You are on page 1of 18

Developmental Psychology © 2012 American Psychological Association

2012, Vol. 48, No. 3, 844 – 861 0012-1649/12/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/a0027432

Dopaminergic Modulation of Incentive Motivation in Adolescence:


Age-Related Changes in Signaling, Individual Differences, and
Implications for the Development of Self-Regulation

Monica Luciana, Dustin Wahlstrom, James N. Porter, and Paul F. Collins


University of Minnesota

Behavioral activation that is associated with incentive-reward motivation increases in adolescence


relative to childhood and adulthood. This quadratic developmental pattern is generally supported by
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

behavioral and experimental neuroscience findings. It is suggested that a focus on changes in dopamine
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

neurotransmission is informative in understanding the mechanism for this adolescent increase in reward-
related behavioral activation and subsequent decline into adulthood. Evidence is presented to indicate that
incentive-reward motivation is modulated by mesoaccumbens dopamine, and that it increases in ado-
lescence before declining into adulthood because of normative developmental changes at the molecular
level. Potential mechanisms of variation in functional mesoaccumbens dopamine transmission are
discussed with a focus on the interplay between tonic and phasic modes of dopamine transmission in
modulating both general incentive-motivational biases and the efficacy of reward learning during
exposure to novel reward experiences. Interactions between individual difference factors and these
age-related trends are discussed.

Keywords: risk-taking, reward, motivation, adolescence, dopamine

Human adolescence has long been recognized as a period of Within this article, we address the nature of the adolescent’s
heightened exploration of novelty relative to levels observed in motivational state and how that state biases response tendencies
earlier childhood and later adulthood (Kelley, Schochet, & Landry, prior to the acquisition of consolidated experience. Our assertion is
2004; Spear, 2000). This observation is intriguing, because al- that adolescents experience an age-related increase in incentive
though novel events and situations readily capture attention (Fried- motivation that is above and beyond what they experience in
man, Cycowicz, & Gaeta, 2001), one can respond in real-world childhood and above and beyond levels experienced in adulthood
contexts with unrestrained approach, caution, or avoidance, de- (see quadratic pattern illustrated in Figure 1).
pending on motivational bias. Approach to novelty reflects a
positive motivational bias, given that novelty presents a mixture of What Is Incentive Motivation?
reward and threat (Hooks & Kalivas, 1995). Because of their
willingness to explore unfamiliar situations and lack of experience Incentive motivation refers to the energizing of instrumental
with high-risk outcomes, adolescents may be vulnerable to various behavior by anticipation of reward acquisition, (Depue & Collins,
forms of risk-related harm. 1999; Wise, 2004) a process fundamentally grounded in geneti-
Individuals may engage in actions that have a high probability cally determined individual differences (Depue & Collins, 1999;
of adverse consequences because they are insensitive to those Koob & LeMoal, 1997) but shaped through experience. Impor-
consequences, because they are more compelled by the prospect of tantly, one cannot gain the requisite experience through inactivity.
gain, or because they do not have the requisite experience to A biologically grounded system is necessary to promote active
develop any meaningful representation of probabilities of gain or exploration to ensure that rewards and reward-learning experi-
loss and thus behave according to an inherent motivational bias. ences are obtained.
Gray (1973) proposed that higher order dispositional traits are
grounded in biologically based neurobehavioral systems that vary
quantitatively across individuals (i.e., they incorporate trait-level
This article was published Online First March 5, 2012. variation). The system with incentive motivation at its core has
Monica Luciana, Dustin Wahlstrom, James N. Porter, and Paul F. been variously referred to as an expectancy, approach, behavioral
Collins, Department of Psychology and Center for Neurobehavioral De- facilitation, or behavioral activation system (see Depue & Collins,
velopment, University of Minnesota. 1999; Luciana, 2001; Wahlstrom, White, Collins, & Luciana,
The preparation of this article was supported by National Institute on
2010, for reviews).
Drug Abuse Grant DA017843 to Monica Luciana, which also provided
support to Paul F. Collins, and by National Institute of Mental Health Grant
It is expressed through behavioral tendencies related to affilia-
MH017069, which supported James N. Porter’s work on this project. tion and agency and the higher order domain of extraversion
Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Monica (Depue & Collins, 1999). Affiliation directs individuals to ap-
Luciana, 75 East River Road, N218 Elliott Hall, Minneapolis, MN 55455. proach others and to gain reward from interpersonal interactions,
E-mail: lucia003@umn.edu whereas agency promotes social dominance, mastery, efficacy, and

844
DOPAMINE AND INCENTIVE MOTIVATION IN ADOLESCENCE 845

well as neurobiological changes involving the structure and func-


tioning of this circuitry.

Evidence for Increased Incentive Motivation in


Adolescence

Epidemiological studies indicate that experimentation with sub-


stances including alcohol, nicotine, and illicit drugs begins in
adolescence. National youth surveys indicate that 21.1% of youths
in the United States have consumed alcohol (more than a few sips),
and 7.5% have tried marijuana before age 13. By the end of high
school, these percentages rise to 72.5% and 36.8%, respectively
(Eaton et al., 2010). Sexual experimentation is also common. By
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

age 15, up to 70% of adolescents, depending on culture, have


This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

engaged in sexual intercourse (Hawes, Wellings, & Stephenson,


2010), many without using adequate protection against pregnancy
or disease (Eaton et al., 2010).
Figure 1. Dopamine-driven acceleration of incentive motivation from
childhood to adolescence and subsequent decline from adolescence to These statistics are supported by the manner in which adoles-
adulthood. cents describe their behavioral biases. For instance, using a mea-
sure of sensation-seeking that emphasized novelty-seeking, Stein-
achievement (Tellegen & Waller, 2008). Although these charac- berg et al. (2008) recently demonstrated that adolescents report
teristics can be measured in humans through questionnaire and high levels of sensation-seeking relative to children and adults.
laboratory paradigms, animal studies have operationalized moti- This tendency appeared to peak in early to mid-adolescence
vation through measures of goal-directed motor activity, rates, (roughly between the ages of 12 and 15 years) and declined
speed and vigor of responding, and the extent to which an animal thereafter, as evidenced by a large-scale cross-sectional study of
is willing to work for a given outcome (Beninger, 1983; Olds & over 900 individuals who ranged in age from 10 to 30 years
Milner, 1954). Developmental psychologists have suggested that (Steinberg et al., 2008). In our lab, we have observed that mid-
this system is represented by the temperament dimension of sur- adolescents (ages 13–17 years) report increases in reward respon-
gency (Rothbart, Ahadi, Hershey, & Fisher, 2001; Segalowitz et sivity (from Carver & White’s, 1994, Behavioral Inhibition Sys-
al., 2011). We have articulated its role in both adolescent and tem/Behavioral Activation System questionnaire) relative to levels
toddler behavior via dopamine activity (Luciana, 2001; Wahl- reported earlier in the adolescent period (around ages 9 –12 years)
strom, White, Collins, & Luciana, 2010; Wahlstrom, White, & and relative to levels reported later, during young adulthood (ages
Luciana, 2010). According to this framework, approach behaviors 18 –23 years; Urosevic, Collins, Muetzel, Lim, & Luciana, 2012).
are primed by positive incentive stimuli (signals of reward), pro- The literature abounds regarding similar examples of self-reported
cessed by neural circuits dedicated to reward processing, and then increases in facets of novelty-seeking, sensation-seeking, and be-
conditioned through instrumental learning using dopamine as a havioral activation from childhood to adolescence as well as
modulator. declines from adolescence to adulthood (see Steinberg et al., 2008,
A dedicated neural circuit is proposed to support incentive 2010, for discussion, as well as Arnett, 1994; Kafry, 1982; Roth,
motivation, where the major nodes include (among other struc- Schumacher, & Brahler, 2005, for examples). At least one longi-
tures) the midbrain ventral tegmental area (VTA), its dopaminergic tudinal twin study reported that genetic variation accounted for one
projections to medium spiny neurons of the nucleus accumbens third of the variance in behavioral activation system activity
(NAcc), as well as the ventral pallidum, the amygdala, hippocam- (Takahashi et al., 2007).
pus, anterior cingulate cortex, and the medial orbitofrontal cortex In addition, adolescents (relative to both children and adults)
(OFC; Depue & Collins, 1999; Kelley et al., 2004). Dopamine have high levels of social affiliation and spend more time with
(DA) is a primary transmitter that modulates the system’s activity peers than with parents, validating the idea that social acceptance
through its facilitation of reward behavior (Damsma, Pfaus, Wenk- and affiliation are strong natural rewards for this age group (Csik-
stern, Phillips, & Fibiger, 1992; R. A. Frank, Manderscheid, Pan- szentmihalyi, Larson, & Prescott, 1977; Sebastian, Viding, Wil-
icker, Williams, & Kokoris, 1992; Gallistel, Schiegal, & Yeo- liams, & Blakemore, 2010; Steinberg, 2008). Affiliative tenden-
manns, 1981; Koob & Volkow, 2010; Panksepp, 1998). In cies are observed in pubescent animals as well (Spear, 2011). In
humans, individual differences in emotional trait dispositions that contrast to other periods in the lifespan, peer affiliation in adoles-
reflect aspects of positive emotion within the construct of incentive cence has been conceptualized as a central component of mutual
motivation are impacted by genetic and neurophysiological vari- promotion and facilitation of risk-taking behavior (Steinberg,
ations in DA function (Depue, Luciana, Arbisi, & Leon, 1994; 2008). Importantly, these behavioral domains (experimentation
Dreher, Kohn, Kolanchana, Weinberger, & Berman, 2009; Hariri, with substances and sexual behavior, peer affiliation in the context
2009; King et al., 1986; Wacker, Chavanon, & Stemmler, 2006; of heightened risk-taking) involve exploration of novelty in the
Zald et al., 2008). pursuit of salient primary rewards, an observation that offers clues
Evidence in support of the developmental trajectory advanced in regarding potential neural substrates that might underlie approach
Figure 1 includes behavioral changes in motivational tendencies as to such stimuli.
846 LUCIANA, WAHLSTROM, PORTER, AND COLLINS

Animal data cohere with the human literature given adolescent- cence (around the age of 7 years) in girls and somewhat later
limited increases in novelty preference, exploration, and risk- (around the age of 10 years) in boys. In contrast, between the ages
taking (Douglas et al., 2003; Spear, 2000; Stansfield & Kirstein, of 4 and 18 years, the amygdala increased in volume throughout
2006), effects that are enhanced by individual dispositions as well adolescence only in boys, whereas hippocampal volumes increased
as environmental stress (Philpot & Wecker, 2008; Toledo- in girls. Yurgelun-Todd, Killgore, and Cintron (2003) also re-
Rodriguez & Sandi, 2011). In addition, adolescent rats show ported increased amygdala volumes with increasing age in adoles-
increased levels of incentive learning relative to adults (Burton, cents, whereas Bramen et al. (2011) found a sexually dimorphic
Noble, & Fletcher, 2011). pattern where age and pubertal status were associated with in-
creased amygdala volumes in boys, whereas maturation in girls
Neurobiological Foundation was associated with smaller volumes. Structures such as the medial
OFC and NAcc were not examined.
Although neural structures that contribute to incentive behavior We recently observed increases in NAcc volumes from early to
have been identified, it is critical to specify the biological mech- mid-adolescence and then a decline into adulthood (Urosevic et al.,
anisms that might underlie its development, given assertions that in press). Like our observation of quadratic changes in self-
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

increases in sensation-seeking (a construct related, but not identi- reported reward responsivity, this patterning was supported by a
cal to, incentive motivation) have been suggested as a core sub- combination of cross-sectional and longitudinal data. Regions such
strate of adolescent risk-taking behavior (Steinberg et al., 2008). as the caudate, amygdala, and hippocampus did not show similar
Adolescent brain and behavioral development often fits a linear trends.
model with a plateau in early adulthood, at least as a first approx- Teicher, Andersen, and Hostetter (1995) examined rates of
imation. Comparatively fewer examples exist of behavioral do- synaptic pruning (operationalized as changes in DA receptor den-
mains characterized by accelerations to supra-adult levels, fol- sity) in the nigrostriatal versus mesolimbic DA systems in rats
lowed by subsequent declines. Thus, the quadratic pattern from pre-puberty into adulthood. They found strong evidence that
observed via epidemiological, behavioral, and animal data limits both D-1 and D-2 receptors are overproduced then pruned during
the number of biological substrates that might be invoked to early adolescence in the corpus striatam (nigrostriatal system) but
account for it. Here we offer two potential explanations, both of little evidence for receptor pruning in the NAcc during the same
which capitalize on the idea that adolescence prepares individuals period. There was moderate evidence for a transient decline in D-1
in an experience-expectant manner (Depue & Collins, 1999) for a receptor density in the NAcc in late adolescence that rebounded by
range of social, emotional, and motivational experiences that come early adulthood.
with emerging adulthood. Together, these experiences allow the Overall, although it is plausible that adolescence could be a
individual to achieve reproductive success (in an evolutionary period of synaptic exuberance followed by pruning in subcortical
sense) but also permit the achievement of a personal sense of regions, it may be difficult to empirically document both phases of
agency in pursuit of adult independence. neurodevelopment, as regressive gray matter changes (e.g., gray
matter volume reductions) appear to dominate adolescent struc-
Structural Neuroplasticity tural development. Synaptogenesis is maximal in early childhood
throughout the cortex with a gradual pruning of synapses during
Studies of adolescent brain development focus on declines in the late childhood and adolescent periods (Bourgeois, Goldman-
gray matter volume throughout the cortex, which is taken as an Rakic, & Rakic, 1994; Gogtay et al., 2004). The pruning process,
index of synaptic pruning (Giedd et al., 2006; Gogtay et al., 2004); inferred from age-related changes in gray matter volumes and
on increases in white matter volume (Schmithorst & Yuan, 2010); cortical thickness, appears to be regionally variant and unfolds in
and on connectivity patterns between regions (Asato, Terwilliger, a posterior-to-anterior gradient (Gogtay et al., 2004). It has been
Wu, & Luna, 2010). Perhaps structures that compose the brain’s generally accepted that subcortical structures stabilize in their
incentive-reward motivational system undergo a period of synaptic maturation earlier than cortical regions, although this conclusion is
exuberance as development proceeds from childhood to adoles- based on limited evidence. Only recently have neuroimaging tech-
cence. If so, this exuberance might be measurable in humans niques allowed volumes of subcortical structures to be resolved
through changes in subcortical gray matter within medial struc- with some reliability (Fischl et al., 2002).
tures, such as the NAcc, dorsal striatum, amygdala, and hippocam- In contrast, animal work indicates that subcortical structures of
pus, and within cortical regions to which they are directly con- the reward system are malleable in the context of relevant expe-
nected (particularly anterior cingulate and ventromedial prefrontal rience. Full coverage of this topic is beyond the scope of this
cortex [PFC]). During later adolescence, a subsequent wave of review, but two models are briefly mentioned, both of which focus
synaptic pruning and enhanced axonal connectivity (perhaps ex- on the NAcc. The first concerns basic studies of incentive-
tending into young adulthood) would refine synaptic connections motivation in animal models, one of which is derived from a
based on incentive-reward experiences uniquely encoded by each research program involving female Syrian hamsters. Meisel and
individual. Mullins (2006) demonstrated that the NAcc is part of a circuit that
Few human structural MRI studies have specifically focused on regulates incentive-motivated behavior, including sexual behavior,
adolescent changes in volumes of subcortical structures involved in this species. A single sexual encounter in a sexually naı̈ve
in mediation of incentive-reward motivation. Giedd et al. (2006) female activates NAcc neurons (Joppa, Meisel, & Gardner, 1995;
reported on gray matter volumes within the caudate nucleus across Kohlert & Meisel, 1999; Meisel, Camp, & Robinson, 1993), and
adolescence with some evidence for a U-shaped sexually dimor- sexual behavior increases c-Fos expression in the NAcc core but
phic developmental trajectory. Volumes peaked prior to adoles- not the shell (Bradley & Meisel, 2001). The NAcc core has greater
DOPAMINE AND INCENTIVE MOTIVATION IN ADOLESCENCE 847

involvement in behavioral response activation based on reward- for this assertion, focusing on changes in limbic/striatal DA sig-
related information associated with conditioned reinforcers, as naling from childhood into adulthood.
derived from information transmitted by limbic inputs, such as the
basolateral amygdala (e.g., Ito, Robbins, & Everitt, 2004). Overview of the Dopamine System
When the sexual behavior of these animals is studied in the
laboratory under maximally controlled conditions, the animals are DA has both neurotransmitter and neuromodulatory effects.
ovariectomized, and then their hormonal status is regulated by Whereas neurotransmitters influence postsynaptic cells via direct
exogenous administrations of gonadal hormones. Estradiol admin- receptor stimulation, neuromodulators may simultaneously regu-
istrations in adult females decreases synaptic density in medium late numerous populations of neurons thereby impacting the func-
spiny neurons of the NAcc core and alter the morphology of spines tional status of other chemical systems, leading combinations of
in that region. Dendritic spines are the primary anatomical sites of excitatory and inhibitory effects (Kazmarek & Levitan, 1987).
excitatory synapses, and this destabilization is thought to lead to a Thus, although DA cell bodies are abundant in midbrain regions
down regulation of accumbens excitability (Staffend, Loftus, & (VTA, substantia nigra), they project to adjacent midbrain and
striatal sites (mesostriatal system), to structures historically de-
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

Meisel, 2011). The reduction is akin to what is observed during


This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

drug withdrawal (Kourrich, Rothwell, Klug, & Thomas, 2007) and fined as core to the limbic system (mesolimbic system), and to the
may lead to a similar state of motivational “craving,” which cortex (mesocortical system; Björklund & Dunnett, 2007). DA is
ultimately promotes sexual motivation. Therefore, the hormonal often co-released with glutamate and/or gamma-Aminobutyric
environment modulates the behavioral expression of incentive- acid (GABA) and impacts information processing through these
motivation by altering synaptic structure within the NAcc (as well systems (Koob & Volkow, 2010; Trudeau, 2004). The VTA-to-
as the hippocampus, not discussed here). In the wild, this dynamic NAcc (mesoaccumbens) projection is particularly relevant to this
would ultimately serve to facilitate motivated behavior (in this review, given that the latter structure represents a core node for
case, receptivity to and seeking of sexual experience). Adoles- behavioral activation associated with incentive-reward motivation;
cence has not been a focus of this work, but given the hormonal it has been estimated that 75% of this projection is dopaminergic
changes that accompany puberty onset, as well as the observation (Swanson, 1982).
that sexual behavior is frequently initiated during adolescence, it is When depolarized, DA cells display two firing rates. One is
possible that these accumbens synaptic alterations may play a role represented by single-spike firing (in the 2–10-Hz frequency,
in motivating normative adolescent escalations in sexual and social termed “tonic”); the other is characterized by bursts of two to six
behaviors. action potentials (in the 15–30-Hz frequency; “phasic”; Grace &
A second model of structural plasticity is offered by studies of Bunney, 1984a, 1984b; Wanat, Willuhn, Clark, & Phillips, 2009).
drug addiction, where it has been observed that drugs of abuse lead The tonic mode is low in amplitude and steady and underpins basal
to changes in synaptic strength in several regions of the reward DA neuron firing patterns, resulting in extracellular DA concen-
system (Lüscher & Bellone, 2008). Structural plasticity in the trations ranging from 5 to 20 nM. DA diffuses several micrometers
NAcc has been hypothesized to account for drug-sensitization away from its synaptic release sites, which accounts for extracel-
effects in psychostimulant-exposed individuals, given that medium lular concentrations (Garris & Rebec, 2002; Rice & Cragg, 2008).
spiny neuron density increases after repeated cocaine ingestions In addition to presynaptic autoreceptor action, extracellular DA
(T. E. Robinson & Kolb, 2004). Spine density changes also ac- levels are regulated by the action of the DA transporter (via
company cocaine exposure following periods of withdrawal and reuptake) and by catabolic enzymes. Tonic firing is controlled by
abstinence (Shen et al., 2009). These effects may be mediated by membrane properties of the neuron and regulated by GABAergic
changes in glutamate levels (Kourrich et al., 2007) and can be inhibition (Floresco, West, Ash, Moore, & Grace, 2003; Goto,
reversed with injections of a glutamate agonist (Zhou & Kalivas, Otani, & Grace, 2007).
2008; Zhou, 2010). Although this particular example highlights a Phasic firing is rapid and transient and results in DA concen-
pathological process, it similarly illustrates the manner in which trations of as much as 1 mM. It leads to high-amplitude release of
experience can alter synaptic structure, strength of responding, and DA into neural synapses and, like extracellular DA from tonic
neurochemistry in this region. Similar experience-driven changes release, is regulated by reuptake mechanisms as well as catabolic
in brain structure could occur selectively during adolescence in a enzymes. The net result is that phasic DA acts locally within the
manner that accounts for an increase then subsequent decline in synaptic cleft. Phasic activity is triggered by environmentally
incentive-driven behavior. salient events (see Schultz, 2000; Wanat et al. 2009; and Willuhn,
Wanat, Clark, & Phillips, 2010, for reviews) and, within the striatal
region, is dependent on glutaminergic excitatory input to DA
Neurochemical Dynamics neurons from sources such as the pontine tegmentum, ventral
pallidum, and subthalamic nucleus.
A second interrelated mechanism to explain this patterning is The neural and behavioral correlates of each firing mechanism
grounded more explicitly in neurochemical changes that occur have been only minimally investigated, and it is still relatively
between childhood and adulthood. Specifically, during adoles- unclear how the firing modes interact or the extent to which they
cence, a transient peak in neurochemical signaling may occur are coupled. Tonic activity has been conceptualized as inhibitory
within circuitry that mediates incentive-reward motivation, thereby to phasic signals (Goto et al., 2007), although it has also been
providing a push toward acquisition of experiences that are essen- suggested that phasic signals potentiate tonic levels (Niv, Joel, &
tial for developing independent, intrinsically motivated adult be- Dayan, 2006). Schultz (2000) demonstrated that phasic DA activ-
havior in a sexually maturing individual. Here we provide support ity increases in the context of unexpected rewards and in response
848 LUCIANA, WAHLSTROM, PORTER, AND COLLINS

to reward during initial phases of instrumental reward learning and through knockout preparations) increase tonic firing and enhance
that tonic DA levels transiently decline when anticipated rewards incentive motivation in the context of well-learned behaviors
are not delivered or when animals encounter aversive stimuli. (Cagniard, Balsam, Brunner, & Zhuang, 2006; Cagniard, Beeler, et
DA acts on D1-like and D2-like receptors (Beaulieu & Gainet- al., 2006). Increases in incentive motivation are associated with
dinov, 2011) found presynaptically on the spines of projection higher levels of tonic DA activity in the striatum and with increas-
neurons (Caillé, Durmartin, & Bloch, 1996; Hersch et al., 1995) ing response vigor, as demonstrated through behavioral and com-
and postsynaptically on the heads of dendritic spines contacted by putational models (Niv et al., 2007; Weiner & Joel, 2002).
glutamatergic terminals (Levey et al., 1993). The tonic and phasic Phasic DA responses are more contextually bound in regard to
modes of firing may interact differentially with each receptor drug reward. Phasic DA responses occur following drug adminis-
subtype. In subcortical projections, phasic DA release activates D1 trations (reward delivery), notably alcohol, cannabinoids and nic-
receptors to facilitate inputs from the VTA to limbic and striatal otine, as well as following instrumental responses during the
structures (Goto et al., 2007). Tonic release has bidirectional course of cue-drug learning. Conditioned drug cues that are fol-
effects on VTA-prefrontal inputs through the action of D2 recep- lowed by drug administration consistently activate phasic DA,
tors. Increases attenuate the activity of PFC afferents, whereas whereas phasic bursts do not reliably occur in the context of
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

decreases facilitate them, perhaps enabling switches to new re- noncontingent pairings. However, spontaneous phasic responses
sponse strategies when current responses fail to yield anticipated due to pharmacological properties of drugs are also evident and
rewards (Goto & Grace, 2005). Importantly, there is an optimal may alter tonic levels of DA, thereby complicating efforts to
level of activity at which DA energizes behavior. An inverted-U- distinguish tonic versus phasic effects.
shaped performance function (Yerkes & Dodson, 1908) has been Willuhn et al. (2010) suggest that different time scales of DA
proposed to characterize individual differences in DA mediation of transmission may have different functions. Distinct aspects of
reward-based learning (M. X. Cohen, Krohn-Grimberghe, Elger, & phasic signaling may relate to both reinforcement learning and
Weber, 2007). approach behavior, whereas tonic signaling enables motivational
The critical question in the context of this review concerns and motor systems but not reinforcement learning. Overall, then,
which physiological parameters of DA neurotransmission are most receptor stimulation by tonic DA activity may index the individ-
tightly coupled to individual differences in the behavioral activa- ual’s ongoing incentive-motivational disposition (Willuhn et al.,
tion associated with incentive-reward motivation. Here we focus 2010; Niv et al., 2007), whereas phasic signals primarily influence
on tonic versus phasic modes of action and their effects on behav- encoding of stimulus and response reward associations by modu-
ioral and neurobiological activation. At this point it is important to lating synaptic plasticity in response to naturally occurring re-
distinguish between two basic sources of individual differences: wards, novel events, and reward learning experiences.
genetic variation and learning experiences. In the case of incentive These models may be integrated through a proposed develop-
motivation, although accounts tend to focus on the energizing of mental cascade through which elevated tonic DA levels that pro-
behavior during a specific incentive-reward learning experience mote incentive motivation and response vigor serve to direct action
(e.g., Schultz, 2000), individuals vary in their capacities for in contexts where initial cues for reward learning experiences are
incentive-related behavioral activation at the outset, prior to any ambiguous or simply absent. This context may characterize early
specific learning experience (Depue & Collins, 1999). Although adolescence. By definition, higher levels of incentive-motivation
phasic DA activity has garnered most of the attention in the (accompanying higher levels of tonic DA) energize exploratory
literature because of its role in reward learning, more enduring and approach behaviors that bring the individual into contact with
cross-situational individual differences in the magnitude and fre- reinforcing experiences of reward acquisition, each of which has
quency of incentive-driven behavioral activation appear to be specific stimulus and response associations. These experiences
modulated by tonic DA levels (Niv, Daw, Joel, & Dayan, 2007; would then drive both incentive-reward learning and phasic DA
Ostlund, Wassum, Murphy, Balleine, & Maidment, 2011; Willuhn responding, and sufficiently potent learning experiences may lead
et al., 2010). to further alterations in tonic DA levels as bouts of phasic DA
The importance of tonic DA levels for motivation can be seen in responses accumulate (Niv et al., 2007). With increasing age,
research on mechanisms of drug reward. For instance, noncontin- prefrontal circuitry and its connections with dorsal and ventral
gent systemic administrations of drugs of abuse (nicotine, alcohol, striatal regions become more directionally organized (Asato et al.,
cannabinoids, opiates, amphetamine and cocaine) consistently in- 2010). This refinement of PFC-striatal connectivity may permit
crease extracellular tonic levels of DA in the NAcc, whereas drugs phasic DA signals to achieve optimal signal-to-noise ratios in the
with low abuse potential do not (Wanat et al., 2009). Drugs such relay to cortical circuits. The medial orbitofrontal region, for
as amphetamine and cocaine impact extracellular concentrations instance, is among the last cortical brain regions to reach full
via clearance mechanisms, notably reuptake inhibition. Alcohol synaptic maturity (Gogtay et al., 2004) but is in receipt of
exerts similar effects but through a more complex cascade of outcome-related signals from the striatum during reward learning
signaling events. When tonic DA levels fall below a certain thresh- (Tobler, O’Doherty, Dolan, & Schulz, 2007). These signals permit
old, animals will titrate their levels of drug self-stimulation to an accurate calculation of expected value to be achieved during
reestablish a given level and to achieve optimal levels of intoxi- learning, allowing an organism to make probabilistic judgments
cation (Willuhn et al., 2010). Thus, tonic DA levels contribute to about potential outcomes during decision making (Schultz, 2000;
the drugs’ reinforcing properties but across a time scale of minutes Tobler et al., 2007). Thus, tonic DA signals code individual
to hours, as opposed to the millisecond precision within which differences in incentive motivation, propelling individuals at suf-
phasic signals operate (Wanat et al., 2009; Willuhn et al., 2010). ficient levels to engage rewarding experiences. These experiences
Moreover, decreases in striatal DA transporter activity (achieved serve as learning contexts through which phasic signals will be
DOPAMINE AND INCENTIVE MOTIVATION IN ADOLESCENCE 849

generated. As phasic DA signaling becomes more reliable with functional decline in activity between middle adulthood and old
advancing age (D. L. Robinson, Zitzman, Smith, & Spear, 2011) age (Bäckman, Lindenberger, Li, & Nyberg, 2010; Dreher, Meyer-
and as the PFC achieves its full maturational potential, the organ- Lindenberg, Kohn, & Berman, 2008).
ism can maximally benefit from this informational stream in Primate and rodent research generally indicates the most exten-
making decisions in potentially risky contexts. sive changes in DA transmission during the prenatal period into
This model is admittedly difficult to operationalize and relies on early childhood, as full synaptic capacity develops. There appear
assessing distinct aspects of DA neurotransmission not only be- to be more subtle but measurable changes in adolescence and
tween childhood and adolescence and between adolescence and declines in DA functional activity thereafter. We have summarized
adulthood but within adolescence as experience consolidates. If this work elsewhere (Wahlstrom, White, Collins, & Luciana, 2010;
tonic levels of DA underlie incentive-reward motivation, and if Wahlstrom, White, & Luciana, 2010), and other recent reviews
such levels increase through adolescence, there should be evidence have discussed it as well (Ernst, Romeo, & Andersen, 2009; Spear,
of increased DA release, decreased DA transporter activity, in- 2011). We emphasize points most salient to the impact of tonic
creased extracellular DA concentrations, and/or decreased autore- versus phasic signaling.
ceptor regulation, all of which are predicted to have effects on
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

Table 1, adapted from Wahlstrom, White, and Luciana (2010),


This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

tonic concentrations of DA (Goto et al., 2007; Willuhn et al.,


summarizes evidence of adolescent-specific changes in DA func-
2010). Similarly, as adolescence progresses, we might expect
tion. The table presents data separately from primate versus rodent
changes in phasic DA responses that have been enabled by this
studies, because distinctions between species have been observed,
tonic shift. Accordingly, shifts in age-related patterning of tonic
particularly in comparing subcortical versus cortical patterns of
versus phasic activity might differentially characterize early ado-
DA activity along several parameters. Both primates and rodents
lescence, when individuals initiate a significant expansion in the
exhibit alterations in substrates of DA signaling during adoles-
scope and intensity of reward experiences, as opposed to later
adolescence as such experience become more consolidated. cence. Subcortical changes are most relevant to this review.
One study reported on changes in midbrain cell firing through
adolescence in the rat (McCutcheon & Marinelli, 2009). Slow
Does the Dopamine System Change During
spike firing, in the range of 3–5 Hz, increased from days 24 to 27
Adolescence?
(late childhood), peaked around day 48 (peri-adolescence), and
Animal data support the notion that various aspects of DA then showed a steady decline through day 70 (young adulthood).
transmission change in the course of the mammalian life span from Others, using microdialysis, have reported a quadratic patterning
childhood to adolescence (see Table 1). Human studies indicate a of basal DA concentrations, with late adolescent peaks relative to

Table 1
Summary of Major Changes in Dopaminergic Signaling in Primates and Rodents

Variable Cortical Subcortical References

Primates
Tissue concentrations Peaks during adolescence Brown & Goldman (1977); Goldman-Rakic
& Brown (1982)
Dopaminergic Peaks in PFC layer III during Rosenberg & Lewis (1994, 1995); Lambe
innervation adolescence et al. (2000)
D1-type density Peaks in childhood; elevated in Peaks in childhood; elevated in Lidow et al. (1991); Lidow & Rakic
adolescents compared with adults adolescents compared with adults (1992); Montague et al. (1999); Seeman
et al. (1987)
D2-like density Peaks in childhood; elevated in Peaks in childhood; elevated in Lidow et al. (1991); Lidow & Rakic
adolescents compared with adults adolescents compared with adults (1992); Seeman et al. (1987)

Rodents
Tissue concentrations Monotonic increases between childhood Monotonic increases between childhood Andersen et al. (1997); Giorgi et al.
and adulthood, but adolescent peaks and adulthood, but adolescent peaks (1987); Nomura et al. (1976); Stamford
in dopamine synthesis in synaptic availability (1989); Ungethüm et al. (1996)
Dopaminergic Monotonic increase between childhood Berger et al. (1985); Kalsbeek et al. (1988)
innervation and adulthood.
D1-type density Monotonic increases between childhood Peaks during periadolescence Andersen et al. (1997); Gelbard et al.
and adulthood. (1989); Giorgi et al. (1987); Rao et al.
(1991); Tarazi et al. (1999); Tarazi &
Baldessarini (2000); Teicher et al. (1995)
D2-like density Monotonic increases between childhood Peaks during periadolescence Andersen et al. (1997); Gelbard et al.
and adulthood. (1989); Rao et al. (1991); Tarazi et al.
(1998b); Teicher et al. (1995)

Note. PFC ⫽ prefrontal cortex. Italicized boxes indicate evidence suggesting heightened dopamine activity compared with adulthood. Adapted from
“Neurobehavioral Evidence for Changes in Dopamine System Activity During Adolescence,” by D. Wahlstrom, T. White, and M. Luciana, 2010,
Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 34, p. 638. Copyright 2010 by Elsevier.
850 LUCIANA, WAHLSTROM, PORTER, AND COLLINS

early adolescence or adulthood (Badanich, Adler, & Kirstein, As comprehensively summarized by Spear (2011), although the
2006; Philpot, Wecker, & Kirstein, 2009). Concordantly, there is a animal evidence is overwhelming for a remodeling of the DA
decline in synthesis-modulating DA autoreceptor function in the system during adolescence, these various changes do not map onto
striatum, NAcc, and—most notably—the PFC between childhood developmental changes in motivated behavior in a straightforward
and adolescence (Andersen, Dumont, & Teicher, 1997), leading to manner. For example, adolescent rats appear to show differential
increases in synaptic availability. In adolescent primates, subcor- responsivity to the motor-activating versus rewarding properties of
tical tissue concentrations of DA increase relative to both child- stimulant drugs, possibly depending on the specific mechanisms of
hood and adulthood (Goldman-Rakic & Brown, 1982; Irwin et al., drug action (e.g., DA release vs. inhibition of the DA transporter;
1994). D1 and D2 densities peak in subcortical structures, such as Walker et al., 2010). Accordingly, although DA projection systems
the striatum and NAcc, in late childhood to early adolescence, as to both dorsal and ventral striatum (NAcc) are functionally imma-
do the densities of D2-like D3 and D4 receptors (Andersen, Thomp- ture in adolescent rats, the functional consequences differ in terms
son, Krenzel, & Teicher, 2002; Tarazi, Tomasini, & Baldessarini, of DA release, reuptake, and behavior, at least in the case of
1998b). Both D1 and D2 densities are heightened during adoles- stimulant drugs. In general, components of DA projection systems
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

cence relative to adulthood, a pattern that characterizes both the are highly interactive, and there is the potential for researchers to
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

cortex and subcortical regions (Lidow & Rakic, 1992; Seeman et reach different conclusions based on the use of different method-
al., 1987). The decline in receptor density from adolescence to ological techniques (see also Ernst et al., 2009; Willuhn et al.,
adulthood is in the range of 35%–50%, depending on region, and 2010, for further discussion of this point), as well as different
is accompanied by dramatic changes in DA basal activity (Ander- experimental designs.
sen et al., 2002). This complexity is illustrated well by D. L. Robinson et al.
We have not previously reviewed changes in transporter density, (2011), who studied phasic DA fluctuations in early adolescent rats
but these could impact adolescents’ tonic DA levels as well as and reported a baseline rate of fluctuation similar to adult rats but
pharmacological responses to DA drugs (Wanat et al., 2009). If an unreliable coupling of phasic DA responses to novel stimuli,
transporter density declines with consequent increases in DA tone, along with a nonhabituating phasic DA responsivity to repeated
then adolescents might be particularly sensitive to pharmacologi- brief social interactions with other rats (unlike adult rats). As
cal manipulations that impact reuptake inhibition. Indeed, adoles- shown by this example, adolescents may differ from adults regard-
cent animals, relative to adults, are more sensitive to the rewarding ing the information carried by fluctuations in DA transmission,
properties of psychostimulant drugs, including cocaine (Brenhouse even when overall transmission levels are equivalent. Therefore, it
& Andersen, 2008), nicotine (Torres, Tejeda, Natividad, & O’Dell, is challenging to pinpoint which developmental changes in DA
2008), and alcohol (Pautassi, Myers, Spear, Molina, & Spear, transmission have the most potent and direct influence over ado-
2008). Spear has demonstrated through an elegant series of studies lescent reward behavior, as well as which changes are antecedents,
(see Doremus-Fitzwater, Varlinska, & Spear, 2010; Spear, 2011) rather than consequences, of reward experiences (Spear, 2011).
that adolescent rats are particularly sensitive to the rewarding This complexity is reflected, too, by human functional neuro-
effects of alcohol and, concordant with the human work of Chein, imaging studies using tasks that deliver rewards and punishments
Albert, O’Brien, Uckert, and Steinberg (2010), to alcohol’s facil- (summarized in Table 2), which generally have not been specifi-
itation of social behavior (Varlinska & Spear, 2002). However, at cally designed to assess DA-related processes but contribute none-
least one study has demonstrated that cocaine-induced blockade of theless to this discussion. Paradigms used across studies vary
the DA transporter does not differentially impact extracellular DA considerably in the extent to which they assess incentive-related
levels in adolescents versus adults (Frantz, O’Dell, & Parsons, learning, brain responses to anticipation versus delivery of re-
2007). wards, whether incentives are designed to promote improvement
Findings regarding postnatal changes in DA transporters have on an otherwise nonrewarding task, and whether outcomes can be
been inconsistent. This is especially true in the substantia nigra and predicted in the course of task performance. Analytic strategies
VTA, where different lines of evidence have indicated no consis- similarly vary from whole-brain analysis to a priori region-of-
tent developmental changes (Moll et al., 2000), steady increases interest approaches with variation in the stringency of applied
between birth and adulthood (Galineau, Kodas, Guilloteau, Vilar, statistical thresholds. Given this heterogeneity in design and anal-
& Chalon, 2004), and peaks in transporter density at postnatal day ysis, the studies vary considerably in the extent to which they
21 in the rat, which is prepubertal (Coulter, Happe, & Murrin, identify age-related differences in activation.
1996). Importantly, different binding agents were used in each For instance, age differences between children and adolescents
study, and the age groups studied are not directly comparable. For were not found in blood-oxygen-level-dependent (BOLD) re-
example, neither the Moll et al. (2000) nor the Galineau et al. sponses to feedback when a guessing game was utilized (May et
(2004) studies examined transporter levels at the age at which al., 2004), but in a comparison of adolescents and adults that
density peaked, according to the evidence of Coulter, Happe, and utilized a “Wheel of Fortune” gambling task (Ernst et al., 2005), a
Murrin (1996). Findings in subcortical regions are more consistent, win versus no-win contrast revealed greater NAcc activation in
with converging lines of evidence indicating that transporter den- adolescents. In contrast, negative feedback (nonwins relative to
sity increases into periadolescence and plateaus thereafter in the wins) elicited greater amygdala activation in adults than adoles-
striatum, NAcc, thalamus, and bed nucleus of the stria terminalis cents, suggesting less sensitivity to punishment for adolescents, a
(Coulter et al., 1996; Galineau et al., 2004; Tarazi, Tomasini, & pattern also observed by van Leijenhorst, Zanolie, et al. (2010) in
Baldessarini, 1998a; but see Moll et al., 2000, for contrary find- the context of orbitofrontal activation and by Bjork, Smith, Chen,
ings). and Hommer (2010) in the anterior cingulated cortex. In addition,
DOPAMINE AND INCENTIVE MOTIVATION IN ADOLESCENCE 851

Table 2
fMRI Studies of Reward Processing in Healthy Adolescents

Reference Age groups Task paradigm Findings

May et al. (2004) 18 children and adols, ages 8– Guessing game with no predictable Many regions activated by reward feedback,
18 contingencies; guesses were rewarded, including the NAcc and orbital frontal
not rewarded, or had neutral outcomes region; no effects of age on patterns of
in a predetermined sequence activation
Bjork et al. (2004) 12 adols (ages 12–17); 12 adults Monetary Incentive Delay Task: Subjects No group differences in performance;
(ages 22–28) saw cues signaling opportunities to anticipation of gain activated NAcc in
win or avoid losing rewards of both groups; right insula, dorsal midbrain,
different magnitudes; task was to thalamus, and ACC also activated; gain
respond quickly to a subsequent target vs. nongain outcomes activated the NAcc,
to receive the reward; feedback was PFC, and putamen; post hoc group
provided on each trial comparison revealed an adolescent
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

decrement in gain anticipation activation


This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

in the right NAcc


Ernst et al. (2005) 18 adols (ages 9–17); 16 adults “Wheel of Fortune”: Subjects selected Win vs. no-win contrast showed greater
(ages 20–40) one slice of the wheel based on color NAcc activation in adols vs. adults;
(red or blue); the wheel was spun; the negative feedback activated the amygdala
subject won the dollar amount paired in adults more so than adols
with the selected slice if the slice
stopped under the pointer. If not, the
subject won nothing.
Eshel et al. (2007) 18 adols, 16 adults; same as “Wheel of Fortune” task; focus was on Increased risky selections in moderate risk
Ernst et al. (2005) risky selections range negatively correlated with age;
risky choice selection was associated with
greater OFC/VLPFC/ACC activation in
adults vs. adols; this activation correlated
with risky selections
Galván et al. (2006) 16 children (aged 7–11), 13 Delayed response two-choice task during Focus on nucleus accumbens and OFC
adols (ages 13–17), 12 adults event-related fMRI: passive pairings responses to large rewards; adols showed
(ages 23–29) of cues and rewards; task was to greater NAcc activity relative to both
select the side of cue presentation; children and adults; adols showed less
responses not explicitly rewarded OFC activation than children and were
equivalent to adults
Galván et al. (2007) 10 children, 7 adols, 9 adults Delayed response two-choice task during Self-reported likelihood of engaging in risk-
(subset of the above sample) event-related fMRI (same as above), taking was associated with magnitude of
plus self-reported measures of risk- NAcc activation in response to large
taking and risk perception rewards (r ⫽ .61); those who anticipated
positive consequences from risk-taking
activated the NAcc more
van Leijenhorst, Zanolie, N ⫽ 53; 15 adults (ages 18–23); Slot Machine Task: three possible When anticipating uncertain rewards, all age
et al. (2010) 18 adol (ages 14–15); 17 pre- outcomes (XYZ [50%], XXY [25%], groups showed increased activation in the
adol (ages 10–12) XXX [25%]); wins only for XXX; striatum; a cluster in the anterior insula
examined how adols respond to showed a linear decrease in activation
uncertain rewards with age; when processing outcomes,
middle adols were more responsive to
received rewards as indicated by
increased activation in the ventral
striatum; young adults responded most to
the omission of rewards as indicated by
increased activation in the OFC
Bjork et al. (2010) 24 adols (ages 12–17) and 24 Compared adol response to reward cues Both adols and adults recruit the NAcc and
adults (ages 22–42) vs. reward delivery using Monetary medial OFC during task performance;
Incentive Delay Task Adols showed reduced NAcc recruitment
by reward-predictive cues compared with
adults in a linear contrast with
nonincentive cues and in a volume-of-
interest analysis of signal change in the
NAcc. Adols showed little difference in
striatal and frontocortical responsiveness
to reward deliveries compared with
adults; suprathreshold activation of ACC
by loss outcomes vs. avoided losses was
present in adults but not adols
(table continues)
852 LUCIANA, WAHLSTROM, PORTER, AND COLLINS

Table 2 (continued)

Reference Age groups Task paradigm Findings

J. R. Cohen et al. 18 children, aged 8–12, 16 adols Probabilistic learning task: Participants Adols had faster reaction times than the
(2010) aged 14–19, and 11 adults classified abstract stimuli into one of other groups to stimuli with large
aged 25–30 two categories; feedback provided on rewards; neural responses to stimulus
each trial; correct responses included a presentation vs. feedback were modeled
monetary reward. using linear and quadratic functions;
neural prediction error signals in the
striatum peaked in adolescence, whereas
neural decision value signals varied
depending on how value was modeled
but decreased with age
Geier et al. (2010) 18 adols (ages 13–17) and 16 Rewarded antisaccade task; antisaccade Both groups performed better under
adults (ages 18–30) trials cued by possibility of reward or incentive conditions; adols vs. adults
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

not on that trial; correct inhibitions showed attenuated VS responses during


This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

were rewarded; no trial-by-trial the incentive cue but heightened VS and


feedback provided sPFC responses during reward
anticipation
Somerville et al. Children (ages 6–12); adols Affective go/no-go paradigm with happy Groups did not differ in hit rate; false
(2010) (ages 13–17); adults (ages vs. calm faces alarms were greatest for adols vs. the
18–29) other two groups for happy targets; adols
activated the VS more so that adults and
children in response to happy faces;
activation of frontal regions (IFG)
showed a linear pattern across
development
van Leijenhorst, 4 age groups: 8–10 yr; 12–14 Goal was to examine whether cognitive All age groups made more high-risk
Gunther Moor, et yr; 16–17 yr; 19–26 yr; total control patterns develop linearly and decisions as rewards increased; there was
al. (2010) n ⫽ 58 whether reward responsiveness is a decrease in risk-taking with age in the
elevated in adol using a two-choice most ambiguous condition; older
decision-making task; participants participants were more risk-averse; across
repeatedly chose between a low-risk ages, risky choices were associated with
gamble and a high-risk gamble; the activation in the medial PFC and the
size of the reward associated with the ventral striatum; cautious choices were
high-risk gamble varied associated with DLPFC activation; there
was an adolescent-specific peak in
activation in a VMPFC/ subcallosal
region during the decision phase and in
the VS/caudate during the outcome phase.
Chein et al. (2011) 14 adols (ages 14–18); young Investigated peer influence on risk- Adolescents took significantly more risks
adults (ages 19–22 years); taking. Stoplight Task: simulates when observed by peers than when alone;
adults (ages 24–29) driving intersections with traffic behavior was predicted by sensation-
lights; at each intersection subjects seeking tendencies and not by
decide whether or not to brake as the impulsivity; significant age by social
vehicle approaches a changing traffic context interactions were found
signal (cycling from green to yellow selectively in the VS and OFC; presence
to red); timing of traffic signals and of peers activated these sites in adols but
probability of crash both not other groups; among adols, greater
unpredictable; incentives offered for activity in the VS and OFC was
completing the drive in a fast time associated with risky decision-making (go
period versus stop trials); adults engaged lateral
PFC sites more robustly than did
adolescents when making decisions; this
age difference did not interact with social
context

Note. Adols ⫽ adolescents; NAcc ⫽ nucleus accumbens; OFC ⫽ orbitofrontal cortex; PFC ⫽ prefrontal cortex; VS ⫽ ventral striatum; DLPFC ⫽
dorsolateral prefrontal cortex; VMPFC ⫽ ventromedial prefrontal cortex; VLPFC ⫽ ventrolateral prefrontal cortex; IFG ⫽ inferior frontal gyrus; ACC ⫽
anterior cingulated cortex; sPFC ⫽ superior prefrontal cortex.

van Leijenhorst, Gunther Moor, et al. (2010) found adults to be the response was sufficiently quick, then the participant either won
behaviorally more risk-averse. or avoided losing money. Feedback was provided. This task would
Bjork et al. (2004) compared 12–17-year-olds with young adults seem to be an excellent candidate for the recruitment of incentive
using the Monetary Incentive Delay Task. On each trial, partici- motivation. Yet there were no group differences in performance.
pants viewed cues that signaled the opportunity to either win or Two aspects of reward processing were assessed in terms of brain
lose money. A subsequent target appeared, and the task was to activation: the anticipation of gain and responses to reward deliv-
respond as quickly as possible to the target with a button press. If ery. Gain anticipation activated the NAcc in adolescents and
DOPAMINE AND INCENTIVE MOTIVATION IN ADOLESCENCE 853

adults, along with other neural structures associated with reward in a ventromedial PFC/subcallosal region during the decision
processing (see Table 2). Post hoc group comparisons revealed that phase of the task and in the ventral striatum/caudate during the
adolescents showed a decrement in gain anticipation (decreased outcome phase.
right NAcc activation), suggesting motivational deficiency in this Chein et al. (2011) potentially lend some interpretive clarity to
age group. A more recent examination of reward anticipation using these disparate findings by showing significant Age ⫻ Social
the same paradigm (Bjork et al., 2010) indicated reduced recruit- Context interactions in the context of a driving simulation. The
ment of the NAcc in adolescents when responses to incentive and presence of peers activated the ventral striatum and OFC in ado-
nonincentive cues were compared. lescents but not other groups. Among adolescents, greater activity
Others found stronger evidence for distinctive adolescent pat- in both regions was associated with risky decision making (run-
terns of activation in reward-linked neural structures (Galván et al., ning yellow lights at stoplight intersections). Concordant with
2006; Somerville, Hare, & Casey, 2010; van Leijenhorst, Zanolie, Ernst et al. (2005), adults engaged lateral PFC sites more robustly
et al., 2010). Galván et al. (2006) focused on NAcc and OFC than did adolescents, perhaps suggesting more sensitivity to pun-
responses to large rewards in a task that required no explicit ishment conditions. This activation did not interact with social
reward-related learning. Adolescents showed greater NAcc activ- context. Thus, under conditions that elicit particularly strong
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

ity relative to both children and adults, but they showed less OFC incentive-motivation in adolescents, (i.e., peer interaction), ado-
activation than children. The magnitude of NAcc activation in lescents may exhibit stronger neural responses to both reward
response to large rewards was associated with the self-reported anticipation (also concordant with Geier, Terwilliger, Teslovich,
likelihood of engaging in risk-taking (Galván, Hare, Voss, Glover, Velanova, & Luna, 2010) and reward delivery and may show
& Casey, 2007), and those who anticipated positive consequences heightened ventral striatal responses.
from risk-taking activated the NAcc more strongly, a finding that Taking this literature as a whole, six studies (Chein et al., 2011;
supports the impact of individual difference factors on observed Galván et al., 2006, 2007; Somerville et al., 2010; van Leijenhorst,
age-related activations. Eshel, Nelson, Blair, Pine, and Ernst Zanolie, et al., 2010) compared children or early adolescents with
(2007) similarly focused on the extent to which adolescents made mid-adolescents and adults, allowing quadratic trends to be as-
risky selections and the neural activations associated with those sessed. Nearly every paradigm was associated with some form of
selections. Increased risky selections in a moderate risk range increased activity in DA-innervated structures of the reward sys-
negatively correlated with age. In addition, these selections were tem in adolescents compared with younger versus older partici-
associated with less activation of the OFC, ventrolateral PFC, and pants, although the conditions that elicit reliable region-specific
anterior cingulated cortex in adolescents versus adults. elevations in activation for adolescents remain to be clarified. The
Somerville et al. (2010) utilized a distinct measure of affective strongest contrary evidence comes from Bjork et al. (2004, 2010),
bias (an affective target detection task) and compared children, who used a measure explicitly designed to elicit incentive moti-
adolescents, and adults in their hit rates, false alarm rates, and vation and failed to find significant age differences. In contrast,
patterns of brain activation. Adolescents were more likely to Somerville et al.’s (2010) finding of a bias for perception of
generate false alarms, misinterpreting calm faces as happy, imply- positive facial affect in adolescents versus children and adults
ing a positive bias. A region-of-interest analysis indicated that coheres with the notion that incentive motivation is primed most
adolescents had greater activation of the ventral striatum than strongly in a social context during this period and, as shown by
adults or children in response to happy faces. Activation of pre- Chein et al. (2011), contributes to elevated risk taking in a peer
frontal cortical areas showed a more linear pattern across devel- context. Although this body of work may be suggestive of a
opment. quadratic trajectory in the development of neural signals associ-
van Leijenhorst, Zanolie, et al. (2010) examined how adoles- ated with reward processing from a broad perspective, at this early
cents, compared with children and adults, responded to reward point in the research, the findings are conflicting in regard to
anticipation versus uncertainty using a gambling task. All age elevated NAcc responsivity and associated incentive-reward acti-
groups showed increased activation in the striatum during reward vation as the specific neurobehavioral substrate for the quadratic
anticipation. However, during the processing of reward outcomes, pattern. In addition, because complex decision-making strategies
adolescents showed a unique pattern of increased activation in the are frequently incorporated into task designs, some experimental
ventral striatum, a pattern that was not observed by Bjork et al. paradigms are more adept at assessing developmental trends in
(2004, 2010) but that is more akin to Ernst et al.’s (2005) findings. cognitive and behavioral control as opposed to incentive-reward
In a second study, van Leijenhorst, Gunther Moor, et al. (2010) motivation.
assessed whether cognitive control patterns develop linearly be- Although any finding of increased adolescent NAcc (or other
tween childhood and adulthood and whether reward responsive- reward structure) activation in the context of a risk-reward para-
ness is elevated in adolescents relative to other groups. They used digm is interesting in terms of the neurobiology of risk-reward
a two-choice decision-making task requiring participants to repeat- decision making in general, direct connections to levels of DA
edly choose between a low-risk gamble and a high-risk gamble. transmission are lacking. Experimentally, the most direct connec-
The size of the reward associated with the high-risk gamble varied. tion to DA phasic responses requires that the task involve trial-
All age groups made more high-risk decisions as rewards in- and-error learning of incentive-reward response contingencies,
creased. However, there was a decrease in risk-taking with age. thereby utilizing the magnitude of the reward prediction error to
Across ages, risky choices were associated with activation in the guide learning and thus improve behavioral performance (e.g.,
medial PFC and the ventral striatum. Cautious choices were asso- J. R. Cohen et al., 2010; Pessiglione, Seymour, Flandin, Dolan, &
ciated with dorsolateral PFC activation. In terms of unique patterns Frith, 2006). Alternatively, a task with tradeoffs between level of
in adolescents, there was an adolescent-specific peak in activation response effort and magnitude of reward may provide an assess-
854 LUCIANA, WAHLSTROM, PORTER, AND COLLINS

ment of individual differences in tonic DA levels (see Niv et al., Hutchison, 2009). For individuals with low levels of basal me-
2007; Pinkston & Lamb, 2011). soaccumbens DA activity, increases during the adolescent period
At present, findings from the varied (albeit interesting) risk- will increase their absolute levels of incentive-reward motivation
reward tasks in this literature tend to obscure important conceptual and behavioral activation but still place them at relatively lower
distinctions. For instance, if punishment contingencies play a large levels than their peers, resulting in less reward seeking and more
role, as opposed to limiting a task to reward-delivery and reward- limited exploration of the novel experiences that typically become
omission contingencies, another emotional-motivational system available during adolescence. For individuals who occupy the
that regulates behavioral inhibition will be activated in simultane- middle of the distribution of basal mesoaccumbens DA activity,
ous competition with the system that regulates incentive-reward adolescent increases may bring them to a maximally adaptive peak
motivation, as suggested by Ernst, Pine, and Hardin (2006). Con- of incentive-reward motivation and behavioral activation, with
versely, if a task induces NAcc activation in response to simply increases in reward seeking and exploration of novelty that do not
viewing outcome-feedback stimuli that do not guide learning or entail frequent exposure to highly dangerous risks. For those
elicit modifications of future responses, the interpretive link to DA entering adolescence with high levels of basal activity, further
phasic responses and incentive-motivation is relatively weak. increases may predispose them toward behavioral engagements
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

Finally, most neuroimaging studies of human adolescent devel- with excessively risky situations, repeated drug and alcohol use,
opment have focused on cortical regions (where structural and inability to maintain focus in an achievement context, and com-
functional changes have been clear cut) and have not been de- plaints of chronic boredom. This can be seen as a form of response
signed to maximize assessment of diencephalic and midbrain perseveration or stereotypy (in accord with the Yerkes-Dodson
structures. Recently D’Ardenne, McClure, Nystrom, and Cohen formulation) in which the individual repeats the prepotent response
(2008) reported results using methods to tailor fMRI to assessment despite the presence of environmental cues that signal a need for
of brain stem responses, including within areas rich in DA cells, caution.
such as the ventral tegmental area. However, fMRI remains tar- This threefold typology was illustrated in adults by Cools et al.
geted at phasic responses, as tonic activation levels typically are (2009), who used baseline PET assessment and pharmacological
removed as sources of nuisance variance in standard statistical manipulation to establish a link between basal striatal DA activity
processing of BOLD signals. Positron emission tomography (PET) and reward-based learning. Specifically, it was demonstrated that
scanning is more suited to assessment of individual differences in DA synthesis capacity impacts learning rates as well as the behav-
tonic DA levels (e.g., Cools et al., 2009), which introduces the risk ioral effects of D2 receptor stimulation. Individuals with relatively
of radiation exposure. In addition, because sample sizes tend to be high baseline DA synthesis (a component of basal DA activity), as
small in fMRI studies, the role of individual differences that cut indexed by uptake of a PET DA synthesis tracer, showed relatively
across age groups cannot be explicitly assessed. This is particularly better reversal learning from unexpected rewards relative to pun-
important given the variability in individual BOLD activation ishments. These individuals were also more impaired by the ad-
responses observed within age groups, which can greatly exceed ministration of a D-2 receptor agonist, supporting the hypothesis
the magnitude of the age group average effects (Bjork et al., 2010). that induction of significantly increased activity in an already
high-functioning DA system results in an “overdose” behavioral
Individual Differences in Dopaminergic Tone Predict reaction, analogous to the dysfunctional motor response stereotypy
the Outcomes of Adolescent-Specific Overactivation observed under conditions of excessive mesoaccumbens DA trans-
mission in animal studies. Conversely, individuals with relatively
The third important point within this review concerns the notion low levels of basal DA synthesis showed relatively poorer reversal
that individual differences in mesoaccumbens DA transmission learning from unexpected rewards relative to punishments but
predict the specific outcomes of normative adolescent elevations in improved their performance significantly following administration
incentive-motivation and associated behavioral activation, as well of the D-2 receptor agonist. Individuals with intermediate levels of
as the eventual adult phenotypes. basal DA synthesis maintained intermediate levels of task perfor-
Despite the general tenor of literature on adolescent risk-taking, mance both at baseline and during the drug challenge condition.
many healthy typically developing teens do not apparently dem- Together these findings suggest that there may be a linkage be-
onstrate unusually strong motivational drives and impulsive re- tween tonic DA levels that influence overall levels of incentive-
sponse tendencies. Any model of adolescent development will motivation and behavioral activation, on the one hand, and the
have greater explanatory power if it can account for both group- efficacy of phasic mesolimbic DA signals that guide incentive-
based profiles as well as individual differences in behavior, and reward learning in specific environmental contexts, on the other.
incorporate interactions between the two sources of variation. To Accordingly, adolescent-limited elevations in mesoaccumbens DA
this point, we have asserted that incentive-reward motivation and transmission would be predicted to differentially affect individu-
behavioral activation are relatively greater in adolescence com- als, depending on their baseline preadolescent levels, as they
pared with other points in the lifespan because of increases in engage the novel opportunities for potential reward acquisition
mesoaccumbens DA tonic activity during this time. Nevertheless, (and exposure to punishment) that arise during adolescence.
we recognize that individual differences undoubtedly set a range It must be emphasized that these illustrative scenarios are con-
around which this adolescent-specific reaction can occur and that structed in a type of “dimensional isolation” for clarity of presen-
individuals enter the adolescent period with preexisting variation tation. The reality of adolescent behavioral change obviously is
in levels of incentive-reward motivation and behavioral activation much more complex, beginning with interactions between the
because of genetic and environmental influences operating magnitude and frequency of incentive-reward activation of behav-
throughout development (Depue & Collins, 1999; M. J. Frank & ior and the strength of constraining influences exerted by behav-
DOPAMINE AND INCENTIVE MOTIVATION IN ADOLESCENCE 855

ioral inhibition and cognitive control systems. Moreover, adoles- relate to motivated behavior, because controversy remains as to
cent reward experiences can induce plasticity in the whether reward sensitivity is modulated by dopamine systems.”
mesoaccumbens DA system that determines forward-going devel- Although Steinberg et al. (2008) suggested that increases in limbic
opmental trajectories in ways that defy direct linear prediction DA activity, in concert with puberty-driven hormonal changes,
from preadolescent behavioral characteristics (see Wahlstrom, might drive adolescents’ increases in sensation-seeking, they noted
White, Collins, & Luciana, 2010, for a similar example of geno- that this connection has not been established. Our view is that a
typically variant patterns of working memory development from comprehensive evaluation of both animal and human research
childhood to adulthood). literature clearly indicates a central role for DA, in concert with
glutaminergic and gabaergic systems, in modulating levels of
Concluding Comments incentive-reward motivation. Moreover, we hypothesize that indi-
viduals’ levels of functional DA activity in ventral striatal regions
Few researchers have investigated the potential uniqueness of are predictive of their behavioral biases in the presence of
the adolescent period in terms of the mechanisms that underlie the incentive-reward cues. We suggest that functional activity in the
time-limited heightening of incentive-reward motivation and be- mesoaccumbens DA system is heightened in the adolescent period
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

havioral activation during adolescence, bracketed by lower levels but with the caveat that this association has relied nearly exclu-
in both earlier childhood and later adulthood. As of now, there sively on animal research. The human functional neuroimaging
have not been structural MRI reports of NAcc expansion then literature is contradictory on this point (see Table 2, as well as
shrinkage that correspond in magnitude to the more dramatic Spear, 2011, for discussion), and within the animal research liter-
changes in cortical gray matter that occur throughout adolescence ature, the primary mechanisms within the mesoaccumbens DA
(Gogtay et al., 2004). Accordingly, we suggest a neurochemical system that may induce these adolescent effects are unclear. Given
account focusing on mesoaccumbens DA transmission that pro- our specific hypotheses regarding tonic mesoaccumbens DA trans-
vides a potential mechanistic explanation for this observed qua- mission levels, we suggest that changes in autoreceptor and trans-
dratic trajectory in incentive-reward behavior. We emphasize that porter function merit particular scrutiny given their roles in regu-
at a molecular level, various aspects of DA neurotransmission are lating extracellular DA.
under both genetic and environmental influence and thus capable We suspect that many investigators in the field of adolescent
of forming a foundation for individual differences, as well as brain development, while questioning our specific hypotheses,
normative trends, in levels of incentive-reward motivation and would agree in principle with this model. Several labs have re-
associated behavioral activation. An important next step in the ported quadratic trajectories for aspects of reward-based respond-
study of adolescent brain development is to generate empirical ing and/or sensation-seeking (Somerville et al., 2010; Steinberg et
data that can be more directly interpreted in terms of DA- al., 2008; van Leijenhorst, Gunther Moor, et al., 2010; van Lei-
modulated variation in levels of ventral striatal activation in ado- jenhorst, Zanolie, et al., 2010). What makes our perspective unique
lescents versus children and adults, as well as direct assessment of is that we view this accumulation of evidence as a foundation for
individual differences that may have as much behavioral impact as moving beyond speculation to direct human pharmacological and
the normative adolescent trends. Acquiring and integrating this neurobehavioral testing of a DA-based model.
evidence is crucial, because the behavioral activation associated Such testing in adolescent samples would not be without chal-
with incentive-reward motivation contributes to the executive load lenges, because systemic pharmacological agents can carry risks
that must be managed by the more linearly developing cognitive and side effects and can modify both tonic and phasic signaling
behavioral control system (Luciana & Collins, 2012). Thus, ado- (Willuhn et al., 2010). However, it may be possible to combine
lescence may be a period of nonlinear challenge to the ongoing pharmacological probes with behavioral tasks that are more spe-
development of emotional self-regulation. cific in assessing tonic DA levels versus phasic DA responses. For
instance, phasic signaling is inferred through prediction error as
Limitations quantified in probabilistic learning paradigms, whereas tonic DA
levels may be more readily associated with response vigor during
This discussion has been restricted to the dynamics of the effortful behavior, as well as individual differences in responding
incentive-reward motivational system and its subcortical substrate, to tradeoffs between level of response effort and magnitude of
particularly with respect to functional variation in the VTA-NAcc reward. Moreover, rather than lumping adolescents into one large
DA projections. To simplify the presentation, we have not dis- group, it may be more productive to assess neural and behavioral
cussed interactions with the cognitive control and behavioral in- distinctions as a function of level of exposure to novel reward
hibition systems, nor mechanisms for adaptive self-regulation of experiences, for example, by separating early, mid- and late ado-
emotional behavior in the context of cortical/subcortical interac- lescents into discrete groups or by quantifying the amount of
tions. In addition, it is important to point out that other groups have exposure using self-report instruments.
suggested a role for DA in adolescent risk-taking behavior (Ernst
et al., 2009), mediated through its facilitation of sensation-seeking Future Directions
(Steinberg et al., 2008, 2009) as well as through the balance of
activation between cortical versus subcortical structures (Casey, Beyond direct assessment of the DA system, which would add
Jones, & Hare, 2008). However, other groups also have expressed considerable interpretive clarity to the neuroimaging and behav-
reservations concerning the centrality of a dopaminergic mecha- ioral findings already reported, other future directions include
nism. For example, Casey and Jones (2010, p. 5) recently stated refining the construct of incentive-reward motivation and behav-
that “It remains unclear how changes in the dopamine system may ioral activation as it applies to children and adolescents, perhaps by
856 LUCIANA, WAHLSTROM, PORTER, AND COLLINS

creating questionnaire measures that provide more developmen- Bäckman, L., Lindenberger, U., Li, S., & Nyberg, L. (2010). Linking
tally appropriate representations the construct. Laboratory and cognitive aging to alterations in dopamine neurotransmitter functioning:
neuroimaging probes, too, could be better informed by the animal Recent data and future avenues. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Re-
pharmacology literature (as with J. R. Cohen et al., 2010), allowing views, 34, 670 – 677. doi:10.1016/j.neubiorev.2009.12.008
the various components of incentive motivation and behavioral Badanich, K. A., Adler, K. J., & Kirstein, C. L. (2006). Adolescents differ
activation to be assessed more cleanly. Good candidates, in our from adults in cocaine-conditioned place preference and cocaine-
induced dopamine in the nucleus accumbens septi. European Journal of
view, would include assessments of response vigor (Niv et al.,
Pharmacology, 550, 95–106. doi:10.1016/j.ejphar.2006.08.034
2007), manipulations of the expenditure of effort to achieve im-
Beaulieu, J. M., & Gainetdinov, R. R. (2011). The physiology, signaling,
mediate goals (Treadway, Buckholtz, Lambert, Schwartzman, & and pharmacology of dopamine receptors. Pharmacology Review, 63,
Zald, 2009), and discrimination of reward valuation as it varies in 182–217. doi:10.1124/pr.110.002642
adolescents compared with children and adults (e.g., see findings Beninger, R. J. (1983). The role of dopamine in locomotor activity and
related to delay discounting tasks reported by Olson et al., 2009, learning. Brain Research, 287, 173–196. doi:10.1016/0165-
and Steinberg et al., 2009). Several recent studies have incorpo- 0173(83)90038-3
rated electrophysiological recordings to examine aspects of puta-
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

Berger, B., Verney, C., Febvret, A., Vigny, A., & Helle, K. (1985).
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

tive phasic DA responding (Eppinger & Kray, 2011; Hämmerer, Postnatal ontogenesis of the dopaminergic innervation in the rat anterior
Li, Müller, & Lindenberger, 2011), an assessment strategy that can cingulate cortex (Area 24): Immunocytochemical and catecholamine
be applied with relative ease to both adolescent and adult samples. fluorescence histochemical analysis. Brain Research, 353, 31– 47. doi:
For instance, Hämmerer et al. (2011) examined the feedback- 10.1016/0165-3806(85)90021-5
related negativity component of the event-related potential during Bjork, J. M., Knutson, B., Fong, G. W., Caggiano, D. M., Bennett, S. M.,
probabilistic learning and found that adolescents and young adults & Homer, D. W. (2004). Incentive-elicited brain activation in adoles-
cents: Similarities and differences from young adults. The Journal of
were superior to children or elderly adults in their differential
Neuroscience, 24, 1793–1802. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4862-03.2004
classification of task-related outcomes, that they needed fewer
Bjork, J. M., Smith, A. R., Chen, G., & Hommer, D. W. (2010). Adoles-
trials to learn from choice outcomes, and that they learned more
cents, adults and rewards: Comparing motivational neurocircuitry re-
from gains than losses. A research program of this type serves to cruitment using fMRI. PLoS One, 5, e11440. doi:10.1371/journal
refine a DA-based model with a more substantial consideration of .pone.0011440
the roles that tonic versus phasic modes of DA transmission have Björklund, A., & Dunnett, S. B. (2007). Dopamine neuron systems in the
in modulating incentive motivation and guiding reward-related brain: An update. Trends in Neurosciences, 30, 194 –202. doi:10.1016/
learning. Longitudinal work would allow Age ⫻ Individual Dif- j.tins.2007.03.006
ference factors to be more cleanly dissociated. Finally, as a whole, Blair, C., & Diamond, A. (2008). Biological processes in prevention and
to the extent that pharmacological probes can be utilized, this work intervention: The promotion of self-regulation as a means of preventing
would elicit targets for future intervention studies given that neu- school failure. Development and Psychopathology, 20, 899 –911. doi:
rochemistry is malleable. 10.1017/S0954579408000436
To conclude, behavioral self-regulation is achieved, at least in Bourgeois, J., Goldman-Rakic, P. S., & Rakic, P. (1994). Synaptogenesis
part, by one’s ability to control motivational impulses (Blair & in the prefrontal cortex of rhesus monkeys. Cerebral Cortex, 4, 78 –96.
Diamond, 2008). With respect to the study of human adolescence, doi:10.1093/cercor/4.1.78
Bradley, K. C., & Meisel, R. L. (2001). Sexual behavior induction of c-Fos
incentive-reward motivational systems show a period of elevated
in the nucleus accumbens and amphetamine-stimulated locomotor ac-
activity between childhood and adolescence and a decline from
tivity are sensitized by previous sexual experience in female Syrian
adolescence into young adulthood. The mechanisms that underlie
hamsters. The Journal of Neuroscience, 21, 2123–2130. Retrieved from
this acceleration and subsequent decline are unknown. Here we http://www.jneurosci.org/
have suggested that adolescent changes in tonic neuromodulatory Bramen, J. E., Hranilovich, J. A., Dahl, R. E., Forbes, E. E., Chen, J., Toga,
activity DA within mesostriatal projections may represent a central W., . . . Sowell, E. R. (2011). Puberty influences medial temporal lobe
mechanism that should be further investigated to understand both and cortical gray matter maturation differently in boys than girls
this nonlinear developmental trend and the individual difference matched for sexual maturity. Cerebral Cortex, 21, 636 – 646. doi:
factors that interact with it. 10.1093/cercor/bhq137
Brenhouse, H. C., & Andersen, S. L. (2008). Delayed extinction and
References stronger reinstatement of cocaine conditioned place preference in ado-
Andersen, S. L., Dumont, N. L., & Teicher, M. H. (1997). Developmental lescent rats, compared to adults. Behavioral Neuroscience, 122, 460 –
differences in dopamine synthesis inhibition by (⫹)-7-OH-DPAT. 465. doi:10.1037/0735-7044.122.2.460
Naunyn-Schmiedeberg’s Archives of Pharmacology, 356, 173–181. doi: Brown, R. M., & Goldman, P. S. (1977). Catecholamines in neocortex of
10.1007/PL00005038 rhesus monkeys: Regional distribution and ontogenetic development.
Andersen, S. L., Thompson, A. P., Krenzel, E., & Teicher, M. H. (2002). Brain Research, 124, 576 –580. doi:10.1016/0006-8993(77)90960-X
Pubertal changes in gonadal hormones do not underlie adolescent do- Burton, C. L., Noble, K., & Fletcher, P. J. (2011). Enhanced incentive
pamine receptor overproduction. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 27, 683– motivation for sucrose-paired cues in adolescent rats: Possible roles for
692. doi:10.1016/S0306-4530(01)00069-5 dopamine and opioid systems Neuropsychopharmacology, 36, 1631–
Arnett, J. (1994). Sensation seeking: A new conceptualization and a new 1643. doi:10.1038/npp.2011.44
scale. Personality and Individual Differences, 16, 289 –296. doi: Cagniard, B., Balsam, P. D., Brunner, D., & Zhuang, X. (2006). Mice with
10.1016/0191-8869(94)90165-1 chronically elevated dopamine exhibit enhanced motivation, but not
Asato, M. R., Terwilliger, R., Woo, J., & Luna, B. (2010). White matter learning, for a food reward. Neuropsychopharmacology, 31, 1362–1370.
development in adolescence: A DTI study. Cerebral Cortex, 20, 2122– doi:10.1038/sj.npp.1300966
2131. doi:10.1093/cercor/bhp282 Cagniard, B., Beeler, J. A., Britt, J. P., McGehee, D. S., Marinelli, M., &
DOPAMINE AND INCENTIVE MOTIVATION IN ADOLESCENCE 857

Zhuang, X. (2006). Dopamine scales performance in the absence of new place conditioning in adolescent and adult male and female rats: Effects
learning. Neuron, 51, 541–547. doi:10.1016/j.neuron.2006.07.026 of social isolation. Physiology & Behavior, 80, 317–325. doi:10.1016/
Caillé, I., Dumartin, B., & Bloch, B. (1996). Ultrastructural localization of j.physbeh.2003.08.003
D1 dopamine receptor immunoreactivity in rat striatonigral neurons and Dreher, J. C., Kohn, P., Kolachana, P., Weinberger, D. R., & Berman, K. F.
its relation with dopaminergic innervation. Brain Research, 730, 17–31. (2009). Variation in dopamine genes influences responsivity of the
doi:10.1016/0006-8993(96)00424-6 human reward system. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sci-
Carver, C. S., & White, T. L. (1994). Behavioral inhibition, behavioral ences, USA, 106, 617– 622. doi:10.1073/pnas.0805517106
activation, and affective responses to impending reward and punishment: Dreher, J. C., Meyer-Lindenberg, A. M., Kohn, P., & Bermann, K. F.
The BIS/BAS Scales. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 67, (2008). Age-related changes in midbrain dopaminergic regulation of the
319 –333. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.67.2.319 human reward system. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sci-
Casey, B. J., & Jones, R. M. (2010). Neurobiology of the adolescent brain ences, USA, 105, 15106 –15111. doi:10.1073/pnas.0802127105
and behavior: Implications for substance use disorders. Journal of the Eaton, D. K., Kann, L., Kinchen, S., Shanklin, S., Ross, J., Hawkins, J., . . .
American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 49, 1189 –1201. Wechsler, H. (2010). Youth risk behavior surveillance—United States,
doi:10.1016/j.jaac.2010.08.017 2009. Morbidity and Mortality Weekly Report, 59(SS-5).
Casey, B. J., Jones, R. M., & Hare, T. A. (2008). The adolescent brain. Eppinger, B., & Kray, J. (2011). To choose or to avoid: Age differences in
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1124, 111–126. doi: learning from positive and negative feedback. Journal of Cognitive
10.1196/annals.1440.010 Neuroscience, 23, 41–52. doi:10.1162/jocn.2009.21364
Casey, B. J., Jones, R. M., & Somerville, L. H. (2011). Braking and Ernst, M., Nelson, E. E., Jazbec, S. P., McClure, E. B., Monk, C. S.,
accelerating of the adolescent brain. Journal of Research on Adoles- Leibenluft, E., . . . Pine, D. S. (2005). Amygdala and nucleus accumbens
cence, 21, 21–33. doi:10.1111/j.1532-7795.2010.00712.x in responses to receipt and omission of gains in adults and adolescents.
Chein, J., Albert, D., O’Brien, L., Uckert, K., & Steinberg, L. (2011). Peers NeuroImage, 25, 1279 –1291. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.12.038
increase adolescent risk-taking by enhancing activity in the brain’s Ernst, M., Pine, D. S., & Hardin, M. (2006). Triadic model of the neuro-
reward circuitry. Developmental Science, 14, F1–F10. biology of motivated behavior in adolescence. Psychological Medicine,
Cohen, J. R., Asarnow, R. F., Sabb, F. W., Bilder, R. M., Bookheimer, 36, 299 –312. doi:10.1017/S0033291705005891
S. Y., Knowlton, B. J., & Poldrack, R. A. (2010). A unique adolescent Ernst, M., Romeo, R. D., & Andersen, S. L. (2009). Neurobiology of the
response to reward prediction errors. Nature Neuroscience, 13, 669 – development of motivated behaviors in adolescence: A window into a
671. doi:10.1038/nn.2558 neural systems model. Pharmacology, Biochemistry and Behavior, 93,
Cohen, M. X., Krohn-Grimberghe, A., Elger, C. E., & Weber, B. (2007). 199 –211.
Dopamine gene predicts the brain response to dopaminergic drug. Eu- Eshel, N., Nelson, E. E., Blair, R. J., Pine, D. S., & Ernst, M. (2007).
ropean Journal of Neuroscience, 26, 3652–3660. doi:10.1111/j.1460- Neural substrates of choices selection in adults and adolescents: Devel-
9568.2007.05947.x opment of the ventrolateral prefrontal and anterior cingulate cortices.
Cools, R., Frank, M. J., Gibbs, S. E., Miyakawa, A., Jagust, W., & Neuropsychologia, 45, 1270 –1279. doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia
D’Esposito, M. (2009). Striatal dopamine predicts outcome-specific .2006.10.004
reversal learning and its sensitivity to dopaminergic drug administration. Fischl, B., Salat, D. H., Busa, E., Albert, M., Dieterich, M., Haselgrove, C.,
The Journal of Neuroscience, 29, 1538 –1543. doi:10.1523/ . . . Dale, A. M. (2002). Whole brain segmentation: Automated labeling
JNEUROSCI.4467-08.2009 of neuroanatomical structures in the human brain. Neuron, 33, 341–355.
Coulter, C. L., Happe, H. K., & Murrin, L. C. (1996). Postnatal develop- doi:0.1016/S0896-6273(02)00569-X
ment of the dopamine transporter: A quantitative autoradiographic study. Floresco, S. B., West, A. R., Ash, B., Moore, H., & Grace, A. A. (2003).
Developmental Brain Research, 92, 172–181. doi:10.1016/0165- Afferent modulation of dopamine neuron firing differentially regulates
3806(96)00004-1 tonic and phasic dopamine transmission. Nature Neuroscience, 6, 968 –973.
Csikszentmihalyi, M., Larson, R., & Prescott, S. (1977). The ecology of Frank, M. J., & Hutchison, K. E. (2009). Genetic contributions to
adolescent activity and experience. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 6, avoidance-based decisions: Striatal D2 receptor polymorphisms. Neuro-
281–294. doi:10.1007/BF02138940 science, 164, 131–140. doi:10.1016/j.neuroscience.2009.04.048
Damsma, G., Pfaus, J. G., Wenkstern, D., Phillips, A. G., & Fibiger, H. C. Frank, R. A., Manderscheid, P. Z., Panicker, S., Williams, H. P., &
(1992). Sexual behavior increases dopamine transmission in the nucleus Kokoris, D. (1992). Cocaine euphoria, dysphoria, and tolerance assessed
accumbens and striatum of male rats: Comparison with novelty and using drug-induced changes in brain-stimulation reward. Pharmacology,
locomotion. Behavioral Neuroscience, 106, 181–191. doi:10.1037/0735- Biochemistry and Behavior, 42, 771–779. doi:10.1016/0091-
7044.106.1.181 3057(92)90028-E
D’Ardenne, K., McClure, S. M., Nystrom, L. E., & Cohen, J. D. (2008). Frantz, K. J., O’Dell, L. E., & Parsons, L. H. (2007). Behavioral and
BOLD responses reflecting dopaminergic signals in the human ventral neurochemical responses to cocaine in periadolescenct and adult rats.
tegmental area. Science, 319, 1264 –1267. doi:10.1126/science.1150605 Neuropsychopharmacology, 32, 625– 637. doi:10.1038/sj.npp.1301130
Depue, R. A., & Collins, P. F. (1999). Neurobiology of the structure of Friedman, D., Cycowicz, Y. M., & Gaeta, H. (2001). The novelty P3: An
personality: Dopamine, facilitation of incentive motivation, and extra- event-related brain potential (ERP) sign of the brain’s evaluation of
version. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 22, 491–569. doi:10.1017/ novelty. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 25, 355–373. doi:
S0140525X99002046 10.1016/S0149-7634(01)00019-7
Depue, R. A., Luciana, M., & Arbisi, P., Collins, P., & Leon, A. (1994). Galineau, L., Kodas, E., Guilloteau, D., Vilar, M., & Chalon, S. (2004).
Dopamine and the structure of personality: Relation of agonist-induced Ontogeny of the dopamine and serotonin transporters in the rat brain: An
dopamine D2 activity to positive emotionality. Journal of Personality autoradiographic study. Neuroscience Letters, 363, 266 –271. doi:
and Social Psychology, 67, 485– 498. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.67.3.485 10.1016/j.neulet.2004.04.007
Doremus-Fitzwater, T. L., Varlinska, E. I., & Spear, L. P. (2010). Moti- Gallistel, C. R., Shizgal, P., & Heomans, J. S. (1981). A portrait of the
vational systems in adolescence: Possible implications for age differ- substrate for self-stimulation. Psychological Review, 88, 228 –273. doi:
ences in substance abuse and other risk-taking behaviors, Brain and 10.1037/0033-295X.88.3.228
Cognition, 72, 114 –123. doi:10.1016/j.bandc.2009.08.008 Galván, A., Hare, T. A., Parra, C. E., Penn, J., Voss, H., Glover, G., &
Douglas, L. A., Varlinskaya, E. I., & Spear, L. P. (2003). Novel-object Casey, B. J. (2006). Earlier development of the accumbens relative to
858 LUCIANA, WAHLSTROM, PORTER, AND COLLINS

orbitofrontal cortex might underlie risk-taking behavior in Adolescents. Yung, K. K., . . . Levey, A. I. (1995). Electron microscopic analysis of
The Journal of Neuroscience, 26, 6885– 6892. doi:10.1523/ D1 and D2 dopamine receptor proteins in the dorsal striatum and their
JNEUROSCI.1062-06.2006 synaptic relationships with motor corticostriatal afferents. Journal of
Galván, A., Hare, T. A., Voss, H., Glover, G., & Casey, B. J. (2007). Neuroscience, 15, 5222–5237. Retrieved from http://www.jneurosci.org/
Risk-taking and adolescent brain: Who is at risk? Developmental Sci- Hooks, M. S., & Kalivas, P. W. (1995). The role of mesoaccumbenspallidal
ence, 10, F8 –F14. doi:10.1111/j.1467-7687.2006.00579.x circuitry in novelty-induced behavioral activation. Neuroscience, 64,
Garris, P. A., & Rebec, G. V. (2002). Modeling fast dopamine neurotrans- 587–597. doi:10.1016/0306-4522(94)00409-X
mission in the nucleus accumbens during behavior. Behavioural Brain Irwin, I., DeLanney, L. E., McNeil, T., Chan, P., Forno, L. S., & Murphy,
Research, 137, 47– 63. doi:10.1016/S0166-4328(02)00284-X G. M., Jr., . . . Langston, J. W. (1994). Aging and the nigrostriatal
Geier, C. F., Terwilliger, R., Teslovich, T., Velanova, K., & Luna, B. dopamine system: A non-human primate study. Neurodegeneration, 3,
(2010). Immaturities in reward processing and its influence on inhibitory 251–265.
control in adolescence. Cerebral Cortex, 20, 1613–1629. doi:10.1093/ Ito, R., Robbins, T. W., & Everitt, B. J. (2004). Differential control over
cercor/bhp225 cocaine-seeking behavior by nucleus accumbens core and shell. Nature
Gelbard, H. A., Teicher, M. H., Faedda, G., & Baldessarini, R. J. (1989). Neuroscience, 7, 389 –397. doi:10.1038/nn1217
Postnatal development of dopamine D1 and D2 receptor sites in rat Joppa, M. A., Meisel, R. L., & Garber, M. A. (1995). Fos expression in
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

striatum. Brain Research, 49, 123–130. doi:10.1016/0165- female hamster brain following sexual and aggressive behaviors. Neu-
3806(89)90065-5 roscience, 68, 783–792. doi:10.1016/0306-4522(95)00179-M
Giedd, J. N., Clasen, L. S., Lenroot, R., Greenstein, D., Wallace, G. L., Kafry, D. (1982). Sensation seeking of young children. Personality and
Ordaz, S., . . . Chrousos, G. P. (2006). Puberty-related influences on Individual Differences, 3, 161–166. doi:10.1016/0191-8869(82)90030-7
brain development. Molecular and Cellular Endocrinology, 254 –255, Kalsbeek, A., Voorn, P., Buijs, R. M., Pool, C. W., & Uylings, H. B. M.
154 –162. doi:10.1016/j.mce.2006.04.016 (1988). Development of the dopaminergic innervation in the prefrontal
Giorgi, O., De Montis, G., Porceddu, M. L., Mele, S., Calderini, G., cortex of the rat. The Journal of Comparative Neurology, 269, 58 –72.
Toffano, G., & Biggio, G. (1987). Developmental and age-related Kazmarek, L. K., & Levitan, I. (1987). Neuromodulation: The biochemical
changes in D1-dopamine receptors and dopamine content in the rat control of neuronal excitability. Oxford, England: Oxford University
striatum. Brain Research, 432, 283–290. doi:10.1016/0165- Press.
3806(87)90053-8 Kelley, A. E., Schochet, T., & Landry, C. F. (2004). Risk-taking and
Gogtay, N., Giedd, J. N., Lusk, L., Hayashi, K. M., Greenstein, D., novelty-seeking in adolescence. Annals of the New York Academy of
Vaituzis, A. C., . . . Thompson, P. M. (2004). Dynamic mapping of Sciences, 1021, 27–32. doi:10.1196/annals.1308.003
human cortical development during childhood through early adulthood. King, R. J., Mefford, I. N., Wang, C., Murchison, A., Caligari, E. J., &
Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of Berger, P. A. (1986). CSF dopamine levels correlate with extraversion in
America, 101, 8174 – 8179. doi:10.1073/pnas.0402680101 depressed patients. Psychiatry Research, 19, 305–310. doi:10.1016/
Goldman-Rakic, P. S., & Brown, R. M. (1982). Postnatal development of 0165-1781(86)90123-X
monoamine content and synthesis in the cerebral cortex of rhesus mon- Kohlert, J. G., & Meisel, R. L. (1999). Sexual experience sensitizes
keys. Brain Research, 256, 339 –349. doi:10.1016/0165-3806 mating-related nucleus accumbens dopamine responses of female Syrian
(82)90146-8 hamsters. Behavioural Brain Research, 99, 45–52. doi:10.1016/S0166-
Goto, Y., & Grace, A. A. (2005). Dopaminergic modulation of limbic and 4328(98)00068-0
cortical drive of nucleus accumbens in goal-directed behavior. Nature Koob, G. F., & LeMoal, M. (1997). Drug abuse: Hedonic homeostatic
Neuroscience, 8, 805– 812. doi:10.1038/nn1471 dysregulation. Science, 278, 52–58. doi:10.1126/science.278.5335.52
Goto, Y., Otani, S., & Grace, A. A. (2007). The yin and yang of dopamine Koob, G. F., & Volkow, N. (2010). Neurocircuitry of addiction. Neuro-
release: A new perspective. Neuropharmacology, 53, 583–587. Re- psychopharmacology, 35, 217–238. doi:10.1038/npp.2009.110
trieved from doi:10.1016/j.neuropharm.2007.07.007 Kourrich, S., Rothwell, P. E., Klug, J. R., & Thomas, M. J. (2007). Cocaine
Grace, A. A., & Bunney, B. S. (1984a). The control of firing pattern in experience controls bidirectional synaptic plasticity in the nucleus ac-
nigral dopamine neurons: Burst firing. Journal of Neuroscience, 4, cumbens. Journal of Neuroscience, 27, 7921–7928. doi:10.1523/
2877–2890. Retrieved from http://www.jneurosci.org/ JNEUROSCI.1859-07.200
Grace, A. A., & Bunney, B. S. (1984b). The control of firing pattern in Lambe, E. K., Krimer, L. S., & Goldman-Rakic, P. S. (2000). Differential
nigral dopamine neurons: Single spike firing. Journal of Neuroscience, postnatal development of catecholamine and serotonin inputs to identi-
4, 2866 –2876. Retrieved from http://www.jneurosci.org/ fied neurons in prefrontal cortex of rhesus monkeys. The Journal of
Grace, A. A., Floresco, S. B., Goto, Y., & Lodge, D. J. (2007). Regulation Neuroscience, 20, 8780 – 8787. Retrieved from http://www.jneurosci
of firing of dopaminergic neurons and control of goal-directed behavior. .org/
Trends in Neurosciences, 30, 220 –227. doi:10.1016/j.tins.2007.03.003 Levey, A. I., Hersch, S. M., Rye, D. B., Sunahara, R. K., Niznik, H. B.,
Gray, J. A. (1973). Causal theories of personality and how to test them. In Kitt, C. A., . . . Ciliax, B. J. (1993). Localization of D1 and D2 dopamine
J. R. Royce (Ed.), Multivariate analysis and psychological theory (pp. receptors in brain with subtype-specific antibodies. Proceedings of the
409 – 463). New York, NY: Academic Press. National Academy of the Sciences, USA, 90, 8861– 8865. doi:10.1073/
Hämmerer, D., Li, S.-C., Müller, V., & Lindenberger, U. (2011). Life span pnas.90.19.8861
differences in dlectrophysiological correlates of monitoring gains and Lidow, M. S., Goldman-Rakic, P. S., & Rakic, P. (1991). Synchronized
losses during probabilistic reinforcement learning. Journal of Cognitive overproduction of neurotransmitter receptors in diverse regions of the
Neuroscience, 23, 579 –592. doi:10.1162/jocn.2010.21475 primate cerebral cortex. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sci-
Hariri, A. R. (2009). The neurobiology of individual differences in com- ences, USA, 88, 10218 –10221. doi:10.1073/pnas.88.22.10218
plex behavioral traits. Annual Reviews of Neuroscience, 32, 225–247. Lidow, M. S., & Rakic, P. (1992). Scheduling of monoaminergic neu-
doi:10.1146/annurev.neuro.051508.135335 rotransmitter receptor expression in the primate neocortex during post-
Hawes, Z. C., Wellings, K., & Stephenson, J. (2010). First heterosexual natal development. Cerebral Cortex, 2, 401– 416. doi:10.1093/cercor/
intercourse in the United Kingdom: A review of the literature. Journal 2.5.401
of Sex Research, 47, 137–152. doi:10.1080/00224490903509399 Luciana, M. (2001). Dopamine-opiate modulation of reward-seeking be-
Hersch, S. M., Ciliax, B. J., Gutekunst, C. A., Rees, H. D., Heilman, C. J., havior: Implications for the functional assessment of prefrontal devel-
DOPAMINE AND INCENTIVE MOTIVATION IN ADOLESCENCE 859

opment. In C. A. Nelson & M. Luciana (Eds.), Handbook of develop- behaviour in humans. Nature, 442, 1042–1045. doi:10.1038/
mental cognitive neuroscience (pp. 647– 661). Cambridge, MA: MIT nature05051
Press. Philpot, R. M., & Wecker, L. (2008). Dependence of adolescent novelty-
Luciana, M., & Collins, P. F. (2012). Incentive motivation, cognitive seeking behavior on response phenotype and effects of apparatus scal-
control, and the adolescent brain: Is it time for a paradigm shift? ing. Behavioral Neuroscience, 122, 861– 875. doi:10.1037/0735-
Manuscript submitted for publication. 7044.122.4.861
Lüscher, C., & Bellone, C. (2008). Cocaine-evoked synaptic plasticity: A Philpot, R. M., Wecker, L., & Kirstein, C. L. (2009). Repeated ethanol
key to addiction? Nature Neuroscience, 11, 737–738. doi:10.1038/ exposure during adolescence alters the developmental trajectory of do-
nn0708 –737 paminergic output from the nucleus accumbens septi. International
May, J. C., Delgado, M. R., Dahl, R. E., Stenger, V. A., Ryan, N. D., Fiez, Journal of Developmental Neuroscience, 27, 805– 815. doi:10.1016/
J. A., & Carter, C. S. (2004). Event-related functional magnetic reso- j.ijdevneu.2009.08.009
nance imaging of reward-related brain circuitry in children and adoles- Pinkston, J. W., & Lamb, R. J. (2011). Delay discounting in C57BL/6J and
cents. Biological Psychiatry, 55, 359 –366. doi:10.1016/j.biopsych DBA/2J mice: Adolescent-limited and life-persistent patterns of impul-
.2003.11.008 sivity. Behavioral Neuroscience, 125, 194 –201. doi:10.1037/a0022919
Rao, P. A., Molinoff, P. B., & Joyce, J. N. (1991). Ontogeny of dopamine
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

McCutcheon, J. E., & Marinelli, M. (2009). Age matters. European Jour-


This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

nal of Neuroscience, 29, 997–1014. doi:10.1111/j.1460-9568 D1 and D2 receptor subtypes in rat basal ganglia: A quantitative auto-
.2009.06648.x radiographic study. Developmental Brain Research, 60, 161–177. doi:
Meisel, R. L., Camp, D. M., & Robinson, T. E. (1993). A microdialysis 10.1016/0165-3806(91)90045-K
study of ventral striatal dopamine during sexual behavior in female Rice, M. E., & Cragg, S. J. (2008). Dopamine spillover after quantal
Syrian hamsters. Behavioral Brain Research, 55, 151–157. doi:10.1016/ release: Rethinking dopamine Transmission in the nigrostriatal pathway.
0166-4328(93)90111-3 Brain Research Reviews, 58, 303–313. doi:10.1016/j.brainresrev
Meisel, R. L., & Mullins, A. J. (2006). Sexual experience in female .2008.02.004
rodents: Cellular mechanisms and functional consequences. Brain Re- Robinson, D. L., Zitzman, D. L., Smith, K. J., & Spear, L. P. (2011). Fast
search, 1126, 66 –75. doi:10.1016/j.brainres.2006.08.050 dopamine release events in the nucleus accumbens of early adolescent
rats. Neuroscience, 176, 296 –307. doi:10.1016/j.neuroscience
Moll, G. H., Mehnert, C., Wicker, M., Bock, N., Rothenberger, A., Rüther,
.2010.12.016
E., & Huether, G. (2000). Age-associated changes in the densities of
Robinson, T. E., & Kolb, B. (2004). Structure plasticity associated with
presynaptic monoamine transporters in different regions of the rat brain
exposure to drugs of abuse. Neuropharmacology, 47, 33– 46. doi:
from early juvenile life to late adulthood. Developmental Brain Re-
10.1016/j.neuropharm.2004.06.025
search, 119, 251–257. doi:10.1016/S0165-3806(99)00182-0
Rosenberg, D. R., & Lewis, D. A. (1994). Changes in the dopaminergic
Montague, D. M., Lawler, C. P., Mailman, R. B., & Gilmore, J. H. (1999).
innervation of monkey prefrontal cortex during late postnatal develop-
Developmental regulation of the dopamine D1 receptor in human cau-
ment: A tyrosine hydroxylase immunohistochemical study. Biological
date and putamen. Neuropsychopharmacology, 21, 641– 649. doi:
Psychiatry, 36, 272–277. doi:10.1016/0006-3223(94)90610-6
10.1016/S0893-133X(99)00062-7
Rosenberg, D. R., & Lewis, D. A. (1995). Postnatal maturation of the
Niv, Y., Daw, N. D., Joel, D., & Dayan, P. (2007). Tonic dopamine:
dopaminergic innervation of monkey prefrontal and motor cortices: A
Opportunity costs and the control of response vigor. Psychopharmacol-
tyrosine hydroxylase immunohistochemical analysis. The Journal of
ogy, 191, 507–520. doi:10.1007/s00213006-0502-4
Comparative Neurology, 358, 383– 400. doi:10.1002/cne.903580306
Niv, Y., Joel, D., & Dayan, P. (2006). A normative perspective on moti-
Roth, M., Schumacher, J., & Brahler, E. (2005). Sensation seeking in the
vation. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 10, 375–381. doi:10.1016/
community: Sex, age and sociodemographic comparisons on a represen-
j.tics.2006.06.010 tative German population sample. Personality and Individual Differ-
Nomura, Y., Naitoh, F., & Segawa, T. (1976). Regional changes in mono- ences, 39, 1261–1271. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2005.05.003
amine content and uptake of the rat brain during postnatal development. Rothbart, M. K., Ahadi, S. A., Hershey, K. L., & Fisher, P. (2001).
Brain Research, 101, 305–315. doi:10.1016/0006-8993(76)90271-7 Investigations of temperament at 3–7 years: The Children’s Behavior
Olds, J., & Milner, P. (1954). Positive reinforcement produced by electrical Questionnaire. Child Development, 72, 1394 –1408. doi:10.1111/1467-
stimulation of septal area and other regions of rat brain. Journal of 8624.00355
Comparative & Physiological Psychology, 47, 419 – 427. doi:10.1037/ Schmithorst, V. J., & Yuan, W. Y. (2010). White matter development
h0058775 during adolescence as shown by diffusion MRI. Brain and Cognition,
Olson, E. A., Collins, P. F., Hooper, C. J., Muetzel, R., Lim, K. O., & 72, 16 –25. doi:10.1016/j.bandc.2009.06.005
Luciana, M. (2009). White matter integrity predicts delay discounting Schultz, W. (2000). Multiple reward signals in the brain. Nature Reviews
behavior in adolescents: A diffusion tensor imaging study. Journal of Neuroscience, 1, 199 –207. doi:10.1038/35044563
Cognitive Neuroscience, 21, 1401–1421. doi:10.1162/jocn.2009.21107 Sebastian, C., Viding, E., Williams, K., & Blakemore, S.-J. (2010). Social
Ostlund, S. B., Wassum, K. M., Murphy, N. P., Balleine, B. W., & brain development and the affective consequences of ostracism in ado-
Maidment, N. T. (2011). Extracellular dopamine levels in striatal sub- lescence. Brain and Cognition, 72(1), 134 –145. doi:10.1016/
regions track shifts in motivation and response cost during instrumental j.bandc.2009.06.008
conditioning. Journal of Neuroscience, 31, 200 –207. doi:10.1523/ Seeman, P., Bzowej, N. H., Guan, H.-C., Bergeron, C., Becker, L. E.,
JNEUROSCI.4759-10.2011 Reynolds, G. P., . . . Tourtellotte, W. W. (1987). Human brain dopamine
Panksepp, J. (1998). Affective neuroscience: The foundations of human and receptors in children and aging adults. Synapse, 1, 399 – 404. doi:
animal emotion. New York, NY: Oxford University Press. 10.1002/syn.890010503
Pautassi, R. M., Myers, M., Spear, L. P., Molina, J. C., & Spear, N. E. Segalowitz, S. J., Santesso, D. L. Willoughby, T., Reker, D. L., Campbell,
(2008). Adolescent, but not adult, rats exhibit ethanol-mediated appeti- K., Chalmers, H., & Rose-Krasnor, L. (2011). Adolescent peer interac-
tive second-order conditioning. Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental tion and trait surgency weaken medial prefrontal cortex responses to
Research, 32, 2016 –2027. doi:10.1111/j.1530-0277.2008.00789.x failure. Social, Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, doi:10.1093/scan/
Pessiglione, M., Seymour, B., Flandin, G., Dolan, R. J., & Frith, C. D. nsq090
(2006). Dopamine-dependent prediction errors underpin reward-seeking Shen, H., Toda, S., Moussawi, K., Bouknight, A., Zahm, D. S., & Kalivas,
860 LUCIANA, WAHLSTROM, PORTER, AND COLLINS

P. W. (2009). Altered dendritic spine plasticity in Cocaine-withdrawn tum but not nucleus accumbens. Developmental Brain Research, 89,
rats. Journal of Neuroscience, 29, 2876 –2884. doi:10.1523/ 167–172. doi:10.1016/0165-3806(95)00109-Q
JNEUROSCI.5638-08.2009 Tellegen, A., & Waller, N. (2008). Exploring personality through test
Somerville, L. H., Hare, T. A., & Casey, B. J. (2010). Frontostriatal construction: Development of the multidimensional personality ques-
maturation predicts behavioral regulation failures to appetitive cues in tionnaire. In G. J. Boyle, G. Matthews, & D. H. Saklofske (Eds.), The
adolescence. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 23, 2103–2114. SAGE handbook of personality theory and assessment. Vol. 2: Person-
Spear, L. P. (2000). The adolescent brain and age-related behavioral ality measurement and testing (pp. 261–292). Thousand Oaks, CA:
manifestations. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 24, 417– 463. Sage.
doi:10.1016/S0149-7634(00)00014-2 Tobler, P. N., O’Doherty, J. P., Dolan, R. J., & Schulz, W. (2007). Reward
Spear, L. P. (2011). Reward, aversions and affect in adolescence: Emerging value coding distinct from risk attitude-related uncertainty coding in
controversies across laboratory, animal and human data. Developmental human reward systems. Journal of Neurophysiology, 97, 1621–1632.
Cognitive Neuroscience, 1, 390 – 403. doi:10.1016/j.dcn.2011.08.001 doi:10.1152/jn.00745.2006
Staffend, N. A., Loftus, C. M., & Meisel, R. L. (2011). Estradiol reduces Toledo-Rodriguez, M., & Sandi, C. (2011). Stress during adolescence
dendritic spine density in the ventral striatum of female Syrian hamsters. increases novelty seeking and risk-taking behavior in male and female
rats. Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience, 5, 17. doi:10.3389/
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

Brain Structure and Function, 215, 187–194. doi:10.1007/s00429-010-


This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

0284-7 fnbeh.2011.00017
Stamford, J. A. (1989). Development and aging of the rat nigrostriatal Torres, O. V., Tejeda, H. A., Natividad, L. A., & O’Dell, L. E. (2008).
system dopamine system studied with fast cyclic voltammetry. Journal Enhanced vulnerability to the rewarding effects of nicotine during the
of Neurochemistry, 52, 1582–1589. doi:10.1111/j.1471-4159.1989 adolescent period of development. Pharmacology, Biochemistry and
.tb09212.x Behavior, 90, 658 – 663. doi:10.1016/j.pbb.2008.05.009
Stansfield, K. H., & Kirstein, C. L. (2006). Effects of novelty on behavior Treadway, M. T., Buckholtz, J. W., Lambert, W. E., Schwartzman, A. N.,
in the adolescent and adult rat. Developmental Psychobiology, 48, 10 – & Zald, D. H. (2009). Worth the ‘EEfRT’? The effort expenditure for
15. doi:10.1002/dev.20127 rewards task as an objective measure of motivation and anhedonia. PLoS
Steinberg, L. (2008). A social neuroscience perspective on adolescent One, 4, e6598. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0006598
Trudeau, L. E. (2004). Glutamate co-transmission as an emerging concept
risk-taking. Developmental Review, 28, 78 –106. doi:10.1016/
in monoamine neuron function. Journal of Psychiatry and Neuroscience,
j.dr.2007.08.002
29, 296 –310.
Steinberg, L. (2010). A dual systems model of adolescent risk-taking.
Urosevic, S., Collins, P., Muetzel, R., Lim, K., & Luciana, M. (2012).
Developmental Psychobiology, 52, 216 –224.
Longitudinal changes in behavioral approach system sensitivity and
Steinberg, L., Albert, D., Cauffman, E., Banich, M., Graham, S., &
brain structures involved in reward processing during adolescence. De-
Woolard, J. (2008). Age differences in sensation seeking and impulsivity
velopmental Psychology. Advance online publication. doi:10.1037/
as indexed by behavior and self-report: Evidence for a dual systems
a0027502
model. Developmental Psychology, 44, 1764 –1778. doi:10.1037/
van Leijenhorst, L., Gunther Moor, B., Op de Macks, Z. A., Rombouts,
a0012955
S. A. R. B., Westenberg, P. M., & Crone, E. A. (2010). Adolescent risky
Steinberg, L., Graham, S., O’Brien, L., Woolard, J., Cauffman, E., &
decision making: Neurocognitive development of reward and control
Banich, M. (2009). Age differences in future orientation and delay
regions. NeuroImage, 51, 345–355. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage
discounting. Child Development, 80, 28 – 44. doi:10.1111/j.1467-
.2010.02.038
8624.2008.01244.x
van Leijenhorst, L., Zanolie, K., Van Meel, C. S., Westenberg, P. M.,
Swanson, L. W. (1982). The projections of the ventral tegmental area and
Rombouts, S. A. R. B., & Crone, E. A. (2010). What motivates the
adjacent regions: A combined fluorescent retrograde tracer and immu- adolescent? Brain regions mediating reward sensitivity across adoles-
nofluorescence study in the rat. Brain Research Bulletin, 9, 321–353. cence. Cerebral Cortex, 20, 61– 69. doi:10.1093/cercor/bhp078
doi:10.1016/0361-9230(82)90145-9 Varlinskaya, E. I., & Spear, L. P. (2002). Acute effects of ethanol on social
Takahashi, Y., Yamagata, S., Kijima, N., Shigemasu, K., Ono, Y., & Ando, behavior of adolescent and adult rats: Role of familiarity of the test
J. (2007). Continuity and change in behavioral inhibition and activation situation. Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research, 26, 1502–
systems: A longitudinal behavioral genetic study. Personality and Indi- 1511. doi:10.1111/j.1530-0277.2002.tb02449.x
vidual Differences, 43, 1616 –1625. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2007.04.030 Wacker, J., Chavanon, M., & Stemmler, G. (2006). Investigating the
Tarazi, F. I., & Baldessarini, R. J. (2000). Comparative postnatal develop- dopaminergic basis of extraversion in humans: A multilevel approach.
ment of dopamine D1, D2, and D4 receptors in rat forebrain. Interna- Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 91, 171–187. doi:
tional Journal of Developmental Neuroscience, 18, 29 –37. doi:10.1016/ 10.1037/0022-3514.91.1.171
S0736-5748(99)00108-2 Wahlstrom, D., White, T., Collins, P. F., & Luciana, M. (2010). Develop-
Tarazi, F. I., Tomasini, E. C., & Baldessarini, R. J. (1998a). Postnatal mental changes in dopamine neurotransmission in adolescence: Behav-
development of dopamine and serotonin transporters in rat caudate- ioral implications and issues in assessment. Brain and Cognition, 72,
putamen and nucleus accumbens septi. Neuroscience Letters, 254, 21– 146 –159. doi:10.1016/j.bandc.2009.10.013
24. doi:10.1016/S0304-3940(98)00644-2 Wahlstrom, D., White, T., & Luciana, M. (2010). Neurobehavioral evi-
Tarazi, F. I., Tomasini, E. C., & Baldessarini, R. J. (1998b). Postnatal dence for changes in dopamine system activity during adolescence.
development of dopamine D4-like receptors in rat forebrain regions: Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 34, 631– 648. doi:10.1016/j
Comparisons with D2-like receptors. Developmental Brain Research, .neubiorev.2009.12.007
110, 227–233. doi:10.1016/S0165-3806(98)00111-4 Walker, Q. D., Morris, S. E., Arrant, A. E., Nagel, J. M., Parylak, S., Zhou,
Tarazi, F. I., Tomasini, E. C., & Baldessarini, R. J. (1999). Postnatal G., . . . . Kuhn, C. M. (2010). Dopamine uptake inhibitors but not
development of dopamine D1-like receptors in rat cortical and stria- dopamine releasers induce greater increases in motor behavior and
tolimbic brain regions: An autoradiographic study. Developmental Neu- extracellular dopamine in adolescent rats than in adult male rats. The
roscience, 21, 43– 49. doi:10.1159/000017365 Journal of Pharmacology and Experimental Therapeutics, 335, 124 –
Teicher, M. H., Andersen, S. L., & Hostetter, Jr., J. C. (1995). Evidence for 132. doi:10.1124/jpet.110.167320
dopamine receptor pruning between adolescence and adulthood in stria- Wanat, M. J., Willuhn, I., Clark, J. J., & Phillips, P. E. (2009). Phasic
DOPAMINE AND INCENTIVE MOTIVATION IN ADOLESCENCE 861

dopamine release in appetitive behaviors and drug addiction. Current correlates of medial temporal lobe development across adolescence: A
Drug Abuse Reviews, 2, 195–213. doi:10.2174/1874473710902020195 magnetic resonance imaging study. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 96,
Weiner, I., & Joel, D. (2002). Dopamine in schizophrenia: Dysfunctional 3–17.
information processing in basal ganglia-thalamocortical split circuits. In Zald, D. H., Cowan, R. L., Riccardi, P., Baldwin, R. M., Ansari, M. S., Li, R., . . .
G. Di Chiara (Ed.), Handbook of experimental pharmacology: Vol. Kessler, R. M. (2008). Midbrain dopamine receptor availability is inversely
154/II. Dopamine in the CNS II (pp. 417– 472). Berlin, Germany: associated with novelty-seeking traits in humans. The Journal of Neuroscience,
Springer-Verlag. 28, 14372–14378. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2423-08.2008
Willuhn, I., Wanat, M. J., Clark, M. J., & Phillips, P. E. M. (2010). Zhou, W. (2010). Normalizing drug-induced neuronal plasticity in nucleus
Dopamine signaling in the nucleus accumbens of animals self- accumbens weakens enduring drug-seeking behavior. Neuropsychop-
administering drugs of abuse. In D. W. Self & J. K. Staley (Eds.), harmacology, 35, 352–353. doi:10.1038/npp.2009.97
Behavioral neuroscience of drug addiction: Current topics in behavioral Zhou, W., & Kalivas, P. W. (2008). N-acetylcysteine reduces extinction
neurosciences (pp. 29 –71). Berlin, Germany: Springer-Verlag. responding and induces enduring reductions in cue- and heroin-induced
Wise, R. A. (2004). Dopamine, learning, and motivation. Nature Reviews drug-seeking. Biological Psychiatry, 63, 338 –340. doi:10.1016/
Neuroscience, 5, 483– 494. doi:10.1038/nrn1406 j.biopsych.2007.06.008
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

Yerkes, R. M., & Dodson, J. D. (1908). The relation of strength of stimulus


This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

to rapidity of habit-formation. Journal of Comparative Neurology and Received June 3, 2011


Psychology, 18, 459 – 482. doi:10.1002/cne.920180503 Revision received October 24, 2011
Yurgelun-Todd, D. A., Killgore, W. D., & Cintron, C. B. (2003). Cognitive Accepted November 21, 2011 䡲

You might also like