findings. This infor­
ered here to demonstrate that some of these mation suggests several reasons to accept
effects occur automatically. the argument that these processes are
occurring automatically. First, decoding of
social relationship information, as evi­
SOCIAL RELATIONSHIPS denced by research using the IPT, often
occurs quickly; clips from the task are less
Information about social relationships is than one minute, suggesting that people’s
relatively easy to infer from people’s nonver­ decoding of this information must also
bal behaviors. In terms of social status, dom­ occur very quickly (reducing the likelihood
inant people tend to stand taller, use more of conscious involvement). Second, certain
personal space, interrupt more, and talk types of conscious instructions interfere
louder than less dominant people (Henley, with the accuracy of decoding relationship
1977; Burgoon & Dunbar, this volume; but information (e.g., Patterson & Stockbridge,
see Hall, 2005). Powerful people look at 1998; Patterson, this volume). When this
others the same amount when speaking as type of interference happens, it suggests
when listening, but less powerful people that people have developed automatic
look more when listening than when they are and efficient strategies for processing the
speaking (Fehr & Exline, 1987). Relationship information, and conscious attention inter­
status can also be inferred from nonverbal feres with the use of the strategies on which
behaviors. Research using the Interpersonal people typically rely.
Perception Task (IPT) (Costanzo & Archer, Third, participants in studies such as the
1989; see Riggio, this volume), which con­ ones reviewed above can rarely identify the
tains video clips of less than a minute, has factors that influenced their judgments
demonstrated that one can accurately iden­ (e.g., Bernieri et al., 1994). One study even
tify when people are related and when they found that confederates who were
are in significant relationships (Smith, instructed to create liking with a partner
Archer, & Costanzo, 1991). Rapport could not identify accurately the behaviors
can also be determined from watching they used and how they used them, even
people interact, at least partially because rap­ though they were successful at their goal
port is related to interactional synchrony, (Palmer & Simmons, 1995). Research on
04-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 7:23 PM Page 65

Automatic Cognitive Processes and Nonverbal Communication–––◆–––65

nonconscious behavioral mimicry (reviewed as facial expressions, tone of voice, and


in the Rapport section below) has also gestures. One particular piece of evidence
demonstrated that people are not con­ that supports the idea that emotions can be
sciously aware that they mimic the behav­ encoded automatically is the fact that some
iors of others or that other people might be emotional expressions are instantaneous
mimicking their behaviors or that this mim­ and cannot be controlled (Ekman &
icry creates liking (Chartrand & Bargh, Davidson, 1994), two defining characteris­
1999; Chartrand, Maddux, & Lakin, tics of automatic processes. Certain emo­
2005). This lack of conscious awareness is tional facial expressions, like the Duchenne
the hallmark of an automatic process. smile (Frank, Ekman, & Friesen, 1993) or
Finally, an interesting line of research has blushing (Leary, Britt, Cutlip, & Temple-
demonstrated recently that postural comple­ ton, 1992), are argued to be spontaneous
mentarity, with regard to dominant and sub­ depictions of enjoyment or embarrassment,
missive nonverbal behaviors, occurs without respectively. The spontaneity associated
conscious intention or control (Tiedens & with these emotional expressions could be a
Fragale, 2003). In these studies, confederates result of an evolutionarily developed auto­
were instructed to engage in dominant (i.e., matic link between experiencing an emo­
postural expansion) or constricted (i.e., pos­ tion and nonverbal indicators of that
tural constriction) nonverbal behaviors, and emotion (Buck, 1984; Ekman, 1992, see
the nonverbal behaviors of participants were Floyd, this volume), similar to the perception-
measured. Results indicated that participants behavior link idea reviewed earlier.
exhibited complementary behaviors to those An alternative approach to understanding
of the confederate and that interactions the spontaneity associated with emotional
where complementarity occurred were expressions is the behavioral ecology view
reported to be more comfortable. Impor­ (Fridlund, 1994; Fridlund & Russell, this
tantly, extensive debriefing of all partici­ volume). This perspective suggests that facial
pants, using a funneled debriefing procedure displays do not reflect expressions of dis­
(see Bargh & Chartrand, 2000), revealed crete, internal, emotional states but rather
that they were not aware that complemen­ are messages that signal people’s intentions
tarity had occurred or that it had affected the within a particular context. Fridlund (1994)
comfort level of the interaction (Tiedens & argues that displays of facial expressions
Fragale, 2003). have evolved to meet specific selection pres­
sures and that because they reveal informa­
tion about people’s intentions, they are
EMOTIONAL EXPRESSION displayed to serve people’s social motives.
Although this approach offers a different
In addition to relationship informa­ explanation for what “emotional” facial
tion, people may encode information expressions mean, this perspective also sug­
automatically about their own emotions gests that facial expressions have evolved to
and decode the emotions of others. In serve specific purposes, can occur quickly
Emotional Contagion, Hatfield, Cacioppo, within a particular context, and often occur
and Rapson (1994) synthesize a wealth of without conscious intention or awareness.
data from a variety of research subareas Some emotional facial expressions are
supporting the idea that emotions are com­ also difficult to create consciously, suggest­
municated automatically and spread to ing that when these expressions occur,
other people. More often than not, this con­ they do so automatically. For example,
tagion occurs through nonverbal cues, such expressions of fear are almost impossible to
04-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 7:23 PM Page 66

66–––◆–––Foundations

re-create without truly being fearful (Ekman, suggesting that this decoding can occur
1985). Finally, a recent meta-analysis sug­ automatically.
gests that observers can detect accurately the
state and trait anxiety of others, although
the magnitude of these effects depends on PREJUDICE
the communication channel (Harrigan,
Wilson, & Rosenthal, 2004). Because anxi­ Prejudice, or negative feelings about
ety is a negative emotional state that most groups of people, can also be communi­
people would not want to convey to others, cated automatically through nonverbal cues
this information is probably being encoded (see Dovidio, Hebl, Richeson, & Shelton,
without intention or awareness. this volume). Because categorical informa­
Tone of voice may also convey informa­ tion (e.g., race, sex; Brewer, 1988; Fiske &
tion automatically about a speaker’s emo­ Neuberg, 1990) is processed automatically
tional state. Neumann and Strack (2000) in most cases, simply seeing a member of
had participants listen to a speech that was a stereotyped group can activate, without
delivered in either a slightly happy or a awareness, thoughts and feelings about that
slightly sad voice. After hearing the text, group (Blair, 2002; Devine, 1989; Fiske,
participants rated their own mood. People’s 1998). It has been argued that a dissocia­
moods were affected by the tone in which tion often exists between people’s conscious
the passage was read and, consistent with and unconscious attitudes toward minority
emotional contagion, participants automat­ groups; therefore, researchers have been
ically encoded this emotional information. able to measure both and determine the
When they repeated the text that they effects that each has on social interactions.
heard, they imitated the tone of the original This work has shown that nonverbal
reading spontaneously. This encoding behaviors like gaze, blinking, body posture,
occurred despite the fact that participants and interpersonal distance can indicate preju­
were not consciously aware of the original diced feelings toward interaction partners
tone of the message and were not given (Crosby, Bromley, & Saxe, 1980; McConnell
instructions to mimic the original tone. & Liebold, 2001; Towles-Schwen & Fazio,
The automaticity of encoding of emo­ 2003). In one demonstration, people who
tional expressions is demonstrated most indicated negative feelings toward Blacks on
clearly in research by Ulf Dimberg and an implicit measure of prejudice also blinked
colleagues (Dimberg 1982, 1997; Dimberg, more (a sign of uncomfortableness) and
Thunberg, & Elmehed, 2000). In his work, made less eye contact during an interaction
Dimberg had participants look at pictures with an African American partner (Dovidio,
of people displaying happy or angry facial Kawakami, Johnson, Johnson, & Howard,
expressions. He then measured their subtle 1997). Dovidio, Kawakami, and Gaertner
facial movements with electromyographic (2002) replicated this effect and showed that
(EMG) technology. The results showed that the less positive nonverbal behaviors of par­
participants moved the muscles associated ticipants who had implicit racial biases led
with smiling when looking at the happy the African American partners of these par­
pictures and the muscles associated with ticipants to feel that the interaction had been
frowning when looking at the angry pic­ less friendly.
tures. This effect occurred even when the Likewise, Vanman, Saltz, Nathan, and
facial expressions were presented outside Warren (2004) have also shown that facial
of conscious awareness (i.e., subliminally), EMG activity can be related to another
04-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 7:23 PM Page 67

Automatic Cognitive Processes and Nonverbal Communication–––◆–––67

person’s race; participants who had cheek instructions (Godfrey, Jones, & Lord,
EMG activity (i.e., activity that would be 1986). People copy the postures of interac­
associated with smiling) when viewing tion partners more when they are told those
pictures of White targets were more likely partners might be helpful than when oppor­
to nominate a White applicant for a tunities to get help from the partners are not
prestigious award than a Black applicant. available (La France, 1985). Nonverbal
Participants in these studies were not aware behaviors are also dependent on the people
of the negative nonverbal behaviors they for whom the display occurs (see DePaulo
encoded or the fact that these nonverbal & Friedman, 1998, for a review). Finally,
behaviors were affecting their interactions the ecological theory of perception suggests
negatively. This work therefore provides that people are able to gain, relatively auto­
evidence that both encoding and decoding matically, information about other people
of prejudicial feelings can occur automati­ from their appearances and movements,
cally. In fact, as Dovidio et al. (2002) have information that Gibson (1979) calls affor­
discussed, researchers first turned to non­ dances. This information is able to be gath­
verbal behaviors as indicators of prejudice ered easily because appearance and behavior
because of their hypothesized “leakiness” express some personality characteristics
(i.e., uncontrollability) and spontaneity. accurately (Zebrowitz & Collins, 1997).
Nonverbal communication is therefore
sensitive to conscious goals and situational
IMPRESSION MANAGEMENT constraints. Although this strategic use of
AND PERSONALITY EXPRESSION nonverbal behaviors suggests that their cog­
nitive basis is conscious, being strategic does
Although impression management and not demand consciousness. There are several
self-presentation can certainly be conscious lines of research that suggest that people
(see Keating, this volume), these commu­ alter their nonverbal behaviors automati­
nicative functions can also occur relatively cally. Tice, Butler, Muraven, and Stillwell
effortlessly and without conscious guid­ (1995) hypothesized that people’s default
ance, particularly in cases where people self-presentational strategy with strangers is
are not particularly motivated to convey a to present a positive image, whereas people’s
desired identity. Moreover, it is also clear default self-presentational strategy with
that people may infer traits and make attri­ friends is to present a modest image.
butions from the nonverbal behaviors of Consistent with this proposition, when par­
others automatically and that these judg­ ticipants behaved consistently with their
ments can be quite accurate. Together, this default tendencies, their self-presentations
literature suggests that people often express used few cognitive resources, which led to
their personality and decode personality increased memory of the details of the inter­
information about others automatically action. Participants who used nondefault
through nonverbal behaviors. self-presentational strategies (e.g., modesty
People tend to use different self-presenta­ with strangers) were not able to pursue these
tional strategies in different situations and strategies automatically and used conscious
with different types of people. For example, cognitive resources to accomplish their goals
when people are instructed to make an (see also Patterson, Churchill, Farag, &
interaction partner like them, they nod, Borden, 1991/1992). Other research has
smile, and make more eye contact than also demonstrated that self-presenting in a
do people who did not receive these way that is inconsistent with personality is
04-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 7:23 PM Page 68

68–––◆–––Foundations

cognitively taxing and requires attentional from nonverbal presentations and that
resources (Pontari & Schlenker, 2000). length of observation time did not signifi­
Attributional processes, whereby people cantly affect accuracy; accuracy was just as
infer traits and make dispositional or good when people observed behavior for
situational attributions, can also happen 30 seconds as it was when people observed
automatically. An extensive literature on behavior for five minutes (Ambady &
spontaneous trait inferences has demon­ Rosenthal, 1992). The fact that people
strated that simply presenting people with can be so accurate at decoding in such
descriptions of other people’s behaviors short time frames suggests that the process
results in inferring traits from them is likely not occurring with much conscious
(Uleman, 1999; Uleman et al., 1996). intention or guidance. Moreover, when
Participants in these studies were not conscious attention is devoted to this type
instructed to infer traits and typically even of task, decoding accuracy decreases (Gilbert
denied that they had done so (Winter & & Krull, 1988; Patterson & Stockbridge,
Uleman, 1984). Related research has found 1998).
that people might also make emotion
inferences when reading descriptions of
other’s behavior automatically (Gernsbacher, EXPECTANCIES
Goldsmith, & Robertson, 1992). Finally,
people make both dispositional and situa­ Just as people convey personality through
tional attributions without the use of limited their nonverbal behaviors, expectancies that a
cognitive resources or conscious intention person holds for others are also conveyed
(Gilbert, Pelham, & Krull, 1988; Krull, through nonverbal behaviors, seemingly with­
1993). Thus, when people perceive the out intention or awareness (see Remland,
behaviors of others, they seem to decode this volume). Communication of these
this information effortlessly and uninten­ expectancies—in certain contexts and under
tionally, at least in most circumstances. certain conditions—results in a self-fulfilling
Automatic self-presentation, impression prophecy, whereby perceivers elicit behav­
management, trait inferences, and attribu­ iors that are consistent with their original
tions are likely to be beneficial to most ideas and then conclude that their expectan­
people in most situations but only to the cies were truthful, without realizing the role
extent that these relatively effortless catego­ that they have played in confirming them
rizations are correct. Ambady and her col­ (Rosenthal, 2003). For example, researchers’
leagues have demonstrated that people’s behaviors can affect the responses of their
automatic decodings of the behaviors of participants (Rosenthal, 1976), and the
others are remarkably accurate (Ambady, expectations of teachers have been found
Bernieri, & Richeson, 2000; Gray & to affect the behaviors of their students
Ambady, this volume). In one empirical (Rosenthal & Jacobson, 1968). That people
demonstration, participants watched video are unaware that they have elicited the
clips of teachers’ nonverbal behaviors, behavior they expected suggests that these
some as short as six seconds, and then expectancies are being conveyed automati­
judged teacher effectiveness. Participants’ cally. This is particularly the case when nega­
ratings were strongly correlated with tive expectancies are conveyed, which would
students’ end-of-semester ratings (Ambady likely be controlled if people were aware of
& Rosenthal, 1993). them (e.g., Word, Zanna, & Cooper, 1974).
A meta-analysis has revealed a medium to There is also evidence that people are
large effect size for accuracy of predictions affected automatically by the expectancies
04-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 7:23 PM Page 69

Automatic Cognitive Processes and Nonverbal Communication–––◆–––69

that others have for them (i.e., automatic conscious awareness (e.g., Bavelas, Black,
decoding of expectancies). Word et al. Lemery, & Mullett, 1986; Bernieri, 1988;
(1974) showed that when White partici­ Bernieri, Reznick, & Rosenthal, 1988;
pants in a second study were treated as La France, 1979, 1982; La France &
Black participants had been treated in a first Broadbent, 1976). Because this research
study (e.g., abrupt questioning, lack of eye does not typically include awareness checks
contact), the quality of the interaction or attempts to rule out conscious involve­
decreased. The White participants stuttered ment, the automaticity of these effects is,
more and made less eye contact as a result of again, inferred from participants’ apparent
the expectancies for a less successful inter­ lack of awareness.
view that the interviewer was conveying. There is more definitive evidence that
Chen and Bargh (1997) found a similar behavioral mimicry can occur noncon­
result: When stereotypes of African Americans sciously, however, and that this automatic
were activated outside of conscious aware­ mimicry results in smoother interactions
ness, participants treated an interaction and the development of liking. Chartrand
partner in a way that was consistent and Bargh (1999) found that participants
with this stereotype, eliciting more hostility. mimicked the nonverbal behaviors of a con­
Participants were unaware that the stereo­ federate without conscious awareness. In a
type had been activated and were unaware second study, they found that mimicry
that their own behavior was influencing the leads to increased liking for the mimicker
behavior of their interaction partner. The and smoother interactions. Thus, mimick­
results of this study are consistent with ing others and being mimicked can commu­
many other research findings that stereotype nicate rapport automatically. This finding
activation results in stereotype-consistent is consistent with the work of Lakin and
behaviors (see Wheeler & Petty, 2001, for Chartrand (2003), who found that partici­
a review). Again, these studies demonstrate pants who pursue an unconscious affilia­
consistently that people are not aware that tion goal are more likely to mimic the
their behavior is affected or is being affected behaviors of an interaction partner than
by the expectancies of others. participants who do not have an active
affiliation goal (see also Lakin, Jefferis,
Cheng, & Chartrand, 2003).
RAPPORT A second study expanded on this finding
by giving participants who had a noncon­
Not all behaviors that occur outside of sciously activated affiliation goal a success
awareness are problematic. For example, or failure experience (Lakin & Chartrand,
many people have been interested in the 2003). Participants either succeeded (i.e.,
relationship between posture sharing (either had a pleasant interaction with a confeder­
mimicking or mirroring) and the develop­ ate) or failed at their goal (i.e., had a rela­
ment and maintenance of rapport (for a tively less pleasant interaction with a
review, see Tickle-Degnen, this volume). confederate). In a subsequent interaction
The fact that people mimic the nonverbal with a different confederate, participants
behaviors of others (both significant others who still had affiliation goals (i.e., those who
and strangers) has long been established initially failed) mimicked the nonverbal
(Chartrand et al., 2005). Further work has behaviors of their interaction partner more
demonstrated that people tend to mimic than participants who had been successful.
others and thereby demonstrate or develop This effect occurred despite the fact that par­
rapport without intention, control, or ticipants did not consciously know they were
04-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 7:23 PM Page 70

70–––◆–––Foundations

pursuing an affiliation goal and did not typically compares participants who are
consciously acknowledge the confederate’s mimicked with participants who are not
behaviors or how the confederate’s behav­ mimicked, these effects occur arguably as a
iors were affecting their own behaviors. direct result of the positive feelings that
A recent social exclusion experience may occur automatically with mimicry. The
also lead to increases in behavioral mimicry majority of this work includes awareness
(Lakin et al., 2005; Lakin & Chartrand, checks and other methodological strategies
2005). The need to belong is one of the that reduce the role of consciousness in
strongest core motivations, and it affects these effects (Bargh & Chartrand, 2000).
people’s thoughts, feelings, and behaviors
frequently (Williams, Forgas, & von Hippel,
2005). It is therefore not surprising that ♦ Discussion and
when people have been excluded from a Future Directions
social group, they will engage in behaviors
that help them to create liking and allow
them to reenter the group. Recent research As the literatures reviewed above demon­
has shown that mimicking the nonverbal strate, nonverbal behaviors can communi­
behaviors of group members may be one cate information automatically about social
such strategy (Lakin et al., 2005). When relationships, emotions, prejudice, person­
participants were excluded from a comput­ ality, and expectations, as well as indicate
erized ball-tossing game and then interacted relationship status and rapport. As a whole,
with a confederate in a different context, this research compellingly argues that non­
they mimicked the behaviors of the confed­ verbal communication has, at least in part,
erate more than when they had not been its basis in automatic processes. There are,
excluded during the ball-toss game. In other however, several important topics that
words, participants who were trying to cre­ deserve research attention.
ate liking and affiliation were able to pur­
sue this goal through mimicking another METHODOLOGICAL CONCERNS
person. Together with the findings from
Lakin and Chartrand (2003), this work Whereas not all the research reviewed here
suggests that people can pursue a goal to has provided definitive evidence that the
develop rapport or liking by mimicking the processes are occurring without awareness,
behaviors of others automatically. intention, control, or cognitive effort, the
Finally, paradigms that involve having a several lines of research that do provide
confederate mimic the behaviors of partici­ this evidence demonstrate that nonverbal
pants have demonstrated that being mimic­ communication can and does occur auto­
ked causes people to be more interdependent matically. Moreover, the research that does
(van Baaren, Maddux, Chartrand, de Bouter, not contain definitive evidence often has
& van Knippenberg, 2003), be more helpful methodological characteristics suggesting
(van Baaren, Holland, Kawakami, & van that people are not aware of what their non­
Knippenberg, 2004), and provide bigger tips verbal behaviors are communicating or the
(van Baaren, Holland, Steenaert, & van extent to which their own thoughts, feelings,
Knippenberg, 2003; for an overview, see and behaviors are being affected by the non­
Tickle-Degnen, this volume). Mimicry also verbal communications of others.
leads to greater persuasion (Bailenson & Although this suggests that a significant
Yee, 2005). Because work on mimicry component of nonverbal communication
04-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 7:23 PM Page 71

Automatic Cognitive Processes and Nonverbal Communication–––◆–––71

is occurring automatically, to determine Future work in nonverbal communication


definitively the role that automatic processes would benefit from explicit acknowledgment
play in nonverbal communication, researchers of the role that both automatic and con­
should consider including methodological trolled processes play in the encoding and
strategies in future research that would reduce decoding of nonverbal behaviors. For
the role of controlled processes. For example, example, Patterson (2001, this volume) has
participants could be encouraged to make taken this approach in his parallel process
judgments and decisions quickly to reduce model. He integrated encoding and decod­
their reliance on conscious processes, or they ing of nonverbal communication into a sin­
could be put under cognitive load so that their gle framework by suggesting that these
available cognitive resources would be split processes occur in parallel and that they
among several tasks. Researchers could also both can be more or less controlled cogni­
include awareness checks or funneled debrief­ tive processes. In addition, he argued that
ings to determine how aware people are of factors like biology, culture, personality, sit­
their cognitive processes (Bargh & Chartrand, uational constraints, interpersonal expectan­
2000). By using these sorts of methodologies, cies, affect, and goals can affect people’s
future research will be better able to deter­ social cognition. The parallel process model
mine the relative contribution of automatic therefore synthesizes much of the work that
processes to nonverbal communication. social psychologists, communication schol­
ars, and others were conducting on nonver­
bal communication already (see Patterson,
ROLE OF CONTROLLED 2001, for a review), which is an important
PROCESSES first step in creating a more complete
understanding of nonverbal communica­
Even though I have argued that much of tion processes.
nonverbal communication appears to occur This model also has several interesting
automatically, it would be a mistake to implications for the study of the cognitive
assume that there is no controlled cognitive processes related to nonverbal communica­
basis to nonverbal communication as well. tion. First, the parallel process model implies
We may be unaware of the nonverbal cues that people are sophisticated users and con­
that we encode typically, but we can direct sumers of nonverbal cues. The fact that
our “internal eye” easily onto these behaviors encoding and decoding occur in parallel
and therefore become more aware of them. demonstrates that neither process requires
People may not process the nonverbal mes­ extensive cognitive resources (i.e., that nei­
sages they receive from others consciously, ther process is particularly effortful). This
but if something causes a disruption in their implication is consistent with my argument
automatic processing (e.g., an unusual event), that much of nonverbal communication
or if people are motivated to learn about occurs automatically, perhaps even that
another person, that process can easily which appears to be used relatively strategi­
become conscious. Finally, people may even cally. Arguably, we have evolved automatic
try to control (i.e., with intention and strategies to deal with the incredible
awareness, and with varying degrees of suc­ amounts of social information that we
cess) their nonverbal behaviors (although must process and to help us accomplish
even control can become automatized with our important objectives and goals. Given
enough practice; see Kawakami, Dovidio, the importance of nonverbal cues, it is
Moll, Hermsen, & Russin, 2000). not surprising that automatic nonverbal
04-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 7:23 PM Page 72

72–––◆–––Foundations

communication would also be evolutionarily relationships, emotions, prejudices, person­


adaptive. alities, and expectations, as well as indicates
existing levels of rapport or the desire to
create rapport. The cognitive processes that
IMPORTANCE OF underlie this important source of informa­
COGNITIVE RESOURCES tion, however, are not always automatic.
Parallel process models, or other models that
Another interesting area for future recognize explicitly the role for both auto­
research to explore is the role of cognitive matic and controlled processes in nonverbal
resources in nonverbal communication. communication, will likely represent the
Because cognitive resources are limited, the future of research in this area. Ironically,
amount of cognitive resources that a person with more awareness of automatic processes,
has will be a crucial factor determining we should have a greater appreciation of the
whether a person is able to use controlled importance of nonverbal communication.
processes to decode nonverbal communi­
cation. For example, the parallel process
model would predict that when cognitive ♦ References
resources are devoted to encoding or decod­
ing for whatever reason (i.e., we are particu­
larly motivated to convey an impression, we Ambady, N., Bernieri, F. J., & Richeson, J. A.
are interested in an interaction partner’s (2000). Toward a histology of social behav­
ior: Judgmental accuracy from thin slices
intentions), they will be less available to the
of the behavioral stream. In M. P. Zanna
other process (because they occur in paral­
(Ed.), Advances in experimental social psy­
lel). In other words, if we have to think chology (Vol. 32, pp. 201–271). San Diego,
about encoding, fewer resources can be CA: Academic Press.
devoted to decoding. This could have impor­ Ambady, N., & Rosenthal, R. (1992). Thin slices
tant implications for situations where people of expressive behavior as predictors of inter­
need to be focused specifically on one personal consequences: A meta-analysis.
process or the other. Alternatively, because Psychological Bulletin, 111, 256–274.
both the sending and the receiving of non­ Ambady, N., & Rosenthal, R. (1993). Half a
verbal cues can be relatively automatic, one minute: Predicting teacher evaluations from
could argue that devoting resources to one thin slices of nonverbal behavior and phys­
process would not necessarily cause the other ical attractiveness. Journal of Personality
and Social Psychology, 64, 431–441.
to deteriorate (because it would be relatively
Bailenson, J. N., & Yee, N. (2005). Digital
automatic). Future research on nonverbal
chameleons: Automatic assimilation of
communication will need to explore these nonverbal gestures in immersive virtual
different possibilities; determining how cog­ environments. Psychological Science, 16,
nitive resources affect nonverbal communi­ 814–819.
cation will provide important insight into Bargh, J. A. (1990). Auto-motives: Preconscious
how automatic these processes really are. determinants of social interaction. In E. T.
Higgins & R. Sorrentino (Eds.), Handbook
of motivation and cognition (Vol. 2,
♦ Conclusion pp. 93–130). New York: Guilford Press.
Bargh, J. A. (1994). The four horsemen of auto­
maticity: Awareness, intention, efficiency,
Nonverbal communication conveys infor­ and control in social cognition. In R. S.
mation automatically about our social Wyer & T. K. Srull (Eds.), Handbook of
04-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 7:23 PM Page 73

Automatic Cognitive Processes and Nonverbal Communication–––◆–––73

social cognition (2nd ed., pp. 1–40). Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin,
Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. 20, 303–311.
Bargh, J. A. (1996). Automaticity in social Bernieri, F. J., Reznick, J. S., & Rosenthal,
psychology. In E. T. Higgins & A. W. R. (1988). Synchrony, pseudosynchrony,
Kruglanski (Eds.), Social psychology: and dissynchrony: Measuring the entertain­
Handbook of basic principles (pp. 169– ment process in mother–infant interactions.
183). New York: Guilford Press. Journal of Personality and Social Psycho­
Bargh, J. A. (1997). The automaticity of everyday logy, 54, 243–253.
life. In R. S. Wyer (Ed.), The automaticity of Blair, I. V. (2002). The malleability of automatic
everyday life: Advances in social cognition stereotypes and prejudice. Personality and
(Vol. 10, pp. 1–61). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. Social Psychology Review, 6, 242–261.
Bargh, J. A., Chaiken, S., Govender, R., & Brewer, M. B. (1988). A dual process model of
Pratto, F. (1992). The generality of the impression formation. In T. K. Srull & R. S.
automatic attitude activation effect. Journal Wyer (Eds.), Advances in social cognition
of Personality and Social Psychology, 62, (Vol. 1, pp. 1–36). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum
893–912. Associates.
Bargh, J. A., & Chartrand, T. L. (1999). The Buck, R. (1984). The communication of emo­
unbearable automaticity of being. American tion. New York: Guilford.
Psychologist, 54, 462–479. Carver, C. S., Ganellen, R. J., Froming, W. J.,
Bargh, J. A., & Chartrand, T. L. (2000). The & Chambers, W. (1983). Modeling: An
mind in the middle: A practical guide to analysis in terms of category accessibility.
priming and automaticity research. In H. T. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology,
Reis & C. M. Judd (Eds.), Handbook of 19, 403–421.
research methods in social and personality Chartrand, T. L., & Bargh, J. A. (1996).
psychology (pp. 253–285). New York: Automatic activation of impression forma­
Cambridge University Press. tion and memorization goals: Nonconscious
Bargh, J. A., Chen, M., & Burrows, L. (1996). goal priming reproduces effects of explicit
Automaticity of social behavior: Direct task instructions. Journal of Personality and
effects of trait construct and stereotype acti­ Social Psychology, 71, 464–478.
vation on action. Journal of Personality and Chartrand, T. L., & Bargh, J. A. (1999). The
Social Psychology, 71, 230–244. chameleon effect: The perception–behavior
Bargh, J. A., Gollwitzer, P. M., Lee-Chai, A., link and social interaction. Journal of
Barndollar, K., & Trötschel, R. (2001). The Personality and Social Psychology, 76,
automated will: Nonconscious activation 893–910.
and pursuit of behavioral goals. Journal of Chartrand, T. L., Maddux, W. W., & Lakin, J.
Personality and Social Psychology, 81, L. (2005). Beyond the perception–behavior
1014–1027. link: The ubiquitous utility and motiva­
Bavelas, J. B., Black, A., Lemery, C. R., & tional moderators of nonconscious mim­
Mullett, J. (1986). “I show how you feel”: icry. In R. R. Hassin, J. S. Uleman, &
Motor mimicry as a communicative act. J. A. Bargh (Eds.), The new unconscious
Journal of Personality and Social Psycho­ (pp. 334–361). New York: Oxford
logy, 50, 322–329. University Press.
Bernieri, F. J. (1988). Coordinated movement Chen, M., & Bargh, J. A. (1997). Nonconscious
and rapport in teacher-student interac­ behavioral confirmation processes: The self-
tions. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 12, fulfilling nature of automatically-activated
120–138. stereotypes. Journal of Experimental Social
Bernieri, F. J., Davis, J. M., Rosenthal, R., & Psychology, 33, 541–560.
Knee, C. R. (1994). Interactional synchrony Choi, Y. S., Gray, H. M., & Ambady, N. (2005).
and rapport: Measuring synchrony in dis­ The glimpsed world: Unintended communi­
plays devoid of sound and facial affect. cation and unintended perception. In
04-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 7:23 PM Page 74

74–––◆–––Foundations

R. R. Hassin, J. S. Uleman, & J. A. Bargh Dimberg, U. (1997). Psychophysiological reac­


(Eds.), The new unconscious (pp. 309–333). tions to facial expressions. In U. Segerstrale
New York: Oxford University Press. & P. Molnar (Eds.), Nonverbal communica­
Costanzo, M., & Archer, D. (1989). Interpreting tion: Where nature meets culture (pp. 47–60).
the expressive behavior of others: The Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Interpersonal Perception Task. Journal of Dimberg, U., Thunberg, M., & Elmehed, K.
Nonverbal Behavior, 13, 225–245. (2000). Unconscious facial reactions to
Crosby, F., Bromley, S., & Saxe, L. (1980). emotional facial expressions. Psychological
Recent unobtrusive studies of Black and Science, 11, 86–89.
White discrimination and prejudice: A liter­ Dovidio, J. F., Kawakami, K., & Gaertner, S. L.
ature review. Psychological Bulletin, 87, (2002). Implicit and explicit prejudice
546–563. and interracial interaction. Journal of
DePaulo, B. M., & Friedman, H. S. (1998). Personality and Social Psychology, 82,
Nonverbal communication. In D. T. 62–68.
Gilbert, S. T. Fiske, & G. Lindzey (Eds.), Dovidio, J. F., Kawakami, K., Johnson, C.,
The handbook of social psychology (4th Johnson, B., & Howard, A. (1997). On the
ed., pp. 3–40). New York: Oxford nature of prejudice: Automatic and con­
University Press. trolled processes. Journal of Experimental
Devine, P. G. (1989). Stereotypes and prejudice: Social Psychology, 33, 510–540.
Their automatic and controlled compo­ Ekman, P. (1985). Telling lies. New York:
nents. Journal of Personality and Social Norton.
Psychology, 56, 5–18. Ekman, P. (1992). An argument for basic emo­
Dijksterhuis, A., Aarts, H., Bargh, J. A., & van tions. Cognition and Emotion, 6, 169–200.
Knippenberg, A. (2000). On the relation Ekman, P., & Davidson, R. J. (Eds.). (1994).
between associative strength and automatic The nature of emotion. New York: Oxford
behavior. Journal of Experimental Social University Press.
Psychology, 36, 531–544. Epley, N., & Gilovich, T. (1999). Just going
Dijksterhuis, A., & Bargh, J. A. (2001). The along: Nonconscious priming and confor­
perception–behavior expressway: Automatic mity to social pressure. Journal of
effects of social perception on social behav­ Personality and Social Psychology, 35,
ior. In M. P. Zanna (Ed.), Advances in 578–589.
Experimental Social Psychology (Vol. 33, Fazio, R. H., Sanbonmatsu, D. M., Powell,
pp. 1–40). San Diego, CA: Academic Press. M. C., & Kardes, F. R. (1986). On the
Dijksterhuis, A., & van Knippenberg, A. (1998). automatic activation of attitudes. Journal of
The relation between perception and behav­ Personality and Social Psychology, 50,
ior or how to win a game of Trivial Pursuit. 229–238.
Journal of Personality and Social Psycho­ Fehr, B. J., & Exline, R. V. (1987). Social visual
logy, 74, 865–877. interaction: A conceptual and literature
Dijksterhuis, A., & van Knippenberg, A. (2000). review. In S. Feldstein & A. W. Siegman
Behavioral indecision: Effects of self-focus (Eds.), Nonverbal behavior and communi­
on automatic behavior. Social Cognition, cation (2nd ed., pp. 225–325). Hillsdale,
18, 55–74. NJ: Erlbaum.
Dijksterhuis, A., Spears, R., & Lepinasse, V. Fiske, S. T. (1998). Stereotyping, prejudice, and
(2001). Reflecting and deflecting stereo­ discrimination. In D. T. Gilbert, S. T. Fiske,
types: Assimilation and contrast in impres­ & G. Lindzey (Eds.), The handbook of
sion formation and automatic behavior. social psychology (4th ed., pp. 357–411).
Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, New York: Oxford University Press.
37, 286–299. Fiske, S. T., & Neuberg, S. L. (1990). A con­
Dimberg, U. (1982). Facial reactions to tinuum of impression formation, from
facial expressions. Psychophysiology, 19, category-based to individuating processes:
643–647. Influences of information and motivation
04-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 7:23 PM Page 75

Automatic Cognitive Processes and Nonverbal Communication–––◆–––75

on attention and interpretation. In Henley, N. M. (1977). Body politics: Power, sex


M. P. Zanna (Ed.), Advances in Exper­ and nonverbal communication. Englewood
imental Social Psychology (Vol. 23, pp. 1–74). Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall.
San Diego, CA: Academic Press. Higgins, E. T. (1996). Knowledge activation:
Frank, M. G., Ekman, P., & Friesen, W. V. Accessibility, applicability, and salience. In
(1993). Behavioral markers and recogniz­ E. T. Higgins & A. W. Kruglanski (Eds.),
ability of the smile of enjoyment. Journal Social psychology: Handbook of basic prin­
of Personality and Social Psychology, 64, ciples (pp. 133–168). New York: Guilford
83–93. Press.
Fridlund, A. J. (1994). Human facial expression. Higgins, E. T., Rholes, W. S., & Jones, C. R.
San Diego, CA: Academic Press. (1977). Category accessibility and impres­
Gernsbacher, M. A., Goldsmith, H. H., & sion formation. Journal of Experimental
Robertson, R. R. (1992). Do readers men­ Social Psychology, 13, 141–154.
tally represent characters’ emotional states? Krull, D. S. (1993). Does the grist change the
Cognition and Emotion, 6, 89–111. mill? The effect of the perceiver’s inferential
Gibson, J. J. (1979). The ecological approach goal on the process of social inference.
to visual perception. Boston: Houghton Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin,
Mifflin. 19, 340–348.
Gilbert, D. T., & Krull, D. S. (1988). Seeing less Kawakami, K., Dovidio, J. F., & Dijksterhuis,
and knowing more: The benefits of percep­ A. (2003). Effect of social category priming
tual ignorance. Journal of Personality and on personal attitudes. Psychological
Social Psychology, 54, 193–202. Science, 14, 315–319.
Gilbert, D. T., Pelham, B. W., & Krull, D. S. Kawakami, K., Dovidio, J. F., Moll, J.,
(1988). On cognitive busyness: When person Hermsen, S., & Russin, A. (2000). Just say
perceivers meet persons perceived. Journal of no (to stereotyping): Effects of training
Personality and Social Psychology, 54, in the negation of stereotypic associations
733–740. on stereotype activation. Journal of
Godfrey, D. K., Jones, E. E., & Lord, C. G. Personality and Social Psychology, 78,
(1986). Self-promotion is not ingratiating. 871–888.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychol­ La France, M. (1979). Nonverbal synchrony and
ogy, 50, 106–115. rapport: Analysis by the cross-lag panel
Grahe, J. E., & Bernieri, F. J. (1999). The impor­ technique. Social Psychology Quarterly,
tance of nonverbal cues in judging rapport. 42, 66–70.
Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 23, La France, M. (1982). Posture mirroring and
253–269. rapport. In M. Davis (Ed.), Interaction
Hall, J. A. (2005). Meta-analysis of nonverbal rhythms: Periodicity in communicative
behavior. In V. Manusov (Ed.), The source- behavior (pp. 279–298). New York:
book of nonverbal measures: Going beyond Human Sciences Press.
words (pp. 483–492). Mahwah, NJ: La France, M. (1985). Postural mirroring and
Erlbaum Associates. intergroup relations. Personality and Social
Harrigan, J. A., Wilson, K., & Rosenthal, R. Psychology Bulletin, 11, 207–217.
(2004). Detecting state and trait anxiety La France, M., & Broadbent, M. (1976). Group
from auditory and visual cues: A meta­ rapport: Posture sharing as a nonverbal
analysis. Personality and Social Psychology indicator. Group and Organization Studies,
Bulletin, 30, 56–66. 1, 328–333.
Hassin, R. R., Uleman, J. S., & Bargh, J. A. Lakin, J. L., & Chartrand, T. L. (2003). Using
(Eds.). (2005). The new unconscious. New nonconscious behavioral mimicry to create
York: Oxford University Press. affiliation and rapport. Psychological Science,
Hatfield, E., Cacioppo, J. T., & Rapson, R. L. 14, 334–339.
(1994). Emotional contagion. Cambridge: Lakin, J. L., & Chartrand, T. L. (2005). Exclusion
Cambridge University Press. and nonconscious behavioral mimicry. In
04-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 7:23 PM Page 76

76–––◆–––Foundations

K. D. Williams, J. P. Forgas, & W. von Patterson, M. L., & Stockbridge, E. (1998).


Hippel (Eds.), The social outcast: Ostracism, Effects of cognitive demand and judgment
social exclusion, rejection, and bullying strategy on person perception accuracy.
(pp. 279–295). New York: Psychology Press. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 22,
Lakin, J. L., Chartrand, T. L., & Arkin, R. M. 253–263.
(2005). I am too just like you: Nonconscious Pontari, B. A., & Schlenker, B. R. (2000). The
mimicry as a behavioral response to social influence of cognitive load on self-presenta­
exclusion. Unpublished manuscript. tion: Can cognitive busyness help as well
Lakin, J. L., Jefferis, V. E., Cheng, C. M., & as harm social performance? Journal of
Chartrand, T. L. (2003). The chameleon Personality and Social Psychology, 78,
effect as social glue: Evidence for the evolu­ 1092–1108.
tionary significance of nonconscious mim­ Posner, M. I., & Snyder, C. R. R. (1975).
icry. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 27, Attention and cognitive control. In R. L.
145–162. Solso (Ed.), Information processing and cog­
Leary, M. R., Britt, T. W., Cutlip, W. D., II., & nition: The Loyola symposium (pp. 55–85).
Templeton, J. L. (1992). Social blushing. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Psychological Bulletin, 112, 446–460. Rosenthal, R. (1976). Experimenter effects in
Levy, B. (1996). Improving memory in old age behavioral research. New York: Irvington.
through implicit self-stereotyping. Journal Rosenthal, R. (2003). Covert communication in
of Personality and Social Psychology, 71, laboratories, classrooms, and the truly real
1092–1107. world. Current Directions in Psychological
Macrae, C. N., & Johnston, L. (1998). Help, Science, 12, 151–154.
I need somebody: Automatic action and Rosenthal, R., & Jacobson, L. (1968).
inaction. Social Cognition, 16, 400–417. Pygmalion in the classroom. New York:
McConnell, A. R., & Liebold, J. M. (2001). Holt, Rinehart, & Winston.
Relations among the Implicit Association Shah, J. Y. (2003). Automatic for the people:
Test, discriminatory behavior, and explicit How representations of significant others
measures of racial attitudes. Journal of implicitly affect goal pursuit. Journal of
Experimental Social Psychology, 37, Personality and Social Psychology, 84,
435–442. 661–681.
Neisser, U. (1967). Cognitive psychology. Shah, J. Y. (2005). The automatic pursuit and
Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall. management of goals. Current Directions in
Neumann, R., & Strack, F. (2000). “Mood con­ Psychological Science, 14, 10–13.
tagion”: The automatic transfer of mood Shiffrin, R. M., & Schneider, W. (1977).
between persons. Journal of Personality Controlled and automatic human informa­
and Social Psychology, 79, 211–223. tion processing: II. Perceptual learning,
Palmer, M. T., & Simmons, K. B. (1995). automatic attending and a general theory.
Communicating intentions through nonver­ Psychological Review, 84, 127–190.
bal behaviors: Conscious and nonconscious Smith, H. J., Archer, D., & Costanzo, M. (1991).
encoding of liking. Human Communication “Just a hunch”: Accuracy and awareness
Research, 22, 128–160. in person perception. Journal of Nonverbal
Patterson, M. L. (2001). Toward a comprehen­ Behavior, 15, 3–18.
sive model of nonverbal communication. In Srull, T. K., & Wyer, R. S. (1979). The role
W. P. Robinson & H. Giles (Eds.), The new of category accessibility in the interpreta­
handbook of language and social psychol­ tion of information about persons: Some
ogy (pp. 159–176). New York: Wiley. determinants and implications. Journal of
Patterson, M. L., Churchill, M. E., Farag, F., & Personality and Social Psychology, 37,
Borden, E. (1991/1992). Impression man­ 1660–1672.
agement, cognitive demand, and inter­ Steele, C. M., & Aronson, J. (1995). Stereotype
personal sensitivity. Current Psychology: threat and the intellectual test perfor­
Research & Reviews, 10, 263–271. mance of African Americans. Journal of
04-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 7:23 PM Page 77

Automatic Cognitive Processes and Nonverbal Communication–––◆–––77

Personality and Social Psychology, 69, of Personality and Social Psychology, 84,
797–811. 1093–1102.
Tice, D. M., Butler, J. L., Muraven, M. B., & Vanman, E. J., Saltz, J. L., Nathan, L. R., &
Stillwell, A. M. (1995). When modesty Warren, J. A. (2004). Racial discrimination
prevails: Differential favorability of self- by low-prejudiced Whites facial movements
presentation to friends and strangers. Journal as implicit measures of attitudes related
of Personality and Social Psychology, 69, to behavior. Psychological Science, 15,
1120–1138. 711–714.
Tiedens, L. Z., & Fragale, A. R. (2003). Power Wheeler, S. C., Jarvis, W. B. G., & Petty, R. E.
moves: Complementarity in dominant and (2000). Think unto others: The self-destructive
submissive nonverbal behavior. Journal impact of negative racial stereotypes.
of Personality and Social Psychology, 84, Journal of Experimental Social Psychology,
558–568. 37, 173–180.
Towles-Schwen, T., & Fazio, R. H. (2003). Wheeler, S. C., & Petty, R. E. (2001). The effects
Choosing social situations: The relation of stereotype activation on behavior: A
between automatically activated racial atti­ review of possible mechanisms. Psychological
tudes and anticipated comfort interacting Bulletin, 127, 797–826.
with African Americans. Personality and Williams, K. D., Forgas, J. P., & von Hippel, W.
Social Psychology Bulletin, 29, 170–182. (Eds.). (2005). The social outcast: Ostracism,
Uleman, J. S. (1999). Spontaneous versus inten­ social exclusion, rejection, and bullying.
tional inferences in impression forma­ New York: Psychology Press.
tion. In S. Chaiken & Y. Trope (Eds.), Winter, L., & Uleman, J. S. (1984). When are
Dual-process theories in social psychology social judgments made? Evidence for the
(pp. 141–160). New York: Guilford Press. spontaneousness of trait inferences. Journal
Uleman, J. S., Newman, L. S., & Moskowitz, of Personality and Social Psychology, 47,
G. B. (1996). People as flexible interpreters: 237–252.
Evidence and issues from spontaneous trait Word, C. O., Zanna, M. P., & Cooper, J.
inference. In M. P. Zanna (Ed.), Advances in (1974). The nonverbal mediation of self-
Experimental Social Psychology (Vol. 28, fulfilling prophecies in interracial interac­
pp. 179–211). San Diego, CA: Academic Press. tion. Journal of Experimental Social
van Baaren, R. B., Holland, R. W., Kawakami, K., Psychology, 10, 109–120.
& van Knippenberg, A. (2004). Mimicry and Zajonc, R. B. (1980). Feeling and thinking:
prosocial behavior. Psychological Science, Preferences need no inferences. American
15, 71–74. Psychologist, 35, 151–175.
van Baaren, R. B., Holland, R. W., Steenaert, B., Zajonc, R. B., Adelmann, P. K., Murphy, S. T.,
& van Knippenberg, A. (2003). Mimicry & Neidenthal, P. M. (1987). Convergence
for money: Behavioral consequences of in the physical appearance of spouses.
imitation. Journal of Experimental Social Motivation and Emotion, 11, 335–346.
Psychology, 39, 393–398. Zebrowitz, L. A., & Collins, M. A. (1997).
van Baaren, R. B., Maddux, W. W., Chartrand, Accurate social perception at zero acquain­
T. L., de Bouter, C., & van Knippenberg, A. tance: The affordances of a Gibsonian
(2003). It takes two to mimic: Behavioral approach. Personality and Social Psychology
consequences of self-construals. Journal Review, 1, 204–223.
04-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 7:23 PM Page 78
05-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 7:24 PM Page 79

5
NONVERBAL SKILLS AND ABILITIES
� Ronald E. Riggio
Kravis Leadership Institute,
Claremont McKenna College

T he practice of nonverbal communication relies on a range of innate


and developed foundational processes. One foundation underlying
the use of nonverbal communication focuses on individual differences in
the abilities to communicate nonverbally. This ability, or skill, approach
is akin to a personality perspective in that it focuses on individual differ­
ences in the sending (encoding), receiving (decoding), and regulation
(management) of nonverbal communication (for more on personality,
see Gifford, this volume). In other words, people vary in their abilities to
convey nonverbal messages to others accurately, “read” others’ nonver­
bal communications, and monitor and control their nonverbal displays.
The skill approach to nonverbal communication was advanced in
a 1979 book edited by Robert Rosenthal, Skill in Nonverbal Com­
munication: Individual Differences, and by the work of Rosenthal and
colleagues (1979) on the Profile of Nonverbal Sensitivity (PONS), a
measure of individual differences in the ability to decode nonverbal
messages. In Rosenthal’s Skill volume, Friedman (1979) argued that the
skill approach to nonverbal communication represented three impor­
tant shifts in conceptualizing the study of human social interaction. The
first was a shift from an emphasis on cognitive processes (e.g., attitudes,
cognitive attributions) in interpersonal relations to a focus on emotional
processes. The second shift moved the focus from abstract traits to more

◆ 79
05-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 7:24 PM Page 80

80–––◆–––Foundations

concrete abilities. The third shifts the Inventory (EQI; Bar-On, 1997). Its measures
concept of nonverbal skill from a study of of emotional intelligence resemble self-report
inferred states to the study of process. A personality assessments typically.
clear example of this last shift is illustrated Not unlike emotional intelligence, non­
by research on expectancy effects (Harris & verbal skills are important for success in var­
Rosenthal, 2005; Rosenthal & Jacobson, ious aspects of social life. Specifically, there is
1968) that prompted scholars to try to evidence that nonverbal skills and abilities
understand the “process” of how positive are important in initiating and maintaining
(and negative) expectations are conveyed. social interaction, developing interpersonal
The work by researchers who were focus­ relationships, and managing impressions.
ing on individual differences in nonverbal Nonverbal skills and abilities are also linked
communication skill represented the ground­ to stress management and to success in
work for the highly popular concept of careers in various business settings (Riggio,
emotional intelligence (Goleman, 1995, 1992a, 2005). Importantly, nonverbal skills,
1998; Mayer & Salovey, 1997; Salovey & unlike personality traits, can be learned and
Mayer, 1990). Indeed, Friedman (1979) improved. For example, research on decep­
used the analogy of an intelligence test, with tion detection suggests that this decoding
its “right” and “wrong” answers, when dis­ ability improves by providing feedback
cussing the measurement advantages of a concerning performance accuracy and with
nonverbal skill approach to personality over practice (Zuckerman, Koestner, & Alton,
traditional traitlike measures. The same 1984; Zuckerman, Koestner, & Colella,
measurement distinction divides the emo­ 1985), as well as by providing decoders
tional intelligence research community with information concerning more accurate non­
its distinction between the “abilities model” verbal clues to deception (de Turck,
and the “mixed model” (Caruso, Mayer, & Harszlak, Bodhorn, & Texter, 1990; see also
Salovey, 2002; Mayer, Salovey, & Caruso, Patterson, Foster, & Bellmer, 2001). There is
2000) for explaining and describing emo­ also evidence that nonverbal expressiveness
tional intelligence. The abilities model of can be improved through training (Taylor,
emotional intelligence focuses on abilities to 2002; see also Vrij, this volume).
perceive, understand, use, and manage emo­ Following the belief in the importance of
tions, and it is represented by the Multifactor nonverbal skills for an array of outcomes,
Emotional Intelligence Scale (MEIS) and the this chapter reviews theory and research
Mayer, Salovey, Caruso, Emotional Intellig­ on the skill approach to nonverbal com­
ence Test (MSCEIT) (Mayer, Salovey, & munication by focusing on three general
Caruso, 1997, 2002) measures. domains of nonverbal skills and abilities:
Both these instruments are performance- (1) nonverbal decoding skill, (2) nonverbal
based assessments of emotional abilities, encoding skill, and (3) skill in regulating
many of which are central to skill in nonver­ nonverbal communication. In addition to
bal communication and, like most measures defining these skill domains, the means of
of emotional intelligence ability, have correct measuring these nonverbal skills will be
and incorrect answers. The mixed model of reviewed, as will research on the impact
emotional intelligence combines emotional of each specific nonverbal skill domain on
skills and abilities with traitlike notions outcomes in human social interaction.
of personality, which are represented by Finally, the research and application poten­
Goleman’s (1995, 1998) work and instru­ tials of the nonverbal skills and abilities
ments like the Bar-On Emotional Quotient perspective will be discussed.
05-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 7:24 PM Page 81

Nonverbal Skills and Abilities–––◆–––81

♦ Skill in Nonverbal Decoding Similarly, Buck and colleagues (Buck,


1978, 2005; Buck, Miller, & Caul, 1974;
Buck, Savin, Miller, & Caul, 1972) devel­
Skill in nonverbal decoding involves sensitiv­ oped a technique to assess individual differ­
ity to the nonverbal messages of others as well ences in the ability to read subtle, natural
as the ability to interpret those messages accu­ (unposed) emotional expressions. Using
rately. For the most part, others’ nonverbal this “slide-viewing technique” (SVT), the
messages involve the communication of emo­ faces of stimulus subjects are videotaped
tions, attitudes (e.g., liking or disliking), and while watching emotion-evoking slides and
cues of status or dominance (Mast, 2002). talking about them. Some of the slides are
Skill in nonverbal decoding is a subset of the meant to evoke positive emotions (a group
broader construct of interpersonal sensitivity, of laughing children, sleeping baby); others
which is defined as “the ability to sense, per­ evoke negative emotions (severe facial
ceive accurately, and respond appropriately injury, crying child with crutch). Others are
to one’s personal, interpersonal, and social simply unusual or scenic slides. Respondents
environment” (Bernieri, 2001, p. 3). view the silent video segments of stimulus
A number of assessment instruments subjects’ faces and try to determine which
have been designed to measure individual slide the stimulus subject was viewing.
differences in nonverbal decoding skill. An Again, a total accuracy score is the measure
examination of the properties of these vari­ of emotional or nonverbal decoding skill,
ous skill instruments can help illustrate but scores can also be obtained for accuracy
the conceptual and methodological issues at decoding specific emotions. Buck (1976)
involved in studying nonverbal skills and also developed a standardized instrument,
abilities. The earliest and simplest instru­ the Communication of Affect Receiving
ments consisted of having respondents Ability Test (CARAT), from some of the
attempt to decode photographs of basic video sequences. The slide-viewing technique
facial expressions of emotions. The Brief measures spontaneous, natural, dynamic
Affect Recognition Test (BART), developed emotional expressions and relies on video­
by Ekman and Friesen (1974), is an example tape segments rather than still photographs,
of this type of measure. Using a tachisto­ providing more of the available nonverbal
scope, test takers are presented with a brief information.
presentation (a fraction of a second) of indi­ The Diagnostic Analysis of Nonverbal
viduals posing “basic” emotional expres­ Accuracy (DANVA) assesses both visual
sions (happiness, sadness, surprise, disgust, cues of basic emotional expressions and
fear, and anger) and must choose the auditory nonverbal cues (i.e., paralanguage)
correct emotion. A total accuracy score of emotion (Nowicki & Duke, 1994). The
represents the measure of nonverbal or DANVA is actually a collection of several
emotional decoding skill. Matsumoto and instruments that includes both posed and
colleagues (2000) developed an improved spontaneous photographs of emotional
version of this instrument, the Japanese and expressions (including subtests measuring
Caucasian Brief Affect Recognition Test facial expressions of emotions, emotions
(JACBART). A limitation of these instru­ expressed via hand gestures, and emotions
ments, however, is the focus on measuring expressed via body posture). In addition,
accuracy in decoding only static, visual cues there are subtests that measure decoding
of distinct facial expressions of emotions, of vocal cues, with audio segments of emo­
devoid of context. tions being expressed while using the same
05-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 7:24 PM Page 82

82–––◆–––Foundations

content-standard sentence (“I am going out aware than those receiving low scores (e.g.,
of the room now but I’ll be back later.”). In Ambady, Hallahan, & Rosenthal, 1995;
all cases, respondents choose the correct Hall & Carter, 1999). In addition, physi­
emotional expression (“happy,” “sad,” cians receiving higher scores on the PONS
“angry,” or “fearful”). Scores are the had more satisfied patients than their col­
number of correct choices on each subtest. leagues with low-sensitivity scores (DiMatteo,
There are both adult and child versions of Friedman, & Taranta, 1979).
the DANVA (Nowicki & Duke, 2001) as The PONS assesses a wide range of sen­
well as a modified version of both measures sitivity to different channels of nonverbal
useful in the field (Duke & Nowicki, 2005). communication: audio versus visual, the
The DANVA assesses sensitivity to both relatively “rich” nonverbal cues provided
visual and auditory cues and increases the by facial expressions versus the more lim­
range of nonverbal expressions by including ited range of cues emitted by the body
the ability to decode both posed and spon­ channel. It also involves nonverbal cues dis­
taneous expressions of emotions. Research played in a number of enacted scenarios,
shows that scores on the DANVA correlate such as “helping a customer” or “talking
with greater overall social competence and about one’s divorce,” sometimes combining
better psychosocial adjustment (Nowicki & the nonverbal display of emotion with cues
Duke, 2001). of dominance-submission (e.g., “talking
Perhaps the most well-known measure of to a lost child” or “asking forgiveness”).
nonverbal decoding skill is the aforemen­ Other items involve reading cues that
tioned PONS (Rosenthal et al., 1979). The represent complex blendings of emotions
PONS consists of 220 brief, two-second and circumstances (e.g., “returning a faulty
audio and video clips of a woman enacting item to a store,” “talking about one’s wed­
multiple, emotionally laden scenes (e.g., ding”), whereas some are seemingly devoid
expressing jealous rage, asking for a favor, of emotional content (e.g., “ordering food
talking about the death of a friend). By in a restaurant”). The breadth of the
masking the spoken words via electronic PONS’s sampling of enacted scenes and the
content filtering or randomized splicing of multiple channels of nonverbal cue displays
the audio track, only nonverbal cues are pre­ seem to capture the diversity of nonverbal
sented. Eleven different channels (e.g., face behavior, but this has led to low internal
only, body only, audio only, and all possible consistency (see Hall, 2001) and can be
combinations) are used to assess specific time-consuming to implement. Other limi­
aspects of nonverbal decoding. After each tations of the PONS are its reliance on a
clip, respondents are presented with a multi- lone sender and the fact that the scenarios
ple-choice item and instructed to choose the are posed rather than genuine enactments
correct portrayal. A total accuracy score rep­ of emotions.
resents general sensitivity to nonverbal cues The Interpersonal Perception Task (IPT;
(Hall, 2001). There are also brief versions of Archer & Costanzo, 1988; Costanzo &
the PONS that use only the audio or visual Archer, 1989) is another measure of non­
cues. The full-length PONS has been used verbal decoding skill but one that focuses
widely in research and has demonstrated more on reading nonverbal, verbal, and sit­
good psychometric properties (Hall, 2001; uational cues to assess the ability to decode
Rosenthal, 1979). Not surprisingly, research the relationships among video interactants,
with the PONS suggests that persons receiv­ their status or dominance in relationship to
ing high scores are more interpersonally one another, their level of intimacy in the
05-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 7:24 PM Page 83

Nonverbal Skills and Abilities–––◆–––83

relationship, and whether they are truth communicator, the IPT has many segments
telling or deceiving. Scenes include trying to with two or more interactants, requiring a
determine a child’s parent (relationship), more sophisticated level of decoding skill,
identifying the person of higher status or the such as the ability to notice inconsistencies
winner of a sporting event (status or domi­ between verbal and nonverbal channels or to
nance), or trying to determine the length of use cues of dyadic rapport (Bernieri & Gillis,
a couple’s dating relationship (intimacy). 2001; Bernieri & Rosenthal, 1991). It is also
The IPT consists of 30 brief video (with full likely that correct decoding of IPT scenes
audio) scenes of one, two, or more stimulus requires not just recognition of specific cues,
persons, communicating with an inter­ as in decoding facial expressions of emotion,
viewer, communicating with another person but also sophisticated interpretation of
on the telephone, or in face-to-face commu­ complex verbal, nonverbal, and social cues.
nication. After watching the scene, respon­ For example, one study found that whereas
dents choose the correct interpretation via Japanese and American participants scored
a multiple-choice question. There is also a about the same in the visual-only condition
brief version of the IPT (IPT-15, Costanzo of the IPT-15, Japanese participants’ accu­
& Archer, 1993) that removed some of the racy in relation to Americans dropped off
problematic items from the longer version considerably when the vocal and verbal cues
(e.g., items that were decoded at about were added (Iizuka, Patterson, & Matchen,
chance or less than chance accuracy). 2002). Furthermore, respondents who rely
The IPT goes beyond nonverbal decod­ on “common sense” heuristics tend to make
ing and presents individuals being assessed systematic errors on the IPT, such as assum­
with both the audio and the video interac­ ing that the older interactant (or the male
tion between or among participants. Most interactant) has the higher status (Archer &
other measures of nonverbal decoding skill Costanzo, 1988). An individual who has
eliminate the verbal content to focus solely great skill in nonverbal and situational
on nonverbal (visual and paralinguistic) decoding should be more likely to avoid
cues. The authors of the IPT suggest that these errors.
it is more closely aligned with the construct A limitation of all the performance
of “social intelligence” than with emotional measures of nonverbal decoding skill is the
intelligence (Archer, Costanzo, & Akert, relatively small sampling of what is obvi­
2001). Research with the IPT has been ously a vast domain of nonverbal behavior.
primarily focused on psychometric issues, For instance, the BART and DANVA focus
although there is evidence that persons exclusively on emotional communication.
scoring high on the IPT are more socially Although the PONS assesses skill in decod­
aware and socially competent (Archer et al., ing an array of nonverbal messages involving
2001). In a study of college roommates, cues of emotion, status, and relationship, it is
high scorers on the IPT had higher-quality limited to one sender; the IPT has multiple
and more supportive relationships with one senders, but it is brief (15 or 30 items) and
another than did low IPT scorers (Hodgins suffers from low internal consistency (Hall,
& Zuckerman, 1990). 2001). The extremely low internal consis­
The IPT has the advantage of using natu­ tency coefficients (KR20s of .38 and .52 for
ralistic interactions as opposed to the posed the brief and long versions of the IPT) sug­
communications used in most other mea­ gest that the IPT may indeed be assessing a
sures of nonverbal decoding skill. More­ broad domain of interpersonal skills. Finally,
over, rather than presenting a single performance measures of nonverbal skill are
05-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 7:24 PM Page 84

84–––◆–––Foundations

time-consuming to develop and often diffi­ particularly empathy and being other-
cult to administer. oriented (Losoya & Eisenberg, 2001).
Another approach to measuring nonver­ Nonverbally sensitive individuals may also
bal skill involves the use of self-report be more prone to emotional contagion
methods. Self-report methods offer the effects, vicariously experiencing others’
advantages of sampling across a broad emotions (Hatfield, Cacioppo, & Rapson,
range of nonverbal skill-related areas, and 1994). Indeed, there is a .48 correlation
they are relatively easy to administer (Riggio between the emotional sensitivity scale
& Riggio, 2001). The earliest published of the SSI and a self-report measure of emo­
attempt to assess nonverbal decoding skill tional contagion (Riggio & Carney, 2003).
via self-reports was by Zuckerman and This suggests that although nonverbal sen­
Larrance (1979) with their Perceived sitivity is considered to be an important
Decoding Ability (PDA) and Perceived component of global nonverbal skill or
Encoding Ability (PEA) tests. As part of a competence, it is possible to be too nonver­
larger “social skill” model, Riggio’s Social bally sensitive. Davis (1983, 1994), in his
Skills Inventory (SSI; Riggio, 1986, 1989, multidimensional model of empathy, con­
2005; Riggio & Carney, 2003) uses self- ceptualizes one aspect of other-oriented
report techniques to measure two decoding sensitivity as taking another’s perspective or
abilities, emotional sensitivity (the ability to showing some empathic concern with
decode emotions and other nonverbal cues) others’ emotional states. A different form
and social sensitivity (the ability to decode of empathy, however, is labeled “personal
and understand social situations, social distress,” suggesting emotional contagion
roles, and social scripts). Unfortunately, effects. Taken to an extreme, nonverbal
measuring nonverbal decoding skill via self- sensitivity may cause people to experience
reports has demonstrated only limited suc­ some personal distress. It is important to
cess. For example, correlations between emphasize that when considering global
self-report measures of decoding skill and skill or competence in nonverbal and emo­
performance measures have been positive tional communication, an individual needs
but low (typically below .20). The emo­ to also possess skills in decoding, encoding,
tional sensitivity subscale of the SSI, how­ and regulating communication and emo­
ever, has had a slightly stronger relationship tional processes (see Riggio, 1986; Riggio
with decoding scores on the DANVA & Carney, 2003).
(decoding faces) and a similar decoding
facial expressions subtest of the Multifactor
Emotional Intelligence Test (correlations of DETECTION OF DECEPTION
.22 and .26, respectively; Riggio & Carney,
2003). In addition, self-reported emotional The ability to detect deception accu­
sensitivity is correlated substantially with rately is a particular nonverbal decoding
relevant self-reported social behaviors, skill. Because of its complexity, the ability
including the size and closeness of social to detect deception is rare, with a very small
networks (Riggio, 1986; Riggio, Watring, percentage of the general population able
& Throckmorton, 1993). to detect deception much above chance
Skill in decoding nonverbal communica­ levels (Ekman, O’Sullivan, & Frank, 1999;
tion is considered a very important compo­ Malone & DePaulo, 2001; Vrij, this vol­
nent of nonverbal ability. It is aligned closely ume). Yet some individuals seem to be par­
with important personality characteristics, ticularly successful at detecting deception
05-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 7:24 PM Page 85

Nonverbal Skills and Abilities–––◆–––85

and are labeled “wizards” (O’Sullivan, detection, are correlated, although the
2005). These rare individuals are particu­ magnitude of relationships is modest. For
larly skilled at reading nonverbal cues, example, the ability to detect one kind of
although they also possess a number of lie is related to detecting the accuracy
other skills and qualities critical for detect­ of another form of lie (Frank & Ekman,
ing deception. These include the ability to 1997). In addition, the ability to detect
notice inconsistencies in and between non­ deception is slightly positively related to
verbal and verbal cues, a strong motivation the ability to decode nonverbal cues of
to both observe and analyze human behav­ emotion (Buller & Burgoon, 1994; Malone
ior, and an ability to avoid the systematic & DePaulo, 2001). Likewise, a review of
cognitive biases that hamper the ability to intercorrelations among standardized per­
detect deception successfully in the general formance measures of decoding skill (e.g.,
population (O’Sullivan, 2005). PONS, IPT, CARAT) shows that there are
A great deal of research suggests why positive relationships among the tests, but
decoding of deception is such a difficult they are quite modest (Hall, 2001). This
skill. First, there is a trusting bias: a ten­ result suggests, perhaps, that nonverbal
dency to believe that others’ communica­ decoding skill is complex and multifaceted,
tions are more likely to be truthful than consisting of multiple abilities that are
deceptive (Ekman, 1985; Riggio, 1992b). somewhat related to, but considerably inde­
This manifests itself in proportionately more pendent from, one another.
judgments of “truth” as opposed to “lie” in
most experimental investigations of decep­
tion detection. Second, people may not be ♦ Skill in Nonverbal Encoding
able to hone their deception detection skills
if they receive inadequate feedback concern­
ing whether someone was lying or telling the Nonverbal encoding skill, also referred to as
truth (i.e., in everyday life, we may never nonverbal expressiveness, involves the abil­
find out for sure if a friend or relation was ity to send nonverbal messages to others
lying to us or not). People may also rely too accurately. Typically, performance mea­
much on stereotypical cues of deception, sures of individual differences in nonverbal
such as an inability to maintain eye contact, encoding consist of videotaping participants
nervous fidgeting, and slow, staccato speech while they are sending emotional expres­
patterns, cues that may be unrelated to sions spontaneously or while posing them
actual deception (DePaulo et al., 2003). on cue. For example, Buck’s (2005) slide-
Furthermore, because deceivers are also viewing technique was reviewed earlier as a
aware of these stereotypical deception cues, method of assessing nonverbal decoding
they may take steps to ensure that they mon­ skill. But the slide-viewing technique can
itor and control these “known” clues to also be used to measure individual differ­
deception. For example, in one study, par­ ences in the spontaneous encoding of
ticipants engaged in significantly greater emotions: A measure of spontaneous encod­
eye contact when deceiving as compared ing ability consists of the percentage of
with truth telling, presumably in an effort judges who can identify the emotion being
to look more honest (Riggio & Friedman, portrayed correctly via facial expressions
1983). while the sender is viewing or discussing the
There is some evidence that nonverbal emotion-evoking slide. More frequently,
decoding abilities, including deception however, participants are asked to pose an
05-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 7:24 PM Page 86

86–––◆–––Foundations

emotion with either a facial expression or as a measure of nonverbal encoding skill,


facially and vocally while reciting some although it is related more to spontaneous
content-standard sentence or phrase. In emotional encoding than to posed sending
other instances, encoders may be asked to of emotions (Riggio & Riggio, 2005).
pose a positive or negative affect (i.e., liking Personality scholars have shown renewed
or disliking) toward a person or object. interest in emotional expressiveness in the
Scores of nonverbal encoding ability consist past decade, and several newer self-report
of the percentage of judges who identify measures have been developed, including the
correctly the posed emotion or affect (e.g., Berkeley Expressivity Questionnaire (BEQ;
Zaidel & Mehrabian, 1969; Zuckerman, Gross & John, 1995) and the Emotional
Lipets, Koivumaki, & Rosenthal, 1975). Expressivity Scale (EES; Kring, Smith, &
Unlike measures of nonverbal decoding Neale, 1994). All these measures are rela­
ability, there are no standardized observation- tively brief, self-report instruments ranging
based tests of nonverbal encoding ability from 13 to 17 items. For a review of these
readily available. Instead, researchers have measures, see Riggio and Riggio (2005).
either used the slide-viewing technique or Whereas the PEA did not correlate sig­
created some form of posed nonverbal nificantly with either posed or spontaneous
encoding task to measure individual differ­ emotional encoding tasks (Zuckerman &
ences. For example, in a series of studies, Larrance, 1979), the other self-report
nonverbal encoding ability was measured measures (ACT, BEQ, and EES) have all
by having participants pose each of six correlated positively and significantly with
basic emotional expressions to a video cam­ emotional encoding tasks (Friedman &
era while saying content-standard sentences Riggio, 1999; Friedman et al., 1980;
(Friedman, Prince, Riggio, & DiMatteo, Gross & John, 1995; Kring et al., 1994).
1980). Persons scoring high on posed emo­ Moreover, persons scoring high on self-
tional encoding were evaluated as better report measures of nonverbal or emotional
speakers and were viewed as more likable encoding ability make more positive first
than individuals scoring low on encoding impressions (Friedman, Riggio, & Casella,
ability (Riggio & Friedman, 1986). Good 1988), appear more attractive to others
encoders of emotion were also more suc­ (DePaulo, Blank, Swaim, & Hairfield,
cessful deceivers than were poor encoders 1992), are more socially self-confident and
(Riggio & Friedman, 1983). less lonely and shy (Friedman et al., 1980;
Assessment of nonverbal or emotional Riggio, 1986), and report larger and more
encoding ability is a costly and time- supportive social networks (Friedman et al.,
consuming procedure that involves the use of 1980; Riggio, 1992a) than do individuals
multiple judges to obtain reliable scores con­ scoring low on self-report measures of
cerning the individual senders’ accuracy. As emotional or nonverbal expressiveness.
a result, researchers have turned to other There is specific evidence of the tie
methods to measure emotional encoding between encoding abilities. In one of the
skill and have had good success using earliest studies of the emotional contagion
self-report means to assess nonverbal or process, for instance, Friedman and Riggio
emotional expressiveness. Zuckerman and (1981) found that nonverbally expressive
Larrance’s (1979) PEA measure, mentioned individuals, as measured by the ACT,
earlier, was the first such published measure. were able to “infect” others with their emo­
The Affective Communication Test (ACT; tions through purely nonverbal channels.
Friedman et al., 1980) has been used widely Nonverbal expressiveness has also been
05-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 7:24 PM Page 87

Nonverbal Skills and Abilities–––◆–––87

implicated in the ability to deceive success­ affect, such as the spontaneous expression
fully, but the relationship is complex. Non- of anger or dislike, can have equally nega­
verbally expressive individuals, as measured tive outcomes (see Burgoon & Bacue,
by a posed emotional encoding task, were 2003). Therefore, it is important to con­
somewhat more successful at deceiving sider the monitoring and regulation of
others, but much of their success was expressive displays when discussing compe­
related to a “demeanor bias.” That is, they tence in nonverbal skills and abilities.
simply looked more honest and truthful
than nonexpressive persons regardless of ♦ Skill in Nonverbal
whether they were truth telling or lying.
Regulation and Control
Presumably, this was due to their tendency
to be animated and expressive and to avoid
“nervous” cues that are stereotypically asso­ Skill in nonverbal communication involves
ciated with deception (Riggio & Friedman, more than just encoding and decoding abil­
1983). ities. The ability to regulate and control
Nonverbal or emotional expressiveness one’s nonverbal communication is also
has often been either equated with the per­ a key component of what it means to
sonality trait of extraversion or considered be “nonverbally skilled” (Riggio, 1986).
a component of it. Consistent evidence sug­ Specifically, the ability to regulate both the
gests, however, that although expressive­ experience of emotions and the expression
ness and extraversion are related, they of those emotions has received a great deal
are different constructs (Friedman, 1983; of attention (e.g., Eisenberg, Champion, &
Riggio & Riggio, 2002). The fact that non­ Ma, 2004; Gross, 1998). People learn to
verbal expressive ability is most often mea­ suppress the expression of emotions, which
sured via self-report instruments (with is likely responsible for the consistent
shared method variance inflating the corre­ differences in emotional expressiveness
lations between measures of expressiveness due to culture (Ekman & Friesen, 1969;
and measures of extraversion) further Matsumoto, 2001, this volume; Matsumoto
drives the misconception that expressive­ & Yoo, 2005; but see Fridlund, 1994, this
ness is “just extraversion.” volume, for an alternative view) and gender
The ability to convey nonverbal mes­ (Hall, Carter, & Horgan, 2000; Riggio &
sages to others, particularly the sending of Carney, 2003). Regulating or managing
emotional messages, is a critical skill for emotions is also one of the core elements in
social success, and a fundamental compo­ the abilities model of emotional intelligence
nent of the larger construct of communica­ (Mayer et al., 2000). Gross (2001) suggests
tion competence (see Greene & Burleson, that regulation of emotion can involve
2003). Performance-based measurement of two processes: reappraisal and suppression.
nonverbal encoding skill is a difficult task; Reappraisal involves altering both the expe­
therefore, it has not received as much atten­ rience and the expression of an emotion,
tion as nonverbal decoding ability. Instead, whereas suppression involves the inhibition
researchers have relied on self-report mea­ of emotionally expressive behavior. In
sures of nonverbal expressiveness. Whereas either case, the nonverbal communication
evidence suggests that the ability to express of emotion can be controlled and can thus
positive emotions creates more favorable be considered an “ability.”
impressions and can positively influence Snyder’s (1974, 1987) research on self-
others’ moods, the expression of negative monitoring represents some of the earliest
05-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 7:24 PM Page 88

88–––◆–––Foundations

work attempting to measure control over Individuals scoring high on EC, for example,
nonverbal communication. By definition, are able to stifle the expression of felt emo­
self-monitoring is, in part, the ability to tions or cover the display of the felt emo­
monitor, regulate, and control one’s non­ tion with another emotional “mask.”
verbal displays. Yet self-monitoring also Tucker and Riggio (1988) found that indi­
involves being attentive to others’ nonverbal viduals scoring high on EC were more diffi­
cues and an ability to adjust one’s own cult to decode in a spontaneous emotional
expressive behavior to try to “fit in” to sending using the slide-viewing technique,
social situations. It has been argued, based suggesting that they monitor and stifle their
on factor analyses of Snyder’s (1974) self- emotional expressions. Research also sug­
monitoring scale, that the SMS also mea­ gests that persons with high levels of EC
sures elements of nonverbal encoding and have greater comfort speaking before large
decoding skill (Riggio & Friedman, 1982). audiences, performed better in a discussion-
Indeed, a key validation study conducted oriented problem-solving group, and had
by Snyder (1974) involved correlating more formal acting experience than persons
SMS scores with posed emotional encoding lacking EC (Riggio, 1986; Riggio, Riggio,
and decoding tasks. Positive relationships Salinas, & Cole, 2003).
between SMS and emotional encoding and Another measure of the ability to control
decoding success suggest that self-monitoring nonverbal and emotional communication
could represent a measure of global social is the Emotion Regulation Questionnaire
skill or competence. Factor analyses of the (ERQ), a 10-item self-report instrument with
original SMS demonstrated separate factors subscales measuring the ability to regulate
that measured other-directedness, a ten­ emotions via reappraisal or suppression
dency to focus on others’ behavior, likely (Gross & John, 2003). Research using the
a prerequisite for nonverbal decoding ERQ suggests that there are psychologically
skill, and extraversion-acting, a factor that “healthy” and “unhealthy” means to regu­
involves not only control or communication late and control the expression of emotion.
but also a form of social acting skill that For example, reappraisal-based EC or regu­
would be related to posed nonverbal encod­ lation leads to more positive outcomes (e.g.,
ing skill (Briggs & Cheek, 1988; Briggs, positive emotions, well-being), whereas con­
Cheek, & Buss, 1980). These results suggest trolling the expression of emotion via sup­
that global nonverbal and social skills or pression leads to more negative outcomes.
competence is composed of encoding, Riggio and Zimmerman (1991) found that
decoding, and regulatory skills, inspiring a persons scoring high on SSI-EC reported
more general, multidimensional model of using social support strategies less to cope
nonverbal and social skills (Riggio, 1986, with stress, whereas persons scoring high on
1989; Riggio & Carney, 2003). emotional expressiveness and sensitivity
In this multidimensional nonverbal and reported having larger supportive social net­
social skill model, emotional control (EC) is works and making greater use of social sup­
one of three core elements of nonverbal skill port strategies to cope with stress.
that involves the ability to control and regu­ The ability to control strong emotions
late emotional and nonverbal displays. The can be an asset in formal social settings,
other two elements in this model, discussed such as in public speaking, in the work­
earlier, are emotional or nonverbal encod­ place, and in positions of authority when
ing (labeled emotional expressivity) and under crisis or stress, where an individual
decoding (labeled emotional sensitivity). would not want to arouse others’ anxiety
05-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 7:24 PM Page 89

Nonverbal Skills and Abilities–––◆–––89

levels further by nonverbally conveying his control of nonverbal communication


or her own anxiety and fears. Similarly, reg­ represent a set of complex skills that are par­
ulation and suppression of anxiety should ticularly important for effective interper­
lead high-control individuals to be more sonal communication. Indeed, each of the
effective deceivers, although this was not three areas of nonverbal skill just reviewed—
the case in a study of deception that did not decoding, encoding, and control—are each
involve strong emotions or arousal (Riggio, internally complex and multifaceted. There
Tucker, & Throckmorton, 1988). Bonanno is still a great deal of research to be done to
and colleagues (2004) argue that abilities understand the domain of nonverbal com­
to both enhance and suppress emotional munication skills fully.
expression lead to greater flexibility and The study of nonverbal communication
positive psychosocial outcomes. This sug­ has been dominated by the study of func­
gests that having a broad repertoire of non­ tions and process. Early research focused
verbal skills is most important for social primarily on the meaning of various non­
effectiveness and psychosocial well-being. verbal behaviors, such as the communi­
Riggio and colleagues (Perez & Riggio, cative function of facial expressions (e.g.,
2003; Riggio, 1986; Riggio & Carney, Ekman, Friesen, & Ellsworth, 1972), ges­
2003) have argued, however, that whereas tures (Birdwhistell, 1970), posture (e.g.,
possession of high levels of nonverbal and Mehrabian, 1969), touch (e.g., Henley,
social skills is important for social success 1977), and vocalics (e.g., Argyle, 1999).
and personal adjustment, there needs to be Later research used the “lens model” per­
a “balance” among the skills. For example, spective (Brunswik, 1956) to try to under­
being overly emotionally expressive with­ stand the process by which, for example, a
out possessing the ability to regulate and sender uses nonverbal cues to affect, per­
control that expressiveness is typically mal­ suade, or influence others or to understand
adaptive. Emotionally expressive individu­ the person perception process (see DePaulo
als who lack the ability to regulate these & Friedman, 1998).
displays may appear emotionally unstable There has been comparatively little
and “out of control.” Similarly, being attention given to individual differences
overly sensitive to nonverbal cues can make in the ability to communicate nonver­
one susceptible to emotional contagion bally. Friedman (1979) saw great potential
processes as discussed earlier. Perez, Riggio, in using the concept of skill in nonverbal
and Kopelowicz (in press) found a connec­ communication to reframe thinking about
tion between lack of balance among nonver­ individual differences from the traditional
bal and social skills and poor psychological trait approach to more objectively measured
adjustment in clinical patients. skills and abilities. More than 25 years ago,
he predicted that “the next few years will
likely see a proliferation of measures of non­
♦ The Skill Approach to verbal sensitivity, nonverbal expressiveness,
Nonverbal Communication: deceptive abilities, rhythm, expressive style,
channel preference, self-monitoring, and the
Unrealized Potential
like” (Friedman, 1979, p. 23). In actuality,
there have been relatively few measures of
There is more to nonverbal skill than just nonverbal skill (most of which are reviewed
the ability to encode and decode nonver­ in this chapter), and these are not used
bal messages. Monitoring, regulation, and widely. Research on individual differences is
05-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 7:24 PM Page 90

90–––◆–––Foundations

still dominated by traditional personality participating in a group discussion, and


measures, with Big Five measures used in other exercises, many of which are designed
the majority of investigations. Indeed, a lit­ to assess, in part, communication and
erature search turned up more than 1,000 “people” skills (Howard, 1997; Thornton,
publications with either “Big Five” or 1992). Trained observers then evaluate
“NEO” (the primary Big Five scale) in the the participants’ performance by rating, for
title as opposed to 35 publications with example, their communication skills, rap­
“nonverbal skill” or “nonverbal ability.” port with others, and decision making.
Admittedly, performance measures of There is evidence that possession of nonver­
nonverbal skill are extremely costly to bal and social skills does predict perfor­
develop, are time-consuming to administer, mance on many assessment center tasks
and sample only a limited “slice” of nonver­ (Riggio et al., 1997). Yet the question of
bal behavior. The last characteristic may not whether it is worth the trouble to get these
be much of a problem, however, because expensive and time-consuming perfor­
thin slices can be quite informative (Ambady, mance-based assessments is one that paral­
LaPlante, & Johnson, 2001; Ambady & lels the work on measuring nonverbal skills
Rosenthal, 1992). It was, in fact, the time and abilities.
and costs issue that led this author to the The recent explosion of interest in emo­
development of self-report measures of non­ tional intelligence has fueled the develop­
verbal and social skill. These measures were ment of measures, particularly those of
supposed to be a “stopgap” until we could Mayer, Salovey, and associates (Mayer et al.,
develop a battery of performance-based 1997, 2002), that assess components of non­
measures assessing the broad range of non­ verbal skill. These researchers are, in many
verbal skills and abilities suggested by non­ ways, reinventing the decades-old measures
verbal communication research. Because the of nonverbal decoding skill and encounter­
self-report measures seemed to do the job ing many of the same problems that plagued
and took on a life of their own, the stan­ the nonverbal communication scholars
dardized performance measures were never (Ciarrochi, Chan, Caputi, & Roberts, 2001).
pursued fully. The technology exists, Many emotional intelligence researchers,
however, to have persons pose or sponta­ however, are simply opting for the easier to
neously express emotions while being video­ administer self-report measures. Despite
taped and then to have those videotaped their conceptual similarity, the two lines of
expressions compared with templates of research—skill in nonverbal communica­
“correct” prototype emotional expressions. tion and emotional intelligence—have rarely
Similar technology could be used to assess crossed. Perhaps, this will change with
performance-based abilities to regulate and increasing recognition that both are focusing
control emotional expressions. on the same phenomena.
A similar situation exists in person­
nel selection. Assessment centers offer
a performance-based alternative to tradi­ ♦ References
tional pencil-and-paper testing in evaluat­
ing job applicants (usually for managerial
Ambady, N., Hallahan, M., & Rosenthal, R.
positions). The assessment center requires (1995). On judging and being judged accu­
applicants to engage in a variety of work- rately in zero-acquaintance situations.
related simulations, such as making a for­ Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
mal presentation, coaching a subordinate, 69, 518–529.
05-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 7:24 PM Page 91

Nonverbal Skills and Abilities–––◆–––91

Ambady, N., LaPlante, D., & Johnson, E. Bonanno, G. A., Papa, A., Lalande, K., Wesphal,
(2001). Thin-slice judgments as a measure M., & Coifman, K. (2004). The importance
of interpersonal sensitivity. In J. A. Hall & of being flexible: The ability to both enhance
F. J. Bernieri (Eds.), Interpersonal sensitiv­ and suppress emotional expression predicts
ity: Theory and measurement (pp. 89–101). long-term adjustment. Psychological Science,
Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. 15, 482–487.
Ambady, N., & Rosenthal, R. (1992). Thin slices Briggs, S. R., & Cheek, J. M. (1988). On the
of expressive behavior as predictors of inter­ nature of self-monitoring: Problems with
personal consequences: A meta-analysis. assessment, problems with validity. Journal
Psychological Bulletin, 111, 256–274. of Personality & Social Psychology, 54,
Archer, D., & Costanzo, M. (1988). The 663–678.
Interpersonal Perception Task. Berkeley, Briggs, S. R., Cheek, J. M., & Buss, A. H.
CA: University of California Extension (1980). An analysis of the self-monitoring
Media Center. scale. Journal of Personality & Social
Archer, D., Costanzo, M., & Akert, R. (2001). Psychology, 38, 679–686.
The Interpersonal Perception Task (IPT): Brunswik, E. (1956). Perception and the repre­
Alternative approaches to problems of sentative design of psychological experi­
theory and design. In J. A. Hall & F. J. ments (2nd ed.). Berkeley, CA: University of
Bernieri (Eds.), Interpersonal sensitivity: California Press.
Theory and measurement (pp. 161–182). Buck, R. (1976). A test of nonverbal receiving
Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. ability: Preliminary studies. Human Commu­
Argyle, M. (1999). Nonverbal vocalizations. nication Research, 2, 162–171.
In L. K. Guerrero, J. A. DeVito, & M. L. Buck, R. (1978). The slide-viewing technique for
Hecht (Eds.). The nonverbal communica­ measuring nonverbal sending accuracy: A
tion reader: Classic and contemporary read­ guide for replication. Catalog of Selected
ings (pp. 135–148). Prospect Heights, IL: Documents in Psychology, 8, 62.
Waveland Press. Buck, R. (2005). Measuring emotional experi­
Bar-On, R. (1997). Bar-On Emotional Quotient ence, expression, and communication: The
Inventory: Technical manual. Toronto, slide-viewing technique. In V. Manusov
Canada: Multi-Health Systems. (Ed.), The sourcebook of nonverbal mea­
Bernieri, F. J. (2001). Toward a taxonomy of sures: Going beyond words (pp. 457–470).
interpersonal sensitivity. In J. A. Hall & Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
F. J. Bernieri (Eds.), Interpersonal sensitiv­ Buck, R., Miller, R. E., & Caul, W. F. (1974).
ity: Theory and measurement (pp. 3–20). Sex, personality and physiological variables
Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. in the communication of emotion via facial
Bernieri, F. J., & Gillis, J. S. (2001). Judging rap­ expression. Journal of Personality and
port: Employing Brunswik’s lens model to Social Psychology, 30, 587–596.
study interpersonal sensitivity. In J. A. Hall Buck, R., Savin, V. J., Miller, R. E., & Caul, W.
& F. J. Bernieri (Eds.), Interpersonal sensi­ F. (1972). Nonverbal communication of
tivity: Theory and measurement (pp. 67– affect in humans. Journal of Personality
88). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. and Social Psychology, 23, 362–371.
Bernieri, F. J, & Rosenthal, R. (1991). Coordinated Buller, D. B., & Burgoon, J. K. (1994).
movement in human interaction. In R. S. Deception: Strategic and nonstrategic com­
Feldman & B. Rime (Eds.), Fundamentals of munication. In J. A. Daly & J. M. Wiemann
nonverbal behavior (pp. 401–432). New (Eds.), Strategic interpersonal communi­
York: Cambridge University Press. cation (pp. 191–224). Hillsdale, NJ:
Birdwhistell, R. L. (1970). Kinesics and context: Erlbaum.
Essays on body motion communication. Burgoon, J. K., & Bacue, A. E. (2003). Nonverbal
Philadelphia: University of Pennsylvania communication skills. In J. O. Greene &
Press. B. R. Burleson (Eds.), Handbook of
05-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 7:24 PM Page 92

92–––◆–––Foundations

communication and social interaction skills practitioner-patient rapport. Journal of


(pp. 179–219). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. Nonverbal Behavior, 4, 18–26.
Caruso, D. R., Mayer, J. D., & Salovey, P. Duke, M., & Nowicki, S., Jr. (2005). The Emory
(2002). Emotional intelligence and Dyssemia Index. In V. Manusov (Ed.), The
emotional leadership. In R. E. Riggio, sourcebook for nonverbal measures: Going
S. E. Murphy, & F. J. Pirozzolo (Eds.), beyond words (pp. 35–46). Mahwah, NJ:
Multiple intelligences and leadership Erlbaum.
(pp. 55–74). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. Eisenberg, N., Champion, C., & Ma, Y. (2004).
Ciarrochi, J., Chan, A., Caputi, P., & Roberts, R. Emotion-related regulation: An emerging
(2001). Measuring emotional intelligence. construct. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 50,
In J. Ciarrochi, J. P. Forgas, & J. D. Mayer 236–259.
(Eds.). Emotional intelligence in everyday Ekman, P. (1985). Telling lies: Clues to deceit in
life: A scientific inquiry (pp. 25–45). the marketplace, marriage, and politics.
Philadelphia: Psychology Press. New York: Norton.
Costanzo, M. A., & Archer, D. (1989). Ekman, P., & Friesen, W. V. (1969). The reper­
Interpreting the expressive behavior of toire of nonverbal behavior: Categories, ori­
others: The Interpersonal Perception Task gins, usage, and coding. Semiotica, 1, 49–98.
(IPT). Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 13, Ekman, P., & Friesen, W. V. (1974). Nonverbal
225–245. behavior and psychopathology. In R. J.
Costanzo, M. A., & Archer, D. (1993). The Friedman & M. Katz (Eds.), The psychol­
Interpersonal Perception Task-15 (IPT-15). ogy of depression: Contemporary theory
Berkeley, CA: University of California and research (pp. 3–31). Washington, DC:
Extension Media Center. Winston & Sons.
Davis, M. H. (1983). Measuring individual differ­ Ekman, P., Friesen, W. V., & Ellsworth, P. C.
ences in empathy: Evidence for a multi­ (1972). Emotions in the human face:
dimensional approach. Journal of Personality Guidelines for research and an integration
and Social Psychology, 44, 113–126. of findings. New York: Pergamon.
Davis, M. H. (1994). Empathy: A social psycho­ Ekman, P., O’Sullivan, M., & Frank, M. G.
logical approach. Boulder, CO: Westview. (1999). A few can catch a liar. Psycho­
DePaulo, B. M., Blank, A. L., Swaim, G. W., & logical Science, 10, 263–266.
Hairfield, J. G. (1992). Expressiveness and Frank, M. G., & Ekman, P. (1997). The ability
expressive control. Personality and Social to detect deceit generalizes across differ­
Psychology Bulletin, 18, 276–285. ent types of high-stake lies. Journal of
DePaulo, B. M., & Friedman, H. S. (1998). Personality and Social Psychology, 72,
Nonverbal communication. In D. T. Gilbert, 1429–1439.
S. T. Fiske, & G. Lindzey (Eds.), The hand­ Fridlund, A. J. (1994). Human facial expres­
book of social psychology (4th ed., sion: An evolutionary view. San Diego, CA:
pp. 3–40). New York: McGraw-Hill. Academic Press.
DePaulo, B. M., Lindsay, J. J., Malone, B. E., Friedman, H. S. (1979). The concept of skill in
Muhlenbruck, L., Charlton, K., & Cooper, nonverbal communication: Implications
H. (2003). Cues to deception. Psychological for understanding social interaction. In
Bulletin, 129, 74–118. R. Rosenthal (Ed.), Skill in nonverbal com­
de Turck, M. A., Harszlak, J. J., Bodhorn, D. J., munication (pp. 2–27). Cambridge, MA:
& Texter, L. A. (1990). The effects of train­ Oelgeschlager, Gunn, & Hain.
ing social perceivers to detect deception Friedman, H. S. (1983). On shutting one’s eyes
from behavioral cues. Communication to face validity. Psychological Bulletin, 94,
Quarterly, 38, 189–199. 185–187.
DiMatteo, M. R., Friedman, H. S., & Taranta, Friedman, H. S., Prince, L. M., Riggio, R. E.,
A. (1979). Sensitivity to bodily non­ & DiMatteo, M. R. (1980). Understanding
verbal communication as a factor in and assessing nonverbal expressiveness:
05-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 7:24 PM Page 93

Nonverbal Skills and Abilities–––◆–––93

The Affective Communication Test. Journal Hall, J. A., Carter, J. D., & Horgan, T. G.
of Personality and Social Psychology, 39, (2000). Gender differences in non­
333–351. verbal communication of emotion. In
Friedman, H. S., & Riggio, R. E. (1981). Effect A. H. Fischer (Ed.), Gender and emotion:
of individual differences in nonverbal Social psychological perspectives (pp. 97–
expressiveness on transmission of emotion. 117). New York: Cambridge University
Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 6, 96–104. Press.
Friedman, H. S., & Riggio, R. E. (1999). Harris, M. J., & Rosenthal, R. (2005). No more
Individual differences in ability to encode teachers’ dirty looks: Effects of teacher
complex affects. Personality and Individual nonverbal behavior on student outcomes.
Differences, 27, 181–194. In R. E. Riggio & R. S. Feldman (Eds.),
Friedman, H. S., Riggio, R. E., & Casella, D. F. Applications of nonverbal communication
(1988). Nonverbal skill, personal charisma, (pp. 157–192). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
and initial attraction. Personality and Social Hatfield, E., Cacioppo, J. T., & Rapson, R.
Psychology Bulletin, 14, 203–211. (1994). Emotional contagion. Cambridge,
Goleman, D. (1995). Emotional intelligence: England: Cambridge University Press.
Why it can matter more than IQ. New Henley, N. (1977). Body politics: Power, sex,
York: Bantam. and nonverbal communication. Englewood
Goleman, D. (1998). Working with emotional Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.
intelligence. New York: Bantam. Hodgins, H., & Zuckerman, M. (1990). The
Greene, J. O., & Burleson, B. R. (Eds.) (2003). effect of nonverbal sensitivity on social
Handbook of communication and interaction. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior,
social interaction skills. Mahwah, NJ: 24, 155–170.
Erlbaum. Howard, A. (1997). A reassessment of assess­
Gross, J. J. (1998). The emerging field of emotion ment centers: Challenges for the 21st
regulation: An integrative review. Review of century. Journal of Social Behavior and
General Psychology, 2, 271–299. Personality, 12(5), 13–52.
Gross, J. J. (2001). Emotion regulation in Iizuka, Y., Patterson, M. L., & Matchen, J. C.
adulthood: Timing is everything. Current (2002). Accuracy and confidence on the
Directions in Psychological Science, 10, Interpersonal Perception Task: A Japanese-
214–219. American comparison. Journal of Non­
Gross, J. J., & John, O. P. (1995). Facets of verbal Behavior, 26, 159–174.
emotional expressivity: Three self-report Kring, A. M., Smith, D. A., & Neale, J. M.
factors and their correlates. Personality and (1994). Individual differences in disposi­
Individual Differences, 19, 555–568. tional expressiveness: Development and vali­
Gross, J. J., & John, O. P. (2003). Individual dation of the emotional expressivity scale.
differences in two emotion regulation Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
processes: Implications for affect, relation­ 66, 934–949.
ships, and well-being. Journal of Personality Losoya, S. H., & Eisenberg, N. (2001). Affective
and Social Psychology, 85, 348–362. empathy. In J. A. Hall & F. J. Bernieri
Hall, J. A. (2001). The PONS test and the (Eds.), Interpersonal sensitivity: Theory and
psychometric approach to measuring inter­ measurement (pp. 21–43). Mahwah, NJ:
personal sensitivity. In J. A. Hall & Erlbaum.
F. J. Bernieri (Eds.), Interpersonal sensitiv­ Malone, B. E., & DePaulo, B. M. (2001).
ity: Theory and measurement (pp. 143– Measuring sensitivity to deception. In J. A.
160). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. Hall & F. J. Bernieri (Eds.), Interpersonal
Hall, J. A., & Carter, J. D. (1999). Gender- sensitivity: Theory and measurement
stereotype accuracy as an individual differ­ (pp. 103–124). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
ence. Journal of Personality and Social Mast, M. (2002). Dominance as expressed
Psychology, 77, 350–359. and inferred through speaking time: A
05-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 7:24 PM Page 94

94–––◆–––Foundations

meta-analysis. Human Communication O’Sullivan, M. (2005). Emotional intelligence


Research, 28, 420–450. and deception detection: Why most people
Matsumoto, D. (2001). Culture and emotion. In can’t “read” others, but a few can. In R. E.
D. Matsumoto (Ed.), The handbook of Riggio & R. S. Feldman (Eds.), Applications
culture and psychology (pp. 171–194). of nonverbal communication (pp. 215–253).
New York: Oxford University Press. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Matsumoto, D., LeRoux, J., Wilson-Cohn, C., Patterson, M. L., Foster, J. L., & Bellmer, C. G.
Raroque, J., Kooken, K., Ekman, P., et al. (2001). Another look at accuracy in social
(2000). A new test to measure emotion judgments. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior,
recognition ability: Matsumoto and Ekman’s 25, 207–219.
Japanese and Caucasian Brief Affect Perez, J. E., & Riggio, R. E. (2003). Nonverbal
Recognition Test (JACBART). Journal of social skills and psychopathology. In
Nonverbal Behavior, 24, 179–209. P. Philippot, E. J. Coats, & R. S. Feldman
Matsumoto, D., & Yoo, S. E. (2005). Culture (Eds.), Nonverbal behavior in clinical set­
and applied nonverbal communication. tings (pp. 17–44). New York: Oxford
In R. E. Riggio & R. S. Feldman (Eds.), University Press.
Applications of nonverbal communication Perez, J. E., Riggio, R. E., & Kopelowicz, A.
(pp. 255–277). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. (in press). Social skill imbalances as indica­
Mayer, J. D., & Salovey, P. (1997). What is tors of psychopathology: An exploratory
emotional intelligence? In P. Salovey & investigation. Personality and Individual
J. D. Sluyter (Eds.). Emotional development Differences.
and emotional intelligence. New York: Riggio, H. R., & Riggio, R. E. (2002).
Basic Books. Emotional expressiveness, extraversion,
Mayer, J. D., Salovey, P., & Caruso, D. R. and neuroticism: A meta-analysis. Journal
(1997). Multifactor Emotional Intelligence of Nonverbal Behavior, 26, 195–218.
Test (MEIS). Needham, MA: Allyn & Riggio, R. E. (1986). Assessment of basic social
Bacon. skills. Journal of Personality and Social
Mayer, J. D., Salovey, P., & Caruso, D. R. Psychology, 51, 649–660.
(2000). Models of emotional intelligence. Riggio, R. E. (1989). Manual for the Social Skills
In R. J. Sternberg (Ed.), The handbook of Inventory: Research edition. Palo Alto, CA:
intelligence (pp. 396–420). New York: Consulting Psychologists Press.
Cambridge University Press. Riggio, R. E. (1992a). Social interaction skills
Mayer, J. D., Salovey, P., & Caruso, D. R. and nonverbal behavior. In R. S. Feldman
(2002). Mayer-Salovey-Caruso Emotional (Ed.), Applications of nonverbal behavioral
Intelligence Test (MSCEIT) user’s manual. theories and research (pp. 3–30). Hillsdale,
Toronto, Canada: MHS Publishers. NJ: Erlbaum.
Mehrabian, A. (1969). Significance of posture Riggio, R. E. (1992b). Detecting lies and decep­
and position in the communication of atti­ tion. The Long Term View, 1, 9–14.
tude and status relationships. Psychological Riggio, R. E. (2005). The Social Skills Inventory
Bulletin, 71, 359–372. (SSI): Measuring nonverbal and social
Nowicki, S., & Duke, M. P. (1994). Individual skills. In V. Manusov (Ed.), The sourcebook
differences in the nonverbal communica­ of nonverbal measures: Going beyond
tion of affect: The Diagnostic Analysis of words (pp. 25–33). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Nonverbal Accuracy scale. Journal of Riggio, R. E., Aguirre, M., Mayes, B. T., Belloli,
Nonverbal Behavior, 18, 9–35. C., & Kubiak, C. (1997). The use of assess­
Nowicki, S., & Duke, M. P. (2001). Nonverbal ment center methods for student outcome
receptivity: The Diagnostic Analysis of assessment. Journal of Social Behavior and
Nonverbal Accuracy (DANVA). In J. A. Personality, 12, 273–288.
Hall & F. J. Bernieri (Eds.), Interpersonal Riggio, R. E., & Carney, D. R. (2003). Social
sensitivity: Theory and measurement Skills Inventory manual (2nd ed.).
(pp. 183–198). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. Redwood City, CA: MindGarden.
05-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 7:24 PM Page 95

Nonverbal Skills and Abilities–––◆–––95

Riggio, R. E., & Friedman, H. S. (1982). The Rosenthal, R., & Jacobson, L. (1968). Pygmalion
interrelationships of self-monitoring factors, in the classroom. New York: Holt, Rinehart,
personality traits, and nonverbal social and Winston.
skills. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 7, Salovey, P., & Mayer, J. D. (1990). Emotional
33–45. intelligence. Imagination, Cognition, and
Riggio, R. E., & Friedman, H. S. (1983). Personality, 9, 185–211.
Individual differences and cues to deception. Snyder, M. (1974). The self-monitoring of
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, expressive behavior. Journal of Personality
45, 899–915. and Social Psychology, 30, 526–537.
Riggio, R. E., & Friedman, H. S. (1986). Snyder, M. (1987). Public appearances/private
Impression formation: The role of expres­ realities: The psychology of self-monitoring.
sive behavior. Journal of Personality and New York: W.H. Freeman.
Social Psychology, 50, 421–427. Taylor, S. J. (2002). Effects of a nonverbal skill
Riggio, R. E., & Riggio, H. R. (2001). Self- training program on perceptions of
report measurement of interpersonal sensi­ personal charisma. Unpublished doctoral
tivity. In J. A. Hall & F. J. Bernieri (Eds.), dissertation, University of California at
Interpersonal sensitivity: Theory and mea­ Riverside.
surement (pp. 127–142). Mahwah, NJ: Thornton, G. C. (1992). Assessment centers in
Erlbaum. human resource management. Reading,
Riggio, R. E., & Riggio, H. R. (2005). Self-report MA: Addison-Wesley.
measures of emotional and nonverbal Tucker, J. S., & Riggio, R. E. (1988). The role
expressiveness. In V. Manusov (Ed.), The of social skills in encoding posed and spon­
sourcebook of nonverbal measures: Going taneous facial expressions. Journal of
beyond words (pp. 105–111). Mahwah, NJ: Nonverbal Behavior, 12, 87–97.
Erlbaum. Zaidel, S. F., & Mehrabian, A. (1969). The abil­
Riggio, R. E., Riggio, H. R., Salinas, C., & Cole, ity to communicate and infer positive and
E. J. (2003). The role of social and emo­ negative attitudes facially and vocally.
tional communication skills in leader emer­ Journal of Experimental Research in
gence and effectiveness. Group Dynamics: Personality, 3, 233–241.
Theory, Research, and Practice, 7, 83–103. Zuckerman, M., Koestner, R., & Alton, A. O.
Riggio, R. E., Watring, K., & Throckmorton, B. (1984). Learning to detect deception. Journal
(1993). Social skills, social support, and of Personality and Social Psychology, 46,
psychosocial adjustment. Personality and 519–528.
Individual Differences, 15, 275–280. Zuckerman, M., Koestner, R., & Colella, M. J.
Riggio, R. E., & Zimmerman, J. A. (1991). Social (1985). Learning to detect deception from
skills and interpersonal relationships: Influ­ three communication channels. Journal of
ences on social support and support seeking. Nonverbal Behavior, 9, 188–194.
In W. H. Jones & D. Perlman (Eds.), Zuckerman, M., & Larrance, D. (1979). Individ­
Advances in personal relationships (Vol. 2, ual differences in perceived encoding and
pp. 133–155). London: Jessica Kingsley decoding abilities. In R. Rosenthal (Ed.), Skill
Press. in nonverbal communication (pp. 171–203).
Rosenthal, R. (Ed.). (1979). Skill in nonverbal Cambridge, MA: Oelgeschlager, Gunn, &
communication. Cambridge, MA: Oelge­ Hain.
schlager, Gunn, & Hain. Zuckerman, M., Lipets, M. S., Koivumaki, J. A.,
Rosenthal, R., Hall, J. A., DiMatteo, M. R., & Rosenthal, R. (1975). Encoding and
Rogers, P. L., & Archer, D. (1979). decoding nonverbal cues of emotion. Journal
Sensitivity to nonverbal communications: of Personality and Social Psychology, 32,
The PONS test. Baltimore, MD: Johns 1068–1076.
Hopkins University Press.
05-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 7:24 PM Page 96
06-Manusov.qxd 7/6/2006 4:53 PM Page 97

6
NONVERBAL AND VERBAL
COMMUNICATION
Hand Gestures and Facial Displays as Part
of Language Use in Face-to-Face Dialogue1

� Janet Beavin Bavelas


University of Victoria

� Nicole Chovil
Independent Researcher and Education Consultant

O f the many different research perspectives on the fundamentals of


nonverbal communication, one of the most taken for granted is
the relationship between verbal communication and co-occurring non­
verbal acts. Most researchers assume that conversational gestures (e.g.,
illustrators) and some facial actions (e.g., eyebrow emphasizers or a
quizzical expression) contribute to the talk-in-progress. This chapter is the

Author’s Note: The Social Sciences and Humanities Research Council of


Canada has generously provided long-term support for the program of
research from which this model developed. We owe a continuing debt to dis­
cussions with members of the research team over the years and a particular
debt to Jennifer Gerwing’s survey of recent gesture research.

◆ 97
06-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 7:26 PM Page 98

98–––◆–––Foundations

next step in a developing model on the combinations of words, prosody, hand


contribution of nonverbal communication gestures, facial displays, or gaze, including
to face-to-face dialogue (proposed originally mixed syntax (Slama-Cazacu, 1976), com­
in Bavelas & Chovil, 2000). In addition prehensive communicative act (Linell, 1982),
to drawing on the evidence so far, we will multichannel process (Leeds-Hurwitz, 1989;
suggest new directions for research in this Sanders, 1987; Scherer, 1980), composite sig­
relatively neglected area. Specifically, we will nal (Clark, 1996; Engle & Clark, 1995), inte­
propose four theoretical propositions regard­ grated message (Bavelas & Chovil, 2000),
ing the subset of nonverbal acts that function and, most recently, multimodal communica­
as part of language use in face-to-face dia­ tion (e.g., Engle, 2000), although the last
logue and will discuss the logical and empir­ term often includes computers or objects as
ical evidence for each. We hope that the well as human actions.
reader will agree that this area of research The broader context of the approach dis­
and theory is at an exciting point, with cussed in this chapter is our interest in the
enough evidence to be promising but with unique features of face-to-face dialogue as a
many more questions and possibilities still primary mode of language use. Changes in
open for investigation. conceptions of language itself have been a
boon for conversational hand gestures and
facial displays. Historically, when linguists
♦ Historical and
and psycholinguists conceptualized language
Theoretical Context as an abstract entity or idealized it as written
text or formal monologues, all nonverbal
Researchers who focus on face-to-face dia­ acts were either irrelevant or a completely
logue have long noted that some nonverbal separate communication channel. Recent
behaviors can work closely with words, interest in how interlocutors ordinarily use
prosody, and each other in ordinary conver­ language has led to an emphasis on conver­
sation. In our view, the beginning of a sys­ sation and, eventually, to face-to-face dia­
tematic theory was in 1955, with the highly logue. Indeed, many authors (e.g., Bavelas,
influential, although mostly unpublished, 1990; Bavelas, Hutchinson, Kenwood, &
Natural History of an Interview project (cf. Matheson, 1997; Clark, 1996, pp. 8–10;
Leeds-Hurwitz, 1987; McQuowan, 1971). Fillmore, 1981; Goodwin, 1981; Levinson,
We can then trace a line of proponents of an 1983; Linell, 1982) have proposed that face­
integrated approach in many disciplines, to-face dialogue, rather than written text or
including Birdwhistell (1966), Scheflen formal monologue, is the fundamental or
(1968), Ekman and Friesen (1969), Kendon basic site of language use, for at least three
(1972, 1980), Blurton-Jones (1972), Pike reasons: (1) face-to-face dialogue is arguably
(1972), Weiner, Devoe, Rubinow, and the first format for human language in evo­
Geller (1972), Slama-Cazacu (1976), Duncan lutionary terms; (2) in typical development,
and Fiske (1977), Poyatos (1980), Scherer it is the individual’s first language; and (3) it
(1980), Linell (1982), McNeill (1985), is the most common format for language use
Goodwin and Goodwin (1986), Sanders in everyday life.
(1987), Leeds-Hurwitz (1989), Chovil (1989), More specifically, we have proposed (e.g.,
Bavelas, Black, Chovil, and Mullett (1990, Bavelas, 1990; Bavelas & Chovil, 2000;
chap. 6), Fridlund (1991a), Streeck and Knapp Bavelas, Coates, & Johnson, 2002; Bavelas,
(1992), Clark (1996, chap. 6), and Jones and Hutchinson, Kenwood, & Matheson, 1997)
LeBaron (2002). These authors have used that there are two features of dialogue that,
a variety of terms for verbal-nonverbal in combination, do not occur in other forms
06-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 7:26 PM Page 99

Nonverbal and Verbal Communication–––◆–––99

of language use such as written text, public Theoretical Propositions


speaking, or e-mail. Face-to-face dialogue is,
first of all, dialogue rather than monologue.
♦ I. Synchrony With
It is a collaborative activity (Clark, 1996)
Spontaneous Speech in
with a high degree of reciprocity and mutual
influence at a micro-social level (Bavelas,
Face-to-Face Dialogue
in press); that is, dialogue involves moment­
by-moment or even simultaneous responses As noted, our focus in this chapter is on
between the interlocutors. Second, once a specific subset within the vast domain of
observed closely, face-to-face dialogue nonverbal behaviors. We do not propose
reveals the ubiquity and integral importance that all nonverbal behaviors function as
of specific nonverbal acts in the moment-by­ part of language use. Instead, we propose
moment interaction. Thus, in addition to sharp limits on the behaviors that might be
rapid social reciprocity, the second key part of integrated verbal and nonverbal
characteristic of face-to-face dialogue is the messages; therefore, our model includes
availability of elements other than words, only certain behaviors when they occur in
such as hand gestures, facial displays, and certain ways in certain settings. Somewhat
some other nonverbal acts. We propose that similar physical behaviors might occur in
these elements serve unique and essential other ways or in other settings, with no
roles in the dialogue. relationship to language use. And there
In the rest of this chapter, we outline are many, perhaps most, nonverbal behav­
four theoretical propositions about the iors that are unlikely to be related directly
subset of nonverbal acts that are part of to language use. The nested criteria of set­
language use in face-to-face dialogue, with ting, timing, and meaning are useful for
an emphasis on features that researchers making these distinctions, as elaborated in
can test both logically and empirically. the following sections.
The first, primary distinguishing character­
istic is their synchrony with spontaneous FACE-TO-FACE DIALOGUE
speech. Second, these are symbolic acts
with referents. Third, they are tightly inte­ To be included in our model of language
grated with words, although not necessar­ use, the setting in which the nonverbal acts
ily redundant with them. Fourth, the occur must be spontaneous face-to-face
participants in dialogue use them to create dialogue. That is, both (or all) participants
and convey shared meanings. We will can see and hear each other and can interact
review data for hand gestures and facial freely as themselves.2 This criterion puts
displays, data that imply possible pro­ many familiar research settings outside our
grams of research for other acts, such as focus of interest. For example, studies of
gaze (Bavelas, Coates, & Johnson, 2002). individuals who are alone or of individuals
Because of space and the intended reader­ who are looking at videos or photographs of
ship of this chapter, we will focus primar­ other individuals do not yield data on face­
ily on experimental or quantitative data to-face dialogue. When the speaker or the
and will not review the rich qualitative addressee is an experimenter or confederate,
work that still leads the way. In our expe­ the dialogue is not reciprocally spontaneous;
rience, both traditions combine to produce that is, one participant is following scripted
a more refined appreciation of how guidelines rather than interacting freely.
skillfully and precisely participants com­ Even within a dialogue, the nonverbal
municate in face-to-face dialogue. actions must be visible to the partner, which
06-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 7:26 PM Page 100

100–––◆–––Foundations

precludes dialogues through a visual barrier. obvious noncommunicative function (e.g.,


Similarly, participants may produce muscle hand movements to reach for or manipu­
movements or physiological patterns that late objects or facial adaptors such as lick­
instruments can measure, but if these are ing dry lips or squinting in bright light).
not visible to the other participant, then None of the above acts is likely to be syn­
they are not part of the overt face-to-face chronous with precise words or phrases. It
dialogue as we conceptualize it. All of the is readily observable, however, that speak­
above settings and conditions can provide ers can co-opt almost any physical action
important background information, espe­ conversationally (e.g., when they demon­
cially in experimental designs that contrast strate blinking, reaching, or gazing; Clark
them to face-to-face dialogue (e.g., manipu­ & Gerrig, 1990). Timing as well as form
lating the visual availability of the receiver; and context make it clear when ordinary
cf. Chovil, 1997; Cohen & Harrison, actions are being used conversationally,
1973). Indeed, such experiments demon­ because only the stylized, communicative
strate that other settings are not the same as form would be synchronous with and sup­
face-to-face dialogue and that we cannot plement speech. In short, we are neither
assume generalizability. claiming nor excluding broad physical
categories of behaviors but rather making
functional distinctions based on identifi­
SYNCHRONY WITH WORDS
able parameters. Our focus is on what the
behavior is doing, not on what kind of
Within the setting of face-to-face dia­
behavior it is.
logue, a further essential criterion for our
model is timing. The nonverbal acts we are
focusing on are synchronized with the CONVERSATIONAL MEANING
words that they accompany, which means
that their typical duration will be seconds So far, we have described two aspects of
or even less. Conversational hand gestures, synchrony between the verbal and nonverbal
unlike emblems or hand signals in non­ acts that are included in our model; the set­
speaking contexts, have a split-second rela­ ting must be face-to-face dialogue, and the
tionship to words (e.g., McNeill, 1992, nonverbal acts must be tightly timed with
pp. 25–29). Similarly, although the face speech. The third criterion is even more spe­
is capable of assuming precise stereotypic cific: The act must have meaning in its partic­
emotional configurations (e.g., Ekman, ular and immediate conversational context.
1993), the face in dialogue can be highly Words and prosody are audible ways of cre­
mobile (Bavelas & Chovil, 1997), and ating meaning in conversation; we propose
many of its actions are synchronous with that certain nonverbal acts are visible acts of
the words of the dialogue rather than the meaning (Bavelas & Chovil, 2000). Just like
emotional state of a participant (Chovil, words, however, their meaning is not intrin­
1989, 1991/1992; Ekman, 1997). sic to the isolated act but depends on the
The timing requirement also excludes linguistic context (see Robinson, this volume).
many other nonverbal acts from our subset, In contrast, emblematic hand gestures and
including the following: involuntary or facial expressions of emotion usually have
reflexive actions (e.g., blinking or breath­ stereotypic forms that are virtually indepen­
ing), static posture or appearance (e.g., arm dent of linguistic context. Similarly, the inter­
or leg positions or cosmetic choices such as pretation of many other nonverbal acts (such
clothing or tattoos), and acts that have an as those indicating intimacy or deception)
06-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 7:26 PM Page 101

Nonverbal and Verbal Communication–––◆–––101

does not necessarily depend on the micro- polysemous, yet each meaning is ordinarily
context in which they occur. unambiguous when it occurs in a particular
The context that determines the mean­ conversational context (i.e., from a wide
ing of both audible and visible acts of turn to wide awake to nationwide).
meaning is multilayered and includes who Similarly, in the following gestural
the participants are, why they are talking, example, the speaker is describing a picture
how they have been using the word or act of a dress with an unusually wide hipline,
so far, the particular topic in that phase of which extends about a meter on either side of
the conversation, the precise point in the the waist (cf. Bavelas, Kenwood, Johnson, &
utterance, and the simultaneous other ele­ Phillips, 2002, Figure 1). The underlining
ments of the integrated message of which indicates where gestures occurred in relation
it is a part at that moment. As an example to the words; the brackets contain an itali­
of the importance of all of these layers cized description of each gesture; and S =
of context for the meaning of words, speaker, A = addressee. (For readers who
the adjective wide has a couple dozen do not usually watch conversational actions
meanings in the Random House Unabridged frame by frame, the best way to understand a
Dictionary (1993). Like most words, it is written example is to act it out oneself.)

Example 1.
S: “OK. Ah, like a huge skirt that goes out like this?” ____________
[both hands move from waist to full out] [holds width]
A: “Like one of the round ones?” ____________
[hands curve out from waist] [holds width]

By moving her hands out and especially is usually inseparable from its linguistic and
by holding them in place at the farthest micro-social contexts.
extent, the speaker indicated, among other Contextual specificity does not apply
things, that the skirt was “wide.” The only to hand gestures. In the first systematic
addressee confirmed his understanding by description of a conversational facial dis­
replicating both movements. play, Ekman (1979) showed that the same
In different conversational moments, physical eyebrow actions can have several
her gesture for a wide skirt could have indi­ different meanings (e.g., as a baton or a
cated the length of a fish that the speaker question mark), depending on conversa­
caught, the metaphorical amount of work tional context. Chovil (1989) found that
the speaker has left to do, or the begin­ even stereotypic expressions can have varied
ing of a tree-hugging gesture. Yet at the meanings. The classic nose-wrinkle of dis­
moment it occurred in its particular conver­ gust can also convey rejection of other kinds,
sational context—even though there was no which have nothing to do with smell (e.g.,
reference to “wide” in the words of either a disliked movie, an unpleasant chore), and
person—it was unambiguous. As Goodwin an angry expression may not indicate con­
(2000) illustrated through a detailed micro­ comitant anger. In the following example
analysis of a hand gesture, simply “locating from Chovil’s data, the speaker was humor­
the lexical affiliate of a gesture does not ously describing a past argument with her
constitute establishing its meaning” (p. 92) sister about whether she should cut her
because the meaning of any word or gesture hair.
06-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 7:26 PM Page 102

102–––◆–––Foundations

Example 2.

S: “I’m goin’ like, ‘I wanna cut my hair!’”_______


[exaggerated, stylized anger display] [smiles]

Exactly as she was describing her own and Gerrig (1990), and Streeck and Knapp
part in the argument, she configured her eyes, (1992) have also noted that hand gestures
brows, and mouth in a classic anger display. can depict, demonstrate, or reenact. There is
As soon as the relevant phrase was over, she a difference between a hand action, which
smiled along with the addressee at her own has a practical function in the material world
humor. She was not angry when she made (e.g., turning on a light switch or holding a
the display; indeed, she may not have been telephone) and a hand gesture, which has a
nearly as angry at the time of the argument communicative function in the social world
as the current display indicated. She was (e.g., as part of telling someone to switch on
exaggerating for effect, as confirmed by her the light or that you will call them later).
immediate smile, which the addressee shared. Practical and material considerations shape
We propose that, just as with words, the the hand action, but social and communica­
addressees seldom have difficulty selecting tive considerations shape the hand gesture.
the correct meaning, largely because of the Because of these considerations, the hand
contextual redundancy that supports it. action and gesture should look different
in predictable ways (Gerwing & Bavelas,
2004). Very few studies have even recorded
♦ II. Symbolic Acts the difference between hand actions and
hand gestures; an exception is LeBaron and
In proposing that a hand gesture or facial Streeck’s (2000) comparison of instrumental
display in dialogue is a symbol, we intend actions to later gestures for the same actions.
the simplest sense of the term: Symbols have Several recent experiments have demon­
referents; they are something that stands for strated a key part of our proposal, namely,
something else (Quine, 1987, p. 763). Put if social and communicative factors shape
in other terms, symbols are encoded acts, hand gestures, then these factors should
although the encoding is ordinarily ana­ cause variation in gestures for the same ref­
logic or iconic (see Bavelas & Chovil, erent. Ozyurek (2000, 2002) showed that
2000). The case for the proposition that speakers made a gesture depicting the same
some nonverbal acts are symbolic involves motion differently depending on their spatial
somewhat different issues for hand gestures relationship to their addressees. Other exper­
and for facial displays, which will be iments have also demonstrated that the ref­
treated separately in the following. erent is not the sole determinant of the form
of a gesture. Rather, linguistic principles
HAND GESTURES AS SYMBOLS unique to dialogue can influence the shape
of gestures: When the participants shared
McNeill (1992) pointed out that “ges­ common ground about an object, they made
tures are not just movements and can never sketchier gestures to depict it than when
be fully explained in purely kinesic terms. the information was new to one of them
They are not just the arms waving in the air (Gerwing & Bavelas, 2004). Similarly,
but symbols that exhibit meaning in their within a dialogue, later gestures for familiar
own right” (p. 105). Kendon (1985), Clark (“given”) information were shorter than
06-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 7:26 PM Page 103

Nonverbal and Verbal Communication–––◆–––103

those for new information (Woods, 2005). Because of the vastly greater research
These effects are identical to the effects of interest in emotional expression, there is
given-versus-new information on verbal remarkably little scholarship on the use of
communication, such as the length of verbal the face for communication (for a more
reference (e.g., Fowler, 1988). The results general discussion of emotional communi­
also accord with Grice’s (1967/1989) max­ cation, however, see Fridlund & Russell,
ims of manner and quantity: The sketchier this volume). We know of three systematic
or shorter gestures were sufficient for their descriptions: Ekman’s (1979) above-
purpose but no more than that. mentioned description of eyebrow move­
ments, Brunner’s (1979) analysis of smiles
FACIAL DISPLAYS AS SYMBOLIC as back-channels, and Chovil’s systematic
identification of conversational facial dis­
The distinction between symbol and plays other than smiles (1989, 1991/1992).
referent is even more subtle and important The latter study documented the wide
for faces, because there is a strong tendency variety of syntactic and semantic functions
to equate a facial expression with an emo­ of participants’ facial displays in sponta­
tional expression. In this view, facial neous face-to-face dialogue. For example,
expressions of emotion are nonsymbolic, speakers facially portrayed themselves as
involuntary acts that reveal information they might have appeared at another time,
about the individual’s intrapsychic state. in another situation (see Example 2, earlier
Ekman, the pioneer in the study of face and in this chapter); they also portrayed others’
emotion, anticipated other functions of the reactions (e.g., a disapproving relative), and
face in his early work, however (e.g., they marked syntactic emphasis, questions,
Ekman, 1979). More recently, he also made and other narrative features, usually with
several distinctions between facial expres­ eyebrow movements.
sions of emotion and facial actions that are One limitation of the research just dis­
conversational signals: cussed is that it has been almost entirely
descriptive, documenting the nonemotional
Most importantly, the conversational sig­ role of facial displays in face-to-face dia­
nals [italics added] are part of the struc­ logue but not offering an alternative theo­
ture of the conversation, part of the flow retical conception of them. We (Bavelas &
of talk, and governed by the rules which Chovil, 1997) found a promising theory in
govern the production of speech. While Clark’s (1996; Clark & Gerrig, 1990) con­
facial expressions of emotion [italics cept of demonstration as a distinct method
added] often occur during conversation, of signaling (adapted from Peirce, cited in
their location in the speech flow is related Buchler, 1940). Clark and Gerrig (1990)
not to the structure of talk but to the proposed that many conversational actions,
semantics, revealing an emotional reac­ such as quoting what someone else said,
tion to what is being said or not being are demonstrations rather than descriptions
said. (Ekman, 1997, p. 340) or indications (the other two methods of
signaling). In addition, “people can demon­
Thus, Ekman’s first criterion, synchrony strate a cough, the rhythm of a part of a
with speech, is the same as ours. Kraut and Chopin prelude, the sound of a car engine,
Johnston (1979) also proposed a distinction . . . or the appearance of a chimpanzee”
between the facial expression of emotion (Clark & Gerrig, 1990, pp. 766–767).
and a socially oriented facial display (which The speaker need not actually have a cough
is the term we use in this chapter). or be playing a Chopin prelude (and is
06-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 7:26 PM Page 104

104–––◆–––Foundations

certainly not a car engine or a chimpanzee), synchronized with words in both timing and
nor is he or she making such a claim, meaning. In this section, we propose that
because a demonstration is not literal (it is these two synchronies of timing and meaning
“non-serious”; Goffman, 1974). The prin­ work together to produce an integrated but
ciple of demonstration means that speakers often complex whole. Most of the available
do not necessarily or even usually use their research relevant to this proposal has focused
facial displays to portray the way they feel on gestures; at present, we have to rely on
at that moment; rather, the speaker is illus­ anecdotal observation for facial displays.
trating some aspect of the conversational What may be the best evidence of the pre­
topic of the moment. cise integration of audible and visible acts is
When demonstrating, the speaker does easy to demonstrate: Speakers usually coor­
not simply reproduce the literal expression; dinate their hand gestures and facial displays
a demonstration is selective, deleting irrele­ to verbal syntax. McNeill (1985) found that
vant features and retaining or even exagger­ “gestures synchronize with parallel linguistic
ating the relevant ones. For example, units [and] almost never cross clause bound­
squinting one’s eyes to indicate skepticism aries” (pp. 160–161). Ekman (1997) made
or disbelief may demonstrate looking more the same general point about facial displays:
closely at something, but it is likely to be a
highly stylized and different in form (e.g., Take for example, a person who says he
quicker) than literally squinting to read fine had been afraid of what he would learn
print. In support of this, Gilbert, Fridlund, from a biopsy report, and was so relieved
and Sabini (1987) showed that individuals when it turned out to be negative. When
who were demonstrating facial displays to the word “afraid” is said, the person
various odors produced facial configura­ stretches back his lips horizontally, refer­
tions that were clearer to observers than ring facially to fear. (p. 340)
when they were actually smelling the odor Ekman went on to point out that the
and having the same reaction sponta­ above facial action, used to “refer to a fear
neously. Arguably, these results illustrate not felt now,” would not only be a trans­
the selective nature of demonstration. We formed version of the emotional expression
proposed at the beginning of this section of fear but “would be likely to be made very
that if conversational hand gestures and quickly, much more quickly than the actual
facial displays are symbols, then social expression of emotion would be” (p. 340).
and communicative considerations would Presumably, the display would be quicker
shape them. What we know about the prin­ in order to synchronize with the word
ciples of this selective process is encourag­ “afraid.” One important facility of the facial
ing but far too little; the determinants of the muscles is that they can track the speed of
form of symbolic nonverbal acts are an words or phrases. In Example 2, presented
important area for further research. earlier in this chapter, the speaker’s face
changed rapidly from an angry expression
♦ III. Integrated (but Not to a smile exactly when her phrase ended.
Necessarily Redundant) We can illustrate the precise integration
of all three elements (hand gestures, facial
With Words
displays, and words) with a brief example
from our data (Bavelas, 2000). The speaker
One of our defining criteria for nonverbal was telling the addressee about a close call
acts that are part of language use in face-to­ he once had, when he fell into a river and
face dialogue was that they must be tightly nearly drowned:
06-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 7:26 PM Page 105

Nonverbal and Verbal Communication–––◆–––105

Example 3.
S: “So, my-my-my head is in the water like this,
[head back, eyes shut, impassive face]
and basically it’s, water’s going over my head.
[head to vertical] [hands sweep beside head]
And it’s str–I grew really, really calm.”
[puzzled ] [serious face, looking at A]

During each underlined phrase, the puzzling. Each phrase of this example
speaker depicted some aspect of his dilemma demonstrates precise coordination of
gesturally or facially. In the first two lines, he words, hand (and head) gestures, and
demonstrated that “like this” meant a par­ facial displays, all serving the immediate
ticular position of his head in the water and narrative purpose.
also his closed eyes and impassive facial dis­ Coordination seems to be an important
play; then he returned his head to vertical factor in creating the meaning of these
and demonstrated that “water’s going over speech-related nonverbal acts. Engle (2000;
my head” meant that the water (represented see also Engle, 1998; Engle & Clark, 1995)
by his hands) was sweeping past the sides of conducted an intensive analysis of multi-
his head. Altogether, this first sentence said modal signals (speech, gestures, diagrams,
and showed that he was on his back with the and object demonstrations), which yielded
water flowing around, but not over, his face. several lines of evidence for the temporal
At the same time, he illustrated his helpless­ and linguistic integration of iconic and
ness facially, with his eyes closed and an indexical conversational gestures with
impassive expression, both also synchronous speech. For example, the gesture and the
with the verbal description of his dilemma. immediately accompanying speech segment
Accomplishing these depictions required a were co-expressive, referring to the same
high degree of coordination and integration. underlying referent:
For example, in order to show how “water’s
going over my head,” he had to return his For all but one of the 108 [communica­
head to vertical and lift his hands up to the tive] nonverbal signals, a co-expressive
sides of his head (the preparatory phase) speech segment could be found within
before he started to say the phrase. [a] two intonation unit time window.
His impassive expression foreshadowed . . . In stark contrast to communicative
the latter part of his next sentence (“I grew nonverbal signals, in 14 of [the] 17
really, really calm”), but first he interrupted non-communicative cases, no co­
his narration to insert a metacommunicative expressive speech was present. (Engle,
comment on his own reaction: He said a 1998, pp. 323–324)
shortened version of “And it’s strange,”
while making a very brief but clear facial One implication of Engle’s findings is
display of puzzlement, as if still unable to that timing is a metacommunicative tool
understand what he was describing as that speakers use to signal what is in the
his strange calmness in the situation. same integrated unit of meaning (Engle,
He then returned to the main narrative 2000). Bavelas, Holt, and Allison (2000)
line by depicting, verbally, prosodically, analyzed over 1,700 gestures to learn how
and facially, the calmness he now found they were connected to co-occurring speech.
06-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 7:26 PM Page 106

106–––◆–––Foundations

The data revealed that, whereas speakers received a single unitary interpretation
sometimes used linguistic markers (e.g., a distinct from the interpretation of either
deictic expression or a dummy noun phrase), constituent, not a preference for one
the most common link was simply timing rather than the other of two discrete
(70% of the gestures). Unfortunately, we do messages. (Sanders, 1987, p. 142; italics
not have comparable information for facial added)
displays. There are to our knowledge no sys­
tematic studies of the precise temporal and These interpretations are consistent with
linguistic relationship of facial displays to Engle’s (1998) proposal that, following
words, although advances in digital analysis Grice’s (1967/1989) “cooperative princi­
make such frame-by-frame analysis possi­ ple,” both speaker and addressee assume
ble, albeit still labor-intensive. that “all signals in a particular composite
signal are intended to be treated from the
start as contributing to a single, unified
REDUNDANCY AND interpretation” (Engle, 1998, p. 321).
NONREDUNDANCY But what is the internal nature of a mul­
timodal message that produces a single,
Engle’s (2000) data also confirmed that, unified interpretation? We propose that, pri­
although the gestures were virtually always marily because of synchronous timing, mul­
consistent with the co-expressive speech, timodal elements can range from completely
they were sometimes complementary rather redundant to highly nonredundant and still
than duplicating the speech. This observa­ remain unified. Our research group has
tion contradicts the possibility that hand examined the degree or rate of redundancy
gestures are simply a redundant mode of in hand gestures with different functions.
expression. Examples 1 and 3 each illus­ Bavelas, Chovil, Lawrie, and Wade (1992),
trated that gestures can convey important for example, examined the degree of redun­
information that is not in the speaker’s dancy of a gesture with its accompanying
words (e.g., the width of the dress and the phonemic clause and found that, across
way his head was in the water). Sometimes several different descriptive tasks, gestures
the simultaneous audible and visible ele­ depicting features of the task topic were
ments of a message, taken separately, might much more redundant with the words than
appear to contradict each other. As Sanders were gestures that referred to the interlocu­
(1987) pointed out, however, receivers inte­ tor or to the interaction itself. The latter
grate these apparent contradictions at the (which we called interactive gestures) were
level of overall meaning (rather than at the usually completely nonredundant, although
level of components or physical source). they depended on and contributed to the
For example, Bugental, Kaswan, and Love meaning of the clause.
(1970; cited in Sanders, 1987) found An example from our data (Bavelas,
Sutton, Gerwing, & Johnson, 2002) illus­
positively valued utterances paired with a trates a nonredundant interactive gesture.
negatively valued facial expression and At the beginning of their getting-acquainted
vocal qualities were judged by respon­ conversation, one participant had answered
dents to be sarcastic. Negatively valued the other’s inquiry by saying that he was a
utterances paired with positively valued Political Science major. A minute later, after
nonverbal displays were judged to involve they had been discussing another topic, the
joking. Thus, these inconsistent pairs same speaker returned to his academic
of utterances and nonverbal displays standing:
06-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 7:26 PM Page 107

Nonverbal and Verbal Communication–––◆–––107

Example 4.
S: “This is my last term, and, ah, Political Science. I was a double major . . .”
[flicks hand to A]

Because his addressee already knew portraying a past or present personal reac­
what his major was, naming it again was tion) were redundant with speech; the other
not new but given (i.e., shared) informa­ 162 semantic displays by speakers were
tion. The speaker’s words (“and, ah, nonredundant. The 315 syntactic facial dis­
Political Science”) were cryptic and did not plays by speakers (e.g., grammatical mark­
fit the syntax of his sentence; they also con­ ers such as emphasis or question markers)
tained no reference to the addressee’s prior were virtually always nonredundant with
knowledge. In our view, it was the hand words, although not necessarily with
flick at the addressee that made the socially prosody. Finally, the 160 facial displays by
necessary reference; we interpret this ges­ listeners were, by definition, nonredundant
ture to mean “as you already know,” that with speech, because the listener was the
is, as citing or acknowledging that the person who was not speaking at the
addressee obviously still remembered what moment. Thus, over 70% of all displays
the speaker’s major was. The effective sen­ conveyed information that was not in the
tence would be, in words, “This is my last words. We speculate that the smiles by either
term and, as you know, I’m in Political speaker or listener, which were not analyzed
Science. I was a double major . . .” in this study, would follow a similar pattern.
One limitation of our analysis in Bavelas In sum, the third defining criterion of the
et al. (1992) was that it did not distinguish nonverbal acts of interest in this chapter is
among different experimental conditions that they form an integrated whole, with
and therefore included some conditions in words and each other. Integration does not
which there was no addressee or in which necessarily or even usually mean duplication,
the speaker and addressee were interacting as there is at least some evidence that the var­
through a partition. More recently, Bavelas, ious modalities can convey different (nonre­
Gerwing, Sutton, and Prevost (2002, 2005) dundant) information from each other.
examined gestural redundancy as a function When and how these diverse but unified
of the presence and visual availability of the elements operate is an important question for
addressee. When speaker and addressee were future research.
face to face, fewer than 20% of the speaker’s
gestures conveyed only information that was
also in their words; over 80% also included ♦ IV. Communication
some nonredundant information. In con­
in Dialogue
trast, when the speakers were on the tele­
phone or talking into a tape recorder to no
one, their gestures were significantly more This final section examines evidence that the
redundant; almost 60% of their gestures speakers and addressees use hand gestures
were entirely redundant with their words. and facial displays to communicate. To do
Likewise, Chovil (1989, 1991/1992; so, it is first necessary to discuss methodol­
see also Bavelas & Chovil, 1997) reported ogy, because there are three different meth­
redundancy data on 880 conversational ods for examining these issues. The first two
facial displays. She found that 243 of the focus on speaker and addressee separately:
405 semantic displays by speakers (e.g., An encoding design tests the conditions
06-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 7:26 PM Page 108

108–––◆–––Foundations

under which speakers do or do not produce Johnston, 1979; Schneider & Josephs,
hand gestures or facial displays; they should 1991). Only Chovil’s (1989, 1991) experi­
be more likely to do so in face-to-face dia­ ment on addressees’ facial displays involved
logue than in other conditions. A decoding a conversational dialogue.
design seeks evidence that those who see In contrast, and for obvious reasons,
such acts also understand their meaning. We encoding studies of conversational gestures
have reviewed most of these studies in other have involved conversations, but virtually
places (Bavelas & Chovil, 2000; Bavelas, none of them have been spontaneous
Gerwing, Sutton, et al., 2005; Chovil, 1997; dialogues. In most of these gesture studies,
see also Kendon, 1994; Gray & Ambady, the addressees were nonreactive confeder­
this volume) and will only summarize the ates or the experimenter, or even an imag­
pattern here. The vast majority of studies ined other (e.g., Alibali, Heath, & Myers,
show evidence for communication, in that 2001; Bavelas, Kenwood, et al., 2002;
(1) individuals tend to produce more ges­ Beattie & Aboudan, 1994; Cohen, 1977;
tures or facial displays when someone Cohen & Harrison, 1973; Emmorey &
would see them and (2) observers can garner Casey, 2001; Krauss, Dushay, Chen, &
accurate information from these acts. Rauscher, 1995). Only three studies, to
Indeed, these studies have been so successful our knowledge, involved spontaneous dia­
that we can now begin to see their limita­ logues between two participants (Bavelas
tions. Therefore, we will point out here et al., 1992; Bavelas, Gerwing, Sutton,
what, in retrospect, appear as deficiencies et al., 2005; Rimé, 1982).
in studies of isolated individuals (including As shown in Bavelas and Chovil (2000),
some of our own experiments) and will there are still far fewer decoding than
suggest a third method, one that examines encoding studies, and virtually all of them
the speaker and addressee together, within involve a similar design, one that tests
their interaction. whether outsiders to the interaction who
later view the gesture or facial display can
ascertain or at least agree on its meaning
ENCODING AND (e.g., Bavelas et al., 1986; Bavelas et al.,
DECODING DESIGNS 1990; Rosenfeld, Shea, & Greenbaum,
1979; Shea & Rosenfeld, 1976).3 In one
The typical encoding design varies study, Graham and Argyle (1975) showed
whether or not the speaker has a visually that the addressees were more accurate at
available recipient, for example, whether drawing figures when the speaker who
the speaker is alone or in the presence of described them had been able to gesture,
another person. The main limitation of the but there was little or no interaction invol­
existing encoding designs is that they sel­ ved between speakers and addressees.
dom include a spontaneous face-to-face Although these studies are encouraging
dialogue, which should be the baseline con­ for demonstrating some decoding of gestures
dition. For example, most studies of facial or facial displays, the viewers were rarely
displays have instead used mere presence, the original addressees, a methodological
eye contact, or social context instead of choice that raises at least two problems.
face-to-face dialogue (e.g., Bavelas, Black, First, most decoder studies do not present
Lemery, & Mullett, 1986; Fernandez-Dols the entire conversation, so that the decod­
& Ruis-Belda, 1995; Fridlund, 1991b; ing outsider has a fraction of the context
Jones, Collins, & Hong, 1991; Jones & that the addressee had. Our unpublished
Hong, 2001; Jones & Raag, 1989; Kraut & pilot studies with these designs showed
06-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 7:26 PM Page 109

Nonverbal and Verbal Communication–––◆–––109

that, in highly controlled presentations, same pattern of relationships to speaker


decoders imagined contexts in order to turn signals as did responses such as
make sense of a gesture, thereby escaping “mhm” or nodding. He concluded that these
experimental control. Second, Schober and smiles also acted as back-channel responses.
Clark’s (1989) experiments on verbal More recently, we (Bavelas, Chovil,
dialogue showed that, even when outsiders Coates, & Roe, 1995) used a similar statis­
had access to the entire conversation tical approach to examine the momentary
between speaker and addressee, they had a effects of interactive gestures (which, as
significantly poorer understanding because described above, are social gestures aimed
they were not part of the dialogue and at the addressee and are usually nonredun­
could not participate in grounding, that is, dant with speech). The analysis required
in the interactive process of establishing independently identifying (1) the perlocu­
mutual understanding (see also Roberts & tionary meaning of each gesture in a large
Bavelas, 1996). The same effect may also sample of interactive gestures and (2) the
apply to gestures or facial displays. immediately following response of the
addressee. In almost all cases, there was a
DIALOGUE DESIGNS significant relationship between the predic­
tions based on the meaning of the gesture
The above criticisms lead us to propose and the addressee’s response to the gesture,
that the best studies of whether hand ges­ even though the meaning did not appear in
tures or facial displays communicate are words.
ones that focus on the original speaker and Furuyama (2000) demonstrated that,
addressee in dialogue. There are two design when one person taught another how to
alternatives: Because of the requirement of make an origami figure without paper avail­
unscripted interaction between the partici­ able, the teachers of course used gestures to
pants, such studies often involve micro­ demonstrate. The learners frequently joined
analysis of events occurring spontaneously in their teacher’s gesture, acting in and on
within the interaction rather than the effects the same gesture space; for example, they
of experimentally manipulation. As shown pointed to or even touched the teacher’s ges­
below, however, there are also true experi­ ture as part of their dialogue about the
ments, which use controlled tasks and figure. Furuyama called these “collaborative
systematic quantitative analysis; the inde­ gestures.” Clark and Krych (2004) analyzed
pendent variable applies to the dyad rather one person teaching another how to build
than to an individual. In any case, the best a Lego structure. The learner would often
method is the one that goes where the phe­ check with the teacher, for example, by
nomenon is happening. pointing at a particular block, by picking it
Two early studies used nonexperimental up and exhibiting it to the teacher, or by
designs. Camras (1977) created a situation poising it over where it might go. These
in which two children would both want the actions were not ones that actually placed
same object. She showed that, when the the blocks; instead, they were arguably ges­
child with the object made an aggressive tural demonstrations of intention or inquiry.
facial display, the other child would stop The teachers seemed to use them as such, as
trying to take it and would wait longer evidenced by their responding immediately
before trying again than when not met to them, even interrupting themselves to
with an aggressive facial display. Brunner change what they were saying in response to
(1979) conducted a statistical analysis what the learner was communicating with
showing that listeners’ smiles followed the the gesture.
06-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 7:26 PM Page 110

110–––◆–––Foundations

There is also an increasing number of research question seems to have shifted


true experiments involving two partici­ from whether participants use gestures to
pants (i.e., neither experimenter nor con­ communicate, using standard rate measures,
federate) in a spontaneous dialogue. For to how they use gestures to communicate,
example, several studies described in an using more subtle and varied measures.
earlier section of this chapter showed that
speakers in dialogue change the form of
their gestures because of their addressee’s ♦ Conclusion
location (Ozyurek, 2000, 2002) or current
knowledge (Gerwing & Bavelas, 2004;
Some nonverbal acts are an intrinsic part of
Woods, 2005). Bavelas et al. (2002, 2005)
language use in face-to-face dialogue. In this
found that speakers who were describing a
chapter, we have focused on conversational
picture of an unusual dress in face-to-face
hand gestures and facial displays because
dialogue (compared with talking on the
there is research support for their use in dia­
telephone or to a tape recorder) made sig­
logue. The data so far suggest the outline of
nificantly larger, life-sized gestures that
a model for how these acts function in dia­
were also less redundant with speech and
logue: First, there are independent criteria
more likely to be marked with a deictic
for identifying this subset of nonverbal
expression.
acts, all of which focus on their synchrony
Bangerter (2004) found that, when one
with speech. Second, these are analogically
person was identifying targets for another
encoded symbolic acts, functionally distin­
to choose, the speakers relied more on
guishable from the actions or objects they
pointing than on words when their dis­
may represent. Third, they form integrated
tance from the target object was short. At
messages with the words they accompany,
greater distances, pointing would be more
although they may frequently convey infor­
ambiguous, and the speakers used words
mation that is not merely redundant with
to describe the object. Finally, Bavelas,
those words. Finally, there is a growing
Gerwing, Allison, and Sutton (2005) asked
body of such evidence that the participants
two participants to design a floor plan
in a dialogue use gestures to communicate
across a table from each other, with no
with each other; at present, there are only
paper to draw on, which elicited a large
a few such studies for faces. New directions
proportion of nonredundant gestures. The
for research could include expanding and
experimental variable was the width of the
refining the evidence presented in any of
table. When the table was narrow enough,
these four areas; exploring other nonverbal
they worked in the same space. When it
acts, such as gaze, that might be added to
was too wide, they had to work in different
gestures and facial displays; and beginning
spaces, but they reached out significantly
to reassemble the parts into the integrated
farther (toward each other), presumably
messages that participants create, in order to
so that the other person could see their
understand how they function as a whole.
gestures.
Thus, there are several examples each of
experimental and nonexperimental studies ♦ Notes
that examine gestures between speakers
and addressees in dialogue. It is worth 1. The present chapter is an extension of the
noting that, in these more recent studies, the model proposed in Bavelas and Chovil (2000). It
06-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 7:26 PM Page 111

Nonverbal and Verbal Communication–––◆–––111

includes a few sections adapted from that article, expression (pp. 334–346). Cambridge:
with the permission of the Journal of Language Cambridge University Press.
and Social Psychology and Sage Publications. Bavelas, J. B., & Chovil, N. (2000). Visible acts
2. Clark (1996, chap. 1) has outlined in of meaning. An integrated message model
fuller detail the characteristics of face-to-face of language use in face-to-face dialogue.
conversation as a fundamental setting for Journal of Language and Social Psychology,
language use. 19, 163–194.
3. We are not including any method that Bavelas, J. B., Chovil, N., Coates, L., & Roe, L.
isolates gestures from their verbal context (e.g., (1995). Gestures specialized for dialogue.
by using only the video without a sound track), Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin,
because such procedures treat conversational 21, 394–405.
gestures as if they were emblems. Bavelas, J. B., Chovil, N., Lawrie, D. A., & Wade,
A. (1992). Interactive gestures. Discourse
Processes, 15, 469–489.
♦ References Bavelas, J. B., Coates, L., & Johnson, T. (2002).
Listener responses as a collaborative process:
The role of gaze. Journal of Communication,
Alibali, M. W., Heath, D. C., & Myers, H. J. 52, 566–580.
(2001). Effects of visibility between speaker Bavelas, J. B., Gerwing, J., Allison, M., & Sutton,
and listener on gesture production: Some C. (2005, June). Evidence for grounding with
gestures are meant to be seen. Journal of non-redundant gestures: Co-constructing vir­
Memory and Language, 44, 169–188. tual spaces. Paper presented at the second
Bangerter, A. (2004). Using pointing and conference of the International Society for
describing to achieve joint focus of atten­ Gesture Studies, Lyon, France.
tion in dialogue. Psychological Science, 15, Bavelas, J. B., Gerwing, J., Sutton, C., & Prevost,
415–419. D. (2002, June). Gestures in face-to-face,
Bavelas, J. B. (1990). Nonverbal and social telephone, and tape recorder conditions.
aspects of discourse in face-to-face interac­ Paper presented at the first conference of the
tion. Text, 10, 508. International Society for Gesture Studies,
Bavelas, J. B. (2000). Nonverbal aspects of flu­ Austin, TX.
ency. In H. Riggenbach (Ed.), Perspectives Bavelas, J. B., Gerwing, J., Sutton, C., &
on fluency (pp. 91–101). Ann Arbor: Prevost, D. (2005). Gesturing on the tele­
University of Michigan Press. phone: Independent effects of dialogue and
Bavelas, J. B. (in press). The micro-social dimen­ visibility. Unpublished manuscript.
sion of face-to-face dialogue. In S. Duncan Bavelas, J. B., Holt, T., & Allison, M. (2000).
& E. Levy (Eds.), Papers in honor of Links between gestures and words.
David McNeill. Washington, DC: Unpublished research report.
American Psychological Association. Bavelas, J. B., Hutchinson, S., Kenwood, C., &
Bavelas, J. B., Black, A., Chovil, N., & Mullett, J. Matheson, D. H. (1997). Using face-to-face
(1990). Equivocal communication. Newbury dialogue as a standard for other communi­
Park, CA: Sage. cation systems. Canadian Journal of Com­
Bavelas, J. B., Black, A., Lemery, C. R., & munication, 22, 5–24.
Mullett, J. (1986). “I show you how you Bavelas, J. B., Kenwood, C., Johnson, T., &
feel.” Motor mimicry as a communicative Phillips, B. (2002c). An experimental study
act. Journal of Personality and Social of when and how speakers use gestures to
Psychology, 50, 322–329. communicate. Gesture, 2, 1–17.
Bavelas, J. B., & Chovil, N. (1997). Faces in dia­ Bavelas, J. B., Sutton, C., Gerwing, J., & Johnson,
logue. In J. A. Russell & J. M. Fernandez- T. (2002d). Analysis of gestures in dialogue
Dols (Eds.), The psychology of facial [Training CD], Department of Psychology,
06-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 7:26 PM Page 112

112–––◆–––Foundations

University of Victoria, Victoria, B.C., Clark, H. H., & Gerrig, R. J. (1990). Quotations
Canada. as demonstrations. Language, 66, 764–805.
Beattie, G., & Aboudan, R. (1994). Gestures, Clark, H. H., & Krych, M. A. (2004). Speaking
pauses and speech: An experimental while monitoring addressees for understand­
investigation of the effects of changing ing. Journal of Memory and Language, 50,
social context on their precise temporal 62–81.
relationships. Semiotica, 99, 239–272. Cohen, A. A. (1977). The communicative
Birdwhistell, R. L. (1966). Some relationships functions of hand illustrators. Journal of
between American kinesics and spoken Communication, 27, 54–63.
American language. In A. Smith (Ed.), Com­ Cohen, A. A., & Harrison, R. P. (1973).
munication and culture (pp. 182–189). Intentionality in the use of hand illustrators
New York: Holt, Rinehart & Winston. in face-to-face communication situations.
Blurton-Jones, N. G. (1972). Criteria for use in Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
describing facial expressions of children. In 28, 276–279.
N. G. Blurton-Jones (Ed.), Ethological stud­ Duncan, S., & Fiske, D. (1977). Face-to-face
ies of child behavior (pp. 365–413). interaction: Research, methods, and theory.
Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. New York: Erlbaum.
Brunner, L. J. (1979). Smiles can be back Ekman, P. (1979). About brows: Emotional and
channels. Journal of Personality and Social conversational signals. In J. Aschoof, M. von
Psychology, 37, 728–734. Cranach, K. Foppa, W. Lepenies, & D. Ploog
Buchler, J. (Ed.). (1940). Philosophical writings of (Eds.), Human ethology (pp. 169–202).
Peirce. London: Routledge and Kegan Paul. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.
Bugental, D. E., Kaswan, J. W., & Love, L. R. Ekman, P. (1993). Facial expression and emo­
(1970). Perception of contradictory mean­ tion. American Psychologist, 48, 384–392.
ings conveyed by verbal and nonverbal Ekman, P. (1997). Should we call it expression
channels. Journal of Personality and Social or communication? European Journal of
Psychology, 16, 647–655. Social Sciences, 10, 333–359.
Camras, L. A. (1977). Facial expressions used Ekman, P., & Friesen, W. V. (1969). The reper­
by children in a conflict situation. Child toire of nonverbal behavior. Categories, ori­
Development, 48, 1431–1435. gins, usage, and coding. Semiotica, 1, 49–98.
Chovil, N. (1989). Communicative functions of Emmorey, K., & Casey, S. (2001). Gesture,
facial displays in conversation. Unpublished thought and spatial language. Gesture, 1,
doctoral dissertation, Department of Psycho­ 35–50.
logy, University of Victoria, Victoria, British Engle, R. A. (1998). Not channels but composite
Columbia, Canada. signals: Speech, gesture, diagrams and object
Chovil, N. (1991). Social determinants of facial demonstrations are integrated in multimodal
displays. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, explanations. In M. A. Gernsbacher & S. J.
15, 141–154. Derry (Eds.), Proceedings of the Twentieth
Chovil, N. (1991/1992). Discourse-oriented Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science
facial displays in conversation. Research Society (pp. 321–326). Mahwah, NJ:
on Language and Social Interaction, 25, Erlbaum.
163–194. Engle, R. A. (2000). Toward a theory of multi-
Chovil, N. (1997). Facing others: A social com­ modal communication combining speech,
municative perspective on facial displays. gestures, diagrams, and demonstrations
In J. A. Russell & J.-M. Fernandez-Dols in instructional explanations. Unpublished
(Eds.), The psychology of facial expression doctoral dissertation, School of Education,
(pp. 321–333). Cambridge: Cambridge Stanford University.
University Press. Engle, R. A., & Clark, H. H. (1995, March).
Clark, H. H. (1996). Using language. Using composites of speech, gestures, dia­
Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. grams and demonstrations in explanations
06-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 7:26 PM Page 113

Nonverbal and Verbal Communication–––◆–––113

of mechanical devices. Paper presented Graham, J. A., & Argyle, M. (1975). A


at the American Association for Applied cross-cultural study of the communication
Linguistics Conference, Long Beach, CA. of extra-verbal meanings by gestures. Inter­
Fernandez-Dols, J.-M., & Ruis-Belda, M.-A. national Journal of Psychology, 10, 57–67.
(1995). Are smiles a sign of happiness? Gold Grice, H. P. (1989). Logic and conversation. In
medal winners at the Olympic Games. Journal Studies in the way of words (pp. 22–40).
of Personality and Social Psychology, 69, Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
1113–1119. (Original date of lectures 1967)
Fillmore, C. (1981). Pragmatics and the descrip­ Jones, S., & Hong, H.-W. (2001). Onset of
tion of discourse. In P. Cole (Ed.), Radical voluntary communication: Smiling looks to
pragmatics (pp. 143–166). New York: mother. Infancy, 2, 353–370.
Academic Press. Jones, S. E., & LeBaron, C. D. (2002). Research
Fowler, C. A. (1988). Differential shortening of on the relationship between verbal and
repeated content words produced in vari­ nonverbal communication: Emerging inte­
ous communicative contexts. Language and grations. Journal of Communication, 52,
Speech, 31, 307–319. 499–521.
Fridlund, A. J. (1991a). Evolution and facial Jones, S. S., Collins, K., & Hong, H.-W. (1991).
action in reflex, social motive, and par­ An audience effect on smile production in
alanguage. Biological Psychology, 32, 10-month-old infants. Psychological Science,
3–100. 2, 45–49.
Fridlund, A. J. (1991b). Sociality of solitary Jones, S. S., & Raag, T. (1989). Smile produc­
smiling: Potentiation by an implicit audi­ tion in older infants: The importance of a
ence. Journal of Personality and Social social recipient for the facial signal. Child
Psychology, 60, 229–240. Development, 60, 811–818.
Furuyama, N. (2000). Gestural interaction Kendon, A. (1972). Some relationships between
between the instructor and the learner in body motion and speech. In A. W. Seigman
origami instruction. In D. McNeill (Ed.), & B. Pope (Eds.), Studies in dyadic com­
Language and gesture (pp. 99–117). munication. Elmsford, NY: Pergamon.
Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Kendon, A. (1980). Gesticulation and speech:
Gerwing, J., & Bavelas, J. (2004). Linguistic Two aspects of the process of utterance.
influences on gesture’s form. Gesture, 4, In M. R. Key (Ed.), The relationship of
157–195. verbal and nonverbal communication
Gilbert, A. N., Fridlund, A. J., & Sabini, J. (pp. 207–227). The Hague: Mouton.
(1987). Hedonic and social determinants of Kendon, A. (1985). Uses of gesture. In
facial displays to odors. Chemical Senses, D. Tannen & M. Saville-Troike (Eds.),
12, 355–363. Perspectives on silence (pp. 215–234).
Goffman, E. (1974). Frame analysis. New York: Norwood, NJ: Ablex.
Harper & Row. Kendon, A. (1994). Do gestures communicate?
Goodwin, C. (1981). Conversational organi­ A review. Research on Language and Social
zation. Interaction between speakers and Interaction, 27, 175–200.
hearers. New York: Academic Press. Krauss, R. M., Dushay, R. A., Chen, Y., &
Goodwin, C. (2000). Gesture, aphasia, and inter­ Rauscher, F. (1995). The communicative
action. In D. McNeill (Ed.), Language and value of conversational hand gestures.
gesture (pp. 99–117). Cambridge: Cambridge Journal of Experimental Social Psychology,
University Press. 31, 533–552.
Goodwin, M. H., & Goodwin, C. (1986). Kraut, R. E., & Johnston, R. E. (1979). Social
Gesture and coparticipation in the activity and emotional messages of smiling: An etho­
of searching for a word. Semiotica, 62, logical approach. Journal of Personality and
51–75. Social Psychology, 37, 1539–1553.
06-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 7:26 PM Page 114

114–––◆–––Foundations

LeBaron, C., & Streeck, J. (2000). Gestures, the mind (pp. 763–765). Oxford: Oxford
knowledge, and the world. In D. McNeill University Press.
(Ed.), Language and gesture (pp. 99–117). Random house unabridged dictionary (2nd ed.).
Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. (1993). New York: Random House.
Leeds-Hurwitz, W. (1987). The social history Rimé, B. (1982). The elimination of visible
of The History of an Interview: A behaviour from social interactions: Effects
multidisciplinary investigation of social on verbal, nonverbal and interpersonal
communication. Research on Language variables. European Journal of Social
and Social Interaction, 20, 1–51. Psychology, 12, 113–129.
Leeds-Hurwitz, W. (1989). Communication Roberts, G. L., & Bavelas, J. B. (1996). The
in everyday life: A social interpretation. communicative dictionary: A collaborative
Parkside, WI: University of Wisconsin- theory of meaning. In J. Stewart (Ed.),
Parkside Press. Beyond the symbol model. Reflections on
Levinson, S. (1983). Pragmatics. Cambridge: the nature of language (pp. 139–164).
Cambridge University Press. Albany: SUNY Press.
Linell, P. (1982). The written language bias in Rosenfeld, N. M., Shea, M., & Greenbaum,
linguistics. Linkoping, Sweden: University of P. (1979). Facial emblems of “right” and
Linkoping, Department of Communication. “wrong”: Topographical analysis and
McNeill, D. (1985). So you think gestures are derivation of a recognition test. Semiotica,
nonverbal? Psychological Bulletin, 92, 26, 15–34.
350–371. Sanders, R. E. (1987). The interconnection of
McNeill, D. (1992). Hand and mind: What utterances and nonverbal displays. Research
gestures reveal about thought. Chicago: on Language and Social Interaction, 20,
University of Chicago Press. 141–170.
McQuowan, N. (Ed.). (1971). The natural history Scheflen, A. (1968). Human communication:
of an interview. Microfilm collection of man­ Behavioral programs and their integration
uscripts on cultural anthropology. Chicago: in interaction. Behavioral Science, 13,
University of Chicago, Joseph Regenstein 44–55.
Library, Department of Photoduplicating. Scherer, K. R. (1980). The functions of nonver­
Ozyurek, A. (2000). The influence of addressee bal signs in conversation. In R. N. St. Clair
location on spatial language and representa­ & H. Giles (Eds.), The social and psycho­
tional gestures of direction. In D. McNeill logical contexts of language (pp. 225–244).
(Ed.), Language and gesture (pp. 99–117). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press. Schneider, K., & Josephs, I. (1991). The
Ozyurek, A. (2002). Do speakers design their expressive and communicative functions of
cospeech gestures for their addressees? The preschool children’s smiles in an achieve­
effects of addressee location on representa­ ment-situation. Journal of Nonverbal
tional gestures. Journal of Memory and Behavior, 15, 185–198.
Language, 46, 665–875. Schober, M. F., & Clark, H. H. (1989).
Pike, K. L. (1972). Towards a theory of the struc­ Understanding by addressees and over-
ture of human behavior. In R. M. Brend hearers. Cognitive Psychology, 21,
(Ed.), Selected writings: To commemorate 211–232.
the 60th birthday of Kenneth Lee Pike Shea, M., & Rosenfeld, H. M. (1976).
(pp. 106–116). The Hague: Mouton. Functional employment of nonverbal social
Poyatos, F. (1980). Interactive functions and limi­ reinforcers in dyadic learning. Journal of
tations of verbal and nonverbal behaviors in Personality and Social Psychology, 34,
natural conversation. Semiotica, 30, 211–244. 228–239.
Quine, W. V. O. (1987). Symbols. In R. L. Slama-Cazacu, T. (1976). Nonverbal com­
Gregory (Ed.), The Oxford companion to ponents in message sequence: “Mixed
06-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 7:26 PM Page 115

Nonverbal and Verbal Communication–––◆–––115

syntax.” In W. C. McCormack & S. A. Weiner, M., Devoe, S., Rubinow, S., & Geller, J.
Wurm (Eds.), Language and man: Anthro­ (1972). Nonverbal behavior and nonverbal
pological issues (pp. 217–227). The communication. Psychological Review, 79,
Hague: Mouton. 185–214.
Streeck, J., & Knapp, M. L. (1992). The inter­ Woods, J. (2005). New vs. given information: Do
action of visual and verbal features in gestures dance to the same tune as words?
human communication. In F. Poyatos (Ed.), Unpublished honours thesis, Department of
Advances in nonverbal communication Psychology, University of Victoria, Victoria,
(pp. 3–24). Amsterdam: Benjamins. B.C., Canada.
06-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 7:26 PM Page 116
07-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 4:17 PM Page 117

PART II

FACTORS OF INFLUENCE
07-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 4:17 PM Page 118
07-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 4:17 PM Page 119

7
THE BIOLOGICAL FOUNDATIONS
OF SOCIAL ORGANIZATION
The Dynamic Emergence of Social Structure
Through Nonverbal Communication

� Ross Buck and Stacie Renfro Powers


University of Connecticut

T he functioning of any system involves communication between


system elements, and this chapter argues that social structure
emerges during individual development via nonverbal communication

Authors’ Note: The authors gratefully acknowledge the contributions of


many students and colleagues to the research on which this chapter is based.
Among those whose contributions have been particularly relevant to this
chapter are R. Thomas Boone of St. Johns University; Benson Ginsburg of the
University of Connecticut; David A. Kenny of the University of Connecticut;
Makoto Nakamura of Utsunomiya University, Japan; Elliott Ross of the
University of Oklahoma VA Medical Center; Edward Vieira of Simmons
College; and Lesley Withers of Central Michigan University. Parts of this
research were supported by NIMH RO1 MH48753; the Harry Frank
Guggenheim Foundation; the EJLB Foundation—National Trust, Canada
Council; the Russell Sage Foundation; and the University of Connecticut
Research Foundation.

◆ 119
07-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 4:17 PM Page 120

120–––◆–––Factors of Influence

between elements—that is, between at the same time they reflect the primordial
individuals. In this chapter, we present a imperatives seen in simple creatures: arousal
view of social structure as an emergent, self- and quiescence, approach and avoidance,
organizing dynamic system, and we seek dominance and submission, cooperation
to demonstrate the critical role of nonver­ and competition, courtship and sex.
bal communication in this emergence. We
review evidence relevant to this thesis,
arguing that even simple organisms show ♦ Selfishness, Altruism, and
evidence of motivational-emotional systems Communicative Genes
underlying basic behavior and sociality.
These systems exist not only within the
A fundamental issue in any discussion of
individual organism but also “out there” in
communication and sociality is the question
the communicative relationship associated
of cooperation and competition. Are living
with genetically structured displays in
creatures responding to genetic influences
senders and preattunements in receivers.
that are fundamentally selfish? Is altruism
The result is a spontaneous communicative
a cultural ideal with no biological under­
interplay from which, over time, social struc­
pinning? Or is cooperation—and attach­
ture emerges. In human beings, this emer­
ment and love—anchored in fundamental
gence proceeds so naturally and effortlessly
biological imperatives? The answers to
that it is often taken for granted, but many of
these questions turn on the technical issue
the deepest meanings conveyed by everyday
of the unit of selection in evolution, and
nonverbal communication reflect these
communication theory has important con­
unseen, primordial biological imperatives.
tributions to make in this domain.
This chapter considers how, through
genetically structured displays and preat­
tunements, spontaneous communicative SELFISH GENES
interplay creates the basis for the social
organization of species. The chapter moves The “selfish gene” theory of Richard
from the biochemical bases of motivation Dawkins (1989) and others was based on
and emotion that humans share with sim­ the interpretation of natural selection as
pler species to the particular ways in which operating at the level of the gene (i.e., that
social and moral emotions emerge from this individuals are motivated to maximize the
foundation. It examines the special role of number of genes contributed to subsequent
language in human social organization and generations; Hauser, 1996). A corollary of
the interaction between spontaneous non­ this gene selectionist account of evolution
verbal communication and linguistically was that the gene is the ultimate unit of evo­
organized communication. Finally, it pre­ lutionary selection: The gene is programmed
sents human interaction as a self-organizing only to make copies of itself. Dawkins
dynamic system involving the direct com­ (1982) argued that the unit of evolutionary
munication of social and moral emotions, selection must be self-replicating across the
including pride and arrogance, guilt and span of evolutionary time. He termed this
shame, envy and jealousy, pity and scorn, unit an active, germ-line replicator. His
and, potentially, moral emotions, such as candidate for replicator was the gene, which
triumph, humiliation, resentment, and con­ is maintained virtually intact from genera­
tempt. These social and moral emotions are tion to generation across evolutionary
the foundation of human interaction, and timescales. Fitness in the evolutionary sense
07-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 4:17 PM Page 121

The Biological Foundations of Social Organization–––◆–––121

is based not on the survival of the individ­ B’s general sending accuracy, A’s general
ual but on the survival of the individual’s receiving ability, and the unique ability of
genes, termed inclusive fitness. A to read B. The latter quantity reflects
One of the implications of this position the unique relationship of A and B and is
is that genuine altruism—sacrificing one’s termed the “relationship effect.”
own genetic fitness to preserve the genetic
fitness of others—should not exist at the COMMa to b = (SENDINGa + RECEIVINGb +

biological level. Instances of apparent UNIQUEa to b)

altruism are explained as reflecting inclu­


COMMb to a = (SENDINGb + RECEIVINGa +
sive fitness, where helping others improves
UNIQUEb to a)
the survival of one’s own genes through kin
selection (helping genetic relatives) and The overall influence of individual and
reciprocity (helping with the probability of relationship factors on communication
return). This position generated consider­ has been assessed in human beings by
able debate, because it suggested that round-robin experimental designs in which
apparently prosocial and selfless acts are A communicates not only with B but also
instances of selfish manipulation. with others—C and D—and B also com­
municates with C and D. This allows one to
measure the abilities of A and B to send and
COMMUNICATIVE GENES receive not only vis-à-vis one another but
also vis-à-vis C and D, so that one can esti­
The communicative gene hypothesis
mate the average sending accuracy of A to
accepts the gene-selectionist account but
B, C, and D; the average receiving ability of
adds that communicative relationships of
A from B, C, and D; and the same for B.
genes can be units of evolutionary selection
Knowing the overall communication
(Buck & Ginsburg, 1991, 1997). Dawkins’s
accuracy of A and B, and the general send­
argument assumed the gene was selected
ing accuracies and receiving abilities of A
as an individual unit: “selection purely at
and B, the unique abilities of A and B to
the level of the individual gene” (Dawkins,
communicate with each other (plus error)
1989, pp. 84–85). This genetic atomism,
can be assessed by solving the equation.
however, does not take into account the fact
For example, using a round-robin design in
that genes do not function alone: Whatever
which young married couples communi­
they do to enhance their own survival is in
cated nonverbally vis-à-vis one another
the context of interactive and communica­
and also vis-à-vis groups of “other males”
tive relationships with other genes.
and “other females,” Sabatelli, Buck, and
Communication involves phenomena
Kenny (1986) found communication from
both at the level of the individual sender
wife to husband, and from husband to wife,
and receiver and at the level of the dyadic
to be composed of the following:
sender-receiver relationship, and the relative
influence of these is potentially measurable. COMMw to h = (48% SENDINGw + 1%
Extending Kenny’s (1994) Social Relations RECEIVINGh + 51%
Model, communication from A to B is a [UNIQUEw to h + error])
function of the general sending accuracy of
A, the general receiving ability of B, and the COMMh to w = (22% SENDINGh + 10%
unique ability of B to “read” A. Conversely, RECEIVINGw + 68%
communication from B to A is composed of [UNIQUEh to w + error])
07-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 4:17 PM Page 122

122–––◆–––Factors of Influence

This analysis can be applied generally motivational and emotional states in


to any communication situation, including human beings: it is based on a biologically
the communication of genes. Gene A has shared and genetically based signal system
a communicative relationship with gene B consisting of displays on the part of the
and also with other genes, potentially all sender and preattunements on the part of
in the genome, just as an individual person the receiver. The sender’s displays are
A has a communicative relationship with signs—readouts of the sender’s internal
person B and also with others. In theory, motivational and emotional state—that
the sending and receiving of specific genes function as social affordances (Gibson,
can be assessed and related to their com­ 1966). Given attention, these are picked
munication with other genes, and unique up by perceptual preattunements in the
communicative relationships between genes receiver, who gains an intuitive and affec­
can be assessed. tive knowledge of the sender. Spontaneous
The communicating genes in question communication is direct, effortless, and
may be in the same cell, in different cells, nonpropositional.
or in different individual organisms. For In contrast, symbolic communication
example, genes that produce pheromones involves the intentional communication of
function in the context of genes in other propositions; it is based on a socially shared
organisms that produce receptors for those signal system that is learned rather than
pheromones. Sending genes persist across innate. Information is transmitted via
evolutionary time, dependent on receiving symbols, including spoken language. These
genes. So the unique relationship of sending two streams of symbolic and spontaneous
and receiving genes can be an active, germ- communication occur in every human
line replicator in Dawkins’s (1982) sense. conversation, although the relative impor­
This allows one to maintain a gene selec­ tance of the two streams varies with the sit­
tionist account of evolution, but by reject­ uation and the personal relationship. All
ing genetic atomism it also allows one to else being equal, the spontaneous stream is
recognize genuine prosocial tendencies, relatively more important in intimate, affec­
including biologically based prosocial emo­ tively charged situations and relationships,
tions and altruism, as based on the selection but it has some role—often unconscious—
of communicative relationships between in every communicative exchange.
genes (Buck, 1999, 2002). Of course, the sender also attempts to
control or manage the display via learned
and culturally variable display rules, and the
SPONTANEOUS, SYMBOLIC, receiver responds to displays in others by
AND PSEUDOSPONTANEOUS learned and culturally variable decoding rules
COMMUNICATION (Buck, 1984; Seyfarth & Cheney, 2003).
Pseudospontaneous communication involves
Communication between genes is a the intentional nonverbal display by the
fundamental example of spontaneous sender to manipulate the receiver: If the
communication, which involves genetically sender is successful, the receiver’s affective
based displays in a sender that can be “buttons are pushed” in a way that may be
“picked up” by genetically based preat­ beneficial to the sender tactically or strategi­
tunements in a receiver (Buck, 1984; cally and, possibly, detrimental to the receiver
Buck & VanLear, 2002). Spontaneous (Buck & VanLear, 2002). For example,
communication includes the nonverbal charismatic leaders are often masters of
and nonintentional communication of pseudospontaneous communication. Through
07-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 4:17 PM Page 123

The Biological Foundations of Social Organization–––◆–––123

practice and refinement of their expressions SUMMARY


of emotion, they are able to persuade their
audiences by turning on preattunements for Communication theory allows a useful
fear, anger, attraction, and so forth. approach to the conceptualization of the
Communicative relationships based on genetic bases of cooperation and competi­
spontaneous communication can persist tion. If communicative relationships between
across evolutionary timescales. An example genes function as units of selection in
is the dominant-submissive relationship in evolution, it follows that genes not only
which one individual displays threat that compete selfishly with other genes but
may be responded to by another individual also depend directly on communica­
with actions such as counterthreat, submis­ tively related genes for their own survival
sion, or withdrawal. Such communicative across evolutionary timescales: commu­
acts form the basis of the dominance hier­ nicatively related genes co-evolve and
archy and territoriality across many species survive as a collectivity. As a result, there
(Hauser, 1996; Wilson, 1975). There is evi­ are fundamental biological imperatives for
dence both in human beings and in other tendencies toward both cooperation and
animals that confident dominance is associ­ competition, resulting in a relationship
ated with the neurochemical serotonin (5­ between these opposed predispositions that
hydroxytriptamine), and the relationship of might be symbolized by the Yin and Yang
serotonin and dominance may underlie the (Buck, 2002). The next section presents
antidepressant effects of selective serotonin evidence that nonverbal communication is
uptake inhibitors (SSRIs), such as Prozac © critical to whether cooperation or competi­
and Zoloft © (see Buck, 1999). tion actually occurs.
Another striking example of a display
crossing evolutionary timescales involves
the sexual pheromone in the yeast Saccha­ ♦ Cooperation, Competition,
romyces cerevisiae, which is virtually iden­
and Deception
tical to gonadotropin-releasing hormone
(GnRH), a peptide neurochemical active
in mammals and associated with erotic In Expression of the Emotions in Man and
feelings in human beings (Panksepp, 1991). Animals, Charles Darwin (1872/1998)
The similarity is so great that the yeast argued that expressive displays evolved to
pheromone functions as the hormone in signal information about the sender’s moti­
rodents, and rodent hormone functions vational-emotional state, and this account
as the pheromone in yeast. Loumaye, was largely accepted by early ethologists.
Thorner, and Catt (1982) stated, “It is This position was challenged by the selfish-
intriguing that a pheromone responsible gene account of evolution, however, which
for mating and zygote formation in a uni­ argued that selection should actually work
cellular organism is both structurally and against individuals whose displays are
functionally related to the peptide serving a informative about their actual motiva­
key role in mammalian reproduction” (p. tional-emotional state (Hauser, 1996). In
1324). So, like the chemical organization of the latter view, displays are actually designed
dominance, the sending and receiving genes like advertisements, to deceive and manipu­
associated with these molecules of sexual late the receiver, concealing the actual state
attraction have endured and maintained of the sender (Dawkins, 1989; Dawkins &
their essential functions across evolutionary Krebs, 1978). The receiver responds by
timescales. “mind reading,” attempting to see through
07-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 4:17 PM Page 124

124–––◆–––Factors of Influence

the deception. The result is an evolutionary noted, honest expressivity comes at a poten­
“arms race” in which senders evolve pro­ tial cost to the sender—those who show
gressively more persuasive and manip­ their feelings accurately put themselves at
ulative “sales pitches,” and receivers risk of exploitation—and costly signals are
become increasingly sales resistant (Krebs more reliable. Thus, the costly nature of the
& Dawkins, 1984). Zahavi (1977) moder­ display is consistent with its representing an
ated this view by suggesting that displays accurate readout of the sender’s motivational-
can be honest but if and only if they are emotional state. Second, there is evidence
costly to the sender: In the absence of costs, that spontaneous emotional expressivity is in
the display becomes unreliable because it is fact valued by others. Sabatelli and Rubin
too easily imitated. (1986) found that emotional expressivity is
The question of honest versus mani­ positively related to judgments of interper­
pulative communication can be approached sonal attraction, independent of physical
from the viewpoint of economic game attractiveness. Third, studies demonstrating
theory. Frank (1988, 2001, 2004) argued that personality judgments based on brief
that cooperation can succeed only in the “thin slices” of nonverbal behavior have
presence of markers that distinguish indi­ accurately predicted interpersonal outcomes
viduals who are willing to cooperate—that (Ambady & Rosenthal, 1992) show that
is, individuals who are “trustworthy”— emotional expressivity has an immediate
from those who will not. Such markers impact, which could allow fast and accu­
must involve nonverbal behaviors that rate evaluation of the trustworthiness of
occur during the communication between strangers.
players (Boone & Buck, 2003). For For nonverbal expressiveness to function
example, Frank, Gilovich, and Regan as an accurate “sincerity detector,” sponta­
(1993) allowed partners to meet and inter­ neous displays must differ from intentional
act before a one-shot Prisoner’s Dilemma pseudospontaneous behaviors. Indeed,
(PD) game but not discuss the game itself. spontaneous expressive behavior does
Cooperation was enhanced overall, but differ from posed or intentionally enacted
more significantly, the participants could behavior, so that there is a signal basis for
apparently predict the decision of partners distinguishing between honest and manipu­
to cooperate or compete, so that dyads lative displays. Cohn and Schmidt (2004)
tended to show either mutual cooperation compared spontaneous smiles elicited by
or mutual defection, with relatively few viewing a comedy film with deliberate
cases of one partner taking advantage of the smiles, using automatic feature tracking to
other. Apparently, the intent to cooperate extract and represent dynamic features of
or compete was somehow discernable in the behavior as well as its morphology.
the partners’ behavior during the face-to­ They found that spontaneous and deliber­
face interaction. ate smiles could be distinguished: There
Boone and Buck (2003) combined con­ were differences in the timing of smile
siderations about whether displays are onsets, and there was a tight coupling
honest or manipulative with evidence about between duration and amplitude in sponta­
superficially self-defeating cooperation in neous smiles consistent with the contention
social dilemmas and suggested that a key that the spontaneous smile is automatic and
marker of trustworthiness is spontaneous involuntary. Also, there is initial empirical
nonverbal expressivity. This argument was evidence that expressiveness predicts coop­
based on several considerations. First, as eration in the PD game. Rauh, Polonsky,
07-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 4:17 PM Page 125

The Biological Foundations of Social Organization–––◆–––125

and Buck (2004) found that cooperative PALEOSOCIALITY IN


behavior, as assessed by the participants’ NONVERTEBRATE FORMS
cooperation on the first move of a com­
puter-simulated PD game, was positively “Paleosociality” refers to the social orga­
related to friendly expressiveness. nization and behavior of relatively simple
In conclusion, genuine cooperation and creatures. In these creatures, fundamental
altruism can be seen to have biological bases principles of social organization can be dis­
tied to nonverbal cues. If communicative cerned. Given the intrinsic conservatism of
relationships of genes are recognized to be evolution, these principles illuminate basic
active, germ-line replicators, it is consistent mechanisms underlying social organization
with a gene-selectionist account of evolu­ by offering specific examples of how social
tion. Therefore, displays and pre-attunements organization arises as an emergent and self-
are spontaneous sending and receiving organizing dynamic system. Moreover, it
mechanisms that co-evolve: Cooperation is becomes clear that the mechanism of emer­
fostered by communication that is friendly, gence through time is communication
open, direct, and genuinely expressive. It between elements—that is, between individ­
exists together with competition, antago­ ual organisms. With nonvertebrates, and
nism, and manipulation, which are also particularly with single-celled creatures,
biologically based and fostered by evasion, the system is uncomplicated enough so
equivocation, and pretense. that the specific mechanisms of communica­
tion—the display molecules and receptor
mechanisms—can be observed directly and
♦ The Spontaneous their genetic origins established. To reveal
Communication/Social this process, we consider two examples: quo­
rum sensing in bacteria and the life cycle of
Structure Relationship
the cellular slime mold. Our goal is to illus­
trate relatively simple examples of social
We have shown that spontaneous commu­ organization as a self-organizing dynamic
nication is biologically based in both its system emerging out of communicative inter­
sending and its receiving aspects and is a actions, which is based on general principles
critical determinant of cooperation and identical to those underlying the emergence
competition. Indeed, we argue that spon­ of human social organization emerging out
taneous communication is the foundation of communicative interactions.
of social organization. In some respects,
this spontaneous communication/social Quorum Sensing in Bacteria. Quorum sens­
structure relationship is easier to under­ ing is a remarkable example of bacterial
stand in simpler creatures, where behavior communication leading to collective action
is under relatively close genetic control. through arousal and quiescence (Hentzer
Despite the relative simplicity of such & Givskov, 2003; Waters & Bassler, 2005).
creatures, their social structures are sur­ “Quorum” refers to having a minimum
prisingly complex and show basic charac­ number of individuals to perform a given
teristics found in all social structures: action, as in a meeting that requires a certain
arousal-quiescence, approach-avoidance, number of participants. Quorum sensing in
dominance-submission, cooperation and bacteria refers to the ability of individual
competition, and some form of courting bacteria to sense through the concentration
and sexuality. of signaling molecules that their numbers in
07-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 4:17 PM Page 126

126–––◆–––Factors of Influence

the area have reached a critical mass, and the life cycle of the slime mold Dictyo­
thereby to coordinate collective behavior. stelium discoideum illustrates the implica­
It is used by certain pathogenic bacteria to tions of cell communication for larger
trick the immune systems of their hosts. The issues of overall social organization: the
bacteria are quiescent—not activating a organization of multicelled organisms.
full immune system response—until their Slime molds are abundant (a spoonful of
numbers are sufficient to overwhelm the rich garden soil contains millions), their
victim. When a quorum is attained, the reg­ principal food is rotting organic material,
ulation of bacterial genes is changed so that and they are responsible for the slimy mate­
toxic “virulence factors” are produced. For rial found on rotting wood. At one stage in
example, in cystic fibrosis, when the bacte­ their life cycle, D. discoideum are single-
ria reach a certain concentration, they pro­ celled amoebae that live independently and
duce a “biofilm,” a tough shell that protects display negative chemotaxis. That is, they
them from attack from the immune system produce pheromones that repel similar
of the victim or from antibiotics. The bacte­ amoebae (Newell, 1981), a function analo­
ria can then reproduce, produce toxins, and gous to territorial/threat displays in more
damage tissues in relative safety (Riedel & complex species, that tend to spread indi­
Eberl, 2002). viduals evenly over the available environ­
The molecules that bacteria use to ment. When food in the environment
communicate have structures similar to becomes scarce, the negative chemotaxis
those used by humans and many other crea­ ends, and a positive chemotaxis begins,
tures. The signals or displays used by bacte­ attracting rather than repelling the amoe­
ria typically involve amino acids or peptides bae. In D. discoideum the molecule on
functioning as pheromones (Gallio, Sturgill, which the positive chemotaxis is based is
Rather, & Kylsten, 2002). Peptides are cri­ cyclic adenosine monophosphate, a ubiqui­
tical signaling molecules in human beings, tous molecule involved in metabolism and
including the endorphins, oxytocin (OXY), in the functioning of many neurotrans­
vasopressin (VP), cholecystokinin, diazepam- mitters in human beings. The result is an
binding inhibitor, GnRH, adrenocortico aggregation center where the single-celled
tropic hormone, and insulin. Many of these amoebae come together and form a multi-
molecules exist virtually unchanged in celled form termed a “grex,” which may be
simple creatures. Moreover, these mole­ made up of millions of individuals.
cules are also associated with subjectively This journey culminates in the differenti­
experienced feelings and desires in human ation of the cells into a “fruiting body.”
beings, including attachment, euphoria, Cells at the front end die and become a cel­
pain, erotic feelings, panic, anxiety, and lulose stalk anchored to a secure footing,
stress. Gallio and colleagues (2002) con­ and a mass of cells from the rear then liter­
cluded that peptide-mediated signaling is ally climb over their fallen colleagues to
a “widely conserved mechanism for signal the top of the stalk and form spores that
release . . . not only the signal releasing become released into the environment.
mechanism seems conserved, but also the Given favorable conditions, the spores
molecule that carries it out . . . (suggesting become individual amoebae and begin the
that) . . . these molecules share a common life cycle anew (Waters & Bassler, 2005). In
ancestry” (pp. 12212–12213). this process, the individual amoebae that
form the stalk give up any possibility for
Slime Molds. Whereas quorum sensing passing on their genes, whereas the individ­
demonstrates that bacteria communicate, uals that climb the stalk carry on the
07-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 4:17 PM Page 127

The Biological Foundations of Social Organization–––◆–––127

species. As noted, giving up the potential to are polygamous. Prairie voles (Microtus
pass on one’s own genes while enhancing ochrogaster) show monogamy as evidenced
the genetic potential of other individuals by selective and lasting partner preferences
is the biological definition of altruism (pair bonds) that are activated by mating
(Wilson, 1975). Thus, D. discoideum in its (Wang & Aragona, 2004). The bonds are
life cycle demonstrates what might be lasting: If the bonded partner dies, a surviv­
termed proto-threat behavior in its negative ing prairie vole will live alone rather than
chemotaxis, proto-attachment behavior in take a new mate. Both male and female
its positive chemotaxis, and proto-altruistic provide extensive care for their pups, with
behavior in its stalk formation. the male helping to build the nest and
There are many other examples of spending almost as much time as the female
paleosociality in simple creatures. In insect with the young. If separated from their
societies, for example, social organization is parents, the pups become agitated and dis­
largely “hard wired”—that is, it is under play ultrasonic distress calls and high stress
close genetic control and has little to do with evidenced by increases in cortisol. In con­
learning or flexibility. The genes accomplish trast, closely related species—the meadow
social organization via spontaneous com­ vole (Microtus pennsylvanicus) and mon­
munication: genetically based displays and tane vole (Microtus montanus)—are non-
preattunements in different individual organ­ monogamous. The latter breed promiscuously
isms interacting with one another over time. and nest independently, and the males play
no parenting role. Even the females aban­
don their pups soon after birth, and the
SOCIALITY IN MAMMALS pups do not appear to be distressed by
abandonment (Carter, Lederhendler, &
In mammals, there is an additional con­ Kirkpatrick, 1997).
sideration for understanding social organi­ There is evidence that attachment in
zation: Unlike most reptiles, the mammal is voles is related to the peptides OXY and
helpless at birth. The brain and spinal cord VP. Brain areas associated with OXY are
do not come preprogrammed: They are much larger in the prairie vole than in the
programmed by the animal’s experience montane vole (Insel, Young, & Wang,
over the course of development in the terri­ 1997), and the distribution of VP receptors
torial and social environment. This experi­ in the male brain is different, with the
ence requires communication mechanisms prairie vole and other monogamous species
involving bonding, nurturance, and protec­ having a relatively high density of VP recep­
tion (i.e., attachment), to ensure that the tors (termed V1aR) in the ventral forebrain,
young animal receives adequate parental in an area of the brain also associated with
attention until it is physically and socially dopamine-mediated reward (Lim et al.,
mature enough to function on its own. An 2004; Young, Lim, Gingrich, & Insel,
examination of voles can help demonstrate 2001). Moreover, OXY and VP are critical
these communicative mechanisms. to the establishment of the pair bonds of
prairie voles. In the natural setting, mating
Polygamous and Monogamous Voles. The is associated with OXY release, which
vole is a particularly interesting case illus­ apparently functions to cement partner
trating the biology of attachment. The vole preferences in that the individuals prefer
is a mouse-size rodent, some species of who they are with when OXY increase
which are monogamous, forming lasting occurs (Carter, DeVries, & Getz, 1995).
female-male bonds, whereas other species Thus, OXY is both necessary and sufficient
07-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 4:17 PM Page 128

128–––◆–––Factors of Influence

for the development of pair bonds in molecules relevant to attachment mecha­


the female prairie vole (Williams, Insel, nisms can have significant effects on complex
Harbaugh, & Carter, 1994). human behavior, and perhaps particularly on
There is recent evidence that, rather behavior involving communication. It is clear
incredibly, the ruggedly individualistic but that these primordial affective motivational-
philandering male meadow vole can be emotional systems can influence complex
made to act like a sweet, sensitive, reliable choices in human beings involving coopera­
male prairie vole by the alteration of a sin­ tion and competition, and this is consistent
gle gene. And, it is certainly a communica­ with our view that such systems underlie
tive gene. Lim et al. (2004) demonstrated basic behavior and sociality in humans.
that V1aR gene transfer into the ventral
forebrain of male meadow voles sub­ Complex Mammalian Social Organization.
stantially increased partner preference More complex social mammals, such as
formation, indicating that “changes in the wolves and nonhuman primates (monkeys
regional expression of a single gene can and apes), demonstrate more complex
have a profound effect on the social behav­ social behavior accompanied by more com­
iour of individuals within a species” plex communication patterns. With these
(p. 756). The V1aR gene transfer increases creatures, bonding cannot be simply turned
VP receptors in the ventral forebrain, and on and off chemically or genetically. Rather,
the authors suggest that this has the effect bonding involves a long process of develop­
of increasing social memory of the partner’s mental experience in communicative rela­
olfactory signature and also associating tionships with other individuals. For
that social memory with dopamine­ example, Woolpy and Ginsburg (1967)
mediated reward: “Monogamous social raised unrelated wolves together from
organization might be the result of the infancy with no adult tutors. Over the
insertion of the V1aR system into the ancient course of development the individuals gen­
pre-existing reward circuit” (p. 756). erated a typical adult pack structure from
Interestingly, dopamine-mediated reward their own communicative interactions.
is also associated with addiction, and it Often these communicative interchanges
may be that the increased V1aRs induce the occur in the context of rough-and-tumble
animal to become physically addicted to play, which may be accompanied by plea­
the partner (in this regard, Panksepp, 1991, sure associated with endorphin mechanisms
noted a number of similarities between (Burgdorf & Panksepp, 2001).
addiction and attachment). Recent studies Studies by Harry Harlow and colleagues
have demonstrated such effects on coopera­ demonstrated that rhesus monkeys show
tive and competitive behavior in the PD specific stages of socioemotional develop­
games considered previously. With human ment based on experience in commu­
participants, Kosfeld, Heinrichs, Zak, nicative relationships (Harlow, 1971).
Fischbacher, and Fehr (2005) found that Interactions with the mother initiate the
OXY presented in an aerosol increased maternal affectional system, which estab­
cooperative behavior in a PD game, and lishes a basic sense of attachment and trust
Wood, Rilling, Sanfey, Bhagwagar, and in other monkeys. This is followed by the
Rogers (2005) reported that the disruption peer affectional system, in which commu­
of central serotonin function increases com­ nicative displays—threats, submissive behav­
petitive tendencies. The implication is that iors, courting behaviors—are “tried out”
these physiological manipulations involving in the context of rough-and-tumble
07-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 4:17 PM Page 129

The Biological Foundations of Social Organization–––◆–––129

play relationships with peers. This sets plays a similar role, but now in interaction
the stage for the heterosexual affectional with language, a new source of social orga­
system, which is the basis for adult monkey nization missing in other animals.
communicative relationships and social
organization. Harlow suggested that these
are analogous to infancy, childhood, and ♦ Language, Nonverbal
adolescence in humans. Communication, and
Harlow’s work also demonstrated that Human Social Organization
early social deprivation can have devastat­
ing effects on an individual animal’s abilities
to participate in social behavior. Contact The human brain shares many features
comfort—contact with a soft skinlike with the brains of other animals, particu­
surface—was found to be sufficient to larly in the phylogenetically older subcorti­
instill a basic sense of trust, but trusting cal and limbic regions, which are associated
individuals raised separately from peers with motivational and emotional process­
showed significant deficits in courting and ing (Buck, 1999). In those regions, similar
sexual behavior after puberty. Animals neurochemicals appear to function in ways
reared in wire cages were extremely fearful analogous to those in other animals, and
and aggressive as adults (Harlow, 1971). they do seem to have the ability in some
There is evidence that these social deficits cases to overwhelm more “rational” linguis­
are related to an inability to communicate. tic control mechanisms. Addiction provides
Specifically, Miller, Caul, and Mirsky many tragic examples. Indeed, it can be
(1967) demonstrated that monkeys isolated argued that these motivational-emotional
in infancy were unable as adults either to systems set the agenda for human commu­
send or to receive accurately in Miller’s nication and underlie the human equiva­
cooperative conditioning paradigm measur­ lents of the primordial characteristics
ing communication in monkeys (Miller, found in all social structures—arousal­
Banks, & Ogawa, 1962, 1963). quiescence, approach-avoidance, dominance-
submission, cooperation-competition, and
courting-sexuality—all of which are commu­
SUMMARY nicated nonverbally.
Language introduces in human beings
Examples of social organization from a source of behavioral control and social
simple bacteria to mammals including organization that is lacking in animals,
primates demonstrate common principles creating a real discontinuity with other
involving an unfolding of biological poten­ species (Buck, 1988). As complex proposi­
tial in the course of development. At all tions become expressible and recordable in
these levels, social organization emerges out language, questions of linguistic organiza­
of communicative interactions as a self- tion that are distinct from biological fac­
organizing dynamic system. In mammals, tors become important determinants of
attachment systems involving bonding, human social organization. On the other
nurturance, and protection of the young, hand, language itself is based on biological
are critical and demand communication mechanisms, associated particularly with
systems through which attachment can func­ the left hemisphere (LH) of the brain in
tion. We now turn to human social organi­ almost all humans. For example, damage
zation, where nonverbal communication to the LH has long been associated with
07-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 4:17 PM Page 130

130–––◆–––Factors of Influence

crippling language deficits collectively SUMMARY


termed aphasia. Damage confined to
Wernicke’s area is associated with recep­ In human beings, spontaneous and sym­
tive aphasia, in which the patient can bolic communication proceeds in simultane­
speak but cannot comprehend. Damage ous streams, spontaneous communication
confined to Broca’s area near the left being entirely nonverbal and symbolic com­
motor cortex, associated with the control munication involving both verbal and non­
of the mouth, tongue, and larynx, is asso­ verbal elements. We have argued that the
ciated with expressive aphasia, in which spontaneous stream guides cooperative and
the patient can comprehend but cannot competitive behavior, effortlessly and often
speak. Damage to the right hemisphere unconsciously. The spontaneous stream is
(RH) in contrast is often associated with also involved in social comparison process­
deficits in spontaneous communication. es that can involve inequalities, where one
Patients damaged in the RH can express partner is in a dominant position relative to
propositions normally, but often show the other. Such inequalities are the founda­
deficits in both the sending and the receiv­ tion of higher level social and moral
ing aspects of spontaneous communica­ emotions. The next sections consider
tion. Specifically, the voice tends to be flat how social and moral emotions emerge,
and lacking in affective prosody, the naturally and effortlessly, from human
face is unexpressive, and there are often interactions.
deficits in understanding emotion and
other affective information in the voices
and faces of others (Buck & Duffy, 1980; ♦ Social and Moral Emotions in
Ross, 1992).
Human Social Organization
Many nonverbal behaviors—gestures,
illustrators, emblems, and so on—are linked
closely with language (see Bavelas & SOCIAL EMOTIONS
Chovil, this volume), and there is evidence
they are organized linguistically. For Social comparison processes occur con­
example, persons who use gestural forms stantly in animal social behavior, ranging
of communication, such as sign language from relatively straightforward considera­
and finger spelling, typically lose their abil­ tions of who is bigger, smellier, louder, or
ities after suffering LH damage. In a meta­ more colorful to more complex issues involv­
analysis, Buck and VanLear (2002) showed ing establishing and maintaining enduring
that abilities at both sending and receiving social relationships (de Waal & Tyack,
via pantomime are generally lost with LH 2003). In humans, linguistic competence
damage, suggesting that the LH is associ­ adds enormously to the complexity of these
ated with intentional, symbolic, proposi­ considerations, but often social comparisons
tional communication and the RH with come down to whether the comparison
spontaneous, automatic, nonpropositional, person is better off or worse off than oneself.
affective communication. The implication But this social comparison is not necessarily
is that the distinction between “sponta­ only rational: It is often fraught with strong
neous” and propositional or “symbolic” emotion. Arguably, the emotional “fire” in
communication is more fundamental social and moral emotions has its origin in
than that between verbal and nonverbal that fundamental mammalian motivational-
communication. emotional system: attachment.
07-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 4:17 PM Page 131

The Biological Foundations of Social Organization–––◆–––131

Fundamental Social Motives. The motiva­ jealousy, and scorn on the other relates to
tion arising from attachment has, arguably, whether the responder is securely or inse­
two aspects: the need to be loved and the curely attached in that particular comparison
need to meet or exceed expectations (Buck, situation. A secure person may experience
1988). People are strongly motivated to be pride, guilt, envy, and pity in situations where
esteemed and loved and to do that which is an insecure person may experience arro­
expected and indeed to exceed expectations. gance, shame, jealousy, and scorn.
Individuals can attain or fail to attain these
two social goals themselves, and they can The Dynamics of Social Emotions. These
compare themselves with other persons who fundamental interpersonal contingencies
attain or fail to attain these goals. The rela­ do not elicit social emotions in isolation
tive strength of these two social motives from one another. Rather, because success
should be related to the security of the and failure are relative, the social emotions
attachment of the person to reference are dynamically related one to the other.
persons evaluating them. Persons who are If P is successful and experiences pride-
insecurely attached are likely to need reas­ arrogance, P will tend to pity-scorn the less
surance that they are loved, and that over­ successful O. On the other hand, O will
whelming need might overshadow needs to tend to be envious-jealous of P and will
meet or exceed expectations. Secure individ­ experience guilt-shame in comparison to P.
uals, in contrast, should be relatively assured The resulting dynamics of the social emo­
of love, and they will instead be able to focus tions are summarized in Figure 7.1a.
on meeting and exceeding expectations. To test these predictions, American and
Japanese students were asked to read a
Fundamental Interpersonal Contingencies. series of six simple scenarios, imagining
The comparisons just described constitute they had gone home for a high school
fundamental interpersonal contingencies, reunion and heard the scenario about an old
which yield eight primary social emotions, acquaintance “O” (Buck, Nakamura, Vieira,
including pride-arrogance when one is & Polonsky, 2005). The scenarios involved
doing comparatively well, guilt-shame positive or negative outcomes for O that
when one is not doing well, envy-jealousy were or were not deserved: for example,
when the other is doing well, and pity- O won the lottery, was hit by lightning,
scorn when the other is not doing well invented a new computer chip, and was
(Buck, 2004). More specifically, a person P jailed for selling drugs to children.
who succeeds experiences what could be Participants were asked to rate how they
labeled in English pride or arrogance; if P would feel about O and how O would feel
fails, P experiences guilt or shame; if com­ about them, using the list of the primary
parison person O succeeds, P tends to expe­ social emotions in English or Japanese as
rience envy or jealousy; if O fails, P tends to appropriate. The results relevant to the pre­
experience pity or scorn. These comparisons dictions about emotion dynamics are sum­
involve relative dominance, which, as we marized in Table 7.1. Briefly, across both
have seen, is a fundamental characteristic of nations, in all cases save one, the average
social structure and organization where one correlations were in the predicted direction
is in a position of relative strength and the and statistically significant. Thus, the predic­
other in a position of relative weakness. The tions about the dynamics of social emotions
difference between pride, shame, envy, and in Figure 7.1a were strongly supported in
pity on the one hand and arrogance, shame, both America and Japan.
07-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 4:17 PM Page 132

132–––◆–––Factors of Influence

PITY-SCORN

PRIDE- GUILT-
ARROGANCE SHAME

ENVY-
JEALOUSY
a.

CONTEMPT

Pity-Scorn

TRIUMPH HUMILIATION

Pride- Guilt-
Arrogance Shame

Envy-
Jealousy

b. RESENTMENT

Figure 7.1 (Continued)


07-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 4:17 PM Page 133

The Biological Foundations of Social Organization–––◆–––133

ADMIRATION-
SYMPATHY

TRUST AND TRUST AND


DIGNITY DIGNITY

ADMIRATION-
c. SYMPATHY

Figure 7.1 Hypothesized Dynamics of Social and Moral Emotions


NOTE: (A) Dynamics of eight primary social emotions. (B) Moral considerations can exacerbate conflict through
the perception of unfairness. (C) Moral considerations can facilitate cooperation through the perception of
fairness.

Table 7.1 Hypotheses About the Dynamics of Primary Social Emotions and Average
Correlations From America and Japan

America Japan

If one experiences the primary social emotion pride, it is


Likely that one would simultaneously experience arrogance .80*** .69***
Unlikely that one would experience guilt-shame −.40*** −.33**
Likely that one would experience pity-scorn toward others .25* .13
Unlikely that one would experience envy-jealousy toward others −.39*** −.41***

If one experiences the primary social emotion pride,


the comparison other is
Unlikely to simultaneously experience pride-arrogance −.55*** −.46***
Likely to experience guilt-shame .36*** .23**
Unlikely to experience pity-scorn −.28** −.31**
Likely to experience envy-jealousy .68*** .77***

NOTE: Pride is given as an example. Data represent the average correlations computed across all relevant
social emotions. English pronunciation of equivalent words in Japanese: Hokori, pride; goumannsa,
arrogance; zaiakukann, guilt; hazukashisa, shame; urayami, envy; sitto, jealousy; awaremi, pity; keibetsu,
scorn.
***p < .001. **p < .01. *p < .05.
07-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 4:17 PM Page 134

134–––◆–––Factors of Influence

This result supports the key proposition moral emotions. We propose that like social
that social emotions emerge as dynamic emotions, moral emotions involve issues of
systems from biologically based prosocial relative success or failure on the part of O:
attachment, in interaction with fundamental If we are winning and perceive that we
interpersonal contingencies involving social deserve it, we respond with feelings of tri­
comparison, over the course of individual umph; if we lose and deserve it, we experi­
development. Although the language used ence humiliation. If O wins and deserves it,
to label the social emotions is cultural, we experience admiration even as we may
attachment and the eight unions of interper­ simultaneously experience envy-jealousy.
sonal contingencies are fundamentally similar But if we believe that O does not deserve
in two nations with different values and lan­ success, our envy-jealousy is mixed with bit­
guages. The implication is that this emer­ ter resentment. If O loses and deserves it, we
gence is a pancultural phenomenon, common feel contempt, but if O’s loss is not deserved,
to all cultures and to all historical times. we feel sympathy. Morality is a double-
The social emotions are “on,” and the edged sword, with the capacity to amelio­
dynamic pattern of social emotions is rate potential conflict arising from inequities
present, in every human communicative or greatly exacerbate such conflict.
exchange. The interchange depicted in
Figure 7.1a is rather competitive, with the Moral Indignation. When unfairness is per­
proud-arrogant individual clearly express­ ceived, moral considerations can greatly
ing dominance to the partner and the exacerbate the conflict shown in Figure
partner responding with a downcast, sub­ 7.1a, evoking strikingly strong negative
missionlike display. Such feelings can be emotion: moral indignation, as illustrated
raised even in trivial encounters, as in in Figure 7.1b. P is triumphant and shows
passing on the street. Generalizing from scorn toward O, who responds with humil­
Ambady and Rosenthal (1992), we can be iation and resentment: a powerful, poiso­
exquisitely sensitive to thin slices suggesting nous combination fraught with potential
even a hint of pride-arrogance: It can set off conflict and aggression. Resentment can
alarm bells that one is being put down, lead to increased conflict that can become
snubbed, and disrespected. Perhaps such deadly, with murderous assaults on O being
signals—a smirk, a quick turning away of perceived to be morally justified and even
the eyes, a subtle raise of the chin—account gratifying, eliciting schadenfreude: a delight
for the ability of people in the Frank et al. in the discomfort and even agony of the
(1993) study to calculate that their partner other. Such perceptions can become socially
is likely to compete. Conversely, we quickly shared, particularly through encourage­
grasp weakness and submission in others— ment via media propaganda, and lead to
responding involuntarily with pity or scorn— long-term conflicts between peoples that
and we can directly perceive honest and can turn into war, ethnic cleansing, and
friendly trustworthiness. genocide.

Civil Exchange. On the other hand, if P


MORAL EMOTIONS and O perceive that each is following the
rules fairly, even though they may differ
When comparative success or failure is greatly in comparative success, the poten­
seen to be deserved or undeserved, it adds tial conflict shown in Figure 7.1 can be
another level of consideration that elicits ameliorated. If P and O perceive that each
07-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 4:17 PM Page 135

The Biological Foundations of Social Organization–––◆–––135

is following the rules fairly, then each can ♦ Summary and Conclusions
view the other with admiration and sym­
pathy. Moreover, they themselves feel a
sense of dignity and trust in the relation­ Signs of basic sociality—arousal-quiescence,
ship. Prosocial emotions can occur, such as approach-avoidance, dominance-submission,
gratitude and appreciation, when the other cooperation-competition, courting-sexual­
follows the rules and does the expected. ity—are rooted in the deepest and most
These and other positive moral feelings ancient regions and molecules of the brain.
constitute the emotional bases of civil Their influence is almost unnoticed—they
exchange (see Figure 7.1c). Even bitter ene­ function largely below the radar of
mies may engage in civil interaction, albeit consciousness—but it is profound and per­
grudgingly, if they can mutually acknowl­ vasive. These signs constitute the mecha­
edge that each is following the rules and nism by which social structure emerges and
acting with dignity. Seemingly trivial ritu­ organizes itself, whether in bacteria and
als of greeting, making brief eye contact, slime molds or in the rough-and-tumble
and saying “good day,” function to remind play of children. The mechanism of the
interactants that each is following the rules emergence is spontaneous nonverbal com­
and acting with dignity. It is possibly the munication—unconscious, intuitive, and
nonverbal expression of civility that under­ effortless—involving the direct perception of
lies the perception of mutual trustworthi­ and preprogrammed responses to displays.
ness that enabled participants in Frank et Spontaneous communication links us one to
al. (1993) to guess correctly that their another in a primordial biological dance.
partner would cooperate—that is, that We have seen that informal human
their partner was trustworthy (Boone & communication involves social comparisons
Buck, 2003). that evoke a dynamic system of social emo­
Moral emotions are based on the same tions including pride and arrogance, guilt
elements as social emotions: an affective and shame, envy and jealousy, and pity and
caring based biologically on attachment, scorn, and potentially also moral emotions,
interacting with fundamental interpersonal such as triumph, humiliation, resentment,
contingencies involving comparative suc­ and contempt. These potentially disruptive
cess and failure. But, with moral emotions emotions are often finessed by common and
there is the additional element of fairness virtually unconscious verbal and nonverbal
and justice, equity, or following the rules. rituals of politeness that establish the social
Moral emotions emerge together with lubricant of civility, evoking moral emotions
social emotions as dynamic systems, intu­ of admiration, sympathy, trust, respect, and
itively and effortlessly, during socioemo­ gratitude. They are leavened by other basic
tional development. They emerge, as Piaget signals, including subtle signs of attraction
(1971) suggested, from experiences in that hint at courtship. These signs may be
social interactions, particularly play. They used to deceive and manipulate, but the very
have animal analogies in the pack structure fact of deception implies their potential to
that emerges spontaneously from the inter­ reveal underlying truth.
actions of young wolves raised together by Human social behavior retains a founda­
Woolpy and Ginsburg (1967) and in the tion in primordial sociality as evidenced
need for peer interactions in young mon­ by emergent systems of social and moral
keys demonstrated in the work of Harlow emotions that naturally, effortlessly, and
(1971). largely unconsciously guide each and every
07-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 4:17 PM Page 136

136–––◆–––Factors of Influence

human communicative exchange, medi­ Buck, R., Nakamura, M., Vieira, E. T., Jr., &
ated by nuances of nonverbal behavior that, Polonsky, M. (2005, November). Dynamics
although subtle, can engender deep and pow­ of higher level social emotions: A cross-
erful emotions ranging from enraged humili­ national comparison of America and Japan.
Paper presented at the annual conference of
ation and resentment to delight and triumph,
the National Communication Association,
tender nurturance and protectiveness, pas­
Boston.
sionate desire, and compassionate love.
Buck, R., & VanLear, C. A. (2002). Verbal
and nonverbal communication: Distin­
guishing symbolic, spontaneous, and
♦ References pseudo-spontaneous nonverbal behavior.
Journal of Communication, 52, 522–541.
Burgdorf, J., & Panksepp, J. (2001). Tickling
Ambady, N., & Rosenthal, R. (1992). Thin induces reward in adolescent rats. Physiology
slices of behavior as predictors of inter­ & Behavior, 72, 167–173.
personal consequences: A meta-analysis. Carter, C. S., DeVries, A. C., & Getz, L. L.
Psychological Bulletin, 2, 256–274. (1995). Physiological substrates of mam­
Boone, R. T., & Buck, R. (2003). Emotional malian monogamy: The prairie vole model.
expressivity and trustworthiness: The role Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews,
of nonverbal behavior in the evolution 19, 303–314.
of cooperation. Journal of Nonverbal Carter, C. S., Lederhendler, I. I., & Kirkpatrick,
Behavior, 27, 163–182. B. (Eds.). (1997). The integrative neurobiol­
Buck, R. (1984). The communication of emo­ ogy of affiliation. Annals of the New York
tion. New York: Guilford Press. Academy of Sciences, 807, 260–272.
Buck, R. (1988). Human motivation and emo­ Cohn, D. F., & Schmidt, K. L. (2004). The tim­
tion (2nd ed.). New York: Wiley. ing of facial motion in posed and sponta­
Buck, R. (1999). The biological affects: A typol­ neous smiles. International Journal of
ogy. Psychological Review, 106, 301–336. Wavelets, Multiresolution, and Information
Buck, R. (2002). The genetics and biology of Processing, 2, 1–12.
true love: Prosocial biological affects and Darwin, C. (1998). The expression of the emo­
the left hemisphere. Psychological Review, tions in man and animals (3rd ed.). New
109, 739–744. York: Oxford University Press. (Original
Buck, R. (2004). The gratitude of exchange and work published 1872)
the gratitude of caring: A developmental­ Dawkins, R. (1982). The extended phenotype:
interactionist perspective of moral emotion. The gene as the unit of selection. Oxford,
In R. A. Emmons & M. McCullough (Eds.), UK: Oxford University Press.
The psychology of gratitude (pp. 100–122). Dawkins, R. (1989). The selfish gene: New edi­
New York: Oxford University Press. tion. New York: Oxford University Press.
Buck, R., & Duffy, R. (1980). Nonverbal com­ Dawkins, R., & Krebs, J. R. (1978). Animal
munication of affect in brain damaged signals: Information or manipulation?
patients. Cortex, 16, 351–362. In J. R. Krebs & N. B. Davies (Eds.), Behav­
Buck, R., & Ginsburg, B. (1991). Emotional com­ ioural ecology: An evolutionary approach
munication and altruism: The communi­ (pp. 282–309). Oxford, UK: Blackwell.
cative gene hypothesis. In M. Clark (Ed.), Frank, R. (1988). Passions within reason: The
Altruism: Review of personality and social strategic role of the emotions. New York:
psychology (Vol. 11, pp. 149–175). Newbury Norton.
Park, CA: Sage. Frank, R. (2001). Cooperation through emo­
Buck, R., & Ginsburg, B. (1997). Communicative tional commitment. In R. M. Hesse (Ed.),
genes and the evolution of empathy. Evolution and the capacity for commit­
In W. Ickes (Ed.), Empathic accuracy ment (pp. 57–76). New York: Russell Sage
(pp. 17–43). New York: Guilford Press. Foundation.
07-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 4:17 PM Page 137

The Biological Foundations of Social Organization–––◆–––137

Frank, R. H. (2004). In defense of sincerity conservation of a reproductive hormone?


detection. Rationality and Society, 16, Science, 218, 1324–1325.
287–305. Miller, R. E., Banks, J., & Ogawa, N. (1962).
Frank, R., Gilovich, T., & Regan, D. T. (1993). Communication of affect in “cooperative
The evolution of one-shot cooperation: conditioning” of rhesus monkeys. Journal
An experiment. Ethology and Sociobiology, of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 64,
14, 247–256. 343–348.
Gallio, M., Sturgill, G., Rather, P., & Kylsten, P. Miller, R. E., Banks, J., & Ogawa, N. (1963).
(2002). A conserved mechanism for extracel­ Role of facial expression in “cooperative­
lular signaling in eukaryotes and prokary­ avoidance conditioning” in monkeys. Journal
otes. Proceedings of the National Academy of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 67,
of Sciences, 99, 12208–12213. 24–30.
Gibson, J. J. (1966). The senses considered as Miller, R. E., Caul, W. F., & Mirsky, I. A.
perceptual systems. Boston: Houghton (1967). Communication of affects between
Mifflin. feral and socially isolated monkeys. Journal
Harlow, H. F. (1971). Learning to love. San of Personality and Social Psychology, 7,
Francisco: Albion. 231–239.
Hauser, M. D. (1996). The evolution of com­ Newell, P. C. (1981). Chemotaxis in the cellular
munication. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. slime moulds. In J. M. Lackie & P. C.
Hentzer, M., & Givskov, M. (2003). Pharma­ Wilkinson (Eds.), Biology of the chemotac­
cological inhibition of quorum sensing for tic response (pp. 89–114). Cambridge, UK:
the treatment of chronic bacterial infections. Cambridge University Press.
Journal of Clinical Investigations, 112, Panksepp, J. (1991). Brain opioids: A neuro­
1300–1307. chemical substrate for narcotic and social
Insel, T. R., Young, L., & Wang, Z. (1997). dependence. In S. Cooper (Ed.), Theory
Molecular aspects of monogamy. In in psychopharmacology (pp. 149–175).
C. S. Carter, I. I. Lederhendler, & London: Academic Press.
B. Kirkpatrick (Eds.), Annals of the New Piaget, J. (1971). Piaget’s theory. In P. Mussen
York Academy of Sciences: The integrative (Ed.). Handbook of child development
neurobiology of affiliation (Vol. 807, (Vol. 1, pp. 703–732). New York: Wiley.
pp. 302–316). New York: New York Rauh, C., Polonsky, M., & Buck, R. (2004,
Academy of Sciences. July). Cooperation on the first move:
Kenny, D. A. (1994). Interpersonal perception: A Emotional expressiveness and avatars in the
social relations analysis. New York: Guilford. Prisoners Dilemma Game. Paper presented
Kosfeld, M., Heinrichs, M., Zak, P. J., Fischbacher, at the meeting of the International Society
U., & Fehr, E. (2005). Oxytocin increases for Research on Emotions, New York.
trust in humans. Nature, 435, 673–676. Riedel, K., & Eberl, L. (2002). N-Acyl hom­
Krebs, J. R., & Dawkins, R. (1984). Animal oserinelactone-mediated cell-cell communi­
signals: Mind-reading and manipula­ cation in bacterial biofilms. Biomedical
tion. In J. R. Krebs & N. B. Davies (Eds.), Progress 15, 69–74.
Behavioural ecology: An evolutionary Ross, E. D. (1992). Lateralization of affective
approach (2nd ed., pp. 380–402). Sunderland, prosody in the brain. Neurology, 42
MA: Sinauer. (Suppl. 3), 411.
Lim, M. M., Wang, Z., Olazabal, D. E., Ren, X., Sabatelli, R. M., Buck, R., & Kenny, D. A.
Terwilliger, E. F., & Young, L. J. (2004). (1986). A social relations analysis of
Enhanced partner preference in a promiscu­ nonverbal communication accuracy in
ous species by manipulating the expression married couples. Journal of Personality, 54,
of a single gene. Nature, 429, 754–757. 513–527.
Loumaye, E., Thorner, J., & Catt, K. J. Sabatelli, R. M., & Rubin, M. (1986). Nonverbal
(1982). Yeast mating pheromone activates expressiveness and physical attractiveness
mammalian gonadotropins: Evolutionary as mediators of interpersonal perceptions.
07-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 4:17 PM Page 138

138–––◆–––Factors of Influence

Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 10, (Microtus ochrogaster). Journal of Endocrin­


120–133. ology, 6, 247–250.
Seyfarth, R. M., & Cheney, D. L. (2003). Wilson, E. O. (1975). Sociobiology: The new
Signalers and receivers in animal communi­ synthesis. Cambridge, MA: Belknap.
cation. Annual Review of Psychology, 54, Wood, R. M., Rilling, J. K., Sanfey, A. G.,
145–173. Bhagwagar, Z., & Rogers, R. D. (2005).
de Waal, F. B. M., & Tyack, P. L. (2003). Animal The effects of altering dietary L-tryptophan
social complexity: Intelligence, culture, and on the performance of an iterated Prisoner’s
individualized societies. Cambridge, MA: Dilemma (PD) game in healthy volunteers.
Harvard University Press. Behavioural Pharmacology, 16, S32.
Wang, Z., & Aragona, B. J. (2004). Neuro­ Woolpy, J. H., & Ginsburg, B. E. (1967). Wolf
chemical regulation of bonding in male socialization: A study of temperament in
prairie voles. Physiology and Behavior, 83, a wild social species. American Zoologist,
319–328. 7, 357–363.
Waters, C. M., & Bassler, B. L. (2005). Quorum Young, L. J., Lim, M. M., Gingrich, B., & Insel,
sensing: Cell-to-cell communication in T. R. (2001). Cellular mechanisms of social
bacteria. Annual Review of Cell and attachment. Hormones and Behavior, 40,
Developmental Biology, 21, 319–346. 133–138.
Williams, J. R., Insel, T. R., Harbaugh, C. R., & Zahavi, A. (1977). The costs of honesty (further
Carter, C. S. (1994). Oxytocin admin­ remarks on the handicap principal). Journal
istered centrally facilitates formation of of Theoretical Biology, 67, 603–605.
a partner preference in prairie voles
08-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 7:26 PM Page 139

8
AN EVOLUTIONARY APPROACH
TO UNDERSTANDING
NONVERBAL COMMUNICATION

� Kory Floyd
Arizona State University

T here are several excellent theories in nonverbal communication


and new models to develop to meet emerging theoretic needs. This
essay delineates how Darwin’s (1859) theory of evolution by means of
natural selection (TNS) can add to the theoretic sophistication in this
area by explaining multiple aspects of nonverbal communication and
behavior in human relationships. I have divided this chapter into three
general sections, the first of which provides an introduction to evolu­
tion, in general, and TNS and evolutionary psychology, in particular.
Such an introduction, although protracted, is essential to an apprecia­
tion of the evolutionary approach. The second section applies the prin­
ciples of TNS and evolutionary psychology to the understanding of
nonverbal communication and gives specific examples of how TNS can
be used to study topics of importance to nonverbal communication
researchers, including emotion and attraction. Finally, I discuss the effi­
cacy of TNS as a guide for nonverbal communication scholars and offer
brief suggestions for researchers wishing to incorporate the theory into
their own work.

◆ 139
08-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 7:26 PM Page 140

140–––◆–––Factors of Influence

♦ Evolution, Natural Selection, third principle, heritability, some of this varia­


and Evolutionary Psychology tion is inherited, or passed down genetically
from parents to their biological children.1 For
example, two parents with brown hair will
Evolution itself is a remarkably simple con­ tend to have brown-haired offspring, because
cept: It refers to change over time in the char­ hair color is heritable.2
acteristics of living organisms. Researchers The fourth (and, at the time it was
generally address two types of evolution. proposed, the most innovative) principle
The first is ontogeny, which references in Darwin’s theory is selection. Heritable
change in an individual person, animal, or characteristics that advantage an organism
plant over time (this term is synonymous with respect to survival and procreation
with development). The physical growth and will be passed to future generations with a
cognitive maturation observed when an greater frequency than characteristics that
infant grows into an adult is an example of do not provide these advantages. That is,
ontogeny. The second is phylogeny, which genetic traits that prove advantageous to an
relates to change in the characteristics of a organism (because they help meet an envi­
group or species over time. For example, an ronmental challenge) are selected for, or
observed change in the average length of the retained from one generation to the next,
giraffe’s neck or in the hue of the moth’s whereas characteristics that do not prove
wings over the course of many generations advantageous are selected against, or not
would be an example of phylogeny. It is retained. For example, a long neck is
this type of change, specifically, that most advantageous to giraffes because it gives
researchers refer to when using the word them access to food, which is necessary for
evolution (see, e.g., Buss, 1999). survival. Giraffes with the longest necks,
It is simple enough, in many cases, to doc­ therefore, have access to more food than
ument that phylogenetic changes occur (albeit other giraffes, and thus, are more likely to
slowly, often over long spans of time). What survive to sexual maturity. Because they are
eluded scientists for centuries, however, was more likely to survive to sexual maturity,
an explanation for how the process of evolu­ they are more likely to procreate, and
tion works. Darwin (1859) offered such an because neck length is heritable, succeed­
explanation in his theory of natural selection ing generations of giraffes will have longer
(a theory that was simultaneously, and average necks than did previous genera­
independently, proposed by Alfred Russel tions. In the parlance of TNS, an environ­
Wallace, 1858). The theory espouses four mental challenge (access to food) caused a
main principles: superfecundity, variation, heritable trait (neck length) to be advanta­
heritability, and selection. The first principle, geous in terms of survival and procreation.
superfecundity, means that in any given gen­ This example illustrates the principle of
eration, many more members of a species are survival of the fittest (a phrase coined by
born than can possibly survive and reproduce, Herbert Spencer): Those organisms best
creating what Darwin referred to as a “strug­ adapted to the demands of their environ­
gle for existence.” The second principle, ment are the most likely to survive and
variation, indicates that all members of a reproduce themselves because the advanta­
species have different combinations of traits. geous heritable characteristics are passed on
Humans, for instance, vary one from another to their progeny at a greater frequency than
in any number of traits, including height, disadvantageous ones. The importance of
body shape, hair and eye color, bone density, the environment in this principle is clear,
sensory ability, and weight. According to the because traits that would prove adaptive
08-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 7:26 PM Page 141

An Evolutionary Approach to Understanding Nonverbal Communication–––◆–––141

in one environment may be maladaptive in on Darwin’s (1859) explanation of evolu­


another. (For example, the physical proper­ tion. First, humans are no less subject to
ties that make many water mammals fast evolution than are any other living organ­
swimmers make them slow movers on land, isms. If a particular attribute advantages
causing these properties to be adaptive for humans with respect to their survival and
evading predators in the water but mal­ procreation abilities, evolutionary psychol­
adaptive for evading predators on land.) ogists would expect that attribute to be
Inherent in the theory of natural selection “selected for” in humans just as it would in
is the premise that all organisms, if they are any other organism. The second principle is
to have evolutionary success, must attend to that psychological characteristics are no less
their needs for survival and procreation con­ subject to the process of evolution than
tinually. Procreation, of course, is impossible physical characteristics are. If, for example,
barring survival to sexual maturity. Mere sur­ intelligence, empathy, emotional control, or
vival is not enough to succeed in the evolu­ a tendency toward jealousy are characteris­
tionary process, however, because without tics that improve a person’s ability to sur­
procreation one’s genetic materials are not vive and/or to procreate, then evolutionary
contributed to future generations. Impor­ psychologists would contend that these
tantly, it need not be one’s own procreation characteristics, to the extent that they are
that contributes one’s genetic materials to at all heritable, can be “selected for” just as
subsequent generations; such a task can be physical stature, strength, or a long neck
accomplished by anyone who also carries can be. Consequently, evolutionists exam­
one’s genes, such as a sibling, cousin, or niece. ine the ways in which mental, emotional, or
According to inclusive fitness theory, any­ psychological attributes, and the behaviors
thing one does that furthers the reproductive that characterize them, might have been
success of these relatives also furthers one’s advantageous to our ancestors with respect
own reproductive success, by a factor equal to survival and procreation.
to the degree of genetic relatedness with that Following this brief overview, the pri­
relative (which is higher for siblings than for mary purpose of this chapter is to delineate
nieces, and higher for nieces than for cousins; some of the ways in which TNS and the
see Hamilton, 1964; Trivers, 1971). principles of evolutionary psychology can
Recognition of these premises has turned be used to model, predict, and explain
the attention of scholars in a number of dis­ nonverbal communication. Before engaging
ciplines toward the question of how partic­ specific examples, however, it is necessary
ular characteristics of organisms contribute to consider some important principles
to survival and procreation. Specifically, about the application of TNS to human
researchers began in the mid-20th century behavior. These concern the nature of
to explore how psychological mechanisms evolutionary adaptations and are explained
might have evolved to meet environmental subsequently.
challenges in much the same way that
physical mechanisms have. The possibility
giving rise to this work was that human IMPORTANT PRINCIPLES OF THE
psychological experiences, such as sexual EVOLUTIONARY APPROACH
attraction, love, or jealousy, are evolved
adaptations in the same way that physical Adaptations deal with proximal and
features such as opposable thumbs are. ultimate levels of causation. The question
The discipline of evolutionary psychol­ of why a particular behavior occurs can be
ogy brings two important principles to bear answered on at least two levels of abstraction.
08-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 7:26 PM Page 142

142–––◆–––Factors of Influence

A proximal cause is the condition or set of fell in love with each other, couldn’t imagine
conditions that appears to facilitate the their lives without each other, and wanted to
behavior in the specific time, place, and spend the rest of their lives together. Few are
manner in which it occurred. In response likely to say that they got married in order to
to the question, “Why did you eat dinner pass their genetic materials on to future gen­
tonight?” one might answer by identifying erations. The theory of natural selection
a proximal cause: “Because I felt hungry.” posits that this is, indeed, the ultimate cause
By contrast, an ultimate cause is the condi­ of pair bonding, irrespective of people’s con­
tion or set of conditions that represents the scious awareness. That is, the evolutionary
original or higher-order cause of a behavior approach does not require individuals to be
(and often dictates the connection between aware of the ultimate causes of their behav­
the behavior and its proximal causes). iors in order for those ultimate causes to be
Thus, an ultimate answer to the question, operative.
“Why did you eat dinner tonight?” might
be, “Because I must eat in order to survive, Adaptations need not be adaptive for
and so I have evolved the sensation of modern living. When people think about
hunger as a way of motivating me to eat on their modern environments, it can be diffi­
a regular basis.” This example illustrates cult to understand how particular adap­
how a given nonverbal behavior (e.g., eat­ tations are beneficial. Why, for instance, do
ing) can be considered to be caused both by humans have a preference for sweet, fatty,
a proximal agent (hunger) and by an ulti­ and salty foods? How could such a prefer­
mate agent (human need for nutrients). ence possibly be adaptive, when overindul­
Although both are valid causes, an evolu­ gence in these types of foods can lead to
tionary approach is concerned primarily obesity, high cholesterol, heart disease, and
with identifying ultimate causes that even death? Evolutionary theories do not
provide some clue as to the environmen­ try to explain human adaptations with ref­
tal challenges that adaptations evolved erence to modern living. Rather, such theo­
to meet. In many cases, however, there is an ries focus on physical and psychological
unmistakable link between ultimate and traits that would have been adaptive in the
proximal causes. societies of our ancestors. Evolution usually
operates slowly, and modern civilization is
Adaptations need not operate at a conscious remarkably young when considered on an
level. When asked to account for their own evolutionary timescale. Agriculture was
behaviors, people commonly identify proxi­ invented only about 10,000 years ago, and
mal causes with little or no regard for ulti­ civilization (let alone, modern civilization)
mate, higher-order causes. This is often is an even more recent phenomenon.
because they are simply unaware of what the Humankind has spent more than 99% of
ultimate causes might be or how they might its history living in hunter-gatherer soci­
be operating through more proximal causes eties, and as Morris (2001) pointed out, it is
(Kenrick & Simpson, 1997). Evolutionary not likely that natural selection has made
theories acknowledge that many ultimate any noticeable modifications in the human
causes operate outside of individuals’ con­ brain in the short period of time repre­
scious awareness of them, and they contend sented by modernity. As a result, some
that this is not problematic. For example, if traits that were adaptive for our hunter-
people are asked to explain why they got gatherer ancestors may be useless or even
married, they will likely say it is because they maladaptive in modern times.
08-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 7:26 PM Page 143

An Evolutionary Approach to Understanding Nonverbal Communication–––◆–––143

Adaptations need not be adaptive for every members, and all group members are
person or in every instance. It can be diffi­ equally cared for. In such a situation,
cult for an individual to see how particular anything that benefits the group as a whole
traits might be adaptive if, for whatever also benefits each member individually,
reason, those traits do not produce the because of the communal sharing of
adaptive outcomes for that individual. The resources. Suppose, however, that a group
human sex drive can provide a useful member were to find a large sum of cash
example. The fact that sex is physically plea­ that the other group members did not
surable for (most) humans can be consid­ know about. Would it benefit this member
ered to be adaptive in the sense that it to contribute the money to the group? Yes,
motivates humans to engage in intercourse, because an economic benefit to the group
which is necessary for reproduction of the would benefit all members. It would bene­
species.3 To be an adaptation, a trait must fit this individual more to keep the money
prove advantageous in solving an evolution­ and not to disclose its existence to the
ary challenge (in this case, the challenge of group, however, because this member
procreation). This does not mean, however, would then be over-benefited in relation to
that the trait must produce its adaptive his or her fellow members.4 In a grave
result for every person. The sex drive is financial crisis, this member might even be
adaptive because the challenge of reproduc­ able to survive, whereas others in the
tion is met more effectively with it than group—or the group as a whole—perished.
without it. Moreover, an adaptation need Adaptations work in much the same way,
not produce its adaptive result in every to advantage the individual rather than
instance. Of course, very few instances of any group to which he or she belongs.
sexual intercourse result in pregnancy, rela­ Therefore, when it comes to situations in
tive to the number of times humans engage which an individual’s priorities conflict
in intercourse overall. Again, however, this with a group’s, evolved adaptations tend to
does not mean that the sex drive is not adap­ privilege the success of the individual over
tive (in fact, it would be quite maladaptive if that of the group.
every instance of intercourse did result in
pregnancy). Adaptations need only provide Summary. These principles illustrate the
advantages relative to their alternatives. application of TNS and related theories to
various aspects of human behavior. Indeed,
Adaptations operate at the individual level, the breadth of behaviors evolutionary theo­
not the group or species level. Humans ries can explain is certainly one of their
belong to a number of important groups, strengths (see Floyd & Haynes, 2005).
including their families, social networks, Whereas many otherwise excellent theories
and professional networks that, in various used commonly in nonverbal communi­
ways, help ensure their survival. In many cation research are limited to explaining
cases, therefore, what is beneficial to the specific behaviors or tendencies, such
group is beneficial to the individual as expectancy violations (Burgoon & Hale,
member, and vice versa. Often, however, 1988), adaptation (Andersen, 1985), or
individual and group priorities are in con­ arousal (Cappella & Greene, 1982), TNS
flict. Consider, for example, a communal and evolutionary psychology can be applied
living situation in which each person’s to multiple aspects of nonverbal behavior.
money and possessions are considered to In the second section of this chapter, I will
be the collective property of all group discuss the application of evolutionary
08-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 7:26 PM Page 144

144–––◆–––Factors of Influence

principles to the study of nonverbal EMOTION DISPLAYS


communication, specifically, and discuss
two examples in detail. Emotion, and its related constructs and
terms, is a topic that has drawn consider­
able interest from nonverbal communica­
♦ Applications of TNS to tion scholars and students (e.g., Andersen
the Understanding of & Guerrero, 1998; see also Fridlund &
Nonverbal Communication Russell, this volume). Evolutionary psy­
chologists, too, have directed attention at
explicating the evolutionary bases for both
TNS provides a rich explanatory basis the experience and the expression of the
for understanding human social behavior, emotions, and their applications have pro­
including nonverbal communication. At vided fertile ground for understanding how
the heart of any application of TNS to a emotions, and the behaviors through which
characteristic or behavior is the question we communicate them, can be adaptive.
of how that characteristic or behavioral It is important to preface this discussion
tendency ultimately serves the purposes by distinguishing clearly between the expe­
of viability or fertility for the individual. rience of an emotion and the expression of
Importantly, not all characteristics or it, the former being an internal affective
behaviors that contribute to viability or state, and the latter encompassing specific
fertility are necessarily evolutionary adap­ behaviors through which such a state is rep­
tations (which evolve specifically in reac­ resented. This distinction is important for
tion to environmental challenges to two reasons. First, the experience and the
viability or fertility). They may nonetheless expression of an emotion may be adaptive
be evolutionarily adaptive through their in different ways or to differing degrees. For
contributions to these superordinate goals. instance, the experience of grief may serve
(A complete discussion of the distinction to make maladaptive situations (e.g., the
between adaptations and evolutionary by- loss of one’s livelihood) aversive, whereas
products is beyond the scope of this chap­ the outward manifestation of grief may sig­
ter; for a more in-depth discussion, see nal distress and elicit needed support and
Tooby & Cosmides, 1990a, 1990b.) protection from others in one’s social
In this section, I offer two examples of network. Second, the experience-expression
how TNS and principles of evolutionary distinction is important because emotional
psychology can be applied to topics of experiences and emotional expressions
interest to nonverbal communication schol­ are, at least, partially independent of one
ars. Because an application of the theory to another. That is, people can experience
all aspects of nonverbal communication is emotions without expressing them (e.g.,
impossible for one chapter, I selected as concealing one’s anger), and they can also
examples two topics that receive a good express emotions without experiencing
deal of empirical attention from nonverbal them (e.g., conveying affection that is not
researchers: emotion displays and attrac­ genuine) (see Guerrero, Andersen, & Trost,
tion. For each, I offer examples of how 1998; Ploog, 1986).
TNS and evolutionary psychology can Much has been written on the evolution­
explain important aspects of the behavior, ary functions of emotional experiences
and then discuss empirical support relevant (e.g., see Buss, 1994a, 1994b; Cannon,
to such explanations. 1929; Frank, 1988; Nesse, 1990). Often of
08-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 7:26 PM Page 145

An Evolutionary Approach to Understanding Nonverbal Communication–––◆–––145

greater interest to nonverbal communica­ Darwin’s third principle is the principle


tion scholars, however, are the behaviors by of actions due to the constitution of the ner­
which emotional experiences are conveyed vous system, which acknowledged that
to others. Seminal work on the evolution­ emotional expression is often the direct
ary function of emotional expression was result of nervous system arousal. Being
conducted by Darwin (1872), whose book, frightened, for instance, can induce sympa­
The Expression of the Emotions in Man thetic arousal that leads directly to certain
and Animals, espoused three general princi­ components of the facial fear expression:
ples regarding the evolution of emotion dilated pupils, an open mouth, increased
displays. muscular tension, and a pronounced
First, the principle of serviceable asso­ increase in perspiration. Darwin excluded
ciated habits acknowledged that people from this third category explicitly those
perform certain physical actions to meet expressions that were due to the force of
specific needs or desires (e.g., plugging habit or its antithesis, distinguishing this
one’s nose in the presence of a foul odor principle from the former two.
to prevent the odor from entering the nasal These three principles are foundational
passages). Whenever people experience to the evolutionary approach to emotional
states of mind similar to those associated expression, which calls for the examination
with those needs or desires, they will per­ of (a) how emotion displays contribute to
form the same actions, even though they human viability and fertility and (b) how
may serve no functional purpose. For they are connected to their associated emo­
example, when presented with a particu­ tional experiences. As an example of the
larly bad idea, one might plug one’s nose former point, the components of the fear
to express contempt because one’s state of expression mentioned above might all be
mind is similar to that experienced in the said to serve particular functions with
presence of an offensive odor; thus, one respect to survival. Pupil dilation allows for
plugs one’s nose out of habit or association. the increased intake of visual information
Such a behavior is functional in the situa­ that may be important in ascertaining
tion that initially provoked it (i.e., it pro­ the nature of the threat that provoked the
tects one from the offensive odor), but in fear response. An open mouth allows for
associated situations it is merely symbolic increased oxygen intake, supplying the
and serves to convey the message that “this body with increased fuel for fighting, or
idea stinks.” fleeing from, the threat. Similarly, increased
The second principle, the principle of muscular tension also readies the body to
antithesis, suggested that when one experi­ fight or flee; additionally, it can serve to
ences a state of mind opposing one that is insulate the body from immediate injury.
associated with a particular habit, one will Increased perspiration is evoked to main­
perform an opposite action. For instance, tain thermoregulation in the face of chang­
when presented with a particularly pleasant ing blood flow to the muscles and skin
idea, one might smile and take in a deep tissues. Thus, from the vantage of evolu­
breath to convey contentment with the idea; tionary psychology, these common compo­
because plugging one’s nose is associated nents of facial fear displays are elicited by
with an opposing state of mind (i.e., that the adaptive actions of the nervous system
summoned in the presence of a bad idea), to invoke increased vigilance and increased
the opposite state of mind elicits the oppo­ energy for the purpose of dealing with the
site behavior. threat that initially evoked the fear.
08-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 7:26 PM Page 146

146–––◆–––Factors of Influence

This is just one example of how the logic primary emotions there are, and what they
behind TNS and evolutionary psychology are (Cornelius, 1996; Ekman, 1972; Izard,
could be used to link an emotion display 1977; Tomkins, 1962, 1963). Included on
to viability, fertility, or both. Of course, nearly every list of primary emotions, how­
increases in vigilance and energy in the face ever, are fear, sadness, surprise, anger, and
of fear may seem to have a direct relation­ happiness or joy. Second, nonverbal com­
ship with survival ability if the threat evok­ munication scholars have debated the
ing the fear is physical (e.g., being chased by extent to which these forms of evidence
an animal, being stalked by a criminal), but provide logical support for Darwin’s propos­
their effect on survival may seem to be more itions (e.g., Ekman, 1992; Russell, 1994).
dubious if the threat is psychological (e.g., Some, such as Fridlund (1994) and Fridlund
facing impending exams or a visit to the and Russell (this volume), have offered
dentist). This may well be the case. To the alternative theoretic accounts. Although
extent that dealing successfully even with an analysis of these competing viewpoints
psychological stressors can contribute to is beyond the scope of this chapter, it
viability, however, even minutely or indi­ is important to note that scholars have
rectly (e.g., conquering one’s fear enough to not always agreed about the merits of
do well in an exam, which helps secure a the Darwinian perspective or the extent to
good grade, which contributes to the attain­ which the existing evidence supports it.
ment of gainful employment and access to
resources), the mechanisms that foster such Cross-Cultural Consistency in Emotion
abilities can be “selected for” through the Displays. As Ekman (1973) explained,
process of natural selection. Because nat­ Darwin’s (1872) proposition that emotion
ural selection operates on an exceedingly displays are innate suggests that they are
slow time scale (e.g., producing changes not the product of cultural socialization
over millennia), characteristics that confer and should, therefore, show more con­
even seemingly inconsequential advantages sistency than variability from culture to cul­
with respect to viability or fertility, and that ture.5 An impressive body of research has
are at least partially heritable, are likely to accumulated that addresses this hypothe­
be selected for. sis with respect to facial emotion displays
With respect to the link between emo­ (arguably the most communicative of all
tion displays and their underlying emo­ nonverbal emotion displays; Knapp, 1978).
tional experiences, Darwin (1872) argued Some early investigations conducted by
that displays of the primary emotions, at researchers attempting to identify cultural
least, had evolved as a product of evolu­ variation in facial emotion displays, instead
tionary pressures and were, therefore, as provided evidence of beyond-chance consis­
innate as the emotional experiences them­ tency in how people from different cultures
selves (Ekman, 1973). Several forms of interpret them. For example, Triandis and
evidence can be examined as sources of Lambert (1958) showed photos of an actress
support for Darwin’s (1872) proposition. expressing different emotions to American
In this section, I identify three such forms college students, Greek college students,
of evidence. Two preliminary observations and Greek villagers from the Island of
are warranted, the first of which is that Corfu; their comparisons indicated that
Darwin’s proposition referred to the pri­ “Greek subjects, even when they come from
mary or “chief” emotions, and contem­ very different populations, rate emotional
porary scholars disagree on how many expressions in the same way as American
08-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 7:26 PM Page 147

An Evolutionary Approach to Understanding Nonverbal Communication–––◆–––147

college students” (p. 323). Other investiga­ are relatively uninhibited in their expressions
tions have also found substantial cultural of emotions such as happiness, sadness, or
consistency, in cultures ranging from fear, whereas older individuals have a
American, Mexican, Japanese, Chinese, and greater capacity to control their emotion
Turkish (e.g., Cüceloglu, 1970; Dickey & displays in the service of politeness or other
Knower, 1941; Vinacke, 1949; Vinacke & social norms. If Darwin’s (1872) proposi­
Fong, 1955; Matsumoto, this volume). tion regarding the innateness of emotion dis­
Although these early studies provided plays is accurate, however, one should find
preliminary evidence for cross-cultural consistency in the forms of emotion display
consistency in facial displays of emotion, from infancy onward through the life cycle.
their contributions were constrained by To address this question, Darwin began
a number of methodological limitations. chronicling expressive behavior in his own
Work by a range of scholars has remedied child, and his observations were offered in
many of these shortcomings and demon­ his 1872 book and in a later essay titled
strated cross-cultural consistency in facial A Biographical Sketch of an Infant (1877).
displays of emotion across both literate His observations, although methodologi­
and preliterate cultures (e.g., Eibl- cally compromised, indicated the presence
Eibesfeldt, 1972; Ekman, 1968; Ekman of at least seven distinctive facial emotion
& Friesen, 1971; Ekman, Friesen, & displays beginning as early as 45 days.
Ellsworth, 1972; Ekman, Sorenson, & Systematic research on the ontology of
Friesen, 1969; Izard, 1971). Their collective emotion displays was relatively scarce in
finding is one of consistency across a range the century following Darwin’s publica­
of cultures with respect to how emotions tions, but several more contemporary stud­
are conveyed (at least, facially). Although ies have addressed the enactment of facial
some studies have questioned the degree of emotion displays during infancy. Research
cross-cultural consistency (e.g., Elfenbein & on smiling, for instance, has indicated that
Ambady, 2002), and others have ques­ externally elicited smiles begin to appear
tioned its very existence (e.g., Fernández- around the end of the first month (Sroufe,
Dols & Ruis-Belda, 1995), much of the 1984), and that by the age of 12 weeks,
evidence supports the hypothesis of cross- infants have begun to smile selectively to
cultural consistency. newly mastered activities and familiar
persons (Lewis, Sullivan, & Brooks-Gunn,
Consistency in Emotion Displays of Infants 1985). Fox and Davidson (1988) also
and Noninfants. If the means of conveying found that 10-month-olds produced more
emotions are learned principally through Duchenne smiles (accompanied by contrac­
socialization and enculturation, then it fol­ tion of the lateral orbicularis oculi) with
lows that newborn infants, being possessed their mothers than with strangers and that
neither of the cognitive capacity nor the only Duchenne smiles were associated
experience to be functionally affected by with left frontal EEG activity (hypothesized
enculturation messages, should display their to be an approach-related pattern). As
emotions differently than children, adoles­ Charlesworth and Kreutzer (1973) noted,
cents, and adults, the latter groups having there is little evidence of a one-to-one rela­
been subjected to the influences of socializa­ tionship between smiling and a specific
tion. Of course, differences between infants class of external stimuli; rather, infant smil­
and noninfants are evident in terms of the ing can be elicited via a variety of auditory,
ability to control emotion displays; infants visual, tactile, and kinesthetic stimuli. Most
08-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 7:26 PM Page 148

148–––◆–––Factors of Influence

relevant to Darwin’s proposition is the age hypothesis, which posits that merely engag­
at which facial emotion displays begin to be ing in a facial display of emotion will initi­
observed in infants; the earlier the displays, ate physiological changes consistent with
the less likely they are products of social­ that emotion (e.g., smiling makes one feel
ization or enculturation. happier).
Several emotion displays, in addition In an experimental test of the facial feed­
to smiling, have been studied with infants, back hypothesis, Ekman, Levenson, and
including displays of interest (Sullivan & Friesen (1983) instructed scientists and pro­
Lewis, 1989), surprise (Camras, 1988), fessional actors to enact facial displays of
disgust (Rosenstein & Oster, 1988), fear six emotions while their autonomic nervous
(Schwartz, Izard, & Ansul, 1982), and system arousal was measured. Importantly,
anger (Stenberg, Campos, & Emde, 1983). participants were told how to configure their
Also instructive is research on congenitally faces physically in particular ways. Rather
blind children, whose condition eliminates than being instructed to “display anger,” for
their ability to imitate visually observed instance, participants were asked to pull
expressive behavior in others. Early work their eyebrows down and together, press
by Freedman (1965) indicated that congen­ their lips together, raise their upper eyelids,
itally blind infants smiled at expected times etc. Each expression was held for 10 sec­
and (by 6 months of age) for expected dura­ onds. Facial displays of anger, fear, sadness,
tions, leading Freedman to conclude that happiness, and surprise were accompanied
smiling is an innate, rather than learned, by increases in heart rate, whereas displays
behavior. Later work by Eibl-Eibesfeldt of disgust elicited a small decrease in heart
(1970) and Charlesworth (1970) further rate. Displays of anger, sadness, and happi­
supported the parallels in expressions of ness also elicited increases in skin tempera­
blind and sighted infants, adding credence ture, whereas displays of fear, surprise, and
to Darwin’s (1872) assertion regarding the disgust were accompanied by decreased skin
evolution of emotion displays. temperature.
These physiological responses to display­
Physiological Correlates of Emotion ing emotion closely resemble the correlat­
Display. A third form of evidence buttress­ es of experiencing emotion (see Levenson,
ing Darwin’s proposition derives from 1992). Hess, Kappas, McHugo, Lanzetta,
research showing that engaging in facial and Kleck (1992) also found that displaying
displays of emotion elicits physiological anger and happiness increased participants’
changes that mimic those associated with heart rates, whereas displaying peaceful­
the experience of those emotions. As ness decreased it. Later experiments by
Levenson (1992) detailed, evidence from Levenson, Carstensen, Friesen, and Ekman
several laboratories has indicated distinc­ (1991) and Levenson, Ekman, and Friesen
tions between emotional experiences with (1990) replicated the findings of Ekman et
respect to their effects on autonomic ner­ al. (1983). Ekman (1989) also replicated
vous system arousal. If Darwin’s conjecture those results with a non-American culture,
regarding the evolution of emotion displays the Minangkabau of Sumatra, Indonesia (a
is correct, one would also expect to find fundamentalist Muslim, matrilineal society).
that emotion displays correspond to physi­ The findings that emotion displays are
ological profiles similar to those character­ judged consistently across cultures, begin to
izing their associated emotions. This is appear early in life, and are associated with
the principle behind the facial feedback specific physiological reactions indicative of
08-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 7:26 PM Page 149

An Evolutionary Approach to Understanding Nonverbal Communication–––◆–––149

the underlying emotions do not unequivo­ physical attractiveness with a high degree of
cally prove the validity of the evolutionary consistency. If beauty really were in the eye
explanation, but these forms of evidence of the beholder (a notion that dates back at
make it more difficult to argue that emotion least to the third century, BC; Rubenstein,
displays are merely social or cultural con­ Langlois, & Roggman, 2002), then evaluat­
structions. A second active area of nonver­ ing attractiveness as a signal of reproduc­
bal communication research in which tive potential would not be a reliable
evolutionary principles can be applied is that strategy. The second assumption is that the
of interpersonal attraction. links between markers of attractiveness and
reproductive potential are real, not socially
or culturally contrived. Just as the evolu­
ATTRACTION tionary perspective focuses attention on
how facial displays of emotion might serve
It is difficult to overstate the importance the evolutionary end of survival (as in the
of attraction in human social interaction. example of the fear expression detailed
Although a number of qualities may lead above), it also focuses on the ways in which
one person to be attracted to another as a markers of physical attractiveness are hon­
friend or potential romantic partner (e.g., est indicators of fitness, genetic quality,
personality, similarity, propinquity), a great health, fertility, and reproductive value. As
many markers of physical attractiveness are above, I will offer in this section several
nonverbal, having to do with personal phys­ forms of evidence that support and are
ical appearance (see Andersen, Guerrero, illustrative of the evolutionary perspective
& Jones, this volume). Thus, for nonverbal on attractiveness.
communication researchers, these compo­
nents of attractiveness are of clear relevance. Intra- and Intercultural Consistency in
From an evolutionary perspective, phys­ Judgments of Attractiveness. The contention
ical attraction is important for one funda­ that “beauty is in the eye of the beholder”
mental reason: It is often the instigating suggests both within-culture and, especially,
force behind the development of reproduc­ between-culture variation in what people
tive relationships. Without attraction to find physically attractive in others. Such
motivate initial interaction, many poten­ variation does exist, particularly between
tially significant relationships might simply cultures; however, research on human
be forgone. The evolutionary approach to attraction has also identified a number of
understanding attractiveness therefore aspects of physical attraction that are
focuses on what aspects of physical appear­ remarkably consistent within and across cul­
ance are judged to be attractive, and how tures. In a series of studies, for example,
attraction to these aspects of physical Cunningham, Roberts, Wu, Barbee, and
appearance is consequential with respect to Druen (1995) asked people of different races
reproductive success. This perspective pro­ to judge the attractiveness of Caucasian,
poses that humans (and many other species) Asian, Hispanic, and Black women and
are adaptively attuned to markers of physi­ found a correlation of .93 between racial
cal attractiveness as signals of fertility and groups in their attractiveness ratings. In their
reproductive potential. second study, the researchers found that
The causal links proffered by the evolu­ Taiwanese participants agreed strongly with
tionary approach rest on at least two impor­ the ratings of the other groups (r = .91) and
tant assumptions. First, people evaluate that the degree of exposure to Western
08-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 7:26 PM Page 150

150–––◆–––Factors of Influence

media did not moderate this effect. In their development, genetic problems such as
third study, Cunningham et al. found that recessive genes and homozygosity, and
Blacks and Caucasians agreed with each environmental stressors such as parasites,
other strongly on attractiveness ratings pollutants, or extreme temperatures, cause
(r = .94). Other studies have documented the individual to exhibit fluctuating asym­
consensus in ratings of attractiveness between metry (FA), or deviations from exact sym­
North Americans and South Africans, among metry on bodily features that tend to be
Chinese, Indians, and the English, between symmetrical (Møller & Pomiankowski,
Black and Caucasian Americans, and between 1993). FA is assessed by taking precise mea­
Americans, Russians, and Ache Indians (see sures of physical features such as the length
Cross & Cross, 1971; Jackson, 1992; Jones, of the ear, the breadth of the elbow, or the
1996; Morse, Gruzen, & Reis, 1976; width of the feet on one side of the body
Thakerar & Iwawaki, 1979). and comparing them to the same measures
In a meta-analysis of the attractiveness taken from the opposite side. Larger dis­
literature, Langlois et al. (2000) examined crepancies indicate higher FA. Only the
919 effect sizes from a sample of over 1,800 most genetically fit individuals can main­
empirical studies focusing on the assess­ tain symmetry under conditions of develop­
ment of physical attractiveness. Their prin­ mental stress; low FA therefore serves as a
cipal findings all supported the notions of marker of genetic quality (Møller, 1997).
intra- and intercultural consistency in how A number of studies verify direct rela­
people rate others’ attractiveness. Specifically, tionships between symmetry and percep­
Langlois et al. found within-culture agree­ tions of attractiveness. Grammer and
ments of .90 for ratings of adults’ attrac­ Thornhill (1994), for example, measured
tiveness and .85 for ratings of children’s the symmetry of adults’ faces and found
attractiveness. Cross-ethnic agreement was strong linear relationships between symme­
.88 and cross-cultural agreement was .94. try and perceptions of the attractiveness
These findings contradict the “eye of the of those faces for both men and women.
beholder” hypothesis and support the Similarly, Hume and Montgomerie (2001)
contention that many signals of physical examined the symmetry-attractiveness rela­
attractiveness are judged consistently both tionship by manipulating the amount of
within and between cultures. symmetry in facial photographs. They
found the same direct relationship between
Relationship of Attractiveness Markers to symmetry and attractiveness ratings (see
Reproductive Success. The evolutionary also Perrett et al., 1999).
explanation assumes not only that people Importantly, symmetry has also demon­
evaluate attractiveness with relative consis­ strated direct effects on reproductive suc­
tency but also that markers of attractiveness cess. Thornhill and Gangestad (1994)
have real connections with reproductive found, for instance, that symmetry was lin­
success. In this section, I examine two phys­ early related to the number of sexual part­
ical characteristics that (1) are associated ners that male and female college students
strongly and consistently with attractive­ reporting having had. This effect held after
ness and (2) have direct relationships with controlling for the confounding effect of
reproductive success. A first predictor of age (see also Gangestad & Thornhill, 1997).
facial and bodily attractiveness is symmetry, These results suggest that more symmetri­
or the extent to which two sides of a body cal people have more sexual opportunities
mirror each other. During an individual’s than less symmetrical people. Moreover,
08-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 7:26 PM Page 151

An Evolutionary Approach to Understanding Nonverbal Communication–––◆–––151

Thornhill, Gangestad, and Comer (1995) exclusively during pregnancy and lactation
found a direct relationship between the man’s (Björntorp, 1987) and because a WHR of
symmetry and his partner’s likelihood of .70 or below corresponds to the absence of
achieving orgasm. This is significant for major obesity-related disorders, such as dia­
reproductive success because female orgasm betes, carcinomas, or heart disease (Barbieri,
increases the proportion of her partner’s 1990). Indeed, research indicates that higher
sperm that is retained in her reproductive WHRs are associated with women’s diffi­
tract, boosting the chances of pregnancy culties in becoming pregnant (Kaye, Folsom,
(see Baxter & Bellis, 1993). Importantly, Prineas, Potter, & Gapstur, 1990). In a
no other variables measured in the study study of 500 women attending a fertility
(including the man’s height, income, and clinic, for instance, Zaadstra, Seidell, van
sexual experience, and the couple’s ratings Noord, te Velde, Habbema, Vrieswijk, and
of their mutual love) predicted the woman’s Karbaat (1993) reported that women who
likelihood of achieving orgasm. had a WHR under .80 were more than twice
A second example, relevant for women, is as likely to get pregnant following 12
their waist-to-hip ratio (WHR), or the ratio rounds of artificial insemination as were
of waist width to hip width. Across cultures, women with ratios exceeding .80.
and even across time periods in Western cul­ Symmetry and WHR are but two
tures, a female WHR of approximately .70 examples of physical characteristics that
has been seen as maximally attractive (i.e., show both strong, reliable associations with
when the waist is about 70% as wide as attractiveness and direct relationships with
the hips; see Singh & Young, 1995). For reproductive success (others may include
example, Singh (1993) found that every sin­ physical stature, skin clarity, and natural
gle Miss America winner from 1923 to 1987 body odor). From the evolutionary perspec­
had a WHR between .69 and .72 and that tive, therefore, one would contend that
the WHRs of Playboy centerfolds also these characteristics are attractive to people
ranged only from .68 to .71. Singh and Luis because attraction to these characteristics is
(1995) demonstrated that the preference for reproductively beneficial. I have argued
a .70 WHR was consistent across cultures. elsewhere (Floyd & Haynes, 2005) that
As with symmetry, WHR also has direct one of the great strengths of the evolution­
effects on reproductive success, and research ary approach is its ability to explain multi­
indicates that a WHR of .70 is the WHR ple aspects of behavior and to do so at a
of maximum female fertility. After puberty, level of explanation that often transcends
estrogens lead to the accumulation of body the social, cultural, or political influ­
fat in the gluteofemoral region (thighs and ences that are often the focus of social
buttocks), whereas androgens (such as science research. Such a perspective allows
testosterone) promote fat accumulation researchers the breadth to investigate how
in the abdominal region. WHR therefore human communicative behaviors are
serves as a reliable index of the distribution related to pervasive, engrained motivations
of fat between the upper and lower body that are often independent of the effects of
and also of the relative proportions of gender, race, class, sexual orientation, or a
intra-abdominal and extra-abdominal fat host of other proximal influences. In this
(Deprés, Prudhomme, Pouloit, Tremblay, manner, human communicative behavior
& Bouchard, 1991). can be understood within a broad context
This is significant for fertility, both that is relatively unbound by time, locale,
because gluteofemoral fat is used nearly or policy, and is often even unrestricted to
08-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 7:26 PM Page 152

152–––◆–––Factors of Influence

humans. The evolutionary perspective may be the same. For example, Floyd and
allows one to understand how nonverbal Morman (2001) compared biological fathers
communicative behaviors fit within a larger and stepfathers in terms of the amount of
and broader picture than many other non­ affection they communicated to their sons.
verbal theories point to. The hypothesis that fathers give more affec­
tion to biological sons than to stepsons can
easily be derived on the basis of inclusive
♦ Using TNS in Nonverbal fitness theory. One might arrive at the same
prediction, however, by reasoning that men
Communication Research
feel closer to their biological sons than to
stepsons because they have longer histories
To conclude this chapter, I offer three recom­ with biological sons.
mendations for researchers wishing to capital­ To test an evolutionary hypothesis fairly,
ize on the advantages of evolutionary theories the researcher must design the test in such a
in their study of nonverbal behavior. The first way that one explanation can be ruled out. If
is to conceptualize research questions in terms it is a difference in closeness that causes
of ultimate causality. That is, researchers fathers to be more affectionate with biologi­
should consider the relationship that specific cal sons than stepsons, for instance, then the
communicative behaviors might have either to difference in affectionate communication
survival or procreation (or both). The links should fail to manifest (or be diminished) if
for some behaviors might be fairly obvious; the level of closeness is covaried out. In fact,
one can certainly identify how a behavior like Floyd and Morman (2001) found that the
flirting is related (eventually) to procreation, mean difference in affectionate communica­
or how the fight-or-flight response to fear is tion was maintained even when closeness
related to survival. The links for other behav­ and other relational characteristics were con­
iors may be less apparent but are there trolled for. Certainly, ruling out one rival
nonetheless (e.g., the reproductive benefits of hypothesis does not provide unequivocal evi­
being attracted to symmetrical sexual part­ dence for another, but it strengthens one’s
ners). Researchers could apply evolutionary claim. By crafting research designs carefully
principles to the study of such diverse behav­ to rule out rival causes, researchers can test
iors as immediacy, deception, touch avoid­ explanations derived from evolutionary the­
ance, and cosmetic use to identify their ories while simultaneously ruling out expla­
implications for survival and procreation. nations offered by other perspectives.
Conceptualizing research questions in terms Finally, researchers must consider con­
of ultimate causality requires researchers to text so as to avoid oversimplified hypothe­
consider the survival and procreative purposes ses. Although the evolutionary paradigm
that a given behavior might serve. focuses on ultimate, rather than proximal,
Equally important is that researchers causes, the characteristics evolution selects
formulate hypothesis tests to rule out for in individuals are often manifested only
rival explanations. This is a fundamental in particular contexts. Some may be more
principle of the scientific design, but it is genetically prone than others to reciprocate
especially important for researchers study­ nonverbal aggression, for instance, but this
ing communication from an evolutionary doesn’t necessarily mean that such people
perspective because one can often deduce have higher mean levels of aggressive­
the same prediction using evolutionary ness than others, because an aggressive
and nonevolutionary theories. Although context is required to bring the tendency
the explanations differ, the basic prediction to fruition. Careful consideration of the
08-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 7:26 PM Page 153

An Evolutionary Approach to Understanding Nonverbal Communication–––◆–––153

environment and social context will help integrations of nonverbal behavior (pp. 1–36).
scholars avoid oversimplified hypotheses. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Researchers must bear in mind that evolu­ Andersen, P. A., & Guerrero, L. K. (Eds.) (1998).
tionary adaptiveness matters, in terms of Handbook of communication and emotion:
Research, theory, applications, and contexts.
predicting behavior, but it is not the only
San Diego, CA: Academic Press.
thing that matters. Contextual influences,
Barbieri, R. L. (1990). The role of adipose tissue
such as the history of a relationship or the
and hyperinsulinemia in the develop­
constraints of a social context must also be ment of hyperandrogenism in women. In
considered in order to avoid the fallacy R. E. Frisch (Ed.), Adipose tissue and reproduc­
that evolutionary pressures are the only tion (pp. 42–57). Basel, Switzerland: Karger.
pressures that exert influence on interper­ Baxter, R. R., & Bellis, M. A. (1993). Human
sonal behavior. sperm competition: Ejaculate manipulation
by females and a function for the female
orgasm. Animal Behavior, 46, 887–909.
♦ Notes Bjorklund, D. F., & Pellegrini, A. D. (2002). The
origins of human nature: Evolutionary
developmental psychology. Washington,
1. This is true, at least, for sexually reproduc­ DC: American Psychological Association.
ing species; see Bjorklund and Pellegrini (2002). Björntorp, P. (1987). Fat cell distribution and
2. Not all characteristics are inherited; if a metabolism. In R. J. Wurtman & J. J.
woman loses her hearing because of an injury, Wurtman (Eds.), Human obesity (pp. 66–72).
this will not make her more likely to have babies New York: New York Academy of Sciences.
who are deaf. Burgoon, J. K., & Hale, J. L. (1988).
3. Of course, advances in reproductive tech­ Nonverbal expectancy violations: Model
nology, such as in vitro fertilization, have elimi­ elaboration and application to immediacy
nated intercourse as an absolute precursor to behaviors. Communication Monographs,
reproduction. Such technology was not available 55, 58–79.
to our hunter-gatherer ancestors, for whom the Buss, D. M. (1994a). The evolution of desire:
adaptive nature of the human sex drive would Strategies of human mating. New York:
have been selected. Basic Books.
4. Such a benefit does require that the others Buss, D. M. (1994b). The strategies of human
in the group be unaware of this member’s good mating. American Scientist, 82, 238–249.
fortune. As Cosmides and Tooby (1989, 1992) Buss, D. M. (1999). Evolutionary psychology:
have suggested, humans have evolved a type of The new science of the mind. Boston: Allyn
cheating detection mechanism by which they & Bacon.
notice, remember, and respond to perceived Camras, L. A. (1988, May). Darwin revisited: An
cheating on the part of others, so as to protect infant’s first emotional facial expressions.
their self-interests. Paper presented at International Conference
5. For brevity’s sake, I will use the term on Infant Studies, Washington, DC.
emotion displays in this section to refer to expres­ Cannon, W. (1929). Bodily changes in pain,
sions of primary emotions, including fear, sad­ hunger, fear, and rage: Research into the
ness, surprise, anger, and happiness. function of emotional excitement. New
York: Harper & Row.
Cappella, J. N., & Greene, J. O. (1982). A dis­
♦ References crepancy-arousal explanation of mutual
influence in expressive behavior for adult and
infant-adult interaction. Communication
Andersen, P. A. (1985). Nonverbal immediacy Monographs, 49, 89–114.
in interpersonal communication. In A. W. Charlesworth, W. R. (1970). Surprise reactions
Siegman & S. Feldstein (Eds.), Multichannel in congenitally blind and sighted children.
08-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 7:26 PM Page 154

154–––◆–––Factors of Influence

Bethesda, MD: National Institute of Mental emotions. American Journal of Sociology,


Health Progress Report. 47, 190–193.
Charlesworth, W. R., & Kreutzer, M. A. Eibl-Eibesfeldt, I. (1970). Ethology: The biology
(1973). Facial expressions of infants and of behavior. New York: Holt.
children. In P. Ekman (Ed.), Darwin and Eibl-Eibesfeldt, I. (1972). Similarities and
facial expression: A century of research in differences between cultures in expressive
review (pp. 91–168). New York: Academic movements. In R. A. Hinde (Ed.), Nonverbal
Press. communication (pp. 297–314). Cambridge,
Cornelius, R. R. (1996). The science of emotion: UK: Cambridge University Press.
Research and tradition in the psychology of Ekman, P. (1968, September). The recognition
emotion. Upper Saddle River, NJ: Prentice and display of facial behavior in literature
Hall. and non-literate cultures. Paper presented
Cosmides, L. L., & Tooby, J. (1989). at symposium on the universality of
Evolutionary psychology and the genera­ emotions of the American Psychological
tion of culture. Part II. Case study: A com­ Association, San Francisco, CA.
putational theory of social exchange. Ekman, P. (1972). Universal and cultural differ­
Ethology and Sociobiology, 10, 51–97. ences in facial expressions of emotion. In
Cosmides, L. L., & Tooby, J. (1992). Cognitive J. K. Cole (Ed.), Nebraska symposium
adaptations for social exchange. In on motivation (pp. 207–283). Lincoln, NE:
J. Barkow, L. Cosmides, & J. Tooby (Eds.), University of Nebraska Press.
The adapted mind (pp. 163–228). New Ekman, P. (1973). Cross cultural studies of
York: Oxford University Press. facial expression. In P. Ekman (Ed.),
Cross, J. F., & Cross, J. (1971). Age, sex, race, Darwin and facial expression: A century
and the perception of facial beauty. of research in review (pp. 169–222). New
Developmental Psychology, 5, 433–439. York: Academic Press.
Cüceloglu, D. M. (1970). Perception of facial Ekman, P. (1989). A cross cultural study
expressions in three cultures. Ergonomics, of emotional expression, language and
13, 93–100. physiology. Symposium conducted at the
Cunningham, M. R., Roberts, A. R., Wu, C-H., annual meeting of the American Association
Barbee, A. P., & Druen, P. B. (1995). “Their for the Advancement of Science, San
ideas of beauty are, on the whole, the same Francisco.
as ours”: Consistency and variability in the Ekman, P. (1992). Are there basic emotions?
cross-cultural perception of female attrac­ Psychological Review, 99, 550–553.
tiveness. Journal of Personality and Social Ekman, P., & Friesen, W. V. (1971). Constants
Psychology, 68, 261–279. across cultures in the face and emotion.
Darwin, C. (1859). On the origin of species. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
London: Murray. 17, 124–129.
Darwin, C. (1872). The expression of the emo­ Ekman, P., Friesen, W. V., & Ellsworth, P.
tions in man and animals. London: Murray. (1972). Emotion in the human face. New
Darwin, C. (1877). A biographical sketch of an York: Pergamon.
infant. Mind, 2, 285–294. Ekman, P., Levenson, R. W., & Friesen, W. V.
Deprés, J. P., Prudhomme, D., Pouloit, M., (1983). Autonomic nervous system activity
Tremblay, A., & Bouchard, C. (1991). distinguishes among emotions. Science,
Estimation of deep abdominal adipose- 221, 1208–1210.
tissue accumulation from simple anthropo­ Ekman, P., Sorenson, E. R., & Friesen, W. V.
metric measurements in men. American (1969). Pan-cultural elements in facial dis­
Journal of Clinical Nutrition, 54, plays of emotion. Science, 164, 86–88.
471–477. Elfenbein, H. A., & Ambady, N. (2002). On the
Dickey, E. C., & Knower, F. H. (1941). A note universality and cultural specificity of emotion
on some of the ethnological differences in recognition: A meta-analysis. Psychological
recognition of simulated expressions of the Bulletin, 128, 203–235.
08-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 7:26 PM Page 155

An Evolutionary Approach to Understanding Nonverbal Communication–––◆–––155

Fernández-Dols, J. M., & Ruiz-Belda, M. A. effect of facial expression on the self-


(1995). Are smiles a sign of happiness? generation of emotion. International Journal
Gold medal winners at the Olympic Games. of Psychophysiology, 12, 251–265.
Journal of Personality and Social Psy­ Hume, D. K., & Montgomerie, R. (2001). Facial
chology, 69, 1113–1119. attractiveness signals different aspects of
Floyd, K., & Haynes, M. T. (2005). “quality” in women and men. Evolution
Applications of the theory of natural and Human Behavior, 22, 93–112.
selection to the study of family communica­ Izard, C. E. (1971). The face of emotion. New
tion. Journal of Family Communication, 5, York: Appleton.
79–101. Izard, C. E. (1977). Human emotions. New
Floyd, K., & Morman, M. T. (2001). Human affec­ York: Plenum.
tion exchange: III. Discriminative parental Jackson, L. A. (1992). Physical appearance and
solicitude in men’s affectionate communication gender: Sociobiological and sociocultural
with their biological and nonbiological sons. perspectives. Albany: State University of
Communication Quarterly, 49, 310–327. New York Press.
Fox, N., & Davidson, R. (1988). Patterns of Jones, D. (1996). Physical attractiveness and
brain electrical activity during facial signs the theory of sexual selection. Ann Arbor:
of emotion in 10 month old infants. University of Michigan Press.
Developmental Psychology, 24, 230–236. Kaye, S. A., Folsom, A. R., Prineas, R. J., Potter,
Frank, R. (1988). Passion within reason: The J. D., & Gapstur, S. M. (1990). The associa­
strategic role of the emotions. New York: tion of body fat distribution with lifestyle and
Norton. reproductive factors in a population study of
Freedman, D. G. (1965). Hereditary control postmenopausal women. International
of early social behavior. In B. Foss (Ed.), Journal of Obesity, 14, 583–591.
Determinants of infant behavior (pp. 149– Kenrick, D. T., & Simpson, J. A. (1997). Why
156). London: Methuen. social psychology and evolutionary psychol­
Fridlund, A. J. (1994). Human facial expression: ogy need one another. In J. A. Simpson & D.
An evolutionary view. San Diego, CA: T. Kenrick (Eds.), Evolutionary social psy­
Academic Press. chology (pp. 1–20). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Gangestad, S. W., & Thornhill, R. (1997). Knapp, M. L. (1978). Nonverbal communica­
Human sexual selection and developmental tion in human interaction (2nd ed.). New
stability. In J. A. Simpson & D. T. Kenrick York: Holt.
(Eds.), Evolutionary social psychology Langlois, J. H., Kalakanis, L., Rubenstein, A. J.,
(pp. 169–195). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. Larson, A., Hallam, M., & Smoot, M.
Grammer, K., & Thornhill, R. (1994). Human (2000). Maxims or myths of beauty?
(Homo sapiens) facial attractiveness A meta-analytic and theoretical review.
and sexual selection: The role of symmetry Psychological Bulletin, 126, 390–423.
and averageness. Journal of Comparative Levenson, R. W. (1992). Autonomic nervous
Psychology, 108, 233–242. system differences among emotions. Psy­
Guerrero, L. K., Andersen, P. A., & Trost, M. R. chological Science, 3, 23–27.
(1998). Communication and emotion: Basic Levenson, R. W., Carstensen, L. L., Friesen, W.
concepts and approaches. In P. A. Andersen V., & Ekman, P. (1991). Emotion, physiol­
& L. K. Guerrero (Eds.), The handbook of ogy, and expression in old age. Psychology
communication and emotion: Research, and Aging, 6, 28–35.
theory, applications, and contexts (pp. 3–27). Levenson, R. W., Ekman, P., & Friesen, W. V.
San Diego, CA: Academic Press. (1990). Voluntary facial action generates
Hamilton, W. D. (1964). The genetical evolution emotion-specific autonomic nervous system
of social behavior. I & II. Journal of activity. Psychophysiology, 27, 363–384.
Theoretical Biology, 7, 1–52. Lewis, M., Sullivan, M., & Brooks-Gunn, J.
Hess, U., Kappas, A., McHugo, G. J., Lanzetta, (1985). Emotional behavior during the
J. T., & Kleck, R. E. (1992). The facilitative learning of a contingency in early infancy.
08-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 7:26 PM Page 156

156–––◆–––Factors of Influence

British Journal of Developmental Psycho­ Singh, D., & Luis, S. (1995). Ethnic and gender
logy, 3, 307–316. consensus for the role of waist-to-hip ratio on
Møller, A. P. (1997). Developmental stability judgment of women’s attractiveness. Human
and fitness: A review. American Naturalist, Nature, 6, 51–65.
149, 916–942. Singh, D., & Young, R. K. (1995). Body weight,
Møller, A. P., & Pomiankowski, A. (1993). waist-to-hip ratio, breasts, and hips: Role
Fluctuating asymmetry and sexual selec­ in judgments of female attractiveness and
tion. Genetica, 89, 267–279. desirability for relationships. Ethology and
Morris, R. (2001). The evolutionists: The strug­ Sociobiology, 16, 483–507.
gle for Darwin’s soul. New York: W. H. Sroufe, L. A. (1984). The organization of emo­
Freeman. tional development. In K. R. Scherer &
Morse, S. T., Gruzen, J., & Reis, H. (1976). The P. Ekman (Eds.), Approaches to emotion
“eye of the beholder”: A neglected variable (pp. 109–128). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
in the study of physical attractiveness. Stenberg, C., Campos, J., & Emde, R. (1983).
Journal of Personality, 44, 209–225. The facial expression of anger in seven
Nesse, R. (1990). Evolutionary explanations of month old infants. Child Development, 54,
emotions. Human Nature, 1, 261–289. 178–184.
Perrett, D. I., Burt, D. M., Penton-Voak, I. S., Sullivan, M., & Lewis, M. (1989). Emotion and
Lee, K. J., Rowland, D. A., & Edwards, R. cognition in infancy: Facial expressions
(1999). Symmetry and human facial attrac­ during contingency learning. International
tiveness. Evolution and Human Behavior, Journal of Behavioral Development, 12,
20, 295–307. 221–237.
Ploog, D. (1986). Biological foundations of Thakerar, J. N., & Iwawaki, S. (1979). Cross-
the vocal expressions of emotions. In cultural comparisons in interpersonal
R. Plutchik & H. Kellerman (Eds.), attraction of females toward males. Journal
Emotion: Theory, research, and experience of Social Psychology, 108, 121–122.
(Vol. 3, pp. 173–197). New York: Thornhill, R., & Gangestad, S. W. (1994).
Academic Press. Human fluctuating asymmetry and
Rosenstein, D., & Oster, H. (1988). Differential sexual behavior. Psychological Science, 5,
facial response to four basic tastes in new­ 297–302.
borns. Child Development, 59, 1555–1568. Thornhill, R., Gangestad, S. W., & Comer, R.
Rubenstein, A. J., Langlois, J. H., & Roggman, (1995). Human female orgasm and mate
L. A. (2002). What makes a face attractive fluctuating asymmetry. Animal Behavior,
and why: The role of averageness in defin­ 50, 1601–1615.
ing facial beauty. In G. Rhodes & L. A. Tomkins, S. S. (1962). Affect, imagery, con­
Zebrowitz (Eds.), Facial attractiveness: sciousness: Vol. 1. The positive affects.
Evolutionary, cognitive, and social perspec­ New York: Springer.
tives (pp. 1–33). Westport, CT: Ablex. Tomkins, S. S. (1963). Affect, imagery, con­
Russell, J. A. (1994). Is there universal recogni­ sciousness: Vol. 2. The negative affects.
tion of emotion from facial expression? New York: Springer.
Psychological Bulletin, 115, 102–141. Tooby, J., & Cosmides, L. (1990a). On the
Schwartz, G., Izard, C. E., & Ansul, S. (1982, universality of human nature and the uni­
May). Heart rate and facial response to queness of the individual: The role of genet­
novelty in 7- and 13-month-old infants. ics and adaptation. Journal of Personality,
Paper presented at International Conference 58, 17–68.
on Infant Studies, Austin, TX. Tooby, J., & Cosmides, L. (1990b). The past
Singh, D. (1993). Adaptive significance of explains the present: Emotional adapta­
waist-to-hip ratio and female physical tions and the structure of ancestral envi­
attractiveness. Journal of Personality and ronments. Ethology and Sociobiology, 11,
Social Psychology, 65, 293–307. 375–424.
08-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 7:26 PM Page 157

An Evolutionary Approach to Understanding Nonverbal Communication–––◆–––157

Triandis, H. C., & Lambert, W. W. (1958). national-race groups in Hawaii: II. Oriental
A restatement and test of Schlosberg’s faces. Journal of Social Psychology, 41,
theory of emotion with two kinds of 185–195.
subjects from Greece. Journal of Abnormal Wallace, A. R. (1858). On the tendency of
and Social Psychology, 56, 321–328. varieties to depart indefinitely from the
Trivers, R. L. (1971). The evolution of recipro­ original type. Journal of the Proceedings
cal altruism. Quarterly Review of Biology, of the Linnean Society (Zoology), 3,
46, 35–37. 53–62.
Vinacke, W. E. (1949). The judgment of facial Zaadstra, B. M., Seidell, J. C., van Noord, P. A.
expressions by three national-race groups H., te Velde, E. G., Habbema, J. D. F.,
in Hawaii: I. Caucasian faces. Journal Vrieswijk, B., et al. (1993). Fat and female
of Personality, 17, 407–429. fecundity: Prospective study of effect of
Vinacke, W. E., & Fong, R. W. (1955). The body fat distribution on conception rates.
judgment of facial expressions by three British Medical Journal, 306, 484–487.
08-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 7:26 PM Page 158
09-Manusov.qxd 7/10/2006 3:23 PM Page 159

9
PERSONALITY AND
NONVERBAL BEHAVIOR
A Complex Conundrum

� Robert Gifford
University of Victoria

A Boldly Sketched Portrait of Albrecht Dürer.

“Whoever examines this countenance


cannot but perceive in it the traits of forti­
tude, deep penetration, determined perse­
verance, and inventive genius. At least every
one will acknowledge the truth of these
observations, when made.”

SOURCE: J. C. Lavater (ca. 1844), Essays on


Physiognomy, translated by Thomas Holcroft,
4th ed., London (publisher unknown), pp. 33–35,
image and caption from Plate 1. (Retrieved
August 9, 2005, from www.newcastle.edu.au/school/fine-art/publications/
lavater; the Dürer image and the quotation from Lavater about Dürer were

Author’s Note: Correspondence should be addressed to Robert Gifford,


Department of Psychology, University of Victoria, Victoria, British Columbia,
Canada, V8W 3P5; e-mail: rgifford@uvic.ca.

◆ 159
09-Manusov.qxd 7/10/2006 3:23 PM Page 160

160–––◆–––Factors of Influence

used, with permission, from the above Web site for future research that incorporates the
of Ross Woodrow.) complexities I detailed, and discuss an
exemplar study that reflects this paradigm.
It is possible to infer character I begin, however, with a brief history of
from features, if it is granted that scholarship connecting nonverbal cues
the body and the soul are changed to personality.
together by the natural affections.

Aristotle, Prior Analytics, 350 BCE


♦ A Short History
Has he not a rogue’s face? Speak
brother, you understand physiog­ The idea that personal qualities are encoded
nomy, a hanging look to me. in human physical features already was at
William Congreve, least 100 years old when Aristotle wrote
Love for Love, 1695 Prior Analytics about 2300 years ago. For
centuries, physiognomy was assumed to be
obvious and true. Until the time of Henry

F rom Aristotle’s time, physiognomists


have been certain they can discern per­
sonality solely from a person’s facial fea­
VIII, according to one Web site (www
.answers.com/topic/physiognomy, retrieved
August 9, 2005), its validity was assumed
tures. The confidence expressed by famous so widely that it was taught in many uni­
physiognomists like the Swiss theologian versities. Writers such as William Congreve
Johann Caspar Lavater turned to respectable apparently accepted physiognomy as fact in
scientific optimism in the 1930s. But by the 16th and 17th centuries, as the quota­
the 1980s, researchers were generally pes­ tion above suggests. The 18th and 19th
simistic regarding the contention that per­ centuries were dominated by the works of
sonality is encoded clearly in nonverbal Lavater, whose pronouncements about the
behavior or that personality could be psychological meaning of facial differences
decoded through nonverbal behavior. The were extremely popular. His books went
pessimism was justifiable, given the mixed through about 150 editions (see www.new­
and unsatisfactory research results in castle.edu.au/school/fine-art/publications/
the area. In this chapter, I argue that what lavater). Nineteenth-century writers such as
appears to be a simple proposition—that Balzac, Hardy, and Dickens used physiog­
nonverbal behavior and personality have nomic character descriptions frequently in
simple or direct connections—actually is a their novels.
very difficult problem with at least 10 types The premise of physiognomy, that per­
of complexity for researchers to manage. sonality is encoded in the face, has been dis­
Nevertheless, some progress has been made credited for decades, in part because static
in the last two decades, based mainly on facial features represent only a small part of
adaptations of Brunswik’s (1956) lens a person’s nonverbal impression on others.
model. To show this development, and fol­ This discrediting has not prevented contem­
lowing discussion of relevant terms, I offer porary pop psychology writers (e.g., Young,
a larger description of these “10 complexi­ 1993) from making claims about what
ties.” I then review research assessing people can discern about one another.
the connections between personality and From Leonardo da Vinci’s portrait of
nonverbal behavior, propose a paradigm Mona Lisa, for example, Young divined
09-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 7:49 PM Page 161

Personality and Nonverbal Behavior–––◆–––161

that she was a liar, stubborn, a gifted Nevertheless, the Allport-Vernon book
abstract thinker with an IQ above average, did not stimulate much new published
greedy, an unreliable friend, and a sneak; research over the next three decades; only a
that she would punish her enemies by any few scattered studies may be found from
means; and that she needed at least 9 hours the 1930s until the early 1960s. Perhaps the
of sleep each night! Unlike a painting, how­ first modern study that would fit comfort­
ever, human faces are dynamic, and they ably in this chapter was Ralph Exline’s
convey different dispositional impressions (1963) investigation of visual interaction in
depending on the person’s state. For groups of men and groups of women who
example, when different emotions are had been categorized in terms of their need
experienced by an actor, different disposi­ for affiliation. He found that need for affil­
tions are inferred by observers (Montepare iation was related to mutual glances, but it
& Dobish, 2003). The first important worked differently for men and women.
scientific study relevant to the present Exline’s study has the further distinction of
chapter was summarized in Allport and recognizing that nonverbal behavior should
Vernon’s (1933) groundbreaking mono­ be examined within interacting groups,
graph. Allport and Vernon did not attempt rather than assuming implicitly that people
to relate nonverbal behavior to personality express themselves nonverbally without
dispositions. Rather, they sought to find reference to others—that is, always in the
unity (or something close to it) among the same way (see the physiognomists).
expressive movements of their subjects. In the four decades since the mid-1960s,
Their hypothesis appears to have been a number of scientific studies (although per­
rooted in the Aristotelian proposition, haps fewer than one might expect) have
quoted at the start of the chapter, that one’s been conducted on the relations between
whole body and personality are a kind of nonverbal behavior and personality, but
unity in which every aspect is mirrored in progress has been slow and fitful. Twenty
every other aspect. This view was champi­ years after Exline’s (1963) study, reviewers
oned by German psychologists such as were forced to conclude that the expression
William Stern, who influenced Allport and of personality in nonverbal behavior “can­
Vernon profoundly. The purpose of the not be said to be strongly supported by the
1933 monograph was to demonstrate the evidence” (Bull, 1983, p. 113) and that “in
existence of consistency among a person’s general, much of the research on personal­
expressive movements, which would, in the ity correlates has shown . . . relatively
Aristotle-Stern sense, support the very con­ weak relationships to nonverbal behavior”
struct and existence of personality. When (Heslin & Patterson, 1982, p. 131).
Allport and Vernon were writing, personal­ Recognizing the complexity of nonver­
ity was not a widely respected construct in bal behavior, however, Patterson (1995,
psychology, and it was therefore in need of this volume) developed his parallel process
empirical support. Allport and Vernon’s model, which posits that nonverbal behav­
results showed promise, in that two clusters ior is a balance of behavior and social cog­
of expressive movements, one “general” nitive judgment processes, often automatic
and one “specific,” were found, albeit with or overlearned, but sometimes under con­
lower than desirable reliability. Their book trol and in the service of a particular goal.
was the basis for some optimism that per­ Plausible as it is, the theory also is difficult
sonality and nonverbal behavior could be to test empirically, as Patterson (this
studied profitably. volume) attests. This chapter, then, while
09-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 7:49 PM Page 162

162–––◆–––Factors of Influence

acknowledging the value of the parallel Although this problem is beyond the scope of
process model, focuses on the more modest, this chapter, suffice it to say there are prob­
but more easily researched, social judgment lems with self-report measures as well as
aspect of communication. As will be seen, problems with ratings by significant others
there are plenty of complexities even within (see Funder, 2003; Kenny, 1994). The second
this limited portion of the whole. main problem involving accuracy is this:
Thus, at the beginning of the 21st How well do decoders detect the (true) level
century, considerable research remains to of a person’s dispositions from the others’
be done before the complex connections nonverbal behavior? In the typical study,
among personality, nonverbal behavior, and decoding accuracy is measured as the dis­
inferences about personality made by crepancy between the decoder’s assessment
observers of others’ nonverbal behavior can of the disposition and the actor’s self-rating
be understood. This chapter summarizes (or the ratings of the actor’s significant
recent work and offers a paradigm that others). Given the uncertainty of self-ratings,
might accelerate progress in the social judg­ and even those of significant others (Kenny,
ment portion of the problem. The paradigm 1994), these assessments should not be
might be considered a subset of the ambi­ granted the status of truth or treated as the
tious framework proposed by Patterson criterion against which observers’ ratings are
(1995) for understanding nonverbal com­ measured. Nevertheless, they certainly have
munication in general. some face validity. They are, after all, the
views of the actors by the actors or by those
who know them well.
♦ Key Distinctions: If the premise that even these assessments
are fallible is accepted, it becomes unrea­
Encoding, Decoding,
sonable to tarnish any lack of agreement
Accuracy, and Agreement
with these assessments on the part of other
observers as a lack of achievement or error.
Encoding is the outward, objective, visi­ The observer’s assessment has its own
ble manifestation of personal dispositions inherent value as the view of another
in nonverbal behavior. The fundamental person after watching the actor’s nonverbal
hypothesis of researchers in this area is that behavior, with the advantage of some per­
valid encoding does occur. A crucial, and spective, detachment, and often, objectivity.
still largely unanswered, question remains, Thus, the two assessments of the disposi­
however. How much encoding occurs and tion should be granted equal ontological
for which dispositions? Decoding is the use status, and it is preferable to characterize
by observers of nonverbal behavior to infer any difference between the two assessments
personal dispositions in others. The inter­ as a discrepancy rather than as observer
esting question in this area concerns the error. In sum, neither the self’s nor the
accuracy of that decoding by different kinds observer’s rating is valid necessarily, and
of observers, for different dispositions, under the neutral term agreement should be used
different conditions. The wild card in this rather than accuracy.
endeavor, however, is accuracy. How valid The decoding of personality is itself
are our inferences of personality from non­ fraught with problems. Assessments of a
verbal behavior? person’s dispositions vary with the type of
Accuracy, in this sense, is itself problem­ information given to the judge. For example,
atic in two important ways. The first issue when judges in an interview study either
concerns the validity of personality measures. saw a silent videotape of an interview with
09-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 7:49 PM Page 163

Personality and Nonverbal Behavior–––◆–––163

a manager (i.e., only the nonverbal behav­ that does not guarantee decoding validity
ior) or read a transcript of the same inter­ (Kenny, 1991), as exemplified by (inaccu­
view (thus, no nonverbal behavior), the rate) stereotyping. The second complexity
correlations between their assessments of concerns the relevance of the disposition’s
the managers’ extraversion and conscien­ domain to the context in which the nonver­
tiousness were r = .27 and r = .30, respec­ bal behavior occurs (e.g., examining inter­
tively; that is, they shared about 9% of their personal traits in interpersonal contexts). A
variance (Motowidlo, Burnett, Maczynski, researcher should not expect conscientious­
& Witkowski, 1996). Given that the over­ ness, for example, to be encoded strongly
lap between the assessments was quite low, in a casual conversation, but the same
they cannot both be accurate. Which assess­ researcher could expect extraversion to be
ment was more accurate, that based on encoded strongly in a conversation.
the manager’s words, as written, or that The third complexity involves the poten­
based on the manager’s wordless, nonver­ tial interference with encoding of situa­
bal behavior? tional factors. One such factor is the mutual
influence of interacting participants. Some
writers have assumed that the presence of
TEN COMPLEXITIES others will always affect the encoding for
any individual (e.g., Kanki, 1985), but the
With these issues in mind, a summary of effect of others can be tested with intraclass
current knowledge may be essayed. All the correlation analyses (e.g., Gifford, 1994).
following findings are subject to a blanket Sometimes the effect of others is found,
cautionary note, however, because most empirically, to be minimal. The fourth
studies have not dealt with all the complex­ complexity relates to another aspect of the
ities of encoding and decoding research. situation, the nature of the activity or inter­
Specifically, I argue that researchers must action during which encoding is investi­
manage (at least) 10 complexities in their gated. For example, individuals may not
research designs and analyses (i.e., Type I merely enact a given behavior (e.g., smiling)
or Type II errors in the relations between more or less frequently depending on
nonverbal behavior and personality that context (e.g., at a party vs. a funeral), but
may occur if a researcher relies solely on a their encoding (the correlation with a given
simple Pearson correlation or fails to take disposition, regardless of the frequency of
into account one or more of the accuracy or the act) may differ with the situation. In
agreement issues). Some complexities are a study that demonstrated this, disposi­
familiar and some less so; some are easier to tional public self-consciousness was encoded
manage than others. in hand movements differently when partic­
The first complexity is perhaps the most ipants were lying than when they were
obvious, but published studies that over­ telling the truth (Vrij, Akehurst, & Morris,
look it have appeared. All the measures 1997).
must have adequate internal consistency The fifth complexity is a matter of clear
and interrater reliability. Ordinarily, this reporting more than a problem in itself. For
should be at least .75, but .80 or better is example, in one study, neuroticism was cor­
very desirable. Adjustments for error and related with touching the self more, fewer
apologies for lower levels of reliability expressive gestures, and more gaze aversion
sometimes are made, but they are not con­ (Campbell & Rushton, 1978). The first two
vincing. Although interrater reliability is encodings were based on a teacher’s rating of
a prerequisite for decoding validity, even the person’s neuroticism, however, whereas
09-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 7:49 PM Page 164

164–––◆–––Factors of Influence

the third encoding was based on a self-report not only are there cultural differences in the
measure of neuroticism. That is, the different amount or frequency of nonverbal behav­
assessments of neuroticism related to differ­ iors (e.g., Hall, 1966; see Matsumoto, this
ent nonverbal behaviors. Such results should volume), but nonverbal behavior may also
be reported as based on different measures of differentially encode (correlate with) disposi­
the disposition. In a study that illustrated the tions in different cultures (e.g., Andersen &
problem of considering self-ratings and rat­ Guerrero, 1998).
ings by others to be equivalent, self-report
measures of emotional expressiveness yielded Summary. To learn whether nonverbal
different relations to a disposition (neuroti­ behavior truly encodes personality disposi­
cism again) than did rated behavioral assess­ tions, researchers must navigate at least 10
ments of emotional expressiveness (Riggio & design and analysis complexities: (1) True
Riggio, 2002). encoding can be obscured through the use of
A sixth complexity is that relations unreliable measures; (2) encoding should be
between personality and nonverbal behav­ studied in a context in which the disposition
ior can differ with different combinations is salient; (3) others in the interaction might
of traits. For example, individuals who are influence an individual’s encoding; (4) encod­
shy and sociable avert their gaze more and ing might occur differently when a person is
engage in more self-manipulation than engaged in different activities or purposes; (5)
others (Cheek & Buss, 1981), but this is not encoding may depend on who (e.g., self or
true of the other combinations (e.g., shy but significant others) assesses the disposition;
not sociable persons). The value of the (6) nonverbal behavior may encode combi­
Cheek and Buss study lies in its demonstra­ nations of dispositions without encoding
tion that combinations of dispositions that combination’s constituent dispositions;
sometimes reveal more about encoding (7) combinations of nonverbal behaviors may
than individual dispositions do. A seventh encode dispositions without the individual
complexity is that dispositions can be behaviors doing so; (8) encoding may depend
encoded by a group of behaviors without on the gender composition of the group; (9)
any particular behavior doing so (Aries, encoding sometimes differs for male and
Gold, & Weigel, 1983). Sometimes a pat­ female individuals; and (10) cultural groups
tern or profile of nonverbal behaviors must vary in their encoding patterns.
be measured before significant encoding
(in their case, dominance) can be detected.
Eighth, in the same study, the encoding ♦ Encoding and
of dominance occurred only in same-sex
Decoding Personality
groups, not in mixed-sex groups. Thus,
encoding may be different depending on the
sex composition of an interacting group. Keeping these complexities in mind, I turn
The ninth form of complexity is also related now toward the research focused on encod­
to sex: A given encoding relation may be true ing and decoding personality. An example
for one sex but not for the other. For example, of relatively straightforward encoding
extraversion correlates strongly with the use of results comes from a study of interacting
broad gestures among women but not among female dyads (Berry & Hansen, 2000). In
men (Lippa, 1998). The tenth complexity is Big Five personality terms, more agreeable
that personality is encoded by nonverbal women gestured more, used more open
behavior differently across cultures. That is, body postures, visually attended to their
09-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 7:49 PM Page 165

Personality and Nonverbal Behavior–––◆–––165

interaction partner more, used fewer visual mouth as shy, and those who smile less as
dominance behaviors, and displayed fewer too serious (Ferrari & Swinkels, 1996).
negative facial expressions than did less Are these accurate assessments of a tar­
agreeable women. Women who were more get person’s personality or mere “decoding
open to experience visually attended to errors” (Bull, 1983)? Observers appear to
their interaction partners more than those decode confidently and with greater con­
who were less open to experience. More sensus (Gifford, 1994; Lippa & Dietz,
extraverted persons seem to use more ani­ 2000), but the evidence that they do so
mated, expressive, and animated gestures— accurately is mixed or even discouraging,
that is, faster and more energetic gestures as shown, for example, by the Cleeton and
using the hands farther from the body Knight study. On the positive side, some
(Lippa, 1998)—than more introverted research shows that if one is willing to define
persons. Children with more internal, accuracy as observer agreement with target
rather than external, locus of control ten­ self-assessments, removing nonverbal behav­
dencies smile more and engage in fewer off- ior from a job interview (by conducting
task activities (Carton & Carton, 1998). it by telephone, as opposed to in person)
Individuals with avoidant attachment styles reduces accuracy (Blackman, 2002). Thus,
tend to choose larger interpersonal distances nonverbal behavior certainly can contribute
(Kaitz, Bar-Haim, Lehrer, & Grossman, to accurate judgments.
2004), as do those with greater trait anxiety A variant on the study of decoding is the
(e.g., Patterson, 1973) and weaker affiliative study of decoding ability, sometimes called
tendencies (e.g., Mehrabian & Diamond, nonverbal sensitivity (e.g., Rosenthal, 1979;
1971). see Riggio, this volume). Decoding as a skill
Whether or not dispositions are encoded related to the judge’s own experience and
in nonverbal behavior, decoders believe background is often applied to constructs
they are. In an early study, personnel man­ other than personality (i.e., emotion; Mullins
agers were quite confident that job applica­ & Duke, 2004). More intelligent judges are
tion photographs revealed the applicants’ also more accurate (Lippa & Dietz, 2000),
character (Viteles & Smith, 1932). Observers’ at least for some dispositions: More intelli­
ratings may be reliable, which suggests gent university-student judges assessed
accuracy, but they often do not correlate dispositional extraversion and an omnibus
with any of the targets’ physical features (across dispositions) measure more accu­
(e.g., Cleeton & Knight, 1924). Researchers rately than less intelligent university-
still investigate alleged nonverbal “power student judges. On the other side of the
codes” (Schwartz, Tesser, & Powell, 1982) lens, which dispositions are easiest to
and the “shared meaning” of postures decode from nonverbal behavior? Several
(Kudoh & Matsumoto, 1985). “High­ studies (e.g., Ambady, Hallahan, & Rosenthal,
persuasive” nonverbal behavior patterns in 1995; Borkenau & Liebler, 1992; Gifford,
actors (direct gaze, more gestures, fewer 1994; Lippa & Dietz, 2000) report that
self-touches) are judged to be more sociability or extraversion is the most legi­
assertive, forceful, powerful, and intelligent ble or accurately discernable disposition.
(Hart & Morry, 1997). Individuals who This, however, may be a function of the
speak in a tight-lipped manner or who turn second complexity mentioned previously.
their heads while speaking may be judged Most studies use conversations as the activ­
as “uptight,” those who speak with a hand ity, and extraversion is particularly salient
over their mouths or smile with a closed for conversations.
09-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 7:49 PM Page 166

166–––◆–––Factors of Influence

♦ A Paradigm Proposal carefully developed nonverbal behavior


scoring system, and (3) observer-raters who
typically are unacquainted with the actors
This section proposes a paradigm that may so that their ratings are not influenced by
deal with the crucial accuracy problems previous personal experience with the
in the most useful way. Its essential feature actor. More particularly or operationally,
is that encoding and decoding both are the paradigm’s structure is an adaptation of
included in the same study. Most prior Brunswik’s (1956) lens model (Figure 9.1).
studies have examined either encoding or Encoding (or what Brunswik called
decoding, which disallows the possibility of ecological validity) is represented by the
understanding the relations between the lines connecting personality to nonverbal
two processes, or they have compared self behavior. Encoding, as defined here, occurs
and observer ratings without investigat­ when reliable self-assessments correlate sig­
ing intervening variables such as nonver­ nificantly with reliably scored nonverbal
bal behavior. For example, one study behaviors. Inferences from these cues
showed that self and acquainted observer (decoding, or what Brunswik called cue uti­
ratings were better correlated than self and lization) are represented by the lines con­
unacquainted observer ratings, but the necting nonverbal behavior and impression
researchers did not investigate the behav­ formation on the part of the observers.
ioral cues on which the ratings were based Decoding, as defined here, occurs when reli­
(Funder & Colvin, 1988). Watson (1989) able observer assessments are correlated
noticed this gap and called for studies of with reliably scored nonverbal behaviors.
judgments that also include behavioral The curved line linking the ratings of the
cues. Nevertheless, “cueless” studies are actors’ dispositions with the observers’
still reported (e.g., Ambady, Hallahan, & ratings of those dispositions represents what
Rosenthal, 1995). For example, “sociable” Brunswik called achievement, or what is here
actors were found to be more “legible” called agreement. The large oval signifies the
(i.e., easier to “read” or accurately decode) context in which the judgments are made.
than less sociable actors, based on actor- Encoding and decoding are influ­
observer agreement, but the pathways enced by the context in which they occur.
or mediating behaviors underlying this What transpires in a conversation may
phenomenon were not examined. A few not flow the same way in a debate as
years later, these results were replicated, during a romantic evening, a business dis­
and many potential mediating cues were cussion, a romantic interaction, or an inter­
investigated. In this work, extraverts used rogation, or in interactions within versus
more energetic gestures, kept their hands across cultures. One illustration of this
farther from their bodies, and changed their comes from a study of deception (Vrij,
facial expression more than introverts Akehurst, & Morris, 1997). In this study,
(Lippa, 1998). actors were interviewed twice, once when
The proposed paradigm includes the fol­ they told the truth and once when they lied.
lowing elements: reliably measured person­ Actors with higher levels of public self-con­
ality constructs that are investigated within sciousness used their hands differently (less)
the context to which they apply, using three when they lied than when they told the
independent groups of raters: (1) actors’ truth. Thus, the adapted lens model in
self-rated personality or raters who know Figure 9.1 requires its surrounding oval to
the actor well, (2) raters trained in a signify the context in which the encoding
09-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 7:49 PM Page 167

Personality and Nonverbal Behavior–––◆–––167

Head Behaviors

Trunk Behaviors
Self-Rated Observer-Rated
Disposition Disposition
Arm and
Hand Behaviors

Leg Behaviors
Ecological Validity Cue Utilization

Achievement

Figure 9.1 The Contextual Lens Model (updated)


SOURCE: Brunswik (1956).

and decoding occur. Few studies have done nomothetic goals (“How do people
what seems most productive, however: (1) decode?”), the study combines the ratings
to investigate all three processes; (2) to of “everyone”—parents, supervisors,
assess the relative strengths of encoding, friends, partners, peers, or members of
decoding, and agreement; and (3) to take other cultures—as the observers of interest.
the context into account to provide some Of course, if observers in general or from a
understanding of how nonverbal behavior particular group use nonverbal cues idio­
communicates (and miscommunicates) per­ syncratically, the interrater reliability of
sonality. Some other notable exceptions their target disposition ratings will be low,
that focus on nonverbal behavior and dis­ and it will be inappropriate to correlate
positions include those by Borkenau and their ratings with the nonverbal behavior
Liebler (1992) and Lippa (1998). scores (decoding correlations) or with the
The paradigm is employed, in part, to targets’ self-ratings (agreement correla­
understand the cue utilization policies of tions). Thus, studies with any sort of nomo­
observers, individually or in aggregate. Some thetic goals depend on the idea, and
early studies focused on individual abilities, therefore must hypothesize, that
such as those of clinicians (e.g., Hoffman, a group of observers will reliably agree on
1960), and found that their judgments, as actors’ dispositions. If a specified group of
revealed through their use of cues, did observers do not agree, then conclusions
not match well with their own impressions about their cue-utilization policies cannot
of how they use those cues. A more be stated, probably because members of
recent individual-level focus has been on that group do not use the same cues.
the ability or sensitivity of individual The earliest study that used this para­
observers (e.g., Rosenthal, 1979). When digm was one by Brunswik himself in 1945
a researcher has more aggregate, (but not reported until later; Brunswik,
09-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 7:49 PM Page 168

168–––◆–––Factors of Influence

1956, pp. 26–29). The central cues he because the actors in the study were
employed were, perhaps appropriately engaged in a conversation; other disposi­
enough for a first and early study, physiog­ tions (e.g., conscientiousness, openness to
nomic: actor’s height of forehead, length of experience, and emotional stability, the
nose, and so on. A decidedly verbal study other three dispositions in the Big Five)
using the paradigm to study extraversion in were not included because they would
relation to vocal behavior was published not have been examined in a context that
two decades later (Scherer, 1978). In one should have made them particularly salient.
study that fulfilled most of the goals of the
proposed paradigm, behavioral cues were
examined as mediators of the encoding- ♦ Interlude: Potential Outcomes
decoding process (Borkenau & Liebler,
of Encoding-Decoding Studies
1992). The same judges served as raters of
the physical cues and as decoders, however,
which compromised the independence of Before describing the exemplar study in detail,
the behavior scores and trait ratings. it may be useful to discuss the generic
Perhaps the first study that examined potential outcomes of studies that use this
nonverbal behavioral mediators and used paradigm. The first assumption is that all
behavior scorers who were independent of the judgments (e.g., self-ratings, behavior
both targets and observers was conducted by scoring, and observer ratings) are reliable;
Gifford, Ng, and Wilkinson (1985). That if some are not, they cannot be used with
study identified nonverbal cues exhibited by any pretence of validity. In general, encod­
job applicants that mediated (and failed to ing, decoding, and agreement may be weak
mediate) agreement between job applicant or strong for any disposition, and the
and personnel officer assessments of the pattern of results may be different for each
applicant’s social skill and motivation to disposition.
work. That study, however, did not employ The first type of potential outcome
personality dispositions as the psychological occurs when, for a given disposition, encod­
constructs. A subsequent study, with a coor­ ing, decoding, and agreement all are weak.
dinated set of independent targets, raters, In this case, (1) personality is not reflected
and observers, that did investigate nonverbal consistently in nonverbal behavior (at least
behaviors and how they encode dispositions not in the behaviors studied), (2) observers
as well as which of these same nonverbal do not use this set of behavior cues to arrive
behaviors are employed by observers to infer at their inferences, and (3) observer infer­
targets’ personality finally appeared just over ences do not agree with the self or knowl­
a decade ago (Gifford, 1994). edgeable other assessments of actors.
Gifford (1994) will be described in some Second, if decoding is strong but encoding
detail as an exemplar of the proposed para­ is weak, observers apparently are using
digm. It examined the eight dispositions invalid stereotypes. (One suspects, without
that comprise the interpersonal aspect of the benefit of data, that this was the case
personality and form a circumplex (Wiggins, with Lavater and his fellow physiogno­
1979). The primary axes of the circumplex mists.) Agreement should be weak in such a
(dominance and warmth) are interpreted by case, because there are no true relations
some theorists as two of the Big Five per­ between personality and nonverbal behav­
sonality domains (McCrae & Costa, 1989; ior for observers to decode legitimately.
Peabody & Goldberg, 1989). Interpersonal Third, if strong encoding but weak
dispositions were selected deliberately decoding is found, observers are unable to
09-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 7:49 PM Page 169

Personality and Nonverbal Behavior–––◆–––169

deduce correctly which nonverbal cues same objective (visible) aspects of reality as
reflect the actors’ personality. The potential encoding. If so, the centuries-old conviction
for strong agreement is present, but it is that dispositions truly are “legible” would
unrealized. Fourth, if agreement is strong receive convincing support.
but both encoding and decoding are weak,
observers must be using nonverbal behav­
iors for decoding that the researcher has not ♦ An Exemplar
measured. Some nonverbal cue or other
must have been providing valid information
about the actor’s personality, or agreement The following study is presented partly for
would not be possible. The researcher must its results, which illustrate many specific
explore the impression formation process, encoding and decoding results with per­
perhaps through interviews with observers, sonality and nonverbal behavior. But it is
to learn which unstudied nonverbal cues the also presented as a way of introducing
observers might have been using to succeed many of the intricacies of conducting
in matching the assessments of the actors. encoding-decoding research, including pro­
Fifth, if weak encoding and high agree­ posed solutions to problems that arise in
ment are found, observers again must be the course of analyzing the data in such
using valid but unmeasured nonverbal cues, studies. Based on the available literature,
unless the unlikely case that the observers the hypotheses of the exemplar study
are clairvoyant holds true (Reichenbach, (Gifford, 1994) were that encoding would
1938). As Wiggins (1973) wryly notes, be weak to moderate but that observers
“Such a possibility is assigned rather low would have a strong tendency to decode.
priority as a contemporary scientific expla­ Agreement, based on recent research that
nation” (p. 159). This is a case in which indicates dispositions are communicated
researchers must rethink their choice of cues, to different degrees (Gifford et al., 1985;
seeking others that do encode the disposi­ John, 1990), was expected to vary across
tion. One way to accomplish this might be dispositions. For dispositions with low
to interview the judges, asking them to agreement, self-observer encoding-decoding
reflect on their inferences. discrepancies were expected to be high.
Finally, if strong encoding, strong For dispositions with high agreement, self-
decoding, and strong agreement are found, observer discrepancies were expected
the researcher may conclude that the whole to be low.
process is working as researchers in this The target participants were 60 under­
area dream it does, and they may be able to graduates drawn from a psychology depart­
supply a satisfying account of this assess­ ment participant pool. Ten all-male and 10
ment process. A sober second thought, all-female triads were formed into conver­
however, is that actors (or their intimates) sational groups, and one group at a time
and observers could be agreeing on an was filmed as it conversed. The participants
inaccurate view of the actors’ personality, were given a list of suggested topics, but
something akin to a folie à deux. A more they were encouraged to converse on any
likely interpretation is that the strong medi­ topic they chose. A week or so prior to the
ation of objective nonverbal behaviors, conversation, the participants were given
assessed reliably by independent observers, Wiggins’ (1979) Interpersonal Adjective
would be substantial evidence that the Scales Inventory (IAS). The IAS covers two
observers’ decoding is valid, given that of the Big Five domains of personality, but
they have been demonstrated to rely on the it was chosen to maximize the relevance of
09-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 7:49 PM Page 170

170–––◆–––Factors of Influence

selected dispositions to the context. From between a nonverbal behavior cue and a
the top of the circumplex, these scales are disposition, however, necessarily is a valid
ambitious-dominant, gregarious-extraverted, encoding link.
warm-agreeable, unassuming-ingenuous, lazy- Three specific threats to the validity or
submissive, aloof-introverted, cold-quarrel­ generalizability of an encoding link may
some, and arrogant-calculating (see Gifford & be identified. First, the correlation could be
O’Connor, 1987). influenced by the actions of others in the
The videotapes were then scored using conversation; a valid encoding link should
the Seated Kinesic Activity Notation System be empirically attributable to an individual,
(SKANS IV; Gifford, 1986), in which 38 uncontaminated by group influence, if it
kinesic and facial behaviors are measured in is to be considered a valid personality-
one of three ways: frequency, duration, or nonverbal behavior link. Second, correla­
time sampling. In a second sample of partic­ tions may be due to chance; to be valid, an
ipants, 21 unacquainted peers of the targets encoding link should have reasonable
were shown 5-minute selections from the strength and be part of an ordered pattern
middle of the conversations over several ses­ of correlations around the interpersonal
sions with the audio track turned off. Each circle. If a behavior is truly relevant to inter­
time the tape was played, each observer was personal behavior, it should not merely
asked to focus on only 1 of the 3 partici­ correlate with one disposition on the circle.
pants shown in the tape. The tape was then Its correlations should rise and fall around
replayed and the observers watched another the interpersonal circle in an ordered man­
participant. Thus, all 21 observers viewed ner (Gifford, 1991). Third, the possibility
all 60 participants. After each tape was of sex differences raises the issue of general­
shown, each observer completed a 40-item izability of a given putative encoding link
short version of the IAS about one target. to both sexes. For example, a valid link
The observers’ task was demanding, so they between a disposition and a nonverbal
made their ratings over several sessions. behavior for women may not be valid for
They were paid $50 for their efforts and men, or vice versa. For example, using most
offered a prize of $50 for being the most of one’s body when gesturing validly signals
accurate (defined as coming the closest to extraversion for women, but it does not
the self-ratings of the 60 target individuals— work as a valid signal for men (Lippa, 1998).
really, as noted, a measure of agreement). Each of these threats was considered in
Most measures (self-assessments, observer preliminary analyses (for details, see
assessments, and SKANS IV measures) Gifford, 1994). In all, because of significant
were adequately reliable. Some behaviors, group influence or failure to conclusively
however, occurred infrequently; they were map onto the interpersonal circle, 19 of the
difficult to score owing to camera place­ 27 remaining nonverbal behaviors were
ment; or interjudge agreement was low. rejected as not demonstrably valid encoders of
Others were combined because they were interpersonal dispositions. The eight nonver­
highly correlated. Thus, the remaining bal behaviors identified as valid encoders of
analyses were based on 27 nonverbal interpersonal circle traits are head orienta­
behaviors. Pearson correlation coefficients tion, nods, arm wrap, gestures, object
between the actors’ self-assessed disposi­ manipulation, left leg lean, leg movement,
tions and their nonverbal behaviors repre­ and leg extension. Their significant links
sent the left, or encoding, half of the lens (p < .05) with the eight dispositions of the
diagram. Not every significant correlation interpersonal circle are displayed in one
09-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 7:49 PM Page 171

Personality and Nonverbal Behavior–––◆–––171

Gregarious-Extraverted
Ecological Validity: Cue Utilization:
Multiple R = .41 Multiple R = .80
R2 = .17 R2 = .65
Head Orientation
.39
Head Recline
–.40***
Nods
*
.28
.27*
Head Shakes .52***

Smiles .32**
.26*
Trunk Recline
Extraverted– –.30**
–.46*** Extraverted–
Arm Wrap
Gregarious .41*** .62*** Gregarious
(Self) Gestures –.24* (Others)
Left Hand Vertical .39**
Hand Extension .35**
.27*
Right Leg Orientation .23*

Right Leg Lean


–.23*
Left Leg Lean
–.24*
Leg Openness

Leg Movement

Achievement = .45

Figure 9.2 The Contextual Lens Model for Gregarious-Extraverted

typical disposition: gregarious-extraverted. cue utilization correlations are displayed on


See Figure 9.2 (also, see Gifford, 1994) for the right half of Figure 9.2; collectively,
the other seven lens models. they describe the way in which typical
Correlations between the nonverbal observers decode.
behaviors and the dispositions as inferred
by the observers were computed. All 27 of
the nonverbal behaviors were used for this THE STRENGTH OF
purpose, rather than the subset of 8 behav­ ENCODING AND DECODING
iors used for the encoding half of the study.
This was because the goal on the left half of Next, the magnitude of encoding and
the lens is to determine which nonverbal decoding was examined. Magnitude was
behaviors actually encode personality (to computed as the multiple correlation and per­
the best of our methods’ abilities), whereas centage of variance in each disposition
on the right side the goal is to determine accounted for by the nonverbal behaviors.
which nonverbal behaviors are believed Only nonverbal behaviors that had shown
by observers to be cues to personality. This significant (p < .05) correlations with the dis­
distinction follows from Brunswik’s origi­ positions were considered. Stepwise multiple
nal labels for the two sides of the lens regression analysis was used for this pur­
model: ecological validity (left half) and cue pose. Magnitudes to be reported are conser­
utilization (right half). Significant (p < .05) vative because, although all variables with
09-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 7:49 PM Page 172

172–––◆–––Factors of Influence

significant Pearson correlations were given generalize to typical individual observers


the opportunity to predict a given disposi­ would conclude that decoding is not partic­
tion, only those that made significant ularly reliable, and this would attenuate the
(p < .05) additional contributions to the seemingly large magnitude of decoding.
equation were included. Figure 9.2 shows the Whether researchers examine group
values of multiple R and R2 for the encoding decoding or typical individual decod­
and decoding for gregarious-extraverted. ing value depends on the study’s purpose. If
One general tendency apparent from it is to understand how observers (in gen­
the results is that decoding is stronger than eral, nomothetically) decode, one would use
encoding. Beginning at the top of the inter­ the full observer sample; if it is to estimate
personal circle and proceeding clockwise, the decoding skill of a single “typical”
multiple correlations were (encoding fol­ observer, the attenuation approach should
lowed by decoding) as follows: ambitious- be used; and if the goal is to understand how
dominant .54 versus .81, extraverted- one particular observer decodes (e.g., a clin­
gregarious .41 versus .80, warm-agreeable ician under training), one could study
.30 versus .79, unassuming-ingenuous .41 decoding with an n of 1. The question for
versus .74, lazy-submissive .62 versus .81, the researcher is, do I wish to learn how and
aloof-introverted .54 versus .82, cold-quar­ how well observers in general decode, how
relsome .00 versus .79, and arrogant-calcu­ and how well a typical single observer (e.g.,
lating .25 versus .77. The median encoding a typical human resource officer in a large
magnitude is .41 and the median decoding organization) decodes, or how and how well
magnitude is .80. Many more significant this observer (e.g., a person applying for a
decoding links than encoding links are found. job as a human resource officer) decodes?

PARTICULAR ENCODING
GROUP VERSUS
AND DECODING LINKS
INDIVIDUAL DECODING
For readers who are interested, consider­
Despite these findings, however, decod­
able information about particular relations
ing actually may not be much stronger than
between interpersonal dispositions and
encoding. As noted, observer decoding rat­
nonverbal behaviors is available in Figures
ings are based on 21 raters. Multiple raters
3 to 10 of Gifford (1994). Here, only one
almost necessarily increase the reliability
lens model is presented, as an example, in
of ratings. When ratings are more reliable,
the interest of saving space.
correlations involving them are stronger
because less error is involved. Stronger cor­
relations are more likely to be statistically AGREEMENT AND NONVERBAL
significant and therefore to be included in COMMUNICATION
the lens diagrams. Analyses that corrected
for attenuation and estimated the reliability Agreement is measured as the correla­
of single judges (see Gifford, 1994, for tion between typical self-assessments and
details) showed that one typical decoding typical assessments by observers, and it
link shrank from r = −.58 to r = −.35. The is represented in Figures 9.1 and 9.2 by
matched encoding link for this decoding the curved line at the bottom. The use
link was r = −.29, not much less than of correlations overcomes several of the
r = −.35. Thus, observers as a group decode classic Cronbach (1955) criticisms of accu­
strongly, but researchers who wish to racy research. Across the eight dispositions
09-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 7:49 PM Page 173

Personality and Nonverbal Behavior–––◆–––173

in the full study, agreement averaged .27 necessary. As noted earlier, however, the
(r− to Z-transformed), which is significant greater number and magnitude of decoding
(p < .02), if moderate in magnitude. links is due partly to psychometric considera­
Agreement ranged from .18 (NS) for both tions—that is, the superior reliability of
lazy-submissive and cold-quarrelsome to decoding. Agreement is higher in general
.45 (p < .001) for gregarious-extraverted when there are more matched links. The exis­
and .41 (p < .001) for aloof-introverted. tence of matched links, with their lines going
The relations between encoding and from the disposition to a behavior and from
decoding fall into two categories, each with the behavior to the observer’s assessment,
two forms. First, matched links may be clearly suggests that agreement increases when
identified. One form of matched link occurs information “flows” via such matched links.
when a nonverbal behavior significantly Conversely, agreement is lower when many
encodes self-assessments and is also used to mismatched links occur. The same trend was
a significant degree by observers to decode also demonstrated earlier in a personnel selec­
or infer that self-assessment. Across the 8 tion context by Gifford et al. (1985).
dispositions, 14 matched links of this form When information does not flow, either
were found. Matched links are underlined encoding has not occurred (no behaviors
in Figure 9.2. Another form of matched link measured encode the disposition) or the
occurs when a link is significant on neither observer has used cues other than those
side of the lens: Observers are saying that that the encoding analysis suggests are
a given behavior does not encode a given valid indicators of a disposition. The com­
trait, and based on the self-assessments, it munication of self-assessed personality was
does not. In this study, 105 such matches quite good (i.e., agreement was relatively
occurred. Second, mismatched links may be high) for some dispositions. Considering
identified. One form of mismatched link that observers saw only 5 minutes of a
occurs when a nonverbal behavior does soundless conversation among individuals
encode a self-assessed disposition, but they had never met, their decoding of gre­
observers do not use that cue. For example, garious-extraverted, r = .45, and aloof-
more lazy-submissive persons manipulate introverted, r = .41, for example, is quite
objects (e.g., their clothing, pen, paper) an “achievement.” This study demon­
more than others do, but observers do not strates that (1) the encoding of interper­
use object manipulation as a cue to lazy- sonal dispositions in nonverbal behavior is
submissive. In this study, six mismatches of moderate (median multiple R = .41), (2)
this kind occurred across the eight interper­ the decoding of the same dispositions is
sonal dispositions. The other form of mis­ moderate by individual observers and
matched link occurs when observers use a strong by groups of observers (median
particular nonverbal cue to form their multiple R = .80), and (3) agreement is low
impression but that cue does not encode to moderate (mean r = .27), yet significant
that disposition. For example (see Figure (p < .02). Each of these results will be dis­
9.2), observers believe that more gregari­ cussed in turn. Apart from the specific
ous-extraverted persons orient their heads magnitudes of these links, the combined
toward their companions more, but (based findings show exactly how information
on self-assessments) this is not so. appears to flow from the actor to the
In this study, 83 mismatches of this kind observer—that is, how personality and non­
were observed across the eight dispositions. verbal behavior are connected, and how
Decoders appear to use many more cues to observers infer (and misinfer) personality
infer self-assessed dispositions than were by watching other people.
09-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 7:49 PM Page 174

174–––◆–––Factors of Influence

ENCODING encoders and vary the social context of their


observations to encourage the expression of
That encoding is at least moderate is dispositions that may not show up in a pleas­
an optimistic note in a literature that can ant exchange among previously unacquain­
be characterized as pessimistic (see Bull, ted individuals before concluding that
1983; Duncan & Fiske, 1985; Heslin & dispositions simply are not encoded in
Patterson, 1982). These results are based on nonverbal behavior.
a conservative strategy that winnowed out
apparently significant encoding links that
manifested group influence or did not meet DECODING
the criteria for behavior mapping (Gifford,
1991) and used a conservative variance- Apparently, strong decoding was evident
accounting strategy. Almost 40% of the for all eight dispositions (the smallest mul­
variance in self-assessed lazy-submissive­ tiple R was .74). Borkenau and Liebler
ness, for example, was accounted for. Even (1992) also report stronger decoding than
more encouraging, certain behaviors that encoding. Because observers as a group
have been linked to dominance or submis­ produce very reliable ratings, they appear
siveness in other encoding studies, such as willing to make strong inferences about the
postural relaxation (Mehrabian, 1981) and targets’ personalities based on nonverbal
looking in relation to speaking (Exline, behavior. Typical individual observers may
Ellyson, & Long, 1975), were not included not decode any more strongly than the
in this study. When these behaviors are equivalent strength of encoding, however.
included in future studies, the evidence for Thus, personality theorists must be careful
encoding should be even stronger. when interpreting observer decodings.
Dispositions that were not well encoded Observer assessments are not necessarily
(e.g., cold-quarrelsome) may be encoded by any more or less valid than are self-ratings,
behaviors that were not included in this particularly if neither self nor observer
study, may not be encoded in nonverbal has any expert training. Observers may
behavior, or may not have been elicited often use “power codes” (Schwartz, Tesser,
in the context of the conversations in this & Powell, 1982), and postures have
study. In a newly acquainted group of three shared meaning for observers (Kudoh &
students who have conversational freedom, Matsumoto, 1985). This does not mean
any tendencies an individual may have necessarily that dominance from the self’s
toward cold-quarrelsome or arrogant-calcu­ perspective is revealed by this same set of
lating have little reason or opportunity to be acts, however; it merely means that
expressed. If so, any nonverbal behaviors observers believe it is.
that signal these dispositions will occur infre­
quently and not have much variance. This, in
turn, means that correlations between dispo­ AGREEMENT
sition and behavior are unlikely to emerge.
The context-based reason for poor encod­ Agreement, as explained earlier, depends
ing, however, may not always apply. For on the decoders’ appropriate use of ecolog­
example, warm-agreeable is a disposition ically valid cues. For example, the observers
that one would expect to be expressed in this believed that 14 nonverbal cues are good
study’s conversational context, but it was indicators of target cold-quarrelsomeness,
encoded poorly. Researchers, then, must con­ but not one of the 14 cues encoded self-
sider other nonverbal behaviors as potential rated cold-quarrelsomeness, and agreement
09-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 7:49 PM Page 175

Personality and Nonverbal Behavior–––◆–––175

was only r = .18 (NS). In contrast, the gregarious-extraverted to aloof-introverted)


observers believed that 14 nonverbal manifests the greatest agreement (the mean r
behaviors signal gregarious-extraverted, for the two dispositions was .43). One
and 3 of those actually do; as a result, implication is that grexalin is the most visi­
agreement was r = .45 (p < .001). Clearly, ble interpersonal axis. All the dispositions
agreement varies across dispositions. The were strongly perceived (decoded) but with­
results indicate that, in general (around the out much agreement. Grexalin is perceived
interpersonal circle), self and observer both more correctly than the other axes. In
agree and disagree about self’s dispositions. attempting to explain this phenomenon, it is
Self-other agreement depends heavily on tempting to assert that grexalin is the most
the observer’s use of appropriate nonverbal legible disposition because it is the most
cues (i.e., those that do encode self’s view truly interpersonal disposition.
of the disposition). “Disagreement” occurs
when observers use inappropriate cues. If a
way can be found to increase the reliability ♦ Conclusion
of self-assessment, perhaps through multi­
ple self-assessments, error on the encoding
IMPLICATIONS FOR EVERYDAY
side will shrink, and the magnitude and SOCIAL INTERACTION
number of encoding links should rise, with
a consequent increase in agreement. One important implication of the find­
ings is that when self and observer believe
that different behaviors signify a given dis­
VARIATIONS IN ENCODING
position (or that a given behavior signifies
ACROSS DISPOSITIONS
different dispositions), misinterpretation
and conflict may result. If one person
Interpersonal dispositions (in Big Five
believes that the other is cold, he or she may
terms, agreeableness and extraversion, as
well behave toward the other in accordance
opposed to the three others that are not par­
with this perception of coldness, that is,
ticularly interpersonal in nature: conscien­
with generally negative responses. The other
tiousness, emotional stability, and openness
person consequently may be expected to be
to experience) are the focus in this study,
unpleasantly mystified by these actions and
because they are the dispositions one would
expect to be salient in the conversational may then respond accordingly (i.e., not very
context we examined. This restriction of dis­ positively). The first person may then react
positions studied to contexts in which they to the second person’s negative reaction neg­
may reasonably be expected to manifest atively, and so on. In this way, the innocent
themselves or to be salient is one of the pre­ use of and consequent misinference from
cepts of this paradigm. In this study, the two certain nonverbal behaviors could seriously
major dimensions are treated as orthogonal damage the development of relations between
axes of a circumplex; around the circumplex persons who meet for the first time.
are eight gradations resulting from the high
and low ends of the two dimensions, plus A PROCESS MODEL
combinations of them. The resulting eight AS A FRAMEWORK
interpersonal constructs vary in the amount
they are encoded in nonverbal behavior. Brunswik’s (1956) lens model serves as a
What I call the “grexalin” axis (the line useful way of conceptualizing social judg­
that bisects the interpersonal circle from ment, including the encoding and decoding
09-Manusov.qxd 8/3/2006 8:30 PM Page 176

176–––◆–––Factors of Influence

of nonverbal behavior by actors and heuristic, in that it can serve as a clear


observers. Besides being clear intuitively, it agenda for future researchers.
offers an unambiguous path for empirical
demonstrations of how encoding, decoding, FINAL COMMENTS
and agreement operate through nonverbal
behavior. A companion framework, which Personality and nonverbal behavior are
I have adapted from Craik’s (1968) frame­ not linked in simple ways. This accounts for
work for understanding environmental per­ the undulations in optimism from the era of
ception and which offers a comprehensive physiognomy and early (1930s) scientific
overview of the different kinds of actors, efforts to the lacunae in research until the
media of presentation, form of judgments, 1960s, followed by the slough of despond in
types of criteria, types of observers, and the early 1980s and the slow rise of opti­
types of analyses, is portrayed in Table 9.1. mism since then. Progress will be difficult,
The process model is at once daunting, in given the 10 (or more) complexities, but
that it suggests the huge number of possi­ if researchers are careful to at least describe
bilities in this research area in contrast to how their studies deal with the complexities,
how little has been accomplished, and understanding will grow. This will be a step

Table 9.1 A Process Model for the Encoding and Decoding of Nonverbal Behavior in
Personality

Mode of Form of Observer Type of


Actor (A) Presentation Judgment Sample Criteria Context (O) Analysis

Child In Vivo Self-report (A) Big 5 Conversation Peer A-O


Agreement

Student Video Rating (O) Trait Personnel Partner O-O


Anxiety Selection Agreement

Partner Audio Scored Self-Monitor Deception Employer Encoding


Behavior Correlation

Friend Transcript Specific Attachment Clinical Employee Decoding


Style Correlation

Peer Drawing Pattern Control Attraction Teacher Mediators/


Orientation Moderators
Other Culture Narcissism Friend Skill/
Sensitivity
Employee Avoidant Parent Gender
Intelligence
Employer Public Self- Stranger Personality
Consciousness Emotional
State
Stranger Secure
09-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 7:49 PM Page 177

Personality and Nonverbal Behavior–––◆–––177

toward a fuller understanding, which future Borkenau, P., & Liebler, A. (1992). Trait infer­
researchers will achieve, of both social judg­ ences: Sources of validity at zero acquain­
ment and the delicate behavioral dance tance. Journal of Personality and Social
involved in nonverbal behavior (Patterson, Psychology, 62, 645–657.
Brunswik, E. (1956). Perception and the represen­
this volume). Table 9.1 is an effort to create
tative design of psychological experiments.
a process model of the problem, one that is
Berkeley, CA: University of California Press.
adapted from Craik (1968), who used such
Bull, P. (1983). Body movement and interper­
a model for another area of research, envi­ sonal communication. New York: Wiley.
ronmental perception. Even this does not Campbell, A., & Rushton, J. P. (1978). Bodily
encompass the range of considerations that communication and personality. British
should be taken into account in any serious Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology,
scientific study of personality and nonverbal 17, 31–36.
behavior, but perhaps other observations Carton, J. S., & Carton, E. E. R. (1998).
made in this chapter will contribute to that Nonverbal maternal warmth and children’s
goal. locus of control of reinforcement. Journal
of Nonverbal Behavior, 22, 77–86.
Cheek, J. M., & Buss, A. H. (1981). Shyness and
♦ References sociability. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, 41, 330–339.
Cleeton, G. U., & Knight, F. B. (1924). Validity
Allport, G., & Vernon, P. (1933). Studies of character judgments based on external
in expressive movement. New York: criteria. Journal of Applied Psychology, 8,
Macmillan. 215–231.
Ambady, N., Hallahan, M., & Rosenthal, R. Craik, K. H. (1968). The comprehension of the
(1995). On judging and being judged accu­ everyday physical environment. Journal
rately in zero-acquaintance situations. Journal of the American Institute of Planners, 34,
of Personality and Social Psychology, 69, 29–37.
518–529. Cronbach, L. J. (1955). Processes affecting scores
Andersen, P. A., & Guerrero, L. K. (1998). The on “understanding of others” and “assumed
bright side of relational communication: similarity.” Psychological Bulletin, 52,
Interpersonal warmth as a social emotion. 177–193.
In P. A. Andersen & L. K. Guerrero (Eds.), Duncan, S. F., Jr., & Fiske, D. W. (1985).
Handbook of communication and emotion: Interaction structure and strategy. New
Research, theory, applications, and contexts York: Cambridge University Press.
(pp. 303–329). San Diego: Academic Press. Exline, R. V. (1963). Explorations in the process
Aries, E. J., Gold, C., & Weigel, R. H. (1983). of person perception: Visual interaction in
Dispositional and situational influences relation to competition, sex, and need for
on dominance behavior in small groups. affiliation. Journal of Personality, 31, 1–20.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychol­ Exline, R. V., Ellyson, S. L., & Long, B. (1975).
ogy, 44, 779–786. Visual behavior exhibited by males differ­
Berry, D. S., & Hansen, J. S. (2000). Personality, ing as to interpersonal control orientation
nonverbal behavior, and interaction quality in one- and two-way communication
in female dyads. Personality and Social Psy­ systems. In P. Pliner, L. Krames, & T.
chology Bulletin, 26, 278–292. Alloway (Eds.), Nonverbal communication
Blackman, M. C. (2002). The employment inter­ of aggression (Vol. 2, pp. 21–51). New
view via the telephone: Are we sacrificing York: Plenum.
accurate personality judgments for cost effi­ Ferrari, J. R., & Swinkels, A. (1996). Classic
ciency? Journal of Research in Personality, cover-ups and misguided messages: Exam­
36, 208–233. ining face-trait associations in stereotyped
09-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 7:49 PM Page 178

178–––◆–––Factors of Influence

perceptions of nonverbal behavior. Journal John, O. P. (1990, August). Self-observer agree­


of Social Behavior and Personality, 11, ment: Differences between traits, differen­
27–42. ces between individuals. Paper presented at
Funder, D. C. (2003). Toward a social psychol­ the annual meetings of the American
ogy of person judgments: Implications for Psychological Association, Boston, MA.
person perception accuracy and self-knowl­ Kaitz, M., Bar-Haim, Y., Lehrer, M., & Grossman,
edge. In J. P. Forgas & K. D. Williams E. (2004). Adult attachment style and
(Eds.), Social judgments: Implicit and interpersonal distance. Attachment and
explicit processes (pp. 115–133). New Human Development, 6, 285–304.
York: Cambridge University Press. Kanki, B. G. (1985). Participant differences and
Funder, D. C., & Colvin, C. R. (1988). Friends interactive strategies. In S. F. Duncan Jr. &
and strangers: Acquaintanceship, agree­ D. W. Fiske (Eds.), Interaction structure and
ment, and the accuracy of personality judg­ strategy. New York: Cambridge University
ment. Journal of Personality and Social Press.
Psychology, 55, 149–158. Kenny, D. A. (1991). A general model of
Gifford, R. (1986). SKANS IV: Seated Kinesic consensus and accuracy in interpersonal
Activity Notation System (Version 4.1) perception. Psychological Review, 98,
[Computer software]. Victoria, BC, Canada: 155–163.
University of Victoria, Department of Kenny, D. A. (1994). Interpersonal perception:
Psychology. A social relations analysis. New York:
Gifford, R. (1991). Mapping nonverbal behav­ Guilford.
ior on the interpersonal circle. Journal Kudoh, T., & Matsumoto, D. (1985). Cross-
of Personality and Social Psychology, 61, cultural examination of the semantic
279–288. dimensions of body postures. Journal of
Gifford, R. (1994). A lens-mapping framework Personality and Social Psychology, 48,
for understanding the encoding and decod­ 1440–1446.
ing of interpersonal dispositions in nonverbal Lippa, R. (1998). The nonverbal display and
behavior. Journal of Personality and Social judgment of extraversion, masculinity,
Psychology, 66, 398–412. femininity, and gender diagnosticity: A lens
Gifford, R., Ng, C. F., & Wilkinson, M. (1985). model analysis. Journal of Research in
Nonverbal cues in the employment inter­ Personality, 32, 80–107.
view: Links between applicant qualities and Lippa, R., & Dietz, J. K. (2000). The relation
interviewer judgments. Journal of Applied of gender, personality, and intelligence to
Psychology, 70, 729–736. judges’ accuracy in judging strangers’ per­
Gifford, R., & O’Connor, B. (1987). The inter­ sonality from brief video segments. Journal
personal circumplex as a behavior map. of Nonverbal Behavior, 24, 25–43.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, McCrae, R. R., & Costa, P. T., Jr. (1989). The
52, 1019–1026. structure of interpersonal traits: Wiggins’s
Hall, E. T. (1966). The hidden dimension. New circumplex and the five-factor model.
York: Doubleday. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
Hart, A. J., & Morry, M. M. (1997). Trait infer­ 56, 586–595.
ence based on racial and behavioral cues. Mehrabian, A. (1981). Silent messages. Belmont,
Basic and Applied Social Psychology, 19, CA: Wadsworth.
33–48. Mehrabian, A., & Diamond, S. G. (1971).
Heslin, R., & Patterson, M. L. (1982). Seating arrangement and conversation.
Nonverbal behavior and social psychology. Sociometry, 34, 281–289.
New York: Plenum. Montepare, J. M., & Dobish, H. (2003). The con­
Hoffman, P. J. (1960). The paramorphic repre­ tribution of emotion perceptions and their
sentation of clinical judgment. Psychological overgeneralizations to trait impressions.
Bulletin, 57, 116–131. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 27, 237–254.
09-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 7:49 PM Page 179

Personality and Nonverbal Behavior–––◆–––179

Motowidlo, S. J., Burnett, J. R., Maczynski, J., & Scherer, K. R. (1978). Personality inference from
Witkowski, S. (1996). Predicting managerial voice quality: The loud voice of extroversion.
job performance from personality ratings European Journal of Social Psychology, 8,
based on a structured interview: An interna­ 467–487.
tional replication. Polish Psychological Schwartz, B., Tesser, A., & Powell, E. (1982).
Bulletin, 27, 139–151. Dominance cues in nonverbal behavior. Social
Mullins, D. T., & Duke, M. P. (2004). Effects of Psychology Quarterly, 45(2), 114–120.
social anxiety on nonverbal accuracy and Viteles, M. S., & Smith, K. R. (1932). The pre­
response time I: Facial expressions. Journal diction of vocational aptitude and success
of Nonverbal Behavior, 28, 3–33. from photographs. Journal of Experimental
Patterson, M. L. (1973). Stability of nonverbal Psychology, 15, 615–629.
immediacy behaviors. Journal of Experimen­ Vrij, A., Akehurst, L., & Morris, P. (1997).
tal Social Psychology, 9, 97–109. Individual differences in hand movements
Patterson, M. L. (1995). A parallel process model during deception. Journal of Nonverbal
of nonverbal communication. Journal of Behavior, 21(2), 87–102.
Nonverbal Behavior, 19, 3–29. Watson, D. (1989). Strangers’ ratings of the five
Peabody, D., & Goldberg, L. R. (1989). Some robust personality factors: Evidence of a
determinants of factor structures from surprising convergence with self-report.
personality-trait descriptors. Journal of Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
Personality and Social Psychology, 57, 57, 120–128.
552–567. Wiggins, J. S. (1973). Personality and predic­
Reichenbach, H. (1938). Experience and predic­ tion: Principles of personality assessment.
tion. Chicago: University of Chicago Press. Reading, MA: Addison-Wesley.
Riggio, H. R., & Riggio, R. E. (2002). Wiggins, J. S. (1979). A psychological taxonomy
Emotional expressiveness, extraversion, of trait-descriptive terms: The interpersonal
and neuroticism: A meta-analysis. Journal domain. Journal of Personality and Social
of Nonverbal Behavior, 26(4), 195–218. Psychology, 37, 395–412.
Rosenthal, R. (Ed.). (1979). Skill in nonver­ Young, L. (1993). The naked face: The essential guide
bal communication: Individual differences. to reading faces. New York: St. Martins Press.
Cambridge, MA: Oelgeschlager, Gunn &
Hain.
09-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 7:49 PM Page 180
10-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 5:00 PM Page 181

10
FACTORING IN AGE
Nonverbal Communication
Across the Life Span

� Robert S. Feldman
University of Massachusetts at Amherst

� James M. Tyler
Purdue University

N onverbal behaviors comprise a fundamental component of human


communication and represent the nature of communication in
its earliest form. To use these behaviors in everyday social activities
necessitates developing the ability to identify nonverbal expressions
accurately and the capacity to respond nonverbally in an appropriate
fashion (see Riggio, this volume). These skills not only serve a variety
of communicative goals and functions but are also vitally important
to the expression of emotions (see Fridlund & Russell, this volume;
Matsumoto, this volume). Beginning as early as infancy and continuing
into late adulthood, the ability to distinguish, interpret, and express
nonverbal behaviors plays an essential role in the successful navigation
of social interactions.
Typically, proficiency in nonverbal communication is described in
terms of encoding and decoding activities, enhancing the course of a
social interaction and the goals of the interactants. With respect to
decoding, skillful nonverbal communication requires that people pos­
sess a “nonverbal sensitivity” to decode other people’s expressions and

◆ 181
10-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 5:00 PM Page 182

182–––◆–––Factors of Influence

behaviors accurately (DePaulo & Rosenthal, Indeed, beginning with infancy and contin­
1982). In comparable fashion, encoding uing throughout adulthood, the ability to
nonverbal behaviors involves the expres­ differentiate, interpret, and understand
sion of internal experiences in a manner facial expressions is a central component
that others can decode accurately, conse­ in developing and maintaining relation­
quently enabling one to achieve one’s ships. Evidence shows typically that the
desired goals (Burgoon, 1994). Importantly ability to identify facial expressions accu­
for this chapter, decoding and encoding rately improves with age, although even
ability improves generally from infancy young infants distinguish among and appro­
through early adulthood, although both priately respond to a variety of expressions
may begin to deteriorate at more advanced (Walker-Andrews & Dickson, 1997).
ages (Malatesta, 1981). It is difficult to assess how infants process
The primary goal of the present chapter facial displays, with most studies necessarily
is to examine the development and progres­ using visual preference or habituation to
sion of nonverbal communication from measure the distinctions that infants make
infancy throughout late adulthood, focus­ between expressions. In broad terms, how­
ing attention on two of the most commonly ever, as early as the first few days of life,
researched channels: facial expressions infants appear to possess some instinctive
and body movements. The first section, capacity for nonverbal communication
Decoding Nonverbal Communication, (i.e., they imitate some facial expressions
involves the ability to decode facial expres­ and gestures; Field, Woodson, Greenberg,
sions, first in infants and children and sub­ & Cohen, 1982). Moreover, numerous
sequently in younger and older adults, and features of infants’ perceptual development
focuses on the ability to decode body move­ indicate sensitivity to visual, acoustic, and
ments, again focusing on infants and motion information, connoting that infants
children and then on adults. The second are responsive to environmental stimuli
section, Encoding Nonverbal Communica­ (Bertenthal, Proffitt, Spetner, & Thomas,
tion, concerns the capacity to encode non­ 1985). This early sensitivity may allow for
verbal communication, following the same the eventual recognition and discrimination
outline as the previous section. In the final of different facial expressions.
section, we describe potential directions for To determine infants’ ability to distin­
future research. guish facial expressions, researchers typically
expose them to numerous stimuli, including
a variety of posed faces. The results of such
♦ Decoding Nonverbal research indicate that within hours of birth,
Communication neonates look longer at and visually track
stimuli that appear more face-like (Johnson,
Dziurawiec, Ellis, & Morton, 1991). Infants
DECODING FACIAL EXPRESSIONS:
INFANTS AND CHILDREN also appear to differentiate between happy
and surprised expressions and between
Facial expressions are characterized happy and fearful expressions, but only if
typically as a fundamental aspect of non­ the happy expressions occur first (Young-
verbal communication, with the ability to Browne, Rosenfeld, & Horowitz, 1977).
decode such expressions playing an impor­ Moreover, infants discriminate between
tant role in the successful navigation of frowning and smiling, especially if posed by
ongoing interactions (Burgoon, 1994). their mother, and they tend to look longer at
10-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 5:00 PM Page 183

Factoring in Age–––◆–––183

joy expressions compared with neutral or animals (Feinman & Lewis, 1983). For
angry ones (Barrera & Maurer, 1981). In instance, both 12- and 18-month-olds pre­
short, this work suggests that infants may sented with a novel toy remained closer
possess an early sensitivity to recognize and to their mother when she posed a fearful
differentiate facial displays. expression, moved to a middistance for
Because neonates can discern only blurry neutral expressions, and wandered farthest
faces (i.e., they distinguish hairline, eyes, when mothers portrayed happy expressions
nose, and mouth), it is probable that they (Klinnert, 1984). Moreover, 12-month-olds
discriminate facial expressions primarily played less with a toy if their mothers
based on feature information (Ludemann, displayed negative expressions toward that
1991). By 6 months, however, visual sharp­ toy (Hornik, Risenhoover, & Gunnar,
ness improves, and infants’ sensitivity to 1987). By preschool, children match facial
contrasts enables them to make finer- expressions of emotion to narrated stories
grained affective distinctions between (Borke, 1971) and label facial displays with
expressions (Gwiazda, Bauer, & Held, basic emotions (i.e., happiness, sadness,
1989). Thus, infants as young as 3 to 4 anger, and fear) at better than chance
months old appear to discriminate between accuracy (Denham & Couchoud, 1990;
facial expressions, based initially on feature Philippot & Feldman, 1990).
differences and later on affectively relevant In broad terms, positive emotions are
information (Kestenbaum & Nelson, 1990). recognized earlier and more accurately than
As infants approach 1 year of age, their negative ones (Camras & Allison, 1985).
gaze tends to focus increasingly on the Specifically, 3- to 6-year-old-children can
facial area, and they start to process infor­ generally identify happiness more accu­
mation conveyed by facial expressions in rately than they can identify sadness and
a more cognitively complex way (McClure, anger, and they identify sadness and anger
2000). Specifically, infants move beyond better than fear, disgust, surprise, and
simply distinguishing among expressions shame (Harrigan, 1990). Moreover, when a
to assessing events and regulating behavior facial expression conflicts with a situation
in accord with information derived from (e.g., receiving a gift but displaying a sad
the facial expressions of adults. This abil­ face), preschoolers attend more to facial
ity—social referencing—is explored empiri­ expressions than to circumstances to iden­
cally by placing an infant in situations that tify the emotion (Wiggers & Van Lieshout,
involve conflicting outcomes and having 1985). For instance, 3- to 5-year-old children
their caretakers react with predetermined use facial cues to interpret whether people
cues when the infant seeks her guidance actually like a drink, particularly when the
(Saarni, Mumme, & Campos, 1998). expressions are highly exaggerated (Eskritt
In a classic study of social referencing, & Lee, 2003). In other words, the degree to
Sorce, Emde, Campos, and Klinnert (1985) which nonverbal cues are salient influences
observed that the majority of 1-year-old preschool children’s interpretation of facial
infants crossed a visual cliff when their expressions.
mothers posed happy expressions, whereas Although the speed that children
no infants crossed the cliff when their process facial expressions improves with
mothers posed fearful expressions. Similar age overall, it varies with the specific emo­
examples of social referencing have been tion. For example, happiness is detected
reported using various stimuli to trigger more quickly than anger, fear, and sad­
emotion, including toys, strangers, and ness (Boyatzis, Chazan, & Ting, 1993).
10-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 5:00 PM Page 184

184–––◆–––Factors of Influence

Moreover, even though young children when judging facial expressions, particu­
identify negative facial expressions half as larly when perceiving negative emotions
slow as adults, their accuracy improves (Levinson, Carstensen, Friesen, & Ekman,
with age. This finding is reflected most 1991). Older adults’ accuracy does improve,
strongly in sadness expressions, with however, when identifying expressions of
children’s error rates decreasing from 17% people their own age (Malatesta, Izard,
at age 7 to 8% at age 10 (DeSonneville Culver, & Nicolich, 1987). In their research,
et al., 2002). Beyond the age of 10, com­ Malatesta et al. found that, across three age
parison with adults makes it clear that groups (young to old), adults performed
children’s processing speed and accuracy best when decoding facial expressions of
increases significantly. Indeed, by adoles­ adults their own age.
cence, most can organize facial expressions In more precise terms, older compared
into six distinct categories (i.e., happiness, with younger adults rate sad, but not
sadness, surprise, anger, fear, and disgust) happy, facial expressions as significantly
and differentiate the intensity within each, more intense, and are generally less accu­
thus allowing for a more detailed assess­ rate at identifying negative and neutral
ment of facial displays (Ley & Strauss, facial expressions in comparison with
1986). Even at very low levels of intensity, positive ones (McDowell, Harrison, &
nearly 100% of older teens can recognize Demaree, 1994). In fact, older adults are
joy expressions, consistent with the “happy­ less likely to attend to negative compared
face advantage,” which shows higher over­ with neutral or positive expressions, thus
all accuracy rates for decoding happy faces decreasing the overall accuracy with which
(Hess, Blairy, & Kleck, 1997). they decode negative displays (Mather &
In short, the ability to decode facial Carstensen, 2003). Moreover, compared
expressions accurately improves with age, with younger adults, older adults’ arousal
with preschool children showing better and amygdala activation diminishes when
than chance accuracy at matching facial viewing negative facial expressions, sug­
displays with emotions. Beyond the age of gesting that older adults’ encoding of
10, children’s accuracy is nearly compara­ negative emotions may be somewhat
ble with adults’. Thus, as children get older, constrained (Mather et al., 2004).
they are able to discriminate and under­ Other research has examined this poten­
stand increasingly complex facial displays tial age-related bias toward negative expres­
across a wide range of social interactions. sions more closely. For instance, in one
study, participants viewed a pair of faces
briefly presented on a computer screen, and
DECODING FACIAL EXPRESSIONS: then one face was subsequently replaced
YOUNGER AND OLDER ADULTS with a dot (Mather & Carstensen, 2003).
Participants were instructed to respond as
Although decoding capacity appears to quickly as possible to indicate which side of
improve from infancy through early adult­ the screen the dot was on. Younger adults’
hood, few studies have examined closely the reaction times remained the same for both
interpretation and decoding of facial expres­ emotional (positive and negative) and neu­
sions in the context of adult developmental tral faces, whereas older adults responded
change over time. The evidence available sug­ significantly faster when the dot was
gests that older adults commit more decoding located where a happy rather than neutral
errors than younger and middle-aged adults face had previously been. Conversely, older
10-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 5:00 PM Page 185

Factoring in Age–––◆–––185

adults’ reaction times were significantly degraded in some respects compared with
slower when the dot was located where an younger adults.
angry or sad face had been in comparison
with a neutral face. In brief, older adults
showed a bias to attend to neutral rather DECODING BODY MOVEMENTS:
than to negative facial expressions, and to INFANTS AND CHILDREN
positive rather than neutral ones, whereas
younger adults did not exhibit these biases. Although body movement has received
In addition, older adults decoded positive considerably less research attention than
faces compared with negative ones more has facial expression, the evidence available
accurately and, in a recognition test, exhib­ indicates that people decode body move­
ited better recall for previously seen posi­ ments relatively less accurately than they
tive rather than negative faces (Mather & decode facial expressions (Ambady &
Carstensen, 2003). Moreover, similar results Rosenthal, 1992). Nonverbal communica­
emerged using a forced choice memory test tion has been investigated using various
in which participants were shown pairs of kinesic forms, including gait, geometric
faces matched for emotional expression and figures, and expressive movement perfor­
asked which face they had seen before. mances. The research reveals that various
Specifically, senior adults were most accurate body movements communicate important
at discriminating between happy-new and personal and social information.
happy-old faces, again also indicating that A variety of evidence suggests that body
they recognize positive facial expressions movements communicate emotions reli­
better than negative ones; in contrast, type ably, whether they are posed, presented
of emotional expression did not significantly through dance, or elicited during scenes por­
affect younger adult’s recognition. traying interpersonal behavior (DeMeijer,
In some respects, then, older adults tend 1989). In general, emotion-based body
not to fare as well as younger adults in movement cues are characterized by varia­
decoding the facial expressions of others. tions in form, tempo, force, and direction
This is not to say that older adults are (Montepare, Koff, Zaitchik, & Albert,
unable to interpret facial displays success­ 1999). For instance, movements character­
fully. Indeed, one study examining the ized by extreme muscle exertion in the
facial expressions of physical therapists absence of an actual physical barrier may
found that older adults decoded both posi­ lead to inferences of fear or anger (Wallbott,
tive and negative facial displays accurately 1980). Similarly, if an individual with no
(Ambady, Koo, Rosenthal, & Winograd, apparent physical reason unexpectedly
2002). Specifically, older adults judged steps backward, it may imply surprise or
therapists accurately as warm, caring, amazement. In short, body movements can
concerned, and empathic when therapists be analyzed as communications that impart
displayed positive expressions and as indif­ gross categories of emotions (e.g., like-dis­
ferent, distant, and cold when they pre­ like), with postural changes potentially
sented negative expressions. Whereas this reflecting corresponding changes in under­
study does not address age-related differ­ lying interpersonal emotion structures
ences, it suggests that older adults certainly (Ekman, 1965).
have the capacity to interpret and decode With respect to development, children
facial expressions accurately, although as are generally more skilled at decoding
other research documents, their skills are compared with encoding body movements,
10-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 5:00 PM Page 186

186–––◆–––Factors of Influence

which may suggest that decoding ability using point light displays presented 4- and
develops first (Kumin & Lazar, 1974). 6-month-old infants with a human running
Specifically, older children are more accu­ in place and a foil (the same form but con­
rate than younger children at decoding verted 180°). Using the observer format as
emotional meaning in body movements and in the previous study, the data revealed that
at using movement cues to judge emotional both infant groups were significantly above
intensity (Michael & Willis, 1968). For chance and that infants preferred the target
example, 6-year-olds who viewed a film rather than the foil. In short, evidence
with a body movement only and a head appears to indicate that sensitivity to body
movement only (masked faced) condition movement may manifest in infants between
displayed better accuracy in the body 4 and 6 months of age.
compared with the head-only condition, With respect to older children, in one
whereas 4-year-olds were less accurate in study, 5-, 8-, 10-, and 12-year-olds made
both conditions (Pendleton & Snyder, emotion attribution judgments, both in nar­
1982). Other studies indicate that even very rative form and by choosing one of four
young children—4- and 5-years-olds— emotions that best matched a particular
when asked to choose a movement (from a body movement (Van Meel, Verburgh, &
video) that corresponds to a gestural mes­ deMeijer, 1993). In both cases (narrative/
sage of a character depicted in a vignette, forced choice), 5-year-olds’ ability to iden­
display a 60% decoding accuracy, suggest­ tify emotion appropriately was significantly
ing that by age 4, children possess sufficient below older children. Although the study
capacity to understand some gestural mean­ did not involve accuracy scores specifically,
ings (Boyatzis & Satyaprasad, 1994). the results suggest that 5-year-olds failed
Other researchers suggest that sensitivity to decode emotion meaning beyond chance
to body movement manifests as early as 4 to level. Another study involving children
6 months of age (Fox & McDaniel, 1982). aged 4, 5, and 8 years and adults, however,
Using Johansson’s (1973) classic point light found that 5-year-olds did exceed chance
technique (i.e., points of lights on the main identification of happiness, sadness, and fear,
limbs and joints represent a moving person although not anger (Boone & Cunningham,
as small luminous dots moving against a 1998). Even 4-year-olds identified some
black background), Fox and McDaniel emotional meaning in body movement by
presented targets to infants (2, 4, and 6 exceeding better than chance identification
months) that consisted of biological body of sadness. Moreover, although 5-year-olds
movement patterns and foils (i.e., moving failed to identify anger beyond chance, they
dots that did not meet criteria for biological used cues that are associated with adult
movement). Independent observers made attributions of anger to make ratings of
target location judgments based on observ­ greater emotional intensity.
ing only the infants and not the stimuli. Relative to 4-year-olds, 5-year-olds
Results indicated that the judgment of appear to show increased skill in the ability
observers associated with 2-month-old to recognize emotional expression via body
infants did not differ from chance. Those movement. The most significant growth in
associated with the 4- and 6-month-olds, this ability appears to occur between the
however, made judgments significantly ages of 5 and 8, with 8-year-olds performing
better than chance, suggesting that sensitiv­ better than chance and showing little differ­
ity to body movement may appear by 4 ence compared with adults in their capacity
months of age. The same experimenters also to recognize emotions in body movements.
10-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 5:00 PM Page 187

Factoring in Age–––◆–––187

Sadness was the earliest emotion to be 5-year-olds, but not 4-year-olds, were
recognized via body movement, in contrast similar to adults.
to facial expression research, which sug­
gests that happiness is the first recognized
(Cunningham & Sterling, 1988). DECODING BODY MOVEMENTS:
Another classic study also suggests that YOUNGER AND OLDER ADULTS
attributions pertaining to social events may,
in part, be grounded in body movement pat­ Although variations in body movements
terns (Heider & Simmel, 1944). When adults convey important personal information con­
describe an animated film in which a large cerning people’s emotions, research investi­
triangle, a small triangle, and a circle move at gating the differences in adults’ ability to
various speeds along different trajectories, decode nonverbal expressions, particularly
they virtually never speak in purely geomet­ in older adults, is rather limited (Aronoff,
ric terms. Instead, they describe a series of Woike, & Hyman, 1992). Moreover, the
interpersonal events that converge on a studies that have examined these differences
common theme. Specifically, they usually tend to focus primarily on decoding facial
describe a scenario in which a woman (cir­ expressions. Consequently, with respect to
cle) is chased and trapped by a male bully age-related differences, relatively little is
(large triangle) until rescued by a second known concerning the extent to which body
helpful male (small triangle). This descrip­ movements provide cues to decode nonver­
tion style suggests patterns of movement that bal communication accurately.
when displayed by people, reveal states such The evidence available, however, sug­
as fear, aggression, and affiliation. gests that certain features involving trunk
The tendency to attribute human charac­ movement, degree of openness, vertical
teristics to the figures depicted in such direction, force, and speed serve as cues
displays has been shown to appear at to the recognition of various emotions
an early age (Kassin & Baron, 1986). For (Harrigan & Rosenthal, 1983). For
example, after viewing the film, adults example, trunk and head position and
and preschoolers produced open-ended degree of body openness are associated with
anthropomorphic descriptions, although sadness, open arm movements are linked
preschoolers did so less than adults (Berry with warmth and empathy, and leaning
& Springer, 1993). Specifically, 75% of forward and tilting one’s head are tied to
adults described the film anthropomorphi­ interpersonal positiveness (Matsumoto &
cally (i.e., attribute human states and rela­ Kudoh, 1987). Moreover, adults who were
tions, e.g., fear and protectiveness, to the shown Heider and Simmel’s (1944) film
geometric figures), whereas only about half judge violence as associated with rapid
of the children did so. The open-ended ver­ movements, gentleness with slower move­
balization task, however, may have under­ ments, and hesitation with sudden reduc­
estimated younger children’s abilities. A tions in speed (Berry, Misovich, Kean, &
second experiment using a fixed-choice Baron, 1992). Another early study that used
question format revealed clear distinctions films of adults playing a ring-toss game also
between the responses of 3- and 4-year­ revealed that people tend to readily attribute
olds and that of 5-year-olds and adults. psychological qualities to body movements
Specifically, adults and 5-year-olds had (Wolff, 1943). Specifically, adults were
higher canonical patterns of responses than characterized as weak, introverted, and pes­
3- and 4-year-olds, and the attributions of simistic if they exhibited a stooped posture,
10-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 5:00 PM Page 188

188–––◆–––Factors of Influence

shuffled their feet, or failed to bend their sadness. In contrast, few age-related differ­
knees while walking. In contrast, adults ences emerge with respect to the accurate
were viewed as carefree and easygoing if decoding of happy and neutral emotions.
they walked with a sauntering gait and as Another interesting line of research using
happy and optimistic if they briskly lifted expressive dance movements provides evi­
their feet while walking. dence that adults use body movements
Recent research using point light displays to decode particular emotions (Boone &
also supports the idea that adults frequently Cunningham, 1998). Specifically, dancers
identify emotion via body movements and enacted trunk and arm movements previ­
suggests further that older and younger ously identified as emotion cues, and
adults use similar cues to decode such move­ decoders rated each movement for 12 dif­
ments (Montepare & Zebrowitz-McArthur, ferent emotions. In broad terms, adults used
1988). For instance, angry gaits are associ­ six cues—upward arm movement, muscle
ated with heavy footedness, sad gaits with tension, forward leaning, directional
less arm swing, and proud and angry gaits changes in face and torso, tempo changes,
with greater stride length. More precisely, and how long arms are kept close to the
anger may be characterized by variations in body—with better than 90% accuracy to
velocity, force, abrupt tempo changes, and distinguish among happiness, sadness,
body angularity, sadness with contracted, anger, and fear. Moreover, as the number of
soft, and smooth movements, and happiness movement cues specifying an emotion
with expanded, action-filled, loose, fast, and increased, the frequency with which adults
somewhat jerkier movements (DeMeijer, decoded the target emotion accurately
1989). In contrast, neutral movements are increased.
related to minimal and contracted actions, Boone and Cunningham (1998) also
in addition to loose, slow, soft, and smooth found similarity between children and
movements. adults in their ability to decode body move­
Evidence also suggests some differences ments accurately. Specifically, adults and
in the ease of recognizing certain body children identified anger by increased face
movement-based emotions based on age and torso movement, happiness by increased
and development. For example, younger upward arm movements, sadness by down­
adults are more adept at decoding anger ward head movement and less muscle ten­
compared with happiness or sadness on the sion, and fear by rigid body posture with
basis of cues provided by a person’s gait head positioned up. More interestingly,
(Montepare, Goldstein, & Clausen, 1987). adults and 8-year-olds displayed better than
With respect to age differences, older adults chance accuracy on all four emotions, and
make fewer accurate identifications than minimal differences arose between adults’
do younger adults, although both groups and 8-year-olds’ ability to identify the dif­
decode emotions from body movement at ferent emotions accurately.
above chance levels (Malatesta et al., In summary, children’s capacity to dis­
1987). Specifically, older adults are less criminate, identify, and understand the
skilled than younger adults at decoding complex facial expressions of others show
negative compared with positive emotion significant age-related improvement. Like­
displays. This is particularly true for anger wise, as children get older, they become
and sadness, which older adults frequently increasingly more aware that people’s
judge as neutral, although they are more body movements may reveal interper­
accurate at identifying anger rather than sonal events, relationships, and personal
10-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 5:00 PM Page 189

Factoring in Age–––◆–––189

attributes. With respect to older adults, expression of joy, sour taste evokes expres­
although they possess the capacity to sions of sadness, and a jack-in-the-box, arm
decode facial expressions accurately, they restraint, and masked stranger situations
commit more decoding errors than younger produce surprise and, in some cases, joy­
adults. Similarly, although older adults ful expressions. Moreover, infants’ facial
identify emotions via body movements at movements display temporal patterns
a better than chance rate, they do so less involving smiling, brow knitting, and pout­
accurately than younger adults, especially ing, and all but one of the facial muscle
when decoding negative emotions. actions visible in adults can also be identi­
fied in infants (Oster, 1978).
Infants as early as 2 months of age can not
♦ Encoding Nonverbal only distinguish among and imitate happy,
Communication sad, and surprised facial expressions but also
convey facial displays of interest, smiling,
anger, and disgust (Izard & Malatesta,
ENCODING FACIAL EXPRESSIONS: 1987). For instance, around 3 to 4 weeks of
INFANTS AND CHILDREN age, infants exhibit social smiling, coalescing
into Duchenne smiles around the end of
An essential aspect underlying people’s the first month, and becoming gradually
capacity to adapt to the social world stronger over the course of the next 5 months
involves knowing how to encode and con­ (Messinger & Fogel, 1998). In addition,
trol nonverbal emotion displays. From a after gazing at another’s face, 1- to 3-month­
developmental perspective, for instance, old infants often smile spontaneously,
instructing children to follow rules or mod­ although around the fourth month smiling
ulate their emotional behavior often repre­ becomes reserved increasingly for the infant’s
sents more a request to control facial caregivers (Oster, 1978). Specifically, infants
expressions rather than the direct control often respond as a function of their mothers’
of emotions (Ceschi & Scherer, 2003). expressions of joy, sadness, and anger, and in
Examples of these expression control behav­ part, they exhibit an age-related increase in
iors can be noted in young children, gradu­ the ability to match their mothers’ facial
ally increasing in frequency throughout expressions (Izard, Fantauzzo, Castle, &
adulthood (Saarni & von Salisch, 1993). Haynes, 1995). For example, 2.5-month-old
Theorists have even argued that very young infants distinguish between and respond
infants possess some basic signaling capaci­ accurately to positive and negative conditions
ties in which facial expressions comprise a based, respectively, on their mother’s expres­
major component (Trevarthen, 1985). sions of interest and joy or sadness, anger,
Although encoding facial displays has and withdrawal (Haviland & Lelwica, 1987).
been examined across a variety of contexts, Notwithstanding this capacity to interpret
systematic investigation of infants’ facial and match their mothers’ expressions, how­
expressions is still somewhat limited (Fox ever, young infants are still prone to exhibit
& Davidson, 1986). Evidence does suggest, positive rather than negative expressions to
however, that young infants exhibit a their mothers’ negative facial displays
variety of distinct facial expressions in (Matias & Cohn, 1993).
response to diverse stimuli (Campos, Moreover, infants 2 to 8 months of age
Campos, & Barrett, 1989). For example, respond with facial expressions of frustra­
the most common response to tickling is the tion, predominantly anger, when expected
10-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 5:00 PM Page 190

190–––◆–––Factors of Influence

rewards or consequences are changed A number of studies have also examined


(Alessandri, Sullivan, & Lewis, 1990). This children’s ability to suppress their expres­
response appears consistent with data that sions to hide their actual feelings or reac­
indicate infants’ sad expressions often tions (e.g., Feldman, Tomasian, & Coats,
co-occur, in part, with anger, although 1999). One study in which 7- and 10-year­
displays of sadness alone do not represent a old children were instructed to suppress or
significant component of a frustration express smiles and laughter freely indicated
response (Camras, 1992). The clearest con­ no age differences in facial expressions,
sensus suggests that by 5 months, infants except that 7-year-olds exhibited more
experience expectancy violations as frustrat­ facial activity than 10-year-olds (Ceschi &
ing stimuli that are associated predominantly Scherer, 2003). Although in the suppres­
with facial expressions of anger (Sullivan & sion condition children did not laugh or
Lewis, 2003). Studies eliciting infants’ nega­ show smiles less frequently; they did so for
tive responses suggest that anger is the most a significantly shorter length of time than
common reaction and sadness the least com­ in the expression condition. In another
mon (Camras & Sachs, 1991). With respect study, 6- and 13-year-old children con­
to expressions of fear, researchers agree sumed a bitter drink and tried to conceal
generally that such displays do not emerge their true response by feigning expressions
until around 7 months of age (Skarin, 1977). of enjoyment. Adult judges detected the
Turning to older children, evidence duplicitous expressions of younger children
suggests that preschoolers as early as 24 successfully, but they were unable to do
months of age use particular displays of so with older children. Thus, 13-year-olds
emotion to elicit support from their care­ were better able to suppress negative expres­
giver (Buss & Kiel, 2004). In general, how­ sions on demand, while producing positive
ever, it is more difficult for them to ones simultaneously (Feldman, Jenkins,
produce facial expressions spontaneously & Popoola, 1979). Similarly, when faced
on demand compared with imitating a with implicit demands to control facial
model’s expressions. When they are able expressions (e.g., an undesirable gift in a
to produce expressions, however, young public situation), Cole (1986) found that
children find happiness the easiest to enact 6-year-olds display more socially negative
and fear the most difficult to reflect spon­ expressions than 10-year-olds, although a
taneously (Fridlund, Ekman, & Oster, similar study reported that even 4-year­
1987). Moreover, as children age they olds suppressed some negative expressions,
become progressively able to mimic spe­ albeit they seemed unaware they were
cific actions embedded in facial expres­ doing so.
sions, and by 4 years of age, they are aware
that distinct facial attributes are associ­
ated with specific emotions (Paliwal & ENCODING FACIAL EXPRESSIONS:
Goss, 1981). Indeed, when asked how they YOUNGER AND OLDER ADULTS
would express or regulate emotion dis­
plays, young children rate the use of facial Although there is abundant evidence
cues as the most common channel (Zeman that associates facial expressions of adults
& Garber, 1996). In addition, they associ­ with basic emotions, studies focusing
ate situations that evoke negative affect specifically on age-related changes in facial
with the increased use of facial displays to expressions is rather limited and mixed
signal or mask emotions. (Carstensen, Gottman, & Levinson, 1995).
10-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 5:00 PM Page 191

Factoring in Age–––◆–––191

Some research suggests, however, that older which may increase the likelihood that older
adults are less successful than younger adults’ expressions are misinterpreted at a
adults at posing facial expressions accu­ higher rate than younger adults’ expressions.
rately (Malatesta, 1981). In contrast, other More precisely, as late adulthood
studies investigating spontaneous facial unfolds, physiological characteristics of
expressions or relived emotions show no emotional experience, including somatic
age-related differences in the accuracy of activity and skin conductance, tend to
facial displays and, in some cases, even indi­ diminish (Levinson et al., 1991). In addi­
cate a more accurate expressivity in older tion, other physical changes including sur­
adults (Levinson et al., 1991). For example, face musculature and wrinkling also make
accuracy ratings for facial expressions gen­ it more difficult to interpret older adults’
erated when adults intentionally recalled an facial expressions accurately (Malatesta et al.,
extreme emotional occurrence revealed no 1987). In short, although getting older does
age-related effects (Malatesta et al., 1987). not necessarily result in decreased affectiv­
Levinson et al.’s (1991) study shows that ity, understanding older adults’ facial
older adults’ relived emotions are experi­ expressions may be obscured by wrinkles
enced just as intensely, are just as likely to and other physiological changes. These
elicit facial expressions, and just as fre­ changes create a situation in which it may
quently elicit emotion-specific autonomic be harder for other people to encode the
nervous system (ANS) activity as are younger older adults’ facial displays successfully
adults’ relived emotions. The capacity of (Malatesta, 1981).
older adults’ facial expressions to recruit Not only may older adults’ facial char­
emotion-specific ANS activity, however, acteristics make it difficult to encode
does show clear and significant age-related expressions, but stereotypic beliefs may
diminishment. also affect negatively their capacity to do
Notwithstanding the conflicting evidence so accurately. For example, Matheson
regarding age-related differences in the capac­ (1997) found that judges’ ratings of pain
ity to encode facial expressions accurately, (facial displays) were more accurate when
researchers agree that as people get older their assessing older adults than younger adults.
expressiveness gradually declines, which may Although judges estimated more pain in
lead to increased difficulty in interpreting older adults’ expressions across all response
their facial expressions. Research supporting types (i.e., genuine, posed, and baseline),
this perspective shows that older adults com­ older adults did not, however, report any
pared with younger adults have more closed more pain than younger adults. This sug­
and less expressive facial expressions, gests that judges may have been biased to
although some self-report studies indicate note pain in older adult’s facial expres­
that older adults inhibit affective displays less sions, undermining the impression that rat­
often than do younger adults (Malatesta, ings of older adults’ pain was more
1981). Studies using more objective measures, accurate. The stereotypic idea that increased
however, indicate that older adults’ facial pain is a “normal process of aging” may
expressions are more difficult to decode than explain, in part, why higher pain ratings
younger adults and are perceived as sadder were ascribed to older adult’s expressions,
(Malatesta et al., 1987). In short, researchers even though they did not necessarily report
agree that older adults’ attempts to encode more pain. This finding adds to the idea
facial expressions may be complicated by that older adults’ success at encoding facial
facial changes associated with increased age, expressions may be affected negatively by
10-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 5:00 PM Page 192

192–––◆–––Factors of Influence

factors other than ability or developmental requests, appeared as early as 9 months,


issues. with significant increments occurring up
to 15 months (Zinober & Martlew, 1985).
By comparison, pointing and showing ges­
ENCODING BODY MOVEMENTS: tures start to increase between 12 and 14
INFANTS AND CHILDREN months, with pointing used to identify ref­
erents clearly by 14 months (Masur, 1983).
As we turn to body movement, evidence In short, it appears that around 1 year of
reveals that infants’ gestural movements can age infants begin to use or elicit nonverbal
express affect, indicate requests, and draw gestural movements for communication
attention to particular objects, which may purposes.
represent a developmental step that suggests Interestingly, evidence also suggests that
an increasing awareness that communication infants’ emotion-related gestures tend to
involves both active and reactive purposes decrease after 11 months, congruent with
(Goodwyn, Acredolo, & Brown, 2000). evidence that infants begin to reduce nega­
These results suggest that even infants’ early tive affective expressions around 1 year of
gestural activity, consisting of physical age (Blake, McConnell, Horton, & Benson,
explorations of the environment, may con­ 1992). Moreover, it appears that specific
note some degree of sensitivity to body body movements are associated with posi­
movements. For example, infants look at, tive and negative affect, suggesting that
orient to, and focus on objects perceived early nonverbal communication may reveal
to be within reach before reaching for the particular emotions (Legerstee, Corter, &
objects (Haslett & Samter, 1997). More Kienapple, 1990). Specifically, infants’ pos­
specifically, 5-month-old infants reach for itive affect seems related to open-handed
close objects, lean toward distant objects, arm extensions, whereas negative affect is
and reduce reaching behavior as the distance associated with closed hands and arms
from an object increases (Field, 1976). They extended down to the sides. Overall, infants
develop awareness that reaching and leaning use a variety of gestures to express affective
movements are related and that leaning is states, to indicate requests, and to attract
simply an extension of reach. Infants at 8 attention (Goodwyn et al., 2000). They also
months recognize that leaning extends reach­ employ showing or reference movements to
ing distance. By 10 months, they grasp the direct adults’ attention to objects; specifi­
limitations of leaning, and by 12 months, cally they hold, offer, or point at objects
they start to understand that physical aids to indicate interest or desire (Messinger &
lengthen their reaching distances (McKenzie, Fogel, 1998). These movements suggest an
Skouteris, Day, & Hartman, 1993). important developmental step indicating
Moreover, longitudinal and interview an infant’s increasing awareness that the
studies indicate that 14- to 16-month-old­ communication process entails both active
infants employ a broad range of request and reactive aspects.
and attribute gestures consistently when For toddlers and preschool children, the
depicting various objects, desires, and use of gestural movements to represent
states (Acredolo & Goodwyn, 1985). For their knowledge of objects is difficult, even
example, gestures that involve offering though they can, in part, substitute hand
and reaching for objects emerge routinely movements for objects (Boyatzis &
around the 1-year mark, although one Watson, 1993). In general, young children’s
study reported that object offering, but not pantomimic representation of actions
10-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 5:00 PM Page 193

Factoring in Age–––◆–––193

progresses developmentally from, first, groups used less forceful movements to


using body parts as substitutes for objects encode sadness, happiness, and anger, used
to, later, using empty-handed gestures in less rotation and fewer and slower move­
which they simply pretend to hold an object ments for sadness and fear, and used more
(O’Reilly, 1995). For example, after watch­ upward movements for happiness and
ing an adult model teeth-brushing behavior anger. Although cues used consistently to
with an imaginary toothbrush, 3-year-old distinguish among these emotions are not
children typically used their finger to repre­ yet fully developed by age 5, children, even
sent the toothbrush, suggesting that they at this early age, have begun to differen­
still depend on substitute objects to facili­ tially use movement cues that are normally
tate their representations. In comparison, associated with how adults encode expres­
although 4-year-olds also relied on body sions of emotion.
parts more than on imaginary objects, they
did so less than 3-year-olds, suggesting a
reduced dependency on substitute objects. ENCODING BODY MOVEMENTS:
In contrast, 5-year-olds largely employed YOUNGER AND OLDER ADULTS
imaginary objects for all representations,
reflecting apparent freedom from the per­ With respect to age-related changes, very
ceptual cues of an actual substitute. little work has investigated directly adults’
Several findings also suggest that young capacity to encode body movements success­
children encode emotion via body move­ fully. One study, though, indicated minimal
ments. For instance, children are able to differences between older and younger
encode body movements accurately using female adults, except older adults used
emotion cues that are depicted in narrative more body parts as imaginary objects and
vignettes (Kumin & Lazar, 1974). In addi­ encoded fewer descriptive gestures than
tion, children are sensitive to music cues younger adults (Ska & Nespoulous, 1987).
that depict positive and negative emotions Notwithstanding the scarcity of direct evi­
(Kastner & Crowder, 1990). Specifically, in dence, a number of related research lines add
one study, 4- and 5-year-olds were prompted indirectly to our understanding of encoding
to manipulate teddy bears to express an body movements. For example, one line of
emotion conveyed by a specific music seg­ research that relates to the capacity to
ment (Boone & Cunningham, 2001). Adult encode body movements involves people’s
judges viewed the manipulations (no sound) gait patterns. Specifically, variations in
to identify the emotion. Results indicated people’s gait may strongly influence how
that children as young as 4 used body move­ others perceive the person (i.e., their traits
ments successfully to encode the specified and emotional state) and thus, with or with­
emotion. Overall, children’s encoding ability out intention, people’s body movements may
was strongest for sadness and happiness and encode information about their internal state
less developed for anger and fear. (Barclay, Cutting, & Kozlowski, 1978).
In broad terms, these results show that With respect to decoding body move­
older children communicate emotions more ments as a function of age, research
effectively than do younger children. Specifi­ indicates that older adults’ gaits compared
cally, 5-year-olds encoded happiness and sad­ with children’s, adolescents’, and younger
ness successfully, whereas 4-year-olds could adults’ gaits are perceived as reflecting less
only encode sadness, and neither group could happiness, as physically weaker, as less
encode anger or fear. In addition, both dominant, and as less sexy (Montepare &
10-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 5:00 PM Page 194

194–––◆–––Factors of Influence

Zebrowitz-McArthur, 1988). These nega­ successful than older adults at posing


tively valenced perceptions suggest that it facial expressions accurately. Older adults’
may be difficult for older adults to use body attempts to encode facial expressions
movements to encode their genuine affec­ may be complicated by age-related facial
tive state accurately. In brief, increased hip changes, however, which increase the
swaying, knee bending, lifting of feet, more likelihood that their expressions will be
steps per second, larger arm swings, looser misinterpreted. Similarly, older children are
joints, and a bouncier pattern are used clearly more effective at communicating
to decode gaits as youthful. Interestingly, emotions through body movement than are
younger adults who are perceived as having younger children, and the cues that older
a youthful versus older gait are also judged children use to encode emotions, although
as more powerful and happy. not yet developed fully, are relatively simi­
Another interesting set of studies, lar to adults’. With respect to older adults’
although using only young adults, examined capacity to encode body movements, there
whether body movements represented in is a clear need for more research. Indeed,
geometric patterns are encoded with the the most that may be said presently is that
same affective meanings that are associated it is simply difficult for many older adults
with facial movements (Aronoff et al., to use body movements to encode their
1992). This work follows classic studies in emotions accurately.
which one ethnic group’s gesture system,
considered an angular one, was contrasted
with the rounded gesture system of another, ♦ Conclusions
revealing the possibility of classifying and
distinguishing among specific types of body
movements (Efron, 1972). In particular, Examining nonverbal behavior as it devel­
Aronoff et al. (1992) found that body move­ ops from infancy throughout late adult­
ments communicated distinct affective hood provides essential information to
meanings; for instance, roundedness was understand the emergent use and interpre­
associated with warm characters and angu­ tation of such behaviors. The channels of
lar and diagonal forms with threatening communication that have received the most
characters. More precisely, “threatening” frequent study from a developmental per­
characters used diagonally oriented poses spective involve facial expressions and
three times more often than “warm” char­ body movements, although the majority of
acters, and warm characters used nearly research is centered on the former. Most
four times as many round poses as did of this work also centers on children and
threatening characters. In addition, round- young adults rather than older adults.
arm movements were associated primarily Notwithstanding these partial limitations,
with warm characters and straight-arm the available evidence recognizes broadly
movements principally with threatening that infants and adults convey personal
characters. information involving their emotions,
Overall, evidence suggests that even desires, and reactions through the distinct
infants convey a variety of facial expres­ use of facial expressions and body move­
sions. And certainly as children get older, ments. In short, how people express these
they mimic facial expressions and associate nonverbal channels, and how they use these
distinct facial characteristics with specific channels to interpret and understand
emotions accurately. Within adulthood, others, has a significant influence on the
it appears that younger adults are more quality of their social interactions.
10-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 5:00 PM Page 195

Factoring in Age–––◆–––195

FUTURE DIRECTIONS an urgent need to understand better how


the nonverbal communication process
Despite the number of strong conclu­ changes over time. In particular, it is impor­
sions that can be drawn from existing tant to clarify how nonverbal processes
research, additional work is required. One unfold during the later stages of adulthood.
empirical strategy that would provide fur­ Although research is increasing with respect
ther insight and knowledge concerning to older adults, there is still an obvious need
the development of nonverbal communi­ to examine the changes that the elderly may
cation involves the use of longitudinal experience with respect to nonverbal com­
research. To our knowledge, no studies munication and its subsequent affect on
have employed such a design. Using such a their interpersonal life.
method would, however, explicate a host of
issues pertaining to the development and
progression (i.e., age-related changes) of ♦ References
people’s nonverbal behavior, ranging from
cultural and biological aspects to more
Acredolo, L. P., & Goodwyn, S. (1985). Symbolic
clinical and health-related issues.
gesturing in language development: A case
Indeed, an important area that lacks study. Human Development, 28, 40–49.
significant investigation involves the broad Alessandri, S. M., Sullivan, M. W., & Lewis, M.
differences among cultures in the develop­ (1990). Violation of expectancy and frus­
ment of people’s nonverbal encoding tration in early infancy. Developmental
and decoding abilities. Such cultural differ­ Psychology, 26, 738–744.
ences particularly include the develop­ Ambady, N., Koo, J., Rosenthal, R., &
ment of nonverbal skills as a product Winograd, C. H. (2002). Nonverbal com­
of parental input and the learned cultural munication predicts geriatric patients’
rules or norms concerning the appropriate­ health outcomes. Psychology and Aging,
ness of showing certain expressions during 17, 443–452.
Ambady, N., & Rosenthal, R. (1992). Thin
certain situations (i.e., display rules; see
slices of expressive behavior as predictors of
Matsumoto, this volume). Although there
interpersonal consequences: A meta­
is a scarcity of direct studies that examine analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 111,
these issues, some indirect ones suggest 256–274.
that parenting factors may contribute sig­ Aronoff, J., Woike, B. A., & Hyman, L. M. (1992).
nificantly to good adult decoding skills. Which are the stimuli in facial displays
Moreover, these studies also report a posi­ of anger and happiness? Configurational
tive relation among family members’ bases of emotion recognition. Journal of
nonverbal abilities, again suggesting the Personality and Social Psychology, 62,
potential affect of parental influences 1050–1066.
(Hodgins & Koestner, 1993). Research on Barclay, C. D., Cutting, J. E., & Kozlowski, L. T.
the development of nonverbal communica­ (1978). Temporal and spatial factors in
gait perception that influence gender recog­
tion would clearly benefit from more of this
nition. Perception and Psychophysics, 23,
systematic exploration of cultural differ­
145–152.
ences, especially those related to parenting Barrera, M. E., & Maurer, D. (1981).
styles and the cultural norms that parents Discrimination of strangers by the three­
invoke when interacting with their children. month-old. Child Development, 52,
Moreover, the population of older 558–563.
adults in the United States is increasing Berry, D. S., Misovich, S. J., Kean, K. J., &
dramatically, and with that increase comes Baron, R. M. (1992). Effects of disruption
10-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 5:00 PM Page 196

196–––◆–––Factors of Influence

of structure and motion on perceptions of Campos, J. J., Campos, R. G., & Barrett, K. C.
social causality, Personality and Social (1989). Emergent themes in the study of emo­
Psychology Bulletin, 18, 237–244. tional development and emotion regulation.
Berry, D. S., & Springer, K. (1993). Structure, Developmental Psychology, 25, 394–402.
motion, and preschoolers’ perceptions of Camras, L. A. (1992). Expressive development
social causality. Ecological Psychology, 5, and basic emotions. Cognition & Emotion,
273–283. 6, 269–283.
Bertenthal, B. I., Proffitt, D. R., Spetner, N. B., Camras, L. A., & Allison, K. (1985). Children’s
& Thomas, M. A. (1985). The development understanding of emotional facial expres­
of infant sensitivity to biomechanical sions and verbal labels. Journal of Non­
motions. Child Development, 56, 531–543. verbal Behavior, 9, 84–94.
Blake, J., McConnell, S., Horton, G., & Camras, L. A., & Sachs, V. B. (1991). Social ref­
Benson, N. (1992). The gestural repertoire erencing and caretaker expressive behavior
and its evolution over the second year. in a day care setting. Infant Behavior and
Early Development and Parenting, 1(3), Development, 14, 27–36.
127–136. Carstensen, L. L., Gottman, J. M., & Levinson,
Boone, R. T., & Cunningham, J. G. (1998). R. W. (1995). Emotional behavior in
Children’s decoding of emotion in expres­ long-term marriage. Psychology and Aging,
sive body movement: Development of cue 10, 140–149.
attunement. Developmental Psychology, Ceschi, G., & Scherer, K. R. (2003). Children’s
34, 1007–1016. ability to control the facial expression
Boone, R. T., & Cunningham, J. G. (2001). of laughter and smiling: Knowledge and
Children’s expression of emotional meaning behavior. Cognition & Emotion, 17,
in music through expressive body movement. 385–411.
Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 25, 21–41. Cole, P. M. (1986). Children’s spontaneous
Borke, H. (1971). Interpersonal perception of control of facial expression. Child Develop­
young children: Egocentrism or empathy? ment, 57, 1309–1321.
Developmental Psychology, 5, 263–269. Cunningham, J. G., & Sterling, R. S. (1988).
Boyatzis, C. J., Chazan, E., & Ting, C. Z. Developmental change in the understanding
(1993). Preschool children’s decoding of of affective meaning in music. Motivation
facial emotions. Journal of Genetic Psycho­ and Emotion, 12, 399–413.
logy, 154, 375–382. DeMeijer, M. (1989). The contribution of gen­
Boyatzis, C. J., & Satyaprasad, C. (1994). eral features of body movement to the attri­
Children’s facial and gestural decoding and butions of emotions. Journal of Nonverbal
encoding: Relations between skills and with Behavior 13, 247–268.
popularity. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, Denham, S. A., & Couchoud, E. A. (1990).
18, 37–55. Young preschoolers’ ability to identify emo­
Boyatzis, C. J., & Watson, M. W. (1993). tions in equivocal situations. Child Study
Preschool children’s symbolic representa­ Journal, 20, 153–169.
tion of objects through gestures. Child DePaulo, B. M., & Rosenthal, R. (1982).
Development, 64, 729–735. Measuring the development of nonverbal
Burgoon, J. K. (1994). Nonverbal signals. In sensitivity. In C. E. Izard (Ed.), Measuring
M. L. Knapp & G. R. Miller (Eds.), emotions in infants and children. New
Handbook of interpersonal communication York: Cambridge University Press.
(2nd ed., pp. 229–285). Thousand Oaks, DeSonneville, L. M. J., Verschoor, C. A.,
CA: Sage. Njiokiktjien, C., Op het Veld, V.,
Buss, K. A., & Kiel, E. J. (2004). Comparison of Toorenaar, N., & Vranken, M. (2002).
sadness, anger, and fear facial expressions Facial identity and facial emotions: Speed,
when toddlers look at their mothers. Child accuracy, and processing strategies in
Development, 75, 1761–1773. children and adults. Journal of Clinical
10-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 5:00 PM Page 197

Factoring in Age–––◆–––197

and Experimental Neuropsychology, 24, Goodwyn, S. W., Acredolo, L. P., & Brown, C. A.
200–213. (2000). Impact of symbolic gesturing on
Efron, D. (1972). Gesture, race, and culture. early language development. Journal of
The Hague: Mouton. Nonverbal Behavior, 24, 81–103.
Ekman, P. (1965). Differential communication Gwiazda, J., Bauer, J., & Held, R. (1989). From
of affect by head and body cues. Journal visual acuity to hyperacuity: A 10-year
of Personality and Social Psychology, 2, update. Canadian Journal of Psychology,
726–735. 43, 109–120.
Eskritt, M., & Lee, K. (2003). Do actions speak Harrigan, J. A. (1990). The effects of task order
louder than words? Preschool children’s use on children’s identification of facial
of the verbal-nonverbal consistency princi­ expressions. Motivation and Emotion, 8,
ple during inconsistent communications. 157–169.
Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 27, 25–41. Harrigan, J. A., & Rosenthal, R. (1983).
Feinman, S., & Lewis, M. (1983). Social refer­ Physicians’ head and body positions as
encing at ten months: A second-order effect determinants of perceived rapport. Journal
on infants’ responses to strangers. Child of Applied Social Psychology, 13, 496–509.
Development, 54, 878–887. Haslett, B. B., & Samter, W. (1997). Children
Feldman, R. S., Jenkins, L., & Popoola, O. communicating: The first 5 years. Hillsdale,
(1979). Detection of deception in adults NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
and children via facial expressions. Child Haviland, J. M., & Lelwica, M. (1987). The
Development, 50, 350–355. induced affect response: 10-week-old infants’
Feldman, R. S., Tomasian, J., & Coats, E. J. responses to three emotion expressions.
(1999). Adolescents’ social competence and Developmental Psychology, 23, 97–104.
nonverbal deception abilities: Adolescents Heider, F., & Simmel, M. (1944). An experimen­
with higher social skills are better liars. tal study of apparent behavior. American
Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 23, Journal of Psychology, 57, 243–259.
237–249. Hess, U., Blairy, S., & Kleck, R. E. (1997). The
Field, J. (1976). The adjustment of reaching intensity of emotional facial expressions
behavior to object distance in early infancy. and decoding accuracy. Journal of Non­
Child Development, 47, 304–308. verbal Behavior, 21, 241–257.
Field, T. M., Woodson, R., Greenberg, R., & Hodgins, H. S., & Koestner, R. (1993). The ori­
Cohen, D. (1982). Discrimination and imi­ gins of nonverbal sensitivity. Personality
tation of facial expressions by neonates. and Social Psychology Bulletin, 19,
Science, 218, 179–181. 466–473.
Fox, N. A., & Davidson, R. J. (1986). Hornik, R., Risenhoover, N., & Gunnar, M.
Psychophysiological measures of emotion: (1987). The effects of maternal positive,
New directions in developmental research. neutral, and negative affective communica­
In C. E. Izard & P. B. Read (Eds.), tions on infant responses to new toys. Child
Measuring emotions in infants and children Development, 58, 937–944.
(Vol. 2, pp. 13–47). Boston: Cambridge Izard, C. E., Fantauzzo, C. A., Castle, J. M., &
University Press. Haynes, O. M. (1995). The ontogeny and
Fox, R., & McDaniel, C. (1982). The perception significance of infants’ facial expressions in
of biological motion by human infants. the first 9 months of life. Developmental
Science, 218, 486–487. Psychology, 31, 997–1013.
Fridlund, A. J., Ekman, P., & Oster, H. (1987). Izard, C. E., & Malatesta, C. Z. (1987).
Facial expressions of emotion. In A. W. Perspectives on emotional development I:
Siegman & S. Feldstein (Eds.), Nonverbal Differential emotions theory of early
behavior and communication (2nd ed., emotional development. In J. D. Osofsky
pp. 143–223). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence (Ed.), Handbook of infant development
Erlbaum. (pp. 494–554). New York: Wiley.
10-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 5:00 PM Page 198

198–––◆–––Factors of Influence

Johansson, G. (1973). Visual perception of bio­ older women. Psychology and Aging, 2,
logical motion and a model for its analysis. 193–203.
Perception and Psychophysics, 14, 201–211. Masur, E. F. (1983). Gestural development,
Johnson, M. H., Dziurawiec, S., Ellis, H., & dual-directional signaling, and the transi­
Morton, J. (1991). Newborns’ preferential tion to words. Journal of Psycholinguistic
tracking of face-like stimuli and its subse­ Research, 12, 93–109.
quent decline. Cognition, 40, 1–19. Mather, M., Canli, T., English, T., Whitfield, S.,
Kassin, S. M., & Baron, R. M. (1986). On the Wais, P., Ochsner, K., et al. (2004).
basicity of social perception cues: Develop­ Amygdala responses to emotionally val­
mental evidence for adult processes? Social enced stimuli in older and younger adults.
Cognition, 4, 180–200. Psychological Science, 15, 259–263.
Kastner, M. P., & Crowder, R. G. (1990). Mather, M., & Carstensen, L. L. (2003). Aging
Perception of the major/minor distinction: and attentional biases for emotional faces.
IV. Emotional connotations in young Psychological Science, 14, 409–415.
children. Music Perception, 8, 189–201. Matheson, D. H. (1997). The painful truth:
Kestenbaum, R., & Nelson, C. A. (1990). The Interpretation of facial expressions of pain
recognition and categorization of upright in older adults. Journal of Nonverbal
and inverted emotional expression by Behavior, 21, 223–238.
infants. Infant Behavior and Development, Matias, R., & Cohn, J. F. (1993). Are max-
13, 497–511. specified infant facial expressions during
Klinnert, M. D. (1984). The regulation of infant face-to-face interaction consistent with
behavior by maternal facial expression. Infant differential emotions theory? Developmen­
Behavior and Development, 7, 447–465. tal Psychology, 29, 524–531.
Kumin, L., & Lazar, M. (1974). Gestural Matsumoto, D., & Kudoh, T. (1987). Cultural
communication in preschool children. similarities and differences in the semantic
Perceptual and Motor Skills, 38, 708–710. dimensions of body postures. Journal of
Legerstee, M., Corter, C., & Kienapple, K. Nonverbal Behavior, 11, 166–179.
(1990). Hand, arm, and facial actions McClure, E. B. (2000). A meta-analytic review
of young infants to a social and nonso­ of sex differences in facial expression pro­
cial stimulus. Child Development, 61, cessing and their development in infants,
774–784. children, and adolescents. Psychological
Levinson, R. W., Carstensen, L. L., Friesen, W. Bulletin, 126, 424–453.
V., & Ekman, P. (1991). Emotion, physiol­ McDowell, C., Harrison, D., & Demaree, H.
ogy, and expression in old age. Psychology (1994). Is right hemisphere decline in the
and Aging, 6, 28–35. perception of emotion a function of aging?
Ley, R. G., & Strauss, E. (1986). Hemispheric International Journal of Neuroscience, 79,
asymmetries in the perception of facial 1–11.
expression by normals. In R. Bruyer (Ed.), McKenzie, B. E., Skouteris, H., Day, R. H., &
The neuropsychology of face perception Hartman, B. (1993). Effective action by
and facial expression (pp. 269–290). infants to contact objects by reaching and
Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. leaning. Child Development, 64, 415–429.
Ludemann, P. M. (1991). Generalized discrimi­ Messinger, D. S., & Fogel, A. (1998). Give and
nation of positive facial expressions by take: The development of conventional
seven- and ten-month-old infants. Child infant gestures. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly,
Development, 62, 55–67. 44, 566–590.
Malatesta, C. Z. (1981). Affective development Michael, G., & Willis, F. N. J. (1968). The devel­
over the lifespan: Involution or growth? opment of gestures as a function of social
Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 27, 145–173. class, education, and sex. Psychological
Malatesta, C. Z., Izard, C. E., Culver, C., & Record, 18, 515–519.
Nicolich, M. (1987). Emotion communi­ Montepare, J., Goldstein, S., Clausen, A. (1987).
cation skills in young, middle-aged, and The identification of emotions from gait
10-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 5:00 PM Page 199

Factoring in Age–––◆–––199

information. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, Skarin, K. S. (1977). Cognitive and contextual


11, 33–42. determinants of stranger fear in six- and
Montepare, J., Koff, E., Zaitchik, D., & Albert, eleven-month-old infants. Child Develop­
M. (1999). The use of body movements ment, 48, 537–544.
and gestures as cues to emotions in younger Sorce, J. F., Emde, R. N., Campos, J. J., &
and older adults. Journal of Nonverbal Klinnert, M. D. (1985). Maternal emo­
Behavior, 23, 133–152. tional signaling: Its effect on visual cliff
Montepare, J. M., & Zebrowitz-McArthur, L. behavior of 1-year-olds. Developmental
(1988). Impressions of people created by Psychology, 21, 195–200.
age-related qualities of their gaits. Journal Sullivan, M. W., & Lewis, M. (2003).
of Personality and Social Psychology, 55, Contextual determinants of anger and other
547–556. negative expressions in young infants.
O’Reilly, A. W. (1995). Using representations: Developmental Psychology, 39, 693–705.
Comprehension and production of actions Trevarthen, C. (1985). Facial expressions of
with imagined objects. Child Development, emotion in mother-infant interaction.
66, 999–1010. Human Neurobiology, 4, 21–32.
Oster, H. (1978). Facial expression and affect Van Meel, J., Verburgh, H., & de Meijer, M.
development. In M. Lewis & L. Rosenblum (1993). Children’s interpretations of dance
(Eds.), The development of affect (pp. 43– expressions. Empirical Studies of the Arts,
76). New York: Plenum Press. 11, 117–133.
Paliwal, P., & Goss, A. E. (1981). Attributes of Walker-Andrews, A. S., & Dickson, L. R. (1997).
schematic faces in preschoolers’ use of names Infants’ understanding of affect. In S. Hala
of emotions. Bulletin of the Psychonomic (Ed.), Development of social cognition
Society, 17, 139–142. (pp. 161–186). Hove, UK: Psychology Press.
Pendleton, K. L., & Snyder, S. S. (1982). Young Wallbott, H. G. (1980). The measurement of
children’s perception of nonverbally human expression. In W. von Raffler-Engel
expressed “preference”: The effects of non­ (Ed.), Aspects of nonverbal communication
verbal cue, viewer age, and sex of actor. (pp. 302–228). Lisse, The Netherlands:
Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 6, 220–237. Swets & Zietlinger.
Philippot, P., & Feldman, R. S. (1990). Age and Wiggers, M., & Van Lieshout, C. F. (1985).
social competence in preschoolers’ decod­ Development of recognition of emotions:
ing of facial expression. British Journal of Children’s reliance on situational and facial
Social Psychology, 29, 43–54. cues. Developmental Psychology, 21,
Saarni, C., Mumme, D. L., & Campos, J. J. 338–349.
(1998). Emotional development: Action, Wolff, W. (1943). The expression of personality.
communication and understanding. In New York: Harper.
W. Damon (Ed.), Handbook of child Young-Browne, G., Rosenfeld, H. M., &
psychology (5th ed., vol. 3), Social, Horowitz, F. D. (1977). Infant discrimination
emotional, and personality development of facial expressions. Child Development, 48,
(pp. 237–309). New York: Wiley. 555–562.
Saarni, C., & von Salisch, M. (1993). The social­ Zeman, J., & Garber, J. (1996). Display rules
ization of emotional dissemblance. In for anger, sadness, and pain: It depends on
M. Lewis & C. Saarni (Eds.), Lying and who is watching. Child Development, 67,
deception in everyday life (pp. 106–125). 957–973.
New York: Guilford. Zinober, B., & Martlew, M. (1985). Develop­
Ska, B., & Nespoulous, J.-L. (1987). Panto­ mental changes in four types of gesture in
mimes and aging. Journal of Clinical relation to acts and vocalizations from 10
and Experimental Neuropsychology, 9, to 21 months. British Journal of Develop­
754–766. mental Psychology, 3, 293–306.
10-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 5:00 PM Page 200
11-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 5:03 PM Page 201

11
WOMEN’S AND MEN’S
NONVERBAL COMMUNICATION
Similarities, Differences,
Stereotypes, and Origins

� Judith A. Hall
Northeastern University

N onverbal cues are the “front lines” of contact between people.


Information is conveyed via appearance, movement, and expres­
sion even before any words are spoken, and once the dialogue begins, a
veritable torrent of cues is at the perceiver’s disposal. People use nonver­
bal cues to draw first, and lasting, impressions about emotions, person­
ality, character, and motives, as artists and novelists have always been
aware. In turn, people know that their own nonverbal behavior is a pri­
mary vehicle by which they project their personas into the world. Thus,
nonverbal behavior is a major medium of self-presentation (DePaulo,
1992; Keating, this volume). When presenting ourselves to the world,
one of the major facets of identity to be presented is our gender.1
Presenting our gender—showing the world that we are male or female
and that we know how to behave accordingly—is thus one contributing
explanation for nonverbal gender differences. But other possible expla­
nations have also been put forth. In this chapter, I review the nature of

◆ 201
11-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 5:03 PM Page 202

202–––◆–––Factors of Influence

men’s and women’s nonverbal behavior and previous research were upheld, and many
I work to provide explanations for differ­ other differences emerged as well.
ences in those behaviors. But, lest the reader Table 11.1 shows the behaviors for
expect too much, let me emphasize that des­ which Briton and Hall (1995a) found
cription is much easier than explanation. significant differences in the ratings of men
Explaining effects as complex as those asso­ and women. The reader will probably find
ciated with gender is not done easily. these results to be unsurprising because
In the present chapter, I summarize stereo­ the beliefs are robust and probably widely
types (i.e., everyday beliefs) about nonverbal shared. Only two behaviors did not show
gender differences, the actual differences as a significant perceived gender difference:
revealed by observational research, and some “frowns at others” and “interacts facing
possible theoretical frameworks within directly.” The stereotypes suggest that
which we might understand the differences. women are seen as more expressive, involved,
The nonverbal differences to be discussed warm, fluent, and skilled in their nonverbal
include specific behaviors such as smiling and communication than are men.
gazing and also accuracy in expression via These beliefs also coincide with well-
nonverbal cues (both deliberate and sponta­ documented stereotypes that depict women
neous) and accuracy in receiving nonverbal as being more emotionally expressive and
cues (both noticing or recalling and interpret­ sensitive than men (e.g., Bem, 1974; Brody
ing cues). My overall goal is to summarize a & Hall, 2000; Johnson & Shulman, 1988;
large literature in a small space and in a way LaFrance & Banaji, 1992; Spence &
that is fair to different perspectives. Helmreich, 1978), and they also coincide
with the ways men and women describe
themselves. Fischer and Manstead (2000)
♦ Stereotypes About Nonverbal found that in all the 37 countries in which
they gathered data, women rated themselves
Gender Differences
higher on the nonverbal expression of
emotion than men rated themselves. In the
Briton and Hall (1995a) asked over 400 United States, studies have shown pre­
college students for their opinions about 19 dictable differences in men’s and women’s
nonverbal behaviors and skills in relation to self-reports of emotional expressiveness (e.g.,
gender. Each student made a separate rating Gross & John, 1998), though sometimes the
for men and for women on a 1–10 scale. For magnitude of the difference varies with the
some behaviors, it was easy to predict the specific emotions being rated (Brody & Hall,
beliefs that would emerge, based on previ­ 2000). Thus, stereotypic beliefs about the
ous stereotype or self-rating studies: People nonverbal behavior of men and women are
would believe men to be louder, to have less well developed and widely shared.
variable voices, to smile less, to gaze less, to
be less emotionally expressive, to use hand
and arm gestures less, and to be less skilled ♦ What Are the Actual
in encoding (sending) and decoding (judg­ Nonverbal Differences
ing) nonverbal cues (Korzenny, Korzenny, Between Men and Women?
& Sanchez de Rota, 1985; Kramer, 1977;
Rosenkrantz, Vogel, Bee, Broverman, &
Broverman, 1968; Zuckerman & Larrance, In 1984, I published a meta-analysis of
1979).2 Indeed, all the predictions based on nonverbal gender differences that covered
11-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 5:03 PM Page 203

Women’s and Men’s Nonverbal Communication–––◆–––203

Table 11.1 Beliefs About Female and Male Nonverbal Behavior

Women Rated Higher Than Men Men Rated Higher Than Women

Smiles at others Touches oneself


Has an expressive face Has restless feet and legs
Gazes at others Speaks with dysfluencies
Uses hand gestures Speaks loudly
Interacts closely Uses “um” and “ah”
Has an expressive voice Interrupts others
Laughs
Has encoding (expression) skill
Pays attention to cues
Recognizes faces
Has decoding skill
SOURCE: Summarized from Briton and Hall (1995a).

specific behaviors as well as communication on the Hall (1984) review. Children are
skills. Effects for both the gender of the defined as 2 through 12 years, and adults
person whose nonverbal behavior is mea­ are defined as college-age and older.
sured and the gender of the interaction part­ Adolescents’ behavior is mentioned when
ner were reviewed (though the latter had possible, but there is much less research on
fewer results than the former), based on this age group. Because the present review
studies published in English up through is necessarily a simplification, the reader is
1983. This was the first quantitative treat­ referred to Hall and the other cited works
ment of this subject and it has stood the for discussion of many additional method­
test of time rather well even though much ological issues and qualifying points.
research has been published since (its conclu­
sions also concurred well with a qualitative
review done by Vrugt & Kerkstra, 1984). NONVERBAL BEHAVIORS
In the following sections, the main con­
clusions of the 1984 review (omitting those Smiling. The 1984 analysis found that
studies that included infants) are summa­ when adolescents and adults were inter­
rized and updated as space allows, under acting with others, there was clear evidence
the two main headings of “nonverbal based on 23 studies that women smiled
behaviors” and “nonverbal skills.” The more than men. Gender differences in smiling
number of studies available for different were negligible, however, when people were
behaviors and skills varies dramati­ observed alone. Based on 20 studies among
cally; because of space limitations, the pre­ children, even when observed in a social
sent chapter focuses on the most well- situation, there was also little difference.
substantiated results. Results that are not There is also a well-documented gender dif­
given a citation to a specific study are based ference in posed photographs. In one such
11-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 5:03 PM Page 204

204–––◆–––Factors of Influence

study, Dodd, Russell, and Jenkins (1999) find this but found the exact opposite
examined over 16,000 school yearbook (Halberstadt, Hayes, & Pike, 1988).
photographs and found, as did Hall (1984), Another measure of apparent sincerity is
that the younger ages (kindergarten through the Duchenne smile (Ekman, Davidson, &
Grade 3) showed little difference, but the Friesen, 1990), the term used to describe
gender difference was pronounced after the combination of upturned mouth (zygo­
that (including among college students and maticus muscle) and crinkles around the
faculty or staff). The generality of the effect eyes (orbicularis oculi muscle). This con­
is evident in a study that found male and figuration has been shown in a number of
female physicians interacting with their studies to reflect felt positive affect more
patients to show a marked difference in than does the upturned mouth alone. Hecht
smiling (Briton & Hall, 1995b). and LaFrance (1998) measured Duchenne
LaFrance, Hecht, and Levy Paluck’s (“enjoyment”) smiles and non-Duchenne
(2003) recent meta-analysis included over (“social”) smiles separately and found that
400 male-female comparisons. They con­ women displayed more of both kinds than
cluded, consistent with Hall (1984), that men. Thus, the idea that women’s expres­
women smiled more than men, that there sions are phony is not well supported by the
was no gender difference in the absence of available research.3
social interaction, that the difference was Hall and Halberstadt (1986) examined
greatest in same-gender pairs, and that social moderating factors for Hall’s (1984) adult
tension was associated with a bigger differ­ studies and found that when the circum­
ence. They also found numerous other mod­ stances of observation were rated as more
erators, including the following: The gender anxiety provoking, the gender difference was
difference was especially large when people larger. Whether women increased their smil­
were aware of being observed, were under ing under such circumstances, or men reduced
explicit instructions to get acquainted, were theirs, could not be ascertained. Finally, Hall
engaged in self-disclosure, and were adoles­ (1984) also found evidence (as did Hinsz &
cents. The latter result is especially interest­ Tomhave, 1991) that women received more
ing considering the overall lack of a gender smiles from others than men did.
difference for children. It seems that adoles­
cence, a time of acute awareness of gender Facial Expressiveness. Research on children’s
norms, is also the time when boys’ and girls’ facial behavior is scanty and inconclusive,
smiling is most discrepant. but five out of six published studies
One potentially important moderator— reviewed by Hall (1984) that measured
the apparent sincerity of the smile—has been objectively the extent or frequency of facial
examined in only very limited fashion, but movements in adults found that women’s
it is important because of the stereotype faces were significantly more expressive
that women use smiles in an emotionally than men’s. It is not clear whether expres­
false way. Bugental, Love, and Gianetto siveness means that more emotions are
(1971), in a much-cited study, found that being shown or that there is simply more
the positivity of women’s facial expressions movement overall.
was less concordant with the positivity of
their spoken words than was the case for Gazing. Twenty-five studies of children and
men. On the basis of this finding, the authors 61 studies of adults were located that mea­
called women’s smiles “perfidious.” A sured overall gazing during conversation
larger, more recent, study not only did not (frequency or duration). Females in both
11-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 5:03 PM Page 205

Women’s and Men’s Nonverbal Communication–––◆–––205

age groups engaged in higher levels of the dominance or power construct (a topic to
gazing than males did, with the differ­ be taken up in a later section).
ence being somewhat larger for adults.
Subsequent research continues to find more Head, Hand, and Arm Gestures. Hand
gazing by women (e.g., McCormick & gestures, as discussed here, are the fluid
Jones, 1989, who observed couples in bars). hand movements that accompany speech,
Hall’s review (1984) also found that women rather than the discrete “emblematic” hand
received more gaze than men did, making movements that can substitute for words
for a pattern also seen for smiling: The least (such as the A-okay sign or the slit-throat
gaze occurred when men interacted with sign; Knapp & Hall, 2005). In terms of
men, and the most gaze occurred when speech-dependent hand movements and
women interacted with women. other movements of the head and arms,
A conspicuous moderator of women’s Hall (1984) concluded, based on 15 studies,
elevated gaze tendencies was uncovered in that women engage in more expressive
a series of studies by Aiello (e.g., Aiello, movements during conversation than men
1977). As the physical distance between do. This is in line with women’s greater
conversing interactants increased, men’s facial expressivity noted above.
gazing increased, as predicted by theories
of intimacy compensation (Argyle & Dean, Body Movements and Positions. The ways
1965). Women’s gazing fell off abruptly, people sit, stand, and move their bodies
however, after a separation of about six feet, have often been noted to distinguish men
creating a reversal in the gender difference. from women. Men were observed to be
It seems that women’s heightened gazing is more restless (e.g., foot and leg move­
tied closely to the higher level of involve­ ments, body shifts, fidgeting; 14 results),
ment produced by closer interactions. more expansive (e.g., wide knees, legs, or
The “visual dominance ratio” is a particu­ arms; six results), more relaxed (e.g., trunk
lar pattern of gazing during interaction, lean, reclining, feet on table; six results),
which refers specifically to the ratio of the and less involved (e.g., nods, forward lean;
percentage of gaze while speaking to the per­ 18 results) than were women (Hall, 1984).
centage of gaze while listening. A higher ratio Finally, based on 11 results, Hall (1984)
indicates that the person is relatively more concluded that women engaged in more
likely to gaze while speaking compared to lis­ self-touching during interaction than men
tening. The visual dominance ratio is higher did, a result also found by McCormick and
in more dominant or powerful persons (hence Jones (1989) for couples observed in bars.
its name), as documented in numerous stud­ Interesting, for this behavior, Briton and
ies (reviewed by Dovidio & Ellyson, 1985). Hall’s (1995a) stereotype data were wrong.
Dovidio, Ellyson, Keating, Heltman, and
Brown (1988) measured the visual domi­ Interpersonal Touch. Hall (1984) and Stier
nance ratio in cross-gender interactions in and Hall (1984) concluded that, in general,
which neither person had a power or exper­ women were more likely to touch others
tise advantage over the other. In these inter­ than men were, based on 20 studies of
actions, women displayed a lower visual children and 18 studies of adults. This
dominance ratio than did men. Although it is result concurs with Willis and Rinck’s
not clear whether this is an effect of the (1983) study, in which college students
actor’s or the target’s gender, it is nevertheless kept logs of touches immediately after they
interesting because of its theoretical links to occurred, and also with later studies of
11-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 5:03 PM Page 206

206–––◆–––Factors of Influence

adults in public places (McCormick & are more likely to initiate nonhand touches
Jones, 1989; Willis & Dodds, 1998). In an with men (e.g., DiBiase & Gunnoe, 2004;
additional study that combined several age Hall & Veccia, 1990). Notably, all this
groups, Berkowitz (1971) observed touches research has been on friendly or at least
in public in six areas of the world. The innocuous touches. For more aggressive
overall tendency of women to touch more touches, the gender differences are unknown,
than men was significant and evident in 23 though one might surmise that they vary with
of the 24 groups observed (four age groups the gender of the target and the specific kind
across six areas of the world). of aggressive touch (Archer, 2002).
Interpersonal touch is, however, a com­
plex topic both methodologically and theo­ Interpersonal Distance and Facing Orienta­
retically, and several moderating variables tion. Hall (1984) summarized 28 studies of
are evident in its use. First, the preponder­ children and 59 studies of adults in which
ance of findings showing more female- interpersonal distance was observed in rela­
initiated touch may be especially dependent tively nonreactive ways (such as unobtru­
on the high levels of touch typically shown sive observation in public). In both age
between females. Stier and Hall (1984) groups, males established larger interper­
and, later, Montemayor and Flannery sonal distances than females did, with the
(1989) noted that the most unambiguous effects being more pronounced in adults
differences were between female-female and than in children. Separate analyses of two
male-male dyads. Because of the cultural other methodologies—staged (where par­
norms and values prevailing in some coun­ ticipants are asked to set comfortable dis­
tries, perhaps especially the United States, tances to others in the laboratory) and
men may be particularly reluctant to touch projective (where participants position
other men in most public settings. humanoid figures on a board or in a
Apart from the gender composition of the picture)—show similar though weaker
dyad, several other moderators have been results; specifically, little gender difference
discussed. Sports settings show a disinhibi­ among children and larger distances for
tion of male-male touch. The type of touch men among adults. For the nonreactive
matters as well, with handshaking being studies, it was also possible to look at the
much more notable between men than gender of target effects. In five studies of
between women (Hall, 1984). Evidence also children and 20 studies of adults, it was very
suggests that when touch in cross-gender evident that males were approached less
dyads is observed, with the question being closely than were females. As was the case
whether the man touches the woman more or for smiling and gazing, the separate actor
vice versa, both the type of touch and the type gender and target gender effects combined
of relationship matter. In younger, less com­ such that distance was smallest in female-
mitted couples, there is a stronger tendency female dyads and largest in male-male dyads.
for the male to touch the female (as in Regarding directness of orientation, five
Henley, 1973), whereas in older and more studies of children and 18 studies of adults
established couples the balance of touching is indicated that females oriented their heads
likely to be shifted to the female (e.g., Hall & and bodies more directly toward a con­
Veccia, 1990; Willis & Dodds, 1998). Several versation partner than males did. Several
studies also suggest that men are more likely studies that used naturalistic observation or
to touch women with their hand, but women confederates who invaded someone’s space
11-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 5:03 PM Page 207

Women’s and Men’s Nonverbal Communication–––◆–––207

also found gender differences in preferred ACCURACY IN NONVERBAL


spatial orientation that varied with the COMMUNICATION
identity of the other person (e.g., Fisher
& Byrne, 1975). When the other was an Sending Nonverbal Cues. A person can be
acquaintance, women preferred side-by­ an accurate expressor in two ways: first,
side positions, and men preferred face-to­ by being spontaneously expressive in a way
face positions, but these preferences were that makes one’s states (such as emotions)
reversed when the other was a stranger. readily apparent to others and second, by
being able to deliberately convey certain
Voice Qualities and Vocal Behaviors. The states so that others can judge them accu­
number of available studies assessing rately. These two kinds of accuracy, which
vocalics is typically smaller than for visible are called spontaneous and posed, are
nonverbal behaviors, but Hall (1984) con­ positively correlated (Cunningham, 1977;
cluded that the following vocal gender dif­ Zuckerman, Hall, DeFrank, & Rosenthal,
ferences were evident. Men’s voices tend to 1976; see Riggio, this volume). Hall (1984)
be louder and lower pitched and to contain concluded, based on 49 studies, that females
more speech disturbances (e.g., repetitions, were more accurate on both definitions of
omissions, stutters, sentence incomple­ accurate sending. Studies of adults showed
tions); men also tend to use fewer listener somewhat larger effects than did studies
responses such as “uh-huh” and “I see” of children. Furthermore, the difference
while listening to another speak (called between males and females was most pro­
back-channel responses) and more filled nounced for visible cues and, in fact, nearly
pauses (filled with sounds such as “uhh”). nonexistent for vocally conveyed cues.
A later study (Viscovich et al., 2003) found
that men’s voices were less variable in pitch Noticing and Recalling Nonverbal Cues.
as well. Research has also found that men’s Hall (1984) reviewed several studies that
voices were rated as different from suggested that women noticed or were
women’s on a number of global qualities— more influenced by nonverbal cues than
for example, less pleasant, more dominant, men were. Recently, Horgan, Schmid Mast,
less enthusiastic, and less anxious. Studies Hall, and Carter (2004) and Schmid Mast
based on listeners’ global impressions may and Hall (2006) documented in eight stud­
be biased by the listeners’ stereotypes, thus ies that women were more accurate than
reducing their value as evidence on actual men in remembering the appearance (cloth­
gender differences. ing, hair, and other physical characteristics)
of others after having live interactions and
Summary. This brief review paints a picture after watching a videotape of people inter­
that most readers will find very familiar: acting. Also, women’s appearance was eas­
Women are observed to be more direct, ier to remember than was the appearance
animated, and warm in interpersonal inter­ of men. Hall, Murphy, and Schimd Mast
action than are men. Conversely, men (in press) demonstrated in two studies that
behave in a less involved manner. These women were also more accurate than men
differences are closely associated with stereo­ at remembering dynamic nonverbal cues of
types of how men and women relate to others such as hand gestures, nodding, and
others nonverbally. I turn now to accuracy in shrugging, based on viewing live or
sending and receiving nonverbal cues. recorded interactions.
11-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 5:03 PM Page 208

208–––◆–––Factors of Influence

Women’s advantage in remembering said in a later section). Furthermore, the


appearance and visible movement is remi­ difference between men’s and women’s
niscent of a related body of literature on scores was just as strong in the non-U.S.
accuracy of recognizing faces. Hall (1984) groups as in the U.S. groups, and the differ­
concluded, based on 28 studies of adults, ence was robust from third grade up into
that women recognized faces better than adulthood.
men, consistent with two previous reviews Later studies have continued to confirm
by other authors; also, several studies that women’s accuracy is higher than men’s
showed that females associated names with for judging the meanings of nonverbal cues
faces more accurately than men did. in a variety of domains and contexts: for
Shapiro and Penrod (1986) also did a meta­ judging (a) personality traits (Ambady,
analysis of gender differences in face recog­ Hallahan, & Rosenthal, 1995; Lippa &
nition, however, and concluded there was Dietz, 1998; Vogt & Colvin, 2003), (b)
no difference. intelligence (Murphy, Hall, & Colvin,
2003), (c) facial expressions of emotion in
Decoding Nonverbal Cues. Many studies six nations (Biehl et al., 1997), (d) vocal
have examined gender differences in accu­ expressions of emotion in nine nations
racy of interpreting the meanings of non­ (Scherer, Banse, & Wallbott, 2001), (e)
verbal cues. The first meta-analysis of this facial emotions judged by children and ado­
topic was by Hall (1978), who located 75 lescents (meta-analysis by McClure, 2000),
studies of children through adults who were and (f) emotions conveyed by the eyes and
asked to identify the meanings of nonverbal brows alone (Baron-Cohen, Wheelwright,
cues in persons presented typically via pho­ Hill, Raste, & Plumb, 2001). Women have
tos, films, videos, or audiotape. Females’ also been shown to have more explicit
advantage was very evident and was rela­ knowledge of nonverbal cues, as demon­
tively constant across a variety of cultures strated on a paper-and-pencil test of knowl­
and age groups of perceivers, as well as edge of nonverbal cue meanings and usages
across the gender of the people whose cues (Rosip & Hall, 2004).
were judged. The greater accuracy for Some domains, however, show markedly
females tended to be more pronounced for reduced or even nonexistent gender differ­
visible than for vocal cues. Hall (1984) ences. A meta-analysis evidenced that
located an additional 50 studies for which women did not excel over men in judging
the same conclusions held. deception (Zuckerman, DePaulo, &
One particular test of decoding nonver­ Rosenthal, 1981), and several studies have
bal cues—the Profile of Nonverbal Sensiti­ now suggested that women are no more
vity (PONS test; Rosenthal, Hall, DiMatteo, accurate than men in judging status or
Rogers, & Archer, 1979)—was given to dominance from nonverbal cues (e.g.,
133 groups of many ages in 11 different Dovidio & Ellyson, 1982; Schmid Mast
countries. Only a small number of these & Hall, 2004b). Both of these results
groups were included in the two reviews are interesting, in part because deception
described in the preceding paragraph so that and status (dominance) are arguably less
the literature review would not be over­ female-stereotypical than are most of the
weighted by one testing instrument. For the other constructs measured in nonverbal
133 PONS test samples, the average magni­ sensitivity tasks (e.g., emotions, relation­
tude was exactly the same as in the preced­ ships, appearance). Perhaps future research
ing summaries (more on magnitude will be will uncover not only areas of gender
11-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 5:03 PM Page 209

Women’s and Men’s Nonverbal Communication–––◆–––209

similarity in nonverbal skill, but even areas I used the correlation metric because it
in which men’s accuracy exceeds women’s. has the broadest comparability across
different domains of research (Lipsey &
Wilson, 2001; Rosenthal, 1991).4
♦ How Accurate To assess accuracy of the gender differ­
Are the Stereotypes? ence beliefs, Briton and Hall (1995a) corre­
lated the effect sizes for the belief data
shown in Table 11.1, separately for male
Thus far, I have worked to document and female perceivers, with the effect sizes
beliefs about, and actual differences in, for the actual differences in Hall (1984). The
men’s and women’s nonverbal behavior resulting correlations were .74 for the aver­
and skill. A comparison between the stereo­ aged group of female perceivers and .68
types and the actual gender differences for the averaged group of male perceivers,
indicates there can be no doubt that the meaning that both men and women held
stereotypes are overwhelmingly correct in very accurate beliefs about the patterning of
substance. There are at least two ways to the nonverbal gender differences. Hall and
appraise the accuracy of people’s beliefs in Carter (1999) also gathered stereotype data
more precise quantitative terms. One way is about nonverbal gender differences and
to make a direct comparison (e.g., to gather were able to duplicate this group accuracy
people’s beliefs about the rate of hand ges­ result almost exactly. In that study, accuracy
turing by men and women during conversa­ was significantly higher for female than for
tion and then to compare this with the male perceivers. Thus, women possessed
actual rate). In most research, however, more accurate knowledge than men about
people are asked to state their beliefs on an how the genders differ in their nonverbal
arbitrary rating scale that cannot be com­ behavior.
pared directly with measured behaviors. A
second way is to correlate the beliefs with
the actual differences, a method that does APPRAISING THE SIZE
not require identical metrics for both beliefs OF THE DIFFERENCES
and actual differences. This method, called
sensitivity correlation (Judd & Park, 1993) How big are nonverbal gender differ­
or profile correlation (Vogt & Colvin, ences? This is an important question that
2003), captures the covariation between can be addressed in several different ways.
believed and actual differences across One way is to ask whether the differences
behaviors. are big enough to be noticeable in everyday
To apply this method, the actual differ­ life. According to Cohen (1988), an effect
ences between males and females must be size correlation (r) of about .25 is large
quantified on a common metric. In my enough to be visible to the naked eye, assum­
meta-analysis (Hall, 1984), I used the ing repeated exposure. The average effect
point-biserial Pearson correlation between size of the nonverbal gender differences is in
gender (coded male = 0, female = 1) and this range (.20 and above) for expression
the behavior in question, although other accuracy, smiling, facial expressiveness,
indices of effect size are available, notably decoding skill, recall of appearance, gazing
Cohen’s d, defined as the difference (both gazing and being gazed at), interper­
between two means divided by their within- sonal distance (both initiated and received),
group standard deviation (Cohen, 1988). body restlessness, bodily expansiveness,
11-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 5:03 PM Page 210

210–––◆–––Factors of Influence

bodily expressiveness (mainly hand ges­ of over 300 meta-analyses on the effects of
tures), self-touching, speech errors, and interventions in education and psychology,
filled pauses. The fact that the observed dif­ finding an overall effect of r = .24. The non­
ferences are this large may help to explain verbal gender differences are comparable
why stereotypes about the differences are with these overall effects.
so accurate. Richard et al.’s (2003) analysis of gender
But there are other ways to ask the mag­ effects (for a long list of psychological
nitude question. One is in terms of the vari­ variables) revealed an average correlation
ance explained by the gender differences. of .12, consistent with a similar quantita­
In other words, in an average study, how tive summary of gender difference meta­
much of the variation in the observed analyses done by Hall (2006), suggesting
behavior is accounted for by gender? The that the nonverbal gender differences are
answer is “not much.” Correlations in the larger on average than other gender differ­
range of .20 to .40 account for only small ences reported in the social-personality lit­
amounts of variance (4%–16%). This erature. Finally, Hall (2006) compiled a
means that in absolute terms, very little of list of other correlates (besides gender) for
the variation in nonverbal behavior and nonverbal sensitivity and smiling and found
skill is due to gender. Another way to that gender correlated with nonverbal sen­
express these differences is to examine the sitivity as strongly (on average) as other
distributions for men and women. With variables did, and it correlated with smiling
effects in the range we find in this area of only slightly more weakly than other vari­
research, the male and female distributions ables did. Thus, within a relative frame­
will overlap a great deal. To be more exact, work, gender differences in nonverbal
only about 30% of their joint distributions communication are within the range typical
will not be overlapped (Cohen, 1988). This of social-personality psychology in general,
means that many men will exhibit behavior larger than other gender differences on
within the range of “women’s behavior” average, and not very different from other
and vice versa. For example, many men will correlates of the nonverbal behaviors.
smile more than the average woman.
Caution must be taken with the “magni­
tude” approach, however. Before we con­ WHERE DO THE
clude that gender is only a trivial predictor DIFFERENCES COME FROM?
of nonverbal behavior, we must ask a
broader and more relativistic question: This most complex question has no
How do the nonverbal gender differences simple answer, and before starting, several
compare to other social psychological important observations must be made. First,
effects, other gender differences, and other because nonverbal behavior is often ambigu­
correlates of the same nonverbal behaviors? ous in meaning (Knapp & Hall, 2005), and
Within these frames of reference, the non­ studies reporting gender differences hardly
verbal gender differences fare rather well. ever ascertain what the behaviors mean to
Richard, Bond, and Stokes-Zoota (2003) the people observed, it is very difficult to
performed a summary of over 450 meta­ know what interpretations to place on the
analyses of social and personality phenom­ gender differences in terms of intentions,
ena (involving over 25,000 studies and 8 motivations, correlated states, and so forth.
million participants). The overall effect was Second, it is not possible to do controlled
an average correlation of .21. Lipsey and experiments that could lead to confident
Wilson (1993) did a nonoverlapping review inferences about cause, given that we
11-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 5:03 PM Page 211

Women’s and Men’s Nonverbal Communication–––◆–––211

cannot assign people randomly to be either strong and high-status people behave.
male or female. Third, because gender is a Women’s behaviors, therefore, are tied
potent social variable whose impact on more to lower social status than to being
behavior begins at birth, it is likely not pos­ female per se. Furthermore, the subtle and
sible to pinpoint “the” cause of a gender nonconscious nature of nonverbal commu­
difference. Indeed, considered in a wider nication provides an effective means not
historical perspective, social expectations, only to display power, status, and domi­
modeling, and force of habit may keep a nance but also to enforce and maintain
gender difference alive even if the circum­ domination over women. Henley’s argu­
stances that were the original “cause” are ment had a great deal of influence because
no longer operative. Fourth, there is no it was consonant with the feminist psychol­
reason to assume that one unified theory is ogy zeitgeist and it unified many findings
required to explain the plethora of nonver­ under one parsimonious explanation (Vrugt,
bal gender differences that exist. Although 1987). As a consequence, it has been pre­
a single grand explanation may succeed sented—often as fact rather than hypothe­
eventually in explaining all or most of the sis—in social psychology and psychology of
differences, it is also possible that there are women textbooks up to the present day.
many different causes that either coexist in Furthermore, Henley’s (1977, 1995) ideas
producing a given gender difference or were more provocative than systematic, in
apply to different behaviors. Below, I offer that she did not claim to put forth a fully
a quick summary of several theoretical developed theory that could account for all
positions concerning possible causes for behaviors, all moderating factors, and all
gender differences in nonverbal behavior. definitions of the power construct. And, of
Andersen (1998) proposed a biologically course, at the time of her seminal work
based explanation for women’s advantage in there was much less empirical research
decoding nonverbal cues. Women may, for available on which to base a comprehensive
example, have evolved to be more sensitive theory. From the perspective of some 30
to nonverbal cues than men because of years later, it is now possible to fill in a cru­
advantages in terms of survival of offspring. cial empirical gap.
Some nonverbal differences (e.g., gazing) do Henley’s argument rested on three inter­
appear in early infancy, which may support locking claims: that men have more power,
an inborn mechanism; on the other hand, dominance, and status than women; that
social learning may occur even within the men and women differ in their nonverbal
first year of life, and furthermore, some dif­ communication; and that people high ver­
ferences only appear years later (e.g., smil­ sus low in power, dominance, and status
ing). Perhaps because it is difficult to prove differ in their nonverbal communication in
the biological case, or because sociocultural the same way that men and women differ.
explanations are more compelling to social The first two of these claims are, broadly
researchers, most writers have pursued an speaking, not in dispute. Interestingly, how­
approach based on social factors. ever, the third claim was assumed to be true
Henley (1977, 1995) made the most without much empirical substantiation, and
ambitious theoretical proposition when she authors since Henley have not often ques­
argued that nonverbal gender differences tioned this assumption. Hall, Halberstadt,
are rooted in gender differences in power, and O’Brien (1997) and Hall, Coats, and
status, and dominance. Women, she argued, Smith LeBeau (2005) undertook an eval­
behave the way weak and low-status people uation of this third claim by conducting
behave, whereas men behave the way meta-analyses of the relations of nonverbal
11-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 5:03 PM Page 212

212–––◆–––Factors of Influence

behavior to power, dominance, and status produced those social norms in the first
(for simplicity, I will just say “power”). place. Thus, power would be a real but
If Henley’s (1977, 1995) power-based more distant cause. The difficulty of proving
theory has viability, the gender differences this “root cause” argument reduces its
should parallel the power differences.5 Such value, however.
parallelism is evident for the visual domi­ Setting the power hypothesis aside for
nance ratio, bodily openness, loudness of the moment, what explanations remain? As
voice, interruptions, and back-channel just noted, societal norms, roles, and expec­
responses. Some nonverbal behaviors that tations are clearly consistent with the
are more prevalent in persons with higher observed gender differences. To review the
power, however, are actually more charac­ literature on gender roles is clearly beyond
teristic of women (more facial expressive­ the scope of this chapter, but it is clear that
ness, smaller interpersonal distances, greater women in many cultures are expected to
decoding skill, greater encoding skill),6,7 be competent in the social domain, indeed
and some nonverbal behaviors that are responsible for the positive outcomes of
more characteristic of women do not show social interaction, and there are well-
overall power effects (smiling, gazing, nod­ established gender norms on dimensions
ding, gesturing, and direct orientation). such as communion, relationship orienta­
Thus, the parallelism required by the tion, concern with feelings, social division of
power-based theory is very inconsistent. labor, and the experience and expression of
Though as an all-encompassing theory affect (especially positive affect; Alexander
Henley’s (1977, 1995) argument appears & Wood, 2000; Clancy & Dollinger, 1993;
not to be viable, future research may still Cross & Madson, 1997; Eagly, 1987; Jansz,
succeed in identifying circumstances and 2000). Nonverbal cues and skills are relev­
behaviors in which power underlies gender ant to such norms and expectations. The
differences. Schmid Mast and Hall (2004b), existence of norms and expectations creates a
for example, found that smiling was not chicken-and-egg relation with observed dif­
related overall to either manipulated power ferences, however: The one brings about and
or trait dominance in a laboratory study, reinforces the other in a cycle.
but that smiling was in fact elevated in one In the most detailed theoretical state­
circumstance: among women (not men) ment to date, Hall, Carter, and Horgan
who wanted, and received, assignment to (2000) offered a conceptual analysis of
the lower power role. This suggests that how various factors may impinge on
future studies could profit from looking women’s smiling, expression skill, and
carefully at moderating factors. interpersonal sensitivity. To illustrate with
If power is not the immediate cause of the the case of smiling, we identified numerous
gender differences, what is? One possibility potential factors that could serve to elevate
is that power is the root cause of whatever is women’s smiling. At the heart of the analy­
the more proximal cause. For example, sis is the experience of positive affect and
although social norms that encourage the facial feedback process whereby smil­
women to be expressive, warm, approach­ ing itself produces positive affect (Strack,
able, interpersonally involved, sensitive, and Martin, & Stepper, 1988). The fact that
the like may be the important direct influ­ women are smiled at more than men could
ences in the lifetime of a given woman (with produce more smiling through the reci­
the complementary norms having direct procity process (Hinsz & Tomhave, 1991),
influences on a man), one might still argue which then produces positive affect
that it was the power differential that through facial feedback, more smiling, and
11-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 5:03 PM Page 213

Women’s and Men’s Nonverbal Communication–––◆–––213

so forth. Women’s superior awareness of associated with concurrently felt positive


social scripts and norms, and their attune­ emotion.
ment to others’ needs, may also lead them Researchers also need to understand
into a heightened amount of prosocial better the meaning of behavior in context
smiling. People expect women to smile (be (see Bavelas & Chovil, this volume). For
socially responsive, etc.), and people example, gazing may take on different
deliver social rewards to women accord­ meanings depending on other nonverbal
ingly, which again feeds into the affect­ behaviors, the task, the emotional tone of
feedback-smiling cycle. the interaction, and so forth. Furthermore,
The differences between females and different processes may be operative for
males, however, may not always be linked men versus women. For example, there
to immediate social purposes and messages. may be situations in which men’s smiling
Nonverbal stylistic habits are undoubtedly is tied to the urge to be influential,
learned, in part, through simple modeling whereas women’s smiling is tied to the
processes of same-gender others. A child belief that smiling makes them look more
raised in isolation from same-gender mod­ attractive. No meaningful conclusion
els would likely display a very different could be drawn without knowing about
profile of nonverbal behavior and skill these contingencies. Research on gender
than typically seen. Furthermore, the pur­ differences in nonverbal behavior and
pose of the gender-linked behaviors may be their explanations is still at a rather gross
metacommunicative. For example, boys stage of description that lumps together
may smile less than girls in part because many different variations with different
they do not want to be perceived as acting possible meanings.
like girls. Clearly, social pressures bear
down on both males and females (Jansz,
2000). Conformity to group standards can ♦ Conclusion
be a potent motivator all by itself, a point
well supported by a number of the modera­
tors uncovered in LaFrance et al.’s (2003) Nonverbal gender differences and their
meta-analysis of smiling (i.e., the effect of associated stereotypes, because they connote
being observed). much about emotion, motivation, and other
To summarize the state of theory regard­ personal characteristics, have implications
ing the origins of nonverbal gender differ­ for how men and women are viewed and
ences, the weight of evidence points away treated. Self-fulfilling prophecies create a
from a power-based explanation and potent but usually invisible web of influence
toward a sociocultural theory based on throughout our lives. Though the nonverbal
social norms, expectations, roles, and asso­ gender differences are small in absolute
ciated affective experiences. It is important terms, they are real and can have real conse­
to emphasize that the typical study that quences. Sometimes the differences may be
simply compares the nonverbal behavior of noticed with gratitude or admiration, but
males and females is not conducive to sometimes they are a source of aggravation
unraveling the mystery of causation. and frustration. Perhaps the ideal to strive
Researchers need to distinguish better for is an adaptable repertoire so that all
among subtly different nonverbal cues that individuals are able to adapt their nonverbal
have different interpersonal meanings. An behavior to the needs of the situation and of
example given earlier is smiling, for which the other people in it, as well as to their own
there are variants that are more and less emotional and strategic needs.
11-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 5:03 PM Page 214

214–––◆–––Factors of Influence

♦ Notes the least, one should find that controlling for


power diminishes the gender differences. When
this kind of analysis has been undertaken, how­
Author’s Note: This chapter is dedicated to ever, no such reduction occurred (e.g., Hall &
Bob and Doris Gordon, who supplied the most Friedman, 1999).
pleasant possible setting in which to undertake 6. Snodgrass’s research (e.g., Snodgrass,
its writing. 1992) has often been cited as showing that low-
power individuals are motivated to be better
1. Rather than prejudge the origins of male- decoders of nonverbal cues in the dyadic context
female differences by attributing some to “sex” than high-power individuals. Later research
and some to “gender,” I prefer to use the terms (Hall, Rosip, Smith Le Beau, Horgan, & Carter,
interchangeably. To be consistent with the edi­ 2006; Snodgrass, Hecht, & Ploutz-Snyder,
tors’ preferences of using only one term, I use 1998), however, showed that this effect was
gender in the present chapter. Also, when I use actually due to the nature of expression and not
the term gender, I refer to people who self- to motivational factors related to decoding.
identify or are identified based on appearance as 7. Fischer and Manstead (2000) used self-
dichotomously male or female without consi­ report of nonverbal emotional expressiveness
deration of possible complexities such as trans­ to test the power hypothesis on a cross-cultural
gendered or transsexual identities, gender level, using questionnaires and archival data
reassignment, or homosexual identity. relating to women’s status in 37 countries. There
2. Kramer’s (1977) respondents also believed were few gender by status interactions, and
that men’s voices are lower pitched and that men those that did emerge showed that the gender
lounge and lean back more than women do. difference in expressiveness was greater in
Neither of these behaviors was included in the more egalitarian countries, in contradiction to
Briton and Hall (1995a) study, though both Henley’s (1977) power-based theory.
stereotypes were supported by Hall’s (1984)
review of actual nonverbal gender differences.
3. Of course, smiles have many meanings
besides spontaneously revealing happy emotion. ♦ References
Smiles can be used as back-channel responses in
conversation (i.e., cues that encourage the other
person to continue speaking; Brunner, 1979), Aiello, J. R. (1977). Visual interaction at
and smiles are used ubiquitously as social extended distances. Personality and Social
messages (greeting, apologizing, reassuring, Psychology Bulletin, 3, 83–86.
acknowledging passersby, etc.). Therefore, even Alexander, M. G., & Wood, W. (2000).
if women showed more non-Duchenne smiles Women, men, and positive emotions: A
than men, it would not necessarily mean that social role interpretation. In A. H. Fischer
women’s smiles are insincere, but rather could (Ed.), Gender and emotion: Social psycho­
indicate more socially skilled behavior or other logical perspectives (pp. 189–210). Paris:
authentic interpersonal motives. The simple Cambridge University Press.
comparison of men’s and women’s Duchenne Ambady, N., Hallahan, M., & Rosenthal, R.
and non-Duchenne smiling is not meaningful (1995). On judging and being judged accu­
without knowledge of current emotional states rately in zero-acquaintance situations.
and motives. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
4. For effects of r = .20 or smaller, the d 69, 518–529.
statistic is simply 2r (Rosenthal, 1991). As r Andersen, P. A. (1998). Researching sex
increases beyond this level, d increases at an differences within sex similarities: The
accelerating rate. For example, the d corre­ evolutionary consequences of reproductive
sponding to r = .45 is 1.00. differences. In D. J. Canary & K. Dindia
5. Of course, demonstrating such paral­ (Eds.), Sex differences and similarities
lelism does not by itself validate the theory. At in communication: Critical essays and
11-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 5:03 PM Page 215

Women’s and Men’s Nonverbal Communication–––◆–––215

empirical investigations of sex and gender Clancy, S. M., & Dollinger, S. J. (1993).
in interaction (pp. 83–100). Mahwah, NJ: Photographic depictions of the self: Gender
Erlbaum. and age differences in social connectedness.
Archer, J. (2002). Sex differences in physically Sex Roles, 29, 477–495.
aggressive acts between heterosexual part­ Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical power analysis for
ners: A meta-analytic review. Aggression the behavioral sciences (2nd ed.). Mahwah,
and Violent Behavior, 7, 313–351. NJ: Erlbaum.
Argyle, M., & Dean, J. (1965). Eye-contact, Cross, S. E., & Madson, L. (1997). Models
distance and affiliation. Sociometry, 28, of the self: Self-construals and gender.
289–304. Psychological Bulletin, 122, 5–37.
Baron-Cohen, S., Wheelwright, S., Hill, J., Raste, Cunningham, M. R. (1977). Personality and
Y., & Plumb, I. (2001). The “Reading the the structure of the nonverbal communica­
Mind in the Eyes” Test Revised Version: tion of emotion. Journal of Personality,
A study with normal adults, and adults with 45, 564–584.
Asperger syndrome or high-functioning DePaulo, B. M. (1992). Nonverbal behavior
autism. Journal of Child Psychology and and self-presentation. Psychological Bulletin,
Psychiatry, 42, 241–251. 111, 203–243.
Bem, S. L. (1974). The measurement of DiBiase, R., & Gunnoe, J. (2004). Gender
psychological androgyny. Journal of and culture differences in touching behav­
Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 42, ior. Journal of Social Psychology, 144,
155–162. 49–62.
Berkowitz, W. R. (1971). A cross-national Dodd, D. K., Russell, B. L., & Jenkins, C.
comparison of some social patterns of (1999). Smiling in school yearbook photos:
urban pedestrians. Journal of Cross- Gender differences from kindergarten to
Cultural Psychology, 2, 129–144. adulthood. The Psychological Record, 49,
Biehl, M., Matsumoto, D., Ekman, P., Hearn, 543–554.
V., Heider, K., Kudoh, T., et al. (1997). Dovidio, J. F., & Ellyson, S. L. (1982). Decoding
Matsumoto’s and Ekman’s Japanese and visual dominance behavior: Attributions of
Caucasian Facial Expressions of Emotion power based on the relative percentages of
(JACFEE): Reliability data and cross- looking while speaking and looking while
national differences. Journal of Nonverbal listening. Social Psychology Quarterly, 45,
Behavior, 21, 3–21. 106–113.
Briton, N. J., & Hall, J. A. (1995a). Beliefs Dovidio, J. F., & Ellyson, S. L. (1985). Patterns
about female and male nonverbal commu­ of visual dominance behavior in humans.
nication. Sex Roles, 32, 79–90. In J. F. Dovidio & S. L. Ellyson (Eds.),
Briton, N. J., & Hall, J. A. (1995b). Gender- Power, dominance, and nonverbal behavior
based expectancies and observer judgments (pp. 128–149). New York: Springer.
of smiling. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, Dovidio, J. F., Ellyson, S. L., Keating, C. F.,
19, 49–65. Heltman, K., & Brown, C. E. (1988). The
Brody, L. R., & Hall, J. A. (2000). Gender, emo­ relationship of social power to visual dis­
tion, and expression. In M. Lewis & J. M. plays of dominance between men and
Haviland-Jones (Eds.), Handbook of emo­ women. Journal of Personality and Social
tions (2nd ed., pp. 338–349). New York: Psychology, 54, 233–242.
Guilford. Eagly, A. H. (1987). Sex differences in social
Brunner, L. J. (1979). Smiles can be back chan­ behavior: A social role interpretation.
nels. Journal of Personality and Social Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Psychology, 37, 728–734. Ekman, P., Davidson, R. J., & Friesen, W. V.
Bugental, D. E., Love, L. R., & Gianetto, R. M. (1990). The Duchenne smile: Emotional
(1971). Perfidious feminine faces. Journal expression and brain physiology: II. Journal
of Personality and Social Psychology, 17, of Personality and Social Psychology, 58,
314–318. 342–353.
11-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 5:03 PM Page 216

216–––◆–––Factors of Influence

Fischer, A. H., & Manstead, A. S. R. (2000). of structured interactions between employ­


The relation between gender and emotions ees of a company. Personality and Social
in different cultures. In A. H. Fischer (Ed.), Psychology Bulletin, 25, 1082–1091.
Gender and emotion: Social psychological Hall, J. A., & Halberstadt, A. G. (1986). Smiling
perspectives (pp. 71–94). Paris: Cambridge and gazing. In J. S. Hyde & M. C. Linn
University Press. (Eds.), The psychology of gender: Advances
Fisher, J. D., & Byrne, D. (1975). Too close for through meta-analysis (pp. 136–158).
comfort: Sex differences in response to Baltimore: The Johns Hopkins University
invasions of personal space. Journal of Press.
Personality and Social Psychology, 32, Hall, J. A., Halberstadt, A. G., & O’Brien, C. E.
15–21. (1997). “Subordination” and nonverbal
Gross, J. J., & John, O. P. (1998). Mapping the sensitivity: A study and synthesis of findings
domain of expressivity: Multimethod evi­ based on trait measures. Sex Roles, 37,
dence for a hierarchical model. Journal of 295–317.
Personality and Social Psychology, 74, Hall, J. A., Murphy, N. A., & Schmid Mast, M.
170–191. (in press). Recall of nonverbal cues:
Halberstadt, A. G., Hayes, C. W., & Pike, K. M. Exploring a new definition of interpersonal
(1988). Gender and gender role differences sensitivity. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior.
in smiling and communication consistency. Hall, J. A., Rosip, J. C., Smith LeBeau, L.,
Sex Roles, 19, 589–604. Horgan, T. G., & Carter, J. D. (2006).
Hall, J. A. (1978). Gender differences in decod­ Attributing the sources of accuracy in
ing nonverbal cues. Psychological Bulletin, unequal-power dyadic communication:
85, 845–857. Who is better and why? Journal of
Hall, J. A. (1984). Nonverbal sex differences: Experimental Social Psychology, 42, 18–27.
Communication accuracy and expressive Hall, J. A., & Veccia, E. M. (1990). More
style. Baltimore, MD: The Johns Hopkins “touching” observations: New insights on
University Press. men, women, and interpersonal touch.
Hall, J. A. (2006). How big are nonverbal gen­ Journal of Personality and Social Psy­
der differences? The case of smiling and chology, 59, 1155–1162.
sensitivity to nonverbal cues. In K. Dindia Hecht, M. A., & LaFrance, M. (1998). License
& D. J. Canary (Eds.), Sex differences and or obligation to smile: The effect of power
similarities in communication (2nd ed., pp. and sex on amount and type of smiling.
59–81). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin,
Hall, J. A., & Carter, J. D. (1999). Gender- 24, 1332–1342.
stereotype accuracy as an individual differ­ Henley, N. M. (1973). Status and sex: Some
ence. Journal of Personality and Social touching observations. Bulletin of the
Psychology, 77, 350–359. Psychonomic Society, 2, 91–93.
Hall, J. A., Carter, J. D., & Horgan, T. G. Henley, N. M. (1977). Body politics: Power,
(2000). Gender differences in nonverbal sex, and nonverbal communication.
communication of emotion. In A. H. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall.
Fischer (Ed.), Gender and emotion: Social Henley, N. M. (1995). Body politics revisited:
psychological perspectives (pp. 97–117). What do we know today? In P. J.
Paris: Cambridge University Press. Kalbfleisch & M. J. Cody (Eds.), Gender,
Hall, J. A., Coats, E. J., & Smith LeBeau, L. power, and communication in human
(2005). Nonverbal behavior and the verti­ relationships (pp. 27–61). Hillsdale, NJ:
cal dimension of social relations: A meta­ Erlbaum.
analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 131, Hinsz, V. B., & Tomhave, J. A. (1991).
898–924. Smile and (half) the world smiles with you,
Hall, J. A., & Friedman, G. B. (1999). Status, frown and you frown alone. Personality and
gender, and nonverbal behavior: A study Social Psychology Bulletin, 17, 586–592.
11-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 5:03 PM Page 217

Women’s and Men’s Nonverbal Communication–––◆–––217

Horgan, T. G., Schmid Mast, M., Hall, J. A., & McClure, E. B. (2000). A meta-analytic review
Carter, J. D. (2004). Gender differences in of sex differences in facial expression pro­
memory for the appearance of others. cessing and their development in infants,
Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, children, and adolescents. Psychological
30, 185–196. Bulletin, 126, 424–453.
Jansz, J. (2000). Masculine identity and restric­ McCormick, N. B., & Jones, A. J. (1989).
tive emotionality. In A. H. Fischer (Ed.), Gender differences in nonverbal flirtation.
Gender and emotion: Social psychological Journal of Sex Education & Therapy, 15,
perspectives (pp. 166–186). Paris: Cambridge 271–282.
University Press. Montemayor, R., & Flannery, D. J. (1989). A
Johnson, J., & Shulman, G. (1988). More alike naturalistic study of the involvement of
than meets the eye: Perceived gender differ­ children and adolescents with their mothers
ences in subjective experience and its dis­ and friends: Developmental differences in
play. Sex Roles, 19, 67–79. expressive behavior. Journal of Adolescent
Judd, C. M., & Park, B. (1993). Definition Research, 4, 3–14.
and assessment of accuracy in social Murphy, N. A., Hall, J. A., & Colvin, C. R.
stereotypes. Psychological Review, 100, (2003). Accurate intelligence assess­
109–128. ments in social interaction: Mediators and
Knapp, M. L., & Hall, J. A. (2005). Nonverbal gender effects. Journal of Personality, 71,
communication in human interaction (6th 465–493.
ed.). Belmont, CA: Wadsworth. Noller, P., & Gallois, C. (1986). Sending emo­
Korzenny, B. A. G., Korzenny, F., & Sanchez de tional messages in marriage: Non-verbal
Rota, G. (1985). Women’s communication behaviour, sex and communication clarity.
in Mexican organizations. Sex Roles, 12, British Journal of Social Psychology, 25,
867–876. 287–297.
Kramer, C. (1977). Perceptions of male and Richard, F. D., Bond, C. F., Jr., & Stokes-
female speech. Language and Speech, 20, Zoota, J. J. (2003). One hundred years
151–161. of social psychology quantitatively
LaFrance, M., & Banaji, M. (1992). Toward a described. Review of General Psychology,
reconsideration of the gender-emotion rela­ 7, 331–363.
tionship. In M. S. Clark (Ed.), Emotion and Rosenkrantz, P., Vogel, S., Bee, H., Broverman,
social behavior (pp. 178–201). Newbury I., & Broverman, D. M. (1968). Sex-role
Park, CA: Sage. stereotypes and self-concepts in college
LaFrance, M., Hecht, M. A., & Levy Paluck, E. students. Journal of Consulting and Clinical
(2003). The contingent smile: A meta­ Psychology, 32, 287–295.
analysis of sex differences in smiling. Rosenthal, R. (1991). Meta-analytic procedures
Psychological Bulletin, 129, 305–334. for social research (rev. ed.). Newbury
Lippa, R. A., & Dietz, J. K. (1998). The relation Park, CA: Sage.
of gender, personality, and intelligence to Rosenthal, R., Hall, J. A., DiMatteo, M. R.,
judges’ accuracy in judging strangers’ per­ Rogers, P. L., & Archer, D. (1979).
sonality from brief video segments. Journal Sensitivity to nonverbal communication:
of Nonverbal Behavior, 24, 25–43. The PONS test. Baltimore: The Johns
Lipsey, M. W., & Wilson, D. B. (1993). The effi­ Hopkins University Press.
cacy of psychological, educational, and Rosip, J. C., & Hall, J. A. (2004). Knowledge
behavioral treatment: Confirmation from of nonverbal cues, gender, and nonverbal
meta-analysis. American Psychologist, 48, decoding accuracy. Journal of Nonverbal
1181–1209. Behavior, 28, 267–286.
Lipsey, M. W., & Wilson, D. B. (2001). Scherer, K. R., Banse, R., & Wallbott, H. G.
Practical meta-analysis. Thousand Oaks, (2001). Emotion inferences from vocal
CA: Sage. expression correlate across languages and
11-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 5:03 PM Page 218

218–––◆–––Factors of Influence

cultures. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psy­ Journal of Personality and Social


chology, 32, 76–92. Psychology, 54, 208–219.
Schmid Mast, M., & Hall, J. A. (2004a). Who Viscovich, N., Borod, J., Pihan, H., Peery, S.,
is the boss and who is not? Accuracy of Brickman, A. M., Tabert, M., et al. (2003).
judging status. Journal of Nonverbal Acoustical analysis of posed prosodic
Behavior, 28, 145–165. expressions: Effects of emotion and sex.
Schmid Mast, M., & Hall, J. A. (2004b). When Perceptual and Motor Skills, 96, 759–771.
is dominance related to smiling? Assigned Vogt, D. S., & Colvin, C. R. (2003). Interpersonal
dominance, dominance preference, trait orientation and the accuracy of personality
dominance, and gender as moderators. Sex judgements. Journal of Personality, 71,
Roles, 50, 387–399. 267–295.
Schmid Mast, M., & Hall, J. A. (2006). Vrugt, A. (1987). The meaning of nonverbal sex
Women’s advantage at remembering differences. Semiotica, 64, 371–380.
others’ appearance: A systematic look at Vrugt, A., & Kerkstra, A. (1984). Sex differences
the why and when of a gender difference. in nonverbal communication. Semiotica,
Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 50, 1–41.
32, 353–364. Willis, F. N., Jr., & Dodds, R. A. (1998). Age,
Shapiro, P. N., & Penrod, S. (1986). Meta­ relationship, and touch initiation. Journal
analysis of facial identification studies. of Social Psychology, 138, 115–123.
Psychological Bulletin, 100, 139–156. Willis, F. N., & Rinck, C. M. (1983). A personal
Snodgrass, S. E. (1992). Further effects of log method for investigating interpersonal
role versus gender on interpersonal sensitiv­ touch. Journal of Psychology: Inter­
ity. Journal of Personality and Social Psy­ disciplinary and Applied, 113, 119–122.
chology, 62, 154–158. Zuckerman, M., DePaulo, B. M., & Rosenthal,
Snodgrass, S. E., Hecht, M. A., & Ploutz- R. (1981). Verbal and nonverbal communi­
Snyder, R. (1998). Interpersonal sensitivity: cation of deception. In L. Berkowitz (Ed.),
Expressivity or perceptivity? Journal of Advances in experimental social psychology
Personality and Social Psychology, 74, (Vol. 14, pp. 1–59). New York: Academic
238–249. Press.
Spence, J. T., & Helmreich, R. (1978). Zuckerman, M., Hall, J. A., DeFrank, R. S., &
Masculinity and femininity: Their psycholog­ Rosenthal, R. (1976). Encoding and decod­
ical dimensions, correlates, and antecedents. ing of spontaneous and posed facial expres­
Austin, TX: University of Texas Press. sions. Journal of Personality and Social
Stier, D. S., & Hall, J. A. (1984). Gender differ­ Psychology, 34, 966–977.
ences in touch: An empirical and theoretical Zuckerman, M., & Larrance, D. T. (1979).
review. Journal of Personality and Social Individual differences in perceived encoding
Psychology, 47, 440–459. and decoding abilities. In R. Rosenthal
Strack, F., Martin, L. L., & Stepper, S. (1988). (Ed.), Skill in nonverbal communication:
Inhibiting and facilitating conditions Individual differences (pp. 171–193).
of the human smile: A nonobtrusive Cambridge, MA: Oelgeschlager, Gunn &
test of the facial feedback hypothesis. Hain.
12-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 5:04 PM Page 219

12
CULTURE AND
NONVERBAL BEHAVIOR

� David Matsumoto
San Francisco State University

♦ Defining Culture

Over the history of time, people have had to solve a host of distinct
social problems in order to adapt and thus achieve reproductive success,
including negotiating complex status hierarchies, forming successful
work and social groups, attracting mates, fighting off potential rivals of
food and sexual partners, giving birth and raising children, and battling
nature (Buss, 1991, 2001). Universal biological imperatives are associ­
ated with a universal set of psychological problems that people need to
solve in order to survive; thus, all individuals and groups of individuals
must create ways to deal with these universal problems. The ways that
each group develops then become their culture.
In my view, culture is the product of the interaction between univer­
sal biological needs and functions, universal social problems created to

Author’s Note: I thank Marija Drezgic, Devon McCabe, and Joanna Schug
for their aid in conducting the literature review; Seung Hee Yoo for her com­
ments on a previous version of this chapter; and Sanae Nakagawa, Andres
Olide, and Akiko Terao for their aid in the functioning of my laboratory.

◆ 219
12-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 5:04 PM Page 220

220–––◆–––Factors of Influence

address those needs, and the contexts in emotion, division of labor by sex, revenge
which people live. Culture is created as and retaliation, mate selection and sexual
people adapt to their environments in order jealousy, self-enhancement, and personality
to survive, and it results from the process can be traced to the core aspect of a univer­
of individuals’ attempts to adapt to their sal human nature based on biological imper­
contexts in addressing the universal social atives and universal social problems of
problems and biological needs. Although adaptation and living.
many different definitions of culture exist But many mental and behavioral
(e.g., Berry, Poortinga, Segall, & Dasen, processes are also culture-specific. Different
1992; Jahoda, 1984; Kroeber & Kluckholn, cultures develop different ways of dealing
1963; Linton, 1936; Rohner, 1984; with the biological imperatives and univer­
Triandis, 1972), I define culture as a shared sal social problems based on their contexts.
system of socially transmitted behavior that Language is an example of a very culture-
describes, defines, and guides people’s ways specific behavior. Each culture has its
of life, communicated from one generation own language, with its own vocabulary,
to the next. syntax, grammar, phonology, and pragmat­
Because people must deal with the same ics (Barnlund & Araki, 1985; Barnlund &
set of biological needs and functions and Yoshioka, 1990; Chen, 1995; Gudykunst
universal social problems, it is very possible & Mody, 2001; Kim et al., 1996; Minami
and in many cases very likely that the ways & McCabe, 1995; Nomura & Barnlund,
in which they are addressed are the same. 1983). The need to have language may be a
That is, universal biological needs and social pancultural universal problem; and having a
problems can lead to similar solutions language may be a universal solution to this
across cultures, especially over time in our problem. But the specific way in which each
evolutionary history. Thus, many aspects of culture solves this problem—that is, devel­
our mental processes and behaviors can be ops its own language—is different in every
considered universal. For example, all culture.
humans appear to have some degree of spe­
cific fears, such as to snakes, spiders,
heights, and darkness, because these types of ♦ The Role of Culture
fears have led in our evolutionary history to
in the Nonverbal
greater probability of survival (Seligman &
Communication Process
Hager, 1972). As well, people have a ten­
dency to perceive their own ingroup as het­
erogeneous, fully recognizing the individual As with verbal communication, culture
differences that exist in that group, whereas influences nonverbal behaviors in profound
they perceive other groups as more homoge­ ways. By far the largest research literature
neous, assuming less diversity within the on this topic is related to facial expressions
group (Linville & Jones, 1980; Triandis, of emotion, which I review later in this
McCusker, & Hui, 1990). People also seem chapter. In this section, I highlight briefly
to have a natural proclivity to fears of the role of culture on other types of
strangers and outgroup members, which nonverbal behaviors before turning to the
may be a universal basis for ethnocentrism, larger discussion of culture and emotional
prejudice, aggression, and even war (Buss, expressions.
2001; see also Dovidio & colleagues, this
volume). Other universal processes, such as Culture and Gestures. The study of culture
incest avoidance, facial expressions of and gestures has its roots in the study by
12-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 5:04 PM Page 221

Culture and Nonverbal Behavior–––◆–––221

David Efron (Boas & Efron, 1936; Efron, engaged in more gazing and had more
1941), who examined the gestures of Sicilian direct orientations when interacting with
and Lithuanian Jewish immigrants in New others, less interpersonal distance, and
York City. Efron found that there were dis­ more touching. Within the United States,
tinct gestures among traditional Jews and there are also differences in gaze and visual
Italians but that the traditional gestures behavior between different ethnic groups
disappeared as people were more assimilated (Exline, Jones, & Maciorowski, 1977;
into the larger American culture. This work LaFrance & Mayo, 1976).
was followed initially by that of Ekman and
his colleagues (Ekman, 1976; Friesen, Ekman, Culture and Interpersonal Space. Hall
& Wallbott, 1979), who documented cultural (1966, 1973) specified four different levels of
differences in emblematic gestures between interpersonal space use depending on social
Japanese, Americans, and New Guineans. relationship type: intimate, personal, social,
Morris and his colleagues (Morris, Collett, and public. Whereas people of all cultures
Marsh, & O’Shaughnessy, 1980) have also seem to make these distinctions, they differ
well documented many cultural differences in in the spaces they attribute to them. Arab
gestures. The American A-OK sign, for males, for example, tend to sit closer to each
example, is an obscene gesture in many cul­ other than American males, with more
tures of Europe, having sexual implications. direct, confrontational types of body orien­
Placing both hands at the side of one’s head tations (Watson & Graves, 1966). They also
and pointing upward with the forefingers sig­ were found to use greater eye contact and to
nals one is angry in some cultures; in others, speak in louder voices. Arabs, at least in the
however, it means that one wants sex. past, learned to interact with others at dis­
tances close enough to feel the other person’s
Culture and Gaze. Research on humans breath (Hall, 1963). Furthermore, Latin
and nonhuman primates has shown that Americans tend to interact more closely than
gaze is associated with dominance, power, do students of European backgrounds
or aggression (Fehr & Exline, 1987) and (Forston & Larson, 1968), and Indonesians
affiliation and nurturance (Argyle & Cook, tend to sit closer than Australians
1976). Fehr and Exline suggested that the (Noesjirwan, 1977, 1978). Italians interact
affiliative aspects of gazing begin in infancy, more closely than either Germans or
as infants attend to adults as their source Americans (Shuter, 1977), and Colombians
of care and protection. Cultures create rules were found to interact at closer distances
concerning gazing and visual attention, than did Costa Ricans (Shuter, 1976).
however, because both aggression and affil­
iation are behavioral tendencies that are Culture and Other Nonverbal Behaviors.
important for group stability and mainte­ Other studies have documented cultural
nance. Cross-cultural research has docu­ differences in other nonverbal behaviors
mented differences in these rules. Arabs, for as well, such as in the semantic meanings
example, have been found to gaze much attributed to body postures (Kudoh &
longer and more directly at their partners Matsumoto, 1985; Matsumoto & Kudoh,
than do Americans (Hall, 1963; Watson & 1987) and vocal characteristics and hand
Graves, 1966). Watson (1970), who classi­ and arm movements (Vrij & Winkel, 1991,
fied 30 countries as either a “contact” cul­ 1992). Collectively, the evidence provides
ture (those that facilitated physical touch or more than ample support for the contention
contact during interaction) or a “noncon­ that culture plays a large role in molding
tact” culture, found that contact cultures our nonverbal behaviors, which comprise
12-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 5:04 PM Page 222

222–––◆–––Factors of Influence

an important part of the communication judgments of faces thought to express


process. The largest research literature emotions panculturally and demonstrated
in the area of culture and nonverbal behav­ that all cultures agreed on the emotions
ior, however, concerns facial expressions portrayed in the expressions, providing the
of emotion. In the next section, I review the first evidence for their universality (Ekman,
most relevant research in this area of study, 1972, 1973; Ekman & Friesen, 1971;
illustrating the universal and culture-specific Ekman, Sorenson, & Friesen, 1969; Izard,
aspects of both the encoding and decoding 1971). Collectively, these findings demon­
of facial expressions of emotion. strated the existence of six universal expres­
sions—anger, disgust, fear, happiness,
sadness, and surprise—as judges from
♦ Culture and Facial around the world agreed on what emotion
Expressions of Emotion was portrayed in the faces.
Yet the judgment studies were not the
THE UNIVERSALITY only evidence that came to bear on the
OF FACIAL EXPRESSIONS question of emotion universality. Some of
the most important findings related to
Questions concerning the universality of universality were from Ekman’s (1972)
facial expression find their roots in Charles cross-cultural study of expressions that
Darwin’s work. Darwin’s thesis, summa­ occurred spontaneously in reaction to
rized in The Expression of Emotion in Man emotion-eliciting films. In that study,
and Animals, suggested that emotions and American and Japanese participants viewed
their expressions had evolved across species, a neutral and highly stressful film (com­
were evolutionarily adaptive, biologically prised of four separate clips), while their
innate, and universal across all human and facial behaviors were recorded throughout
even nonhuman primates. According to the entire experiment. Ekman coded the last
Darwin (1872/1998), humans, regardless 3 minutes of facial behavior videotaped
of race or culture, possess the ability to during the neutral films and the entire 3
express emotions in exactly the same ways, minutes of the last stress film clip. The cod­
primarily through their faces. Between the ing identified facial muscle configurations
time of Darwin’s original writing and associated with the six emotions mentioned
the 1960s, however, only seven studies previously; all corresponded to the facial
attempted to test the universality of facial expressions portrayed in the stimuli used in
expression. These studies were flawed their judgment studies (Ekman, 1972;
methodologically in a number of ways, so Ekman et al., 1969, 1972). Research fol­
that unequivocal data speaking to the issue lowing Ekman’s original study described
of the possible universality of emotional above and using American, Japanese,
expression did not emerge at that time German, Canadian, and French partici­
(Ekman, Friesen, & Ellsworth, 1972). pants has continued to mount convincing
It was not until the mid-1960s when evidence for the universality of facial
psychologist Sylvan Tomkins, a pioneer in expressions of emotion (see Table 12.1).
modern studies of human emotion, joined Considerable evidence documenting and
forces independently with Paul Ekman and converging in their support of the univer­
Carroll Izard to conduct the first of what sality of facial expressions of emotion has
have become known today as the “univer­ come from studies with different bases
sality studies.” These researchers obtained than those following Ekman (1972). For
12-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 5:04 PM Page 223

Culture and Nonverbal Behavior–––◆–––223

Table 12.1 Studies Examining Spontaneous Facial Expressions of Emotion

Measurement
Citation Participants Eliciting Stimuli System Emotionsa

Rosenberg & American Videos selected FACS Disgust, sadness,


Ekman, 1994 university for their ability to fear, happiness,
students elicit primarily contempt, and
disgust and anger
secondarily fear
Ruch, 1995 German Slides of jokes FACS Happiness
university and cartoons
students
Ruch, 1993 German Slides of jokes FACS Happiness
university and cartoons
students
Frank, Ekman, American Films designed to FACS Happiness
& Friesen, university elicit various
1993, Study 1 students emotions
Gosselin, Actors from the Actors were FACS Happiness, fear,
Kirouac, & Conservatory of asked to interpret anger, surprise,
Dore,1995, Dramatic Arts in 2 of 24 scenarios sadness, and
Study 1 Quebec designed to elicit disgust
happiness, fear,
anger, surprise,
sadness, and
disgust
Ekman, Depressed Intake and FACS and Happiness,
Matsumoto, & inpatients discharge EMFACS contempt, anger,
Friesen, 1997 interviews disgust, fear, and
sadness
Berenbaum & German Engaging in a EMFACS Contempt, disgust,
Oltmanns, schizophrenic political anger, sadness,
1992 and conversation with fear, surprise, and
psychosomatic a partner they happiness
patients, and had never met
healthy controls before
Ellgring, 1986 German Interviews FACS Happiness
depressed
patients
Heller & French Interviews with FACS and Contempt
Haynal, 1994 depressed the patient’s EMFACS
patients psychiatrists
Keltner, Moffitt, American Administration EMFACS Anger, fear, and
& Stouthamer­ adolescents of the WISC-R sadness
Loeber, 1995 with behavior
problems

(Continued)
12-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 5:04 PM Page 224

224–––◆–––Factors of Influence

Table 12.1 (Continued)

Measurement
Citation Participants Eliciting Stimuli System Emotionsa

Chesney et al., American Structured FACS Disgust, fear,


1990 salaried interview sadness,
employees in designed to happiness, anger,
managerial assess Type A contempt, and
positions at an behavior surprise
aerospace firm
Camras, Oster, American and Arm restraint that FACS Anger, sadness,
Campos, Japanese infants produces distress fear, and
Miyake, & happiness
Bradshaw, 1992

NOTE: FACS, Facial action coding system; EMFACS, emotion facial action coding system; JACFEE, Japanese
and Caucasian facial expressions of emotions; WISC-R, Weschler Intelligence Scale for Children—Revised.
a. Corresponding to facial muscle configurations coded in the face that match those in JACFEE.

instance, studies have shown that the uni­ 1997). This similarity exists in people of
versal facial expressions of emotion occur in as widely divergent cultures as the United
congenitally blind individuals (Charlesworth States and the Minangkabau of West
& Kreutzer, 1973). Research on nonhuman Sumatra, Indonesia. In addition, there is uni­
primates has also demonstrated that the versality in the antecedents that bring about
expressions that are universal to humans emotion (Scherer, 1997a, 1997b).
also occur in animals, and that animals
have many different yet stable signals of
emotion (Chevalier-Skolnikoff, 1973; Geen, CULTURAL DIFFERENCES
1992; Hauser, 1993; Snowdon, 2003). IN EXPRESSING EMOTION:
Likewise, the emotions portrayed in the uni­ CULTURAL DISPLAY RULES
versal facial expressions correspond to emo­
tion taxonomies in different languages Despite the existence of universal facial
around the world (Romney, Boyd, Moore, expressions of emotion, people around the
Batchelder, & Brazill, 1996; Romney, world do express emotions differently. The
Moore, & Rusch, 1997; Shaver, Murdaya, first evidence for cultural differences in
& Fraley, 2001; Shaver, Wu, & Schwartz, expression was Friesen’s (1972) study, in
1992). which the spontaneous expressions of
There is also cross-cultural similarity in Americans and Japanese were examined as
the physiological responses to emotion when they viewed highly stressful films in two
these facial expressions are used as markers, conditions, first alone and then a second
in both the autonomic nervous system and time in the presence of an older, male
brain activity (Davidson, 2003; Ekman, experimenter. In the first condition, the
Levenson, & Friesen, 1983; Levenson, American and Japanese participants
Ekman, & Friesen, 1990; Levenson, Ekman, were similar in their expressions of dis­
Heider, & Friesen, 1992; Tsai & Levenson, gust, sadness, fear, and anger; in the second
12-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 5:04 PM Page 225

Culture and Nonverbal Behavior–––◆–––225

condition, however, cultural differences effects of culture on expression were not


emerged. Whereas the Americans continued limited to negative emotions.
to express their negative emotions, the Ekman and Friesen (1969) coined the
Japanese were more likely to smile. term cultural display rules to account for
Other researchers have also examined cultural differences in facial expressions of
cultural differences in emotional expression emotion. These are rules learned early in
(Argyle, Henderson, Bond, Iizuka, & childhood that help individuals manage and
Contarello, 1986; Edelmann et al., 1987; modify their emotional expressions depend­
Gudykunst & Nishida, 1984; Gudykunst & ing on social circumstances. Ekman and
Ting-Toomey, 1988; Noesjirwan, 1978; Friesen used the concept to explain the
Waxer, 1985). A recent study from my American–Japanese cultural differences in
laboratory extended Ekman and Friesen’s expression they observed, suggesting that in
(Ekman, 1972; Friesen, 1972) original the first condition of their experiment there
findings. In this study (Matsumoto & was no reason for display rules to modify
Kupperbusch, 2001), European American expressions because the participants were
females were classified as either individual­ alone and their display rules were inopera­
istic or collectivistic based on their respon­ tive; in the second condition display rules
ses to an individual difference measure dictated that the Japanese mask their nega­
(Matsumoto, Weissman, Preston, Brown, & tive emotions in the presence of the experi­
Kupperbusch, 1997) and were then video­ menter (Ekman, 1972; Friesen, 1972).
taped unobtrusively as they watched films After the original inception and docu­
designed to elicit positive and negative emo­ mentation of display rules, published cross-
tion, first alone and then in the presence cultural research was dormant until
of an experimenter. They self-rated their Matsumoto’s (1990) study examining dis­
emotional responses to both films in both play rules in Americans and Japanese.
conditions, and samples of their emotional Participants saw faces portraying seven
expressions were judged by a separate group emotions and rated the appropriateness
of decoders. of each in eight social situations involv­
Both individualists and collectivists expe­ ing people of varying intimacy and status.
rienced the films as intended, and there was Americans rated negative emotions more
no difference in their expressions when they appropriately than did the Japanese in
were alone. With the experimenter, how­ ingroups, whereas the Japanese rated nega­
ever, the collectivists attenuated their nega­ tive emotions more appropriately than
tive expressions and more often masked Americans in outgroups; the Japanese also
them with smiles. This finding is the same rated negative emotions more appropriately
that Ekman and Friesen (Ekman, 1972; than Americans toward lower status indi­
Friesen, 1972) reported previously, and the viduals. Matsumoto (1993) used the same
remarkable thing about this study is that the methodology to document differences in
entire sample was of European American display rules among four ethnic groups
females who were classified based solely on within the United States.
their responses to a questionnaire assessing When the concept of display rules
individualism and collectivism. The collec­ was proposed originally as a mechanism of
tivists also attenuated their expressions of expression management, Ekman and Friesen
positive emotion when in the presence of the (1969, 1975) noted six ways in which expres­
experimenter (Ekman and Friesen’s studies sions may be managed when emotion is
did not test positive emotions); thus, the aroused. Of course, individuals can express
12-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 5:04 PM Page 226

226–––◆–––Factors of Influence

emotions as they feel them with no modifi­ would do if they felt each emotion in four
cation. But individuals can also amplify social situations: with family members, close
(exaggerate) or deamplify (minimize) their friends, colleagues, and strangers. These
expressions; for instance, feelings of sadness categories were chosen because they repre­
may be intensified (amplification) at funerals sent a broad range of social categories within
or minimized (deamplification) at weddings. which people interact, and because previous
People can mask or conceal their emotions research has demonstrated considerable vari­
by expressing something other than what ability in cultural values and attitudes across
they feel, as when nurses or physicians hide these social situations (Brewer & Kramer,
their emotions when speaking with patients 1985; Tajfel, 1982).
with terminal illness, or when employees in In our first study using the DRAI
service industries (e.g., flight attendants) (Matsumoto, Takeuchi, Andayani, Kouznet­
interact with customers. Individuals may sova, & Krupp, 1998), participants from the
also learn to neutralize their expressions, United States, Japan, South Korea, and
expressing nothing, such as when playing Russia completed the DRAI along with an
poker (poker face) and to qualify their feel­ individual-level measure of individualism-
ings by expressing emotions in combination, collectivism. Our results showed that
such as when feelings of sadness are mixed Russians exerted the highest control over
with a smile, with the smile commenting on their expressions, followed by South Koreans
the sadness, saying “I’ll be OK.” All these and Japanese; Americans had the lowest
behavioral responses have been found to scores. Significant sex differences were also
occur when spontaneous expressive behav­ found, with females exerting more control on
iors have been studied (Cole, 1986; Ekman anger, contempt, disgust, and across all emo­
& Rosenberg, 1998). tions when with family members, and males
Recently, my colleagues and I created exerting more control on fear and surprise.
the Display Rule Assessment Inventory Our most recent study involving the
(DRAI), in which participants choose a DRAI (Matsumoto, Yoo, Hirayama, &
behavioral response when they experi­ Petrova, 2005) provided evidence for its
ence different emotions in different social internal and temporal reliability and for
situations (Matsumoto, Takeuchi, Andayani, its content, convergent (with measures of
Kouznetsova, & Krupp, 1998; Matsumoto, emotion regulation), discriminant (correla­
Choi, Hirayama, Domae, & Yamaguchi, tions with personality controlling for emo­
2005). The emotions were those that previ­ tion regulation), external, and concurrent
ous research has shown to be universally predictive validity (with personality). The
expressed and recognized: anger, contempt, findings also indicated that expression reg­
disgust, fear, happiness, sadness, and sur­ ulation occurs in the various ways discussed
prise; these were selected because universal­ earlier, and not on a simple expression-
ity served as a basis by which to examine suppression dimension. Additionally, there
display rules initially and by which compar­ were consistent and predictable cultural
isons across cultures would be meaningful. differences among American, Russian,
To build internal consistency, a synonym for and Japanese participants. For instance,
each emotion label was also included in the Americans and Russians both expressed
initial DRAI—hostility, defiance, aversion, anger and contempt more than Japanese.
worry, joy, gloom, and shock, respectively— Americans expressed fear and disgust more
resulting in a total of 14 emotions terms. than Russians, and Americans expressed
Participants are asked to consider what they happiness more than did Russians and
12-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 5:04 PM Page 227

Culture and Nonverbal Behavior–––◆–––227

Japanese. The Japanese participants de- antecedents and elicitors of emotion, or in


amplified more than both the Americans and the facial configurations themselves.
the Russians. Americans amplified more There are many cultural differences in
than Russians on sadness and disgust, emotion judgments as well. Although people
whereas Japanese amplified surprise and of all cultures recognize the universal faces
fear more than Russians. Japanese qualified at levels well beyond chance, they differ
sadness more than Russians, but the on the absolute level of recognition (Biehl
Russians qualified their happiness more et al., 1997; Elfenbein & Ambady, 2002;
than both Japanese and Americans. Matsumoto, 1989, 1992; Matsumoto et al.,
2002). In an attempt to explain why cul­
tures differ in emotion recognition rates,
CULTURAL INFLUENCES ON Matsumoto (1989) compiled recognition
JUDGMENTS OF EMOTION accuracy data from 15 cultures reported
in four studies, and correlated them with
As discussed earlier, studies examining Hofstede’s (1980) four cultural dimensions.
judgments of facial expressions were instru­ Individualism was positively correlated with
mental in the original universality studies recognition rates of negative emotions. An
and have been replicated by many authors, independent meta-analysis by Schimmack
and Elfenbein and Ambady’s (2002) meta­ (1996) also indicated that individualism
analysis of judgment studies of emotion predicted emotion recognition levels. These
(not limited to facial expressions) demon­ findings may be related to the fact that
strated convincingly that people around the individualism is also correlated positively
world recognize emotions at levels well with emotional expression (Matsumoto &
above chance accuracy. Research of the last Koopmann, 2004). Individualistic cultures
decade and a half has demonstrated that may foster the free and open expression of
people of different cultures are similar in emotion, thereby promoting the more accu­
other aspects of emotion judgment as well. rate judgment of emotion as well. Just as
For example, there is pancultural similarity cultures have display rules that govern the
in judgments of relative intensity among management of emotional expression, they
faces; that is, when comparing expressions, may have “cultural decoding rules” that help
people of different countries agree on which manage the judgments of emotions in others.
is more strongly expressed (Ekman et al., There are cultural differences in judg­
1987; Matsumoto & Ekman, 1989). There ments of the intensity of expressions as
is also evidence of pancultural agreement in well. Ekman et al.’s (1987) study of 10
the association between perceived expres­ countries was the first to document such
sion intensity and inferences about subjec­ differences, with Asians rating emotions at
tive experiences (Matsumoto, Kasri, & lower intensity than non-Asians. Although
Kooken, 1999). People of different cultures this finding has been replicated a number of
have also been found to agree on the sec­ times (Biehl et al., 1997; Matsumoto, 1990,
ondary emotions portrayed in an expres­ 1993), more recent research indicated that
sion (Biehl et al., 1997; Ekman et al., 1987; the cultural differences differ depending on
Matsumoto & Ekman, 1989), suggesting whether observers rate the external display
pancultural agreement in the multiple or the presumed internal experience.
meanings derived from universal faces. This Matsumoto et al. (1999) tested this idea
agreement may exist because of overlap by comparing American and Japanese
in the semantics of the emotion categories, judgments on both types of ratings and
12-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 5:04 PM Page 228

228–––◆–––Factors of Influence

found that Americans rated external dis­ were interpreted as occurring as a function
play more intensely than the Japanese, of cultural display rules, none actually mea­
but that the Japanese rated internal experi­ sured display rules and linked them to the
ence more intensely than Americans. judgments. A recent study from our labora­
Within-country analyses indicated no sig­ tory, however, has closed this loop. In this
nificant differences between the two ratings study, American and Japanese participants
for the Japanese; the Americans, however, completed the DRAI and viewed a series of
rated external displays more intensely than facial expressions of emotion portrayed at
they rated subjective experience. high and low intensities (Matsumoto, Choi,
These findings were extended by et al., 2005). They made three judgments for
Matsumoto and colleagues (2002) by hav­ each face: a categorical judgment of which
ing American and Japanese observers rate emotion was portrayed, and intensity rat­
expressions expressed at 0%, 50%, 100%, ings of the strength of the external display
and 125% intensities. The data for the and the presumed subjective experience of
100% and 125% expressions replicated the the expressor. American and Japanese
previous findings: Americans rated external judges thought that the expressors of high
display significantly higher than internal intensity expressions displayed the emotions
experience, whereas there were no differ­ more strongly than they felt them. When
ences for the Japanese. Also, there were no judging the low intensity expressions,
differences between external and internal Americans and Japanese also rated the
ratings for either Americans or Japanese on expressor’s internal experience higher than
0% expressions, which were expected. On they did the external display, but the effect
50% expressions, however, the findings was significantly larger for the Japanese. All
were intriguing. Whereas there was no these differences were mediated by display
difference between external and internal rules as assessed by the DRAI, suggesting
ratings for the Americans, the Japanese that one’s own rules for expression manage­
rated internal experience higher than exter­ ment influences one’s judgments of expres­
nal display. We interpreted these findings as sion management in others.
suggesting that for weaker expressions,
Japanese may assume that a display rule is
operating, and may thus infer more emo­ A POSSIBLE INGROUP ADVANTAGE
tion being felt than is actually displayed. IN RECOGNIZING EMOTIONS?
When Americans see a weak expression,
however, there need not be any such One type of cultural difference in judg­
assumption; thus, they interpret the same ment that has recently received attention
amount of emotion felt as expressed. For concerns the possibility of an ingroup advan­
strong expressions, Japanese may assume tage in emotion recognition (Elfenbein &
that the context was such that the expres­ Ambady, 2002). This is defined as the ten­
sion was justified; thus, they infer a level dency for members of a cultural group to be
of emotion felt that is commensurate with more accurate in recognizing the emotions
what is shown. When Americans see a strong of members of their own cultural group
expression, however, they know that there than of other, relatively more disparate
is a display rule to exaggerate one’s feelings; groups. Although previous research testing
thus, they compensate for this display rule this hypothesis (Boucher & Carlson, 1980;
by inferring less emotion felt. Kilbride & Yarczower, 1983; Markham &
One limitation of all the studies cited in Wang, 1996) provided mixed results,
this section was that, although the findings Elfenbein and her colleagues have recently
12-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 5:04 PM Page 229

Culture and Nonverbal Behavior–––◆–––229

reported a number of studies in support of Four of these were associated with non­
it (Elfenbein & Ambady, 2002, 2003a, significant interaction Fs that test the
2003b; Elfenbein, Mandal, Ambady, & ingroup effect. Two involved studies of
Harizuka, 2002). facial expressions (Kilbride & Yarczower,
Elsewhere, I have suggested that studies 1983; Mehta et al., 1992), and both these
must meet two methodological require­ involved facial action coding system
ments to test the ingroup hypothesis ade­ (FACS) coding of the facial muscles in the
quately (Matsumoto, 2002). First, studies expressions. The FACS codes were equiva­
should employ balanced designs in which lent but not exactly the same across the
all judge cultures view expressions por­ expressor ethnicities as they are in the
trayed by members of all the other cul­ Japanese and Caucasian facial expressions
tures in the study. Second, because balanced of emotion (JACFEE), thus allowing for
studies include stimuli expressed by people minor cultural differences in the expres­
of multiple cultures, it is necessary to sions to exist (perhaps, corresponding to
ensure that the stimuli are equivalent Elfenbein and Ambady’s, 2002, 2003a;
across the cultural groups in terms of their Elfenbein et al., 2002, “emotion dialects”).
physical signaling properties related to When balanced studies employ expres­
emotion. Given both of these concerns, sions that are equivalent in their physical
Matsumoto (2002) concluded that Elfenbein signaling properties (the JACFEE), there
and Ambady’s (2002) original meta-analysis is no support for the ingroup hypothesis
could not support the ingroup hypothesis (Matsumoto, 2002; Matsumoto & Choi,
because they did not review the studies 2004). This is the case whether the expres­
as to whether or not they met these two sions being judged are full-face, high
requirements. intensity expressions, or low intensity
When balanced studies are examined as expressions where signal clarity is weaker
to whether or not they employed stimuli (Matsumoto & Choi, 2004). Future studies
that were equivalent in their physical sig­ will need to isolate differences in expres­
naling properties or not, the data are clear: sions across encoder cultures while holding
All the studies reported by Elfenbein and constant nonmorphological features of the
colleagues to date supporting the ingroup face that may contribute to emotion signal­
hypothesis have used stimuli that were ing. There are many aspects of the face that
not equivalent across the cultural groups may contribute to emotion signaling,
(Elfenbein & Ambady, 2003a, 2003b; including facial physiognomy, cosmetics,
Elfenbein et al., 2002; Elfenbein, Mandal, and hairstyle, in addition to the actual
Ambady, Harizuka, & Kumar, 2004). expressions themselves (Ekman, 1979;
Furthermore, a close examination of the Matsumoto & Choi, 2004). Research is yet
balanced studies they reviewed in Table 4 to test the possible contributory roles of
of their original meta-analysis (Elfenbein these aspects of the face to emotion signal­
& Ambady, 2002) shows that only five ing, which is a possible rich source of infor­
studies provide evidence that the physical mation in the future.
signaling properties of the expressions used
as stimuli were equivalent across the
expressor ethnicities (Albas, McCluskey, ♦ Conclusion
& Albas, 1976; Kilbride & Yarczower,
1983; McCluskey, Albas, Niemi, Cuevas,
& Ferrer, 1975; McCluskey & Albas, In considering cultural influences on
1981; Mehta, Ward, & Strongman, 1992). nonverbal behavior, it is first important to
12-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 5:04 PM Page 230

230–––◆–––Factors of Influence

recognize the universal bases of those variations in relationship rules. International


behaviors, and to realize that culture’s influ­ Journal of Psychology, 21, 287–315.
ence on nonverbal behaviors occurs above Barnlund, D., & Araki, S. (1985). Intercultural
and beyond the universal bases of those encounters: The management of compli­
ments by Japanese and Americans. Journal
behaviors that we are all born with. With
of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 16, 9–26.
regard to emotion communication, we all
Barnlund, D., & Yoshioka, M. (1990).
start with the same base of universal, pan­
Apologies: Japanese and American styles.
cultural expressions. We learn rules about International Journal of Intercultural
how to modify and manage these expres­ Relations, 14, 193–206.
sions based on social circumstance (cultural Berenbaum, H., & Oltmanns, T. (1992).
display rules), and we learn rules about Emotional experience and expression in
how to manage our judgments of them (cul­ schizophrenia and depression. Journal of
tural decoding rules). Whereas we all rec­ Abnormal Psychology, 101, 37–44.
ognize universal emotions at levels well Berry, J. W., Poortinga, Y. H., Segall, M. H., &
beyond chance, there are cultural influences Dasen, P. R. (1992). Cross-cultural psy­
on the absolute levels of recognition accu­ chology: Research and applications. New
York: Cambridge University Press.
racy and on judgments of external intensity
Biehl, M., Matsumoto, D., Ekman, P., Hearn,
and internal subjective experience.
V., Heider, K., Kudoh, T., et al. (1997).
Most of our knowledge concerning cul­
Matsumoto and Ekman’s Japanese and
ture and nonverbal behaviors comes from Caucasian facial expressions of emotion
studies of facial expressions of emotion. The (JACFEE): Reliability data and cross-
few cross-cultural studies on other nonver­ national differences. Journal of Nonverbal
bal behaviors that do exist suggest consider­ Behavior, 21, 3–21.
able cultural differences in these. Yet there Boas, F., & Efron, D. F. (1936). A comparative
may be universal aspects to these other non­ investigation of gestural behavior patterns
verbal behaviors that research has just not in “racial” groups living under different as
yet uncovered. Examples include the raising well as similar environmental conditions.
of one or both arms in achievement or clap­ Psychological Bulletin, 33, 760.
Boucher, J. D., & Carlson, G. E. (1980).
ping as a sign of approval. Future research
Recognition of facial expression in three cul­
will not only continue to unravel the influ­
tures. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology,
ence of culture on facial expressions but will
11, 263–280.
also need to delve into these other possibili­ Brewer, M. B., & Kramer, R. M. (1985). The
ties for other nonverbal behaviors. psychology of intergroup attitudes and
behavior. Annual Review of Psychology,
36, 219–243.
♦ References Buss, D. M. (1991). Evolutionary personality
psychology. Annual Review of Psychology,
42, 459–491.
Albas, D. C., McCluskey, K. W., & Albas, C. A. Buss, D. M. (2001). Human nature and culture:
(1976). Perception of the emotional An evolutionary psychological perspective.
content of speech: A comparison of two Journal of Personality, 69, 955–978.
Canadian groups. Journal of Cross-Cultural Camras, L. A., Oster, H., Campos, J., Miyake, K.,
Psychology, 7, 481–490. & Bradshaw, D. (1992). Japanese and
Argyle, M., & Cook, M. (1976). Gaze American infants’ responses to arm restraint.
and mutual gaze. New York: Cambridge Developmental Psychology, 28, 578–583.
University Press. Charlesworth, W. R., & Kreutzer, M. A. (1973).
Argyle, M., Henderson, M., Bond, M., Iizuka, Y., Facial expressions of infants and children.
& Contarello, A. (1986). Cross-cultural In P. Ekman (Ed.), Darwin and facial
12-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 5:04 PM Page 231

Culture and Nonverbal Behavior–––◆–––231

expression (pp. 91–168). New York: Sight, sound, and sense (pp. 124–156).
Academic Press. Bloomington: Indiana University Press.
Chen, G. M. (1995). Differences in self- Ekman, P., & Friesen, W. (1969). The repertoire
disclosure patterns among Americans ver­ of nonverbal behavior: Categories, origins,
sus Chinese: A comparative study. Journal usage, and coding. Semiotica, 1, 49–98.
of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 26, 84–91. Ekman, P., & Friesen, W. (1971). Constants across
Chesney, M. A., Ekman, P., Friesen, W. V., culture in the face and emotion. Journal of
Black, G. W., et al. (1990). Type a behavior Personality and Social Psychology, 17,
pattern: Facial behavior and speech com­ 124–129.
ponents. Psychosomatic Medicine, 52, Ekman, P., & Friesen, W. V. (1975). Unmasking
307–319. the face; a guide to recognizing emotions
Chevalier-Skolnikoff, S. (1973). Facial expres­ from facial clues. Englewood Cliffs, NJ:
sion of emotion in nonhuman primates. Prentice Hall.
In P. Ekman (Ed.), Darwin and facial Ekman, P., Friesen, W. V., & Ellsworth, P.
expression (pp. 11–89). New York: (1972). Emotion in the human face: Guide­
Academic Press. lines for research and an integration of find­
Cole, P. M. (1986). Children’s spontaneous con­ ings. New York: Pergamon Press.
trol of facial expression. Child Development, Ekman, P., Friesen, W. V., O’Sullivan, M.,
57, 1309–1321. Chan, A., Diacoyanni-Tarlatzis, I., Heider,
Darwin, C. (1998). The expression of emotion K., et al. (1987). Universals and cultural dif­
in man and animals. New York: Oxford ferences in the judgments of facial expres­
University Press. (Original work published sions of emotion. Journal of Personality
1872) and Social Psychology, 53, 712–717.
Davidson, R. J. (2003). Parsing the subcompo­ Ekman, P., Levenson, R. W., & Friesen, W. V.
nents of emotion and disorders of emotion: (1983). Autonomic nervous system activity
Perspectives from affective neuroscience. In distinguishes among emotions. Science,
R. J. Davidson, K. R. Scherer, & H. H. 221, 1208–1210.
Goldsmith (Eds.), Handbook of affective Ekman, P., Matsumoto, D., & Friesen, W. (1997).
sciences (pp. 8–24). New York: Oxford Facial expression in affective disorders. In P.
University Press. Ekman & E. L. Rosenberg (Eds.), What the
Edelmann, R. J., Asendorpf, J., Contarello, A., face reveals: Basic and applied studies of
Georgas, J., Villanueva, C., & Zammuner, spontaneous expression using the facial
V. (1987). Self-reported verbal and non­ action coding system (FACs) (pp. 331–341).
verbal strategies for coping with embarrass­ New York: Oxford University Press.
ment in five European cultures. Social Ekman, P., & Rosenberg, E. L. (Eds.). (1998).
Science Information, 26, 869–883. What the face reveals: Basic and applied
Efron, D. (1941). Gesture and environment. studies of spontaneous expression using the
Oxford, England: King’s Crown Press. facial action coding system (FACs). New
Ekman, P. (1972). Universal and cultural differ­ York: Oxford University Press.
ences in facial expression of emotion. In J. Ekman, P., Sorenson, E. R., & Friesen, W. V.
R. Cole (Ed.), Nebraska symposium on (1969). Pancultural elements in facial dis­
motivation, 1971 (pp. 207–283). Lincoln: plays of emotion. Science, 164, 86–88.
Nebraska University Press. Elfenbein, H. A., & Ambady, N. (2002). On the
Ekman, P. (1973). Darwin and facial expres­ universality and cultural specificity of
sion; A century of research in review. New emotion recognition: A meta-analysis.
York: Academic Press. Psychological Bulletin, 128, 205–235.
Ekman, P. (1976). Movements with precise mean­ Elfenbein, H. A., & Ambady, N. (2003a).
ings. Journal of Communication, 26(3), Cultural similarity’s consequences: A dis­
14–26. tance perspective on cross-cultural differen­
Ekman, P. (1979). Facial signs: Facts, fantasies, ces in emotion recognition. Journal of
and possibilities. In T. Sebeok (Ed.), Cross-Cultural Psychology, 34, 92–110.
12-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 5:04 PM Page 232

232–––◆–––Factors of Influence

Elfenbein, H. A., & Ambady, N. (2003b). When programs for expressive behavior. Journal of
familiarity breeds accuracy: Cultural expo­ Research in Personality, 26, 273–280.
sure and facial emotion recognition. Jour­ Gosselin, P., Kirouac, G., & Dore, F. (1995).
nal of Personality and Social Psychology, Components and recognition of facial
85, 276–290. expression in the communication of emo­
Elfenbein, H. A., Mandal, M. K., Ambady, N., tion by actors. Journal of Personality and
& Harizuka, S. (2002). Cross-cultural Social Psychology, 68, 83–96.
patterns in emotion recognition: Highlighting Gudykunst, W. B., & Mody, B. (Eds.). (2001).
design and analytic techniques. Emotion, 2, Handbook of international and intercul­
75–84. tural communication. Newbury Park, CA:
Elfenbein, H. A., Mandal, M. K., Ambady, N., Sage.
Harizuka, S., & Kumar, S. (2004). Gudykunst, W. B., & Nishida, T. (1984).
Hemifacial differences in the in-group Individual and cultural influences on uncer­
advantage in emotion recognition. Cognition tainty reduction. Communication Mono­
and Emotion, 18, 613–629. graphs, 51, 23–36.
Ellgring, H. (1986). Nonverbal expression of Gudykunst, W. B., & Ting-Toomey, S. (1988).
psychological states in psychiatric patients. Culture and affective communication.
European Archives of Psychiatry and American Behavioral Scientist, 31, 384–400.
Neurological Sciences, 236, 31–34. Hall, E. T. (1963). A system for the nota­
Exline, R. V., Jones, P., & Maciorowski, K. tion of proxemic behaviors. American
(1977, August). Race, affiliative-conflict Anthropologist, 65, 1003–1026.
theory and mutual visual attention during Hall, E. T. (1966). The hidden dimension. New
conversation. Paper presented at the York: Doubleday.
American Psychological Association Annual Hall, E. T. (1973). The silent language. New
Convention, San Francisco. York: Anchor.
Fehr, B. J., & Exline, R. V. (1987). Social visual Hauser, M. (1993). Right hemisphere domi­
interactions: A conceptual and literature nance for the production of facial expres­
review. In A. W. Siegman & S. Feldstein sion in monkeys. Science, 261, 475–477.
(Eds.), Nonverbal behavior and communi­ Heller, M., & Haynal, V. (1994). Depression
cation (Vol. 2, pp. 225–326). Hillsdale, NJ: and suicide faces. Cahiers Psychiatriques
Erlbaum. Genevois, 16, 107–117.
Forston, R. F., & Larson, C. U. (1968). The Hofstede, G. H. (1980). Culture’s consequences:
dynamics of space: An experimental study in International differences in work-related
proxemic behavior among Latin Americans values. Beverly Hills: Sage.
and North Americans. Journal of Commu­ Izard, C. E. (1971). The face of emotion. East
nication, 18, 109–116. Norwalk, CT: Appleton-Century-Crofts.
Frank, M. G., Ekman, P., & Friesen, W. V. Jahoda, G. (1984). Do we need a concept of cul­
(1993). Behavioral markers and recogniz­ ture? Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology,
ability of the smile of enjoyment. Journal 15, 139–151.
of Personality and Social Psychology, Keltner, D., Moffitt, T., & Stouthamer-Loeber,
64, 83–93. M. (1995). Facial expressions of emotion
Friesen, W. V. (1972). Cultural differences in and psychopathology in adolescent boys.
facial expressions in a social situation: An Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 104,
experimental test of the concept of display 644–652.
rules. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Kilbride, J. E., & Yarczower, M. (1983). Ethnic
University of California, San Francisco. bias in the recognition of facial expressions.
Friesen, W. V., Ekman, P., & Wallbott, H. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 8, 27–41.
(1979). Measuring hand movements. Journal Kim, M. S., Hunter, J. E., Miyahara, A., Horvath,
of Nonverbal Behavior, 4, 97–112. A. M., Bresnahan, M., & Yoon, H. J. (1996).
Geen, T. (1992). Facial expressions in socially iso­ Individual vs. culture-level dimensions of
lated nonhuman primates: Open and closed individualism and collectivism: Effects on
12-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 5:04 PM Page 233

Culture and Nonverbal Behavior–––◆–––233

preferred conversation styles. Communica­ Matsumoto, D. (2002). Methodological require­


tion Monographs, 63, 29–49. ments to test a possible ingroup advantage
Kroeber, A. L., & Kluckholn, C. (1963). in judging emotions across cultures:
Culture: A critical review of concepts Comments on Elfenbein and Ambady
and definitions. Cambridge, MA: Harvard and evidence. Psychological Bulletin, 128,
University. 236–242.
Kudoh, T., & Matsumoto, D. (1985). Cross- Matsumoto, D., & Choi, J. W. (2004).
cultural examination of the semantic dimen­ Exploring decoder sources of the ingroup
sions of body postures. Journal of Personality advantage effect in recognizing facial
and Social Psychology, 48, 1440–1446. expressions of emotions. Unpublished man­
LaFrance, M., & Mayo, C. (1976). Racial uscript.
differences in gaze behavior during conver­ Matsumoto, D., Choi, J. W., Hirayama, S.,
sations: Two systematic observational stud­ Domae, A., & Yamaguchi, S. (2005).
ies. Journal of Personality and Social Culture, display rules, emotion regulation,
Psychology, 33, 547–552. and emotion judgments. Manuscript sub­
Levenson, R. W., Ekman, P., & Friesen, W. V. mitted for publication.
(1990). Voluntary facial action gener­ Matsumoto, D., Consolacion, T., Yamada, H.,
ates emotion-specific autonomic nervous Suzuki, R., Franklin, B., Paul, S., et al.
system activity. Psychophysiology, 27, (2002). American-Japanese cultural differ­
363–384. ences in judgments of emotional expres­
Levenson, R. W., Ekman, P., Heider, K., & sions of different intensities. Cognition and
Friesen, W. V. (1992). Emotion and auto­ Emotion, 16, 721–747.
nomic nervous system activity in the Matsumoto, D., & Ekman, P. (1989).
Minangkabau of West Sumatra. Journal American-Japanese cultural differences in
of Personality and Social Psychology, intensity ratings of facial expressions of
62, 972–988. emotion. Motivation and Emotion, 13,
Linton, R. (1936). The study of man: An intro­ 143–157.
duction. New York: Appleton. Matsumoto, D., Kasri, F., & Kooken, K. (1999).
Linville, P. W., & Jones, E. E. (1980). Polarized American-Japanese cultural differences in
appraisals of out-group members. Journal judgments of expression intensity and sub­
of Personality and Social Psychology, 38, jective experience. Cognition and Emotion,
689–703. 13, 201–218.
Markham, R., & Wang, L. (1996). Recognition Matsumoto, D., & Koopmann, B. (2004).
of emotion by Chinese and Australian Ecology, culture, personality, and emo­
children. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psy­ tional expression. Unpublished manuscript.
chology, 27, 616–643. Matsumoto, D., & Kudoh, T. (1987). Cultural
Matsumoto, D. (1989). Cultural influences on similarities and differences in the semantic
the perception of emotion. Journal of dimensions of body postures. Journal of
Cross-Cultural Psychology, 20, 92–105. Nonverbal Behavior, 11, 166–179.
Matsumoto, D. (1990). Cultural similarities and Matsumoto, D., & Kupperbusch, C. (2001).
differences in display rules. Motivation and Idiocentric and allocentric differences in
Emotion, 14, 195–214. emotional expression and experience. Asian
Matsumoto, D. (1992). American-Japanese Journal of Social Psychology, 4, 113–131.
cultural differences in the recognition of Matsumoto, D., Takeuchi, S., Andayani, S.,
universal facial expressions. Journal of Kouznetsova, N., & Krupp, D. (1998). The
Cross-Cultural Psychology, 23, 72–84. contribution of individualism-collectivism
Matsumoto, D. (1993). Ethnic differences to cross-national differences in display
in affect intensity, emotion judgments, dis­ rules. Asian Journal of Social Psychology,
play rule attitudes, and self-reported emo­ 1, 147–165.
tional expression in an American sample. Matsumoto, D., Weissman, M., Preston, K.,
Motivation and Emotion, 17, 107–123. Brown, B., & Kupperbusch, C. (1997).
12-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 5:04 PM Page 234

234–––◆–––Factors of Influence

Context-specific measurement of individu­ Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 15,


alism-collectivism on the individual level: 111–138.
The IC interpersonal assessment inven­ Romney, A. K., Boyd, J. P., Moore, C. C.,
tory (ICIAI). Journal of Cross-Cultural Batchelder, W. H., & Brazill, T. J. (1996).
Psychology, 28, 743–767. Culture as shared cognitive representations.
Matsumoto, D., Yoo, S. H., Hirayama, S., & Proceedings of the National Academy of
Petrova, G. (2005). Validation of an indi­ Sciences of the United States of America,
vidual-level measure of display rules: The 93, 4699–4705.
display rule assessment inventory (DRAI). Romney, A. K., Moore, C. C., & Rusch, C. D.
Emotion, 5, 23–40. (1997). Cultural universals: Measuring the
McCluskey, K., Albas, D., Niemi, R., Cuevas, semantic structure of emotion terms in
C., & Ferrer, C. (1975). Cultural differ­ English and Japanese. Proceedings of the
ences in the perception of the emotional National Academy of Sciences of the
content of speech: A study of the develop­ United States of America, 94, 5489–5494.
ment of sensitivity in Canadian and Rosenberg, E. L., & Ekman, P. (1994).
Mexican children. Developmental Psychol­ Coherence between expressive and experi­
ogy, 11, 551–555. ential systems in emotion. Cognition and
McCluskey, K. W., & Albas, D. C. (1981). Emotion, 8, 201–229.
Perception of the emotional content of Ruch, W. (1993). Extraversion, alcohol, and
speech by Canadian and Mexican children, enjoyment. Personality and Individual
adolescents and adults. International Differences, 16, 89–102.
Journal of Psychology, 16, 119–132. Ruch, W. (1995). Will the real relationship
Mehta, S. D., Ward, C., & Strongman, K. between facial expression and affective
(1992). Cross-cultural recognition of posed experience stand up: The case of exhilara­
facial expressions of emotion. New Zealand tion. Cognition and Emotion, 9, 33–58.
Journal of Psychology, 21, 74–77. Scherer, K. R. (1997a). Profiles of emotion-
Minami, M., & McCabe, A. (1995). Rice balls antecedent appraisal: Testing theoretical
and bear hunts: Japanese and North predictions across cultures. Cognition and
American family narrative patterns. Journal Emotion, 11, 113–150.
of Child Language, 22, 423–445. Scherer, K. R. (1997b). The role of culture in
Morris, D., Collett, P., Marsh, P., & emotion-antecedent appraisal. Journal of
O’Shaughnessy, M. (1980). Gestures: Their Personality and Social Psychology, 73,
origins and distribution. New York: 902–922.
Scarborough. Schimmack, U. (1996). Cultural influences on
Noesjirwan, J. (1977). Contrasting cultural pat­ the recognition of emotion by facial expres­
terns on interpersonal closeness in doctors: sions: Individualist or Caucasian cultures?
Waiting rooms in Sydney and Jakarta. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 27,
Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 8, 37–50.
357–368. Seligman, M. E., & Hager, J. (1972). Bio­
Noesjirwan, J. (1978). A rule-based analysis logical boundaries of learning. New York:
of cultural differences in social behavior: Appleton-Century-Crofts.
Indonesia and Australia. International Shaver, P., Murdaya, U., & Fraley, R. C. (2001).
Journal of Psychology, 13, 305–316. The structure of the Indonesian emotion lex­
Nomura, N., & Barnlund, D. (1983). Patterns of icon. Asian Journal of Social Psychology, 4,
interpersonal criticism in Japan and the 201–224.
United States. International Journal of Shaver, P. R., Wu, S., & Schwartz, J. C. (1992).
Intercultural Relations, 7, 1–18. Cross-cultural similarities and differences
Rohner, R. P. (1984). Toward a conception in emotion and its representation. In
of culture for cross-cultural psychology. M. S. Clark (Ed.), Emotion: Review of
12-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 5:04 PM Page 235

Culture and Nonverbal Behavior–––◆–––235

personality and social psychology (Vol. 13, American and European American dating
pp. 175–212). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. couples during interpersonal conflict.
Shuter, R. (1976). Proxemics and tactility in Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 28,
Latin America. Journal of Communication, 600–625.
26, 46–52. Vrij, A., & Winkel, F. W. (1991). Cultural
Shuter, R. (1977). A field study of nonverbal com­ patterns in Dutch and Surinam nonverbal
munication in Germany, Italy, and the United behavior: An analysis of simulated police/
States. Communication Monographs, 44, citizen encounters. Journal of Nonverbal
298–305. Behavior, 15, 169–184.
Snowdon, C. T. (2003). Expression of emotion Vrij, A., & Winkel, F. W. (1992). Cross-cultural
in nonhuman animals. In R. J. Davidson, police-citizen interactions: The influence of
K. Scherer, & H. H. Goldsmith (Eds.), race, beliefs, and nonverbal communication
Handbook of affective sciences (pp. 457– on impression formation. Journal of Applied
480). New York: Oxford University Press. Social Psychology, 22, 1546–1559.
Tajfel, H. (1982). Social psychology of inter­ Watson, O. M. (1970). Proxemic behavior:
group relations. Annual Review of Psychol­ A cross-cultural study. The Hague, The
ogy, 33, 1–39. Netherlands: Mouton.
Triandis, H. C. (1972). The analysis of subjec­ Watson, O. M., & Graves, T. D. (1966).
tive culture. New York: Wiley. Quantitative research in proxemic behavior.
Triandis, H. C., McCusker, C., & Hui, C. H. American Anthropologist, 68, 971–985.
(1990). Multimethod probes of individual­ Waxer, P. H. (1985). Video ethology: Television
ism and collectivism. Journal of Personality as a data base for cross-cultural studies in
and Social Psychology, 59, 1006–1020. nonverbal displays. Journal of Nonverbal
Tsai, J. L., & Levenson, R. W. (1997). Cultural Behavior, 9, 111–120.
influences of emotional responding: Chinese
12-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 5:04 PM Page 236
13-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 5:05 PM Page 237

13
CASTING NONVERBAL
BEHAVIOR IN THE MEDIA
Representations and Responses

� Valerie Manusov
University of Washington

� Adam Jaworski
Cardiff University

N onverbal behavior is often regarded as a set of actions that occur


between interactants in face-to-face settings. Indeed, many of the
nonverbal cues that we enact are done for and with other people in our
everyday interactions. We also learn a great deal about how to behave
nonverbally and what certain cues may mean through those interac­
tions, particularly in our encounters with family (Halberstadt, 1986).
Simultaneously, however, we are presented with a nonverbal code in
our exposure to an array of mediated sources. By providing normative
behaviors, interpretations for cues in context, and potential sanctions
for not adhering to the code, media present and encourage a particular
set of communication practices and ideologies.
This chapter focuses on the portrayal of nonverbal behavior in what
have been defined as “traditional” media: television, film, and the press (for

◆ 237
13-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 5:05 PM Page 238

238–––◆–––Factors of Influence

a review of online communication, see include when not to talk (i.e., to use silence;
Walther, this volume). We argue that non­ Jaworski, 1997), how to gesture (Morris,
verbal cues as they occur within mediated Collett, Marsh, & O’Shaughnessy, 1980),
coverage1—and the talk about those cues and rules for the use of time (Levine, 1997).
within the coverage—affect and reflect Media play a large part in teaching the non­
many deeply held beliefs about the nature, verbal code, at least within some societies.
importance, and meaning of nonverbal
behavior. To make this argument, we try to
answer four questions: (1) How are nonver­ AFFECTIVE SOCIALIZATION
bal codes learned? (2) What is the content of
those codes? (3) What codes exist in media An important part of being socialized
talk about nonverbal cues? and (4) What into a particular communication commu­
consequences do such codes have for media nity, and one that is particularly important
users? Our goal in answering these ques­ for understanding nonverbal behavior and
tions is to reveal the processes—and the media exposure, involves learning the ways
implications—of the media as an influenc­ in which emotions ought to be expressed
ing factor for our understanding and use of and experienced. Nonverbal cues are con­
nonverbal cues. We begin with how nonver­ nected commonly with emotional expres­
bal cues may be learned. sion (Ekman, 1972; but see Fridlund &
Russell, this volume), and research has
found that media perform a number of
♦ Learning the Nonverbal roles in the development and understanding
of affect. These roles include the normative
Code: Socialization
occurrence of nonverbal expressions of
emotion (i.e., how often such emotions
Socialization can be defined as a process of occur in everyday life) and the type of emo­
acquiring the cultural (including verbal and tion presented as most common in interac­
nonverbal) knowledge and skills needed tion. Research has also examined media
to become competent members of particu­ exposure and the development of particular
lar communities. Communicative compe­ affective skills. In addition, researchers have
tencies are included within the larger investigated the connection between non­
socialization process (Hymes, 1972) and verbal images in media and peoples’ affec­
involve “socialization through language tive reactions to those presentations. This
and socialization to use language” (Ochs & section reviews some arguments regarding
Schieffelin, 1986, p. 2) and learned nonver­ the connection between media and affective
bal communication (see Feldman & Tyler, understanding and response.
this volume). Saville-Troike (2003), in One of the most common areas of
examining the socialization processes research connecting nonverbal media dis­
involved in nonverbal communication, dis­ plays and affective socialization revolves
cusses how, apart from universal nonverbal around what children may learn about
patterns, such as some of the facial expres­ emotional expression from media exposure,
sions of emotion identified by Darwin particularly from television. Coats and
(1872) (see also Ekman, 1972), children Feldman (1995) (see also Coats, Feldman, &
in different societies are exposed to— Philippot, 1999) argue that television is a pri­
or taught explicitly—appropriate age-, mary means through which children (and
gender-, class-, and other-related nonverbal others) learn how to encode and decode
behavior. These learned behavioral patterns emotions and to discover when specific
13-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 5:05 PM Page 239

Casting Nonverbal Behavior in the Media–––◆–––239

emotional expressions are appropriate (see viewing are less easy to predict. For
also Saarni, 1985). More specifically, Wilson example, Coats and Feldman (1995)
and Smith (1998) noted that many “young assessed some of the potential outcomes
children encounter certain emotions and of exposure to television on the use of
affective situations on television long before nonverbal emotional expressions and
they experience those same phenomena in found that children who were frequent
real life” (p. 533). Some children’s shows TV watchers were better encoders of
even focus on teaching viewers to recognize those emotions expressed commonly on
and label feelings (Wilson & Smith, 1998). television shows than were infrequent
Researchers have speculated on the watchers; they were also better at encod­
reason for television’s primacy in teaching ing spontaneous (but poorer at encoding
young viewers about affect. For instance, posed) emotional displays. The authors
Collins (1983) argues that young children interpreted the findings about overall
are particularly affected by television encoding skill cautiously, suggesting that
portrayals of nonverbal expressions children who watch TV frequently are less
because, in part, their cognitive develop­ able to regulate their emotional expres­
ment may not yet allow them to focus as sions than are children less often exposed
fully on verbal content; thus, nonverbal ele­ to television. An additional study found
ments are more likely to capture their atten­ that frequent viewers could better decode
tion. Researchers suggest, however, some others’ facial expressions, but they “had a
problems with such learning. For Houle less differentiated, more simplistic view
and Feldman (1991), “Television presents of the consequences of nonverbal self-
salient, engaging, even prestigious mod­ presentation” (Feldman, Coats, & Spielman,
els of emotionality and emotional expres­ 1996, p. 1718).
sion . . . [But] it appears that the models Other researchers have focused on what
television presents . . . are different from is being learned from media: the affective
[the] models presented in children’s social content and responses to affect. Weiss and
environments” (p. 329). Specifically, (1) Wilson (cited in Wilson & Smith, 1998),
televised emotional expressions appear, in for instance, found that subplots in a
general, much more often than they do in television show that were humorous led
everyday interactions and (2) certain emo­ children to have less negative evaluations
tions (e.g., happiness, sadness, and anger) of the story’s main plot (i.e., an earth­
occur more commonly than others and in quake). Specifically, children who saw
different proportions than they are likely to shows with a humorous subplot rated the
do in nonmediated contexts (Houle & main character’s mood more positively
Feldman, 1991). Furthermore, several stud­ and, perhaps more important, judged the
ies have documented that families on televi­ occurrence of earthquakes in real-life as
sion are more likely to deal with simple, less severe than did those who did not
rather than complex, emotions (e.g., have the positively affective subplot. Media
Larson, 1993), which may be one explana­ images have also been found to induce feel­
tion for why certain simple skills, but not ings of horror: Wilson and Smith (1998)
complex skills, are facilitated by increased argued that children tend to be frightened
television viewing. by horrific images, with younger children
As would be expected, the amount of more likely to be scared by visually fright­
exposure appears to play a role in affec­ ening characters and older children fright­
tive socialization. But the specific findings ened by seeing a televised victim’s terror
regarding affective learning and television reactions.
13-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 5:05 PM Page 240

240–––◆–––Factors of Influence

COGNITIVE SOCIALIZATION found to be distortions from “actual”


appearances. Most notably, “current soci­
The work just reviewed suggests that etal standards for beauty inordinately
media, particularly television, may play a emphasize the desirability of thinness”
role in emphasizing the expression of certain (Hargreaves & Tiggemann, 2002, p. 288),
emotions over others, increasing skill for the and the media—particularly fashion mag­
spontaneous expression and interpretation azines—are a primary source of those stan­
of those emotions but possibly lessening dards. Thus, a likely schema resulting from
other affective abilities. Socialization rele­ media exposure is that thinness is desirable.
vant to nonverbal communication is not Such exposure does not mean that people’s
only about affect, however. Semioticians schemata reflect the belief that most bodies
and critical discourse analysts have long are thin, however. Instead, one study showed
emphasized the role the media play in that viewing appearance-related advertise­
developing group consciousness: construct­ ments led people to believe that people’s
ing, reinforcing, and legitimizing popular actual body size was, on average, larger than
ideologies—or commonsense and normative it is (Myers & Biocca, 1999).
attitudes and beliefs—about social reality Such beauty schemata are more common
(see Barthes, 1977; Fairclough, 1995). for females than for males, reflecting part
Some of these ideas are, inevitably, of what Goffman (1976) labeled gender
about affect. As Wilson and Smith (1998) stereotypes and showing the importance
argue, for example, “Children’s beliefs of media in cognitive socialization about
about emotional events in the real world gender (which is often linked to physical
can be influenced by television” (p. 540). appearance schemata). Using print advertis­
Furthermore, the type and frequency of ing as a source of data, Goffman showed
emotional expressions on television are not that pose, facial expression, and other
consistent with what occurs in everyday forms of “body language” worked to depict
interactions, often leading frequent viewers women as shy, dreamy, gentle, helpless, and
to have an unrealistic image of “normal” likely to be manipulated. Males’ behaviors,
social expression and self-presentation what Browne (1998) calls “executive behav­
(Coats & Feldman, 1995; Zillmann, 1991). iors” (i.e., active and aggressive), engen­
In particular, the typical straightforward dered images of power, control, and
connection between what a character is dominance. For both female and male
feeling and his or her expression of that images, however, Goffman (1976) contended
feeling may lead to a simplified view of that the advertisements worked to create
the strategic nature of nonverbal behavior, beliefs about sex differences that were unre­
one that does not take dissemblance into alistic. More recent analyses of advertise­
account (Feldman et al., 1996; Houle & ments reflect that such images—represented
Feldman, 1991). in nonverbal cues—persist in adult-targeted
Additionally, however, the media pro­ advertisements (Klassen, Jasper, & Schwartz,
vide cognitive structures about physical 1993) as well as advertisements oriented
appearance. Specifically, scholars argue to children (Browne, 1998; Kolbe, 1990;
that media provide repeated images of ideal for a broader discussion of sex or gender
physical appearance that help to create and nonverbal communication, see Hall,
relatively stable appearance schemata this volume).
(Hargreaves & Tiggemann, 2002). Like affect Gender schemata related to appear­
schemata, appearance schemata have been ance are instituted in other ways. In his
13-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 5:05 PM Page 241

Casting Nonverbal Behavior in the Media–––◆–––241

assessment of television commercials in (p. 1173). Although learning about prod­


India, for example, Roy (2005) followed ucts and their linkage to self-identity may
other scholars in asserting that females are occur in the process of shopping with
often presented as the province and prop­ family and friends (Derscheid et al., 1996),
erty of men by the nonverbal cues directed it also is brought about through exposure
toward them in photographic images. In to media (Seiter, 1993).
his study, Roy addressed specifically
where males in two commercials looked.
His analysis confirmed that in addition SUMMARY
to other choices regarding camera angle,
dress, and music, males gazed at females in The research on socialization relevant
the commercial, but females did not gaze to nonverbal cues has focused primarily on
at males, and the gaze direction suggested two areas: (1) patterns of use in emotional
that the females were there to be looked at expressions and development and (2)
and, in some cases, owned. Roy argued as effects of schemata regarding ideal physical
well that the females in the commercials types and other aspects of identity. In both
acted in a way that was complicit non- kinds of socialization, certain patterns and
verbally with the gendered ideology (i.e., types of nonverbal cues are portrayed as
accepting the gaze). more common or appropriate than others,
Additionally, researchers have exam­ and individuals’ view of the self may be
ined what children learn from media about created and judged in relation to those
being consumers. For example, Derscheid, patterns and types. In addition, media rep­
Kwon, and Fang (1996) focused their resentations of emotional expressions, body
argument on the processes of consumer size, and gender cues are often inconsistent
socialization (i.e., how children learn about with actual, everyday behavior, creating a
shopping, products, and stores). They code that exists in the media but not within
argued that young children cannot read the larger communicative system in which
labels on products but can and do recog­ the code is embedded.
nize nonlinguistic symbols (McNeal, 1987).
This recognition may increase as media ♦ The Content of Media
exposure increases. Furthermore, Derscheid
Representations:
et al. (1996) found that more exposure to
TV or books by preschoolers was related
What Is the Code?
to a greater ability to recognize nonlinguis­
tic product symbols. More important, To discern more fully the prevalence and
perhaps, this recognition often leads to nature of media portrayals of nonverbal
product preference, and such preference has behavior, a range of studies have investi­
been linked with developing identities. gated the content of nonverbal cues in the
Specifically, products and their affective media. Many of these projects have used
symbols may become part of how children content analysis (or other quantitative
choose to identify themselves to others approaches) to discern the message value of
(Seiter, 1993). As Derscheid et al. (1996) nonverbal cues within a particular media
assert, “TV, videos, and books allow source; others provide a qualitative (often
children to imitate different personalities semiotic or critical discourse) analysis to
and act out fantasies while they construct identify the content of media coverage. The
their identity from media role models” current section reviews some of this work,
13-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 5:05 PM Page 242

242–––◆–––Factors of Influence

including scholarship whose aim is to soap operas and discerned that female
critique the code’s content. characters tend to reflect only a few body
types, whereas males are portrayed with
more variation (although televised body
UNCOVERING THE CODE types are, overall, thinner than in the actual
population). Also, characters with larger
Considerable research has focused on bodies tended to be cast in fewer “roman­
media sources to discover the content of tic” contexts and are outfitted less reveal­
certain types of coverage, the relative fre­ ingly than thinner characters. Larger bodied
quency of such coverage, and possible links characters also tend to be older than their
between the coverage and other important thinner counterparts (White et al., 1999).
variables. Most commonly, researchers
have investigated the coverage of particular
ideologies and their potential associations CRITIQUING THE CODE
with viewers’ reactions. Content analyses
have revealed, for instance, that televi­ Although research has not investigated
sion emphasizes physical attractiveness for all aspects of the cultural code presented
women (Furnham & Mak, 1999), although nonverbally in media sources, it has
recent trends suggest that attractiveness is revealed an overall tendency to present ide­
emphasized increasingly for males as well alized images (young, attractive, thin con­
(Bordo, 1999; Coupland, 2005). sumers). In response to this apparent code,
The focus on appearance appears to a number of sociologic and discourse
reflect other ideologies. Coupland (2003) analytic scholars have critiqued the presen­
argues that advertisements, epitomized in tation of these ideologies (e.g., Bancroft,
skin-care and other beauty-product market­ 1998; Jaworski, 2003; Shaw, 1998), with a
ing, display an ageist ideology, representing particular concern with those ideologies
getting older solely in physical terms and surrounding the body. Their studies share a
as fundamentally negative. In an analysis common premise that in late-modern, capi­
of the texts in British magazine skin-care talist societies, personal identities have
advertisements, Coupland (2003) found become more fluid and changeable than
that discourses are “scientized” and “mar­ they used to be in traditional, premodern
ketized” and employed to provide desirable societies. In the contemporary world, schol­
solutions to alleviate “premature ageing,” ars argue that identity has taken the form of
particularly for women. She also argues a highly reflexive “project of self” linked to
that the ethos of body management and lifestyle choices and stances, consumption
modification for the sake of maintaining of goods and services, and mass media
a youthful appearance is sanctioned by mediation of personal experience (i.e., a
the normalization of plastic surgery proce­ persistent but contingent and ongoing,
dures, to which cosmetics have become often aspirational “program” that provides
an “inexpensive” precursor or temporary a focus for our daily activities, narratives,
replacement (Coupland, 2005). and encounters with others; c.f. Bauman,
Other research has focused on the repre­ 1998; Coupland, Nussbaum, & Grossman,
sentation of different physical types in the 1993; Giddens, 1991).
media, presenting an ideology regarding a More central to nonverbal cues, Feather-
preference for thinness. For example, White, stone (1991) and Shilling (1993) provide
Brown, and Ginsburg (1999) content evidence that the body has become a project
analyzed prime-time shows and afternoon site, fueled by media images of the “ideal”
13-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 5:05 PM Page 243

Casting Nonverbal Behavior in the Media–––◆–––243

body, leading some to strive for “perfec­ birth, who says she would rather have a
tion” and leaving the others frustratingly nose job than her hearing. Needless to
“inadequate.” Moreover, the theme of the say, there is nothing wrong with her nose
ideal and “deviant” bodies is the dominant at all. (Edwards, 1998, p. 29)
one in the literature dealing with media
representations of the body and the viewers’ The concept of deviant bodies can be
or readers’ reactions to these images. related to Goffman’s presentation of stigma
For example, a clear pattern emerged in as “an undesired differentness from what
Jaworski’s (2003) discourse analytic study we had anticipated” (Goffman, 1997,
of the interviews with “ordinary” members p. 74). The fear of the deviant body implies
of the public in the BBC four-part series and can only be made meaningful in relation
Naked, broadcast in the United Kingdom in to the ideal, desirable, and normal body. Of
November and December 1998. Specifically, course, neither is the ideal or idealized image
Jaworski observed that the four, roughly of the body a given, nor is it the same and
identified, age groups of the interviewees— unchanging for all people and at all times.
middle aged, young adults, teenagers, and Just as individuals’ identities, self-percep­
elderly—positioned talk about their bodies tions, other representations, and so on are
in terms of an ideal of youthful looks, good flexible, changeable, and multiplex, so are
health, and sexual attractiveness and fulfill­ the representations of the ideal body. At any
ment. In contrast, the groups associated the given time in a specific community (social,
deviant body image with prepuberty, old age, gender, professional, or any other group),
illness, and loss of sexual drive or partners. however, there are certain well-recognized
The findings of this work are consistent and accepted principles on which the idea of
with Urla and Terry’s scholarship (1995) an ideal body is based. This is, for example,
distinguishing between normal and deviant evident from the different representations of
bodies (where normal does not equate with the “ideal” body in the history of (Western)
natural). Urla and Terry argued, like art, whether the representation is Leonardo
Jaworski (2003), that the power constraints da Vinci’s drawing of the classically perfect
imposed on body representations have led to Vitruvian Man (ca. 1490), Jacques-Louis
the widespread acceptance of a binary split David’s painting, Napoleon in His Study
between acceptable and unacceptable body (1812), depicting the Emperor as fusing
images: “normal versus deviant” or “healthy the monarchichal body politic with revolu­
versus pathological.” This is problematic tionary symbolism, or the practices of “nor­
because “deviance” is not so much linked to malizing” whiteness through the colonial
a particular set of body traits as to their per­ photography of the 19th century represent­
ceived connotations imposed by a hegemonic ing the imperfect body of the racial “other”
social order and value system. In the case (Mirzoeff, 1995).
of the Naked documentary, the problem is The discursive or visual representations
exemplified in the comment by The Sunday of such ideal bodies are constituted by and
Times TV critic, Edwards (1998), who sums are constitutive of the power relations
up the program’s impact as follows: operating within the community. With
regard to the issue of social preoccupation
[Whereas] some people might tune in to with the “ideal versus deviant” bodies,
Naked because of the promise of nudity, Douglas (1966) argues that because the
what keeps you watching is the stories, body can be used as a symbol of any
not the pictures, most dramatically that bounded system, such as society, negotiat­
of Louise, a 15-year-old girl, deaf since ing the margins of bodily acceptability can
13-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 5:05 PM Page 244

244–––◆–––Factors of Influence

be indicative of negotiating the margins of subvert and challenge these ideologies. This
society, and the shifting of these margins reflexivity of the interviewees is consistent
may have consequences for the centers of with other forms of reflexivity associated
power. This preoccupation can explain with the late-modern era (Beck, 1992,
our fear of, and at the same time fascina­ 1994; Giddens, 1990, 1991), allowing indi­
tion with, “freaks,” “monsters,” “cyborgs,” viduals to tackle their anxieties and uncer­
“aliens,” and so on (Eubanks, 1996). tainties concerning the changing beliefs and
Yet our fascination with such patholo­ value systems and their own shifting identi­
gized and stigmatized bodies also has more ties as seen and experienced through their
of a personal dimension. Shildrick (2002) bodies (Lupton & Tulloch, 1998).
argues that “monsters” (i.e., the excluded
bodies that fail to conform to any corpo­
real norm) may sometimes turn up in our SUMMARY
own self-perceptions.
Instead of remaining at the outer regions In this section, we discussed scholarship
of our embodied selves, “monsters” may at that has investigated and critiqued the con­
times reflect aspects of our own subjectivi­ tent of media portrayals of at least some
ties, creating uncertainties and anxieties of nonverbal cues (i.e., those focusing primar­
our self-perception and self-identification ily on appearance cues or the body). This
(Stafford, 1991). For example, as far as work reflects several ideologies common to
our humanity is defined in contrast to non­ media representations, helping to reveal the
human creatures (e.g., monsters), we often larger cultural code reflected in these repre­
project a “monstrous” image onto our own sentations, which may be, at once, both
bodies through negative self-perception utilitarian and problematic for the overall
(the extreme case being dysmorphophobia) culture. In some cases, these implied ideolo­
or, in an instance of carnivalesque play gies are derived from the nonverbal images.
with identities, by putting on a mask of a In other cases, they are apparent in how
Halloween monster. As Shaw (1998) media talk about the nonverbal cues. The
observes, however, the women she inter­ latter, an analysis of metacommunication, is
viewed on the role of media images of the at the core of studying ideological stances in
body, their self-perception, and identity media texts (as well as in face-to-face inter­
action), because it is at this level that
are not deluded “cultural dopes.” . . . we can gain insight into how social groups
Rather, they are an active, interpreting, value and orient to language and communi­
knowledgeable and diverse audience, cation (varieties, processes, effects) through
who attribute meaning to cultural images the study of folk beliefs about commu­
of female “beauty,” and negotiate their nication, people’s attitudes, and awareness
relationship with their own bodies about their verbal and nonverbal conduct.
within the constraints of the “fashion We turn now more directly to the metacom­
beauty complex.” (p. 22) municative role of the media.

Although most of the discourses in


Naked (Jaworski, 2003) seem to conform ♦ Metapragmatic Discourse
to the dominant ideologies of the body that
conceptualize the normative body as young,
attractive, sexualized, and so on, a number In the previous section, we discussed several
of participants in the program appear to studies dealing with media discourses to
13-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 5:05 PM Page 245

Casting Nonverbal Behavior in the Media–––◆–––245

assess what media sources (e.g., talk media. In a qualitative discourse analytic
show guests) presume about various study, for example, Jaworski and Galasiński
nonverbal cues (e.g., Greenberg, Sherry, (2002) examined U.K. press reports of
Busselle, Hnilo, & Smith, 1997; Shaw, President Clinton’s testimony to the grand
1998). Additional work has concerned jury in the Clinton-Lewinsky affair as seen
itself with the media’s own portrayals of in the video released on September 21,
specific nonverbal acts (e.g., Featherstone, 1998. This study focuses on the papers’
1991). These discourses or “stories” form choices of the descriptors and interpreta­
part of what we term here verbal and visual tions of Clinton’s nonverbal behavior in
metadiscourses of nonverbal behavior (i.e., the video, particularly in the ways they
talk or visual presentation that comments combined written text and still pictures
on the meaning of the nonverbal cues). In from the video in reporting how Clinton
our own work, which we review in this “looked” and “behaved” during the
section, we argue that metadiscursive repre­ testimony.
sentations may enter the public conscious­ The relative indeterminacy and immedi­
ness and come to constitute structured acy of nonverbal behavior (e.g., facial
understandings, perhaps even “common­ expressions, gestures, body posture) allow
sense” understandings of how communica­ the media as well as the social actors
tion works, what it is usually like, what in noninstitutional, face-to-face contexts
certain ways of speaking and nonverbal to use metapragmatic manipulation of
behavior connote and imply, and what they nonverbal behavior (i.e., strategic [re-]
ought to be like. That is, metadiscourse interpretation) or glossing of a particular
works at an ideological level and influences nonverbal expression. Jaworski and
people’s actions and priorities in a wide Galasiński’s analysis revealed that the
range of ways, some clearly visible and metapragmatic nature of the reports was
others much less so. not so much aimed at the “accurate”
reporting of what happened regarding
Clinton’s facial expressions, gestures, tone
JAWORSKI AND GALASIŃSKI of voice, and certain physiological reactions
(e.g., perspiration). Rather, they aimed at
Through metadiscourse commentary constructing a particular version of reality,
(remarking on communicative perfor­ a version that is ideologically compatible
mance, style, rhetorical function, or silence; with the dominant ideologies subscribed to
Jaworski, 1997), people can influence and by the newspapers.
negotiate how an utterance is or should Overall, the comparison of the eight
have been heard or try to modify the values newspapers’ accounts of Clinton’s nonver­
attributed to it (e.g., “What I meant to say bal behavior demonstrates a rather clear
was . . .”). At the metalevel of communica­ split between broadsheet and tabloid
tion, we can mark our personal or group newspapers. The former tended to be posi­
identities, display expertise, claim incompe­ tive (The Guardian, Financial Times, The
tence, and do many other sorts of “personal Independent, The Daily Telegraph) or at
identification work” or “social relationship least nonjudgmental (The Western Mail).
work” (Jaworski, Coupland, & Galasiński, Of the broadsheets, only The Daily
2004). Therefore, as an important site of Telegraph in its editorial was strongly criti­
“ideological work,” metadiscursive studies cal of Clinton, which is not very surprising
have been especially fruitful in unraveling given the newspaper’s conservative bias.
the underlying code (or codes) held by the The tabloids, on the other hand, tended to
13-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 5:05 PM Page 246

246–––◆–––Factors of Influence

be mildly critical (The Daily Mail), at a event. Across 218 texts, derived from an
minimum, or, more commonly, sensation­ array of print, radio, and television discus­
ally negative and disparaging (The Sun, The sions of the handshake, the authors argued
Mirror). With regard to the last three that three potentially competing views on
papers, despite its conservatism, The Daily nonverbal behavior emerged. These por­
Mail’s position was relatively uncritical trayed nonverbal cues
of the Democratic President. The other
two tabloids, despite their opposing politi­ (1) as informative (i.e., reflecting emo­
cal leanings (The Sun to the right and The tions, statements of the relationship
Mirror to the left), seemed to adopt a unan­ between the two men and their cultures,
imous stance with regard to the coverage and indicators of personal characteris­
of this story by overriding their political tics), (2) as performative (i.e., as strate­
differences with the overarching ethos of gic, symbolic, and performed for a larger
sensationalism and focusing on human inter­ audience), and (3) as transformative
est stories in tabloid publishing (Allan, 1999). (having the potential to alter the nature
of interaction and future events). (p. 2;
italics ours)
MANUSOV AND COLLEAGUES
The same behaviors (the handshake and
As critical discourse analysts, Jaworski surrounding nonverbal cues, such as facial
and Galasiński’s (2002) work involves expressions and other arm movements) were
assessing the performative value of meta­ legitimately given a range of interpretations,
discursive commentary. Manusov and her reflecting very different conceptions of how
colleagues (Manusov & Bixler, 2003; nonverbal cues work in communication.
Manusov & Milstein, 2005), on the other These findings comment on the “relative
hand, focused on metadiscourse as a way to indeterminacy of nonverbal behavior” as
learn about how the media talk about non­ argued by Jaworski and Galasiński (2002),
verbal cues and, in doing so, reflect a way and they are consistent with Patterson
of thinking about the nature and meaning (1983) and Manusov (1990) in showing that
of nonverbal cues. To explore media reflec­ the same behaviors allow for diverse inter­
tions regarding the way nonverbal cues may pretations, even of their very origin.
work and what they may mean, Manusov Manusov and Milstein (2005) found a
and her colleagues accessed media texts similar variability in frames for the hand­
that commented on the 1993 handshake shake and its accompanying cues in their
between Yitzhak Rabin and Yasser Arafat. analysis of Israeli and Palestinian press cov­
The handshake event, while not representa­ erage of the same event. Somewhat similar to
tive of everyday interaction, is recognized Manusov and Bixler (2003), Manusov and
as a highly significant and public nonverbal Milstein (2005) found two primary frames
cue. It has also been talked about in media for presenting the meaning of nonverbal cues
sources for the past 13 years. in this press, which they labeled representa­
Manusov and Bixler (2003) set out to tions (a combination of nonverbal cues’
look at U.S. metadiscourse specifically to informative and performative natures) and
uncover the media’s implied portrayal of transformations. Representations involved
the nature of nonverbal behavior (i.e., the conceptualization of nonverbal cues as
whether it was a set of natural signs, strate­ a “stand-in” for a larger process, issue, or
gic symbols, etc.) in its coverage of the state, often something intangible or abstract
13-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 5:05 PM Page 247

Casting Nonverbal Behavior in the Media–––◆–––247

and based in the larger event or context in from particular kinds of advertising
which the behaviors occurred. The more strategies. Specifically, Chaudhuri and Buck
specific meanings for the nonverbal cues (1995) argued that there is an important
framed as representations were peace­ link between an advertisement model’s
hope-optimism, violence, betrayal, anguish, facial expression and the likelihood that an
authority-legitimacy, agreement-promise, audience will share the emotion vicariously
and dislike. Finally, transformative meanings (for a more general discussion of emotional
focused on the potential of nonverbal cues to contagion, see Tickle-Degnen, this volume).
bring about a new state or process or to serve Furthermore, the authors argued that, “The
as a time marker from which events could be consumer comes to associate the brand with
measured. The more specific transformations the emotion generated (happiness, or relief
included gaining legitimacy, increasing status, from fear and anxiety) and sees the brand as
working as a curse, and moving backward. a status instrument that obtains rewards
and stays punishment” (Chaudhuri & Buck,
1995, p. 425). In their study, advertising
SUMMARY strategies designed to enhance mood were
associated positively with actual affective
Together, the work on metadiscourse response. Attempts to portray status were
provides an indirect, or even ironic, means linked positively with audience arousal.
for understanding the role of the media in
nonverbal communication by focusing on
the “talk” about the nonverbal cues. Thus, SELF-IMAGE
unlike much of the work described earlier,
our work focusing on metadiscourse allows The work locating a link between media
the larger nonverbal code to be understood portrayals of ideal attractiveness and the
by investigating the way it is discussed or development of attractiveness schemata have
presented in the media, with an assumption also found important connections to the
that such presentations may work to pro­ effects or consequences of such schemata.
mote particular beliefs and ideologies rather Many of these consequences revolve around
than others. We turn now toward other one’s own image and esteem. In particular,
implications of the media code for non­ activating appearance schemata prior to
verbal cues. watching appearance-related commercials
has led female viewers to feel more anger,
less confidence, and greater overall body
♦ The Consequences dissatisfaction (Hargreaves & Tiggemann,
of the Code 2002). Body dissatisfaction is often
associated with such outcomes as lowered
self-esteem (Henderson-King & Henderson-
Many of the studies mentioned in the previ­ King, 1997; Richards, Casper, & Larson,
ous sections concern not only what is cov­ 1990) and eating disorders (Harrison, 1997;
ered in media portrayals of nonverbal cues. Twamley & Davis, 1999). Activation of
They are also concerned deeply with the appearance schemata may also lead to dis­
consequences or outcomes of the portrayals, crimination against larger body types (White
especially if viewed over time and over et al., 1999).
contexts. For instance, immediate affective The way people talk about the body,
responses have been investigated as resulting as revealed by Shaw (1998) and Jaworski
13-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 5:05 PM Page 248

248–––◆–––Factors of Influence

(2003), is equally ideologized, informed frequency also predicted accurately the


by scientific, medical, and especially com­ popular vote outcomes across all eight elec­
mercial (Coupland, 2003) discourses of tions they analyzed, confirming the authors’
the body. As Foucault (1978, 1979) has contention about the importance of vocal
argued, body representations, both lay and cues in determining status and the effect of
scientific, are guided by similar truth- status on election outcomes.
seeking principles leading to modes of But looks or sound work differently, as
regulation, containment, incitement, and would be expected, depending on the media
resistance. As such, these representations form used. Across two studies, Patterson,
turn bodies from purely “natural” to Churchill, Burger, and Powell (1992)
socially “constructed” entities (Lupton, focused on this issue, which they labeled
2000; Synnott, 1993), constrained by rela­ presentation modality, and found a range of
tions of power (Foucault’s systems of interesting results. Using tapes of the 1984
knowledge or knowledge production) and debates between presidential candidates
by social and material inequality (see Urla Ronald Reagan and Walter Mondale,
& Terry, 1995, p. 3). Patterson et al. found evidence for a visual
modality effect. Specifically, in their second
study, the authors noted that viewers who
PERSUASION only saw the debates (i.e., had visual infor­
mation but no audio) judged Mondale much
Whereas esteem (especially low esteem) lower than they judged Reagan on expres­
is unlikely to be the persuasive intent of siveness and physical attractiveness. This
mediated nonverbal cues, the larger ideolo­ “benefit” for Reagan had not occurred in the
gies mentioned in this chapter may well be. audiovisual and audio presentations when
Indeed, research has shown that nonverbal the audiences had the opportunity to hear—
cues in the media appear to have addi­ or could be distracted by listening to—what
tional, widespread consequences, and most the debaters said. Thus, certain nonverbal
of these implications center around the cues (e.g., the tendency to blink more,
persuasive impact of nonverbal cues. One exchange gaze, or use fewer head move­
notable body of research has looked at the ments) appeared to work against Mondale,
effects of politicians’ nonverbal behaviors but only when those cues were the sole focus
(as well as the behaviors of people talking of the participants’ evaluations.
about or to politicians) on outcomes such In addition to televised debates, research
as voting. Much of this research focuses on on nonverbal cues in other election coverage
nonverbal cues during televised debates. has also found that such cues may play
Gregory and Gallaher (2002) suggest an important role in image creation and,
that in addition to appearing presidential, ultimately, in audiences’ voting behavior.
candidates fare better when they also sound Rosenberg, Bohan, McCafferty, and Harris
presidential. In their analysis of the funda­ (1986), who argue for the importance of
mental frequency of candidates’ voices dur­ candidate perception in election outcome,
ing 19 televised debates from the 1960 also found specifically that a candidate’s
to 2000 presidential elections, they found appearance (as judged from a photograph)
that certain frequencies were linked with affects judgments of the candidate.
the perceived dominance and “command­ Specifically, certain candidates were seen as
ing presence” of some candidates. The “more congressional” or as having more
metric the authors determined from this integrity and competence than were others.
13-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 5:05 PM Page 249

Casting Nonverbal Behavior in the Media–––◆–––249

Those rated more favorably were more about Ronald Reagan than when discussing
likely to win a mock election by both Walter Mondale) also had an audience that
university students and a sample from the was significantly more likely to vote for
general population. Although not stated by Reagan than the audiences of CBS or NBC.
the researchers, the candidates in the favor­ In the study, the authors provide a case for
able conditions tended to be smiling and the role of facial expressions, rather than
had good posture, which, arguably, is what audience selectivity, as the likely causal
affected the election outcome. variable in voting behavior.
An affective consequence of televised Such bias may occur when talking to, in
nonverbal behaviors was found in a study addition to talking about, politicians. In
of political leaders’ facial displays. In their three studies of Israeli television interview­
analysis of the emotional responses viewers ers, Babad (1999) asked U.S. participants
had to videotaped excerpts of the candidates (who did not understand the language in the
in the 1984 U.S. presidential election, interviews) to judge the interviewers’ non­
Sullivan and Masters (1988) found that for verbal behaviors. Despite Israeli law that
most of the candidates, neutral and happy demands broadcasters act neutrally to can­
or reassuring facial displays evoked (self­ didates of all parties, Babad’s first study
reported) positive emotions in audience reflected that all six interviewers studied
members. These reactions were more intense used different nonverbal behaviors with dif­
in displays that came closest to the election, ferent interviewees, although some of the
particularly for Ronald Reagan (the pattern interviewers were more consistent than were
was reversed for Reagan’s primary Demo­ others. In the second study, analysis of one
cratic rival, Walter Mondale). Importantly, interviewer’s behaviors when talking to two
the authors found that emotional responses candidates for prime minister in 1996
predicted attitudes toward the candidates, (Shimon Peres and Benjamen Netanyahu)
such that audience members viewed more showed that the interviewer’s nonverbal
favorably those candidates (such as Gary cues were judged as much more favorable
Hart) whose facial expressions evoked the when talking to one candidate than to the
most positive self-reported emotional other. The third study included a more
responses (Sullivan & Masters, 1988). detailed analysis of the first set of data. In it,
Newscasters’ nonverbal behaviors when Babad found that the positive impressions
discussing a candidate also appear to play a emitted by the interviewers were based on
role in audiences’ judgments about political more smiling and nodding, a relaxed face,
candidates. An initial analysis of 227 seg­ and more round hand movements. Negative
ments of the 1976 U.S. presidential election impressions were tied to “beating” hand
coverage documented that newscasters may movements, nonverbal regulation of the
use more positive facial expressions when interview, forward lean, head thrusting,
talking about one candidate or another blinking, and appearing sarcastic. Overall,
(Friedman, Mertz, & DiMatteo, 1980). more “aggressive” interviewers were those
Later research (e.g., Mullen et al., 1986) likely to use nonverbal cues more differen­
found evidence that such facial “bias” may tially across interviewees (Babad, 1999).
affect viewers’ attitudes toward the candi­ But it is not just interviewers who may
dates: The only U.S. station (ABC) whose act or react differently and affect audience’s
broadcaster showed facial favoritism views. Other research has looked at the
toward a candidate in 1984 (Peter Jennings nonverbal reactions of other parties in and
used more positive facial cues when talking watching an interaction. Seiter (2001), for
13-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 5:05 PM Page 250

250–––◆–––Factors of Influence

example, noted that “cues communicated audiences, negative nonverbal reaction


by one person may influence other people’s played a greater role than did positive reac­
perceptions of a third party’s veracity” tions, encouraging participants to take a
(p. 335). Specifically, and following (in less favorable view of the politician. From
some of the experimental conditions) the this, Wiegman concluded that “the audi­
split-screen format of televised political ence is a mediating factor between the
debates when an opponent’s nonverbal reac­ source and the receiver” (p. 38).
tions are shown, Seiter asked students to Davis (1999) also found an important
watch a videotaped debate that allowed connection between actual audience reac­
him to assess the role of opponents’ non­ tion shots on television and viewers’
verbal cues of apparent disbelief for what postmessage opinions on issues that were
the speaker was saying. He found that both modified, in her study, by issue position.
the speaker’s and his opponent’s veracity Nabi and Hendricks (2003) also revealed
were harmed by the nonverbal expressions, that positive audience reactions could affect
even when the expressions of disbelief persuasion, but only if they were consistent
were only moderate. Seiter cites previous with the talk show host’s nonverbal cues.
research that shows an opponent’s negative Given that “reaction shots are one of the
nonverbal cues are more likely to influence most commonly used editing devices used
audiences’ judgment of the opponent in a neg­ to capture and manipulate non-verbal
ative way (i.e., lowering perceptions of com­ cues in film and television” (Davis, 1999,
petence and character) than of the speaker. p. 477), such effects, even when moderated
Additionally, research has examined by prior opinion and others’ behaviors, are
the persuasive impact of “overheard audi­ vital to assess.
ences,” sounds of approval or disapproval
made by a real or apparent studio of listen­
SUMMARY
ers. Axsom, Yates, and Chaiken (1987)
found that auditory reactions by an audi­
Overall, research focusing on the impli­
ence (in their case, enthusiastic or unethusi­
cations of the mediated nonverbal code has
astic responses) had an effect on listeners’
addressed image and, more commonly, per­
persuasion, but only if the listeners were
suasion. This work has found that certain
not highly involved in the issue. For those
presentations of nonverbal cues, viewed
with low involvement, audience response
over time, are likely to affect, often nega­
affected their postmessage opinions in the
tively, people’s own identities. More force­
direction of the audience’s affect. “Seen”
fully, research has looked at the persuasive
audience reactions also appear to have
effects of visual and aural cues on voting,
an important effect on viewers’ attitudes
character judgments, affective responses,
but in an interesting and complex way.
and issue position, among other outcomes.
Wiegman’s (1987) analysis of an interview
These results suggest that the media may
with the Liberal leader in the Dutch
work effectively as a factor underlying the
Parliament found that the audience’s attrac­
use and interpretation of nonverbal cues.
tiveness played a central role in the effects
of their reactions. For audiences judged as
attractive, research participants (members ♦ Conclusion
of the Dutch Socialist and Liberal parties)
were particularly influenced by their posi­
tive reactions, tending to be more favorable Whereas nonverbal cues are studied often
toward the politician. For unattractive as they occur between—and are affected
13-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 5:05 PM Page 251

Casting Nonverbal Behavior in the Media–––◆–––251

by—people in interaction, the media are terms of how we act, how we look (or
also, for many, an important factor of influ­ aspire to look), or who we find desirable,
ence. We argue in this chapter that they powerful, or trustworthy, we need to
work to shape their audience to have cer­ continue to examine critically the portray­
tain expectations, which are often at odds als and the (meta)commentaries of non­
with what everyday life would lead audi­ verbal displays in the media.
ences to expect. Those expectations involve,
among other things, the frequency and type
of emotional expressions, standards of ♦ Note
appearance, and gender roles. The affective
and cognitive responses that audiences have
1. In this chapter, we discuss research
to, and take away from, media are part of a
primarily on media use in developed (First World)
larger communicative code, which includes
nations.
judgments of others, expected behaviors,
and interpretations of actions, that the
media work to affect and reflect. The code,
as revealed in media talk and visual and ♦ References
aural content, also appears to bring about
certain kinds of behaviors, such as particular Allan, S. (1999). News culture. Buckingham:
consumption and voting patterns. Looking Open University Press.
at the media as a “factor of influence” creat­ Axsom, D., Yates, S., & Chaiken, S. (1987).
ing and re-creating a larger code for its users Audience response as a heuristic cue in per­
necessitates a larger critique of the code. In suasion. Journal of Personality and Social
this chapter, we reviewed some of the pub­ Psychology, 53, 30–40.
lished critiques that others have done. We Babad, E. (1999). Preferential treatment in tele­
offer here one additional comment. vision interviewing: Evidence from non­
As Ron Scollon (1998) has argued, all verbal behavior. Political Communication,
16, 337–358.
mediated communication is, at some level,
Bancroft, A. (1998). The model of a man:
produced as face-to-face, interpersonal
Masculinity and body image in men’s
communication. In this sense, nonverbal lifestyle magazines. In J. Richardson &
cues displayed in the media, although more A. Shaw (Eds.), The body in qualitative
self-conscious, scripted, rehearsed, and research (pp. 26–38). Aldershot, UK:
aided by image consultants and makeup Ashgate.
artists, are inevitably subject to the same Barthes, R. (1977). Image–music–text. London:
rules of production and interpretation as Fontana.
nonverbal behavior in other, unmediated Bauman, Z. (1998). Globalization: The human
contexts. What is important, however, is consequences. Cambridge, UK: Polity Press.
the immense influence the media have on Beck, U. (1992). Risk society: Towards a new
their audiences through the propagation of modernity. London: Sage.
Beck, U. (1994). The reinvention of politics:
certain modes of behavior and types of
Towards a theory of reflexive moderniza­
body image while suppressing and devalu­
tion. In U. Beck, A. Giddens, & S. Lash
ing others. Some of these processes are not (Eds.), Reflexive modernization: Politics,
new. After all, looking at ancient Greek tradition and aesthetics in the modern
statues or 17th-century European portraits, social order (pp. 1–55). Cambridge, UK:
we tend to see mostly idealized images of Polity Press.
men and women. As the media wield a huge Bordo, S. (1999). The male body. New York:
amount of power in steering our lives in Farrar, Straus and Giroux.
13-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 5:05 PM Page 252

252–––◆–––Factors of Influence

Browne, B. A. (1998). Gender stereotypes in Douglas, M. (1966). Purity and danger.


advertising on children’s television in the Baltimore, MD: Penguin.
1990s: A cross-national analysis. Journal of Edwards, M. (1998, November 22). I just can’t
Advertising, 27, 83–97. bare it. The Sunday Times, Culture, 29.
Chaudhuri, A., & Buck, R. (1995). Affect, Ekman, P. (1972). Universals and cultural
reason, and persuasion: Advertising differences in facial expressions of emo­
strategies that predict affective and analytic tions. In J. K. Cole (Ed.), Nebraska sympo­
cognitive responses. Human Communica­ sium on motivation (pp. 207–283).
tion Research, 21, 422–441. Lincoln: University of Nebraska Press.
Coats, E. J., & Feldman, R. S. (1995). The role Eubanks, V. (1996). Zones of dither: Writing the
of television in the socialization of nonver­ postmodern body. Body & Society, 2, 73–88.
bal behavioral skills. Basic and Applied Fairclough, N. L. (1995). Media discourse.
Social Psychology, 17, 327–341. London: Edward Arnold.
Coats, E. J., Feldman, R. S., & Philippot, P. Featherstone, M. (1991). The body in con­
(1999). The influence of television on sumer culture. In M. Featherstone,
children’s nonverbal behavior. In P. Philippot, M. Hepworth, & B. S. Turner (Eds.), The
R. S. Feldman, & E. J. Coates (Eds.), The body: Social processes and cultural theory
social context of nonverbal behavior (pp. (pp. 170–196). London: Sage.
156–181). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge Feldman, R. S., Coats, E. J., & Spielman, D. A.
University Press. (1996). Television exposure and children’s
Collins, W. A. (1983). Interpretation and inference decoding of nonverbal behavior. Journal of
in children’s television. In J. Bryant & Applied Social Psychology, 26, 1718–1733.
D. R. Anderson (Eds.), Children’s understand­ Foucault, M. (1978). The history of sexuality:
ing of television. New York: Academic Press. An introduction (R. Huxley., Trans.).
Coupland, J. (2003). Ageist ideology and dis­ London: Penguin.
courses of control in skincare product Foucault, M. (1979). Discipline and punish: The
marketing. In J. Coupland & R. Gwyn birth of the prison (A. M. Sheridan Smith,
(Eds.), Discourse, the body and identity Trans.). New York: Vintage Books.
(pp. 127–150). Basingstoke, UK: Palgrave Friedman, H. S., Mertz, T. J., & DiMatteo,
Macmillan. M. R. (1980). Perceived bias in the facial
Coupland, J. (2005). Let surgery wait: expressions of television news broadcasters.
Consumerised discourses on the “problem” Journal of Communication, 30, 103–111.
of ageing. Unpublished manuscript. Furnham, A., & Mak, T. (1999). Sex-role stereo­
Coupland, N., Nussbaum, J. F., & Grossman, typing in television commercials: A review
A. (1993). Introduction: Discourse, self­ and comparison of fourteen studies done on
hood, and the lifespan. In N. Coupland & five continents over 25 years. Sex Roles, 41,
J. F. Nussbaum (Eds.), Discourse and 413–437.
lifespan identity (pp. x–xxviii). Newbury Giddens, A. (1990). The consequences of
Park, CA: Sage. modernity. Cambridge, UK: Polity Press.
Darwin, C. (1872). The expression of emotions Giddens, A. (1991). Modernity and self-identity:
in men and animals. London: John Murray. Self and society in the late modern age.
Davis, S. (1999). The effects of audience reaction Cambridge, UK: Polity Press.
shots on attitudes towards controversial Goffman, E. (1976). Gender advertisements.
issues. Journal of Broadcasting & Electronic Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Media, 43, 476–491. Goffman, E. (1997). The stigmatized self.
Derscheid, L. E., Kwon, Y.-H., & Fang, S-R. In C. Lemert & A. Branman (Eds.), The
(1996). Preschoolers’ socialization as Goffman reader (pp. 73–79). Oxford, UK:
consumers of clothing and recognition of Blackwell. (Originally published as Stigma:
symbolism. Perceptual and Motor Skills, Notes on the management of spoiled iden­
82, 1171–1181. tity. Boston: Simon & Schuster, 1963)
13-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 5:05 PM Page 253

Casting Nonverbal Behavior in the Media–––◆–––253

Greenberg, B. S., Sherry, J. L., Busselle, R. W., Jaworski, A., Coupland, N., & Galasiński, D.
Hnilo, L. R., & Smith, S. L. (1997). (Eds.). (2004). Metalanguage: Social and
Daytime television talk shows: Guests, ideological perspectives. Berlin, Germany:
content, and interactions. Journal of Mouton de Gruyter.
Broadcasting & Electronic Media, 41, Jaworski, A., & Galasiński, D. (2002). The ver­
412–426. bal construction of non-verbal behaviour:
Gregory, S. W., Jr., & Gallagher, T. J. (2002). British press reports of President Clinton’s
Spectral analysis of candidates’ nonverbal grand jury testimony video. Discourse &
vocal communication: Predicting U.S. presi­ Society, 13, 629–649.
dential election outcomes. Social Psychology Klassen, M. L., Jasper, C. M., & Schwartz, A.
Quarterly, 65, 298–308. M. (1993). Men and women: Images of
Halberstadt, A. (1986). Family socialization of their relationships in magazine advertise­
emotional expression and nonverbal com­ ments. Journal of Advertising Research,
munication of styles and skills. Journal of 22, 30–39.
Personality and Social Psychology, 51, Kolbe, R. H. (1990). Gender roles in children’s
827–836. advertising: A longitudinal content analysis.
Hargreaves, D., & Tiggemann, M. (2002). The In J. H. Leigh & C. R. Martin (Eds.),
effect of television commercials on mood Current issues and research in advertising
and body dissatisfaction: The role of (pp. 197–206). Ann Arbor: University of
the appearance–schema activation. Journal Michigan, Graduate School of Business
of Social and Clinical Psychology, 21, Administration.
287–308. Larson, M. S. (1993). Family communication on
Harrison, K. (1997). Does interpersonal attrac­ prime-time television. Journal of Broadcast­
tion to thin media personalities promote ing and Electronic Media, 37, 349–357.
eating disorders? Journal of Broadcasting Levine, R. (1997). A geography of time. New
& Electronic Media, 41, 478–500. York: Basic Books.
Henderson-King, E., & Henderson-King, D. Lupton, D. (2000). The social construction of
(1997). Media effects on women’s body medicine and the body. In G. L. Albert,
esteem: Social and individual difference fac­ R. Fitzpatrick, & S. C. Scrimshaw (Eds.),
tors. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, Handbook of social studies in health and
27, 167–173. medicine (pp. 50–63). London: Sage.
Houle, R., & Feldman, R. S. (1991). Emotional Lupton, D., & Tulloch, J. (1998). The adolescent
displays in children’s television program­ “unfinished body,” reflexivity and HIV/
ming. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 15, AIDS risk. Discourse & Society, 4, 19–34.
261–271. Manusov, V. (1990). An application of attribu­
Hymes, D. (1972). On communicative com­ tion principles to nonverbal messages in
petence. In J. Pride & J. Holmes (Eds.), romantic dyads. Communication Mono­
Sociolinguistics (pp. 269–293). Harmonds­ graphs, 57, 104–118.
worth, UK: Penguin. Manusov, V., & Bixler, N. R. (2003,
Jaworski, A. (1997). “White and white”: November). The polysemous nature of non­
Metacommunicative and metaphorical verbal behavior: Variety in media framing
silences. In A. Jaworski (Ed.), Silence: Inter­ of the Rabin-Arafat handshake. Paper
disciplinary perspectives (pp. 381–401). presented to the National Communication
Berlin, Germany: Mouton de Gruyter. Association, Miami.
Jaworski, A. (2003). Talking bodies: Representa­ Manusov, V., & Milstein, T. (2005). Inter­
tions of norm and deviance in the BBC preting nonverbal behavior: Representation
Naked programme. In J. Coupland & and transformation frames in Israeli and
R. Gwyn (Eds.), Discourse, the body and Palestinian coverage of the 1993 Rabin-
identity (pp. 151–176). Basingstoke, UK: Arafat handshake. Western Journal of
Palgrave Macmillan. Communication, 69, 183–201.
13-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 5:05 PM Page 254

254–––◆–––Factors of Influence

McNeal, J. U. (1987). Children are consumers: Roy, A. (2005). The “male gaze” in Indian
Insights and implications. Lexington, MA: television commercials: A rhetorical analy­
Lexington Books. sis. In T. Carilli & J. Campbell (Eds.),
Mirzoeff, N. (1995). Bodyscape: Art, Modernity Women and the media: National and global
and the Ideal Figure. London: Routledge. perspectives (pp. 3–18). Lanham, MD:
Morris, D., Collett, P., Marsh, P., & University Press of America.
O’Shaughnessy, M. (1980). Gestures: Their Saarni, C. (1985). Indirect processes in affect
origins and distribution. New York: socialization. In M. Lewis & C. Saarni
Scarborough. (Eds.), The socialization of emotions
Mullen, B., Futrell, D., Stairs, D., Tice, D. M., (pp. 187–212). New York: Plenum.
Baumeister, R. F., Dawson, K. E., et al. Saville-Troike, M. (2003). The ethnography of
(1986). Newscasters’ facial expressions and communication: An introduction (3rd ed.).
voting behavior of viewers: Can a smile Oxford: Blackwell.
elect a president? Journal of Personality and Scollon, R. (1998). Mediated discourse as social
Social Psychology, 51, 291–295. interaction. London: Longman.
Myers, P. N., & Biocca, F. A. (1999).The effect Seiter, E. (1993). Sold separately: Children
of television advertising and program­ and parents in consumer culture. New
ming on body image distortions in young Brunswick, NJ: Rutgers University Press.
women. In L. K. Guerrero, J. A. Devito, & Seiter, J. S. (2001). Silent derogation and
M. L. Hecht (Eds.), The nonverbal commu­ perceptions of deceptiveness: Does com­
nication reader: Classic and contemporary municating nonverbal disbelief during an
readings (2nd ed., pp. 92–100). Prospect opponent’s speech affect perceptions
Heights, IL: Waveland Press. of a debater’s veracity? Communication
Nabi, R. L., & Hendricks, A. (2003). The Research Reports, 18, 334–344.
persuasive effect of host and audience reac­ Shaw, A. (1998). Images of the female body:
tion shots in television talk shows. Journal Women’s identities and the media. In
of Communication, 53, 527–543. J. Richardson & A. Shaw (Eds.), The body
Ochs, E., & Schieffelin, B. (1986). Language in qualitative research (pp. 7–25).
socialization across cultures. New York: Aldershot, UK: Ashgate.
Cambridge University Press. Shildrick, M. (2002). Embodying the monster:
Patterson, M. L. (1983). Nonverbal behavior: Encounters with the vulnerable self.
A functional perspective. New York: Springer. London: Sage.
Patterson, M. L., Churchill, M. E., Burger, Shilling, C. (1993). The body and social theory.
G. K., & Powell, J. L. (1992). Verbal and London: Sage.
nonverbal modality effects on impressions Stafford, B. M. (1991). Body criticism: Imaging
of political candidates: Analysis from the the unseen in enlightenment art and medi­
1984 presidential debates. Communication cine. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Monographs, 59, 231–242. Sullivan, D. G., & Masters, R. D. (1988).
Richards, M. H., Casper, R. C., & Larson, “Happy warriors”: Leaders’ facial displays,
R. W. (1990). Weight and eating disorders viewers’ emotions, and political support.
among pre- and young adolescent boys and American Journal of Political Science, 32,
girls. Journal of Adolescent Health Care, 345–368.
11, 85–89. Synnott, A. (1993). The body social: Symbolism,
Rosenberg, S. W., Bohan, L., McCafferty, P., & self and society. London: Routledge.
Harris, K. (1986). The image and the vote: Twamley, E. W., & Davis, M. C. (1999). The
The effect of candidate presentation on sociocultural model of eating disturbance in
voter preference. American Journal of young women: The effects of personal
Political Science, 30, 108–127. attributes and family environment. Journal
13-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 5:05 PM Page 255

Casting Nonverbal Behavior in the Media–––◆–––255

of Social and Clinical Psychology, 18, an interview with a Dutch politician.


467–489. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 17,
Urla, J., & Terry, J. (1995). Introduction: 37–49.
Mapping embodied deviance. In J. Terry & Wilson, B. J., & Smith, S. L. (1998). Children’s
J. Urla (Eds.), Deviant bodies: Critical responses to emotional portrayals on
perspectives on difference in scenic and television. In P. A. Andersen & L. K.
popular culture (pp. 1–18). Bloomington: Guerrero (Eds.), Handbook of communica­
Indiana University Press. tion and emotion: Theory, application, and
White, S. E., Brown, N. J., & Ginsburg, S. L. contexts (pp. 533–569). San Diego, CA:
(1999). Diversity of body types in network Academic Press.
television programming: A content analysis. Zillmann, D. (1991). Television viewing and
Communication Research Reports, 16, physiological arousal. In J. Bryant & D.
386–392. Zillmann (Eds), Responding to the screens:
Wiegman, O. (1987). Attitude change in a Reception and reaction processes (pp. 103–
realistic experiment: The effect of party 133). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
membership and audience reaction during
13-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 5:05 PM Page 256
14-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 5:06 PM Page 257

PART III

FUNCTIONS
14-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 5:06 PM Page 258
14-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 5:06 PM Page 259

14
NONVERBAL BEHAVIOR IN
INTIMATE INTERACTIONS AND
INTIMATE RELATIONSHIPS

� Peter A. Andersen
San Diego State University

� Laura K. Guerrero
Arizona State University

� Susanne M. Jones
University of Minnesota, Twin Cities

I ntimacy is a complex concept about which researchers legitimately


create various conceptualizations and reach different conclusions
(Acitelli & Duck, 1987; Prager, 1995). For this reason, intimacy has
been conceptualized variously as a relationship type, as an emotion, as
interpersonal warmth or closeness, as a subjective experience, as a com­
munication trait, as an interpersonal process, as a motive, as a behav­
ior, as sexual interaction, and as an interpersonal goal (Andersen &
Guerrero, 1998; McAdams, 1988; Prager, 1995; Reis & Shaver, 1988).
Whereas all these conceptualizations have value, consistent with Prager
(2000), we are concerned primarily in this chapter with intimacy as a

◆ 259
14-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 5:06 PM Page 260

260–––◆–––Functions

Relational Context

Relationship Enhancement

Intimate Experience Nonverbal Expression


• Positive Affect of Intimacy
• Shared Knowledge • Positive Involvement
and Understanding • Reciprocity
• Feelings of Warmth • Immediacy Behaviors
and Closeness • Signs of Closeness

Figure 14.1 An Interaction-Centered Model of Intimacy Processes Related to Nonverbal


Behavior Relational Context

type of interaction, a focus that has two we argue that nonverbal communication is
primary foundations: (1) that intimate inter­ the sine qua non of intimacy. Although
actions are necessary to develop and main­ intimacy can be created by talk, we con­
tain intimate relationships and (2) that tend that nonverbal communication is
emotions and behaviors associated with the intimacy’s primary vehicle.
experience of intimacy are displayed within As a framework for exploring the role
the context of intimate interaction. that nonverbal behavior plays in the
Although intimacy can be concept­ intimacy process, we present an interaction-
ualized as an experience consisting of felt centered model of intimacy processes related
emotions and perceptions of understand­ to nonverbal behavior (see Figure 14.1).
ing, or as a relationship that is character­ This model modifies and extends Prager’s
ized by affection and trust, ultimately (1995, 2000; Prager & Roberts, 2004) con­
intimacy is located in interaction (Prager, ceptualization of intimacy. Positive non­
2000). Intimate interaction is the vehicle verbal involvement cues occupy a central
through which people exchange intimate position in the model. Although individuals
actions, thoughts, and feelings. Relation­ typically manifest positive involvement when
ships are creations of interaction, with they experience intimate thoughts and feel­
partners labeling relationships as intimate ings, it is the expression of intimacy (through
(or nonintimate) based on the communica­ positive involvement cues) that sustains
tion patterns that have occurred between and enhances those intimate thoughts and
them. Thus, we argue that intimacy is feelings. Ultimately, both the experience and
experienced and expressed in interaction. expression of intimacy lead to relationship
Although verbal factors are an important enhancement; relational partners who
component of intimate interaction, we engage in intimate interaction frequently
shall demonstrate in this chapter that and routinely are more likely to be satisfied
nonverbal behaviors play a critical role in and committed as well as to manage conflict
creating and sustaining intimate interac­ effectively (Prager, 2000). Variables associ­
tions and relationships. More pointedly, ated with relational context, such as the
14-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 5:06 PM Page 261

Nonverbal Behavior in Intimate Interactions and Intimate Relationships–––◆–––261

type and stage of a relationship, frame the 1995). In his triangular theory of love,
experience and expression of intimacy and Sternberg (1986) conceptualized intimacy as
provide guidelines for what constitutes appro­ the experience of warm, affectionate feel­
priate levels of intimacy within interactions. ings that occur during interaction with close
With our interaction-centered model friends and others we hold dear. Similarly,
serving as a guide, this chapter is organized Clarke, Allen, and Dickson (1985) defined
around three issues. First, we distinguish warmth as a positive emotion that charac­
between the experience and expression terizes close, intimate relationships, and
of intimacy. Second, we examine nonverbal Andersen and Guerrero (1998) argued that
cues of positive involvement and argue that warmth is a “pleasant, contented, intimate
these cues are the basic building blocks of feeling that occurs during positive inter­
intimate interaction. Third, we examine actions” with others (p. 306). As these
positive involvement behaviors in the con­ conceptualizations suggest, intimate feel­
text of intimate relationships, showing that ings are sustained, enhanced, and created
these behaviors are associated with relation­ through intimate interaction with others.
ship maintenance and enhancement. This illustrates the complexity of intimacy.
Intimacy is experienced typically during
interaction in close relationships in the pres­
THE EXPERIENCE VERSUS THE ence of positive verbal and nonverbal
EXPRESSION OF INTIMACY behavior that reflects and creates feelings of
warmth. The experience of interpersonal
Scholars have distinguished between
warmth can lead people to engage in more
the experience and expression of intimacy,
positive behavior, just as positive behavior
sometimes referring to these components as
can trigger feelings of warmth and intimacy.
latent versus manifest intimacy, respectively
(e.g., Sternberg, 1986). The experience of
intimacy is located in internal processes NONVERBAL INVOLVEMENT CUES
related to perceptions and felt emotions. At
the perceptual level, Prager and Roberts The expression of intimacy involves
(2004) argued that intimacy is experienced verbal and nonverbal behavior (Prager,
through shared knowledge. This knowl­ 2000). Verbal expressions of intimacy
edge, which is gained through intimate include self-disclosure, verbal responsive­
interaction, is stored in cognitive schemas. ness, and intimate words (Andersen, 1998).
The extent to which these schemas are Self-revealing statements that express vul­
accurate and reflect shared meaning defines nerable emotions are especially conducive
the quality of intimate relationships. The to intimacy (Prager & Roberts, 2004).
perception of understanding is also impor­ Statements expressing agreement and vali­
tant. Prager (2000) noted that perceiving dation also foster intimacy (Andersen &
oneself to be “liked, accepted, understood, Guerrero, 1998). Nonverbal expressions of
cared for, or loved” is an essential part of intimacy include a wide range of behaviors
the intimacy experience (p. 231; see also, that reflect both positive affect and involve­
Reis & Shaver, 1988). ment, such as gaze, smiling, forward lean,
At the emotional level, people experience and affirming head nods. According to
intimacy as an affective state characterized Prager (2000), positive involvement behav­
by subjective feelings of warmth and affec­ iors contribute “substantially to people’s
tion (Andersen & Guerrero, 1998; Prager, intimate experiences” (p. 233) and are a
14-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 5:06 PM Page 262

262–––◆–––Functions

defining feature of interactions in close, help predict how intimate an interaction


satisfying relationships. is (e.g., Byrne, 1997). In developed relation­
Most nonverbal behavior is spontaneous ships, intimate interaction is positively asso­
and exerts a powerful effect because of ciated with a number of relational quality
its perceived authenticity (Andersen, 1999; indicators, such as satisfaction and trust
Prager, 2000). We contend that nonverbal (Prager, 1995).
behavior plays a special role in the creation Importantly, however, if intimate feel­
and maintenance of intimacy for two addi­ ings and perceptions are not manifest in
tional reasons. First, in contrast to verbal communication, they remain internal
communication, nonverbal communica­ processes and have little direct effect on
tion is multimodal; people can say only relationships. Intimate interaction helps
one word at a time, yet they can engage sustain and enhance intimate feelings and
in numerous positive involvement cues perceptions that enhance relationships. In
simultaneously. Indeed, scholars have con­ some cases, intimate interaction even
tended that intimacy is communicated creates the experience of intimacy. For
through multichanneled composites of example, an individual may feel neutral
nonverbal cues (Andersen, 1999; Patterson, toward an acquaintance until the pair is
1983), and nonverbal involvement behav­ thrown together in a situation that leads
iors are perceived typically as a gestalt, with them to disclose personal information and
people processing a package of behaviors act warmly toward one another. Relational
rather than focusing on a single cue context also influences how intimacy is
(Andersen, 1985, 1998). Second, people experienced and expressed. In beginning
tend to express emotions such as warmth relationships, trust, shared knowledge, and
and affection via nonverbal rather than understanding are still developing, so inti­
verbal cues (Burgoon, Buller, & Woodall, mate thoughts and feelings may be hidden
1996; Planalp, DeFrancisco, & Rutherford, or expressed more cautiously (e.g., Altman
1996). Although different models of inti­ & Taylor, 1973; Aune, Aune, & Buller,
macy offer diverse predictions for how this 1994). Prager (2000; Prager & Roberts,
process works (see Patterson, this volume), 2004), as well as other scholars (e.g.,
increases in nonverbal involvement behav­ Hatfield, 1984) have noted that intimacy
ior (such as eye contact, touch, and smil­ expression is inhibited when people feel
ing) often lead to emotional reactions and vulnerable, as is often the case during initial
changes in arousal that can trigger affective interactions. In fact, Prager (2000) argued
states related to intimacy (Andersen, 1985, that the expression of thoughts and emo­
1998; Cappella & Greene, 1982). tions that make one vulnerable is a critical
As depicted in Figure 14.1, the experi­ component of both intimate interactions
ence and expression of intimacy are inter­ and intimate relationships.
dependent processes. When people feel
positive affect and perceive shared knowl­
edge and understanding, they are likely to POSITIVE INVOLVEMENT
express those internal experiences through BEHAVIOR IN INTIMATE
communication. In initial interactions, vari­ RELATIONSHIPS
ables such as social and physical attraction
(which involve positive affect) and per­ The search for a catalog of behaviors
ceived similarity (which is likely related to that foster and express intimacy has led
shared knowledge and understanding) may down various paths to lists of behaviors
14-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 5:06 PM Page 263

Nonverbal Behavior in Intimate Interactions and Intimate Relationships–––◆–––263

High Involvement

Intimacy/ Dominance/
Affiliation Aggression

Positive Affect Negative Affect

Social Avoidance/
Politeness Withdrawal

Low Involvement

Figure 14.2 Involvement and Affect as Dimensions Underlying Relational Messages

labeled representing involvement, positive involvement is a behavioral reflection of


affect, or immediacy, but ultimately these the intimacy level of an interaction. For
lists are similar. Prager’s (2000) term posi­ example, Patterson (1988) described inti­
tive involvement behavior represents the macy as the degree of union or openness
intersection of these lines of research. with another person, which is manifested
Consistent with Prager’s theorizing, we by a high degree of spontaneous nonverbal
believe that nonverbal behaviors related to involvement. Earlier, Patterson (1983)
intimacy have two fundamental character­ noted, “the construct of nonverbal involve­
istics: They reflect involvement and positive ment overlaps with proxemics, intimacy,
affect. Figure 14.2 shows how the dimen­ and immediacy, but it is more comprehen­
sions of involvement (or intensity) and sive than each of those constructs” (p. 5).
affect combine to produce various rela­ According to Patterson, involvement is
tional messages. To understand the behav­ communicated via nonverbal behaviors
iors that communicate intimacy we turn such as decreased distance, more gaze and
to a discussion of work on involvement and touch, direct body orientation, forward
immediacy. Although various scholars use lean, facial and vocal expressiveness, and
these two terms somewhat differently, postural openness.
each approach leads to the same conclu­ Burgoon and Newton (1991) defined
sion: Behaviors that reflect engagement involvement via five dimensions: immediacy
and positive affect contribute to intimate (e.g., touch), expressiveness (e.g., facial ani­
interaction. Moreover, regardless of the mation), altercentrism (e.g., attention to the
approach taken, behaviors identified as partner), smooth interaction management
constituting intimate interaction are strik­ (e.g., coordinated turn-taking), and com­
ingly similar, indicating that they are part posure (e.g., few vocal pauses). Importantly,
of the same construct. altercentrism reflects caring and shared
Nonverbal involvement is the degree to understanding, which Prager (2000) identi­
which a person is an engaged, active partic­ fied as essential to the intimacy experience.
ipant in a social interaction (Coker & Shared knowledge may also be related to
Burgoon, 1987). Scholars have argued that smooth interaction management because
14-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 5:06 PM Page 264

264–––◆–––Functions

partners would be more familiar with one (Marston, Hecht, & Robers, 1987; Shaver,
another’s communication style. Burgoon Schwartz, Kirson, & O’Connor, 1987).
and Newton (1991) added a sixth dimen­ Liking is communicated through eye contact,
sion, affect, as relevant but not essential to smiling, facial and gestural animation, and
nonverbal involvement. According to this head nodding (Floyd & Ray, 2003; Palmer
perspective, the simultaneous manifestation & Simmons, 1995). Finally, joy is associated
of involvement behaviors and positive affect with being physically energetic, smiling,
cues (such as smiling and vocal warmth) laughing, approaching others, and sound­
helps create or sustain intimacy. In contrast, ing enthusiastic (Shaver et al., 1987). When
when involvement behaviors are paired these emotions are exchanged, an intimate
with negative affect cues (e.g., scowling and interaction is created that helps produce and
a loud, angry voice), dominance or aggres­ sustain an intimate relationship.
sion is communicated. Instead of using the term positive
Dillard, Solomon, and Palmer (1999) involvement, some scholars use the term
argued that involvement is conceptualized immediacy to describe a set of behaviors
by the level of intensity or engagement that communicates both involvement and
present within an interaction. Involvement positive affect (Andersen, 1985). Mehrabian
cues are present in interactions characterized (1967, 1969b) coined the term immediacy
as either affiliative-intimate or dominant- to describe approach behaviors that reflect
aggressive, depending on the type of affect the intensity level of interaction. Later,
present in the interaction (see also, Mehrabian (1981) argued that immediacy
Cappella, 1983; Guerrero, 2004; Prager, behaviors also signal attentiveness, heighten
1995; see Figure 14.2). Thus, nonverbal sensory stimulation, and communicate lik­
involvement and displays of positive affect ing, with individuals moving toward
combine to produce intimate interactions. people and things they like, and away from
Studies demonstrate that both participants people and things they dislike. On the basis
and observers perceive people to be more of these characteristics, Mehrabian’s con­
intimate when they use involvement behav­ ceptualization of immediacy seems to cap­
iors such as eye contact and forward leans, ture intensity (or involvement) and liking
coupled with positive affect cues such as (or warm feelings). Andersen (1985) added
smiling (e.g., Burgoon, Buller, Hale, & that immediacy behaviors increase physi­
deTurck, 1984; Burgoon & Le Poire, 1999; cal and psychological closeness, signal
Burgoon & Newton, 1991). availability for interaction, are physiolog­
Researchers have also identified a host ically arousing, and communicate positive
of behaviors associated with positive affect affect.
and interpersonal warmth (see Andersen & Mehrabian’s (1967, 1969b) work
Guerrero, 1998, for a review). Interpersonal focused on five immediacy behaviors:
warmth is part of a cluster of social emotions interpersonal distance, touch, gaze, body
related to intimacy and affection that orientation, and lean. Andersen (1985)
includes love, liking, and joy. These emo­ expanded the domain of immediacy behav­
tions are expressed via positive involvement iors to include a wide variety of kinesic,
(Guerrero & Andersen, 2000; Guerrero & vocalic, and chronemic cues (e.g., smiling,
Floyd, 2006). Love is communicated non- warm vocal tone, and time spent together).
verbally by physical closeness, positive As noted earlier, some scholars view imme­
touch, smiling, mutual gaze, spending time diacy as one of several dimensions under
together, warm vocal tones, and giving gifts the broader construct of involvement
14-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 5:06 PM Page 265

Nonverbal Behavior in Intimate Interactions and Intimate Relationships–––◆–––265

(Burgoon & Newton, 1991; Dillard et al., chronemics. When discussing the behaviors
1999). For these scholars, immediacy is associated with each of these subcodes, we
limited to involvement behaviors reflect­ also comment on relational context vari­
ing physical and psychological closeness ables (such as type and stage of relationship)
between two people during interaction, that influence the display and interpretation
with Mehrabian’s (1969b) original list of of various positive involvement cues.
immediacy behaviors—distance, touch,
gaze, body orientation, and lean—providing Proxemics. At least four types of proxemic
the best exemplars. Regardless of the behavior are related to the experience or
approach taken, however, scholars study­ communication of intimacy: interpersonal
ing involvement and immediacy appear to distance, lean, body orientation, and the
agree that behaviors representing the inter­ physical plane (Andersen, 1999). Interper­
section of involvement and positive affect sonal distance refers to the physical space
are the building blocks of intimate inter­ between two people. Hall’s (1966) classic
action. Like Prager (1995), we refer to these work on conversational distances suggests
behaviors as positive involvement cues. that in North American culture, the dis­
tance ranging from 0 to 18 inches is the
“intimate” zone, reserved generally for inti­
Specific Behaviors Reflecting mate interaction with close relational part­
Positive Involvement
ners. Mehrabian (1969a) contended that
Next, we identify specific nonverbal close interpersonal distances are related to
behaviors that have been categorized as liking under most circumstances, and early
positive involvement cues. This discussion is empirical research showed that close dis­
organized by examining various subcodes tances are related to positive interpersonal
of nonverbal communication, such as prox­ attitudes (Mehrabian & Ksionsky, 1970),
emics and haptics. Our review focuses on liking, and friendship (Priest & Sawyer,
dynamic nonverbal behaviors rather than 1967).
nonverbal cues that are typically static, Mehrabian (1969a) also suggested,
such as appearance and the environment. however, that close distances can lead to
Although cues related to appearance and less liking and intimacy under certain cir­
the environment undoubtedly shape percep­ cumstances. Burgoon and her colleagues
tions and intimate experiences, we believe (e.g., Burgoon & Hale, 1988; Burgoon,
static cues have less potential for influencing Manusov, Mineo, & Hale, 1985) have
the ongoing process of intimate interaction shown that close distance only produces
than do dynamic cues such as touch and increased liking and positive perceptions
gesturing, which frequently change through­ if a person is perceived to be rewarding
out the course of interaction (Guerrero & (e.g., attractive, high status). When some­
Floyd, 2006). This perspective is consistent one judged as nonrewarding gets close,
with work by scholars studying involve­ people tend to evaluate that person more
ment and immediacy; although these schol­ negatively. Research shows that relation­
ars have not differentiated between dynamic ships also matter. Morton (1977), for
and static nonverbal cues, the behaviors example, verified that people are less com­
they have identified as constituting involve­ fortable standing or sitting close to
ment and immediacy have tended to fall a stranger than a friend. Guerrero (1997)
under the subcodes of proxemics, haptics, revealed that people tend to sit closer
kinesics, vocalics, and to a lesser extent, to romantic partners than to friends.
14-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 5:06 PM Page 266

266–––◆–––Functions

Together, these studies suggest that inter­ vulnerable body parts such as the face or
personal distance reflects the intimacy level torso “always . . . experienced as intimate”
of relationships and that relational context (Prager, 2000, p. 233).
helps determine how people interpret and Despite the consistency with which touch
respond to positive involvement behavior. is perceived as intimate, touch may function
Lean, body orientation, and the physical to express intimacy differentially depending
plane decrease the vertical and horizontal on the stage of a relationship. In field stud­
distance between people. Forward leans ies conducted at airports, zoos, and theaters,
communicate immediacy, involvement, and for example, more touch was associated
affection and tend to lead to perceptions with feelings of greater intimacy during rela­
of greater intimacy (e.g., Burgoon, 1991; tionship escalation (Guerrero & Andersen,
Palmer, Cappella, Patterson, & Churchill, 1991; Heslin & Boss, 1980; McDaniel &
1990; Ray & Floyd, 2000). People comfort­ Andersen, 1998). In these studies, touch was
able with intimacy expression are more observed unobtrusively and correlated posi­
likely to lean forward than are those fearful tively to participants’ or observers’ ratings
of intimacy (Guerrero, 1996). Face-to-face of intimacy in developing relationships. In
body orientation is related to intimacy long-term relationships, touch tends to level
(Andersen, 1999). In one study, couples who off or decline. Likewise, in a study of dating
expressed support for each other during dis­ and married couples, Emmers and Dindia
agreements (with support potentially being a (1995) found a nonlinear relationship
reflection of intimacy) were more likely to between touch and relational intimacy in
use direct body orientation and close dis­ self-reported private touch, with touch
tancing (Newton & Burgoon, 1990). Studies peaking and then leveling off or decreasing
suggest that women friends are especially slightly at the highest reported levels of
likely to use direct body orientation to intimacy. These studies suggest that touch is
express intimacy (e.g., Guerrero, 1997). more than a reflection of intimate experi­
Communicating on the same physical plane ence. Instead, touch may be an essential part
also reduces height differentials, leading to of developing and escalating intimate rela­
more intimate interaction (Andersen, 1999). tionships. Once the experience of intimacy is
stable in longer-term relationships, touch
Haptics. Touch is vital to human develop­ may become less necessary.
ment; loving physical contact, for example, Touch also appears to be important
enables children to reach full social and in communicating intimacy across a variety
intellectual potential and helps them become of relationship types. Monsour (1992) arg­
comfortable with intimacy (Guerrero, 2000; ued that physical contact is essential to
Montagu, 1978). Some scholars have even perceptions of intimate interaction for
suggested that intimacy is impossible in the friends. Similarly, Marston, Hecht, Manke,
absence of touch (e.g., Morris, 1971). McDaniel, and Reeder (1998) found
Research has confirmed that across many that tactile behavior (e.g., embracing, hug­
contexts, including friendships, romances, ging, or kissing) is the primary way people
family relationships, and therapeutic and communicate intimacy in romantic rela­
medical treatments, touch is associated tionships. The criss-cross hug may be per­
with intimacy. Indeed, Prager (1995, 2000) ceived as especially intimate across various
considers touch to be a fundamental types of relationships (Floyd, 1999).
component of intimate interaction. As she Studies comparing other types of non­
put it, “Touch further intensifies the experi­ sexual touch suggest that face touching is
ence of intimacy” with welcome touch on seen as particularly intimate, presumably
14-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 5:06 PM Page 267

Nonverbal Behavior in Intimate Interactions and Intimate Relationships–––◆–––267

because the face is a sensitive and vulnera­ behavior, body movements, and gestures
ble part of the body (Burgoon, 1991; Lee & (Burgoon et al., 1996). Intimate kinesic
Guerrero, 2001), which comports with expressions include smiling, facial pleasant­
Prager’s (1995, 2000) argument that inti­ ness, increased eye contact, and gestures that
macy requires one to become vulnerable. In connote immediacy, affection, closeness, and
Burgoon et al.’s (1984) study, observers warmth (Kleinke, 1986). The face is consid­
who viewed videotaped tactile interactions ered the primary and most trusted source of
rated touch conditions as more intimate emotional information (Ekman & Friesen,
than nontouch conditions. Because touch is 1975; Knapp & Hall, 2006; Planalp et al.,
considered an intimate behavior, however, 1996). Therefore, the face carries important
interactants must be sure that touch is messages of positive affect that help create
welcome (Andersen, 1998; Prager, 2000). intimate interaction. Burgoon and Newton
Studies show that some people are highly (1991) found that both facial and gestural
touch avoidant and do not like intimate animation predict relational intimacy.
touch, will not volunteer for touch experi­ Additionally, when couples are intimate,
ments, stand and sit out of reach of other they also tend to synchronize their gestures,
interactants, and dislike a situation if they body movements, and facial expressions
are touched (see Andersen, 2005). Touch­ (Tickle-Degnen & Rosenthal, 1990; see also
avoidant people will adjust to greater levels Tickle-Degnen, this volume).
of touch, however, depending on rela­ Eye behavior typically is crucial to
tional stage and their partner’s tactile pref­ the experience of intimacy. The primary
erences. In short, the relationship trumps oculesic behaviors are gaze (i.e., a person
the trait when the two conflict (Guerrero looks at another person) and eye contact
& Andersen, 1991, 1994), illustrating the (i.e., simultaneous gaze). The latter is par­
importance of relational context. ticularly important for initiating social
Of course, touch is inappropriate in some interactions because it serves as an invita­
relationships. Interpretations of excessively tion to communicate and is vital to attribu­
intimate behaviors, such as touch, can lead tions of intimacy (Andersen, 1985). Argyle
to employees’ reports of sexual harassment. (1972) and Breed (1972) found mutually
Lee and Guerrero (2001) found that among causal relationships between eye contact
ambiguous touches, a gentle facial touch and intimacy. Eye contact is essential for
or an arm around a coworker’s waist were communicating positive involvement and
rated as the most intimate and, therefore, fostering the experience of intimacy in face­
inappropriate for coworkers. In another to-face contexts (Andersen & Andersen,
study, soft touches were viewed as less harm­ 1984; Mehrabian, 1981). Experiments by
ful and harassing than were hard touches Burgoon, Coker, and Coker (1986) showed
(Black & Gold, 2003). Whether touch is that less eye contact had a negative effect on
perceived as sexually harassing is not just a perceived intimacy compared with normal
function of the area touched and the type of or greater levels of eye contact.
touch, but also of the physical attractiveness
of the transgressor (Black & Gold, 2003; Vocalics. Most of the research focusing on
Cartar, Hickes, & Slane, 1996). Generally, vocal cues has not addressed their poten­
the more attractive the transgressor, the tial for communicating intimacy. Rather,
more acceptable the tactile behavior is rated. research has focused on the related
constructs of affect, liking, closeness, imme­
Kinesics. Kinesics encompass nonverbal diacy, and affection. Next to the face,
behaviors that include facial expressions, eye the vocal channel is the key medium for
14-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 5:06 PM Page 268

268–––◆–––Functions

transmitting emotional information (Knapp respectively. Hatfield and Rapson (1987)


& Hall, 2006). Given that subtle vocal cues suggested that intimacy is comprised of
are hard to control, the voice is a reliable several elements, including behavioral
indicator of emotion. Planalp et al. (1996) intimacy, operationalized primarily as
found that people relied most frequently spending time in close proximity to one’s
on vocal cues, especially loudness, speed of partner. Factor analytic studies have
talking, and amount of talking when inter­ found that spending time with someone
preting emotional expressions from others. is a central component of nonverbal
People tend to match vocal cues to signal intimacy or immediacy (Andersen &
liking and closeness (Tickle-Degnen & Andersen, 2005). In a study of the subjec­
Rosenthal, 1990). Likewise, vocal pleasant­ tive nature of intimacy, Marston et al.
ness is an important facet of positive (1998) demonstrated that an important
involvement that helps people initiate and component of intimacy was time together,
maintain intimate relationships (Guerrero, including private moments.
2004). Positive affect is conveyed via vocal As discussed above regarding kinesics,
warmth, vocal pleasantness, and relaxed interactional matching or synchrony
laughter (Burgoon & Newton, 1991; between two interactants is vital to the
Guerrero, 1997). As Knapp and Hall creation of intimacy. Theories of interper­
(2006) pointed out, the presence of a smile sonal adaptation (e.g., Burgoon, Stern, &
is also evident in the voice. Affection, a Dillman, 1995), communication accom­
construct closely related to intimacy, is also modation theory (Giles & Street, 1994),
communicated via pitch (number of vocal rapport (Tickle-Degnen & Rosenthal,
vibrations per second) as well as variance in 1990), and interpersonal sensitivity (Hall
pitch. Floyd and Ray (2003) found that & Bernieri, 2001) suggest that timing and
men and women were rated as showing synchronicity are essential to establishing
more affection when their voices varied in intimacy. More specifically, studies indi­
pitch. Men were perceived as more affec­ cate that interactional timing is crucial for
tionate, however, when their average pitch developing intimacy in both adult-adult
level was low, whereas women were per­ and infant-adult interaction (Cappella,
ceived as more affectionate when their 1981, this volume; Stern, 1980). Children
average pitch level was high. tend to be more secure and comfortable
with intimacy when they have parents
Chronemics. Spending time with a rela­ who respond to their needs consistently
tional partner often sends compelling, and appropriately, using moderate levels
intimate messages. Studies reveal that a of stimulation and highly synchronized
potent predictor of relational satisfaction, nonverbal behavior (Ainsworth, Blehar,
interpersonal understanding, and inti­ Waters, & Wall, 1978; Isabella & Belsky,
macy, is the amount of time people spend 1991). In addition, many researchers
together. Egland, Stelzner, Andersen, report that careful timing and sequencing
and Spitzberg (1997) reported that among of intimacy behaviors such as eye contact
20 nonverbal behaviors, spending time and proximity are essential to perceptions
together was the most powerful predic­ of interpersonal intimacy (see Andersen,
tor of both relational satisfaction and 1998, for a review). Initiating intimacy
perceived interpersonal understanding, too quickly, too slowly, or in the wrong
accounting for 30% and 25% of the vari­ sequence can be perceived as excessive inti­
ance in satisfaction and understanding, macy, sexual harassment, or interpersonal
14-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 5:06 PM Page 269

Nonverbal Behavior in Intimate Interactions and Intimate Relationships–––◆–––269

desperation (Wertin & Andersen, 1996). and how much shared understanding one
Similarly, dissynchronous behaviors such perceives initially, is likely to exert a strong
as arriving late for a meeting or social influence on how people react to positive
engagement communicate less friendliness involvement behavior. The second condi­
and sociability (Baxter & Ward, 1975). tion revolves around the concept of reci­
procity. If one person engages in positive
involvement behavior and the other person
Interpreting Positive Involvement
Cues as a Reflection of Intimacy responds by compensating (e.g., backing
away) rather than reciprocating (e.g., smil­
Intensifications of positive involvement ing and increasing eye contact), intimate
behavior usually have a direct, positive rela­ interaction has not occurred. It is our
tionship with experienced intimacy. This position that intimate interaction requires
is the position of the direct effects model participation by both partners rather than
(Andersen, 1985, 1999) and a related per­ the simple display of positive involvement
spective, the social meaning model (Burgoon behavior by one partner.
et al., 1986), both of which have been widely Our position regarding reciprocity is
supported. The direct effects model posits supported by scholarship on intimacy as
that although immediacy (or positive well as theories of nonverbal communica­
involvement behavior) is moderated by tion. In Prager’s (2000) model of intimacy,
situational, relational, cultural, and person­ partners are viewed as highly interdepen­
ality factors, in most circumstances greater dent. Prager described the interdependent
nonverbal immediacy inherently produces processes underlying intimate interaction
positive person perceptions and greater rela­ this way: “The experiences (feelings and per­
tional intimacy (Andersen, 1998, 1999). ceptions) that Partner A’s behavior elicits in
The social meaning model is based on the Partner B prompt Partner B to behave inti­
principle that there are consensually recog­ mately. The intimate behaviors performed
nized meanings for nonverbal communi­ by Partner B then shape the experiences of
cation within social communities or the Partner A and so on” (p. 230). As this sce­
broader society (Burgoon & Newton, 1991; nario illustrates, the creation of intimate
Burgoon et al., 1985). Research on the interaction requires individuals to express
social meaning model has confirmed that their intimate feelings to each other, as well
people interpret positive involvement cues as to respond positively to one another’s
as a reflection of intimacy (Burgoon & expressions of intimacy. Theories focusing
Le Poire, 1999; Burgoon & Newton, 1991). on patterns of nonverbal communication
Positive involvement behavior is, how­ come to similar conclusions. For example,
ever, most likely to lead to intimate inte­ according to Andersen’s (1985, 1998) cog­
ractions and intimate relationships when nitive valence theory, positive reactions to
two conditions are met. The first of these immediacy behavior lead to reciprocity and
conditions—that the behavior is welcome— increased intimacy. Similarly, in Cappella
was alluded to earlier in this chapter. and Greene’s (1982) discrepancy arousal
Behaviors such as touch and close proxemic theory, individuals who feel positive emo­
distancing, in particular, can be interpreted tion in response to a partner’s increase in
as threatening or aggressive if uninvited and expressive, warm behavior are theorized to
unwanted, even if positive affect cues are reciprocate by engaging in similarly expres­
present. The experience of intimacy, in sive and warm behavior that creates inti­
terms of how much positive affect one feels mate interaction. Research stemming from
14-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 5:06 PM Page 270

270–––◆–––Functions

an expectancy violations theory has also relationships from casual relationships


demonstrated that interactions are rated (Hays, 1988; Monsour, 1992; Prager, 2000).
as more intimate when positive involvement Because positive involvement behaviors
cues are reciprocated (see Burgoon et al., reflect intimacy (Prager, 2000), these
1995, for a review). behaviors help people maintain and
Scholarship demonstrating an emo­ enhance close relationships. Nonverbal
tional contagion effect (Hatfield, Cacioppo, involvement cues, however, vary across
& Rapson, 1994) also suggests that relationship type. In one study, people used
reciprocity contributes to intimacy. Emo­ more head nods, vocal fluency, vocal inter­
tional contagion occurs when one person est, and shorter response latencies when
catches the emotions of the other, creating a interacting with same-sex friends than with
likely reciprocity effect. If an individual does cross-sex friends or romantic partners
not express her or his intimate feelings, (Guerrero, 1997), yet people maintained
however, there is no opportunity for conta­ larger distances with same-sex friends.
gion to occur. Catching one another’s emo­ Women friends were most likely to use
tions may also foster shared understanding, direct body orientation, and romantic part­
which is an important construct in Prager ners used the most eye contact, touch, and
and Roberts’ (2004) model of intimacy. closest proximity (Guerrero, 1997), sug­
gesting different positive involvement cues
emerge in intimate interactions depending
♦ Intimate Interactions as on the type of relationship.
Relationship Enhancing Positive involvement cues are also related
to emotional support and comforting, both
of which are associated with intimacy and
Partners who display and reciprocate posi­ relationship maintenance (Burleson &
tive involvement also enhance their relation­ Samter, 1994). Jones and Guerrero (2001)
ships. Prager (2000) took the position that found that both verbal person-centeredness
“intimate relationships are built on fre­ (i.e., verbal comments validating the dis­
quently occurring intimate interactions” tressed person) and nonverbal immediacy
(p. 236), with intimate interaction fostering exerted strong effects on comforting quality.
satisfaction, trust, and understanding. Such Participants also reported feeling better
a view is consistent with the perspective of (Jones, 2004) and liking the helper more
communication scholars who have argued (Jones & Burleson, 2003) when he or she
that relationships emerge across ongoing was nonverbally immediate rather than
interactions. Wilmot (1995) contended that nonimmediate. Comforting is most likely to
relationships represent a collection of all occur in the context of an intimate relation­
the interactions that two people have ship, suggesting a bidirectional relationship
engaged in over time. Cappella (1988) between intimacy and comforting (Burleson
argued, “Interactions reflect the kind of & Goldsmith, 1998).
relationship that exists between the part­ Extensive evidence also links positive
ners” (p. 325). This position is consistent involvement behavior, including the expres­
with the idea that relationships are charac­ sion of positive emotion, to relational
terized as intimate because partners have a satisfaction and the experience of intimacy
history of using positive involvement behav­ (Guerrero & Floyd, 2006). As Kelly,
iors when interacting with one another. Fincham, and Beach (2003) concluded, “it is
Scholars have demonstrated that intimacy often not the verbal content that stands out”
is the vital characteristic differentiating close when distinguishing between happy and
14-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 5:06 PM Page 271

Nonverbal Behavior in Intimate Interactions and Intimate Relationships–––◆–––271

unhappy couples. Instead, “what is remark­ engage in hostile or aggressive behaviors


able is the pleasurable emotions couples that reflect the intersection of involvement
appear to be experiencing—the smiles, and negative affect (see Figure 14.2).
laughs, affection, and warmth that couples In a study on nonverbal correlates of
show” (p. 729). During ordinary interaction conflict, Newton and Burgoon (1990)
and conflict episodes, satisfied couples found that people who used antisocial ver­
express more positive affect than do dissat­ bal statements also tended to use nonverbal
isfied couples (e.g., Broderick & O’Leary, behaviors such as a loud or sharp vocal
1986; Gottman, Markman, & Notarius, tone, head shaking, fast speaking rate, and
1977). Satisfied couples in intimate relation­ animated gestures—all of which suggest
ships also reciprocate nonverbal displays involvement but not positive affect. By con­
of involvement, activity, and positive affect trast, Newton and Burgoon showed that
more than dissatisfied couples (Manusov, people who made supportive statements
1995). Although these studies measured during conflict interactions were more
satisfaction rather than intimacy, the results likely to display positive involvement
are consistent with Prager’s (2000) theoriz­ behaviors such as direct body orientation,
ing about the connection between intimate kinesic animation, vocal warmth, and vocal
interaction and relationship enhancement. interest. Given the considerable body of
Couples who create intimate interaction research showing that people tend to recip­
through the mutual display of positive rocate negative nonverbal behavior during
involvement are likely to reinforce intimate conflict interaction (for a review see
feelings and perceptions while also enhanc­ Guerrero & Floyd, 2006), being able to
ing their relationships. break this negative cycle by using positive
Expressing positive involvement may involvement cues may not only help couples
also counterbalance negativity within deal with conflict more effectively, but also
romantic relationships. Fincham, Bradbury, help them sustain intimate feelings and
Arias, Byrne, and Karney (1997) found that perceptions that enhance their relationships.
among couples displaying negative behav­ The experience of intimacy is also
ior, those who exhibited more positive related to reading emotions accurately,
behavior rated themselves as happier. Prager particularly positive ones. Decoding posi­
(2000) also noted that effective conflict tive emotions accurately is linked to satis­
management is essential for sustaining inti­ faction (Gottman & Porterfield, 1981) as
macy within relationships. Of course, it is well as to perceptions of shared under­
often difficult to engage in positive involve­ standing, which are part of the intimacy
ment behavior in the midst of the negative experience (Prager, 2000). Gaelick,
emotion that tends to characterize many Bodenhausen, and Wyer (1985) reported
conflict interactions. Often, people respond that people reciprocated emotions they
to conflict by withdrawing or becoming thought their spouses were experiencing.
demanding and competitive (Heavey, People had more difficulty decoding affec­
Christensen, & Malamuth, 1995). When tionate emotions than hostile emotions,
people withdraw from conflict, they are however, leading to more negative commu­
likely to engage in behaviors that indicate a nication cycles. Similarly, Manusov, Floyd,
lack of involvement, such as using less gaze, and Kerssen-Griep (1997) found that part­
a more closed posture, and more head down ners in intimate dyads were more likely to
positions (Feeney, Noller, Sheehan, & notice negative nonverbal cues than posi­
Peterson, 1999). When individuals become tive cues, and positive perception of non­
demanding or competitive, they are likely to verbal behavior was linked to relational
14-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 5:06 PM Page 272

272–––◆–––Functions

satisfaction. Partners in happy relationships intimacy in developed relationships,


attribute negative affect expression to external researchers will need to examine how
causes and positive affect expression to the intimacy experience and expression influ­
relationship (Koerner & Fitzpatrick, 2002). ence one another as well as relational char­
Decoding nonthreatening emotional displays acteristics such as trust, love, satisfaction,
accurately is associated with relational close­ and commitment. The perceptions and
ness and experienced intimacy for couples, behaviors of both relational partners need
whereas accurately decoding relationship- to be explored and patterns of reciprocity
threatening emotion is associated with less should be investigated. Researchers study­
intimacy (Simpson, Orina, & Ickes, 2003). ing relational maintenance may also want
to look at how intimate interaction helps
sustain and enhance close relationships.
Intimacy is a complex construct that
♦ Summary and Conclusion
emerges from dynamic interaction patterns.
To appreciate this complexity fully, schol­
Intimate interactions play a pivotal role ars must understand the specific nonverbal
in the development and maintenance of and verbal behaviors that work in tandem
relationships. Nonverbal cues of positive with perceptions, emotions, and relation­
involvement are a critical component that ship characteristics in creating and sustain­
helps define interaction as intimate. ing intimate interaction.
Behaviors such as gaze, smiling, touch, and
close distancing are all related to per­
ceptions of intimacy and liking, especially ♦ References
when they are welcome and reciprocated.
These behaviors also differentiate close
Acitelli, L. K., & Duck, S. W. (1987). Intimacy
relationships from casual relationships, and
as the proverbial elephant. In D. Perlman &
satisfied couples from dissatisfied couples.
S. W. Duck (Eds.), Intimate relationships:
Along with self-disclosure and verbal Development, dynamics, and deterioration
responsiveness, nonverbal behaviors that (pp. 297–308). Newbury Park, CA: Sage.
signal positive involvement are key ingredi­ Ainsworth, M. D. S., Blehar, M. C., Waters, E.,
ents in the recipe for intimate interaction. & Wall, S. (1978). Patterns of attachment:
Taking an interaction-based approach to A psychological study of the strange situa­
the study of intimacy provides scholars tion. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
with many challenges and exciting direc­ Altman, I., & Taylor, D. A. (1973). Social pene­
tions for new research. First, scholars need tration: The development of interpersonal
to continue examining interactions between relationships. New York: Holt, Rinehart &
people to determine which specific positive Winston.
Andersen, P. A. (1985). Nonverbal immediacy
involvement behaviors are associated most
in interpersonal communication. In A. W.
strongly with intimacy across various types
Siegman & S. Feldstein (Eds.), Multichannel
of relationships. Second, scholars should integrations of nonverbal behavior (pp.
investigate the extent to which variables 1–36). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
such as attraction and perceived similarity Andersen, P. A. (1998). The cognitive valence
influence both intimacy experience and theory of intimate communication. In M. T.
intimacy expression in initial interaction. Palmer & G. A. Barnett (Eds.). Progress in
Third, to get a more complete picture of communication sciences, Volume XIV:
14-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 5:06 PM Page 273

Nonverbal Behavior in Intimate Interactions and Intimate Relationships–––◆–––273

Mutual influence in interpersonal commu­ behavior to marital satisfaction. Journal of


nication: Theory and research in cognition, Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 54,
affect, and behavior (pp. 39–72). Stamford, 514–517.
CT: Ablex. Burgoon, J. K. (1991). Relational message
Andersen, P. A. (1999). Nonverbal communica­ interpretations of touch, conversational dis­
tion: Forms and functions. Mountain View, tance, and posture. Journal of Nonverbal
CA: Mayfield. Behavior, 15, 233–259.
Andersen, P. A. (2005). The touch avoidance mea­ Burgoon, J. K., Buller, D. B., Hale, J. L., &
sure. In V. Manusov (Ed.), The sourcebook deTurck, M. A. (1984). Relational mes­
of nonverbal measures: Going beyond words. sages associated with nonverbal behaviors.
(pp. 57–66). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. Human Communication Research, 10,
Andersen, P. A., & Andersen, J. F. (1984). The 351–378.
exchange of nonverbal immediacy: A criti­ Burgoon, J. K., Buller, D. B., & Woodall, W. G.
cal review of dyadic models. Journal of (1996). Nonverbal communication: The
Nonverbal Behavior, 8, 327–349. unspoken dialogue (2nd ed.). New York:
Andersen, P. A., & Andersen, J. F. (2005). McGraw-Hill.
Measurement of perceived nonverbal Burgoon, J. K., Coker, D. A., & Coker, R. A.
immediacy. In V. Manusov (Ed.), The (1986). Communicative effects of gaze
sourcebook of nonverbal measures: Going behavior: A test of two contrasting expla­
beyond words. (pp. 113–126). Mahwah, nations. Human Communication Research,
NJ: Erlbaum. 12, 495–524.
Andersen, P. A., & Guerrero, L. K. (1998). The Burgoon, J. K., & Hale, J. L. (1988). Nonverbal
bright side of relational communication: expectancy violations: Model elaboration
Interpersonal warmth as a social emotion. and application to immediacy behaviors.
In P.A. Andersen & L. K. Guerrero (Eds.), Communication Monographs, 55, 58–79.
Handbook of communication and emotion: Burgoon, J. K., & Le Poire, B. A. (1999).
Research, theory, applications and contexts Nonverbal cues and interpersonal judg­
(pp. 303–324). San Diego, CA: Academic ments: Participant and observer perceptions
Press. of intimacy, dominance, composure, and
Argyle, M. (1972). The psychology of inter­ formality. Communication Monographs,
personal behavior (2nd ed.). London: 66, 105–124.
Penguin. Burgoon, J. K., Manusov, V., Mineo, P., &
Aune, K. S., Aune, R. K., & Buller, D. B. (1994). Hale, J. L. (1985). Effects of eye gaze on
The experience, expression, and perceived hiring, credibility, attraction, and relational
appropriateness of emotion across levels of message interpretation. Journal of Non­
relationship development. Journal of Social verbal Behavior, 9, 133–146.
Psychology, 134, 141–150. Burgoon, J. K., & Newton, D. A. (1991).
Baxter, L., & Ward, J. (1975). Newsline. Applying a social meaning model to rela­
Psychology Today, 8, 28. tional message interpretations of conversa­
Black, K. A., & Gold, D. J. (2003). Men’s and tional involvement: Comparing observer
women’s reactions to hypothetical sexual and participant perspectives. Southern
advances: The role of initiator socioeco­ Communication Journal, 56, 96–113.
nomic status and level of coercion. Sex Burgoon, J. K., Stern, L. A., & Dillman, L.
Roles, 49, 173–178. (1995). Interpersonal adaptation: Dyadic
Breed, G. (1972). The effect of intimacy: interaction patterns. New York: Cambridge
Reciprocity or retreat? British Journal of University Press.
Social and Clinical Psychology, 2, 135–142. Burleson, B. R., & Goldsmith, D. J. (1998).
Broderick, J. E., & O’Leary, K. D. (1986). How the comforting process works:
Contributions of affect, attitude and Alleviating emotional distress through
14-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 5:06 PM Page 274

274–––◆–––Functions

conversationally induced reappraisals. In nonverbal encoding patterns. Human Com­


P. A. Andersen & L. K. Guerrero (Eds.), munication Research, 13, 463–494.
Handbook of communication and emo­ Dillard, J. P., Solomon, D. H., & Palmer, M. T.
tion: Research, theory, applications and (1999). Structuring the concept of rela­
contexts (pp. 245–280). San Diego, CA: tional communication. Communication
Academic Press. Monographs, 66, 49–65.
Burleson, B. R., & Samter, W. (1994). A social Egland, K. I., Stelzner, M. A., Andersen, P. A.,
skills approach to relationship mainte­ & Spitzberg, B. S. (1997). Perceived under­
nance: How individual differences in com­ standing, nonverbal communication, and
munication skills affect the achievement relational satisfaction. In J. E. Aitken &
of relationship functions. In D. J. Canary & L. J. Shedletsky (Eds.), Intrapersonal
L. Stafford (Eds.), Communication and communication processes (pp. 386–396).
relational maintenance (pp. 61–90). San Annandale, VA: Speech Communication
Diego, CA: Academic Press. Association.
Byrne, D. (1997). An overview (and underview) Ekman, P., & Friesen, W. V. (1975). Unmasking
of research and theory within the attraction the face: A guide to recognizing emotions
paradigm. Journal of Social and Personal from facial clues. Englewood Cliffs, NJ:
Relationships, 14, 417–431. Prentice Hall.
Cappella, J. N. (1981). Mutual influence in Emmers, T. M., & Dindia, K. (1995). The effect
expressive behavior: Adult-adult and of relational stage and intimacy on touch:
infant-adult dyadic interaction. Psycholo­ An extension of Guerrero and Andersen.
gical Bulletin, 89, 101–132. Personal Relationships, 2, 225–236.
Cappella, J. N. (1983). Conversational involve­ Feeney, J. A., Noller, P., Sheehan, G., &
ment: Approaching and avoiding others. In Peterson, C. (1999). Conflict issues and
J. M. Wiemann & R. P. Harrison (Eds.), conflict strategies as contexts for nonver­
Nonverbal interaction (pp. 113–148). bal behavior in close relationships. In
Beverly Hills, CA: Sage. P. Philippot, R. S. Feldman, & E. J. Coats
Cappella, J. N. (1988). Personal relationships, (Eds.), The social context of nonverbal
social relationships and patterns of interac­ behavior (pp. 348–371). Paris: Cambridge
tion. In S. Duck (Ed.), Handbook of per­ University Press.
sonal relationships: Theory, research and Fincham, F. D., Bradbury, T. N., Arias, I.,
interventions (pp. 325–342). Chichester, Byrne, C. A., & Karney, B. R. (1997).
UK: Wiley. Marital violence, marital distress, and
Cappella, J. N., & Greene, J. O. (1982). A dis­ attributions. Journal of Family Violence,
crepancy-arousal explanation of mutual 11, 367–372.
influence in expressive behavior for adult Floyd, K. (1999). All touches are not created
and infant-adult interaction. Communica­ equal: Effects of form and duration on
tion Monographs, 49, 89–114. observers’ perceptions of an embrace.
Cartar, L., Hicks, M., & Slane, S. (1996). Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 23,
Women’s reactions to hypothetical male 283–299.
touch as a function of initiator attractive­ Floyd, K., & Ray, G. B. (2003). Human affection
ness and level of coercion. Sex Roles, 35, exchange: VI. Vocalic predictors of perceived
737–750. affection in initial interactions. Western
Clarke, D. D., Allen, C. M. B., & Dickson, S. Journal of Communication, 67, 56–73.
(1985). The characteristic affective tone of Gaelick, L., Bodenhausen, G. V., & Wyer, R. S.,
seven classes of interpersonal relationships. Jr. (1985). Emotional communication in
Journal of Social and Personal Relation­ close relationships. Journal of Personality
ships, 2, 117–130. and Social Psychology, 49, 1246–1265.
Coker, D. A., & Burgoon, J. K. (1987). The Giles, H., & Street, R. L. (1994). Com­
nature of conversational involvement and munication characteristics and behavior.
14-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 5:06 PM Page 275

Nonverbal Behavior in Intimate Interactions and Intimate Relationships–––◆–––275

In M. L. Knapp & G. R. Miller (Eds.) Hall, E. T. (1966). The hidden dimension.


Handbook of interpersonal communication Garden City, NY: Doubleday.
(2nd ed., pp. 103–161). Thousand Oaks, Hall, J. A., & Bernieri, F. J. (2001).
CA: Sage. Interpersonal sensitivity theory and mea­
Gottman, J. M., Markman, H. J., & Notarius, surement. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
C. I. (1977). The topography of marital Hatfield, E. (1984). The dangers of intimacy. In
conflict: A sequenced analysis of verbal and V. J. Derlega (Ed.), Communication, inti­
nonverbal behaviors. Journal of Marriage macy, and close relationships (pp. 191–217).
and the Family, 39, 461–477. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press.
Gottman, J. M., & Porterfield, A. L. (1981). Hatfield, E., Cacioppo, J. T., & Rapson, R. L.
Communicative competence in the nonver­ (1994). Emotional contagion. New York:
bal behavior of married couples. Journal of Cambridge University Press.
Marriage and the Family, 43, 817–824. Hatfield, E., & Rapson, R. L. (1987). Gender
Guerrero, L. K. (1996). Attachment-style difference in love and intimacy: The fantasy
differences in intimacy and involvement: vs. the reality. Journal of Social Work and
A test of the four-category model. Com­ Human Sexuality, 5, 12–26.
munication Monographs, 63, 269–292. Hays, R. B. (1988). Friendship. In S. W. Duck
Guerrero, L. K. (1997). Nonverbal involvement (Ed.), Handbook of personal relationships
across interactions with same-sex friends, (pp. 391–408). New York: Wiley.
opposite-sex friends, and romantic partners: Heavey, C. L., Christensen, A., & Malamuth,
Consistency or change? Journal of Social and N. M. (1995). The longitudinal impact of
Personal Relationships, 14, 31–58. demand and withdrawal during marital
Guerrero, L. K. (2000). Intimacy. In D. Levinson, conflict. Journal of Consulting and Clinical
J. Ponzetti, & P. Jorgensen (Eds.), Ency­ Psychology, 63, 797–801.
clopedia of human emotions. (pp. 403–409) Heslin, R., & Boss, D. (1980). Nonverbal inti­
New York: Macmillan Reference. macy in arrival and departure at an airport.
Guerrero, L. K. (2004). Observer ratings of Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin,
nonverbal involvement and immediacy. In 6, 248–252.
V. Manusov (Ed.), The sourcebook of Isabella, R. A., & Belsky, J. (1991). Interactional
nonverbal measures: Going beyond words synchrony and the origins of infant-mother
(pp. 221–235). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. attachment: A replication study. Child
Guerrero, L. K., & Andersen, P. A. (1991). Development, 62, 373–384.
The waxing and waning of relational Jones, S. M. (2004). Putting the person into
intimacy: Touch as a function of relational person-centered and immediate emotional
stage, gender and touch avoidance. Journal support: Emotional change and perceived
of Social and Personal Relationships, 8, helper competence as outcomes of comfort­
147–165. ing in helping situations. Communication
Guerrero, L. K., & Andersen, P. A. (1994). Research, 32, 338–360.
Patterns of matching and initiation: Touch Jones, S. M., & Burleson, B. R. (2003). Effects
behavior and avoidance across romantic of helper and recipient sex on the experi­
relationship stages. Journal of Nonverbal ence and outcomes of comforting messages:
Behavior, 18, 137–153. An experimental investigation. Sex Roles,
Guerrero, L. K., & Andersen, P. A. (2000). 48(1/2), 1–19.
Emotion in close relationships. In Jones, S. M., & Guerrero, L. K. (2001).
C. Hendrick & S. S. Hendrick (Eds.), Close Nonverbal immediacy and verbal person
relationships: A sourcebook (pp. 171–186). centeredness in the emotional support
Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. process. Human Communication Research,
Guerrero, L. K., & Floyd, K. (2006). Nonverbal 27, 567–596.
communication in close relationships. Kelly, A. B., Fincham, F. D., & Beach, S. R. H.
Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. (2003). Communication skills in couples:
14-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 5:06 PM Page 276

276–––◆–––Functions

A review and discussion of emerging perspec­ Mehrabian, A. (1967). Orientation behaviors


tives. In J. O. Greene & B. R. Burleson (Eds.), and nonverbal attitude in communicators.
Handbook of communication and social Journal of Communication, 17, 324–332.
skills (pp. 723–751). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. Mehrabian, A. (1969a). Significance of posture
Kleinke, C. L. (1986). Gaze and eye contact: A and position in the communication of atti­
research review. Psychological Bulletin, tude and status relationships. Psychological
100, 78–100. Bulletin, 71, 359–372.
Knapp, M. L., & Hall, J. A. (2006). Nonverbal Mehrabian, A. (1969b). Some referents and
communication in human interaction (6th measures of nonverbal behavior. Behavior
ed.). Belmont, CA: Wadsworth-Thomson. Research Methods and Instrumentation, 1,
Koerner, A. F., & Fitzpatrick, M. A. (2002). 203–207.
Nonverbal communication and marital Mehrabian A. (1981). Silent messages: Implicit
adjustment and satisfaction: The role of communication of emotions and attitudes
decoding relationship relevant and relation­ (2nd ed.). Belmont, CA: Wadsworth.
ship irrelevant affect. Communication Mehrabian, A., & Ksionsky, S. (1972).
Monographs, 69, 33–51. Categories of social behavior. Comparative
Lee, J. W., & Guerrero, L. K. (2001). Types Group Studies, 3, 425–436.
of touch in cross-sex relationships between Monsour, M. (1992). Meanings of intimacy
co-workers: Perceptions of relational and in cross- and same-sex friendships. Journal
emotional messages, inappropriateness, and of Social and Personal Relationships, 9,
sexual harassment. Journal of Applied 277–295.
Communication, 29, 197–220. Montagu, A. (1978). Touching: The human
Manusov, V. (1995). Reacting to changes in significance of the skin (2nd ed.). New
nonverbal behaviors: Relational satisfac­ York: Harper and Row.
tion and adaptation patterns in romantic Morris, D. (1971). Intimate behavior. New
dyads. Human Communication Research, York: Random House.
21, 456–477. Morton, T. L. (1977). The effects of acquain­
Manusov, V., Floyd, K., & Kerssen-Griep, J. tance and distance on intimacy and reci­
(1997). Yours, mine, and ours: Mutual attri­ procity. Unpublished doctoral dissertation,
butions for nonverbal behaviors in couples’ University of Utah, Salt Lake City.
interactions. Communication Research, 24, Newton, D. A., & Burgoon, J. K. (1990).
234–260. Nonverbal conflict behaviors: Functions,
Marston, P. J., Hecht, M. L., Manke, M. L., strategies, and tactics. In D. D. Cahn (Ed.),
McDaniel, S., & Reeder, H. (1998). The sub­ Intimates in conflict: A communication
jective experience of intimacy, passion, and perspective (pp. 77–104). Hillsdale, NJ:
commitment in heterosexual loving relation­ Erlbaum.
ships. Personal Relationships, 5, 15–30. Palmer, M. T., Cappella, N. J., Patterson, M. L., &
Marston, P. J., Hecht, M. L., & Robers, T. Churchill, M. (1990, June). Mapping conver­
(1987). True love ways: The subjective expe­ sation behaviors onto relational inferences I:
rience and communication of romantic love. A cross-sectional analysis. Paper presented at
Journal of Social and Personal Relation­ the meeting of the International Communica­
ships, 4, 387–407. tion Association, Dublin, Ireland.
McAdams, D. P. (1988). Personal needs and Palmer, M. T., & Simmons, K. B. (1995).
personal relationships. In S. Duck (Ed.), Communicating intentions through nonver­
Handbook of personal relationships bal behaviors: Conscious and unconscious
(pp. 7–22). New York: Wiley. encoding of liking. Human Communication
McDaniel, E., & Andersen, P.A. (1998). Research, 22, 128–160.
International patterns of tactile communi­ Patterson, M. L. (1983). Nonverbal behavior:
cation: A field study. Journal of Nonverbal A functional perspective. New York:
Behavior, 21, 59–75. Springer.
14-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 5:06 PM Page 277

Nonverbal Behavior in Intimate Interactions and Intimate Relationships–––◆–––277

Patterson, M. L. (1988). Functions of nonverbal Reis, H. T., & Shaver, P. (1988). Intimacy as
behavior in close relationships. In S. Duck an interpersonal process. In S. Duck (Ed.),
(Ed.), Handbook of personal relationships Handbook of personal relationships
(pp. 41–56). New York: Wiley. (pp. 367–389). New York: Wiley.
Planalp, S., DeFrancisco, V. I., & Rutherford, D. Shaver, P. R., Schwartz, J., Kirson, D., &
(1996). Varieties of cues to emotion in nat­ O’Connor, C. (1987). Emotion knowl­
urally occurring situations. Cognition and edge: Further explorations of a prototype
Emotion, 10, 137–153. approach. Journal of Personality and Social
Prager, K. J. (1995). The psychology of inti­ Psychology, 52, 1061–1086.
macy. New York: Guilford. Simpson, J. A., Orina, M., & Ickes, W. (2003).
Prager, K. J. (2000). Intimacy in personal rela­ When accuracy hurts and when it helps: A
tionships. In C. Hendrick & S. S. Hendrick test of the empathic accuracy model in mar­
(Eds.), Close relationships: A sourcebook ital interactions. Journal of Personality and
(pp. 229–242). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. Social Psychology, 85, 881–893.
Prager, K. J., & Roberts, L. J. (2004). Deep Stern, D. (1980). The first relationship: Mother
intimate connection: Self and intimacy in and infant. Cambridge, MA: Harvard
couple relationships. In D. J. Mashek & University Press.
A. P. Aron (Eds.), Handbook of closeness Sternberg, R. J. (1986). A triangular theory of
and intimacy (pp. 43–60). Mahwah, NJ: love. Psychological Review, 93, 119–135.
Erlbaum. Tickle-Degnen, L., & Rosenthal, R. (1990). The
Priest, R. F., & Sawyer, J. (1967). Proximity and nature of rapport and nonverbal correlates.
peership: Bases of balance in interpersonal Psychology Inquiry, 1, 285–293.
attraction. American Journal of Sociology, Wertin, L., & Andersen, P. A. (1996, February).
72, 633–649. Cognitive schema and the perceptions of
Ray, G. B., & Floyd, K. (2000, May). Nonverbal sexual harassment: A test of cognitive
expressions of liking and disliking in initial valence theory. Paper presented at the
interaction: Encoding and decoding per­ annual meeting of the Western States
spectives. Paper presented at the meeting Communication Association, Pasadena, CA.
of the Eastern States Communication Asso­ Wilmot, W. W. (1995). Relational communica­
ciation, Pittsburgh, PA. tion. New York: McGraw-Hill.
14-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 5:06 PM Page 278
15-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 5:06 PM Page 279

15
NONVERBAL EXPRESSIONS OF
DOMINANCE AND POWER IN
HUMAN RELATIONSHIPS

� Judee K. Burgoon
University of Arizona

� Norah E. Dunbar
California State University, Long Beach

F undamental to all social species is the negotiation and expression


of dominance and power relationships. Whether it is establishing a
pecking order or a marching order, proclaiming privileges or prohibi­
tions, exercising leadership or intimidation, humans, like other mam­
mals, have evolved intricate means of signaling in any social encounter
who are “one up” or “one down,” who can “have” or “have not,” who
“goes before” or “goes after.” Such signaling is a necessity for creating

Authors’ Note: Preparation of this manuscript was supported by funding


from the U.S. Air Force Office of Scientific Research under the U.S. Depart­
ment of Defense University Research Initiative (Grant No. F49620-01-1­
0394). The views, opinions, and/or findings in this report are those of the
authors and should not be construed as an official Department of Defense
position, policy, or decision.

◆ 279
15-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 5:06 PM Page 280

280–––◆–––Functions

and maintaining social order. And, as with Koch, 1998). This chapter is meant to
so many other aspects of social behavior, further that discussion.
much of this work is done nonverbally. It is
the nonverbal signals of dominance-
submission and power-powerlessness that ♦ Conceptualizing Power,
constitute the focus of this chapter.
Dominance, and Status
The social significance of nonverbal
expressions of dominance and power is
underscored by the panoply of scientific Decades of scholarly debate on what con­
research and commentary it has attrac­ stitutes power have produced a consensual
ted. Ellyson and Dovidio (1985) note such view of power as the capacity to produce
diverse tributaries as Charles Darwin’s intended effects, and in particular, the
The Expression of Emotion in Man and ability to influence the behavior of another
Animals (1872/1965), Sigmund Freud’s person (Berger, 1994; Burgoon et al.,
psychoanalytic treatises, works by person­ 1998; Dunbar, 2004; French & Raven,
ality and social psychologists in the early 1959). Power takes many forms, and
1900s, and anthropological works by its multidimensional nature is reflected in
Ray Birdwhistell (1970) and Edward Hall the classification of power into three
(1959, 1966). A confluence of research on domains: power bases, power processes,
such interrelated constructs as status, and power outcomes (Olson & Cromwell,
authority, rank, control, influence, exper­ 1975). Power bases refer to resources such
tise, leadership, domineeringness, assertive­ as rewards or knowledge that form the
ness, and aggressiveness has galvanized foundation for control over others (French
interest further in this fundamental social & Raven, 1959). Several of the nonverbal
dimension. Though these various con­ display patterns to be discussed in this
structs are not synonymous, they all fall chapter are linked to these bases of power.
under what Edinger and Patterson (1983) Power processes, on the other hand, are
referred to as the social control aspects of the specific strategies (often nonverbal)
interaction and what Hall, Coats, and used to exert power in interactions. Power
Smith LeBeau (2005) labeled the vertical outcomes are the compliance, conformity,
dimension of human relationships. cooperation, or obedience that one
In this chapter, we conceptualize domi­ secures; that is, they are the actual influ­
nance and power as incorporating not ence that is achieved over others’ beliefs
only reflexive, fixed action patterns that and actions (Wheeless, Barraclough, &
are under the control of external stimuli Stewart, 1983).
but also deliberate, adaptive, and change­ The conceptualization of dominance has
able ones that are under the control of varied according to disciplinary perspective.
actors themselves (consistent with some For personality psychologists, dominance is
ethologists and behavioral ecologists, e.g., considered an enduring individual trait that
Bernstein, 1980; Fridlund, 1991a, 1991b; designates one’s characteristic temperament
Liska, 1992). Greater emphasis on the and behavioral predispositions (e.g., Cattell,
strategic aspects of dominance squares Eber, & Tatsuoka, 1970; Ridgeway, 1987).
with our writings on interpersonal domi­ Social skills are part of this equation, as the
nance as social, interactional, situational, ability to be forceful, to take initiative, and
intentional, and dynamic (Burgoon & to be expressive yet relaxed and poised
Dunbar, 2000; Burgoon, Johnson, & are all facets of dominance displays that
15-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 5:06 PM Page 281

Nonverbal Expressions of Dominance and Power in Human Relationships–––◆–––281

correspond with characterizations of a skill­ Overall, power, dominance, and status


ful communication style (Burgoon & are best conceptualized as interrelated
Dunbar, 2000). For biologists and socio­ though not synonymous. High status often
biologists, dominance designates a pattern gives the appearance of power and may
of imbalance in interactions within a dyadic facilitate dominance because one is endowed
relationship (Hinde, 1978) or an organism’s with legitimate authority, and legitimate
position in a social hierarchy (Sebeok, authority confers on the individual the
1972), which accords it preferential access potential for greater influence (Rollins &
to resources (Omark, 1980). For sociolo­ Bahr, 1976). But high status does not guar­
gists, power and dominance are intertwined antee the exercise of power or the display of
with status, which designates one’s position dominant behavior, and dominance displays
in a socially agreed-upon hierarchy, some­ in the absence of legitimate power may fail
thing that is prevalent in all types of societies to achieve influence (Ridgeway, Diekema,
(Lips, 1991). A person’s social position is & Johnson, 1995). In this chapter, we focus
often based on possession of commodities on “power” and “dominance” and use
valued by the society (e.g., money, occupa­ them as shorthand for the dimensions of
tion, good looks) or position within a pres­ dominance-submission and power-power­
tige hierarchy of relations in a social unit. lessness. We ask the reader to interpret
Communication and social psychology dominance and power, not as discrete cate­
scholars largely view dominance as a social gories, but as continua that span the
rather than organismic variable but one that extremes of each dimensional pole and all
is defined at the interpersonal level (i.e., in points in between.
relation to another actor). Dominance con­
stitutes one of two superordinate topoi of
relational communication (Burgoon & ♦ Operationalizing
Hale, 1984). Whereas power may remain Power and Dominance
latent, dominance is manifest and behav­
ioral, referencing those communicative acts
by which one actor’s assertion of influence Power and dominance can be understood
and control is met with acquiescence from more concretely through their typical oper­
another (Burgoon & Dunbar, 2000; ationalizations. Power is often operational­
Rogers-Millar & Millar, 1979). Moreover, ized as potential influence and measured
even though dominance-submission is con­ as a perceptual variable, despite claims
ceptualized as a universal dimension along that perceptions do not necessarily consti­
which all social relationships can be tute “real” power differences (e.g., Olson
arrayed, dominance displays may be insti­ & Cromwell, 1975). Measures range from
gated by a combination of individual tem­ self-reports and overlapping circles that rep­
perament and situational features that resent various levels of equality in a rela­
encourage dominant behavior (Aries, Gold, tionship, to role-plays, projective tests, and
& Weigel, 1983; Burgoon & Dunbar, 2000) story writing (e.g., Berger, 1994; Dunbar &
and that are responsive to changing goals, Burgoon, 2005a; Neff & Harter, 2002).
interlocutors, situations, and time, among Such measures often make little or no
other factors. Thus, within the same mention of nonverbal behavior.
episode, individuals may adjust their By contrast, dominance is operationalized
dominance-submission displays to changing commonly in ways that incorporate nonver­
circumstances. bal behavior, whether through self-reports of
15-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 5:06 PM Page 282

282–––◆–––Functions

an individual’s own dominance, partner or through an individual’s perceptions based


observer perceptions of another person’s on his or her relationship to another,
dominance, or trained coders’ ratings of whereas dominance is measured by the
particular markers of dominance (Dunbar & behavioral manifestations of that power,
Burgoon, 2005b). Measurements can be whether recorded by third parties or the
made at the microlevel, in which case they interactants themselves.
entail objective physical behaviors, or at the
macrolevel, in which case they entail holistic
interpretations of whether an actor appears ♦ Theoretical Perspectives
dominant or submissive (see Dunbar &
Burgoon, 2005b, for samples of both types
There is an array of theories and models that
in separate studies and Burgoon & Le Poire,
provide the foundation for principles of non­
1999, or Moscowitz, 1988, 1990, for
verbal dominance, power, and status.
examples of both measurement approaches
Organismic approaches view dominance and
in the same study). Dominant microbehav­
power as inhering in characteristics or
iors that have been measured include talk
behavioral patterns of individuals, and status
time, loudness, gaze, eyebrow raise, posture,
as associated with classes of individuals.
arm and leg positions, physical assertiveness,
More social models envision dominance and
smiling, threat gestures, proximity, and
power as transacted between actors at
touch (e.g., Aries et al., 1983; Schwartz,
dyadic or group levels. Uniting these respec­
Tesser, & Powell, 1982).
tive perspectives on social conduct is an
In contrast to examinations of particular
interest in socially acquired behavioral rou­
behaviors made by an individual, rela­
tines. Several of these models are discussed in
tionally based measurement examines
dominance at the dyadic level. A relational the following sections.
perspective distinguishes between domi­
neeringness—individual attempts to con­ PERSONALITY AND
trol the interaction—and dominance. The EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY
former is assessed as an individual-level
characteristic; the latter is defined accord­ The history of personality assessment is
ing to pairs of adjacent conversational rife with constructs that are linked explic­
turns by which interlocutors position them­ itly or implicitly to dominance and power:
selves as “one up” or “one down” vis-à-vis nDominance, nPower, authoritarianism,
the other. Specifically, dominance is said locus of control, and Machiavellianism,
to occur when one individual’s assertive among others. Because personality is
actions elicit complementary acquiescence expected to be fairly stable, nonverbal indi­
by another. Hence, dominance or submis­ cators associated with personality traits
sion is defined according to “interacts” should form stable behavioral profiles by
(pairs of acts) rather than individual acts which individuals could be typed. In sup­
(Rogers-Millar & Millar, 1979; for an port of this view, Gifford (1994) used a
incorporation of nonverbal cues and the Brunswikian lens model to analyze 27
application in group settings, see Siegel, behaviors and concluded that eight were
Friedlander, & Heatherington, 1992). valid indicators that accounted for 30% of
Overall, however, the operationalizations the variance in ambitious-dominant per­
of power and dominance reflect their defin­ sonality and 39% of the variance in lazy-
itional differences. Power is often measured submissive personality. Other research,
15-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 5:06 PM Page 283

Nonverbal Expressions of Dominance and Power in Human Relationships–––◆–––283

discussed in detail below, points to a fairly dominance is also socially negotiated;


stable behavioral profile associated with away from partitioning variance into sepa­
expressivity, which some (e.g., Gallaher, rate individual and social components,
1992) have viewed as a personality style toward understanding the interaction
that would be associated closely with dom­ between organisms and their social envi­
inance or extraversion. ronment (Burgoon & Dunbar, 2000).
Ethology and evolutionary psychol­ Representative of this view is the behavioral
ogy perspectives, on the other hand, focus ecology perspective, a term coined by
on physiological, anatomical, and behav­ Fridlund (1991a, 1991b), that acknowl­
ioral features that are adaptive in respond­ edges the fundamentally social and commu­
ing to survival-related problems (see Floyd, nicative nature of these dominance
this volume). Within the ethological per­ displays, their inborn origins notwithstand­
spective is a bifurcation between viewing ing. Display forms and intensity are seen as
dominance-submission as (1) inborn attri­ responsive to the relationship between
butes possessed by individual organisms sender and receiver and no longer viewed as
that confer on them greater or lesser sur­ primitive reflexes but rather as inclinations,
vival success than other species mates or (2) because unequivocally announcing one’s
emergent properties of social interaction intentions would risk heightened resistance
that entail the ability to manipulate others and therefore be detrimental to survival.
and form alliances. Those who subscribe Appeasement displays are linked closely
to the former viewpoint see in many to the submissive end of the dominance-
intraspecific and interspecific displays ves­ submission continuum. Keltner, Young,
tiges of inborn tendencies to exert power and Buswell (1997) conjecture that emo­
and domination, to elicit deference and tional states such as modesty, shyness,
acquiescence, or to appease and submit to a embarrassment, and shame have all evolved
stronger conspecific (see Andrew, 1972; from appeasement systems in other species
Keltner, 1995; Smith, 1974; Thorpe, 1972). and serve appeasement-related functions in
Displays such as fight-flight, or more abbre­ humans. Appeasement signals are intended
viated intention signals of approach- to placate or pacify others when social rela­
avoidance, are thought to be universal and tionships are disrupted and conflicts arise.
innate, the result of natural selection lead­ Violations of social rules or breeches of
ing to ritualized displays. Behaviors such as expected social distance may prompt
sprawling, relaxed postures and sweeping appeasement gestures in the form of sub­
gestures that signal lack of fear among missiveness, affiliative displays, or inhibi­
humans find parallels in the expansive pos­ tions of other actions. Acceptance of an
ture or tail-in-the-air gait of other warm­ appeasement gesture may take the form of
blooded creatures. reduced aggressiveness and increased social
Yet many behaviors that appear to be approach.
homologs across species do not necessarily
share a genetic origin. Some signals may actu­
ally be conventionalized, the result of learning SOCIAL EXCHANGE MODELS
at critical periods in an animal’s development
(Fridlund, 1991a). Contemporary ethological Social exchange models move domi­
work is shifting away from viewing domi­ nance, power, and status from the person­
nance strictly as an innate pattern based ality or biological into a social arena and
on aggressiveness, toward one in which share the assumption that individuals will
15-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 5:06 PM Page 284

284–––◆–––Functions

act to maximize their interpersonal rewards actions, and perceived by their partners as
and minimize their interpersonal costs nondominant.
(Rusbult & Arriaga, 1997; Thibaut &
Kelley, 1959). Social exchange theories
view power as a characteristic of a relation­ GENDER POLITICS
ship, not an individual. Power is achieved
dyadically when a person is valued as an Another social model of power is what
exchange partner and there are few alterna­ has become known as the “gender politics
tives; people depend more on partners who hypothesis” (Henley, 1977, 1995; LaFrance
hold high exchange value (Emerson, 1962). & Henley, 1993; also see Hall, this volume).
This power-dependence relationship may In this model, nonverbal behaviors are a
be expressed nonverbally through influence primary means by which those in positions
strategies related to making oneself appear of power (usually men) exercise social con­
more attractive as a partner or signaling trol and interpersonal dominance. Henley
one’s interest or disinterest in an exchange argued that many behaviors that may seem
relationship (Burgoon, Dunbar, & Segrin, unimportant are actually reflections of soci­
2002). etal biases founded in power differences. For
One social exchange theory that focuses example, she claimed that women are more
on nonverbal cues specifically is dyadic likely to exhibit circumspect demeanor,
power theory (DPT) (Dunbar, 2004; tense posture, gaze aversion or vigilant
Rollins & Bahr, 1976). It proposes that watching, more smiling, touch avoidance,
perceptions of legitimate authority and and greater emotional expressivity. Henley
access to resources increase individuals’ per­ drew a parallel between the behaviors asso­
ceptions of power compared with interac­ ciated with status and the behaviors associ­
tion partners. Perceptions of power, in turn, ated with sex: “The same behaviors
influence the use of dominant communica­ exhibited by superior to subordinate are
tion to control the interaction, which those exhibited by men to women; and
results in greater influence over decisions. women exhibit to men the behaviors typical
In DPT, the relationship between power of subordinate to superior” (Henley, 1977,
and dominance is theorized to be cur­ p. 180). Undergirding the gender politics
vilinear, because power is sometimes latent hypothesis are three key premises: (1) that
rather than overt (Komter, 1989). For nonverbal differences between men and
example, powerless individuals may remain women are substantial; (2) that observed
silent if they fear retaliation or termination nonverbal patterns reflect social disparities;
of the relationship from their more power­ that is, nonverbal patterns are systematically
ful partner (Cloven & Roloff, 1993; Leung, correlated with degree of power or domi­
1988). On the other hand, extremely pow­ nance; and (3) those who occupy the sub­
erful individuals may maintain control ordinate role (usually women) are more
without ever having to initiate any control socially perceptive and vigilant by virtue
attempts (Bugental & Shennum, 2002; of their position of greater weakness and
Christensen & Heavey, 1990; Dunbar, vulnerability.
2004). In a test of DPT, Dunbar and Research has documented large sex or
Burgoon (2005a) found that the most pow­ gender differences in nonverbal behavior.
erful individuals (i.e., with the most influ­ For example, several reviews by Hall (1998,
ence) were the most facially and gesturally this volume; Hall et al., 2005) and others
expressive, the least controlled in their body (e.g., Andersen, 1998; Burgoon & Dillman,
15-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 5:06 PM Page 285

Nonverbal Expressions of Dominance and Power in Human Relationships–––◆–––285

1995; LaFrance, Hecht, & Levy Paluck, around expectations that establish a “power
2003; Riggio, this volume) have found and prestige order” (Berger, Conner, &
that, compared with men, women generally Fisek, 1974; Ridgeway & Berger, 1986;
smile, gaze, nod, and gesture more; use Ridgeway & Walker, 1995). According to
more direct body orientation; receive but do this theory, group members develop expec­
not give more touch; are less relaxed postur­ tations about others’ likely contributions to
ally but also exhibit less shifting of body and the task based on status characteristics, and
feet; have more speech errors, and give more these performance expectations confer an
back-channel responses. Women are also “expectation advantage or disadvantage,”
more skilled decoders and encoders of non­ depending on whether the individual is
verbal and emotional communication and expected to contribute favorably or unfa­
exhibit nonverbal behaviors indicative of vorably to successful task completion.
docility and openness to others. Status characteristics within this model
Nevertheless, many of the empirical find­ involve any quality of actors around which
ings do not comport with the gender politics evaluations of and beliefs about them come
hypothesis. For example, the meta-analysis to be organized (e.g., age, sex, race, ethnicity,
by Hall et al. (2005) found only four behav­ education, occupation, physical attractive­
iors that were associated with higher actual ness, and intelligence; Berger, Rosenholtz, &
verticality (their term for dominance, status, Zelditch, 1980). Furthermore, expectation
and power): closer physical distances to states theory differentiates between specific
others, using more open body postures, and diffuse status characteristics. Specific
interrupting more, and speaking more status characteristics are socially valued
loudly (see also, Hall, Rosip, Smith LeBeau, skills, expertise, or social accomplishments
Horgan, & Carter, 2006; Snodgrass, Hecht, that imply a specific and bounded range of
& Ploutz-Snyder, 1998). The great hetero­ competencies, such as computer or mathe­
geneity in results and lack of parallelism matical skills. Diffuse status characteristics,
between nonverbal gender differences and such as gender or race, not only are associ­
power, dominance, or status have led many ated culturally with some specific skills but
scholars to conclude that research results on also carry general expectations for compe­
the gender politics hypothesis are equivocal, tence that are diffuse and unbounded in
that there are more commonalities than range (Ridgeway & Walker, 1995). Many of
differences between the sexes in dominance these characteristics are signaled nonverbally
displays, that purported sex differences through demeanor and appearance, making
are often based on stereotypes, and that this theory especially relevant to nonverbal
observed differences may not be attributable dominance. Those who possess status-
to women holding more subservient roles valued external characteristics “are more
(Burgoon & Dillman, 1995; Dindia & likely (1) to have chances to perform, (2) to
Canary, 2006; Hall et al., 2005). initiate problem-solving performances, (3)
have their performances positively evaluated,
and (4) are less likely to be influenced when
EXPECTANCY THEORIES there are disagreements” (Berger, Ridgeway,
Fisek, & Norman, 1998, p. 381) than
Other models of power draw upon those lacking such characteristics or those
expectations. For example, expectation who possess negatively valued ones.
states theory, which focuses on influence Related to performance expectations
and task performance in groups, revolves are reward expectations: expectations
15-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 5:06 PM Page 286

286–––◆–––Functions

about whether the status characteristics are individual that are appropriate, desired, or
more or less likely to create benefits for preferred. Deviant or unexpected behaviors,
individual perceivers or the group. Three by virtue of their novelty, can heighten
classes of reward structures that have attention to the violation and the commu­
been identified are categorical, ability, and nicator committing the violation. Reward
outcome (Ridgeway & Berger, 1986). valence refers to whether interactions with
Categorical structures are related to diffuse the communicator are viewed as desirable
social status characteristics such as age, or not. Someone who is physically attrac­
gender, or physical strength. These expecta­ tive, has high status, controls valued
tions are like the physical attractiveness resources, or gives positive feedback, for
stereotypes discussed earlier in their ability instance, should be more positively regarded
to engender attraction and confer credibil­ than someone who is physically repulsive,
ity. Ability structures are associated with has lower status, controls nothing of
the specific task to be performed. Speaking value, or gives negative feedback. Tests of
with an authoritative voice or using dra­ EVT have demonstrated that for highly
matic gestures may imply greater confi­ rewarding communicators, violations of
dence and expertise (i.e., greater ability). expectancies can engender more positive
Outcome structures are associated with interpretations of ambiguous or polysemic
actual accomplishments during the group nonverbal behaviors, such that positive vio­
task. Those with high expectation advan­ lations produce more favorable outcomes
tages not only are more likely to take than expectancy confirmations. The same
the initiative (talking first, establishing act committed by low-reward communica­
seating arrangements, etc.) and to be more tors can backfire, eliciting uncharitable
participative but also are likely to be evaluations that make it a negative violation.
accorded more deferential treatment by For low-reward communicators, expectancy
others. In this manner, they will have more confirmation is therefore best (Burgoon,
of their recommendations acknowledged 1991; Burgoon, Newton, Walther, &
and accepted. Baesler, 1989; Le Poire & Burgoon, 1994).
Another expectancy-based theory is Nonverbal indicators of status, power, and
expectancy violations theory (EVT), dominance may be understood in many
which addresses the effects of noticeable contexts from an EVT frame to the extent
deviations from both societal and individ­ that they impinge on reward valence, con­
ual expectations for nonverbal communi­ stitute expectancy violations, or are the
cation (Burgoon, 1978, 1995; Burgoon & result of violations.
Burgoon, 2001). EVT is relevant to nonver­
bal dominance and power in several respects.
Nonverbal behaviors are the locus of the SUMMARY
expectations (or their violations). Inter­
pretations of nonverbal behavior may include Scholars have identified many theoreti­
dominance connotations, and effects may cal explanations for differences in status,
include perceived power and actual influ­ dominance, or power. Whereas some theo­
ence. Additionally, effects may be moderated rists place the emphasis on personality or
by characteristics of the actor that include his individual characteristics, many scholars
or her status, dominance, and power. consider perceptions of power and the
In EVT, expectancies are enduring pat­ resultant dominance displays to be interper­
terns of anticipated behavior for a particular sonal or relational in nature. They are often
15-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 5:06 PM Page 287

Nonverbal Expressions of Dominance and Power in Human Relationships–––◆–––287

influenced by context in that interactants’ These displays map onto the most primitive
past experiential history or larger societal fight-flight and approach-avoidance response
norms can influence the dynamic inter­ patterns (Keltner, Gruenfeld, & Anderson,
play of dominance-submission. This view 2003). Threats may signal not only readi­
allows us to examine the particular ways in ness to strike but also the ability to fight
which power-powerlessness or dominance- if provoked (Maynard Smith, 1982).
submission are typically manifest in inter­ Less indicative of impending physical
action. The next section introduces three aggression, but still intimidating, are threat
general principles for the nonverbal expres­ stares and penetrating gazes (Ellsworth,
sion of dominance and power. Carlsmith, & Henson, 1972; Exline,
Ellyson, & Long, 1975; Le Poire &
Burgoon, 1994). Although Hall et al.
♦ Nonverbal Strategies (2005) found that actual gaze was not asso­
for Signaling Dominance- ciated with their dimension of verticality, it
Submission and Power- is possible that distinguishing among glares,
Powerlessness stares, and timing of breaks in eye contact
might produce differences. Hinde (1978,
1985) proposed that threat displays show
Based on an extensive survey of the empiri­ conflicting intentions to attack and to
cal nonverbal literature, Burgoon and col­ escape. Such ambivalence and the likely
leagues (Burgoon, Buller, & Woodall, 1996; development of finely graded rather than
Burgoon & Hoobler, 2002) have proposed crude signals of threat may account for
a number of principles (rules or abstrac­ mixed empirical findings.
tions) underlying the nonverbal expression Another symbolic form of threat comes
of dominance and power. We group these from silence, which can convert its target
principles into three general categories: (1) from personhood to nonperson (or object)
physical potency, (2) resource control, and status, with concomitant loss of belonging­
(3) interaction control. ness and protection accorded members of
the same social unit. As explained by
Bruneau (1973), the “silent treatment” can
PHYSICAL POTENCY force

Physical potency represents most closely subordinates into awkward positions


the stereotypic views of what it means to whereby they exhibit behaviors detri­
signify dominance or submission, power mental to their own cause—because
or powerlessness. The components that are their frustration is aggravated by silent
included within the general conception of response to their efforts. Silence as
potency are (1) threat, (2) size or strength, absence of response to or lack of recog­
and (3) expressivity. nition of subordinates may very well be
the main source of protection of power
Threat. Perhaps the most obvious signals in socio-political orders where physical
of dominance and submission recognized restraint has lost repute. (p. 39)
in the ethological literature (e.g., Keltner,
1995; Smith, 1974; Thorpe, 1972) are Other signals intended to show subor­
threat and fright displays, the former to dination, supplication, and appeasement
intimidate and the latter to show timidity. include stooped and contractive postures,
15-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 5:06 PM Page 288

288–––◆–––Functions

crouching, drawing the head into the responses to visible physical attributes of
shoulders, a hesitant gait, a slow and ten­ potential harm or harmlessness that can
tative approach, retreating body orienta­ be deciphered from a safe distance. Mature
tions, and exposing vulnerable body faces are broader ones with square jaws,
regions such as the jugular vein or palm larger noses, more prominent eyebrows,
(Mehrabian, 1981). Targets of impending thinner lips, and smaller ratios of eye size to
attack may rely on these fright signals to face size, whereas baby-faced features
show submission symbolically. Those low included more rounded and softer features,
in power and in subservient roles may also smaller noses, less pronounced brows and
exhibit far less expressivity due to a host of eyebrows, larger lips, and larger eye sizes
inhibitory and avoidant tendencies associ­ relative to total face size (Keating, 1985,
ated with the position of vulnerability this volume; Rhodes & Zebrowitz, 2002;
(Keltner et al., 2003). Zebrowitz, Fellous, Mignault, & And­
reoletti, 2003). Baby-faced and smiling
Size and Strength. All species appear to faces are seen as submissive, weak, and
respond to speed, agility, and energy expen­ helpless; mature and unsmiling or frowning
diture as indicators of potency. By contrast, faces with furrowed brows are seen as dom­
lethargy and torpidity are associated typi­ inant, threatening or aggressive. Similarly,
cally with weakness and ineffectualness. high-pitched, thin voices are babyish;
Thus, any nonverbal action that entails a deep-pitched, louder, and more resonant
high degree of intensity and dynamic action ones are considered mature (Montepare &
is likely to connote power and to secure Zebrowitz-McArthur, 1987).
avoidance, flight, or submission from less
dominant others. In the human repertoire, Expressivity. In addition to threat and
height, weight, bulk, and muscularity may size, dominance has been associated with
signal sufficient physical strength and more energetic and animated behavior
endurance to render the displayer victorious such as variable facial expressions,
in a physical conflict. Other nonverbal inflected speech, high-pitched voice, head
behaviors that connote size or strength are shaking and nodding, frequent and broad
rapid gait, erect postures, firm stances, ani­ gestures, wide smiles, peppy, not sluggish
mated gesturing, loud and deep-pitched or lethargic movement, erect posture,
voices, rapid speaking tempo, clear articula­ quick movement, upright torso, vertical
tion, and clothing or hair styles that create a sitting posture, more expansive and
bulky appearance (Apple, Streeter & Krauss, emphatic postures and vocalizations, loud
1979; Burgoon, Birk, & Pfau, 1990; Hall voice, vigorous behaviors, heavy step, legs
et al., 2006; Mehrabian, 1981; Schwartz et wide apart, elbows akimbo, hands away
al., 1982). Size and strength can be signaled from body, emphatic and centrifugal ges­
also by “strength in numbers,” such as a tures and movements, and more coordi­
celebrity’s entourage, a gang member’s com­ nated nonverbal behavior (e.g., smooth
rades, or an army battalion’s compatriots. voice, fluid and graceful walk, rhythmic
Implicit signals of strength or weakness speech, flowing voice and speech)
are facial and vocal maturity or babyish­ (Gallaher, 1992). These behaviors con­
ness. Nonverbal appearance features that note a high degree of actual or potential
connote maturity or immaturity, and energy expenditure.
actions that emphasize or deemphasize Gifford (1994) found several behaviors
these features, may capitalize on innate associated with perceptions of individuals
15-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 5:06 PM Page 289

Nonverbal Expressions of Dominance and Power in Human Relationships–––◆–––289

as more dominant-ambitious (direct head space is “taken”), have easy access to


orientation, lack of forward head tilt, others, and may have others’ access regu­
more head shaking, direct trunk orienta­ lated by gatekeepers—people such as recep­
tion, less arm wrap, more gestures, more tionists who can prevent intrusions. In
self-manipulations, right leg orientation, addition to access to space, dominance may
more right leg lean, and more leg exten­ also be expressed by taking up more physi­
sion). Additionally, gregarious-extraverted cal space (i.e., a combination of enlarging
individuals, who also might be regarded as one’s size and occupying more space).
dominant, displayed more nods, less arm Dominant individuals often sit in more
wrap, more gestures, and more left leg open body positions (as opposed to defen­
lean; those individuals perceived as gregar­ sive positions), adopt stances with arms
ious-extraverted displayed most of the akimbo, and use more expansive gestures.
same behaviors as associated with domi­ Submissive people take up less space by
nance plus more nods, smiles, forward contracting their postures, sitting with
lean, and hand extension and less left-hand closed arm and leg positions, and using
verticality, leg openness, and leg move­ diminutive, if any, gestures (Burgoon,
ment. In general, then, behaviors that Buller, Hale, & deTurck, 1984).
connote a high degree of actual or poten­
tial energy expenditure are associated with Precedence. Precedence refers to “who
dominance and power. gets to go first” and likely extends from an
evolutionary basis (Burgoon & Hoobler,
2002). Just as alpha males in the animal
RESOURCE CONTROL kingdom are the first to feast on prey,
so dominant humans are the first to appro­
In addition to the displays of potency priate the spoils of war. High-ranking
just described, nonverbal cues signaling personages also have the first right of
dominance and power do so because they refusal on acquiring socially valued goods
constitute displays of “resource holding and services. The principle of precedence is
potential” (Fridlund, 1991a). Animals sig­ reinforced through rituals that symboli­
nify privileged access to such valued cally signify one’s social position, such as
resources as food, protective shelter, or entering a space first, walking ahead of
fecund females. In humans, these may be others of lower rank, going to the head of
signified through (1) command of space, the line, leading a parade, or being given a
(2) precedence, (3) prerogative, and (4) pos­ first turn.
session of other valued commodities. We
discuss each of these briefly. Prerogative. People may mark their power
through having the “right” to behave in
Command of Space. Powerful people have certain ways. Consistent with EVT
access to more space, larger territories, and (Burgoon, 1978; Burgoon & Burgoon,
more private territories (Remland, 1981), 2001; Burgoon & Ebesu Hubbard, 2005)
which also afford their occupants or own­ and the gender politics hypothesis, domi­
ers greater insulation from intrusion by nant, powerful, and high-ranking person­
others and more space in which to display ages are free to deviate from norms and
other visible indications of their status and expectations, and may actually accrue
power. They may display more territorial more power by doing so, compared with
markers (i.e., tangible objects that signify a those in subordinate positions, who must
15-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 5:06 PM Page 290

290–––◆–––Functions

conform to social norms or risk adverse territorial advantage is so universally recog­


consequences. Dominant individuals have nized that international diplomacy and
the prerogative to initiate touch as well other serious negotiations are slated to take
as to determine the frequency, intensity, and place on neutral ground to prevent any one
intimacy of touch (Burgoon, 1991; Burgoon party from having a territorial advantage.
et al., 1989; Burgoon & Hale, 1988), and Among the principles that enable interac­
people high in verticality commonly interact tion control are (1) centrality, (2) elevation,
at closer interaction distances than those (3) initiation, (4) nonreciprocation, and
low in verticality (Hall et al., 2006). (5) task performance cues.

Possession of Valued Commodities. Centrality. Centrality of position in a social


Beyond the survival-related resources of setting arguably affords the central figure
food, shelter, and safety are the goods not only insulation from threats on the
and services that every society designates perimeter of the group but also maximal
as status symbols. Those who can afford capacity to monitor and influence the
luxuries and other status symbols, or who actions of those in immediate proximity.
can appropriate such valued intangibles as Within social interactions, leaders sit or
another’s time, should accrue both rank stand in more central positions in a group,
and influence. Thus, symbolic actions that such as the head of a table or wherever
mark one’s position explicitly or implicitly visual access to the most people is maxi­
in a status hierarchy may be socially ritu­ mized (Sommer, 1971). Centrality may also
alized extensions of an evolutionary-based be marked by gaze patterns. Submissive or
principle of resource control. Together low-status individuals look at superiors
with the other means just mentioned, more when they are listening (as a sign of
the possession of valued items works attention and respect) than when they are
to reflect people’s power, dominance, and speaking, whereas powerful people show
status. relatively more gaze at others when speak­
ing than when listening. This produces
what is called a visual dominance ratio
INTERACTION CONTROL (Exline et al., 1975). Central positioning
can also confer status on those whose
Like the prerogative to control offices, workspaces, parking spaces, and
resources, powerful people are able to con­ the like are centrally located. For example,
trol interactions with others (e.g., by sum­ offices that are located closer to the “center
moning others to their home turf, calling of power” are typically inhabited by higher-
for and adjourning meetings, and changing status organizational members, with the
the direction of a conversation). They can center of power defined by the office inhab­
dictate that interactions take place in their ited by the highest-ranking member of
territory, which may elicit deferential and all. Interestingly, however, this principle is
submissive behavior from visitors, espe­ overridden by the principle of privileged
cially if the territory is personalized with access. Where the central territory has been
plaques, framed degrees, elegant furnishings, “contaminated” (Lyman & Scott, 1967), as
and other symbolic markers of status (Edney, in deteriorating inner cities and graffiti-
1976). Those who interact on their “home ridden neighborhoods, powerful and domi­
court” gain greater confidence typically from nant individuals seek and secure more
the familiarity of their surroundings. This protected spaces. Thus, centrality can be
15-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 5:06 PM Page 291

Nonverbal Expressions of Dominance and Power in Human Relationships–––◆–––291

associated with the most and least powerful speaker through eye contact, gesturing, or
in a society. direct verbal address. They also may con­
trol the conversational floor by initiating
Elevation. Like physical mass and size that and switching topics, picking up conversa­
convey control of the horizontal sphere, tional turns more rapidly, interrupting
height may convey control of vertical space. others, and talking longer (Hall et al., 2005;
Elevated perches give predators an advan­ Wiemann, 1985), which results in them
tage over their prey; raised thrones, daises, talking more, influencing others more,
theater “box seats,” pedestals, penthouses, and being perceived as leaders (Leffler,
top-floor offices, and prison guard posts Gillespie, & Conaty, 1982).
give people greater surveillance and control
over others. The power bias toward Nonreciprocation. Another form of conver­
height “is deeply embedded in the visual sational control is nonreciprocation of
grammar of western civilization. For a others’ nonverbal behavior patterns.
speaker, it has functional advantages. The Whereas people on an equal plane may sig­
elevation . . . gives him or her a much larger nal their equality through matching and
field of vision. Elevation gathers and keeps mirroring of another’s kinesic, proxemic,
attention” (King, cited in Jaworski, 1993, and vocalic patterns, dominant individuals
p. 14). Behaviors that increase height differ­ may meet another’s smile with a blank
entials, such as standing over another expression or counter an expressive voice
person or “looking down at someone,” with a bored one. Dominant individuals
likewise function as dominant behaviors. It may also become the zeitgeber, the one who
follows, then, that tall people would be seen sets the interactional pace and to whom
as more powerful than short people (Frieze, others orient as they attempt to establish
Olson, & Good, 1990). Social rituals, such interactional synchrony (Burgoon, Stern, &
as bowing to the higher status individual in Dillman, 1995). All these patterns work to
Japan (Nixon & West, 1995), have evolved reflect the lack of power symmetry.
to accord elevation symbolically to individ­
uals of higher rank. Task Performance Cues. A final principle of
conversational control is a culmination of
Initiation. A corollary to the principle of many of the foregoing principles applied
precedence discussed earlier is initiation. to a task context. Task performance cues
Deciding where people will sit or stand, are nonverbal indicators of status and task-
changing interactional distances, initiat­ related ability from which group members
ing touch, starting or stopping conversa­ infer one’s potential to contribute effec­
tion, and setting interaction rhythms are all tively to a group’s task performance (Berger
interaction-based extensions of the “going et al., 1980; Ridgeway & Walker, 1995).
first” principle. For example, dominant High-status apparel, possession of artifacts
individuals may initiate handshakes, dictate that are culturally defined status symbols
conversational distancing, and set the (e.g., an expensive briefcase or pen), and
pattern for seated or standing interaction. actions indicative of centrality, privileged
Sheer proximity to, and surveillance of, access, precedence, and prerogative, con­
more individuals enables a powerful person note likely expertise, experience, and lead­
to dictate who talks to whom and when. By ership. These set up expectations for the
being accorded the privilege of speaking person to contribute favorably to a group’s
first, they then can nominate the next task and set into motion a self-fulfilling
15-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 5:06 PM Page 292

292–––◆–––Functions

prophecy whereby such individuals are should not expect simple and unidimen­
accorded more and longer speaking turns, sional behavioral profiles of status, domi­
are allowed to initiate or change topics, and nance-submission, or power-powerlessness.
thus exert more influence on the group, Moreover, because human actors are not
which then reinforces their prestige and bundles of instincts but rather active agents
position of power. In this manner, who choose among alternative strategies to
the “strong” may become stronger and the accomplish their goals, variability in forms
“weak,” weaker. of dominance and power expressions is to
be expected.
The nonexhaustive review we have
presented here illustrates the various ways
♦ Conclusions
in which nonverbal behaviors can be
enacted to accomplish these ends. These
The expression of dominance-submission include principles for displaying physical
and power-powerlessness is a form of com­ potency, signifying resource holding poten­
munication that has a universal vocabulary. tial, and accomplishing interaction control.
The theories, lines of research, and princi­ We hope this review promotes insights into
ples for nonverbal display of dominance what behavioral patterns carry dominance-
and power that we have reviewed here submission and power-powerlessness
point to a strong grounding in ethological connotations as well as what theoretical
and related behavioral ecology perspectives. explanations can be advanced for under­
Even those forms of expression that have standing their meanings and impact. It
clear social derivations and constitute sym­ provides a template for future research into
bolic rather than sign behavior often have the critical role played by nonverbal behav­
more primitive biological analogs. Thus, ior in negotiating this elemental facet of
the study of nonverbal expressions of social life.
dominance, power, and status offer insights
more generally into societal hierarchies and
social functioning.
♦ References
Notwithstanding the evidence of univer­
sality and possible innate foundations for
many power and dominance displays, Anderson, J. R. (1998). Cognitive psychology
we share with Hall et al. (2006) a convic­ and its implications. New York: Freeman.
tion that such displays are also responsive Andrew, R. J. (1972). The information poten­
to culture, context, relationships, social tially available in mammal displays. In
motives, and the psychological makeup R. A. Hinde (Ed.), Non-verbal communica­
of actors. This responsivity to a panoply of tion (pp. 179–204). Cambridge, UK:
antecedent factors helps explain what Cambridge University Press.
might otherwise seem to be a dismal record Apple, W., Streeter, L. A., & Krauss, R. M.
(1979). Effects of pitch and speech rate
of nonverbal expressions of status, power,
on personal attributions. Journal of
and dominance in predicting the vertical
Personality and Social Psychology, 37,
dimension of human relationships. The 715–727.
need for flexibility and adaptability for all Aries, E. J., Gold, C., & Weigel, R. H. (1983).
species argues for behavioral repertoires Dispositional and situational influences on
that are rich in alternative forms of expres­ dominance behavior in small groups.
sion and that can be adjusted or substituted Journal of Personality and Social Psycho­
as circumstances demand. We, therefore, logy, 44, 779–786.
15-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 5:06 PM Page 293

Nonverbal Expressions of Dominance and Power in Human Relationships–––◆–––293

Berger, C. R. (1994). Power, dominance, Burgoon, J. K., Buller, D. B., Hale, J. L., &
and social interaction. In M. L. Knapp & deTurck, M. (1984). Relational messages
G. R. Miller (Eds.), Handbook of inter­ associated with nonverbal behaviors.
personal communication (2nd ed., pp. 450– Human Communication Research, 10,
507). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. 351–378.
Berger, J., Conner, T. L., & Fisek, M. H. (1974). Burgoon, J. K., Buller, D. B., & Woodall, G. W.
Expectation states theory: A theoreti­ (1996). Nonverbal communication: The
cal research program. Cambridge, MA: unspoken dialogue. New York: McGraw-
Winthrop. Hill.
Berger, J., Ridgeway, C. L., Fisek, M. H., & Burgoon, J. K., & Burgoon, M. (2001).
Norman, R. Z. (1998). The legitimation Expectancy theories. In P. Robinson &
and delegitimation of power and prestige H. Giles (Eds.), Handbook of language and
orders. American Sociological Review, 63, social psychology (2nd ed., pp. 79–101).
379–405. Sussex, UK: Wiley.
Berger, J., Rosenholtz, S. J., & Zelditch, M., Jr. Burgoon, J. K., & Dillman, L. (1995). Gender,
(1980). States organizing processes. Annual immediacy and nonverbal communication.
Review of Sociology, 6, 479–508. In P. J. Kalbfleisch & M. J. Cody (Eds.),
Bernstein, I. S. (1980). Dominance: A theoreti­ Gender, power, and communication in
cal perspective for ethologists. In human relationships (pp. 63–81). Hillsdale,
D. R. Omark, F. F. Strayer, & D. G. NJ: Erlbaum.
Freedman (Eds.), Dominance relations Burgoon, J. K., & Dunbar, N. (2000). An interac­
(pp. 71–84). New York: Garland. tionist perspective on dominance-submission:
Birdwhistell, R. (1970). Kinesics and context: Interpersonal dominance as a dynamically,
Essays on body motion communication. situationally contingent social skill. Com­
Philadelphia: University of Pennsylvania munication Monographs, 67, 96–121.
Press. Burgoon, J. K., Dunbar, N. E., & Segrin, C.
Bruneau, T. J. (1973). Communicative silences: (2002). Nonverbal influence. In M. Pfau &
Forms and functions. Journal of Commun­ J. P. Dillard (Eds.), The persuasion hand­
ication, 23, 17–46. book: Theory and practice (pp. 445–473).
Bugental, D. B., & Shennum, W. (2002). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
Gender, power, and violence in the family. Burgoon, J. K., & Ebesu Hubbard, E. (2005).
Child Maltreatment, 7, 56–64. Expectancy violations theory and interac­
Burgoon, J. K. (1978). A communication model tion adaptation theory. In W. Gudykunst
of personal space violations: Explication (Ed.), Theorizing about intercultural com­
and an initial test. Human Communication munication (pp. 149–171). Thousand
Research, 4, 129–142. Oaks, CA: Sage.
Burgoon, J. K. (1991). Relational message inter­ Burgoon, J. K., & Hale, J. L. (1984). The fun­
pretations of touch, conversational dis­ damental topoi of relational communi­
tance, and posture. Journal of Nonverbal cation. Communication Monographs, 51,
Behavior, 15, 233–258. 193–214.
Burgoon, J. K. (1995). Cross-cultural and inter­ Burgoon, J. K., & Hale, J. L. (1988). Nonverbal
cultural applications of expectancy viola­ expectancy violations: Model elaboration
tions theory. In R. L. Wiseman (Ed.), and application to immediacy behaviors.
Intercultural communication theory (Inter­ Communication Monographs, 55, 58–79.
national and intercultural communication Burgoon, J. K., & Hoobler, G. (2002).
annual, Vol. 19, pp. 194–214). Thousand Nonverbal signals. In M. L. Knapp &
Oaks, CA: Sage. J. Daly (Ed.), Handbook of interpersonal
Burgoon, J. K., Birk, T., & Pfau, M. (1990). communication (pp. 240–299). Thousand
Nonverbal behaviors, persuasion, and Oaks, CA: Sage.
credibility. Human Communication Burgoon, J. K., Johnson, M. L., & Koch, P. T.
Research, 17, 140–169. (1998). The nature and measurement of
15-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 5:06 PM Page 294

294–––◆–––Functions

interpersonal dominance. Communication sourcebook of methods for measuring


Monographs, 65, 309–335. nonverbal cues (pp. 361–374). Mahwah,
Burgoon, J. K., & Le Poire, B. A. (1999). NJ: Erlbaum.
Nonverbal cues and interpersonal judg­ Edinger, J. A., & Patterson, M. L. (1983).
ments: Participant and observer perceptions Nonverbal involvement and social control.
of intimacy, dominance, composure, and Psychological Bulletin, 93, 30–56.
formality. Communication Monographs, Edney, J. J. (1976). Human territories: Comment
66, 105–124. on functional properties. Environment and
Burgoon, J. K., Newton, D. A., Walther, J. B., & Behavior, 8, 31–47.
Baesler, E. J. (1989). Nonverbal expectancy Ellsworth, P. C., Carlsmith, J. M., & Henson, A.
violations and conversational involvement. (1972). The stare as a stimulus to flight in
Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 13, 97–120. human subjects: A series of field experi­
Burgoon, J. K., Stern, L., & Dillman, L. (1995). ments. Journal of Personality and Social
Interpersonal adaptation: Interaction Psychology, 33, 117–122.
adaptation theory. New York: Cambridge Ellyson, S. L., & Dovidio, J. F. (1985). Power,
University Press. dominance, and nonverbal behavior: Basic
Cattell, R. B., Eber, H. W., & Tatsuoka, M. M. concepts and issues. In S. L. Ellyson &
(1970). Handbook for the sixteen per­ J. F. Dovidio (Eds.), Power, dominance,
sonality factor questionnaire (16PF). and nonverbal behavior (pp. 1–28). New
Champaign, IL: Institute for Personality York: Springer.
and Ability Testing. Emerson, R. M. (1962). Power-dependence
Christensen, A., & Heavey, C. L. (1990). relations. American Sociological Review,
Gender and social structure in the 27, 31–41.
demand/withdraw pattern of marital Exline, R. V., Ellyson, S. L., & Long, B. (1975).
conflict. Journal of Personality and Social Visual behavior as an aspect of power role
Psychology, 59, 73–81. relationships. In P. Pliner, L. Krames, &
Cloven, D. H., & Roloff, M. E. (1993). The T. Alloway (Eds.), Nonverbal communication
chilling effect of aggressive potential on of aggression (pp. 21–52). New York: Plenum.
the expression of complaints in intimate French, J. R. P., & Raven, B. (1959). The bases
relationships. Communication Monographs, of social power. In D. Cartwright (Ed.),
60, 199–219. Studies in social power (pp. 150–167). Ann
Darwin, C. (1965). The expression of the Arbor, MI: Institute for Social Research.
emotions in man and animals. Chicago: Frieze, I. H., Olson, J. E., & Good, D.C. (1990).
University of Chicago Press. (Original work Perceived and actual discrimination in
published 1872) the salaries of male and female managers.
Dindia, K., & Canary, D. J. (Eds.). (2006). Sex Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 20,
differences and similarities in communica­ 46–67.
tion (2nd ed.). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. Fridlund, A. J. (1991a). Evolution and facial
Dunbar, N. E. (2004). Dyadic power theory: action in reflex, social motive, and paralan­
Constructing a communication-based theory guage. Biological Psychology, 32, 3–100.
of relational power. Journal of Family Fridlund, A. J. (1991b). Sociality of solitary
Communication, 4, 235–248. smiling: Potentiation by an implicit audi­
Dunbar, N. E., & Burgoon, J. K. (2005a). ence. Journal of Personality and Social Psy­
Perceptions of power and interactional dom­ chology, 60, 229–240.
inance in interpersonal relationships. Journal Gallaher, P. (1992). Individual differences in
of Social and Personal Relationships, 22, nonverbal behavior: Dimensions of style.
231–257. Journal of Personality and Social Psy­
Dunbar, N. E., & Burgoon, J. K. (2005b). The chology, 63, 133–145.
measurement of nonverbal dominance. Gifford, R. (1994). A lens-mapping frame­
In V. Manusov (Ed.), Beyond words: A work for understanding the encoding and
15-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 5:06 PM Page 295

Nonverbal Expressions of Dominance and Power in Human Relationships–––◆–––295

decoding of interpersonal dispositions in Keltner, D. (1995). The signs of appeasement:


nonverbal behavior. Journal of Personality Evidence for the distinct displays of embar­
and Social Psychology, 66, 398–412. rassment, amusement, and shame. Journal
Hall, E. T. (1959). The silent language. Garden of Personality and Social Psychology, 68,
City, NY: Anchor/Doubleday. 441–454.
Hall, E. T. (1966). The hidden dimension (2nd Keltner, D., Gruenfeld, D. H., & Anderson, C.
ed.). Garden City, NY: Anchor/Doubleday. (2003). Power, approach, and inhibition.
Hall, J. A. (1998). How big are nonverbal sex Psychological Review, 110, 265–284.
differences? The case of smiling and non­ Keltner, D., Young, R. C., & Buswell, B. N.
verbal sensitivity. In D. Canary & K. Dindia (1997). Appeasement in human emotion,
(Eds.), Sex differences and similarities in social practice, and personality. Aggressive
communication (pp. 59–81). Mahwah, NJ: Behavior, 23, 359–374.
Lawrence Earlbaum Associates. Komter, A. (1989). Hidden power in marriage.
Hall, J. A., Coats, E., & Smith LeBeau, L. Gender & Society, 3, 187–216.
(2005). Nonverbal behavior and the LaFrance, M., Hecht, M. A., & Levy Paluck, E.
vertical dimension of social relations: A (2003). The contingent smile: A meta­
meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 131, analysis of sex differences in smiling.
898–924. Psychological Bulletin, 129, 305–334.
Hall, J. A., Rosip, J. C., Smith LeBeau, L., LaFrance, M., & Henley, N. M. (1993). On
Horgan, T. G., & Carter, J. D. (2006). “oppressing hypotheses” or differences in
Attributing the sources of accuracy nonverbal sensitivity revisited. In L. Radtke
in unequal-power dyadic communication: & H. Stam (Eds.), Power/gender: Social
Who is better and why? Journal of relations in theory and practice (pp.
Experimental Social Psychology, 42, 18–27. 287–311). London: Sage.
Henley, N. M. (1977). Body politics: Power, Leffler, A., Gillespie, D. L., & Conaty, J. C.
sex, and nonverbal communication. (1982). The effect of status differentiation
Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall. on nonverbal behavior. Social Psychology
Henley, N. M. (1995). Gender politics revis­ Quarterly, 45, 153–161.
ited: What do we know today? In P. J. Le Poire, B. A., & Burgoon, J. K. (1994). Two
Kalbfleisch & M. J. Cody (Eds.), Gender, contrasting explanations of involvement
power, and communication in human violations: Expectancy violations theory
relationships (pp. 27–61). Hillsdale, NJ: versus discrepancy arousal theory. Human
Erlbaum. Communication Research, 20, 560–591.
Hinde, R. A. (1974). Biological bases of human Leung, K. (1988). Some determinants of con­
social behavior. New York: McGraw-Hill. flict avoidance. Journal of Cross-Cultural
Hinde, R. A. (1978). Dominance and role—two Psychology, 19, 125–136.
concepts with dual meanings. Journal of Lips, H. M. (1991). Women, men and power.
Social and Biological Structures, 1, 27–38. Mountain View, CA: Mayfield.
Hinde, R.A. (1985). Expression and negotia­ Liska, J. (1992). Dominance-seeking language
tion. In G. Zivin (Ed.), The development strategies: Please eat the floor, dogbreath,
of expressive behavior (pp. 103–l16). or I’ll rip your lungs out, okay? In S. A.
Orlando, FL: Academic Press. Deetz (Ed.), Communication yearbook 15
Jaworski, A. (1993). The power of silence: Social (pp. 427–456). New Brunswick, NJ: Inter­
and pragmatic perspectives. Newbury Park, national Communication Association.
CA: Sage. Lyman, S. M., & Scott, M. B. (1967).
Keating, C. F. (1985). Human dominance Territoriality: A neglected sociological
signals: The primate in us. In S. L. Ellyson dimension. Social Problems, 15, 236–249.
& J. F. Dovidio (Eds.), Power, dominance, Maynard Smith, J. (1982). Evolution and the
and nonverbal behavior (pp. 89–108). New theory of games. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge
York: Springer. University Press.
15-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 5:06 PM Page 296

296–––◆–––Functions

Mehrabian, A. (1981). Silent messages: Implicit Ridgeway, C. L., Diekema, D., & Johnson, C.
communication of emotions and attitudes (1995). Legitimacy, compliance, and gender
(2nd ed.). Belmont, CA: Wadsworth. in peer groups. Social Psychology Quar­
Montepare, J. M., & Zebrowitz-McArthur, L. terly, 58, 298–311.
(1987). Perceptions of adults and children Ridgeway, C. L., & Walker, H. A. (1995).
with childlike voices in two cultures. Status structures. In K. S. Cook, G. A. Fine,
Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, & J. S. House (Eds.), Sociological perspec­
23, 331–349. tives on social psychology (pp. 281–310).
Moskowitz, D. S. (1988). Cross-situational Boston: Allyn & Bacon.
generality in the laboratory: Dominance Rogers-Millar, E. L., & Millar, F. E. (1979).
and friendliness. Journal of Personality and Domineeringness and dominance: A trans­
Social Psychology, 54, 829–839. actional view. Human Communication
Moskowitz, D. S. (1990). Convergence of Research, 5, 238–246.
self-reports and independent observers: Rollins, B. C., & Bahr, S. J. (1976). A theory of
Dominance and friendliness. Journal of power relationships in marriage. Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology, 58, Marriage and the Family, 38, 619–627.
1096–1106. Rusbult, C. E., & Arriaga, X. B. (1997).
Neff, K. D., & Harter, S. (2002). The role of Interdependence theory. In S. Duck (Ed.),
power and authenticity in relationship styles Handbook of personal relationships (2nd
emphasizing autonomy, connectedness, or ed., pp. 221–250). Chichester, UK: Wiley.
mutuality among couples. Journal of Social Schwartz, B., Tesser, A., & Powell, E. (1982).
and Personal Relationships, 19, 835–857. Dominance cues in nonverbal behavior.
Nixon, J. C., & West, J. F. (1995). Intercultural Social Psychology Quarterly, 45, 114–120.
preparation for managers going to Japan. Sebeok, T. (1972). Perspectives in zoosemiotics.
Mid-American Journal of Business, 10, The Hague, The Netherlands: Mouton.
57–64. Siegel, S. M., Friedlander, M. L., &
Olson, D. H., & Cromwell, R. E. (1975). Power Heatherington, L. (1992). Nonverbal rela­
in families. In R. E. Cromwell & D. H. tional control in family communica­
Olson (Eds.), Power in families (pp. 3–11). tion. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 16,
Beverly Hills, CA: Sage. 117–139.
Omark, D. R. (1980). The group: A factor Smith, W. J. (1974). Displays and messages
or an epiphenomenon in evolution. in intraspecific communication. In S. Weitz
In D. R. Omark, F. F. Strayer, & (Ed.), Nonverbal communication: Readings
D. G. Freedman (Eds.), Dominance rela­ with commentary (pp. 331–340). New
tions (pp. 21–64). New York: Garland. York: Oxford University Press.
Remland, M. (1981). Developing leadership Snodgrass, S. E., Hecht, M. A., & Ploutz-
skills in nonverbal communication: A situa­ Snyder, R. (1998). Interpersonal sensitivity:
tional perspective. Journal of Business Expressivity or perceptivity? Journal of
Communication, 18, 18–31. Personality and Social Psychology, 74,
Rhodes, G., & Zebrowitz, L. A. (Eds.). (2002). 238–249.
Facial attractiveness: Evolutionary, cognitive, Sommer, R. (1971). Spatial parameters in
and social perspectives. Westport, CT: Ablex. naturalistic research. In A. H. Esser (Ed.),
Ridgeway, C. L. (1987). Nonverbal behavior, Behavior and environment: The use of
dominance, and the basis of status in task space in animals (pp. 281–290). New York:
groups. American Sociological Review, 52, Plenum.
683–694. Thibaut, J. W., & Kelley, H. H. (1959). The
Ridgeway, C. L., & Berger, J. (1986). social psychology of groups. New York:
Expectations, legitimation, and dominance Wiley.
behavior in task groups. American Socio­ Thorpe, W. H. (1972). The comparison of
logical Review, 62, 218–35. vocal communication in animals and man.
15-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 5:06 PM Page 297

Nonverbal Expressions of Dominance and Power in Human Relationships–––◆–––297

In R. A. Hinde (Ed.), Non-verbal commu­ Street Jr., & J. N. Cappella (Eds.), Sequence
nication (pp. 27–48). Cambridge, UK: and pattern in communicative behaviour
Cambridge University Press. (pp. 85–102). London: Edward Arnold.
Wheeless, L. R., Barraclough, R., & Stewart, R. Zebrowitz, L. A., Fellous, J-M., Mignault, A., &
(1983). Compliance-gaining and power in Andreoletti, C. (2003). Trait impressions as
persuasion. Communication yearbook, 7 overgeneralized responses to adaptively sig­
(pp. 105–145). Newbury Park, CA: Sage. nificant facial qualities: Evidence from con­
Wiemann, J. M. (1985). Interpersonal control nectionist modeling. Personality and Social
and regulation in conversation. In R. L. Psychology Review, 7, 194–215.
15-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 5:06 PM Page 298
16-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 5:09 PM Page 299

16
THE FUNCTIONS
OF FACIAL EXPRESSIONS
What’s in a Face?

� Alan J. Fridlund
University of California, Santa Barbara

� James A. Russell
Boston College

H umans are quintessentially social beings, and our faces occupy a


central role in our social interactions. How and why they do so is
the focus of this chapter. This chapter is not intended to be comprehen­
sive, an inevitable choice given the explosion of research on facial expres­
sions that has occurred in the past 20 years. Instead, we focus on one
central contention: that the traditional focus on emotion as the way to
explain our facial expressions is erroneous. We argue instead that facial
cues are better seen as “social tools” that modify the trajectory of our
social interactions. We begin with a brief look at how faces have been
interpreted historically. Next, we examine more current views of the face
and facial expression. In doing so, we touch upon the face’s anatomy and
physiology, the properties of the face that can convey information, and
the ways that others are affected by our faces. We end by discussing

◆ 299
16-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 5:09 PM Page 300

300–––◆–––Functions

facial paralanguage and suggest that the out” the Cartesian passions, much like the
traditional division between facial paralan­ face of the clock reads out the time. The
guage and “facial expressions of emotion” is French neurologist Guillaume Duchenne,
illusory and counterproductive. who invented the biopsy and discovered
Duchenne muscular dystrophy, pioneered
the scientific study of facial movement,
♦ Older Views of based on the assumption that the ability to
Facial Expression express emotions was God-given. But the
chief expositor of this theological view of
the facial muscles was the celebrated
History shows the variety of possible Scottish surgeon, anatomist, and artist
interpretations of faces. “Face reading,” or Charles Bell (discoverer of Bell’s palsy), who
physiognomy, goes back to antiquity (see published his celebrated Essays on the
Fridlund, 1994, for a more detailed review of Anatomy of Expression in Painting in 1806.
this history). It existed in ancient Egypt and Bell’s theological view was the implicit tar­
Arabia and was a profession in China before get for Charles Darwin, whose Expression of
Confucius. Pythagoras probably originated the Emotions in Man and Animals (1872)
the scientific study of physiognomy, which aimed systematically to replace creationist
Hippocrates and Galen applied to their med­ accounts of human behavior by provid­
ical diagnoses. Aristotle constructed a system ing an onslaught of natural-history ones. As
of physiognomy which likened people’s per­ Fridlund (1992a) discussed, Darwin (1872)
sonalities to the animals whose faces they countered Bell (1806) by arguing that
resembled. For example, people with promi­ human facial expressions were not there for
nent upper lips were said to be stupid like expression at all: They were either vestigial
apes and donkeys, and those with “hawk” reflexes or accidents of nervous system
noses were magnanimous. Two thousand wiring. If faces were vestiges or accidents,
years later, the Renaissance figure, then the argument of divine design was
Giambattista Della Porta, joined Aristotle’s undermined. Thus, for Darwin, we don’t
physiognomy with Hippocrates’ system of bare our teeth when we’re angry in order to
temperament, arguing that a person’s face show anger; we just do it because evolution­
indicated whether he was sanguine, choleric, ary ancestors tended to bite when they were
phlegmatic, or melancholic. But physiog­ aggressive, and we simply inherited this out­
nomy reached its zenith with the 18th-cen­ dated habit.
tury Swiss pastor and poet, Johann Caspar In the 20th century, accounts of facial
Lavater, who believed he could recognize the expression bifurcated (Russell & Fernandez-
God in man by divining his subjects’ traits Dols, 1997). Almost no one continued
from the shapes and lines in their faces. Darwin’s (1872) nonadaptationist account.
Much of physiognomy concerned static Those who focused on humans continued to
features of the face, but changes in the face see facial expressions as readouts of
were also thought to convey changes in the the passions, modernized with an evolution­
person. Aristotle had read faces as indicat­ ary patina to say that facial expressions
ing the “passions,” which Descartes under­ evolved to express the passions. Those who
stood as turbulence in the bodily humors. studied nonhuman animals saw faces as sig­
The 17th-century French painter Charles nal movements, which, like other animal
LeBrun, who ruled the arts in France for signals, evolved to convey likely actions and
two generations, argued that the face “reads thereby alter the course of ongoing social
16-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 5:09 PM Page 301

The Functions of Facial Expressions–––◆–––301

interaction. Later in this chapter, we con­ this volume). Craniofacial anomalies or


trast these as the Facial Expression Program deformities like harelips, scarring, eye folds
(FEP) and the Behavioral Ecology View or skin discoloration (e.g., blotching or
(BEV). We will show that the latter view vitiligo), and overbites can lead to aversion
both restores continuity between our con­ reactions and, like all stigma, result in social
ceptions of human facial expressions and marginalization (Heatherton, Kleck, Hebl,
nonhuman displays and provides a more & Hull, 2000). Mental disorder was once
productive way to understand how we use thought to be deducible strictly from phys­
our faces in everyday social commerce. For iognomy, and history is replete with racist
now, however, we turn toward the face stereotypes about personality and criminal­
itself. ity based on facial features (see Gilman,
1985, 1996).
Although such inferences as these
♦ Components of (whether valid or not) play a role in guiding
Facial Expression the social interactions we have with their
owners, it is less clear whether some static
facial features were designed to be signals.
Our faces are exquisitely complex, in both To use Goffman’s (1959) terms, are they
appearance and movement, and people given (produced with the aim of communi­
use both to make inferences about us. In cation) or given off (where any communi­
describing appearance and movement we cation is incidental)? The best answer is
are, technically, speaking of the face’s static maybe, for some. Facial angularity, for
and dynamic features. instance, is an approximate indicator of
circulating testosterone (as is a plethora of
other signs like height, hairiness, and mus­
STATIC FEATURES OF THE FACE cularity), and females tend to prefer angular
faces when they are ovulating more than
Our faces influence our social inter­ when they are menstruating (Penton-Voak
actions even before they move, because of et al., 1999). Youthful (“neotenous”) fea­
how much we draw from the face’s static tures among females of reproductive age
features. Although some reviews omit their (e.g., wide eyes, full lips) are an approxi­
discussion, these features form the back­ mate indicator of greater fecundity
ground against which our facial expressions (Johnson & Franklin, 1993) and more
arise, and their inclusion is critical. These reproductive years ahead, and males judge
features are as follows. youthful females to be more beautiful
First, we use static facial features to (Cunningham, Roberts, Barbee, Druen, &
infer the owner’s sex, age, and race. Beyond Wu, 1994; Perrett, May, & Yoshikawa,
that, group identifications are often inferred 1994). In several accounts, angularity in
from cultural ornamentation that can males and big eyes in females were shaped
include grooming, face painting, tattooing, by sexual selection. On the other hand,
nose or lip piercing, or hair coloration. both features may simply be incidental off­
Second, we use the face’s static features to shoots of other traits; so for example,
make inferences about the owner’s person­ females may have preferred mating with
ality, much along the lines of ancient stronger males, with angularity “tagging
physiognomy (although we are likely to be along,” as a correlated variation, to use
incorrect in our judgments; see Gifford, Darwin’s (1872) term.
16-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 5:09 PM Page 302

302–––◆–––Functions

Because evolutionary accounts of behavior blood through the dense, superficial vascula­
amount to historical reconstructions, which ture of the face (and, often, the hands and
account applies is difficult to determine. the chest) exhausts heat. Prolonged flushing
The argument hinges on whether the sign in is accompanied by burning and sweating,
question is exaggerated beyond what would and this combination is not only painful but
be expected of an offshoot, often to the embarrassing (Gerlach, Wilhelm, Gruber, &
point that it becomes a physiological liabil­ Roth, 2001).
ity or handicap. For example, a male widow Vasodilation can also occur rapidly, as
bird drags along a tail of feathers four to in intense exercise. In blushing, the entire
five times the length of the female’s tail. His face (and usually, the ears and upper neck)
tail adds weight, is metabolically costly to reddens within 2 seconds of the event that
grow, and hinders flight. It does attract evoked it. Blushes last on average about 20
females, and that seems to be its only virtue seconds, although they may endure up to
(Andersson, 1982). Thus, it is reasonable to 15 min (Shields, Mallory, & Simon, 1990).
conclude that the male’s tail evolved to do People blush commonly in various kinds
so and that it is not simply an offshoot of of social interactions. The interactions are
the hormonal requirements of maleness. usually intense ones, such as when we (1)
No comparable clear-cut argument for lash out and yell (“reddening with anger”),
any human static facial feature has been (2) grieve, and (3) are made the subject
made. Perhaps the best candidate is the of undue social attention, whether being
eyebrows, which enhance the visibility of praised (“flushed with pride”), found guilty
our brow movements and—although some of a transgression, or caught off guard and
have conjectured that they are sweat catch­ made to go off role (e.g., embarrassment
ers (see Porter, 1993)—they have no other and other “self-presentational predica­
convincing raison d’etre. Although static ments”; see Schlenker, 1980). As one might
features of the face provide information, predict, blushing is more likely among
much more research has focused on the people with low self-esteem or those who
dynamic features of the face, specifically its are especially sensitive about others’ judg­
coloration and muscular movements. ments of them (Leary, Britt, Cutlip, &
Templeton, 1992).
Based on the fact that most blushing
DYNAMIC FEATURES OF THE FACE is social, Leary et al. (1992) suggested that
blushes are, at least in part, intrinsically
Facial Coloration Changes. Faces change communicative. In their account, our
color when the blood vessels constrict or blushes notify others that we care about
dilate, thereby regulating how much blood what they think of us, a strategy that may
flows through the facial muscles and skin. In appease those who would judge us harshly.
facial flushing, all or part of the face reddens, Social blushes can occur in solitude, as well,
often over a period of hours. Facial flushing when we imagine the appropriate social
may occur after changes in ambient temper­ circumstances, and then place ourselves
ature, eating spicy foods, or with fever. It can before an imaginary audience (see Fridlund,
also occur in a variety of disorders, such as Sabini, et al., 1990; Fridlund, Kenworthy,
rosacea, and after taking certain vasoactive & Jaffey, 1992; for the influence of imagi­
substances, like alcohol or niacin. Flushing is nary audiences of facial expressions).
also common during menopause. It appears Whether blushes evolved as a signal
to be thermoregulatory, because running hinges on whether the coloration changes are
16-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 5:09 PM Page 303

The Functions of Facial Expressions–––◆–––303

Corrugator supercilii
Levator labii superioris aleque nasii Galea aponeurotica
Frontalis
Levator palpebrae superioris Procerus
Tarsus superioris Lacrimalis
Tarsus inferioris
Corrugator supercilii
Orbicularis oculi, Orbicularis oculi,
pars palpebralis pars orbitalis
Orbicularis oculi,
Levator labii
pars palpebralis
superioris
Orbicularis oculi,
Orbicularis oculi,
pars ciliaris
pars orbitalis
Septum/Lateral wall

Zygomatic minor Nasalis, pars transversa


Levator anguli oris
Zygomatic major Nasalis, pars alaris

Orbicularis oris
Buccinator
Risorius Risorius
Depressor septi/ Modiolus
Platysma
Orbicularis oris
Buccinator
Modiolus
Depressor anguli oris
Depressor labii Labial Salivary Orbicularis oris
inferioris Glands Mentalis

Figure 16.1 The Superficial (Mimetic) Muscles of the Face Seen From the Inside Out
SOURCE: Reprinted from Pernkopf (1938), with permission from Elsevier.

beyond what would be expected as inciden­ participant off to one side, the participant’s
tal offshoots of other vascular reactions. cheek temperature was higher on that side.
Specifically, the circumstances that might
make us blush socially are often those in Muscular Changes. Our facial coloration
which “the heat is on,” and when we would changes proceed over periods of seconds
blush as a natural part of thermoregulation. or minutes, but it is the muscular changes
That others might read blush as a sign of in our face that allow it to accomplish split-
that reaction would be incidental. The jury is second signaling and social engagement. Our
still out on the issue, but some evidence of exquisitely complex facial movements are
an intrinsic display component was found caused by fewer than 30 muscles (Figure 16.1)
by Drummond and Mirco (2004), who precisely controlled by more nerves than
measured the cheek temperatures of partici­ any other muscles save those of the fingers.
pants who were asked either to sing (an The face collects together the organs
embarrassing task) or read aloud (a less- for tasting, smelling, eating (including suck­
embarrassing task). During the singing, ling), seeing, and speaking. All these involve
cheek temperatures were symmetrical if the orifices—the mouth, nose, and eyes—that
experimenter watched the participant sing require control and protection. These are
head-on. But if the experimenter watched the the primary function of our facial muscles.
16-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 5:09 PM Page 304

304–––◆–––Functions

At least some of the information a face reliability, and validity remains question­
provides to an observer stems from these able. Terms such as “smile” and “frown”
simple functions: shouting or whispering, are vague, and we have no guarantee that
looking toward or away or down, sniffing they cover the range of facial behavior
or tasting, orienting or listening—information exhaustively or parse nature at its joints.
given off. The facial muscles also display The Maximally Discriminative Facial
messages—information given. But the same Movement Coding System developed by
question arises for the facial muscles as it Izard (1979) uses theoretically defined cate­
did for static features and vascular changes: gories of facial features.
Were the muscles themselves shaped by nat­ A major advance in facial measurement
ural selection, at least in part, for these was an anatomically based system devised
display purposes? by the anatomist Hjortsjö (1970). His
Here, ironically, the case is harder to system was later revised by Ekman and
make. Any proof that muscles were shaped Friesen (1978) as their Facial Action
for signaling would require demonstrating Coding System (FACS). Specific facial
that they possess shapes or actions that do actions are given “action unit” codes; for
not accord with their physiological func­ example, brow knitting is an “Action Unit
tions, or perhaps even handicap their phys­ 4” (AU 4) and cheek-corner retraction is
iological functions (like the piloerection AU 12. A second major technique for facial
of feathers on the peacock’s tail). Muscles measurement is facial electromyography
used in producing speech, for example, (EMG), in which tiny electrodes are affixed
were likely just co-opted (technically, to the skin over major facial muscles. The
preadapted or exapted; Gould & Vrba, electrical signals produced by the contract­
1982) because mammalian suckling had ing muscles are then digitized and recorded.
already required fine control and flexibility. The facial EMG technique can render pre­
There is also little evidence on this issue. Of cise estimates of even weak muscle contrac­
course, humans use facial muscles to create tions, but its limitations include lack of
displays, and the role of natural selection in selectivity (frequently, there is crosstalk
this use is a separate question. from neighboring muscles), reactivity
(participants are usually quite aware of the
Measuring Facial Muscular Changes. What­ electrodes), and imperfect relationship to
ever their use, studying the facial muscles visual appearance (co-contracting antago­
requires measuring them precisely, and this nistic muscles can be electrically quite active
task is difficult. Most of the facial muscles but result in little change in facial appear­
originate in bone and insert into skin. ance) (see Fridlund & Cacioppo, 1986).
Because they distend skin, we are seldom
able to measure the contractions of our
facial muscles exactly; we measure instead SUMMARY
the displacement of facial features as an
indication of their contractions. Perhaps the As we have seen, people make a wide
most common method of doing so is direct range of inferences about others from both
observation. In its simplest form, raters the appearance and the movements of faces.
watch people and tally the numbers of By far, most research on facial expression
smiles, frowns, grimaces, pouts, and so on, has focused on the face’s muscular move­
in a given period. In this case, many raters ments, under the presupposition that they
are typically needed to achieve adequate “express emotion.” We now turn to the use
16-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 5:09 PM Page 305

The Functions of Facial Expressions–––◆–––305

of facial musculature in expression and the same specific basic emotions to those
evaluate this presupposition. universal facial patterns; and (3) those same
facial patterns are, indeed, manifestations of
those very same emotions in all humans.
♦ The Facial The principal method used in support of this
Expression Program premise was to show people of various cul­
tures photographs of posed facial expres­
sions (see Figure 16.2) and then ask each
Scientists since the end of the 18th century participant to match the photos with emo­
have adopted the ancient and by then tionally laden scenarios or emotion-words.
common-sense assumption that what faces Peoples from many parts of the world were
express are emotions. Indeed, by the 1980s, tested, including, most famously, relatively
the psychology of facial expression was isolated, non-Western and illiterate peoples
dominated by a network of assumptions, from Papua New Guinea.1
theories, and methods that Russell and Russell (1994) reviewed the 34 cross-
Fernandez-Dols (1997) termed the Facial cultural studies that used the standard
Expression Program (FEP). Table 16.1 sum­ method just described. The overall findings
marizes the FEP. from the matching-to-word design are
What we offer is but a prototype, and so depicted in Figure 16.3. The leftmost set of
no one theorist would support all 12 of the bars comes from Western literate societies
following claims. The FEP guided research (mostly college students). The second set of
on facial expression, indeed on emotion, for bars comes from non-Western societies,
several decades. Some of the basic assump­ although the participants were also largely
tions have been severely questioned by the college students. The rightmost set is com­
data; for evidence against the 12th claim, prised of the findings from the isolated, non-
for example, see Russell and Fehr (1987) Western, illiterate cultures. All groups
and Carroll and Russell (1996). More showed matching of still faces to words at
recently, questions have arisen about all of greater-than-chance rates (Elfenbein &
the assumptions (for reviews see Fridlund, Ambady, 2003; Russell, 1994), but as
1994; Russell, 1994; Russell & Fernandez- the participants’ level of westernization
Dols, 1997). Here we consider two central decreased, so did their matching rates. In the
issues: universality and the relationship of case of the studies involving non-Western,
facial expressions to emotions. isolated, illiterate participants, even the lower
rates are likely to be inflated by a concatena­
ARE FACIAL EXPRESSIONS tion of technical shortcomings within the
OF EMOTION UNIVERSAL? studies themselves—namely, that they used a
within-subjects design (known to inflate
There are many arguments regarding matching relative to a between-subjects
the universality of facial expression. design), using a forced-choice response for­
Demonstration of Strong Universality as mat (another known source of inflation),
presupposed in the FEP would require three with experimenters who were not blind to the
sets of findings: (1) the same patterns of stimuli or the hypotheses, and with question­
facial movement occur in all normal able experimental control (Sorenson, 1975).
humans, with the specific patterns now The studies show something, to be sure,
offered illustrated in Figure 16.2; (2) but rejecting the null hypothesis of ran­
observers in different societies attribute dom matching does not confirm Strong
16-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 5:09 PM Page 306

306–––◆–––Functions

Table 16.1 Stipulations of the Facial Expression Program

1. There is a small number (7 ± 2) of basic emotions.


2. Each basic emotion is genetically determined, universal, and discrete, and includes a
unique conscious experience, physiology, instrumental action, and—most important—a
characteristic facial expression.
3. The production (encoding) and recognition (decoding) of these distinct facial expressions
constitute a signaling system, similar across species, which is an evolutionary adaptation
to some of life’s major problems. The 7 (± 2) facial expressions are thus easily recognized
by all human beings regardless of their culture.
4. Any state lacking its own facial expression is not a basic emotion, and so discovering
which facial expressions signal the same emotion universally provides a list of candidate
basic emotions. The seven candidates found so far are happiness, surprise, fear, anger,
contempt, disgust, and sadness.
5. All emotions other than the basic ones are subcategories or mixtures (patterns, blends,
combinations) of the basic emotions.
6. Voluntary facial movements can simulate spontaneous expressions. Voluntary expressions
are deceptive in nature, and culturally conditioned according to “display rules” dictating
when an expression can be displayed freely or inhibited, exaggerated, or masked with a
different expression. The true emotion “leaks” through the camouflage and can be
detected through facial measurement.
7. Emotional state is revealed by facial measurement. Thus, verbal reports can be bypassed,
permitting access to the emotions of newborns and of others unable or unwilling to speak
truthfully.
8. The distinct subjective feelings associated with each emotion are due, at least in part, to
proprioceptive feedback from facial movements. This “facial feedback hypothesis” has
been offered as one means by which an individual “knows” which emotion he or she is
feeling.
9. Deliberately manipulating the face into the appropriate configuration creates the
neurological pattern of the corresponding emotion. For instance, wrinkling the nose
creates the neurological pattern of disgust. Facial manipulation can then be used in the
laboratory to reveal the physiological signature of each emotion.

10. The ability to recognize the emotion in a facial expression is innate rather than culturally
determined, and may be seen as early as just after birth.

11. The mental categories by means of which recognition occurs (in the self as facial
feedback or in others through facial signaling) are genetically rather than culturally
determined, and the emotion-words we use thus designate innate and universal
categories. Other languages may use other names, but the categories named are the
same.

12. The meaning (“signal value”) of a facial expression is fixed by nature and invariant across
changes in the context in which it occurs.

Universality stated above. Many interpreta- (1995) labeled Minimal Universality, as


tions are possible. For example, one parsi- stated above (1) Certain patterns of facial
monious alternative account, which Russell movement occur in all human beings;
16-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 5:09 PM Page 307

The Functions of Facial Expressions–––◆–––307

Figure 16.2 Four of the Facial Expressions of Emotion (“Happy,” “Sad,” “Angry,” and “Fear” Faces,
Respectively) That Are Claimed Within the Facial Expression Program to Be Innate,
Universally Produced and Understood, and Tied Intimately to Emotional State
SOURCE: Photographs by Alan Fridlund (model: Jason Fridlund)

(2) facial movements are coordinated with relationship between facial expressions
psychological states, which might include and emotion. At least in Western societies,
actions, preparations for actions, physical people interpret many stimuli—not just
states, emotional states, cognitive states, faces—in terms of emotion: angry storms,
and other psychological conditions (which weeping willows, joyous birdsong, and the
they are would require other evidence); melancholic baying of wolves. Conversely,
(3) most people everywhere can infer some­ humans interpret faces in many ways, not
thing of another’s psychological state from just as emotions. For example, people
facial movement, just as they can from interpret faces in terms of action tendencies
almost anything else that other person does; (e.g., Frijda & Tcherkassof, 1997) and
and (4) people in Western cultures have a social messages (e.g., Yik & Russell, 1999).
set of beliefs in which specific types of facial The assumption of a privileged relation is
actions are expressions of specific types of that faces and emotions are linked in some
emotions (e.g., the smile is a familiar sym­ more fundamental and specific way: Speci­
bol for happiness). These four premises pre­ fic “basic” emotions cause specific facial
dict that, when asked, people everywhere expressions. Specific facial expressions were
can form nonrandom associations between shaped by evolution to express precisely
faces and emotions. People in Western cul­ those specific, basic emotions. What are
tures will be particularly adept at associat­ these “basic emotions” that are held to
ing faces with emotions. At this time, we cause specific facial expressions and to be
argue that anything beyond Minimal expressed by them? How would one deter­
Universality has yet to be proved.2 mine them? Ortony and Turner (1990)
exposed a host of conceptual problems with
the supposition of “basic emotions,” but
IS THE RELATIONSHIP BETWEEN pragmatically, one could imagine sidestep­
FACIAL EXPRESSIONS AND ping the complexities. Only three simple
EMOTION PRIVILEGED? steps would be required: (1) develop inclu­
sion and exclusion criteria for stipulating
Untested in the famous cross-cultural whether people are having a particular emo­
studies was the assumption of a privileged tion. In other words, what specific signs and
16-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 5:09 PM Page 308

308–––◆–––Functions

100

Median Percentage Agreement


80

60

40

20

0
Western Literate Non-Western Literate Isolated Illiterate
(N = 20) (N = 11) (N = 3)
Culture Group

Expression
“Happy” “Surprise” “Anger”
“Sadness” “Fear” “Disgust”

Figure 16.3 Overall Face-Matching Rates in Cross-Cultural Studies


SOURCE: From Russell (1994).

symptoms must be present to say that the present at once. The absence of agreed upon
emotion is present, and what specific signs inclusion and exclusion criteria for “emotion”
and symptoms must be present to say that it means that the hypothesis of a privileged rela­
is absent? This criterion cannot include the tion of emotion to facial expression remains
face, else one would simply be proving a fuzzy. It will remain untestable so long as
tautology; (2) measure what these people do “emotion” is operationally undefinable.
with their faces; and (3) investigate whether Setting aside the fuzzy edges of the con­
there is a relationship between Steps 1 and 2. cept of emotion, one runs into difficulty
We argue that Step 3 has not been accom­ even with the prototypical cases created in
plished, because the prerequisite Step 1 has the laboratory. Empirical examination of
not been accomplished. For some, “emotion” specific emotions as highly coherent pack­
means subjective emotional experience; for ages has produced surprisingly weak
others, it means emotional behavior or results. Research has repeatedly uncovered
changes in peripheral physiology; for still weak associations among components
others, it means certain kinds of cognition. of an emotion (Lang, 1968; Lazarus,
For many theorists, “emotion” is a complex Speisman, & Mordkoff, 1963; Mandler,
of all these components, and not all need be Mandler, Kremen, & Sholiton, 1961).
16-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 5:09 PM Page 309

The Functions of Facial Expressions–––◆–––309

In the most sophisticated set of laboratory Such correlations are compatible with many
studies on this topic, Reisenzein (2000) theories about facial expression, including
examined the intercorrelations among those that assume no privileged relation.
four components of surprise: & cognitive Altogether, two major planks of the
appraisal, self-reported experience of sur­ FEP—Strong Universality and a privile­
prise, reaction time, and facial expression. ged link between facial expressions and
Correlations were modest, with the exception emotion—have been shown to be quite
of one relation that is close to a tautology: a controvertible. As a result, beginning in the
correlation between self-reported feeling of 1980s, theorists began to search for
surprise and self-reported appraisal of the accounts of our facial expressions that
stimulus as unexpected. better suited the evidence.
The assumption that emotions cause
facial expressions has fared poorly as well.
Little evidence is available on production of ♦ The Behavioral Ecology View
faces, and what exists largely goes against
the hypothesis that emotions are the imme­
diate cause. Fernandez-Dols and Ruiz- Modern evolutionary theory has inspired
Belda (1997) reviewed available evidence new accounts of facial and vocal expres­
on the question, “What is the actual behav­ sion (e.g., Owren & Bachorowski, 2001;
ior of a happy person, an angry person, and Owren & Rendall, 2001). Here we focus
so on?” (p. 256); their review led them to on Fridlund’s (1991a, 1992b, 1994, 1997,
conclude that the “conventional answer, 2003) BEV. BEV is a pragmatic, function­
known to artists, actors, and everyone else alist view of facial displays centered on
throughout the ages, is wrong” (p. 256). In how we use our faces in everyday life. BEV
support of this contention, Camras (1992) assumes facial displays are simply signals
examined one child for over a year and that influence others’ actions. No assump­
failed to find the predicted expressions in tion is made that the recipient’s response
the predicted emotional circumstances. is mediated by a conscious reading of
More notably, Fernandez-Dols and the sender’s display or a classification
Ruiz-Belda (1995) studied people in a clearly of that display into categories like
ecstatic state (having just won a gold medal “emotion.”
at the Olympic games) and noted that they Evolutionary theory provides strong
failed to smile except in specific social cir­ basic premises for the BEV of facial displays
cumstances. Reisenzein (in press) extended (see Floyd, this volume). In the first place,
his research program on surprise and found the generation of facial displays co-evolves
that the predicted “facial expression of sur­ with others’ vigilance for them. The display
prise” occurred rarely in surprised persons, must benefit the displayer, but the recipient
although, tellingly, those same persons is also indispensable: Signals that do not
reported (mistakenly) that their faces had end up influencing the behavior of recipi­
shown the predicted pattern. In Parkinson’s ents, either because they are not received
(2005) thoughtful and thorough review of or because they are moot, would not be
the literature most relevant to the relation of selected. Furthermore, recipients of displays
emotion to faces, he argued that overall, the should attend only to cues that provide
research shows only moderate correlations predictions about the future behavior of
between facial expressions and emotion. the displayer. Therefore, facial displays
16-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 5:09 PM Page 310

310–––◆–––Functions

and vigilance for them must co-evolve like the signals of other animals, should be
and could do so only if displays provided no less dependent on motive and context.
mutually beneficial signals of contingent Because displays exert their influence in the
future action. These signals would allow particular context of their issuance, they
interactants to proceed with reciprocation may be interpretable only within that
or counteraction that promoted mutual context. The evidence supports this: For
survival (Alcock, 1984; Hinde, 1985; example, a face interpreted as “afraid” in
Smith, 1977, 1986). This does not imply one context may be interpreted as “angry”
that recipients can use only formalized cues; in another (Carroll & Russell, 1996).
everyday behavior in a given context also
allows prediction of the others’ next moves
(Argyle, 1972; Kendon, 1981). EMOTIONS OR MOTIVES?
Second, for the human face to broadcast
its owner’s ongoing internal state, just From these premises, the BEV offers
as the face of a clock reads out the time, an alternative to the FEP. In Table 16.2,
would be foolhardy in its altruism, because we provide contrasting interpretations of
it ignores the potential conflict of interest commonly studied facial displays. Just as
between signaler and recipient. Signals do Yik and Russell (1999) showed that people
not evolve to provide information detri­ in three different societies interpret faces in
mental to the signaler. Signalers must not terms of social motive with about as much
be honest automatically, but only when it is agreement as they do in terms of emotion,
beneficial to do so. Automatic readouts of in each case in the table, an emotion is
emotion would thus be extinguished early contrasted with a social motive or inten­
in phylogeny in the service of deception, tion. The table must be interpreted with
economy, and privacy. two strong caveats, however. First, the
Third, individuals who survived conflict prototypical facial displays used in FEP
by signaling their intentions would include research are highly selected, posed, melo­
not only those who produced more schema­ dramatic faces of unknown ecological
tized facial behavior (technically, “ritualized” validity (indeed, their ecological validity is
if the evolution is genetic and “conven­ dubious; see Russell & Carroll, 1999). The
tionalized” if it is cultural) but also those more appropriate faces to examine are
with a heightened sensitivity to faces (see those that actually occur in the ecology.
Nelson & de Haan, 1997). This “ecology” Second, as we discussed above, for BEV—
of signaling and vigilance, countersignaling but not for FEP—the meaning of a facial
and countervigilance, is analogous to the display depends on its context. As such, the
balance of resources and consumers, and interpretations offered for the BEV in
predator and prey, which characterize all Table 16.2 are but samples, and they would
natural ecosystems. change along with the context in which the
Fourth, the costs and benefits of signal­ faces occurred.
ing, and of emitting a particular kind of sig­ A common criticism of the BEV is that it
nal, would vary with the momentary social supplants one ineffable, internal psychologi­
context and the animal’s intentions within cal construct, emotion, with another, social
it. This sociality of animal signals is well motive, or that “emotion” can be defined
documented in a number of animals and easily so as to subsume social motive. Doing
a variety of social settings (Fridlund, 1994; so would make “emotion” even slipperier
Marler & Evans, 1997). Human signals, than it already is. A better approach is to
16-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 5:09 PM Page 311

The Functions of Facial Expressions–––◆–––311

Table 16.2 Emotions and Behavioral Ecology Interpretations of Common Human Facial
Displays

Facial Expression Program


(“facial expressions Behavioral Ecology View (signification of
of emotion”) intent and verbal equivalent)

“Felt” (“Duchenne”) smile (readout Readiness to affiliate or play (“Good to see you,”
of happiness) “Let’s play [keep playing],” or “Let’s be friends”)

“False” smile (feigned happiness) Readiness to appease (“Whatever you say” or


“I give in”)

“Sad” face (readout of sadness) Recruitment of succor (“Take care of me,” “Hold
me” or “Look what you [he/she] did to me”)

“Anger” face (readout of anger) Readiness to attack (“Back off or I’ll attack”)

“Leaked” anger (inhibited anger) Conflict about attacking (“I want to attack and
I don’t want to attack” or “I’m so close to
attacking you”)

“Fear” face (readout of fear) Readiness to submit or escape (“Don’t hurt me!” or
“Take what you want!”)

“Contentment” face (readout of Readiness to continue current interaction


contentment) (“Everything [you’re doing] is just fine”)

“Contempt” face (readout of Declaration of superiority (“I can’t even bother with
contempt you” or “You’re not worth the trouble”)

“Poker” face (suppressed emotion) Declaration of neutrality (“I’m taking no position


[on what you’re doing/saying]”)

sharpen and narrow concepts through empir­ interaction trajectories, just like our words,
ical confrontation of competing hypotheses. our tone of voice, and our gestures. All these
Motives as used in BEV are not about feelings are like switches on a railway, diverting the
or, indeed, any necessarily conscious state. interactional train this way and that as it bar­
Rather, motive is the projected plan of action rels down the track. Emotion is the accom­
and its goal. BEV therefore offers a different panying plume of steam. We offer two
program of research, with different ques­ additional lines of research that add fuel to
tions, premises, and hypotheses than those this perspective.
comprising the FEP. Although Fridlund
(1994) has been critical of the concept of
emotion, within the BEV “emotion” is, at SOCIALITY OF FACES
best, secondary in understanding of faces. We
argue that the concept of emotion is not If facial displays are intrinsically social,
needed to understand how our facial expres­ as we contend, then it would seem that
sions evolved or operate in modern life. Our when alone we wouldn’t produce them.
expressions participate in and guide our As Ekman, Davidson, and Friesen (1990)
16-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 5:09 PM Page 312

312–––◆–––Functions

argued, “Facial expressions do occur when solitary faces has been documented in
people are alone . . . and contradict the the­ several experiments of affective imagery,
oretical proposals of those who view expres­ which showed that, controlling for self-
sions solely as social signals” (p. 351). We reported emotion, subjects showed more
know of no evidence that the kind of proto­ facial behavior in high- than in low-sociality
typical facial expressions of emotion seen in imagery. In other words, people display to
Figure 16.2 occur when the displayer is the people “in their heads” (Fridlund,
alone, and Reisenzein’s (in press) data that Sabini, et al., 1990; Fridlund, Kenworthy,
people believe they show faces when in fact & Jaffey, 1992).
they do not suggest that evidence ought to Fridlund (1991b) attempted to circum­
replace assumption here. But being social vent the imprecision of imagery manipula­
versus being alone does not conform to the tions by manipulating implicit audiences
all-or-none law. Even when an interactant directly. Subjects watched an amusing
has been removed physically from the room, videotape in one of four viewing con­
he or she may still be present psychologi­ ditions: (1) alone; (2) alone, but with the
cally. Actually, the physical presence of belief that a friend nearby was performing
others is one of the least important ways of an irrelevant task; (3) alone, but with
assessing the sociality of facial displays. the belief that a friend nearby was viewing
There are several ways in which people the same videotape in another room; and
can be structurally alone, with their facial (4) when a friend was physically present.
behavior implicitly social (see Fridlund, Viewers’ smiles were measured using facial
1994, for a complete exposition). First, EMG over the zygomatic major muscles
when we are alone we often treat ourselves responsible for smiling. Smiling among
as interactants. We reward or punish our­ solitary viewers who believed a friend was
selves, hit, touch, and stroke ourselves, and viewing nearby equaled that shown in the
likely move our faces in the course of these actual presence of the friend, but it was
acts. Most important, we speak to ourselves greater than that shown by subjects who
(without undermining the social and simply viewed alone. Reported happiness
communicative nature of language), and did not differ among the viewing condi­
the facial expressions involved may be a tions, and within conditions it correlated
part (see paralanguage section below). negligibly with smiling.
Second, we often act as if others are present Similar findings emerged from another
when they are not. In their absence, we study involving gradations in sociality.
curse them and utter words of love to them. Chovil (1991b) visually coded the types of
In many of these acts we deploy facial dis­ gestures made in different social contexts.
plays. These faces, too, are social and com­ Her subjects (all females) heard stories about
municative, although they are emitted when close calls in one of four conditions: (1) alone,
we are alone. from an audiotape recording; (2) alone, over
Third, we often imagine that others are the telephone; (3) from another subject across
present when they are not. In our imagina­ a partition; and (d) talking to another subject
tion we engage in interactions with others face to face. When these conditions were
who are not there; that is, we “simulate” ordered according to their “psychological
interaction with them. We imagine talking presence,” as determined by separate raters,
to them, arguing with them, making love Chovil’s subject’s exhibited facial displays—
with them, and we often deploy facial dis­ largely wincing and grimacing—that
plays. That “implicit audiences” mediate increased monotonically with sociality, a
16-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 5:09 PM Page 313

The Functions of Facial Expressions–––◆–––313

finding that mapped nearly identically onto smile when they had just made a spare or
that provided by Fridlund (1991b). strike, but were likely to do so when they
These kinds of audience effects have been turned around to meet the gaze of those in
found in many studies, although not all their bowling party. In analogous studies
found facial behavior to be independent of involving infants and their play toys, smil­
reported emotion; the relationship of the ing was almost entirely dependent on visual
two has become a focus of debate (Kappas, contact with the caregiver (Jones, Collins,
1997, and the special 1997 issue of Journal & Hong, 1991; Jones & Raag, 1989).
of Nonverbal Behavior). Some studies Fifth, we often treat nonhumans, and ani­
have found independence (Devereux & mate and inanimate objects, as interactants.
Ginsburg, 2001; Jakobs, Manstead, & That we often treat nonhuman animals as
Fischer, 1999a), whereas others have not humans is self-evident to any pet owner. We
(Hess, Banse, & Kappas, 1995; Jakobs, also treat inanimate objects as though they
Manstead, & Fischer, 1999b). In these social­ were social interactants. Devotees of indoor
ity studies, face-to-face interaction resulted in gardening talk, gesture, and make faces to
maximal facial behavior. This should not their houseplants. Children do the same to
always be the case, however, because in their stuffed animals, dolls, or toy soldiers
many contexts we do not issue communica­ (see exposition by Fridlund & Duchaine,
tions to others. Friends sharing a humorous 1996). Thus, even those “solitary” facial dis­
experience face to face should exhibit plays that are offered as decisive evidence
greater facial behavior than if they are sepa­ against BEV actually support it, and they
rated by a partition; friends asked to play reveal the extent to which we are social, even
poker should exhibit less. Likewise, facial when we are alone.
behavior that is socially censored (e.g., cry­
ing in front of strangers or casual friends)
may produce less facial behavior with FACIAL PARALANGUAGE
increasing sociality (see Jakobs, Manstead, AND GESTURE
& Fischer, 2001). Indeed, the social role we
play toward others has proven critical in For FEP theorists, facial expressions of
understanding the displays we make toward emotion are a thing apart from language.
them, whether they are present or not (see Indeed, Ekman and Friesen’s (1978) FACS
Wagner & Smith, 1991). specifies that certain Action Units cannot
Fourth, we often rehearse, forecast, or be scored if they occur during speech. This
prepare for interaction and deploy dis­ omission is not trivial, because most facial
plays appropriately, consciously or not, displays occur amid speech. Indeed, we
even though no interactant is immediately have seen various indications that facial dis­
present. Some displays function to deter plays are tied to speech more closely than
interaction, like scowls, or the tongue- is commonly understood. Fridlund and
showing display seen in both gorillas and Gilbert (1985) argued that the face’s chief
humans (Dolgin & Sabini, 1982; Smith, display role was to accompany and supple­
Chase, & Lieblich, 1974). On the other ment speech. Such a close association
hand, “readiness to interact” displays (cf., between facial movements and speech
Smith, 1977) are omnipresent. We deploy a might be expected, given that the entire
smile seconds before greeting a neighbor at orofacial apparatus (lips, cheeks, tongue,
the front door. Indeed, Kraut and Johnston pharynx) changes in the course of making
(1979) found that bowlers were unlikely to speech sounds.
16-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 5:09 PM Page 314

314–––◆–––Functions

TYPOLOGIES OF FACIAL hierarchical scheme that accounted for 99%


PARALANGUAGE of these movements. Overall, the frequen­
cies of facial movements in her typology did
An early typology of facial paralan­ not change with either the conversation
guage was the inductive scheme suggested topic or the sex composition of the dyad.
by Ekman and Friesen (1969). Their typol­ Chovil identified five primary types of
ogy distinguished four main types: (1) facial displays, along with several illustra­
emblems, which are arbitrary symbols that tive subtypes. (In parentheses, we give the
substitute for speech (a nod in place of percentage of the 1,184 instances so classi­
“yes”) and can be used, for example, to cir­ fied.): First are syntactic displays (27%),
cumvent speech taboos; (2) [self-] manipu­ which are connected with either speech into­
lators (adaptors), which include biting or nation or syntactical features and are redun­
wiping our lips, or running our tongues in dant with what we are saying. They consist
the crevices between our teeth and cheeks, largely of eyebrow raising and lowering,
clamping and then widening our eyelids, with tightening or widening of the eyes.
working our jaws, or bruxing our teeth; These syntactic displays included “empha­
(3) illustrators, which, as Darwin (1872, sizers,” which place stress on words (“He’s
p. 366) put it, “give vividness and energy to really bad”), and “question markers,”
our spoken words,” and serve to accent our which indicate that the utterance should be
speech or substitute for finger-pointing, as taken as a question. Second are speaker
when we tilt our heads to indicate a location illustrators (21%) that depict or represent
or object; and (4) regulators, which, within a what we are saying. Like Ekman and
stream of conversation, prevent either awk­ Friesen’s (1969) “illustrators,” they include
ward speech gaps or colliding utterances; “personal reactions,” which depict our sen­
these include facial and head movements to timents about what we are saying (e.g., our
help us hold or yield the floor, indicate that making a “yuck” face when we say, “Rap
we have gotten the point, show that we wish music is vile”), and “facial shrugs”
to squeeze in a word or two, or enable us to (“You’ve got me”). Third are speaker com­
regulate the other speakers’ content and pac­ ments (14%) that convey information that
ing. Precise, quantitative evidence would be is nonredundant with what we are saying.
required to determine how often each of These comments include personal reactions,
Ekman and Friesen’s categories occurs and which add our sentiments to our otherwise
whether their scheme is exhaustive. neutral utterances, thinking or remembering
These questions prompted Chovil displays, which signify that we are thinking
(1991a) to videotape 12 dyads of different or reminiscing, facial shrugs, which are sim­
sex compositions while they were having ilar to speaker illustrators but with non-
three short conversations. She then coded equivalent words, and interactive displays,
the participants’ facial movements from which recruit or enhance another’s atten­
the videotape records. Chovil developed a tion or reaction. Fourth are listener com­
typology of facial paralanguage inductively ments (14%), made when we have yielded
based on whether a facial movement the floor, that have connotations typically
(1) was issued by speakers or listeners and distinct from what the other is saying. The
(2) conveyed syntactic, semantic, or nonlin­ most common listener comments are back-
guistic information. Across the 12 dyads channel displays, personal reactions to the
and 3 conversations, Chovil and her judges speaker’s utterances, motor mimicry dis­
scored 1,184 facial movements. From this plays that echo the speaker’s sentiments,
data set, Chovil achieved a parsimonious and understanding displays, which signify
16-Manusov.qxd 7/6/2006 4:58 PM Page 315

The Functions of Facial Expressions–––◆–––315

our comprehension of the substance of the look away, stare, listen, taste, sniff, attend
speaker’s utterance, as if to say, “I know or ignore, speak or shout, and so on. Little
what you mean.” Fifth (25%) nonlinguistic we do fails to change the face, and
adaptors, occurring with no clear relation to observers make use of this information
ongoing speech, that include biting the lips “given off.” During conversation, the face
or wiping them with the tongue. supplements and complements the verbal
Chovil’s (1991a) findings are reminiscent message. The research we have reviewed in
of findings on facial expressions, in that her this chapter suggests that the boundary
facial paralanguage categories do not specify between facial paralanguage and facial
precise facial muscular patterns (except for expressions of emotion may be illusory: The
the brow movements of syntactic displays), face and the spoken word each modifies the
because she found no facial movements that other, and together, they form a package
were peculiar to any specific paralinguistic that modifies the trajectory of ongoing social
category. Rather, specific movements were interaction. We can indicate disbelief, for
categorizable only in the context of the example, by rolling our eyes, “scrunching
ongoing conversation. In addition, most up” one side of our face, or saying “Yeah,
facial movements (with the exception of the right” as innuendo, and each form of com­
“personal reaction displays”) had nothing munication has a similar effect. In everyday
obvious to do with emotion. social commerce, face, voice, and gesture are
Chovil’s (1991a) data demonstrate all co-conspirators. They are all paralan­
the overwhelming predominance of the guage, all signals that serve the same social
paralinguistic role of the face. Given this end, and none of them has a privileged rela­
predominance, we argued that facial tionship to emotion (see Bavelas & Chovil,
expressions of emotion have received too 2000; this volume, for their Integrated
much attention relative to paralinguistic Message Model from this understanding).
functions of faces. Furthermore, the distinc­ New neurological findings support this
tion between faces that accompany speech suggestion by pointing to common neuro­
and those that do not is superficial, and sev­ logical underpinnings and, possibly, evolu­
eral explanations can be offered for it. One tionary origins of vocal, facial, and gestural
possibility is that “facial expressions” are a expressions (see Rizzolatti & Arbib, 1998;
type of interjection into speech. Like the Rizzolatti & Craighero, 2004). These find­
verbal interjections we insert in our ongo­ ings begin with the discovery in monkeys
ing speech, the facial expressions of emo­ of the so-called mirror neurons in the
tion that are bracketed off by FEP theorists frontal lobe. These mirror neurons do
may actually be nonverbal interjections, not fire when the animal possessing them
once necessarily linked to vocalization but sees an object but rather when he sees a
now separable from it, making them appear certain action performed toward the
autonomous and wholly apart from the object—whether he is executing the action
stream of our facial paralanguage. or watching someone else do it. Mirror
neurons were found for actions that
included grasping between index finger
♦ Concluding Comment and thumb, placement on a plate, or
tearing.
Of course, the discovery of mirror
Faces differ in their static configura­ neurons is in one sense trivial. Because we
tions, and the face changes dynamically in imitate, we must have neurons for imita­
coloration and movement as we look, tion. But the location of these neurons is
16-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 5:09 PM Page 316

316–––◆–––Functions

surprising. The area containing them is ♦ References


the monkey homolog to our Broca’s area,
which is intimately involved with generat­
ing the orofacial motor sequences associ­ Alcock, J. (1984). Animal behavior: An evolu­
tionary approach (3rd ed.). Sunderland,
ated with speech. Just as surprising is the
MA: Sinauer.
finding from neuroimaging studies that,
Andersson, M. (1982). Female choice selects for
in humans, this so-called “speech area” is extreme tail length in a widowbird. Nature,
also active when we move our arms and 99, 818–820.
hands (or imagine moving them), and in Argyle, M. (1972). Non-verbal communication
deaf people it is Broca’s area that becomes in human social interaction. In R. A. Hinde
activated when the individual uses sign (Ed.), Nonverbal communication (pp.
instead of spoken language. The data suggest 243–269). New York: Cambridge University
that, during our evolution, we developed Press.
both “brachio-manual” and “oro-facial” Bavelas, J. B., & Chovil, N. (2000). Visible acts
communication systems in which we, by of meaning. An integrated message model
virtue of our “mirror systems,” could enact of language in face-to-face dialogue.
Journal of Language and Social Psycho­
what we saw others do (Rizzolatti & Arbib,
logy, 19, 163–194.
1998). In this general model, gestural, facial,
Bell, C. (1806). Essays on the anatomy of
and vocal displays may have all co-evolved, expression in painting. London: Longman.
from the same sets of circuits, and they may Camras, L. (1992). Expressive development
have constituted the background against and basic emotions. Cognition & Emotion,
which speech arose. These signs, readily mir­ 6, 269–283.
rorable, could have formed the “toolset” for Carroll, J. M., & Russell, J. A. (1996). Do facial
social signaling. expressions signal specific emotions?
Judging emotion from the face in context.
Journal of Personality and Social Psycho­
♦ Notes logy, 70, 205–218.
Chovil, N. (1991a). Discourse-oriented facial
displays in conversation. Research on
1. Having participants match posed Language and Social Interaction, 25,
faces to emotion terms or scenarios says noth­ 163–194.
ing about how people deal with the fleeting, Chovil, N. (1991b). Social determinants of facial
ever-changing faces of everyday life, or what displays. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior,
information they naturally draw from them. 15, 141–154.
Moreover—and this is an essential point—the Cunningham, M. R., Roberts, A. R., Barbee,
logic of this design means that the results A. P., Druen, P. B., & Wu, C. (1995). “Their
address the second premise. The first and third ideas of beauty are, on the whole, the same
remain untested. as ours”: Consistency and variability in the
2. We argue that the assumption that cross-cultural perception of female physical
universal faces and our reading of them must attractiveness. Journal of Personality and
be part of our biological heritage is unwarranted Social Psychology, 68, 261–279.
(and would remain so even, counterfactually, Darwin, C. R. (1872). Expression of the
had the studies supported Strong Universality). emotions in man and animals. London:
Natural selection can spawn both uniformity Albemarle.
and diversity. Both evolutionary and sociocul­ Devereux, P. G., & Ginsburg, G. P. (2001).
tural accounts are compatible with either cross- Sociality effects on the production of laugh­
cultural commonality or diversity (see Fridlund, ter. Journal of General Psychology [Special
1994). Issue Humor and Laughter], 128, 227–240.
16-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 5:09 PM Page 317

The Functions of Facial Expressions–––◆–––317

Dolgin, K. M., & Sabini, J. (1982). Experimental Fridlund, A. J. (1992b). The behavioral ecology
manipulation of a human non-verbal dis­ and sociality of human faces. In M. S. Clark
play: The tongue show affects an observer’s (Ed.), Review of personality and social psy­
willingness to interact. Animal Behaviour, chology (Vol. 13, pp. 90–121). Newbury
30, 935–936. Park, CA: Sage.
Drummond, P. D., & Mirco, N. (2004). Staring Fridlund, A. J. (1994). Human facial expression:
at one side of the face increases blood flow An evolutionary view. San Diego, CA:
on that side of the face. Psychophysiology, Academic Press.
41, 281–287. Fridlund, A. J. (1997). The new ethology
Ekman, P., Davidson, R. J., & Friesen, W. V. of human facial expressions. In J. A. Russell
(1990). The Duchenne smile: Emotional & J.-M. Fernandez-Dols (Eds.), The
expression and brain physiology II. Journal psychology of facial expression (pp. 103–
of Personality and Social Psychology, 58, 129). New York: Cambridge University
342–353. Press.
Ekman, P., & Friesen, W. V. (1969). The reper­ Fridlund, A. J. (2003). The behavioral ecology
toire of nonverbal behavior: Categories, ori­ view of smiling and other facial expres­
gins, usage, and coding. Semiotica, 1, 49–98. sions. In M. Abel (Ed.), An empirical reflec­
Ekman, P., & Friesen, W. V. (1978). Facial tion on the smile (pp. 45–82). New York:
action coding system: A technique for the Mellen Press.
measurement of facial movement. Palo Fridlund, A. J., & Cacioppo, J. T. (1986).
Alto, CA: Consulting Psychologists Press. Publication guidelines for human elec­
Elfenbein, H. A., & Ambady, N. (2003). When tromyographic research. Psychophysiology,
familiarity breeds accuracy: Cultural 23, 567–589.
exposure and facial emotion recognition. Fridlund, A. J., & Duchaine, B. (1996). “Facial
Journal of Personality and Social Psycho­ expressions of emotion” and the delusion
logy, 85, 276–290. of the hermetic self. In R. Harré & W. G.
Fernandez-Dols, J.-M., & Ruiz-Belda, M.-A. Parrott, The emotions (pp. 259–284).
(1995). Are smiles a sign of happiness? Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University
Gold medal winners at the Olympic Games. Press.
Journal of Personality and Social Psycho­ Fridlund, A. J., & Gilbert, A. N. (1985).
logy, 69, 1113–1119. Emotions and facial expressions (Letter to
Fernandez-Dols, J.-M., & Ruiz-Belda, M.-A. the Editor). Science, 230, 607–608.
(1997). Spontaneous facial behavior during Fridlund, A. J., Kenworthy, K. G., & Jaffey, A.
intense emotional episodes: Artistic truth K. (1992). Audience effects in affective
and optical truth. In J. A. Russell & J.-M. imagery: Replication and extension to
Fernandez-Dols (Eds.), The psychology of affective imagery. Journal of Nonverbal
facial expression (pp. 255–274). New Behavior, 16, 191–212.
York: Cambridge University Press. Fridlund, A. J., Sabini, J. P., Hedlund, L. E.,
Fridlund, A. J. (1991a). Evolution and facial Schaut, J. A., Shenker, J. I., & Knauer,
action in reflex, social motive, and paralan­ M. J. (1990). Social determinants of
guage. Biological Psychology, 32, 3–100. facial expressions during affective imagery:
Fridlund, A. J. (1991b). Sociality of solitary Displaying to the people in your head.
smiling: Potentiation by an implicit audi­ Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 14,
ence. Journal of Personality and Social Psy­ 113–137.
chology, 60, 229–240. Frijda, N. H., & Tcherkassof, A. (1997). Facial
Fridlund, A. J. (1992a). Darwin’s anti- expressions as modes of action readiness.
Darwinism and the Expression of the emo­ In J. A. Russell & J.-M. Fernández-Dols
tions in man and animals. In K. T. Strongman (Eds.), The psychology of facial expres­
(Ed.), International review of emotion (Vol. sion (pp. 78–102). New York: Cambridge
2, pp. 117–137). New York: Wiley. University Press.
16-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 5:09 PM Page 318

318–––◆–––Functions

Gerlach, A. L., Wilhelm, F. H., Gruber, K., & 10-month-old infants. Psychological Science,
Roth, W. T. (2001). Blushing and physio­ 2, 45–49.
logical arousability in social phobia. Journal Jones, S. S., & Raag, T. (1989). Smile produc­
of Abnormal Psychology, 110, 247–258. tion in older infants: The importance of a
Gilman, S. L. (1985). Difference and pathology: social recipient for the facial signal. Child
Stereotypes of sexuality, race and madness. Development, 60, 811–818.
Ithaca, NY: Cornell University Press. Kappas, A. (1997). The fascination with faces:
Gilman, S. L. (1996). Seeing the insane. Lincoln: Are they windows to our soul? Journal of
University of Nebraska Press. Nonverbal Behavior, 21, 157–161.
Goffman, E. (1959). The presentation of self in Kendon, A. (1981). Introduction: Current issues
everyday life. Garden City, NY: Doubleday. in the study of “nonverbal communi­
Gould, S. J., & Vrba, E. S. (1982). Exaptation: cation.” In A. Kendon (Ed.), Nonverbal
A missing term in the science of form. communication, interaction, and gesture
Paleobiology, 8, 14–15. (pp. 1–53). Paris: Mouton.
Heatherton, T. F., Kleck, R. E., Hebl, M. R., & Kraut, R. E., & Johnston, R. E. (1979). Social and
Hull, J. G. (Eds.). (2000). The social psy­ emotional messages of smiling: An ethologi­
chology of stigma. New York: Guilford. cal approach. Journal of Personality and
Hess, U., Banse, R., & Kappas, A. (1995). The Social Psychology, 37, 1539–1553.
intensity of facial expression is determined Lang, P. J. (1968). Fear reduction and fear
by underlying affective state and social behavior: Problems in treating a construct. In
situation. Journal of Personality and Social J. M. Shlien (Ed.), Research in Psycho­
Psychology, 69, 280–288. therapy, 1, 90–102.
Hinde, R. A. (1985). Expression and negotia­ Lazarus, R. S., Speisman, J. C., & Mordkoff, A.
tion. In G. Zivin (Ed.), The development of M. (1963). The relationship between
expressive behavior (pp. 103–116). Orlando, autonomic indicators of psychological
FL: Academic Press. stress: Heart rate and skin conductance.
Hjortsjö, C. B. (1970). Man’s face and mimic Psychosomatic Medicine, 25, 19–30.
language. Lund: Student-litteratur. Leary, M. R., Britt, T. W., Cutlip, W. D., &
Izard, C. E. (1979). The Maximally Discriminative Templeton, J. L. (1992). Social blushing.
Facial Movement Coding System (MAX). Psychological Bulletin, 112, 446–460.
Unpublished manuscript. (Available from Mandler, G., Mandler, J. M., Kremen, I.,
Instructional Resource Center, University of & Sholiton, R. D. (1961). The response
Delaware, Newark, DE) to threat: Relations among verbal and phys­
Jakobs, E., Manstead, A. S. R., & Fischer, A. H. iological indices. Psychological Mono­
(1999a). Social motives and emotional graphs, 75(9, Whole No. 513), 22.
feelings as determinants of facial displays: Marler, P., & Evans, C. (1997). Animal sounds
The case of smiling. Personality and Social and human faces: Do they have anything in
Psychology Bulletin, 25, 424–435. common? In J. A. Russell & J.-M. Fernandez-
Jakobs, E., Manstead, A. S. R., & Fischer, A. H. Dols (Eds.), The psychology of facial expres­
(1999b). Social motives, emotional feel­ sion (pp. 133–157). New York: Cambridge
ings, and smiling. Cognition & Emotion, University Press.
13, 321–345. Nelson, C. A., & de Haan, M. (1997). A neu­
Jakobs, E., Manstead, A. S. R., & Fischer, A. H. robehavioral approach to the recognition
(2001). Social context effects on facial activ­ of facial expressions in infancy. In J. A.
ity in a negative emotional setting. Emotion, Russell & J.-M. Fernandez-Dols (Eds.),
1, 51–69. The psychology of facial expression
Johnson, V. S., & Franklin, M. (1993). Is beauty (pp. 176–204). New York: Cambridge
in the eye of the beholder? Ethology and University Press.
Sociobiology, 14, 183–199. Ortony, A., & Turner, T. J. (1990). What’s so
Jones, S. S., Collins, K., & Hong, H.-W. (1991). basic about basic emotions? Psychological
An audience effect on smile production in Review, 97, 315–331.
16-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 5:09 PM Page 319

The Functions of Facial Expressions–––◆–––319

Owren, M. J., & Bacharowski, J.-A. (2001). The Russell, J. A, & Carroll, J. M. (1999). The
evolution of emotional experience: A “self­ phoenix of bipolarity: Reply to Watson and
ish-gene” account of smiling and laughter Tellegen (1999). Psychological Bulletin,
in early hominids and humans. In T. J. 125, 611–617.
Mayne & G. A. Bonanno (Eds.), Emotions: Russell, J. A., & Fehr, B. (1987). Relativity in
Current issues and future directions. the perception of emotion in facial expres­
Emotions and social behavior (pp. 152– sions. Journal of Experimental Psychology:
191). New York: Guilford Press. General, 116, 223–237.
Owren, M. J., & Rendall, D. (2001). Sound on Russell, J. A., & Fernandez-Dols (Eds.). (1997).
the rebound: Bringing form and function The psychology of facial expression
back to the forefront in understanding (pp. 295–320). New York: Cambridge
nonhuman primate vocal signaling. Evolu­ University Press.
tionary Anthropology, 10, 58–71. Schlenker, B. R. (1980). Impression manage­
Parkinson, B. (2005). Do facial movements ment: The self-concept, social identity, and
express emotions or communicate motives? interpersonal relations. Monterey, CA:
Personality and Social Psychology Review, Brooks/Cole.
9, 278–311. Shields, S. A., Mallory, M. E., & Simon, A.
Penton-Voak, I. S., Perrett, D. I., Castles, D. L., (1990). The experience and symptoms of
Kobayashi, T., Burt, D. M., Murray, L. K., blushing as a function of age and reported
et al. (1999). Menstrual cycle alters face frequency of blushing. Journal of Nonver­
preferences. Nature, 399, 741–742. bal Behavior. 14, 171–187.
Pernkopf, E. (1938). Atlas of topographical and Smith, W. J. (1977). The behavior of com­
applied human anatomy. Philadelphia: municating. Cambridge, MA: Harvard
Saunders. University Press.
Perrett, D. I., May, K. A., & Yoshikawa, S. Smith, W. J. (1986). An “informational” per­
(1994). Facial shape and judgments of spective on manipulation. In R. W. Mitchell
female attractiveness. Nature, 368, 239–242. & N. S. Thompson (Eds.), Perspectives on
Porter, A. M. W. (1993). Sweat and thermo­ human and nonhuman deceit (pp. 71–87).
regulation in hominids. Journal of Human Albany: State University of New York Press.
Evolution, 25, 417–423. Smith, W. J., Chase, J., & Lieblich, A. K. (1974).
Reisenzein, R. (2000). Exploring the strength Tongue showing: A facial display of
of association between the components of humans and other primate species.
emotion syndromes: The case of surprise. Semiotica, 11, 201–246.
Cognition & Emotion. 14, 1–38. Sorenson, E. R. (1975). Culture and the expres­
Reisenzein, R. (in press). Evidence for strong sion of emotion. In. T. R. Williams (Ed.),
dissociation between emotion and facial Psychological anthropology (pp. 364–372).
displays: The case of surprise. Journal of The Hague: Mouton.
Personality and Social Psychology. Wagner, H. L., & Smith, J. (1991). Facial
Rizzolatti, G., & Arbib, M. A. (1998). Language expression in the presence of friends and
within our grasp. Trends in Neurosciences, strangers. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior,
21, 188–194. 15, 201–214.
Rizzolatti, G., & Craighero, L. (2004). The Yik, M. S. M., & Russell, J. A. (1999). Inter­
mirror-neuron system. Annual Review of pretation of faces: A cross-cultural study of
Neuroscience, 27, 169–192. a prediction from Fridlund’s theory.
Russell, J. A. (1994). Is there universal recogni­ Cognition & Emotion, 13, 93–104.
tion of emotion from facial expression?
Psychological Bulletin, 115, 102–141.
Russell, J. A. (1995). Facial expressions of
emotion: What lies beyond minimal univer­
sality? Psychological Bulletin, 118, 379–391.
16-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 5:09 PM Page 320
17-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 5:09 PM Page 321

17
WHY AND HOW THE
SILENT SELF SPEAKS VOLUMES
Functional Approaches to Nonverbal
Impression Management

� Caroline F. Keating
Colgate University

M y mother said it wasn’t important to be confident, but it was


important to look confident. Martha Stewart’s mother must
have told Martha the same thing. On the day the celebrity CEO appeared
in court to receive a 10-month sentence for lying to federal prosecutors
about a stock sale, those who watched the defendant stride into the court­
room could only imagine what Martha was feeling and thinking. To man­
age, perhaps, the audience’s imaginings, Martha crafted an impression of
herself using nonverbal signals: Her posture was erect, her walk was ener­
getic, and she projected just enough anger to seem powerful but not out
of control. Her crinkly, narrow blue eyes and firm, well-defined chin con­
veyed determination and toughness. But the long blond bangs that tum­
bled across her forehead made her look girlishly innocent, and her voice
cracked with emotion. In essence, Martha presented herself as the entre­
preneurial Joan of Arc, marching toward an uncertain future, affected
but undaunted by her downturn in fortune.

◆ 321
17-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 5:09 PM Page 322

322–––◆–––Functions

Martha’s calibrated self-presentation their distinctive and shared features.


throughout the ordeal seems to have been The final section exposes some of impres­
effective: Her release from prison coincided sion management’s important unfinished
with a 6% spike in the value of the Martha business.
Stewart Company. On paper, she was esti­
mated to be much wealthier after leaving
prison than when she entered. Martha ♦ Impression Management and
returned to a million dollar a year position
Nonverbal Communication
in her company and set to star in two new
television shows (Kaufman, 2005). Yet it is
doubtful that in her heart, Martha Stewart Broadly defined, impression management
was as sanguine about her personal fate and is “the goal-directed activity of controlling
corporate future as she appeared to be on information in order to influence the
the day of her sentence. impressions formed by an audience”
What Martha Stewart’s courtroom (Schlenker, 2003, p. 492). Audience impres­
drama illustrates is that faces and bodies sions of people (e.g., self, family members,
do not speak solely from the heart. The job and political candidates), groups (e.g.,
corpus may be moved or silenced by pur­ organizations, nations), objects (e.g., prod­
pose, effort, and habit. Its signal is checked ucts, gifts), events (e.g., performances, disas­
and balanced by processes that warp spon­ ters), and ideas (e.g., policies, theories) may
taneous expression to influence audiences all be managed (Schlenker, 2003). This
strategically. In these ways, nonverbal chapter pinpoints the first category of man­
communication is tuned to motives and ageable entities—people—in which individ­
orchestrated to accomplish goals. Thus, uals serve as units of analysis.
displays of anger can be used to cloak signs The control of information about the self
of guilt (Ekman, 1992), smiles can serve as is sometimes referred to as impression man­
a disguise for psychological pain (Bonanno agement and sometimes as self-presentation
et al., 2002), and “blank” looks may be (Goffman, 1959; Schlenker & Pontari,
enacted to convey irony or sarcasm 2000). Many researchers use these con­
(Attardo, Eisterhold, Hay, & Poggi, 2003). structs interchangeably (e.g., Jones &
This brand of nonverbal impression man­ Pittman, 1982; Leary, 1995; Vohs,
agement reflects how individuals “spin” Baumeister, & Ciarocco, 2005), and they
nonlanguage cues in ways intended to are considered together here. I emphasize,
project images that produce desired social however, their application to social outcomes
outcomes. rather than to intrapersonal consequences
My main objective in this chapter is to such as self-concept or self-regulation. In
integrate functional approaches to nonver­ social contexts, the actor’s impression man­
bal communication with theoretical per­ agement goal is not simply to wield momen­
spectives on impression management and tary influence over others in the sense of
self-presentation. To accomplish this task, “selling” something or getting compliance.
the introductory section identifies essential Instead, the goal is to gain advantage by
aspects of the relationship between non­ projecting an image or identity to interac­
verbal communication and impression tants and to the observers of interactions
management. The second section applies (Burgoon, Buller, & Woodall, 1996;
specific types of functional approaches Patterson, 2001). Such nonverbal forms of
to impression management and identifies self-presentation have surprising power. For
17-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 5:09 PM Page 323

Why and How the Silent Self Speaks Volumes–––◆–––323

instance, whereas blatant, verbal self- behavior has with impression formation.
promotion can create a backlash against She noted that the impressions nonverbal
women who use it (Rudman, 1998), non­ cues generate are typically “off the record”
verbal tactics such as firm handshakes can in that they are resistant to precise identifi­
be an effective way for women to self- cation and assessment (p. 206). Nonverbal
promote in a sexist environment (Chaplin, cues are like journalists’ anonymous
Phillips, Brown, Clanton, & Stein, 2000). sources: Their messages are crucial but hard
Functional explanations for impression to name. A literary example comes from
management combine how individuals columnist Maureen Dowd (2004), who
control the presentation of self-relevant attributed the Bush Administration’s deci­
information with how others respond to sion to invade Iraq to “body language” that
it (Schlenker, 2003; Schlenker & Pontari, amplified verbal exchanges between the
2000). As DePaulo (1992) pointed out, President and his advisors. In this case,
nonverbal cues are more accessible to audi­ the President’s nonverbal actions made his
ences than to communicators, because words “speak louder” by rendering images
those signaling cannot see the messages of a man whose mind was already made up.
they send. Signalers are stuck relying largely Nonverbal impression management can
on audience reactions to gauge the impres­ also distract audiences from verbal mes­
sions they leave. In the end, impression sages. Adaval and Wyers (2004) found that
management may depend as much on the when memory for impressions and nonver­
audience as on the actor because it requires bal actions was sharp, recall for verbal mes­
the dynamic interplay between the two sages was dull. In this instance, actions
(Goffman, 1959; Schlenker, 2003). Thus, appeared to speak louder than words,
the way to successful self-presentation is perhaps even drowning them out.
both to “Know Thy Self” and “Know Thy Muscling nonverbal cues into sculpted
Audience.” impressions is not always easy. It is particu­
The importance of actor and audience larly difficult to manage impressions that
dynamics has encouraged some research­ require suppressing or neutralizing sponta­
ers to analyze nonverbal interaction using neous nonverbal responses (Ekman, 1992).
dyads or groups as units of analysis (e.g., Individuals pressured to appear invulnerable
Bernieri, Gillis, & Davis, 1996). Approaches can often control verbal self-reports better
like these fuse the moment-by-moment than they can monitor nonverbal behavior.
interdependence of one person’s action with Gay child care workers, for example, may
another’s reaction. However, most analyses express little anxiety verbally when faced
of impression management processes have with stereotype threats, whereas kinesic cues
been extracted from the vantage point of the belie their verbal expression of confidence
individual actor or encoder (Burgoon et al., by conveying tension (Bosson, Haymovitz,
1996). To fortify these types of approaches, & Pinel, 2004). Moreover, the successful
Patterson (2001; see also Patterson, this neutralization of nonverbal expression is no
volume) hinged actor-audience dynamics guarantee that a desirable image will be pro­
together by analyzing the actors’ dual tasks jected. For instance, overcontrolling nonver­
as encoders of messages and decoders of bal output can make others suspicious of
audience reactions. highly motivated liars (DePaulo, Lindsay,
In her overview of nonverbal behavior Malone, & Muhlenbruck, 2003; see also
and self-presentation, DePaulo (1992) artic­ Vrij, this volume). Lack of expressivity
ulated the special relationship nonverbal tends to convey disinterest, aloofness, and
17-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 5:09 PM Page 324

324–––◆–––Functions

coldness (Burgoon et al., 1996; Mehrabian, assigned to them (Albright, Forest, &
1972). When it comes to the nonverbal Reiseter, 2001). How can perceivers be
engine that powers self-presentation, there so readily beguiled by others’ nonverbal
seems to be no “neutral” gear, only “for­ performances? At times, perceivers may
ward” and “reverse.” Cues such as mutual “want” to be fooled and accept impressions
gaze while smiling, nodding, and forward at face value. For example, socially anxious
lean generally draw approach, whereas people are especially poor lie detectors
mouth and brow frowns, gaze avoidance (DePaulo & Tang, 1994) and seem to avoid
or aggressive stares, tense body postures, and gazing at emotion-laden faces presumably
interpersonal space violations usually propel because they fear negative social appraisals
avoidance (Burgoon et al., 1996; Mehrabian, (Mansell, Clark, Ehlers, & Chen, 1999).
1972). Women often resist probing the veracity of
Managing images to regulate approach ingratiating comments (DePaulo, Stone, &
and avoidance could be considered a cross- Lassiter, 1985). Thus, accepting as well as
species phenomenon. As any horse rancher projecting contrived images may have a lot
knows, stallions never limp in the presence to do with the fear of looking too closely
of their mares: Revealing signs of lameness into the proverbial social mirror (Tice &
telegraphs vulnerability to mates and com­ Baumeister, 2001).
petitors and would be a stallion’s reproduc­ There are, nevertheless, nonverbal checks
tive undoing. Primate-care workers in and balances on human gullibility in
laboratories and zoos are often amazed to response to others’ managed impressions.
discover animals that, overnight, seem to One unlikely defense against skewed non­
fall fatally ill and die. Sick or injured indi­ verbal presentations is the snap judgment.
viduals apparently protect themselves from Nonverbal behavior is often decoded early
becoming the target of conspecifics’ rejec­ and quickly during interactions (DePaulo,
tion and aggression by cloaking signs of 1992), and researchers have found, for
weakness up to the bitter end. The best- example, that deception is best detected
known human examples of this kind of rapidly (Vrij, Evans, Akehurst, & Mann,
impression management may come from 2004) before a person’s “acting” takes
the White House. The ability of American effect. Audiences also have defenses against
presidents to disguise physical and psycho­ bad acting: Self-presentations that do not
logical illness and project false images of seem genuine are not effective. So when
health and fortitude has kept many in office women read a script meant to project pow­
despite the odds (Gilbert, 1998). From a erful leadership, they were not nearly as
functional perspective, honesty about one’s effective as when they performed the identi­
physical condition may not always be the cal script in a mindful way, not straying from
best policy when the social goal is to main­ the content but adding personal, feminine
tain power. Indeed, the difficulty in detect­ touches to the tone (Kawakami, White, &
ing pain from gestures has stymied Langer, 2000). Overlearned, scripted behav­
veterinarians and physicians alike for years ior can lead to stilted performances that are
(e.g., Hyden & Peolsson, 2002; Leary, not compelling, especially if the image one
Tchividjian, & Kraxberger, 1994). attempts to project does not come naturally
Individuals are proficient at “spinning” (Schlenker & Pontari, 2000).
images, even in experimental settings where Nevertheless, some individuals have
the image “spun” has been arbitrarily better self-presentational skills than others.
17-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 5:09 PM Page 325

Why and How the Silent Self Speaks Volumes–––◆–––325

Researchers have attributed these differ­ to do so (Hassin & Trope, 2000). At the
ences to variations in temperament, same time, perceivers are unaware that mor­
appearance, or environment or to combi­ phological cues often guide their assessment
nations of traits, skills, and experiences and treatment of others (Keating, Randall,
(e.g., Anderson, John, Keltner & Kring, & Kendrick, 1999; Keating, Randall,
2001; DePaulo et al., 2003; Gangestad & Kendrick, & Gutshall, 2003; Todorov,
Snyder, 2000). Examples come from stud­ Mandisodza, Goren, & Hall, 2005).
ies of leadership. Undergraduates identi­ People direct remarkable amounts of
fied by their same-sex peers as socially time, effort, and resources toward modify­
dominant seem to be especially good at ing outward appearances. Across cultures
disguising the truth (Keating & Heltman, and millennia, face and body parts have
1994). Presenting an image of intellectual been dressed, painted, pierced, shaved,
competence is just as good as the real thing plucked, injected, molded, stretched, cut,
(i.e., possessing intelligence) when leaders and sewn to manage images of self and iden­
are judged for intelligence (Rubin, Bartels, tity (Guerrero & DeVito, 1999; Zebrowitz,
& Bommer, 2002). Furthermore, charis­ 1997). These (pre)occupations often reflect
matic nonverbal performances are conta­ cultural values. In parts of the West, fasci­
gious and enhance leadership effectiveness nation with individual physical appearance
and liking (Cherulnick, Donely, Wiewel, has led to the popularity of television series
& Miller, 2001). Yet deciphering exactly such as ABC’s Extreme Makeover, which
what these powerfully appealing nonver­ on a typical night musters an audience of
bal impression management skills are and over 8 million U.S. viewers, who watch as
teaching them has proven to be difficult plastic surgeons, cosmetic artists, and
(Riggio, Riggio, Salinas, & Cole, 2003). physical trainers transform appearances
(C. Whipple, personal communication,
July 26, 2005; see Manusov & Jaworski,
NONVERBAL IMPRESSION this volume).
MANAGEMENT: Clothing is part of this nonverbal arsenal
TECHNIQUES OF THE TRADE of impression management techniques.
Women and men select clothing styles
Static Physical Appearance Cues. The sizes, strategically to make their bodies appear
shapes, qualities, and spatial relationships to fit cultural ideals (e.g., Frith & Gleeson,
of static morphological cues influence how 2004; see also Guerrero & DeVito, 1999).
individuals are perceived (see Rhodes & These physical ideals, and their accompany­
Zebrowitz, 2002). Moreover, static signals ing fashions, change with the times in what
can alter the interpretation of dynamic non­ could arguably be an adaptive pattern
verbal cues: Thus, the same behavior may (Pettijohn & Jungeberg, 2004). Even the
get a different “read” when displayed by color of clothing can spark impressions in a
individuals with different facial structures, big way. For example, the aggressiveness of
body types, or genders (Keating, 2002). national athletic teams has been linked to
After all, morphological cues “arrive” first the color of their uniforms. Football and
and set expectations about traits and abili­ hockey teams wearing black uniforms
ties (Zebrowitz, 1997). So potent are these receive disproportionately high numbers of
cues that they are difficult for perceivers to penalties, in part because they are perceived
ignore even when given explicit instructions as more aggressive; cued by their own
17-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 5:09 PM Page 326

326–––◆–––Functions

clothing, team members may actually allowed spontaneous expression in the


behave that way (Frank & Gilovich, 1988). service of self-presentation (Burgoon et al.,
On or off the athletic field, height con­ 1996). Despite their dependence on motion,
veys status and power (Montepare, 1995). expressive behavioral cues are often
The taller of U.S. Presidential contenders indexed as if they were static entities. Only
win elections a disproportionate amount of a few researchers have probed for informa­
the time (Cialdini, 2001). Moreover, adult tion transmitted purely by motion (e.g.,
height is correlated with salary (Collins & Berry, 1990; Grammer, Honda, Juette, &
Zebrowitz, 1995). In China, job advertise­ Schmitt, 1999).
ments sometimes contain minimum height The meaning of movements can be
requirements, and individuals go to surpris­ altered by the context of speech. Some
ing lengths, literally, to achieve greater actions regulate or complement speech
height. Chinese physicians have nearly per­ (Duncan, 1972; see Bavelas & Chovil, this
fected surgical techniques that succeed in volume), cue speakers’ or learners’ memory
permanently increasing adult height by as for speech-related cognitions (e.g., Krauss,
much as 5 or 6 inches. In this procedure, 1998; Singer & Goldin-Meadows, 2005),
the legs are broken below the knee and or serve as signs and have specific meanings
extenders are attached to the leg bones. The in particular cultures (e.g., Birdwhistell,
patients spend months in hospital, during 1970). Paralinguistic or vocalic cues consist
which time an apparatus gradually stretches of dynamic information about the voice
the broken bones apart just enough for (e.g., variation in pitch, tone, timbre, loud­
them to regrow in between (Gifford, 2004). ness, and tempo) and nonlanguage charac­
Apparently, size does matter in China and teristics of speech that find their way into
elsewhere. conversations (e.g., pauses, silences, sighs,
Physical appearance cues can also be laughs, throat clearing) (Burgoon et al.,
manipulated through weight loss or gain 1996). Speakers routinely alter their voice
and molded through exercise routines qualities and speech characteristics to
designed to shift distributions of fat and “play” to different audiences (DePaulo,
muscle in the body. Drugs are sometimes 1992), and these alterations impact the
used to enhance these effects: In particular, impressions they leave behind. Judgments
the use of anabolic steroid develops muscle about speakers’ politeness, for example, are
more quickly than weight training alone affected by voice tone as well as language
(Wroblewska, 1997). These types of prac­ content (LaPlante & Ambady, 2003). They
tices can be carried to extremes: Machismo also happen quickly: Impressions that form
nervosa describes a psychological disorder in the earliest moments of interaction are
characterized by excessive weight training, largely based on nonverbal information
abnormal eating habits, and distorted body and possess the tenacity of other types of
image (Connan, 1998). For many, morph­ primacy effects (e.g., Kenny, Horner, Kashy,
ing body parts to transform physical images & Chu, 1992). Perceivers tend to attribute
is worth considerable effort and risk. durable character traits from initial exposures
to strangers’ nonverbal behavior (Gifford,
Dynamic Behavioral Cues. Nonverbal 1994; Manusov & Rodriguez, 1989).
messages activated by body movements First impressions are not only both
include facial expressions, gestures, pos­ durable and influential, but tiny samples of
tures, gaze, touch, and paralanguage. behavior are sometimes all it takes to create
Behavioral signals may be intensified, atten­ them. Perceivers may draw conclusions
uated, masked, neutralized, ritualized, or about others based on exceedingly “thin
17-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 5:09 PM Page 327

Why and How the Silent Self Speaks Volumes–––◆–––327

slices” of behavior (i.e., in a matter of 5 or functional approaches to understanding why


10 seconds; Ambady, Bernieri, & Richeson, and how these platforms operate. The basic
2000). Once impressions are fixed, per­ premises, promises, and shortcomings of
ceivers are generally motivated to go about each approach are described and illustrated.
confirming what they already believe (e.g., Implications of their common attributes are
Dougherty, Turban, & Callender, 1994). considered in the final section of this chapter.
For instance, job applicants’ initial hand­
shakes predict who is likely to be offered
the job by the end of the interview process ♦ Functional Approaches
(Ambady et al., 2000). Whether or not to Nonverbal Impression
these brief, initial impressions are linked to Management
real dispositional qualities (for more on this
issue, see Albright et al., 2001), interpreta­
tions of subsequent movements, gazes, The intellectual centerpiece of functional
vocalizations, and smiles are often guided approaches is that behavioral systems are
by them. goal directed or organized by purpose.
The manipulation of the relative timing Classic designs of what it means to engage
of body movements and expression can also in functional analyses are found in the writ­
leave lasting impressions. Depending on ings of Darwin (1872/1991) and Brunswik
the situation, mimicry, complementarity, or (1955). These theorists fashioned natural
synchrony of action can enhance impres­ selection and adaptation as ultimate expla­
sions. Actors and perceivers who mimic one nations for trait development and behavior.
another’s behavior generally report greater Many current functional approaches to
rapport and greater mutual liking (Hess, nonverbal impression management can be
Philippot, & Blairy, 1999; see also Lakin, traced to the thinking of these theorists
this volume; Tickle-Degnen, this volume). (e.g., Cunningham, Druen, & Barbee, 1997;
Interactants’ motives can, however, shift Keating, 2002). Other kinds of functional
this formula. For example, individuals seek­ analyses project more immediate or proxi­
ing to dominate others are more favorably mate intentions and outcomes as opposed to
impressed by actors who behave in a com­ distal, biological adaptations (e.g., Patterson,
plementary, appeasing manner than in a 2001; Saarni & Weber, 1999). These mod­
matching, assertive one (Tiedens & Fragale, els identify relatively short-term communi­
2003). Human courtship behavior—the cation goals and specify the processes by
successful kind, anyhow—usually has a which they are achieved.
synchrony to it, too (Grammer, Kruck, & The theoretical approaches featured in
Magnujsson, 1998). Characteristics of this section are meant to represent a variety
motion, such as the speed of offset and onset of functional perspectives on nonverbal
of behaviors, rather than the specific behav­ impression management; they do not com­
iors themselves, predict female interest in prise an exhaustive accounting of view­
males (Grammer et al., 1999). points. Contemporary, empirically based
theoretical perspectives are included that
Summary. Nonverbal communication (1) are concerned directly with strategic
provides powerful platforms for impression impression management or the outcomes of
management: Both static (morphological) self-presentation, (2) identify goal-directed
and dynamic (behavioral) nonverbal cues functions, (3) focus on nonverbal means of
can be managed to shape impressions. The crafting impressions, and (4) specify a role
next section of this chapter presents different for audiences. Approaches meeting these
17-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 5:09 PM Page 328

328–––◆–––Functions

Table 17.1 Selected Functional Approaches to Nonverbal Impression Management

Main Theme Function Nonverbal Cues Research Examples

Evolutionary Biological Neoteny, senescence, Cunningham, Barbee,


fitness expressive, grooming and Philhower (2002)
Status cues Keating (2002) and
Mueller and Mazur (1996)
Sexual dimorphism Perrett et al. (1998)
Fluctuating asymmetry Gangestad, Simpson,
DiGeronimo, and Biek
(1992)

Ecological Adaptive social Physiognomic Zebrowitz (1997) and


responding maturity/immaturity Montepare and Zebrowitz
(1998)
Gestures and body Bernieri, Gillis, and Davis
movements (1996) and Gifford (1994)

Emotional Regulation of Emotional displays Saarni and Weber (1999)


others’ emotions and Clark, Pataki, and
Carver (1996)
Self-regulation Saarni and Weber (1999)
norms
Ekman (1971)

Social-cognitive Social goal Encoding and decoding Patterson (1999, 2001)


attainment Courtship behaviors Grammer, Kruck, and
Magnujsson (1998)
Thin slices Ambady, Bernieri, and
Richeson (2000)
Deception DePaulo, Lindsay, Malone,
and Muhlenbruck (2003)

criteria were distinguishable by one of four and what messages are likely to be advan­
overlapping, theoretical emphases: evolu­ tageous (or disadvantageous) to impression
tionary, ecological, emotional, or social- formation (for a more general discussion of
cognitive. Table 17.1 outlines distinguishing evolutionary perspectives on nonverbal
features and cites research related to each. communication, see Floyd, this volume).
The intellectual inspiration for evolutionary
approaches can be traced to Darwin
EVOLUTIONARY PERSPECTIVES (1872/1991), who applied notions of ran­
ON IMPRESSION MANAGEMENT dom variation and selective retention of
genetically based traits to communication
Evolutionary perspectives are perhaps in animals and humans. Darwinian logic
best at projecting why individuals manage requires that genetic substrates (however
nonverbal cues to produce particular images direct or indirect) underlie appearances and
17-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 5:09 PM Page 329

Why and How the Silent Self Speaks Volumes–––◆–––329

communication abilities. Gene-based with the idea of a multiple-message advan­


appearances and behaviors that confer tage, Keating and Doyle (2002) found that
reproductive benefits to individuals and the physiognomies of desirable dates and
to their kin are selected for and retained in mates contained mixed signals of domi­
offspring. nance and warmth rather than strong forms
Courtship displays in birds, piloerection in of either message.
felines, play bows in canines, appeasement The multiple fitness model specifies five
grimaces in primates, and neoteny in humans types of features that skew perceptions of
are all examples of cues rooted in phyletic faces and bodies. These include (1) the
histories. They illustrate that the essential appearance of neonate features such as large
function or “why” of signaling systems is the eyes, small nose, and smooth skin, which
enhancement of biological fitness. Behavioral signal dependence, cuteness, and vulnerabil­
and appearance cues that signal sex and sex­ ity; (2) sex-linked sexual maturity features,
ual interest, developmental maturity, status, which enhance sexual attractiveness; (3)
health, and reproductive potential are imbued expressive features such as large smiles, high
with information essential to fitness. For eyebrows, and relaxed vocal tones, which
example, symmetrical faces and bodies look invite social interchange; (4) grooming fea­
attractive presumably because symmetry tures including hairstyle, cosmetics, cloth­
reflects pathogen resistance, health, good ing; and (5) senescence features such as male
genes, and, ultimately, reproductive potential pattern baldness, gray hair, and slow gait,
(e.g., Gangestad, Thornhill, & Yeo, 1994), which signal nurturance and appeasement
and there are image management techniques (Cunningham et al., 1997, 2002). These
for those who do not measure up to the ideal. features may be augmented, altered, or
For instance, some believe that the disguise contrived to achieve desirable impressions.
of body asymmetry may be accomplished Blond hair, for example, is a neonate fea­
through the careful design and placement of ture that can be mimicked by adults. The
tattoos and piercings (Singh & Bronstad, offspring of European parents often sig­
1997). naled their ontogenetic status by remaining
Cunningham’s multiple fitness model blond until after puberty (Cunningham
(Cunningham, Barbee, & Philhower, 2002; et al., 1997, 2002; Guthrie, 1976). Adults
Cunningham et al., 1997) provides a rela­ who dye their hair blond can mimic, to
tively comprehensive approach to impression some extent, the youthful impressions
management from an evolutionary point of blond locks convey. So when Cunningham
view. For Cunningham and his colleagues, and his colleagues portrayed 21 women as
“each face and body provides an opportunity blonds and as brunettes, trait attributions
for natural and sexual selection to increase or differed. As blonds, the women were
decrease the success of the individual convey­ perceived as more attractive, feminine, emo­
ing that appearance” (Cunningham et al., tional, and pleasure seeking. As brunettes,
2002, p. 109). Success is achievable physically they were rated as more intelligent
(e.g., by developing traits that enhance health (Cunningham et al., 1997). Though consis­
or strength) and socially (e.g., by developing tent with evolutionary thinking, it is impos­
traits that enhance dominance or devotion). sible to tell from such data whether
According to the model, individuals display ultimate (evolutionary) or proximate
multiple fitness messages that function in (learned through association) mechanisms
complementary ways to influence hetero­ direct this perceptual bias. Much evolution­
sexual attractiveness and bonding. Consistent ary theorizing is vulnerable to this kind of
17-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 5:09 PM Page 330

330–––◆–––Functions

uncertainty, because deriving testable validity and cue utilization. It could be said
predictions and reasonable measures of fit­ that Brunswikian approaches are more gen­
ness is difficult. In other words, the ques­ erous than evolutionary ones in the degree to
tion to be posed in this instance is not which they anticipate and tolerate error in
whether blonds have more fun, but whether the (social) perceptual system.
they have more offspring and kin who The application of Brunswik’s paradigm
themselves are reproductively successful. to nonverbal self-presentation reveals that
the cues encoded by a communicator may
or may not match those used (decoded) by
ECOLOGICAL PERSPECTIVES ON observers to derive impressions of the com­
IMPRESSION MANAGEMENT municator. Gifford (1994) noted this slip­
page when he adapted a Brunswikian lens
Although displays ultimately enhance model to nonverbal impression formation.
reproductive potential far downstream, He found that communicators who scored
proximate goals, say for social approval or high on the measurements of warmth and
financial reward, may be served in their agreeableness nodded their heads often dur­
more immediate wake. From an ecological ing interactions. When observers judged the
perspective, the benefits of impression man­ communicators’ traits, however, they relied
agement involve adaptive social functioning on more than just the valid cue of nods;
in which the fit between signal and social observers used a host of postural cues
context or “ecology” is imperative. The the­ unassociated with the dispositions of com­
oretical perspectives categorized as following municators. Similarly, object manipulation
an ecological tradition reflect the thinking predicted communicators’ scores on mea­
of early functionalists like Brunswik (1955), surements of dominance and submissive­
who applied his ideas to perception and ness, yet observers neglected to use this cue
social perception (e.g., to face cues, nations). in their assessments of them. One explana­
Brunswik argued that perceptual systems tion for the discrepancy may relate to per­
were adapted to their environments by ceiver’s motivation. When the dispositions
expectancies developed through experience judged are highly relevant to perceivers,
in a particular environment or ecology. they tend to increase their use of valid cues
Perception operated probabilistically, mean­ (Gangestad, Simpson, DiGeronimo, & Biek,
ing that cue perception was biased toward 1992).
interpretations that had worked in the past Ecological approaches relevant to
(e.g., Segall, Campbell, & Herskovitz, impression management include research
1966). Because probabilistic judgments were on social perception derived from static
mostly correct in specific ecologies, these physical appearances as well as behavior.
perceptual habits or attunements were The Brunswikian idea of affordances has
thought to be adaptive (McArthur & Baron, been championed by Zebrowitz and her
1983). Like a Vegas gambler at the blackjack colleagues (e.g., Montepare & Zebrowitz,
table, Brunswik (1955) reckoned that per­ 1998; Zebrowitz, 1997). For example,
ceivers need only beat the odds some per­ facial structures may convey affordances
centage of the time to come away with a defined as opportunities for certain types
winning perceptual formula. This implies of interactions. Sensitivity toward these
that a certain degree of error is acceptable signals is adaptive but can result in over-
in social perception. That is, there may be interpretation, a kind of perceptual error
some “slippage” in the matchup between cue we may be biologically prepared to
17-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 5:09 PM Page 331

Why and How the Silent Self Speaks Volumes–––◆–––331

make (Zebrowitz, 1997). Affordances largely through nonverbal means, is a key


proffered by “age-related physical quali­ component of emotional intelligence (see
ties” (Montepare & Zebrowitz, 1998, Bar-On & Parker, 2000). Expressions may
p. 95) such as infantile facial cues, for forecast intentions and the nature of subse­
example, overgeneralize when displayed by quent interactions (Fridlund, 1994; Fridlund
adults and influence cognitions about social & Russell, this volume). They can be conta­
traits. Hence, baby-faced adults are per­ gious and used to “infect” others’ internal
ceived as having characteristics associated states and cognitions (Hatfield, Cacioppo,
with babies: dependent, weak, approach­ & Rapson, 1994). Knowing which emo­
able, and warm (Montepare & Zebrowitz, tions to project, to what degree, when, and
1998; Zebrowitz, 1997). By creating initial with whom constitutes a form of impression
impressions, facial structure thus sets the management aimed at strategic emotional
stage for impression management strategies self-presentation. Such self-presentation
(e.g., Keating, 2002; Zebrowitz, 1997). may also enhance emotion regulation and
coping (Holodynski, 2004; Saarni &
Weber, 1999). The “bad management” of
EMOTIONAL PERSPECTIVES ON emotional displays can have dramatic—
IMPRESSION MANAGEMENT actually historic—consequences: Democratic
candidate Howard Dean arguably wrecked
Humans seem hot-wired to read and his 2004 bid for the U.S. presidency in under
write emotion-related nonverbal communi­ 5 seconds by letting loose over the airwaves
cation. Emotions can project from brain to a volatile, hot-blooded, untamed scream
body quickly with little input from higher, that was captured and immortalized in the
cortical areas of the brain (LeDoux, 1996). national media as the “Dean Scream”
Emotion decoding can be fast, too. Brain (Stolberg, 2004).
responses to facial displays of fear, anger, Saarni (1989; Saarni & Weber, 1999)
happiness, sadness, disgust, and surprise emphasizes the self-presentational functions
register distinct patterns of processing activ­ of managed emotional displays and identi­
ity that appear rapid and automatic (Batty fies the proximate, social goals they serve. In
& Taylor, 2003). But the fact that the pres­ her view, emotional displays are calibrated
ence of others modifies the quality and to cast desired self-images and to cope with
intensity of an individual’s expression (e.g., stressful situations. Saarni distinguishes
Ekman, 1971; Fridlund, 1994) indicates emotional display management, or what
that humans come biologically prepared others derive about a person’s emotional
not simply to express and detect affective experience, from emotion regulation, or a
states but also to control their display person’s internal experience of emotion.
(LeDoux, 1996). These “audience effects” Display management involves the strategic
are complex: Audiences and co-actors or habitual dissembling of expressive behav­
sometimes attenuate expression, sometimes ior (Saarni & Weber, 1999). The expression
amplify it, and at other times alter the of internal emotion states may be attenu­
type of emotion conveyed (e.g., Manstead, ated, exaggerated, replaced by feigned emo­
Fischer, & Jakobs, 1999). tion, or suppressed and neutralized.
When drafted into the service of impression Whereas Ekman (1971) attributed
management, emotional cues become power­ behavioral modifications like these to cul­
ful allies. In fact, both the regulation of one’s tural “display rules” or norms for public
own and others’ emotions, accomplished expressivity, Saarni (1989), a functionalist
17-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 5:09 PM Page 332

332–––◆–––Functions

at heart, attaches the dissembling of (Tedeschi & Norman, 1985). Baumeister


emotional expressivity to an individuals’ (1993) projected three main goals: (1)
social goal striving. According to Saarni, social belonging and acceptance, (2) the
children learn to dissemble to avoid nega­ construction of self-identity with desirable
tive consequences, to craft displays so as qualities, and (3) the establishment and
to avoid hurting another’s feelings, and to protection of self-esteem or positive self-
adopt social conventions for dissembling. images. But attempts to delineate particular
Whether these or other lessons are part of a social goals and images, and to prescribe
cross-cultural curriculum of emotional self- the social conditions most favorable to
presentation remains to be studied (e.g., impression management, led to specifica­
Manstead et al., 1999). But it is clear that tions that clashed (see DePaulo, 1992;
control over emotional expression is a skill Schlenker, 2003; Schlenker & Pontari,
that individuals are prepared to learn and 2000). For instance, emotional displays
use strategically to accomplish social goals and scripted behaviors were considered
throughout their lives (Keating & Heltman, “management-free” episodes by some and
1994; see Feldman & Tyler, this volume). viewed as exemplary impression manage­
Emotional displays aimed at attaining ment opportunities by others. There were
social goals sometimes score as congruent, controversies as to whether managed self-
and at other times as incongruent, with presentations ever exposed the authentic
privately held feelings. In one experiment self and whether awareness was needed to
(Pataki & Clark, 2004), men expressed produce them.
happiness publicly, but not privately, just Conceptual clashes like these could be
before meeting with a socially undesirable rectified, argued Schlenker (2003), by
woman. Prior to meeting a socially desir­ unleashing impression management from
able woman, men tended to report more ties to singular social goals frozen in time.
happiness privately than they expressed Schlenker and his colleagues (e.g., Schlenker
publicly. Apparently the men’s social goal­ & Pontari, 2000) made the case for broad­
posts moved from “confident politeness” ening the conceptualization of impression
in the first instance to “don’t appear too management. In essence, they argued that
accommodating” in the second. These impression management comprises a dynamic
findings help show that emotional displays process serving a hierarchy of goals and that
may reflect social motives and goals, acting it glides continually between the cognitive
skill, emotional intelligence, and more (e.g., fore and aft of interactions depending on
Clark, Pataki, & Carver, 1996; Fernandez- the resources individuals direct toward it.
Dols, 1999; Fridlund, 1994; Manstead Resource allocation depends on many things,
et al., 1999). including the relative importance of the goals
served, the effort needed to perform goal-rel­
evant behavior, competition from additional
SOCIAL-COGNITIVE PERSPECTIVES tasks at hand, features of the audience
ON IMPRESSION MANAGEMENT such as their expertise or attractiveness, and
the skills and personality characteristics of
Functional social-cognitive approaches the actor.
identify particular proximate social goals as These ideas help explain how signals
energizing presentations of the self. Some from our body and face sometimes under­
impose single primary goals, such as inter­ mine us just when we need them the
personal power or social attractiveness most: when managing impressions to look
17-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 5:09 PM Page 333

Why and How the Silent Self Speaks Volumes–––◆–––333

credible. Situations in which communica­ Summary of Approaches. The variety of


tors have a large personal stake in being approaches presented in this section reveal
believed may be made tense by that fact three tasks of nonverbal impression manage­
alone (DePaulo et al., 2003; Ekman, 1992). ment: the enhancement of biological fitness,
Add to this factor certain characteristics the production of adaptive social responses,
of the communicator (e.g., low confidence) and the pursuit of social goals. The ultimate
plus features of the audience (e.g., suspi­ and proximate functions they comprise are
ciousness) and competition for attentional interwoven into the fabric of human life his­
resources (e.g., impressing cohorts), and the tory and fashion two important qualities of
derived formula predicts either an unsuc­ nonverbal impression management.
cessful bid for a date or a collapsed court­ The first quality is that nonverbal
room testimony, each undermined by impression management may be performed
nonverbal tension leakage and untrustwor­ without awareness. This is despite the fact
thy appearances. that its operation is described typically
This way of thinking about impression as “strategic,” thereby implying conscious
management highlights important issues for processes and control (Burgoon et al.,
nonverbal communication processes. First, 1996; DePaulo, 1992). Implicitly or explic­
neither goals nor the situations that trigger itly, functional approaches accept that non­
them need be conscious to produce nonver­ verbal impression management may result
bal impression management strategies (e.g., from either conscious or nonconscious
Cheng & Chartrand, 2003; see Lakin, this (automatic) processes. Second, func­
volume). Second, resource availability and tional approaches treat the integrity of
allocation may influence the success of the nonverbal signal as relatively arbi­
presentations (e.g., Vohs et al., 2005). trary: imposters, self-deceivers, honest
These two key elements of impression signalers, and the misread attract equal
management are contained in the parallel attention on the impression management
process model of nonverbal communica­ runway. These two qualities have impor­
tion presented by Patterson (2001; see also tant implications for understanding impres­
Patterson, this volume). According to sion management and are the focus of this
Patterson, processes at or below the level of chapter’s final section.
consciousness can launch the dynamics of
sending and receiving nonverbal messages.
Patterson’s parallel process model could be ♦ The Unfinished Business
helpful in determining how cognitive and
of Nonverbal Impression
affective mediators or filters operate differ­
Management
ently for individuals high and low in traits
such as social anxiety or self-monitoring.
Parts of the model could also be used to Connie, a young graduate student who
predict when and how cognitive resources showed up at class each day wearing jeans
would be redirected, for example, under and a tee shirt, charged her professor with
different status conditions (e.g., Snodgrass, sexual harassment. But as the professor saw
1992) or for different age groups or in dif­ it, Connie was the one behaving in sexually
ferent cultures. To date, components of the provocative ways. The professor claimed
parallel processing model have heuristic that Connie flirted with him regularly by
value, but specific hypotheses are yet to be smiling and gazing at him with open legs as
formulated and tested. she sat around the conference table in the
17-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 5:09 PM Page 334

334–––◆–––Functions

graduate seminar classroom. Connie was within us sought to control our social world
stunned by this accusation; she did not feel (see Leary, 1995; Schlenker & Pontari,
sexually attracted to her professor. What 2003, for discussions). Contemporary evolu­
kind of impression management was this? tionary and ecological perspectives give
Or was it? And whose was it? Maybe impression management, especially its non­
Connie’s nonverbal behavior expressed an verbal forms, a place in the larger scheme
attraction toward her professor that she of adaptive social behavior. New knowledge
was unaware of and Connie was simply about connections between emotion and
self-deceptive. Alternatively, the professor cognition offers fresh insights into nonverbal
was perhaps motivated to misread Connie’s emotional self-presentation, self-regulation,
body cues and perceive messages that were and social context. Contemporary social-
not really there. But either way, could the cognitive approaches put conscious and
professor defend himself by claiming that nonconscious processes within the bound­
he was victimized by Connie’s noncon­ aries of nonverbal impression management
scious goal of seduction played out on a frameworks and highlight the importance of
nonverbal stage? Is Connie responsible? Is nonverbal measurement techniques.
her professor responsible? At the same time, these new insights
This example highlights the fact that have a troubling side. Freed from the impo­
nonconscious goal activation and its inter­ sition of conscious control, where is the
section with deceptive and self-deceptive “management” in nonverbal impression
cognitive processes have potentially impor­ management? How is successful nonverbal
tant practical consequences for the per­ impression management different from just
formers and audiences of nonverbal getting lucky in the context of impression
impression management. At the same time, formation? Is impression management
they present conceptual and measurement simply the mirror image of person percep­
challenges for researchers studying impres­ tion, a contest for best performance of
sion management. These conceptual and expressive scripts, or a relatively honest
measurement issues are intertwined. Given form of deception? The usefulness of the
that nonverbal impression management can impression management concept may lie
be driven by nonconscious goals and stimu­ in a renewed emphasis on its most unique
lated by nonconscious processes (e.g., aspect: the interdependence of actor-
Cheng & Chartrand, 2003), nonverbal audience psychologies (Goffman, 1959;
behavior may be the best—or only—way to Patterson, 2001). Those who invest in
track their operation. These measurements future research on nonverbal impression
will have especially complex iterations in management must give due diligence by dis­
“live” impression management situations tinguishing its functions and effects from
where nonconscious processes simultane­ those of other social influence processes,
ously energize actors, audiences, and their testing them in an orderly way, and reveal­
relationships. Teasing nonverbal commu­ ing their superior predictive validity.
nicative processes apart as well as pasting
them together will be necessary to elucidate
how nonverbal impression management is ♦ References
orchestrated between actors and audiences.
Previous conceptualizations of impres­ Adaval, R., & Wyers, R. S. (2004).
sion management characterized it as a Communicating about a social interaction:
Machiavellian enterprise of strategy and Effects on member for protagonists’ state­
manipulation in which the “mini-Mach” ment and nonverbal behaviors. Journal
17-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 5:09 PM Page 335

Why and How the Silent Self Speaks Volumes–––◆–––335

of Experimental Social Psychology, 40, words do not: facial expressions of


450–465. emotion, smiling, and the willingness to dis­
Albright, L., Forest, C., & Reiseter, K. (2001). close childhood sexual abuse. Journal of
Acting, behaving, and the selfless basis of Personality and Social Psychology, 83,
metaperception. Journal of Personality and 94–110.
Social Psychology, 81, 910–921. Bosson, J. K., Haymovitz, E. L., & Pinel, E. C.
Ambady, N., Bernieri, F. J., & Richeson, J. A. (2004). When saying and doing diverge:
(2000). Toward a histology of social behav­ The effects of stereotype threat on self-
ior: Judgmental accuracy from thin slices reported versus nonverbal anxiety. Journal
of the behavioral stream. In M. P. Zanna of Experimental Social Psychology, 40,
(Ed.), Advances in experimental social psy­ 247–255.
chology (Vol. 32, pp. 201–271). San Diego, Brunswik, E. (1955). Representative design and
CA: Academic Press. probabilistic theory in a functional theory.
Anderson, C., John, O. P., Keltner, D, & Kring, Psychological Review, 62, 193–217.
A. M. (2001). Who attains social status? Burgoon, J. K., Buller, D. B., & Woodall, W. G.
Effects of personality and physical attractive­ (1996). Nonverbal communication (2nd
ness in social groups. Journal of Personality ed.). New York: McGraw-Hill.
and Social Psychology, 81, 116–132. Chaplin, W. F., Phillips, J. B., Brown, J. D.,
Attardo, S., Eisterhold, J., Hay, J, & Poggi, I. Clanton, N.R., & Stein, J. L. (2000).
(2003). Multimodal markers of irony and Handshaking, gender, personality, and first
sarcasm. International Journal of Humor impressions. Journal of Personality and
Research, 16, 243–260. Social Psychology, 79, 110–117.
Bar-On, R., & Parker, J. D. A. (2000). Cheng, C. M., & Chartrand, T. L. (2003).
Handbook of emotional intelligence. San Self-monitoring without awareness: Using
Francisco: Jossey-Bass. mimicry as a nonconscious affiliation strat­
Batty, M., & Taylor, M. J. (2003). Early pro­ egy. Journal of Personality and Social
cessing of the six basic facial emotional Psychology, 85, 1170–1179.
expressions. Cognitive Brain Research, 17, Cherulnick, P. D., Donely, K. A.,Wiewel, T. S.
613–620. R., & Miller, S. R. (2001). Charisma is con­
Baumeister, R. (1993). Self-presentation: tagious: The effect of leaders’ charisma on
Motivational, cognitive, and interpersonal observers’ affect. Journal of Applied Social
patterns. In G. van Heck, P. Bonaiuto, I. J. Psychology, 31, 2149–2159.
Deary, & W. Nowack (Eds.), Personality Cialdini, R. B. (2001). Influence: Science and
psychology in Europe (Vol. 4, pp. 257– practice (4th ed.). Boston: Allyn & Bacon.
280). Tilburg, The Netherlands: Tilburg Clark, M. S., Pataki, S. P., & Carver, V. H.
University Press. (1996). Some thoughts and findings on
Bernieri, F. J., Gillis, J. S., & Davis, J. M. (1996). self-presentation of emotions in relation­
Dyad rapport and the accuracy of its judg­ ships. In G. J. O. Fletcher & J. Fitness
ment across situations: A lens model analy­ (Eds.), Knowledge structures in close rela­
sis. Journal of Personality and Social tionship: A social psychological approach
Psychology, 71, 110–129. (pp. 247–274). Hillsdale NJ: Erlbaum.
Berry, D. S. (1990). What can a moving face Collins, M. A., & Zebrowitz, L. A. (1995). The
tell us? Journal of Personality and Social contributions of appearance to occupa­
Psychology, 58, 1004–1014. tional outcomes in civilian and military
Birdwhistell, R. (1970). Kinesics and context: settings. Journal of Applied Social Psycho­
Essays on body motion communication. logy, 25, 129–163.
Philadelphia: University of Pennsylvania Connan, F. (1998). Machismo nervosa: An omi­
Press. nous variant of bulimia nervosa. European
Bonanno, G. A., Keltner, D., Noll, J. G., Eating Disorders Review, 6, 154–159.
Putnam, F. W., Trickett, P. K., LeJeune, J., Cunningham, M. R., Barbee, A. P., &
et al. (2002). When the face reveals what Philhower, C. L. (2002). Dimensions of
17-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 5:09 PM Page 336

336–––◆–––Functions

facial physical attractiveness: The intersec­ Fernandez-Dols, J. (1999). Facial expression


tion of biology and culture. In G. Rhodes and emotion: A situationist view. In
& L. A. Zebrowitz (Eds.), Facial attractive­ P. Philippot, R. S. Feldman, & E. J. Coats
ness: Evolutionary, cognitive, and social (Eds.), The social context of nonverbal
perspectives (pp. 193–238). Westport, CT: behavior (pp. 242–261). Cambridge, UK:
Ablex. Cambridge University Press.
Cunningham, M. R., Druen, P. B., & Barbee, Frank, M. G., & Gilovich, T. (1988). The dark
A. P. (1997). Angels, mentors, and friends: side of self- and social perception: Black
Trade-offs among evolutionary, social, and uniforms and aggression in professional
individual variables in physical appearance. sports. Journal of Personality and Social
In J. A. Simpson & D. T. Kendrick (Eds.), Psychology, 54, 74–85.
Evolutionary social psychology (pp. 109– Fridlund, A. J. (1994). Human facial expression:
140). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. An evolutionary view. San Diego, CA:
Darwin, C. (1991). The expression of the emo­ Academic Press.
tions in man and animals. London: John Frith, H., & Gleeson, K. (2004). Clothing and
Murray. (Original work published 1872) embodiment: Men managing body image
DePaulo, B. M. (1992). Nonverbal behavior and appearance. Psychology of Men and
and self-presentation. Psychological Bulletin, Masculinity, 5, 40–48.
111, 203–243. Gangestad, S. W., Simpson, J. A., DiGeronimo,
DePaulo, B. M., Lindsay, J. J., Malone, B. E., & K., & Biek, M. (1992). Differential accu­
Muhlenbruck, L. (2003). Cues to decep­ racy in person perception across traits:
tion. Psychological Bulletin, 129, 74–118. Examination of a functional hypothesis.
DePaulo, B. M., Stone, J. I., & Lassiter, G. D. Journal of Personality and Social Psycho­
(1985). Telling ingratiating lies: Effects of logy, 62, 688–698.
target sex and target attractiveness on ver­ Gangestad, S. W., & Snyder, M. (2000). Self-
bal and nonverbal deceptive success. Journal monitoring: Appraisal and reappraisal.
of Personality and Social Psychology, 48, Psychological Bulletin, 126, 530–555.
1191–1203. Gangestad, S. W., Thornhill, R., & Yeo, R. A.
DePaulo, B. M., & Tang, J. (1994). Social anxi­ (1994). Facial attractiveness, developmen­
ety and social judgment: The example of tal stability, and fluctuating asymmetry.
detecting deception. Journal of Research in Ethology and Sociobiology, 15, 73–85.
Personality, 28, 142–153. Gifford, R. (1994). A lens-mapping framework
Dougherty, T. W., Turban, D. B., & Callender, for understanding the encoding and decod­
J. C. (1994). Confirming first impression ing of interpersonal dispositions in nonver­
in the employment interview: A field study bal behavior. Journal of Personality and
of interviewer behavior. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 66, 398–412.
Psychology, 79, 659–665. Gifford, R. (2004, December 5). Weekend edi­
Dowd, M. (2004, April 22). The body politic. tion [Radio broadcast]. Washington, DC:
New York Times. Retrieved April 22, 2004, National Public Radio.
from http://www.nytimes.com Gilbert, R. E. (1998). The mortal presidency:
Duncan, S. D., Jr. (1972). Some signals and rules Illness and anguish in the White House.
for taking speaking turns in conversations. New York: Fordham University Press.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychol­ Goffman, E. (1959). The presentation of self in
ogy, 23, 283–292. everyday life. Garden City, NY: Doubleday.
Ekman, P. (1971). Universal and cultural differ­ Grammer, K., Honda, M., Juette, A., & Schmitt,
ences in facial expressions of emotion. In A. (1999). Fuzziness of nonverbal court­
J. K. Coles (Ed.), Nebraska symposium ship communication unblurred by motion
on motivation (pp. 207–283). Lincoln: energy detection. Journal of Personality and
University of Nebraska Press. Social Psychology, 77, 487–508.
Ekman, P. (1992). Telling lies. New York: Grammer, K., Kruck, K. B., & Magnujsson,
Norton. (Original work published 1985) M. S. (1998). The courtship dance: Patterns
17-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 5:09 PM Page 337

Why and How the Silent Self Speaks Volumes–––◆–––337

of nonverbal synchronization in opposite- Keating, C. F., & Heltman, K. R. (1994).


sex encounters. Journal of Nonverbal Dominance and deception in children and
Behavior, 22, 3–29. adults: Are leaders the best misleaders?
Guerrero, L. K., & DeVito, J. A. (Eds.). (1999). Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin,
Nonverbal communication reader: Appear­ 20, 312–321.
ance and adornment cues. Prospect Keating, C. F., Randall, D. W., & Kendrick, T.
Heights, IL: Waveland Press. (1999). Presidential physiognomies: Altered
Guthrie, R. D. (1976). Body hotspots. New images, altered perceptions. Political Psy­
York: Van Nostrand Reinhold. chology, 20, 593–610.
Hassin, R., & Trope, Y. (2000). Facing faces: Keating, C. F., Randall, D., Kendrick, T.,
Studies on the cognitive aspects of physiog­ & Gutshall, K. A. (2003). Do babyfaced
nomy. Journal of Personality and Social adults receive more help? The (cross­
Psychology, 78, 837–852. cultural) case of the lost resume. Journal
Hatfield, E., Cacioppo, J. T., & Rapson, R. of Nonverbal Behavior, 27, 89–109.
(1994). Emotional contagion. Cambridge, Kenny, R. D., Horner, C., Kashy, D. A., & Chu,
UK: Cambridge University Press. L. (1992). Consensus at zero acquaintance:
Hess, U., Philippot, P., & Blairy, S. (1999). Replication, behavioral cues, and stability.
Mimicry: Facts and fiction. In P. Philippot, Journal of Personality and Social Psy­
R. S. Feldman, & E. J. Coats (Eds.), chology, 62, 88–97.
The social context of nonverbal behavior Krauss, R. M. (1998). Why do we gesture when
(pp. 213–241). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge we speak? Current Directions in Psycholo­
University Press. gical Science, 7, 54–60.
Holodynski, M. (2004). The miniaturization of LaPlante, D., & Ambady, N. (2003). On how
expression in the development of emotional things are said: Voice tone, voice intensity,
self-regulation. Developmental Psychology, verbal content, and perceptions of politeness.
40, 16–28. Journal of Language and Social Psychology,
Hyden, L., & Peolsson, M. (2002). Pain ges­ 22, 434–441.
tures: The orchestration of speech and body Leary, M. R. (1995). Self-presentation:
gestures. Health, 6, 325–345. Impression management and interper­
Jones, E. E., & Pittman, T. S. (1982). Toward a sonal behavior. Madison, WI: Brown &
general theory of strategic self-presentation. Benchmark.
In J. Suls (Ed.), Psychological perspectives Leary, M. R., Tchividjian, L. R., & Kraxberger,
on the self (Vol. 1, pp. 231–263). Hillsdale, B. E. (1994). Self-presentation can be haz­
NJ: Erlbaum. ardous to your health: Impression manage­
Kaufman, W. (2005, March 3). All things con­ ment and health risk. Health Psychology,
sidered [Radio broadcast]. Washington, 13, 461–470.
DC: National Public Radio. LeDoux, J. (1996). The emotional brain. New
Kawakami, C., White, J. B., & Langer, E. J. York: Simon & Schuster.
(2000). Mindful and masculine: Freeing Mansell, W., Clark, D. M., Ehlers, A., & Chen, Y.
women leaders from the constraints of gen­ (1999). Social anxiety and attention away
der roles. Journal of Social Issues, 56, 49–63. from emotional faces. Cognition and
Keating, C. F. (2002). Charismatic faces: Social Emotion, 13, 673–690.
status cues put face appeal in context. In Manstead, A. S. R., Fischer, A. H., & Jakobs,
G. Rhodes & L. A. Zebrowitz (Eds.), Facial E. B. (1999). The social and emotional
attractiveness: Evolutionary, cognitive, and functions of facial displays. In P. Philippot,
social perspectives (pp. 153–192). Westport, R. S. Feldman, & E. J. Coats (Eds.),
CT: Ablex. The social context of nonverbal behavior
Keating, C. F., & Doyle, J. (2002). The faces (pp. 287–316). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge
of desirable mates and dates contain mixed University Press.
social status cues. Journal of Experimental Manusov, V., & Rodriguez, J. S. (1989).
Social Psychology, 38, 414–424. Intentionality behind nonverbal messages:
17-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 5:09 PM Page 338

338–––◆–––Functions

A perceiver’s perspective. Journal of Riggio, R., Riggio, H. R., Salinas, C., & Cole,
Nonverbal Behavior, 13, 15–24. E. J. (2003). The role of social and emotional
McArthur, L. Z., & Baron, R. M. (1983). communication skills in leaders emergence
Toward an ecological theory of social and effectiveness. Group Dynamics: Theory,
perception. Psychological Review, 90, Research, and Practice, 7, 83–103.
215–238. Rubin, R. S., Bartels, L. K., & Bommer. W. H.
Mehrabian, A. (1972). Nonverbal communica­ (2002). Are leaders smarter or do they just
tion. Chicago: Aldine-Atherton. seem that way? Exploring perceived intel­
Montepare, J. M. (1995). The impact of varia­ lectual competence and leadership emer­
tions in height on young children’s impres­ gence. Social Behavior and Personality,
sions of men and women. Journal of 30, 105–118.
Nonverbal Behavior 19, 31–47. Rudman, L. A. (1998). Self-promotion as a risk
Montepare, J. M., & Zebrowitz, L. A. (1998). factor for women: The costs and benefits
Person perception comes of age: The of counterstereotypical impression manage­
salience and significance of age in social ment. Journal of Personality and Social
judgments. Advances in Experimental Psychology, 74, 629–645.
Social Psychology, 30, 93–161. Saarni, C. (1989). Children’s understanding of
Mueller, U., & Mazur, A. C. (1996). Facial strategic control of emotional expression in
dominance in Homo sapiens as honest sig­ social transactions. In C. Saarni & P. Harris
naling of male quality. Behavioral Ecology, (Eds.), Children’s understanding of emotion
8, 569–579. (pp. 181–208). New York: Cambridge
Pataki, S. P., & Clark, M. S. (2004). Self- University Press.
presentations of happiness: Sincere, polite, Saarni, C., & Weber, H. (1999). Emotional
or cautious? Personality and Social Psycho­ displays and dissemblance in childhood:
logy Bulletin, 30, 905–914. implications for self-presentation. In
Patterson, M. L. (1999). The evolution of a par­ P. Philippot, R. S. Feldman, & E. J. Coats
allel process model of nonverbal communi­ (Eds.), The social context of nonverbal
cation. In P. Philippot, R. S. Feldman, & behavior (pp. 317–347). Cambridge, UK:
E. J. Coats (Eds.), The social context of non­ Cambridge University Press.
verbal behavior (pp. 317–347). Cambridge, Schlenker, B. R. (2003). Self-presentation. In
UK: Cambridge University Press. M. R. Leary & J. P. Tangney (Eds.),
Patterson, M. L. (2001). Toward a comprehen­ Handbook of self and identity (pp. 462–
sive model of non-verbal communication. 518). New York: Guilford Press.
In W. P. Robinson & H. Giles (Eds.), The Schlenker, B. R., & Pontari, B. A. (2000). The
new handbook of language and social psy­ strategic control of information: Impression
chology (pp. 159–176). New York: Wiley. management and self-presentational in
Perrett, D. I., Lee, K. J., Penton-Voak, daily life. In A. Tesser, R. Felson, & J. Suils
I., Rowland, D., Yoshikawa, S., Burt, D. M., (Eds.), Perspective on self and identity
et al. (1998). Effects of sexual dimorphism (pp. 199–232). Washington, DC: American
on facial attractiveness. Nature, 394, Psychological Association.
884–887. Segall, M. H., Campbell, D. T., & Herskovitz,
Pettijohn, T. E., III, & Jungeberg, B. J. (2004). M. J. (1966). The influence of culture on
Playmate curves: Changes in facial and visual perception. Indianapolis, IN: Bobbs
body feature preferences across social Merrill.
and economic conditions. Personality and Singer, M. A., & Goldin-Meadows, S. (2005).
Social Psychology Bulletin, 30, 1186–1197. Children learn when their teacher’s gestures
Rhodes, G., & Zebrowitz, L. A. (2002). (Eds.). and speech differ. Psychological Science,
Facial attractiveness: Evolutionary, cogni­ 16, 85–89.
tive, and social perspectives. Westport, CT: Singh, D., & Bronstad, P. M. (1997). Sex
Ablex. differences in the anatomical locations of
17-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 5:09 PM Page 339

Why and How the Silent Self Speaks Volumes–––◆–––339

human body scarification and tattooing Personality and Social Psychology, 84,
as a function of pathogen prevalence. 558–568.
Evolution and Human Behavior, 18, Todorov, A., Mandisodza, A. N., Goren, A., &
403–416. Hall, C. C. (2005). Inferences of compe­
Snodgrass, S. E. (1992). Further effects of role tence from faces predict election outcomes.
versus gender on interpersonal sensitivity. Science, 308, 1623–1626.
Journal of Personality and Social Psycho­ Vohs, K. D., Baumeister, R. F., & Ciarocco,
logy, 62, 154–158. N. J. (2005). Self-regulation and self-pre­
Stolberg, S. G. (2004, January 25). Whoop, oops sentation: Regulatory resource depletion
and the state of the political slip. New York impairs impression management and
Times Week in Review, sec. 4, pp. 1, 3. effortful self-presentation depletes regula­
Tedeschi, J. T., & Norman, N. (1985). tory resources. Journal of Personality and
Social power, self-presentation, and the Social Psychology, 88, 632–657.
self. In B. R. Schlenker (Ed.), The self in Vrij, A., Evans, H., Akehurst, L., & Mann, M.
social life (pp. 293–321). New York: (2004). Rapid judgements in assessing verbal
McGraw-Hill. and nonverbal cues: Their potential for
Tice, D. M., & Baumeister, R. F. (2001). deception researches and lie detection.
The primacy of the interpersonal self. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 18, 283–296.
In C. Sedikedes & M. Brewer (Eds.), Wroblewska, A. M. (1997). Androgen-anabolic
Individual self, relational self, collective self steroids and body dysmorphia in young
(pp. 71–88). Philadelphia: Psychology Press. men. Journal of Psychosomatic Research,
Tiedens, L. Z., & Fragale, A. R. (2003). Power 42, 225–234.
moves: Complementarity in dominant and Zebrowitz, L. A. (1997). Reading faces: Window
submissive nonverbal behavior. Journal of to the soul? Boulder, CO: Westview Press.
17-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 5:09 PM Page 340
18-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 5:10 PM Page 341

18
NONVERBAL COMMUNICATION
AND DECEPTION

� Aldert Vrij
University of Portsmouth, United Kingdom

A re there behavioral differences between liars and truth tellers?


Most people think there are. Can we spot whether people are
lying by looking at their behavior? Many police officers (and parents)
think they can (Boon & McLeod, 2001; Elaad, 2003). These beliefs
reflect the assumption that communicative behavior, particularly non­
verbal cues, function as signals of deception or truth telling. Despite our
assumptions about our abilities, however, most people are poor at
detecting deception (Bond & DePaulo, 2005; Vrij, 2000a; see also
Riggio, this volume). This inability may be due to the expectations that
people have about what cues reveal deception. Unlike the clarity of
Pinocchio’s growing nose, however, researchers have found no single
cue that is related to deception uniquely and reliably; and deception the­
ories, such as those included in this chapter, do not predict that such
cues exist. To help make sense of the general inability to detect decep­
tion, this chapter discusses the discrepancy between attempts at decep­
tion detection and cues to deception. Several reasons why people fail
to spot deceit will also be discussed, including ways in which they can
improve. I start this chapter with discussing the importance of nonver­
bal communication in deception more generally.

◆ 341
18-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 5:10 PM Page 342

342–––◆–––Functions

♦ Importance of Nonverbal speech, they cannot be silent nonverbally.


Communication in Deception When a question takes one by surprise,
one can afford a little rest to think of an
appropriate answer. Nonverbally, however,
People can rely on what people say or how there is no possibility of taking such a
they act when attempting to determine their rest. That person will display behavior
truthfulness. To assess what cues are relied throughout the entire conversation, even
on under what circumstances, Park, Levine, when remaining silent, and the receiver
McCornack, Morrison, and Ferrara (2002) can observe and interpret this behavior
asked college students to recall an instance (DePaulo & Kirkendol, 1989).
in their life in which they had detected that A second reason why people often
another person had lied to them. They then attend to nonverbal communication is that
reported how they had discovered the lie. observers may not know which verbal cues
The results suggested that people’s main to pay attention to. Speech content can
method of detecting lies was by compar­ reveal a lie if only observers know what to
ing someone’s speech content with other pay attention to (Vrij, 2005). Third, unlike
sources of information (third parties and verbal-only lie detection for which written
physical evidence) to prove that the state­ transcripts of the statements are required
ment was incorrect. When people hear dif­ (Vrij, 2005), nonverbal lie detection can
ferent statements from the same person take place without any tools or equipment.
about a particular topic, they focus primar­ It is therefore the only lie detection method
ily on speech content, checking for consis­ that can be employed spontaneously and
tency between the different statements the only method that could be used in situ­
(Granhag & Vrij, 2005). In situations where ations where immediate observations are
there is no information to check and only required. Nonverbal lie detection can only
one statement is made, however, people pay be successful, however, if the behavior
primary attention to nonverbal communica­ shown by liars differs from that shown by
tion to detect lies (Vrij & Mann, 2005). truth tellers. The next two sections address
There are several reasons for this focus. this issue. In the first of these sections, I dis­
First, nonverbal communication might cuss three theories addressing how and why
actually be more revealing than speech nonverbal behavior could be linked to
(DePaulo, 1992; Vrij, 2000a). Specifically, deception. This is followed by an overview
there may be automatic links between of research examining nonverbal cues to
strongly felt emotions and certain behaviors deceit, discussed in light of these theories.
(Ekman, 1985/2001), but there are no such
links between emotions and speech con­
tent. Also, because we exchange informa­ ♦ Deception Theories
tion predominantly, certainly consciously,
via words, we are more practiced in using
ZUCKERMAN, DEPAULO,
words than in using other behaviors. The
AND ROSENTHAL’S
predominance of words in the exchange of MULTIFACTOR MODEL
information also makes people more aware
of what they are saying than of how All three deception theories discussed in
they are behaving. Being aware of one’s this chapter have one important feature in
own speech and other behavior underlies common: The mere fact that people lie will
attempts to control those behaviors. not necessarily affect their nonverbal behav­
Finally, although people can refrain from ior. Sometimes, however, liars may show
18-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 5:10 PM Page 343

Nonverbal Communication and Deception–––◆–––343

different behaviors than do truth tellers. carefully to assess whether they are getting
According to Zuckerman, DePaulo, and away with their lie.
Rosenthal (1981), three factors could influ­ The extent to which lying is demanding
ence cues to deception: (1) emotional reac­ often depends on the type of lie. Telling an
tions, (2) content complexity, and (3) outright lie might be more cognitively chal­
attempted behavioral control. Each of these lenging than concealing information, and
factors may influence a liar’s nonverbal telling an elaborate lie might well be more
behavior and emphasize a different aspect of demanding than providing short yes or no
deception. answers. Lying may be more demanding
Telling a lie is associated most com­ when the lie is not well prepared or
monly with three different emotions: fear, rehearsed. People engaged in cognitively
guilt, or delight (Ekman, 1985/2001). Liars complex tasks make more speech hesita­
might feel guilty because they are lying, tions (e.g., stutters) and speech errors, speak
might be afraid of getting caught, or might slower, pause more, and wait longer before
be excited about having the opportunity to giving an answer (Goldman-Eisler, 1968).
fool someone. The strength of these emo­ Cognitive complexity also leads to fewer hand
tions depends on the personality of the liar and arm movements (Ekman & Friesen,
and on the circumstances under which 1972) and to more gaze aversion, because
the lie takes place (Ekman, 1985/2001; looking the conversation partner in the eye
Vrij, 2000a). Importantly, the experience of can be distracting (Doherty-Sneddon, Bruce,
guilt, fear, and excitement may influence a Bonner, Longbotham, & Doyle, 2002).
liar’s behavior. For example, guilt might Liars may realize that observers look at
result in gaze aversion if the liar does not their behavioral reactions to judge whether
dare to look the target straight in the eye they are lying and may, therefore, attempt
while telling a lie. Fear and excitement to control their behavior to appear credible.
might result in signs of stress, such as an To be successful, typically, liars must sup­
increase in movements, an increase in press their nervousness while masking evi­
speech hesitations (mm’s and errrr’s) and dence of having to think hard. They should
speech errors (stutters, repetition of words, also be able to show “honest-looking”
omission of words), or a higher pitched behaviors and avoid “dishonest-looking”
voice. The stronger the emotion, the more behaviors (Hocking & Leathers, 1980).
likely it is that some of these behaviors will These “requirements” mean that liars may
reveal deceit (Ekman, 1985/2001). need to act, but they must also avoid behav­
Lying may be a cognitively demanding ior appearing planned, rehearsed, and
task. To avoid getting caught, liars need to lacking in spontaneity. According to this
provide plausible answers while avoiding theoretical perspective, liars’ motivation and
contradicting themselves. They must tell a efforts to control their behavior will increase
lie that is consistent with everything when the stakes (negative consequences of
the observer knows or might find out. getting caught or positive consequences of
Liars also need to remember what they succeeding) increase (Ekman, 1985/2001).
have said, so that they can say the same
things again when asked to repeat their
story. They may also feel an urge to control DEPAULO’S SELF­
their demeanor so that they will appear PRESENTATIONAL PERSPECTIVE
honest (as emphasized in the attempted
control process below), and they may Zuckerman et al.’s (1981) perspective
observe the target person’s reactions predicts that the more liars experience one
18-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 5:10 PM Page 344

344–––◆–––Functions

or more of the three factors (emotion, con­ face-to-face encounters, liars must accom­
tent complexity, behavioral control), the plish numerous communication tasks simul­
more likely it is that cues to deception will taneously. They must produce a credible
occur. These factors are present only to a verbal message while projecting credible
limited extent in the majority of lies that nonverbal behavior simultaneously. They
people tell (DePaulo, Kashy, Kirkendol, must also manage their emotions, attend to
Wyer, & Epstein, 1996). In her self- their conversation partner while keeping the
presentational perspective, DePaulo (1992; dialogue running smoothly, send desired
DePaulo et al., 2003) argues that emotions, relational messages to their conversation
content complexity, and behavioral control partner and respond appropriately to what
might also influence truth tellers’ behavior. is said, and be discreet about any intentions
Thus, liars may be afraid of not being to deceive their partner. IDT embraces
believed in high-stakes situations, but so will Zuckerman et al.’s (1981) factors (emotion,
truth tellers, because they too could face content complexity, and attempted behavior
negative consequences if they fail to con­ control) as underlying reasons for cues to
vince others. Given the similarities between deceit (Burgoon, Buller, White, Afifi, &
liars and truth tellers, this perspective thus Buslig, 1999). In addition, it emphasizes
predicts that clear, diagnostic nonverbal that when deception occurs in interactive
cues to deception are unlikely to exist. contexts, it is not a unidirectional activity.
According to DePaulo et al. (2003), liars Rather, both liar and receiver mutually
and truth tellers will succeed in their social influence each other (Burgoon, Buller,
interaction goals only if they appear sincere. Floyd, & Grandpre, 1996).
The difference between lying and truth According to IDT, receivers’ behavior
telling is that the liar’s claim to honesty is may influence senders’ behavioral displays
illegitimate, and this lack of legitimacy has both directly, via synchrony, and indirectly,
two implications. First, deceptive self-presen­ because it may trigger behavioral adjust­
tations might be less embraced as truthful ments (Burgoon et al., 1999). Regarding the
self-presentations less convincingly (e.g., direct effects, when people communicate
because liars have moral scruples, lack emo­ with each other, matching and synchrony
tional investment in their false claims, or lack may take place (Burgoon et al., 1999;
the knowledge and experience to back up Chartrand & Bargh, 1999; see Tickle-
their deceptive statements convincingly). Degnen, this volume). People may mirror
Second, liars typically experience a greater each other’s posture, or they may converge
sense of awareness and deliberateness in in how quickly and how loudly they speak.
their performances than truth tellers, because They may also reciprocate each other’s
they may take their credibility less for gazing, nodding, accents, and smiling behav­
granted than truth tellers. Trying to appear ior (DePaulo & Friedman, 1998). This
convincing deliberately, however, might be “chameleon effect” (Chartrand & Bargh,
counterproductive. 1999) emerges even when strangers interact
with each other, and it happens typically
within a few minutes (Chartrand & Bargh,
BULLER AND BURGOON’S
INTERPERSONAL 1999). Furthermore, the indirect effects are
DECEPTION THEORY related to feedback from the receiver: When
liars are exposed to negative feedback from
A third perspective on deception, Buller the receiver, expressed through either verbal
and Burgoon’s (1996) interpersonal decep­ comments or through nonverbal behavior,
tion theory (IDT), postulates that during liars might realize that their performance is
18-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 5:10 PM Page 345

Nonverbal Communication and Deception–––◆–––345

lacking credulity. Consequently, liars might review to date assessing the consistency
respond by making behavioral adjustments and strength of certain nonverbal cues
to diminish suspicions. indicating deception. Their meta-analysis
includes 116 studies, although not all these
SUMMARY projects focus on nonverbal cues to decep­
tion, and involves 102 different nonverbal
The three perspectives discussed here cues. Most of the studies were experimental
make clear that the relationship between studies where university students lied or
lying and deceptive behavior is complex. told the truth for the sake of the experi­
Zuckerman et al.’s (1981) assumptions that ment. Sometimes efforts were made to
liars might show signs of emotions and cog­ motivate the participants, for example, by
nitive load seem straightforward, yet liars promising them a financial reward if they
often do not experience emotions and were convincing.
high cognitive load (DePaulo et al., 1996). Significant findings emerged for 23 cues,
DePaulo et al.’s (2003) self-presentation per­ and these are listed in Table 18.1. Nine of
spective stresses that such experiences are not those cues, listed in the bottom half of
the exclusive domain of liars. Truth tellers Table 18.1, were investigated in only a few
may experience them as well, and, as a result, studies and will not be discussed further.
may also display nonverbal cues associated The cues are ranked in terms of their effect
with emotion or cognitive load. The sizes. Cohen (1977) suggested that effect
attempted behavioral control prediction is sizes of .20, .50, and .80 should be inter­
not straightforward given that the behaviors preted as small, medium, and large effects,
shown by deceptive senders, as a result of this respectively. The effect sizes in the meta­
deliberate control, will depend on both their analysis were typically small. No one cue
perceptions of what constitutes a credible was related significantly to deception across
nonverbal display and their acting skills in studies. The highest effect sizes were found
performing this display. Finally, IDT’s inter­ in the cues that were not often investigated
active approach implies that deceptive behav­ (bottom half of Table 18.1), but if the con­
ior might be influenced directly by the centration is only on the cues that were
behavior of the receiver (a result of the investigated more often, then the largest
chameleon effect) or indirectly influenced by effect size was found for verbal and vocal
the suspicions raised by the receiver (Burgoon immediacy, d = –.55. The second highest
et al., 1999). The complex relationship cue, pupil dilation, obtained a d-score
between nonverbal communication and of .39. Most other cues obtained effect sizes
deception thus makes it unlikely that clear, of around .20 (see DePaulo et al., 2003, for
diagnostic, nonverbal cues to deception exist. all effect sizes, information about the indi­
Deception research, summarized in the next vidual studies, definitions of the nonverbal
section, has supported this view. cues, and the impact of several moderating
factors on these cues).

♦ Liars and Nonverbal Cues


SUPPORT FOR THE
NO CUE AKIN PINOCCHIO’S THEORETICAL PERSPECTIVES
GROWING NOSE
The results of the meta-analysis provide
DePaulo et al.’s (2003) meta-analysis of general support for the theoretical perspec­
cues to deception is the most comprehensive tives discussed previously. Several cues
18-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 5:10 PM Page 346

346–––◆–––Functions

Table 18.1 Nonverbal Cues to Deception

Cues d

Nonverbal cues to deception


Verbal and vocal immediacy −.55
Pupil dilation .39
Talking time −.35
Discrepant/ambivalent .34
Verbal and vocal uncertainty .30
Nervous, tense .27
Vocal tension .26
Chin raise .25
Words and phrase repetitions .21
Verbal and vocal involvement −.21
Pitch, frequency .21
Lip pressing .16
Illustrators −.14
Facial pleasantness −.12

Cues based on a small number of studies


Changes in foot movements 1.05
Pupillary changes .90
Genuine smile −.70
Indifferent, unconcerned .59
Interrupted words and repeated words .38
Specific hand and arm movements −.36
Seems planned, not spontaneous .35
Intensity of facial expression −.32
Direct orientation −.20

SOURCE: Derived from DePaulo et al. (2003).

(pupil dilation, nervousness, vocal tension, control their behavior in a convincing


and pitch) indicate that liars may be tenser manner (DePaulo et al., 2003; Zuckerman
than are truth tellers. The findings that liars et al., 1981).
talk less, make more word and phrase rep­ In the overwhelming majority of decep­
etitions, and make fewer illustrators (move­ tion studies, however, no interaction takes
ments tied to speech directly and serving to place between sender and receiver, mak­
illustrate what is being said verbally; ing them inappropriate for Buller and
Ekman & Friesen, 1969) suggest that lying Burgoon’s (1996) IDT. Studies in which an
is somewhat more cognitively demanding interactional interview style has been
than truth telling. The remaining cues employed have provided mixed results
reveal that liars appear less spontaneous, regarding whether liars avoid displaying
more ambivalent, less involved, and more suspicious behaviors (Levine & McCornack,
uncertain, and this fits well with the predic­ 1996). It might be that liars aim to suppress
tions that liars endorse their statements less all behaviors that they believe are suspi­
convincingly than do truth tellers (DePaulo cious, but they often do not succeed (Buller,
et al., 2003) and that liars often fail to Stiff, & Burgoon, 1996; Vrij, 2000a).
18-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 5:10 PM Page 347

Nonverbal Communication and Deception–––◆–––347

♦ Reasons for Few smiled as frequently as liars (Ekman, Friesen,


Nonverbal Cues to Deception & O’Sullivan, 1988). Other differences
between felt and false smiles include that
false smiles are more asymmetrical, appear
The complex relationship between nonver­ too early or too late, and often last longer
bal behavior and deception, outlined above, (Ekman, Davidson, & Friesen, 1990; see
already predicted that research would Ekman, 1985/2001, for more examples
reveal only a few, and usually weak, rela­ about how facial expressions could be
tionships between nonverbal cues and related to deceit).
deception. There are more explanations for Similar patterns may occur with differ­
the limited success that people have when ent behaviors. Nonverbal communication
they attempt to detect deception. This researchers have identified numerous types
section highlights two such reasons. of hand movements (Bavelas, Chovil,
Coates, & Roe, 1995; Ekman & Friesen,
SOME CUES ARE OVERLOOKED 1969, 1972; McNeill, 1985, 1992). For
example, based on the work of Efron
One explanation for not finding consis­ (1941), Ekman and Friesen (1969) made
tent and reliable cues to deception is that a distinction between five movement catego­
some nonverbal cues are overlooked by ries: emblems, illustrators, affect displays,
researchers, sometimes because the scoring regulators, and adaptors. In their later writ­
systems used to measure them are not ings (Ekman & Friesen, 1972; Friesen,
detailed enough. Ekman (1985/2001) has Ekman, & Wallbott, 1979), they restricted
identified a number of different smiles, themselves to only three of these cate­
including a distinction between felt and gories, emblems, illustrators, and adaptors,
false smiles. Felt smiles include smiles in because “these three classes include all
which the person actually experiences a hand movement except for those times
positive emotion, whereas false smiles are when the hand moves simply to establish a
deliberately contrived to convince another new position or rest” (Friesen et al., 1979,
person that a positive emotion is felt, when p. 99). This three-class categorization is
in fact it is not. Felt smiles are accompanied often used in deception research, and all
by the action of two muscles: the zygomatic three categories appear in DePaulo et al.’s
major that pulls the lip corners upward (2003) meta-analysis.
toward the cheekbone and the orbicularis Ekman and Friesen (1972) make further
oculi that raises the cheek and gathers skin distinctions into eight types of illustrators,
inward from around the eye socket. The lat­ but these subdivisions are not used by
ter change produces bagged skin below the deception researchers typically. In one
eyes and crow’s-feet creases beyond the eye experiment, however, Caso, Maricchiolo,
corners. In false smiles, the action of the Bonaiuto, Vrij, and Mann (2006) did dif­
orbicularis oculi muscle causing the effects ferentiate between different types of illus­
around the eye is often missing (Frank, trators. Truth tellers described objects they
Ekman, & Friesen, 1993). Ekman and col­ had in their possession, whereas liars had to
leagues found that truth tellers displayed imagine that they had these objects in their
more felt smiles than did liars, whereas liars possession. Liars made fewer deictic move­
displayed more false smiles than did truth ments (pointing gestures) than did truth
tellers. When the distinction between felt tellers, perhaps due to the lack of real
and false smiles was not made, truth tellers objects they could point at, but liars made
18-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 5:10 PM Page 348

348–––◆–––Functions

more metaphoric gestures, which are typi­ What happens in situations where the
cally made when people describe abstract stakes are really high, for example, when
ideas (McNeill, 1992). Illustrators as a guilty suspects lie in police interviews, when
whole entity (i.e., all the different types com­ smugglers go through customs at airports,
bined) were not linked to deception. Like or when adulterous husbands are chal­
smiles, it was only when specific types of lenged by their wives? To examine how
distinctions were made among subclasses of liars respond in high-stakes situations, one
behavior that deception cues were detected. of the few options is to analyze such real-
life high-stakes situations. It is difficult,
however, to capture such lies on tape and to
INDIVIDUAL AND establish the ground truth in such situa­
SITUATIONAL DIFFERENCES tions, that is, to know for certain that some­
one was actually lying or telling the truth
Another reason for limited number of (Vrij & Mann, 2003), making such studies
nonverbal indicators of deceit may be that a difficult to undertake.
meta-analysis cannot capture signs of deceit Mann, Vrij, and Bull (2002) published
at an individual level. That is, different the most comprehensive study to date
individuals may give their lies away in dif­ about people’s behavior in real-life high-
ferent ways (DePaulo et al., 2003); such stakes situations. They examined the
idiosyncratic cues do not become apparent behavioral responses of 16 suspects while
when the focus is across studies. Similarly, they lied and told the truth during their
cues to deception could be dependent on the police interviews. The police interviews
situational context for the lie. A meta-analy­ were videotaped, and the tapes were
sis that accumulates findings across contexts made available for detailed scoring of the
would not apprehend those trends either. suspects’ behavioral responses. The sus­
Furthermore, more cues to deception are pects were interviewed in connection with
likely to occur when the stakes are high serious crimes such as murder, rape, and
rather than low. In high-stakes situations, arson and were facing long custodial sen­
liars might feel stronger emotions, might tences if found guilty. Regarding the
experience more cognitive demand, and ground truth, clips of video footage were
might be more motivated to manage their selected where other sources (reliable wit­
behavior to appear credible. In their meta­ ness statements and forensic evidence) pro­
analysis, DePaulo et al. (2003) com­ vided conclusive evidence that the suspect
pared higher-stakes studies (e.g., studies lied or told the truth.
where financial incentives were promised if Results revealed that compared with
the participant appeared credible) with when they told the truth, the suspects
lower-stakes studies. Some cues to decep­ exhibited more pauses, fewer eye blinks,
tion, such as an increase in blinking, a and fewer hand and arm movements (by
decrease in leg and foot movements, and an male suspects) when they lied. Indicators of
increased speech rate, appeared only in being tense (such as fidgeting and gaze aver­
higher-stakes situations. The differences sion) did not emerge. These indicators are
between liars and truth tellers were still the behavioral patterns that police officers
small, however, perhaps because a high- expect typically in lying suspects. Mann
stakes situation will also affect truth tellers et al.’s results suggest that the suspects’ cues
or, alternatively, because the stakes were still to deception were more likely the result
not high enough in these laboratory-based, of increased cognitive demand than of
higher-stakes studies. nervousness. The strongest evidence for this
18-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 5:10 PM Page 349

Nonverbal Communication and Deception–––◆–––349

was the reduction in eye blinks during the person was lying or telling the truth.
deception. Research has shown that ner­ There is no opportunity for the lie detectors
vousness results in an increase in eye to check the veracity of the statement via
blinking (Harrigan & O’Connell, 1996), physical evidence, third parties, and so on,
whereas increased cognitive load results in so the only source of information is the
a decrease in eye blinking (Wallbott & verbal and nonverbal behavior displayed in
Scherer, 1991). the videos. In such a study, simply guessing
The apparent predominance of cogni­ whether the person lied or spoke the truth
tive load processes compared with emo­ would result in an accuracy rate (percent­
tional processes in those suspects is age of correct classifications) of 50%.
perhaps not surprising. Many of the sus­ Vrij (2000a) reviewed 37 of such stud­
pects included in Mann et al.’s (2002) ies. In most studies, the accuracy rates
study have had regular contact with the varied between 45% and 60%, with an
police and were probably familiar with the average accuracy rate of 56.6%, only just
police interview situation, thereby decreas­ above the level of chance. When accuracy
ing their nervousness. Suspects in police at detecting lies was computed separately
interviews are typically of below average from accuracy at detecting truth, results
intelligence, however (Gudjonsson, 2003). showed that observers were reasonably
There is evidence that less intelligent good at detecting truths (correctly judging
people will have particular difficulty in that someone was telling the truth: 67%
inventing plausible and convincing stories accuracy rate) but particularly poor at
(Ekman & Frank, 1993). Alternatively, it detecting lies (correctly judging that some­
might well be that the suspects were more one was lying: 44% accuracy rate). This
tense when they lied but that this was rate is below the level of chance, and
momentarily suppressed when they had to people would be more accurate at detecting
think hard. There is evidence that cognitive lies if they simply guessed.
demand results in an automatic and momen­ The superior accuracy rate for truthful
tary suppression of arousal (Jennings, messages is likely caused at least in part by
1992; Leal, 2005). the truth bias: Judges are more likely to
The absence of clear diagnostic nonver­ consider that messages are truthful than
bal cues to deceit makes detecting deceit a deceptive and, as a result, truthful messages
difficult task. People’s ability to detect are identified with more accuracy than are
deceit and reasons why they sometimes fail deceptive messages. There are at least four
to catch liars will be discussed in the next explanations for the truth bias (Gilbert,
two sections. 1991; Vrij & Mann, 2003). First, in daily
life people are more often confronted with
truthful than with deceptive statements,
♦ Accuracy in Detecting Deceit so they are more inclined to assume that
the behavior they observe is honest (what
LAYPERSONS’ LIE has been labeled the availability heuristic;
DETECTION IN STRANGERS O’Sullivan, Ekman, & Friesen, 1988).
Second, social conversation rules discour­
In a typical lie detection study, observers age people from displaying suspicion. It is
(normally undergraduate students) are often necessary, however, to challenge what
shown video clips of strangers who are the person is saying and ask for more infor­
either lying or telling the truth. They mation to detect deceit (Vrij, 2000a,
are asked to indicate after each clip whether 2004b). Third, people may be unsure as to
18-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 5:10 PM Page 350

350–––◆–––Functions

whether deception is actually occurring. The results of these studies did not sup­
Given this uncertainty, the safest and port the idea that it is easier to detect lies in
most polite strategy may be to believe friends or lovers than in strangers. As rela­
what is expressed overtly (DePaulo, Jordan, tionships become more intimate, partners
Irvine, & Laser, 1982). Fourth, based on a develop a strong tendency to judge the
Spinozan model of knowledge representa­ other, a tendency that has been called the
tion, Gilbert (1991) argues that everything relational truth-bias heuristic (Stiff, Kim, &
is taken to be true initially, and that disbe­ Ramesh, 1992). As the relationship between
lief requires extra effort. In other words, two people intensifies, they often become
the truth bias is the default setting when more confident that they can detect each
interacting with strangers. other’s lies. High levels of confidence tend
Lie detection is not only difficult when to result in the belief that the other person
adults are involved. When adults are asked to would probably not dare to lie, which sub­
detect truths and lies in children with whom sequently reduces the need of trying to dis­
they are not familiar, particularly in situa­ cover whether that person is lying (Levine
tions where they can only rely on the child’s & McCornack, 1992).
verbal and nonverbal behavior, they do not Anderson et al. (1999) provided addi­
perform much better than when they are tional explanations as to why people may
asked to detect lies in adults. Parents, how­ fail to detect deceit in their close friends
ever, seem to be better than other adults at or romantic partners. They suggested that
detecting lies told by their own children (see when close relationship partners attempt
Vrij, 2002, for a review of child deception to detect deceit in each other, they bring
literature). Deception is also harder to spot to mind a great deal of information about
across cultures (e.g., when natives and immi­ each other. This information could be over­
grants in a country interact) than within the whelming, and the lie detector might deal
same culture (see Bond & Rao, 2004, for a with this load by processing the informa­
review of cross-cultural lie detection). tion heuristically instead of searching care­
fully for genuine cues to deceit. Also, in
close relationship interactions, the lie detec­
LAYPERSONS’ LIE DETECTION tor must engage simultaneously in social
IN FRIENDS, PARTNERS, behavior (e.g., the need to appear support­
OR CHILDREN ive in those interactions) and social cogni­
tion (e.g., decoding possible cues to
Boon and McLeod (2001) reported that deception) (Patterson, 1995). This might be
people believe that they are fairly successful too much for the lie detector and, as a
in detecting lies in their partners. They also result, valuable cues may remain unnoticed.
believe that they themselves are more suc­ Finally, it could be that as relationships
cessful in deceiving their partners than their develop, the partners become more
partners are at deceiving them. In several skilled at crafting communications designed
experiments, however, observers were uniquely to fool each other.
shown videotaped truthful or deceptive
statements of people they know, and were
asked to judge the veracity of each state­ PROFESSIONAL LIE CATCHERS
ment (Anderson, Ansfield, & DePaulo,
1999; Vrij, 2000a). As in most studies, Granhag and Vrij (2005) reviewed 10
there was no physical evidence or third- studies in which professional lie catchers,
party information to rely on. such as police officers, participated as
18-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 5:10 PM Page 351

Nonverbal Communication and Deception–––◆–––351

judges. In these studies, the professionals cue uniquely related to deception. The
saw clips of people they did not know, and other reasons are a little more complicated
no physical or third-party evidence was and merit more discussion.
available to them. Granhag and Vrij found
an accuracy rate (lies and truths combined)
of 55%. This accuracy rate is similar to the THE USE OF HEURISTICS
accuracy rate found with students. Results
revealed, however, that professionals are Rather than scrutinizing another’s reac­
equally good at detecting truths (55%) and tions actively for cues to deceit, observers
lies (55%), suggesting that the truth bias, may instead rely on rule of thumb decision
typically found in students, does not seem rules or cognitive heuristics (Levine, Park,
to occur with professional lie catchers. & McCornack, 1999). Such heuristics
In another study, Mann, Vrij, and Bull easily lead to systematic errors and biases.
(2004) showed 99 police officers videotapes Two heuristics that are thought to influence
consisting of a total of 54 truths and lies veracity judgments, the availability heuris­
told by suspects during their police inter­ tic and the relational truth-bias heuristic,
views. These clips were derived from the were discussed earlier. Another heuristic
Mann et al. (2002) study described previ­ sis the probing heuristic (Levine &
ously. Again, no physical or third-party evi­ McCornack, 2001), and it refers to judges’
dence was available to the observers. In this tendency to believe a source more after the
study, however, the officers were capable source has been probed. Receivers often
of detecting, on average, 63% of the truths have a strong belief in the efficacy of prob­
and 66% of the lies. These accuracy rates ing as a lie detection strategy. In cases
were higher than those found in most pre­ where probing does not result in clear signs
vious lie detection studies. One explanation of deceit, and it often will not (Levine &
for this greater accuracy is that the stakes McCornack), the source is more likely to
for the suspects in this study were consider­ be believed. The representativeness heuris­
ably higher than the stakes for the liars and tic (Stiff et al., 1989) refers to the tendency
truth tellers in most lie detection studies, to evaluate a particular reaction (e.g., ner­
and as noted, high-stake lies may be easier vous behavior) as an example of a broader
to detect than low-stake lies (see, Vrij, category (e.g., deception). The expectancy-
2000b, for a review). violation heuristic (Vrij, 2004b) relates to
observers’ tendency to judge reactions that
are odd or infrequent, such as keeping the
♦ Reasons Why Lies eyes closed or staring while speaking, as
deceptive (Bond et al., 1992). In addition,
Remain Undetected
the facial appearance heuristic (Vrij,
2004b) refers to observers’ tendency to
Despite some evidence that certain kinds of judge people with attractive faces or with
lies may be easier to detect, most lies are not a baby-faced appearance as more honest
detected easily. There are numerous reasons (Aune, Levine, Ching, & Yoshimoto, 1993;
why lies often remain undetected (Vrij, Bull & Rumsey, 1988).
2004a, 2004b). Those related to nonverbal Furthermore, O’Sullivan (2003) demon­
communication are discussed here. As strated that the fundamental attribution
already argued, the most basic reason for error (FAE) can undermine lie detection.
the failure to detect lies is that there is no The FAE is the tendency, when forming
single verbal, nonverbal, or physiological impressions of others, to overestimate
18-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 5:10 PM Page 352

352–––◆–––Functions

dispositional causes of that person and beliefs are established, people tend to seek
to underestimate situational causes. When information that confirms rather than dis-
observers believe that someone is generally confirms these beliefs (confirmation bias)
a trustworthy person, they will have the and therefore have a biased evaluation of
tendency to judge that person as truthful new information. Consequently, they will
in any given situation. Similarly, when discount information that contradicts
observers believe that someone is an their beliefs (belief perseverance). These
untrustworthy person, they will be inclined processes result in people thinking that
to judge that person as dishonest in any their beliefs are more grounded in reality
given situation. than they are. In turn, this makes it unlikely
that they will change their beliefs (Granhag
& Vrij, 2005; Strömwall et al., 2004).
LOOKING AT THE WRONG CUES Fourth, it is often difficult for people to
test the accuracy of their beliefs because of
Studies investigating how people think a lack of reliable feedback (i.e., we seldom
liars behave have been carried out world­ learn whether our lie detection strategy is
wide, albeit overwhelmingly with Caucasian accurate). Interestingly, research suggests
participants. These studies suggest that that prisoners have the most accurate
people predominantly expect liars to react beliefs about deception (Hartwig, Granhag,
nervously, with “liars look away” being Strömwall, & Andersson, 2004; Vrij &
the most often mentioned belief (see Bond Semin, 1996). They may well live in a
& Rao, 2004; Strömwall, Granhag, & world that involves a lot of deception.
Hartwig, 2004; Vrij, Akehurst, & Knight, Success in such a culture may depend in
in press; Vrij, 2004b, for reviews). The part on being able to tell when one is the
belief that liars increase their movements is target of deception.
also widespread (Strömwall et al., 2004).
Gaze, however, does not appear to be reli­
ably related to deception (DePaulo et al., PEOPLE DO NOT TAKE
2003; Vrij, 2000a). In addition, liars tend INDIVIDUAL AND SITUATIONAL
to decrease rather than increase their move­ DIFFERENCES INTO ACCOUNT
ments (DePaulo et al., 2003; Vrij, 2000a;
see Table 18.1). There are large individual differences in
There are several reasons why these people’s behavior. People may fail to catch
incorrect beliefs exist. First, because media liars because they do not take such individ­
often promote the idea that liars look away ual differences into account when they
and fidget, many people may have been attempt to detect deceit. People whose
exposed to and absorbed this information. normative behavior looks suspicious are in
Even police manuals often suggest that liars a particularly disadvantageous position.
fidget and look away, which may explain Some individuals’ nonverbal behavior gives
why police officers also endorse these the impression that they are telling the truth
beliefs overwhelmingly (Vrij, 2004b). (honest demeanor bias), whereas others’
Second, people believe mistakenly that they natural behavior leaves the impression that
show nervous behaviors themselves when they are lying (dishonest demeanor bias)
they lie (Vrij, Edward, & Bull, 2001a), and, (Frank & Ekman, 2004; Riggio, Tucker, &
during lie detection, observers may look for Throckmorton, 1988).
cues they mistakenly believe they themselves Such demeanor biases are related to
show while lying. Third, once incorrect personality traits. Expressive people, for
18-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 5:10 PM Page 353

Nonverbal Communication and Deception–––◆–––353

example, exude credibility, regardless of the differences); the same person also behaves
truth of their assertions, because their spon­ differently across situations (intrapersonal
taneity tends to disarm suspicion (Riggio, differences). For example, people show dif­
1986). People with a strong sense of public ferent behaviors when with friends in a
self-consciousness tend to make a less credi­ restaurant than while interviewed for a
ble impression on others. When these indi­ job application. Establishing how people
viduals lie, they are concerned about being behave in the former setting can therefore
scrutinized by others, which changes their not be used to establish whether they are
behavior in such a way that it appears dis­ lying in the latter setting. Yet police manuals
honest. Introverts and socially anxious advise police detectives to establish a sus­
people also tend to make a suspicious pect’s baseline behavior on the initial small
impression. The social clumsiness of many talk part of the interrogation, and to com­
introverts and the impression of tension, pare this with the behavior shown during
nervousness, or fear of socially anxious indi­ the accusational part of the interrogation
viduals are often interpreted by observers as (Inbau, Reid, Buckley, & Jayne, 2001).
indicators of deceit. Interestingly, introverts’
demeanor seems not to reflect their actions
accurately. Specifically, research has found ♦ Discussion
that introverted people lie infrequently
(Kashy & DePaulo, 1996).
Errors in interpreting someone’s behavior This chapter makes clear that the relation­
arise easily in cross-ethnic or cross-cultural ship between deception and nonverbal
interactions because of differences in behav­ communication is complex, and distin­
ior that may be displayed by different groups. guishing accurately between lies and truths
For example, African Americans display is often challenging. Having difficulty with
more gaze aversion generally than do Euro- detecting deceit, however, does not harm
Americans (LaFrance & Mayo, 1976). Such people in many daily life situations. On the
differences are, in part, based on the fact that contrary, being ignorant about the truth
gaze patterns are influenced by culture, and often serves us well. What would we think
that looking into the eyes of a conversation of ourselves if we come to know the truth
partner is regarded as polite in some Western about every flattering comment we receive?
cultures but is considered to be rude in sev­ And how would we respond if other people
eral other cultures (Vrij & Winkel, 1991). In could detect every lie we tell, including our
support of this argument, several experimen­ white lies? Telling lies is very much part
tal studies revealed that behaviors displayed of everyday life (DePaulo et al., 1996;
typically by members of some ethnic groups Goffman, 1959; Vrij, in press) and some­
living in the Netherlands make a suspicious thing that is expected in the smooth work­
impression on Caucasian Dutch police offi­ ing of social relations. Sometimes, however,
cers, a phenomenon labeled cross-cultural it is important to be able to detect lies. For
nonverbal communication errors (see Vrij, example, it would benefit police investiga­
2000a, for an overview of these studies). That tions if police detectives were able to tell
is, nonverbal behavioral patterns normative when suspects are lying and when they are
for certain ethnic groups may be interpreted telling the truth during police interviews.
by members of other ethnicities as revealing There are opportunities for improving lie
attempts to hide the truth. detection ability by paying attention to
Not only do different people behave dif­ people’s behavior. To conclude this chap­
ferently in the same situation (interpersonal ter, I will discuss these ideas.
18-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 5:10 PM Page 354

354–––◆–––Functions

PAY ATTENTION TO might be the result of conversation rules,


DIAGNOSTIC CUES which regulate politeness. It is impolite or
undesirable typically to accuse someone of
Observers can become more accurate being a liar (e.g., “I do not believe you”),
when they are trained to attend to more but it might be possible to challenge the
diagnostic cues (Frank & Feeley, 2003). words of a speaker more subtly (e.g., “Do
Determining the greatest number of diag­ you really like that person so much?”).
nostic cues may, however, be difficult. One Alternatively, people might look at dif­
means for doing so is to examine the strate­ ferent cues when detecting lies than when
gies that good lie detectors use. For applying an indirect method. In Vrij,
example, Frank and Ekman (1997) found Edward, and Bull’s (2001b) lie detection
that accurate lie detectors appear to be experiment, half of the sample of police
good at spotting facial micro-expressions of officers was asked to indicate whether or
emotion. Other studies have revealed that not certain people were lying. The other
good lie detectors report paying attention half was asked to indicate whether or not
to vocal cues while attempting to detect these people had to think hard while mak­
deceit (Anderson, DePaulo, Ansfield, ing their statements. Police officers could
Tickle, & Green, 1999; Feeley & Young, only distinguish between truths and lies by
2000). This finding matches well with using the latter, implicit, method. Moreover,
DePaulo et al.’s (2003) meta-analysis, only in the implicit method did they pay
which showed that many of the cues distin­ attention to the cues that actually discrimi­
guishing liars from truth tellers are vocal nated between the truth tellers and liars,
cues. Other studies have shown that people such as a decrease in hand movements.
become better lie detectors when they can­ In the explicit method they paid attention
not see the person’s face (DePaulo, Stone, to stereotypical, but unreliable, cues to
& Lassiter, 1985). Lie detectors are inclined deceit such as an increase in foot and leg
to look at someone’s eye movements when movements.
they are available to them, even though eye
movements are quite easy to control and
not related to deception in most cases. COMPARABLE TRUTH

Because of individual differences in


IMPLICIT LIE DETECTION nonverbal behavior, the response of one
individual in a specific situation cannot be
There is evidence that people are better used as a comparison to judge the veracity
lie detectors when they are asked indirectly of the response of another person in the
whether or not someone is lying (DePaulo same situation. Intraindividual comparisons
& Morris, 2004). In some studies, after (comparing the responses of the same
watching a truthful or deceptive story, par­ person in different situations) are, in that
ticipants were asked to detect deception respect, better, as they control for individual
both in a direct way (i.e., Is the person differences in responses. For this technique
lying?) and in an indirect way (i.e., Does the to be effective, however, it is essential that
speaker sincerely like the person he or she appropriate comparisons are made (Vrij &
just described?). These studies found Mann, 2003) and that the known truthful
greater accuracy when using the indirect response to be used as a comparison (base­
measures (see Vrij, 2001, for a review). This line) is truly comparable with the response
18-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 5:10 PM Page 355

Nonverbal Communication and Deception–––◆–––355

under investigation. Thus, if the response instantly the nonverbal behavior displayed
under investigation is a high-stakes situa­ by target persons (see Vrij, 2004b).
tion, then the baseline situation should also
be a high-stakes situation. Moreover,
there is evidence that some behaviors are INTERVIEWING TO
topic related. While analyzing the behavior DETECT DECEPTION
shown by Saddam Hussein in an interview
with Peter Arnett during the First Gulf War Mann et al.’s (2002) examination of
(the interview was broadcasted on CNN), real-life police interviews, discussed earlier,
Davis and Hadiks (1995) found that the suggested that when suspects lie, they
topic Hussein discussed was related to the experience high cognitive demand. Police
illustrators he made. Only when discussing interviewers could use this knowledge by
Israel and Zionism did Hussein make spe­ employing interview techniques that increase
cific movements with his left forearm. Likely, the cognitive demand in suspects. This
Israel has a special meaning to an Arab should have a greater effect on liars than on
leader, and issues such as personal involve­ truth tellers, thus facilitating discrimination
ment should be taken into account as well. between them. There are several ways in
Vrij and Mann (2001) employed the which cognitive demand could be increased.
comparable truth technique in an actual For example, suspects could be asked to
police interview. During a videotaped real- elaborate on or repeat what they have just
life police interview, a man suspected and said. Using this technique, lies could well
later convicted of murder was asked to fail if the liar did not plan the lie in suffi­
describe his activities on a particular day. cient detail.
The murder suspect described his activities Moreover, liars tend to speak of events
during the morning (went to work), after­ in a more fixed chronological order (this
noon (visited a market), and evening (vis­ happened first, and then this, and then that,
ited a neighbor). Detailed analyses of the etc.) than truth tellers. It has been suggested
videotape revealed a sudden change in that it is difficult for liars to fabricate a
behavior as soon as he started to describe story in a nonchronological order (Vrij,
his activities during the afternoon and 2005). Lie detectors could exploit this diffi­
evening. One reason for this change may culty by asking interviewees to tell their
have been that he was lying and evidence stories in a nonchronological order, for
supported this view. Police investigations example, in reverse order. Police officers
could confirm his story with regard to his could also use the evidence they have
morning activities but revealed that his against a suspect strategically. Inbau et al.
statements about the afternoon and even­ (2001), for example, advise the police to
ing were fabricated. In reality, he met the present such evidence at the beginning of
victim in the afternoon and killed her later the interview (e.g., “Our CCTV footage
on that day. In this case, my colleague and shows that you were in Commercial Road
I were able to make a good comparison. on Saturday evening at 8 p.m.”). The task
The comparable truth method would bene­ the lying suspect then faces is to fabricate
fit, however, from instant, accurate, and an alibi that is consistent with this factual
detailed coding of all sorts of behaviors evidence. Alternatively, the police do not
shown by a target person. Progress is being reveal the evidence initially but first let the
made in developing technical equipment suspect talk about his whereabouts. In an
that can observe nonintrusively and code experiment where the timing of presenting
18-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 5:10 PM Page 356

356–––◆–––Functions

the evidence was manipulated (it was expectancy violation. Journal of Personality
presented either before or after the intervie­ and Social Psychology, 63, 969–977.
wee was given the opportunity to discuss Bond, C. F., & Rao, S. R. (2004). Lies travel:
his or her activities) lies were more readily Mendacity in a mobile world. In P. A.
Granhag & L. A. Strömwall (Eds.), Deception
detected by observers when the evidence
detection in forensic contexts (pp. 127–147).
was presented at a later stage (Hartwig,
Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.
Granhag, Strömwall, & Vrij, 2005).
Boon, S. D., & McLeod, B. A. (2001). Deception
People have considerable difficulty in in romantic relationships: Subjective esti­
accurately distinguishing between truths mates of success at deceiving and attitudes
and lies, among other factors, because they toward deception. Journal of Social and
often fail to take individual and situational Personal Relationships, 18, 463–476.
differences into account; attend to the Bull, R., & Rumsey, N. (1988). The social psy­
wrong, nondiagnostic, cues, and use heuris­ chology of facial appearance. New York:
tics that lead to systematic errors and biases. Springer-Verlag.
It is argued in this chapter, however, that Buller, D. B., & Burgoon, J. K. (1996).
people could become better lie detectors if Interpersonal deception theory. Communi­
cation Theory, 6, 203–242.
they used alternative lie detection strategies.
Buller, D. B., Stiff, J. B., & Burgoon, J. K.
(1996). Behavioral adaptation in deceptive
transactions. Human Communication
♦ References Research, 22, 589–603.
Burgoon, J. K., Buller, D. B., Floyd, K., &
Grandpre, J. (1996). Deceptive realities:
Anderson, D. E., Ansfield, M. E., & DePaulo, B. Sender, receiver, and observer perspectives
M. (1999). Love’s best habit: Deception in in deceptive conversations. Communication
the context of relationships. In P. Philippot, Research, 23, 724–748.
R. S. Feldman, & E. J. Coats (Eds.), Burgoon, J. K., Buller, D. B., White, C. H.,
The social context of nonverbal behavior Afifi, W., & Buslig, A. L. S. (1999). The
(pp. 372–409). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge role of conversation involvement in decep­
University Press. tive interpersonal interactions. Personality
Anderson, D. E., DePaulo, B. M., Ansfield, M. and Social Psychology Bulletin, 25,
E., Tickle, J. J., & Green, E. (1999). Beliefs 669–685.
about cues to deception: Mindless stereo­ Caso, L., Maricchiolo, F., Bonaiuto, M., Vrij,
types or untapped wisdom? Journal of A., & Mann, S. (2006). The impact of
Nonverbal Behavior, 23, 67–89. deception and suspicion on different hand
Aune, R. K., Levine, T., Ching, P., & movements. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior,
Yoshimoto, J. (1993). The influence of per­ 30, 1–19.
ceived source reward value and attributions Chartrand, T. L., & Bargh, J. A. (1999). The
of deception. Communication Research chameleon effect: The perception-behavior
Reports, 10, 15–27. link and social interaction. Journal of
Bavelas, J. B., Chovil, N., Coates, L., & Roe, L. Personality and Social Psychology, 76,
(1995). Gestures specialized for dialogue. 893–910.
Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, Cohen, J. (1977). Statistical power analysis
21, 394–405. for the behavioral sciences. New York:
Bond, C. F., & DePaulo, B. M. (2005). Accuracy Academic Press.
of deception judgments. Unpublished Davis, M., & Hadiks, D. (1995). Demeanor and
manuscript. credibility. Semiotica, 106, 5–54.
Bond, C. F., Omar, A., Pitre, U., Lashley, B. R., DePaulo, B. M. (1992). Nonverbal behavior and
Skaggs, L. M., & Kirk, C. T. (1992). Fishy- self-presentation. Psychological Bulletin,
looking liars: Deception judgment from 111, 203–243.
18-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 5:10 PM Page 357

Nonverbal Communication and Deception–––◆–––357

DePaulo, B. M., & Friedman, H. S. (1998). and deception in everyday life (pp. 184–
Nonverbal communication. In D. T. Gilbert, 201). New York: Guildford Press.
S. T. Fiske, & G. Lindzey (Eds.), The hand­ Ekman, P., & Friesen, W. V. (1969). The reper­
book of social psychology (pp. 3–40). toire of nonverbal behavior. Semiotica, 1,
Boston, MA: McGraw-Hill. 49–98.
DePaulo, B. M., Jordan, A., Irvine, A., & Laser, Ekman, P., & Friesen, W. V. (1972). Hand
P. S. (1982). Age changes in the detection movements. Journal of Communication,
of deception. Child Development, 53, 22, 353–374.
701–709. Ekman, P., Friesen, W. V., & O’Sullivan, M.
DePaulo, B. M., Kashy, D. A., Kirkendol, S. E., (1988). Smiles when lying. Journal of
Wyer, M. M., & Epstein, J. A. (1996). Lying Personality and Social Psychology, 54,
in everyday life. Journal of Personality and 414–420.
Social Psychology, 70, 979–995. Elaad, E. (2003). Effects of feedback on the
DePaulo, B. M., & Kirkendol, S. E. (1989). The overestimated capacity to detect lies and the
motivational impairment effect in the com­ underestimated ability to tell lies. Applied
munication of deception. In J. C. Yuille Cognitive Psychology, 17, 349–363.
(Ed.), Credibility assessment (pp. 51–70). Feeley, T. H., & Young, M. J. (2000). The
Dordrecht, The Netherlands: Kluwer. effects of cognitive capacity on beliefs about
DePaulo, B. M., Lindsay, J. L., Malone, B. E., deceptive communication. Communication
Muhlenbruck, L., Charlton, K., & Cooper, Quarterly, 48, 101–119.
H. (2003). Cues to deception. Psychological Frank, M. G., & Ekman, P. (1997). The ability to
Bulletin, 129, 74–118. detect deceit generalizes across different types
DePaulo, B. M., & Morris, W. L. (2004). of high-stake lies. Journal of Personality and
Discerning lies from truths: Behavioral cues Social Psychology, 72, 1429–1439.
to deception and the indirect pathway Frank, M. G., & Ekman, P. (2004). Appearing
of intuition. In P. A. Granhag & L. A. truthful generalizes across different decep­
Strömwall (Eds.), Deception detection in tion situations. Journal of Personality and
forensic contexts (pp. 15–40). Cambridge, Social Psychology, 86, 486–495.
UK: Cambridge University Press. Frank, M. G., & Ekman, P., & Friesen, W. V.
DePaulo, B. M., Stone, J. L., & Lassiter, G. D. (1993). Behavioral markers and recogniz­
(1985). Deceiving and detecting deceit. In ability of the smile of enjoyment. Journal of
B. R. Schlenker (Ed.), The self and social Personality and Social Psychology, 64,
life (pp. 323–370). New York: McGraw- 83–93.
Hill. Frank, M. G., & Feeley, T. H. (2003). To catch
Doherty-Sneddon, G., Bruce, V., Bonner, L., a liar: Challenges for research in lie detec­
Longbotham, S., & Doyle, C. (2002). tion. Journal of Applied Communication
Development of gaze aversion as disengage­ Research, 31, 58–75.
ment of visual information. Developmental Friesen, W. V., Ekman, P., & Wallbott, H.
Psychology, 38, 438–445. (1979). Measuring hand movements.
Efron, D. (1941). Gesture and environment. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 4, 97–112.
New York: King’s Crown. Gilbert, D. T. (1991). How mental systems
Ekman, P. (2001). Telling lies. New York: W. believe. American Psychologist, 46,
W. Norton. (Original work published 107–119.
1985) Goffman, E. (1959). The presentation of self in
Ekman, P., Davidson, R. J., & Friesen, W. V. everyday life. New York: Doubleday.
(1990). The Duchenne smile: Emotional Goldman-Eisler, F. (1968). Psycholinguistics:
expression and brain physiology II. Journal Experiments in spontaneous speech. New
of Personality and Social Psychology, 58, York: Doubleday.
342–353. Granhag, P. A., & Vrij, A. (2005). Deception
Ekman, P., & Frank, M. G. (1993). Lies that detection. In N. Brewer & K. Williams
fail. In M. Lewis & C. Saarni (Eds.), Lying (Eds.), Psychology and law: An empirical
18-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 5:10 PM Page 358

358–––◆–––Functions

perspective (pp. 43–92). New York: heuristic-based explanations of the probing


Guilford Press. effect. Human Communication Research,
Gudjonsson, G. H. (2003). The psychology of 27, 471–502.
interrogations and confessions: A hand­ Levine, T. R., Park, H. S., & McCornack, S. A.
book. Chichester, UK: Wiley. (1999). Accuracy in detecting truths
Harrigan, J. A., & O’Connell, D. M. (1996). Facial and lies: Documenting the “veracity
movements during anxiety states. Personality effect.” Communication Monographs, 66,
and Individual Differences, 21, 205–212. 125–144.
Hartwig, M., Granhag, P. A., Strömwall, L. A., Mann, S., Vrij, A., & Bull, R. (2002). Suspects,
Andersson, L. O. (2004). Suspicious minds: lies and videotape: An analysis of authen­
Criminals’ ability to detect deception. Psy­ tic high-stakes liars. Law and Human
chology, Crime, and Law, 10, 83–95. Behaviour, 26, 365–376.
Hartwig, M., Granhag, P. A., Strömwall, L. A., Mann, S., Vrij, A., & Bull, R. (2004). Detecting
& Vrij, A. (2005). The strategic use of true lies: Police officers’ ability to detect
disclosing evidence. Law and Human deceit. Journal of Applied Psychology, 89,
Behavior, 29, 469–484. 137–149.
Hocking, J. E., & Leathers, D. G. (1980). McNeill, D. (1985). So you think gestures are
Nonverbal indicators of deception: A new nonverbal? Psychological Review, 92,
theoretical perspective. Communication 350–371.
Monographs, 47, 119–131. McNeill, D. (1992). Hand and mind. Chicago:
Inbau, F. E., Reid, J. E., Buckley, J. P., & Jayne, B. University of Chicago Press.
C. (2001). Criminal interrogation and con­ O’Sullivan, M. (2003). The fundamental attri­
fessions (4th ed.). Gaithersburg, MD: Aspen. bution error in detecting deception: The
Jennings, J. R. (1992). Is it important that the boy-who-cried-wolf-effect. Personality and
mind is in a body? Inhibition and the heart. Social Psychology Bulletin, 29, 1316–1327.
Psychophysiology, 29, 369–383. O’Sullivan, M., Ekman, P., & Friesen, W. V.
Kashy, D. A., & DePaulo, B. M. (1996). Who (1988). The effect of comparisons on
lies? Journal of Personality and Social detecting deceit. Journal of Nonverbal
Psychology, 70, 1037–1051. Behavior, 12, 203–216.
LaFrance, M., & Mayo, C. (1976). Racial differ­ Park, H. S., Levine, T. R., McCornack, S. A.,
ences in gaze behavior during conversations: Morrisson, K., & Ferrara, M. (2002). How
Two systematic observational studies. Journal people really detect lies. Communication
of Personality and Social Psychology, 33, Monographs, 69, 144–157.
547–552. Patterson, M. L. (1995). Invited article: A parallel
Leal, S. (2005). Central and peripheral physiol­ process model of nonverbal communication.
ogy of attention and cognitive demand: Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 19, 3–29.
Understanding how brain and body work Riggio, R. E. (1986). Assessment of basic social
together. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, skills. Journal of Personality and Social
University of Portsmouth, England. Psychology, 51, 649–660.
Levine, T. R., & McCornack, S. A. (1992). Riggio, R.E., Tucker, J., & Throckmorton, B.
Linking love and lies: A formal test of the (1988). Social skills and deception ability.
McCornack and Parks model of deception Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin,
detection. Journal of Social and Personal 13, 568–577.
Relationships, 9, 143–154. Stiff, J. B., Kim, H. J., & Ramesh, C. N. (1992).
Levine, T. R., & McCornack, S. A. (1996). A crit­ Truth biases and aroused suspicion in rela­
ical analysis of the behavioral adaptation tional deception. Communication Research,
explanation of the probing effect. Human 19, 326–345.
Communication Research, 22, 575–588. Stiff, J. B., Miller, G. R., Sleight, C., Mongeau,
Levine, T. R., & McCornack, S. A. (2001). P., Garlick, R., & Rogan, R. (1989).
Behavioral adaptation, confidence, and Explanations for visual cue primacy in
18-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 5:10 PM Page 359

Nonverbal Communication and Deception–––◆–––359

judgments of honesty and deceit. Journal Vrij, A., Edward, K., & Bull, R. (2001a).
of Personality and Social Psychology, 56, People’s insight into their own behaviour
555–564. and speech content while lying. British
Strömwall. L. A., Granhag, P. A., & Hartwig, M. Journal of Psychology, 92, 373–389.
(2004). Practitioners’ beliefs about decep­ Vrij, A., Edward, K., & Bull, R. (2001b). Police
tion. In P. A. Granhag & L. A. Strömwall officers’ ability to detect deceit: The benefit of
(Eds.), Deception detection in forensic con­ indirect deception detection measures. Legal
texts (pp. 229–250). Cambridge, UK: and Criminological Psychology, 6, 185–197.
Cambridge University Press. Vrij, A., & Mann, S. (2001). Telling and detect­
Vrij, A. (2000a). Detecting lies and deceit: The ing lies in a high-stake situation: The case
psychology of lying and its implications for of a convicted murderer. Applied Cognitive
professional practice. Chichester: Wiley. Psychology, 15, 187–203.
Vrij, A. (2000b). Telling and detecting lies as a Vrij, A., & Mann, S. (2003). Telling and detect­
function of raising the stakes. In C. M. ing true lies: Investigating and detecting the
Breur, M. M. Kommer, J. F. Nijboer, & lies of murderers and thieves during police
J. M. Reintjes (Eds.), New trends in criminal interviews. In M. Verhallen, G. Verkaeke,
investigation and evidence II (pp. 699–709). P. J. van Koppen, & J. Goethals (Eds.),
Antwerpen, Belgium: Intersentia. Much ado about crime: Chapters on psy­
Vrij, A. (2001). Implicit lie detection. The chology and law (pp. 185–208). Brussels,
Psychologist, 14, 58–60. Belgium: Uitgeverij Politeia.
Vrij, A. (2002). Deception in children: A Vrij, A., & Mann, S. (2005). Police use of non­
literature review and implications for verbal behavior as indicators of deception.
children’s testimony. In H. Westcott, G. In R. E. Riggio & R. S. Feldman (Eds.),
Davies, & R. Bull (Eds.), Children’s testi­ Applications of nonverbal communication
mony (pp. 175–194). Chichester: Wiley. (pp. 63–94). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Vrij, A. (2004a). Guidelines to catch a liar. In Vrij, A., & Semin, G. R. (1996). Lie experts’
P. A. Granhag & L. A. Strömwall (Eds.), beliefs about nonverbal indicators of decep­
Deception detection in forensic contexts tion. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 20,
(pp. 287–314). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge 65–80.
University Press. Vrij, A., & Winkel, F. W. (1991). Cultural pat­
Vrij, A. (2004b). Invited article: Why profes­ terns in Dutch and Surinam nonverbal
sionals fail to catch liars and how they behaviour: An analysis of simulated police/
can improve. Legal and Criminological citizen encounters. Journal of Nonverbal
Psychology, 9, 159–181. Behavior, 15, 169–184.
Vrij, A. (2005). Criteria-based content analysis: Wallbott, H. G., & Scherer, K. R. (1991). Stress
A qualitative review of the first 37 studies. specifics: Differential effects of coping style,
Psychology, Public Policy, and Law, 11, gender, and type of stressor on automatic
3–41. arousal, facial expression, and subjective
Vrij, A. (in press). Deception: A social lubricant feeling. Journal of Personality and Social
and a selfish act. In K. Fiedler (Ed.), Psychology, 61, 147–156.
Frontiers of social psychology: Social Zuckerman, M., DePaulo, B. M., & Rosenthal,
communication. New York: Psychology R. (1981). Verbal and nonverbal communi­
Press. cation of deception. In L. Berkowitz (Ed.),
Vrij, A., Akehurst, L., & Knight, S. (in press). Advances in experimental social psychology
Police officers’, social workers’, teachers’ (Vol. 14, pp. 1–57). New York: Academic
and general public’s beliefs about deception Press.
in children, adolescents and adults. Legal
and Criminological Psychology.
18-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 5:10 PM Page 360
19-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 5:12 PM Page 361

19
THE INTERACTION MANAGEMENT
FUNCTION OF NONVERBAL CUES
Theory and Research About Mutual
Behavioral Influence in Face-to-Face Settings

� Joseph N. Cappella
University of Pennsylvania

� Darren M. Schreiber
University of California, San Diego

T here are two important senses in which conversations are regu­


lated. The more typical connotation of the word regulate implies
that a person seeks intentionally to alter the content, tenor, or events of
a conversation toward some preordained end. Regulation of this type
exhibits control in the sense that actions are undertaken to achieve what
one perceives to be an important need or purpose. Such conversational
behaviors are sometimes called “deliberate.” The second sense assumes
that regulation of interaction is more “automatic” (i.e., weighed less
cognitively; for more on this topic, see Lakin, this volume).
The research that will be reviewed in this chapter will show that this
automatic sense of management involves control over the more micro­
scopic events during interaction. People are, in general, quite unaware
that such influences exist and, under most circumstances, do not
employ such responses intentionally (Berger & Roloff, 1980; Langer,
1978). More generally, this chapter updates recent reviews of the

◆ 361
19-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 5:12 PM Page 362

362–––◆–––Functions

patterns of behavioral coordination that some linguistic behaviors have been


characterize social interaction and focuses included as well (see especially Burgoon
on the explanations behind these patterns. et al., 1995; Cappella, 1994). This section
We will show that recent research has will not repeat the content of these prior
added to the fact base about coordination detailed reviews but instead will report
in ways that strengthen and extend previ­ selectively on key studies that advance the
ous research while, at the same time, offer­ research base on coordination.
ing some new empirical puzzles that need Recent research in three broad arenas
resolving. We will explore some possible pertinent to mutual coordination of behav­
answers beyond those available in the ior will be presented in this section: studies
current theoretical literature. of (1) mutual coordination, (2) the relation­
ship between behavioral coordination and
outcome for the relationship or social group,
♦ Coordination in Interaction and (3) the first two types but focused on
infants and children. In contrast to previous
reviews, this one will not focus on behav­
Interaction is not simply the generation
ioral coordination exclusively but will
of social symbols or signals; neither is it
expand to include emotional contagion in
reducible solely to the reception or interpre­
the sense of yoked emotional response (i.e.,
tation of such symbols or signals. In this
self-reported affect) among interactants.
chapter, interaction is conceptualized as the
This expansion recognizes that behavioral
regularized patterns of messages from one
coordination in emotions may also produce
person that influence the messages sent in
emotional synchrony, which in turn can be
turn by the other over and above what they
consequential in relational connection and
would otherwise be (Cappella, 1985). This
performance.
definition emphasizes the pattern of exchange
between two persons and not the behavior of
one or the other person, even though that
MUTUAL COORDINATION
behavior occurred in the context of an inter­
action with another. These regularized pat­
A substantial body of research indicates
terns are labeled coordination.
that social interactions exhibit mutual coor­
dination for behaviors as diverse as accents,
♦ Research on Coordination speech rate, vocal intensity, postural and
gestural behaviors, movement, gaze, facial
affect, self-disclosure, excuses, and other
Numerous reviews of the literature on behaviors (Cappella, 1981, 1985, 1998).
coordination in adult dyads and adult-infant The variety of behaviors implicated is testi­
dyads have appeared. These include reviews mony to the centrality of this process, and
by Cappella (1981, 1991, 1993, 1994) and the mechanisms behind it, in human behav­
by Burgoon (1978, 1993, 1994) and her col­ ior. Recent research in this area has devel­
leagues (Burgoon, Stern, & Dillman, 1995), oped in two directions: one increasingly
among others (Bernieri & Rosenthal, 1991; microscopic and physiological and the
Hatfield, Cacioppo, & Rapson, 1994; Hess, other more macroscopic and less behav­
Philippott, & Blairy, 1999; Patterson, 1976, ioral. Both developments are welcome.
1982, 1999). These reviews have focused on The first moves from the study of non­
vocal and kinesic behaviors primarily, but verbal behaviors, such as eye gaze, that
19-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 5:12 PM Page 363

The Interaction Management Function of Nonverbal Cues–––◆–––363

are often “multifunctional.” These behav­ M. levator labii, the frontal M. lateralis, and
iors often have several interpretations the M. corrugator supercilii muscle regions
depending on the context. The physiolo­ were obtained as were emotional experi­
gical responses are less susceptible— ences. The results revealed that people both
but certainly not immune—to interpretive mimicked and experienced an emotion sim­
variation. The less behavioral, more macro­ ilar to that expressed by the stimulus person.
scopic behaviors, move the research on The focus on coordination in emotions
coordination into a different research has extended to vocal emotion. Neuman
domain (e.g., the role of emotional and and Strack (2000) had people listen
other types of contagion in groups, organi­ to philosophical essays read in a slightly
zations, and other social units). The impli­ happy or slightly sad tone of voice.
cation of these two developments is to Listeners reported a more positive mood
direct that theories be capable of explaining with the happier reading and a more
both the automatic responses characteristic negative mood with the sadder reading. In
of physiological coordination and the a second study, they also repeated the essay
more subjective, deliberate processes of in a tone that independent raters found
emotional yoking between partners, per­ to be happier or sadder depending on
haps with expressive imitation mediating hearing the slightly happy or sad initial
the subjective yoking of emotional experi­ rendition—a kind of vocal coordination
ence. The following details examples of of emotion. Additionally, Hietanen et al.
research with these characteristics. (1998) obtained facial EMG responses to
The first of these concerns microcoordi­ vocal affect expressions as participants lis­
nation. Electromyography (EMG) is the tened to single words uttered by two actors
study of facial muscle activity in response stimulating different emotions. Three cate­
to various stimuli. Visual stimuli can elicit gories of expressions were selected: emo­
facial muscle activity depending on the tional neutrality, anger, and contentment.
affective valence of the stimuli to the The EMG activity over two facial muscle
subject. For example, studies suggest that regions was measured: corrugator supercilii
stimuli related to positive affect increase (the muscle that knits the brows together)
activity in the cheek muscle region— and orbicularis oculi (the muscle that pro­
smiling—and stimuli related to negative duces bagging below the eyes and wrinkles
affect increase activity in the brow muscle in the corners of the eyes). Hearing the
region—frowning (Hietanen, Surakka, & expression of anger increased EMG activity
Linnankoski, 1998). Some studies have in the participants’ brow region more than
investigated facial electromyographic respon­ did hearing the expression of contentment.
ses during actual interaction. Lundqvist In contrast, the expression of contentment
(1995), for example, explored whether activated the periocular muscle region more
people exposed to facial expressions than did anger. The results support the view
responded with specific facial muscle that negative and positive affects are “con­
reaction patterns that correspond to spe­ tagious” from hearing human vocal affect
cific emotional experiences. Participants expressions.
were shown pictures of faces expressing The linkage between emotional expres­
sadness, anger, fear, surprise, disgust, sion and felt emotion within the person is
happiness, as well as neutral facial expres­ an important set of facts that theories must
sions. At the same time, facial electromyo­ explain. In interpersonal contexts, if person
graphs from the M. zygomaticus major, the A mimics person B and if both A and B
19-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 5:12 PM Page 364

364–––◆–––Functions

experience emotion consistent with their depression between spouses where one was
facial displays, then we might reasonably primarily the caretaker and one the care­
expect coordination between A and B in giver. Depression scores for the care receiver
their subjective experiences of emotional tended to determine those of the caretaker
or mood. This coordination in subjective at a later point in time, suggesting coordina­
emotion and mood is examined next. tion but with the caregiver dominant. The
Specifically, Totterdell, Kellett, Teuchmann, opposite influences were not found with
and Briner (1998) explored “mood link­ regard to depression or a variety of other
age” in the context of work groups. They emotional states. Specifically, Goodman and
investigated whether people’s moods are Shippy (2002) studied elderly spouses where
influenced by the collective mood of their one was experiencing serious vision prob­
work teammates over time. lems. After controlling for other factors,
In their first study, a time-series analysis depression by one spouse predicted the
showed a significant association between partner’s depression.
the nurses’ moods and the collective mood Individual differences in sensitivity to
of their teammates independent of shared emotions from others and the ability to trans­
hassles. In their second study, a team of mit emotions to others may affect the exis­
accountants rated their own moods and the tence and strength of observed contagion.
moods of their teammates three times a day Verbeke (1997) explored whether these
for 4 weeks using pocket computers. The individual differences are assets or liabilities
accountants’ moods and their judgments of over the long term for salespersons. Doherty
their teammates’ moods were significantly (1997) explored the individual differences in
associated with the collective mood of susceptibility to emotional coordination by
their teammates. The findings suggest that crafting an Emotional Contagion Scale, a
people’s moods can become linked to the 15-item measure of individual differences in
mood of their coworkers, offering a com­ susceptibility to catching others’ emotions.
pelling line of evidence for affective or, Participants were videotaped while watching
in this case, mood synchrony during adult videotapes of emotionally expressive stimulus
interaction. persons relating their happiest and saddest
Similar effects have been reported for memories. Doherty found that susceptibility
teacher burnout (i.e., emotional exhaus­ to emotional contagion was positively related
tion and depersonalization; Bakker & to reactivity, emotionality, sensitivity to
Schaufeli, 2001), in sales contexts (Verbeke, others, social functioning, and self-esteem.
1997), and in clinical environments (Hsee, Doherty, Orimoto, Singelis, Hatfield, and
Hatfield, Carlson, & Chemtob, 1992). Hebb (1995) showed further that women
Thompson, Nadler, and Kim (1999), for in a variety of occupations illustrated
example, argued that the ability of negotia­ higher total emotional contagion scores than
tors at the bargaining table is enhanced to did men.
the extent that they are successful in per­ Findings on mood transfer observed
ceiving emotions of participants, reacting in more applied settings have also been
appropriately to them, and being “in tune” obtained in more controlled environments
with those emotions. Pugh (1998) found (Gump & Kulik, 1997). In one study
that in a service context sales people were (Neuman & Strack, 2000), participants
more effective when they were emotionally were tested on their listening comprehen­
congruent with their customers. Furthermore, sion in response to a neutral text that was
Ingram (1997) studied the coordination of read to them in a happy or sad tone of
19-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 5:12 PM Page 365

The Interaction Management Function of Nonverbal Cues–––◆–––365

voice. Listeners reported being in an (but see Ullstadius, 1998, who offered
emotional state that was like that of the contrary evidence for the imitation of
read materials and when required to repeat tongue protrusions and mouth openings in
portions of the text read to them, employed 18 newborn infants). Other studies support
a vocal tone similar to the one they had the claim of mutual coordination between
heard. Similar findings by Hess and Blairy infants and adults. Stack and Arnold
(2001) suggest that aspects of facial mim­ (1998), for example, focused on maternal
icry of emotion may account for the subjec­ touch and its ability to influence infants’
tive emotional reactions that viewers of the gaze and affect during interactions. The
videotaped faces reported. Direct evidence results from this study indicated that (1)
of mediation from facial stimuli to facial infants were sensitive to subtle changes
mimicry by viewers to reports of emotional in maternal tactile-gestural behavior, (2)
state by viewers was not obtained. These maternal touch and hand gestures can
reactions to facial displays are likely to attract infants’ attention to their faces even
occur quickly (presentations less than half when still and expressionless, and (3) there
a second) and exhibit a dose-response rela­ were associations between infant expres­
tionship with more intense displays eliciting siveness and gaze at mothers’ faces and
more intense emotional reactions (Wild, hands during these periods.
Erb, & Bartels, 2001). Symons and Moran (1994) extended the
idea of maternal influence to mutual influ­
Summary. Overall, four conclusions can be ence—that is, observing infants’ smiling
drawn from the recent literature: (1) rela­ behavior as being both responsive to and
tively automatic responses to emotional dependent on maternal smiling behavior.
stimuli are manifest in facial and vocal reac­ Twenty-five mothers were observed engaged
tions; (2) these automatic reactions are in face-to-face interactions with their 8-,
sometimes accompanied by subjective feel­ 12-, 16-, and 20-week-old infants, the ages
ings of emotion; (3) mood and emotional at which face-to-face interaction is most
contagion in subjective experience—emo­ common. Maternal dependency and infant
tional yoking—is common in applied and responsiveness were not found to have
more controlled settings; and (4) the ques­ occurred at significant levels, but maternal
tion of which mechanisms might account responsiveness and infant dependency
for emotional yoking is an open question, were, and at all ages. Although mothers
although the possibility of expressive mim­ were responsive to their infants, mothers
icry is suggested. smiled a lot independently of their infants’
smiling behavior; hence, infant behavior
is sufficient but not necessary to elicit
MUTUAL COORDINATION IN smiles from the mother. Infants responded
INFANT-MOTHER INTERACTION to their mothers’ smiles with smiles of
their own. The proportion of mother
Studies reviewed by Cappella (1997, smiles followed by infant smiles did not
1998) and Field (1987) show that infants exceed the levels expected by chance.
weeks and, in a few cases, even hours old Mother smiling behavior seems necessary,
adapt to their adult partners in vocal, gaze, but not sufficient, to elicit smiles from the
facial, and movement behaviors. Such evi­ infants.
dence underscores the centrality of mutual In an important, and related, line of
coordination in human social interaction study, empathic responsiveness and affective
19-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 5:12 PM Page 366

366–––◆–––Functions

reactivity to infant stimuli were studied in interaction video exhibited more behavioral
mothers at high and low risk for physical co-occurrences between infant and mother
child abuse (Milner, Halsey, & Fultz, involving vocal, touch, and gaze behaviors.
1995). Compared with baseline, high-risk A meta-analysis tracked the value of inter­
mothers reported no change in empathy ventions in improving parental sensitivity
across infant conditions such as baseline, in interacting with their infants across
smiling, quiet, and crying. Low-risk moth­ available studies (Bakermans-Kranenburg,
ers did report an increase in empathy. van Ijzendoorn, & Juffer, 2003). Inter­
Following the presentation of a crying ventions that altered parental sensitivity
infant, high-risk mothers reported more also had favorable consequences on subse­
distress and hostility. These data agree with quent attachment, adding evidence to the
other studies showing that child abusers are claim that responsive interactions are impor­
less empathic and more hostile in response tant for the development of attachment.
to a crying child. The absence of responsive interaction in
Although these conclusions are com­ the early weeks and months of an infant’s
pelling, one of the problems with many of life can be deleterious to the infant’s devel­
the studies of infant-adult interaction is that opment in the absence of other buffer­
the samples are usually small and unrepre­ ing social and psychological conditions.
sentative of the population at large. The Despite this understanding, many parents
National Institute of Child Health and may not be responsive to their child. There
Human Development (NICHD), however, are many reasons for lower responsive­
has undertaken a large-scale study of more ness by parents, including depression. Field
than 1,100 infants and their mothers at 10 (1998) argues that early maternal depres­
different sites around the country. One of sion is associated with two different inter­
the first reports from this study (NICHD actional styles: withdrawn or intrusive.
Early Child Care Research Team, 1997) Both can affect the infant’s physiology and
focused on the relationship between child biochemistry through inadequate stimula­
care and quality of later attachment between tion or its opposite, overstimulation. Field
the mother and her infant. Although the argues that others in the infant’s life who
quality, amount, and type of child care out­ are not themselves depressed may buffer the
side the home (6–15 months) were unre­ negative consequences of interaction with a
lated to attachment quality at 15 months, depressed mother.
maternal responsiveness to the infant did In addition to the more one-sided
interact with child care arrangements. responsiveness, synchrony in the expressed
When mothers were low in responsiveness behaviors between infants and their care­
and their children also experienced poor givers has become a staple of researchers
quality child care outside the home, the and is slowly achieving the stature of a
infants tended to be less securely attached diagnostic tool in assessing developmental
at 15 months. progression and responsive parenting.
Parents can also be trained to be more Some recent studies have used synchrony
responsive to their infants. Wendland- between infant and mother (and sometimes
Carro, Piccinini, and Millar (1999) exposed father) to assess the risk status of triplets
parents of newborns (2–3 days old) to a in comparison to twins and singletons
video on the importance of parental inter­ (Feldman & Eidelman, 2004), the develop­
action or a video on basic caregiving. ment of symbolic competence at 2 years
One month later, those exposed to the from synchrony at 3 and 9 months
19-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 5:12 PM Page 367

The Interaction Management Function of Nonverbal Cues–––◆–––367

(Feldman & Greenbaum, 1997), the devel­ evaluations have been associated, for
opment of self-control at 2 years (Feldman, example, with coordination in speech latency
Greenbaum, & Yirmiya, 1999), and the (Welkowitz & Kuc, 1973), speech rate and
development of emotional differentiation duration (Street, 1982), and pronunciation
with mothers and with fathers (Feldman, (Giles & Smith, 1979). Generalized respon­
2003). Not only does infant-adult coordi­ siveness is associated with attraction (Davis
nation occur early in the infant’s develop­ & Martin, 1978) and the provision of plea­
ment, but the presence of these behaviors is surable stimulation (Davis & Perkowitz,
also predictive of subsequent attachment 1979). Movement synchrony and mimicry
and, very possibly, other cognitive and are associated with rapport (Bernieri, 1988;
behavioral advances. The fact that parents Hess et al., 1999; see Tickle-Degnen, this
can be primed to be responsive and sensi­ volume). Our own research has produced
tive to their infants’ behaviors is evidence modest positive correlations between mea­
that intervention can benefit infants and sures of dynamic coordination and inter­
their parents by enhancing the bonding personal attraction (see Cappella, 1996,
process as well as other desirable social and 1998; Cappella & Flagg, 1992; Cappella,
cognitive competencies. Palmer, & Donzella, 1991).
In the context of marital relationships,
Summary. Despite the occasional contrary Gottman’s (1979) widely cited findings are
study, research on coordination between still the exemplar. Although all his couples
infants and adults (1) has been consistent tended to show reciprocity in hostile affect
with earlier research; (2) has been consis­ in discussions about common problems in
tent across studies in meta-analytical sum­ their marriages, the less well-adjusted cou­
maries; (3) has been manifest in more ples showed greater hostile affect than did
representative populations; and (4) has the better-adjusted couples. Pike and Sillars
begun to be treated as an indicator of nor­ (1985) also found greater reciprocity in
mal behavioral, emotional, and cognitive negative vocal affect for dissatisfied as
development. These conclusions imply that opposed to satisfied married couples. Using
coordination in adult social interaction has face-directed gaze rather than negative
its roots in infant-adult social interaction. affect, Noller’s (1984) satisfied couples
exhibited greater correlation between part­
ners than did the dissatisfied couples (see
MUTUAL COORDINATION also Manusov, 1995). Overall, partners in
AND RELATIONAL AND satisfying, established relationships appear
INDIVIDUAL CONDITIONS to differ in the type of mutual influence that
their interaction shows relative to those in
Coordination between partners in less satisfied relationships.
expressed emotion (and other behaviors) Some of the recent studies in emotional
and in subjectively experienced emotion does contagion and relational outcomes have
not imply necessarily desirable or undesir­ focused more on the similarity of reported
able outcomes for the persons or their rela­ emotion than on expressed emotion (e.g.,
tionship. Earlier research on this issue Totterdell, 2000; Totterdell, Wall, Holman,
(e.g., Cappella, 1991, 1998) has shown Diamond, & Epitropaki, 2004). Anderson,
mostly, but not exclusively, beneficial out­ Keltner, and John (2003) completed a con­
comes for infants and mostly favorable out­ trolled version of the contagion hypothesis.
comes in adult relationships. Positive social Their study evaluated the development of
19-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 5:12 PM Page 368

368–––◆–––Functions

emotional similarity over time. They made from the available empirical results
defined emotional similarity as the coordi­ without inducing some skepticism. The
nation of thoughts and behaviors leading to “causality problem” is both theoretical and
greater understanding and cohesion among empirical. On empirical grounds, even if
partners. Studies 1 and 2 in Anderson et al. there is both covariation and temporal
were longitudinal, investigating dating part­ order, as is the case in Gottman’s research
ners and college roommates at two points and in many of the infant-adult studies,
in time separated by 6 months. The third these criteria do not eliminate the possibil­
was experimental. Study 1 showed an ity of spurious correlation or mutual attrac­
increase in reported emotional similarity— tion prior to the interaction affecting the
both positive and negative—over time, initial levels of coordination. Whereas the
whereas personality similarity remained rel­ data for infants are more convincing, those
atively stable in the same time period. Over for adults are less convincing. On theoreti­
the same time period, positive emotional cal grounds, the mechanisms through
convergence was associated with relational which rapport might grow from behavioral
satisfaction, but negative emotional similar­ coordination between partners are not well
ity was not. Relational breakup from Time described by prevailing explanations. In the
1 to Time 2 was also predicted by emo­ remainder of this section, we will focus on
tional similarity at Time 1: Couples with empirical considerations.
greater emotional similarity at Time 1 Although this issue was not their pri­
stayed together, whereas those without mary motivation, Chartrand and Bargh
emotional similarity tended to part. (1999) have taken on the causal question
Although emotional contagion is com­ directly in a series of studies. In their first
mon among coworkers, dating couples, study, the authors established a mimicry
college roommates, and spouses, and its effect between confederate and respondent
suppression can disrupt communication using two uncommon behaviors: face rub
and relationship formation and elevate and foot shake. When confederates used
blood pressure (Butler et al., 2003), it is less one of the unusual behaviors, then so did
clear whether emotional contagion and syn­ the respondent. In effect, there was imita­
chrony in behavior are as consequential to tion of the behavior over and above base­
the success of a relationship. Gottman and line. These imitations were independent of
Levenson (1999) compared four classes of whether the confederate was smiling or not.
predictors in accounting for deterioration In their second study, confederates imitated
in marital interaction over a 4-year period. the “posture, movement, and mannerisms”
Two classes of predictors were physiologi­ (p. 902) of the respondent while maintain­
cal, one cognitive, and one interactional. ing a neutral facial expression and avoiding
The ratio of positive to negative expressions gaze. Results indicated that when the
was the best predictor of deterioration of confederates imitated the behavior of
interaction, which, in turn, was an excellent the participants, the participants rated the
predictor of future marital dissolution and interactions as smoother and reported lik­
dissatisfaction. ing the confederate more. Careful checking
Despite the association between some of the confederates’ other nonverbal behav­
type of coordination and some form of rap­ iors indicated no differences in rated eye
port or attachment for adult and infant- contact, smiling, friendliness, or liking of
adult dyads, a strong claim of a causal the participant by the confederates across
relationship between the two cannot be the mimicry/no mimicry conditions. The
19-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 5:12 PM Page 369

The Interaction Management Function of Nonverbal Cues–––◆–––369

authors employed a careful debriefing mimicry. Their results suggest that being
procedure to determine whether the partic­ mimicked enhanced a prosocial attitude in
ipants were aware of imitation by the con­ general. Several personality and situation
federate. They were not. factors enhance or retard the likelihood
The results of this study establish a clear of mimicry, including self-monitoring
causal relationship between imitation (Cheng & Chartrand, 2003), context depen­
and positive relational consequences. What dence (Van Baaren, Horgan, Chartrand, &
differentiates this study from several others Dijkmans, 2004), self-construal orientation
that seem to test the same hypothesis is the (Van Baaren, Maddux, Chartrand, de
careful control over confederate behavior Bouter, & van Knippenberg, 2003), affilia­
and attention to initial levels of liking or tion goals (Lakin & Chartrand, 2003), and
cues to liking and attraction (such as smil­ attachment patterns (Sonnby-Borgstrom &
ing or other nonverbal cues). Other studies Jonsson, 2004).
have used confederates to enact behaviors
that are themselves clear indicators of posi­ Summary. The research on the association
tive feeling, such as eye gaze and smiling. between coordination and relational out­
These behaviors can create positive regard comes finds that at both the micro- and
right away. Instead of studying the impact macrolevels, coordination affects relational
of coordination on subsequent positive feel­ outcomes. Studies at the microlevel show
ing, a design allowing initially positive non­ clearly that mimicry of unobtrusive behav­
verbal behavior confounds initial positive iors is causally linked to rapport, and those
regard with coordination. In the Chartrand at the macrolevel show that emotional yok­
and Bargh (1999) study, however, the imi­ ing between partners is a necessary condi­
tated behaviors are hardly noticed by tion for bonding.
participants, and initial behaviors by the
confederate are not inherently positive. Yet
they create positive social perceptions auto­ ♦ Implications for Theory
matically. The authors argue strongly for
an automatic perception-behavior linkage
based on these and other data. The review of previous and newer empirical
This recent work in social psychology on findings produces four broad conclusions
imitation of behaviors has produced a surge that should guide our tour into the theoret­
of other studies operating under the label ical realms. The first of these is automatic­
mimicry. These studies have reinvigorated ity. Many behaviors produce coordination
the study of contagion and coordination between partners automatically, operating
processes but have done so, at least initially, well below conscious awareness (see Lakin,
in ignorance of a long history of research on this volume). The consistent observation of
similar, if not exactly the same, processes. automatic coordination of behaviors sug­
New findings supporting and extending gests strongly that theory must be based on
Chartrand and Bargh’s initial work have mechanisms that allow for automatic, non-
cascaded into the literature. Van Baaren, conscious behavioral and emotional coordi­
Holland, Kawakami, and van Knippenberg nation. Second, the evidence establishing a
(2004), for example, found in three sepa­ causal linkage between behavioral coordi­
rate studies that people who were mimicked nation and some form of positive relational
by others were more helpful and generous outcome, particularly rapport, is difficult
toward third parties not involved in the to treat with skepticism any longer. The
19-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 5:12 PM Page 370

370–––◆–––Functions

absence of a good explanation for this limitations will not allow a comprehensive
relationship leaves the empirical causal review, but we will discuss extant theories
findings without a strong explanatory in terms of their ability to account for
basis, however, and therefore undermines the recent findings reported above (see
their believability. Patterson, this volume). Specifically, what
Third, research in various domains has can theories tell us about automatic
begun to produce findings showing that responding, the linking between synchrony
persons in work groups, in relationships, and outcome, emotional yoking, and how
in social groups, living together, and so coordination came to be so central a
on develop (and report) similar emotional process in human social interaction?
responses over the course of time. This sug­
gests an emotional yoking in social groups.
The impact for successful relationships and EVALUATING THEORIES
performance of emotional similarity and OF COORDINATION
dissimilarity is only beginning to be under­
stood. Theories must begin to explain the A number of competing accounts have
mechanisms through which yoked emotion been put forward to meet the basic require­
develops (other than through spurious ments of explaining coordination. These
external events that must be controlled), include drive explanations (Argyle & Dean,
especially given the failure of several studies 1965; Firestone, 1977), arousal-mediated
to show that expressed emotion mediates explanations (Andersen, 1985; Burgoon,
the relationship between partners’ reports of 1978; Burgoon & Jones, 1976; Burgoon
yoked emotion. Theory must account for & Hale, 1988; Cappella & Greene, 1982;
the link between synchrony in expressed Patterson, 1976, 1982), cognitive explana­
emotion and emotional contagion. Finally, tions (Giles & Powesland, 1975; Giles,
the strong and consistent findings of syn­ Mulac, Bradac, & Johnson, 1987; Street
chrony in infant-adult interactions and the & Giles, 1982), and various combinations
possibility that synchrony (or its absence) of these (Andersen, Guerrero, Buller, &
might be a diagnostic tool for normal devel­ Jorgensen, 1998; Burgoon et al., 1995).
opmental progression of infants argue that With the upsurge of research emphasizing
coordination is a deep-seated and abiding the automatic nature of certain aspects of
process in human social and behavioral life. coordination in the adult (Chartrand &
Explaining how and why these processes Bargh, 1999) and in the infant-adult arenas
came to be is an important goal for theory, (De Wolff & van Ijzendoorn, 1997), how­
particularly evolutionary theories, which ever, and the clear causal evidence for a
aim to explain how processes came to be in coordination-rapport link, theories must be
the first place (Cappella, 1991; Buck & capable of accounting for these develop­
Renfrow Powers, this volume). ments, not as an afterthought but as a
central feature.
Despite their elegance, careful attention
♦ Theories About Coordination to the research literature, and attempts to
be comprehensive, none of these theories
has risen to the top of the empirical heap in
In this section, we take up theoretical contrastive tests. In three such tests, the
approaches to explaining the existing data findings are mostly mixed, with one or the
on coordination in social interaction. Space other theory taking precedence in some
19-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 5:12 PM Page 371

The Interaction Management Function of Nonverbal Cues–––◆–––371

results but no one theory clearly accounting explanation. In particular, two components
for all findings. O’Connor and Gifford of their findings need explaining: mimicry
(1988) tested their social cognitive approach and rapport. What explains people’s
against arousal labeling and discrepancy mimicry of each other’s (inconsequential)
arousal theories, reporting that the social behaviors? Furthermore, what explains
cognitive model fared best in accounting for why mimicry should be associated with
behavioral responses but the self-report positive social regard for the partner? Other
results were less clearly supported. Other issues arise as we interrogate this process.
contrastive tests have produced a mixture Do people always imitate? The answer of
of findings favoring no one explanation course is “no,” but what are the conditions
indicative of the complexity of realistic promoting mimicry and its absence? Can
social interactions or the difficulty of pro­ mimicry create hostility rather than rap­
ducing true critical tests (Andersen et al., port?
1998; Le Poire & Burgoon, 1996). Bargh and his colleagues have offered
One possible explanation for the inabil­ some answers (Bargh, 2003; Bargh &
ity of one of the several extant theories to Chartrand, 1999; Bargh & Ferguson, 2000;
account successfully for the results is not Ferguson & Bargh, 2004), even carrying
only the complexity of the testing environ­ their views into the evolutionary domain
ments and the requirements of careful, con­ (Lakin, Jefferis, Cheng, & Chartrand,
trolled manipulation by confederates but 2003). Their essential claim is that a much
also the breadth of the theories themselves. larger proportion of human activity is dri­
Each of the theories makes a concerted ven by automatic processes than people and
effort to encompass the full range of behav­ psychology have been willing to admit.
iors, explain the conditions promoting Although there has been a very rich
compensation and reciprocity, and take tradition of environmental primes stimulat­
into account mitigating and aggravating con­ ing cognitions and evaluations auto­
ditions. For example, discrepancy-arousal matically (Bargh & Ferguson, 2000), it is
theory (Cappella & Greene, 1982) particu­ only recently that automatic primes for
larly tries to offer an account of infant- behavior have been investigated and found
adult as well as adult-adult patterns of operational.
coordination. This very strength, however, Bargh and his colleagues posit a percep­
might produce generalities in the theories tion-behavior linkage that shows itself
that make them less able to predict particu­ in media effects on behavior (Berkowitz,
lar outcomes in particular social contexts. 1984, 1997), in behavior consistent with
A different strategy is to craft theories of the activation of stereotypes (Dijksterhuis
much narrower scope that seek to provide & van Knippenberg, 1998), and, of course,
very specific predictions of specific empiri­ in social interaction (Chartrand & Bargh,
cal phenomena. We turn now to such a 1999). The mechanisms through which
case, which provides explanations for perception leads to behavior and the
results about behavioral mimicry. limiting conditions are as yet not com­
pletely explored or understood. “Mirror
neurons” are a viable candidate for per­
EXPLAINING MIMICRY ception-behavior activation and inter­
personal facial feedback (IFF) a potential
The intriguing findings reported by mechanism for establishing rapport
Chartrand and Bargh (1999) beg for an through imitation.
19-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 5:12 PM Page 372

372–––◆–––Functions

INTERPERSONAL The IFFH has, to our knowledge, never


FACIAL FEEDBACK been tested directly. A study by Kleinke
and Walton (1982), however, comes clos­
The interpersonal facial feedback hypoth­ est. They used techniques of reinforcement
esis (IFFH; Cappella, 1993) offers a specula­ to alter the frequency of smiles emitted by
tion that accounts for the development of subjects. Those who emitted more smiles
relational outcomes from behavioral coordi­ gave the interview and the interviewer
nation. None of the causal theories currently higher ratings than those who emitted
in the literature accounts specifically for the fewer smiles even though they were not able
association between coordination patterns to ascertain that they were being reinforced
and relational outcomes, either in adults or to smile. Although suggestive, this study did
in children. Neither do the mimicry-rapport not check the quality and number of rein­
explanations make a good case for the forcements, and so the results could be due
mechanism through which mimicry might to differences in reinforcement frequency or
breed rapport. The IFFH may help with this quality rather than differences in smiling.
set of issues. Although we have not carried out a seri­
The IFFH is a series of simple claims. ous test of the IFFH, a secondary analysis of
First, facial displays, especially emotional some previous data is suggestive. To assess
ones, tend to be imitated by both infants whether one’s own smiling affects one’s
and adults. Imitation can be overt (i.e., attraction to a conversational partner, as
observable by others) or covert (i.e., observ­ the IFFH would suggest, we began with the
able only via micromomentary displays predictors that accounted for variance in
or through changes in muscle potential attraction due to experimental condition
[EMG]). Second, the act of producing a (attitude similarity, relational history, and
facial display of emotion alters the underly­ their interaction) and to effects from the
ing experience of emotion, intensifying it partner’s behavior (in this case the interac­
toward the more positive or more negative tion of the partner’s gaze) (Cappella &
valence. The mechanism for this intensifica­ Palmer, 1990, p. 175). We added one’s
tion effect may be through the phenomenon own smiling at the partner to this regres­
of facial efferance (Adelman & Zajonc, sion predicting one’s attraction (in both
1989). Third, if person A expresses a linear and quadratic forms). The results
valenced emotion, and B imitates with a (Cappella, 1993) suggest that the effects of
similar display, the subjective experiences experimental condition and partner’s behav­
of emotion between A and B are yoked ior are roughly the same as reported previ­
through facial feedback, so that subjective ously by Cappella and Palmer (1990)
emotional similarity accompanies expres­ without any additional predictors, but that
sive similarity. If the IFFH is correct, it there is a positive linear effect and a negative,
explains several results from the coordina­ albeit small and marginal, quadratic effect of
tion-mimicry literature, including (1) the one’s own smiling on one’s own attraction to
link between coordination and relational the partner. In effect, one’s own smiling adds
outcomes, at least for facial displays; (2) significant and positive variation to the pre­
how attachment between infants and their diction of attraction to the partner.
parents might come about; and (3) how the With the IFFH and its more speculative
recent observations of subjective emotional counterparts pertinent to vocal and physi­
contagion (e.g., coworkers having yoked cal imitation (see Cappella, 1993), certain
emotional responses) might be produced. puzzles in the interactional literature are
19-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 5:12 PM Page 373

The Interaction Management Function of Nonverbal Cues–––◆–––373

explained. The IFFH assumes that behav­ action of another. Additional research soon
ioral adaptation is temporally prior to identified clusters of mirror neurons in a
one’s own affective response. By invoking number of different locations in the brain
the IFFH, physiological and, as we will see (Rizzolatti & Craighero, 2004). Support
below, neurological pathways are impli­ for the theory that these neurons were pro­
cated as the causal linkages from behavioral viding mental representations of the action
activation in the muscles of the face and increased when it was discovered that neu­
voice to the subjective affective response rons representing the final part of a motor
(Zajonc, Murphy, & Englehart, 1989). In sequence continued to fire even when the
effect, a clear, if controversial, causal mech­ final portion of the sequence was hidden
anism is posited to account for the associa­ from the monkey’s view (Umilta et al.,
tion between behavioral coordination and 2001).
interpersonal affect. Mirror neurons have been posited as the
foundation on which imitation (Buccino
et al., 2004), empathy (Carr, Iacoboni,
NEUROLOGICAL BASES FOR Dubeau, Mazziotta, & Lenzi, 2003), and
INTERPERSONAL FACIAL even our capacity to understand another’s
FEEDBACK, INTERPERSONAL state of mind rests (Gallese & Goldman,
VOCAL FEEDBACK, AND MIMICRY 1998; Schulkin, 2000; Williams, Whiten,
Suddendorf, & Perrett, 2001). Evidence
One line of research that supports from a variety of sources including neuro­
Bargh’s (2003) explanation of mimicry as logical impairment, direct neuron record­
well as the IFFH is found in recent work in ings, evolutionary biology, and neuroimaging
neuroscience, specifically the isolation of have been marshaled in support of these
a mirror neuron reflex. In the mid-1990s, arguments, at least in part because mirror
neuroscientist Vitorio Gallese was observing neurons enable a plausible story to be told
neural activity in the cortex of a macaque about how we have come to the capacity to
monkey during object manipulation. After communicate emotionally.
returning to his laboratory with an ice- There is also mounting evidence suggest­
cream cone, Gallese noticed that each time ing that mirror neurons allow humans to
he licked the ice-cream cone, the neurons use the same neural mechanisms both to
in the monkey’s premotor cortex fired. This express emotions and perceive the expres­
was intriguing, given that the monkey was sion of emotion in others. Furthermore,
not making any motor movements. these neural substrates appear connected to
Further study revealed a set of 92 neu­ the emotion (changes in body and brain
rons in the premotor cortex that were active states triggered by the content of percep­
both when the monkey performed an tions) as well as feelings (those changes
action and when the experimenter per­ in brain state that reach sufficient intensity
formed the same action (Gallese, Fadiga, to be perceived by conscious awareness)
Fogassi, & Rizzolatti, 1996; Rizzolatti, (Damasio, 1999, 2003). The insula, for
Fadiga, Gallese, & Fogassi, 1996). Although instance, appears to become activated not
canonical neurons in the premotor cortex only when we experience a disgusting smell
would activate only when the monkey per­ but also when we perceive someone else’s
formed a motor hand action, these neurons experience of disgust or imitate a disgusted
were described as mirror neurons because expression. The region appears to facilitate
of their apparent ability to represent the our recognition of our physical, emotional,
19-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 5:12 PM Page 374

374–––◆–––Functions

and mental states and the physical, emo­ others in the social environment. Importantly,
tional, and mental states of others. And it the evidence is strong with causal direction
may well be that the insula is one part of clear and possible confounders minimized.
the network involved in the experience Third, coordination in the form of syn­
of disgust after adopting a disgusted facial chrony between infants and their parents
expression. has become sufficiently well established as
Mirror neurons offer a plausible, causal to be a sign of normal (and, in its absence,
mechanism for explaining the “perception­ abnormal) social development. These find­
behavior” linkage that is central to the new ings are evidence of the maturation of a
research on automatic mimicry and older field of study.
research on automatic responding in infants At the theoretical level, the active formu­
and adults as well. Coupled with the IFF lation of general explanations of coordina­
hypothesis, the two mechanisms provide a tion that has marked the past 40 years of its
plausible, if speculative, account of auto­ study has not produced conceptual or theo­
matic behavioral responding, imitation (both retical consensus on which of several simi­
gestural and facial), the occurrence of rap­ lar competitors is the most effective. We
port, and emotional yoking. Empirical test­ speculate that the move toward broad the­
ing for these regulative processes awaits. ories that encompass the range of behaviors
and circumstances characteristic of coordi­
nation may be part of the problem along
with the difficulty of providing unequivocal
♦ Conclusions
contrastive tests. One alterative is explana­
tions that are more limited in scope. We
In this chapter, we have tried to bring explored “automatic perception-behavior”
previous reviews of the literature on non­ account of mimicry, arguing that it offers
verbal coordination up to date by high­ clear predictions and links well with other
lighting key studies and trends in the theories of automatic responding. Whereas
literature. In addition, we have tried to the perception-behavior link works well, it
offer some speculations for theory develop­ is also clear that the behavior-rapport link is
ment that would help to resolve some puz­ less obvious a consequence of mimicry. As a
zles and paradoxes in the existing literature. suggestive resolution, we presented IFFH to
In closing, however, three empirical conclu­ account for the behavior-rapport link and
sions should be brought out, lest they be some evidence from the neurosciences on
lost in the details that the review entails. mirror neurons to strengthen the association
First, yoked emotional response among between perception and behaviors enacted
social actors characterizes work groups, through imitation (or coordination).
dating and longer term relationships, room­
mates, and even professional sports teams.
The mechanism of the production of this ♦ References
yoking of emotion is less well established
and its consequences—for good or ill—
Adelman, P. K., & Zajonc, R. B. (1989).
have not yet been fully explored. Second, Facial efference and the experience of
coordination shows itself through mimicry emotion. Annual Review of Psychology,
of unnoticed behaviors. Mimicry, in turn, 40, 249–280.
produces a sense of rapport with the person Andersen, P. A. (1985). Nonverbal immedi­
mimicking that may even generalize to acy in interpersonal communication. In
19-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 5:12 PM Page 375

The Interaction Management Function of Nonverbal Cues–––◆–––375

A. W. Siegman & S. Feldstein (Eds.), Bernieri, F., & Rosenthal, R. (1991). Inter­
Multichannel integrations of nonverbal personal coordination: Behavior matching
behavior (pp. 1–36). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. and interactional synchrony. In R. S. Feldman
Andersen, P. A., Guerrero, L. K., Buller, D. B., & B. Rime (Eds.), The fundamentals of non­
& Jorgensen, P. F. (1998). An empirical verbal behavior (pp. 401–432). New York:
comparison of three theories of nonverbal Cambridge University Press.
immediacy exchange. Human Communica­ Buccino, G., Vogt, S., Ritzl, A., Fink, G. R.,
tion Research, 24, 501–535. Zilles, K., Freund, H. J., et al. (2004).
Anderson, C., Keltner, D., & John, O. P. (2003). Neural circuits underlying imitation learn­
Emotional convergence between people ing of hand actions: An event-related fMRI
over time. Journal of Personality and Social study. Neuron, 42, 323–334.
Psychology, 84, 1054–1068. Burgoon, J. K. (1978). A communication model
Argyle, M., & Dean, J. (1965). Eye contact, of interpersonal space violations: Explica­
distance, and affiliation. Sociometry, 28, tion and initial test. Human Communication
289–304. Research, 4, 129–142.
Bakermans-Kranenburg, M. J., van Ijzendoorn, Burgoon, J. K. (1993). Interpersonal expecta­
M. H., & Juffer, F. (2003). Less is more: tions, expectancy violations, and emotional
Meta-analyses of sensitivity and attachment communication. Journal of Language and
interventions in early childhood. Psycholo­ Social Psychology, 12, 30–48.
gical Bulletin, 129, 195–215. Burgoon J. K. (1994). Nonverbal signals. In
Bakker, A. B., & Schaufeli, W. B. (2001). Burnout M. L. Knapp & G. R. Miller (Eds.), Hand­
contagion processes among teachers. Journal of book of interpersonal communication (2nd
Applied Social Psychology, 30, 2289–2308. ed., pp. 229–271). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
Bargh, J. A. (2003). Why we thought we could Burgoon, J. K., & Hale, J. L. (1988). Nonverbal
prime social behavior. Psychological Inquiry, expectancies violations: Model elaboration
14, 216–218. and application to immediacy behaviors.
Bargh, J. A., & Chartrand, T. L. (1999). The Communication Monographs, 55, 58–79.
unbearable automaticity of being. Burgoon, J. K., & Jones, S. B. (1976). Toward a
American Psychologist, 54, 462–479. theory of personal space expectations and
Bargh, J. A., & Ferguson, M. J. (2000). Beyond their violations. Human Communication
behaviorism: On the automaticity of higher Research, 2, 131–146.
mental processes. Psychological Bulletin, Burgoon, J. K., Stern, L. A., & Dillman, L.
126, 925–945. (1995). Interpersonal adaptation: Dyadic
Berkowitz, L. (1984). Some effects of thoughts interaction patterns. New York: Cambridge
on anti- and prosocial influences of media University Press.
events: A cognitive neo-association analy­ Butler, E. A., Egloff, B., Wilhelm, F. H., Smith,
sis. Psychological Bulletin, 95, 410–427. N. C., Erickson, E. A., & Gross, J. J.
Berkowitz, L. (1997). Some thoughts extending (2003). The social consequences of expres­
Bargh’s argument. In R. S. Wyer (Ed.), sive suppression. Emotion, 3, 48–67.
Advances in social cognition (Vol. 10, Cappella, J. N. (1981). Mutual influence in
pp. 83–94). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. expressive behavior: Adult and infant-adult
Berger, C. R., & Roloff, M. E. (1980). Social cog­ dyadic interaction. Psychological Bulletin,
nition, self awareness, and interpersonal 89, 101–132.
communication. In B. Dervin & M. J. Voight Cappella, J. N. (1985). The management of con­
(Eds.), Progress in communication sciences versations. In M. L. Knapp & G. R. Miller
(Vol. 2, pp. 1–50). Norwood, NJ: Ablex. (Eds.), The handbook of interpersonal commu­
Bernieri, F. J. (1988). Coordinated movement nication (pp. 393–438). Beverly Hills, CA: Sage.
and rapport in teacher-student interac­ Cappella, J. N. (1991). The biological origins of
tions. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 12, automated patterns of human interaction.
120–138. Communication Theory, 1, 4–35.
19-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 5:12 PM Page 376

376–––◆–––Functions

Cappella, J. N. (1993). The facial feedback expressive groups. Paper presented at the
hypothesis in human interaction: Review International Communication Association
and speculations. Journal of Language and conference, Chicago.
Social Psychology, 12, 13–29. Carr, L., Iacoboni, M., Dubeau, M. C.,
Cappella, J. N. (1994). The management of Mazziotta, J. C., & Lenzi, G. L. (2003).
conversational interaction in adults and Neural mechanisms of empathy in humans:
infants. In M. L. Knapp & G. R. Miller A relay from neural systems for imitation to
(Eds.), Handbook of interpersonal commu­ limbic areas. Proceedings of the National
nication (2nd ed., pp. 380–419). Thousand Academy of Sciences, 100, 5497–5502.
Oaks, CA: Sage. Chartrand, T. L., & Bargh, J. A. (1999). The
Cappella, J. N. (1996). Dynamic coordination chameleon effect: The perception-behavior
of vocal and kinesic behavior. In J. Watt & link and social interaction. Journal of
C. A. VanLear (Eds.), Dynamic patterns Personality and Social Psychology, 76,
in communication processes (pp. 353–386). 893–910.
Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. Cheng, C. M., & Chartrand, T. L. (2003). Self-
Cappella, J. N. (1997). The development of monitoring without awareness: Using
theory about automated patterns of face­ mimicry as a non-conscious affiliation strat­
to-face human interaction. In G. Philipsen egy. Journal of Personality and Social
& T. Albrecht (Eds.), Theories in human Psychology, 85, 1170–1179.
communication (pp. 57–84). Albany, NY: Damasio, A. R. (1999). The feeling of what hap­
SUNY Press. pens: Body and emotion in the making of
Cappella, J. N. (1998). The dynamics of nonver­ consciousness. New York: Harcourt Brace.
bal coordination and attachment: Problems Damasio, A. R. (2003). Looking for Spinoza:
of causal direction and causal mechanism. Joy, sorrow, and the feeling brain. New
In M. T. Palmer (Ed.), Progress in commu­ York: Harcourt.
nication sciences: Vol. 14. Mutual influence Davis, D., & Martin, H. J. (1978). When plea­
in interpersonal communication: Cognitive, sure begets pleasure: Recipient responsive­
behavioral, and affective components ness as a determinant of physical pleasuring
(pp. 19–37). Greenwich, CT: Ablex. between heterosexual dating couples and
Cappella, J. N., & Flagg, M. E. (1992, July). strangers. Journal of Personality and Social
Interactional adaptation, expressiveness Psychology, 36, 767–777.
and attraction: Kinesic and vocal respon­ Davis, D., & Perkowitz, W. T. (1979).
siveness patterns in initial liking. Paper pre­ Consequences of responsiveness in dyadic
sented at the 6th International Conference interaction: Effects of probability of
on Personal Relationships, University of response and proportion of content-related
Maine, Orono. responses on interpersonal attraction.
Cappella, J. N., & Greene, J. O. (1982). A Journal of Personality and Social Psycho­
discrepancy-arousal explanation of mutual logy, 37, 534–550.
influence in expressive behaviors for De Wolff, M. S., & van Ijzendoorn, M. H.
adult-adult and infant-adult interactions. (1997). Sensitivity and attachment: A meta­
Communication Monographs, 49, 89–114. analysis on parental antecedents of infant
Cappella, J. N., & Palmer, M. T. (1990). Attitude attachment. Child Development, 68,
similarity, relational history, and attraction: 571–591.
The mediating effects of kinesic and vocal Dijksterhuis, A., & van Knippenberg, A. (1998).
behaviors. Communication Monographs, The relation between perception and
57, 161–183. behavior, or how to win a game of Trivial
Cappella, J. N., Palmer, M. T., & Donzella, B. Pursuit. Journal of Personality and Social
(1991, May). Individual consistency in tem­ Psychology, 74, 865–877.
poral adaptations in nonverbal behavior Doherty, R. W. (1997). The Emotional
in dyadic conversations: High and low Contagion Scale: A measure of individual
19-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 5:12 PM Page 377

The Interaction Management Function of Nonverbal Cues–––◆–––377

differences. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, Giles, H., Mulac, A., Bradac, J. J., & Johnson, P.
21, 131–153. (1987). Speech accommodation theory:
Doherty, R. W., Orimoto, L., Singelis, T. M., The first decade and beyond. In M. L.
Hatfield, E., & Hebb, J. (1995). Emotional McLaughlin (Ed.), Communication year­
contagion: Gender and occupational differ­ book 10 (pp. 13–48). Newbury Park, CA:
ences. Psychology of Women Quarterly, Sage.
19, 355–371. Giles, H., & Smith, P. M. (1979).
Feldman, R. (2003). Infant-mother and infant- Accommodation theory: Optimal levels
father synchrony: The coregulation of posi­ of convergence. In H. Giles & R. St. Clair
tive arousal. Infant Mental Health Journal, (Eds.), Language and social psychology
24, 1–23. (pp. 45–65). Baltimore, MD: University
Feldman, R., & Eidelman, A. I. (2004). Parent- Park Press.
infant synchrony and the social-emotional Goodman, C. R., & Shippy, R. A. (2002). Is it
development of triplets. Developmental contagious? Affect similarity among
Psychology, 40, 1133–1147. spouses. Aging & Mental Health, 6,
Feldman, R., & Greenbaum, C. W. (1997). 266–274.
Affect regulation and synchrony in mother- Gottman, J. M. (1979). Marital interaction.
infant play as precursors to the develop­ New York: Academic Press.
ment of symbolic competence. Infant Gottman, J. M., & Levenson, R. W. (1999).
Mental Health Journal, 18, 4–23. What predicts change in marital interaction
Feldman, R., Greenbaum, C. W., & Yirmiya, N. over time? A study of alternative models.
(1999). Mother-infant affect synchrony Family Process, 38, 143–158.
as an antecedent of the emergence of Gump, B. B., & Kulik, J. A. (1997). Stress, affil­
self-control. Developmental Psychology, iation, and emotional contagion. Journal
35, 223–231. of Personality and Social Psychology, 72,
Ferguson, M. J., & Bargh, J.A. (2004). How 305–319.
social perception can automatically influ­ Hatfield, E., Cacioppo, J., & Rapson, R. L.
ence behavior. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, (1994). Emotional contagion. Paris:
8, 33–39. Cambridge University Press.
Field, T. (1987). Affective and interactive distur­ Hess, U., & Blairy, S. (2001). Facial mimicry
bances in infants. In J. D. Osofsky and emotional contagion to dynamic emo­
(Ed.), Handbook of infant development tional facial expressions and their influence
(2nd ed., pp. 972–1007). New York: Wiley. on decoding accuracy. International Journal
Field, T. (1998). Maternal depression effects on of Psychophysiology, 40, 129–141.
infants and early interventions. Preventive Hess, U., Philippot, P., & Blairy, S. (1999).
Medicine: An International Journal Devoted Mimicry: Facts and fiction. In P. Philippot,
to Practice & Theory, 27, 200–203. R. S. Feldman, & E. J. Coats (Eds.), The
Firestone, I. (1977). Reconciling verbal and non­ social context of nonverbal behavior (pp.
verbal models of dyadic communication. 213–241). New York: Cambridge University
Environmental Psychology and Nonverbal Press.
Behavior, 2, 30–44. Hietanen, J. K., Surakka, V., & Linnankoski, I.
Gallese, V., Fadiga, L., Fogassi, L., & Rizzolatti, (1998). Facial electromyographic responses
G. (1996). Action recognition in the premo­ to vocal affect expressions. Psychophysio­
tor cortex. Brain, 119, 593–609. logy, 35, 530–536.
Gallese, V., & Goldman, A. (1998). Mirror neu­ Hsee, C. K., Hatfield, E., Carlson, J. G., &
rons and the simulation theory of mind read­ Chemtob, C. (1992). Assessments of the
ing. Trends in Cognitive Science, 2, 493–501. emotional state of others—Conscious judg­
Giles, H., & Powesland, P. F. (1975). Speech style ments versus emotional contagion. Journal
and social evaluation. London: Academic of Social and Clinical Psychology, 11,
Press. 119–128.
19-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 5:12 PM Page 378

378–––◆–––Functions

Ingram, L. A. (1997). Congruence and conta­ infant-mother attachment security: Results


gion of affect in elderly caregiver-care recip­ of the NICHD study of early child care.
ient spousal pairs. Dissertation Abstracts Child Development, 68, 860–879.
International, 58 (3-B), 1595B. Noller, P. (1984). Nonverbal communication and
Kleinke, C. L., & Walton, J. H. (1982). Influence marital interaction. Oxford: Pergamon Press.
of reinforced smiling on affective responses O’Connor, B. P., & Gifford, R. (1988). A test
in an interview. Journal of Personality and among models of nonverbal immediacy
Social Psychology, 42, 557–565. reactions: Arousal-labeling, discrepancy-
Lakin, J. L., & Chartrand, T. L. (2003). Using arousal, and social cognition. Journal of
non-conscious behavioral mimicry to Nonverbal Behavior, 12, 6–30.
create affiliation and rapport. Psychological Patterson, M. L. (1976). An arousal model
Science, 14, 334–339. of interpersonal intimacy. Psychological
Lakin, J. L., Jefferis, V. E., Cheng, C. M., & Review, 83, 235–245.
Chartrand, T. L. (2003). The chameleon Patterson, M. L. (1982). A sequential functional
effect as social glue: Evidence for the evolu­ model of nonverbal exchange. Psychological
tionary significance of nonconscious mim­ Review, 89, 231–249.
icry. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 27, Patterson, M. L. (1999). The evolution of a par­
145–162. allel process model of nonverbal communi­
Langer, E. J. (1978). Rethinking the role of cation. In P. Philippot, R. S. Feldman, &
thought in social interaction. In J. H. E. J. Coats (Eds.), The social context of
Harvey, W. J. Ickes, & R. F. Kidd (Eds.), nonverbal behavior (pp. 317–347). New
New directions in attribution research (Vol. York: Cambridge University Press.
2, pp. 35–58). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Pike, G. R., & Sillars, A. L. (1985). Reciprocity
Le Poire, B. A., & Burgoon, J. K. (1996). and marital communication. Journal of
Usefulness of differentiating arousal res­ Personal and Social Relationships, 2,
ponses within communication theories: 303–324.
Orienting response or defensive arousal Pugh, S. D. (1998). Service with a smile: The
within nonverbal theories of expectancy contagious effects of employee affect on
violation. Communication Monographs, customer attitudes. Dissertation Abstracts
63, 208–230. International, 58 (11-B), 6267B.
Lundqvist, L.O. (1995). Facial EMG reactions Rizzolatti, G., & Craighero, L. (2004). The
to facial expressions: A case of facial emo­ mirror-neuron system. Annual Review of
tional contagion. Scandinavian Journal of Neuroscience, 27, 169–192.
Psychology, 36, 130–141. Rizzolatti, G., Fadiga, L., Gallese, V., & Fogassi,
Manusov, V. (1995). Reacting to changes in L. (1996). Premotor cortex and the recog­
nonverbal behaviors: Relational satisfac­ nition of motor actions. Cognitive Brain
tion and adaptation patterns in romantic Research, 3, 131–141.
dyads. Human Communication Research, Schulkin, J. (2000). Theory of mind and mirror­
21, 456–477. ing neurons. Trends in Cognitive Science,
Milner, J. S., Halsey, L. B., & Fultz, J. (1995). 4, 252–254.
Empathic responsiveness and affective reac­ Sonnby-Borgstrom, M., & Jonsson, P. (2004).
tivity to infant stimuli in high- and low-risk Dismissing-avoidant pattern of attachment
for child abuse mothers. Child Abuse and and mimicry reactions at different levels
Neglect, 19, 767–780. of information processing. Scandinavian
Neumann, R., & Strack, F. (2000). “Mood con­ Journal of Psychology, 45, 103–13.
tagion”: The automatic transfer of mood Stack, D. M., & Arnold, S. L. (1998). Changes
between persons. Journal of Personality in mothers’ touch and hand gestures influ­
and Social Psychology, 79, 211–223. ence infant behavior during face-to-face
NICHD Early Child Care Research Team. interchanges. Infant Behavior and Develop­
(1997). The effects of infant child care on ment, 21, 451–468.
19-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 5:12 PM Page 379

The Interaction Management Function of Nonverbal Cues–––◆–––379

Street, R. L. (1982). Evaluation of noncontent Van Baaren, R. B., Holland, R. W., Kawakami,
speech accommodation. Language and K., & Van Knippenberg, A. (2004). Mimicry
Communication, 2, 13–31. and prosocial behavior. Psychological
Street, R. L., & Giles, H. (1982). Speech accom­ Science, 15, 71–74.
modation theory: A social cognitive approach Van Baaren, R. B., Horgan, T. G., Chartrand,
to language and speech behavior. In T. L., & Dijkmans, M. (2004). The forest,
M. Roloff & C. R. Berger (Eds.), Social the trees, and the chameleon: Context depen­
cognition and communication (pp. 193– dence and mimicry. Journal of Personality &
226). Beverly Hills, CA: Sage. Social Psychology, 86, 453–459.
Symons, D., & Moran, G. (1994). Responsiveness Van Baaren, R. B., Maddux, W. W., Chartrand,
and dependency are different aspects of T. L., de Bouter, C., & van Knippenberg, A.
social contingencies: An example from (2003). It takes two to mimic: Behavioral
mother and infant smiles. Infant Behavior consequences of self-construals. Journal of
and Development, 17, 209–214. Personality and Social Psychology, 84,
Thompson, L. L., Nadler, J., & Kim, P. H. 1093–1102.
(1999). Some like it hot: The case for the Verbeke, W. (1997). Individual differences in
emotional negotiator. In L. L. Thompson emotional contagion of salespersons: Its
& J. M. Levine (Eds.), Shared cognition in effect on performance and burnout.
organizations: The management of knowl­ Psychology & Marketing, 14, 617–636.
edge (pp. 139–161). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. Welkowitz, J., & Kuc, M. (1973). Inter­
Totterdell, P. (2000). Catching moods and relationships among warmth, genuineness,
hitting runs: Mood linkage and subjective empathy, and temporal speech patterns in
performance in professional sport teams. interpersonal interaction. Journal of
Journal of Applied Psychology, 85, Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 41,
848–859. 472–473.
Totterdell, P., Kellett, S., Teuchmann, K., & Wendland-Carro, J., Piccinini, C. A., & Millar,
Briner, R. B. (1998). Evidence of mood W. S. (1999). The role of an early interven­
linkage in work groups. Journal of Per­ tion on enhancing the quality of mother-
sonality and Social Psychology, 74, infant interaction. Child Development,
1504–1515. 70, 713–721.
Totterdell, P., Wall, T., Holman, D., Diamond, Wild, B., Erb, M., & Bartels, M. (2001). Are emo­
H., & Epitropaki, O. (2004). Affect net­ tions contagious? Evoked emotions while
works: A structural analysis of the relation­ viewing emotionally expressive faces: Quality,
ship between work ties and job-related quantity, time course and gender differences.
affect. Journal of Applied Psychology, 89, Psychiatry Research, 102, 109–124.
854–867. Williams, J. H., Whiten, A., Suddendorf, T., &
Ullstadius, E. (1998). Neonatal imitation in a Perrett, D. I. (2001). Imitation, mirror neu­
mother-infant setting. Early Development rons and autism. Neuroscience Biobehavior
and Parenting, 7, 1–8. Review, 25, 287–295.
Umilta, M. A., Kohler, E., Gallese, V., Fogassi, Zajonc, R. B., Murphy, S. T., & Englehart, M.
L., Fadiga, L., Keysers, C., et al. (2001). I (1989). Feeling and facial efference:
know what you are doing: A neurophysio­ Implications of the vascular theory of emo­
logical study. Neuron, 31, 155–165. tion. Psychological Review, 96, 395–416.
19-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 5:12 PM Page 380
20-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 5:12 PM Page 381

20
NONVERBAL BEHAVIOR
AND ITS FUNCTIONS IN
THE ECOSYSTEM OF RAPPORT

� Linda Tickle-Degnen
Boston University

T he term rapport is used to indicate a meaningful human


experience of close and harmonious connection that involves
common understanding (Compact Oxford English Dictionary, 2005),
and being in rapport with someone requires mutual responsiveness
to intention, attitude, and affect (Park & Burgess, 1924). Rapport is
self-affirming, satisfying, and enjoyable for the individual—a desired
internal state of optimal experience (Csikszentmihalyi, 1990)—and it
involves the experiential bonding of individuals into relationship with
one another. This bonding sustains mutual involvement in single inter­
actions of short duration as well as across repeated interactions over
extended periods of time.

Author’s Note: This chapter was supported in part by Grant No. NS048059
from the National Institute of Neurological Disorders and Stroke of the
National Institutes of Health (NIH). Its contents are solely the responsibility
of the author and do not necessarily represent the official views of NIH.

◆ 381
20-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 5:12 PM Page 382

382–––◆–––Functions

Rapport creates a powerful medium for perceived qualities (Dijksterhuis & Bargh,
social influence (Freud, 1914/1924; LaFrance, 2001), or a strategic process (Brunswik,
1990) and for the accomplishment of tasks 1955) is beyond the scope of this chapter
that are challenging and require mutual com­ (but see Lakin, this volume), although
mitment to accomplish (Tickle-Degnen & arguably both automaticity and cognitive
Gavett, 2003). Individuals in rapport are mediation are involved (Patterson, 1999,
likely to cooperate with one another to 2003; Strack & Deutsch, 2004). Once in
accomplish tasks and objectives that could rapport, however, individuals form a phys­
not be accomplished alone as effectively, ical and social ecosystem—an interde­
as efficiently, or at all. Cooperation facili­ pendence of two entities’ perceptions and
tates many of the tasks of everyday living, actions—that operates in relation to the
including those most basic to species larger physical and social environment in
survival—eating, protection against harm, which it is embedded. People develop, main­
or procreation—as well as those involved in tain, and indicate their rapport through a
the actualization of human potential. The stream of interlinked signals and responses
child in rapport with the parent is fed and that are shaped by their personal physical
nourished, workers in rapport achieve the and psychological properties, the parame­
project deadline, and the student and ters of the task in which they are engaged,
teacher in rapport pass knowledge on to the and the physical and social environment of
next generation. their actions.
Importantly for this chapter, rapport is Research studies on rapport, with a
created, sustained, and marked by action. few exceptions (Bernieri, Gillis, Davis, &
More specifically, it involves the coupling Grahe, 1996; Puccinelli, Tickle-Degnen,
of internal experience and purposeful, often & Rosenthal, 2003, 2004), have not
nonverbal, action into a perception-action addressed nonverbal behavior as emergent
system that has adaptive value for the indi­ from context explicitly and as part of an
vidual, the dyad, and the human species ecosystem of rapport, that is, from an eco­
(Dijksterhuis & Bargh, 2001; Fridlund, logical perspective. As a result, accumulat­
1997; Patterson, 1999). The perception- ing evidence has little theoretical coherence
action system of rapport is not separate and appears to be complex and conflicting.
from its context but is rather emergent The purpose of this chapter is to explore
within it (Gibson, 1986; McArthur & the role of nonverbal behavior within the
Baron, 1983; Zebrowitz & Collins, 1997), ecosystem of rapport and to demonstrate
and the immediate contextual elements for that a systematic investigation of context
one individual are the features of the other resolves apparently conflicting results and
individual, including static nonverbal fea­ promotes the theoretical coherence needed
tures such as facial structure and signs of to move this area of research forward. To
gender as well as dynamic features from do so, the chapter describes the functions
verbal and nonverbal behavior. of nonverbal behavior and presents a three-
Perceivers respond rapidly to informa­ component model of rapport that is consis­
tive physical features in the face, body, and tent with an ecological perspective.
voice of potential cooperative partners as The chapter also reviews recent research
they negotiate the natural social environ­ findings on nonverbal behavior and rapport
ment. Whether this response is due to an and describes how a model of optimal expe­
attunement of attention that is learned rience developed by Csikszentmihalyi and
(Gibson, 1986), an automatic expression of his colleague (Csikszentmihalyi, 1990;
20-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 5:12 PM Page 383

Nonverbal Behavior and Its Functions in the Ecosystem of Rapport–––◆–––383

Csikszentmihalyi & Csikszentmihalyi, 1988) dyad (Ambady, Bernieri, & Richeson, 2000;
can be applied flexibly to the systematic Patterson, 1999). This ecosystem as a whole
investigation of rapport and nonverbal serves the function of regulating dyadic
behavior in context. Specifically, this model connection and rapport to accomplish inter­
places optimal experiences such as rapport personal tasks and meet personal, dyadic,
within the broader theoretical perspective and group needs.
of how dyads regulate themselves to achieve In contrast to a view that specific
smooth, harmonious, and enjoyable interac­ patterns of nonverbal behavior map one-
tion as constrained by the tasks that they are to-one with discrete emotions (Ekman &
doing. The model provides a framework for Keltner, 1997; Izard, 1994) or that nonver­
showing how nonverbal behavior varies in bal behavior expresses an actor’s social
relation to the degree that task parameters intentions, not emotions (Fridlund, 1997),
support or undermine the achievement of the position taken here is that a nonver­
rapport. The chapter concludes with implica­ bal action or event is best understood as
tions for future research. expressive of many psychological states,
traits, and action tendencies at once. The
stream of perception and behavior is such
♦ The Functions of that participants’ intentions, attitudes, and
Nonverbal Behavior in the feelings are not parsed out into entities that
are expressed in mutually exclusive behav­
Ecosystem of Rapport
ioral markers. Rather internal states, per­
ceptions, and behaviors are convergent
As part of the dyadic ecosystem, nonverbal aspects of a meaningful event that is under­
behavior is functional (Patterson, 1994). stood within context (Heft, 2003; James,
Nonverbal behavior provides information 1890). That is, from an ecological perspec­
about each interacting participant’s initial tive, nonverbal behavior is constrained, or
intentions, attitudes, and feelings and, as shaped, in relation to (a) the capacities and
such, serves an expressive function in the tendencies of the interacting individuals to
development and maintenance of rapport. experience, perceive, and express rapport-
Furthermore, each participant’s nonverbal relevant intentions, attitudes, and feelings;
behavior functions to create affordances (b) parameters of the interactive task; and
(Gibson, 1986) or opportunities for action (c) features of the physical and societal
by the other participant. To the perceiver, environment.
an actor’s behavior affords differing degrees More specifically, individuals vary in
of interpersonal contact, enjoyment, infor­ their sensitivity to experiencing rapport,
mation exchange, mutual task engagement, their expressiveness of internal states, and
and so on. The nonverbal reactions of the their ability to pick up information rele­
perceiver, now actor, function to signal the vant to others’ states. For one individual, a
degree of “pick up” of the other partici­ vertical wrinkle between the brows may be
pant’s intentions, attitudes, and feelings as an expression of anger, an emotion rele­
well as the acceptance or agreement with vant to rapport, and, in another, it may be
them. These functions operate within a con­ an indication of far-sightedness, a condi­
tinuous signal-perception-action-signal loop tion less relevant to rapport. Also, interac­
of interpersonal interaction, and they often tive tasks vary in difficulty, required
occur spontaneously, within seconds and in actions and object manipulations, bodily
parallel across both participants within a positioning to accomplish the task, and
20-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 5:12 PM Page 384

384–––◆–––Functions

amounts of verbal and nonverbal commu­ generally as consisting of the components


nication. Furrowing the brow during an of mutual attentiveness, positivity, and
easy task can indicate confusion and be interpersonal coordination.
associated with feelings not conducive to Mutual attentiveness refers to partici­
rapport, such as incompetence, but during pants’ focus on one another or on the same
a challenging task can indicate careful, object or environmental feature. Behavioral
effortful decision making and be associated attention demonstrates and affords interper­
with feelings congruent with rapport sonal interest and engagement. Positivity
(Tamir, Robinson, Clore, Martin, & refers to an affectively positive rather than
Whitaker, 2004). Finally, physical and negative experience of one another. Behav­
social environments vary in features such ioral positivity demonstrates and affords
as type and position of furniture, respect and beneficence toward another
size of interactive space, the presence and person. Interpersonal coordination refers to
number of potential observers to the an immediate and spontaneous behavioral
interaction, and the cultural setting and and experiential mutual responsiveness, a
social roles of the participants. A couple’s being “in sync” rather than “out of sync”
frowning at each other in public, when with one another, and the affordance of
social pressures would tend to suppress well-regulated and effective interaction.
negative affect, may indicate stronger neg­ Although this three-component model did
ativity than the same degree of frowning not involve the concept of affordance explic­
in the privacy of home, where intimacy is itly, the components relate implicitly to an
expected to generate more open expression affordance function, as do many social psy­
of frustration. chological constructs (McArthur & Baron,
1983; Zebrowitz & Collins, 1997).
The concepts of attentiveness, positivity,
♦ A Three-Component and coordination are socially meaningful
because they require a context-dependent
Model of Rapport
assessment of behavior. To come back to
the example of brow furrowing, a brow
For nonverbal behavior to serve the func­ furrow is not separable from context in nat­
tions relative to rapport (i.e., as information uralistic and daily life interaction, just as
about internal states, as affordances, and as the meaning of the word “furrowing” is not
information about the congruence of inter­ separable from the utterance and context
nal states between interactive partners), the in which it is spoken (Fridlund, 1997). In
perceiver must be able to glean information some contexts, the furrow may indicate
relevant to rapport from variable and con- high levels of attentiveness, positivity, and
text-dependent combinations and physical coordination, yet in other contexts it may
forms of behavior (such as a brow furrow signify low levels. Rosenthal (1982) cap­
and eye contact). A problem for research tured this contention in his advocacy to
and theory in this area is how these variable augment the standard molecular coding of
combinations can indicate similar experi­ physical behavior with a molar judging of
ences (i.e., being in or out of rapport) across the social meaning of behavior. The molar
varying contexts. One solution proposed by approach, now identified with the “thin
Tickle-Degnen and Rosenthal (1987, 1990) slice” method, has been validated exten­
is that the experience and behavioral sively in the prediction of important social
manifestations of rapport can be described phenomena and outcomes (Ambady et al.,
20-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 5:12 PM Page 385

Nonverbal Behavior and Its Functions in the Ecosystem of Rapport–––◆–––385

2000) and in the explanation of how own needs and inner states. Expressivity
individuals form their judgments of rapport tends to elicit liking, rapport, and other
(Grahe & Bernieri, 2002). It is the approach positive social outcomes (Riggio & Riggio,
taken in the three-component model of 2002). In an experimental manipulation
rapport (Tickle-Degnen & Rosenthal, of the suppression of emotional behavior
1987, 1990). (Butler et al., 2003), female dyads watched
an upsetting film and, then, after receiving
the experimental manipulation, discussed it.
♦ Research on Nonverbal Women who had been instructed to sup­
Behavior and Rapport press their emotional behavior were less
expressive, both positively and negatively,
less responsive, and more distracted than
The three-component model is partially were those women who had not received
represented in the recent research on non­ any specific instructions. The uninstructed
verbal behavior and rapport. The bulk of partners of the “suppressing” women report­
recent and relevant research is on interper­ edly felt less rapport and willingness to form
sonal coordination, followed by nonverbal friendships with those women. Dyads in
expressivity, a construct that encompasses which both members were uninstructed,
but is not limited to positivity. Little and, therefore, more expressive, had better
research has systematically examined rapport outcomes.
the component of mutual attentiveness. The
review that follows describes first the Expressivity and Positivity. That expression
findings for expressivity, and second, of both positive and negative feelings is
the findings for coordination, because non­ conducive to rapport is counter to the
verbal expressivity appears to be the raw proposal that positivity alone, not general
behavioral material for the development expressivity, is a basic component of rap­
of interpersonal coordination. Both sets of port (Tickle-Degnen & Rosenthal, 1987,
findings demonstrate the need for a more 1990). Some research studies, however,
systematic investigation of context and of have implied the importance of positivity
more complex patterns among the different by finding that negative expressivity is a
behavioral components of rapport. negative correlate of rapport. In a study of
women who interviewed one another about
their daily lives and aspirations, dyads
NONVERBAL EXPRESSIVITY experienced lower rapport when the more
expressive partner of the dyad was also the
From their review of the literature on partner with the more negatively emotional
emotional expressivity, Boone and Buck personality (Tickle-Degnen & Puccinelli,
(2003) conclude that to be openly self- 1999). Rapport was higher when the less
expressive is a claim to one’s own trust­ negatively emotional partner was the more
worthiness and one’s ability to trust. By expressive partner of the dyad. Other find­
displaying feelings openly, vividly, and ings suggest that dyads containing partners
dynamically, people invite others to know who are sensitive to detecting negative
their feelings, intentions, and attitudes. Such emotionality in others tend to have com­
openness allows the perceiver to determine promised experiences of rapport (Elfenbein
the potential for an interaction with the & Ambady, 2002; Puccinelli & Tickle-
actor that is responsive to the perceiver’s Degnen, 2004).
20-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 5:12 PM Page 386

386–––◆–––Functions

Context factors may explain the inconsis­ (2003) note that expressivity of an unusu­
tency in research results about negative expres­ ally intense or chaotic nature among newly
sivity. In Tickle-Degnen and Puccinelli’s acquainted individuals would be perceived
(1999) study, the task of discussing one’s daily as untrustworthy and not conducive to
life and aspirations with a relatively new cooperation. From this perspective, a mod­
acquaintance implicitly required a pleasant erate level of positivity and negativity of
self-presentation (DePaulo, 1992). In this con­ expression would be more favorable to rap­
text, rapport was higher when expressivity port than either very low levels (unexpres­
was more positive than negative. Expressed sive) or very high levels (unusually intense).
negativity by one partner may have been off- Related to this nonlinear perspective is
putting given the context demands. In con­ Gottman’s (1994) finding that when hap­
trast, in Butler et al.’s (2003) study, the task pily married couples openly express nega­
of watching an upsetting film with another tive emotion in a manner that is modulated
person would have implicitly required a by positive interpersonal strategies, such
social acknowledgment of the shared nega­ as humor, they return to a nonnegative
tive experience. Suppression of negative state. Unhappy couples, on the other hand,
expressivity may have been off-putting in this descend into unalleviated and intense nega­
context. Perhaps, this suppression was a mes­ tivity. Positive and negative expressivity
sage of dissimilarity or nonacceptance of not events may cycle relative to one another,
only the suppressing woman’s inner state and the dynamic pattern of these cycles may
but also the partner’s inner state. Further­ drive the rapport experience.
more, suppression would afford the partner Tickle-Degnen and Gavett (2003)
little responsive action, such as commis­ observed nonlinear and dynamic patterning
eration. In the context of this study, unsup­ of nonverbal behavior in six speech thera­
pressed negative expressivity would be pists and their new child clients over eight
“positive” because it would imply mutual weekly clinic sessions. There appeared to be
acceptance and understanding of one a higher degree of fluctuating, up-down
another’s experience. changes in levels of nonverbal positivity
If such an interpretation is correct, the and attentiveness within dyads from session
implication is that expressivity can have to session during the first four sessions
meaning that is positive, neutral, or nega­ compared with the last four sessions, which
tive, depending on the context. Specifically, showed a stable leveling of behavior. The
it is expressivity that has positive meaning­ therapists’ self-reported experience of rap­
in-context that is the correlate or facilitator port, competence, and success, as well as
of rapport. Lending support to this contex­ their supervisors’ analysis of the sessions,
tual explanation, Bernieri et al. (1996) suggested that the greater degree of behav­
found that nonverbal expressivity had a ioral fluctuation in the earlier set of sessions
lower correlation with experienced rapport was due to conditions that challenged effec­
in an adversarial context, in which expres­ tive communication and collaboration. The
sivity may have had a less positive, more greater degree of behavioral stability in the
negative quality, than in a cooperative one, later set appeared to be due to less chal­
in which expressivity may have had a more lenging conditions. More concretely, the
positive, less negative quality. therapists and children were learning about
each other and how to communicate and
Nonlinear and Dynamic Patterns of work together effectively in their earlier ses­
Nonverbal Expressivity. Boone and Buck sions, and they were more informed and
20-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 5:12 PM Page 387

Nonverbal Behavior and Its Functions in the Ecosystem of Rapport–––◆–––387

skilled in their interactive patterns in later patterns that are not mutually exclusive of
sessions. one another. Three frequently measured
Furthermore, across the entire set of ses­ coordinative patterns are matching, interac­
sions, therapists who felt the highest level of tional synchrony, and mimicry (Burgoon,
rapport demonstrated intermediate levels of Stern, & Dillman, 1995). Matching refers to
attentiveness and positivity. Therapists who behavioral similarity, often measured as a
felt the lowest level of rapport demonstrated couple’s shared positioning of arms and legs.
low or high—not intermediate—attentive­ In my work, I have operationalized matching
ness and positivity. These findings support as the similarity in the degree of observed
a view of nonlinearity in the association attentiveness and positivity of partners
between nonverbal behavior and rapport (Tickle-Degnen & Gavett, 2003). Inter­
that is consistent with Boone and Buck’s actional synchrony refers to similarity in the
(2003) view that a moderate level of expres­ rhythm of behavior and to a measurable
sivity is more conducive to feelings of trust­ enmeshment or interdependence of the tim­
worthiness and motives to cooperate than ing of movements. Mimicry is a matching of
extremely low or high levels of expressivity. the behavior of one participant, such as foot
shaking, by another participant close in time
INTERPERSONAL COORDINATION to the original behavior.
Of the three hypothesized components
The research on nonverbal expressivity of rapport (Tickle-Degnen & Rosenthal,
points toward rapport as emerging from 1987, 1990), interpersonal coordination has
multicomponent, nonlinear, and dynamic the most evidence supporting its validity.
behavior in context. The research on inter­ Parent-infant studies suggest that there are
personal coordination leads to a similar innate pressures for individuals to accom­
conclusion. The concept of interpersonal modate to one another behaviorally (Crown,
coordination is particularly useful for Feldstein, Jasnow, Beebe, & Jaffe, 2001;
understanding rapport because the concept Papoušek & Papoušek, 1997). When an
of interpersonal coordination incorporates interaction is going well or supported by
the dynamic and bidirectional, person-to­ the context, coordinated patterning tends
person interdependence of behavior. Each to increase over the course of an encounter
partner is part of the context for the other (Bernieri, Reznick, & Rosenthal, 1988;
partner, and each partner’s expressivity is Cappella, 1996, this volume; Warner,
the “raw action” material that affords the 1992). Such patterning, however, does not
responsive action necessary to link part­ always occur. Kritzer and Valenti (1990)
ners into a coordinative unit. Coordination showed that synchrony did not increase
emerges from the context of interpersonal among untrained therapists and their
action. For example, individuals who move clients, but it did so with trained therapists.
more are more likely to show a behavioral Bernieri et al. (1988) found synchrony to
responsiveness to one another beyond that increase in mothers with their own infants
expected by chance alone (Grammer, Kruck, but not among mothers with infants
& Magnusson, 1998). It is this above-chance with whom they were interacting for the
responsiveness that defines interpersonal first time.
coordination. It is possible that individuals who coordi­
The terms coordination, responsiveness, nate behaviorally recognize consciously that
and accommodation are used interchange­ their actions are easy, predictable, efficient,
ably to refer to a whole class of coordinative and coordinated and, consequently, infer
20-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 5:12 PM Page 388

388–––◆–––Functions

that these qualities are due to an experiential degrees of synchrony than those who were
understanding of one another (Bem, 1972). less involved. Renfro and Rauh (2005)
But recent research demonstrates that con­ found that manipulated disruption of
sciousness is not required. Studies in which interpersonal coordination during video-
mimicry is manipulated experimentally sug­ mediated discussions had a more negative
gest that another’s mimicry of one’s own effect on communication satisfaction and
behavior induces liking of and rapport with emotion when individuals were more rather
the mimicker unconsciously (Chartrand than less interested and involved in the
& Bargh, 1999; Lakin, Jefferis, Cheng, & topic of their discussion.
Chartrand, 2003; see Lakin, this volume)
and promotes prosocial behavior toward Interpersonal Coordination and Positivity.
mimickers as well as those not involved Another example that coordination is not a
in mimicking (van Baaren, Holland, sufficient component of rapport is demon­
Kawakami, & van Knippenberg, 2004). strated in the mixed findings of research
on postural matching, one type of coordi­
Interpersonal Coordination and Attentive­ native structure. In naturalistic interactions
ness. Despite the persuasive evidence that of people in established relationships,
interpersonal coordination is a behavioral matching is a positive correlate of rapport
correlate and antecedent of rapport across experience (LaFrance, 1979; LaFrance &
various contexts, it is possible that coordi­ Broadbent, 1976). Alternatively, in interac­
nation is an automatic and spontaneous tions of strangers involved in experimental
correlate of any type of interpersonal expe­ tasks that had unusual or ambiguous
rience that involves engagement and mutual purposes, matching has been found to be
attention (Bernieri et al., 1996; Burgoon indicative of communication anxiety or
et al, 1995; Dijksterhuis & Bargh, 2001; nervousness rather than rapport (Bernieri,
Hatfield, Cacioppo, & Rapson, 1994). An 1988; LaFrance & Ickes, 1981). The match­
example that coordination is not a suffi­ ing in these latter contexts was relatively sta­
cient component of rapport is demonstrated tic and unchanging, appearing rigid and
in the findings of a study of people interact­ strained (Bernieri, 1988). LaFrance and
ing via typing in an Internet-style chat Ickes (1981) have suggested such matching
room (Niederhoffer & Pennebaker, 2002). in anxiety-provoking contexts may reflect
Accommodation to one another’s linguistic an intention or effort to connect rather than
styles occurred regardless of rapport levels an actual expression of rapport. Even
(Giles & Coupland, 1991). Engagement in though coordinative behavior in these con­
the form of attentive interest appeared to be texts reflects feelings that are negative, such
a better indicator of rapport than the coordi­ patterns also reflect a prosocial, and there­
native process of accommodation. fore, positive intention; that is, the intention
Other studies suggest that attentiveness or desire to be in rapport. Such an inten­
and synchrony are additive or interactive tion, in the long run, actually may promote
with respect to rapport. Kendon (1970) rapport but, perhaps, not in short-lived,
found that observers who were present and experimental encounters.
attentive to, but not involved in, a conver­
sation between two people tended to syn­ Nonlinear and Dynamic Patterns of
chronize their movement to the speaker. Interpersonal Coordination. Some research
The two people who were engaged with has demonstrated that interpersonal coordi­
each other showed more pronounced nation varies along a looseness-to-tightness
20-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 5:12 PM Page 389

Nonverbal Behavior and Its Functions in the Ecosystem of Rapport–––◆–––389

dimension, and that this dimension is with the therapist accommodating more to
important for understanding its role in rap­ the client than vice versa (Lichtenberg et al.,
port (Bernieri & Rosenthal, 1991; Burgoon 1998).
et al., 1995; Cappella, 1996; Gottman, These findings suggest that the varia­
1994; Warner, Malloy, Schneider, Knoth, tion in degree of coordination reflects the
& Wilder, 1987; Watzlawick, Beavin, & dyad’s effort to optimize dyadic function­
Jackson, 1967). In general, a moderate ing. Dyadic functioning involves maintain­
degree of coordination between individuals ing connection to others in order to
indicates effective communication, bond­ accomplish a socially mediated task. Just as
ing, and positive interpersonal outcomes the attainment and maintenance of connec­
(Bernieri et al., 1988; Jaffe, Beebe, Feldstein, tion to others drives much social behavior
Crown, & Jasnow, 2001; Kritzer & (e.g., Burgoon et al., 1995; Cappella, 1996;
Valenti, 1990; Warner, 1992). Based on Goffman, 1967), it also appears, more
this research, Tickle-Degnen and Gavett specifically, to drive nonverbal behavior
(2003) divided speech therapy dyads into associated with rapport. Research on the
three categories—those having interper­ automatic functions of simple forms of
sonal coordination that was loose, tight, behavioral mimicry (e.g., face touching
or intermediate—based on the absolute or foot shaking) suggests that individuals
difference score between the therapist’s and mimic and respond to mimicry in order to
child’s rated levels of attentiveness or posi­ affiliate with one another (Lakin et al.,
tivity, across three slices in time within a 2003). Many factors (including subliminal
session. The higher the difference score, priming and recent failure to affiliate satis­
the “looser” the pattern of coordination. factorily with another person) that stimu­
Consistent with previous research findings, late affiliation and affiliation goals induce
therapists experienced higher rapport when mimicry and associated feelings of liking
there was an intermediate degree of (Lakin & Chartrand, 2003).
matched attentiveness or matched positiv­
ity, and experienced lower rapport when
the match was loose or tight. CONTEXT AND DIMENSIONALITY
The results from some studies suggest IN THE NONVERBAL BEHAVIOR
that very tightly coordinated action, as OF THE RAPPORT ECOSYSTEM
reflected in a relatively high degree of
symmetry, regularity, periodicity, or pre­ This overview of the recent research on
dictability in the interaction pattern, rapport demonstrates that the behavioral
appears to be indicative of a challenging, components of prosocial or positive expres­
uncomfortable social situation or an effort sivity, interpersonal coordination, and an
to overcome a problematic interaction. attentive engagement combine in function to
Cappella (1996), for example, found that initiate, produce, indicate, and sustain rap­
strangers were more likely to accommodate port. These behavioral components of rap­
to one another in their speech turn-taking port emerge in relation (a) to challenges in
than were well-acquainted individuals, sug­ the context of interaction, including chal­
gesting that “hyperpoliteness” operates in lenges to effective communication, connec­
initial encounters. In another study, thera­ tion, and relationship and (b) to achieving
pist-client dyads with ultimately unsuccess­ other beneficial dyadic outcomes. There is
ful, compared with successful, outcomes moderation, or a slight degree of “loose­
were characterized by tighter coordination, ness,” in the intensity and structure of
20-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 5:12 PM Page 390

390–––◆–––Functions

behavior when individuals experience rap­ different interpersonal tasks. Bernieri et al.
port. Relatively low or high levels of inten­ (1996) and Puccinelli et al. (2003, 2004)
sity in the behavioral components of rapport had each of their participating dyads per­
may indicate that individuals are either not form two tasks: one in which the purpose
connecting or attempting to do so in condi­ was to communicate important personal
tions that are challenging for preserving the information via face-to-face conversation,
interpersonal connection required to achieve and another one, in which the purpose was
beneficial dyadic outcomes. to solve a problem using props and physical
Outcome goals and process subgoals objects creatively in an object-focused task.
operate simultaneously and interactively Bernieri et al. (1996) compared an adver­
during interpersonal interaction (Austin sarial debate with a cooperative trip-
& Vancouver, 1996; Dijksterhuis & Bargh, planning task. They found more behavioral
2001) and shape the physical, spatial, elements to be associated with the dyad
and temporal structure of interaction. With members’ experienced rapport during the
respect to rapport, relevant outcome goals debate than during the trip-planning task.
include affiliation, enjoyment, and the In addition, the behavioral elements indica­
accomplishment of an interpersonal task tive of rapport during the debate were less
(e.g., getting a child to eat, completing a intercorrelated—that is, more multidimen­
project on deadline, or facilitating student sional—compared with those elements
learning). Relevant process subgoals include indicative of rapport during the trip-plan­
communicating internal states, enabling ning task, which were more intercorre­
partner responses, and showing agreement lated—that is, more unidimensional. In the
and cooperation. debate task, behaviors associated with rap­
There may be, at times, a simpler goal port included those involved in expressing
structure, that is, fewer goals and more and connecting, such as synchrony, proxim­
integrated or congruent goals, in contexts ity, and to a small degree expressivity, as
that support rapport than in contexts that well as those involved in conversing effec­
do not. Under a simple goal structure, tively face to face, such as the back-channel
behavioral positivity, coordination, and responses of head nodding and “uh hmms,”
attentiveness may covary with one another eye contact, and talkativeness. In the trip-
in a relatively stable fashion across the planning task, on the other hand, individu­
interaction, all being correlated positively als were active and expressive or were
with rapport. A complex goal structure relatively quiet and nonexpressive, and if
may occur in contexts that do not support they were active and expressive they had
rapport. Individuals may express negativity, higher rapport. Furthermore, observers
poor responsiveness, and inattention, and were able to detect the rapport experienced
mix or alternate these signals with others by the participants more accurately from the
that indicate their intention to attempt to more simple configuration of behavior
establish or restore rapport. In these situa­ encoded during the trip-planning task
tions, there may be more than one dimen­ than from the more complex configuration
sion in the correlational structure of encoded during the debate.
behavior and the experience of rapport. In a similar vein, Puccinelli et al. (2003,
The most direct support for this idea 2004) compared conversations about daily
of uni- versus multidimensionality to the life activities and aspirations with behav­
correlational structure of behavior and iors during a puzzle construction task. In
experienced rapport comes from studies of contrast with Bernieri et al. (1996), who
20-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 5:12 PM Page 391

Nonverbal Behavior and Its Functions in the Ecosystem of Rapport–––◆–––391

measured much of the dyads’ nonverbal decoded accurately by observers as indica­


behavior at a molecular level (e.g., coded tive of felt rapport.
amount of eye contact), Puccinelli et al. A set of studies (Dunkerley, Tickle-
(2003, 2004) measured nonverbal behavior Degnen, & Coster, 1997; Tickle-Degnen
at a more molar level (e.g., raters’ judgments & Coster, 1995) of occupational therapy
of degree of attentive behavior). Despite this sessions in a pediatric clinic demonstrated
difference in methods, Puccinelli et al.’s find­ similar findings to those of Bernieri et al.
ings were similar to those of Bernieri et al. (1996) and Puccinelli et al. (2003, 2004).
(1996), in that there was a higher degree of Two periods within a session were studied:
rapport-relevant behavior in the conversa­ The beginning minutes of the session, when
tion than puzzle task, yet observers were therapists and their child clients undertook
more sensitive to dyads’ experience of rap­ light and easy warm-up sensory-motor
port in the puzzle task. tasks, and the middle minutes, when the
Bernieri et al. (1996) and Puccinelli et al. dyads worked on more challenging tasks. In
(2003, 2004) made sense of their findings by these studies, experienced rapport was not
suggesting that their conversation tasks were assessed. Rather, the degree of dyadic rap­
more difficult interpersonally than their port in the videotaped sessions was judged
object-focused tasks. Individuals had to be by pediatric therapists.
expressive and talkative to accomplish the In the less-challenging beginning min­
purposes of the conversations successfully utes, rated child behaviors showed a unidi­
regardless of how much rapport they felt. mensional association with the dyad’s
The verbal content during these tasks would judged rapport. Rapport was judged to
be more likely to expose partners’ differences be higher as children showed a uniformly
in intention, attitude, and affect (and thus enjoyable, less anxious, and successful
threaten rapport). Nevertheless, the social performance, and judged to be lower as
context required behavior to be supportive children showed a more uniformly nega­
and accepting implicitly. Actual feelings may tive, more anxious, and unsuccessful per­
have been encoded less clearly under these formance. In the middle minutes, the
circumstances, and a high degree of social children appeared to be a bit overchal­
skill was required. The object-focused tasks lenged relative to their ability levels, and the
of trip planning and puzzle construction, on associations between child behaviors and
the other hand, did not require as much talk­ judged rapport showed a more complex
ing or negotiation of personal or intimate pattern than that shown in the easier,
territory for success. Thus, there was little beginning minutes. The pattern involved
possibility of the partners finding them­ two dimensions: positivity and anxious
selves at odds with one another, and self- effort. Dyadic rapport was judged as higher
presentation was of little concern. Success in children who showed enjoyment and
was determined clearly, simply, and imper­ success, but the children also showed a
sonally by using up the money for the trip or slight edge of anxiety, considerable physical
by making a puzzle design that matched a effort, and more help seeking. Rapport was
model. There would be little need to hide judged as lower when children showed less
feelings in this context and, thus, less social enjoyment, success, anxiety, and effort.
skill required than in the conversation tasks. Together these findings suggest that the
It is not surprising, then, that in the object- goals and procedures of interpersonal tasks
focused tasks, nonverbal expressivity held a create challenges that constrain and shape
direct association with rapport, and was the physical, spatial, and temporal structure
20-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 5:12 PM Page 392

392–––◆–––Functions

of nonverbal behavior indicative of rap­ incremental progress toward goal achieve­


port. Specifically, linearity, dynamics, and ment. Optimal experience is found more
dimensionality in the correlational struc­ frequently in structured than unstruc­
ture of experienced rapport and nonverbal tured tasks, and in work than nonwork
behavior appear to vary according to task activities, where task goals and progress are
goals and procedures. To understand better often overt and systematically monitored
the role of nonverbal behavior in rapport, (Csikszentmihalyi & LeFevre, 1989;
there is a need for research that systemati­ Massimini, Csikszentmihalyi, & Delle
cally relates task attributes and other con­ Fave, 1988).
textual factors to the complexity of patterns The optimal experience has both emo­
of behavior. tional and cognitive dimensions. A deeply
enjoyable activity is one in which individu­
als’ attention becomes absorbed in the doing
♦ Nonverbal Behavior in of an activity. Activity and awareness merge
the Regulation of Rapport as attention is focused on the task at hand.
During Interpersonal Tasks Individuals have feelings of competence.
Worries, hassles, distractions, and self-
consciousness fade, and although there is
A model developed by Csikszentmihalyi engaged intention, the experience is one of
and his colleague (Csikszentmihalyi, 1990; ease, enjoyment, and harmony. In
Csikszentmihalyi & Csikszentmihalyi, Csikszentmihalyi’s (1990) words, there is a
1988) may be useful for guiding future feeling of being in flow. Suboptimal experi­
investigations in this area. The model ences result from task conditions that fail to
explains optimal experience as emerging support challenging and skillful interaction
from individuals’ skill levels and as interac­ with the environment. According to this
tive with the challenges of the tasks they are model, boredom is the result of tasks and
performing. Although the model does not activities that offer too little challenge rela­
address nonverbal behavior directly, its tive to skill and that have little overt struc­
framework is general and flexible enough ture, purpose, and interactive feedback.
to include nonverbal behavior. It serves as Anxiety, on the other hand, occurs when
an exemplar for how task parameters and tasks and activities offer too great a chal­
other contextual factors might operate with lenge relative to skill and that have compli­
respect to nonverbal behavior and rapport. cated structure, ambiguous goals, and little
Csikszentmihalyi and colleague (Csiks­ feedback enabling successful performance.
zentmihalyi, 1990; Csikszentmihalyi & Optimal experience has been shown to
Csikszentmihalyi, 1988) have shown that be associated with dyadic rapport experi­
activities evoking an optimal experience are ence and nonverbal behavior. Puccinelli
ones that are challenging, but not too chal­ and Tickle-Degnen (2004) measured the
lenging, for one’s skill level. As applied to experience of rapport from items that ask
dyads, tasks that challenge dyads at their level dyad members to assess how much they
of interactive capacity or skill intellectually, and their partner felt attentive, positive,
physically, emotionally, or socially are most and coordinated, and measured the
likely to generate an optimal level of rapport. experience of flow from items developed
In addition to the match of task challenge by Csikszentmihalyi and Csikszentmihalyi
with skill, activities that evoke an optimal (1988). As self-reported rapport in dyads
experience are ones in which there are clear increased, so did shared feelings of enjoy­
goals and clearly marked steps in the ment and having the capacity to meet the
20-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 5:12 PM Page 393

Nonverbal Behavior and Its Functions in the Ecosystem of Rapport–––◆–––393

challenges of the situation. At the same time, skill. A complete research agenda requires
self-consciousness declined. Similarly, in that additional features and pressures, such
studies of therapist-child interactions as those related to individual and group
(Dunkerley et al., 1997; Tickle-Degnen & differences, be investigated systematically
Coster, 1995), as behavioral attentiveness, as well. For example, the individual in the
positivity, and coordination increased so dyad who has the highest level of trait
did the match between the challenge of expressivity drives rapport (Sullins, 1991;
therapeutic tasks and the child’s demon­ Tickle-Degnen & Puccinelli, 1999).
strated ability level increasingly. On the Relative to individuals who are less accu­
other hand, these behavioral correlates of rate in interpreting others’ inner states from
rapport showed very little relationship to nonverbal behavior, individuals who are
the task challenge as measured indepen­ more accurate have rapport experiences
dently of the child’s ability, which indicates that are more reactive or sensitive to the
that task challenge alone did not moderate other’s emotional traits (Puccinelli &
this behavior. Rather, nonverbal behavior Tickle-Degnen, 2004). Similarly, individu­
was associated with the child’s own capaci­ als who have strong social motives, such as
ties in interaction with the context of action. a need to belong or a tendency toward high
Table 20.1 shows the distinctions between self-monitoring, are more attentive to non­
optimal and suboptimal rapport experi­ verbal behavior and respond with greater
ences based on Csikszentmihalyi’s (1990) empathic accuracy (Pickett, Gardner, &
model, which I have modified for the pur­ Knowles, 2004) and behavioral mimicry
poses of understanding the rapport ecosys­ (Cheng & Chartrand, 2003).
tem. When conditions provide the just-right Emotional expressivity, nonverbal sensi­
challenge (i.e., not over- and not under- tivity, and social motives vary according to
challenging) to a dyad’s skill level, the gender, health conditions, and age as well.
dyad’s nonverbal behavior would clearly Females are more emotionally expressive
and simply express the rapport experience. generally, their own emotions are more
When conditions are overchallenging to responsive to the behavior of others
dyad skill, the behavior would show signs (Fischer, 2000), and their interactions are
of effort and nervousness and, perhaps, driven more by mutual participation goals
more behavioral elements or more intense (Strough & Berg, 2000) compared with
behavior indicative of attempts to establish males. Some health conditions, such as the
or recover rapport. When underchalleng­ movement disorder of Parkinson’s disease,
ing, there would be very little purposeful prevent individuals from expressing their
interpersonal behavior, unless there are inner states with clarity and responding
other implicit contextual pressures to estab­ to others’ states in a coordinated manner.
lish rapport. Observers often have negative impressions
and presume social apathy or negative social
motives, such as deceptiveness, inaccurately
♦ Individual and Group in these individuals, leading to stigmatiza­
Differences in the tion processes that prevent or erode rapport
(Lyons & Tickle-Degnen, 2003; Tickle-
Rapport Ecosystem
Degnen & Lyons, 2004). Interestingly, the
movement disorder of Parkinson’s disease
The nonverbal correlates of rapport are mimics the expressive behavior of the very
shaped by many contextual features and old (Singer, 1974) and can occur in younger
pressures besides task challenge and dyad and older adults who, otherwise, are
20-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 5:12 PM Page 394

394–––◆–––Functions

Table 20.1 Optimal Experience and Nonverbal Behavior in Rapport

Sub-optimal Optimal Experience: Sub-optimal


Experience: Bored Calm and Energized Experience: Anxious

Task conditions
Too little of a challenge Task challenge matches Overly challenging for dyad
for dyad members dyad members’ skills members
Unstructured Structured Complicated structure
Purposeless Clear goals Ambiguous goals
Feedback irrelevant Ongoing feedback about Little feedback about success
success in achieving goals in achieving goals

Thoughts and feelings


One’s competence is Competent Incompetent
irrelevant
Negative: bored Positive: calm and energized Negative: anxious
No concern, no interest Absorbed in interaction Overriding concern about
self
Partner is irrelevant Partner accepts one’s attitudes, Partner may not be accepting
intentions, and feelings of and responding to self
Emptiness Harmony, flow, in rapport Disharmony, disorder

Nonverbal behavior
Unfocused Absorbed attention Vigilant attention
Inactive, perhaps Expressive, prosocial Disregulated affect, perhaps
prosocial intentions affect and intentions prosocial intentions
No effort Engaged but relaxed Effortful, intense
Lethargic, off-task Calm, socially clear Nervous or socially
behavior behavior ambiguous behavior
Hypo-responsivity to Moderate responsivity to Excessive responsivity to
partner’s behavior partner’s behavior partner’s behavior

Rapport ecosystem
Unengaged Resilient, secure bond Insecure bond
Failure to develop or Ease in sustaining Difficulty effectively
maintain bond. There interaction toward interacting toward goal
may be attempts to goal achievement achievement. Failure to
create or restore bond. develop or maintain bond.
There may be attempts to
create or restore bond.
NOTE: Table influenced by Csikszentmihalyi (1990).

developmentally normal. Research in this In addition, cultural contexts influence


area can inform the role of aging on the the properties of nonverbal behavior related
nonverbal correlates of rapport. to rapport. In Asian cultures, moderation of
20-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 5:12 PM Page 395

Nonverbal Behavior and Its Functions in the Ecosystem of Rapport–––◆–––395

emotion and expression are seen as central to to the rapport experience is far from
achieving harmonious relationships with understood and requires more research.
others, an achievement highly valued within Despite the call in this chapter to integrate
the Confucian tradition and collectivistic cul­ an understanding of rapport with optimal
ture (Chang & Holt, 1994). This emphasis experience, rapport has been studied rarely in
on moderation contrasts to the emphasis contexts in which conditions are favorable
on personal expression in individualistic for such experience. Yet social enjoyment
cultures. For example, Asians compared is one of the primary types of satisfaction
with North Americans show more control of that individuals experience and seek out
facial expressivity in response to emotionally (Csikszentmihalyi & LeFevre, 1989). To fur­
evocative stimuli (Okazaki, Liu, Longworth, ther understand the implications of this con­
& Minn, 2002). tention, more research is needed on describing
people’s tasks when they have optimal social
experience and what their behavior looks like
♦ Implications for
under these optimal conditions. Specifically,
Future Research the review of the literature on rapport and
nonverbal behavior suggests the following
People experience rapport with their hypothesis: When contextual factors support
family members, friends, physicians, rapport, nonverbal behavior will show a clear
teachers, and coworkers as they engage in and simple relationship to experienced rap­
tasks that vary in how challenging, struc­ port. On the others hand, when contextual
tured, comfortable, or intimate they are. In factors threaten or undermine rapport, non­
contrast to the variety of task conditions verbal behavior will show a complex or
found in natural social life, the laboratory, ambiguous relationship to experienced rap­
in which most research on rapport is con­ port. To test this hypothesis, we need to
ducted, manipulates or represents only a clarify what is meant by “simple” or “com­
limited array of conditions. It presents spe­ plex” relationship, and the following con­
cialized and unique sets of task constraints structs require more careful development and
that influence patterns of nonverbal operationalization: linearity, loose versus tight
behavior and their functional associations coordination, and dimensionality in the asso­
with outcomes (Grahe & Sherman, 2005; ciation of nonverbal behavior with rapport.
Tamir et al., 2004). Experimental interac­ The experience of rapport also requires
tion tasks involve unacquainted or mildly more careful elaboration from a temporal
acquainted participants typically, and or dynamic perspective (see Cappella &
implicit norms require pleasantness and Schreiber, this volume). Individuals may
appropriate modulation of emotion. fluctuate in the strength of their rapport
Research on rapport and nonverbal com­ over time, yet they may have strong motives
munication must take into account these to retain rapport. At times, they may not
constraints operating on behavior. We feel in rapport with one another, but their
should also consider how laboratory task behavior demonstrates features that are
constraints related to interpersonal expres­ somewhat congruent with what would be
sivity and coordination are similar to or demonstrated if their rapport experience
deviate from the everyday situations of were stronger. Tracking the development,
participants outside the laboratory. waning, and restoration of rapport
Finally, how individual and group differ­ across time in enduring relationships will
ences interact with task constraints as shed light on these differences in rapport
nonverbal behavior mediates and responds experiences. Finally, there is a need for
20-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 5:12 PM Page 396

396–––◆–––Functions

measures that assess not only being in rap­ of Personality and Social Psychology, 54,
port but also desire to be in rapport. 243–253.
Together, this work will enable a theo­ Bernieri, F. J., & Rosenthal, R. (1991). Inter­
retically more coherent approach to study­ personal coordination: Behavior matching
and interactional synchrony. In R. Feldman
ing the nonverbal correlates of rapport and
& R. Rime (Eds.), Fundamentals of nonver­
interpreting the results. Models such as
bal behavior (pp. 401–432). Cambridge,
that of Csikszentmihalyi (Csikszentmihalyi,
UK: Cambridge University Press.
1990; Csikszentmihalyi & Csikszentmihalyi, Boone, T., & Buck, R. (2003). Emotional
1988) are helpful in guiding this work for­ expressivity and trustworthiness: The role
ward. They illustrate how optimal experi­ of nonverbal behavior in the evolution
ences, such as rapport, emerge in the of cooperation. Journal of Nonverbal
context of accomplishing interpersonal Behavior, 27, 163–182.
tasks to meet personal, dyadic, and group Brunswik, E. (1955). Representative design and
needs. These models can be used to organize probabilistic theory. Psychological Review,
the systematic investigation of how nonver­ 62, 193–217.
bal behavior functions in context to initiate, Burgoon, J. K, Stern, L. A., & Dillman, L.
(1995). Interpersonal adaptation: Dyadic
produce, indicate, and sustain rapport.
interaction patterns. Cambridge, UK:
Cambridge University Press.
Butler, E. A., Egloff, B., Wilhelm, F. H., Smith,
♦ References N. C., Erickson, A., & Gross, J. J. (2003).
The social consequence of expression sup­
pression. Emotion, 3, 48–67.
Ambady, N., Bernieri, F. J., & Richeson, J. A. Cappella, J. N. (1996). Dynamic coordination of
(2000). Toward a histology of social vocal and kinesic behavior in dyadic inter­
behavior: Judgmental accuracy from thin action: Methods, problems, and interper­
slices of the behavioral stream. Advances in sonal outcomes. In J. H. Watt & C. A.
Experimental Social Psychology, 32, VanLear (Eds.), Dynamic patterns in
201–271. communication processes (pp. 353–386).
Austin, J. T., & Vancouver, J. B. (1996). Goal Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
constructs in psychology: Structure, process, Chang, H-C, & Holt, G. R. (1994). A Chinese
and content. Psychological Bulletin, 120, perspective on face as inter-relational con­
338–375. cern. In S. Ting-Toomey (Ed.), The chal­
Bem, D. J. (1972). Self-perception theory. In lenge of facework: Cross-cultural and
L. Berkowitz (Ed.), Advances in experimen­ interpersonal issues (pp. 95–132). Albany:
tal social psychology (Vol. 6, pp. 1–62). State University of New York Press.
New York: Academic Press. Chartrand, T. L., & Bargh, J. A. (1999). The
Bernieri, F. (1988). Coordinated movement and chameleon effect: The perception-behavior
rapport in teacher-student interactions. link and social interaction. Journal of
Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 12, Personality and Social Psychology, 76,
120–138. 893–910.
Bernieri, F. J., Gillis, J. S., Davis, J. M., & Grahe, Cheng, C. M., & Chartrand, T. L. (2003). Self-
J. E. (1996). Dyad rapport and the accuracy monitoring without awareness: Using mim­
of its judgments across situations: A lens icry as a nonconscious affiliation strategy.
model analysis. Journal of Personality and Journal of Personality and Social Psy­
Social Psychology, 71, 110–129. chology, 85, 1170–1179.
Bernieri, F. J., Reznick, J. S., & Rosenthal, R. Compact Oxford English dictionary. (2005).
(1988). Synchrony, pseudosynchrony, and Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.
dissynchrony: Measuring the entrainment Retrieved May 5, 2005, from www
process in mother-infant interactions. Journal .askoxford.com
20-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 5:12 PM Page 397

Nonverbal Behavior and Its Functions in the Ecosystem of Rapport–––◆–––397

Crown, C. L., Feldstein, S., Jasnow, M. D., J. M. Fernandez-Dols (Eds.), The psychol­
Beebe, B., & Jaffe, J. (2001). The cross- ogy of facial expression (pp. 103–129).
modal coordination of interpersonal tim­ Paris: Cambridge University Press.
ing: Six-week-old infants’ gaze with adults’ Gibson, J. J. (1986). The ecological approach to
vocal behavior. Journal of Psycholinguistic visual perception. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Research, 30, 1–23. (Original work published 1979).
Csikszentmihalyi, M. (1990). Flow: The psy­ Giles, H., & Coupland, N. (1991). Language:
chology of optimal experience. New York: Contexts and consequences. Pacific Grove,
HarperCollins. CA: Brooks/Cole.
Csikszentmihalyi, M., & Csikszentmihalyi, I. S. Goffman, E. (1967). Interaction ritual: Essays on
(1988). Optimal experience: Psychologi­ face-to-face behavior. New York: Pantheon.
cal studies of flow in consciousness. Gottman, J. M. (1994). What predicts divorce?
Cambridge,UK: Cambridge University Press. The relationship between marital proc­
Csikszentmihalyi, M., & LeFevre, J. (1989). esses and marital outcomes. Hillsdale, NJ:
Optimal experience in work and leisure. Erlbaum.
Journal of Personality and Social Psycho­ Grahe, J. E., & Bernieri, F. J. (2002). Self-
logy, 56, 815–822. awareness of judgment policies of rapport.
DePaulo, B. M. (1992). Nonverbal behavior and Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin,
self-presentation. Psychological Bulletin, 28, 1407–1418.
111, 203–243. Grahe, J. E., & Sherman, R. A. (2005). An eco­
Dijksterhuis, A., & Bargh, J. A. (2001). The per­ logical examination of rapport using
ception-behavior expressway: Automatic a dyadic puzzle task. Unpublished manu­
effects of social perception on social behav­ script.
ior. In M. Zanna (Ed.), Advances in exper­ Grammer, K. Kruck, K. B., & Magnusson, M. S.
imental social psychology (Vol. 33, (1998). The courtship dance: Patterns of
pp. 1–40). San Diego, CA: Academic Press. nonverbal synchronization in opposite-sex
Dunkerley, E., Tickle-Degnen, L., & Coster, W. encounters. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior,
(1997). Therapist-child interaction in the 22, 3–29.
middle minutes of sensory integration treat­ Hatfield, E., Cacioppo, J. T., & Rapson, R. L.
ment. American Journal of Occupational (1994). Emotional contagion. Paris:
Therapy, 51, 799–805. Cambridge University Press.
Ekman, P., & Keltner, D. (1997). Universal Heft, H. (2003). Affordances, dynamic
facial expressions of emotion: An old con­ experience, and the challenge of reifica­
troversy and new findings. In U. Segerstråle tion. Ecological Psychology, 15, 149–180.
& P. Molnár (Eds.), Nonverbal communi­ Izard, C. (1994). Innate and universal facial
cation: Where nature meets culture (pp. expressions: Evidence from developmen­
27–46). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. tal cross-cultural research. Psychological
Elfenbein, H. A., & Ambady, N. (2002). Bulletin, 115, 288–299.
Predicting workplace outcomes from the Jaffe, J., Beebe, B., Feldstein, S., Crown, C. L., &
ability to eavesdrop on feelings. Journal of Jasnow, M. D. (2001). Rhythms of dialogue
Applied Psychology, 87, 963–971. in infancy: Coordinated timing in develop­
Fischer, A. H. (Ed.). (2000). Gender and emo­ ment. Monographs of the Society for
tion: Social psychological perspectives. Research in Child Development, 66(2),
Paris: Cambridge University Press. vi-131.
Freud, S. (1924). On the history of the psycho­ James, W. (1890). Principles of psychology.
analytic movement. In J. Riviere (Trans.) New York: Holt.
& E. Jones (Ed.), Collected papers (Vol. I, Kendon, A. (1970). Movement coordination in
pp. 287–359). London: Hogarth. (Original social interaction: Some examples described.
work published 1914) Acta Psychologica, 32, 100–125.
Fridlund, A. J. (1997). The new ethology of Kritzer, R., & Valenti, S. S. (1990, March).
human facial expression. In J. A. Russell & Rapport in therapist-client interactions:
20-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 5:12 PM Page 398

398–––◆–––Functions

An ecological analysis of the effects of Niederhoffer, K. G., & Pennebaker, J. W.


nonverbal sensitivity and interactional (2002). Linguistic style matching in social
synchrony. Paper presented at the 61st interaction. Journal of Language and Social
annual meeting of the Eastern Psychological Psychology, 21, 337–360.
Association, Philadelphia. Okazaki, S., Liu, J. F., Longworth, S. L., &
LaFrance, M. (1979). Nonverbal synchrony and Minn, J. Y. (2002). Asian American-
rapport: Analysis by the cross-lag panel White American differences in expressions
technique. Social Psychology Quarterly, of social anxiety: A replication and
42, 66–70. extension. Cultural Diversity and Ethnic
LaFrance, M. (1990). The trouble with rapport. Minority Psychology, 8, 234–247.
Psychological Inquiry, 1, 318–320. Papoušek, H., & Papoušek, M. (1997).
LaFrance, M., & Broadbent, M. (1976). Group Preverbal communication in humans and
rapport: Posture sharing as nonverbal indi­ the genesis of culture. In U. Segerstråle &
cator. Group and Organization Studies, 1, P. Molnár (Eds.), Nonverbal communica­
328–333. tion: Where nature meets culture (pp.
LaFrance, M., & Ickes, W. (1981). Posture 87–108). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
mirroring and interactional involvement: Park, R. E., & Burgess, E. W. (1924).
Sex and sex typing effects. Journal of Introduction to the science of sociology.
Nonverbal Behavior, 5, 139–154. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
Lakin, J. L., & Chartrand, T. L. (2003). Using Patterson, M. L. (1994). Strategic functions of
nonconscious behavioral mimicry to nonverbal behavior. In J. A. Daly & J. M.
create affiliation and rapport. Psychological Wiemann (Eds.), Strategic interpersonal
Science, 14, 334–339. communication (pp. 273–293). Hillsdale,
Lakin, J. L., Jefferis, A. E., Cheng, C. M., & NJ: Erlbaum.
Chartrand, T. L. (2003). The chameleon Patterson, M. L. (1999). The evolution of a par­
effect as social glue: Evidence for the evolu­ allel process model of nonverbal communi­
tionary significance of nonconscious mim­ cation. In P. Philippot, R. S. Feldman, &
icry. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 27, E. J. Coats (Eds.), The social context of
145–162. nonverbal behavior (pp. 317–347). Paris:
Lichtenberg, J. W., Wettersten, K. B., Mull, H., Cambridge University Press.
Moberly, R. L., Merkley, K. B., & Corey, Patterson, M. L. (2003). Evolution and nonver­
A. T. (1998). Relationship formation and bal behavior: Functions and mediating
relational control as correlates of psy­ processes. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior,
chotherapy quality and outcome. Journal of 27, 201–207.
Counseling Psychology, 45, 322–337. Pickett, C. L., Gardner, W. L., & Knowles, M.
Lyons, K. D., & Tickle-Degnen, L. (2003). (2004). Getting a cue: The need to belong
Dramaturgical challenges of Parkinson’s and enhanced sensitivity to social cues.
disease. Occupational Therapy Journal of Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin,
Research: Occupation, Participation, and 30, 1095–1107.
Health, 23, 27–34. Puccinelli, N. M., & Tickle-Degnen, L. (2004).
Massimini, F., Csikszentmihalyi, M., & Delle Fave, Knowing too much about others: Modera­
A. (1988). Flow and biocultural evolution. In tors of the relationship between eavesdrop­
M. Csikszentmihalyi & I. S. Csikszentmihalyi ping and rapport in social interaction.
(Eds.), Optimal experience: Psychological Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 28,
studies of flow in consciousness (pp. 60–81). 223–243.
Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press. Puccinelli, N. M., Tickle-Degnen, L., &
McArthur, L. Z., & Baron, R. M. (1983). Rosenthal, R. (2003). Effect of dyadic
Toward an ecological theory of social context on judgments of rapport: Dyad
perception. Psychological Review, 90, task and partner presence. Journal of
215–238. Nonverbal Behavior, 27, 211–236.
20-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 5:12 PM Page 399

Nonverbal Behavior and Its Functions in the Ecosystem of Rapport–––◆–––399

Puccinelli, N. M., Tickle-Degnen, L., & In P. Philippot, R. S. Feldman, & E. J. Coats


Rosenthal, R. (2004). Effect of target (Eds.), Nonverbal behavior in clinical settings
position and target task on judge sensitiv­ (pp. 75–110). Oxford, UK: Oxford
ity to felt rapport. Journal of Nonverbal University Press.
Behavior, 28, 211–220. Tickle-Degnen, L., & Lyons, K. D. (2004).
Renfro, S., & Rauh, C. (2005, May). Video delay Practitioners’ impressions of patients with
effects on emotions, communication out­ Parkinson’s disease: The social ecology of
comes, and involvement. Paper presented at the expressive mask. Social Science &
the 55th annual conference of the Inter­ Medicine, 58, 603–614.
national Communication Association, New Tickle-Degnen, L., & Puccinelli, N. (1999). The
York. nonverbal expression of negative emotions:
Riggio, H. R., & Riggio, R. E. (2002). Peer and supervisor responses to occupa­
Emotional expressiveness, extraversion, tional therapy students’ emotional attrib­
and neuroticism: A meta-analysis. Journal utes. Occupational Therapy Journal of
of Nonverbal Behavior, 26, 195–218. Research, 19, 18–39.
Rosenthal, R. (1982). Conducting judgment Tickle-Degnen, L., & Rosenthal, R. (1987).
studies. In K. R. Scherer & P. Ekman (Eds.), Group rapport and nonverbal behavior.
Handbook of methods in nonverbal behav­ In C. Hendrick (Ed.), Review of per­
ior research (pp. 287–361). New York: sonality and social psychology: Volume 9,
Cambridge University Press. Group processes and intergroup relations
Singer, E. (1974). Premature social aging: The (pp. 113–136). Newbury Park, CA: Sage.
social-psychological consequences of a Tickle-Degnen, L., & Rosenthal, R. (1990). The
chronic illness. Social Science & Medicine, nature of rapport and its nonverbal corre­
8, 143–151. lates. Psychological Inquiry, 1, 285–293.
Strack, F., & Deutsch, R. (2004). Reflective and van Baaren, R. B., Holland, R. W., Kawakami,
impulsive determinants of social behavior. K., & van Knippenberg, A. (2004). Mimicry
Personality and Social Psychology Review, and prosocial behavior. Psychological
8, 220–247. Science, 15, 71–74.
Strough, J., & Berg, C. A. (2000). Goals as a Warner, R. M. (1992). Speaker, partner and
mediator of gender differences in high- observer evaluations of affect during social
affiliation dyadic conversations. Develop­ interaction as a function of interaction
mental Psychology, 36, 117–125. tempo. Journal of Language and Social
Sullins, E. S. (1991). Emotional contagion revis­ Psychology, 11, 253–266.
ited: Effects of social comparison and Warner, R. M., Malloy, D., Schneider, K.,
expressive style on mood convergence. Knoth, R., & Wilder, B. (1987). Rhythmic
Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, organization of social interaction
17, 166–174. and observer ratings of positive affect
Tamir, M., Robinson, M. D., Clore, G. L., Martin, and involvement. Journal of Nonverbal
L. L., & Whitaker, D. J. (2004). Are we pup­ Behavior, 11, 57–74.
pets on a string? The contextual meaning of Watzlawick, P., Beavin, J., & Jackson, D. D.
unconscious expressive cues. Personality and (1967). Pragmatics of human communica­
Social Psychology Bulletin, 30, 237–249. tion: A study of interactional patterns,
Tickle-Degnen, L., & Coster, W. (1995). pathologies, and paradoxes. New York:
Therapeutic interaction and the management Norton.
of challenge during the beginning minutes of Zebrowitz, L. A., & Collins, M. A. (1997).
sensory integration treatment. Occupational Accurate social perception at zero acquain­
Therapy Journal of Research, 15, 122–141. tance: The affordances of a Gibsonian
Tickle-Degnen, L., & Gavett, E. (2003). approach. Personality and Social Psychology
Changes in nonverbal behavior during the Review, 1, 204–223.
development of therapeutic relationships.
20-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 5:12 PM Page 400
21-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 6:11 PM Page 401

PART IV

CONTEXTS AND
CONSEQUENCES
21-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 6:11 PM Page 402
21-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 6:11 PM Page 403

21
NONVERBAL COMMUNICATION
IN CLOSE RELATIONSHIPS

� Patricia Noller
University of Queensland

T he importance of communication in close relationships may be


seen in Wood’s (1995) claim that “communication is not only a
central, generative process of intimacy, but is actually what we experi­
ence as relationships” ([italics added] p. 125). Whereas communication
can be both verbal and nonverbal, evidence suggests that the nonverbal
channels may be particularly crucial to relationship processes and out­
comes (Gottman, Markman, & Notarius, 1977; Noller, 1984; Nowicki
& Duke, 2001). The communicative potential of nonverbal cues con­
tributes to their importance in relationships. As Manusov (2002) points
out, “One of the most intriguing aspects of nonverbal communication
is its ability to be interpreted in myriad ways” (p. 15).
Nowhere is this fact more evident, or more problematic, than in close
relationships, in which nonverbal communication performs two impor­
tant functions: the expression of emotions (expressing one’s feelings
about the self, the partner, or some other person or thing) and the dis­
play of relational messages (indicating the type of relationship, e.g.,
close vs. distant; Manusov, 2002). In particular, their ability to “signal
how participants regard each other, their relationship, and themselves in
the relationship” (Burgoon & Dillman, 1995, p. 63) makes nonverbal

◆ 403
21-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 6:11 PM Page 404

404–––◆–––Contexts and Consequences

cues particularly pertinent to relationships. chapter. In this chapter, my goal is to


In Noller’s (1995) study, for example, the review the literature on nonverbal commu­
most frequent way in which negative mes­ nication in close relationships from both an
sages were sent to partners, irrespective of empirical and a theoretical perspective. I
gender or relationship satisfaction, was focus first on interpersonal sensitivity, or
with a smile accompanied by negative the success in decoding nonverbal commu­
words or tone of voice. This set of behav­ nication, in close relationships. I follow
iors can be interpreted in terms of spouses this with discussion of nonverbal expres­
wishing to get their complaint across to siveness, or the encoding of nonverbal
the partner but wishing to send a positive communication in close relationships. I
message about the relationship at the then discuss what is known about the role
same time. of nonverbal communication in courtship.
In addition to (and perhaps because of) Finally, I focus on nonverbal communica­
their ability to be interpreted in an array of tion in the context of two other influential
ways, nonverbal cues are also particularly theories of close relationships: attachment
able to be misunderstood. Misunderstand­ theory and attribution theory.
ings may occur either because the message
was not sent clearly (an encoding error) or
because the message, although sent clearly, ♦ Decoding Sensitivity
was not decoded accurately (a decoding
error; Noller, 1984; for a general discussion
of encoding and decoding skills, see Riggio, Interpersonal sensitivity concerns the ability
this volume). Friedman (2001) notes that to decode others’ nonverbal communication
the success of nonverbal communication accurately and thus be sensitive to the emo­
depends on factors related to the communi­ tional material they are expressing through
cator and factors relating to the perceiver. the nonverbal channel. Friedman (2001)
The factors that he specifies as relevant to notes that there are several paradoxes with
encoding include that person’s communica­ regard to interpersonal sensitivity: “In vari­
tion skills and his or her motivation to be ous situations it appears that a tremendous
accurate or not in conveying the message, amount of important interpersonal knowl­
the situational context (e.g., an argument edge is being rapidly communicated, mostly
or an intimate conversation, public or pri­ nonverbally” (p. 351). Nevertheless, or per­
vate), and the communicator’s current emo­ haps precisely because so much information
tional state (such as angry or excited). For is being transmitted, “there is a great deal
the perceiver, decoding is affected by fac­ of mis-information and misunderstanding
tors such as personality, the extent to which in face-to-face human relations” (Friedman,
he or she is paying attention or is distracted, 2001, p. 351).
and the motivation to understand what the According to Zebrowitz (2001), increas­
partner is seeking to communicate. ing our understanding of the processes
The possibility for errors—and what involved in decoding nonverbal behavior is
may contribute to more successful encod­ important because decoding sensitivity pre­
ing and decoding of nonverbal cues—is dicts more effective social functioning in
perhaps most germane for looking at the general (Losoya & Eisenberg, 2001; Noller,
consequentiality of nonverbal communica­ 2001; Nowicki & Duke, 2001). Losoya and
tion in relationships. For this reason, it Eisenberg (2001) focus on affective empathy,
provides the primary focus of the present which is the ability to recognize a person’s
21-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 6:11 PM Page 405

Nonverbal Communication in Close Relationships–––◆–––405

emotion and also feel that emotion with the less close friends were more accurate than
person. The authors review studies con­ close friends, particularly if the senders had
cerned with empathy-related responding (or been asked to conceal the negative emotion
responding in a way that lets the other (making the message more ambiguous).
person know that one empathizes with their These researchers suggest that close friends
situation), including those studies involving may have more to lose than do less close
the assessment of facial and gestural cues of friends if they perceive concealed negativity
young children (e.g., Miller, Eisenberg, accurately. These friends may believe that
Fabes, & Shell, 1996; Zahn-Waxler, if the negative message is ignored, the issue
Radke-Yarrow, Wagner, & Chapman, 1992; will go away.
Zahn-Waxler, Robinson, & Emde, 1992). Another relational feature is overall sat­
Using actual facial expressions of emo­ isfaction, and it too may be related to sensi­
tion in research studies to assess people’s tivity. In support of this suggestion, Noller
empathy can help deal with the problem (1984, 2001), using the standard-content
of social desirability affecting self-report paradigm (a research procedure that
responses and the tendency, particularly involves the use of ambiguous words to
of older children and adults, to react in ensure that decoders are focusing on the
socially expected ways in more constrained nonverbal cues; see Noller, 2005a), found
research paradigms. Losoya and Eisenberg a relation between decoding accuracy and
(2001) show that affective empathy can be relationship satisfaction: Those who were
distinguished from sympathy (which does happy in their marriages, and particularly
not involve feeling the person’s distress), husbands, were more accurate at decoding
however, and that affective empathy is their spouses than those who were unhappy
correlated with the overall quality of social in their marriages. The study also showed
functioning. that satisfied spouses were more accurate
Besides individual skill in affective empa­ at decoding their spouses than they were
thy, there is also some evidence for a “close­ at decoding strangers, whereas those who
ness of relationship effect” on decoding were unhappy with their marriages were
sensitivity, with close relationship partners’ more accurate at decoding strangers than
greater knowledge about each other’s lives they were at decoding their spouses. In addi­
leading to greater accuracy at decoding tion, Noller and Ruzzene (1991) showed
nonverbal behavior. For example, romantic that happy couples were more accurate
partners (Noller & Ruzzene, 1991) and than unhappy couples at identifying the
friends (Fleming, Darley, Hilton, & type of affect being experienced by their
Kojetin, 1990; Stinson & Ickes, 1992) are partners and were also better able to iden­
generally better at decoding one another’s tify their partners’ goals and intentions in
nonverbal cues than are strangers. More an interaction.
specifically, Sternglanz and DePaulo (2004) In a later study involving newlyweds,
compared the accuracy of friends and and also using the standard-content para­
strangers at decoding nonverbal cues to digm, Noller and Feeney (1998) reported
emotion and found that, overall, friends that nonverbal accuracy increased over
were more accurate than strangers at time for all three message types: positive,
decoding cues to emotion, and close friends neutral, and negative. This finding suggests
were more accurate at decoding clearly that couples may become better at under­
expressed negative emotions. For some standing each other’s nonverbal behavior
negative emotions in their study, however, over time, although practice effects, while
21-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 6:11 PM Page 406

406–––◆–––Contexts and Consequences

unlikely, cannot be ruled out (see Noller & affected negatively in relationships char­
Feeney, 1994, for more discussion of this acterized by high levels of interdependence,
issue). We also found that wife-to-husband high levels of insecurity, and consequent
communications were decoded more accu­ concerns about the vulnerability of the rela­
rately than were husband-to-wife commu­ tionship. On the other hand, Simpson,
nications, particularly if the messages were Ickes, and Grich (1999) found that anx­
positive. This finding is similar to that in iously attached women became hypervigi­
the earlier study where the effect was a lant and hence more accurate when the
function of the more accurate encoding relationship was under threat. This latter
of positive messages by wives. Husbands’ finding suggests that a complex model
accuracy at decoding positive messages is needed to account for the role of cogni­
from their wives was associated with their tive biases in explaining decoding in close
current marital satisfaction, and husbands’ relationships.
earlier satisfaction predicted their later Simpson, Orina, and Ickes (2003) tested
accuracy at decoding their wives’ commu­ their empathic accuracy model further in a
nications across all three types of messages. sample of married couples and found sup­
It seems likely that these husbands’ decod­ port for their predictions. Feelings of close­
ing was more accurate because their ability ness decreased for those who were more
was not affected by negative perceptions of empathically accurate when their partners
the relationship with the partner. reported relationship-threatening thoughts
and feelings, and feelings of closeness
increased for those accurate decoders whose
MOTIVATED INACCURACY partners reported nonthreatening thoughts
and feelings. Similar results were found
Ickes and Simpson (1997, 2001) have when outsiders rated the level of threat con­
proposed an empathic accuracy model that tained in the partners’ thoughts and feel­
may help to explain some of the findings ings. There was also evidence in these data
just discussed. They suggested that there are that an individual’s own perception of rela­
times when those in close relationships are tionship vulnerability, and not just his or
actually motivated to be inaccurate in terms her perception of the partner’s sense of rela­
of their decoding of one another’s nonver­ tionship vulnerability, affected the sense of
bal behavior. The authors argue that this closeness with the partner. As before, these
motivated inaccuracy can be explained in findings fit with those of Sternglanz and
terms of the threatening versus nonthreat­ DePaulo (2004), who found some evidence
ening nature of the discussion in which for motivated inaccuracy in the decoding of
the partners are engaged. They proposed a close friends.
positive relation between accuracy and
relationship satisfaction when the material
being discussed is relatively nonthreatening BIAS IN DECODING
and a negative relation when the discussion
is contentious, conflicted, and threatening The decoding of nonverbal cues may be
to the relationship. biased in various ways, particularly by
Simpson, Ickes, and their colleagues cognitive processes that make individuals
have carried out several studies to test this prone to distortions of perception in a nega­
model. For example, Simpson, Ickes, and tive direction. For example, although Noller
Blackstone (1995) showed, in a sample of (1984) did not find an expected bias related
dating couples, that empathic accuracy was to marital satisfaction, the study did reveal a
21-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 6:11 PM Page 407

Nonverbal Communication in Close Relationships–––◆–––407

bias related to gender. Wives tended to maintained irrespective of the valence of the
make errors in a positive direction, decod­ stimuli. In a more complex empathic accu­
ing negative messages as neutral and neutral racy task, these researchers were also able
messages as positive, whereas husbands to show an effect of need to belong on indi­
tended to make them in a negative direction, viduals’ sensitivity to the thoughts and feel­
decoding positive messages as neutral and ings of another. Thus, those with a strong
neutral messages as negative. Gaelick, need to belong are likely to be highly sensi­
Bodenhausen, and Wyer (1985) found a tive to close others’ nonverbal communi­
similar bias: Spouses reciprocated the emo­ cation. In a study assessing the extent to
tion they thought their partner was express­ which need to belong and rejection sensitiv­
ing, but because they were less accurate at ity could predict accuracy on the social per­
decoding partners’ expressions of love, only ception task, known as The Diagnostic
hostility tended to be reciprocated. Analysis of Nonverbal Accuracy 2 (Nowicki
Downey and Feldman (1996) have & Duke, 1994), however, Pickett et al.
explored the construct of rejection sensitiv­ (2004) found that accuracy was predicted
ity as leading to bias in the decoding of by the need to belong, but it was not pre­
nonverbal behavior. They argued that rejec­ dicted by rejection sensitivity. They explain
tion-sensitive individuals are prone, because this difference in terms of the nature of
of their heightened anxiety and sensitiv­ the task, arguing that rejection sensitivity
ity, to perceive ambiguous nonverbal cues is likely to be more influential in a task
as indications of dislike and rejection. involving ambiguous stimuli that could be
Rejection sensitivity tends to be maladap­ interpreted as rejecting.
tive for relationships (Downey, Freitas,
Michaelis, & Khouri, 1998), because rejec­
tion-sensitive individuals tend to focus on SUMMARY
any potential negativity in a message and
ignore or fail to perceive positivity. Likewise, Overall, decoding accuracy tends to be
Pickett, Gardner, and Knowles (2004) argue affected by the closeness of the relationship,
that individuals who are chronically high in although accuracy may be lower in very
the need to belong are likely to be hypervig­ close relationships when communications
ilant in scanning the social environment for are perceived as threatening the stability of
indications of acceptance or rejection, espe­ the relationship. The quality of the relation­
cially if they are lonely or have experienced ship also influences decoding accuracy, at
rejection continually (Gardner, Jefferis, least in some studies. There is also evidence
Knowles, & Pickett, 2003; Gardner, Pickett that decoding accuracy can be affected by
& Brewer, 2000). gender and by such cognitive processes as
Need to belong and rejection sensitivity rejection sensitivity and the need to belong.
are likely to be related in terms of their As shall be discussed in a later section of
effects on nonverbal decoding. Pickett et al. this chapter, decoding accuracy may also be
(2004) explored the association between affected by attachment insecurity.
the need to belong and accuracy in the iden­
tification of facial expression and vocal
tone. They found that accuracy in identify­ ♦ Nonverbal Expressiveness
ing both facial expressions and vocal tone
was positively correlated with scores on
a measure of need to belong. The authors Accurate expression of emotion (or inten­
also reported that this relationship was tion, according to Fridlund, 1994; Fridlund
21-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 6:11 PM Page 408

408–––◆–––Contexts and Consequences

& Russell, this volume) is important in negative behavior) when wives were most
close relationships. Much of the time, each stressed. Neff and Karney argue, on the
partner needs to know how the other is feel­ basis of these data, that husbands and wives
ing so that those feelings can be taken into are equally capable of providing support
account in their attempts to solve problems to their partners, but that wives seem able
and make decisions. It is also important for to provide that support when it is most
partners to let one another know when they needed. It may be that husbands do not rec­
are feeling sad or disappointed so that the ognize the cues indicating their wives’
appropriate level of social support can be greater levels of stress or that they believe
provided. Nevertheless, individuals differ support will be more effective if offered
in their willingness to express their feelings, in “calmer” moments. Alternatively, hus­
and this willingness can be affected by gen­ bands more than wives may resent the
der, by their ambivalence about the value expectation that they will support their
of communicating feelings, by the type of wives at times of distress and become nega­
relationship in which they are involved, tive in response. A further possibility is that
and by the quality of their relationship. husbands, because of their greater emphasis
Specifically, nonverbal behaviors are on autonomy and independence, are less
central to the expression of love in close sympathetic to those who need to seek
relationships. A study by Gonzaga, Keltner, support (MacGeorge, 2003).
Londahl, and Smith (2001) indicated that Willingness to express feelings depends,
behaviors, such as head nods, forward at least to some extent, on the individuals’
leans, and Duchenne (felt as opposed to “ambivalence about emotional expression”
contrived) smiles were central to the expres­ (King & Emmons, 1990, p. 864), which has
sion of love. In addition, nonverbal behav­ been defined as experiencing a conflict
iors that create closeness, such as gaze, between “the competing goals of wanting
close distance, body orientation, touch, and to show how one is feeling yet fearing
smiling, are important in the nonverbal the consequences of such self-expression”
expression of love (Andersen, 1985). These (Mongrain & Vettese, 2003, p. 545).
behaviors (often termed immediacy behav­ Mongrain and Vettese found that women’s
iors) are seen as signaling warmth, commu­ self-reported ambivalence over the expres­
nicating availability, decreasing distance, sion of emotion was related to their reports
and promoting involvement in interactions of suppressed anger toward their boyfriends.
(see Andersen, Guerrero, & Jones, this In other words, “ambivalent” women
volume). reported that they were not prepared to
Because social support is important for express the anger that they felt about the
relationship satisfaction (Coyne & Smith, behavior of their boyfriends. In addition,
1994; Pasch & Bradbury, 1998), it often when these highly ambivalent (about emo­
needs to be expressed clearly, as does the tional expression) women were observed in
supportive response. As Neff and Karney a conflict resolution task, they were less
(2005) have pointed out, the timing of such congruent in their communication (i.e., there
a response may be critical. These research­ was a mismatch between their verbal and
ers found that husbands and wives pro­ nonverbal channels). Furthermore, when
vided equal proportions of supportive mood and personality constructs were con­
behaviors, but wives tended to provide the trolled for, they displayed more overt sub­
most support when the husbands’ levels of missiveness toward their boyfriends than did
stress were highest, whereas husbands pro­ women who were low in ambivalence. These
vided the least support (in fact the most findings suggest that women are likely to
21-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 6:11 PM Page 409

Nonverbal Communication in Close Relationships–––◆–––409

tolerate bad behavior from their boyfriends of negative danger signs, such as invalida­
and even be submissive toward them in such tion, escalation, and withdrawal, these
circumstances. The chances of these couples researchers found that those who cohabited
having healthy, egalitarian, and respectful before engagement reported more danger
relationships seem bleak. signs both before and after marriage than
Guerrero (1997) explored whether type of did those who cohabited only after engage­
relationship affects encoding. She explored ment or marriage.
nonverbal behavior using a partial round- Cohan and Kleinbaum (2002) proposed
robin design, so that an individual’s behav­ that cohabiters treat each other less posi­
ior could be compared across relationships: tively than other couples and that this ten­
with a same-sex friend, an opposite-sex dency to less positive behavior carries over
friend, and a romantic partner. She found into the marriage and may contribute to
that romantic relationships could be distin­ the higher divorce rate among those who
guished from other types of relationships by cohabit before marriage (see, also, Heaton,
the closer proximity and higher levels of 2002; Kieran, 2002). In the study by Kline
touch and gaze in those relationships. In et al. (2004), those who had cohabited
addition, there were more silences, longer before their engagement were coded as more
response latencies, and less fluency in negative than those who cohabited only
romantic relationships. Nodding and vocal after engagement or after marriage. Those
interest were more common in friendships who cohabited before marriage were also
than in romantic relationships, and there coded as less positive than those who cohab­
was more evidence of postural congruence ited only after marriage, and those who
in same-sex than in opposite-sex dyads. cohabited after engagement were also coded
Particular patterns of nonverbal expres­ as less positive than those who cohabited
sion are also linked to relationship quality after marriage. These differences in the pos­
or satisfaction. Unhappy couples display itivity of communication were found before
more negative nonverbal behaviors and less marriage and were also in evidence after the
positive emotion than more satisfied cou­ marriage. These findings also fit with those
ples (Burgoon, Buller, & Woodall, 1996). of Cohan and Kleinbaum (2002): that mar­
Specifically, Gottman (1994) has argued ried couples who had cohabited before
that happy couples tend to display five pos­ marriage tended to treat each other less pos­
itive behaviors for every negative behavior, itively than those who had not.
whereas unhappy couples have a much Other research has explored the associa­
lower ratio of positive to negative behaviors tions between emotional expression and
and may even display more negative behav­ marital satisfaction in couples. Waldinger,
iors than positive. Thus, if we think of this Schulz, Hauser, Allen, and Crowell (2004),
issue in terms of a bank account model, for example, used naive coders’ observa­
these couples would not have any positive tional data based on conflict interactions to
resources to draw on in terms of expressing assess four factors of emotional expression:
their feelings about their relationships. hostility, empathy, affection, and distress.
In recent years, there has been an The researchers found that men who were
increased interest in comparing married part of satisfied couples expressed more
couples with different histories of cohabi­ empathy and personal distress, such as sad­
tation. In one study, Kline et al. (2004) ness and anxiety, less hostility, and margin­
assessed the association between the timing ally more affection than did less satisfied
of cohabitation and couple interaction men. Women in satisfied couples expressed
before and after marriage. Using self-reports greater empathy in their marital interactions,
21-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 6:11 PM Page 410

410–––◆–––Contexts and Consequences

and marginally more affection, than did less Holtzworth-Munroe, Smutzler, and Stuart
satisfied women. These results were similar (1998) have also suggested that withdrawal
whether the marriage qualities were assessed can lead to ongoing power struggles and
using spouse self-report or an interview- “a pattern of coercive efforts can gradually
based assessment. The researchers also develop, creating a rigid pattern of negative
explored the association between emotional polarized interaction” (p. 72) (see also
expression and relationship dissolution Gottman et al., 2002).
(within 5 years). Using the four emotion Researchers have pointed to a gendered
scales scores in a logistic regression, the pattern in the use of withdrawal in mar­
researchers were able to identify correctly riage, particularly with regard to the
85% of those whose relationships had demand-withdraw pattern of conflict inter­
remained intact, and overall relationship action. Females are more likely to be in the
status at follow-up was predicted with role of demander and males in the role of
83% accuracy using the women’s emotional withdrawer, although whose issue is being
expression and 81% accuracy using the discussed is also important (Christensen
men’s emotional expression. & Heavey, 1990). For example, Noller,
The encoding of nonverbal behavior also Feeney, Roberts, and Christensen (2005)
involves the expression of both power and reported an observational study of the
intimacy. Burgoon and Dillman (1995), nonverbal concomitants of withdrawal in
for example, have argued that knowing married couples engaged in conflict interac­
who wields power and how that power is tions. They were interested in whether the
expressed through nonverbal communica­ nonverbal behavior related to withdrawal
tion is central to understanding the dynam­ would be affected by gender and whose
ics of any relationship (see also Burgoon issue was being discussed. They found that
& Dunbar, this volume). Nonverbal behav­ the clearest pattern of nonverbal behavior
iors, such as touch, gaze, body movements, related to withdrawal was for husbands
and spacing, can be used to express power during discussion of their wives’ issues.
and control (Edinger & Patterson, 1983; Husbands’ withdrawal was indicated by a
Guerrero, Andersen, & Afifi, 2001). A lack of gaze and open gestures and by head
study comparing violent couples with dis­ down and head turn. These behaviors
tressed nonviolent and nondistressed nonvi­ may reflect avoidance and a lack of imme­
olent couples found that distressed couples diacy, with husbands seeming to avoid
engaged in lower levels of mutual gaze any eye contact with their wives. Wives’
than did other couples, although the vio­ withdrawal was indicated by a lack of
lent couples seemed to be maintaining gaze open or neutral gestures and head down.
with different affect (e.g., the angry stare; Interestingly, and as would be expected,
Gottman, Driver, Yashimoto, & Rushe, highly satisfied spouses tended to be more
2002). Violent couples also rated their open and less withdrawn in their conflict
interaction less positively than did either of interactions with partners than did spouses
the nonviolent groups. low in satisfaction.
One behavior generally seen as problem­
atic in couple relationships is withdrawal
from conflict. Because the likely conse­ SUMMARY
quences of withdrawal from conflict are
that issues are not resolved and resentment Overall, nonverbal communication is
may increase, withdrawal is generally seen as important for the expression of love, for the
having a negative impact on relationships. expression of support, and to indicate the
21-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 6:11 PM Page 411

Nonverbal Communication in Close Relationships–––◆–––411

need for support. Individuals differ in their involvement desired are particularly likely
willingness to express their feelings, with to be problematic, especially if the female is
women often not prepared to express their signaling interest in “getting to know you,”
anger in romantic relationships. Gender and the male decodes that message as inter­
also affects the extent to which individuals est in sexual activity.
are able to express support to their partners Studies of nonverbal courtship behavior,
and express that support when it is needed however, have generally failed to find a
most. In addition, researchers have found specific list of behaviors that could be con­
a pattern of men’s nonverbal withdrawal sidered as signs of interest in pursuing a rela­
when issues raised by their wives are being tionship. Moore (1985), for example,
discussed. There is also evidence of effects catalogued 52 behaviors that occurred in a
related to the type and quality of relation­ singles bar; however, because of the large
ships including the following: (1) negativity amount of variability between participants,
is particularly marked in low-quality rela­ he was unable to identify specific behaviors
tionships, (2) empathy and lack of hostility that could be classified as courtship behav­
are especially noticeable in high-quality iors. Grammer, Honda, Juette, and Schmitt
relationships, and (3) nonverbal behaviors (1999), in their study of initial interactions
indicating withdrawal are less evident in the between opposite-sex strangers in two cul­
conflict interactions of satisfied couples. tures (Germany and Japan), were unable to
identify behaviors that could be consistently
classified as courtship behaviors. They did
♦ Nonverbal Behavior find, however, that when female interest was
high, mutual gaze was initiated and partners
and Courtship
made a similar number of comments. They
also found that women were more nonver­
The research on decoding and encoding just bally involved and open in their interactions
reviewed has centered largely on ongoing than were men during initial conversations.
relationships, such as marriage and friend­ Argyle (1988) also identified a number of
ship. In addition to nonverbal cues in estab­ courtship behaviors including high levels
lished relationships, courtship, or the of mutual gaze, touch, movement, smiling,
potential establishment of romantic rela­ erect and open posture, and proximity.
tionships, is also an area where nonverbal Differences between males and females
behavior is critical. Expressions of interest have been found, at least within hetero­
in specific members of the opposite sex sexual courtship displays. Analysis of
are often nonverbal (Renninger, Wade, & courtship interactions by Grammer et al.
Grammer, 2004). In addition, although (2000), for example, showed that unless
males tend to be seen as the initiators of males received nonverbal indicators of
courtship, they may seek nonverbal signs interest, such as eye contact, they were less
that females are interested before making likely to approach females. But females may
any approaches (Grammer, Kruck, Juette, rely on males’ signals in deciding whether
& Fink, 2000). Because nonverbal signals to encourage the approach of a male. In
of interest are likely to be somewhat support of this, Renninger et al. (2004)
ambiguous, there is a strong possibility that observed the nonverbal behaviors of males
misunderstandings can occur, both in terms in a bar and found that those who made
of the level of interest expressed and in contact with females successfully tended to
terms of the desired level of involvement. engage in more glancing behaviors, more
Misunderstandings about the levels of space-maximization movements (generally
21-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 6:11 PM Page 412

412–––◆–––Contexts and Consequences

seen as signs of dominance), more intrasexual problematic communication in couples


touching (generally seen as indicating social where at least one partner is insecure in
power), and fewer closed body movements attachment. They claim that
(suggesting that they are more interperson­
ally oriented). Renninger et al. note that, it is possible that the feelings of discom­
commonly, females need to show enough fort or ambivalence over closeness that
interest to elicit courtship behavior from are experienced by insecure individuals,
males, whereas males need to display such coupled with the negative affect they feel
qualities as their status, health, and strength within the relationship, are communi­
to attract female attention. cated to their partners through non­
Henningsen (2004) focused his attention verbal behaviors. The communication of
on the decoding of flirting behaviors, or these feelings, in turn, should be associ­
behaviors that indicate interest in a cross- ated with lower relationship satisfaction
sex target and are likely to promote contact for both partners. (p. 112)
with that target. These behaviors are
typically nonverbal. He found that men Attachment security or insecurity can be
attributed different motivations to flirting seen as working like a filter in the communi­
behaviors than did women, with men tend­ cation process, blocking sensitivity to certain
ing to interpret flirting behaviors as indicat­ nonverbal messages and also discouraging
ing sexual interest to a greater extent than the expression of particular nonverbal sig­
did women. Abbey (1982) discovered like­ nals (Noller, 2005b). In other words, both
wise that males tended to see the same the encoding and the decoding of those
nonverbal cues as more “seductive” that who are insecure in attachment are likely
female observers rated as “friendly.” to be affected by their working models of
Whereas there is evidence that flirting is attachment. Those categorized as anxious
often associated with a desire for sexual in terms of attachment, for example, are
contact (Greer & Buss, 1994; Simpson, likely to have problems decoding positive
Gangestad, & Biek, 1993), there is also evi­ messages from a close other because of their
dence that not all flirting is motivated by sex­ negative views of themselves (Bartholomew,
ual desire or interest (Guerrero et al., 2001). 1990) and to have difficulty decoding nega­
These findings suggest that there is consider­ tive messages, as such messages would serve
able room for miscommunication between to increase their already potent fears about
the sexes because males are likely to believe being rejected.
that women who flirt are more interested in As Mikulincer and Shaver (2003) have
sexual contact than they may actually be. suggested, anxious individuals’ tendency to
focus on their own needs and vulnerabilities
and to be concerned excessively about rejec­
♦ Attachment and tion may make it difficult for them to be
sensitive to their partner’s nonverbal signals
Nonverbal Communication
and to respond appropriately. Because of
their tendency to distrust the good will of
Several different theoretical perspectives relational partners and to strive to maintain
have been used to explain the role of com­ independence and emotional distance from
munication in developing and sustaining them, those high in avoidance are also likely
relationships. For instance, Tucker and to be impaired in terms of their nonver­
Anders (1998) discuss the possibility of bal sensitivity. As Schachner, Shaver, and
21-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 6:11 PM Page 413

Nonverbal Communication in Close Relationships–––◆–––413

Mikulnicer (2005) note, anxious individuals difficult for them to believe that their
are likely to have difficulty decoding nega­ partner cares. A similar filter can make it
tive messages that arouse unwanted attach­ difficult for individuals to express their
ment needs and positive messages that feelings. Specifically, “avoidant” individuals
promote intimacy and closeness. were less expressive in interactions with their
In studying the association between partners (Fraley & Shaver, 2000; Guerrero,
attachment security and the decoding of 1996; Le Poire, Sheppard, & Duggan, 1999;
nonverbal messages by young married cou­ Tucker & Anders, 1998), perhaps because
ples (Noller & Feeney, 1994), my colleague of their lack of trust in others. Alternatively,
and I found that decoding accuracy was expressivity may be perceived as involving
affected by both gender and attachment the type of intimacy likely to threaten their
security. Husbands’ attachment anxiety autonomy and independence.
was associated with low levels of accuracy Tucker and Anders also assessed the
in decoding wives’ messages, irrespective encoding of nonverbal behavior in dating
of whether the messages were positive, neu­ couples and related expressions of nonver­
tral, or negative, suggesting a general bal closeness to attachment style. They
decoding deficit related to anxiety. For found that individuals with a secure attach­
wives, however, anxiety did not affect ment style were rated as less tense, as more
decoding of husbands’ messages, but avoid­ nonverbally expressive, and as seeming to
ance was associated with low levels of enjoy the conversation more than other
accuracy in decoding neutral and negative individuals. Their interactions with their
messages only. Thus, highly anxious partners tended to involve more laughing,
husbands were less accurate decoders touching, gazing, and smiling than was true
overall, and wives who were high in com­ for insecure individuals. In contrast, the
fort with closeness tended to be more accu­ interactions of “preoccupied” individuals
rate for neutral and negative messages. were rated as less nonverbally expres­
Also, attachment security after a year of sive, and these individuals were rated as
marriage predicted decoding accuracy after experiencing less enjoyment of the conversa­
almost 2 years, following the same pattern tion. Both avoidants and preoccupieds
as reported above for husbands and wives. tended to smile and gaze less than secures. It
Attachment security or insecurity is also is interesting to note, however, that preoccu­
likely to influence people’s willingness to pieds were not seen as engaging in more
express their feelings to a partner. The clingy behaviors, as may have been expected.
behaviors most likely to be affected are Noller et al. (2005) report a study of
those related to expressing discomfort and attachment and emotional reactions in a
distress and to seeking support and comfort. partner-distant interaction (where a partner
Moreover, because of problems with decod­ or a confederate was asked to act distant
ing, people may assume incorrectly that with his or her partner). For females, com­
their partner does not care about their dis­ fort with closeness was related to low levels
tress. Thus, “insecures” (i.e., people catego­ of negative affect (both hostile and worried
rized as having an insecure form of affects), and anxiety over abandonment
attachment) may struggle in their close rela­ was related to high levels of avoidance
tionships, not because their partner does not (turning the head or body away from the
care about their distress, but because the partner or resisting contact) and low levels
filter of their attachment insecurity distorts of touch (hugging, hand holding, etc.). For
their perception in such a way that it is males, comfort with closeness predicted
21-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 6:11 PM Page 414

414–––◆–––Contexts and Consequences

high levels of touch and low levels of Manusov (1990) laid out a set of princi­
avoidance, and anxiety over abandon­ ples detailing the ways in which attribution
ment was unrelated to nonverbal behavior. theories applied to the meanings people give
Highly anxious males did, however, engage to nonverbal cues. Following work by other
in negative verbal behavior in response to relational scholars who used attribution the­
their partner’s distancing. ories to predict couples’ sense-making for
Overall, attachment security seems to act other behaviors such as conflict (e.g.,
as a filter and affect both the encoding and Fincham & Bradbury, 1992), she argued
the decoding of nonverbal behavior in close that couples make attributions to explain
relationships. Highly anxious individuals, why the partner acted as he or she did (i.e.,
especially males, seem to have a general sat closer, sighed, avoided eye contact).
decoding deficit, to be less nonverbally Furthermore, these attributions are linked to
expressive than other partners, and to marital satisfaction, such that more satisfied
engage in more avoidance. Thus, highly anx­ couples interpret the same behaviors differ­
ious individuals are likely to have difficulty ently than less satisfied couples.
in maintaining close relationships. But it is In a follow-up study, Manusov et al.
important to keep in mind that attachment (1997) again looked to see if those in satis­
theory is not the only theory relevant to fying relationships make more “spouse­
nonverbal behavior in close relationships. enhancing attributions” and those in
Attributions for nonverbal behavior are also unhappy relationships make more “dis­
important, particularly for understanding tress-maintaining attributions,” as tends to
where communication differences may arise. occur for other marital events (Fincham,
Beach, & Baucom, 1987; Fincham &
Bradbury, 1992). Spouse-enhancing attri­
♦ Attributions for butions tend to involve internal and stable
attributions for positive behaviors and
Nonverbal Behavior
external and unstable attributions for nega­
tive behavior. Distress-maintaining attribu­
Attributions are the explanations we con­ tions, on the other hand, tend to involve
struct to explain our own and others’ behav­ external and unstable attributions for
ior. Because of the ambiguity of nonverbal positive behavior and internal and stable
cues noted earlier, it is particularly important attributions for negative behavior.
in the context of close relationships to under­ As with previous research, Manusov
stand the attributions that those in intimate et al. (1997) found that, in general, couples
relationships make for one another’s non­ were more likely to notice (and hence
verbal behavior (Abbey, 1982; Manusov, report) negative behavior than positive
Floyd, & Kerssen-Griep, 1997). In support behavior, although relationship satisfaction
of this proposition, Solomon (2001) notes tended to increase the likelihood of noticing
that the interplay among affect, attributions, positive behavior, particularly for males. In
and communication is particularly impor­ addition, those in satisfying relationships
tant in the context of close relationships. In were more likely to adopt a spouse-enhanc­
this section, I will focus on Manusov’s work, ing attribution pattern by attributing simi­
which has explored (1) the kinds of attri­ lar causes to their own and their spouse’s
butions partners in romantic relationships behavior. In contrast, those who were more
make for partners’ nonverbal behaviors and dissatisfied were more likely to attribute
(2) the effect of the attribution on the attrib­ negative causes to their spouse’s behavior
utors’ own nonverbal response. than to their own behavior. In spite of this
21-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 6:11 PM Page 415

Nonverbal Communication in Close Relationships–––◆–––415

tendency, however, all couples reported critical in courtship situations where a


more benign attributions for their own female’s interest in getting to know a male
behavior than their partner gave for the may be misinterpreted as an indication
same behavior. Overall relationship satis­ of interest in sexual activity. Although
faction was not as clearly tied to relation­ researchers have been unsuccessful in their
ship-enhancing and distress-maintaining attempts to find a specific list of behaviors
attributions as has been the case in previous related to the early stages of courtship,
research. Perhaps, the inherent ambiguity there is evidence that both males and
of many nonverbal behaviors is again females rely on nonverbal behavior in
relevant here. deciding whether to approach and whether
Looking beyond attributions, Manusov to encourage approach. Because nonverbal
(2002) focused on the effect of one behavior can be ambiguous, however, accu­
member’s attributions for the partner’s racy in decoding and clarity in encoding
behavior on his or her responses to that are particularly important in developing
behavior. In other words, what was the close relationships.
association between individuals’ attribu­ The quality of the relationship affects
tions for their partner’s behavior and their decoding consistently, with those in satisfy­
own nonverbal behavior? She found a pos­ ing relationships generally more accurate
itive association between attributions that than those in unhappy relationships, and
were relationship enhancing and a response those in unhappy relationships even less
involving positive facial and vocal behav­ accurate at decoding their partners’ mes­
ior. The pattern for distress-maintaining sages than they are at decoding the mes­
attributions was not as clear, but there was sages of strangers. There is also evidence
still evidence of an association between dis­ that couples become more accurate at
tress-maintaining attributions for partner decoding one another’s messages over time.
behavior and the negativity of one’s own Decoding sensitivity can be affected by the
nonverbal behavior, behavior indicative of closeness of the relationship, with those in
personal discomfort. Apparently, then, rela­ very close relationships likely to ignore neg­
tionship satisfaction affects the attributions ativity in partners’ communications, partic­
made for a partner’s nonverbal behavior, ularly if the negativity is concealed. This
with those in satisfying relationships more unwillingness to process negative messages
likely to adopt a spouse-enhancing attribu­ has been termed motivated inaccuracy and
tion pattern. Attributions influence the is an issue particularly where the content of
decoding of nonverbal behavior, with a messages is seen as threatening and there­
spouse-enhancing attribution pattern increas­ fore best ignored. Other biases in decoding
ing the likelihood that partners would are related to gender, rejection sensitivity,
respond positively to the partner’s behavior. need to belong, and attachment insecurity.
An important aspect of understanding
decoding sensitivity is exploring the attribu­
♦ Conclusion tions couples make for their partner’s non­
verbal behavior. These attributions can be
either more spouse enhancing or more
As noted at the outset of this chapter, it is distress maintaining. Spouse-enhancing
important to keep in mind the essential attributions tend to be made by those
ambiguity of nonverbal behavior, making in satisfying relationships, whereas distress-
it prone to misinterpretation within close rela­ maintaining attributions tend to be
tionships. This ambiguity may be particularly made by those in distressed relationships.
21-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 6:11 PM Page 416

416–––◆–––Contexts and Consequences

Moreover, there appear to be “self-spouse” interpreting positive messages because of


differences in the ways attributions are their negative views of themselves, and dif­
made. All these differences point toward ficulty decoding negative messages because
the complexity of meaning for nonverbal these messages arouse unwanted attach­
cues in relationships. ment needs. Secure individuals, on the other
Nonverbal behaviors are, however, hand, are likely to be more nonverbally
important to the expression of love and expressive and more comfortable in interac­
of social support in relationships (Trees, tion situations.
2000). Yet individuals differ in their will­ Whereas there is no doubt that all com­
ingness to express their feelings, with munication is critical to the development
gender, ambivalence about the value of and maintenance of relationships, it seems
communicating feelings, and the type and that nonverbal cues, because of their modi­
quality of the relationship in which they are fying effect on words and the overall inter­
involved all affecting willingness to express pretation of the message and its tie to affect,
feelings. For example, there is an interesting may be the more important “side” of that
contrast in the circumstances of husbands communication. Those who seek to help
and wives expressing support. Wives express couples communicate more constructively
the most support when husbands are expe­ need to ensure that they focus on what is
riencing the most stress, whereas husbands happening in the nonverbal channels; help­
provide the least support when wives are ing individuals to understand the rather
experiencing the most stress. trite saying, “It’s not what you say but
Satisfaction and nonverbal cues also the way that you say it,” may have the
seem to be linked in important ways. strongest impact in—and the most impor­
Unhappy couples tend to display more neg­ tant consequences for—a close relationship.
ative and less positive nonverbal behavior.
Those who have cohabited before marriage
tend to be more negative than other cou­ ♦ References
ples, particularly if they have cohabited
before engagement. In addition, men in sat­
Abbey, A. (1982). Sex differences in attributions
isfying relationships express more empathy
for friendly behavior: Do males misperceive
and personal distress, and less hostility, females’ friendliness? Journal of Personality
than those in less satisfying relationships. and Social Psychology, 42, 830–838.
There is also evidence that emotional Andersen, P. A. (1985). Nonverbal immediacy
expression is a good predictor of relation­ in interpersonal communication. In A. W.
ship stability. High-satisfaction spouses Siegman & S. Feldstein (Eds.), Multi­
tend to be more open and less withdrawn in channel integrations of nonverbal behavior
their conflict interactions. A clear pattern (pp. 1–36). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
of withdrawal is reflected in avoidance and Argyle, M. (1988). Bodily communication (2nd
a lack of immediacy by husbands during ed.). London: Methuen.
discussion of issues raised by their wives. Bartholomew, K. (1990). Avoidance of inti­
macy: An attachment perspective. Journal
Attachment insecurity also affects both
of Social and Personal Relationships, 7,
the encoding and the decoding of nonver­
147–178.
bal behavior, blocking sensitivity to certain Burgoon, J. K., Buller, D. B., & Woodall, W. G.
nonverbal messages and discouraging the (1996). Nonverbal communication: The
expression of particular nonverbal signals. unspoken dialogue (2nd ed.). New York:
Insecure individuals may have difficulty McGraw-Hill.
21-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 6:11 PM Page 417

Nonverbal Communication in Close Relationships–––◆–––417

Burgoon, J. K., & Dillman, L. (1995). Gender, Fraley, R. C., & Shaver, P. R. (2000). Adult
immediacy, and nonverbal communication. romantic attachment: Theoretical develop­
In P. J. Kalbfleisch & M. J. Cody (Eds.), ments, emerging controversies, and unan­
Gender, power and communication in swered questions. Review of General
human relationships (pp. 63–81). Hillsdale, Psychology, 4, 132–154.
NJ: Erlbaum. Fridlund, A. J. (1994). Human facial expression:
Christensen, A., & Heavey, C. L. (1990). An evolutionary view. San Diego, CA:
Gender and social structure in the demand/ Academic Press.
withdraw pattern of marital conflict. Friedman, H. S. (2001). Paradoxes of nonverbal
Journal of Personality and Social Psy­ detection, expression and responding:
chology, 59, 73–81. Points to ponder. In J. A. Hall & F. J.
Cohan, C., & Kleinbaum, S. (2002). Toward a Bernieri (Eds.), Interpersonal sensitivity:
greater understanding of the cohabitation Theory and measurement (pp. 351–362).
effect: Premarital cohabitation and subse­ Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
quent marital stability. Journal of Marriage Gaelick, L., Bodenhausen, G. V., & Wyer, R.
and the Family, 64, 180–192. (1985). Emotional communication in close
Coyne, J. C., & Smith, D. A. F. (1994). Couples relationships. Journal of Personality and
coping with a myocardial infarction: Contex­ Social Psychology, 49, 1246–1265.
tual perspective on patient self-efficacy. Gardner, W. L., Jefferis, V., Knowles, M. L., &
Journal of Family Psychology, 8, 43–54. Pickett, C. L. (2003). Loneliness and selec­
Downey, G., & Feldman, S. (1996). Implica­ tive memory: A general bias for remember­
tions of rejection sensitivity for intimate ing social events. Unpublished manuscript.
relationships. Journal of Personality and Gardner, W. L., Pickett, C. L., & Brewer, M. B.
Social Psychology, 70, 1327–1343. (2000). Social exclusion and selective mem­
Downey, G., Freitas, A. L., Michaelis, B., & ory: How the need to belong influences
Khouri, H. (1998). The self-fulfilling memory for social events. Personality and
prophecy in close relationships: Do rejec­ Social Psychology Bulletin, 26, 486–496.
tion-sensitive women get rejected by Gonzaga, G. C., Keltner, D., Londahl, E. A., &
romantic partners? Journal of Personality Smith, M. D. (2001). Love and the commit­
and Social Psychology, 75, 545–560. ment problems in romantic relations and
Edinger, J. A., & Patterson, M. L. (1983). friendship. Journal of Personality and
Nonverbal involvement and social control. Social Psychology, 81, 247–262.
Psychological Bulletin, 93, 30–56. Gottman, J. M. (1994). What predicts divorce:
Fincham, F. D., Beach, S. R. H., & Baucom, The relationship between marital processes
D. H. (1987). Attribution processing in and marital outcomes. Hillsdale, NJ:
distressed and nondistressed couples: 4. Erlbaum.
Self-partner attribution differences. Journal Gottman, J. M., Driver, J., Yoshimoto, D., &
of Personality and Social Psychology, 52, Rushe, R. (2002). Approaches to the study
739–748. of power in violent and nonviolent mar­
Fincham, F. D., & Bradbury, T. N. (1992). riages, and in gay male and lesbian cohabit­
Assessing attributions in marriage: The ing relationships. In P. Noller & J. A.
relationship attribution measure. Journal Feeney (Eds.), Understanding marriage:
of Personality and Social Psychology, 62, Developments in the study of couple
457–468. interaction (pp. 323–347). New York:
Fleming, J. H., Darley, J. M., Hilton, J. L., & Cambridge University Press.
Kojetin, B. A. (1990). Multiple audience Gottman, J. M., Markman, H. J., & Notarius,
problem: A strategic communication per­ C. I. (1977). The topography of marital
ception on social perception. Journal of conflict: A sequential analysis of verbal and
Personality and Social Psychology, 58, nonverbal behavior. Journal of Marriage
593–609. and the Family, 39, 461–477.
21-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 6:11 PM Page 418

418–––◆–––Contexts and Consequences

Grammer, K., Honda, M., Juette, A., & Schmitt, In A. Booth & A. Crouter (Eds.), Just living
A. (1999). Fuzziness of nonverbal court­ together: Implications of cohabitation
ship communication unblurred by motion for families, children and social policy
energy detection. Journal of Personality and (pp. 3–32). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Social Psychology, 77, 487–508. King, L. A., & Emmons, R. A. (1990). Conflict
Grammer, K., Kruck, K., Juette, A., & Fink, B. over emotional expression: Psychologi­
(2000). Nonverbal behavior as courtship cal and physical correlates. Journal of
signals: The role of control and choice in Personality and Social Psychology, 58,
selecting partners. Evolution and Human 864–877.
Behavior, 21, 371–390. Kline, G. H., Stanley, S. M., Markman, H. J.,
Greer, A. E., & Buss, D. M. (1994). Tactics Olmos-Gallo, P. A., St Peters, M., Whitton,
for promoting sexual encounters. Journal of S. W., et al. (2004). Timing is everything:
Sex Research, 31, 185–201. Pre-engagement cohabitation and increased
Guerrero, L. K. (1996). Attachment-style differ­ risk for poor marital outcomes. Journal of
ences in intimacy and involvement: A test of Family Psychology, 18, 311–318.
the four-category model. Communication Le Poire, B. A., Sheppard, C., & Duggan, A.
Monographs, 63, 269–292. (1999). Nonverbal involvement, expressive­
Guerrero, L. K. (1997). Nonverbal involvement ness, and pleasantness as predicted by
across interactions with same-sex friends, parental and partner attachment style.
opposite-sex friends and romantic partners: Communication Monographs, 66, 293–311.
Consistency or change? Journal of Social Losoya, S. H., & Eisenberg, N. (2001). Affective
and Personal Relationships, 14, 31–58. empathy. In J. A. Hall & F. J. Bernieri
Guerrero, L. K., Andersen, P. A., & Afifi, W. A. (Eds.), Interpersonal sensitivity: Theory and
(2001). Close encounters: Communicat­ measurement (pp. 21–43). Mahwah, NJ:
ing in relationships. Mountain View, CA: Erlbaum.
Mayfield. MacGeorge, E. L. (2003). Gender differences
Heaton, T. B. (2002). Factors contributing in attributions and emotions in helping
to increasing marital stability in the U. S. contexts. Sex Roles, 48, 175–182.
Journal of Family Issues, 23, 392–409. Manusov, V. (1990). An application of attribu­
Henningsen, D. D. (2004). Flirting with mean­ tion principles to nonverbal behavior in
ing: An examination of miscommunication romantic dyads. Communication Mono­
in flirting interactions. Sex Roles, 50, graphs, 57, 104–118.
481–489. Manusov, V. (2002). Thought and action:
Holtzworth-Munroe, A., Smutzler, N., & Stuart, Connecting attributions to behaviors in
G. L. (1998). Demand and withdraw com­ married couples’ interactions. In P. Noller
munication among couples experiencing & J. A. Feeney (Eds.), Understanding mar­
husband violence. Journal of Consulting riage: Developments in the study of marital
and Clinical Psychology, 66, 731–743. interaction (pp. 14–31). New York:
Ickes, W., & Simpson, J. A. (1997). Managing Cambridge University Press.
empathic accuracy in close relationships. Manusov, V., Floyd, K., & Kerssen-Griep, J.
In W. Ickes (Ed.), Empathic accuracy (1997). Yours, mine, and ours: Mutual attri­
(pp. 218–250). New York: Guilford Press. butions for nonverbal behavior in couples’
Ickes, W., & Simpson, J. A. (2001). Moti­ interactions. Communication Research, 24,
vational aspects of empathic accuracy. In 234–260.
G. J. O. Fletcher & M. Clark (Eds.), The Mikulincer, M., & Shaver, P. R. (2003). The
Blackwell handbook of social psychology: attachment behavioral system in adulthood:
Interpersonal processes (pp. 229–249). Activation, psychodynamics, and interper­
Oxford, UK: Blackwell. sonal processes. In M. P. Zanna (Ed.),
Kieran, K. (2002). Cohabitation in Western Advances in experimental social psychology
Europe: Trends, issues and implications. (Vol. 35). New York: Academic Press.
21-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 6:11 PM Page 419

Nonverbal Communication in Close Relationships–––◆–––419

Miller, P. A., Eisenberg, N., Fabes, R. A., & tion (pp. 11–43). New York: Cambridge
Shell, R. (1996). Relations of moral rea­ University Press.
soning and vicarious emotion to young Noller, P., Feeney, J. A., Roberts, N., &
children’s prosocial behavior towards peers Christensen, A. (2005). Withdrawal in cou­
and adults. Developmental Psychology, ple interactions: Exploring the causes and
32, 210–219. consequences. In R. E. Riggio & R. S.
Mongrain, M., & Vettese, L. C. (2003). Conflict Feldman (Eds.), Applications of nonverbal
over emotional expression: Implications communication (pp. 195–213). Mahwah,
for interpersonal communication. Person­ NJ: Erlbaum.
ality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 29, Noller, P., & Ruzzene, M. (1991). Com­
545–555. munication in marriage: The influence of
Moore, M. (1985). Nonverbal courtship pat­ affect and cognition. In G. J. O. Fletcher &
terns in women: Context and consequences. F. D. Fincham (Eds.), Cognition in close
Ethology and Sociobiology, 6, 237–247. relationships (pp. 203–233). Hillsdale, NJ:
Neff, L. A., & Karney, B. R. (2005). Gender dif­ Erlbaum.
ferences in social support: A question of skill Nowicki, S., & Duke, M. P. (1994). Individual
or responsiveness. Journal of Personality differences in the nonverbal communica­
and Social Psychology, 88, 79–90. tion of affect: The Diagnostic Analysis of
Noller, P. (1984). Nonverbal communication Nonverbal Accuracy scale. Journal of
and marital interaction. Oxford, UK: Nonverbal Behavior, 18, 9–35.
Pergamon. Nowicki, S., & Duke, M. P. (2001). Nonverbal
Noller, P. (1995). Parent-adolescent relation­ receptivity: The Diagnostic Analysis of
ships. In M. A. Fitzpatrick & A. L. Nonverbal Accuracy (DANVA). In J. A.
Vangelisti (Eds.), Explaining family interac­ Hall & F. J. Bernieri (Eds.), Interpersonal
tions (pp. 77–111). Thousand Oaks, CA: sensitivity: Theory and measurement
Sage. (pp. 183–198). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Noller, P. (2001). Using standard content Pasch, L. A, & Bradbury, T. N. (1998). Social
methodology to assess nonverbal sensitivity support, conflict, and the development of
in dyads. In J. A. Hall & F. J. Bernieri marital dysfunction. Journal of Consulting
(Eds.), Interpersonal sensitivity: Theory and and Clinical Psychology, 66, 219–230.
measurement (pp. 243–264). Mahwah, NJ: Pickett, C. L., Gardner, W. L., & Knowles,
Erlbaum. M. (2004). Getting a cue: The need to
Noller, P. (2005a). Standard content methodol­ belong and enhanced sensitivity to social
ogy: Controlling the verbal channel. In cues. Personality and Social Psychology
V. Manusov (Ed.), The sourcebook of non­ Bulletin, 30, 1095–1107.
verbal measures: Going beyond words Renninger, L. A., Wade, J. T., & Grammer, K.
(pp. 417–430). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. (2004). Getting that female glance: Patterns
Noller, P. (2005b). Attachment insecurity as a fil­ and consequences of male nonverbal behav­
ter in the encoding and decoding of nonver­ ior in courtship contexts. Evolution and
bal behavior in close relationships. Journal Human Behavior, 25, 416–431.
of Nonverbal Behavior, 29, 171–176. Schachner, D. A., Shaver, P. R., & Mikulincer,
Noller, P., & Feeney, J. A. (1994). Relationship M. (2005). Patterns of nonverbal behavior
satisfaction, attachment, and nonverbal and sensitivity in the context of attach­
accuracy in early marriage. Journal of ment relationships. Journal of Nonverbal
Nonverbal Behavior, 18, 199–221. Behavior, 29, 141–169.
Noller, P., & Feeney, J. A. (1998). Communica­ Simpson, J. A., Gangestad, S. W., & Biek, M.
tion in early marriage: Responses to con­ (1993). Personality and nonverbal social
flict, nonverbal accuracy and conversational behavior: An ethological perspective of
patterns. In T. N. Bradbury (Ed.), The relationship initiation. Journal of Experi­
developmental course of marital dysfunc­ mental Social Psychology, 29, 434–461.
21-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 6:11 PM Page 420

420–––◆–––Contexts and Consequences

Simpson, J. A., Ickes, W., & Blackstone, T. Trees, A. R. (2000). Nonverbal communication
(1995). When the head protects the heart: and the support process: Interactional
Empathic accuracy in dating relation­ sensitivity in interactions between mothers
ships. Journal of Personality and Social and young adult children. Communication
Psychology, 69, 629–641. Monographs, 67, 239–261.
Simpson, J. A., Ickes, W., & Grich, J. (1999). Tucker, J. S., & Anders, S. L. (1998). Adult
When accuracy hurts: Reactions of anxious, attachment style and nonverbal closeness
uncertain individuals to a relationship- in dating couples. Journal of Nonverbal
threatening situation. Journal of Personality Behavior, 22, 109–124.
and Social Psychology, 76, 754–769. Waldinger, R. J., Schulz, M. S., Hauser, S. T.,
Simpson, J. A., Orina, M. M., & Ickes, W. (2003). Allen, J. P., & Crowell, J. A. (2004).
When accuracy hurts, and when it helps: Reading others’ emotions: The role of intu­
A test of the empathic accuracy model in itive judgments in predicting marital satis­
marital interactions. Journal of Personality faction, quality and stability. Journal of
and Social Psychology, 85, 881–893. Family Psychology, 18, 58–71.
Solomon, D. H. (2001). Affect, attribution, Wood, J. T. (1995). Relational communication:
and communication: Uniting interaction Continuity and change in personal relation­
episodes and global relationship judgments. ships. Belmont, CA: Wadsworth.
In V. Manusov & J. H. Harvey (Eds.), Zahn-Waxler, C., Radke-Yarrow, M., Wagner,
Attribution, communication behavior, and E., & Chapman, M. (1992). Development
close relationships (pp. 79–90). Cambridge, of concern for others. Developmental
UK: Cambridge University Press. Psychology, 28, 126–136.
Sternglanz, R. W., & DePaulo, B. M. (2004). Zahn-Waxler, C., Robinson, J., & Emde, R. N.
Reading nonverbal cues to emotions: The (1992). The development of empathy in
advantages and liabilities of relationship twins. Developmental Psychology, 28,
closeness. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 1038–1047.
28, 245–266. Zebrowitz, L. A. (2001). Groping for the ele­
Stinson, L., & Ickes, W. (1992). Empathic accu­ phant of nonverbal sensitivity. In J. A. Hall
racy in the interactions of male friends ver­ & F. J. Bernieri (Eds.), Interpersonal
sus male strangers. Journal of Personality sensitivity: Theory and measurement
and Social Psychology, 62, 787–797. (pp. 333–350). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
22-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 5:16 PM Page 421

22
NONVERBAL COMMUNICATION
IN INSTRUCTIONAL CONTEXTS
� James C. McCroskey and
Virginia P. Richmond
University of West Virginia

� Linda L. McCroskey
California State University, Long Beach

I nstructional communication is the area of communication theory and


research that centers attention on the role of communication in teach­
ing and training contexts. It focuses on communication in the instruc­
tional environment and assesses its effects on cognitive, affective, and
psychomotor learning of students and trainees. Instructional communi­
cation examines applications of general theories of communication to
the instructional environment as well as generates theory applicable
specifically to the instructional environment. Whereas this area consid­
ers applications of instructional communication theory in any content
area, it is also concerned with applications of instructional communica­
tion to instruction in communication (commonly referred to as “speech
education” or “communication education”).
This chapter focuses on the relationship between nonverbal commu­
nication research and instructional communication research. In the
beginning, we provide an historical perspective of how the relationships
between these two areas of scholarship in communication evolved. Then
we examine several choices that early researchers made in their studies

◆ 421
22-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 5:16 PM Page 422

422–––◆–––Contexts and Consequences

of nonverbal behavior in the instructional graduate course to be designed for in-


context. In doing so, we direct attention to service elementary and secondary teachers,
nonverbal constructs, including both cate­ “Communication in the Classroom.” This
gorical constructs and collective constructs, class led ultimately to the development of a
that have been prominent in instructional full program leading to an M.A. degree in
research. Finally, we examine briefly some Instructional Communication. As had been
of the implications of this research, high­ the case with nonverbal communication
lighting ways in which the research can previously, there was no book available on
work to increase teacher effectiveness. instructional communication or communica­
tion in instruction. Not surprisingly, given
that one of the faculty members and a doc­
toral student involved in the program were
♦ An Historical Account
students in the first author’s previously
described nonverbal communication semi­
The origins of research on nonverbal behavior nar, nonverbal communication was deemed
and on instructional behavior have a great to be a critical element of communication in
deal in common. Both began prior to the 20th instruction. Hence, it was included in the first
century as a wide array of unrelated studies, course on communication in the classroom.
none of which was driven by a theoretical When Janis Andersen was beginning to
understanding of human communication. develop the idea for her doctoral dissertation
They also both appeared as research in disci­ (Andersen, 1978), she undertook the assign­
plines other than Communication: Nonverbal ment of finding out what educational
behavior was studied originally in a wide research had determined what good teachers
range of the social sciences and humani­ do that poor teachers do not do. Her litera­
ties but primarily in Psychology, whereas ture review identified many published and
instructional behavior was researched in unpublished works that related to effective
Education predominantly. For a thorough teaching. After categorizing these works to
discussion of this early work, see Andersen find commonalities, she concluded that
(1978, pp. 60-90). a large number of these works dealt with
In 1970, the first author taught what nonverbal communication behaviors and
may have been the first graduate seminar in appeared to be associated with Albert
nonverbal communication. The bibliogra­ Mehrabian’s (1971) concept of immediacy,
phy of nonverbal research developed by particularly nonverbal immediacy. The
the graduate students in that seminar (in results of her dissertation confirmed some of
response to the unavailability of any text­ these links and stimulated the research that
book on nonverbal communication) identi­ followed and that established the currently
fied hundreds of published (in over 25 strong theoretical association between non­
disciplines) and unpublished works (theses verbal communication and instructional
and dissertations) that dealt with nonverbal communication. It is to this research, and the
behavior. Quite a few of these works on directions it has taken, that we now turn.
nonverbal behavior provided information
that could be interpreted as relating to com­
munication, but they were not presented as ♦ Choices Made and Why
research on nonverbal “communication.”
In 1973, the first author received
permission from the administration at Over the course of years, researchers in any
West Virginia University to initiate a new area make many choices. Some of these are
22-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 5:16 PM Page 423

Nonverbal Communication in Instructional Contexts–––◆–––423

well thought out in advance; others are advanced what he called the principle of
recognized as “choices” only long after they immediacy. He suggested that “people are
were actually made. Instructional/nonver­ drawn toward persons and things they like,
bal researchers have made four important evaluate highly, and prefer; they avoid or
choices: (1) to focus on but reframe the focus move away from things they dislike, evalu­
of the “immediacy principle,” (2) to focus on ate negatively, or do not prefer” (p. 1). This
affective outcomes, (3) to focus on short- principle advances immediacy as a marker
term outcomes, and (4) to focus on the forest for how people feel about other people,
rather than the trees. Within this discussion which can be considered a psychological
we assess the implications of these choices perspective. Even though many credit
for past, current, and future research. Mehrabian’s writings as the foundation for
their work, communication scholars have
moved away from this psychological orien­
REFRAMING THE tation and assumed that the causal path was
IMMEDIACY PRINCIPLE the reverse of what Mehrabian assumed.
He argued that orientation toward another
Immediacy is defined as the degree of per­ produces one’s immediacy choices, whereas
ceived physical or psychological closeness communication scholars suggest that
between two (or more) people (Mehrabian, one’s immediacy choices produce others’
1971; see Andersen, Guerrero, & Jones, this orientations.
volume). Immediacy behaviors (verbal and Richmond and McCroskey (2000b)
nonverbal) are those that result in people formalized this “production” approach
perceiving others to be closer or more distant recently in their principle of immediate
interactionally. Whereas Mehrabian (1971) communication. As they put it, “The more
suggested that there were both verbal and communicators employ immediate behav­
nonverbal elements related to immediacy, iors, the more others will like, evaluate
communication research has dealt primar­ highly, and prefer such communicators;
ily with nonverbal immediacy. This may be and the less communicators employ imme­
in part because development of measures of diate behaviors, the more others will dis­
verbal immediacy has proven to be prob­ like, evaluate negatively, and reject such
lematic. Although there is no extant valid communicators” (p. 212). Immediate or
measure of verbal immediacy (for a critique nonimmediate communication behaviors
of existing measures, see Robinson & are seen as causes of communication out­
Richmond, 1995), research reported by comes, reflecting what can be called a com­
Mottet and Richmond (1998) suggests munication orientation. This view is
there are 10 verbal strategies (approach consistent with a larger perspective of com­
messages) that are likely to increase percep­ munication researchers, particularly those
tions of immediacy, and there are 8 verbal interested in social influence: that commu­
strategies (avoidance messages) that are nicative messages produce outcomes (e.g.,
likely to decrease perceptions of imme­ McCroskey, 1988).
diacy. All of these strategies are likely Whereas these two conceptualizations of
to be employed simultaneously with com­ immediacy may appear to be the opposite of
patible nonverbal behaviors to produce one another, it is not asserted (at least in
increased or decreased perceptions of published papers) that one is correct and the
immediacy. other is incorrect. Research in Psychology,
To help make sense of the influence that for example, has provided strong support for
immediacy may have, Mehrabian (1971) the principle of immediacy (e.g., Mehrabian,
22-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 5:16 PM Page 424

424–––◆–––Contexts and Consequences

1971), and research in several areas of Whereas considerable research has exam­
Communication has provided strong ined both cognitive and affective learning
support for the principle of immediate outcomes, that which has centered on non­
communication (e.g., Richmond, Lane, & verbal communication generally has focused
McCroskey, 2006). Indeed, organizational most attention on affective learning. This
research results suggest that, rather than focus has been chosen for two reasons: (1)
being at odds with one another, psycho­ the belief that verbal communication has
logical and communicative approaches to its largest impact on cognitive learning and
immediacy may well coexist and together nonverbal communication has its largest
produce a cyclical process leading to both impact on affective learning, and (2)
higher or lower immediacy and higher or measuring cognitive learning has been con­
lower liking between individuals over time fronted with the numerous challenges that
(Richmond & McCroskey, 2000a). Such have not yet been overcome to everyone’s
research suggests that the immediacy behav­ satisfaction. These two areas are reviewed
iors of either a supervisor or a subordinate here.
tend to produce reciprocal immediacy on Although verbal and nonverbal messages
the part of the other person and are associ­ are both present in the vast majority of
ated with more subordinate job satis­ instructional environments, it has been
faction. This result is likely to transfer to argued that they perform different func­
instructional contexts. tions in those environments. More specifi­
cally, researchers assert that the verbal
messages stimulate primarily cognitive
FOCUS ON AFFECTIVE meanings in receivers (what the students
LEARNING OUTCOMES learn about the subject matter), whereas
the nonverbal messages stimulate affective
In addition to a focus on immediacy, meanings in receivers (i.e., the feelings and
instructional communication researchers attitudes toward the content as well as feel­
have been concerned with learning out­ ings and attitudes toward the teacher)
comes associated with communication (McCroskey, Richmond, & McCroskey,
behaviors, including immediacy. The tradi­ 2006; Richmond & McCroskey, 2000b).
tional view of learning advanced in From this perspective, subject competence
Educational Psychology posits three distinct of the instructor is said to be the critical ele­
types of learning: cognitive, psychomotor, ment in cognitive learning, whereas behav­
and affective (Bloom, 1956; Krathwohl, ior, particularly nonverbal communication,
Bloom, & Masia, 1964). From the earliest produces a relational impact on the student
reported instructional research involving attitudes and feelings. Although this view
nonverbal communication (Andersen, 1978), cannot be said to be scientifically proven,
instructional communication researchers virtually all the available research results
have embraced this conceptualization. are consistent with it (see Mottet & Beebe,
Most of this work has sought to determine 2006).
relationships between various communica­ Besides a theoretical push toward affec­
tion variables and cognitive and/or affective tive learning, scholars may focus less on cog­
learning (e.g., Richmond et al., 2006). Little nitive learning for methodological reasons.
work has been directed toward psychomo­ Specifically, the development of measures,
tor learning, but some studies have exam­ or even approaches to the measurement, of
ined affect toward behaviors learned cognitive learning has frustrated instructional
instead (e.g., Andersen, 1978). communication researchers consistently. No
22-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 5:16 PM Page 425

Nonverbal Communication in Instructional Contexts–––◆–––425

approach to measuring cognitive learning, Whereas more research is needed before


much less a specific measuring instrument, a firm conclusion can be drawn, the instruc­
has met with acceptance by a majority of tional research to date suggests that non­
scholars (for a discussion of this issue, see verbal factors may have a much stronger
Richmond et al., 2006). This is also true of impact on learning in the classroom than do
scholars in Education, who have been work­ verbal factors. This is particularly true for
ing on this problem far longer than have affective learning. The available results
Communication scholars. have led to an increased focus on nonverbal
An analysis of these issues, discussed in immediacy in the instructional arena, as
Richmond et al. (2006), concludes that well as in organizational and health con­
measuring the impact of nonverbal commu­ texts (see, Robinson, this volume for
nication behaviors (such as immediacy) on more discussion of immediacy in health
students’ cognitive learning outside the care­ contexts).
fully controlled experimental environment
is very difficult. Some approaches have been
found to be successful and valid in con­ SHORT- VERSUS
trolled experiments, including methods LONG-TERM LEARNING
developed by instructional communication
researchers (e.g., Chesebro & McCroskey, The concern with learning outcomes
2000). The use of these measures in typical forces another choice that researchers must
classroom environments has yet to be vali­ make in the area of nonverbal communica­
dated to the point that they are accepted by tion and instruction: whether to assess short-
some scholars. or long-term learning outcomes. The over­
Given that nonverbal communication whelming majority of instructional commu­
behaviors likely have their greatest impact nication research, including that focused on
on affective learning, and given that some nonverbal communication, has centered on
scholars are not willing to accept the validity short-term learning effects (Richmond et al.,
of the cognitive learning measures that are 2006). It is most likely that this is the case
available currently, nonverbal communica­ because studying long-term effects is so diffi­
tion in instruction has therefore been con­ cult. This assumption is bolstered by the fact
cerned with affective learning primarily. that many of the researchers, when reporting
Instructional researchers have studied both short-term effects, note the need for more
verbal and nonverbal aspects of instructional long-term research.
communication. One of the most extensive It can also be argued, however, that
programs of research in instructional com­ affective learning has its primary impact on
munication focused on the verbal messages long-term, not short-term, cognitive learn­
that teachers use to influence students. This ing. If students learn to like a teacher and/or
“power in the classroom” series of studies a subject matter, it is likely they will choose
determined that use of different categories of to take another class from that teacher
verbal influence messages produced marked and/or in that subject matter. Hence, these
differences in the effectiveness of instruction students will have greater opportunities for
(McCroskey, Richmond, Plax, & Kearney, cognitive learning than will students who
1985; Richmond, 1990). When both verbal do not develop this positive affect toward
and nonverbal variables were considered, the teacher or the subject matter. Students
however, the nonverbal variables proved to who study a subject longer learn more than
have considerably more influence (Plax, students who don’t. Given this view,
Kearney, McCroskey, & Richmond, 1986). affective learning probably should be the
22-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 5:16 PM Page 426

426–––◆–––Contexts and Consequences

primary focus for scholars interested in the communication, and in textbooks con­
role of nonverbal communication in learn­ cerned with nonverbal communication,
ing environments. As noted, this decision is communication scholars have chosen to
reflected in much of the current research take two different approaches, which we
being published in this area. refer to as the “trees” or the “forest.” The
A theoretical view advanced by some “tree” approach (also referred to as the
instructional communication researchers “categorical” approach; Knapp, 1972;
(including the present authors) is that long- Malandro & Barker, 1983) separates the
term learning is the most important form of elements into categories and centers on how
learning (McCroskey, 1998). This view each of these elements affects communica­
holds that schools are created primarily to tion outcomes across contexts. The “for­
acculturate young people and prepare them est” approach, also referred to as the
for success in the surrounding culture. “functional” approach (Burgoon & Saine,
Whereas schools are seen as teaching the 1978), centers on how multiple elements of
content of many disciplines, as well as the nonverbal behavior influence communica­
values and attitudes of the people in that tion outcomes collectively in specific con­
culture, their primary function is, arguably, texts. Most researchers have chosen one or
to teach the students the differences between the other of these to organize or frame their
right and wrong (as seen by their culture), work on nonverbal communication. Some
and prepare them with lifelong learning textbook authors also have followed this
skills. Much of this socialization deals with pattern, whereas others have chosen to
matters of affect, such as developing posi­ include both approaches in their works
tive affect for learning and the norms and (e.g., Hickson & Stacks, 1985; Richmond
values of the culture. Given that nonverbal & McCroskey, 2004).
communication has been found to have a Instructional communication researchers,
major impact on the development of affect for the most part, have taken the “forest”
(Richmond et al., 2006), it may well be the approach, and their emphasis on nonverbal
critical element producing the desired (as immediacy is representative of this choice.
well that which is not desired) outcomes of They recognize that single nonverbal
educational institutions. The long-term behaviors rarely manifest alone (e.g.,
impact of nonverbal communication, there­ Hickson & Stacks, 1985) and that, typi­
fore, should be a major focus of future cally, there are many nonverbal messages
instructional and nonverbal communica­ transmitted simultaneously in instructional
tion researchers. communication by both teachers/trainers
and students. Whereas single nonverbal
behaviors may have a direct impact in
STUDYING THE TREES this context, it is most likely that outcomes
OR THE FOREST are also influenced by multiple nonver­
bal behaviors interacting with one another.
In addition to choices made regarding This impact of both single nonverbal
what is to be the focus of research and at behaviors and the interaction of nonver­
what point the focus is captured, scholars bal behaviors has been demonstrated
also make choices as to the perspective in experimental research (e.g., Kelley &
from which to approach the topics. Gorham, 1988).
From the beginning of formal communi­ Yet, because most of the nonverbal cate­
cation research relating to nonverbal gories were conceptualized by nonverbal
22-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 5:16 PM Page 427

Nonverbal Communication in Instructional Contexts–––◆–––427

communication researchers prior to the will produce the opposite response.


onset of the blending of nonverbal behav­ Whereas nonverbal messages associated
iors within instructional research, we will with physical appearance may have a
first examine the “tree” approach. This con­ strong initial impact, even to the point that
ceptualization involves the categorization one or both people determine not to com­
or classification of the types of nonverbal municate any further with the other. In the
behaviors that are available to instructional instructional environment, it is presumed
communicators. Subsequently, we will that this process will function in much the
examine the “forest”: collections of multiple same manner that it does in other contexts.
nonverbal behaviors that are the founda­ Available research suggests that teachers or
tions of larger conceptualizations that have students who are deemed physically attrac­
been studied in the instructional context. tive will be chosen for more positive future
communication and other positive treat­
ment than will those perceived as less
Categories of Nonverbal attractive (Richmond, 1997).
Behaviors: The “Tree” Approach
In addition to the many cues involved in
Researchers have identified 10 primary physical appearance, there are many nonver­
categories of nonverbal elements: physical bal behaviors that are included in the classi­
appearance, gesture, bodily movement, fication of gesture. Any movements of the
face, eye, touch, vocal, space, environment, hands and/or arms qualify as gestures,
and time. Each of these exists within most although not all these movements are
instructional settings (mediated instruction received as meaningful. Research on gestures
prevents some of these from influencing in the instructional context has been limited,
communication), at least within the context but some work has found, for example, that
of the general North American culture. teachers vary in the amount of gesture in
Given that culture often has a dramatic which they engage, and students report that
influence on nonverbal behaviors and inter­ they learn more from teachers who gesture
pretation of nonverbal behaviors (see more (Richmond, Gorham, & McCroskey,
Matsumoto, this volume), our discussion 1987). Gestures are part of a larger category
should not be assumed to be generalizable of body movement. This class of nonverbal
to other cultures. Much of the information behaviors references the actual movement of
for the following material is drawn from a teacher within the classroom (e.g., walking
summaries provided by Richmond (1997) and leaning). Students report that they learn
and Richmond and McCroskey (2000a, b). more when teachers walk around in the
Rather than citing all of the sources they classroom, and learn less when they teach
provide and the description in that review, while seated or standing behind a podium
readers interested in more detail and refer­ (Richmond et al., 1987). Experimental
ences to research can find such information research indicates that teachers who lean
in those sources. toward the students have students who learn
The nonverbal message received first more than teachers who lean away from the
when people meet is, in most cases, that students (Kelley & Gorham, 1988).
of the physical appearance of the other Another subset of body movements are
person. Responses may be triggered instantly those that occur on the face. There are
by such messages: Some of these cognitive many potential facial expressions that
and affective reactions will stimulate the teachers may express in the classroom.
person to be attracted to the other; some Most of these are yet to be researched.
22-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 5:16 PM Page 428

428–––◆–––Contexts and Consequences

Research has indicated, however, that under most circumstances (occasionally


students report greater learning from vocal communication is produced without
teachers who smile frequently in the class­ the use of verbal language). This nonverbal
room than from those who do not behavior has been referred to as vocalics
(Richmond et al., 1987). Similarly, eye or paralanguage. Among the many ele­
behavior has been studied in the classroom. ments of vocalic behavior is variety in vocal
Research on gaze has found that teachers use. Students report that vocal variety is the
may be looking anywhere in a classroom at nonverbal behavior that is most positively
any given time. But students report that associated with their learning. In contrast,
they learn more from those teachers who the use of a vocal monotone is reported to
look at the students most of the time (estab­ be the behavior that is most negatively asso­
lishing eye contact), and less from those ciated with learning (Richmond et al.,
teachers who look at the board or their 1987). Another vocalic element is the
notes more frequently (Richmond et al., accent of the spoken language. All speakers
1987). Another potentially important eye have an accent, but some are clearer to
behavior for teachers is the “teacher stare,” student listeners than are others. In her
which is often used to help manage research, Linda McCroskey (2003) deter­
students’ classroom behavior. Research has mined that clarity of a teacher was an
not yet confirmed this behavior to be effec­ extremely strong predictor of the evalua­
tive, however. tion of the teacher, students’ affect toward
Touch can also be considered a subset of the subject matter, and both cognitive and
body movements, and it has been alleged that affective learning in U.S. classrooms. This
in the general North American culture, touch was found for both domestic and foreign
is the most powerful nonverbal message one teachers.
can send (Richmond & McCroskey, 2000a, Quite a different subset of nonverbal
b). Touch also is the most suspect of all the cues in the classroom concerns the use of
nonverbal behaviors in which people in this space. The two primary components of
culture can be engaged. When we teach mate­ space in research are territory and inter­
rial on nonverbal communication to groups personal space. Particularly in the lower
of adults working in organizational environ­ grades, territoriality of students is a major
ments, we commonly ask them what is the concern, and it is the number one cause of
most important rule that their organization students’ physical conflicts (Richmond,
demands. Their answers consistently rate 1997). Whereas adults often have learned
“Don’t touch” as Number 1. This response is to control their territorial instinct, young
also the number one answer given by children usually have not. The nonverbal
teachers, particularly male teachers, in educa­ behavior of violating another’s space often
tional organizations. Touch is another area results in strong verbal responses, and it is
that instructional researchers have rarely not unusual to find these conflicts to result
studied. Research indicates, however, that in physical attacks and subsequent retalia­
even college students who report that their tions. Although little kids (often referred to
teachers touch students report a higher level as “space invaders”) usually don’t know
of learning from those teachers than from any better, older students (often referred to
those who touch less or not at all (Richmond as “bullies”) do understand what they are
et al., 1987). doing, and do it on purpose. This is an area
In addition to movement, vocal commu­ of research in Education; instructional com­
nication accompanies verbal communication munication researchers have shown little
22-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 5:16 PM Page 429

Nonverbal Communication in Instructional Contexts–––◆–––429

interest in this nonverbal behavior problem, Summary. As we have noted above, there
however. are very many different kinds of nonverbal
Many factors of the environment either behaviors that may individually affect
send constant messages to people or control instruction. Each has the potential to
the communication that exists within the enhance or impede communication in this
environment directly. These effects occur context. This concludes our discussion of
in educational environments. Considerable the specific categories of nonverbal behav­
research on instructional environments has iors. Now we turn our attention to collec­
been conducted, but virtually none by tions of these variables.
instructional communication researchers.
Such research has been reported in fields such
Collective Nonverbal Constructs:
as Architecture, Education, Psychology, and
The “Forest” Approach
Sociology. What little research that has been
reported by instructional communication As noted, it is rare that a single non­
researchers, however, indicates that even the verbal variable will exist alone in an
arrangement of seating in a classroom can instructional environment. Whereas many
have an important impact on student com­ nonverbal cues function within and affect
munication behavior and student learning most instructional contexts, their collective
(e.g., McCroskey & McVetta, 1978). impact may be of much greater theoretical
The use of time, also known as chrone­ and practical importance. Hence, instruc­
mics, can be highly communicative in the tional researchers have sought to develop or
classroom. How people react to time will adopt constructs that can explain the gen­
vary greatly based on the culture in which eral effects of the combined forces of multi­
they were born or in which they have been ple nonverbal behaviors (i.e., the nonverbal
enculturated. Not only do large national cul­ “forest”). The collective nonverbal con­
tures have a system of time, but individual structs we focus on here are immediacy,
organizations have a culture as well that clarity, and sociocommunicative style.
addresses the time rules for its members. As noted, immediacy is the degree of per­
Schools, at least within the general North ceived physical or psychological closeness
American culture, are examples of the most between people (Mehrabian, 1971), and it
rigid ways of dealing with time. Everything may be reflected in or a result of immediacy
within a given day usually is timed down to behaviors. Nonverbal immediacy research
the minute. One of the major concerns in has determined that it has a powerful impact
most elementary schools is teaching the on instructional outcomes. These outcomes
system of time required by its rules to young include (1) increased affinity for the teacher,
children who have not already been taught (2) increased affective learning (affect for the
the system enforced in their culture. Time content of the subject matter), (3) increased
imposes rules for communicating (or not student perceptions of their own cognitive
communicating). It determines when students learning, and (4) the granting of increased
can talk, how long they can talk, where they referent power to teachers by students. Each
can talk, and what they can talk about. of these will be discussed in turn.
Penalties that can be very severe are imposed The affinity construct was first explored
on students who do not follow the commu­ by interpersonal communication research­
nication time rules. This is an area that ers (Bell & Daly, 1984; McCroskey &
instructional communication researchers Wheeless, 1976) who argued that develop­
have generally ignored up until this time. ment of affinity is the “first function of
22-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 5:16 PM Page 430

430–––◆–––Contexts and Consequences

communication” (McCroskey & Wheeless, associated with both actual content learned
1976, pp. 21–22). Having affinity for (Kelley & Gorham, 1988) and students’
another person essentially means liking perceptions of their own learning (e.g.,
that other person. Nonverbal immediacy is Christophel, 1990; McCroskey et al., 2004;
considered one of the main means one McCroskey, Sallinen, Fayer, Richmond,
person can use to get another person to like & Barraclough, 1996; Richmond, 1990;
him or her. This “liking” is believed to Richmond et al., 1987). Actual learning
include a number of positive affects—for (measured via a content test) and student
example, having increased interpersonal perceived learning have been found to be
attraction (social and/or task) for the other correlated substantially (r = .50; Chesebro
person, having increased respect for the & McCroskey, 2000). Collectively, the
other person, seeing the other person as research evidence suggests a strong positive
more credible (competence, caring, and/or association between teacher nonverbal
trustworthiness), being more responsive to immediacy and cognitive learning of their
the other person, and so on (McCroskey & students.
Wheeless, 1976). Instructional communica­ The final factor involved with immediacy
tion research has observed that teachers to be discussed here that can have a strong
employ nonverbal immediacy consciously impact on students is the teacher’s referent
to increase students’ affinity for them power. French and Raven (1959) conceptu­
(McCroskey & McCroskey, 1986). It also alized five types of power. Referent power
has produced consistent results showing is one of those, the one that has been found
that teachers’ increased nonverbal immedi­ to produce the most positive influence on
acy is associated with similar increases in students when used by teachers (Richmond
students’ evaluations of the teacher, both in & McCroskey, 1984). Opinion leaders,
the United States and in other cultures (e.g., mentors, and others who are seen as positive
McCroskey, Richmond, Sallinen, Fayer, & models and/or people whom others wish to
Barraclough, 1995; McCroskey, Valencic, emulate are said to have referent power.
& Richmond, 2004). These individuals are seen to be both credi­
As noted earlier in this chapter, the con­ ble and task attractive. Teachers who are
struct of affective learning was advanced in nonverbally immediate have been found to
educational psychology (Krathwohl et al., be perceived as both more credible and task
1964). It refers to students’ development of attractive than are teachers who are less
positive (or negative) affect toward the nonverbally immediate (McCroskey et al.,
subject matter taught. Many studies have 2004; Thweatt & McCroskey, 1998).
produced results indicating a strong, positive Overall, then, nonverbally immediate
relationship (in the United States and other teachers tend to produce substantially more
cultures) between the nonverbal immediacy positive instructional outcomes. Students
of teachers and the affective learning of have more affinity for immediate teachers,
their students (e.g., Christophel, 1990; they have more affective learning, they have
McCroskey, Fayer, Richmond, Sallinen, & more cognitive learning, and they grant the
Barraclough, 1996; McCroskey et al., 2004; teacher more referent power in the instruc­
Richmond, 1990). Cognitive learning was tional environment.
also advanced in educational psychology Another collective construct implicating
(Bloom, 1956). Whereas the measurement nonverbal cues is clarity. Clarity has long
of cognitive learning has been controversial, been believed to be an important factor in
nonverbal immediacy has been found to be effective instruction. Considerable research
22-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 5:16 PM Page 431

Nonverbal Communication in Instructional Contexts–––◆–––431

on teacher clarity conducted by Education The final collective nonverbal construct


scholars indicates a strong relationship to be discussed here, sociocommunicative
between teacher clarity and both student style, is a blend of both verbal and nonverbal
achievement and satisfaction (Hines, components. It is also correlated with both
Cruickshank, & Kennedy, 1985). Most of immediacy and clarity. This construct was
this work has centered on organizational developed primarily by researchers interested
and verbal factors. Some researchers, how­ in interpersonal communication (McCroskey
ever, have linked a lack of clarity to & Richmond, 1996; Richmond & Martin,
nonverbal factors (McCroskey, 2003). For 1998; Richmond & McCroskey, 1990) but
example, vocal monotony and vocalized was discovered to be important in instruc­
pauses have been identified as factors which tional communication as well (Richmond,
students identify as interfering with clarity. 2002). The sociocommunicative style and
A possibly very important nonverbal orientation constructs are based on earlier
component, the teacher’s accent, has been variables related to communicator style
discussed widely by people concerned with (Norton, 1983), social style (Merrill & Reid,
teachers who teach in their second lan­ 1981), and androgyny (Bem, 1974).
guage, or any language other than their Sociocommunicative style is conceptual­
first, or teach students whose language is ized as the way the receiver perceives the
the same as the teacher’s but employ a dif­ style of the source. Sociocommunicative
ferent accent of that language (e.g., a Texan orientation is defined as the way people
teaching in Maine). perceive their own style of communication.
Recent research (Sidelinger & McCroskey, These constructs are seen as highly related
1997) has found that clarity is correlated to communication competence across com­
highly with teacher evaluation and affective munication contexts. Three components
learning. This research also found that both are argued to be common to both of these
teacher nonverbal immediacy and teacher constructs: assertiveness, responsiveness,
sociocommunicative style are highly related and versatility (or flexibility) (McCroskey
with both clarity and both of these instruc­ & Richmond, 1996). Because of the diffi­
tional outcomes. Chesebro and McCroskey culty in developing a measure of versatility
(1998, 2001) also found that nonverbal (one that would be independent of the other
immediacy was associated positively with two components), however, most of the
clarity. They also determined that both research in this area has focused on only
increased clarity and nonverbal immedi­ assertiveness and responsiveness.
acy were associated with reductions in Assertiveness is seen as communication
students’ receiver apprehension. In addition, behavior that reflects individuals’ ability to
increased clarity was associated with both stand up for themselves and prevent others
increased affective learning and increased from taking advantage of them, while not
cognitive learning. These research results aggressing against the other person. These
suggest a strong association between clarity individuals are likely to make requests,
and positive instructional outcomes. That express their feelings, and manage conver­
association, however, may be influenced by sations in ways consistent with their com­
nonverbal immediacy and sociocommu­ municative goals. Bem (1974) has argued
nicative style as well. There are indications that this type of communication behavior is
that these constructs are interwoven and stereotypically characteristic of males in the
all are critical nonverbal aspects of effective general North American culture (and many
instruction. other cultures). Assertive communication is
22-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 5:16 PM Page 432

432–––◆–––Contexts and Consequences

viewed as a positive characteristic for both more positive instructional outcomes: more
females and males, however, and necessary positive student affect, more affective learn­
for attaining communication competence ing, more cognitive learning, more moti­
for members of both sexes. vated students, more student trust for the
Likewise, responsiveness is seen as other- teacher, and so on (e.g., Kearney &
oriented communication behavior that McCroskey, 1980; McCroskey et al., 2004;
reflects an individual’s ability to recognize Thomas, 1994; Wanzer & McCroskey,
the needs of others without being submis­ 1998; Wooten & McCroskey, 1996).
sive to the others. Communicators who are Those who are less assertive and/or respon­
responsive are sensitive to others, consider sive are less effective. As was the case with
others’ feelings, and listen carefully to their teacher clarity, teacher immediacy is corre­
communication. Bem (1974) has argued lated with teacher assertiveness and res­
that this type of communication behavior is ponsiveness. Although teacher assertiveness
stereotypically characteristic of females in and teacher responsiveness have been found
the general North American culture (and not to be statistically or meaningfully corre­
many other cultures). As is the case with lated with each other, they have been found
assertive communication, however, respon­ to be correlated consistently with nonver­
sive communication is viewed as a positive bal immediacy (McCroskey et al., 2004;
characteristic of both males and females, Thomas, Richmond, & McCroskey, 1994).
one that is often necessary for attaining
communication competence for members Summary. Based on the research reported
of both sexes. up to this time, it is clear that nonverbal
Whereas both assertiveness and respon­ communication has a major impact on
siveness are seen as characteristic traits of teachers’ effectiveness in the classroom. In
an individual’s behavior, versatility is seen particular, nonverbal immediacy, clarity,
as an individual’s ability to be flexible and and sociocommunicative style all are impor­
adaptable with regard to his or her use of tant factors in that success or failure. Recent
either or both assertiveness and responsive­ research indicates that immediacy and two
ness. Competent communicators, then, are components of sociocommunication style,
seen as being assertive when it is needed responsiveness and assertiveness, have a
and responsive when it is needed as well as strong genetic base (McCroskey, Heisel, &
reducing their assertiveness and/or respon­ Richmond, 2001); however, no research of
siveness when that would be appropriate. this type related to clarity has been reported.
In the instructional environment, for
example, a teacher who is teaching a mass
♦ Implications
lecture class might need to be more
assertive, whereas that teacher, when teach­
ing an interactive class such as a seminar, The research choices reviewed in this chap­
might need to be more responsive. Teachers ter, and the subsequent findings of that
who are highly assertive or highly respon­ research, help us understand much of what
sive, and not versatile, might be more effec­ nonverbal behaviors do in instructional
tive in one type of class and less effective in contexts. This research is important to
another type of class. those who study instructional communica­
In support of this, research has indicated tion and nonverbal communication. It is
that, in general, teachers who are more also important to those who may use non­
assertive and more responsive produce verbal cues in the instructional context. We
22-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 5:16 PM Page 433

Nonverbal Communication in Instructional Contexts–––◆–––433

close this chapter with a discussion of some constructs as predictors of the differences
recent research that focuses specifically on between student perceptions of domestic
the importance of teacher effectiveness in and foreign teachers as well as their pre­
multi-cultural classrooms. dictability in predicting the outcomes
Two studies reported by Linda McCroskey within the two teacher groups separately.
(2002, 2003) have provided considerable The specific constructs she employed were
assistance in determining what nonverbal the ones discussed in the previous section:
constructs are particularly important to immediacy, clarity, and sociocommunica­
the effectiveness of teachers in the instruc­ tive style. Student ethnocentrism was also
tional environment. In these two studies, included as a predictor of the outcome vari­
McCroskey sought to determine whether, ables (affect toward instructor, instructor
and, if so, why, foreign instructors are eval­ evaluation, affective learning, and perceived
uated less positively than are domestic cognitive learning). This study employed a
instructors. The first study (McCroskey, more reliable and valid measure of ethno­
2002) examined student ethnocentrism and centrism than the one used in the first
several student communication traits (e.g., study. The results of the research replicated
intercultural communication apprehension, the results of the first study with regard to
willingness to communicate). The student ethnocentrism. The correlations of ethno­
participants were asked to respond to two centrism with all the outcome variables
of their current teachers: one a domestic were trivial (r = .04 to .11) and nonsignifi­
teacher and the other a foreign teacher. cant. Ethnocentrism could not predict
Her results confirmed the expected students’ varying responses to foreign and
difference between students’ reactions to domestic instructors.
domestic and foreign teachers. The mean The students’ responses to each of the
differences between the two groups outcomes were substantially different when
were significant and large. The results also they were responding to a domestic or a
indicated that student ethnocentrism had foreign instructor. All the mean differences
a low but significant relationship with strongly favored the domestic teachers. It is
the various outcome variables (perceived important to note, however, that approxi­
effectiveness of the teacher, willingness to mately 30% of the students responded
initiate communication with the teacher, more positively to their foreign teacher than
student motivation to work with the their domestic teacher. Where the teacher
teacher, affective learning, perceived cogni­ came from was not a consistent indicator of
tive learning). The student trait predictors student preferences. In contrast, each of the
produced correlations that were trivial communication variables served individu­
(average r = .11) and nonsignificant. The ally as a significant predictor of all the out­
“bottom line” in this study was that come variables. The multiple correlations
although the students perceived very large of just assertiveness and responsiveness
differences between the foreign and domes­ with each of the outcome variables were
tic teachers, neither ethnocentrism nor the extremely strong, ranging from .54 to .72.
student communication traits could predict The multiple correlations involving imme­
a large amount of this variation between diacy and clarity predicting the outcome
domestic and foreign teachers. variables were equally strong, ranging from
The second study (McCroskey, 2003) r = .52 to .81. The results of this research
was designed to examine the predictability suggest that effective instruction is based on
of teacher nonverbal communication such teacher behaviors as are collectively
22-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 5:16 PM Page 434

434–––◆–––Contexts and Consequences

included in the constructs of nonverbal Burgoon, J. K., & Saine, T. J. (1978). The
immediacy, clarity, and sociocommunica­ unspoken dialogue: An introduction to
tive style, all of which include at their base nonverbal communication. Boston, MA:
nonverbal communication. Houghton Mifflin.
Chesebro, J. L., & McCroskey, J. C. (1998). The
relationship of teacher clarity and teacher
immediacy with students’ experiences of
♦ Conclusion state receiver apprehension. Commu­
nication Quarterly, 46, 446–456.
Chesebro, J. L., & McCroskey, J. C. (2000). The
Effective teaching is dependent upon
relationship between students’ reports of
“appropriate” nonverbal communication
learning and their actual recall of lecture
of teachers. In our opinion, certification of
material: A validity test. Communication
teachers without substantial instruction in Education, 49, 297–301.
nonverbal communication concepts and Chesebro, J. L., & McCroskey, J. C. (2001). The
skills would be pure folly. The success of relationship of teacher clarity and immedi­
teachers at all levels depends on how they acy with student state receiver apprehen­
communicate nonverbally. It may be that sion, affect, and cognitive learning.
some teachers are genetically programmed Communication Education, 50, 59–68.
to be more effective, whereas others are Christophel, D. M. (1990). The relationships
equally programmed to fail. Individuals among teacher immediacy behaviors,
considering teaching as a profession need to student motivation, and learning. Com­
munication Education, 39, 323–340.
be aware that these nonverbal behaviors are
French, J. R. P., & Raven, B. (1959). The bases
expected of them, at least in the U.S. cul­
of social power. In D. Cartwright (Ed.),
ture, and measure that in their decision.
Studies in social power (pp. 150–167). Ann
Similarly, administrators of teacher educa­ Arbor, MI: Institute for Social Research.
tion and teacher/trainer selection should Hickson, M. L., III, & Stacks, D. W. (1985).
consider these communication abilities (or Nonverbal communication: Studies and
lack of) in their decisions to admit or hire applications. Dubuque, IA: Wm. C. Brown.
people for their programs. Hines, C., Cruickshank, D., & Kennedy, J.
(1985). Teacher clarity and its relationship
to student achievement and satisfaction.
♦ References American Educational Research Journal,
22, 87–99.
Kearney, P., & McCroskey, J. C. (1980).
Andersen, J. A. (1978). The relationship Relationships among teacher communica­
between teacher immediacy and teach­ tion style, trait and state communication
ing effectiveness. Unpublished doctoral apprehension, and teacher effectiveness.
dissertation, West Virginia University, Communication Yearbook, 4, 533–551.
Morgantown. Kelley, D. H., & Gorham, J. (1988). Effects
Bell, R., & Daly, J. A. (1984). The affinity-seek­ of immediacy on recall of information.
ing function of communication. Commu­ Communication Education, 37, 198–207.
nication Monographs, 51, 91–115. Knapp, M. L. (1972). Nonverbal communica­
Bem, S. L. (1974). The measurement of psycho­ tion in human interaction. New York: Holt,
logical androgyny. Journal of Consulting Rinehart, and Winston.
and Clinical Psychology, 47, 155–162. Krathwohl, D. R., Bloom, B. S., & Masia, B. B.
Bloom, B. S. (1956). Taxonomy of educational (1964). Taxonomy of educational objec­
objectives (Handbook I: Cognitive domain). tives (Handbook II: Affective domain).
New York: McKay. New York: McKay.
22-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 5:16 PM Page 435

Nonverbal Communication in Instructional Contexts–––◆–––435

Malandro, L. A., & Barker, L. (1983). McCroskey, J. C., Valencic, K. M., &
Nonverbal communication. Reading, MA: Richmond, V. P. (2004). Toward a general
Addison-Wesley. model of instructional communication.
McCroskey, J. C. (1998). An introduction to Communication Quarterly, 53, 197–210.
communication in the classroom (2nd ed.). McCroskey, J. C., & Wheeless, L. R. (1976).
Acton, MA: Tapestry Press. Introduction to human communication.
McCroskey, J. C., Fayer, J. M., Richmond, Boston, MA: Allyn & Bacon.
V. P., Sallinen, A., & Barraclough, R. A. McCroskey, L. L. (2002). Domestic and interna­
(1996). A multi-cultural examination of the tional college instructors: An examination
relationship between nonverbal immediacy of perceived differences and their correlates.
and affective learning. Communication Journal of Intercultural Communication
Quarterly, 44, 297–307. Research, 31, 63–83.
McCroskey, J. C., Heisel, A. D., & Richmond, McCroskey, L. L. (2003). Relationships of instruc­
V. P. (2001). Eysenck’s BIG THREE and tional communication styles of domestic
communication traits: Three correlational and foreign instructors with instructional
studies. Communication Monographs, 68, outcomes. Journal of Intercultural Commu­
360–366. nication Research, 32, 75–96.
McCroskey, J. C., & McCroskey, L. L. (1986). Mehrabian, A. (1971). Silent messages. Belmont,
The affinity-seeking of classroom teachers. CA: Wadsworth.
Communication Research Reports, 3, Merrill, D. W., & Reid, R. H. (1981). Personal
158–167. styles and effective performance: Make
McCroskey, J. C., & McVetta, R. W. (1978). your style work for you. Radnor, PA:
Classroom seating arrangements: Instruc­ Chilton Book.
tional communication theory versus student Mottet, T. P., & Beebe, S. A. (2006).
preferences. Communication Education, Foundations of instructional communica­
27, 99–111. tion. In T. P. Mottet, V. P. Richmond, &
McCroskey, J. C., & Richmond, V. P. (1996). J. C. McCroskey (Eds.), The handbook
Fundamentals of human communication. of instructional communication: Rhetorical
Prospect Heights, IL: Waveland. and relational perspectives (pp. 3–32).
McCroskey, J. C., Richmond, V. P., & Boston, MA: Allyn & Bacon.
McCroskey, L. L. (2006). An introduction Mottet, T., & Richmond, V. P. (1998). Verbal
to communication in the classroom: The approach and avoidance items. Communica­
role of communication in teaching and tion Quarterly, 46, 25–40.
training. Boston, MA: Allyn & Bacon. Norton, R. (1983). Communicator style: Theory,
McCroskey, J. C., Richmond, V. P., Plax, T. G., applications, and measures. Beverly Hills,
& Kearney, P. (1985). Power in the class­ CA: Sage.
room V: Behavior alteration techniques, Plax, T. G., Kearney, P., McCroskey, J. C.,
communication training, and learning. & Richmond, V. P. (1986). Power in
Communication Education, 34, 214–226. the classroom VI: Verbal control strategies,
McCroskey, J. C., Richmond, V. P., Sallinen, A., nonverbal immediacy, and affective learn­
Fayer, J. M., & Barraclough, R. A. (1995). ing. Communication Education, 35, 43–55.
A cross-cultural and multi-behavioral Richmond, V. P. (1990). Communication in
analysis of the relationship between non­ the classroom: Power and motivation.
verbal immediacy and teacher evaluation. Communication Education, 39, 181–195.
Communication Education, 44, 281–290. Richmond, V. P. (1997). Nonverbal communi­
McCroskey, J. C. Sallinen, A., Fayer, J. M., cation in the classroom. Acton, MA:
Richmond, V. P., & Barraclough, R. A. Tapestry Press.
(1996). Nonverbal immediacy and cogni­ Richmond, V. P. (2002). Socio-communicative
tive learning: A cross-cultural investigation. style and orientation in instruction. In
Communication Education, 45, 200–211. J. L. Chesebro & J. C. McCroskey (Eds.),
22-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 5:16 PM Page 436

436–––◆–––Contexts and Consequences

Communication for teachers (pp. 104–115). Richmond, V.P., & McCroskey, J.C. (2004).
Boston, MA: Allyn & Bacon. Nonverbal behavior in interpersonal rela­
Richmond, V. P., Gorham, J. S., & McCroskey, tions (5th ed.). Boston, MA: Allyn & Bacon.
J. C. (1987). The relationship between Robinson, R. Y., & Richmond, V. P. (1995).
selected immediacy behaviors and cognitive Validity of the verbal immediacy scale.
learning. Communication Yearbook, 10, Communication Research Reports, 12,
574–590. 80–84.
Richmond, V. P., Lane, D. R., & McCroskey, Sidelinger, R. J., & McCroskey, J. C. (1997).
J. C. (2006). Teacher immediacy and the Communication correlates of teacher clarity
teacher–student relationship. In T. P. Mottet, in the college classroom. Communication
V. P. Richmond, & J. C. McCroskey (Eds.), Research Reports, 14, 1–10.
Handbook of instructional communication: Thomas, C. E. (1994). An analysis of teacher
Rhetorical and relational perspectives. socio-communicative style as a predictor
Boston, MA: Allyn & Bacon. of classroom communication behaviors,
Richmond, V. P., & Martin, M. M. (1998). student liking, motivation, and learning.
Socio-communicative style and socio-com­ Unpublished doctoral dissertation, West
municative orientation. In J. C. McCroskey, Virginia University, Morgantown.
J. A. Daly, M. M. Martin, & M. J. Beatty Thomas, C. E., Richmond, V. P., & McCroskey,
(Eds.), Communication and personality: J. C. (1994). Is immediacy anything more
Trait perspectives (pp. 133–148). Cresskill, than just being nice? The association
NJ: Hampton Press. between immediacy and socio-communica­
Richmond, V. P., & McCroskey, J. C. (1984). tive style. Communication Research
Power in the classroom II: Power and Reports, 11, 107–115.
learning. Communication Education, 33, Thweatt, K. S., & McCroskey, J. C. (1998). The
125–136. impact of teacher immediacy and misbehav­
Richmond, V. P., & McCroskey, J. C. (1990). iors on teacher credibility. Communication
Reliability and separation of factors on Education, 47, 348–358.
the assertiveness–responsiveness measure. Wanzer, M. B., & McCroskey, J. C. (1998).
Psychological Reports, 67, 449–450. Teacher socio-communicative style as a
Richmond, V. P., & McCroskey, J. C. (2000a). correlated of student affect toward teacher
The impact of supervisor and subordinate and course material. Communication Edu­
immediacy on relational and organizational cation, 47, 43–52.
outcomes. Communication Monographs, Wooten, A. G., & McCroskey, J. C. (1996).
66, 85–95. Student trust of teacher as a function
Richmond, V. P., & McCroskey, J. C. (2000b). of socio-communicative style of teacher
Nonverbal behavior in interpersonal and socio-communicative orientation of
relations (5th ed.). Boston, MA: Allyn & student. Communication Research Reports,
Bacon. 13, 94–100.
23-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 7:34 PM Page 437

23
NONVERBAL COMMUNICATION
AND PHYSICIAN-PATIENT
INTERACTION
Review and New Directions

� Jeffrey D. Robinson
Rutgers University

O nce considered to be an intangible aspect of “bedside manner,”


the scientific study of nonverbal communication during visits
between patients and medical physicians is now well documented.
Research suggests that physicians’ nonverbal behavior shapes partici­
pants’ visit communication (e.g., patients’ self-disclosure); ratings of
physicians’ rapport, dominance, and medical-technical skills; patients’
satisfaction with physicians; patients’ understanding and recall of visit
information; and patients’ adherence to physicians’ medical recommen­
dations. Physicians’ nonverbal behavior is consequential in other ways
as well. For example, both the accreditation of residency programs and
the certification of physicians require assessment of physicians’ compe­
tence in “interpersonal skills,” which involve “inherently relational”

Author’s Note: The author thanks James Dillard, Jenny Mandelbaum, Valerie
Manusov, and Richard Street for comments on earlier drafts.

◆ 437
23-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 7:34 PM Page 438

438–––◆–––Contexts and Consequences

and “humanistic” aspects of nonverbal communication has at least two underlying


and verbal communication (Duffy, Gordon, dimensions: medical-technical (i.e., instru­
Whelan, Cole-Kelly, & Frankel, 2004). mental) and affective-relational. The affec­
There is also some evidence that training tive-relational dimension appears to be
in nonverbal communication skills aids suc­ particularly salient to patients. At the point
cess in the American Board of Surgery’s when physical medical problems drive
oral-certification exam, which is designed patients to seek professional medical help,
primarily to test candidates’ medical-technical such problems may create uncertainty, anx­
skills or their “clinical reasoning, problem iety, and feelings of fear, frustration, and
solving ability, and clinical judgment” vulnerability in patients, who (1) must dis­
(Rowland-Morin, Burchard, Garb, & Coe, close private (and sometimes socially deli­
1991, p. 655). cate, embarrassing, illegal, etc.) information
This chapter begins by providing a ratio­ (e.g., sexual history, drug use) to physicians
nale for studying nonverbal communication who are relative strangers and (2) are
between medical physicians and their largely dependent on physicians, who are
patients,1 reviews findings related to individ­ relatively knowledgeable experts and legiti­
ual nonverbal behaviors or variables, and mate brokers of treatment (Mishel, 1988).
discusses new directions for nonverbal Patients can discriminate between
research. The premise of the final section is medical-technical and affective-relational
that the social meaning of individual non­ dimensions of physicians’ communication
verbal behaviors—and thus their produc­ (Bensing & Dronkers, 1992) and, within the
tion, understanding, and effects—is shaped latter set of messages, they are able to dis­
fundamentally and irremediably by, and tinguish between “positive” and “negative”
must be studied relative to, their situation nonverbal affective-relational styles, such
within a variety of aspects of interactional as those communicating “attention or con­
context. This context includes other modal­ cern” rather than “inattention or distance”
ities of communication (e.g., other nonver­ (Aruguete & Roberts, 2002). Whereas
bal and verbal behaviors), as well as norms patients base their evaluations of physicians’
and rules that structure the interaction itself, communicative competence on both dimen­
such as those dealing with turn taking, sions (Cegala, McNeilis, McGee, & Jonas,
social action, and sequences of action. 1995), and although patients’ evaluations of
these dimensions are positively correlated
♦ Rationale for Studying such that an increase in one tends to result
in an increase in the other (Ben-Sira, 1982;
Nonverbal Communication
Street & Buller, 1987), there is an accumu­
lation of evidence suggesting that patients’
Patients do not abide strictly by a rational- evaluations of the quality of physicians
consumer model of medicine. That is, they and their medical care are influenced
seldom select and retain physicians, nor more heavily by the affective-relational
do they evaluate physicians and their med­ (vs. medical-technical) dimension of physi­
ical care or competence, based solely on cians’ communication (Ben-Sira, 1982;
physicians’ medical-technical skills and Griffith, Wilson, Langer, & Haist, 2003;
patients’ health outcomes (Glassman & Mechanic & Meyer, 2000).
Glassman, 1981). Akin to organizational This evidence leads to the conclusion
communication generally (Farace, Monge, that successful medical treatment involves
& Russell, 1977), physicians’ and patients’ physicians’ competent management of the
23-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 7:34 PM Page 439

Nonverbal Communication and Physician-Patient Interaction–––◆–––439

affective-relational dimension of commu­ speaking (Goodwin, 1981; Sacks, Schegloff,


nicative action. For example, practices of & Jefferson, 1974). Research on gaze in
interaction that address the affective- physician-patient interaction has focused on
relational dimension “positively” (e.g., its effects on both verbal communication and
reassurance) have been associated with visit or medical outcomes. Concerning ver­
decreases in patients’ requests for post­ bal communication, Verhaak (1988) and
operative narcotics (Egbert, Battit, Welch, Bensing, Kerrsens, and van der Pasch (1995)
& Bartlett, 1964; Langer, Janis, & Wolfer, examined physicians’ gaze toward patients,
1975) and increases in patients’ levels of and Van Dulmen, Verhaak, and Bilo (1997)
physical functioning, such as their levels of examined the amount of time physicians
blood glucose and diastolic blood pres­ gazed directly at patients’ faces, and these
sure (Kaplan, Greenfield, & Ware, 1989). studies showed that physicians’ gaze orienta­
Although the affective-relational dimension tion was associated positively with the
of communication (including empathy and amount of psychosocial (rather than
rapport) is managed partially through verbal somatic) information given by patients.
behavior, such management involves nonver­ Along similar lines, Duggan and Parrott
bal behavior primarily (Ekman & Friesen, (2001) found physicians’ lack of direct facial
1969; Harrigan & Rosenthal, 1986). orientation toward patients to be negatively
The next section reviews associations associated with patients’ self-disclosure (e.g.,
between a variety of communicative, social, about life beyond symptoms). Some results
and psychological outcomes and the follow­ are non-intuitive and beg further investiga­
ing nonverbal behaviors: gaze orientation, tion. For example, Van Dulmen et al.
head nodding, and body orientation (includ­ (1997), found the amount of time that physi­
ing proximity). Space prevents an exhaustive cians gazed directly at patients’ faces
review of all nonverbal variables, includ­ was negatively associated with the amount
ing smiling, touch, tone of voice, physical of agreements given by patients and the
appearance, and skill at encoding and decod­ amount of reassurance, orientation, and
ing emotion (for more on the latter, see medical counseling given by physicians.
Riggio, this volume). Given the concern in A somewhat inconsistent picture
this section of the Handbook with the impli­ emerges when the research focus concerns
cations of our work, the variables reviewed visits or medical outcomes. For instance,
here are relatively more “controllable” by, Larsen and Smith (1981) discerned that
and thus “teachable” to, physicians. physicians’ direct facial orientation toward
patients was negatively associated with
patients’ post-visit satisfaction with medical
care. Relatedly, Harrigan, Oxman, and
♦ Findings Related to Individual
Rosenthal (1985) found physicians’ increased
Nonverbal Behaviors and decreased mutual gaze with patients to
be negatively and positively associated with
PHYSICIANS’ GAZE ORIENTATION external raters’ evaluations of physicians’
rapport, respectively.2 These research stud­
Gaze orientation communicates one’s ies suggest that gaze and face orientation
current attention to, availability for, and par­ toward patients may increase disclosure but
ticipation with others’ actions (or lack decrease patients’ satisfaction. In possible
thereof; Goodwin, 1981; Kendon, 1990) contradiction to these findings, however,
as well as who one is addressing when Smith, Polis, and Hadac (1981) showed
23-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 7:34 PM Page 440

440–––◆–––Contexts and Consequences

that physicians’ time spent reading patients’ delivering bad medical news). The analytic
medical records, which was also physicians’ “payoff” of looking at the larger interac­
time spent gazing away from patients, was tion, rather than discrete variables, will be
negatively associated with patients’ post- discussed later in this chapter.
visit satisfaction and understanding of
medical information. Furthermore, Bensing
(1991) showed that physicians’ gaze toward PHYSICIANS’ HEAD NODDING
patients was positively associated with
external physician-raters’ evaluations of the When people gaze at speakers, especially
quality of participant-physicians’ psychoso­ when speakers are producing multi-unit
cial care. turns (e.g., when patients produce illness
One explanation of this possible con­ narratives or when physicians explain treat­
tradiction lies in an analysis of where physi­ ments), gazers nod their head frequently,
cians are gazing—and what physicians are which, at a minimum, communicates atten­
doing—while patients are talking. For tion (Schegloff, 1982). Hall, Irish, Roter,
example, Harrigan et al. (1985) also found Ehrlich, and Miller (1994) found that, com­
that, compared with low-rapport physicians, pared with male physicians, female physi­
high-rapport physicians gazed at (i.e., read) cians nod more overall and they nod more to
patients’ medical records more often when female patients. Harrigan and Rosenthal
not gazing at patients, but they were more (1983) discovered external raters’ evalua­
likely to continue to gaze at patients when tions of physicians’ nodding to be positively
patients were talking. In support of this, Giron, associated with raters’ perceptions of physi­
Manjon-Arce, Puerto-Barber, Sanchez-Garcia, cians’ rapport. In a later study, however,
and Gomez-Beneyto (1998) revealed that Harrigan et al. (1985) found no association
physicians’ eye contact while patients spoke between nodding and rapport. Nodding is,
was positively associated with physicians’ however, more commonly studied in associ­
psychodiagnostic abilities. ation with other variables than as an isolated
In sum, physicians’ gaze toward (rather cue, and the findings when nodding is viewed
than away from) patients appears to be as part of a larger communicative function are
positively associated with patients’ giving more robust. For example, Weinberger,
of psychosocial information, which may Greene, and Mamlin (1981) found physicians’
explain the concomitant positive associa­ nonverbal encouragement—operationalized
tions with physicians’ psychodiagnostic in terms of nodding and gesture—was posi­
abilities and patients’ positive evaluations tively associated with patients’ post-visit sat­
of at least the affective-relational dimension isfaction. Duggan and Parrott (2001)
of physicians’ communication. These find­ showed likewise that physicians’ facial rein­
ings are confounded by a lack of control forcement—operationalized in terms of nod­
for what physicians and patients are doing ding and facial animation—was positively
while gazing, however, as well as where associated with patients’ self-disclosure (e.g.,
patients are gazing (e.g., toward or away about life beyond symptoms).
from physicians). A particularly salient
issue seems to be whether physicians and
patients are talking generally and, specifi­ PHYSICIANS’ BODY ORIENTATION
cally, what social actions are getting accom­
plished through such talk (e.g., instructing Although head movement and gaze
patients to sit on the examination table vs. orientation communicate persons’ current
23-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 7:34 PM Page 441

Nonverbal Communication and Physician-Patient Interaction–––◆–––441

engagement, the front of a person’s body body orientation and patients’ satisfaction
communicates a frame of dominant orienta­ with physicians. As with gaze, however,
tion: a frame of space wherein long-term and these findings may be confounded by a lack
dominant social actions are most likely of control for what physicians and patients
to be focused (Goodwin, 1981; Kendon, are doing.
1990; Schegloff, 1998). The orientation of
persons’ bodies communicates their availabil­
ity or nonavailability for collaborative action. SUMMARY
When two persons bring each other into (or
remove the other from) their frame of domi­ A variety of individual nonverbal behav­
nant orientation, they establish (or dismantle) iors have been associated with communica­
a participation framework (Goodwin, 1981). tive, social, and psychological outcomes. The
This orientation appears to have a majority of these outcomes relate to the affec­
number of outcomes. Larsen and Smith tive-relational (vs. medical-technical) dimen­
(1981) found that the amount of time sion of communication, such as ratings of
physicians spend with their bodies oriented physicians’ dominance, rapport, and likeabil­
toward patients was positively associated ity, and to psychosocial (vs. somatic) aspects
with patients’ post-visit satisfaction and of care, such as patients’ self-disclosure of
understanding. Street and Buller (1987) lifeworld events and physicians’ psychodiag­
showed that physicians’ indirect body ori­ nostic abilities. Together, they suggest the
entation (i.e., away from patients) was pos­ important role that nonverbal cues may play
itively associated with patients’ perceptions in physician-patient interactions.
of physicians’ dominance. Harrigan et al. Despite the overall strength of their con­
(1985) reported that physicians’ body ori­ clusions, the findings just reviewed can be
entation away from patients was negatively extended in a number of ways that highlight
associated with external raters’ evaluations new directions for research on nonverbal
of physicians’ rapport. In a different vein, communication. Put generally, to under­
Giron et al. (1998) found physicians’ open stand the process more completely—that is,
face-to-face posture while patients spoke how these behaviors come to have the con­
to be positively associated with physicians’ sequences that they do—research needs to
psychodiagnostic abilities. A range of stud­ be situated within a larger framework
ies has also found positive relationships for understanding communication per se.
between physicians’ proximity to and lean Whereas communication simultaneously
toward patients and outcomes such as involves multiple, mutually influential
patients’ post-visit understanding (Larsen & modalities of meaning (nonverbal, verbal,
Smith, 1981; Smith, Polis, & Hadac, 1981) artifactual) and is interactive inherently,
and external raters’ evaluations of physi­ much of the research done in the medical
cians’ rapport (Harrigan & Rosenthal, 1983; context focuses in isolation on one modal­
Harrigan et al., 1985). ity of behavior (i.e., nonverbal) produced
Overall, this work shows that physicians’ by one participant (i.e., the physician). To
body orientation toward (and physical make its largest contribution—to help
proximity to) patients appears to be posi­ understand how communication in the
tively and negatively associated with ratings medical context comes to work as it does—
of physicians’ rapport and dominance, research on physician-patient communica­
respectively, and this may partially explain tion needs to be situated within (i.e., needs
the positive association between physicians’ to control for) aspects of interactional
23-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 7:34 PM Page 442

442–––◆–––Contexts and Consequences

context that have been shown to shape how conceptualizing the relationship between
nonverbal behavior is produced and under­ verbal and nonverbal behavior that focus
stood. The following section suggests how on their co-occurrence in social meaning.
this may be done. The first conceptualization is that verbal
behavior and nonverbal behavior consti­
tute two distinct channels of communica­
♦ New Directions for tion that are attended to and processed
Research Dealing With separately by receivers (e.g., Ekman &
Interactional Context Friesen, 1969). Researchers adopt this posi­
tion tacitly whenever they examine phe­
nomena whose functions entail both verbal
Street (2003) proposed an ecological model
and nonverbal communication (e.g., domi­
of communication that recognizes that “visit
nance) yet analyze such phenomena exclu­
communication” and its outcomes are orga­
sively in terms of one channel, or modality,
nized by reference to organizational, politi­
of meaning.
cal, media, cultural, and interpersonal
The second, alternative, conceptualiza­
contexts. This section of the chapter extends
tion (which this chapter adopts) is that the
Street’s model generally, and his interper­
meaning of communicative events is shaped
sonal context specifically (which includes
by, and thus depends on, the “context” in
verbal and nonverbal communication), by
which it is situated and that verbal and non­
recognizing the organizing effects of interac­
verbal behavior are each forms of context
tional context. That is, in addition to tradi­
(Goodwin, 1995; Sanders, 1987). From this
tional conceptions of context, interaction has
perspective, the relationship between verbal
its own, independent orders of social organi­
and nonverbal behavior is neither additive
zation (Goffman, 1983) that can affect both
nor multiplicative, in the sense that each
the production and the understanding of
constitutes a separate yet combinable factor
nonverbal communication. Over the past 30
of meaning. Rather, their relationship is
years, three of the most robust “interaction
holistic and metamorphic, with a multitude
orders” involve turn taking, social action,
of modalities (e.g., verbal, nonverbal, arti­
and sequences of talk and action. Before
factual) working together to convey a single
addressing these issues, however, this section
meaning (for more on this, see McNeill,
begins with a discussion of the inseparability
Cassell, & McCullough, 1994).
of nonverbal and verbal behavior.
This second conceptualization shifts
analytic attention away from the function
THE INSEPARABILITY of individual nonverbal behaviors to how
OF NONVERBAL AND they achieve their social meanings in and
VERBAL BEHAVIOR through interaction—that is, to the multi-
modal array of communication practices
Almost 15 years ago, Streeck and Knapp that participants rely on to accomplish cer­
(1992) asserted that “the classification of tain meanings (Sanders, 1987). This con­
communicative behavior as either ‘verbal’ ceptualization is in line with Burgoon’s
or ‘nonverbal’ is misleading and obsolete” (1994) message perspective and Stamp and
(p. 3). Although this position is not new, Knapp’s (1990) interaction perspective on
and has continued to be a mantra of the nature of nonverbal communication,
research reform (see Bavelas & Chovil, this and Robinson and Stivers (2001) supported
volume), its implications often go ignored. the validity of a multimodal perspective in
There are at least two different ways of physician-patient interaction specifically.
23-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 7:34 PM Page 443

Nonverbal Communication and Physician-Patient Interaction–––◆–––443

From this multimodal perspective, the first operationalizing talk is unitization. In stud­
conceptualization (stated above) is statisti­ ies of physician-patient communication,
cally reified (rather than supported) by nonverbal behavior has been unitized his­
physician-patient studies showing that, torically in terms of its duration and fre­
when controlling for verbal variables, non­ quency (e.g., in seconds) across (sometimes
verbal variables retain independent signifi­ randomly selected) segments of (or entire)
cance (e.g., Bensing, 1991; Griffith, Wilson, visits. Contrary to this, physicians and
Langer, & Haist, 2003). patients organize much of their nonverbal
behavior relative to talk, and organize their
talk according to turn-taking rules for
THREE ASPECTS OF
ordinary conversation (Sacks, Schegloff, &
INTERACTIONAL CONTEXT
Jefferson, 1974; for a review, see Robinson,
2001a).3
Taking into account the multimodal con­
One example occurs with gaze orienta­
ceptualization, and because in face-to-face
tion. Because gaze can communicate that
interaction, nonverbal behavior is produced
one is “listening,” interactants orient to the
and understood largely by reference to talk,
general rule that recipients (e.g., physicians)
researchers studying nonverbal communi­
should gaze at speakers (e.g., patients)
cation need to account for the organizing
when being spoken to (Goodwin, 1981).
effects of at least three aspects of interac­
Turns of talk have consequential “posi­
tional context: (1) turn taking, (2) social
tions,” such as beginnings, middles, and
action, and (3) sequences of talk and action.
endings (Schegloff, 1996), and Goodwin
Furthermore, researchers need to account
(1981) showed that recipients’ gaze toward
for the fact that (4) individual nonverbal
speakers is particularly salient at turn
behaviors are (almost always) produced and
beginnings. For instance, in Extract 1, when
understood by reference to each other.
the physician begins to ask his question
These four issues are discussed in order.
(Line 1), he is gazing at the computer screen
(see Figure 23.1, which corresponds posi­
Turn Taking
tionally to the “1” in the transcript).
Buller and Street (1992) noted that tra­ Precisely at the completion of his ques­
ditional measures of nonverbal behavior tion—that is, just as he is about to become
“do not account for how communicators a recipient of the patient’s talk—the physi­
qualitatively interpret the behaviors being cian shifts his gaze to the patient (Figure
quantified” (p. 135, emphasis in original). 23.2; commas “,” symbolize movement of
The underlying issue in their statement is the physician’s head, the “X” symbolizes
whether operationalizations of nonverbal the point at which the physician’s gaze
behavior are ecologically valid (i.e., relevant reaches the patient, and brackets “[ ]” sym­
to participants). An integral component of bolize simultaneous behavior).

Extract 1 [MC:12:03]

1 2

01 DOC: What’s wrong=with your ea:r[s. (.)]

02 DOC: [,,,,,X]

03 PAT: I think they’re both infected.


23-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 7:34 PM Page 444

444–––◆–––Contexts and Consequences

Figure 23.1

Figure 23.2
23-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 7:34 PM Page 445

Nonverbal Communication and Physician-Patient Interaction–––◆–––445

Figure 23.3

Due to the rules of gaze orientation in her answer with “Uh:m.” In this case, the
ordinary conversation (Goodwin, 1981), if patient does not succeed in securing the
speakers (e.g., patients) do not secure recip­ physician’s gaze, and she continues to pro­
ients’ (e.g., physicians’) gaze at the begin­ duce a self-diagnosis: “I believe I have a
ning of their turns, speakers use vocal sinus infection.” (Line 2).4 Research shows,
hitches and perturbations—such as pauses, however, that although recipients’ gaze is
cutoffs, and other marked prosodic pat­ particularly salient at the beginning of
terns—to secure recipients’ gaze prior to speakers’ turns, it is relevant throughout
beginning or completing their turns. This is speakers’ turns, and speakers continue fre­
equally true in physician-patient interaction quently to work to secure recipients’ gaze
(Heath, 1986; Ruusuvuori, 2001). In (e.g., Goodwin, 1981; Heath, 1986). When
Extract 1, the patient does secure the the patient in Extract 2 continues to produce
physician’s gaze prior to beginning her more talk (Line 3), she again attempts to
turn, and she produces a fluent response secure the physician’s gaze by cutting herself
(Line 3). This, however, is not the case in off (symbolized by the hyphen, “a-”) and
Extract 2. When the physician completes pausing for three tenths of a second (each
his question “What’s up.” (Line 1), he is tenth is symbolized by a dash, “(—)”). This
gazing down at the computer keyboard time, she succeeds—that is, immediately
(Figure 23.3). after her cutoff and two tenths of a second
At the outset of her response (Line 2), the of silence, the physician shifts his gaze
patient attempts to secure the physician’s toward the patient (Figure 23.4). The
gaze by pausing briefly (i.e., breathing in, patient begins to speak again precisely as the
symbolized by “.h”) and further delaying physician’s gaze arrives.
23-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 7:34 PM Page 446

446–––◆–––Contexts and Consequences

Extract 2 [MC:20:02]

01 DOC: What's up.

02 PAT: .h Uh:m I believe I have a sinus infection.

03 I’ve had=a- (— – [-) c]o:ld yyou know [stuf]fy nose


fer about a week

04 DOC: [,,,X] [,,,O]

4 5

05 PAT: an’ then Saturday (0.2) it’s: kinda stayed

06 ri:ght, (- [—)] in thee e:ye e:a[r ]

07 DOC: [,,X] [Y:]eah.

Recipients’ gaze is relevant not merely turn-constructional units (Sacks et al.,


once (i.e., fleetingly) but throughout speak­ 1974). In Extract 2, it can be argued that
ers’ turns (Goodwin, 1981), and speakers the patient’s turn includes three turn-
may work to recover recipients’ lost gaze. constructional units (“I believe I have a
For example, in Extract 2, as the patient sinus infection.”, “I’ve had a cold you
describes her “stuffy nose” (Line 3), the know stuffy nose fer about a week”, and
physician removes his gaze back to the “an’ then Saturday it’s kinda stayed right
computer screen (Figure 23.5). The patient, in the eye ear”) and that the patient orients
however, is not finished speaking. Instead, to the relevance of the physician’s gaze in
as the patient describes (and gestures with (and before the completion of) each unit.
her right hand toward) the location of In sum, gaze orientation is organized
her problem (Lines 5–6), she markedly largely by reference to turn construction.
stretches and inflects “ri:ght,” (symbolized Not only have there been specific calls for
by the colon and the comma, respectively) understanding physicians’ and patients’
and pauses for three tenths of a second, gaze orientation in terms of turn taking
which succeeds in resecuring the physician’s (Irish, 1997), but there is preliminary evi­
gaze (Figure 23.6). dence that doing so can produce analytic
Extract 2 raises an additional issue deal­ payoff. For example, Harrigan et al. (1985)
ing with unitization. Physician-patient talk found that high (vs. low) rapport physicians
has been coded historically in terms of are more likely to gaze at patients when
vaguely operationalized “thought units” or patients are speaking. Giron et al. (1998)
“utterances.” However, turns (1) are con­ measured both physicians’ gaze and body
structed from particular types of turn- orientation toward patients separately
constructional units, (2) can contain more depending on whether physicians or
than one turn-constructional unit, and (3) patients were speaking and found these
are produced and understood in terms of variables to be positively associated with
23-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 7:34 PM Page 447

Nonverbal Communication and Physician-Patient Interaction–––◆–––447

Figure 23.4

Figure 23.5
23-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 7:34 PM Page 448

448–––◆–––Contexts and Consequences

Figure 23.6

Figure 23.7
23-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 7:34 PM Page 449

Nonverbal Communication and Physician-Patient Interaction–––◆–––449

physicians’ psychodiagnostic abilities only across Extract 3, the physician maintains his
when patients were speaking. gaze and body orientation toward the patient
Other nonverbal behaviors, such as head (Figure 23.7). In the transcript, “/” and “\”
nodding, also appear to be organized by ref­ symbolize the upward and downward nod­
erence to turn construction. For example, ding of the physician’s head, respectively.

Extract 3 [MC:11:02]

01 DOC: When=did that sta:rt. (.) la- °°e-=uh-°° beginning

02 a last week? .hhhhhhhh thuh co[:ld? ]

03 PAT: [Y:eah] might even

04 sta:rted duh week be[fore a little bit. an’ the:n] (.)

05 DOC: [ /\/ ]

06 PAT: la:st week w’s- thuh sore throat [ca:me an’- (.)] no:w

07 DOC: [ /\ ]

08 thuh si:nus. [(--)]

09 DOC: [ \/ ]

10 DOC: °Okay.°

The physician’s three instances of If nodding is organized relative to turn-


nodding (Lines 5, 7, and 9) map finely onto constructional units, however, then it may
places where the patient’s turn-constructional have different meanings depending on its
units (in this case, sentences) are possibly (or position within turn-constructional units,
projectably) complete. The first nod begins as such as being positioned before or after they
the patient is saying “before” (Line 4), which are possibly complete. Importantly, the rela­
is a possible sentence ending (i.e., “might even tionship between nonverbal behavior and
started duh week before”); the second nod turn taking does not stop at gaze orientation
begins after “throat” (Line 6), which is a pos­ and head nodding. For example, Harrigan
sible sentence ending (i.e., “An’ then last week (1985) found that physicians and patients
w’s thuh sore throat”); and the third nod self-touched more at the beginning and mid­
occurs in the two tenths of a second of silence dle (vs. the end) of utterances. In line with
after “si:nus.” (Line 8), which is a possible evidence from ordinary conversation (e.g.,
sentence ending (i.e., “An’ now thuh sinus.”). Streeck, 1993), a variety of physicians’ and
Like gaze, nodding has been coded patients’ nonverbal behaviors are organized
historically in terms of gross frequency. by reference to turn construction, such as
23-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 7:34 PM Page 450

450–––◆–––Contexts and Consequences

touching, gesturing, and smiling (Beach & sensitive or embarrassing events (Beach &
LeBaron, 2002; Haakana, 2002). LeBaron, 2002; Heath, 1986, 1988), or
when they receive bad medical news (e.g.,
Social Action cancer diagnoses; Maynard, 2003), they
sometimes look away from their interlocu­
Once nonverbal and verbal communica­ tors or cover their faces. One systematic
tion are reunited, researchers must recognize finding, then, is that while physicians
that (1) persons produce and understand engage in physical examination, patients
all communication primarily in terms of the tend to adopt a middle distance gaze orien­
action(s) it performs (e.g., explaining, advis­ tation “away from the doctor yet at no
ing, informing; Schegloff, 1995); (2) actions particular object within the local environ­
are organized by social rules that transcend ment” (Heath, 1988, p. 149).
individual actors (Heritage, 1984); and (3) The possibility that the social organiza­
different social rules—for example, the rules tion of different verbal actions structures
for (or practices of) providing good (vs. bad) nonverbal behavior differently has direct
diagnostic news (e.g., Maynard, 2003)— implications for the types of outcomes dis­
shape at least verbal behavior differentially cussed earlier. For example, it has already
(Heritage, 1984). been documented that physicians’ nonverbal
Although the claim that the social behavior affects raters’ evaluations of the
organization of action structures nonverbal affective-relational dimension of communi­
behavior is in need of further support, it is cation. Such evaluations are, however, also
buttressed by a variety of findings. Harrigan affected by physicians’ verbal behaviors,
(1985) found that patients self-touched more which shape outcomes. For example, when
when answering questions than when being physicians are more verbally empathetic
asked questions and concluded that “the (e.g., provide more reassurance), patients are
semantic context of an utterance may be more satisfied and adherent to medical rec­
expected to exert the strongest influence on ommendations and less willing to sue for
the expression of self touching” (p. 1164), malpractice (for a review, see Frankel,
which “is more complex than a simple cue 1995). When providers are less verbally
of anxiety” (p. 1167). Certain actions, such domineering or controlling (e.g., less direc­
as apologizing for causing emotional pain tive), patients are more assertive, expressive,
(Beach & LeBaron, 2002), may make physi­ and disclose more information (Street,
cians’ touching of patients more normative 1992b); are more satisfied (Street, 1992a);
relative to other actions and thus affect and experience better physical-health out­
patients’ evaluations of physicians’ touch. comes (e.g., metabolic control; Street et al.,
In line with Goffman’s (1963) notion of 1993). Researchers need to determine if
civil inattention, while patients engage in a these types of verbal behaviors structure
variety of private actions such as undress­ nonverbal behavior in particular ways, and
ing (Heath, 1986), it may be more norma­ they need to analyze nonverbal behaviors in
tive for physicians to avert their gaze, even conjunction with them.
if engaged in conversation. The action of If researchers are going to link nonverbal
“remembering” publicly is associated com­ behavior to talk in interaction, in this case
monly with gazing away from interlocutors physical-patient interaction, and if different
(Beach & LeBaron, 2002; Goodwin, social organizations of verbal action struc­
1987). When patients discuss personally ture nonverbal behavior differentially, then,
23-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 7:34 PM Page 451

Nonverbal Communication and Physician-Patient Interaction–––◆–––451

at the very least, researchers need to refine First, first-pair parts affect second-pair
extant coding schemata to better control parts by normatively obligating, and
for social action. The bulk of most coding constraining what counts as, relevant
schemata categorize talk not in terms of responses. For example, “Yes”/“No”-for­
social action but rather in terms of a com­ matted questions constrain initial responses
bination of grammatical form and topical to versions of “Yes” or “No” (Raymond,
content. These limitations have been 2003), and such constraints can have impli­
addressed partially by narrowing analyses cations for patient participation (Heritage
to, and developing new coding schemata & Robinson, 2006; Robinson, 2001a).
around, particular (classes of) social actions, Second, first-pair parts establish frame­
such as patients’ requests for medical works for understanding second-pair parts,
services (Kravitz, Bell, & Franz, 1999).5 and thus the meaning of communicative
behavior is heavily influenced by its sequen­
tial positioning (Schegloff & Sacks, 1973).
Sequencing
For example, in Extract 3, the physicians’
One of the most fundamental interac­ first two head nods (Lines 5 and 7) are pro­
tional contexts organizing social action is the duced after the patient’s response and are
adjacency-pair sequence (Schegloff & Sacks understood as acknowledging the patient’s
1973). In its basic form, the adjacency-pair talk and as encouragement to continue
sequence is composed of two turns of talk: a (Schegloff, 1982). Evidence for this claim is
first-pair part, produced by one speaker, that in each case, the patient continues to
which initiates a course of action, and a sec­ produce a new turn-constructional unit. In
ond-pair part, produced by a different contrast, in Extract 4, the physician’s head
speaker, which responds to the initiated nod (Line 4) is produced as a response to the
action. Space limitations prohibit full expli­ patient’s request for confirmation (Line 1);
cation of the adjacency-pair sequence, and because of its sequential positioning, it gets
only two points are made here. understood differently as a confirmation.

Extract 4 [MC:14:02]

01 PAT: That takes effect right away? ((gazing at doctor))

02 (1.2)

03 PAT: Thuh [flu shot.]

04 DOC: [ /\[/

05 DOC: [°Mm hm,°

06 PAT: ((Shifts gaze from doctor to computer))

Research attempting to discover vari- not accounting for “the two-way and con­
ables that affect physicians’ and patients’ tingent (i.e., sequential) nature of physician-
communication has been criticized for (1) patient interaction” (Hall, Harrigan, &
23-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 7:34 PM Page 452

452–––◆–––Contexts and Consequences

Rosenthal, 1995, p. 25) and (2) implying physician’s gaze shift to a female patient’s
claims about causation while using the cor­ chest can lead directly to the patient gazing
relation statistic (for other critiques, see at her own chest. Similarly, Heath (1986)
Buller & Street, 1992; Street & Buller, showed that physicians’ gaze shifts to
1987). Because correlations do not reveal objects of discussion, such as X rays, can
the direction of causality, because much of lead directly to patients’ gazing at the same
social action is organized by the adjacency- objects. In the same way that patients can
pair sequence, and because first-pair parts use vocal hitches and perturbations to solicit
affect second-pair parts, if sequencing is not physicians’ gaze (see above), patients can
accounted for, then significant correlations use nonverbal behaviors—such as gaze
are constantly in danger of being sequen­ shifts toward physicians, hand gestures,
tially spurious. torso shifts, and leg movements—to solicit
It also appears that nonverbal behavior both physicians’ gaze and talk (Heath,
can be sequenced independent of verbal 1986). For example, patients sometimes
adjacency-pair sequences (although nonver­ seek physicians’ gaze nonvocally for pur­
bal sequences are still often organized by poses of exhibiting embodied characteristics
reference to talk). That is, there is prelimi­ of their suffering (Beach & LeBaron, 2002;
nary evidence that physicians’ and patients’ Heath, 1986, 2002). In Figure 23.6, in addi­
nonverbal behaviors are nonrandom, pat­ tion to using vocal hitches and perturbations
terned, and synchronized. For instance, to solicit the physician’s gaze, the patient
Street and Buller (1987) provided evidence additionally gestures with her right hand to
that physicians and patients “matched” locate the position of her symptoms (i.e.,
gaze orientation, body orientation, and her “e:ye e:ar”). Although more research is
illustrative gestures, and Street and Buller necessary, Heath (1986) noted that some
(1988) demonstrated physician-patient rec­ of these nonverbal sequences operate in a
iprocation or convergence regarding body fashion similar to verbal summons-answer
orientation. Koss and Rosenthal (1997) sequences (Schegloff, 1968).
found external raters’ evaluations of physi­
cian-patient nonverbal synchrony to be
The Inseparability of Nonverbal
positively associated with raters’ evalua­
Behaviors From Themselves
tions of physician-patient rapport. Several
reviews (Kiesler & Auerbach, 2003; Lepper, In addition to turn taking, social action,
Martin, & DiMatteo, 1995) have suggested and sequences, nonverbal behavior embod­
that the presence or absence of physicians’ ies its own interactional context. That is,
and patients’ nonverbal synchrony (or the social meaning of individual nonverbal
exchange) plays a role in a variety of par­ behaviors can be altered when they are
ticipants’ affective-relational attributions, employed simultaneously. For example,
such as affiliation and dominance, respec­ Harrigan and Rosenthal (1983) found
tively (for more on synchrony, see Tickle- external raters’ evaluations of physicians’
Degnen, this volume). rapport to be associated with interactions
The above correlational research is sup­ between physicians’ torso position (i.e.,
ported by focused studies of interaction. For forward or backward lean), head nodding,
example, in line with the observation that and leg position (i.e., crossed or uncrossed).
gaze communicates persons’ current focus Based on these types of findings, Harrigan
of attention, Heath (1988) showed that a and Rosenthal (1986) later asserted,
23-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 7:34 PM Page 453

Nonverbal Communication and Physician-Patient Interaction–––◆–––453

Nonverbal units of behavior are diffi­ though gaze orientation communicates


cult, if not impossible, to study in total persons’ current foci of attention, relative to
isolation from one another. While the upper-body segments (e.g., the head), lower-
head is nodding, the trunk may be body segments (e.g., the legs) more strongly
angled forward or back, the limbs may communicate persons’ frames of dominant
be still or moving, the face expression­ orientation (Kendon, 1990).
less or animated, and the gaze steady, For example, in Figure 23.5, the physi­
averted, or darting. Each unit of nonver­ cian’s head, torso, and legs are in alignment
bal behavior is interrelated in that each is and face the desk or computer; the patient
capable of influencing the evaluation of is gazing at the physician. When physicians
another behavior. (p. 45) arrange their body segments to have diver­
gent orientations—for example, in Figure
Studies have not often tested for interac­ 23.6 (compared with Figure 23.5), when
tion effects between individual nonverbal the physician keeps his torso and legs ori­
behaviors. In fact, such interactions are ented toward the desk or computer yet
obscured when individual nonverbal behav­ rotates his head 30° to the right of his body
iors are collapsed into larger-order vari­ to gaze at the patient—they may communi­
ables, such as immediacy, which has been cate (1) postural instability (e.g., of their
operationalized in terms of decreased phys­ head relative to their body); (2) potential
ical proximity and increased touch, for­ resolutions to such instability by reference
ward lean, gaze, and body orientation to the more stable segments of the body
(Larsen & Smith, 1981; see also the section (e.g., returning their head into alignment
on head nodding). This is not to suggest with their body); (3) an orientation to mul­
that the aggregation of individual nonver­ tiple courses of action (e.g., one by their
bal behaviors is completely unprincipled. head, such as engaging the patient to ask
For instance, researchers often evaluate them a question, and another by their body,
their coherence with statistical techniques, such as documenting the patient’s answer in
such as factor analysis. Aggregation is less the medical records); and (4) a ranking of
principled, however, when it is motivated these multiple courses of action in terms of
by professional demands involving accept­ level of orientation (e.g., the action of doc­
able levels of interrater reliability or signifi­ umenting is more primary and long term
cance, statistical demands involving cell than that of engaging the patient in talk;
size, and so on. Aggregation does, however, Robinson, 1998).
obscure the effects of individual nonverbal The interrelated social organizations of
behaviors, as well as the fact that individual gaze and body orientation can affect raters’
nonverbal behaviors do interact. attributions. For example, in a nonmed­
One of the most well-documented inter­ ical context, Mehrabian (1967) found the
relationships is between gaze and body amount of time senders maintained head
orientation (Mehrabian, 1967). Although orientation toward receivers to be posi­
different segments of the body (e.g., the tively associated with external raters’ evalu­
head, torso, and legs) can be oriented in ations of senders’ positive attitudes toward
different directions (Kendon, 1990), there receivers, but only when senders’ bodies
remains a socially understood body-segment were also oriented toward receivers. In a
hierarchy in terms of persons’ levels of medical context, Ruusuvuori (2001) exam­
attention and engagement. Specifically, even ined patients’ responses to physicians’
23-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 7:34 PM Page 454

454–––◆–––Contexts and Consequences

opening questions (e.g., “What can I do for empathy, rapport, and generally a “positive”
you today?”) and showed that when physi­ affective-relational communication style—
cians removed their gaze from patients has wide-ranging effects (direct and indirect)
prior to patients having completed their on patients’ communicative, social, psycho­
responses, patients tended to produce dis­ logical, and physiological health outcomes.
fluencies to (re)solicit physicians’ gaze. Despite these empirical strides, research on
Ruusuvuori found that patients produced nonverbal communication can be improved.
fewer disfluencies when physicians’ bodies The bulk of prior research has focused
were oriented toward (vs. away) from exclusively on individual nonverbal behav­
patients (e.g., Figure 23.7 compared with iors (e.g., body orientation) or coherent
Figure 23.6). Ruusuvuori’s findings suggest aggregates of nonverbal behaviors (e.g.,
that in terms of physicians’ levels of reinforcement). This chapter argues that the
engagement with patients (i.e., attention social meaning of nonverbal behavior—and
to patient’s responses), patients understand thus its production, understanding, and
the absence or removal of physicians’ gaze effects—is fundamentally and irremediably
differently depending on the orientation of shaped by, and thus must be studied rela­
physicians’ bodies. tive to, its situation within a variety of
The interrelationship between gaze and aspects of interactional context. This is not
body orientation can be complicated by to say that the effects of nonverbal behavior
other nonverbal behaviors. For example, in are relative. Rather, nonverbal behavior is
a nonmedical context, Goodwin (1981) sug­ organized systematically and finely by ref­
gested that recipients’ lack of attention com­ erence to both talk and other nonverbal
municated by shifting their gaze away from behavior.
speakers can be partially offset by recipients This chapter demonstrates that physi­
nodding during and after their gaze cians’ production of individual nonverbal
removal. To further complicate matters, cues (e.g., gaze, body orientation, head
because gaze and body orientation are used nodding), and patients’ understandings of
primarily to communicate (dis)engagement, such cues, is organized by rules associated
their understanding cannot be separated with turn taking, the construction of partic­
from that of the objects being (dis)engaged. ular social actions, the sequencing of
Patients can differentiate between physi­ actions, and the organization of other non­
cians’ gaze at patients’ eyes versus other verbal behaviors (e.g., the interorganization
body parts (e.g., legs, breasts, backs), versus of gaze and body orientation). Not taking
medical records, with different interactional these aspects of interactional context into
and attributional implications (Heath, account obscures our understanding of the
1986, 1988; Robinson, 1998). cause-effect relationships between nonver­
bal (as well as verbal) communication vari­
ables and their outcomes.
♦ Conclusion Relative to research on nonverbal com­
munication between 1965 and 1995,
research over the last 10 years has
This chapter reviews findings related to languished. New directions in research
nonverbal behavior and physician-patient point toward developing new ways of
interaction and shows that physicians’ classifying—that is, conceptualizing and
nonverbal behavior—which is integral to measuring—nonverbal communication. On
the construction and management of the one hand, “without classification, there
23-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 7:34 PM Page 455

Nonverbal Communication and Physician-Patient Interaction–––◆–––455

could be no advanced conceptualization, 2001). As this section goes on to discuss, this


reasoning, language, data analysis or, for extract demonstrates both the interrelated and
that matter, social science research” (Bailey, the negotiated character of verbal and nonverbal
1994, p. 1). Traditional methodologies for communication.
5. The argument concerning social action
coding interaction will always (for a variety
can be extended to social activity. In physician-
of statistical and other reasons) be blunt
patient communication, medical action is fre­
to the ecological validity of interaction
quently produced within specific medical phases
(Mishler, 1984). As such, advances in or activities. For instance, primary-care acute
research will require large-scale partner­ visits have roughly six phases: opening, problem
ships between multiple teams of researchers presentation, information gathering such as his­
representing multiple methodological and tory taking and physical examination, diagnosis,
ontological perspectives. Largely qualita­ treatment, and closing (Robinson, 2003). These
tive research on the interorganizational phases can have their own forms of social orga­
relationships between multiple modes of nization that can shape the production and
communication (e.g., verbal, nonverbal, understanding of their constituent actions, as
artifactual) must proceed both simultane­ does opening (Robinson, 1998), problem-
presentation (Robinson & Heritage, 2005), and
ously and in conjunction with largely quan­
closing (Robinson, 2001b). Along these lines, a
titative, effects-based research, preferably
number of studies of nonverbal communication
without too many analytic compromises on
have controlled for medical phase (e.g., Duggan
either side. & Parrott, 2001; Harrigan, Oxman, &
Rosenthal, 1985; Larsen & Smith, 1981; Street
& Buller, 1987).
♦ Notes

♦ References
1. Because of space limitations, this
chapter excludes research on patient-nurse
and patient-psychotherapist interaction (for Aruguete, M. S., & Roberts, C. A. (2002).
a review, see Caris-Verhallen, Kerkstra, & Participants’ ratings of male physicians
Bensing, 1999, and Hall, Harrigan, & who vary in race and communication style.
Rosenthal, 1995, respectively). Psychological Reports, 91, 793–806.
2. Compared with self-report measures of Bailey, K. D. (1994). Typologies and tax­
emotional expressiveness, those that are based onomies: An introduction to classification
on external raters’ observations of concrete techniques. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
behaviors are more conservative and thus more Beach, W. A., & LeBaron, C. D. (2002). Body
appropriate for isolating nonverbal “predictors” disclosures: Attending to personal problems
of behavior (Riggio & Riggio, 2002). and reported sexual abuse during a medical
3. Overall, researchers seldom measure encounter. Journal of Communication, 52,
physicians’ and patients’ turns. For an excep­ 617–639.
tion, see Street and Buller (1988), who found Bensing, J. M. (1991). Doctor-patient communi­
physicians’ turns to be longer than those of cation and the quality of care. Social
patients and older patients’ turns to be longer Science & Medicine, 32, 1301–1310.
than those of younger patients. Bensing, J. M., & Dronkers, J. (1992).
4. The fact that the patient’s “.h Uh:m” does Instrumental and affective aspects of physi­
not secure the physician’s gaze in this particular cian behavior. Medical Care, 30, 283–298.
instance does not invalidate it as being a member Bensing, J. M., Kerssens, J. J., & van der Pasch,
of a class of practices designed to do so M. (1995). Patient-directed gaze as a tool
(Goodwin, 1981; Heath, 1986; Ruusuvuori, for discovering and handling psychosocial
23-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 7:34 PM Page 456

456–––◆–––Contexts and Consequences

problems in general practice. Journal of Frankel, R. M. (1995). Emotion and the physi­
Nonverbal Behavior, 19, 223–242. cian-patient relationship. Motivation &
Ben-Sira, Z. (1982). Stress potential and eso­ Emotion, 19, 163–173.
tericity of health problems: The signifi­ Giron, M., Manjon-Arce, P., Puerto-Barber, J.,
cance of the physician’s affective behavior. Sanchez-Garcia, E., & Gomez-Beneyto
Medical Care, 20, 414–424. (1998). Clinical interview skills and identi­
Buller, D. B., & Street, R. L., Jr. (1992). fication of emotional disorders in primary
Physician-patient relationships. In R. S. care. American Journal of Psychiatry, 155,
Feldman (Ed.), Applications of nonverbal 530–535.
behavior theories and research (pp. 119– Glassman, M., & Glassman, N. (1981). A mar­
141). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. keting analysis of physician selection and
Burgoon, J. K. (1994). Nonverbal signals. In patient satisfaction. Journal of Health Care
M. L. Knapp & G. R. Miller (Eds.), Marketing, 1, 25–31.
Handbook of interpersonal communication Goffman, E. (1963). Behavior in public places.
(2nd ed., pp. 229–285). Thousand Oaks, New York: Free Press.
CA: Sage. Goffman, E. (1983). The interaction order.
Caris-Verhallen, W. M., Kerkstra, A., & American Sociological Review, 48, 1–17.
Bensing, J. M. (1999). Non-verbal behav­ Goodwin, C. (1981). Conversational organiza­
iour in nurse-elderly patient communica­ tion: Interaction between speakers and
tion. Journal of Advanced Nursing, 29, hearers. New York: Academic Press.
808–818. Goodwin, C. (1987). Forgetfulness as an interac­
Cegala, D. J., McNeilis, K. S., McGee, D. S., tive resource. Social Psychology Quarterly,
& Jonas, A. P. (1995). A study of doctors’ 50, 115–131.
and patients’ perceptions of information Goodwin, C. (1995). Seeing in depth. Social
processing and communication competence Studies of Science, 25, 237–274.
during the medical interview. Health Griffith, C. H., Wilson, J. F., Langer, S., &
Communication, 7, 179–203. Haist, S. A. (2003). House staff nonverbal
Duffy, F. D., Gordon, G. H., Whelan, G., Cole- communication skills and standardized
Kelly, K., & Frankel, R. (2004). Assessing patient satisfaction. Journal of General
competence in communication and inter­ Internal Medicine, 18, 170–174.
personal skills: The Kalamazoo II Report. Haakana, M. (2002). Laughter in medical
Academic Medicine, 79, 495–507. interaction: From quantification to analy­
Duggan, A. P., & Parrott, R. L. (2001). sis, and back. Journal of Sociolinguistics,
Physicians’ nonverbal rapport building and 6, 207–235.
patients’ talk about the subjective compo­ Hall, J. A., Harrigan, J. A., & Rosenthal, R.
nent of illness. Human Communication (1995). Nonverbal behavior in clinician-
Research, 27, 299–311. patient interaction. Applied and Preventive
Egbert, L. D., Battit, G. E., Welch, C. E., & Psychology, 4, 21–37.
Bartlett, M. K. (1964). Reduction of post­ Hall, J. A., Irish, J. T., Roter, D. L., Ehrlich,
operative pain by encouragement and C. M., & Miller, L. H. (1994). Gender in
instruction of patients: A study of doctor- medical encounters: An analysis of physician
patient rapport. New England Journal of and patient communication in a primary care
Medicine, 270, 825. setting. Health Psychology, 13, 384–392.
Ekman, P., & Friesen, W. V. (1969). The reper­ Harrigan, J. A. (1985). Self-touching as an indi­
toire of nonverbal behavior: Categories, ori­ cator of underlying affect and language
gins, usage, and coding. Semiotica, 1, 49–98. processes. Social Science & Medicine, 20,
Farace, R. V., Monge, P. R., & Russell, 1161–1168.
M. (1977). Communicating in organiza­ Harrigan, J. A., Oxman, T. E., & Rosenthal, R.
tions. Reading, MA: Addison-Wesley. (1985). Rapport expressed through nonverbal
23-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 7:34 PM Page 457

Nonverbal Communication and Physician-Patient Interaction–––◆–––457

behavior. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, interaction: The interpersonal circumplex.


9, 95–110. Social Science & Medicine, 57, 1707–1722.
Harrigan, J. A., & Rosenthal, R. (1983). Koss, T., & Rosenthal, R. (1997). Interactional
Physicians’ head and body positions as synchrony, positivity, and patient satisfac­
determinants of perceived rapport. Journal tion in the physician-patient relationship.
of Applied Social Psychology, 13, Medical Care, 35, 1158–1163.
496–509. Kravitz, R. L., Bell, R. A., & Franz, C. E.
Harrigan, J. A., & Rosenthal, R. (1986). (1999). A taxonomy of requests by
Nonverbal aspects of empathy and rapport patients (TORP): A new system for under­
in physician-patient interaction. In P. D. standing clinical negotiation in office
Blanck, R. Buck, & R. Rosenthal (Eds.), practice. Journal of Family Practice, 48,
Nonverbal communication in the clinical 872–878.
context (pp. 36–73). University Park: The Langer, E. J., Janis, I. L., & Wolfer, J. A. (1975).
Pennsylvania State University Press. Reduction of psychological stress in surgi­
Heath, C. (1986). Body movement and speech cal patients. Journal of Experimental Social
in medical interaction. Cambridge, UK: Psychology, 11, 155.
Cambridge University Press. Larsen, K. M., & Smith, C. K. (1981).
Heath, C. (1988). Embarrassment and inter­ Assessment of nonverbal communication
actional organization. In P. Drew & in the patient-physician interview. The
A. Wootton (Eds.), Erving Goffman: Journal of Family Practice, 12, 481–488.
Exploring the interaction order (pp. 136– Lepper, H. S., Martin, L. R., & DiMatteo, M. R.
160). Boston, MA: Northeastern University (1995). A model of nonverbal exchange
Press. in physician-patient expectations for
Heath, C. (2002). Demonstrative suffering: patient involvement. Journal of Nonverbal
The gestural (re)embodiment of symptoms. Behavior, 19, 207–222.
Journal of Communication, 52, 597–616. Maynard, D. W. (2003). Bad news, good news:
Heritage, J. C. (1984). Garfinkel and eth­ Conversational order in everyday talk
nomethodology. New York: Polity Press. and clinical settings. Chicago: University of
Heritage, J., & Robinson, J. D. (2006). The Chicago Press.
structure of patients’ presenting concerns: McNeill, D., Cassell, J., & McCullough, K.
Physicians’ opening questions. Health (1994). Communicative effects of speech-
Communication, 19, 89–102. mismatched gestures. Research on Language
Irish, J. T. (1997). Deciphering the physician- and Social Interaction, 27, 223–237.
older patient interaction. International Mechanic, D., & Meyer, S. (2000). Concepts of
Journal of Psychiatry in Medicine, 27, trust among patients with serious illness.
251–267. Social Science & Medicine, 51, 657–668.
Kaplan, S. H., Greenfield, S., & Ware, J. E. Mehrabian, A. (1967). Orientation behaviors
(1989). Impact of the doctor-patient rela­ and nonverbal attitude communication.
tionship on the outcomes of chronic dis­ Journal of Communication, 17, 324–332.
ease. In M. Stewart & D. Roter (Eds.), Mishel, M. H. (1988). Uncertainty in illness.
Communicating with medical patients Image: Journal of Nursing Scholarship,
(pp. 228–245). Newbury Park, CA: Sage. 20, 225–232.
Kendon, A. (1990). Conducting interaction: Mishler, E. (1984). The discourse of medicine:
Patterns of behavior in focused encounters. Dialectics of medical interviews. Norwood,
Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University NJ: Ablex.
Press. Raymond, G. (2003). Grammar and social orga­
Kiesler, D. J., & Auerbach, S. M. (2003). nization: Yes/no interrogatives and the struc­
Integrating measurement of control and ture of responding. American Sociological
affiliation in studies of physician-patient Review, 68, 939–967.
23-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 7:34 PM Page 458

458–––◆–––Contexts and Consequences

Riggio, H. R., & Riggio, R. E. (2002). Schegloff, E. A. (1982). Discourse as an interac­


Emotional expressiveness, extraversion, tional achievement: Some uses of “uh-huh”
and neuroticism: A meta-analysis. Journal and other things that come between sentences.
of Nonverbal Behavior, 26, 195–218. In D. Tannen (Ed.), Analyzing discourse: Text
Robinson, J. D. (1998). Getting down to business: and talk (pp. 71–93). Washington, DC:
Talk, gaze, and body orientation during Georgetown University Press.
openings of doctor-patient consultations. Schegloff, E. A. (1995). Discourse as an interac­
Human Communication Research, 25, tional achievement III: The omnirelevance
98–124. of action. Research on Language and Social
Robinson, J. D. (2001a). Asymmetry in action: Interaction, 28, 185–211.
Sequential resources in the negotiation of a Schegloff, E. A. (1996). Turn organization: One
prescription request. Text, 21, 19–54. intersection of grammar and interaction. In
Robinson, J. D. (2001b). Closing medical E. Ochs, E. Schegloff, & S. Thompson (Eds.),
encounters: Two physician practices and Interaction and grammar (pp. 52–133).
their implications for the expression of Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.
patients’ unstated concerns. Social Science Schegloff, E. A. (1998). Body torque. Social
& Medicine, 53, 639–656. Research, 65, 535–596.
Robinson, J. D. (2003). An interactional struc­ Schegloff, E. A., & Sacks, H. (1973). Opening
ture of medical activities during acute visits up closings. Semiotica, 7, 289–327.
and its implications for patients’ participa­ Smith, C. K., Polis, E., & Hadac, R. R. (1981).
tion. Health Communication, 15, 27–59. Characteristics of the initial medical inter­
Robinson, J. D., & Heritage, J. (2005). The view associated with patient satisfaction
structure of patients’ presenting concerns: and understanding. Journal of Family
The completion relevance of current symp­ Practice, 12, 283–288.
toms. Social Science and Medicine, 61, Stamp, G. H., & Knapp, M. L. (1990). The
481–493. construct of intent in interpersonal commu­
Robinson, J. D., & Stivers, T. (2001). Achieving nication. Quarterly Journal of Speech, 76,
activity transitions in primary-care consulta­ 282–299.
tions: From history taking to physical exam­ Streeck, J. (1993). Gesture as communication I:
ination. Human Communication Research, Its coordination with gaze and speech.
27, 253–298. Communication Monographs, 60, 275–299.
Rowland-Morin, P. A., Burchard, K. W., Garb, Streeck, J., & Knapp, M. (2002). Culture,
J. L., & Coe, N. P. W. (2002). Influence meaning, and interpersonal communica­
of effective communication by surgery tion. In M. L. Knapp & J. A. Daly (Eds.),
students on their oral examination scores. Handbook of interpersonal communica­
Academic Medicine, 66, 169–171. tion (pp. 286–319). Thousand Oaks, CA:
Ruusuvuori, J. (2001). Looking means listening: Sage.
Coordinating displays of engagement in Street, R. L., Jr. (1992a). Analyzing communica­
doctor-patient interaction. Social Science & tion in medical consultations: Do behav­
Medicine, 52, 1093–1108. ioral measures correspond to patients’
Sacks, H., Schegloff, E. A., & Jefferson, G. perceptions? Medical Care, 30, 976–988.
(1974). A simplest systematics for the orga­ Street, R. L., Jr. (1992b). Communicative styles
nization of turn-taking for conversation. and adaptations in physician-parent consul­
Language, 50, 696–735. tations. Social Science & Medicine, 34,
Sanders, R. E. (1987). The interconnection of 1155–1163.
utterances and nonverbal displays. Research Street, R. L. Jr. (2003). Communication in med­
on Language and Social Interaction, 20, ical encounters: An ecological perspective.
141–170. In T. Thompson, A. M. Dorsey, K. I.
Schegloff, E. A. (1968). Sequencing in conversa­ Miller, & R. Parrott (Eds.), Handbook
tional openings. American Anthropologist, of health communication (pp. 63–89).
70, 1075–1095. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
23-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 7:34 PM Page 459

Nonverbal Communication and Physician-Patient Interaction–––◆–––459

Street, R. L., Jr., & Buller, D. B. (1987). Nonverbal Van Dulmen, A. M., Verhaak, P. F. M., & Bilo,
response patterns in physician-patient interac­ H. J. G. (1997). Shifts in doctor-patient
tions: A functional analysis. Journal of communication during a series of outpa­
Nonverbal Behavior, 11, 234–253. tient consultations in non-insulin-dependent
Street, R. L., Jr., & Buller, D. B. (1988). Patients’ diabetes mellitus. Patient Education &
characteristics affecting physician-patient Counselling, 30, 227–237.
nonverbal communication. Human Com­ Verhaak, P. F. M. (1988). Detection of psycho­
munication Research, 15, 60–90. logical complaints by general practitioners.
Street, R. L., Jr., Piziak, V. K., Carpentier, W. S., Medical Care, 26, 1009–1020.
Herzog, J., Hejl, J., Skinnner, G., et al. Weinberger, M., Greene, J. Y., & Mamlin, J. J.
(1993). Provider-patient communication (1981). The impact of clinical encounter
and metabolic control. Diabetes Care, 16, events on patient and physician satisfaction.
714–721. Social Science & Medicine, 15E, 239–244.
23-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 7:34 PM Page 460
24-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 7:35 PM Page 461

24
NONVERBAL DYNAMICS IN
COMPUTER-MEDIATED
COMMUNICATION,
OR :( AND THE NET :( ’S WITH
YOU, :) AND YOU :) ALONE

� Joseph B. Walther
Michigan State University

I t may seem ironic, at first glance, to review research on nonverbal


communication in the realm of computer-mediated communication
(CMC). A considerable history of theory and research suggests that CMC
differs from face-to-face (FtF) communication precisely on account of the
lack of nonverbal cues in the new medium, and, that as a result, CMC
offers meager social meaning and limited value. As is known to the blind
and deaf, who cannot use all the cues that those with sight and hearing
can use, or to distant lovers, who depend on written letters to express
their love, however, this chapter will show that there are indeed a variety
of cues and adaptations for affective and comprehensible communication
when a larger set of cues is unavailable, even in the textually oriented mode
of CMC. Specifically, some nonverbal cues—those involving chronemics—
traverse CMC and are quite potent. As well, textual symbols—emoticons—
that are presumed to work as surrogates for nonverbal cues are widely

◆ 461
24-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 7:35 PM Page 462

462–––◆–––Contexts and Consequences

known and easily recognized although their theoretical and technological development
utility is questionable. requires more exacting research on nonver­
The limited research on extant nonverbal bal communication in an area once thought
cues or their substitutes, as well as emerging to be devoid of such features.
research on specific reintroductions of non­
verbal features through avatars, videocon­
ferencing, and virtual reality systems is
♦ Hypothesizing About the
leading to a more functionally oriented per­
spective on mediated human communica­
“Lack” of Nonverbal Cues
tion. Newer research is focusing on what in Early CMC Theories
people communicate, and the variety of
means by which to do so, some of which SOCIAL PRESENCE THEORY
means were considered previously the exclu­
sive domain of nonverbal cues. As a result, a The earliest predictions applied to CMC
major consequence of contemporary CMC stressed the depersonalizing, predominantly
research is to help us learn more about com­ negative effects of communication without
munication symbol systems and their func­ nonverbal cues. The first of these theories
tions in general, by observing both their was social presence theory (Short, Williams,
absence and their systematic replacement. & Christie, 1976), the original treatment of
This chapter reviews the major theories which is noteworthy for its comprehensive
and their research traditions on CMC and treatment of the role of nonverbal cues
the similarities and differences among them in communication. Originally focused on
with respect to how the relative absence of video- and audioconferencing, its theoreti­
nonverbal cues may affect communication cal specifications have also been applied to
and social perceptions. As will be argued, text-only communication (Hiltz, Johnson,
most of these approaches have relegated & Agle, 1978; Rice, 1984; Rice & Case,
nonverbal communication to a “black box,” 1983). The theory deals with decrements in
in a kind of all-or-nothing fashion, assum­ interpersonal affect as communication
ing that all nonverbal cues lead to a variety systems incrementally reduce the cue
of functions, and that the cues and func­ systems that users may employ. Thus, as
tions are isomorphic (i.e., that nonverbal communicators shift from FtF to videocon­
cues are tied directly and exclusively to ferencing, many proxemic, as well as
communicative social functions, such that haptic, cues are unavailable. Moving
the absence of such cues precludes func­ to audioconferencing, kinesics and any
tional effects from occurring). remaining proxemic cues are also removed.
The chapter then discusses the potency Short et al. (1976) equate the uses or
of chronemics in CMC (e.g., alternative absence of these cue systems with the
temporal scales, time pressure, and the degree of “social presence” that communi­
implicit and explicit effects of timing cues cators may experience, positing that social
on interpersonal judgments online). It turns presence declines as the number of cue
next to a variety of ways in which users or systems declines. Social presence, in turn,
technology designers attempt to reintro­ is conceptualized as the communicator’s
duce nonverbal cues into CMC or other involvement with the target of the conver­
electronic communication systems. Finally, sation, and it is associated with warmth
considering exemplary approaches to online and friendliness. Many studies have sup­
deception and some research employing ported the premises of social presence
virtual reality systems, we see that future theory (for review, see Walther & Parks,
24-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 7:35 PM Page 463

Nonverbal Dynamics in Computer-Mediated Communication–––◆–––463

2002), although it has also received much to use natural and varied language, and
criticism insofar as its application to CMC the extent to which message exchanges
is concerned (e.g., Lea, 1991; Walther, offer immediate feedback (i.e., sender and
1992). receiver exchanges are bidirectional, or
they are asynchronous and responses are
delayed). Together, these dimensions define
THE LACK OF SOCIAL “media richness.”
CONTEXT CUES HYPOTHESIS The second important difference
between this theory and the others is the
A similar perspective to social presence specification regarding the predicted effec­
theory is the lack of social context cues tiveness and efficiency of richer versus
hypothesis (Kiesler, 1987; Kiesler, Siegel, & leaner media when considering the degree
McGuire, 1984; Siegel, Dubrovsky, Kiesler, of equivocality and uncertainty involved in
& McGuire, 1986; Sproull & Kiesler, 1986). the communication task. Thus, for highly
This position argues that nonverbal cues in equivocal tasks, richer media are posited to
FtF settings establish the social context of be more efficient, whereas for simpler tasks,
interaction, and with the awareness of social although a rich or lean medium might be
context, participants infer and perform equally as effective, a leaner medium may
normative behavior. Without social context be more efficient (Daft & Lengel, 1984).
cues, participants are deindividuated and For instance, to ask a colleague what time a
thus behave aberrantly, including being meeting is scheduled to take place, one may
self- rather than other-focused, task- go down the hallway FtF, but a phone call
oriented, and disinhibited. These states lead or e-mail would work as well, possibly
not only to colder and more task-oriented more quickly, and with less effort.
communication, it is argued, but also to The third difference is that, whereas this
engage in “flaming” (name-calling, swearing, theory, like others, places a premium on
or other uninhibited expressions) online and nonverbal and other aspects of communica­
more attitude polarization. This position, like tive flexibility, it stresses the role of multiple
social presence theory, suggests that the cues as sources to facilitate the comprehen­
absence of nonverbal cues is the causal factor sion of information rather than as a source
distinguishing FtF and online interaction. of individuation, social presence, or social
context. In media richness theory, the avail­
MEDIA RICHNESS ability of nonverbal cues (without differen­
tiation) and other communication system
A third theory also regards the differ­ attributes are expected to help make media,
ences among media and their effects due to and messages, richer, leading to the reduc­
the range of nonverbal cue systems media tion of equivocality in shorter periods of
carry, although media richness theory (Daft time. Although interpersonal effects have
& Lengel, 1984, 1986) differs from the pre­ been imputed as derivatives of this theory
vious positions in three important respects. (Markus, 1994), the original formulation of
First, although the number of cue systems the theory makes no such claim.
supported is a primary difference among
communication media in this theory, cue
systems are joined by three other elements OVERVIEW AND SUMMARY
in differentiating media capacity: the ability
to personalize messages (i.e., to tailor mes­ As a group, all three of these theories sug­
sages for a specific recipient), the capacity gest a “black box” approach to the role of
24-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 7:35 PM Page 464

464–––◆–––Contexts and Consequences

nonverbal cues in communication. They socially oriented responses to the lack of


each seem to assume that if the capacity nonverbal cues in CMC. But it relies on an
to exhibit and detect the use of nonverbal assumption that nonverbal cues, and there­
codes is supported by alternative media, fore their individuating identification func­
users will be or must be using these codes tions, are occluded by electronic text
and attending to them, without privileging systems. SIDE, or the social identification
one code over another. They also presume, model of deindividuation effects (see for
as some critiques have suggested (Culnan review Postmes, Spears, Lea, & Reicher,
& Markus, 1987), that there is a one-to-one 2000) is derived from the social identifica­
correspondence between nonverbal codes tion/self-categorization theory (see Hogg
and the social functions with which they are & Abrams, 1988), which holds that people
associated (e.g., increases in close proximity, often identify on the basis of common
gaze, and touch always mean intimacy group membership, or ingroup and out-
and never mean threat). Moreover, there group identifications, and that certain
appears to be an assumption that nonverbal contextual factors encourage or discourage
codes have a monotonic, additive associa­ these forms of identification.
tion with those functions (i.e., the more SIDE theorists argue that the process
codes that may be used, or the more codes of CMC interaction often facilitates group
that will be used, the more warm or under­ identification: There may be a salient group
standable a given communication episode or social category associated with an online
will be). The perspectives do not consider interaction event, and, most important with
that the more cue systems available, or by respect to nonverbal cues, communicators
the use of text alone, for that matter, the operate under visual anonymity and are
better communicators may be able to reach therefore deindividuated. Because they do
intended or desired levels of affect, even if not see that they differ from one another
those targets are homeostasis (Danchak, idiosyncratically, as would be apparent FtF,
Walther, & Swan, 2001) or, as later CMC they are more likely to experience their part­
research has shown, disaffiliation and psy­ ners and interpret others’ behavior as reflect­
chological distance from others (Douglas & ing group norms, which they value and to
McGarty, 2001; Markus, 1994; O’Sullivan, which they themselves then adhere. Both
2000; Walther, Loh, & Granka, 2005; SIDE and the lack of social context cues
Walther, Slovacek, & Tidwell, 2001). Other approaches argue that the reintroduction of
models do, however, consider the potential visual cues ameliorates deindividuation.
for cues of all kinds—multimodal or text The SIDE model is more specific with
alone—to affect relationships differentially. respect to visual cues than are other tradi­
tions. SIDE research has looked at both
between-media and within-media varia­
tions. That is, not only have SIDE dynamics
♦ Adaptation Theories
been supported in comparisons between
CMC and FtF conditions, but they have
SOCIAL IDENTIFICATION MODEL also been found between CMC alone and
OF DEINDIVIDUATION EFFECTS CMC in a room where people can see
one another (Lea & Spears, 1992), CMC
The next theory discussed is not associ­ with only text compared with CMC with
ated traditionally with the above cues­ a photo of one’s partners (see Postmes,
filtered-out perspective, because it specifies Spears, & Lea, 1998), and CMC alone
24-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 7:35 PM Page 465

Nonverbal Dynamics in Computer-Mediated Communication–––◆–––465

versus CMC plus videoconferencing (Lea, framework acknowledges that receivers


Spears, & de Groot, 2001). With respect stereotype and idealize their partners when
to nonverbal cues, however, SIDE treats all they receive messages without the informa­
visual cues the same theoretically. No dif­ tion about the partner’s idiosyncratic char­
ferentiation is made on the basis of whether acteristics (although the hyperpersonal
visual cues are dynamic (as in videocon­ model does not dictate group- or categori­
ferencing) or static (as in photographs). cal-level stereotypes; Walther, 1997). The
Within SIDE, the function of visual infor­ hyperpersonal model also considers the ide­
mation is to cue individuating identifi­ alizing potential of CMC that eliminates
cations, whereas its absence can promote potentially undesirable dynamic nonverbal
immersion in group identity. Like the previ­ behavior, such as interruptions and other
ous positions reviewed, SIDE treats text- distracting vocalizations, unconventional
based CMC as bereft of cues about the gaze patterns, and unattractive physical
individuals using it and asserts that without appearance characteristics (Walther &
visual information, users do not identify Parks, 2002), although these particular
with one another as individuals. From this elements have not yet been tested.
perspective, it is only possible to achieve Beyond perceptions of partners, how­
interpersonal relationships online by intro­ ever, the reduction of nonverbal cues in
ducing visual, individuating nonverbal cues CMC is pivotal in other specifications
such as photographs or video (Rogers & of hyperpersonal interaction. One is that
Lea, 2004). In contrast to previous theories, senders, in the process of message construc­
according to SIDE, the outcome of the dein­ tion, engage in selective self-presentation to
dividuated, nonvisual state may be proso­ a degree not afforded in FtF interaction.
cial (raising attraction and evaluations of Because many nonverbal cues are more dif­
partners relative to FtF interaction), or it ficult to control (from body shape and
can increase bias and intergroup denigra­ other physical appearance features, vocalic
tion (Douglas & McGarty, 2001; Lea attributes, to kinesic behaviors that are less
et al., 2001; Postmes et al., 1998; Postmes consciously controlled) compared with ver­
& Spears, 2002). bal behaviors, CMC users can create more
intentional messages and avoid uninten­
tional cues. The ability to edit text messages
HYPERPERSONAL CMC enhances this effect (Walther, in press).
Finally, as the CMC process frees users
One additional model of CMC interac­ from needing to attend to one’s own non­
tion also places the absence of physical cues verbal behavior, as well as attending to
as a causal factor in explaining the differ­ partners’ nonverbal affect, information, or
ences between CMC and FtF communica­ conversation management cues, CMC
tion. The hyperpersonal model of CMC users recapture cognitive resources that
strived originally to explain how CMC would normally be allocated to those
interactions may lead to levels of intimacy processes and apply them instead to mes­
and social orientation exceeding those of sage creation, allowing for further expres­
FtF interactions in parallel social contexts sive selectivity.
(Walther, 1996), but it has been expanded Empirical investigations have supported
to predict both hyperpositive and hyperneg­ several aspects of this model. In a test of
ative outcomes (Walther et al., 2001). self-presentation, CMC dyads exchanged
Like the SIDE model, the hyperpersonal more self-disclosure and more intimate
24-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 7:35 PM Page 466

466–––◆–––Contexts and Consequences

personal questions in an online “get-to­ other theories to create contexts in which


know-you” session than did FtF partners, these dynamics emerge.
who relied to a greater extent on environ­
mental characteristics, physical attributes,
and kinesic behaviors to reduce uncertainty SOCIAL INFORMATION
about their partners (Tidwell & Walther, PROCESSING
2002). In a direct test of the impact of facial
photographs as a benefit or detriment to A final theoretical model of CMC
hyperpersonal online relationship forma­ employs a different perspective on the rela­
tion, Walther et al. (2001) employed tionship between nonverbal cues offline
groups, half of which had interacted via and text-only CMC. The social information
CMC over several tasks, whereas members processing theory (SIP; Walther, 1992)
of the other half were unknown to one argues that impression-bearing and rela­
another. In each of these conditions, half of tional functions, for which communicators
them experienced the presence or absence rely on nonverbal cues FtF, are translated
of photographs of their partners’ faces into verbal content, linguistic, stylistic, and
immediately prior to a chat. Results showed chronemic cues in the CMC environment.
that those who came to know one another Given that all functions—task, social, and
online and did not see each other’s pictures otherwise—must be conveyed through the
rated their partners as more affectionate single conduit of text, it may take more
and socially attractive, but the introduction messages, over a longer time, to imbue
of photos reduced attraction among those exchanges with sufficient information for
who were familiar with each other only participants to decode and aggregate in
online. Only among strangers, a photo order to construct impressions and manage
enhanced affection and social attraction relationships. Add to this slowdown that
relative to no photo. Moreover, interesting CMC messages may be exchanged in fits
correlations emerged between participants’ and spurts intermittently (as in e-mail)
self-reported impression management efforts and even further retardation of evolving
and ratings of their physical attractiveness social dynamics is expected relative to FtF
by partners. When there was no photo, processes. Central to SIP, however, is the
physical attractiveness ratings were posi­ premise that all other things being equal,
tively correlated with self-presentation CMC is as capable as FtF communication
efforts, but when pictures were shown, self- of sharing impressions and managing rela­
presentation effort and physical attractive­ tional communication, based on the substi­
ness were negatively correlated. When one’s tutability of verbal and nonverbal cues in
photo shows, the more one tries to enhance the service of social functions.
one’s impression, the worse it seems to get This premise is disconcerting to those
(Walther et al., 2001). who hold that there is unique value to non­
The hyperpersonal perspective may be the verbal cues that cannot be replaced. Indeed,
most specific CMC framework with respect Jones and LeBaron’s (2002) review of non­
to the role of nonverbal communication and verbal communication literature concluded
its functions in FtF interaction, and how they that it has been assumed that “verbal and
are transformed online. Not all dimensions nonverbal behaviors are generally different
of the model have been tested directly, and kinds of messages with rather different
the model is less specific about the kinds of meanings and potential functions” (p. 501).
partners for which hyperpersonal processes SIP, on the other hand, argues that infor­
should be expected to adhere, drawing on mation is information and that it can be
24-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 7:35 PM Page 467

Nonverbal Dynamics in Computer-Mediated Communication–––◆–––467

expressed through a variety of modalities. and CMC transcripts for verbal indications
Like media richness theory (Daft & Lengel, of affinity. Regression analyses of the cues
1984), SIP acknowledges that written cues in both conditions were used to identify the
alone may be less efficient within a given variations in cues and channels that most
time interval compared with a simultane­ strongly predicted the variations in partner
ously multimodal (i.e., kinesic, vocalic, ratings of affect. As expected, that FtF part­
and verbal) exchange, but given sufficient ners expressed affinity through nonverbal
time and exchange, the two systems may cues primarily, with vocalic cues (pleasant­
be functionally equivalent, and CMC users ness, vocal sharpness, vocal condescension,
make these adaptations fluidly. and timber) predominating over kinesics;
The SIP theory has been supported in verbal cues were not significantly associated
several empirical studies (for review, see with FtF liking in comparison with these
Walther & Parks, 2002). For instance, Liu, nonverbal variations. In CMC conversa­
Ginther, and Zelhart (2002) found that tions, however, an equivalent proportion
impression development in CMC was sensi­ of the variance in liking was accomplished
tive to both the length of e-mail messages through verbal behaviors (explicit verbal
and the frequency of e-mail messages from a statements of affection, changing the
partner over time, and Walther and Burgoon subject, and various forms of disagree­
(1992) found that relational communica­ ment), demonstrating comparability and
tion levels changed more or less in parallel substitutability of verbal cues in CMC for
between CMC and FtF groups in response to vocalic and kinesic cues in FtF interaction.
time accrual rather than to the differences
between communication conditions (see
also Chidambaram & Bostrom, 1993). SUMMARY
These studies lend credence to the model’s
causal factors and predicted effects; they did The major theories of CMC each portray
not examine the microprocesses implicated significant effects of the reduction of non­
by the theory—that is, the substitution of verbal cues online. Positions range from the
verbal cues in the service of functions for austere, early formulations, where nonver­
which nonverbal cues are employed offline. bal cues were isomorphic with certain com­
A recent study addressed this gap by municative functions, to the more adaptive
assessing the specific behaviors in alternative models of hyperpersonal CMC and SIP, in
channels that express affinity. Walther et al.’s which users exploit or work through the rel­
(2005) experiment employed decision- ative lack of nonverbal cues. Cutting across
making dyads meeting FtF or via synchro­ these models, other research has focused on
nous computer chat. One member was specific cues—natural or stylized—and the
prompted to enact greater or lesser levels of degree to which CMC users adapt affective
liking toward his or her partner after an ini­ meaning to their usage.
tial interaction period, by whatever way he
or she chose to display the affect. The other
dyad partner rated the ad hoc confederates’ ♦ The Cues That
performance on perceived immediacy and Remain: Chronemics
affection. Coders rated kinesic cues from
videotapes of the FtF confederates and
independently rated the vocalic perfor­ Whereas physical behavior, voice, space,
mances as heard through a content-filtering and appearance cues are indeed absent in
device. Additional coders analyzed both FtF text-based CMC, the chronemic cue system
24-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 7:35 PM Page 468

468–––◆–––Contexts and Consequences

remains, although it is frequently over­ (Reid, Ball, Morley, & Evans, 1997; Reid,
looked in descriptions of CMC’s nonver­ Malinek, Stott, & Evans, 1996) determined
bal capacity. Hesse, Werner, and Altman that the relational tone of CMC is more
(1988) were among the first to recognize sensitive to time scarcity than is FtF inter­
the potential for temporal dynamics and action: In CMC groups, more than in
cues to play a significant role in CMC, FtF groups, discourse became less rational,
noting potential departures from traditional and less affective content appeared, as pres­
interaction patterns in terms of temporal sure increased with shorter time limits.
scale (the temporal scope and duration of Conversely, it appears that a long duration
events and relationships), the sequencing of of time spent in CMC with a partner is
actions, the pace, and the salience of past, inferred as a token of relational intimacy
present, or future issues in ongoing CMC (Henderson & Gilding, 2004).
interactions. Although several studies can Response latencies are another familiar
now be said to address some of these issues, chronemic characteristic, and their effects
few of them have noted Hesse et al.’s origi­ have been studied in several CMC contexts.
nal thinking. Members make biased attributions for
Among those studies examining tem­ response delays, assuming personal rather
poral factors in CMC, chronemic dynamics than situational causes for lags by distant
are potent forces in the experience of team members (Cramton, 2001). Failure
CMC users. As Kalman and Rafaeli to get responses may erode initial levels of
(2005) observed, for example, “One of the trust in virtual groups (Jarvenpaa, Knoll, &
unknowns of emailing is the time it will Leidner, 1998), and frequent messaging is
take the receiver to form and post a reply. noted consistently as a critical factor in
Response times vary considerably, and virtual group trust, affective relations, and
the chronemics of email are an important effectiveness (Walther & Bunz, 2005), espe­
non-verbal cue which can convey meaning” cially with regard to partners’ replies to
(p. 1). According to Rice (1990), e-mail an individual’s conversational initiations or
users attend to the time stamps that are requests (Iacono & Weisband, 1997).
placed on messages automatically, inferring Latencies also have mixed effects in dyadic,
from them when a message was sent and synchronous CMC. In organizational
how much latency occurred before one of settings where members use Instant Messen­
their own messages received a reply. ger, a query that goes without a response is
Temporal dynamics affect virtual groups frequently attributed to one’s partner being
in a variety of ways, although specific busy (Nardi, Whittaker, & Bradner, 2000).
chronemic cues may or may not play a role In social chatting, however, individuals
in these effects. Orlikowski and Yates who find themselves waiting for replies
(2002) found that virtual groups’ activity grow increasingly frustrated if not hostile
cycles became oriented more toward critical (Rintel & Pittam, 1997; see also Feenberg,
events, such as the occasional exchange of 1989).
collaborative documents, than toward the One study tested the interpersonal impres­
influence of predetermined deadlines. In sions affected by variations in e-mail response
a field study of organizational CMC, latency, as well as whether messages were
Steinfield (1986) found that CMC becomes sent at night or during the day (Walther &
more task oriented and less socially ori­ Tidwell, 1995). Researchers created several
ented as collaborators get closer to project pairs of e-mail message facsimiles featuring an
deadlines. In a closer inspection of time initial message and a reply that appeared to be
pressure and CMC, Reid and colleagues initiated by a vice president and replied to by
24-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 7:35 PM Page 469

Nonverbal Dynamics in Computer-Mediated Communication–––◆–––469

a manager who were separated geographi­ and emerging technologies selectively rein­
cally within a corporation. The time stamps troduce additional cues into communicative
on these e-mail facsimiles made one pair exchanges among people who do not meet
appear to have been sent shortly after 10 a.m. FtF. Whether executed by users or technol­
and another pair after 10 p.m. This factor was ogy designers, these developments and their
crossed by the apparent response lag. In some impacts inform nonverbal communication
pairs, the reply seemed to occur several min­ principles.
utes after the initial message; alternatively, 24
hours and several minutes appeared to have
elapsed. The time stamps were further crossed CUE SURROGATES: EMOTICONS
over two kinds of message exchanges, a task-
oriented request versus social banter. A considerable amount of attention has
Ratings of these various stimuli con­ been devoted to the use of “emoticons” in
firmed chronemics-based hypotheses. When CMC. Emoticons are the presentation of
task messages were sent at night the sender keyboard symbols used in such manner as
was rated highest on dominance compared to resemble facial expressions. They are
with the same message sent during the day. assumed widely to express emotion and are
The pattern was opposite for social mes­ frequently described as emotional surro­
sages, which signaled more dominance by gates in CMC for facial expressions and
day than at night. The amount of affection other nonverbal cues to emotion. “Because
ascribed to a sender’s message was affected the use of e-mail eliminates visual cues such
by an interaction between day and night, the as head nodding, facial expressions, pos­
promptness of the reply, and the thematic ture, and eye contact found in FtF commu­
content. The most affection accorded to nication, CMC users often incorporate
task exchanges occurred when there was a emoticons as visual cues to augment the
quick reply to a daytime request, and the meaning of textual electronic messages”
least affection was associated with a prompt (Rezabek & Cochenour, 1998, pp. 201–
response to a nighttime message. As for 202). The use of emoticons in CMC dates
social messages, more affection was per­ back at least as far the early 1980s, and for
ceived in a slower reply to a daytime mes­ many years “smiley dictionaries” circulated
sage than a fast reply, but a fast reply at in the Internet, containing hundreds of vari­
night showed more affection than a slow ations and the verbal labels of their alleged
one. Consistent with Hall’s (1959) observa­ emotional equivalents (e.g., Godin, 1993;
tions about FtF speech lags, it appears that Sanderson, 1993). The best known of these
expectations of quick e-mail replies are symbols are “a smile, wink, and frown,
relaxed within established social relation­ respectively: :-) ;-) :-(” (Danet, Ruedenberg-
ships, although reactions to response latency Wright, & Rosenbaum-Tamari, 1997,
are quite different within impersonal rela­ n.p.). These symbols are well recognized
tions, both online and offline. within the CMC-using community. Among
one college student sample, basic emoticons
were interpreted more reliably than were
♦ Reintroducing Cues photos of human facial expressions of emo­
tion: Whereas Ekman and Friesen (1975)
report percentages of agreement about the
Whereas chronemic cues have always been association of facial photos depicting basic
available, thus countering the ideas that human emotions from 97% for happiness
nonverbal cues are lacking from CMC, new to 67% for anger, Walther and D’Addario
24-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 7:35 PM Page 470

470–––◆–––Contexts and Consequences

(2001) found that the :) and :( emoticons sender’s affective state and attitude about
achieved 98% consensus for happiness and the course. There was very little effect of
sadness, respectively, and associations of emoticons on attitude and interpretation;
other emoticons with anger, disgust, and what impact they did exhibit was not in
fear ranged from 88% to 85%. accord with the hypotheses from facial
Although the literature on emoticons expression research. Specifically, smiley
asserts frequently that they function as emoticons had no effect on message inter­
nonverbal (facial) expressions, very little pretation whatsoever. Frown emoticons
research has examined the functional reduced the positivity of a positive verbal
impact of these symbols. Most of the message, but frowns did not affect interpre­
research on emoticons has analyzed pat­ tations of negative verbal messages—that
terns of their use based on demographic is, did not make them even more negative.
factors: Females use them more frequently Overall, there appeared to be a negativity
than do males (Witmer & Katzman, 1997) effect: When any negative message element
and their usage even depends on which part appeared, whether it was an emoticon or
of the United States e-mail users reside a verbal statement, the interpretation was
(Rezabek & Cochenour, 1998). Walther negative. Additionally, the combination of
and D’Addario (2001) explored their func­ verbal statements and their opposite emoti­
tional dynamics. Reviewing the facial affect con were not significantly different in
literature, they derived hypotheses predict­ sarcasm from other combinations; only a
ing relationships between emoticons and positive verbal message with a ;) emoticon
accompanying verbal messages on affective was rated higher in sarcasm than other
message interpretation. These relationships combinations, suggesting that the wink
included a variety of additive effects, by symbol has some iconic value in CMC but
which the emotional valence of the emoti­ that a negative verbal statement may over­
con would be added to the emotional ride the emoticon effect. Given that only
valence of a verbal message, leading to sup­ the frown emoticon affected meaning, it
plementation (for a positive emoticon plus appears to be the case that :( and the net
a positive verbal message, a negative emoti­ :( ’s with you, but :) and you :) alone.
con plus a negative verbal message) or mod­
ification (a positive element plus a negative
“canceling out” or neutralizing overall AVATARS AND VIDEO
affect). Alternatively, visual primacy was
posited: An emoticon’s valence might over­ In addition to stylized affective cues such
ride that of the verbal statement. The com­ as emoticons, developers and users explore
bination of positive and negative messages, the utility of reintroducing certain visual
among emoticon and verbal statements, cues into distributed interaction. This has
might also result in an interpretation of sar­ been done primarily through the use of
casm, as might the iconic ;) or “winkie.” avatars, icons, and videoconferencing.
In a 4 by 2 experimental procedure, :) ;)
:( or no emoticon were inserted alternately Avatars. Avatars are two-dimensional
in simulated e-mail message mock-ups that representations on a computer screen that
contained either a positive or a negative chat users can select and move around the
verbal statement about a college course. screen during online interaction. Avatars
Participants viewed one of these mock-ups are used frequently in various multiplayer
and then rated the supposed message computer games such as Everquest and the
24-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 7:35 PM Page 471

Nonverbal Dynamics in Computer-Mediated Communication–––◆–––471

SimsOnline, although they have a somewhat (see also Bengtsson, Burgoon, Cederberg,
longer history in multiuser chat spaces such Bonito, & Lundberg, 1999).
as the Palace (www.thepalace.com/; see Krikorian, Lee, and Chock (2000)
Suler, 1999). In most environments, one examined proxemic responses using
selects an avatar initially from among a avatars. Within a Palace chat space, partic­
stock of available figures. Avatars are often ipants engaged in a get-to-know-you con­
cartoonish and range from very generic versation online, exchanging text and
with few distinguishing features to rather manipulating the positions of their respec­
elaborate in design. It is also possible to tive avatars. Researchers captured the
create an individual avatar using graphics video images and developed an automated
software or to craft an avatar from a pho­ system for measuring the dynamic distance
tograph and upload it to the interaction between avatars based on the pixels in the
space. During interaction, dialogue often center of the avatars and the relative dis­
appears as text, as though emanating from tances between them. Results showed rela­
an avatar like the conversational bubbles tively even proportions among the pairs of
that appear in comic strips. Advocates of participants who moved their avatars
these systems argue that they help orient closer, farther, or not at all over the course
players and that they reduce the impersonal of the conversation. Among those who
nature of text-based systems. Much of the moved farther apart, there was also an
research employing avatars focuses on the increase in avatar expressive movement—
psychoanalytic dimensions of avatar selec­ as if being too close inhibited other
tion and usage, such as how an avatar both kinesics—which was accompanied by self-
reflects aspects of the user’s personalities as reports of greater conversational appropri­
it also shapes the online persona through ateness and conversational involvement
social interaction (Suler, 1999). In some among participants. There was also a
online multiplayer games, avatars are used curvilinear trend on other ratings, how­
to duel or fight, although users socialize ever. In general, the correspondence of par­
through text to a large extent alongside the ticipants’ social attraction ratings and
avatar battles (Peña & Hancock, 2006). avatar distances mapped onto the predic­
Two avatar studies bear immediate tions of nonverbal expectancy violations
relevance to nonverbal communication theory (Burgoon & Hale, 1988), in that
research. Nowak and Biocca (2003) exam­ attraction was greater when avatars inter­
ined avatars varying in anthropomorphic acted either at relatively close or far dis­
appearance, representing conversational tances rather than at median ranges.
partners. The more anthropomorphic
representations depicted 3-D drawings of Anthropomorphic Icons. A less manipula­
heads and faces, whereas less anthropo­ ble form of avatars is anthropomorphic
morphic versions featured disembodied, graphics, or icons, accompanying CMC
cartoonish pairs of eyes and lips. Contrary messages or appearing fixed on a screen
to hypotheses, the less anthropomorphic during chat. Icons have the capacity to
the avatar, the greater the participants’ influence receiver’s interpretations of mes­
responses on various measures of presence. sages, even if receivers are aware that the
The authors concluded that the more realis­ icon does not necessarily represent the char­
tic but imperfect human resemblances frus­ acteristics of the actual message sender.
trated users’ expectations, whereas the Isotalus (2003) found that receivers’
more abstract images drew greater interest responses to news stories delivered to
24-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 7:35 PM Page 472

472–––◆–––Contexts and Consequences

handheld computers differed based on the conditions obtained few differences on task
apparent gender of an icon accompanying performance quality or interpersonal per­
the story. Participants paid more attention ceptions due to main effects of the inter­
when the icon appeared to be female. faces (Nowak, Watt, & Walther, 2005).
Furthermore, males found the news more There was greater perceived involvement
credible when accompanied by a male icon, with others group members in those condi­
whereas females’ credibility assessments tions with fewer visual cues, which, in turn,
were higher for female icons; evaluations of led to increases in social attraction and
the stories’ entertainment followed an credibility ratings of partners.
opposite pattern. Lee (2005) also used gen­ Interestingly, by focusing video on the
dered icons to accompany spontaneous, objects that collaborators discuss rather
dyadic CMC chat messages, but the partic­ than on facial displays (but including par­
ipants were aware that the gendered icon ticipants’ voices) seems to be superior to
had been randomly assigned to users; that face-oriented videoconferencing in many
is, there was an even chance that the gender cases (Brittan, 1992). This may be due to
of the icon and the user were mismatched. the communication efficiency with which
Despite this awareness, participants (espe­ humans process multimodal messages,
cially female participants) attributed the when one level of content traverses the
gender of the chat partner on the basis of vocal-to-auditory channel, leaving vision
the icon’s gender. This over-interpretation free to focus on a common object. When
of gender based on a simple physical appear­ videoconferencing depicts the communica­
ance representation suggests, as previous tors rather than the objects they are dis­
perspectives have argued, that CMC plus a cussing, the objects and the image of
little nonverbal representation leads to partners compete for visual attention,
potentially exaggerated perceptions. leading to decrements in efficiency and
performance.
Videoconferencing. Research exploring Fussell and colleagues (Fussell, Kraut, &
videoconferencing to enhance social pres­ Siegel, 2000; Gergle, Kraut, & Fussell, 2004;
ence and improve remote collaborations Kraut, Fussell, & Siegel, 2003) have
predates the Internet and digital technol­ employed audio/videoconferencing with the
ogy considerably (see for review Chapanis, visual field aimed at an object that one part­
Ochsman, Parrish, & Weeks, 1972). Most ner manipulates but both can see. In one
videoconferencing arrangements and stud­ study (Kraut et al., 2003) a head-mounted
ies involve real-time visual conveyance of camera focused on a bicycle that one partner
participants’ faces to remote partners, repaired while an expert helper elsewhere
accompanied by their voices. The results of viewed the bicycle (and the repairer’s manip­
this research have been generally disap­ ulations of it) via video, and instructed the
pointing. Whereas users report greater sub­ repairer via audio while both looked at the
jective presence when video is available, bike. In another study (Gergle et al., 2004),
their communication effectiveness and task both partners viewed puzzle pieces on an
output tends to be no better, and sometimes electronic video display, while one partner
worse, than non-visual interfaces provide guided the other via voice toward the puz­
(Gale, 1991; Storck & Sproull, 1995). zle’s completion. Compared with other video
Similarly, a recent study comparing asyn­ foci, or no video, participants performed
chronous videoconferencing with synchro­ more accurately and quickly at the tasks
nous video, text-only systems, and FtF that were employed in these studies. In the
24-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 7:35 PM Page 473

Nonverbal Dynamics in Computer-Mediated Communication–––◆–––473

scenarios, the face’s physical appearances simultaneously facilitate both. As designers


and expressive dynamics are less useful, and, advance systems that can support these
instead of distracting users with these irrele­ conversations, it would be useful for
vant data, their visual attention is directed to designers to recall who, in FtF interaction,
objects providing what Clark and Brennan gets to choose the view. In contemporary
(1991) conceptualize as “communicative videoconferencing systems, the message
grounding.” sender is often the party who chooses where
As promising as this line of research on to point the camera; the sender chooses the
the role of video appears to be, its promise receiver’s field of vision. In FtF interaction,
is limited to those collaborative activities however, the receiver chooses what he or
in which physical objects are the focus of she sees; the receiver chooses the receiver’s
the conversation. There are many conversa­ visual field. Advancing new telecommuni­
tions, however, where the focus is not on cation systems that replace nonverbal cues
tangible items but rather on abstract issues in electronic form will do well to attend to,
that reside in the thoughts and feelings of and build into new systems, these funda­
communicators. A conversational efficiency mentals of opportunistic visual choice.
framework—that complementary receptors
such as the ears and the eyes are well suited
to multimodal presentations of voice and ♦ Critique and Consequences
visual data—can be extended to the
realm of subjective data. Object-oriented
conversation can be distinguished from A variety of consequences for further theo­
person-oriented conversation, however, retical and system development may be
with the latter referring to conversation inferred from current research trends, and
about persons’ ideas, attitudes, and feelings. as new technologies develop, we may
The most useful and efficient combination predict that the need for conceptual and
of verbal, vocal, and visual cues in person- empirical specificity about nonverbal cues,
oriented videoconferencing would be, quite their functions, and their re-representations
traditionally, verbal content accompanied will become even more consequential. As
by vocalic and facial/kinesic cues supple­ the Internet becomes a permanent fixture in
menting the verbiage with affective infor­ contemporary life, notions about CMC are
mation. There is little novelty in this applied to other domain-specific theories.
proposition, except that in the present Such applications often revert to the
argument we may advance that these com­ premises of some of the older theories
binations are most useful and efficient for reviewed above, regardless of the current
person-oriented conversations, and not for state of support for those theories.
object-oriented discussions. In the case of It is not uncommon, for instance, for
mediated interaction with video, more researchers to assume that without nonver­
advanced research should explore whether bal cues, communicators cannot accom­
the focus of video on objects or faces inter­ plish certain functions that they do in
acts with the orientation of the conversation full-cue environments, and to apply this
in predicting conversational effectiveness. assumption to other communication theo­
Moreover, in many conversations, par­ ries. In persuasion, for example, it has been
ticipants switch between object orientation suggested that the lack of nonverbal cues
and person orientation ad hoc and often, in CMC prevents receivers from forming
and technological systems need to adapt or liking assessments of the online persuader,
24-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 7:35 PM Page 474

474–––◆–––Contexts and Consequences

reducing the likelihood of “peripheral cues, leading to relatively effective (unde­


processing” and promoting instead atten­ tected) deception. Thus, when CMC masks
tion to persuasive arguments (Guadagno receivers’ feedback cues, deceivers do not
& Cialdini, 2005). When such claims are have these guideposts with which to adjust
accompanied by supporting data, such find­ their performances. Their unfolding decep­
ings often obtain in experiments employing tive performance is less tailored to the
CMC in relatively compressed time periods. receiver’s suspicions, and ultimately, the
Such conclusions are untenable from the deception is performed more poorly in
perspective of the SIP model, which would CMC, with more frequent deception detec­
qualify such findings as occurring when tion in CMC rather than less. Related
participants lack sufficient motivation research has varied and measured the cues
and/or online experience with one another available in FtF conditions, whereas CMC
to have formed impressions. Indeed, in conditions have been varied with regard to
many such studies reflecting the dampening synchrony and interactivity, and both have
of affect, influence, or sociability of the been examined with respect to the effects
medium, the incapacity of CMC to allow of receiver suspiciousness and sender moti­
normal performance is often unquestioned, vation to remain undetected (Burgoon,
even though these effects might disappear if Stoner, Bonito, & Dunbar, 2003; George,
CMC-using subjects had ample time. More Marrett, & Tilley, 2004; Woodworth,
attention to the corpus of CMC research Hancock, & Goorha, 2005), all of which
may prevent theoretical and empirical mis­ tell us about the functional aspect of cues
steps as people examine CMC in new func­ absent in CMC, and the combinations of
tional domains. In the future, more specific factors that alter interpersonal deception
consideration of nonverbal cues, those online.
missing and those that are replaceable, will The importance of precise conceptual
be critical to the development of more and empirical specifications of nonverbal
sophisticated theories and better interfaces. cues is also seen in research employing vir­
One exception to the undifferentiated tual reality (VR) systems to facilitate dis­
approach to nonverbal cues in CMC tributed interaction. When any nonverbal
appears in recent studies on interpersonal behavior can be detected and represented
deception theory (IDT; Buller & Burgoon, virtually (see Biocca & Delaney, 1995), it is
1996) applied to the CMC context critical to represent it in meaningful ways in
(Carlson, George, Burgoon, Adkins, & order to elicit particular responses. The
White, 2004). Most commentary on CMC development of remote haptic capabilities,
and deception suggests that the absence of for instance, has dealt with measuring and
nonverbal cues should make deception less conveying subjects’ exertion, and resistance
likely to detect, due to an assumed connec­ by objects, formulating the “collision
tion between the availability of kinesic point” at which an actor’s representation
and vocalic indicators of deception and meets and object’s, to provide propriocep­
receivers’ deception detection success (see, tive feedback (Kim et al., 2004). In repre­
e.g., Hollingshead, 2000). IDT, however, senting people, a study revisiting Argyle
recognizes the transactional nature of and Dean’s (1965) equilibrium theoretic
deception. Because receivers nonverbally predictions was conducted using varied
signal suspicion and incredulity to deceivers levels of eye contact by virtual projections in
in FtF settings, deceivers monitor and learn an immersive 3-D environment. Bailenson,
to accommodate to these suspiciousness Blascovich, Beall, and Loomis (2001) had
24-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 7:35 PM Page 475

Nonverbal Dynamics in Computer-Mediated Communication–––◆–––475

research participants don head-mounted International Conference on Systems


VR eyewear and interact with a virtual man Science. Retrieved July 30, 2005 from
who varied his gaze at precise intervals, http://ieeexplore.ieee.org/search/wrapper
including eyes shut, to persistent gaze, to .jsp?arnumber=772736
Biocca, F., & Delaney, B. (1995). Immersive
gaze with head turns, to gaze with pupil
virtual reality technology. In F. Biocca &
dilation when the subject approached. As
M. R. Levy (Eds.), Communication in the
predicted, there were correlations between
age of virtual reality (pp. 57–124).
the levels of gaze exhibited by the virtual Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
man and the distances to him adopted by Brittan, D. (1992). Being there: The promise
human participants. of multimedia communications. Techno­
This study not only demonstrates the logy Review, 95(4), 42–50.
robustness of equilibrium theory but also Buller, D. B., & Burgoon, J. K. (1996).
contrasts the icon and avatar studies in find­ Interpersonal deception theory. Com­
ing that more is more (rather than less is munication Theory, 6, 203–242.
more) by isolating an aspect of nonverbal Burgoon, J. K., & Hale, J. L. (1988). Nonverbal
behavior rooted in theoretical understanding. expectancy violations: Model elaboration
and application to immediacy behaviors.
It illustrates the means to test extant theories
Communication Monographs, 55, 58–79.
and apply them to new settings, and to
Burgoon, J. K., Stoner, G. M., Bonito, J. A., &
employ methods to create effective interfaces
Dunbar, N. E. (2003). Trust and deception
through careful attention to defined, con­ in mediated communication. Proceedings
trolled reintroduction of specific nonverbal of the 36th Annual Hawaii International
elements through communication technol­ Conference on Systems Sciences. Retrieved
ogy. It will be imperative for designers of new July 30, 2005 from http://ieeexplore.ieee
electronic communication systems to know .org/search/wrapper.jsp?arnumber=
what nonverbal, behavioral cues precisely 1173792
affect particular functions, if new systems will Carlson, J. R., George, J. F., Burgoon, J. K.,
be successful at representing those signals Adkins, M., & White, C. (2004). Deception
through alternative symbols such as text, in computer-mediated communication.
Group Decision and Negotiation, 13, 5–28.
time, icons, video, or VR representations.
Chapanis, A., Ochsman, R. B., Parrish, R. N., &
Weeks, G. D. (1972). Studies in interactive
communication: I. The effects of four com­
♦ References munication modes on the behavior of teams
during cooperative problem-solving.
Human Factors, 14, 487–509.
Argyle, M., & Dean, J. (1965). Eye-contact, Chidambaram, L., & Bostrom, R. P. (1993).
distance and affiliation. Sociometry, 28, Evolution of group performance over time:
289–304. A repeated measures study of GDSS effects.
Bailenson, J. N., Blascovich, J., Beall, A. C., & Journal of Organizational Computing, 3,
Loomis, J. M. (2001). Equilibrium theory 443–469.
revisited: Mutual gaze and personal space Clark, H. H., & Brennan, S. E. (1991).
in virtual environments. Presence, 10, Grounding in communication. In L. B.
583–598. Resnick, R. M. Levine, & S. D. Teasley
Bengtsson, B., Burgoon, J. K., Cederberg, C., (Eds.), Perspectives on socially shared cog­
Bonito, J., & Lundberg, M. (1999). The nition (pp. 127–149). Washington DC:
impact of anthropomorphic interfaces on American Psychological Association.
influence, understanding, and credibility. Cramton, C. D. (2001). The mutual knowledge
Proceedings of the 32nd Annual Hawaii problem and its consequences for dispersed
24-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 7:35 PM Page 476

476–––◆–––Contexts and Consequences

collaboration. Organization Science, 12, Retrieved January 30, 2005, from


346–371. http://www.cs.cmu.edu/~sfussell/pubs/Man
Culnan, M. J., & Markus, M. L. (1987). us cripts/p21-fussell.pdf
Information technologies. In F. M. Jablin, Gale, S. (1991). Adding audio and video to an
L. L. Putnam, K. H. Roberts, & L. W. office environment. In J. M. Bowers &
Porter (Eds.), Handbook of organizational S. D. Benford (Eds.), Studies in computer-
communication: An interdisciplinary per­ supported collaborative work (pp. 49–62).
spective (pp. 420–443). Newbury Park, North-Holland: Elsevier Science.
CA: Sage. George, J. F., Marrett, K., & Tilley, P. (2004).
Daft, R. L., & Lengel, R. H. (1984). Information Deception detection under varying elec­
richness: A new approach to managerial tronic media and warning conditions.
behavior and organization design. In B. M. Proceedings of the 34th Annual Hawaii
Staw & L. L. Cummings (Eds.), Research International Conference on System
in organizational behavior (pp. 191–233). Sciences. Retrieved July 30, 2005 from
Greenwich, CT: JAI Press. http://ieeexplore.ieee.org/ie15/8934/28293/
Daft, R. L., & Lengel, R. H. (1986). Organiza­ 01265080.pdf?tp=&arnumber=1265080
tional information requirements, media &isnumber=28293
richness, and structural determinants. Gergle, D., Kraut, R. E., & Fussell, S. R. (2004).
Management Science, 32, 554–571. Language efficiency and visual technology:
Danchak, M. M., Walther, J. B., & Swan, K. P. Minimizing collaborative effort with visual
(2001, November). Presence in mediated information. Journal of Language and
instruction: Bandwidth, behavior, and Social Psychology, 23, 491–517.
expectancy violations. Paper presented at Godin, S. (1993). The smiley dictionary. Berkeley,
the conference on Asynchronous Learning CA: Peachpit Press.
Networks, Orlando, FL. Guadagno, R. E., & Cialdini, R. B. (2005). Online
Danet, B., Ruedenberg-Wright, L., & persuasion and compliance: Social influence
Rosenbaum-Tamari, Y. (1997). “HMMM on the Internet and beyond. In Y. Amichai-
. . . WHERE’S THAT SMOKE COMING Hamburger (Ed.), The social net: The social
FROM?” Writing, play and performance psychology of the Internet (pp. 91–113).
on Internet Relay Chat. Journal of New York: Oxford University Press.
Computer-Mediated Communication, 2. Hall, E. T. (1959). The silent language. New
Retrieved March 15, 2005, from http://jcmc York: Anchor Books.
.indiana.edu/vol2/issue4/danet.html Henderson, S., & Gilding, M. (2004). “I’ve
Douglas, K. M., & McGarty, C. (2001). Iden­ never clicked this much with anyone in my
tifiability and self-presentation: Computer- life”: Trust and hyperpersonal communica­
mediated communication and intergroup tion in online friendships. New Media &
interaction. British Journal of Social Psy­ Society, 6, 487–506.
chology, 40, 399–416. Hesse, B. W., Werner, C. M., & Altman, I.
Ekman, P., & Friesen, W. V. (1975). Unmasking (1988). Temporal aspects of computer-
the face: A guide to recognizing emotions mediated communication. Computers in
from facial cues. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Human Behavior, 4, 147–165.
Prentice Hall. Hiltz, S. R., Johnson, K., & Agle, G. (1978).
Feenberg, A. (1989). A user’s guide to the prag­ Replicating Bales’ problem solving experi­
matics of computer mediated communica­ ments on a computerized conference: A pilot
tion. Semiotica, 75, 257–278. study (Research report no. 8). Newark:
Fussell, S. R., Kraut, R. E., & Siegel, J. (2000). New Jersey Institute of Technology,
Coordination of communication: Effects Computerized Conferencing and Com­
of shared visual context on collaborative munications Center.
work. In Proceedings of CSCW 2000 Hogg, M. A., & Abrams, D. (1988). Social
(pp. 21–30). New York: ACM Press. identifications: A social psychology of
24-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 7:35 PM Page 477

Nonverbal Dynamics in Computer-Mediated Communication–––◆–––477

intergroup relations and group processes. Krikorian, D. H., Lee, J., & Chock, T. M.
London: Routledge. (2000). Isn’t that spatial? Distance and
Hollingshead, A. B. (2000). Truth and lying in communication in a 2D virtual environ­
computer-mediated groups. In M. A. Neale, ment. Journal of Computer-Mediated
E. A. Mannix, & T. Griffith (Eds.), Communication, 5. Retrieved March 7,
Research in managing groups and teams 2006, from http://www.jcmc.indiana.edu/
(Vol. 3: Technology and teams, pp. 157– vol5/issue4/krikorian.html
173). Greenwich, CT: JAI Press. Lea, M. (1991). Rationalist assumptions in
Iacono, C. S., & Weisband, S. (1997). cross-media comparisons of computer-
Developing trust in virtual teams. mediated communication. Behaviour &
Proceedings of the 30th Annual Hawaii Information Technology, 10, 153–172.
International Conference on System Lea, M., & Spears, R. (1992). Paralanguage and
Sciences. Retrieved July 30, 2005, from social perception in computer-mediated
http://ieeexplore.ieee.org/search/wrapper communication. Journal of Organizational
.jsp?arnumber=665615 Computing, 2, 321–341.
Isotalus, P. (2003, November). Gender and Lea, M., Spears, R., & de Groot, D. (2001).
interface agents in the on-line news. Paper Knowing me, knowing you: Anonymity
presented at the annual meeting of the effects on social identity processes within
National Communication Association, groups. Personality and Social Psychology
Miami Beach, FL. Bulletin, 27, 526–537.
Jarvenpaa, S. L., Knoll, K., & Leidner, D. E. Lee, E. (2005). Effects of the influence agent’s
(1998). Is anybody out there? The implica­ sex and self-confidence on informational
tions of trust in global virtual teams. Journal social influence in computer-mediated com­
of Management Information Systems, 14, munication: Quantitative versus qualitative
29–64. presentation. Communication Research,
Jones, S. E., & LeBaron, C. D. (2002). Guest 32, 29–58.
editors’ introduction. Journal of Communi­ Liu, Y. L., Ginther, D., & Zelhart, P. (2002).
cation, 52, 499–521. An exploratory study of the effects of
Kalman, Y. M., & Rafaeli, S. (2005). Email frequency and duration of messaging on
chronemics: Unobtrusive profiling of impression development in computer-
response times. Proceedings of the 38th mediated communication. Social Science
Annual Hawaii International Conference Computer Review, 20, 73–80.
on System Sciences. Retrieved July 30, Markus, M. L. (1994). Finding a happy
2005, from http://ieeexplore.ieee.org/search/ medium: Explaining the negative effects of
wrapper. jsp?arnumber =1385456 electronic communication on social life at
Kiesler, S. (1987). Social aspects of computer work. ACM Transactions on Information
environments. Social Science, 72, 23–28. Systems, 12, 119–149.
Kiesler, S., Siegel, J., & McGuire, T. W. (1984). Nardi, B., Whittaker, S., & Bradner, E. (2000).
Social psychological aspects of computer- Interaction and outeraction: Instant mes­
mediated communication. American Psy­ saging in action. In Proceedings of the 2000
chologist, 39, 1123–1134. ACM Conference on Computer Supported
Kim, J., Kim, H., Tay, B. K., Muniyandi, M., Cooperative Work (pp. 79–88). New York:
Srinivasan, M. A., Jordan, J., et al. (2004). ACM Press. Retrieved March 1, 2006, from
Transatlantic touch: A study of haptic http://www.dis.shef.ac.uk/stevewhittaker/
collaboration over long distance. Presence, outeraction_cscw2000.pdf
13, 328–337. Nowak, K. L., & Biocca, F. (2003). The effect
Kraut, R. E., Fussell, S. R., & Siegel, J. (2003). of the agency and anthropomorphism on
Visual information as a conversational users’ sense of telepresence, copresence, and
resource in collaborative physical tasks. social presence in virtual environments.
Human-Computer Interaction, 18, 13–49. Presence, 12, 481–494.
24-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 7:35 PM Page 478

478–––◆–––Contexts and Consequences

Nowak, K., Watt, J. H., & Walther, J. (2005). The new media: Communication, research, and
influence of synchrony and sensory modality technology (pp. 129–156). Beverly Hills,
on the person perception process in computer CA: Sage.
mediated groups. Journal of Computer- Rice, R. E. (1990). Computer-mediated commu­
Mediated Communication, 10(3), article 3. nication system network data: Theoretical
Retrieved July 1, 2005, from http://jcmc. concerns and empirical examples. Inter­
indiana.edu/vol10/issue3/nowak.html national Journal of Man-Machine Studies,
Orlikowski, W. J., & Yates, J. (2002). It’s about 32, 627–647.
time: Temporal structuring in organiza­ Rice, R. E., & Case, D. (1983). Electronic
tions. Organization Science, 13, 684–700. message systems in the university: A
O’Sullivan, P. B. (2000). What you don’t know description of use and utility. Journal of
won’t hurt me: Impression management Communication, 33, 131–154.
functions of communication channels in Rintel, E. S., & Pittam, J. (1997). Strangers
relationships. Human Communication in a strange land: Interaction management
Research, 26, 403–431. on Internet Relay Chat. Human Com­
Pen, J., & Hancock, J. T. (2006). An analysis munication Research, 23, 507–534.
of socioemotional and task communication Rogers, P., & Lea, M. (2004). Cohesion in
in online multiplayer videogames. Com­ online groups. In K. Morgan, C. A.
munication Research, 33, 92–109. Sanchez, C. A. Brebbia, & A. Vioskounsky
Postmes, T., & Spears, R. (2002). Contextual (Eds.), Human perspectives on the Internet
moderators of gender differences and society: Culture, psychology and gender
stereotyping in computer-mediated group (pp. 115–124). Southampton, England:
discussions. Personality and Social WIT Press.
Psychology Bulletin, 28, 1073–1083. Sanderson, D. (1993). Smileys. Sebastopol, CA:
Postmes, T., Spears, R., & Lea, M. (1998). O’Reilly & Associates.
Breaching or building social boundaries? Short, J., Williams, E., & Christie, B. (1976).
SIDE-effects of computer-mediated com­ The social psychology of telecommunica­
munication. Communication Research, 25, tions. London: Wiley.
689–715. Siegel, J., Dubrovsky, V., Kiesler, S., & McGuire,
Postmes, T., Spears, R., Lea, M., & Reicher, S. T. W. (1986). Group processes in computer-
D. (2000). SIDE issues centre stage: Recent mediated communication. Organizational
developments in studies of deindividuation Behavior and Human Decision Processes,
in groups. Amsterdam: Royal Netherlands 37, 157–187.
Academy of Arts and Sciences. Sproull, L., & Kiesler, S. (1986). Reducing social
Reid, F. J. M., Ball, L. J., Morley, A. M., & context cues: Electronic mail in organiza­
Evans, J. S. B. T. (1997). Styles of group tional communication. Management
discussion in computer-mediated decision Science, 32, 1492–1512.
making. British Journal of Social Psy­ Steinfield, C. W. (1986). Computer-mediated
chology, 36, 241–262. communication in an organizational
Reid, F. J. M., Malinek, V., Stott, C., & Evans, setting: Explaining task-related and
J. S. B. T. (1996). The messaging threshold socioemotional uses. In M. L. McLaughlin
in computer-mediated communication. (Ed.), Communication yearbook 9 (pp. 777–
Ergonomics, 39, 1017–1037. 804). Beverly Hills, CA: Sage.
Rezabek, L. L., & Cochenour, J. J. (1998). Storck, J., & Sproull, L. (1995). Through a
Visual cues in computer-mediated commu­ glass darkly: What do people learn in video-
nication: Supplementing text with emoti­ conferences? Human Communication
cons. Journal of Visual Literacy, 18, Research, 22, 197–219.
210–215. Suler, J. (1999). The psychology of avatars and
Rice, R. E. (1984). Mediated group communica­ graphical space in multimedia chat commu­
tion. In R. E. Rice & Associates (Eds.), The nities, or, how I learned to stop worrying
24-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 7:35 PM Page 479

Nonverbal Dynamics in Computer-Mediated Communication–––◆–––479

and love my Palace props. Retrieved Walther, J. B., Loh, T., & Granka, L. (2005).
March 6, 2005, from http://www.rider Let me count the ways: The interchange of
.edu/~suler/psycyber/psyav.html verbal and nonverbal cues in computer-
Tidwell, L. C., & Walther, J. B. (2002). mediated and face-to-face affinity. Journal
Computer-mediated communication effects of Language and Social Psychology, 24,
on disclosure, impressions, and interper­ 36–65.
sonal evaluations: Getting to know one Walther, J. B., & Parks, M. R. (2002). Cues
another a bit at a time. Human Com­ filtered out, cues filtered in: Computer-
munication Research, 28, 317–348. mediated communication and relationships.
Walther, J. B. (1992). Interpersonal effects in In M. L. Knapp & J. A. Daly (Eds.),
computer-mediated interaction: A relational Handbook of interpersonal communication
perspective. Communication Research, 19, (3rd ed., pp. 529–563). Thousand Oaks,
52–90. CA: Sage.
Walther, J. B. (1996). Computer-mediated com­ Walther, J. B., Slovacek, C., & Tidwell, L. C.
munication: Impersonal, interpersonal, and (2001). Is a picture worth a thousand
hyperpersonal interaction. Communication words? Photographic images in long term
Research, 23, 3–43. and short term virtual teams. Communica­
Walther, J. B. (1997). Group and interpersonal tion Research, 28, 105–134.
effects in international computer-mediated Walther, J. B., & Tidwell, L. C. (1995).
collaboration. Human Communication Nonverbal cues in computer-mediated
Research, 23, 342–369. communication, and the effect of chrone­
Walther, J. B. (in press). Selective self-presenta­ mics on relational communication. Journal
tion in computer-mediated communication: of Organizational Computing, 5, 355–378.
Hyperpersonal dimensions of technology, Witmer, D., & Katzman, S. (1997). On-line
language, and cognition. Computers in smiles: Does gender make a difference in
Human Behavior. the use of graphic accents? Journal of
Walther, J. B., & Bunz, U. (2005). The rules of Computer-Mediated Communication, 2(4).
virtual groups: Trust, liking, and perfor­ Retrieved March 16, 2006, from http://
mance in computer-mediated commu­ www.jcmc.indiana.edu/vol2/issue4/wit
nication. Journal of Communication, 55, mer1.html
828–846. Woodworth, M. T., Hancock, J. T., & Goorha,
Walther, J. B., & Burgoon, J. K. (1992). Relational S. (2005). The motivational enhancement
communication in computer-mediated inter­ effect: Implications for our chosen modes
action. Human Communication Research, of communication in the 21st century.
19, 50–88. Proceedings of the 38th Annual Hawaii
Walther, J. B., & D’Addario, K. P. (2001). The International Conference on System
impacts of emoticons on message interpre­ Sciences. Retrieved July 30, 2005, from
tation in computer-mediated communica­ http://ieeexplore.ieee.org/search/wrapper
tion. Social Science Computer Review, 19, .jsp?arnumber=1385273
323–345.
24-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 7:35 PM Page 480
25-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 7:37 PM Page 481

25
NONVERBAL COMMUNICATION,
RACE, AND INTERGROUP
INTERACTION

� John F. Dovidio
University of Connecticut

� Michelle Hebl
Rice University

� Jennifer A. Richeson
Northwestern University

� J. Nicole Shelton
Princeton University

P erceiving others and oneself in terms of group identity influences


the way people interact with others. When group memberships are
salient, interactions are often guided by social roles (Eagly & Wood,
1999). To the extent that people rely on category-based, rather than
individual-based, processing in their interaction, their perceptions will
be influenced by group stereotypes and attitudes that can be activated
automatically and without full awareness. When people think of
themselves as members of a group, they view themselves not only as a

◆ 481
25-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 7:37 PM Page 482

482–––◆–––Contexts and Consequences

representative of that group but also as the hypothesized dynamics of intergroup iden­
embodiment of that group’s values, beliefs, tity and nonverbal behavior.
and interests (Turner, Hogg, Oakes, Reicher,
& Wetherell, 1987). Thus, interpersonal
interactions under these conditions become, ♦ Group Differences in
in essence, intergroup encounters. Because
Nonverbal Behavior:
of the importance of social identity in
everyday activity (Tajfel & Turner, 1979),
Status and Race
and the need to manage the complexity and
demands of social functioning (Fiske, Lin, In this section of the chapter, we provide
& Neuberg, 1999), such intergroup inter­ an overview of theoretical perspectives that
actions are common. suggest why and how members of majority
This chapter examines nonverbal behav­ and minority groups in general may differ
ior in an intergroup context. We begin by in the types of nonverbal behaviors they
considering how members of different display regularly. The focus, thus, is on sys­
groups, defined by majority and minority tematic group differences in nonverbal
status, may differ systematically in their behavior during intragroup interaction. We
intragroup nonverbal behavior. Next, we summarize the empirical literature building
focus on the dynamics of interactions between on Halberstadt’s (1985) comprehensive
members of different groups, identifying review of racial differences in nonverbal
important elements that can influence non­ behavior by drawing on more recent work.
verbal behavior. We present a general model Because of personal and group motiva­
of “mixed social interaction” and explore tions for esteem and social dominance, status
the role of nonverbal behavior in these inter­ is a fundamental dimension of intergroup
actions, considering the causes and conse­ encounters (see Burgoon & Dunbar, this
quences of these nonverbal behaviors. We volume). Power and status are relational
conclude with a discussion of the impor­ concepts that are often determined contex­
tance of understanding the reciprocal tually. For instance, a college senior may be
relationship between interpersonal and high status in an interaction with a college
intergroup interaction and the fundamental freshman but low status in a meeting with a
importance of nonverbal behavior in these professor. In general, high-status individu­
interactions. als may have more freedom of movement
Although we examine a range of inter­ and thus may be more open in their pos­
group contexts in this chapter, we illustrate tures and approach others more closely
the relationship between intergroup rela­ than do low-status individuals (Ellyson
tions and nonverbal behavior mainly within & Dovidio, 1985). Low-status individuals
the context of Black-White relations and may be more inhibited in their actions and
primarily within the cultural context of the monitor their partners more closely than do
United States. Even though there has been high-status individuals. Keltner, Gruenfeld,
substantial recognition of the importance of and Anderson (2003) hypothesize that high
culture (see Matsumoto, this volume) and power and status are associated with a gen­
gender (see Hall, this volume) for nonverbal eral approach orientation, whereas lower
behavior, the literature on nonverbal power and status are related to inhibition.
behavior and intergroup relations is rela­ Consistent with this view, individual sta­
tively sparse. Focusing on Black-White tus has been found to exert a strong influ­
relations thus helps provide a coherent ence on nonverbal behavior between people
test, within a defined context, of the (see Hall, Coats, & Smith LeBeau, 2005).
25-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 7:37 PM Page 483

Nonverbal Communication, Race, and Intergroup Interaction–––◆–––483

In a comprehensive meta-analytic review expression and nonverbal behaviors (Frable,


of the literature, Judith Hall and her col­ Blackstone, & Sherbaum, 1990).
leagues found that people who have higher When socially meaningful and enduring
status or social power showed greater facial groups, such as racial groups, are involved,
expressiveness, held more open postures cultural differences also need to be consid­
(with arms and legs), interacted with others ered to understand intergroup relations
at closer distances, and interrupted others and nonverbal behavior. Jones (1986), for
more often than did those with lower status example, observed that Black culture is
or power. Whereas Hall and her colleagues composed of both reactive and evolutionary
did not find systematic differences for components. The reactive component refers
touch, gesturing, postural relaxation, or to the collective adjustments U.S. Blacks
visual contact, it is possible that such have made to cope with oppression; the
behaviors can be tied to status but that the evolutionary component refers to aspects of
relationship is more complex. For instance, Black culture that represent “the unfolding
in the case of visual contact, lower status of a cultural core laid in an African past and
people look more while listening but less characterized in function, if not form, across
while speaking than do higher status people the cultures of the African Diaspora” (Jones,
(Dovidio & Ellyson, 1985). 1986, p. 294). In particular, Jones identified
Because societies are structured strongly key elements of many Blacks’ cultural orien­
in terms of group hierarchy, group identi­ tation, reflected in five dimensions: time,
ties also can produce relatively stable core rhythm, improvisation, oral expression, and
experiences of status. For example, in the spirituality. These cultural values guide a
United States, men have had higher status range of social behaviors, including nonver­
and greater social power traditionally than bal behaviors. E. T. Hall (1966) emphasized
have women, and Whites have had higher the importance of culture in understanding
status and greater social power than Blacks. racial differences in nonverbal behavior,
These chronic differences in status have hypothesizing that Black culture, relative
been hypothesized to produce consistent to that of Whites, reflects a closer and more
differences in the nonverbal behavior of “sensorially involved” orientation. These
minority- and majority-group members. ideas can be organized theoretically as the
The oppression hypothesis (Henley, 1977; cultural hypothesis. Both the oppression
LaFrance & Henley, 1994) posits that the hypothesis and the cultural hypothesis sug­
chronic stigmatization of certain groups gest different patterns of racial differences in
produces adaptations that lead members of nonverbal behavior.
oppressed groups to exhibit systematic non­ Systematic patterns of behavior in inter­
verbal skills and behaviors that differ from group relations can influence nonverbal
those of members of dominant groups and behavior and produce chronic racial differ­
that are functional for coping with their ences in nonverbal skill and behavior. In
low status. In particular, members of terms of skill at decoding other’s nonverbal
chronically oppressed groups are hypothe­ behavior, Halberstadt’s (1985) meta­
sized to be more sensitive and attentive to analysis of racial differences revealed that
their social environment, making them although Black children (ages 4–11)
better at decoding others’ nonverbal behav­ showed equivalent or slightly lower levels
iors and leading to higher levels of visual of decoding accuracy relative to Whites,
contact in social interaction; they also tend Black college students showed a higher
to be more vigilant and guarded, making level of accuracy than did White college
them more inhibited in their emotional students. Halberstadt interpreted these
25-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 7:37 PM Page 484

484–––◆–––Contexts and Consequences

results as consistent with the oppression same-race interactions, Blacks displayed sig­
hypothesis. In general, though, Whites and nificantly lower levels of visual contact than
Blacks are more accurate in decoding the did Whites. In addition, other studies of
nonverbal behavior of members of their adults (Smith, 1983) and children (Reid
own race than they are of other races et al., 1989), beyond the literature Halberstadt
(Bailey, Nowicki, & Cole, 1998; Weathers (1985) reviewed, have also shown that
et al., 2004). Greater intragroup than Blacks display lower levels of visual contact
intergroup accuracy is a function of greater than do Whites.
familiarity and more experience with in- In summary, the results of studies that
group members than with out-group mem­ examined racial differences in nonverbal
bers (Elfenbein & Ambady, 2002). behavior have revealed systematic differ­
Racial differences in nonverbal behav­ ences in spatial behavior, touch, and visual
iors have been studied most commonly in contact. The pattern of findings does not
terms of proxemics (interpersonal distance provide clear support for either the oppres­
and body orientation), touch, and visual sion hypothesis or Hall’s (1966) cultural
contact (gaze). Halberstadt’s (1985) review hypothesis, however. In particular, the
of studies on interpersonal distances main­ findings for spatial behavior (i.e., greater
tained between Blacks and between Whites interpersonal distance among Black than
showed no overall racial differences. Age White adults) are more consistent with
was an important moderating factor, oppression than with the cultural hypothe­
however. Halberstadt’s review revealed that sis, the results for touch (more touching by
Black children maintained closer interper­ Blacks than Whites) are more consistent
sonal distances than did White children, but with the cultural hypothesis, and the find­
Black adults maintained greater interper­ ings for visual behaviors (less eye contact by
sonal distance than did White adults. Reid, Blacks than by Whites) are inconsistent
Tate, and Berman (1989), however, found with both positions. Although racial differ­
that Black children do not always maintain ences in nonverbal behavior do not con­
closer distances than White children. They form uniformly to predictions derived from
found that Black children (ages 4–7) stood the oppression hypothesis or the cultural
farther away from an infant of the same race hypothesis, it is important to recognize that
than did White children. nonverbal behavior is highly sensitive to
Across the eight studies of body orienta­ social context. Racial differences may there­
tion reviewed by Halberstadt (1985), Blacks fore be more pronounced, apparent, and
exhibited a less direct body orientation than consistent in situations in which social
did Whites when interacting with others, and identity is salient, such as in dyadic interac­
this difference tended to increase with age. tions between members of different social
With respect to touch, Halberstadt’s review identity groups.
of eight studies revealed that Blacks touched
one another during their interactions more
often than Whites did. Finally, with respect ♦ Social Identity and
to visual contact, both the oppression Nonverbal Behavior in
hypothesis and E. T. Hall’s (1966) cultural Intergroup Interaction
hypothesis predict that Blacks will exhibit
higher levels of eye gaze than will Whites.
The results of Halberstadt’s (1985) analysis One of the most influential theories of
of eight visual contact studies, however, group influences on intergroup interaction
are inconsistent with both positions. In and nonverbal behavior is expectation
25-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 7:37 PM Page 485

Nonverbal Communication, Race, and Intergroup Interaction–––◆–––485

states theory (Berger, Wagner, & Zelditch, latter conditions, collective needs, goals,
1985; Correll & Ridgeway, 2003). Accord­ and standards are primary.
ing to this sociological framework, interac­ Social categorization and social identity
tions between members of different groups can influence the nature of communication,
are accompanied by differential expecta­ and thus nonverbal behavior, through a
tions about the status of the interactants in broad range of cognitive, affective, and
cases in which group membership is associ­ motivational mechanisms. Social catego­
ated systematically with prestige and status rization activates, often without awareness
in everyday life. Berger et al. (1985) or control, stereotypic associations that can
referred to characteristics of individuals influence expectations of the encounter and
that give rise to differential status expecta­ online attributions of the behavior of the
tions as “diffuse status characteristics.” other person in the interaction (see Lakin,
These expectations, in turn, can generalize this volume). In addition, the general evalu­
to a broad range of situations and, through ative biases that accompany recognition of
a process of behavioral confirmation of different group memberships can produce
expectancies similar to the self-fulfilling general approach or avoidance tendencies
prophecy, affect power-related behavior that are systematically reflected in nonver­
and perceptions across a variety of social bal behaviors. These prejudices may be bla­
contexts. Expectation states theory has tant or subtle (Gaertner & Dovidio, 1986),
received substantial empirical support with and people who have explicitly nonpreju­
respect to interaction between women and diced attitudes may still harbor implicit
men, and it has received some support in intergroup biases (Dovidio & Gaertner,
the realm of interracial behavior (see Berger 2004).
et al., 1985; Correll & Ridgeway, 2003). Recognition of different group member­
Whereas expectation states theory views ships in the interaction also typically arouses
the effect of group membership on interper­ intergroup anxiety (Stephan & Stephan,
sonal interaction as a relatively rational, 1985, 2000). Part of this anxiety may be due
albeit not necessarily conscious, process of to uncertainty about how to behave in this
evaluating the relative contributions of intergroup context, and part may be a func­
interactants in creating social organization, tion of feelings of real or symbolic threat. In
psychological perspectives have posited a addition to anxiety, intergroup interactions
much more pervasive and fundamental can arouse a number of different motiva­
influence of group membership and iden­ tions. For members of majority groups, for
tity. Social identity theory (Tajfel & Turner, instance, intergroup interactions can arouse
1979) and self-categorization theory (Turner dominance orientations (Sidanius & Pratto,
et al., 1987) view the distinction between 1999) or the desire to appear nonprejudiced
personal identity and social identity as (Gaertner & Dovidio, 1986). For members of
critical. According to these theories, when minority groups, the desire to detect and
personal identity is salient, a person’s potentially compensate for anticipated dis­
individual needs, standards, beliefs, and crimination by majority-group interaction
motives determine behavior. In contrast, partners may be activated (Miller & Myers,
when social identity is salient, “people 1998; Shelton, Richeson, & Salvatore, 2005).
come to perceive themselves as more inter­ In general, these positions suggest that
changeable exemplars of a social category majority-group members would be likely to
than as unique personalities defined by display nonverbal behaviors associated with
their individual differences from others” lower levels of liking or attraction and with
(Turner et al., 1987, p. 50). Under these higher levels of social dominance or power
25-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 7:37 PM Page 486

486–––◆–––Contexts and Consequences

than would minority-group members in In this model, Patterson explained and


intergroup interaction. Minority-group predicted changes in the levels of nonverbal
members would be expected to show non­ involvement that social interactants display
verbal behaviors associated with greater vig­ toward each other. More specifically,
ilance (e.g., greater sensitivity to nonverbal Patterson proposed that people approach
cues) and defensiveness or inhibition (e.g., each other with preexisting orientations
less direct orientation) more strongly than (antecedent conditions), such as personal fac­
would majority-group members. tors, experiential factors, and relational or sit­
Overall, there is some support for these uational factors, that can influence whether
expectations. For instance, young and and how they interact. These antecedent con­
middle-aged adults have been shown to ditions trigger preinteraction variables in the
patronize older adults by speaking in a context, such as cognitions and affective reac­
higher pitch and modified register similar to tions, levels of arousal, and behavioral
that used to address babies and pets propensities to act, that mediate the effect of
(Caporael, 1981; Caporael & Culbertson, these preexisting orientations and determine
1983; Kemper, Vandeputte, Rice, Cheung, whether an individual will engage in interac­
& Gubarchuk, 1995). Similarly, Whites tion. Once individuals decide to interact, they
show less intimate, attentive, and involved make functional judgments about an interac­
nonverbal behaviors with Blacks than with tion and determine the extent to which they
Whites (Feldman, 1985; Weitz, 1972; Word, should become behaviorally involved. In this
Zanna, & Cooper, 1974). In addition, Blacks stage, interactants also determine whether
tend to show heightened attentiveness and their expressed level and their partner’s
sensitivity to nonverbal cues of prejudice expressed level of involvement match their
(Richeson & Shelton, 2005; Rollman, expectations, which in turn influence future
1978). Nevertheless, because of the com­ cognitions and affect and can produce stable
plexity of intergroup interaction, generaliza­ or unstable exchanges. Ultimately, these
tions about differences in the behavior of interaction variables determine whether
members of majority and minority groups in interactants will terminate or continue the
intergroup interaction may obscure impor­ interaction.
tant dynamics and reciprocal relationships in Hebl and Dovidio (2005) broadened the
intergroup communication. Thus, in the next scope of Patterson’s (1982) model to address
section we present a model of interactions the uniquely complex interactions that occur
between majority- and minority-group between majority- and minority-group inter­
members that incorporates nonverbal com­ actants. This model is depicted in Figure
munication as part of a general interaction 25.1. The shaded areas represent the exten­
process. sions by Hebl and Dovidio (2005) from
Patterson’s (1982) model to address key ele­
♦ Interaction in an Intergroup ments of an intergroup interaction. In the
remainder of the chapter, we review each
Context: A Model of Mixed
component of the model (i.e., antecedents,
Social Interaction preinteraction mediators, and interaction
phase), addressing within each component
One of the first comprehensive models to both majority and minority interactants’ per­
articulate the components of nonverbal inter­ spectives. Where the data exist, we discuss
action was Patterson’s (1982) Sequential how each of the components influences or
Functional Model of Nonverbal Exchange. is influenced by nonverbal behaviors.
25-Manusov.qxd
Antecedents Pre-Interaction Mediators Interaction Phase

6/30/2006
S8
Type of Motivations
Stigma and Goals
S10
Personal Nonverbal

7:37 PM
S7 Behaviors
S3 Behavioral
Predisposition S6 Assessment of
S4 Meeting Goals
Experiential S2
S1 Affect and Verbal

Page 487
Arousal Assessment
S5 Behaviors of Interactant
Relational-
Situational Stereotypes
and Cognitions Coping Costs/Benefits
Strategies
S9
STIGMATIZER
Termination
TARGET T9
Relational- Coping Costs/Benefits
Situational Stereotypes Strategies
and Cognitions
Assessment
T1 Affect and T5 Verbal of Interactant
Experiential Arousal Behaviors

T2 T4 Assessment of
T10 Behavioral
T3 T6 Meeting Goals
Personal Predisposition T7 Nonverbal
Behaviors
Type of Motivations
Stigma and Goals
T8

Figure 25.1 Hebl and Dovidio’s (2005) Model of Mixed Social Interactions Based on Patterson’s (1982) Sequential Functional Model of Nonverbal

Exchange
487
25-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 7:37 PM Page 488

488–––◆–––Contexts and Consequences

ANTECEDENTS considerations when examining the way in


which interracial interactions unfold. Social
Majority- and minority-group interac­ norms and legal requirements differ across
tants bring different resources and back­ situations (Crandall & Eshleman, 2003),
ground experiences to social interactions. and thus they produce very different pat­
Three critical factors described by Patterson terns of nonverbal expression and interaction
(1982) are (a) personal, (b) experiential, outcomes. In general, Whites’ discrimination
and (c) relational-situational. The fourth against Blacks is less pronounced in situa­
factor, type of stigma, was identified by tions with stronger and clearer normative
Hebl and Dovidio (2005). These four fac­ expectations (see Dovidio & Gaertner,
tors lay the groundwork for creating an 2004). Thus, Whites tend to experience
ideology that individuals use to express greater discomfort and invest more effort
themselves verbally and nonverbally in in consciously regulating their behavior in
interactions. interracial interactions that are less struc­
Personal variables include differences in tured or in which they receive feedback
attitudes or ideologies that predispose that they may be responding in a biased
people to act in particular ways during an and inappropriate fashion (Richeson &
interaction. For example, Whites who are Trawalter, 2005).
more prejudiced toward Blacks will be more Individuals’ power in the interaction also
predisposed to behave more negatively influences their nonverbal displays. People
toward Blacks in the interaction (Dovidio, in power show less restraint from taking
Kawakami, & Gaertner, 2002). Similarly, action (Galinsky, Gruenfeld, & Magee,
Blacks with more in-group-favoring atti­ 2003), are more likely to express anger
tudes are more likely to avoid contact (Tiedens, 2000), and show greater variabil­
with Whites (Ashburn-Nardo, Knowles, & ity in their interactive behaviors than do
Monteith, 2003; Patchen, 1983). Further­ less powerful people (Guinote, Judd, &
more, individual differences in racial iden­ Brauer, 2002). Low power, by contrast,
tity may predispose Blacks to perceive the produces a tendency to inhibit responses
prejudice and discrimination displayed (Keltner et al., 2003). Furthermore, when
toward them as well as to shape their affec­ members of dominant groups are in a
tive and coping responses (Sellers & high-power position, implicit biases
Shelton, 2003). toward lower status partners become acti­
Experiential variables also serve as vated (Richeson & Ambady, 2003) and
antecedent conditions predicting nonverbal are expressed openly (e.g., explicit bias in
involvement. People who have more inter­ verbal evaluation) or more subtly (e.g.,
group contact tend to have more positive standoffish nonverbal behavior; Dovidio,
intergroup attitudes and display less anxiety Kawakami, Johnson, Johnson, & Howard,
in intergroup interaction (Pettigrew & 1997).
Tropp, 2000). In addition, Whites with What makes power even more influen­
more previous interracial contact showed tial in relationships is the finding that inter­
less cardiovascular evidence of threat during actants tend to assume complementarity in
interracial interactions (Blascovich, Mendes, behavioral interactions (Tiedens & Fragale,
Hunter, Lickel, & Kowai-Bell, 2001). 2003). That is, individuals exposed to dom­
Relational-situational variables, such as inant, powerful individuals tend to react
the social domain, the type of relationship, in very submissive ways, which reinforces
and power balances, are also critical the power imbalance. Similarly, individuals
25-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 7:37 PM Page 489

Nonverbal Communication, Race, and Intergroup Interaction–––◆–––489

exposed to submissive behavioral displays disentangled from each other clearly, we


tend to react by assuming more dominant discuss them separately to maximize the
stances. Such complementarity, which may readers’ understanding of each compo­
be present in some situations more than in nent’s potential contribution to nonverbal
others, serves to reinforce the power differ­ behavior during intergroup interactions.
entials that exist in mixed interactions Much of past research on the dynamics
(Dovidio, Brown, Keating, Heltman, & of interracial interaction can be captured
Ellyson, 1988). within the stereotypes and cognitions com­
In addition to personal, experiential, and ponent of the model. A vast amount is
relational or situational factors, the nature known about how Whites’ attitudes, expec­
of intergroup biases and relations differs tations, stereotypes, and prejudices regard­
substantially as a function of the types of ing racial minorities influence their
stigma attached to people (i.e., obesity, behavior during interracial interactions.
facial disfigurement, devalued racial group; Overall, this literature largely finds that
Hebl & Dovidio, 2005). Whereas Whites’ Whites often react negatively to Black inter­
attitudes toward Blacks are characterized action partners; however, Whites’ responses
by both negative attitudes and status differ­ are not necessarily simple and direct. For
entials, sexism involves strong subordinate instance, people’s unconscious (implicit)
role prescriptions for women but not attitudes, which are often measured with
antipathy toward women generally (Eagly response latency techniques (Fazio &
& Karau, 2002). In addition, some types of Olson, 2003), and their overt (explicit)
intergroup biases have a stronger emotional expressions of bias, which are measured
component than others (Stangor, Sullivan, with self-reports, frequently diverge
& Ford, 1991), and some biases (e.g., (Dovidio et al., 2002).
racism) are inhibited more strongly by In addition, interactants in intergroup
social norms than are others (e.g., hetero­ encounters not only possess stereotypes of
sexism; Crandall & Eshleman, 2003). members of other groups but also possess
Thus, the type of stigma can be an impor­ beliefs about how the members of other
tant antecedent factor that may determine groups will perceive them. Whites antici­
whether and how members of these differ­ pating interracial interaction often express
ent groups will interact. concerns that they will be viewed as preju­
diced (Vorauer, Main, & O’Connell,
1998). Similarly, Blacks anticipating inter­
PREINTERACTION MEDIATORS racial interactions often express concerns
that they will be stereotyped by Whites
Antecedent conditions influence the (Pinel, 1999). These beliefs about how they
preinteraction states with which Whites will be perceived can alter the behavior that
and ethnic minorities enter into interracial individuals display during intergroup inter­
interaction (see paths labeled S1 and T1 in actions (Shelton et al., 2005; Vorauer &
Figure 25.1). These states include stereo­ Turpie, 2004).
types and prejudices, affective reactions and There is considerable evidence that affect
arousal levels, behavioral predispositions and arousal, such as feelings of threat
and propensities to act, and motivations and anxiety, are also significant factors
and goals (Hebl & Dovidio, 2005). in mixed interaction (Hebl, Tickle, &
Although these interaction elements often Heatherton, 2000; Stephan & Stephan,
work together and cannot always be 2000). Negative affect may be particularly
25-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 7:37 PM Page 490

490–––◆–––Contexts and Consequences

likely to translate into the display of behavioral tendencies or predispositions,


nonverbal behaviors in an interaction (see which can influence verbal and nonverbal
path S2 of Figure 25.1). Individuals may be communication within an interaction (see
able to monitor their cognitions (e.g., path S2 of Figure 25.1). These behavioral
explicit attitudes and verbal behaviors) rel­ predispositions may be activated automati­
atively easily, but they may be less skilled at cally and without awareness in response
monitoring and controlling their affective to racial categorization. Chen and Bargh
reactions. Instead, affective reactions may (1997), for instance, demonstrated that
“leak out” through nonverbal and paraver­ White participants who were primed sub­
bal channels (Ekman, Friesen, & O’Sullivan, liminally with photographs of Blacks, com­
1988). Affective reactions may arise from pared with those primed with photographs
different processing modes than those that of Whites and those in a no-photograph
trigger cognitive reactions, such that affec­ control condition, behaved with more
tive reactions may be more experiential or hostility in a subsequent interaction with
immediate, whereas cognitive reactions may another White participant and elicited more
be more rational and deliberative. hostile behavior from the partner in return.
Whereas Whites’ anxiety may relate to Both context-dependent, as well as
increased cognitive demand associated with chronic, motivations and goals shape indi­
not wanting to appear biased (Dovidio & viduals’ responses to each other during
Gaertner, 2004; Richeson & Shelton, 2003; intergroup interactions. For example, Whites’
Richeson & Trawalter, 2005; Shelton, motivation to appear nonprejudiced in
2003), Blacks’ anxiety and arousal may be interracial interaction may be internally dri­
related to ways of coping with potential ven, based on personal standards of behav­
prejudice and discrimination, which may ior, as well as externally oriented, rooted in
involve greater vigilance and mindfulness a concern with social norms and sanctions
than would otherwise occur (Hyers & (Plant & Devine, 1998). The motivation to
Swim, 1998). Consistent with this possibil­ appear nonprejudiced can influence Whites’
ity, Ickes (1984) found that Blacks were nonverbal behavior in interracial interac­
particularly anxious during interactions tions (Shelton, 2003). Blacks, in contrast,
with White partners who generally avoid are often motivated to avoid the stigmatiza­
interracial contact. In addition, Tropp tion process altogether: Targets do not
(2003) found that ethnic minorities who want to be the target of stereotypes typi­
had reason to believe their White interac­ cally or to be devalued across social inter­
tion partners were prejudiced against their action contexts or be the recipient of
group experienced considerable anxiety in interpersonal rejection, social discrimina­
anticipation of the interaction. Similarly, tion, and financial disadvantage. Research
Shelton (2003) found that Blacks who were suggests, however, that concern about
led to believe that their interaction partners being the target of prejudice can sometimes
might be prejudiced fidgeted more during facilitate smooth interactions between
the interaction than Blacks who were not Whites and Blacks. Specifically, Whites
given the prejudice expectancy. experienced less negative affect and enjoyed
Both Blacks’ and Whites’ interaction interactions more with Black individuals
outcomes are influenced by the propensi­ who had been primed to expect prejudice
ties, the intentions, and the past ways in compared with Black individuals who were
which they have responded. That is, the not primed to expect prejudice (Shelton
antecedent conditions combine to create et al., 2005).
25-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 7:37 PM Page 491

Nonverbal Communication, Race, and Intergroup Interaction–––◆–––491

INITIAL INTERACTION PHASE These explicit and implicit attitudes are


dissociated frequently.
According to Hebl and Dovidio’s (2005) Moreover, implicit and explicit attitudes
model, the preinteraction mediators just are hypothesized to influence behavior
discussed influence the expression of verbal in different ways. Dovidio et al. (1997)
and nonverbal behaviors within a social proposed that explicit attitudes primarily
interaction (see paths S2 and T2 in Figure predict deliberative forms of behavior,
25.1). As discussed earlier, intergroup atti­ which people have the ability to formulate
tudes and stereotypes, because of the nega­ carefully, monitor, and control. Dovidio
tive feelings and assumptions of status et al. argued further that implicit attitudes,
differences embedded in them, influence in contrast, predict spontaneous behaviors,
both verbal and nonverbal behaviors in which people have little or no ability to
intergroup interaction. Weitz (1972), for control and which typically occur without
example, demonstrated that Whites use awareness or reflection. Consistent with
colder voice tones for interactions with this hypothesis, Dovidio et al. (1997) found
Blacks than with Whites. Word et al. that explicit (i.e., self-reported) prejudice
(1974) found that Whites terminated inter­ predicted overt bias in how Whites judged
actions sooner with Blacks than with other and evaluated Blacks, but implicit prejudice
Whites, and they exhibited greater physical primarily predicted their nonverbal behav­
distance during the interactions. Similarly, iors reflecting anxiety (rate of blinking) and
Fugita, Wexley, and Hillery (1974) reported dislike (gaze aversion) in interactions with
that Whites maintained less eye contact and Blacks. McConnell and Leibold (2001)
shorter glances with Black interviewers also reported that Whites’ implicit racial
than with White interviewers. Feldman attitudes—but not their explicit racial
(1985) likewise reported that both White attitudes—predicted Whites’ speaking
and Black teachers behaved more positively time, speech errors, and speech hesitations,
with members of their own race than with and the attitudes tended to correlate with
members of the other race. Consistent with how much the Whites leaned away from
the status differences associated with their partner and seating distance during
race, Turkstra, Ciccia, and Seaton (2003) interracial interactions.
found that Whites tended to take the Hebl, Foster, Mannix, and Dovidio
floor more often, whereas Blacks tended (2002) found parallel results, with more
to answer more questions in intergroup evidence of bias for subtle and sponta­
conversations. neous behaviors than for overt and formal
Antecedent factors and preinteraction actions, for another type of intergroup bias,
mediators can not only influence the the prejudice of potential employers toward
expression of verbal and nonverbal behav­ gay men and lesbians. In this study,
iors in intergroup interaction but may also employers did not discriminate against con­
affect the correspondence between these federates portrayed as homosexual on for­
behaviors. Although many Whites report mal employment behaviors, such as
that they are nonprejudiced on self-report permission to complete a job application
measures, and presumably believe that they and callbacks for further consideration.
are not prejudiced at a conscious level, they Bias was expressed more subtly in employ­
commonly harbor negative feelings and ers’ interaction behaviors, however.
beliefs at an unconscious, implicit level Employers spent less time and used fewer
(Dovidio, Kawakami, & Beach, 2001). words when interacting with the stigmatized
25-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 7:37 PM Page 492

492–––◆–––Contexts and Consequences

applicants than with the nonstigmatized majority-group members, the less frequent
applicants. research conducted on minority-group
Systematic differences in communication members suggests that they, too, behave in
are also evident in other important types ways that influence intergroup interaction.
of intergroup encounters. Johnson, Roter, In particular, minority-group members
Powe, and Cooper (2004), for example, attempt to act in strategic ways to manage
found that physicians displayed greater ver­ intergroup interactions. These behaviors
bal dominance, less positive affect, and less may take a number of different forms, but
patient-centered communication with Black they are characterized generally as ways to
patients than with White patients. It is cope with actual or anticipated discrimina­
perhaps because of such differences in tion. Majority and minority members engage
communication orientation that same-race in coping strategies in intergroup interac­
interactions between physicians and patients tions. They are adjusting to different types of
(Cooper et al., 2003), and same-race inter­ threats, however, and thus the methods and
actions between teachers and students consequences of coping are quite different.
(Feldman & Donohoe, 1978) are experi­ As suggested by Gaertner and Dovidio
enced more positively than cross-race (1986) (see also Dovidio & Gaertner, 2004),
interactions. Whites commonly find interracial interac­
Importantly, subtle differences in these tion to be anxiety arousing and, thus, they
types of interaction behaviors can exert sig­ may avoid interracial interactions when pos­
nificant impact on the nature and outcomes sible. When they cannot avoid these inter­
of intergroup interactions. Specifically, non­ actions, they may become focused on not
verbal behavior is an important mechanism acting inappropriately, particularly in ways
in self-fulfilling prophecies. Word et al. that can be attributed to racism.
(1974) demonstrated that Whites exhibited Preoccupation with behaving in a non-
less immediate, more negative nonverbal prejudiced manner can further contribute to
behaviors when interviewing Black relative inconsistencies in Whites’ verbal and nonver­
to White confederates. In addition, they bal behaviors in interracial interaction. Hebl
showed that participants with interviewers and Dovidio (2005) found in their review
who showed low-immediacy behaviors that across a range of different types of
(behaviors mirroring Whites’ interactions interactions, stigmatizers’ (i.e., members of
with Blacks), compared with participants majority or socially dominant groups) dis­
with interviewers who displayed high- play of negative nonverbal behaviors was
immediacy behaviors, responded in a less frequently at odds with their verbal behav­
favorable and responsive way during the iors (see paths S4 and T4 in Figure 25.1). In
interaction and were judged by independent general, stigmatizers often report feeling pos­
raters as less suitable for the position. Thus, itively toward targets, whereas their nonver­
the nonverbal behaviors of the interviewers, bal and paraverbal behaviors indicate more
which reflected the differences displayed in negative reactions. This divergence between
interactions with Blacks and Whites, elicited self-reported favorable orientations and
complementary verbal and nonverbal respon­ negative nonverbal behaviors is typically
ses from the interviewees. This study illus­ observed for Whites in interracial interac­
trates the powerful role of nonverbal tions (see Crosby, Bromley, & Saxe, 1980).
behavior in perpetuating racial disparities. Because Whites may be concerned about
Whereas researchers have amassed a great acting in a prejudiced or otherwise inappro­
many insights regarding the perspective of priate way in interracial interactions
25-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 7:37 PM Page 493

Nonverbal Communication, Race, and Intergroup Interaction–––◆–––493

(Gaertner & Dovidio, 1986), they may focus Repeated disengagement may lead to a
the majority of their attention on managing more long-term strategy of disidentifica­
their verbal behaviors, which can be easier to tion, in which Black individuals disengage
monitor and control than nonverbal behav­ permanently from the domain of evalua­
iors. Moreover, to the extent that monitor­ tion. Disengagement may be one reason
ing and controlling of verbal responses why Blacks tend to be less emotionally
involve high cognitive demand, these activi­ expressive in interracial interaction than are
ties may actually facilitate the expression Whites (Ickes, 1984).
of more spontaneous responses (see also Alternatively, minority-group members,
Patterson, 1995). As a consequence, Whites such as U.S. Blacks, often cope with antici­
(and other types of stigmatizers) may be less pated discrimination by either being
adept at managing affect-driven behaviors particularly vigilant to cues of bias or com­
that occur in interactions spontaneously and pensating for potential bias. With respect to
without time for deliberation. ethnic minority/White relations, daily
Vorauer and Turpie (2004) found simi­ encounters with potential discrimination
lar effects in interactions between Canadian may lead ethnic minorities to interpreta­
majority- (White) and minority- (First tions that confirm and reconfirm that prej­
Nations) group members. Whereas higher udice exists and to label ambiguous
evaluative concerns reduced bias in intimacy- behaviors as discriminatory (Sellers &
building behaviors (e.g., eye contact, self- Shelton, 2003). Instead of monitoring for
disclosure) among high-prejudiced Whites, bias that is occurring, minorities can also
higher evaluative concerns interfered with compensate for potential bias before it has
intimacy-building behaviors among low- the opportunity to affect the interracial
prejudiced Whites. Vorauer and Turpie interactions in which they engage (Miller &
interpreted these results as low-prejudiced Myers, 1998). Specifically, when they are
Whites “choking” under the pressure of concerned about the potential bias of their
high evaluative concerns. White interaction partners, ethnic minority
Blacks’ coping strategies in interracial participants often engage in compensatory
interaction are directed generally at coping strategies, such as smiling and talking more,
with actual, perceived, or anticipated bias. to ward off potentially negative outcomes
Like Whites, Blacks may also avoid interra­ (Shelton et al., 2005). Thus, coping styles
cial interactions when possible (Patchen, can influence the assessment processes iden­
1983), but when avoidance is not an tified in the secondary process phase sys­
option, they can make use of several coping tematically (see Figure 25.1).
strategies, including disengagement, vigi­
lance, and compensation. Disengagement
involves limiting the extent to which feel­ SECONDARY INTERACTION PHASE
ings of self-worth are dependent on feed­
back within the interaction. If Blacks After individuals have exchanged verbal
disengage, they do not allow the biases of and nonverbal behaviors, they are likely to
Whites to influence them in substantially assess the interaction in an attempt to con­
negative, or at least direct, ways. As a con­ tinue or terminate it. In deciding which
sequence, Blacks may be less responsive course to pursue, both individuals often
to feedback, either positive or negative, engage in an assessment of their goals, the
from Whites than are Whites in interac­ other interactant, and their outcomes. In
tions (Major, Quinton, & McCoy, 2002). terms of assessing one’s goals, interactants,
25-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 7:37 PM Page 494

494–––◆–––Contexts and Consequences

for example, might examine their social behaved in the interaction were essentially
goals (e.g., did they make a good impres­ uncorrelated. Other researchers have
sion on their interaction partner?) or their shown similarly that Whites’ implicit inter­
task-oriented goals (e.g., did they get the group attitudes and stereotypes, of which
job or other outcome they sought?). In they have limited awareness, predict the
terms of assessing the interactant, interac­ impressions that ethnic minorities form of
tants both evaluate and judge each other them during interactions (Fazio, Jackson,
actively during the interaction (e.g., what Dunton, & Williams, 1995; McConnell
does the other person think of me and my & Leibold, 2001; Sekaquaptewa, Espinoza,
contributions?). Thompson, Vargas, & von Hippel, 2003).
The dissociation of majority-group Because of heightened awareness and
members’ explicit and implicit attitudes and anticipated rejection in interracial interac­
their consequent effect on verbal and non­ tions, Whites tend to overestimate the
verbal behavior can produce a significant extent to which racial minorities will per­
divergence in their self-assessments and how ceive their behavior as friendly (Vorauer
they are evaluated by their interaction part­ & Sakamoto, 2005). Furthermore, both
ners. Dovidio et al. (2002) showed that Whites and Blacks often misinterpret anxi­
Whites’ explicit attitudes and verbal behav­ ety-related behaviors, such a shorter gaze
iors were related, whereas their implicit atti­ durations and more frequent self-touching,
tudes and nonverbal behaviors were related. as signals of unfriendliness more frequently
Specifically, Whites’ explicit racial attitudes in interracial than in intraracial encounters
predicted the positivity of their verbal com­ (Devine & Vasquez, 1998; Dovidio &
munications with Black interaction part­ Johnson, 2005). This vigilance may pro­
ners, but their implicit racial attitudes duce more accurate sensitivity to racial bias
predicted the positivity of their nonverbal among Blacks, however (Rollman, 1978).
communication. In addition, Whites based Richeson and Shelton (2005) found, for
their impressions of how friendly they instance, that Black judges (college student
behaved during their interracial interactions participants) were, on average, better able
on the attitudes that were accessible (i.e., to detect both the explicit and the implicit
their explicit attitudes) and the behaviors racial bias levels of White individuals from
that they could readily monitor (i.e., their 20 seconds of their nonverbal behavior dur­
verbal behaviors). Because most of the ing interracial interactions than were White
Whites in the study perceived themselves as judges. Specifically, Black judges’ ratings
nonprejudiced, they generally believed that of how positively a sample of White targets
they behaved in a friendly and unbiased behaved during an interracial interaction
manner toward their Black partners. were more highly correlated (albeit nega­
Blacks, in contrast, relied on their White tively) with those targets’ automatic racial
interaction partners’ nonverbal behavior in bias scores than were the ratings made by
making their assessments of how friendly White judges. Furthermore, Black judges’
their partners behaved. Because Whites’ ratings of the White targets’ prejudice levels
nonverbal behavior was correlated with were more highly correlated with those
their implicit attitudes that were, on aver­ targets’ explicit prejudice scores than were
age, negative, Blacks often left the interac­ the same ratings made by White judges.
tion with a negative perception of their In addition to assessing their goals and
White partners. Thus, Whites’ and Blacks’ their interaction partner, individuals
assessments of how the White person engage in ongoing assessment of the costs
25-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 7:37 PM Page 495

Nonverbal Communication, Race, and Intergroup Interaction–––◆–––495

and benefits of the interaction (see Figure ♦ Implications of the


25.1). This assessment can be very con­ Model for Intergroup
scious and deliberative, but it can also Interaction Outcomes
involve the use of relatively nonconscious
global impressions. The assessment of costs
and benefits involves not only the likeli­ In this chapter, we made use of Hebl and
hood of gaining (or losing) tangible out­ Dovidio’s (2005) model of mixed social
comes or resources but also the cost of interactions to explore how nonverbal
self-presentation. In addition, as we noted behavior may be expressed and interpreted
earlier, whereas the assessment of major­ during intergroup interactions. Our exami­
ity-group members may be based on the nation of each phase of the interaction
more overt aspects of the exchange, minor­ reveals how susceptible intergroup interac­
ity-group members may weigh more subtle tions are to misunderstandings, if not cate­
behaviors in their evaluation of the costs gorically negative outcomes. Nonverbal
and benefits of continuing the interaction. behavior thus can be a critical element of
If the ratio indicates net personal gains for interpersonal relations that reflects and
the interactant, he or she may choose to reinforces intergroup relations.
continue the interaction, whereas the accu­ With respect to race relations, in particu­
mulation of personal losses may lead to lar, as our analysis has revealed, Whites’ self-
interaction termination. consciousness in interracial interaction may
lead them to focus primarily on the control­
lable aspects of their behavior, such as the
verbal content of their speech, but to increase
♦ Interaction Continuation
the signs of discomfort and other negative
or Termination
states they exhibit nonverbally. Because they
interpret their behavior based on the behav­
Majority- and minority-group interaction iors they can monitor most easily, Whites
partners decide ultimately, either unilater­ tend to overestimate how favorably they are
ally or consensually, whether to continue or appearing. In contrast, Blacks, because of
to terminate the interaction. This critical their self-consciousness, may be particularly
decision is based largely on the verbal and vigilant to cues of Whites’ bias in these inter­
nonverbal behaviors that have been dis­ actions. As a result, they may rely on
played during the interaction and individu­ nonverbal behavior primarily, largely dis­
als’ assessments of themselves, of their counting the verbal content, in forming
partners, and of the costs and benefits of impressions of Whites and the interaction.
the interaction. The continuation of an Given their vigilance to cues of bias, Blacks
interaction is a negotiation in which both are likely to attribute their White partners’
interactants contribute. Disparities may negative nonverbal behavior to racial bias
exist, however; for instance, one interac­ (Dovidio & Johnson, 2005; Richeson &
tant’s strong desire to continue the interac­ Shelton, 2005). Thus, racial distrust influ­
tion may overwhelm the other interactant’s ences how Blacks interpret Whites’ nonver­
weak desire to end the interaction. In addi­ bal behavior, and the discrepancy between
tion, because higher status people often Whites’ overt expressions and their nonver­
exert more control over conversation and bal behavior reinforces this distrust.
interaction, they may be more influential in Furthermore, the different reliance on ver­
determining the length of the interaction. bal and nonverbal behavior by Whites and
25-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 7:37 PM Page 496

496–––◆–––Contexts and Consequences

Blacks in forming their impressions can lead involved in intergroup as well as intragroup
to vastly divergent views not only about their interactions. Building on Patterson’s (1982)
interpersonal relations but also ultimately to Sequential Functional Model of Nonverbal
race relations in general. These dynamics pro­ Exchange, we presented a model of mixed
vide insight into why Blacks and Whites view social interaction (Hebl & Dovidio, 2005)
race relations so differently. For instance, in that outlines key elements in the dynamics
the United States, whereas most (69%) of of communication in an intergroup context.
Whites perceive that Blacks are treated “the Thus, although nonverbal behavior can be
same as Whites”, the majority of Blacks studied in terms of separate encoding and
(59%) report that Blacks are treated worse decoding processes, the dynamic nature
than Whites (Gallup Organization, 2002). of nonverbal communication can best be
Understanding the role of nonverbal behav­ studied during interactions. Unfortunately,
ior in interaction can thus provide funda­ studies of actual intergroup interaction,
mental insights for understanding and including measures of verbal and nonverbal
improving intergroup relations. behavior, remain all too rare. Yet we believe
that this type of research, along with appro­
priate theoretical development, is crucial to
understanding the important role of nonver­
♦ Conclusion
bal behavior in intergroup communication
and ultimately intergroup relations.
Despite its obvious practical importance
and theoretical value, nonverbal behavior
in intergroup contexts is a curiously under­ ♦ References
studied topic. In 1985, Halberstadt observed
specifically that “the first research on race
and socioeconomic differences in nonverbal Ashburn-Nardo, L., Knowles, M. L., &
Monteith, M. J. (2003). Black Americans’
communication was conducted in the
implicit racial associations and their impli­
1930s . . . , but interest in these issues was
cations for intergroup judgment. Social
not sustained until the early 1970s” Cognition, 21, 61–87.
(p. 228). Our review of the literature Bailey, W., Nowicki, S., Cole, S. P. (1998).
reveals that the interest in race and nonver­ The ability to decode nonverbal informa­
bal communication peaked in the 1970s; tion in African American, African, and
research activity on this topic has waned Afro-Caribbean and European American
since then. The bulk of this seminal work adults. Journal of Black Psychology, 24,
on nonverbal behavior, race, and inter­ 418–431.
group interaction focuses on the contention Berger, J., Wagner, D. G., & Zelditch, M.
that majority- and minority-group members, (1985). Introduction: Expectation states
perhaps because of differences in status, theory. In J. Berger & M. Zelditch
Jr. (Eds.), Status, rewards, and influence
social power, and stigmatization, are likely
(pp. 1–71). San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.
to develop different styles of communica­
Blascovich, J., Mendes, W. B., Hunter, S. B., Lickel,
tion, reflected in nonverbal and verbal B., & Kowai-Bell, N. (2001). Perceiver threat
behaviors. The data, however, are sugges­ in social interactions with stigmatized individ­
tive but not yet conclusive. uals. Journal of Personality and Social
We propose that a comprehensive under­ Psychology, 80, 253–267.
standing of nonverbal behavior requires a Caporael, L. R. (1981). The paralanguage of care-
consideration of the complex processes giving: Baby talk to the institutionalized
25-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 7:37 PM Page 497

Nonverbal Communication, Race, and Intergroup Interaction–––◆–––497

aged. Journal of Personality and Social Advances in experimental social psychol­


Psychology, 40, 876–884. ogy (Vol. 36, pp. 1–51). San Diego, CA:
Caporael, L. R., & Culbertson, G. H. (1983). Academic Press.
Verbal response modes of baby talk and Dovidio, J. F., & Johnson, J. (2005). Racial bias
other speech at institutions for the aged. in interpreting cues of anxiety. Unpublished
Language and Communication, 6, 99–112. data, University of Connecticut, Storrs.
Chen, M., & Bargh, J. A. (1997). Nonconscious Dovidio, J. F., Kawakami, K., & Beach, K.
behavioral processes: The self-fulfilling (2001). Implicit and explicit attitudes:
consequences of automatic stereotype Examination of the relationship between
activation. Journal of Experimental Social measures of intergroup bias. In R. Brown &
Psychology, 33, 541–560. S. L. Gaertner (Eds.), Blackwell handbook of
Cooper, L. A., Roter, D. L., Johnson, R. L., social psychology, Vol. 4: Intergroup rela­
Forde, D. E., Steinwachs, D. M., & Powe, tions (pp. 175–197). Oxford, UK: Blackwell.
N. R. (2003). Patient-centered communi­ Dovidio, J. F., Kawakami, K., & Gaertner, S. L.
cation, ratings of care, and concordance of (2002). Implicit and explicit prejudice
patient and physician race. Annals of and interracial interaction. Journal of
Internal Medicine, 139, 907–915. Personality and Social Psychology, 82,
Correll, S. J., & Ridgeway, C. L. (2003). 62–68.
Expectation states theory. In J. Delamater Dovidio, J. F., Kawakami, K., Johnson, C.,
(Ed.), Handbook of social psychology (pp. Johnson, B., & Howard, A. (1997). On the
29–51). New York: Kluwer Academic/ nature of prejudice: Automatic and con­
Plenum. trolled process. Journal of Experimental
Crandall, C. S., & Eshleman, A. (2003). A justi­ Social Psychology, 33, 510–540.
fication suppression model of the expression Eagly, A. H., & Karau, S. J. (2002). Role congruity
and experience of prejudice. Psychological theory of prejudice toward female leaders.
Bulletin, 129, 414–446. Psychological Review, 109, 573–598.
Crosby, F., Bromley, S., & Saxe, L. (1980). Eagly, A. H., & Wood, W. (1999). The origins
Recent unobtrusive studies of black and of sex differences in human behavior:
white discrimination and prejudice: A liter­ Evolved dispositions versus social roles.
ature review. Psychological Bulletin, 87, American Psychologist, 54, 408–423.
546–563. Ekman, P., Friesen, W. V., & O’Sullivan, M.
Devine, P. G., & Vasquez, K. A. (1998). The (1988). Smiles when lying. Journal of
rocky road to positive intergroup relations. Personality and Social Psychology, 54,
In J. L. Eberhardt & S. T. Fiske (Eds.), 414–420.
Confronting racism: The problem and the Elfenbein, H. A., & Ambady, N. (2002).
response (pp. 234–262). Thousand Oaks, On the universality and cultural specificity
CA: Sage. of emotion recognition: A meta-analysis.
Dovidio, J. F., Brown, C. E., Keating, C. F., Psychological Bulletin, 128, 203–235.
Heltman, K., & Ellyson, S. L. (1988). Ellyson, S. L., & Dovidio, J. F. (1985). Power,
Power displays between women and men in dominance, and nonverbal behavior: Basic
discussions of gender-linked tasks: A multi­ concepts and issues. In S. L. Ellyson & J. F.
channel study. Journal of Personality and Dovidio (Eds.), Power, dominance, and
Social Psychology, 55, 580–587. nonverbal behavior (pp. 1–27). New York:
Dovidio, J. F., & Ellyson, S. L. (1985). Visual Springer-Verlag.
dominance behavior in humans. In S. L. Fazio, R. H., Jackson, J. R., Dunton, B. C., &
Ellyson & J. F. Dovidio (Eds.), Power, Williams, C. J. (1995). Variability in auto­
dominance, and nonverbal behavior (pp. matic activation as an unobtrusive measure
129–149). New York: Springer-Verlag. of racial attitudes: A bona fide pipeline?
Dovidio, J. F., & Gaertner, S. L. (2004). Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
Aversive racism. In M. P. Zanna (Ed.), 69, 1013–1027.
25-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 7:37 PM Page 498

498–––◆–––Contexts and Consequences

Fazio, R. H., & Olson, M. A. (2003). Implicit Hall, J. A., Coats, E. J., & Smith LeBeau, L.
measures in social cognition research: Their (2005). Nonverbal behavior and the verti­
meaning and uses. Annual Review of cal dimension of social relations: A meta­
Psychology, 54, 297–327. analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 131,
Feldman, R. S. (1985). Nonverbal behavior, 898–924.
race, and the classroom teacher. Theory Hebl, M. R., & Dovidio, J. F. (2005). Promoting
Into Practice, 24, 45–49. the “social” in the examination of social
Feldman, R. S., & Donohoe, L. F. (1978). stigmas. Personality and Social Psychology
Nonverbal communication of affect in Review, 9, 156–182.
interracial dyads. Journal of Educational Hebl, M. R., Foster, J., Mannix, L. M., &
Psychology, 70, 979–987. Dovidio, J. F. (2002). Formal and interper­
Fiske, S. T., Lin, M., & Neuberg, S. L. (1999). sonal discrimination: A field study under­
The continuum model: Ten years later. In standing of applicant bias. Personality and
S. Chaiken & Y. Trope (Eds.), Dual process Social Psychological Bulletin, 28, 815–825.
theories in social psychology (pp. 231– Hebl, M., Tickle, J., & Heatherton, T. F. (2000).
254). New York: Guilford. Awkward moments in interactions between
Frable, D. E. S., Blackstone, T., & Scherbaum, nonstigmatized and stigmatized individuals.
C. (1990). Marginal and mindful deviants In T. F. Heatherton, R. E. Kleck, M. R.
in social interactions. Journal of Personality Hebl, & J. G. Hull (Eds.), Stigma: Social
and Social Psychology, 59, 140–149. psychological perspectives (pp. 273–306).
Fugita, S. S., Wexley, K. N., & Hillery, J. M. New York: Guilford Press.
(1974). Black–white differences in nonver­ Henley, N. M. (1977). Body politics: Power,
bal behavior in an interview setting. sex, and nonverbal communication. Engle­
Journal of Applied Social Psychology, wood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall.
4, 343–350. Hyers, L. L., & Swim, J. K. (1998). A compari­
Gaertner, S. L., & Dovidio, J. F. (1986). The son of experiences of dominant and minor­
aversive form of racism. In J. F. Dovidio & ity group members during an intergroup
S. L. Gaertner (Eds.), Prejudice, discrimina­ encounter. Group Processes and Intergroup
tion, and racism (pp. 61–89). Orlando, FL: Relations, 12, 143–163.
Academic Press. Ickes, W. (1984). Compositions in black and
Galinsky, A. D., Gruenfeld, D. H., & Magee, J. C. white: Determinants of interaction in inter­
(2003). From power to action. Journal of racial dyads. Journal of Personality and
Personality and Social Psychology, 85, Social Psychology, 47, 330–341.
453–466. Johnson, R. L., Roter, D., Powe, N. R., &
Gallup Organization. (2002). Poll topics & Cooper, L. A. (2004). Patient race/ethnicity
trends: Race relations. Washington, DC: and quality of patient–physician communi­
Author. Retrieved October 3, 2004, from cation during medical visits. American
http://www.gallup.com/poll/topics/race.asp Journal of Public Health, 94, 2084–2090.
Guinote, A., Judd, C. M., & Brauer, M. (2002). Jones, J. M. (1986). Racism: A cultural analysis
Effects of power on perceived and objective of the problem. In J. F. Dovidio & S. L.
group variability: Evidence that more Gaertner (Eds.), Prejudice, discrimination,
powerful groups are more variable. Journal and racism (pp. 279–314). Orlando, FL:
of Personality and Social Psychology, 82, Academic Press.
708–721. Keltner, D., Gruenfield, D. H., & Anderson, C.
Halberstadt, A. G. (1985). Race, socioeconomic (2003). Power, approach, and inhibition.
status, and nonverbal behavior. In A. W. Psychological Review, 110, 265–284.
Siegman & S. Feldstein (Eds.), Multichannel Kemper, S., Vandeputte, D., Rice, K., Cheung,
integrations of nonverbal behavior (pp. 227– H., & Gubarchuk, J. (1995). Spontaneous
266). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. adoption of elderspeak during referential
Hall, E. T. (1966). The hidden dimension. New communication tasks. Journal of Language
York: Doubleday. and Social Psychology, 14, 40–59.
25-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 7:37 PM Page 499

Nonverbal Communication, Race, and Intergroup Interaction–––◆–––499

LaFrance, M., & Henley, N. M. (1994). On situations with infants: Effects of race
oppressing hypotheses: Or differences in and sex. Child Development, 60,
nonverbal sensitivity revisited. In H. L. 710–714.
Radtke & H. J. Stam (Eds.), Power/gender: Richeson, J. A., & Ambady, N. (2003). Effects
Social relations theory and practice of situational power on automatic racial
(pp. 287–311). London: Sage. prejudice. Journal of Experimental Social
Major, B., Quinton, W., & McCoy, S. (2002). Psychology, 39, 177–183.
Antecedents and consequences of attribu­ Richeson, J. A., & Shelton, J. N. (2003). When
tions to discrimination: Theoretical and prejudice does not pay: Effects of interracial
empirical advances. In M. P. Zanna contact on executive function. Psychological
(Ed.), Advances of Experimental Social Science, 14, 287–290.
Psychology (Vol. 34, pp. 251–330). New Richeson, J. A., & Shelton, J. N. (2005). Thin
York: Academic Press. slices of racial bias. Journal of Nonverbal
McConnell, A. R., & Leibold, J. M. (2001). Behavior, 29, 75–86.
Relations among the Implicit Association Richeson, J. A., & Trawalter, S. (2005). Why
Test, discriminatory behavior, and explicit do interracial interactions impair execu­
measures of racial attitudes. Journal of tive function? A resource depletion
Experimental Social Psychology, 37, account. Journal of Personality and Social
435–442. Psychology, 88, 934–947.
Miller, C. T., & Myers, A. M. (1998). Rollman, S. A. (1978). The sensitivity of Black
Compensating for prejudice: How heavy­ and White Americans to nonverbal cues
weight people (and others) control out­ of prejudice. Journal of Social Psychology,
comes despite prejudice. In J. K. Swim & 105, 73–77.
C. Stangor (Eds.), Prejudice: The target’s Sekaquaptewa, D., Espinoza, P., Thompson, M.,
perspective (pp. 191–218). San Diego, Vargas, P., & von Hippel, W. (2003).
CA: Academic Press. Stereotypic explanatory bias: Implicit
Patchen, M. (1983). Students’ own racial atti­ stereotyping as a predictor of discrimina­
tudes and those of peers of both races, as tion. Journal of Experimental Social
related to interracial behaviors. Sociology Psychology, 39, 75–82.
and Social Research, 68, 59–77. Sellers, R. M., & Shelton, J. N. (2003). The role
Patterson, M. L. (1982). A sequential functional of racial identity in perceived racial discrim­
model of nonverbal exchange. Psychological ination. Journal of Personality and Social
Review, 89, 231–249. Psychology, 84, 1079–1092.
Patterson, M. L. (1995). Invited article: A parallel Shelton, J. N. (2003). Interpersonal concerns
process model of nonverbal communication. in social encounters between majority
Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 19, 3–29. and minority group members. Group
Pettigrew, T. F., & Tropp, L. R. (2000). Does Processes & Intergroup Relations, 6,
intergroup contact reduce prejudice? Recent 171–185.
meta-analytic findings. In S. Oskamp (Ed.), Shelton, J. N., Richeson, J. A., & Salvatore, J.
Reducing prejudice and discrimination (2005). Expecting to be the target of preju­
(pp. 93–114). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. dice. Implications for interethnic interac­
Pinel, E. C. (1999). Stigma consciousness: tions. Personality and Social Psychology
The psychological legacy of social stereo­ Bulletin, 39, 1189–1202.
types. Journal of Personality and Social Sidanius, J., & Pratto, F. (1999). Social
Psychology, 76, 114–128. dominance: An intergroup theory of social
Plant, E. A., & Devine, P. G. (1998). Internal hierarchy and oppression. New York:
and external motivation to respond without Cambridge University Press.
prejudice. Journal of Personality and Social Smith, A. (1983). Nonverbal communication
Psychology, 75, 811–832. among Black female dyads: An assessment
Reid, P. T., Tate, C. S., & Berman, P. W. (1989). of intimacy and race. Journal of Social
Preschool children’s self-presentations in Issues, 39, 55–67.
25-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 7:37 PM Page 500

500–––◆–––Contexts and Consequences

Stangor, C., Sullivan, L. A, & Ford, T. E. Rediscovering the social group: A self-
(1991). Affective and cognitive determi­ categorization theory. Oxford, UK: Basil
nants of prejudice. Social Cognition, 9, Blackwell.
359–380. Vorauer, J., Main, K. J., & O’Connell, G. B.
Stephan, W., & Stephan, C. W. (1985). (1998). How do individuals expect to be
Intergroup anxiety. Journal of Social Issues, viewed by members of lower status groups?
41, 157–175. Content and implications of meta­
Stephan, W. G., & Stephan, C. W. (2000). An stereotypes. Journal of Personality and
integrated threat theory of prejudice. In Social Psychology, 75, 917–937.
S. Oskamp (Ed.), Reducing prejudice and Vorauer, J. D., & Sakamoto, Y. (2005).
discrimination: The Claremont Symposium I thought we could be friends, but
on Applied Psychology (pp. 23–45). . . . . Systematic miscommunication and
Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. defensive distancing as obstacles to cross-
Tajfel, H., & Turner, J. C. (1979). An integra­ group friendship formation. Unpublished
tive theory of intergroup conflict. In W. G. manuscript.
Austin & S. Worchel (Eds.), The social psy­ Vorauer, J. D., & Turpie, C. (2004). Disruptive
chology of intergroup relations (pp. 33–48). effects of vigilance on dominant group
Monterey, CA: Brooks/Cole. members’ treatment of outgroup members:
Tiedens, L. Z. (2000). Powerful emotions: The Choking versus shining under pressure.
vicious cycle of social status positions and Journal of Personality and Social Psy­
emotions. In N. Ashkanasy, W. Zerbe, & C. chology, 27, 706–709.
Hartel (Eds.), Emotions in the workplace: Weathers, M. D., Kitsantas, P., Lever, T.,
Research, theory, and practice (pp. 71–81). O’Brien, P., Campbell, S., & Rastatter, M.
Westport, CT: Quorum Books. (2004). Recognition accuracy and reaction
Tiedens, L. Z., & Fragale, A. R. (2003). Power time of vocal expressions of emotion by
moves: Complementarity in dominant and African-American and Euro-American col­
submissive nonverbal behavior. Journal of lege women. Perceptual and Motor Skills,
Personality and Social Psychology, 3, 99, 662–668.
558–568. Weitz, S. (1972). Attitude, voice, and behavior:
Tropp, L. R. (2003). The psychological impact A repressed affect model of interracial
of prejudice: Implications for intergroup interaction. Journal of Personality and
contact. Group Processes and Intergroup Social Psychology, 24, 14–21.
Relations, 6, 131–149. Word, C. O., Zanna, M. P., & Cooper, J.
Turkstra, L., Ciccia, A., & Seaton, C. (2003). (1974). The nonverbal mediation of self-
Interactive behaviors in adolescent conver­ fulfilling prophecies in interracial interac­
sation dyads. Language, Speech, and tion. Journal of Experimental Social
Hearing Services in Schools, 34, 117–127. Psychology, 10, 109–120.
Turner, J. C., Hogg, M. A., Oakes, P. J.,
Reicher, S. D., & Wetherell, M. S. (1987).
26-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 5:21 PM Page 501

26
USES AND CONSEQUENCES OF
NONVERBAL COMMUNICATION
IN THE CONTEXT OF
ORGANIZATIONAL LIFE
� Martin S. Remland
West Chester University

N onverbal communication is a vital part of our personal and pro­


fessional lives. As the chapters in this section show, nonverbal
cues affect various measures of success in personal relationships, edu­
cation, health care, computer-mediated interactions, and intergroup
relations. It is no less important in the context of organizational life,
where face-to-face interaction with superiors, subordinates, and peers
consumes much of our time and energy. Some features of these routine
interactions—status differences, chain of command, division of labor,
measurable performance objectives—are unique to the organizational
context and create special communication challenges for the members
of any organization.
Many of these challenges arise from the natural intersection of context
and function. For example, one of the primary functions of nonverbal
communication is signaling one’s identity (Patterson, 1983; Remland,
2004). In the context of work life, people learn to manage the tension that
exists between expressing their individuality or their membership in some

◆ 501
26-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 5:21 PM Page 502

502–––◆–––Contexts and Consequences

group (e.g., unique styles of dress) and indi­ (e.g., “I’m telling you to do this”), the
cating their affiliation with the organization greater ambiguity of nonverbal displays
(e.g., dressing like everyone else). People also carries the potential to produce unintended
confront and cope with discriminatory prac­ consequences that interfere with the goals
tices based on a person’s race, sex, age, and of an organization. Moreover, because we
so forth. are generally less aware of these “below
In this chapter, I discuss those challenges the radar” signals, the consequences may
about which there is sufficient empirical be especially difficult to avoid. The section
research of use to scholars and practition­ below addresses three possible conse­
ers. The first major set of challenges, which quences of status cues: dysfunctional lead­
stems most directly from the identification ership, sexual harassment, and workplace
and relationship functions of nonverbal discrimination.
communication, is how to manage the dis­
plays of status that are such an integral part
of organizational life. The second major set STATUS DISPLAYS AND
of challenges deals with the emotion func­ DYSFUNCTIONAL LEADERSHIP
tion and focuses on the impact of emotional
Social exchange theory provides a useful
exchanges on task performance. The third
framework for appreciating how status
major set of challenges addresses the
differentials between leaders and followers
delivery function, which involves the
can result in a leader’s loss of power (Blau,
coordination and integration of verbal and
1964; Jacobs, 1970; see also Burgoon &
nonverbal channels of communication.
Dunbar, this volume). Based on social
Table 26.1 offers one view of how these
exchange theory principles, a leader can lose
communication challenges emerge from the
influence over subordinates when subordi­
interplay of context and function. The table
nates begin to assess their relationship with
also serves to identify and organize much of
a leader as more costly than rewarding—
the research that has been done on the uses
that is, in a state of disequilibrium. A social
and consequences of nonverbal communi­
exchange view of leadership emphasizes the
cation in organizations.
interactive or transactional nature of the
leadership process. In each superior-subor­
♦ Managing Nonverbal dinate interaction, an exchange of resources
is negotiated in a way that is perceived as
Displays of Status
equitable by both parties. Effective com­
munication allows the leader to keep the
Research suggests that the nonverbal com­ exchange in a state of equilibrium. Jacobs
munication of high-status persons differs in (1970) suggests, for example, that supervi­
fundamental ways from that of lower-sta­ sors avoid interactions “that make evident
tus persons (Andersen & Bowman, 1990; power or status differentials” (p. 237).
Burgoon & Dunbar, this volume; Edinger The interactive nature of social exchange
& Patterson, 1983; Hall, 2005). These non­ theory suggests that the nonverbal commu­
verbal displays of status serve a useful func­ nication of both superior and subordinate
tion: They clarify and reinforce the role should shape perceptions of leadership, as
relationships that exist in an organization, the status displays of either person can steer
helping to sustain the organization’s hierar­ the relationship into a state of disequilib­
chy. But unlike verbal reminders of status rium (Remland, 1981). Whereas there is no
26-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 5:21 PM Page 503

Uses and Consequences of Nonverbal Communication–––◆–––503

Table 26.1 A Functional View of Nonverbal Communication Research in Organizations

Function Definition Communication Challenges Outcomes

Identification Identification of self Overcoming stereotypes Workplace


as individual or group discrimination
member
Balancing desire for
individuality and need
for conformity

Relationship Creation and Managing displays of Leadership


maintenance of status and power perceptions
relations based on
control and intimacy
Negotiating relational Organizational
expectations (friend, boss, climate
coworker, romantic partner)
Job satisfaction
Sexual harassment

Emotion Expression and Managing emotional Job stress


recognition of displays (e.g., emotional
emotions contagion, emotional labor)
Job satisfaction
Burnout
Motivation
Productivity
Accurately inferring Rapport
emotional states

Delivery Coordination and Developing speaking and Performance


integration of verbal listening competencies assessments
and nonverbal (expectancy effects; mixed
Credibility
channels messages; involvement
judgments
behavior)
Leadership
perceptions
Productivity

lack of research on the effects of a leader’s however, Remland (1984) produced four
nonverbal cues (e.g., Heintzman, Leathers, videotapes of the same two male actors,
Parrott, & Cairns, 1993; Remland, 1984; role-playing a scene in which a superior
Richmond & McCroskey, 2000), there is reprimands his subordinate. Although the
little regarding the impact of a subordi­ script was the same in each role play, the
nate’s nonverbal behavior. In one study, actors altered their nonverbal cues so that
26-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 5:21 PM Page 504

504–––◆–––Contexts and Consequences

each had a high-status and a low-status person spoke. As the researchers point out,
performance. In the high-status (HS) perfor­ “this seemingly paradoxical pattern is
mance, they used a relaxed posture, indirect understandable if the [high-status] person is
body orientation, loud voice, inattentive motivated to downplay his or her own sta­
behavior, and an act of spatial invasion. In tus in the service of comfortable social
the low-status (LS) performance, they used a interaction by (as one example) encourag­
tense posture, direct body orientation, soft ing the partner to speak more” (Hall &
and hesitating speech, and attentive gaze. Friedman, 1999, p. 1088). But, despite the
Not surprisingly, judges rated the supe­ benefit of minimizing status differentials,
rior as more considerate when he used LS some research suggests that leaders often
behaviors than when he used HS behaviors. prefer asymmetrical relations. For example,
But they also rated him as more considerate Yukl (2002) reports studies showing a pos­
when the subordinate he interacted with itive correlation between ingratiation by
used HS behaviors rather than LS behav­ subordinates and leaders’ liking for those
iors. This finding suggests that observers subordinates. Moreover, a recent study
may perceive a male leader as more consid­ found that persons in low-status positions
erate when status differentials are reduced, often choose low-status behavior, which
either from a reduction in the leader’s dis­ may be more “comfortable” for them,
plays of status, or an increase in the subor­ when interacting with higher-status persons
dinate’s. Perhaps the leader receives some in task-oriented encounters (Tiedens &
“credit” for allowing or encouraging subor­ Fragale, 2003).
dinates to behave in a high-status manner, The maintenance of status differentials is
empowering them in the process. Unfortu­ not just about exchange, however. It is also
nately, however, the results of this experi­ a cultural artifact. Hofstede (1982) main­
ment do not permit any inferences about tains that cultures classified as high in
how subordinates judge such interactions “power distance” tend to embrace authori­
with their superiors, a central component in tarian values and encourage actions that
the social exchange hypothesis. perpetuate status distinctions. For example,
Some research indicates that the nonver­ Kowner and Wiseman (2003) asked
bal communication of leaders frequently Japanese and American participants to imag­
includes a mix of high- and low-status cues ine various interactions between high-status
that reduces the differential that exists and lower-status individuals. Although
between them and their subordinates. In there was considerable agreement on the
one study, Hall and Friedman (1999) found specific behaviors differentiating high- from
that higher-status persons spoke more, used low-status persons, the magnitude of the dif­
more hand gestures, and leaned forward ferences varied, with Japanese, a more hier­
less than did lower-status persons. But the archical, collectivistic, and high-context
higher-status persons also nodded more people, reporting greater differences than did
frequently. Unlike the first set of behaviors, Americans, representing a more egalitarian,
all of which signal higher status, head nod­ individualistic, and lower context society.
ding, which implies attentiveness, agree­ Thus, what seems “excessive” in one culture
ment, or the desire for approval, tends to may seem quite ordinary in another.
signal lower status. The challenge of managing displays of
One especially interesting finding in the status is complicated further by the effects
study was that the greater the disparity was of sex-role stereotyping and the correspond­
between the high-status person and the ing claim that sex constitutes a diffuse status
low-status person, the less the high-status characteristic, with women viewed as lower
26-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 5:21 PM Page 505

Uses and Consequences of Nonverbal Communication–––◆–––505

in status (Lockheed & Hall, 1976). A sizeable judged as less effective by males than were
body of research shows how status takes root male speakers who used the same task style.
in the communication behavior of men and Moreover, when female speakers injected
women. Specifically, and although contested some warmth and friendliness into their
in the larger literature, research shows that presentations (a social style) they were
women’s nonverbal communication differs more persuasive with male judges than
to some degree from that of men’s along the when they used the “cooler” task style; this
dimension of status and power (Hall, 1984, was not true for the male speakers. The
this volume; Henley, 1995). But researchers male judges also rated female speakers
also find that many of these differences dis­ using the task style as less likeable and more
appear when women assume positions of threatening than the male speakers who
leadership or possess levels of power equal to used the same style. Although the results of
those of men. That is, the influence of author­ this study show that women were, in this
ity and power on nonverbal communication case, better off using a task-oriented style
may be greater than that of gender (Johnson, than one that highlights feminine (submis­
1994; Dovidio, Ellyson, Keating, Heltman, sive) or masculine (dominant) traits, it still
& Brown, 1988). In addition, women’s reveals the presence of a double standard:
nonverbal cues become more “powerful” For the same performance, women appar­
than men’s when men and women work ently get less credit from men than do their
together on “feminine” tasks (Dovidio, male counterparts.
Brown, Heltman, Ellyson, & Keating, 1988). Almost certainly there is some link
Research also shows that women use more between nonverbal displays of status and
assertive (high status) nonverbal communica­ leadership success. Social exchange theory
tion, such as a more confident tone of voice, raises one possibility for this link: Dysfuncti­
when they interact with superiors and subor­ onal leadership results from asymmetrical
dinates compared with their interactions with patterns of nonverbal communication in
peers (Steckler & Rosenthal, 1985). superior-subordinate interactions that favor
But women in leadership positions may the superior or the subordinate to excess,
still face the challenge of overcoming creating a state of disequilibrium. Yet despite
sex-role expectations. Carli, LaFleur, and the intuitive nature of this proposition, it has
Loeber (1995) compared the effectiveness of not yet been adequately tested. Research on
a task style of nonverbal communication gender differences in nonverbal communica­
(competent) with that of a dominant style, a tion suggests another possibility: that female
submissive style, and a social style (friendly leaders may be judged more harshly than
and competent). They prepared videotapes their male counterparts for using the same
of a male or female speaker using one of the displays of status. But researchers are still a
four styles to deliver the same persuasive long way from identifying systematically the
message to a seated listener. The judges who conditions under which organizational
watched the tapes were persuaded most by members are most likely to impose such
male and female speakers when those speak­ double standards.
ers used the task and social styles.
Contrary to expectations, female speak­
ers were not penalized more than male STATUS DISPLAYS AND
speakers were for using a dominant (i.e., SEXUAL HARASSMENT
masculine) style: Male and female speakers
using this style were equally ineffective. But Another potential workplace problem
female speakers who used a task style were involving nonverbal displays of status is
26-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 5:21 PM Page 506

506–––◆–––Contexts and Consequences

sexual harassment. Although many cases there is some degree of ambiguity in the
of harassment involve the deliberate abuse actions of either party (Jones & Remland,
of authority and power, other cases likely 1997; for an application to courtship con­
occur because one person misses or mis­ texts, see Noller, this volume).
reads the signals of another. For example, The danger of being misunderstood is
a recent experiment by Woodzicka and particularly acute in asymmetrical relation­
LaFrance (2005) demonstrates how a smile ships, where a nonverbal display of status
in response to sexually provocative ques­ can take on sexual connotations. A superior’s
tions, which do at times occur in the work­ use of immediacy behaviors—touching, star­
place, has the potential for prompting ing, and getting close, for example—has
inappropriate sexual conduct. Based on the long been the prerogative of higher-status
idea that women use social smiles for a individuals. But because these actions are
variety of reasons, they discovered that subject to multiple interpretations (e.g.,
female job applicants were more likely to friendliness, intimidation, sexual interest),
use “masking” smiles (concealing negative there is always the chance of misreading the
feelings) in response to questions such as signals (Le Poire, Burgoon, & Parrot,
“Do you have a boyfriend?” than in 1992). In addition, a subordinate’s use of
response to questions such as, “Do you submissive or low-status behaviors, such
have a best friend?” Interestingly, these as smiling, head nodding, silence, eye con­
smiling responses (coded by the researchers tact, and direct body orientation, can
as unfelt or “non-Duchenne” smiles) were make it equally difficult to tell whether
correlated with perceptions of the inter­ the subordinate welcomes the superior’s
viewer as sexist and sexually harassing. advances or is behaving like a subordinate
What is more, men were less able to read is expected to behave. Moreover, the ambi­
these uncomfortable smiles correctly than guity of nonverbal signals makes it possible
women were, and men who scored higher for harassers to deny the charges against
on an instrument that measures likelihood them (“I didn’t mean anything by it”).
to sexually harass were most likely to inter­ Nonverbal displays of status may also
pret the smiles as flirtatious. reveal whether someone is prone to engage
Explanations of sexual harassment usu­ in sexual harassment. Studies on the atti­
ally refer to the actions of both the perpe­ tudes, beliefs, and perceptions of persons
trator and the victim: inappropriate sexual likely to sexually harass show that such
behavior by the perpetrator alongside some persons tend to describe themselves in
form of resistance, or at least disapproval, ways that emphasize social and sexual
by the victim. Studies show that judgments dominance (Pryor, 1987). Some research
of whether an individual is guilty of sexual indicates that nonverbal displays of status
harassment depend on the actions of both may be symptomatic of persons likely to
parties (e.g., Jones & Remland, 1997). The sexually harass. In one study, participants
more people view a behavior as inappropri­ viewed silent clips of videotaped interviews
ate, and the more unwelcome the behavior of men being interviewed by an attractive
is, the more likely those people are to define female subordinate (who could not be seen
it as a case of sexual harassment. But stud­ by the viewers). Only observing the men’s
ies also show that men and women often nonverbal behavior, the participants were
don’t agree on what actions constitute sex­ able to predict which men scored high on
ual harassment. In general, men are less a test that measured likelihood to sex­
likely than women to see the same actions ually harass and which men scored low
as sexually harassing, particularly when (Driscoll, Kelly, & Henderson, 1998).
26-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 5:21 PM Page 507

Uses and Consequences of Nonverbal Communication–––◆–––507

In a follow-up study to Driscoll et al. “powerless stereotypes”—being seen as


(1998), male undergraduate participants, weak, incompetent, unreliable, lower class,
classified as high or low on likelihood to and so on—afflict people who share certain
sexually harass, were interviewed individu­ physical and behavioral attributes. Much of
ally by a female confederate posing as a high this research focuses on a person’s physical
school senior (Murphy, Driscoll, & Kelly, appearance. For instance, numerous studies
1999). To put the men in a more power­ document a beauty bias in the workplace
ful position relative to the interviewer, the that affords attractive individuals greater
researchers told them they would be evalu­ opportunities for success when compared
ating the performance of the female inter­ with less attractive persons, including
viewer following the interview. Men greater compensation (French, 2002;
classified as more likely to sexually harass Hammermesh & Biddle, 1994) and more
expressed greater dominance through their job offers (Marlowe, Schneider, & Nelson,
nonverbal behavior (e.g., less time in the 1996). Studies also confirm discriminatory
interview, less forward leaning, more indi­ practices against individuals who are obese
rect body orientation, and more direct eye (Cawley, 2004), short (Loh, 1993), and
contact) compared with those less likely to baby faced (Zebrowitz, 1997). Other stud­
sexually harass. Underscoring the idea that ies show differential treatment of light-
sexual harassment is more about power and skinned relative to darker-skinned Blacks.
control than it is about sexual attraction, the For example, using interview data from
researchers found no differences between Black adults as well as data obtained from a
the two groups in any nonverbal signals of national survey of Black (U.S.) Americans,
sexual interest (e.g., smiles, sexual glances). Kieth and Herring (1991) reported an
association between socioeconomic status
and skin-tone variations. Complexion was
STATUS DISPLAYS AND a stronger predictor of occupation and
WORKPLACE DISCRIMINATION income than factors such as family back­
ground, thus providing support for the view
In addition to dysfunctional leadership that darker Americans are less likely to ele­
and sexual harassment, nonverbal displays vate their status than are those with lighter
of status can lead to a form of differential complexions in the United States.
treatment that often escapes the awareness Much less research is available on the
of organizational members. This “invisi­ existence of a bias in the workplace against
ble” discrimination stems from the ten­ behaviors defined as low status. Whereas
dency to think less of someone whose numerous studies show discrimination
appearance or actions are indicative of low against persons with certain nonstandard
status. For example, an employer might not accents and dialects (Bradac, 1990), for
reject a job applicant consciously because example, these studies do not extend to any
the applicant is short in stature or has a discernable pattern of job discrimination
high-pitched voice. Yet these cues may still in terms of pay, promotion, or other such
influence the employer’s perception of the practices. One intriguing study, however,
applicant (e.g., “He isn’t very mature,” points to the possibility of a demeanor bias
“She doesn’t seem very confident”). against persons who exhibit signs of nega­
Some studies suggest a bias in the work­ tive affectivity typical of many bullying
place that downgrades individuals who, in victims (e.g., anxiety, sadness, defensive­
some way, communicate a low status iden­ ness, insecurity). In their study of city gov­
tity. In particular, evidence is available that ernment employees, Aquino and Bradfield
26-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 5:21 PM Page 508

508–––◆–––Contexts and Consequences

(2000) discovered that negative affectivity with this surge of interest in emotions gen­
correlated with the reporting of indirect erally has come a focused examination of
rather than direct forms of victimization nonverbal communication in three particu­
(e.g., sabotaging work). They suggest that lar areas: emotional contagion, emotional
displays of negative affect are more likely to labor, and emotion recognition.
elicit acts of contempt than of anger or
aggression, making persons who exhibit
such behavior “appear as vulnerable targets EMOTIONAL CONTAGION
for exploitation, gossip, and other less
obtrusive forms of mistreatment” (Aquino Emotional contagion refers to a phenom­
& Bradfield, 2000, p. 533). enon in which emotions spread from person
The research reviewed in this section to person. Primitive emotional contagion
supports a connection between nonverbal theory maintains that we “catch” others’
communication and various organizational emotions by means of automatic mimicry of
outcomes originating from the identification emotional expressions and the subsequent
and relationship functions of nonverbal feedback that results from our emotional
communication. In the context of organi­ displays (Hatfield, Cacioppo, & Rapson,
zational life, nonverbal displays of status 1994). Laboratory experiments show that
include an array of behaviors that symbolize mere exposure to a facial expression is suffi­
one person’s power over another. These dis­ cient to produce muscle contractions in
plays not only reinforce the role relation­ observers that mirror the expression they
ships that exist in an organization, but they see (Dimberg & Ohman, 1996). In addition,
also can create conditions that can lead to studies confirm the facial feedback hypothe­
dysfunctional leadership, sexual harass­ sis: that an individual’s facial expression of
ment, and workplace discrimination. an emotion can influence the person’s expe­
rience of that emotion directly and immedi­
ately (McIntosh, 1996).
♦ Managing Nonverbal The implications of facial feedback for
organizations has not escaped the attention
Displays of Emotion
of scholars, such as Goleman et al. (2002),
who consider it a ubiquitous process that
The preceding discussion addressed the uses leaders should harness for the good of the
and consequences of nonverbal communica­ organization. Among the studies they cite
tion in the service of an organization’s hier­ in their review is one where researchers
archy: maintaining the social order. In this observed 70 work teams across diverse
section, I direct attention to how nonverbal industries and found that members who sat
displays of emotion may affect the routine in meetings together ended up sharing
performances of organizational members. moods in a relatively short period of time
The subject of emotions in organizations is (Bartel & Saavedra, 2000). Goleman et al.
worthy of serious investigation (Fineman, (2002) claim that the more cohesive a work
1993), and a great deal of interest has been group is, the more contagious the emo­
shown specifically in the development of tional displays will be. Furthermore, they
“emotional intelligence” in the workplace, argue that leaders are most likely to control
as advocated fervently by Goleman (1998), the contagion that takes place because
Goleman, Boyatzis, and McKee (2002), group members generally see the leader’s
and Dulewicz and Higgs (2003). Along emotional reaction as the most valid
26-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 5:21 PM Page 509

Uses and Consequences of Nonverbal Communication–––◆–––509

response, and therefore, members tend to and suppressing a felt emotion are acts of
model their own reactions on the leader’s, emotional labor in the workplace, which
particularly in emotionally ambiguous situ­ Hochschild (1983) defined as “the manage­
ations. They also suggest that a leader’s ment of feeling to create a publicly observ­
ability to spread emotions depends on his able facial and bodily display [that] is sold
or her capacity to convey those emotions. for a wage [and] therefore has exchange
That is, a leader with a highly expressive value” (p. 7). The management of emo­
face, voice, and body is more likely to acti­ tions, according to Hochschild, requires
vate the emotional contagion process than a worker to engage in either surface acting
is a leader who is much less expressive. or deep acting. Whereas surface acting only
Emotional contagion may occur wher­ requires the actor to display an emotion
ever individuals work together in face-to­ with no attendant feelings, deep acting
face groups, or meet directly with the requires the actor to elicit the correspond­
public. But does it affect task performance? ing emotion in some way, as a method
Some research suggests that the spread of actor might do to prepare for an emotion­
positive emotions can boost the perfor­ ally charged scene. Curiously, whereas the
mance of work groups (Barsade, 2002), short-term effort involved in deep acting
predict job satisfaction among employees may surpass that needed for surface acting,
(Fisher, 2000), increase cooperation and the long-term effort required for the latter
minimize conflict (Barsade, 2002), improve appears to take a heavier toll (Grandey,
sales performance, and increase customer 2003; Totterdell & Holman, 2003).
satisfaction (Homburg & Stock, 2004; In her early research, Hochschild (1983)
Verbeke, 1997). Researchers have also dis­ estimated that “roughly one-third of
covered, however, that the spread of nega­ American workers have jobs that subject
tive emotions is a contributing factor to them to substantial demands for emotional
stress and burnout among physicians labor” (p. 11). Mann (1999) surveyed 12
(Bakker, Schaufeli, Sixma, & Bosveld, U.K. companies and found moderate levels
2001), nurses (Omdahl & O’Donnel, of emotional labor in almost two thirds of
1999), teachers (Bakker & Schaufeli, the communications reported by respon­
2000), and sales personnel (Verbeke, dents and high levels in about one third of
1997). Thus, studies show that emotional the reported communications. More than
contagion may have positive and negative half of the participants reported that they
effects on the health and well-being of an laughed or frowned, not because they
organization. wanted to but because they were expected
to. Sixty percent of the reported communi­
cations involved suppressing an emotion,
EMOTIONAL LABOR mostly anger. In addition, those higher up
in the organization reported less emotional
The contagion process depends on the labor than did those lower in the chain of
genuine (i.e., spontaneous) expression of command, supporting Van Maanen and
emotions. But the workplace often also Kunda’s (1989) astute observation that
demands that individuals engage in various “only the dominant and the dormant have
kinds of emotional dissimulation, pretend­ relative freedom from emotional con­
ing to be cheerful when really annoyed or straints in organizational life” (p. 55).
frustrated, for instance. Expressing an Early qualitative studies of flight atten­
unfelt emotion, exaggerating a felt emotion, dants, nurses, cashiers, and others led to a
26-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 5:21 PM Page 510

510–––◆–––Contexts and Consequences

conception of emotional labor as a multidi­ training and experience rather than their
mensional construct consisting of (1) the fre­ looks or demeanor.
quency, duration, and intensity of emotional
displays; (2) the variety of emotions dis­
played; (3) attentiveness to display rules; EMOTION RECOGNITION
and (4) the discrepancy between the felt and
the displayed emotion, referred to as emo­ Whereas the research on emotional con­
tional dissonance (Mann, 1999; Morris & tagion and emotional labor generally
Feldman, 1996). Subsequent surveys have focuses on the expression and regulation
identified emotional dissonance consistently of emotion, other studies have examined
as a strong predictor of job dissatisfaction, the recognition of emotion. The ability to
emotional exhaustion, depersonalization, recognize emotions in others is a mainstay
and other factors contributing to job in the research on interpersonal sensitivity,
burnout (Diefendorff & Richard, 2003; Lee which Riggio (2001, this volume) contends
& Ashforth, 1996; Pugliesi, 1999). There is is necessary for leadership success, person­
also evidence that emotional dissonance, nel functions of hiring and performance
particularly the suppression of negative appraisal, the development and functioning
emotions, can produce health consequences of work teams, and successful customer
related to prolonged stress (Maslach, 1982). service. Emotion recognition is also the
But the research also points to factors that most reliably valid component of emotional
moderate the impact of these negative intelligence (Elfenbein & Ambady, 2002).
consequences, such as job autonomy and Early research on nonverbal decoding
social support (Morris & Feldman, 1996; ability using the profile of nonverbal sensi­
Wharton, 1996). tivity (PONS) reported positive correlations
Although most of the research on emo­ between PONS scores and measures of job
tional labor highlights negative effects, effectiveness of foreign service officers,
some work identifies benefits that arise leadership skills of school principals, and
under certain conditions. For example, the job ratings of human service workers
use of “deep acting” and the regular display (Rosenthal, Hall, DiMatteo, Rogers, &
of positive emotions can result in decreased Archer, 1979). More recently, it has been
dissonance, improved performance, and linked to effective leadership in organiza­
increased satisfaction (Diefendorff & tions (Goleman et al., 2002).
Richard, 2003; Grandey, 2003; Totterdell But recent studies have also begun to
& Holman, 2003). Other researchers con­ raise questions about the benefits of emo­
tend that any requirement to display posi­ tion recognition, finding support for the
tive emotions leads ultimately to improved counterintuitive claim that “people reading”
performance (e.g., increased sales) and a has a downside. Using the diagnostic analy­
heightened sense of accomplishment (Rafaeli sis of nonverbal accuracy (DANVA),
& Sutton, 1987). In a qualitative study of Elfenbein and Ambady (2002) found that
sales workers, for example, Abiala (1999) the ability to read negative emotions con­
found that emotional labor was most likely veyed through the voice rather than the face
to produce positive effects when interacting damaged workplace evaluations received
with customers was a small part of the from peers and supervisors. The ability to
workers’day, there were few rules to fol­ pick up emotions from less controllable
low, the intent to sell was not concealed, nonverbal communication channels, which
and the workers were hired for their the researchers call “eavesdropping,” may
26-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 5:21 PM Page 511

Uses and Consequences of Nonverbal Communication–––◆–––511

burden individuals with difficult or unpleas­ confirm the importance of expectancy


ant information that was not meant to be effects, mixed messages, and nonverbal
shared. Similarly, Puccinelli and Tickle- involvement behaviors.
Degnen (2004), using the PONS, found a
negative correlation between emotional
eavesdropping (increased ability to read EXPECTANCY EFFECTS
body cues and decreased ability to read
facial cues) and ratings of rapport from an Rosenthal (1966) showed how an exper­
interaction partner. imenter’s nonverbal communication could
Scholarly interest in the emotional unwittingly prod human subjects into
domain of the workplace includes studies behaving the way the experimenter hoped
of emotional contagion, emotional labor, they would rather than the way they might
and emotion recognition. Whereas system­ in the absence of the experimenter’s influ­
atic studies in these areas lag behind the ence, a finding that demonstrated how
promise of improved organizational perfor­ a researcher’s nonverbal communication
mance, research confirms the short-term can damage the validity of a scientific
benefits of spreading positive emotions and experiment. Turning their attention to the
the likely costs of spreading negative emo­ classroom, Rosenthal and Jacobsen (1968)
tions, the potentially negative consequences discovered that a teacher’s nonverbal cues
of emotional labor, and, with the possible could produce a similar kind of self-fulfilling
exception of emotional eavesdropping, prophecy. Studies confirm that teachers’
the advantage of recognizing emotions in nonverbal cues influence the performance of
others. The next section highlights research students in ways that confirm the teachers’
on the delivery of verbal messages in expectations, assisting some students and
organizations—that is, the ability to coordi­ penalizing others (Babad, 1992). Expect­
nate and integrate multiple channels of ancies are probably present in a variety of
communication. workplace contexts (Eden, 2003; Rosenthal,
1994). For example, judges often get the
verdicts they expect after signaling their
♦ Managing the Delivery expectations to members of the jury (Blanck
& Rosenthal, 1992). In job interviews,
of Verbal Messages
employers have been found to elicit, inad­
vertently, the undesirable behavior they
Increasingly, organizational life seems to expect to see from a job applicant (Word,
involve less rather than more time in face­ Zanna, & Cooper, 1974).
to-face interactions, as technology offers Although there may be some deleterious
greater options for long distance communi­ effects from expectancies (e.g., groupthink),
cations. Yet despite these advances, text overall, research confirms that workplace
messages are not likely to replace the much performances improve in response to positive
richer, multichanneled communication envi­ expectancies, although these results seem to
ronment afforded by face-to-face exchanges apply more to military personnel than to
(see Walther, this volume). The challenge in business organizations, to male workers than
multichannel communication lies in being to females, and to workers for whom low
able to deliver messages with the desired expectations are initially held (McNatt,
effects. In the organizational context, stud­ 2000). According to Rosenthal (1994),
ies focusing on the delivery of messages efforts to cope with expectancy effects should
26-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 5:21 PM Page 512

512–––◆–––Contexts and Consequences

focus more on getting authority figures to persons were more likely to use positive
raise their expectations than on the more nonverbal cues, regardless of verbal con­
difficult task of having them monitor and tent. Apparently, low status prompts indi­
control their nonverbal cues. Some research viduals to follow a distinct set of rules that
shows, however, that training managers to emphasize clarity when delivering news,
hold higher expectations for their subordi­ and politeness when making requests.
nates, as well as to convey those expectations In their study of gender and mixed mes­
to the subordinates, is difficult at best (Eden sages, LaPlante and Ambady (2002) varied
et al., 2000; White & Locke, 2000). the performance feedback of a supervisor in
terms of content and delivery (tone of voice).
They found that male supervisors were most
MIXED MESSAGES successful when the content was negative
and their tone of voice was positive. In con­
One of the properties of communication trast, female supervisors were most success­
is the potential for sending mixed messages, ful with feedback that included positive
where a message conveyed across one content paired with a negative tone of voice.
channel (e.g., facial) may not be consistent Overall, however, a positive tone of voice
with messages conveyed across one or more was only more effective in the male dyads.
other channels (e.g., vocal, verbal). Early The results for verbal content are consistent
experiments revealed that message recei­ with sex-role expectations: People are more
vers may use a “weighted sum” method of likely to expect compliments from women
inferring the attitudes of a speaker, placing and criticism from men. The difference in
more weight on facial and vocal expression tone of voice seems to suggest that a negative
than on words (Mehrabian & Wiener, tone adds seriousness to the female supervi­
1967). In the workplace, Newcombe and sor’s feedback, reinforcing her legitimate
Ashkanasy (2000) reported that percep­ authority, whereas the positive tone softens
tions of a leader delivering feedback to sub­ the impact of criticism given to male subor­
ordinates were affected more by the leader’s dinates, making it less threatening.
facial expressions than by the verbal con­
tent of the message. Negative facial expres­
sions elicited the most unfavorable judgments NONVERBAL
of the leader. Other studies show that INVOLVEMENT BEHAVIOR
receivers weigh verbal and nonverbal mes­
sages when making inferences about polite­ Most research findings on the uses and
ness (LaPlante & Ambady, 2003; Trees & consequences of nonverbal communication
Manusov, 1998) and sexual harassment in formal workplace interactions, such as
(Remland & Jones, 1985). customer service transactions, interviews,
The uses and consequences of mixed and oral presentations, converge on the
messages may be related to status and gen­ construct of nonverbal involvement behav­
der. LaPlante (2001) found status differ­ ior. Gaze, body orientation, facial expres­
ences in the delivery of mixed messages. siveness, gesticulation, head nods, vocal
When delivering news, low-status persons animation, and more indicate the degree
were more likely than higher-status persons to which a person is overtly involved in
to use nonverbal communication that an interaction (Coker & Burgoon, 1987;
was consistent with their verbal content; Edinger & Patterson, 1983; see also
but when making requests, lower-status Andersen, Guerrero, & Jones, this volume).
26-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 5:21 PM Page 513

Uses and Consequences of Nonverbal Communication–––◆–––513

Generally, research shows that nonver­ presentations. One study found that profes­
bal involvement behavior leads to positive sional buyers rated a salesperson as more
outcomes. Richmond and McCroskey believable when he used a steady gaze,
(2000) found that perceptions of a supervi­ and more interesting and persuasive when
sor’s nonverbal immediacy behavior were he avoided speech hesitations (Leigh &
associated with favorable evaluations of the Summers, 2002). Awamleh and Gardner
supervisor, subordinate motivation, and (1999) manipulated the speech delivery of
job satisfaction. In customer service trans­ a bogus CEO and found, not surprisingly,
actions, studies generally show that the use that the CEO’s presentation was more effec­
of touch and eye contact generates more tive with eye contact, fluency, smiles, and
positive reactions from patrons (Crusco dynamic gestures. They also found that
& Wetzel, 1984; Kaufman & Mahoney, delivery was a more important predictor of
1999). Information gathering interviews performance than either the leader’s vision
also tend to benefit from involvement cues. or the organizational performance of the
Certain forms of touching and making eye leader’s company. Howell and Frost (1989)
contact with a person increase the likeli­ reported higher levels of task performance
hood that the person will comply with a and satisfaction when a leader’s delivery
request to participate in a survey (Hornik included vocal variety, eye contact, relaxed
& Ellis, 1988). Even apprehensive respon­ gestures, and animated facial expressions.
dents will talk more and like their inter­ Holladay and Coombs (1993, 1994) found
viewer more when the interviewer uses high that a leader’s nonverbal cues were more
levels of nonverbal involvement—direct predictive of charisma than was the “vision­
body orientation, forward lean, head nods, ary content” of the leader’s message.
backchannels, and gazing while listening— Furthermore, Kirkpatrick and Locke (1996)
as opposed to much lower levels (Remland documented that delivery boosted percep­
& Jones, 1989). tions of a leader’s charisma, but it did not
In the employment interview, research help the performance of subordinates.
confirms the positive impact of an applicant’s This section reviewed the research on the
nonverbal involvement behavior (Gifford, delivery function of nonverbal communi­
Cheuk, & Wilkinson, 1985; McGovern cation, which refers to the coordination and
& Tinsley, 1978; Young & Beier, 1977). integration of verbal and nonverbal mes­
Instances of such behavior may even predict sages. In the context of organizational life,
subsequent job performance evaluations researchers have been interested chiefly in
(DeGroot & Motowidlo, 1999). The impor­ outcomes associated with expectancy effects,
tance of nonverbal communication notwith­ mixed messages, and various nonverbal
standing, some research suggests that what involvement behaviors. In this regard, there
applicants say, the verbal content, influ­ is strong support for the claim that a
ences hiring decisions more than how they speaker’s nonverbal cues are often more
say it (Riggio & Throckmorton, 1988). In important than the speaker’s words (see also
addition, because social skills are more Giles & Le Poire, this volume). Specifically,
important for some types of jobs than through nonverbal channels, a speaker can
others, the impact of nonverbal communi­ signal positive or negative expectations,
cation may depend on the skills needed for modify the meaning of a verbal message,
a particular job (DePaulo, 1992). and influence the reactions and judgments
Research also recommends the use of of listeners. Studies show that in each of
nonverbal involvement behaviors in oral these ways, a speaker’s delivery can have a
26-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 5:21 PM Page 514

514–––◆–––Contexts and Consequences

powerful impact on various measures of behaviors in context, they frequently rely on


organizational performance. laboratory methods that fall short of meeting
the highest standards of ecological validity.
Given the powerful consequences of
♦ Conclusion nonverbal cues in organizations, particu­
larly those that promote some people and
not others, create unhealthy environments,
Research on the uses and consequences of and the like, more work is needed, despite
nonverbal communication in the context of these very real challenges to doing research
organizational life addresses in a limited in organizations. As well, much of the
way each of the four primary functions of research reviewed in this chapter assesses
nonverbal communication (see Table 26.1). only short-term outcomes, such as immedi­
As this chapter shows, studies focusing on ate reactions to a prerecorded oral perfor­
the identification and relationship functions mance. Longitudinal studies in particular
indicate that when mismanaged or misread, would provide much needed data about the
nonverbal displays of status can create con­ consequences for team leaders and work
ditions leading to dysfunctional leadership, groups of elusive phenomena such as status
sexual harassment, and workplace discrim­ differentials, emotional contagion, expec­
ination. Recent studies focusing on the tancy effects, and nonverbal involvement
emotion function confirm the potential behavior. Long-term study would also yield
benefits and costs of emotional contagion, critical insights into the organization-wide
emotional labor, and emotion recognition. consequences of nonverbal cues responsible
Lastly, studies concerned with the delivery for sexual harassment, workplace discrimi­
function (i.e., the coordination and integra­ nation, and emotional labor, with respect
tion of verbal and nonverbal messages) to law suits, lost productivity, absenteeism,
show that expectancy effects, mixed mes­ turnover, job burnout, and so forth.
sages and nonverbal involvement behaviors In recent years, researchers have demon­
can strengthen or weaken the impact of strated that the nonverbal cues contained in
workplace communications. “thin slices” (Ambady & Rosenthal, 1992;
Although the research reviewed in this see Gray & Ambady, this volume) of natu­
chapter clearly points to the importance of rally occurring social interactions (e.g.,
nonverbal communication in the workplace, 30-second segments) can predict long-term
the organizational environment continues to outcomes, as evidenced in studies of marital
be somewhat neglected by social scientists interactions that predict divorce (Carrere &
whose interest centers on nonverbal cues. Gottman, 1999) and teacher-student inter­
Riggio (2005) cites several reasons for this actions that predict end-of-term evalua­
neglect, among them the fact that organiza­ tions (Ambady & Rosenthal, 1993). In his
tions are reluctant to grant researchers access hugely popular book, Blink: The Power
to naturally occurring interactions for of Thinking Without Thinking, Malcolm
observation and analysis and the equally Gladwell (2005) has succeeded in bringing
important fact that nonverbal researchers a mass audience to the subject of thin slices.
themselves are guilty of not paying enough Perhaps organizations in the future will be
attention to business and organizational more receptive to researchers who are inter­
applications. Regrettably, when researchers ested in studying the impact of nonverbal
turn their attention to the study of nonver­ communication where a brief episode may
bal communication, instead of observing predict a big consequence.
26-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 5:21 PM Page 515

Uses and Consequences of Nonverbal Communication–––◆–––515

♦ References Blanck, P. J., & Rosenthal, R. (1992).


Nonverbal behavior in the courtroom. In
R. S. Feldman (Ed.), Applications of non­
Abiala, K. (1999). Customer orientation and sales verbal behavioral theories (pp. 167–190).
situations: Variations in interactive service Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
work. Acta Sociologica, 42, 207–222. Blau, P. M. (1964). Exchange and power in
Ambady, N., & Rosenthal, R. (1992). Thin social life. New York: Wiley.
slices of expressive behavior as predictors of Bradac, J. J. (1990). Language attitudes and
interpersonal consequences: A meta-analysis. impression formation. In H. Giles (Ed.),
Psychological Bulletin, 111, 256–274. Handbook of language and social psychol­
Ambady, N., & Rosenthal, R. (1993). Half a ogy (pp. 387–412). Oxford, England: Wiley.
minute: Predicting teacher evaluations from Carrere, S., & Gottman, J. (1999). Predicting
thin slices of nonverbal behavior and phys­ divorce among newlyweds from the first
ical attractiveness. Journal of Personality three minutes of a marital conflict discus­
and Social Psychology, 64, 431–441. sion. Family Process, 38, 293–301.
Andersen, P. A., & Bowman, L. (1990). Carli, L. L., LaFleur, S. J., & Loeber, C. C.
Positions of power: Nonverbal influence (1995). Nonverbal behavior, gender,
in organizational communication. In J. A. and influence. Journal of Personality and
DeVito & M. L. Hecht (Eds.), The nonver­ Social Psychology, 68, 1030–1041.
bal communication reader (pp. 391–411). Cawley, J. (2004). The impact of obesity on
Prospect Heights, IL: Waveland. wages. Journal of Human Resources, 39,
Aquino, K., & Bradfield, M. (2000). Perceived 451–475.
victimization in the workplace: The role of Coker, D. A., & Burgoon, J. K. (1987). The
situational factors and victim characteris­ nature of conversational involvement and
tics. Organization Science, 11, 525–537. nonverbal encoding patterns. Human
Awamleh, R., & Gardner, W.L. (1999). Per­ Communication Research, 13, 463–494.
ceptions of leader charisma and effective­ Crusco, A. H., & Wetzel, C. G. (1984). The
ness: The effects of vision content, delivery, Midas touch: The effects of interpersonal
and organizational performance. Leadership touch on restaurant tipping. Personality and
Quarterly, 10, 345–373. Social Psychology Bulletin, 10, 512–517.
Babad, E. (1992). Teacher expectancies and DeGroot, T., & Motowidlo, S. J. (1999). Why
nonverbal behavior. In R. S. Feldman (Ed.), visual and vocal interview cues can affect
Applications of nonverbal behavioral theo­ interviewers’ judgments and predict job per­
ries and research (pp. 167–190). Hillsdale, formance. Journal of Applied Psychology,
NJ: Erlbaum. 84, 986–993.
Bakker, A. B., & Schaufeli, W. B. (2000). Burnout DePaulo, P. J. (1992). Applications of nonverbal
contagion processes among teachers. Journal behavior research in marketing and manage­
of Applied Social Psychology, 30, 2289–2309. ment. In R. S. Feldman (Ed.), Applications of
Bakker, A. B., Schaufeli, W. B., Sixma, H. J., nonverbal behavioral theories and research
& Bosveld, W. (2001). Burnout contagion (pp. 63–87). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
among general practitioners. Journal of Diefendorff, J. M., & Richard, E. M. (2003).
Social and Clinical Psychology, 20, 82–98. Antecedents and consequences of emotional
Barsade, S. G. (2002). The ripple effect: display rule perceptions. Journal of Applied
Emotional contagion and its influence on Psychology, 88, 284–294.
group behavior. Administrative Science Dimberg, U., & Ohman, A. (1996). Behold the
Quarterly, 47, 644–675. wrath: Psychophysiological responses to
Bartel, C. A., & Saavedra, R. (2000). The facial stimuli. Motivation and Emotion,
collective construction of work group 20, 149–182.
moods. Administrative Science Quarterly, Dovidio, J. F., Brown, C. E., Heltman, K.,
45, 197–242. Ellyson, S. L., & Keating, C. F. (1988).
26-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 5:21 PM Page 516

516–––◆–––Contexts and Consequences

Power displays between women and men in Gladwell, M. (2005). Blink: The power of thinking
discussions of gender-linked tasks: A multi­ without thinking. New York: Little, Brown.
channel study. Journal of Personality and Goleman, D. (1998). Working with emotional
Social Psychology, 55, 580–587. intelligence. New York: Bantam.
Dovidio, J. F., Ellyson, S. L., Keating, C. F., Goleman, D., Boyatzis, R., & McKee, A. (2002).
Heltman, K., & Brown, C. E. (1988). The Primal leadership. Boston, MA: Harvard
relationship between social power and Business School Press.
visual displays of dominance between men Grandey, A. A. (2003). When “the show must
and women. Journal of Personality and go on”: Surface acting and deep acting
Social Psychology, 54, 233–242. as determinants of emotional exhaustion
Driscoll, D. M., Kelly, J. R., & Henderson, W. and peer-rated service delivery. Academy of
M. (1998). Can perceivers identify likeli­ Management Journal, 46, 86–96.
hood to sexually harass? Sex Roles, 38, Hall, J. A. (1984). Nonverbal sex differences:
557–588. Communication accuracy and expressive
Dulewicz, V., & Higgs, M. (2003). Leadership style. Baltimore, MD: Johns Hopkins
at the top: The need for emotional intelli­ University Press.
gence in organizations. The International Hall, J. A. (2005). Meta-analysis of nonverbal
Journal of Organizational Analysis, 11, behavior. In V. Manusov (Ed.), The source-
193–210. book of nonverbal measures (pp. 483–492).
Eden, D. (2003). Self-fulfilling prophecies in orga­ Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
nizations. In J. Greenberg (Ed.), Organiza­ Hall, J. A., & Friedman, G. (1999). Status, gender,
tional behavior: The state of the science and nonverbal behavior: A study of struc­
(pp. 91–122). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. tured interactions between employers of a
Eden, D., Geller, D., Gerwirtz, A., Gordon- company. Personality and Social Psychology
Terner, R., Inbar, I., Liberman, M., et al. Bulletin, 25, 1082–1091.
(2000). Implanting Pygmalion leadership Hammermesh, D., & Biddle, J.E. (1994). Beauty
style through workshop training: Seven and the labor market. American Economic
field experiments. Leadership Quarterly, Review, 84, 1174–1194.
11, 171–210. Hatfield, E., Cacioppo, J. T., & Rapson, R.
Edinger, J. A., & Patterson, M. L. (1983). (1994). Emotional contagion. Cambridge,
Nonverbal involvement and social control. UK: Cambridge University Press.
Psychological Bulletin, 93, 30–56. Heintzman, M., Leathers, D. G., Parrott, R. L., &
Elfenbein, H. A., & Ambady, N. (2002). Cairns, A. B. (1993). Nonverbal rapport-
Predicting workplace outcomes from the building behaviors’ effects on perceptions of
ability to eavesdrop on feelings. Journal of a supervisor. Management Communication
Applied Psychology, 87, 963–971. Quarterly, 7, 181–209.
Fineman, S. (1993). Emotion in organizations. Henley, N. M. (1995). Body politics revisited:
Newbury Park, CA: Sage. What do we know today? In P. J.
Fisher, C. D. (2000). Mood and emotions while Kalbfleisch & M. J. Cody (Eds.), Gender,
working: Missing pieces of job satisfaction? power, and communication in human
Journal of Organizational Behavior, 21, relationships (pp. 27–62). Hillsdale, NJ:
185–202. Erlbaum.
French, M. T. (2002). Physical appearance and Hochschild, A. (1983). The managed heart:
earnings: Further evidence. Applied Eco­ Commercialization of human feeling.
nomics, 34, 569–572. Berkeley: University of California Press.
Gifford, R., Cheuk, F. N., Wilkinson, M. Hofstede, G. (1982). Culture’s consequences
(1985). Nonverbal cues in the employment (abridged ed.). Beverly Hills, CA: Sage.
interview: Links between applicant qualities Holladay, S. J., & Coombs, W. T. (1993).
and interviewer judgments. Journal of Communicating visions: An exploration of
Applied Psychology, 4, 729–736. the role of delivery in the creation of leader
26-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 5:21 PM Page 517

Uses and Consequences of Nonverbal Communication–––◆–––517

charisma. Management Communication asymmetric dyads. Cross-Cultural Research,


Quarterly, 6, 405–427. 37, 178–210.
Holladay, S. J., & Coombs, W. T. (1994). LaPlante, D. A. (2001). Mixing up politeness
Speaking of visions and visions being theory: Channel consistency and conflict
spoken: An exploration of the effects of in the expression of politeness. (Doctoral
content and delivery on perceptions of dissertation, Harvard University, 2001).
leader charisma. Management Communi­ Dissertation Abstracts International, 62
cation Quarterly, 8, 165–189. (4-B), 2113.
Homburg, D., & Stock, R. M. (2004). The link LaPlante, D. A., & Ambady, N. (2002). Saying
between salespeople’s job satisfaction and it like it isn’t: Mixed messages from men
customer satisfaction in a business-to­ and women in the workplace. Journal of
business context: A dyadic analysis. Journal Applied Social Psychology, 32, 2435–2457.
of the Academy of Marketing Science, 32, LaPlante, D. A., & Ambady, N. (2003). On how
144–159. things are said: Voice tone, voice intensity,
Hornik, J., & Ellis, S. (1988). Strategies to verbal content, and perceptions of polite­
secure compliance for a mall intercept ness. Journal of Language and Social
interview. Public Opinion Quarterly, 52, Psychology, 22, 434–441.
539–551. Lee, R. T., & Ashforth, B. E. (1996). A meta­
Howell, J. M., & Frost, P. J. (1989). A laboratory analytic examination of three dimensions of
study of charismatic leadership. Organi­ job burnout. Journal of Applied Psychology,
zational Behavior and Human Decision 81, 123–133.
Processes, 43, 243–269. Leigh, T. W., & Summers, J. O. (2002). An ini­
Jacobs, T. O. (1970). Leadership and exchange tial evaluation of industrial buyers’ impres­
in formal organizations. Alexandria, VA: sions of salespersons’ nonverbal cues.
Human Resources Research Organization. Journal of Personal Selling and Sales
Johnson, C. (1994). Gender, legitimate authority, Management, 22, 41–53.
and leader-subordinate conversations. Le Poire, B. A., Burgoon, J. K., & Parrott, R.
American Sociological Review, 59, 122–135. (1992). Status and privacy restoring com­
Jones, T. S., & Remland, M. S. (1997). An munication in the workplace. Journal of
ounce of prevention: Suggestions for Applied Communication Research, 20,
training to prevent sexual harassment. In 419–436.
C. D. Brown, C. Snedeker, & B. Sykes Lockheed, M. E., & Hall, K. P. (1976).
(Eds.), Conflict and diversity (pp. 251– Conceptualizing sex as a status characteris­
266). Cresskill, NJ: Hampton Press. tic: Applications to leadership training strate­
Kaufman, D., & Mahoney, J. M. (1999). The gies. Journal of Social Issues, 32, 111–125.
effect of waitresses’ touch on alcohol Loh, E. S. (1993). The economic effects of phys­
consumption in dyads. Journal of Social ical appearance. Social Science Quarterly,
Psychology, 139, 261–267. 74, 420–438.
Kieth, V. M., & Herring, C. (1991). Skin Mann, S. (1999). Emotion at work: To what
tone and stratification in the black commu­ extent are we expressing, suppressing, or
nity. American Journal of Sociology, 97, faking it? European Journal of Work
760–779. and Organizational Psychology, 8,
Kirkpatrick, S. A., & Locke, E. A. (1996). Direct 347–369.
and indirect effects of three core charis­ Marlowe, C. M., Schneider, S. L., & Nelson,
matic leadership components on perfor­ C. E. (1996). Gender and attractiveness
mance and attitudes. Journal of Applied biases in hiring decisions: Are more experi­
Psychology, 81, 36–51. enced managers less biased? Journal of
Kowner, R., & Wiseman, R. (2003). Culture Applied Psychology, 81, 11–21.
and status-related behavior: Japanese and Maslach, C. (1982). Burnout: The cost of car­
American perceptions of interaction in ing. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall.
26-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 5:21 PM Page 518

518–––◆–––Contexts and Consequences

McGovern, T. V., & Tinsley, H. E. A. (1978). Rafaeli, A., & Sutton, R. I. (1987). Expression of
Interviewer evaluations of interviewee emotion as part of the work role. Academy
nonverbal behavior. Journal of Vocational of Management Review, 12, 23–37.
Behavior, 13, 163–171. Remland, M. S. (1981). Developing leadership
McIntosh, D. N. (1996). Facial feedback skills in nonverbal communication: A situa­
hypotheses: Evidence, implications, and tional perspective. Journal of Business
directions. Motivation and Emotion, 20, Communication, 18, 17–29.
121–147. Remland, M. S. (1984). Leadership impressions
McNatt, D. B. (2000). Ancient Pygmalion and nonverbal communication in a superior-
joins contemporary management: A meta­ subordinate interaction. Communication
analysis of the result. Journal of Applied Quarterly, 32, 41–48.
Psychology, 85, 314–322. Remland, M. S. (2004). Nonverbal communica­
Mehrabian, A., & Wiener, M. (1967). Decoding tion in everyday life (2nd ed.). Boston:
of inconsistent communications. Journal Houghton Mifflin.
of Personality and Social Psychology, 6, Remland, M. S., & Jones, T. S. (1985). Sex dif­
108–114. ferences, communication consistency and
Morris, J. A., & Feldman, D. C. (1996). The judgments of sexual harassment in a perfor­
dimensions, antecedents, and consequences mance appraisal interview. Southern Speech
of emotional labor. Academy of Manage­ Communication Journal, 50, 156–176.
ment Review, 21, 986–1010. Remland, M. S., & Jones, T. S. (1989). The
Murphy, J. D., Driscoll, D. M., & Kelly, J. R. effect of nonverbal involvement and com­
(1999). Differences in the nonverbal behav­ munication apprehension on state anxi­
ior of men who vary in the likelihood to ety, interpersonal attraction, and speech
sexually harass. Journal of Social Behavior duration. Communication Quarterly, 37,
and Personality, 14, 113–128. 170–183.
Newcombe, M. J., & Ashkanasy, N. M. (2002). Richmond, V. P., & McCroskey, J. C. (2000).
The role of affect and affective congruence The impact of supervisor and subordinate
in perceptions of leaders: An experimental immediacy on relational and organizational
study. Leadership Quarterly, 13, 601–615. outcomes. Communication Monographs,
Omdahl, B. L., & O’Donnel, C. (1999). 67, 85–95.
Emotional contagion, empathic concern, and Riggio, R. E. (2001). Interpersonal sensitivity
communicative responsiveness as variables and organizational psychology: Theoretical
affecting nurses’ stress and occupational and methodological applications. In J. A.
commitment. Journal of Advanced Nursing, Hall & F. J. Bernieri (Eds.), Interpersonal
29, 1351–1360. sensitivity: Theory and measurement
Patterson, M. L. (1983). Nonverbal behavior: (pp. 305–318). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
A functional perspective. New York: Riggio, R. E. (2005). Business applications of
Springer-Verlag. nonverbal communication. In R. E. Riggio
Pryor, J. B. (1987). Sexual harassment proclivi­ & R. S. Feldman (Eds.), Applications of
ties in men. Sex Roles, 17, 269–290. nonverbal communication (pp. 121–138).
Puccinelli, N. M., & Tickle-Degnen, L. (2004). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Knowing too much about others: Modera­ Riggio, R. E., & Throckmorton, B. (1988). The
tors of the relationship between eavesdrop­ relative effects of verbal and nonverbal
ping and rapport in social interaction. behavior, appearance, and social skills in
Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 28, evaluations made in hiring interviews.
223–243. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 18,
Pugliesi, K. (1999). The consequences of 331–348.
emotional labor: Effects on work stress, Rosenthal, R. (1966). Experimenter effects in
job satisfaction, and well-being. Motivation behavioral research. New York: Appleton­
and Emotion, 23, 125–154. Century-Crofts.
26-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 5:21 PM Page 519

Uses and Consequences of Nonverbal Communication–––◆–––519

Rosenthal, R. (1994). Interpersonal expectancy behavior (Vol. 11, pp. 43–103). Greenwich,
effects: A 30-year perspective. Current CT: JAI.
Directions in Psychological Science, 3, Verbeke, W. (1997). Individual differences in
176–179. emotional contagion of salespersons: Its
Rosenthal, R., Hall, J. A., DiMatteo, M. R., effect on performance and burnout.
Rogers, P. L, & Archer, D. (1979). Psychology and Marketing, 14, 617–637.
Sensitivity in nonverbal communication: Wharton, A. S. (1996). Service with a smile:
The PONS test. Baltimore: Johns Hopkins Understanding the consequences of emo­
University Press. tional labor. In C. L. McDonald &
Rosenthal, R., & Jacobson, L. (1968). C. Sirianni (Eds.), Working in the service
Pygmalion in the classroom. New York: society (pp. 91–112). Philadelphia: Temple
Holt, Rinehart & Winston. University Press.
Steckler, N. A., & Rosenthal, R. (1985). Sex White, S. S., & Locke, E. A. (2000). Problems with
differences in nonverbal and verbal com­ the Pygmalian effect and some proposed solu­
munication with bosses, peers, and subordi­ tions. Leadership Quarterly, 11, 389–415.
nates. Journal of Applied Psychology, 70, Woodzicka, J. A., & LaFrance, M. (2005).
157–163. Working on a smile: Responding to sexual
Tiedens, L. Z., & Fragale, A. R. (2003). Power provocation in the workplace. In R. E.
moves: Complementarity in dominant and Riggio & R. S. Feldman (Eds.), Applications
submissive nonverbal behavior. Journal of nonverbal communication (pp. 141–155).
of Personality and Social Psychology, 84, Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
558–568. Word, C. O., Zanna, M. P., & Cooper, J.
Totterdell, P., & Holman, D. (2003). Emotion (1974). The nonverbal mediation of self-
regulation in customer service roles: Testing fulfilling prophecies in interracial inter­
a model of emotional labor. Journal of action. Journal of Experimental Social
Occupational Health Psychology, 8, 55–73. Psychology, 10, 109–120.
Trees, A. R., & Manusov, V. (1998). Managing Young, D. M., & Beier, E. G. (1977). The role
face concerns in criticism: Integrating non­ of applicant nonverbal communication
verbal behaviors as a dimension of polite­ in the employment interview. Journal of
ness in female friendship dyads. Human Employment Counseling, 14, 154–165.
Communication Research, 24, 564–582. Yukl, G. (2002). Leadership in organizations.
Van Maanen, J., & Kunda, G. (1989). Real Upper Saddle River, NJ: Prentice Hall.
feelings: Emotional expression and organi­ Zebrowitz, L. A. (1997). Reading faces: Window
zational culture. In L. L. Cummings & to the soul? Boulder, CO: Westview Press.
B. M. Staw (Eds.), Research in organizational
26-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 5:21 PM Page 520
Part-V-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 5:25 PM Page 521

PART V

FINAL THOUGHTS
Part-V-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 5:25 PM Page 522

NONVERBAL COMMUNICATION
Basic Issues and Future Prospects

� Miles L. Patterson
University of Missouri, St. Louis

� Valerie Manusov
University of Washington

T he chapters in this Handbook reflect the breadth and vitality of


research on nonverbal communication. Looking across these chap­
ters, we see a range of sophisticated arguments regarding the commu­
nicative potential of nonverbal behavior. Because the authors have done
a commendable job representing the field, it is neither our intention in
this chapter, nor do we feel it desirable, to attempt a general commen­
tary on the chapters in this volume. Rather, we focus attention on a
select set of issues important for understanding the field today and for
anticipating its future direction. To appreciate where we are and where
we may be headed, however, it is useful to consider from whence we
came. So, first, we consider, in very broad strokes, how the field has
changed in the last 50 years.

522 ◆
Part-V-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 5:25 PM Page 523

Final Thoughts–––◆–––523

♦ Emerging Trends More important perhaps, focusing atten­


tion on one, isolated behavior may come
at the expense of a broader appreciation of
There are many threads shaping the current the complexity and subtlety of the larger
status of research and theory on nonverbal system of communication. This cue-based
communication (see Knapp, this volume). focus may have contributed to the popular­
Nevertheless, widespread and systematic ity of the decoding strategy in studying
research on nonverbal communication can be nonverbal communication (see Gray &
traced back to the period from the mid-1950s Ambady, this volume, who talk about
to the early 1960s with work on spatial methods for assessing particular stimulus
behavior (Sommer, 1959), gaze (Exline, displays that might be photographed or
1963), facial expressions (Ekman, 1965), and videotaped). Later, judgments of the dis­
vocal cues (Mahl, 1956). Not surprisingly, plays (e.g., overall affect, vocal comfort)
most of the early research focused on a single would inform researchers of the meaning
behavior at a time, providing a “channel” and impact of specific nonverbal behaviors.
approach to nonverbal communication. In contrast, the actual measurement of
Thus, researchers who studied interpersonal nonverbal patterns, reflected in an encoding
distance did not also measure gaze patterns, strategy, was typically more complicated.
and those who studied gaze patterns were not This was the case, not only because the
interested typically in facial expressions. As a nonverbal system is a multidimensional
result, the field was structured nominally one, but also because the instrumentation
around behaviors rather than issues or was much more limited as the field was first
processes in nonverbal communication. This developing. In the 1950s and 1960s, for
is still reflected in the organization of some example, videotaping equipment was
textbooks and scholarly volumes. In such expensive and relatively cumbersome.
books, there may be a chapter on spatial Although video cameras then might have
behavior, one on gaze, another on touch, been on rollers, they were hardly portable.
then vocal cues, and so on. Without such formidable and expensive
It is not surprising that this kind of orga­ pieces of equipment, the study of interactive
nization has been dominant in the study behavior required one or more observers
of nonverbal behavior. It is difficult to counting, estimating, or timing the relevant
describe and analyze a complicated system behaviors from behind an observation win­
of communication without segmenting the dow. Although it was possible to measure
content by behavior. But there are also more than one fluid behavior at time, it was
some problems with this approach. Although not a simple endeavor. The various options
certain components of this system can be employed in the decoding and encoding
relatively well represented by a single met­ strategies are discussed in Gray and
ric, others are much more complex. For Ambady’s chapter in this volume (see also
example, distance can be operationalized in the set of assessments in Manusov, 2005).
terms of feet or meters, gaze in terms of There were, however, important excep­
duration, and lean and body orientation in tions to the channel approach in the way
angular changes. This is not the case with that nonverbal communication was con­
highly complex and multidimensional ceptualized. For example, Argyle and
behaviors such as gestures, tone of voice, Dean’s (1965) nonverbal “intimacy” and
and, especially, facial expressions. Mehrabian’s (1969) “immediacy” both
Part-V-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 5:25 PM Page 524

524–––◆–––The SAGE Handbook of Nonverbal Communication

emphasized the multivariate nature of non­ by Remland, this volume), expectations for
verbal communication in a coordinated set normative behavior (see Robinson, this vol­
of behaviors. Conceptual recognition of the ume), and an awareness of what a particu­
multidimensional nature of nonverbal com­ lar speech community values or stigmatizes
munication was not, however, the same as (see Manusov & Jaworksi, this volume).
actually measuring it. Even with the Ethnographic work on nonverbal behav­
advances in videotaping and sophisticated ior is one way to reveal more qualitatively
techniques of computerized recording and how some of these issues work themselves
analysis, multivariate studies of nonverbal out in interaction. One exemplar by Donal
communication are still complicated and Carbaugh (1999) provided a discussion
time-consuming. Nevertheless, conceptualiz­ of the functions of silence among members
ing nonverbal communication as multivari­ of the Blackfeet, a confederacy of three
ate in nature set the stage for two important independent Indian tribes presently living in
developments in the field: (1) the formula­ Montana and Alberta, Canada. In his
tion of theories of interactive behavior (see analysis, Carbaugh described the process
Patterson, this volume) and (2) emphasis on of “listening” (i.e., being silent, usually in
the various functions of nonverbal commu­ sacred spaces) as both a mode of learning
nication (Argyle, 1972; Patterson, 1983; see and a reflection of cultural values. This
chapters in Part III of this volume). practice does not occur—at least not in the
Consistent with these two developments, same form—in other cultural groups, and
the focus of research also changed as the it can only be understood by reference to
field matured. In the 1970s and 1980s, the values of that group. In providing this
research moved away from the study of spe­ description, Carbaugh was able to show
cific channels as isolated aspects of subtle effectively how certain rules for and inter­
communication to a greater focus on the pretation of nonverbal behaviors can only
processes mediating nonverbal communica­ be understood when the larger communica­
tion and the consequences and applications tive context is also understood.
of nonverbal communication (see the chap­ Although these issues—mediating pro­
ters in Part IV of this volume). Furthermore, cesses, applications, and moderating
there was increased attention to the way in variables—continue to affect the contempo­
which the moderating variables of culture, rary study of nonverbal communication,
sex or gender, age, and personality affected another emerging development from the
nonverbal communication (see the chapters mid-1980s through the present day changed
in Part II of this volume). the way that researchers viewed nonverbal
The focus on various determinants facil­ communication. Specifically, and in some
itated an appreciation of nonverbal behav­ ways inconsistent with the more relativistic
ior as part of a larger set of communicative perspectives of studies that look at differ­
practices shaped and constrained by the ences across groups, there was a growing
cultural context or code in which they recognition of the importance of evolution
occur. Thus, for many researchers, under­ in shaping nonverbal communication (see
standing the communicative value of non­ the Floyd and Buck & Renfro Powers chap­
verbal communication depends on its ters, this volume). Basic elements of every­
integration with language (see Bavelas & day life, particularly those related to
Chovil, this volume). For others, the non­ survival, reproduction, and care of off­
verbal behaviors can be understood only spring were linked to natural selection.
when seen within a framework of gender Thus, there is an increased appreciation of
rules and power (see chapters by Hall and hardwired reactions predisposing people to
Part-V-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 5:25 PM Page 525

Final Thoughts–––◆–––525

behave and to make judgments in a fashion active agents, initiating behavior and testing
promoting survival of the species. As preliminary judgments in social settings.
noted, other factors, such as culture (see Fourth, although people possess some adap­
Matsumoto, this volume), individual dif­ tive flexibility in relating to others, and
ferences (see Gifford, this volume), and some more than others (e.g., high self-
social norms (see Dovidio, Hebl, Richeson, monitors), we are constrained substantially
& Shelton, this volume) can moderate in our social judgments and behavior. That
this hardwiring. There is also the pre­ is, the residual effects of biology, culture,
dictable disagreement over the relative gender, and personality constitute the “bag­
impact of these hardwired predispositions, gage” that limits the range of our judgments
but it is clear that the biological bases and behavior (for more on this, see Part II of
of nonverbal communication cannot be this volume), while also providing a rich set
ignored. of interpretive practices that reflect those
At the risk of oversimplifying the field in forces.
its contemporary form, what do these So from this basic foundation, where are
developments suggest about the nature of we headed and what merits special atten­
nonverbal communication? First, nonver­ tion in the years to come? In the remainder
bal communication is best represented as a of this chapter, we highlight a few issues
system comprising interdependent compo­ that may shape the direction of research
nents (see chapters by Bavelas & Chovil and theory in the future. Specifically, we
and Patterson, this volume) and not a hap­ discuss the automaticity of much nonverbal
hazard collection of unrelated cues and communication, advances in methods for
behaviors. We cannot attend to all the assessing nonverbal communication, and
pieces in any single study, but we can rec­ some ways in which new technologies may
ognize that any specific focus is part of a work to extend how we conceptualize and
larger system of communication, embedded what we attend to in the study of nonverbal
within a larger set of communicative prac­ communication.
tices. Second, social judgments and behav­
ior are pragmatic, serving a variety of
♦ A Glimpse at Future Issues
different functions and having important
social, personal, and relational conse­
quences (see Part IV of this volume). These AUTOMATIC PROCESSES
consequences can be valuable (as in the
case of developing close relationships) or A growing body of research and theory
harmful (as in the case of unwarranted points to the dominance of automatic
affection). The reflection of functions in processes in the sending and receiving
goals can even be activated outside of of nonverbal communication (see Bargh,
awareness (Bargh, 1997). That is, particular 1997; Lakin, this volume). Although we
situations, such as an interview or a social may prefer to see ourselves as rational and
gathering, may be sufficient to prime a spe­ deliberate in our social contacts, automatic
cific goal and activate adaptive social judg­ processes seem to be the “default setting” in
ment and behavioral processes, all outside relating to others. In many cases, neither
of awareness (see chapters by Lakin and judgments nor behaviors can wait for a log­
Patterson, this volume). ical assessment of alternatives before some
Third, individuals are not simply reactive decision or action is required. Even when
in their social environments, as the early individuals have the luxury of adequate
interactive theories suggested. They are also time, they are biased toward the quick and
Part-V-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 5:25 PM Page 526

526–––◆–––The SAGE Handbook of Nonverbal Communication

easy solutions. That is, people are inclined more accurate than chance still leaves a lot
to be “cognitive misers,” avoiding unneces­ of room for knowing when we can be con­
sary (and sometimes, necessary) cognitive fident in the accuracy of our judgments.
effort (Fiske & Taylor, 1995). This is especially the case when confidence
It is clear that receivers’ automatic judg­ in judgments does not predict their accu­
ments are typically neither arbitrary nor racy (DePaulo, Charlton, Cooper, Lindsay,
maladaptive (Bargh, 1997). Perceivers make & Muhlenbruck, 1997; Patterson, Foster,
relatively accurate judgments of others & Bellmer, 2001). Future research might
quickly on the basis of limited appearance well consider the circumstances related to
and behavioral information (Ambady & accuracy in judgments, especially automatic
Rosenthal, 1992). Greater accuracy might judgments. There is research documenting
be expected in practical, affordance judg­ individual differences in judgment accu­
ments dealing with how we relate to others racy, but different measures of decoding
than in more abstract trait judgments accuracy are not highly correlated (see
(McArthur & Baron, 1983; Swann, 1984; Riggio, this volume).
Zebrowitz & Collins, 1997). Of course, So is there a simple, general dimension
automatic judgments are sometimes wrong underlying decoding accuracy? What are
(e.g., the fundamental attribution error). the particular circumstances that contribute
Correcting such errors requires not only the to increased accuracy in judgments? Are
availability of cognitive resources, but it also some specific pieces of appearance and
necessitates the motivation to apply those behavioral information especially diagnos­
resources in resolving inconsistencies (Gilbert tic or is the overall gestalt, even in very brief
& Malone, 1995). Thus, without the neces­ glimpses of others, more important? In
sary cognitive resources and motivation, ini­ everyday experience, are we more attuned
tial, automatic judgments will dominate. On to reading the emotions of others or to
the other hand, because the automatic judg­ reading their behavioral intentions (see
ments are relatively accurate, more is not Fridlund & Russell, this volume)? To what
necessarily better when it comes to applying extent can training improve accuracy in
cognitive resources to our judgments. judgments, and if training makes us more
Thinking too much about initially auto­ aware, what does that do to the automatic­
matic judgments can actually decrease accu­ ity of judgments? These are just a few of the
racy (Patterson & Stockbridge, 1998; questions that may merit attention in future
Wilson & Schooler, 1991). research.
So there is something of a dilemma here. Recent research on encoding nonverbal
Automatic judgments, particularly affor­ communication has also emphasized the
dance judgments, are generally accurate. primacy of automatic processes for a wide
But when they are not, cognitive effort range of behaviors. Like social judgment, it
must be applied to correct the judgments. is possible to initiate controlled, deliberate
Without independent evidence, however, patterns of behavior. But individuals must
we do not usually know when our judg­ be motivated enough to expend the effort
ments are inaccurate. Occasionally, such and have the necessary cognitive resources
mistakes catch up with us, as problems to monitor and manage the behavior.
arise in relationships or bank accounts are Nevertheless, thinking more about manag­
drained, but by then the damage is done. ing one’s behavior is not necessarily desir­
Documenting that automatic judgments are able, especially with behavioral routines
Part-V-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 5:25 PM Page 527

Final Thoughts–––◆–––527

that are otherwise automatic. Vallacher and support, automatic mimicry provided an
Wegner (1987) make this point in their efficient and adaptive way to increase the
action identification theory. Specifically, social bonds.
when actions are well learned or automatic, Although it is convenient to discuss the
people tend to conceptualize them in gen­ social judgment and behavioral “sides” of
eral terms, focused primarily on the goals interaction as isolated processes, they are
or consequences of actions. For example, interdependent elements in a larger system of
deciding to go to the supermarket to get communication (see Patterson, this volume).
milk and bread is sufficient to get into the In social settings, specific goals activate adap­
car and drive 3 miles to the store. A person tive judgments and behavior simultaneously.
does not have to think about the specific Much of this process is initiated automati­
elements of driving a car, including the cally and often proceeds to conclusion with­
coordinated movements involved with a out any reflection. But unexpected or unusual
stick shift. If attention is directed to the par­ behavior from others can prompt attention
ticular components’ actions (e.g., the timing and effort in forming judgments, just as our
of applying the clutch, shifting the gear, and own awkward behavior can prompt attempts
accelerating), the efficiency of the sequence at behavior management. It seems likely that
may suffer. This is similar to Bargh’s (1997) once awareness and effort are applied in one
goal-dependent automaticity. For Bargh, process, they are likely to spread to the com­
individuals are aware of particular goals, plementary process.
but once they decide to pursue them, the For example, if a friend behaves in an
instrumental behaviors frequently run on unusual fashion, controlled processes are
automatic. activated to determine just what this means.
In addition, when goals are commonly At the same time, our own behavior may be
pursued in a particular setting, the context more deliberately managed to facilitate an
itself may be sufficient to activate goals and accurate judgment. In a similar fashion, the
goal-directed behavior. A common form of realization of one’s own behavioral faux
automaticity in social settings is behavioral pas directs attention to correcting the mis­
mimicry. Individuals mimic speech pat­ take and, simultaneously, focuses attention
terns, facial expressions, posture, and on the reactions of those around us.
movement in interactions routinely and Following these kinds of events, it may take
automatically (Lakin, Jefferis, Cheng, & some time before automaticity becomes the
Chartrand, 2003; see also chapters by norm again. Although automatic processes
Cappella & Schreiber and Tickle-Degnen, generally constitute the “default” setting
this volume). Behavioral mimicry is clearly for social judgments and behavior, as
adaptive, not only facilitating the immedi­ circumstances change, more controlled
ate interaction but also increasing liking processes can be activated. Of course, indi­
and interdependence between partners. viduals have to be sufficiently motivated
One suggestion for how mimicry happens and have adequate cognitive resources to
so quickly and smoothly is that there is apply in evaluating their judgments or man­
a direct perception-behavior link selected aging their behavior. Nevertheless, more
over the course of evolution because it was is not necessarily better when it comes to
critical for our survival (Dijksterhuis & thinking about our judgments and manag­
Bargh, 2001). That is, because social ani­ ing our behavior. Automaticity is there for
mals depend on others for affiliation and a reason.
Part-V-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 5:25 PM Page 528

528–––◆–––The SAGE Handbook of Nonverbal Communication

NEW METHODS onto representations of individuals in vir­


tual reality (avatars) as it actually occurs
Over the last 50 years, our knowledge (Blascovich et al., 2002; see Walther, this
of nonverbal communication has increased volume). Furthermore, because these are
dramatically. How we study this system of digital signals, they can be transformed and
communication has also changed. In this experimentally manipulated to examine
section, we highlight a few newer methods the effects of precise changes in the avatar’s
that offer considerable promise for under­ behavior on the reactions of another person.
standing the subtleties of nonverbal commu­ In a recent study, for example, evidence
nication. First, the recent advances in the was found for the effects of subtle behav­
biology of nonverbal communication (see ioral mimicry initiated by an embodied
Buck & Renfro Powers, this volume) are agent (humanlike digital representation
likely to continue and provide new insights under computer control) on subsequent
into how this system works. Particularly persuasion and impressions (Bailenson &
important here are brain-imaging techniques Yee, 2005). Specifically, when the agent
that relate the understanding (social judg­ mimicked the participant’s head move­
ment) and execution (behavior) of actions to ments with a 4-second delay, compared
particular patterns of neural activity. In with nonmatching head movements, the
recent years, there is even evidence at the agent was more persuasive and was rated
neurological level that “mirror neurons” more positively. It is impressive that even
may provide the mechanism underlying the under the artificial circumstances of inter­
direct perception-behavior link involved in acting with a virtual person, the influence
mimicry (Rizzolatti & Craighero, 2004; see of mimicry is evident.
also Cappella & Schreiber, this volume). One ingenious study combined a brain-
Specifically, the same neurons seem to be imaging measure, functional magnetic reso­
activated when individuals view a particular nance imaging (fMRI), with a manipulation
behavior as when they actually initiate the on a virtual reality apparatus. When partic­
same behavior. The close link between per­ ipants received a 1-second glance from an
ceiving and behaving is also consistent with approaching agent, compared with gaze
the view of nonverbal communication as an avoidance, brain activity increased dramat­
integrated system of parallel processes oper­ ically in the right hemisphere superior tem­
ating in the service of particular interper­ poral sulcus (Pelphrey, Viola, & McCarthy,
sonal goals (see Patterson, this volume). In 2004). The superior temporal sulcus is appar­
the future, it may even be possible to map the ently involved in deriving social meaning
circuitry of such a system as people engage in from facial and bodily movement (Kolb &
coordinated, goal-oriented social judgments Whishaw, 2003, p. 375). The elaborate
and behavior. instrumentation involved in brain imaging
A second technological advance is the and virtual reality provides impressive, new
development of immersive virtual environ­ alternatives for researchers. Nevertheless,
ments for studying interactive behavior. these high-tech devices are expensive and not
One form, collaborative virtual environ­ easily accessible to most investigators. In
ments (CVEs), allows researchers to study addition, the control and precision of these
continuous patterns of behavior from inter­ measures comes with another cost: decreased
actants in remote physical locations. That external validity or representativeness.
is, the verbal and nonverbal behavior of There is also reason to consider the
interactants in different locations is rendered potential for low-tech alternatives in
Part-V-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 5:25 PM Page 529

Final Thoughts–––◆–––529

studying subtle behavioral changes in people manage these common events


public settings. This is really a suggestion to (Patterson, Iizuka, Tubbs, Ansel, & Anson,
reemphasize the study of unfocused interac­ 2006). There are countless opportunities
tions (Goffman, 1963, pp. 83–88) in a new for studying nonverbal communication in
and systematic fashion with field experi­ the unfocused interactions that all of us
ments. Unfocused interactions are social experience in everyday life. This field exper­
settings where people simply share a com­ iment approach, maximizing external valid­
mon presence without any expectation of ity, provides a useful complement to precise
having a conversation. They are “interac­ and controlled methods such as brain
tions” because the behavior of one person imagery and virtual reality techniques.
affects others in close proximity. For
example, as we share a ride on an elevator,
stand in line at the grocery store, or choose NEW MEDIA
a seat in our physician’s waiting room, we
make subtle adjustments to the close pres­ Not only are new technologies impor­
ence of others. These settings are important tant for expanding our options in managing
because individuals necessarily negotiate and exchanging information, but they also
their relationships to others through their bear on the understanding of what non­
nonverbal behavior. verbal communication entails. As Walther
A particularly interesting example of (this volume) points out, much of the schol­
unfocused interactions is the simple experi­ arship on computer-mediated communica­
ence of passing another pedestrian as we tion (CMC) started with the premise that
walk down the sidewalk. By employing this technology was largely, if not entirely,
confederates and observers walking behind devoid of nonverbal communication. This
the confederates, these microinteractions “cues-filtered-out” perspective suggested
can be reliably studied in the real-world set­ that CMC was different inherently from
tings where they normally occur (Patterson, face-to-face interaction specifically because
2005). In fact, people are sensitive to the it was less rich communicatively. The
gender and level of recognition (avoid, “lack” of nonverbal cues meant, for many,
look, or smile) from a pedestrian as they a deficit to communicating online.
approach and pass the person (Patterson But this scholarship has shown us a
& Tubbs, 2005; Patterson, Webb, & more complicated picture. First, it reveals
Schwartz, 2002). In the 1 to 2 seconds it that people will work to make up for cer­
takes for people to cover a “passing zone” tain nonverbal cues not being available.
of 10 to 12 feet, they make rapid adjust­ The quick rise of user-created emoticons
ments to an approaching stranger. to reflect facial expressions or intentions
The utility of this kind of field-experiment provided a way of reducing the potential
technique extends well beyond simply dis­ ambiguity of verbal messages. Users also
covering norms for pedestrian passings. began writing in capital letters to reflect
Because many of our contacts with out- particular vocal cues their conversational
group members take this form, the subtle partners could not hear online. Not long
adjustments that individuals make in after these user-generated practices emerged,
these microinteractions may also serve as technologies were created for replicating
an unobtrusive measure of out-group them. Now, for example, smiley faces are
attitudes. In addition, there is also evidence built into word processing programs directly.
for cultural differences in the way that These examples highlight our reliance on
Part-V-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 5:25 PM Page 530

530–––◆–––The SAGE Handbook of Nonverbal Communication

nonverbal communication and its integra­ have not received much attention in research
tion with language (Bavelas & Chovil, this (for a notable exception, see Levine, 1997).
volume). They also reflect the degree to Specifically, Walther argues that chrone­
which people can use an array of behaviors mics make up an important set of cues in
to communicate the same messages or per­ CMC and are available without any
form the same functions: When one cue is technological advances. For example,
not available, others take its place. how quickly people respond to one
Second, an exploration of online commu­ another’s messages can send important
nicating reveals the importance of nonverbal communicative messages of relationships,
cues to an array of vital social issues (see importance, and communicative skill (see
chapters in Part IV of this volume). Riggio, the volume). Yet chronemic cues
Specifically, research on CMC has revealed have been neglected as relevant in the
the extent to which we rely on nonverbal cues CMC context.
to display—and constrain—identity. When Closer attention to the new technologies
our physical attractiveness, cues to gender, may provide a new perspective on how
race, and age, and physical abilities are absent nonverbal communication works and what
from the interaction, as occurs in many but is most central in our research endeavors.
not all online interactions, we get a chance to Many of the assumptions that we hold
see the ways in which the nonverbal cues of (including the automaticity of our cue use)
identification sometimes limit, and certainly can be reinvestigated when nonverbal com­
configure, our face-to-face interactions. So, munication takes on a more strategic form.
for example, research reveals the extent to Finally, although the new technologies
which we require nonverbal cues to personal­ advance the range and efficiency of interper­
ize another; without them, people may sonal communication, there may also be
categorize others vaguely as “out-group” unwanted side effects. As people spend more
members and be less likely to treat them fairly time online, talking on cell phones, and text
(Postmes, Spears, Lea, & Reicher, 2000; see messaging, what happens to the important
also, Dovidio et al., this volume). social benefits possible only in face-to-face
At the same time, the absence of certain interactions? Some scholars suggest that the
nonverbal cues that work typically to impersonal nature of our new technologies
categorize a person into a stigmatized reduces our sense of belonging and affects
group (e.g., skin color, body size) may the social fabric adversely (see Bugeja, 2005).
allow for enhancement of certain peoples’ The effect of technologically mediated com­
identities when they can minimize the influ­ munication on the frequency and quality
ence of the cue. Furthermore, as Walther of face-to-face interactions is an important
(this volume) notes, CMC allows for issue for future research.
selective identity presentation in a way that
face-to-face communication does not. For
example, people can take more time to ♦ Conclusion
think through what they want to say, they
may choose to send a photograph of them­
selves that is particularly flattering (or sev­ The study of nonverbal communication has
eral years old), or they may state that their changed dramatically over the last 50 years.
age or sex is other than what it actually is. Conceptual and methodological advances
Third, research on CMC reveals the have facilitated a growing appreciation of
importance of certain nonverbal cues that the influence of nonverbal communication
Part-V-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 5:25 PM Page 531

Final Thoughts–––◆–––531

in our social world. We now have a better Blascovich, J., Loomis, J., Beall, A., Swinth, K.,
understanding of how biology, culture, gen­ Hoyt, C., & Bailenson, J. N. (2002).
der, and personality help shape the course Immersive virtual environment technology
of nonverbal communication. In face-to­ as a methodological tool for social psychol­
ogy. Psychological Inquiry, 13, 103–124.
face interactions, there is a growing appreci­
Bugeja, M. (2005). Interpersonal divide.
ation for the interdependence of parallel
Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.
processes of sending and receiving infor­
Carbaugh, D. (1999). “Just listen”: “Listening”
mation in the service of specific goals. and landscape among the Blackfeet.
Furthermore, this system of communication Western Journal of Communication, 63,
operates primarily on automatic, but it can 250–270.
shift to more controlled processes as correc­ DePaulo, B. M., Charlton, C., Cooper, H.,
tions and adjustments are required in inter­ Lindsay, J. J., & Muhlenbruck, L. (1997).
actions. New methods, such as brain imaging The accuracy-confidence correlation in
and virtual reality techniques, provide us the detection of deception. Personality and
with interesting and powerful alternatives for Social Psychology Review, 1, 346–357.
studying nonverbal communication. There is, Dijksterhuis, A., & Bargh, J. A. (2001). The per­
ception-behavior expressway: Automatic
however, still much to learn from careful
effects of social perception on social behav­
research in mundane public settings where
ior. In M. P. Zanna (Ed.), Advances in
nonverbal communication is the means of
experimental social psychology (Vol. 33,
negotiating unfocused interactions. New pp. 1–46). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.
media and computer technologies also enlist Ekman, P. (1965). Differential communication
nonverbal elements, but not in the fashion of affect by head and body cues. Journal
we have seen in the past. Thus, as some of Personality and Social Psychology, 2,
issues seem closer to resolution, new ques­ 726–735.
tions will keep us busy in the future. Exline, R. V. (1963). Explorations in the process
of person perception: Visual interaction in
relation to competition, sex, and need for
affiliation. Journal of Personality, 31, 1–20.
♦ References Fiske, S. T., & Taylor, S. E. (1995). Social cog­
nition (2nd ed.). New York: McGraw-Hill.
Gilbert, D. T., & Malone, P. S. (1995). The cor­
Ambady, N., & Rosenthal, R. (1992). Thin respondence bias. Psychological Bulletin,
slices of behavior as predictors of inter­ 117, 21–38.
personal consequences: A meta-analysis. Goffman, E. (1963). Behavior in public places.
Psychological Bulletin, 111, 256–274. New York: Free Press.
Argyle, M. (1972). The psychology of interper­ Kolb, B., & Whishaw, I. Q. (2003).
sonal behavior (2nd ed.). London: Penguin. Fundamentals of human neuropsychology
Argyle, M., & Dean, J. (1965). Eye-contact, (5th ed.). New York: Worth.
distance, and affiliation. Sociometry, Lakin, J. L., Jefferis, V. E., Cheng, C. M., &
28, 289–304. Chartrand, T. L. (2003). The chameleon
Bailenson, J. N., & Yee, N. (2005). Digital effect as social glue: Evidence for the
chameleons: Automatic assimilation of non­ evolutionary significance of nonconscious
verbal gestures in immersive virtual environ­ mimicry. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior,
ments. Psychological Science, 16, 814–819. 27, 145–162.
Bargh, J. A. (1997). The automaticity of every­ Levine, R. (1997). A geography of time. New
day life. In R. S. Wyer Jr. (Ed.), Advances York: Basic Books.
in social cognition (Vol. 10, pp. 1–61). Mahl, G. F. (1956). Disturbances and silences
Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. in the patient’s speech in psychotherapy.
Part-V-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 5:25 PM Page 532

532–––◆–––The SAGE Handbook of Nonverbal Communication

Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, and visibility on recognition and avoidance
53, 1–15. in passing encounters. Western Journal of
Manusov, V. (2005). (Ed.). The sourcebook of Communication, 69, 219–231.
nonverbal measures: Going beyond words. Patterson, M. L., Webb, A., & Schwartz, W.
Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. (2002). Passing encounters: Patterns of recog­
McArthur, L. Z., & Baron, R. (1983). Toward nition and avoidance in pedestrians. Basic
an ecological theory of social perception. and Applied Social Psychology, 24, 57–66.
Psychological Review, 90, 215–238. Pelphrey, K. A., Viola, R. J., & McCarthy, G.
Mehrabian, A. (1969). Significance of posture (2004). When strangers pass: Processing of
and position in the communication of atti­ mutual and averted gaze in the superior
tude and status relationships. Psychological temporal sulcus. Psychological Science,
Bulletin, 71, 359–372. 15, 598–603.
Patterson, M. L. (1983). Nonverbal behavior: Postmes, T., Spears, R., Lea, M., & Reicher, S. D.
A functional perspective. New York: (2000). SIDE issues centre stage: Recent
Springer-Verlag. developments in studies of deindividuation
Patterson, M. L. (2005). The passing encounters in groups. Amsterdam: Royal Netherlands
paradigm: Monitoring microinteractions Academy of Arts and Sciences.
between pedestrians. In V. Manusov (Ed.), Rizzolatti, G., & Craighero, L. (2004). The
The sourcebook of nonverbal measures: mirror-neuron system. Annual Review of
Going beyond words (pp. 431–440). Neuroscience, 27, 169–192.
Mahwah NJ: Erlbaum. Sommer, R. (1959). Studies in personal space.
Patterson, M. L., Foster, J. L., & Bellmer, C. D. Sociometry, 22, 247–260.
(2001). Another look at accuracy and con­ Swann, W. B., Jr. (1984). Quest for accuracy in
fidence in social judgments. Journal of person perception: A matter of pragmatics.
Nonverbal Behavior, 25, 207–219. Psychological Review, 91, 457–477.
Patterson, M. L., Iizuka, Y., Tubbs, M., Ansel, Vallacher, R. R., & Wegner, D. M. (1987).
J., & Anson, J. (2006, January). Passing What do people think they’re doing? Action
encounters East and West: A comparison of identification and human behavior. Psy­
Japanese and American pedestrian interac­ chological Review, 94, 3–15.
tions. Poster presented at the annual con­ Wilson, T. D., & Schooler, J. W. (1991).
vention of the Society of Personality and Thinking too much: Introspection can
Social Psychology, Palm Springs, CA. reduce the quality of preferences and deci­
Patterson, M. L., & Stockbridge, E. (1998). sions. Journal of Personality and Social
Effects of cognitive demand and judgment Psychology, 60, 181–192.
strategy on person perception accuracy. Zebrowitz, L. A., & Collins, M. A. (1997).
Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 22, Accurate social perception at zero acquain­
253–263. tance: The affordances of a Gibsonian
Patterson, M. L., & Tubbs, M. E. (2005). approach. Personality and Social Psychology
Through a glass darkly: Effects of smiling Review, 1, 204–223.
AU-Index.qxd 6/30/2006 5:25 PM Page 533

AUTHOR INDEX

Aarts, H., 62 Allen, J. P., 409


Abbey, A., 412 Allesandri, S. M., 190
Abelson, R. P., 34 Allison, K., 183
Abiala, K., 510 Allison, M., 105, 110
Aboudan, R., 108 Alloy, L. B., 32
Abrahams, M. F., 32 Allport, G. W., 8, 161
Abrams, D., 464 Altman, I., 262, 468
Abramson, L. Y., 32 Alton, A. O., 80
Acitelli, L. W., 259 Alvaro, E. M., 29
Acredolo, L. P., 192 Ambady, N., xvii, 13, 34, 42, 43,
Adams, R. B., 42, 44 44, 45, 46, 47, 53, 64, 68, 82,
Adaval, R., 323 90, 124, 134, 147, 165, 166,
Adelman, P. K., 64, 372 185, 208, 227, 228, 229, 305,
Adkins, M., 474 326, 327, 383, 384, 385, 484,
Adolphs, R., 46, 53 488, 510, 512, 514, 525, 535
Afifi, W. A., 344, 345, 346, Andayani, S., 226
410, 412 Anders, S. L., 412, 413
Agle, G., 462 Andersen, J. A., 422, 424
Aiello, J. R., 205 Andersen, J. F., xxi, 268
Ainsworth, M. D. S., 268 Andersen, J. R., 284
Akehurst, L., 163, 166, 324, 352 Andersen, P. A., 13, 24, 25, 143,
Akert, R., 44, 83 144, 164, 211, 259, 261, 262,
Albas, C. A., 229 264, 265, 266, 267, 268, 269,
Albas, D. C., 229 370, 371, 408, 410, 412, 502,
Albert, M., 185 535–536
Albright, L., 48, 324, 327 Anderson, C., 287, 288, 325, 367,
Alcock, J., 310 482, 488
Alexander, M. G., 212 Anderson, D. E., 350, 354
Alibali, M. W., 108 Andersson, L. O., 352
Alkema, F., xvi Andersson, M., 302
Allan, S., 246 Andreoletti, C., 288
Allen, C. M. B., 261 Andrew, R. J., 283

◆ 533
AU-Index.qxd 6/30/2006 5:25 PM Page 534

534–––◆–––The SAGE Handbook of Nonverbal Communication

Ansel, J., 528 Barclay, C. D., 193


Ansfield, M. E., 350, 354 Bargh, J. A., 13, 30, 32, 33, 34, 35, 36, 49,
Anson, J., 528 52, 59, 60, 61, 62, 63, 65, 69, 70, 71,
Ansul, S., 148 344, 368, 369, 370, 371, 373, 382, 388,
Apple, W., 288 390, 490, 524, 525, 526
Aquino, K., 507, 508 Bargh, S. J., 284
Aragona, B. J., 127 Bar-Haim, Y., 165
Araki, S., 220 Barker, M., xvi
Arbib, M. A., 315, 316 Barker, R. G., 32
Archer, D., 44, 45, 46, 47, 64, 82, Barndollar, K., 63
83, 208, 510 Barnlund, D., 220
Archer, J., 206 Baron, R., 525
Argyle, M., xvi, 11, 13, 22, 35, 89, 108, Bar-On, R., 331
205, 221, 225, 267, 310, 370, 411, Baron, R. M., 30, 45, 187, 330, 382, 384
474, 522, 523 Baron-Cohen, S., 43, 208
Arias, I., 271 Barraclough, R. A., 280, 430
Aries, E. J., 164, 281, 282 Barratt, M. S., xviii, xix
Arkin, R. M., 63 Barrera, M. E., xviii, 183
Arnold, S. L., 365 Barrett, K. C., 189
Aronoff, J., 187, 194 Barsade, S. G., 509
Aronson, J., 62 Bartel, C. A., 508
Arriaga, X. B., 284 Bartels, L. K., 325
Arrow, B., 53 Bartels, M., 365
Aruguete, M. S., xvii, 438 Barthes, R., 240
Asendorpf, J., 8, 225 Bartholomew, K., 412
Ashburn-Nardo, L., 488 Bartlett, M. K., 439
Ashkansy, N. W., 512 Bassler, B. L., 125, 126
Attardo, S., 322 Batchelder, W. H., 224
Auerbach, S. M., 452 Battit, G. E., 439
Aune, K. S., 262 Batty, M., 331
Aune, R. K., 262, 351 Baucom, D. H., xx, 414
Austin, J. T., 390 Bauer, J., 183
Awamleh, R., 513 Baum, K., 45, 46
Axsom, D., 250 Baumeister, R. F., 249, 322, 324, 332, 333
Baumrind, D., xix
Babad, E., 249 Bavelas, J. B., 13, 69, 98, 99, 100, 101,
Bachorowski, J. A., 309 102, 105, 106, 107, 108, 109, 110,
Bacue, A. E., 87 315, 347, 536
Baesler, E. J., 286, 290 Baxter, L., 269
Bahr, S. J., 281 Baxter, R. R., 151
Bailenson, J. N., 70, 474, 527 Beach, K., 491
Bailey, K. D., 455 Beach, S. R. H., 270, 414
Bailey, W., 484 Beach, W. A., 450, 452
Bakermans-Kraneburg, M. J., 366 Beall, A. C., 474, 527
Bakker, A. B., 364, 509 Beattie, G., 108
Ball, L., 468 Beaumont, S. L., xvi
Banaji, M. R., 43, 53, 202 Beavin, J., 389
Bancroft, A., 242 Beck, U., 244
Bangerter, A., 110 Bee, H., 202
Banks, J., 129 Beebe, B., xix, 387, 389
Banse, R., 44, 208, 313 Beebe, S. A., 424
Barbee, A. P., 149, 301, 327, 329 Beier, E. G., 513
AU-Index.qxd 6/30/2006 5:25 PM Page 535

Author Index–––◆–––535

Bell, R., 429 Bodenhausen, G. V., 271, 407


Bellis, M. A., 151 Bodhorn, D. J., 80
Bellmer, C. D., 525 Bohan, L., 248
Bellmer, C. G., 80 Boich, L., xxi
Belsky, J., 268 Bommer, W. H., 325
Bem, D. J., 25, 388 Bonaiuto, M., 347
Bem, S. L., 202, 431, 432 Bonanno, G. A., 49, 89, 322
Bengtsson, B., 471 Bond, C. F., 341, 350, 351, 352
Bensing, J. M., 438, 439, 440, 443, 455 Bond, C. F., Jr., 210
Ben-Sira, Z., 438 Bond, M., 225
Benson, B., xvii Bonito, J. A., 471, 474
Benson, N., 192 Bonnell, D., xxi
Berg, C. A., 393 Bonner, L., 343
Berger, C. B., 32, 33, 34 Boon, S. D., 341, 350
Berger, C. R., 361 Boone, R. T., xxiii, 124, 135, 186, 188, 193
Berger, J., 280, 285, 286, 291, 485 Boone, T., 386, 387
Berkowitz, L., 371 Bordeaux, A. R., 53
Berkowitz, W. R., 206 Borden, E., 67
Berman, P. W., 484 Bordo, S., 242
Bernieri, F. J., 12, 13, 46, 47, 64, 68, 69, 81, Borke, H., 183
83, 268, 323, 327, 362, 382, 383, 384, Borkenau, P., 165, 167, 168, 174
385, 386, 387, 388, 389, 390, 391 Bornstein, M. H., xviii
Bernstein, I. S., 280 Borod, J., 207
Berry, D. S., 46, 164, 187, 326 Boss, D., 266
Berry, J. W., 220 Bosson, J. K., 323
Berscheid, E., 13 Bostrom, R. P., 467
Bertenthal, B. I., 182 Bosveld, W., 509
Bhagwagar, Z., 128 Bouchard, C., 151
Biddle, J. E., 507 Boucher, J. D., 228
Biehl, M., 208, 227 Bovatzis, C. J., 183
Biek, M., 330, 412 Bowman, L., 502
Billings, A., xxii Boyatzis, C. J., 186, 192
Bilo, H. J. G., 439 Boyatzis, R., 508, 510
Biocca, F., 240, 471, 474 Boyd, J. P., 224
Birdwhistell, R. L., xxiii, 9, 89, 280 Bradac, J. J., 370, 507
Bissonnette, V., 25 Bradbury, T. N., 271, 408, 414
Bixler, T., 246 Bradfield, M., 507, 508
Bizzell, P., 5 Bradley, B. P., 50
Björntorp, P., 151 Bradner, E., 468
Black, A., 13, 69, 98, 108 Brauer, M., 488
Black, K. A., 267 Brazill, T. J., 224
Blackman, M. C., 165 Breed, G., 23, 267
Blackstone, T., 406, 483 Bremmer, J., 3
Blair, I. V., 66 Brennan, S. E., 473
Blairy, S., 184, 327, 362, 365, 367 Bresnahan, M., 220
Blake, J., 192 Brewer, M. B., 30, 34, 62, 66, 226, 407
Blank, A. L., 86 Brickman, A. M., 207
Blascovich, J., 474, 488 Briggs, S. R., 88
Blehar, M. C., 268 Briner, R. B., 364
Bloom, B. S., 424, 430 Bristol-Power, M. M., xvii
Blurton-Jones, N. G., 98 Briton, N. J., 202, 204, 214
Boas, F., 221 Britt, T. W., 65, 302
AU-Index.qxd 6/30/2006 5:25 PM Page 536

536–––◆–––The SAGE Handbook of Nonverbal Communication

Broadbent, M., 69, 388 Burrows, L., 61


Broderick, J. E., xx, 271 Burt, D. M., 44, 150, 301
Brody, L. R., 202 Buslig, A. L. S., 344, 345, 346
Bromley, S., 66, 492 Buss, A. H., 88, 164
Bronstad, P. M., 329 Buss, D. M., 140, 144, 219, 412
Bronstein, P., xix Buss, K. A., 190
Brooks-Gunn, J., 147 Busselle, R. W., 245
Broverman, D. M., 202 Buswell, B. N., 283
Broverman, I., 202 Butler, E. A., 368, 385, 386
Brown, B., 225 Butler, J. L., 67
Brown, C. A., 192 Butterworth, G., xix
Brown, C. E., 205, 489, 505 Buunk, B., xxi
Brown, J. D., 323 Byrne, C. A., 271
Brown, N. J., 242 Byrne, D., 207, 262
Browne, B. A., 240
Bruce, V., 343 Cacioppo, J. T., 51, 65, 84, 270, 304, 331,
Bruneau, T. J., 287 362, 388, 508
Brunner, L. J., 103, 109 Cairns, A. B., 503
Brunswik, E., 89, 160, 166, 167, 330, 382 Calhoun, J. B., 11
Brunz, U., 468 Callan, V., xvi, xxi
Buccino, G., 373 Callender, J. C., 327
Buck, R., xxiii, 43, 65, 81, 85, 121, 122, Campbell, A., 163
124, 125, 129, 130, 131, 135, 247, Campbell, D. T., 330
386, 387, 536 Campbell, S., 484
Buckley, J. P., 353, 355 Campos, J. J., 148, 183, 189
Bugeja, M., 529 Campos, R. G., 189
Bugental, D. B., 284 Camras, L. A., 109, 148, 183, 190, 309
Bugental, D. E., 106, 204 Canary, D. J., 285
Bull, P., 165 Canli, T., 184
Bull, R., 51, 348, 349, 351, 352, 354 Caporael, J., 486
Buller, D. B., 25, 51, 85, 262, 264, 267, 287, Cappella, J. N., xvi, 24, 25, 53, 143,
289, 322, 323, 324, 326, 333, 344, 345, 262, 264, 266, 268, 269, 270,
346, 370, 371, 409, 438, 441, 443, 452, 362, 365, 367, 370, 371, 372,
455, 474 387, 389, 536–537
Burchard, K. W., 438 Caputi, P., 90
Burgdorf, J., 128 Caris-Verhallen, W. M., 455
Burger, G. K., 248 Carli, L. L., 505
Burgess, E. W., 381 Carlsmith, J. M., 287
Burgess, P., xvi Carlson, G. E., 228
Burgoon, J. K., xv, xvi, xxi, xxii, 12, 13, 23, Carlson, J. G., 364
24, 29, 35, 48, 85, 87, 143, 182, 262, Carlson, J. R., 474
263, 264, 265, 266, 267, 268, 269, 271, Carney, D. R., 84, 87, 88, 89
280, 281, 282, 283, 284, 285, 286, 287, Caron, A. J., 50
289, 290, 291, 322, 323, 324, 326, 333, Caron, R. F., 50
344, 345, 346, 362, 370, 371, 387, 388, Carpentier, W. S., 450
389, 403, 409, 410, 426, 442, 471, 474, Carr, L., 373
506, 512, 536 Carrère, S., 52
Burgoon, M., 286, 289 Carrere, S. J., 514
Buri, J. R., xix Carroll, J. M., 305, 310
Burleson, B. R., 87, 270 Carstensen, L. L., 148, 184, 185, 190, 191
Burnett, C. K., xx Cartar, L., 267
Burnett, J. R., 163 Carter, C. S., 127, 128
AU-Index.qxd 6/30/2006 5:25 PM Page 537

Author Index–––◆–––537

Carter, J. D., 87, 207, 209, 212, 214, Cheng, C. M., 69, 333, 334, 369, 371, 388,
285, 288, 292 389, 393, 526
Carton, E. E. R., 165 Cherulnick, P. D., 325
Carton, J. S., xix, 165 Chesebro, J. L., 425, 430, 431
Caruso, D. R., 80, 87, 90 Cheuk, F. N., 513
Carver, C. S., 62 Cheung, H., 486
Carver, V. H., 332 Chevalier-Skolnikoff, S., 224
Case, D., 462 Chiao, J. Y., 53
Casella, D. F., 86 Chidambaram, L., 467
Casey, F., 108 Ching, P., 351
Casey, S., 108 Chiu, P., 53
Caso, L., 347 Chock, T. M., 471
Casper, R. C., 247 Choi, J. W., 226, 228, 229
Cassell, J., 442 Choi, Y. S., 47, 64
Castel, J. M., 189 Chovil, N., 13, 98, 100, 101, 102, 103,
Castles, D. L., 301 106, 107, 108, 109, 110, 312, 314,
Catt, K. J., 123 315, 347, 537
Cattell, R. B., 280 Christensen, A., xx, xxi, 271, 284, 410, 413
Caughlin, J. P., xx Christie, B., 462
Caul, W. F., 81, 129 Christophel, D. M., 430
Cawley, J., 507 Chu, L., 326
Cederberg, C., 471 Churchill, M. E., 67, 248, 266
Cegala, D. J., 438 Cialdini, R. B., 326, 474
Ceschi, G., 189, 190 Ciarocco, N. J., 322, 333
Chaiken, S., 62, 250 Ciarrochi, J., 90
Chambers, W., 62 Ciccia, A., 491
Champion, C., 87 Cicero, M. T., 4
Chan, A., 90, 227 Clancy, S. M., 212
Chang, H. C., 395 Clanton, N. R., 323
Chapanis, A., 472 Clark, D. M., 324
Chaplin, W. F., 323 Clark, H. H., 99, 102, 103, 105, 109,
Chapman, A. J., 23 110, 473
Chapple, E., 8 Clark, M. S., 332
Charlesworth, W. R., 147, 148, 224 Clarke, D. D., 261
Charlton, C., 525 Clarke, H. H., 98
Charlton, K., 85, 344, 345, 346, 347, 348, Clausen, A., 188
352, 354 Clayton, F., 50
Chartrand, T. L., 32, 49, 52, 60, 61, Cleeton, G. U., 165
63, 65, 69, 70, 71, 333, 334, Clore, G. L., 384
344, 368, 369, 370, 371, 388, Cloven, D. H., 284
389, 393, 526 Coates, L., 98, 99, 109, 347
Chase, J., 313 Coats, E., 13, 190, 212, 238, 239, 240, 280,
Chasiotis, A., xviii 284, 287, 291, 295, 482
Chassin, L., xviii Cochenour, J. J., 469, 470
Chaudhuri, A., 247 Coe, N. P. W., 438
Chaumeton, N., 47, 53 Cohan, C., 409
Chazan, E., 183 Cohen, A. A., 11, 100, 108
Cheek, J. M., 88, 164 Cohen, D., 182
Chemtob, C., 364 Cohen, J., 209, 210, 345
Chen, M., 61, 69, 490 Cohn, D. F., 124
Chen, Y., 108, 324 Cohn, J. F., 189
Cheney, D. L., 122 Coifman, K., 89
AU-Index.qxd 6/30/2006 5:25 PM Page 538

538–––◆–––The SAGE Handbook of Nonverbal Communication

Coker, D. A., 263, 267, 512 Crowell, J. A., 409


Coker, R. A., 267 Crown, C. L., 387, 389
Cole, E. J., 88, 325 Cruickshank, D., 431
Cole, P. M., 190, 226 Crumley, L. P., xx
Cole, S. P., 484 Crusco, A. H., 513
Cole-Kelly, K., 438 Csikszentmihalyi, I. S., 383, 392, 396
Colella, M. J., 80 Csikszentmihalyi, M., 381, 382, 383, 392,
Collett, P., 221, 238 393, 394, 395, 396
Collins, K., 108, 313 Cuber, J. F., xxi
Collins, M. A., 32, 67, 326, 382, 384 Cuceloglu, D. M., 147
Collins, W. A., 239 Cuevas, C., 229
Colvin, C. R., 48, 166, 208, 209 Culbertson, G. H., 486
Comer, R., 151 Culnan, M. J., 464
Conan, F., 326 Culver, C., 184, 188, 191
Conaty, J. C., 291 Cunningham, J. G., 186, 187, 188, 193
Condon, W. S., 12 Cunningham, M. R., 149, 207, 301, 327, 329
Conner, B., 46 Cutler, W. D., 302
Conner, T. L., 285 Cutlip, W. D., II, 65
Connor, B., 45 Cutrona, C. E., xx
Consolacion, T., 227, 228 Cutting, J. E., 46, 193
Contarello, A., 225
Cook, M., 13, 221 Dabbs, J. M., 47
Coombs, W. T., 513 D’Addario, K. P., 469
Cooper, H., 85, 344, 345, 346, 347, 348, Daft, R. L., 463, 467
352, 354, 525 Dailey, A., xvii
Cooper, J., 68, 69, 486, 491, 492, 511 Daly, J. A., 12, 429
Cooper, L. A., 492 Damasio, A. R., 373
Corey, A. T., 389 Damasio, H., 46, 53
Cornelius, R. R., 146 Danchak, M. M., 464
Cornetto, K., 12 Danehy, J. J., 9
Correll, S. J., 485 Danet, B., 469
Corter, C., xviii, 192 Darley, J. M., 405
Cosmides, L., 144 Darwin, C., 140, 141, 145, 146, 147, 148,
Costa, P. T., Jr., 168 222, 238, 300, 301, 314, 327, 328
Costanzo, M. A., 44, 47, 64, 82, 83 Dasen, P. R., 220
Coster, W., 391, 393 Davidson, R. J., 65, 147, 189, 204, 224,
Couchoud, E. A., 183 311, 347
Coupland, J., 242 Davis, D., 367
Coupland, N., 245, 248, 388 Davis, F., 8, 11
Coutts, L. M., 25 Davis, J. M., 64, 323, 382, 386, 388,
Coyne, J. C., 408 390, 391
Craighero, L., 315, 373, 527 Davis, M., 355
Craik, K. H., 176 Davis, M. C., 247
Cramton, C. D., 468 Davis, M. H., 84
Crandall, C. S., 488, 489 Davis, S., 250
Cromwell, R. E., 280, 281 Davitz, J. R., 13
Cronbach, L. J., 172 Dawkins, R., 120, 121, 122, 123, 124
Crosby, F., 66, 492 Dawson, K. E., 249
Cross, J. F., 150 Day, R. H., 192
Cross, S. E., 212 Dean, J., 13, 22, 35, 205, 370, 474, 522
Crouch, W. W., 12 de Bouter, C., 70, 369
Crowder, R. G., 193 DeFrancisco, V. I., 262, 267, 268
AU-Index.qxd 6/30/2006 5:25 PM Page 539

Author Index–––◆–––539

DeFrank, R. S., 207 Dodds, R. A., 206


de Groot, D., 465 Doherty, R. W., 364
DeGroot, T., 513 Doherty-Sneddon, L., 343
de Haan, M., 310 Dolgin, K. M., 313
Delaney, B., 474 Dollinger, S. J., 212
Delaumosne, A., 6 Domae, A., 226, 228
Deldin, P., 53 Dominguez, M. M., xix
Delia, J. G., 7 Donely, K. A., 325
Delle Fave, A., 392 Donohoe, L. F., 492
Demaree, H., 184 Donzella, B., 367
DeMeijer, M., 185, 186, 188 Dougherty, T. W., 327
Denham, S. A., 183 Douglas, K. M., 464, 465
DePaulo, B. M., 7, 49, 63, 67, 84, 85, 86, Douglas, M., 243
89, 182, 201, 208, 323, 324, 325, 332, Dovidio, J. F., 27, 62, 66, 67, 71, 205, 208,
333, 341, 342, 343, 344, 345, 346, 280, 482, 485, 486, 488, 489, 490, 491,
347, 348, 350, 352, 353, 354, 386, 405, 492, 493, 494, 495, 496, 505, 537
406, 513, 525 Dowd, M., 323
Deprés, J. P., 151 Downey, G., 407
Derscheid, L. E., 241 Doyle, C., 343
DeSonneville, M. J., 184 Doyle, J., 329
deTurck, M. A., 80, 264, 267, 289 Driscoll, D. M., 506, 507
Deutsch, R., 34, 35, 382 Driver, J., xv, 410
Devereux, P. G., 313 Dronkers, J., 438
Devine, P. G., 62, 66, 490, 494 Druen, P. B., 149, 301, 327, 329
DeVito, J. A., 325 Drummond, P. D., 303
Devoe, S., 11, 98 Dubeau, M. C., 373
DeVries, A. C., 127 Dubrovsky, V., 463
de Waal, F. B. M., 130 Duck, S. W., 259
De Wolff, M. S., 370 Duffy, F. D., 438
Diacoyanni-Taratzis, I., 227 Duffy, R., 130
Diamond, H., 367 Duggan, A. P., 413, 439, 440, 455
Diamond, S. G., 165 Duggan, R., xvii
DiBiase, R., 206 Duke, M. P., 43, 81, 82, 165, 403, 404, 407
Dickey, E. C., 147 Dulewicz, V., 508
Dickson, L. R., 182 Dunbar, N. E., 280, 281, 282, 283,
Dickson, S., 261 284, 474, 537
Diefendorff, J. M., 510 Duncan, S., 98
Diekema, D., 281 Duncan, S. F., Jr., 11, 12, 174, 326
Dietz, J. K., 165, 208 Dunkerley, E., 391, 393
DiGeronimo, K., 330 Dunton, B. C., 494
Dijkmans, M., 369 Dushay, R. A., 108
Dijksterhuis, A., 61, 62, 371, 382, Dziurawiec, S., 182
388, 390, 526
Dillard, J. P., 264, 265 Eagly, A. H., 212, 481, 489
Dillman, L., 29, 35, 48, 268, 284, 285, 291, East, P. L., xvii
362, 370, 387, 388, 389, 403, 410 Eber, H. W., 280
DiMatteo, M. R., 45, 46, 82, 86, 208, Eberl, L., 126
249, 452, 510 Ebesu, A. S., 29
Dimberg, U., 42, 50, 66 Ebsu Hubbard, E., 289
Dindia, K., 266, 285 Eden, D., 512
Dittus, P. J., xvii Edinger, J. A., 280, 410, 502, 512
Dobish, H., 161 Edlemann, R. J., 225
AU-Index.qxd 6/30/2006 5:25 PM Page 540

540–––◆–––The SAGE Handbook of Nonverbal Communication

Edward, K., 51, 352, 354 Evans, H., 324


Edwards, M., 243 Evans, J. S. B. T., 468
Edwards, R., 150 Exline, R. V., 13, 22, 64, 161, 174,
Efron, D., 8, 194, 221, 347 221, 290, 522
Egbert, L., 439
Egland, K. I., 268 Fabes, R. A., xxi, 405
Egloff, B., 368, 385, 386 Fadiga, L., 373
Ehlers, A., 324 Fairclough, N. L., 240
Ehninger, D., 6 Fang, S. R., 241
Ehrlich, C. M., 440 Fantauzzo, C. A., 189
Eibl-Eibsfeldt, I., 10, 13, 147, 148 Farace, P. R., 438
Eidleman, A. I., 366 Farace, R. V., 438
Eisenberg, A. M., 11 Farag, F., 67
Eisenberg, N., xxi, 84, 87, 404, 405 Fayer, J. M., 430
Eisterhold, J., 322 Fazio, R. H., 62, 66, 489, 494
Ekman, P., 7, 10, 11, 12, 13, 42, 43, 45, 49, Featherstone, M., 242, 245
50, 65, 81, 84, 85, 89, 98, 100, 101, 103, Feeley, T. H., 354
104, 146, 147, 148, 184, 185, 190, 191, Feenberg, A., 468
204, 208, 221, 222, 225, 226, 227, 238, Feeney, J. A., xxi, 14, 271, 405, 406,
267, 304, 311, 313, 314, 322, 323, 331, 410, 413
342, 346, 347, 349, 354, 383, 439, 442, Fehr, B. J., 13, 64, 221, 305
469, 490, 522 Fehr, E., 128
Elaad, F., 341 Feinman, S., 183
Elfenbein, H. A., 32, 43, 44, 45, 147, 227, Feldman, D. C., 510
228, 229, 305, 385, 484, 510 Feldman, R. S., xvi, 13, 14, 183, 190,
Ellgring, H., 51 238, 239, 240, 366, 367, 486, 491,
Ellis, H., 182 492, 537–538
Ellis, S., 513 Feldman, S., 407
Ellsworth, P., 7, 13, 89, 147, 222, 287 Feldstein, S., 387, 389
Ellyson, S. L., 174, 205, 208, 280, 290, Fellous, J. M., 288
482, 489, 505 Ferguson, M. J., 371
Elmehed, K., 42, 50, 66 Fernandez-Dols, J. M., 11, 108, 147, 300,
Emde, R. N., 148, 183, 405 305, 309, 332
Emerson, R. M., 284 Ferrara, M., 342
Emmers, T. M., 266 Ferrari, J. R., 165
Emmons, R. A., 408 Ferreira, D. K., xvii
Emmorey, K., 108 Ferrer, C., 229
Engle, R. A., 98, 105, 106 Feyereisen, P., 14
Englehart, M., 373 Field, J., 192
English, T., 184 Field, T., 13, 182, 365, 366
Epitropaki, O., 367 Fincham, F. D., 270, 271, 414
Epley, N., 62 Fineman, S., 508
Epstein, J. A., 344, 345, 353 Fink, B., 411
Erb, M., 365 Fink, G. R., 373
Erickson, A., 385, 386 Firestone, I., 370
Erickson, E. A., 368 Fischer, A. H., 202, 214, 313, 331, 332, 393
Erlich, P. R., 11 Fisek, M. H., 285
Eshleman, A., 488, 489 Fishbacher, U., 128
Eskritt, M., 183 Fisher, C. D., 509
Espinoza, P., 494 Fisher, J. D., 25, 207
Eubanks, V., 244 Fiske, D. W., 12, 98, 174
Evans, C., 310 Fiske, S. T., 30, 34, 62, 66, 482, 525
AU-Index.qxd 6/30/2006 5:25 PM Page 541

Author Index–––◆–––541

Fitzpatrick, M. A., 272 Frieze, I. H., 291


Flagg, M. E., 367 Frijda, N. H., 307
Flannery, D. J., 206 Frith, H., 325
Fleming, A. S., xviii Froming, W. J., 62
Fleming, J. H., 405 Frost, P. J., 513
Floyd, D., 266 Fugita, S. S., 491
Floyd, K., xxii, 143, 151, 152, 264, 265, Fultz, J., 366
266, 268, 270, 271, 414, 538 Funder, D. C., 32, 48, 162, 166
Fogassi, L., 373 Furnham, A., 242
Fogel, A., 189, 192 Furuyama, N., 109
Follette, W. C., xxi Fussell, S. R., 472
Folsom, A. R., 151 Futrell, D., 249
Ford, T. E., 489
Forest, C., 324, 327 Gaelick, L., 271, 407
Forgas, J. P., 47, 70 Gaertner, S. L., 66, 485, 488, 489,
Forrest, J. A., xvi 490, 492, 494
Forston, R. F., 221 Galasinski, D., 245, 246
Foster, J. L., 80, 491, 525 Galbraith, K. A., xvii
Foucault, M., 248 Gale, A., 23
Fowler, C. A., 103 Gale, S., 472
Fox, N. A., 147, 186, 189 Galinsky, A. D., 488
Frable, D. E. S., 483 Gallaher, P., 283, 288
Fragale, A. R., 52, 65, 327, 488, 504 Gallaher, T. J., 248
Fraley, R. C., 224, 413 Gallese, V., 373
Frank, L. K., 10 Gallio, M., 126
Frank, M. G., 45, 65, 84, 85, 326, 347, Gallois, C., xvi
349, 352, 354 Ganellen, R. J., 62
Frank, R., 124, 134, 135, 144 Gangestad, S. W., 150, 151, 325, 329,
Frankel, R., 438 330, 412
Frankel, R. W., 450 Gapstur, S. M., 151
Franklin, B., 227, 228 Garb, J. L., 438
Franklin, M., 301 Garber, J., 190
Freedman, D. G., 148 Garcia, S., 25
Freitas, A. L., 407 Gardner, W. L., 393, 407, 513
French, J. R. P., 280, 430 Garlick, R., 351
French, M. T., 507 Gavett, E., 382, 386, 387, 389
Frerker, D., 25 Geen, T., 224
Freud, S., 382 Geller, D., 512
Freund, H. J., 373 Geller, J., 11, 98
Frick-Horbury, D., xvi Genova, K. L., 12
Fridlund, A. J., 13, 34, 65, 98, 104, 108, 146, Georgas, J., 225
190, 280, 283, 289, 300, 302, 304, 305, George, J. F., 474
309, 310, 311, 312, 313, 316, 331, 332, Gergle, D., 472
382, 383, 384, 407, 538 Gerlach, A. L., 302
Friedlander, M. L., 282 Gernsbacher, M. A., 68
Friedman, G. B., 214, 504 Gerrig, R. J., 100, 102, 103
Friedman, H. S., 7, 63, 67, 79, 80, 82, 85, 86, Gerwing, J., 102, 106, 107, 108, 110
87, 88, 89, 249, 344, 404 Gerwirtz, A., 512
Friesen, W. V., 7, 10, 11, 12, 13, 43, 45, 49, Getz, L. L., 127
50, 65, 89, 98, 147, 148, 184, 191, 204, Gianetto, R. M., 204
221, 225, 227, 267, 304, 311, 313, 314, Gibson, J. J., 67, 122, 382, 383
343, 346, 347, 349, 439, 442, 469, 490 Giddens, A., 242, 244
AU-Index.qxd 6/30/2006 5:25 PM Page 542

542–––◆–––The SAGE Handbook of Nonverbal Communication

Gifford, R., 163, 165, 168, 169, 170, 171, Gotlib, I. H., 53
172, 173, 282, 288, 326, 330, 371, 513 Gottman, J. M., xv, xix, xx, xxi,
Gilbert, A. N., 104, 313 52, 190, 271, 368, 386, 389,
Gilbert, D. T., 30, 34, 47, 68, 349, 350, 525 403, 409, 410
Gilbert, R. E., 324 Gould, S. J., 304
Gilding, M., 468 Govender, R., 62
Giles, H., xxi, xxii, 51, 367, 370, 388, 538 Graham, J. A., 108
Gillespie, D. L., 291 Grahe, J. E., 382, 385, 386, 388, 390,
Gillis, J. S., 83, 323, 382, 386, 388, 390, 391 391, 395
Gilman, S. L., 301 Grammer, K., 150, 326, 327, 387, 411
Gilmour, R., xvi Granhag, P. A., 342, 350, 352, 356
Gilovich, T., 62, 124, 134, 135, 326 Granka, L., 464, 467
Gingrich, B., 127 Graves, T. D., 221
Ginsburg, B. E., 121, 128, 135 Gray, A. R., 47
Ginsburg, G., xix, 313 Gray, H. M., 47, 64, 538–539
Ginsburg, S. L., 242 Green, E., 354
Giron, M., 440, 441, 446 Green, J. Y., 440
Givskov, M., 125 Greenbaum, C. W., 367
Gladwell, M., 514 Greenbaum, P., 108
Glaser, R., xvii, xviii Greenberg, B. S., 245
Glassman, M., 438 Greenberg, R., 182
Glassman, N., 438 Greene, J. O., 24, 25, 87, 143, 262,
Gleeson, K., 325 269, 370, 371
Godin, S., 469 Greenfield, S., 439
Goffman, E., 10, 52, 104, 240, 243, 301, 322, Greenwald, A. G., 43, 53
323, 334, 353, 389, 442, 450, 528 Greer, A. E., 412
Gold, C., 164, 281, 282 Gregory, S. W., 51
Gold, D. J., 267 Gregory, S. W., Jr., 248
Goldberg, L. R., 168 Grice, H. P., 103, 106
Goldin-Meadow, S., 326 Grich, J., 406
Goldman, A., 373 Griffith, C. H., 438, 443
Goldman-Eisler, F., 10, 343 Gronbeck, B. E., 6
Goldsmith, D. J., 270 Gross, J. J., 87, 88, 202, 368, 385, 386
Goldsmith, H. H., 68 Grossman, A., 242
Goldstein, S., 188 Grossman, E., 165
Goleman, D., 80, 508, 510 Gruber, K., 302
Gollwitzer, P. M., 63 Gruenfeld, D. H., 287, 288, 482, 488
Gomez-Beneyto, M., 440, 441, 446 Gruzen, J., 150
Gonzaga, G. C., 408 Guadagno, R. E., 474
Good, D. C., 291 Gubarchuk, J., 486
Goodman, C. R., 364 Gudjonsson, G. H., 349
Goodwin, C., 12, 98, 101, 439, 441, 442, Gudykunst, W. B., 220, 225
443, 445, 446, 450, 454, 455 Guerrero, L. K., 25, 49, 144, 164, 259, 261,
Goodwin, M. H., 98 264, 265, 266, 267, 268, 270, 271, 325,
Goodwyn, S. W., 192 370, 371, 409, 410, 412, 413, 539
Goorha, S., 474 Guinote, A., 488
Gordon, G. H., 438 Gump, B. B., 364
Gordon, V. V., xvii Gunnar, M., 183
Gordon-Terner, R., 512 Gunnoe, J., 206
Goren, A., 325 Guthrie, I. K., xxi
Gorham, J. S., 426, 427, 428, 430 Guthrie, R. D., 329
Goss, A. E., 190 Gwiazda, J., 183
AU-Index.qxd 6/30/2006 5:25 PM Page 543

Author Index–––◆–––543

Haakana, M., 450 Hatfield, E., 65, 84, 262, 268, 270, 331,
Habbema, J. D. F., 151 362, 364, 388, 508
Hadac, R. R., 439, 441 Hauser, M. D., 120, 123, 224
Hadiks, D., 355 Hauser, S. T., 409
Haefner, P., xx Hay, J., 322
Hager, J., 12, 220 Hayes, C. W., 204
Haidt, J., 45 Haymovitz, E. L., 323
Hairfield, J. G., 86 Haynes, M. T., 143, 151
Haist, S. A., 438, 443 Haynes, O. M., 189
Halberstadt, A., 237 Hays, R. B., 270
Halberstadt, A. G., 204, 212, 482, Hearn, V., 208, 227
483, 484, 496 Heatch, C., 452
Hale, J. L., xxi, 23, 24, 143, 264, 265, Heath, C., 445, 446, 450, 454, 455
267, 281, 289, 370, 471 Heath, D. C., 108
Hall, C. C., 325 Heatherington, L., 282
Hall, E. T., 9, 11, 164, 221, 265, 280, Heatherton, T. F., 301, 489
469, 483, 484 Heaton, T. B., 409
Hall, J. A., xvii, 13, 14, 45, 46, 53, 64, 82, Heavey, C. L., xxi, 271, 284
83, 85, 87, 202, 203, 204, 205, 206, 207, Hebb, J., 364
208, 209, 210, 212, 214, 267, 268, 280, Heberlein, A. S., 46, 53
284, 285, 287, 288, 291, 292, 295, 440, Hebl, M. R., 27, 301, 486, 488, 489, 491,
451, 455, 482, 502, 504, 505, 510, 539 492, 495, 496, 539
Hall, K. P., 505 Hecht, M. A., 204, 213, 214, 285
Hallahan, M., 45, 46, 82, 165, 166, 208 Hecht, M. L., 264, 266, 268
Hallam, M., 150 Hedlund, L. E., 302, 312
Halsey, L. B., 366 Heft, H., 383
Halverson, H. M., 8 Heider, F., 45, 187
Hamilton, W. D., 141 Heider, K., 208, 224, 227
Hammermesh, D., 507 Heinrichs, M., 128
Hancock, J. T., 471, 474 Heintzman, M., 503
Hansen, J. S., 164 Heisel, A. D., 432
Harbaugh, C. R., 128 Hejl, J., 450
Hargreaves, D., 240, 247 Held, R., 183
Harizuka, S., 44, 229 Helmreich, R., 202
Harlow, H. F., 10, 128, 129, 135 Heltman, K. R., 205, 325, 332, 489, 505
Haroff, P., xxi Henderson, M., 225
Harpham, B., 23 Henderson, S., 468
Harrigan, J. A., 66, 183, 187, 349, 439, Henderson, W. M., 506, 507
440, 441, 446, 449, 450, 451, 452, 455 Henderson-King, D., 247
Harris, K., 248 Henderson-King, E., 247
Harris, M., xix, 42, 80 Hendricks, A., 250
Harrison, D., 184 Henggeler, S. W., xvii
Harrison, K., 247 Henley, N. M., 11, 89, 206, 211, 212, 214,
Harrison, R. P., 11, 100, 108 284, 483, 505
Harszlak, J. J., 80 Henningsen, D. D., 412
Hart, A. J., 165 Henry, C., 45
Harter, S., 281 Henson, A., 287
Hartman, B., 192 Hentzer, M., 125
Hartwig, M., 352, 356 Heritage, J., 450, 455
Harwood, J., xxii Hermsen, S., 71
Haslett, B. B., 192 Herring, C., 507
Hassin, R., 59, 325 Herskovitz, M. J., 330
AU-Index.qxd 6/30/2006 5:25 PM Page 544

544–––◆–––The SAGE Handbook of Nonverbal Communication

Herzberg, B., 5 Hoyt, C., 527


Herzog, J., 450 Hsee, C. K., 364
Heslin, R., 161, 174, 266 Hui, C. H., 220
Hess, E. H., 13 Hull, J. G., 301
Hess, U., 148, 184, 313, 327, Hume, D. K., 150
362, 365, 367 Hummert, M. L., xxi, 45
Hesse, B. W., 468 Hunter, J. E., 220
Hickes, M., 267 Hunter, S. B., 488
Hickson, M. L., III, 426 Huston, T. L., xx
Hietanen, J. K., 363 Hutchinson, A., 8
Higgins, E. T., 61 Hutchinson, S., 98
Higgs, M., 508 Hyden, L., 324
Hill, J., 43, 208 Hyers, L. L., 490
Hilton, J. L., 405 Hyman, L. M., 187, 194
Hiltz, S. R., 462 Hymes, D., 238
Hinde, R. A., 281, 287, 310
Hines, C., 431 Iacoboni, M., 373
Hinsz, V. B., 204, 213 Iacono, B., 468
Hirayama, S., 226 Ickes, W., 25, 26, 48, 272, 388,
Hjortsjö, C. B., 304 405, 406, 493
Hnilo, L. R., 245 Iizuka, Y., 83, 225, 528
Hochschild, A., 509 Inbar, I., 512
Hockett, C. F., 9 Inbau, F. E., 353, 355
Hocking, J. E., 343 Ingram, L. A., 364
Hodgins, H. S., 83, 195 Insel, T. R., 127, 128
Hofstede, G., 227, 504 Irish, J. T., 440, 446
Hogan, M. B., 25 Irvine, A., 350
Hogg, M. A., 464, 482, 485 Isabella, R. A., 268
Holcroft, T., 159 Isotalus, P., 471
Holladay, S. J., 513 Iwawaki, S., 150
Holland, R. W., 369, 388 Izard, C. E., 50, 146, 148, 184, 188, 189,
Hollingshead, A. B., 474 191, 222, 304, 383
Holman, D., 367, 509, 510
Holodynski, M., 331 Jaccard, J., xvii
Holt, G. R., 395 Jackson, D. D., 389
Holt, T., 105 Jackson, J. R., 494
Holtzworth-Munroe, A., 410 Jackson, L. A., 150
Homburg, D., 509 Jacobs, T. O., 502
Honda, M., 326, 327, 411 Jacobsen, L., xix, 10, 68, 80, 511
Hong, H. W., 108, 313 Jacobson, N. S., xxi
Hoobler, G., 287, 289 Jaffe, J., 387, 389
Horgan, T. G., 87, 207, 212, 214, 285, Jaffey, A., 302, 312
288, 292, 369 Jahoda, G., 220
Horner, C., 326 Jakobs, E. B., 313, 331, 332
Hornik, J., 513 James, J. W., 23
Hornik, R., 183 James, W., 30, 383
Horowitz, F. D., 182 Janis, S., 439
Horton, G., 192 Jansz, J., 213
Horvath, A. M., 220 Jarvenpaa, S. L., 468
Houle, R., 239, 240 Jarvis, W. B., 62
Howard, A., 66, 67, 90, 488, 491 Jasnow, M. D., 387, 389
Howell, J. M., 513 Jasper, C. M., 240
AU-Index.qxd 6/30/2006 5:25 PM Page 545

Author Index–––◆–––545

Jaworski, A., 238, 242, 243, 244, 245, 246, Kaitz, M., 165
247, 291, 539 Kalakanis, L., 150
Jaworski, J. S., 325 Kalman, Y. M., 468
Jayne, B. C., 353, 355 Kanki, B. G., 163
Jefferis, A. E., 388, 389 Kaplan, S. H., 439
Jefferis, V. E., 69, 371, 526 Kappas, A., 148, 313
Jefferson, G., 439, 443, 446 Karau, S. J., 489
Jenkins, L., 190 Kardes, F. R., 62
Jennings, J. R., 349 Karney, B. R., 271, 408
Johansson, G., 46, 186 Kash, K. L., 53
John, O. P., 88, 169, 202, 325, 367 Kashy, D. A., 326, 344, 345, 353
Johnson, B., 66, 67, 488, 491 Kasri, F., 227
Johnson, C., 66, 67, 281, 488, 491, 505 Kassin, S. M., 187
Johnson, E., 13, 90 Kastner, M. P., 193
Johnson, J., 202, 495 Kaswan, J. W., 106
Johnson, K., 462 Katzman, S., 470
Johnson, M. H., 182 Kaufman, D., 513
Johnson, M. L., 280 Kaufman, W., 322
Johnson, P., 370 Kawakami, C., 324
Johnson, R. L., 492 Kawakami, K., 62, 66, 67, 71, 369, 388,
Johnson, S. L., 53 488, 489, 491, 494
Johnson, T., 98, 99, 101, 106, 108 Kaye, S. A., 151
Johnson, V. S., 301 Kean, K. J., 187
Johnston, A., 45 Kearney, P., 425, 432
Johnston, L., 62 Keating, C. F., 205, 288, 325, 329, 332,
Johnston, R. E., 103, 108 489, 505
Johnstone, T., 45 Kees, W., 11
Jonas, A. P., 438 Keith, V. M., 507
Jones, A. J., 205 Keller, H., xviii
Jones, B. C., 44 Kellett, S., 364
Jones, C. R., 61 Kelley, D. H., 426, 427, 430
Jones, D., 150 Kelley, H. H., 284
Jones, E., xvi Kelly, A. B., 270
Jones, E. E., 220, 322 Kelly, J. R., 506, 507
Jones, J. M., 483 Keltner, D., 45, 49, 283, 287, 288, 322,
Jones, P., 221 325, 367, 383, 408, 482, 488
Jones, S. B., 23, 370 Kemper, S., 486
Jones, S. E., 98, 466 Kendon, A., 6, 7, 9, 10, 11, 12, 14, 98,
Jones, S. M., 270, 539–540 102, 108, 310, 388, 439, 441, 453
Jones, S. S., 108, 313 Kendrick, T., 325
Jones, T. S., 506, 512, 513 Kennedy, G., 4
Jonsson, P., 369 Kennedy, J., 431
Joorman, J., 53 Kenny, D. A., 48, 121, 162, 163
Jordan, A., 25, 350 Kenny, R. D., 326
Jordan, J., 474 Kenrick, D. T., 142
Jorgensen, P. F., 25, 370, 371 Kenwood, C., 98, 101, 108
Josephs, I., 108 Kenworthy, K. G., 302, 312
Judd, C. M., 209, 488 Kerkstra, A., xxi, 203, 455
Juette, A., 326, 327, 411 Kersens, J. J., 439
Juffer, F., 366 Kerssen-Griep, J., 271, 414
Jungeberg, B. J., 325 Kestenbaum, R., 183
Jussim, L., 32 Khouri, H., 407
AU-Index.qxd 6/30/2006 5:25 PM Page 546

546–––◆–––The SAGE Handbook of Nonverbal Communication

Kiecolt-Glaser, J. K., xvii, xviii Koopmann, B., 227


Kiel, E. J., 190 Kopelowicz, A., 89
Kienapple, K., 192 Korzenny, B. A. G., 202
Kieran, K., 409 Korzenny, F., 202
Kiesler, D. J., 452 Kosfeld, M., 128
Kikuchi, T., 29 Koslowski, L. T., 46
Kilbride, J. E., 228, 229 Koss, T., 452
Kim, H., 350, 474 Kouznetsova, N., 226
Kim, J., 474 Kowai-Bell, N., 488
Kim, M. S., 220 Kowner, R., 504
Kim, P. H., 364 Kozlowski, L. T., 193
King, L. A., 408 Kramer, C., 202, 214
King, M. C., 23 Kramer, R. M., 226
Kirk, C. T., 351 Krathwohl, D. R., 424, 430
Kirkendol, S. E., 342, 344, 345, 353 Krauss, R. M., 108, 288
Kirkpatrick, B., 127 Kraut, R. E., 103, 108, 472
Kirkpatrick, S. A., 513 Kravitz, R. L., 451
Kirson, D., 264 Kraxberger, B. E., 324
Kitsantas, P., 484 Krebs, J. R., 123, 124
Klassen, M. L., 240 Kremen, I., 309
Kleck, R. E., 42, 44, 148, 184, 301 Kretschmer, E., 8
Kleinbaum, S., 409 Kreutzer, M. A., 147, 224
Kleinke, C. L., 267, 372 Krikorian, D. H., 471
Kline, G. H., 409 Kring, A. M., 86, 325
Klinnert, M. D., 183 Kritzer, R., 387, 389
Kluckholn, C., 220 Kroeber, A. L., 220
Knapp, M. L., 11, 12, 14, 98, 146, 205, 210, Kruck, K. B., 327, 387, 411
267, 268, 426, 442 Kruglanski, A. W., 44
Knauer, M. J., 302, 312 Krull, D. S., 30, 34, 47, 68
Knee, C. R., 64 Krupp, D., 226
Knight, B., 45 Krych, M. A., 109
Knight, F. B., 165 Ksionsky, S., 265
Knight, S., 352 Kuc, M., 367
Knoll, K., 468 Kudoh, T., 165, 174, 187, 208, 221, 227
Knoth, R., 389 Kulik, J. A., 364
Knower, F. H., 147 Kulp, C., 12
Knower, R. W., 147 Kumar, S., 44
Knowles, M. L., 393, 407, 488 Kumin, L., 186, 193
Knowlton, S. W., 32 Kunda, G., 509
Kobayashi, T., 301 Kunda, Z., 30
Koch, P., 29 Kupperbusch, C., 225
Koch, P. T., 280 Kwon, Y. H., 241
Koerner, A. F., 272 Kylsten, P., 126
Koestner, R., 80, 195
Koff, E., 185 Laban, R., 8
Koivumaki, J., 11, 86 LaFleur, S. J., 505
Kojetin, B. A., 405 LaFrance, M., 67, 69, 202, 204, 213, 284,
Kolb, B., 527 285, 353, 382, 388, 483, 506
Kolbe, R. H., 240 Lakin, J. L., 52, 63, 65, 69, 70, 371, 388,
Komter, A., 284 389, 526, 540
Koo, J., xvii, 185 Lalande, K., 89
Kooken, K., 227 Lambert, W. W., 146
AU-Index.qxd 6/30/2006 5:25 PM Page 547

Author Index–––◆–––547

Lane, D. R., 424, 425, 426 Lever, T., 484


Langer, E. J., 324, 361 Levine, R., 238, 529
Langer, S., 438, 439, 443 Levine, T. R., 342, 346, 350, 351
Langlois, J. H., 149, 150 Levinson, R. W., xix, xx, xxi, 184, 190, 191
Lannoy, J. D., 14 Levinson, S., 98
Lanzetta, J. T., 148 Levinson, W., 47, 53
LaPlante, D., 13, 44, 47, 53, 90, 326, 512 Levy Paluck, E., 204, 213, 285
Larrance, D., 84, 86, 202 Lewis, M., 147, 148, 183, 190
Larsen, K. M., 439, 441, 453, 455 Ley, R. G., 184
Larson, A., 150 Liberman, M., 512
Larson, C. U., 221 Lichtenberg, J. W., 389
Larson, M. S., 239 Lickel, B., 488
Larson, R. W., 247 Liebler, A., 165, 167, 168, 174
Laser, P. S., 350 Lieblich, A. K., 313
Lashley, B. R., 351 Liebold, J. M., 491
Lassiter, G. D., 324, 354 Lim, M. M., 127, 128
Lavater. J. C., 159–160 Lin, M., 482
Lawrie, D. A., 108 Lindsay, J. J., 85, 323, 325, 333, 525
Lazar, M., 186, 193 Lindsay, J. L., 344, 345, 346, 347,
Lazarus, R. S., 309 348, 352, 354
Lea, M., 463, 464, 465, 529 Linell, P., 98
Leal, S., 349 Linnankoski, I., 363
Leary, M. R., 65, 302, 322, 324, 334 Linton, R., 220
Leathers, D. G., 343, 503 Linville, P. W., 220
Leavitt, L. A., xix Lipets, M. S., 86
LeBaron, C. D., 98, 102, 450, 452, 466 Lippa, R., 164, 165, 166, 167, 208
Lederhendler, I. I., 127 Lips, H. M., 281
LeDoux, J., 331 Lipsey, M. W., 209
Lee, E., 472 Liska, J., 280
Lee, J., 471 Little, A. C., 44
Lee, J. W., 267 Liu, J. F., 395
Lee, K., 150, 183 Liu, Y. L., 467
Lee, R. T., 510 Locke, E. A., 512, 513
Lee-Chai, A., 63 Lockheed, M. E., 505
Leeds-Hurwitz, W., 9, 98 Loeber, C. C., 505
LeFevre, J., 392, 395 Loh, T., 464, 467
Leffler, A., 291 Londahl, E. A., 408
Legerstee, M., xviii, 192 Long, B., 174, 290
Lehrer, M., 165 Longbotham, S., 343
Leibold, J. M., 494 Longworth, S. L., 395
Leidner, D. E., 468 Loomis, J., 474, 527
Leigh, T. W., 513 Losoya, S. H., xxi, 84, 404, 405
LeJeune, J., 322 Louiselle, P. A., xix
Lemery, C. R., 69, 108 Loumaye, E., 123
Lengel, R. H., 463, 467 Love, L. R., 106, 204
Lenzi, G. L., 373 Lucas, B., 23
Lepinasse, V., 62 Ludemann, P. M., 183
Le Poire, B. A., xvi, xvii, xxiii, 49, 264, 269, Luis, S., 151
282, 286, 287, 371, 413, 506, 541 Lundberg, M., 471
Lepper, H. S., 452 Lundqvist, L. O., 363
Leung, D. B., 284 Lupton, D., 244
Levenson, R. W., 148, 224, 368 Lyman, S. M., 290
AU-Index.qxd 6/30/2006 5:25 PM Page 548

548–––◆–––The SAGE Handbook of Nonverbal Communication

Lyons, J., 9 Martin, L. R., 452


Lyons, K. D., 393 Martin, M. M., 431
Martlew, M., 192
Ma, Y., 87 Masataka, N., xviii
MacGeorge, E. L., 408 Masia, B. B., 424, 430
Maciorowski, K., 221 Maslach, C., 510
MacLean, D., 50 Maslow, A. H., 10
Macrae, C. N., 62 Massimini, F., 392
Maczynski, J., 163 Mast, M., 81
Maddux, W. W., 65, 69, 70, 369 Masters, R. D., 249
Madson, L., 212 Masur, E. F., 192
Magee, J. C., 488 Matchen, J. C., 83
Magnusson, M. S., 327, 387 Mather, M., 184, 185
Mahl, G. F., 10 Matheson, D. H., 98, 191
Main, K. J., 489 Matias, R., 189
Major, B., 493 Matsumoto, D., 43, 81, 87, 165, 174, 187,
Mak, T., 242 208, 221, 225, 226, 227, 228, 540
Malamuth, N. M., xxi, 271 Maurer, D., 183
Malatesta, C. Z., 182, 184, 188, 189, 191 May, K. A., 301
Mallory, M. E., 302 Mayer, J. D., 80, 87, 90
Malloy, D., 389 Maynard, D. W., 450
Malloy, T. E., 48 Mayo, C., 11, 353
Malone, B. E., 84, 85, 323, 325, 333, 344, Mazloff, D., 45
345, 346, 347, 348, 352, 354 Mazziotta, J. C., 373
Malone, P. S., 525 McAdams, D. P., 259
Mamlin, J. J., 440 McArthur, L. Z., 30, 45, 330, 382, 384, 525
Mandal, M. K., 44, 229 McBride, G., 23
Mandisodza, A. N., 325 McCabe, A., 220
Mandler, G., 309 McCafferty, P., 248
Mandler, J. M., 309 McCarthy, G., 527
Manjon-Arce, P., 440, 441, 446 McClure, E. B., 183, 208
Manke, M. L., 266, 268 McCluskey, K. W., 229
Mann, M., 324 McConnell, A. R., 491, 494
Mann, S., 53, 342, 347, 348, 349, 351, 354, McConnell, S., 192
355, 509, 510 McCormick, N. B., 205, 206
Mannix, L. M., 491 McCornack, S. A., 342, 346, 350, 351
Mansell, W., 324 McCoy, S., 493
Manstead, A. S. R., 202, 214, 313, 331, 332 McCrae, R. R., 168
Manusov, V., 246, 265, 271, 325, 326, 367, McCroskey, J. C., 423, 424, 425, 426, 427,
403, 414, 415, 512, 522, 533 428, 429, 430, 431, 432, 503, 513, 541
Maricchiolo, F. M., 347 McCroskey, L. L., 424, 430, 433, 541
Markham, R., 228 McCulloch, R. E., 432
Markman, H. J., 271, 403, 409 McCullough, K., 442
Markus, M. L., 463, 464 McCusker, C., 220
Marler, P., 310 McDaniel, E., 266
Marlowe, C. M., 507 McDaniel, R. J., 186
Marrett, K., 474 McDaniel, S., 266, 268
Marsh, A. A., 42, 44, 45 McDowell, C., 184
Marsh, P., 221, 238 McGarty, C., 464, 465
Marston, P. J., 264, 266, 268 McGee, D. S., 438
Martin, H. J., 367 McGovern, T. V., 513
Martin, L. L., 213, 384 McGuire, L., xvii, xviii
AU-Index.qxd 6/30/2006 5:25 PM Page 549

Author Index–––◆–––549

McGuire, T. W., 463 Mishler, E., 455


McHugo, G. J., 148 Misovich, S. J., 187
McIntosh, D. N., 508 Misukanis, T. M., xix
McKee, A., 508, 510 Miyahara, A., 220
McKenzie, B. E., 192 Moberly, R. L., 389
McKerrow, R. E., 6 Mody, S., 220
McLagen, M., xviii Mogg, K., 50
McLeod, B. A., 341, 350 Moll, J., 71
McNatt, D. M., 511 Moller, A. P., 150
McNeal, J. U., 241 Molnar, P., 14
McNeilis, K. S., 438 Mongeau, P., 351
McNeill, D., 14, 98, 100, 104, 347, 348, 442 Mongrain, M., 408
McQuowan, N., 98 Monroe, A. H., 6
McVetta, R. W., 429 Monsour, M., 266, 270
Mechanic, D., 438 Montagu, A., 266
Mehrabian, A., xxi, 11, 13, 86, 89, 165, 174, Montagu, M. F. A., 11
264, 265, 267, 288, 324, 422, 423, 429, Monteith, M. J., 488
453, 512, 522 Montemayor, R., 206
Mehta, S. D., 229 Montepare, J. M., 161, 185, 188, 193,
Mendes, W. B., 488 288, 330, 331
Merkley, K. B., 389 Montgomerie, R., 150
Merrill, D. W., 431 Montgomery, S., 25
Mertz, T. J., 249 Moore, C. C., 224
Messinger, D. S., 189, 192 Moore, M., 411
Metcalfe, J., 34 Moran, G., 365
Meyer, S., 438 Mordkoff, A. M., 309
Michael, G., 186 Morissette, P., xix
Michaelis, B., 407 Morley, A. M., 468
Mignault, A., 288 Morman, M. T., 152
Mikkelson, A. C., xxii Morris, D., 11, 221, 238, 266
Mikulincer, M., 412, 413 Morris, J. A., 510
Millar, F. E., 281, 282 Morris, P., 163, 166
Millar, N., 50 Morris, R., 142
Millar, W. S., 366 Morris, W. L., 354
Miller, B. C., xvii Morrison, K., 342
Miller, C. T., 485, 493 Morry, M. M., 165
Miller, G. R., 351 Morse, S. T., 150
Miller, L. H., 440 Morton, J., 182
Miller, P. A., 405 Morton, T. L., 265
Miller, R. E., 81, 129 Moscowitz, D. S., 282
Miller, S. R., 325 Moskowitz, G. B., 61, 68
Milner, J. S., 366 Motowidlo, S. J., 163, 513
Milstein, T., 246 Mottet, T., 423, 424
Minami, M., 220 Mueller, R. A., xix
Mineo, P., 265 Muhlenbruck, L., 85, 323, 325, 333, 344,
Minn, J. Y., 395 345, 346, 347, 348, 352, 354, 525
Mintz, N. L., 10 Mulac, A., 370
Mirco, N., 303 Mull, H., 389
Mirsky, I. A., 129 Mullen, B., 249
Mirzoeff, N., 243 Mullett, J., 13, 69, 98, 108
Mischel, W., 34 Mullins, D. T., 165
Mishel, M. H., 438 Mumme, D. L., 183
AU-Index.qxd 6/30/2006 5:25 PM Page 550

550–––◆–––The SAGE Handbook of Nonverbal Communication

Muniyandi, M., 474 Nowak, K. L., 471, 472


Muraven, M. B., 67 Nowicki, S., 43, 45, 46, 81, 82, 403, 404,
Murdaya, U., 224 407, 484
Murphy, N. A., 207, 208 Nussbaum, J. F., 242
Murphy, S. T., 64, 373 Nuzzo, C., xviii
Murray, L. K., 301
Myers, A. M., 485, 493 Oakes, P. J., 482, 485
Myers, H. J., 108 O’Brien, C. E., 212
Myers, P. N., 240 O’Brien, P., 484
Ochs, E., 238
Nabi, R. L., 250 Ochsman, R. B., 472
Nadler, J., 364 Ochsner, K., 184
Nakamura, M., 131 O’Connell, D. M., 349
Nardi, B., 468 O’Connell, G. B., 489
Nathan, L. R., 66 O’Connor, B., 170, 371
Neale, J. M., 86 O’Connor, C., 264
Neff, K. D., 281 Ogawa, N., 129
Neff, L. A., 408 Ogston, W. D., 12
Neidenthal, P. M., 64 Okazaki, S., 395
Nelson, C. A., 183, 310 Olazabal, D. E., 127, 128
Nelson, C. E., 507 O’Leary, K. D., xx, 271
Nespoulous, J. L., 193 Oller, D. K., xviii
Nesse, R., 144 Olmos-Gallo, P. A., 409
Neuberg, S. L., 30, 62, 66, 482 Olson, D. H., 280, 281
Neuman, R., 363, 364 Olson, J. E., 291
Neumann, R., 45, 66 Olson, M. A., 489
Newcome, M. J., 512 Omar, A., 351
Newell, P. C., 126 Omark, D. R., 281
Newman, L. S., 61, 68 Ophet Veld, V., 184
Newton, D. A., 263, 264, 265, 266, 267, O’Reilly, A. W., 193
268, 269, 271, 286, 290 Orilikowski, W. J., 468
Ng, C. F., 168, 169, 173 Orimoto, L., 364
Nguyen, T., 47, 53 Orina, M., 272, 406
Nicholson, H., xvi Ortony, A., 307
Nicolich, M., 184, 188, 191 O’Shaughnessy, M., 221, 238
Niederhoffer, K. G., 388 Oster, H., 148, 189, 190
Niemi, R., 229 O’Sullivan, M., 84, 85, 227, 347, 349,
Nisbett, R., 49 351, 490
Nishida, T., 225 O’Sullivan, P. B., 464
Nissim, R., 23 Owren, M. J., 309
Nixon, J. C., 291 Oxman, T. E., 439, 440, 441, 446, 449,
Njiokiktjien, C., 184 450, 455
Noesjirwan, J., 221, 225 Ozyurek, A., 102, 110
Noll, J. G., 49, 322
Noller, P., xx, xxi, 14, 45, 271, 367, 403, Pagle, M., xxi
404, 405, 406, 410, 412, 413, 541 Paliwal, P., 190
Nomura, N., 220 Palmer, M. T., 64, 264, 265, 266, 367, 372
Norman, N., 332 Panksepp, J., 123, 128
Norman, R. Z., 285 Papa, A., 89
Norton, R., 431 Papousek, H., xviii, 387
Nosek, B. A., 43, 53 Papousek, M., xviii, 387
Notarius, C. I., xx, 271, 403 Park, B., 209
AU-Index.qxd 6/30/2006 5:25 PM Page 551

Author Index–––◆–––551

Park, H. S., 342, 351 Planalp, S., 262, 267, 268


Park, R. E., 381 Plant, E. A., 490
Parker, J. D. A., 331 Plax, T. G., 425
Parkinson, B., 309 Ploog, D., 144
Parks, M. R., 462, 465, 467 Ploutz-Snyder, R., 214, 285
Parrish, R. N., 472 Plumb, I., 208
Parrott, R. L., 439, 440, 455, 503, 506 Poggi, I., 322
Pash, L. A., 408 Polis, E., 439, 441
Pataki, S. P., 332 Polonsky, M., 124, 131
Patchen, M., 488 Pomiankowski, A., 150
Patterson, M. L., 13, 22, 25, 26, 27, 34, 35, Pontari, B. A., 68, 322, 323, 324, 332, 334
36, 52, 64, 67, 68, 71, 80, 83, 161, 162, Poortinga, Y. H., 220
165, 174, 248, 263, 266, 280, 322, 323, Popoola, O., 190
333, 334, 350, 382, 383, 410, 426, 486, Porter, A. M. W., 302
488, 493, 496, 501, 512, 523, 525, 528, Porterfield, A. L., 271
533–534 Posner, M. I., 60
Paul, S., 227, 228 Postmes, T., 464, 465, 529
Peabody, D., 168 Potter, J. D., 151
Peery, S., 207 Pouloit, M., 151
Pelham, B. W., 30, 34, 47, 68 Powe, N. R., 492
Pelphrey, K. A., 527 Powell, E., 165, 174, 282, 288
Peña, J., 471 Powell, J. L., 248
Pendleton, K. L., 186 Powell, M. C., 62
Pennebaker, J. W., 388 Powers, S. R., 541–542
Penrod, S., 208 Poyatos, F., 98
Penton-Voak, I. S., 150, 301 Prager, K. J., 259, 260, 261, 262, 263, 264,
Peolsson, M., 324 266, 267, 269, 270, 271
Perez, J. E., 89 Pratto, F., 62, 485
Perkowitz, W. T., 367 Preston, K., 225
Perrett, D. I., 44, 150, 301, 373 Prevost, D., 107, 108
Peterson, C., 14, 271 Priest, R. F., 265
Petrova, G., 226 Prince, L. M., 86
Pettigrew, T. F., 488 Prineas, R. J., 151
Pettijohn, T. E., III, 325 Proffitt, D. R., 182
Petty, R. E., 61, 62, 69 Prudhomme, D., 151
Philhower, C. L., 329 Pryor, J. B., 506
Philippot, P., 13, 183, 238, 327, 362, 367 Puccinelli, N. M., 382, 385, 386, 390, 391,
Phillips, B., 101, 108 392, 393, 511
Phillips, J. B., 323 Puerto-Barber, J., 440, 441, 446
Piaget, J., 135 Pugh, S. D., 364
Piccinini, C. A., 366 Putnam, F. W., 49, 322
Pickett, C. L., 393, 407
Pihan, H., 207 Quine, W. V. O., 102
Pike, G. R., 367 Quintilian, M. F., 4
Pike, K. L., 98 Quinton, W., 493
Pike, K. M., 204
Pinel, E. C., 323, 489 Raag, T., 108, 313
Pitre, U., 351 Radke-Yarrow, M., 405
Pittam, J., xxii, 468 Rafaeli, A., 510
Pittinger, R. E., 9 Rafaeli, S., 468
Pittman, T. S., 322 Rajecki, D. W., 25, 26
Piziak, V. K., 450 Ramesh, C. N., 350
AU-Index.qxd 6/30/2006 5:25 PM Page 552

552–––◆–––The SAGE Handbook of Nonverbal Communication

Randall, D. W., 325 Riggio, R. E., 14, 53, 80, 84, 85, 86, 87, 88,
Rao, S. R., 350, 352 89, 164, 325, 352, 353, 385, 455, 510,
Rapson, R. L., 65, 84, 268, 270, 331, 362, 513, 514, 542–543
388, 508 Rilling, J. K., 128
Rastatter, M., 484 Rime, B., 108
Raste, Y., 208 Rinck, C. M., 206
Rather, P., 126 Rintel, E. S., 468
Rauh, C., 124, 388 Risau Peters, J., xviii
Raven, B., 280, 430 Risenhoover, N., 183
Ray, G. B., 264, 266, 268 Ritts, V., 32
Raymond, G., 451 Ritzl, A., 373
Reeder, H., 266 Rizzolatti, G., 315, 316, 373, 527
Reeder, M. E., 268 Roach, M. A., xix
Regan, D. T., 124, 134, 135 Robers, T., 264
Reichenbach, H., 169 Roberts, A. R., 149, 301
Reicher, S. D., 464, 482, 485, 529 Roberts, C. A., xvii, 438
Reid, F. J. M., 468 Roberts, K. P., 51
Reid, J. E., 353, 355 Roberts, L. J., 49, 260, 261, 270
Reid, P. T., 484 Roberts, N., 410, 413
Reid, R. H., 431 Roberts, R., 90
Reis, H. T., 150, 259, 261 Robertson, R. R., 68
Reisenzein, R., 309 Robins, R. W., 43
Reiser, M., xxi Robinson, E., 405
Reiseter, K., 324, 327 Robinson, H., xxii
Remland, M. S., 289, 501, 502, 503, 506, Robinson, J., 405
512, 513, 542 Robinson, J. D., 442, 451, 454,
Ren, X., 127, 128 455, 543
Rendall, D., 309 Robinson, M. D., 384
Renfro, S., 388 Robinson, R. Y., 423
Renninger, L. A., 411 Robles, T. F., xvii, xviii
Restemeier, R., xviii Rodriguez, J. S., 326
Rezabek, L. L., 469, 470 Roe, L., 109, 347
Reznick, J. S., 69, 387, 389 Rogan, R., 351
Rhodes, G., 13, 42, 325 Rogers, P. L., 45, 46, 82, 208, 465, 510
Rholes, W. S., 61 Rogers, R. D., 128
Rice, K., 486 Rogers-Millar, E. L., 281, 282
Rice, R. E., 462, 468 Roggman, L. A., 149
Richard, E. M., 510 Rohner, R. P., 220
Richard, F. D., 210 Rollins, B. C., 281, 284
Richards, M. H., 247 Rollman, S. A., 486, 494
Richeson, J. A., 42, 46, 68, 327, 383, Roloff, M. E., 284, 361
384, 485, 486, 488, 489, 490, 493, 494, Romney, A. K., 224
495, 542 Roodenburg, H., 3
Richmond, V. P., 423, 424, 425, Rose, S. N., xviii
426, 427, 428, 430, 432, 503, Rosenbaum, R., 9
513, 542 Rosenbaum-Tamari, Y., 469
Ridgeway, C. L., 280, 281, 285, 286, Rosenberg, E., 13, 226
291, 485 Rosenberg, S. W., 248
Riedel, K., 126 Rosenfeld, H. M., 108, 182
Riesler, S., 463 Rosenholtz, S. J., 285, 291
Riggio, H. R., 53, 84, 86, 87, 88, 164, Rosenkrantz, P., 202
325, 455 Rosenstein, D., 148
AU-Index.qxd 6/30/2006 5:25 PM Page 553

Author Index–––◆–––553

Rosenthal, R., xvii, xix, 10, 12, 32, 34, 42, Saltz, J. L., 66
44, 45, 46, 47, 53, 64, 66, 68, 69, 80, Salvatore, J., 485, 489, 490, 493
82, 83, 90, 124, 134, 165, 166, 182, 185, Samter, W., 192, 270
187, 207, 208, 209, 214, 267, 268, 343, Sanbonmatsu, D. M., 62
344, 345, 362, 382, 384, 385, 387, 389, Sanchez de Rota, G., 202
390, 391, 439, 440, 441, 446, 449, 450, Sanchez-Garcia, E., 440, 441, 446
452, 455, 505, 511, 514, 525 Sanders, R. E., 98, 106, 442
Rosip, J. C., 53, 208, 214, 285, 288, 292 Sanderson, D., 469
Rosnow, R. L., 44 Sanfey, A. G., 128
Ross, E. D., 130 Sargent, J., 53
Roter, D., 440, 492 Satyaprasad, C., 186
Roth, W. T., 302 Saville-Troike, M., 238
Rothman, A. D., 46 Savin, V. J., 81
Rowland, D. A., 150 Sawyer, J., 265
Rowland-Morin, P. A., 438 Saxe, L., 66, 492
Roy, A., 241 Schaap, C., xxi
Rubenstein, A. J., 149, 150 Schachner, D. A., 412
Rubin, D. B., xix Schachter, S., 23
Rubin, M., 124 Schaufeli, W. B., 364, 509
Rubin, R. S., 325 Schaut, J. A., 302, 312
Rubinow, S., 11, 98 Scheflen, A., 9, 98
Rudman, L. A., 323 Schegloff, E. A., 439, 440, 441, 443, 446,
Ruedenberg-Wright, L., 469 450, 451, 452
Ruesch, J., 10, 11 Scherer, K. R., 12, 13, 44, 45, 46, 51, 98,
Ruiz-Belda, M. A., 108, 147, 309 168, 189, 190, 208, 224, 349
Rumsey, N., 351 Schieffelin, B., 238
Rusch, C. D., 224 Schiffrin, R. M., 60
Rushbult, C. E., 284 Schimmack, U., 227
Rushe, R., 410 Schlenker, B. R., 68, 302, 322, 323, 324,
Rushton, J. P., 163 332, 334
Russell, J. A., 11, 32, 43, 44, 146, 300, 305, Schmid Mast, M., 207, 208, 212
306, 307, 310, 331, 543 Schmidt, K. L., 124
Russell, M., 438 Schmitt, A., 326, 327, 411
Russin, A., 71 Schneider, F. W., 25
Rutherford, D., 262, 267, 268 Schneider, K., 108, 389
Ruusuvuori, J., 445, 453, 455 Schneider, S. L., 507
Ruzzene, M., 405 Schneider, W., 60
Ryan, E. B., xxi Schober, M. F., 108
Schooler, J. W., 34, 525
Saarni, C., 183, 189, 239, 327, 331 Schreiber, D. M., 543
Saavedra, R., 508 Schulkin, J., 373
Sabatelli, R. M., 124 Schwartz, A. M., 240
Sabini, J. P., 104, 302, 312, 313 Schwartz, B., 165, 174, 282, 288
Sachs, V. B., 190 Schwartz, G., 148
Sacks, E. A., 451 Schwartz, J., 224, 264
Sacks, H., 439, 443, 446 Schwartz, W., 528
Saine, T. J., 426 Scollon, R., 251
Sakamoto, Y., 494 Scott, M. B., 290
Salinas, C., 88, 325 Seaton, C., 491
Sallinen, A., 430 Sebeok, T., 281
Salovey, P., 80, 87, 90 Segall, M. H., 220, 330
Salter, V., xvi Segerstrale, U., 14
AU-Index.qxd 6/30/2006 5:25 PM Page 554

554–––◆–––The SAGE Handbook of Nonverbal Communication

Segrin, C., 284 Singh, D., 151, 329


Seidell, J. C., 151 Sixma, H. J., 509
Seilgman, M. E., 220 Ska, B., 193
Seiter, E., 249 Skaggs, L. M., 351
Seiter, J. S., 241 Skarin, K. S., 190
Sekaquaptewa, D., 494 Skinner, G., 450
Sellers, R. M., 488, 493 Skouteris, H., 192
Semin, G. R., 352 Slama-Cazacu, T., 98
Seyfarth, R. M., 122 Slane, S., 267
Shah, J. Y., 63 Sleight, C., 351
Shapiro, P. N., 208 Slovacek, C., 464, 465, 466
Shaver, P. R., 224, 259, 261, 264, 412, 413 Smith, A., 484
Shaw, A., 242, 244, 245, 247 Smith, C. K., 439, 441, 455
Shawn, T., 6 Smith, D. A., 86
Shea, M., 108 Smith, D. A. F., 408
Sheehan, G., 14, 271 Smith, H. J., 64
Sheldon, W. H., 9 Smith, J., 313
Shell, R., 405 Smith, K. R., 165
Shelton, J. N., 42, 485, 486, 488, 489, 490, Smith, L., 214
493, 494, 495, 543–544 Smith, M., 287
Shenk, J. L., xx Smith, M. D., 408
Shenker, J. I., 302, 312 Smith, N. C., 368, 385, 386
Shennum, W., 284 Smith, P. M., 51, 367
Sheppard, C., 413 Smith, R. R., 11
Sherbaum, C., 483 Smith, S. L., 239, 240, 245
Sherman, R. A., 395 Smith, W. J., 283, 287, 313
Sherry, J. L., 245 Smith LeBeau, L., 212, 280, 284, 285, 287,
Shields, S. A., 302 288, 291, 292, 295, 482
Shildrick, M., 244 Smoot, M., 150
Shilling, C., 242 Smutzler, N., 410
Shippy, R. A., 364 Snodgrass, S. E., 214, 285, 333
Sholiton, R. D., 309 Snowdon, C. T., 224
Short, J., 462 Snyder, C. R. R., 60
Shulman, G., 202 Snyder, M., 87, 88, 325
Shulz, M. S., 409 Snyder, S. S., 186
Shuter, R., 221 Solomon, D. H., 264, 265, 414
Sidanius, J., 485 Sommer, R., 22, 290, 522
Sidelinger, R. J., 431 Sonnby-Borgstrom, M., 369
Siegel, A. E., 13 Sorce, J. F., 183
Siegel, J., 463, 472 Sorenson, E. R., 10, 147, 222, 305
Siegel, S. M., 282 Spears, R., 62, 464, 465, 529
Siegman, A. W., 51 Speisman, J. C., 309
Sigman, S. J., 9, 10 Spence, J. T., 202
Sillars, A. L., 367 Spetner, N. B., 182
Simmel, M., 45, 187 Spielman, D. A., 239, 240
Simmons, K. B., 64, 264 Spinella, G., xvii
Simon, A., 302 Spitz, H. H., 13
Simpson, J. A., 142, 272, 330, 406, 412 Spitzberg, B. S., 268
Singelis, T. M., 364 Springer, K., 187
Singer, E., 393 Sproull, L., 472
Singer, J. E., 23 Srinivasan, M. A., 474
Singer, M. A., 326 Srull, T. K., 61, 62
AU-Index.qxd 6/30/2006 5:25 PM Page 555

Author Index–––◆–––555

St. Peters, M., 409 Street, R. L., Jr., xvi, 367, 438, 441, 443,
Stack, D. M., 365 450, 452, 455
Stacks, D. W., 426 Streeter, L. A., 288
Stafford, B. M., 244 Stromwall, L. A., 352, 356
Stairs, D., 249 Strong, R. K., 47
Stallings, J., xviii Strongman, K., 229
Stamp, G. H., 442 Strough, J., 393
Stangor, C., 489 Stuart, G. L., 410
Stanley, S. M., 409 Sturgill, G., 126
Stark, R. E., xviii Suddendorf, T., 373
Steckler, N. A., 505 Suler, J., 471
Steele, C. M., 62 Sullivan, D. G., 249
Steenaert, B., 70 Sullivan, L. A., 489
Stein, J. L., 323 Sullivan, M., 147, 148
Steinberg, M., 12 Sullivan, M. W., 190
Steiner, M., xviii Summers, J. O., 513
Steinfield, C. W., 468 Surakka, V., 363
Stelzner, M. A., 268 Sutton, C., 106, 108, 110
Stenberg, C., 148 Sutton, M., 107, 108
Stennett, J., 45 Sutton, R. I., 510
Stenson, L. L., 25 Suzuki, R., 227, 228
Stephan, C. W., 485, 489 Swaim, G. W., 86
Stephan, W., 485, 489 Swan, K. P., 464
Stepper, S., 213 Swann, W. B., Jr., 32, 47, 525
Sterling, R. S., 187 Swim, J. K., 490
Stern, D. N., xix Swinkels, A., 165
Stern, L. A., 29, 35, 48, 268, 291, 362, 370, Swinth, K., 527
387, 388, 389 Symmes, D., xviii
Sternberg, R. J., 261 Symons, D., 365
Sternglanz, R. W., 405, 406 Synnott, A., 248
Stevenson, M. B., xix
Stewart, R., 280 Tabert, M., 207
Stice, E., xviii Tajfel, H., 226, 482, 485
Stier, D. S., 205, 206 Takeuchi, S., 226
Stiff, J. B., 346, 350, 351 Tamir, M., 384
Stillwell, A. M., 67 Tanford, S., 25, 26
Stinson, L., 405 Tang, J., 324
Stivers, T., 442 Taranta, A., 82
Stock, R. M., 509 Tascovich, J., 527
Stockbridge, E., 34, 64, 68, 525 Tassinary, L. G., 51
Stokes-Zoota, J. J., 210 Tate, C. S., 484
Stolberg, S. G., 331 Tatsuoka, M. M., 280
Stone, J. I., 324 Tay, B. K., 474
Stone, J. L., 354 Taylor, D. A., 262
Stoner, G. M., 474 Taylor, M. J., 331
Storck, J., 472 Taylor, S. E., 30, 34, 525
Stott, C., 468 Taylor, S. J., 80
Stoufe, L. A., 147 Tcherkassof, A., 307
Strack, F., 34, 35, 45, 66, 213, 363, Tchividjian, L. R., 324
364, 382 Tedeschi, J. T., 332
Strauss, E., 184 Templeton, J. L., 65, 302
Streeck, J., 98, 102, 442, 449 Terry, J., 243, 248
AU-Index.qxd 6/30/2006 5:25 PM Page 556

556–––◆–––The SAGE Handbook of Nonverbal Communication

Terwilliger, E. F., 127, 128 Trotschel, R., 63


Tesser, A., 165, 174, 282, 288 Tsai, J. L., 224
Teuchmann, K., 364 Tubbs, M., 528
te Velde, E. G., 151 Tucker, J. S., 88, 89, 352, 412, 413
Texter, L. A., 80 Tulloch, J., 244
Thakerar, J. N., 150 Turban, D. B., 327
Thibaut, J. W., 284 Turkstra, L., 491
Thomas, C. E., 432 Turner, J. C., 482, 485
Thomas, M. A., 182 Turner, T. J., 307
Thompson, L. L., 364 Turpie, C., 489, 493
Thompson, M., 494 Twamley, E. W., 247
Thorner, J., 123 Tyack, P. L., 130
Thornhill, R., 150, 151, 329 Tyler, J. M., 544
Thornton, G. C., 90
Thorpe, W. H., 287 Uleman, J. S., 59, 61, 68
Throckmorton, B., 84, 89, 352, 513 Ullstadius, E., 365
Thunberg, M., 42, 50, 66 Urla, J., 243, 248
Thweatt, K. S., 430
Tice, D. M., 67, 249, 324 Valencic, K. M., 430, 432
Tickle, J. J., 354, 489 Valenti, S. S., 387, 389
Tickle-Degnen, L., 267, 268, 382, Vallacher, R. R., 34, 525
384, 385, 386, 387, 389, 390, 391, 393, van Baaren, R. B., 70, 369, 388
511, 544 Vancouver, J. B., 390
Tidwell, L. C., 464, 465, 466, 468 Vandeputte, D., 486
Tiedens, L. Z., 52, 65, 327, 488, 504 Van der Pasch, M., 439
Tiggemann, M., 240, 247 Van Dulmen, A. M., 439
Tilley, P., 474 Van Egeren, L. A., xviii, xix
Ting, C. Z., 183 Vangelisti, A. L., xx
Ting-Toomey, S., 225 van Ijzendoorn, M. H., 366, 370
Tinsley, H. E. A., 513 van Knippenberg, A., 62, 70, 369,
Todorov, A., 325 371, 388
Tomasian, J., 190 VanLear, C. A., 122, 130
Tomhave, J. A., 204, 213 Van Lieshout, C. F., 183
Tomkins, S. S., 146 Van Maanen, J., 509
Tooby, J., 144 Vanman, E. J., 66
Toorenaar, N., 184 Van Meel, J., 186
Totterdell, P., 364, 367, 509, 510 van Noord, P. A. H., 151
Towles-Schwen, T., 66 van Swol, L. M., xvi
Tracy, J. L., 43 Vargas, P., 494
Trager, G. L., 9 Vasquez, K. A., 494
Trail, S., 53 Veccia, E. M., 206
Tranel, D., 46, 53 Verbeke, W., 364, 509
Trawalter, S., 488, 490 Verburgh, H., 186
Trees, A. R., 415, 512 Verhaak, P. F. M., 439
Tremblay, A., 151 Ver Hoeve, J. N., xix
Trevarthen, C., 189 Vernon, P., 8, 161
Triandis, H. C., 146, 220 Verschoor, C. A., 184
Trickett, P. K., 49, 322 Vettese, L. C., 408
Trivers, R. L., 141 Vieira, T. Jr., 131
Trope, Y., 325 Villanueva, C., 225
Tropp, L. R., 490 Vinacke, W. E., 147
Trost, M. R., 144 Viola, R. J., 527
AU-Index.qxd 6/30/2006 5:25 PM Page 557

Author Index–––◆–––557

Viscovich, N., 207 Watzlawick, P., 389


Viteles, M. S., 165 Waxer, P. H., 225
Voelker, S., xviii Weathers, M. D., 484
Vogel, S., 202 Webb, A., 528
Vogt, D. S., 208, 209 Weber, H., 327, 331
Vogt, S., 373 Webster, S., 51
Vohs, K. D., 322, 333 Weeks, G. D., 472
Volkmar, F. R., 13 Wegner, D. M., 13, 34, 526
von Hippel, W., 70, 494 Weigel, R. H., 164
von Salisch, M., 189 Weinberger, M., 440
Vorauer, J. D., 489, 493, 494 Weiner, M., 98, 512
Vranken, M., 184 Weisband, S., 468
Vrba, E. S., 304 Weissman, M., 225
Vrieswiik, B., 151 Weitz, S., 486, 491
Vrij, A., 51, 53, 163, 166, 221, 324, 341, Welch, C. E., 439
342, 346, 347, 348, 349, 350, 351, 352, Welkowitz, J., 367
353, 354, 355, 356, 544 Wendleand-Carro, J., 366
Vrugt, A., 203, 211 Werner, C. M., 468
Wertin, L., 269
Wade, A., 106, 108 Wesphal, M., 89
Wade, J. T., 411 West, J. F., 291
Wagner, D. G., 485 Wetherell, M. S., 482, 485
Wagner, H. L., 313 Wettersten, K. B., 389
Wagner, S. L., xvi Wexley, K. N., 491
Wais, P., 184 Wharton, A. S., 510
Waldinger, R. J., 409 Wheeler, S. C., 61, 62, 69
Walker, H. A., 285, 291 Wheeless, L. R., 280, 429, 430
Walker-Andrews, A. S., 182 Wheelwright, S., 43, 208
Wall, S., 268 Whelan, G., 438
Wall, T., 367 Whipple, C., 325
Wallace, A. R., 140 Whishaw, I. Q., 527
Wallbott, H. G., 8, 185, 208, 221, 347, 349 Whitaker, D. J., 384
Walster, E. H., 13 Whitcher, S. J., 25
Walther, J. B., 286, 290, 462, 463, White, C. H., 29, 53, 344, 345, 346, 474
464, 465, 466, 467, 468, 469, 470, 472, White, J. B., 324
544–545 White, S. E., 242
Walton, J. H., 372 White, S. S., 512
Wang, L., 228 Whiten, A., 373
Wang, Z., 127, 128 Whitfield, S., 184
Wanzer, M. B., 432 Whittaker, S., 468
Ward, C., 229 Wicker, A. W., 32
Ward, J., 269 Wiegel, R. H., 281, 282
Ware, J. E., 439 Wiegman, O., 250
Warner, R. M., 389 Wiemann, J. M., 12, 291
Warren, J. A., 66 Wiener, M., 11
Waters, C. M., 125, 126 Wiewel, T. S. R., 325
Waters, E., 268 Wiggers, M., 183
Watring, K., 84 Wiggins, J. S., 169
Watson, D., 166 Wilcox, B., 50
Watson, M. W., 192 Wild, B., 365
Watson, O. M., 221 Wilder, B., 389
Watt, J. H., 472 Wilhelm, F. H., 302, 368, 385, 386
AU-Index.qxd 6/30/2006 5:25 PM Page 558

558–––◆–––The SAGE Handbook of Nonverbal Communication

Wilkinson, M., 168, 169, 173, 513 Yamada, H., 227, 228
Williams, C. J., 494 Yamaguchi, S., 226, 228
Williams, E., 462 Yarczower, M., 228, 229
Williams, J. H., 373 Yashimoto, D., 410
Williams, J. R., 128 Yates, S., 250
Williams, K. D., 70 Yates, Y., 468
Williams, M., xvi Yee, N., 70, 527
Willis, F. N., 186, 206 Yeo, R. A., 329
Wilmot, W. W., 270 Yik, M. S. M., 307, 310
Wilson, B. J., 239, 240 Yirmiya, N., 367
Wilson, D. B., 209 Yoo, S. E., 87
Wilson, E. O., 123, 127 Yoo, S. H., 226
Wilson, J. F., 438, 443 Yoon, H. J., 220
Wilson, K., 66 Yoshikawa, S., 301
Wilson, T., 49 Yoshimoto, J., 351
Wilson, T. D., 34, 525 Yoshioka, M., 220
Winkel, F. W., 221, 353 Young, D. M., 513
Winograd, C. H., xvii, 185 Young, L., 127, 128, 160
Winter, L., 61 Young, M. J., 354
Wiseman, R., 504 Young, R. C., 283
Witkowski, S., 163 Young, R. K., 151
Witmer, D., 470 Young, S. L., xx
Witton, S. W., 409 Young-Browne, G., 182
Woike, B. A., 187, 194 Yukl, G., 504
Wolfer, J. E., 439
Wolff, W., 187 Zaadstra, B. M., 151
Wood, J. T., 403 Zach, U., xviii
Wood, R. M., 128 Zahn-Waxler, C., 405
Wood, W., 212, 481 Zaidel, S. F., 86
Woodall, W. G., 262, 267, 287, 322, 323, Zaitchik, D., 185
324, 326, 333, 409 Zajonc, R. B., 62, 64, 372, 373
Woods, J., 102, 110 Zak, P. J., 128
Woodson, R., 182 Zammuner, V., 225
Woodworth, M., 474 Zanna, M. P., 68, 69, 486, 491, 492, 511
Woodzicka, J. A., 506 Zebrowitz, L. A., 13, 31, 32, 42, 43,
Woolpy, J. H., 128, 135 67, 288, 325, 326, 330, 331, 382, 384,
Wooten, A. G., 432 404, 507
Word, C. O., 68, 69, 486, 491, 492, 511 Zebrowitz-McArthur, L., 188, 194, 288
Worthman, C., xviii Zelditch, M., 285, 291, 485
Wroblewska, A. M., 326 Zeman, J., 190
Wu, C. H., 149, 301 Zhou, Q., xxi
Wu, S., 224 Zilles, K., 373
Wyer, M. M., 344, 345, 353 Zimmerman, J. A., 88
Wyers, R. S., 61, 62, 271, 323, 407 Zinober, B., 192
Zuckerman, M., 80, 83, 84, 86, 202, 207,
Xin, K. R., xxiii 208, 343, 344, 345
Sub-Index.qxd 6/30/2006 5:25 PM Page 559

SUBJECT INDEX

Abilities model of emotional Affect/arousal and race/intergroup


intelligence, 80, 87 interaction, 489–490
Ability reward structures, 286 See also Emotions/emotional
Absence of nonverbal cues, xxii displays
Accuracy and studying Affect-based theories, 23–26
emotional expressions, 44 Affective Communication Test
See also Skill approach to (ACT), 86
nonverbal communication Affective empathy, 404–405
Adaptation(s) Affective learning outcomes and
computer-mediated instructional communication,
communication, 464–465 424–425, 430
evolutionary approach, 141–144 Affective-relational dimension to
methods for studying, physician-patient interaction,
review of, 48 438–439
natural selection, 31 Affective socialization and the
See also media, 238–240
Coordination/management Affinity construct and
of interactions instructional communication,
Adjacency-pair sequence, 451–452 429–430
Adolescents, xviii Affordances and automatic
See also Age, factoring in; cognitive processes, 67
Children Affordances and rapport between
Affect, xvii, xx, xxii, 28-30, 32, 35, individuals, 383, 384
49-50, 52, 71, 86-87, 190, African-Americans. See Race and
192, 204, 212-213, 238-240, intergroup interaction
250, 260, 261-265, 267-269, Age, factoring in
271-272, 362-363, 365, 367, infants/children, decoding body
373, 381, 384, 391, 394, 405, movements in, 185–187
410, 412, 413, 414, 416, infants/children, decoding facial
424-426, 428, 430, 432, 433, expressions in, 182–184
462, 464, 465, 467, 470, 474, infants/children, encoding body
486, 487, 492, 508, 522 movements in, 192–193

◆ 559
Sub-Index.qxd 6/30/2006 5:25 PM Page 560

560–––◆–––The SAGE Handbook of Nonverbal Communication

infants/children, encoding facial expressions emotional expressions, 65–66


in, 189–190 expectancies, 68–69
overview, 181–182 future directions, 70–72, 524–526
summary/conclusions, 194–195 goals, 60–61, 63, 526
younger/older adults, decoding body impressions of other people, 61–62, 67–68
movements in, 187–189 methodological concerns, 70–71
younger/older adults, decoding facial mimicry, 69, 70
expressions in, 184–185 overview, 59–60, 63–64
younger/older adults, encoding body prejudice, 66–67
movements in, 193–194 rapport, 69–70
younger/older adults, encoding facial relationships, social, 64–65
expressions in, 190–192 resources, importance of cognitive, 72
Allport, Gordon, 161 role of, 71–72
Altruism, 121 self-presentations, 67–68
Angularity, facial, 301 summary/conclusions, 72
Anthropomorphic icons and See also Coordination/management of
computer-mediated communication, interactions, automatic; Impression
471–472, 475 management; Parallel process model
Antithesis principle and evolution of emotion Avatars, 470–471, 475
displays, 145 Awareness and automatic vs. controlled
Anxiety and race/intergroup interaction, cognitive processes, 60
485, 490
Anxiety and rapport between individuals, 391 Bar-On Emotional Quotient Inventory
Aphasia, 130 (EQI), 80
Appeasement displays and power/dominance Bateson, Gregory, 9, 10
in human relationships, 282 BBC, 243
Arafat, Yasser, 246 Beauty, fascination with physical, xxii
Aristotle, 4, 160, 300 Behavioral ecology view of facial
Arnett, Peter, 355 expressions, 309–315
Arousal-labeling theory, 23–25 Behavior and automatic cognitive
Arte of Rhetorique, The (Wilson), 5 processes, 62
Assertiveness and instructional Bell, Charles, 300
communication, 431–432 Berkeley Expressivity Questionnaire (BEQ), 86
Attachment and biological foundations of Bias in decoding, intimate
social organization, 127–129 interactions/relationships and, 406–407
Attachment security/insecurity and intimate Biographical Sketch of an Infant,
interactions/relationships, 412–414 A (Darwin), 147
Attentiveness, rapport and mutual, 384, 388 Biological causes for human behavior, 10, 527
Attitudes and automatic cognitive See also Natural selection; Social
processes, 62 organization, biological foundations of
Attraction, evolutionary perspective Biology and parallel process model, 31
on, 149–152 Birdwhistell, Ray L., 9–10
Attributions Black box approach to role of nonverbal
automatic cognitive processes, 68 cues in communication, 463–464
intimate interactions/relationships, 414–415 Blended expressions, 44
physician-patient interaction, 453 Blink: The Power of Thinking Without
Austin, Gilbert, 5, 6 Thinking (Gladwell), 514
Autism, xvii Blushing, 302–303
Automatic cognitive processes Boas, Franz, 8
attitudes, 62 Body appearance/orientation
behavior, 62 deception, detecting, 351
controlled vs., 60–61 facial cues/expressions, 303–304
Sub-Index.qxd 6/30/2006 5:25 PM Page 561

Subject Index–––◆–––561

gender differences, 205–207 Cicero, Marcus T., 4


impression management, 321–322, Civil exchange and moral emotions, 134–135
325–327, 329 Civil inattention and physician-patient
instructional communication, 427 interaction, 450
intimate interactions/relationships, Clarity and instructional communication,
266, 411–412 430–431
media, codes encouraged by the, 242–244, Classroom environment. See Instructional
247–248 communication
methods for studying, review of, 52–53 Clinton, Bill, 245
personality traits and, relationship Clothing and impression management,
between, 8–9 325–326
physician-patient interaction, 440–441, Cognitive-affective mediators and parallel
448–449, 453–454 process model, 32
power/dominance in human See also Automatic cognitive processes
relationships, 288 Cognitive socialization and the media,
race and intergroup interaction, 484 240–241
See also Space, interpersonal Cohabitation, 409
Brachio-manual communication system, 316 Collaborative virtual environments
Brief Affect Recognition Test (BART), 81, 83 (CVEs), 527
Broca’s area and mirror neurons, 316 Colleges, 1970s and courses in nonverbal
communication offered at, 11
Categorical approach and instructional Coloration changes, facial, 302–303
communication, 426, 427–429 Communication of Affect Receiving Ability
Categorical reward structures, 286 Test (CARAT), 81
Categorization, race and social, 485 Communication scholars and
Center for Advanced Behavioral Studies, 9 power/dominance in human
Centrality and power/dominance in human relationships, 281
relationships, 290–291 Communication theory and instructional
Chameleon effect, 52 communication, 421
Channel approach to studying nonverbal Communicative gene hypothesis, 121–122
communication, 522 Comparable truth techniques and detecting
Children deception, 354–355
coordination/management of interactions, Compensation and equilibrium theory, 22
automatic, 365–367 Complementary behaviors and automatic
deception, detecting, 350 cognitive processes, 65
development impacted by nonverbal Computer-mediated communication (CMC)
communication, xviii–xix adaptation theories, 464–465
evolution of emotion displays, 147–148 anthropomorphic icons, 471–472, 475
health impacted by nonverbal avatars, 470–471, 475
communication, xvii black box approach to role of nonverbal
media, codes encouraged by the, 241 cues in communication, 463–464
smiling and gender differences in nonverbal chronemics, 467–469
communication, 204 critique and consequences, 473–475
See also Age, factoring in deception, detecting, 474
China, 326 emoticons, 469–470
Chironomia (Austin), 5, 6 hyperpersonal, 465–466
Chronemics lack of social context hypothesis, 463
computer-mediated communication, media richness theory, 463
467–469 overview, 461–462, 528–529
instructional communication, 429 reintroducing cues, 469–473
intimate interactions/relationships, social identification model of
268–269 deindividuation effects, 464–465
Sub-Index.qxd 6/30/2006 5:25 PM Page 562

562–––◆–––The SAGE Handbook of Nonverbal Communication

social information processing, 466–467 emotion, facial expressions of, 222–229


social presence theory, 462–463 evolution of emotion displays, 146–147
videoconferencing, 472–473 facial cues/expressions, 225, 299,
virtual reality systems, 474–475 300, 305–309
Conflict interaction paradigm, 52 gaze, 221
Conflict withdrawal and intimate gestures, 220–221
interactions/relationships, 410 immediacy cues, xxi–xxii
Confucius/Confucianism, 4, 395 impression management, 325
Congreve, William, 160 ingroup advantage in emotion recognition,
Consistency and relationship between 228–229
personality/nonverbal behavior, 163 instructional communication, 433
Context analysis, 9, 163 intimate interactions/relationships, 411
See also individual subject headings judgments of emotion, 227–228
Control/regulation parallel process model, 32
automatic cognitive processes, 60–61 personality/nonverbal behavior, relationship
conversation control, 290–292 between, 164
skill approach to nonverbal race and intergroup interaction, 483, 484
communication, 87–89 rapport between individuals, 394–395
See also Automatic cognitive processes; space, interpersonal, 221
Coordination/management of summary/conclusions, 229–230
interactions, automatic;
Power/dominance in human Daily Mail, The, 246
relationships Daily Telegraph, The, 245
Cooperative principle, 106 Darwin, Charles, 6–7, 300
See also Rapport, the ecosystem of David, Jacques-Louis, 243
Coordination/management of Dean, Howard, 331
interactions, automatic Deception, detecting
defining terms, 362 accuracy in, 349–351
emotional expression linkages, comparable truth techniques, 354–355
363–365, 370 computer-mediated communication, 474
evaluating theories of coordination, diagnostic cues, pay attention to, 354
370–371 heuristics, the use of, 351–352
facial cues/expressions, 363, 371–373 implicit vs. explicit lie detection, 354
implications for theory, 369–370 improving abilities in, 354–356
infant-mother interactions, 365–367 individual and situational differences,
microcoordination, 363 348–349, 352–353
mimicry, 368–371, 373–374, 387 interpersonal deception theory, 344–345
mutual coordination, 362–369 interviewing process, 355–356
neurological bases, 315–316, 373–374, 527 meta-analysis of cues to deception, 345–346
overview, 361–362 multifactor model, 342–343
rapport, individuals in, 384, 387–389 overlooked, cues, 347–348
relational and individual conditions, overview, 341–342
367–369 in partners/friends/children, 350
research on, 362–369 presentational perspective, 343–344
summary/conclusions, 374 professional lie catchers, 350–351
theories on, 369–374 skill approach to nonverbal communication,
Corrugator muscle and facial cues/ 84–85
expressions, 50 social organization, biological foundations
Courtship displays, 411–412 of, 123–125
Culture in strangers, 349–350
deception, detecting, 353 wrong cues, looking at the, 352
defining, 219–220 See also Impression management
Sub-Index.qxd 7/10/2006 11:56 AM Page 563

Subject Index–––◆–––563

Decoding, nonverbal, 81–85 Emoticons, 469–470


See also Personality and nonverbal Emotional Contagion Scale, 364
behavior; individual subject headings Emotional Expressivity Scale (EES), 86
de Jorio, Andrea, 6–7 Emotion Facial Affect Coding Scheme
Delsarte, Francois, 5–6 (EMFACS), 50
Demand-withdrawal patterns during marital Emotion Regulation Questionnaire (ERQ), 88
conflict, xx–xxi Emotions/emotional displays
Demeanor bias, 87 automatic cognitive processes, 65–66
Dependent variables, nonverbal displays coordination/management of interactions,
conceptualized as, 41, 42, 49–53 automatic, 363–365, 370
Descartes, 300 culture, 222–229
Developmental outcomes impacted by facial cues/expressions, 225, 299, 300,
nonverbal communication, xviii–xix 305–309
Deviant body, fear of the, 243 impression management, 331–332
Diagnostic Analysis of Nonverbal Accuracy intelligence, abilities/mixed model of
(DANVA), 43, 46, 81–83, 407, 510 emotional, 80, 87
Diagnostic cues and detecting deception, 354 media, codes encouraged by the, 238–240
Dictyostelium discoideum, 126 natural selection, 144–149
Digital editing software, 44 organizational life, 508–511
Discrepancy-arousal theory, 24 parallel process model, 32
Discrimination, status displays and, 507–508 rapport, individuals in, 385–387
See also Race and intergroup interaction skill approach to nonverbal communication,
Disengagement and race/intergroup 80, 86–90
interaction, 493 social organization, biological foundations
Display Rule Assessment Inventory of, 130–135
(DRAI), 226, 228 two-factor theory of emotions, 23
Dispositions and parallel process model, 32 See also Affect/affective listings; Intimate
Dissonance, emotional, 510 interactions/relationships; Methods for
Distance. See Space, interpersonal the study of nonverbal communication;
Divorce, xx, xxi individual subject headings
Dominant-submissive relationships and Empathic accuracy, studying, 48, 84
communicative genes, 123 See also Intimate interactions/relationships
See also Power/dominance in human Encoding, nonverbal, 85–87
relationships See also Personality and nonverbal
Dress styles, xxii behavior; individual
Duchenne, Guillaume, 300 subject headings
Duchenne smile, 147, 204 Environment, nonverbal behavior as a
Dürer, Albrecht, 159–160 product of the, 8, 10, 32
Dyadic power theory, 284 Environment and theory of natural selection,
Dysmorphophobia, 244 140–141
Equilibrium theory, 22–23, 475
Ecological perspectives Essays on Physiognomy (Lavater), 159
impression management, 330–331 Essays on the Anatomy of Expression in
physician-patient interaction, 442 Painting (Bell), 300
rapport model consistent with, 382 Ethnocentrism and instructional
social perception, 30 communication, 433
Education. See Instructional communication Ethology and power/dominance in human
Electromyography (EMG), facial, 50, 51, relationships, 282
66, 363 Evolution/evolutionary theory
Elevation and power/dominance in human children and emotion displays, 147–148
relationships, 291 facial cues/expressions, 309–310
Elocution movement, 5–6 impression management, 328–330
Sub-Index.qxd 6/30/2006 5:25 PM Page 564

564–––◆–––The SAGE Handbook of Nonverbal Communication

power/dominance in human Facial Action Coding System


relationships, 282 (FACS), 304
spontaneous communication persisting Facial appearance heuristic, 351
across evolutionary timescales, 123 Facial cues/expressions
See also Natural selection; Social behavioral ecology view, 309–315
organization, biological foundations of coloration changes, 302–303
Expectancy-violation heuristic, 351 coordination/management of interactions,
Expectations automatic, 363, 371–373
automatic cognitive processes, 68–69 culture, 222–229
deception, detecting, 351 deception, detecting, 351
expectancy violations theory, 286 dynamic features of the face, 302–304
organizational life, 511–512 evolutionary theory, 309–310
parallel process model, 32 Facial Expression Program, 305–309
power/dominance in human relationships, facial feedback hypothesis, 148
285–286 gender differences, 204–207, 301
race and intergroup interaction, 484–485 instructional communication, 427
skill approach to nonverbal methods for studying, review of,
communication, 80 45, 49–51
space, personal, 23–24 motives, emotions or, 310–311
Experience-expression distinction for muscular changes, 303–304
emotions, 144 older views of, 300–301
Experiential variables and race/intergroup overview, 299–300
interaction, 488 paralanguage and gesture, 313–315
Expression of the Emotions in Man and physician-patient interaction, 440
Animals (Darwin), 6–7, 123, 145, 222, power/dominance in human relationships,
280, 300 287, 288
Expression psychology, German, 8 privileged relationship between emotions
Expressive aphasia, 130 and, 307–309
Expressivity, 288–289, 385–387 sociality of faces, 311–313
See also Emotions/emotional displays; static features of the face, 301–302
Facial cues/expressions; Intimate summary/conclusions, 315–316
interactions/relationships universal, are facial expressions of emotion,
Extraversion-acting, 88 305–307
Extraverted individuals and power/dominance See also Gaze/gazing; individual
in human relationships, 289 subject headings
Eye behavior used to infer mental Fast, Julius, 11
processes, 50 Financial Times, 245
See also Face-to-face dialogue; Gaze/gazing Fluctuating asymmetry (FA), 150
Eyebrows and facial cues/expressions, 302 Flushing, 302–303
Forced-choice response formats and
Face-to-face dialogue studying emotional expressions,
communication in dialogue, 107–110 44–45
encoding and decoding designs, 107–109 Forest approach and instructional
historical/theoretical framework, 98–99 communication, 426, 429–432
integrated audible/visible acts, 104–107 Freud, Sigmund, 280
meaning in conversation and hand Functional approach and instructional
gestures/facial displays, 100–102 communication, 426, 429–432
organizational life, 511–514 Functional magnetic resonance imaging
summary/conclusions, 110 (fMRI), 527
symbolic acts, 102–104 Functional perspective model, 26–28, 30
synchrony with spontaneous speech, 99–102 Fundamental attribution error (FAE),
See also Gaze/gazing 351–352
Sub-Index.qxd 6/30/2006 5:25 PM Page 565

Subject Index–––◆–––565

Galen, 300 Goals


Gallese, Vitorio, 373 automatic cognitive processes, 60–61,
Gaze/gazing 63, 526
culture, 221 parallel process model, 32, 33–34
gender differences, 205 rapport, the ecosystem of, 390
instructional communication, 428 Goffman, Erving, 10
intimate interactions/relationships, 267, 409 Greek rhetoricians and historical overview of
organizational life, 513 nonverbal research, 4
physician-patient interaction, 439–440, Gregarious individuals and power/dominance
443–448, 453–454 in human relationships, 289
power/dominance in human relationships, Group identity. See Race and intergroup
287, 290 interaction
race and intergroup interaction, 483, 484 Guardian, The, 245
See also Face-to-face dialogue
Gender differences Hand gestures. See Face-to-face dialogue
accuracy in sending/receiving nonverbal Haptics and intimate
cues, 207–209 interactions/relationships, 266–267
body movements/positions, 205–207 Hart, Gary, 249
computer-mediated communication, 472 Head nodding and physician-patient
distance and facing orientation, interaction, 440, 448–449
interpersonal, 206–207 Health conditions, xvi–xviii, 393–394
facial cues/expressions, 204–207, 301 See also Physician-patient interaction
gazing, 205 Height and power/dominance in human
head/hand/arm gestures, 205 relationships, 291
media, codes encouraged by the, 240–241 Heritability and theory of natural
organizational life, 504–507 selection, 140
parallel process model, 32 Heterosexual affectional system, 129
power/dominance in human relationships, Heuristics and detecting deception, 351–352
211–212, 284–285 Hippocrates, 300
rapport, individuals in, 393 Historical overview of nonverbal research
size of the differences, appraising the, 19th century: influential works, 6–7
209–210 20th century, the early, 7–9
smiling, 203–204 1950s: laying a foundation, 9–11
stereotypes about, 202–203, 209 1960s/1970s, 11–12
summary/conclusions, 213–214 20th century, the late, 12–13
touch, interpersonal, 205–206 Confucius, 4
voice qualities and vocal behaviors, 207 elocution movement, 5–6
where do the differences come from, Greek rhetoricians, 4
210–213 rhetoric and the delivery canon, 4–6
See also Intimate interactions/relationships Roman orators, 4–5
Genetics. See Evolution/evolutionary theory; summary/conclusions, 13–14
Natural selection; Social organization, See also Theories of interactive behavior,
biological foundations of evolution of
German expression psychology, 8 Honest vs. manipulative communication,
Gerwing, Jennifer, 97 123–125
Gestural Expression of the Ancients in Hussein, Saddam, 355
the Light of Neapolitan Gesturing Hyperpersonal model of computer-mediated
(de Jorio), 6–7 communication, 465–466
Gesture, 13
Gestures, 4–5, 220–221 Identity, race and personal/social, 485
See also Face-to-face dialogue; individual Illness, impression management and disguising
subject headings physical/psychological, 324
Sub-Index.qxd 6/30/2006 5:25 PM Page 566

566–––◆–––The SAGE Handbook of Nonverbal Communication

Immediacy behaviors/cues Informed consent, 49


culture, xxii Initiation and power/dominance in human
defining terms, xxi relationships, 291
instructional communication, 422–424 Institutio Oratoria (Quintilian), 5
multivariate nature of nonverbal Instructional communication
communication, 522–523 affective learning outcomes, 424–425, 430
See also Intimate interactions/ forest approach, 426, 429–432
relationships; Rapport, the historical account, 422
ecosystem of; individual subject immediacy behaviors/cues, 422–424
headings overview, 421–422
Implicit vs. explicit lie detection, 354 perspective from which to approach the
Impression management topics, 426
approach/avoidance, managing images to race and intergroup interaction, 492
regulate, 324 research on, recent, 433–434
automatic cognitive processes, 61–62, short- vs. long-term learning, 425–426
67–68 summary/conclusions, 434
behavioral cues, dynamic, 326–327 tree approach, 426, 427–429
body appearance/orientation, 321–322, Intensity and rapport between individuals,
325–327, 329 389–390
contrived images, accepting/ Intentionality, 60
projecting, 324 Intentions, xii, 65, 72, 211, 283, 287,
defining terms, 322 310, 327, 331, 344, 383, 385, 394, 405,
dual tasks of encoding and decoding, 323 490, 525, 528
ecological perspectives, 330–331 Interaction adaptation theory (IAT),
emotional perspectives, 331–332 27, 29, 30
evolutionary perspectives, 328–330 Interaction chronograph, 8
identification/assessment issues, 323 Interpersonal Adjective Scales Inventory
illness, disguising physical/ (IAS), 169
psychological, 324 Interpersonal deception theory (IDT),
measurement and conceptual challenges, 344–345, 474
333–334 Interpersonal facial feedback (IFF), 371–373
overview, 327–328 Interpersonal Perception Task (IPT), 47, 64,
power of self-presentation, 322–323 82–83
self-deceptive cognitive processes, 334 Interviews
skills involved, 324–325 deception, detecting, 355–356
social-cognitive perspectives, 332–333 organizational life, 513
Stewart, Martha, 321–322 race/intergroup interaction, 492
summary/conclusions, 334 Intimate interactions/relationships
suppressing/neutralizing spontaneous attachment security/insecurity, 412–414
nonverbal responses, 323–324 attributions, 414–415
See also Deception, detecting automatic cognitive processes, 64–65
Inaccuracy, intimate interactions/relationships bias in decoding, 406–407
and motivated, 406 chronemics, 268–269
Inattention and physician-patient communicative potential of nonverbal cues,
interaction, 450 403–404
Inclusive fitness theory, 121, 141 coordination/management of interactions,
Independent, The, 245 automatic, 365–367
Independent variables, nonverbal displays courtship displays, 411–412
conceptualized as, 41–49 deception, detecting, 350
Induced displays, 7–8, 43–44, 47 decoding accuracy, 404–407
Infant vocalizations, xviii–xix empathy-related responding, review of
See also Age, factoring in; Children studies on, 405
Sub-Index.qxd 6/30/2006 5:25 PM Page 567

Subject Index–––◆–––567

enhancing relationships, 270–272 Love, 264


experience vs. expression of intimacy, 261 Love for Love (Congreve), 160
expressiveness, nonverbal, 407–411 Lying. See Deception, detecting;
haptics, 266–267 Impression management
health impacted by nonverbal
communication, xviii Machismo nervosa, 326
immediacy behaviors, 264–265, 408 Magnetic resonance imaging (MRI), 527
inaccuracy, motivated, 406 Mammals and biological foundations of
interaction-centered model, 260–261 social organization, 127–129
interpreting positive involvement cues, Management, interaction. See
269–270 Coordination/management of
involvement cues, positive, 261–270 interactions, automatic
kinesics, 267 Marriage, xviii–xxi, 386
multivariate in nature, conceptualizing See also Intimate interactions/
nonverbal communication as, 522–523 relationships
overview, 259–260 Matching and rapport between
proxemics, 265–266 individuals, 387
sensitivity, interpersonal, 404–407 Maternal affectional system, 128
summary/conclusions, 272, 415–416 Maximally Descriptive Facial Movement
vocal clips/cues, 267–268, 409 Coding System (MAX), 50, 304
See also Rapport, the ecosystem of Mayer, Salovey, Caruso, Emotional
Introverted people and detecting Intelligence Test (MSCEIT), 80
deception, 353 Mead, George H., 7
Mead, Margaret, 10
Japanese and Caucasian Brief Affect Media, codes encouraged by the
Recognition Test (JACBART), 81 affective socialization, 238–240
Journal of Environmental Psychology, 12 cognitive socialization, 240–241
Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 12, 313 consequences of the code, 247–250
Joy, 264 critiquing the code, 242–244
Judgment/behavior and parallel process metacommunicative role of the
model, social, 30, 32, 33–35 media, 245–247
overview, 237–238
Kees, Weldon, 11 persuasion, 248–250
Kinesics, 9, 267 politics, 245–250
self-image, 247–248
Language and biological foundations of social socialization, 238–241
organization, 129–130 summary/conclusions, 250–251
Latencies and computer-mediated uncovering the code, 242
communication, 468–469 Media richness theory, 463
Lavater, Johann C., 159, 160, 300 Metacommunicative role of the
Leadership and organizational life, 502–505, media, 245–247
508–509 Methods for the study of nonverbal
Learning. See Instructional communication communication
LeBrun, Charles, 300 bodily movements, 52–53
Lens model and relationship between dependent variables, 49–53
personality/nonverbal behavior, 160, 166, facial cues, 45, 49–51
167, 175–176 independent variable, nonverbal behavior
Lewinsky, Monica, 245 used as an, 42–49
Life span. See Age, factoring in interactions between study participants,
Linguistic and nonverbal codes, similar constructing, 47–49
structure of, 9–10 overview, 41–42
Lorenz, Konrad, 10 photographs, 42–45
Sub-Index.qxd 6/30/2006 5:25 PM Page 568

568–––◆–––The SAGE Handbook of Nonverbal Communication

summary/conclusions, 53 psychology, evolutionary, 141


visual clips, 46–47 researchers, three recommendations for,
vocal clips/cues, 45–46, 51–52 152–153
Michigan State University, 11 See also Evolution/evolutionary theory;
Microcoordination, 363 Social organization, biological
Mimicry, behavioral, 526 foundations of
See also Automatic cognitive processes; Negativity and rapport between
Coordination/management of individuals, 386
interactions, automatic; Rapport, Nervous system and evolution of emotion
the ecosystem of displays, 145
Minority groups. See Race and intergroup Netanyahu, Benjamin, 249
interaction Neurology and automatic
Mirror, The, 246 coordination/management of interactions,
Mirror neurons, 315–316, 373–374, 527 315–316, 373–374, 527
Mixed model of emotional Nodding (head) and physician-patient
intelligence, 80 interaction, 440, 448–449
Molar judging of the social meaning of Nonreciprocation and power/dominance in
behavior, 384–385 human relationships, 291
Mona Lisa, physiognomy and portrait of, Nonverbal Behavior, 12
160–161 Nonverbal communication
Mondale, Walter, 248, 249 active agents, individuals as, 524
Monkeys and biological foundations of social automatic processes, 524–526
organization, 128–129 context, importance of understanding the
Mood linkages, 364 larger communicative, 523
See also Coordination/management of emerging trends, 522–524
interactions, automatic; future issues, 524–529
Emotions/emotional displays interdependent components, system
Moral emotions and biological foundations comprising, 524
of social organization, 134–135 isolated behaviors, moving away from
Mother-infant interactions and mutual focusing on, 522
coordination, 365–367 judgments/behaviors as pragmatic, 524
Motives and facial cues/expressions, limited range of judgments/behaviors, 524
310–311 media for understanding, new, 528–529
Multidimensionality and rapport between as multivariate in nature, 522–523
individuals, 390 negativity, xx–xxi, 386
Multifactor Emotional Intelligence Scale new methods for understanding, 527–528
(MEIS), 80 processes mediating, greater focus on, 523
Multifactor model and detecting deception, summary/conclusions, 529–530
342–343 See also Historical overview of nonverbal
Multiple fitness model and impression research; Methods for the study of
management, 329 nonverbal communication; individual
subject headings
Naked, 243, 244
Napoleon in His Study, 243 Older adults. See Age, factoring in
National Institute of Child Health and Ontogeny, 140
Human Development (NICHD), 366 Oppression hypothesis, 483, 484
Natural selection Organizational life
adaptive patterns, common, 31 discrimination, status displays
attraction, 149–152 and, 507–508
emotion displays, 144–149 emotional displays, 508–511
evolution, introduction to, 140–144 expectancy effects, 511–512
overview, 139, 523–524 face-to-face dialogue, 511–514
Sub-Index.qxd 6/30/2006 5:25 PM Page 569

Subject Index–––◆–––569

involvement behavior, nonverbal, 512–514 process model as a framework, 175–176


leadership issues, 502–505, 508–509 study on, example of a, 169–175
mixed messages, 512 summary/conclusions, 176–177
overview, 501–502 Personal variables and race/intergroup
sexual harassment, status displays and, interaction, 488
505–507 Persuasion and the media, 248–250
status, 502–508 Photographs used to study nonverbal
summary/conclusions, 514 communication, 42–45
verbal messages, managing the delivery of, Phylogeny, 140
511–514 Physical potency and power/dominance in
Oro-facial communication system, 316 human relationships, 287–289
Other-directedness, 88 Physician-patient interaction
Outcome reward structures, 286 body orientation, physicians’, 440–441
gaze orientation, physicians’, 439–440,
Paleosociality in nonvertebrate forms, 443–448, 453–454
125–127 inseparability of nonverbal behaviors
Paralanguage, facial, 313–315, 428 from themselves, 452–454
Parallel process model inseparability of nonverbal/verbal
cognitive-affective mediators, 32–33 behavior, 442–443
context, setting the, 30–31 nodding, head, 440, 448–449
coordinating parallel processes, 35 overview, 437–438
determinants, 31–32 race and intergroup interaction, 492
environment, social, 32 rationale for studying nonverbal
goals, 32, 33–34 communication, 438–439
judgment/behavior, social, 33 sequences of task and action,
Passing encounters paradigm, 52 451–452
Peer affectional system, 128–129 social action, 450
Perceived Decoding Ability (PDA) Test, 84 summary/conclusions, 454–455
Perceived Encoding Ability (PEA) Test, 84, 86 turn taking, 443–450
Perception-action system and rapport between Physiognomists, 159–160
individuals, 382, 383 Police officers and detecting deception,
Peres, Shimon, 249 350–351, 354, 355–356
Performance-based measurement of nonverbal Politics and the media, 245–250
encoding skill, 87 Porta, Giambattista della, 300
Personality/personality traits Positive involvement behaviors. See Intimate
accuracy/agreement/encoding/decoding, interactions/relationships
distinctions between, 162–163 Positivity and intimate
body appearance/shape and, relationship interactions/relationships, 409
between, 8–9 Positivity and rapport between individuals,
complexities, ten, 163–164 384–388, 391
encoding and decoding personality, Posture and automatic cognitive processes,
164–165 65, 69
history of scholarship on, 160–162 Power/dominance in human relationships
misinterpretation and conflict, 175 classification of power into three
paradigm proposal dealing with accuracy domains, 280
problems, 166–168 conceptualizing power/dominance/status,
parallel process model, 32 280–281
physiognomists, 159–160 control, conversation, 290–292
potential outcomes of encoding/decoding expectancy theories, 285–286
studies, 168–169 gender differences, 211–212, 284–285
power/dominance in human relationships, interaction control, 290–292
282–283 intimate interactions/relationships, 410
Sub-Index.qxd 6/30/2006 5:25 PM Page 570

570–––◆–––The SAGE Handbook of Nonverbal Communication

operationalizing power/dominance, 281–282 affect and arousal, 489–490


organizational life, 502–508 anxiety, 485, 490
overview, 279–280 assessing the interaction, 493–495
personality and evolutionary psychology, background experiences brought to
282–283 situations, 488–489, 491
physical potency, 287–289 body appearance/orientation, 484
physician-patient interaction, 441, 450 categorization, social, 485
principles for expression of, four, 287–292 continuing/terminating the interaction, 495
race and intergroup interaction, 482–483, coping strategies, 493
485–486, 488–489 cultural values/hypothesis, 483, 484
resource control, 289–290 decoding accuracy, 483–484
social exchange models, 283–284 expectation states theory, 484–485
summary/conclusions, 292 eye contact, 484
theoretical perspectives, 282–287 identity, personal/social, 485
Precedence and power/dominance in human implications for the model of mixed social
relationships, 289 interactions, 495–496
Preconscious automaticity, 60 initial interaction phase, 491–493
Preinteraction states/mediators and model of mixed social interaction,
race/intergroup interaction, 489–491 486–495
Prejudice, 43, 66–67 motivations and goals, 490
See also Race and intergroup interaction non-prejudiced manner, preoccupation with
Prerogative and power/dominance in human behaving in a, 492–493
relationships, 289–290 oppression hypothesis, 483, 484
Presentational perspective and detecting organizational life, 507–508
deception, 343–344 overview, 481–482
Prior Analytics (Aristotle), 160 power/dominance in human relationships,
Prisoner’s Dilemma (PD) game, 124–125 482–483, 485–486, 488–489
Probing heuristic, 351 preinteraction states/mediators, 489–491
Procreation and theory of natural selection, 141 prejudices, 489
Profile correlation, 209 secondary interaction phase, 493–495
Profile of Nonverbal Sensitivity (PONS), space, interpersonal, 484
45–46, 79, 82, 83, 208, 510, 511 status, 482–483
Proxemics, 9, 265–266 stereotypes, 489
See also Body appearance/orientation; summary/conclusions, 496
Space, interpersonal tendencies/predispositions, behavioral, 490
Proximal causes and evolutionary approach, Random House Unabridged Dictionary, 101
141–142 Rapport, the ecosystem of
Prozac, 123 automatic cognitive processes, 69–70
Pseudospontaneous communication, context and dimensionality, 389–392
122–123 cooperating with others to accomplish
Psychological well-being and understanding tasks, 382
vocal cues to emotion, 45–46 coordination, interpersonal, 384, 387–389
Psychology, evolutionary, 141 expressivity, nonverbal, 385–387
See also Natural selection functions of nonverbal behavior in,
Purdue University, 11 383–384
Pythagoras, 300 future research, implications for, 395–396
individual and group differences, 393–395
Quintilian, Marcus F., 4–5 overview, 381–382
Quorum sensing in bacteria, 125–126 perception-action system, 382, 383
tasks, context/dimensionality of, 390–393
Rabin, Yitzhak, 246 Reagan, Ronald, 248, 249
Race and intergroup interaction Receptive aphasia, 130
Sub-Index.qxd 6/30/2006 5:25 PM Page 571

Subject Index–––◆–––571

Redundancy and hand gestures/facial displays Sequencing and physician-patient interaction,


in face-to-face dialogue, 106–107 451–452
Regulation. See Control/regulation Sequential Functional Model of Nonverbal
Rejection sensitivity, 407 Exchange, 486
Relational-situational variables and Serotonin and dominance, relationship
race/intergroup interaction, 488 between, 123
Relational truth-bias heuristic, 350 Serviceable associated habits and evolution of
Relationships, personal/social. See Intimate emotion displays, 145
interactions/relationships; Rapport, the Sex composition of groups and
ecosystem of relationship between personality/
Reliability and relationship between nonverbal behavior, 164
personality/nonverbal behavior, 163 Sexual harassment, status displays and,
Reporting and relationship between 505–507
personality/nonverbal behavior, Sexual pheromones, 123
163–164 Sheridan, Thomas, 5
Representativeness heuristic, 351 Silence and power/dominance in human
Research. See Historical overview of relationships, 287
nonverbal research; Methods for the Skill approach to nonverbal communication
study of nonverbal communication; control/regulation, 87–89
Theories of interactive behavior, decoding nonverbal messages, 81–85
evolution of encoding nonverbal messages, 85–87
Resource control and power/dominance in impression management, 324–325
human relationships, 289–290 potential, unrealized, 89–90
Resources, cognitive, 32–33, 72 shifts, three conceptual, 79–80
Response formats and studying emotional Skill in Nonverbal Communication
expressions, 44–45 (Rosenthal), 79
Reward expectations/structures, 285–286 Slide-viewing technique (SVT), 81
Reward valence, 286 Slime molds and biological foundations of
Ritualized displays and power/dominance in social organization, 126–127
human relationships, 282 Smiling, 203–204, 212–213
Roman orators and historical overview of See also Facial cues/expressions
nonverbal research, 4–5 Social action and physician-patient
Romantic relationships, xviii–xxi, 386 interaction, 450
See also Intimate interactions/relationships Social-cognitive perspectives on impression
Ruesch, Jurgen, 10–11 management, 332–333
Social control aspects of interaction, 280
Saccharomyces cerevisiae, 123 Social emotions and biological foundations of
Schools. See Instructional communication social organization, 130–134
Seated Kinesic Activity Notation System Social exchange models and power/dominance
(SKANS IV), 170 in human relationships, 283–284
Selection and theory of natural selection, 140 Social identification model of deindividuation
Selective serotonin reuptake inhibitors effects (SIDE), 464–465
(SSRIs), 123 Social identity theory, 485
Self-image and the media, 247–248 Social impact of nonverbal communication
Selfish genes, 120–121, 123 beauty, fascination with physical, xxii
Self-monitoring scale (SMS), 88 categorizing others into social
Self-presentations and automatic cognitive groups, xxii
processes, 67–68 developmental outcomes, xviii–xix
See also Impression management dress styles, xxii
Self-report methods for measuring nonverbal health outcomes, xvi–xviii
skills, 84, 90 immediacy cues, xxi–xxii
Sensitivity, nonverbal, 84, 165, 209, 404–407 marriage, xix–xxi
Sub-Index.qxd 6/30/2006 5:25 PM Page 572

572–––◆–––The SAGE Handbook of Nonverbal Communication

possessions and status, xxii Spontaneous vs. posed expressions,


summary/conclusions, xxii–xxiii 7–8, 43–44
Social information processing (SIP) theory, See also Automatic cognitive processes
466–467 Stanford University, 9
Sociality of facial displays, 311–313 Stares and power/dominance in human
Socialization and the media, 238–241 relationships, 287
Social organization, biological foundations of Status, 290
communicative genes, 121–122 See also Power/dominance in human
cooperation/competition and deception, relationships
123–125 Stereotypes, 43, 202–203, 209
language and, 129–130 See also Race and intergroup interaction
mammals, sociality in, 127–129 Stern, William, 161
moral emotions, 134–135 Stewart, Martha, 321–322
overview, 119–120 Stigmas and race/intergroup interaction, 489
paleosociality in nonvertebrate forms, Stonewalling and stress in a marriage, xx
125–127 Substance abusers, xvii
pseudospontaneous communication, Sun, The, 246
122–123 Sunday Times, The, 243
quorum sensing in bacteria, 125–126 Superfecundity and theory of natural
selfish genes, 120–121 selection, 140
slime molds, 126–127 Supportive nonverbal communication, health
social emotions, 130–134 outcomes tied to, xviii
spontaneous communication, 122, 123, Symbolic acts and hand gestures/facial
125–126 displays in face-to-face dialogue, 102–104
summary/conclusions, 135–136 Symbolic communication, 122
symbolic communication, 122 Symmetry and perceptions of attractiveness,
Social presence theory, 462–463 150–151
Social referencing, 183 Synchrony, 12–13, 99–102, 387
Social Sciences and Humanities Research See also Automatic cognitive processes;
Council of Canada, 97 Coordination/management of
Sociobiologists and power/dominance in interactions, automatic; Rapport,
human relationships, 281 the ecosystem of
Sociocommunicative style and instructional
communication, 431 Task performance cues and power/dominance
Space, interpersonal in human relationships, 291–292
culture, 221 Tasks, rapport and context/dimensionality
expectancy-violations model, 23–24 of, 390–393
gender differences, 206–207 Teachers. See Instructional communication
instructional communication, 428 Television. See Media, codes encouraged
intimate interactions/relationships, by the
265–266 Territorial markers and power/dominance in
power/dominance in human human relationships, 289
relationships, 289 Theories of interactive behavior,
race and intergroup interaction, 484 evolution of, 13
See also Body appearance/orientation affect-based theories, 23–26
Spearman-Brown formula, 44 early theories, 22–26
Speech accommodation theory, 51 equilibrium theory, 22–23
Speech delivery and historical overview of functional perspective, 26–28
nonverbal research, 4–6 interaction adaptation theory, 27, 29
Spontaneous communication and biological overview, 21–22
foundations of social organization, parallel process model, 30–35
122, 123, 125–126 summary/conclusions, 35–36
Sub-Index.qxd 6/30/2006 5:25 PM Page 573

Subject Index–––◆–––573

See also Historical overview of nonverbal Virtual reality (VR) systems,


research; Methods for the study of 474–475, 527
nonverbal communication Visual clips, studying, 46–47
Threat and power/dominance in human See also Gaze/gazing
relationships, 287–288 Visual dominance ratio, 205, 290
Tinbergen, Niko, 10 Visual habituation procedure, 50
Touch Visual preference method, 50
gender differences, 205–206 Vitruvian Man, 243
instructional communication, 428 Vocal clips/cues
intimate interactions/relationships, gender differences, 207
266–267, 408 historical overview of nonverbal
Trait differences and relationship between research, 4–6
personality/nonverbal behavior, 164 impression management, 326
Trait inferences and automatic cognitive infant vocalizations, xviii–xix
processes, 68 instructional communication, 428
Tree approach and instructional intimate interactions/relationships,
communication, 426, 427–429 267–268, 409
Turn taking and physician-patient interaction, methods for studying, review of,
443–450 45–46, 51–52
organizational life, 511–514
Ubiquity of nonverbal communication, xv–xvi See also Face-to-face dialogue
Ultimate causes and evolutionary approach, Voles and biological foundations of social
141–142 organization, 127–128
Unconscious adaptations and evolutionary
approach, 142 Waist-to-hip ratio (WHR) and perceptions of
Unitization and physician-patient attractiveness, 151
interaction, 443 Warmth, interpersonal, xvii–xviii, 264
University of California, 11 Western Mail, The, 245
West Virginia University, 422
Validity, ecological, 166 White people. See Race and intergroup
Variable analytic tradition, 13 interaction
Variation and theory of natural selection, 140 Withdrawal, intimate interactions/
Verbal exchanges. See Face-to-face dialogue; relationships and conflict, 410
Vocal clips/cues Women’s movement, 11
Vernon, Philip, 161 See also Gender differences in nonverbal
Vertical dimension of human relationships, 280 communication
Videoconferencing, 472–473 Workplace. See Organizational life
Videotape recorders and growing interest in
studying nonverbal behavior, 11–12 Zoloft, 123
Violence and intimate Zygomatic muscle and facial cues/
interactions/relationships, 410 expressions, 50
Sub-Index.qxd 6/30/2006 5:25 PM Page 574
ABE-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 5:21 PM Page 575

ABOUT THE EDITORS

Valerie Manusov is Professor in the Department of Communication at


the University of Washington. She has published two previous edited
volumes: Communication, Attribution, and Close Relationships with
John Harvey (2001) and The Sourcebook of Nonverbal Measures:
Going Beyond Words (2005). She has been Associate Chair of her
department and Associate Editor of the Journal of Social and Personal
Relationships. Her work focuses primarily on patterns of nonverbal
behavior and the meanings given to nonverbal cues at a relational and
cultural level. She also serves as Leadership Fellow for the College of
Arts & Sciences at the University of Washington and teaches courses
in nonverbal communication, interpersonal communication, intercul­
tural communication, and research methods. One of her favorite
achievements is completing the Avon 3-Day Walk for breast cancer
prevention and research. She received her PhD from the University of
Southern California in 1989 and worked for 4 years after that at
Rutgers University.

Miles L. Patterson is Professor and former Chairperson of the


Psychology Department at the University of Missouri at St. Louis
(UMSL). He is the author of two books and more than 70 chapters and
articles, mostly on nonverbal communication. Some of this research
was supported by grants from the National Institute of Mental Health.
He was the editor of the Journal of Nonverbal Behavior from 1986 to
1992 and has been on the editorial boards of several other journals in
psychology, communication, and sociology. He was the 1990 recipient
of the UMSL Chancellor’s Award for Research and Creativity and is a
fellow of both the American Psychological Association and the
American Psychological Society. His current research interests focus

◆ 575
ABE-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 5:21 PM Page 576

576–––◆–––The SAGE Handbook of Nonverbal Communication

primarily on theory in nonverbal commu­ as an expert commentator on communica­


nication, social behavior in public settings, tion and politics. In his spare time, he is an
and the role of nonverbal communication avid jogger, with over 24,000 miles run,
in interpersonal influence. His teaching and a golfer, always with high hopes for
interests include social psychology, nonver­ the next round. He received his PhD from
bal communication, and environmental Northwestern University in 1968 and has
psychology. He has also appeared on St. been at UMSL since 1969.
Louis radio and television a number of times
ATC-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 7:41 PM Page 577

ABOUT THE CONTRIBUTORS

Nalini Ambady is Associate Professor at Tufts University. Before


joining the Tufts faculty in the spring of 2004, she taught at Holy Cross
College and in 1994 joined the faculty at Harvard. Her research inter­
ests include examining the accuracy of social, emotional, and perceptual
judgments; how personal and social identities affect cognition and per­
formance; dyadic interactions, especially those involving status differ­
entiated dyads; and nonverbal communication. She is particularly
interested in applying innovative and integrative methods to examine
these phenomena from multiple perspectives, ranging from the biologi­
cal to the sociocultural. She received her PhD in social psychology from
Harvard University in 1991.

Peter A. Andersen, Professor of Communication at San Diego State


University, has authored five books and more than 150 book chapters,
research papers, and journal articles. His four most recent books are
The Handbook of Communication and Emotion (1998), Nonverbal
Communication: Forms and Functions (1999), Close Encounters:
Communicating in Relationships (2001), and The Complete Idiot’s
Guide to Body Language (2004). He has recently published papers on
communication and emotion, nonverbal communication, interpersonal
relationships, risk communication, helmet safety, health communica­
tion, homeland security, and communication and technology. He has
served as the President of the Western Communication Association, as
Editor of the Western Journal of Communication, and as Director of
Research for the Japan-U.S. Telecommunications Research Institute. He
won the Robert Kibler Award for personal and professional excellence
from the National Communication Association in 2003. He is a
co-investigator on six federal grants in health communication and

◆ 577
ATC-Manusov.qxd 7/6/2006 5:28 PM Page 578

578–––◆–––The SAGE Handbook of Nonverbal Communication

disaster preparedness. He received his PhD altruism; brain mechanisms of emotional


from Florida State University in 1975. communication; emotion in persuasion,
including safe-sex communication; cross-
Janet Beavin Bavelas is the author of cultural studies of social and moral emotion;
Pragmatics of Human Communication emotional communication in clinical sam­
(with Watzlawick & Jackson) and Equivocal ples; emotional factors in maintaining drug
Communication (with Chovil, Black, & regimens; and higher-level social, cognitive,
Mullett) and has published about 60 and moral emotions as aspects of self-
research articles or chapters, primarily on organizing dynamic systems emerging effort­
interpersonal communication. As of 2005, lessly from experience in social interaction.
she is Professor Emeritus of Psychology at
the University of Victoria, where she leads a Judee K. Burgoon is Professor of
research team doing experimental, microan­ Communication, Professor of Family Studies
alytic studies of the unique features of face­ and Human Development, Director of
to-face dialogue, with an emphasis on Human Communication Research for the
speech-related nonverbal acts (hand and Center for the Management of Information,
facial gestures) and on collaboration in and Associate Director of the Media Interface
dyadic interaction. Her applied research Network Design Lab at the University of
includes microanalysis of communication in Arizona. She is also a Visiting Professor of
psychotherapeutic, medical, and electronic Communication at Michigan State University.
interactions and in texts related to social jus­ She has authored or coauthored seven books
tice issues. Her AB (psychology), AM (com­ and monographs and over 240 articles, chap­
munication research), and PhD (psychology) ters, and reviews related to deception, nonver­
are from Stanford University, and she is a bal and relational communication, dyadic
fellow of the International Communication interaction patterns, and computer-mediated
Association, the Canadian Psychological communication. Her current research on
Association, and the Royal Society of Canada. interpersonal communication and deception
detection has been funded by several federal
Ross Buck is Professor of Communication agencies. She is a former Chairperson of the
Sciences and Psychology at the University of National Communication Association’s
Connecticut, Storrs. His books include Interpersonal Communication Division; a
Human Motivation and Emotion and The recipient of NCA’s Distinguished Scholar,
Communication of Emotion, and he is Golden Monographs, and Charles E.
the author of over 100 other publications. Woolbert awards; and an elected fellow of the
He has received grants from the National International Communication Association.
Institute of Mental Health, the Harry She received her EdD from West Virginia
Frank Guggenheim Foundation, the University.
EJLB Foundation, and the Russell Sage
Foundation, and his research has been Joseph N. Cappella is Professor of
featured on ABC News, 20–20, and FUJI­ Communication and holds the Gerald R.
TV, Japan. He is Organizer and Charter Miller Chair at the Annenberg School
Chair of the Nonverbal Communication for Communication at the University of
Division of the National Communication Pennsylvania. His research has produced
Association. His present work centers on more than 90 articles and book chapters
emotional experience, expression, and and three coauthored books focusing on
communication in human cooperation political communication, health, social
and competition, trustworthiness, and interaction, media effects, and statistical
ATC-Manusov.qxd 7/6/2006 5:28 PM Page 579

About the Contributors–––◆–––579

methods. His research has been supported received SPSSI’s Kurt Lewin Award in 2004
by grants from the National Institutes (with S. L. Gaertner) for his career contribu­
of Mental Health, the National Institute tions to the study of prejudice and discrimi­
on Drug Abuse, the National Science nation. He received the Gordon Allport
Foundation, the National Cancer Institute, Intergroup Relations Prize in 1985, 1998,
the Twentieth Century Fund, and the and 2001 for his research on intergroup rela­
Markle, Ford, Carnegie, Pew, and Robert tions. He has an MA and PhD from the
Wood Johnson foundations. He has served University of Delaware.
on the editorial boards of 15 different jour­
nals. He is a Fellow of the International Norah E. Dunbar is Associate Professor of
Communication Association, a Distinguished Communication Studies at California State
Scholar of the National Communication University Long Beach. Her research inter­
Association, a Past President of ICA, and a ests are in relational conflict, deception,
recipient of the B. Aubrey Fisher Mentorship power and dominance, and nonverbal
Award. He received his PhD in 1974 from communication. Currently, she is working
Michigan State University. on several projects, including a study on
the nonverbal expressions of dominance
Nicole Chovil is an author of Equivocal in close relationships. Her work can be
Communication (with Bavelas, Black, & found in journals such as the Journal
Mullett), plus 15 articles and chapters on of Social and Personal Relationships and
equivocal communication, hand and facial the Journal of Family Communication. She
displays in dialogue, and motor mimicry. She teaches undergraduate and graduate classes
conducted the first systematic experimental in interpersonal communication, per­
studies of nonemotional functions of facial suasion, nonverbal communication,
displays in face-to-face dialogue. She was also research methods, and communication
co-investigator and collaborator on a project theory. She received her PhD in 2000 from
using discourse analysis to study the language the University of Arizona.
characterizing sexualized assault in legal judg­
ments. She has a BA, with honors (psychol­ Robert S. Feldman is Associate Dean for
ogy), from the University of Victoria; an MA Faculty and Student Development and
(psychology) from Simon Fraser University; Professor of Psychology at the University of
and a PhD (psychology) from the University Massachusetts at Amherst. A winner of
of Victoria. In 2006, she left her position as the College Distinguished Teacher award,
Director of Education for the British he is a fellow of the American Psychological
Columbia Schizophrenia Society to become Association and the American Psycho­
an independent research and education con­ logical Society. He is a winner of a
sultant specializing in mental illness. Fulbright Senior Research Scholar and
Lecturer award, and he has written more
John F. Dovidio is Professor of Psychology at than 100 books, book chapters, and scien­
the University of Connecticut. He is cur­ tific articles. His research has been sup­
rently Editor of the Journal of Personality ported by grants from the National
and Social Psychology—Interpersonal Institute of Mental Health and the National
Relations and Group Processes and has pre­ Institute on Disabilities and Rehabilitation
viously been Editor of Personality and Social Research. His research interests include
Psychology Bulletin. His research interests the development of nonverbal behavior in
are in intergroup relation, nonverbal impression management and honesty and
communication, and prosocial behavior. He deception.
ATC-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 7:41 PM Page 580

580–––◆–––The SAGE Handbook of Nonverbal Communication

Kory Floyd is Associate Professor of and in the middle of Canada, he is now


Human Communication and Director of Professor of Psychology at the University of
the Communication Sciences Laboratory at Victoria. He is a fellow of the American and
Arizona State University. His research the Canadian Psychological Associations, the
focuses on the communication of affection author of three editions of Environmental
in personal relationships and on the inter­ Psychology: Principles and Practice, and
play between communication, physiology, Editor of the Journal of Environmental
and health. Currently, he is studying the Psychology. His current research includes
ability of affectionate behavior to reduce studies of nonverbal behavior in relation­
the hormonal, cardiovascular, and hemato­ ships, a theory of social evaluation, coopera­
logical effects of stress. He has written tion in resource dilemmas, and the
or edited five books, including Communi­ habitability of the International Space
cating Affection: Interpersonal Behavior Station. He thinks everyone should have a
and Social Context (in press), and has pub­ grocery store within a 10-minute walk.
lished nearly 70 journal articles and book
chapters on the topics of affection, family Howard Giles is Professor of Com­
communication, nonverbal behavior, and munication at the University of California,
physiology. He is currently Chair of the Santa Barbara, where he is also Assistant
Family Communication Division of the Dean of Undergraduate Studies, Director of
National Communication Association and the Center for Police Practices and
Editor of the Journal of Family Communi­ Community, and Affiliated Professor in
cation. He received his PhD from the Psychology and Linguistics. Although his
University of Arizona. current interests relate to diverse areas
of intergroup communication, he has sus­
Alan J. Fridlund is Associate Professor
tained a long-standing interest in nonverbal
at the University of California, Santa
communication by studying the social con­
Barbara. He is a social and clinical
sequences of social dialects.
psychologist whose interests lie in human
ethology (especially nonverbal communi­
cation), neuroethology, psychopathology, Heather M. Gray is a doctoral candidate in
and sexology. He won the Distinguished the Department of Psychology at Harvard
Early Career Contribution Award of the University. As an undergraduate, she
Society for Psychophysiological Research became interested in the manner in which
and was a member of the Faculty in Exper­ transient fluctuations in mood state
imental Psychopathology at the University influence social information processing.
of Pennsylvania. He is the author of Recently, she has been exploring the poten­
Human Facial Expression: An Evolution­ tial effects of mood state on the ability to
ary View (1994) and has coauthored with make accurate inferences about others on
Dan Reisberg (Reed College) and Henry the basis of minimal information. Together
Gleitman (U. Penn.) the introductory text with Nalini Ambady, she has investigated
Psychology (2003, 6th ed.). the effects of sadness on social acuity in a
number of domains, including the thin-slice
Robert Gifford was born near where the paradigm, computer-based social sensitivity
gold rush began in northern California but tasks, and more naturalistic social interac­
almost 100 years too late to join in. He tions. In a second line of research, she is
migrated up the coast to British Columbia in exploring the manner in which relevance
the late 1960s. After teaching at both ends to the self influences the allocation of
ATC-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 7:41 PM Page 581

About the Contributors–––◆–––581

attentional resources to incoming stimuli. Theory and Measurement (coedited with


She has turned recently to psychophysiolog­ Frank Bernieri). She earned her doctorate at
ical techniques to more precisely investigate Harvard University.
how self-relevance affects the early stages of
social information processing. Michelle “Mikki” Hebl is Associate
Professor of Psychology at Rice University.
Laura K. Guerrero is Professor in the Hugh She joined the Rice faculty in 1998, was
Downs School of Human Communication, named the Radoslav Tsanoff Assistant
where she specializes in relational and non­ Professor of Psychology in 2001, and was
verbal communication. She has published promoted to Associate Professor in 2004.
over 60 articles and chapters in these areas. She is part of the industrial/organizational
Her work in nonverbal communication program at Rice University, and her
has focused on tactile behavior and other research examines issues related to diversity
nonverbal immediacy cues, particularly in and discrimination. She is particularly inter­
the context of romantic relationships and ested in identifying remediation strategies
friendships. Her book credits include available to both individuals and organiza­
The Handbook of Communication and tions in addressing discrimination in the
Emotion (coedited with Peter Andersen) and workplace and other settings. She received
Nonverbal Communication in Close her bachelor’s degree from Smith College in
Relationships (coauthored with Kory Floyd). 1991 and her doctorate from Dartmouth
She was awarded the Gerald R. Miller Early College in 1997.
Achievement Award from the International
Association for Relationship Research in Adam Jaworski is Professor at the Centre
2001 and twice received the Dickens Best for Language and Communication Research,
Article Award from the Western States Cardiff University. His latest books are
Communication Association. She received Metalanguage: Social and Ideological
her PhD in 1994 from the University of Perspectives (2004, with Nikolas Coupland
Arizona. and Dariusz Galasiñski) and Discourse,
Communication and Tourism (2005, with
Judith A. Hall is a professor in the Annette Pritchard). His research interests
Department of Psychology at Northeastern include discourse analysis, visual commu­
University. She held positions at Johns nication, and nonverbal communica­
Hopkins University and the Harvard tion. He coedits the book series Oxford
Medical School before coming to Studies in Sociolinguistics (with Nik
Northeastern in 1986. Her interests are in Coupland).
nonverbal communication, gender differ­
ences and gender roles, and physician- Susanne M. Jones is Assistant Professor in
patient communication, with special focus the Department of Communication Studies
on interpersonal sensitivity and the impact at the University of Minnesota, Twin Cities.
of hierarchical roles on nonverbal behavior She examines nonverbal and verbal com­
and communication. Her books include forting and emotional support behaviors, as
Nonverbal Sex Differences; Nonverbal well as the communication of emotion. Her
Communication in Human Interaction work has been published in Communication
(coauthored with Mark Knapp), Doctors Monographs, Human Communication
Talking With Patients/Patients Talking Research, Communication Research, and
With Doctors (coauthored with Debra Sex Roles. She received her PhD in 2000
Roter), and Interpersonal Sensitivity: from Arizona State University.
ATC-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 7:41 PM Page 582

582–––◆–––The SAGE Handbook of Nonverbal Communication

Caroline F. Keating is Professor of Jessica L. Lakin is Assistant Professor of


Psychology at Colgate University. She stud­ Psychology at Drew University in Madison,
ies the nonverbal skills and physical New Jersey. Her main research interest is
appearances associated with social domi­ nonverbal behavior, specifically noncon­
nance, leadership, and charisma in children scious behavioral mimicry, and its relation­
and adults. Together with collaborators, ship to affiliation and the development of
she has demonstrated that humans convey liking and rapport. Her other research
dominance through facial expressions akin interests include automatic processes, more
to those of other primates, facial features generally, the self, and motivated social
that make people appear powerful also cognition. Her work has appeared in
make them seem untrustworthy, people Psychological Science, the Journal of
who are socially powerful have unusually Nonverbal Behavior, and Personality and
good acting skills, and persuasive perfor­ Social Psychology Bulletin, as well as in
mances begin with kidding oneself. She several edited volumes. She received her BA
also studies the charismatic processes by in psychology from Butler University in
which groups inspire a following. Her 1998 and her PhD in social psychology
studies of dominance, leadership, and from Ohio State University in 2003.
deception (funded by the Harry Frank
Guggenheim Foundation) have been fea­ David Matsumoto is Professor of
tured in the print media, on radio talk Psychology and Director of the Culture and
shows, and on television. At Colgate, she Emotion Research Laboratory at San
teaches in specialty seminars on leadership, Francisco State University. He has studied
social bonds, and cross-cultural psychol­ culture, emotion, social interaction, and
ogy. She received her PhD from Syracuse communication for 20 years. His books
University. include well-known titles such as Culture
and Psychology: People Around the World
Mark L. Knapp is the Jesse H. Jones (translated into Dutch and Japanese), The
Centennial Professor in Communication Handbook of Culture and Psychology
and Distinguished Teaching Professor at (translated into Russian), and The New
the University of Texas at Austin. His pub­ Japan (translated into Chinese). He is the
lications include Nonverbal Communica­ recipient of many awards and honors in
tion in Human Interaction (with J. A. the field of psychology, including being
Hall), Interpersonal Communication and named a G. Stanley Hall lecturer by the
Human Relationships (with A. L. American Psychological Association. He is
Vangelisti), and Handbook of Inter­ the series editor for Culture, Cognition, and
personal Communication (coedited with Behavior for Oxford University Press. He is
John A. Daly). He is a Past President of the also Associate Editor for the Journal of
International Communication Association Cross-Cultural Psychology and is on the
and the National Communication Associa­ editorial boards of the Asian Journal of
tion, a fellow of the International Social Psychology, Asian Psychologist, the
Communication Association, and a Distin­ Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, Motivation
guished Scholar in the National Communi­ and Emotion, Cognition and Emotion, and
cation Association. He has served as Editor Human Communication.
of Human Communication Research, and
he developed and edited the Sage Series in James C. McCroskey is Professor of
Interpersonal Communication. Communication Studies at West Virginia
ATC-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 7:41 PM Page 583

About the Contributors–––◆–––583

University. He has authored or coauthored organizational communication, and inter­


over 50 books and 250 journal articles cultural communication to launch and
and book chapters. He coauthored the manage her own private business for 5
first book on instructional communication years and to serve as a consultant for a
in the field and taught one of the first major international business organization.
graduate courses in nonverbal communi­
cation in the field. His research, writing, Beth A. Le Poire is currently Associate
and teaching have focused on instructional Professor of Communication at California
communication, interpersonal communi­ Lutheran University. She has authored over
cation, nonverbal communication, organi­ 45 research papers in the areas of family
zational communication, intercultural and interpersonal communication and
communication, communication traits, specializes in nonverbal communication
social influence, and communibiology. He research. She recently published in the text
has received numerous awards for his Family Communication (Sage, 2006) and is
teaching and research from his university in the process of completing an edited vol­
and a wide variety of national and inter­ ume on interpersonal, socially meaningful
national professional associations in com­ research with Rene Dailey at the University
munication, teacher education, and of Texas at Austin. She was a professor at
pharmacy education. He has been recog­ the University of California at Santa
nized as the most prolific published Barbara when she completed this introduc­
scholar in the history of the field of com­ tory chapter with Howard Giles.
munication.
Patricia Noller is currently Emeritus
Linda L. McCroskey is Associate Professor Professor of Psychology at the University of
of Communication Studies at California Queensland. For 7 years, she was Director
State University, Long Beach. Her research, of the University of Queensland Family
writing, and teaching have focused on inter­ Centre. She has published extensively in the
cultural communication, organizational area of marital and family relationships,
communication, business communication, including 12 books and over 100 journal
instructional communication, communica­ articles and book chapters. She is a fellow
tion traits, and communication theory. She of the Academy of the Social Sciences in
has published in several leading journals in Australia and of the National Council on
the field of communication, including Family Relationships in the United States.
Communication Quarterly, Communication She has served on a number of editorial
Research Reports, and the Journal of boards and was appointed as Foundation
Intercultural Communication Research. Editor of Personal Relationships: Journal of
She is a coauthor of books on instructional the International Society for the Study of
communication and organizational com­ Personal Relationships, a position she held
munication and a forthcoming book on from 1993 to 1997. She was President of
business communication. She is an active that society from 1998 to 2000.
member of the International Communica­
tion Association, the National Communica­ Stacie Renfro Powers is a third-year doc­
tion Association, the Eastern Communication toral student in communication sciences
Association, and the Western States at the University of Connecticut. She is
Communication Association. She also the recipient of a University of Connecti­
has employed her education in business, cut Outstanding Scholars predoctoral
ATC-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 7:41 PM Page 584

584–––◆–––The SAGE Handbook of Nonverbal Communication

fellowship and has worked as a research race and gender influence the way people
assistant on projects relating to the com­ think, feel, and behave. More specifically,
munication of trustworthiness (funded by her research investigates multiple dynamics
the Russell Sage Foundation), the commu­ of prejudice and stereotyping from the per­
nication of emotion in Huntington’s spectives of members of both traditionally
Disease patients and caregivers (funded by stigmatized and dominant social groups.
the University of Connecticut Health She is currently working on three primary
Center Huntington’s Disease Program), lines of research: the dynamics and conse­
and brain mechanisms of empathic ability quences of interracial contact, detecting
(funded by the Olin Neuropsychiatry and controlling racial bias, and racial cate­
Center of the Hartford Hospital Institute gorization and identity. She earned her BS
of Living). She is certified in the Facial in psychology from Brown University in
Action Coding System (FACS) and has 1994 and her PhD in social psychology
received training in functional brain imag­ from Harvard University in 2000.
ing (fMRI) study design and data analysis.
Her main research interests are in facial Virginia P. Richmond is Professor of
expressivity, empathic ability, interper­ Communication Studies at West Virginia
sonal coordination, and sex differences in University. She has authored or coau­
emotion communication. thored over 25 books and 150 journal
articles and book chapters. Her book on
Martin S. Remland is Associate Professor of
nonverbal communication, Nonverbal
Communication Studies at West Chester
Behavior in Interpersonal Relations (coau­
University of Pennsylvania. His research
thored with J. C. McCroskey), is now in
interests include nonverbal displays of sta­
its fifth edition (2004). Her research,
tus and power and cross-cultural differ­
writing, and teaching have focused on
ences in nonverbal involvement behaviors.
nonverbal communication, instructional
He is the author of Nonverbal Com­
communication, interpersonal communi­
munication in Everyday Life (Houghton
cation, organizational communication,
Mifflin, 2004) and coauthor of Inter­
communication traits, social influence,
personal Communication Through the
and training and development. She has
Lifespan (Houghton Mifflin, in press). His
received numerous awards for her teach­
work has appeared in numerous journals in
ing and research from her university and a
the fields of communication and psychol­
wide variety of national professional asso­
ogy. He received his BA from Western
ciations in communication, teacher educa­
Illinois University, his MA from Central
tion, and pharmacy education. She has
Michigan University, and his PhD from
been recognized as one of the top five pro­
Southern Illinois University.
lific published scholars in the history of
Jennifer A. Richeson is Associate Professor the field of communication.
in the Department of Psychology and
Faculty Fellow at the Institute for Policy Ronald E. Riggio is the Henry R. Kravis
Research, both at Northwestern Uni­ Professor of Leadership and Organizational
versity. Her research is in the areas of Psychology and Director of the Kravis
prejudice, stereotyping, and intergroup Leadership Institute at Claremont McKenna
relations. Her work considers the ways in College. He is the author of over 100 books,
which social group memberships such as book chapters, and research articles in the
ATC-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 7:41 PM Page 585

About the Contributors–––◆–––585

areas of leadership, assessment centers, orga­ together in a model called the psychological
nizational psychology, and social psychology. construction of emotion. He is a fellow of
His research work has included studies on the the APA and the APS. He received his PhD
role of social skills and emotions in leadership from the University of California at Los
potential and success, empathy, social intelli­ Angeles in 1974.
gence, emotional skill, and charisma. He is
Associate Editor of The Leadership Darren M. Schreiber is Assistant Professor
Quarterly and is on the editorial boards of of Political Science at the University of
Leadership and the Journal of Nonverbal California at San Diego. His research centers
Behavior. His recent books are Multiple on emergence and complexity in political
Intelligences and Leadership and The Future systems. He studied politics, philosophy,
of Leadership Development (coedited with and economics as an undergraduate at
Susan Murphy, 2002, 2003), Improving Claremont McKenna College, later attend­
Leadership in Nonprofit Organizations ing the U.C. Davis School of Law, where he
(coedited with Sarah Smith Orr, 2004), focused on civil rights litigation. He then spe­
Applications of Nonverbal Behavior cialized in federal litigation at the law firm of
(coedited with Robert S. Feldman, 2005), Neumiller and Beardslee. His dissertation
and Transformational Leadership (2nd ed., research used functional brain imaging
coauthored with Bernard M. Bass, 2006). (fMRI) to study the neural substrates of
political cognition and affect. He has shown
Jeffrey D. Robinson is Associate Professor that ideological sophisticates differ from
in the Department of Communication at political novices in their heightened use of
Rutgers University. He is broadly interested the posterior cingulate, a brain region associ­
in interpersonal communication, health ated with automatic social evaluation. His
communication, and language and social goal is to integrate agent-based models of
interaction. He specializes in conversation macropolitical dynamics with his computa­
analysis and physician-patient interaction. tional model of political cognition in individ­
He received his BA (communication) from uals in order to illuminate the emergence of
the University of California, Santa Barbara, political ideology in the mass public. He
his MA (communication), from the served as Research Director at the Center
University of Southern California, and his of Excellence in Cancer Communication
PhD (sociology) from the University of Research at the Annenberg School of
California at Los Angeles. Communication, University of Pennsylvania,
during 2004 to 2005. He earned his PhD in
James A. Russell is Professor and Chair in political science at the University of
the Psychology Department at Boston California at Los Angeles in 2005.
College. He spent most of his academic
career at the University of British J. Nicole Shelton is Associate Professor of
Columbia. An initial interest in the Psychology at Princeton University. She
emotional impact of large-scale physical was a postdoctoral fellow at the University
environments led to studies on the of Michigan from 1998 to 2000. Her pri­
language of emotion, taxonomies of emo­ mary research, which has been funded by
tion, facial expressions of emotion, cultural the National Institute of Mental Health
differences in emotion, the developmental and the National Science Foundation,
course of emotion knowledge, and theories focuses on how whites and ethnic minori­
of emotion. This research was brought ties navigate issues of prejudice in
ATC-Manusov.qxd 7/6/2006 5:04 PM Page 586

586–––◆–––The SAGE Handbook of Nonverbal Communication

interracial interactions. Specifically, she is self in the context of the self-regulation


interested in how whites’ concerns with and self-presentation of interpersonal
appearing prejudiced and ethnic minori­ behavior, examines how people’s behav­
ties’ concerns with being the target of prej­ iors and emotions are influenced by their
udice influence affective, cognitive, and concerns about others’ impressions and
behavioral outcomes during interracial acceptance of them. His current projects
interactions. Her secondary line of research range from examining the relationship
focuses on the consequences of confronting between the self’s regulatory resources and
perpetrators of prejudice. Specifically, she the capacity to monitor the social environ­
is interested in the interpersonal conse­ ment for relational value cues to the influ­
quences of confronting perpetrators and ence that threatening social circumstances
the intrapersonal consequences of not con­ exert on people’s self-presentation efforts.
fronting perpetrators of prejudice. She He received his PhD in social psychology
earned her BA in psychology from the from the University of Massachusetts
College of William and Mary in 1993 and Amherst in 2006.
her PhD in psychology from the University
of Virginia in 1998. Aldert Vrij is Professor of Applied
Social Psychology at the University of
Linda Tickle-Degnen is Associate Professor Portsmouth. His main research interest is
of Occupational Therapy in Sargent deception, particularly nonverbal aspects
College of Health & Rehabilitation of deception (e.g., how liars behave), ver­
Sciences at Boston University. Her research bal aspects of deception (e.g., what they
is directed toward understanding the social- say), people’s ability to detect deceit, and
psychological implications of Parkinson’s ways to improve this ability. He has pub­
disease and other chronic health conditions, lished almost 300 articles and book chap­
specifically as related to cross-cultural ters and six books to date, the majority of
health care interactions, interpersonal rap­ which are related to deception. He cur­
port, and quality of life. Her research is rently holds research grants from the
funded by the National Institute of Economic and Social Research Council
Neurological Disorders and Stroke and the (ESRC), the British Academy, and the
National Institute of Aging at the National Nuffield Foundation, and in the past he
Institutes of Health. She received her doc­ has held grants from the ESRC, the
torate in experimental social psychology Nuffield Foundation, the Leverhulme
from Harvard University, her master’s Trust, and the Dutch Ministry of Justice.
degree in occupational therapy from the All these research grants were related to
University of Southern California, and her deception. He is the Editor of Legal
bachelor’s degree in anthropology from and Criminological Psychology and sits
Stanford University. on the editorial boards of Law and
Human Behavior, Human Communi­
James M. Tyler is an Assistant Professor cation Research, and the Journal of
in the Department of Communication Nonverbal Behavior.
at Purdue University. He was recently
awarded an American Psychological Joseph B. Walther is Professor in the
Association Dissertation Research Award Department of Communication and the
and he has written nearly 20 journal arti­ Department of Telecommunication, Infor­
cles and book chapters. His research, mation Studies, and Media at Michigan
which focuses on the social aspects of the State University. His research focuses on the
ATC-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 7:41 PM Page 587

About the Contributors–––◆–––587

interpersonal dynamics of communication and the Communication and Technology


via computers in personal relationships, division of the International Communication
work groups, and educational settings. He Association. His professional honors include
has held regular or visiting appointments in the National Communication Association’s
psychology, information technology, educa­ 2002 Woolbert Award for an article that has
tion and social policy at universities in the US stood the test of time and influenced thinking
and UK. He was Chair of the Organizational in the discipline for more than 10 years. He
Communication and Information Systems received his PhD from the University of
division of the Academy of Management Arizona in 1990.
ATC-Manusov.qxd 6/30/2006 7:41 PM Page 588

You might also like