You are on page 1of 2

Magazine

R551

Human sleep and effects were observed on wakefulness


(WASO) or time in slow wave (SWS,
REM 56.5 minutes later around new
moon than at full moon (interaction
cortical reactivity low frequency (0.5 Hz–2 Hz) high
amplitude (>75 μV) EEG activity), non-
of sensitivity and lunar class F(2,35) =
4.09, p = 0.014; Figure 1C). Since the
are influenced by REM or REM sleep. Additionally, no effect is attributable to sensitivity, it is

lunar phase effects were found on total number of


sleep stage changes, awakenings or
plausible that there were insufficient
sensitive individuals in the Cajochen
arousals. Cajochen et al. found effects et al. trials to influence their findings.
Michael Smith, Ilona Croy, on melatonin, electroencephalography The significance of REM latency per
and Kerstin Persson Waye (EEG)-delta activity and stage 4 sleep, se is difficult to interpret. The first
parameters not measured in the sleep cycle can be defined as the
Various human biological functions current work. interval between sleep onset latency
adhere to a circadian rhythm that Further analysis of total sleep time and cessation of the first REM period,
to some extent may be affected by revealed a significant (p = 0.046) and hence one interpretation is that
environmental factors, including reduction around full moon of 24.8 sleep cycle duration (and subsequent
light and temperature [1]. Recent minutes relative to quarter moon structure) is influenced by lunar
evidence from Cajochen et al. (Figure 1A). This corresponds very phase.
[2] indicates that human sleep is closely to the 24.2 minutes reported Despite the relatively small study
influenced by the cycle of the moon, previously [2]. The significant population and subsequently wide
measured in conditions precluding interaction between lunar class and confidence intervals, particularly
the potential impact of nocturnal gender (F(2,35) = 5.6, p = 0.008) around full moon, we found significant
lunar illumination Here in a similarly shows that this effect was driven by effects of lunar phase on total
retrospective study of 47 healthy males, who slept for 51.1 minutes sleep time and sleep onset latency,
volunteers (mean age 23.3, S.D. less than females around full moon, with males being more affected
±2.9 years) we demonstrate that corresponding to reduced sleep than females. This contrasts with
total sleep time decreases by 25 efficiency of 10.6% (Figure S1). previous work using sleep diaries,
minutes and cortical reactivity to Although no main effects of lunar which suggested that women were
environmental stimuli during sleep class were observed for sleep onset more affected according to moon
increases around full moon, and latency, further analysis revealed a phase, although the interaction in
rapid eye movement (REM) sleep significant interaction with gender this study was statistically weak
latency lengthens by 30 minutes (F(2,35) = 5.6, p = 0.008), showing [3]. These findings support those
around new moon. The findings that unlike females, males take reported previously by Cajochen
strengthen the notion that human between 35–40.5 minutes longer et al., although possible gender
sleep is modulated by lunar phase to fall asleep around full moon effects warrant further research. Also
but point to important deviations than during other phases (Figure in agreement, we found no effect
from the study of Cajochen 1B). Interactions were also found on WASO, SWS, non-REM or REM.
et al. that need to be addressed, between gender and lunar class for Findings that did not support the
particularly with regard to individual N1 (F(2,35) = 6.8, p = 0.003) and N2 earlier research were REM latency
susceptibility. sleep (F(2,35) = 6.6, p = 0.004). Males being shortest around full moon,
We retrospectively analyzed had increased time in N1 (colloquially potentially due to our inclusion of
sleep architecture data from young, known as ‘light sleep’ and typified particularly sensitive individuals, and
healthy volunteers in quiet, controlled by low amplitude, mixed frequency subjective sleep quality for which
conditions. The sleep laboratory activity in the EEG), corresponding we found no effect. However, only
was constructed and equipped to to a reduction in N2 (‘intermediate volunteers with good self-reported
simulate a residential apartment as sleep’, characterized by the presence sleep in general were recruited
much as possible, rather than the of K complexes and sleep spindles) and may therefore represent a
clinical settings typically used in closer to the full moon. particularly subjectively resilient
such research, in order to ensure REM latency was longest group. The presence of objective
high ecological validity and thereby around new moon (100.6 minutes), effects may be reconciled with
minimize any influence of the approximately 30 minutes longer than self-reported measures of sleep not
environment on measured outcomes. at full and quarter moons (p < 0.01). necessarily correlating accurately
Lunar phase was classified in the This is in contrast to the findings with physiological response [4].
same manner as by Cajochen of Cajochen et al., who found the However, despite the controlled
et al. into three lunar classes [2] longest REM latency at full moon lighting conditions during the sleep
(see Experimental Procedures in (89.3 minutes) with shorter latencies period time, it cannot be completely
Supplemental Information, published during quarter and new moons. In our excluded that external conditions
with this article online). There were studies, we specifically investigated prior to arriving at the laboratory
no indications that the number the effects of environmental sensitivity (e.g., increased nocturnal illumination
of daylight hours was unevenly on response and recruited subjects around full moon) did not contribute
distributed between lunar classes accordingly. Grouping our sample towards measured sleep outcomes.
(Table S1). Main effects of lunar by persons with and without high Our data furthermore allow us to
class were observed for REM latency sensitivity, we found the non-sensitive examine the effect of lunar cycle in
and total sleep time (Table S2). In group was unaffected by moon phase, nights with external stimuli. Four
accordance with the literature, no whereas sensitive persons entered nights following the quiet baseline
Current Biology Vol 24 No 12
R552

A B Male Female C Non sensitive Sensitive yet unidentified circalunar clock


140
460
450 480 120
exists in humans, such as has been

REM latency
460
demonstrated in marine worms in
TST (min)

TST (min)
440 100
440

(min)
430 80
420
420
400 60
which the transcript levels of the
410 380
40 gene period were modulated by the
circalunar clock [10]. Future work into

N1 (% of TST) Sleep latency (min)


0.6

Cortical reaction
60
REM latency

100
0.4 such mechanisms is required to gain a

prob.
(min)

80 40
20 deeper understanding of how human
60 0.2

40
0 sleep is modulated by the phases of
20 Full moon (±4) Quarter moon (±9) New moon (±14)
the moon.
Cortical reaction

0.4
Lunar phase (±days from full moon)
0.35 15
0.3
prob.

0.25 10
0.2 5 Supplemental Information
0.15
60
Supplemental Information includes two
0.4
tables and experimental procedures, and
N2 (% of TST)
SSC prob.

0.35 55
0.3 50 can be found with this article online at http://
0.25 45
0.2 dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2014.05.018.
0.15 40

Full moon (±4) Quarter moon (±9) New moon (±14) Full moon (±4) Quarter moon (±9) New moon (±14)
Lunar phase (±days from full moon) Lunar phase (±days from full moon)
Acknowledgements
Current Biology
The authors thank Mikael Ögren and Agneta
Agge for assistance in conducting the work,
Figure 1. Effects of lunar cycle on sleep. and the volunteers for their participation.
(A) Effects of lunar class on total sleep time (TST), and REM latency obtained from quiet control The original research project was supported
conditions (upper two panes). Effects of lunar class on sleep stage change (SSC) and cortical by the EU 7th Framework Programme (grant
reaction probabilities obtained from exposure conditions (lower two panes). Significant main
266248).
effects of lunar class were observed for all. (B) Effects of lunar class on females and males for
variables yielding a significant interaction between lunar class and gender. (C) Effects of lunar
class on non-sensitive and sensitive groups for variables yielding a significant interaction be- References
tween lunar class and sensitivity. Error bars indicate 95% confidence intervals. 1. Bartok, O., Kyriacou, C.P., Levine, J., Sehgal,
A., and Kadener, S. (2013). Adaptation
of molecular circadian clockwork to
involved exposing participants to and odorous/pungent chemicals, and environmental changes: a role for alternative
simulated freight rail vibration and is subsequently associated with an splicing and miRNAs. Proc. Biol. Sci. 280,
20130011.
noise [5]. Such stimuli are associated increase in overall general sensitivity. 2. Cajochen, C., Altanay-Ekici, S., Munch, M.,
with inducing sleep stage changes Such sensitivity manifests itself Frey, S., Knoblauch, V., and Wirz-Justice, A.
and cortical reactions (EEG arousals in part as heightened attendance (2013). Evidence that the lunar cycle influences
human sleep. Curr. Biol. 23, 1485–1488.
and awakenings), so we examined to threat [7]. Thus, the increased 3. Röösli, M., Jüni, P., Braun-Fahrländer, C.,
whether such event-related reactivity reaction likelihood of sensitive, i.e. Brinkhof, M.W.G., Low, N., and Egger, M.
(2006). Sleepless night, the moon is bright:
is affected by lunar phase. Main threat-attendant, individuals around longitudinal study of lunar phase and sleep. J.
effects of lunar class were found for full moon supports the notion of the Sleep Res. 15, 149–153.
alterations of sleep depth (F(2,35) = advantage of reaction to predation. 4. Baker, F.C., Maloney, S., and Driver, H.S.
(1999). A comparison of subjective estimates
3.3, p = 0.05) and cortical reactions REM latency of sensitive persons of sleep with objective polysomnographic data
(F(2,35) = 4.7, p = 0.016), reflecting did not differ from the non-sensitive in healthy men and women. J. Psychosom.
Res. 47, 335–341.
increased reactivity around full group around full or quarter moon, 5. Smith, M.G., Croy, I., Ogren, M., and Persson
moon (Figure 1A). However, further but was 31.7 minutes longer around Waye, K. (2013). On the influence of freight
analysis of the interaction between new moon, suggesting lower REM trains on humans: a laboratory investigation
of the impact of nocturnal low frequency
sensitivity and lunar phase (F(2,35) = sleep pressure. Additionally, in our vibration and noise on sleep and heart rate.
3.5, p = 0.041) for cortical reactions work sensitive individuals had on PLoS One 8, e55829.
6. Rockhill, A.P., DePerno, C.S., and Powell, R.A.
reveals that only the sensitive group average 11.2 minutes less SWS than (2013). The effect of illumination and time of
was more reactive to environmental non-sensitive persons. REM sleep day on movements of bobcats (Lynx rufus).
stimuli around full moon (Figure pressure and time in SWS has been PLoS One 8, e69213.
7. Stansfeld, S.A. (1992). Noise, noise sensitivity
1C). Animals with limited nocturnal found to differ between groups with and psychological studies. Psychol. Med.
vision are more readily able to alter polymorphisms in period3 (PER3), a Suppl. 22, 1–44.
8. Dijk, D.J., and Archer, S.N. (2010). PERIOD3,
hunting behaviors to take advantage gene associated with sleep regulation circadian phenotypes, and sleep homeostasis.
of increased light around full moon, [8]. Although purely speculative, Sleep Med. Rev. 14, 151–160.
and accordingly predators have been and taken with earlier suggestions 9. Heinonen-Guzejev, M., Vuorinen, H.S.,
Mussalo-Rauhamaa, H., Heikkila, K.,
found to have higher movement that noise sensitivity has a genetic Koskenvuo, M., and Kaprio, J. (2005). Genetic
rates around nighttime periods with basis [9], the data hint at possible component of noise sensitivity. Twin Res. Hum.
Genet. 8, 245–249.
higher illumination [6]. An increased underlying genetic mechanisms in the 10. Zantke, J., Ishikawa-Fujiwara, T., Arboleda,
physiological responsiveness during regulation of sleep between different E., Lohs, C., Schipany, K., Hallay, N., Straw,
sleep around such times would allow population groups. A.D., Todo, T., and Tessmar-Raible, K. (2013).
Circadian and circalunar clock interactions in a
greater opportunity to react to such The existence of endogenous marine annelid. Cell Rep. 5, 99–113.
predation. Sensitive individuals were circadian clocks in humans is well
classified according to their noise documented. In both the present and
Occupational and Environmental Medicine,
sensitivity, a trait which overlaps previous [2] studies, lunar phase was The Sahlgrenska Academy at the University
with intolerance towards other found to impact sleep architecture. of Gothenburg, Gothenburg 405 30, Sweden.
environmental factors, including light It is therefore plausible that an as E-mail: michael.smith@amm.gu.se

You might also like