You are on page 1of 10

Journal of Psychiatric Research 153 (2022) 149–158

Contents lists available at ScienceDirect

Journal of Psychiatric Research


journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/jpsychires

Bipolar spectrum traits and the space between Madness and Genius: The
Muse is in the Dose
Tiffany A. Greenwood a, *, Lauren J. Chow a, Ruben C. Gur b, John R. Kelsoe a, c
a
Department of Psychiatry, University of California San Diego, La Jolla, CA, USA
b
Department of Psychiatry, University of Pennsylvania, Philadelphia, PA, USA
c
Institute for Genomic Medicine, UC San Diego, La Jolla, CA, USA

A R T I C L E I N F O A B S T R A C T

Keywords: Creativity has long been associated with the bipolar spectrum, particularly among unaffected first-degree rela­
Bipolar disorder tives and those with milder expressions of bipolar traits, suggesting that some aspects of the bipolar spectrum
Creativity may confer advantages for creativity. Here we took a multifaceted approach to better define the shared
Mood
vulnerability between creativity and bipolar disorder. We recruited 135 individuals with bipolar disorder, 102
Temperament
Personality
creative controls, and 103 non-creative controls for a total of 340 participants. All participants completed a
Cognition comprehensive assessment battery that included several self-report temperament and personality questionnaires,
a computerized test of cognitive function across multiple domains, and an evaluation of creative performance
and achievement. Significant group differences were observed for the hypothesized shared vulnerability traits of
hypomanic personality, cyclothymic temperament, impulsivity, and positive schizotypy. While both the creative
and bipolar groups demonstrated superior creative ability, the creative group alone revealed enhanced cognitive
performance. Accounting for intercorrelations between traits, a combination of openness, hypomanic person­
ality, divergent thinking, and reasoning ability emerged as the strongest predictors of creativity, collectively
explaining 34% of the variance in creative achievement and correctly classifying 85% of individuals with high
achievement irrespective of diagnosis. These results confirm and extend earlier observations of a shared
vulnerability between creativity and bipolar disorder and suggest that mild to moderate expressions of bipolar
spectrum traits are associated with enhanced cognitive functioning and creative expression. Further investigation
of these traits is needed to clarify the nature of this shared vulnerability and suggest individualized treatment
strategies to improve clinical outcomes in bipolar disorder.

1. Introduction symptoms. However, this practice has failed to capture the extensive
phenotypic heterogeneity of bipolar disorder and has yielded a limited
Bipolar disorder is a lifelong mood disorder characterized by extreme understanding of the underlying pathology. Further insight and progress
mood swings between the highs of mania and the lows of depression toward a precision medicine approach may be gained by viewing the
(Goodwin and Jamison, 2007). Mania is accompanied by pathological illness as a dimensional construct existing at the extreme of normal
elevations in energy and mood, grandiosity, racing thoughts, and risk population variation in mood, personality, and cognition (Burmeister
taking; whereas depression is associated with low energy and motiva­ et al., 2008; Akiskal, 2002).
tion, as well as feelings of sadness and hopelessness. Up to 50% of pa­ It has long been observed that certain positive traits or enhanced
tients also experience symptoms of psychosis in the form of abilities exist within the bipolar spectrum, including creativity.
hallucinations and delusions (Goodwin and Jamison, 2007). Bipolar Numerous biographical studies of the eminently creative combined with
disorder is common, affecting more than 5.7 million adults in the United large population-based studies of everyday creativity provide strong
States each year with a lifetime prevalence of 3.9% (Kessler et al., 2005). support for a connection between creativity and bipolar disorder
In an effort to inform treatment, current diagnostic systems group pa­ (Jamison, 1993; Tremblay et al., 2010; Kyaga et al., 2011). While these
tients into discrete categories based on the pattern and severity of mood studies have found a ten-fold increase in the rate of bipolar disorder

* Corresponding author. Department of Psychiatry, University of California San Diego, 9500 Gilman Drive, MC 0689, La Jolla, CA, 92093, USA.
E-mail address: tgreenwood@health.ucsd.edu (T.A. Greenwood).

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jpsychires.2022.07.009
Received 4 October 2021; Received in revised form 18 June 2022; Accepted 1 July 2022
Available online 5 July 2022
0022-3956/© 2022 The Authors. Published by Elsevier Ltd. This is an open access article under the CC BY license (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
T.A. Greenwood et al. Journal of Psychiatric Research 153 (2022) 149–158

among creative individuals, they have also shown that creativity and current occupation and creative involvement.
professional success are particularly enhanced among unaffected The final sample of 316 participants included 111 individuals with
first-degree relatives and individuals expressing bipolar spectrum traits bipolar disorder (BP), of whom 57% were bipolar I and 43% were bi­
(Verdoux and Bourgeois, 1995; Richards et al., 1988; Kyaga et al., 2013; polar II. All BP participants were judged to be euthymic at the time of
Coryell et al., 1989; Simeonova et al., 2005; Akiskal and Akiskal, 2007). assessment with only mild symptoms or impairment present over the
These findings suggest that some bipolar spectrum traits may confer past month, as assessed by the clinician-administered Global Assessment
advantages for creativity. of Functioning, (GAF, range 60–100, mean = 75). Of the 205 compari­
A shared vulnerability model and inverted-U relationship has been son subjects, 102 reported involvement in a creative profession, whether
suggested, according to which creativity would increase with genetic full time or freelance (e.g., visual artist, musician, creative writer, etc.)
risk for bipolar disorder up to a threshold and then diminish with the and were considered creative controls (creCTL), while the remaining
increasing impairment that accompanies a more severe expression of 103 reported no significant involvement in creative pursuits and were
symptoms (Richards et al., 1988; Greenwood, 2017, 2020; Nettle, 2001; considered non-creative controls (nCTL). Approximately 38% of BP
Carson, 2011). Creativity is typically defined as behaviors or thoughts participants also reported engagement in a creative profession.
that are both novel and useful and represents a complex, multidimen­ The age, gender, and education distributions were similar across the
sional construct with both cognitive and affective components (Eysenck, three groups, as shown in Table 1. The racial and ethnic composition of
1995; Russ, 1993; Srivastava et al., 2010; Feist, 1998). Traits related to the sample reflects local demographics with 64% white, 17% Hispanic,
mood instability (hypomanic traits, cyclothymic temperament), “expe­ 12% Asian, and 6% Black by self-report. Written informed consent was
riencing” personality types (openness to experience, positive schizotypy, obtained for all subjects according to the local institutional review
impulsivity), and conceptual over-inclusiveness are associated with board.
creativity, bipolar disorder, and creativity in bipolar disorder and may
represent aspects of the shared vulnerability. While mild to moderate 2.2. Temperament and personality assessment
expression of these shared traits may enhance creativity, more extreme
expressions result in symptoms associated with illness, such as mood We evaluated multiple aspects of temperament and personality
cycling, psychosis, risk taking, racing thoughts, and cognitive disorga­ through self-report questionnaires, all of which have validated in both
nization. Creative individuals may also possess cognitive protective healthy and clinical samples. The rationale for these particular ques­
traits in the form of high IQ or cognitive flexibility, which help them to tionnaires is discussed in detail elsewhere and summarized here as a
more effectively integrate original ideas into artistic expression and Supplement (Greenwood, 2017, 2020). The Hypomanic Personality Scale
novel inventions (Carson, 2011). However, it has been suggested that evaluates aspects of hypomanic traits related to high mood, activation,
the eventual success of individuals possessing these cognitive abilities grandiosity, and impulsivity and has been shown to predict risk for bi­
relies heavily on personality factors, like openness (Jauk et al., 2013). polar spectrum disorders (Eckblad and Chapman, 1986; Kwapil et al.,
While previous studies have provided an abundance of data to sup­ 2000). The Barratt Impulsiveness Scale evaluates impulsivity in terms of
port the connection between bipolar disorder and creativity, most cognitive instability, attention, self-control, cognitive complexity,
focused on a limited number of phenotypes in a small sample, creativity motor, and perseverance (Patton et al., 1995). The Temperament Evalu­
in unaffected individuals, or the correlation between diagnosis and ation of Memphis, Pisa, Paris, and San Diego-Autoquestionnaire quantifies
creative occupation in a population-based setting. Here we build on lifelong, milder aspects of bipolar symptomatology according to 5
these earlier investigations with an assessment of multiple dimensions of temperament domains: hyperthymic, dysthymic, cyclothymic, irritable,
affective temperament, personality, and cognition in a cohort comprised and anxious (Akiskal et al., 2005; Kawamura et al., 2010). The Schizo­
of individuals with bipolar disorder and both creative and non-creative typal Personality Questionnaire captures aspects of positive schizotypal
comparison subjects. We expect the shared traits relating to mood and traits relating to cognitive disorganization (eccentric behavior and odd
personality to follow an intermediate phenotype model, with moderate speech) and cognitive-perceptual experiences (ideas of reference,
expressions among creative individuals and more extreme expressions in magical thinking, unusual experiences), as well as negative schizotypal
bipolar disorder. Conversely, shared cognitive and creative traits are traits (constricted affect, social anxiety, no close friends, and suspi­
expected to follow an inverted-U pattern, with enhanced performance ciousness) (Raine, 1991; Carson et al., 2003; Higier et al., 2014). The
on such tasks observed for creative individuals. From these observations, NEO Personality Inventory is the standard questionnaire for the Five
we hope to discover the traits that are most predictive of high creative Factor Model, encompassing the personality domains of neuroticism,
achievement, as well as gain additional insight into the nature of the extraversion, openness, agreeableness, and conscientiousness, as well as
shared vulnerability. six facets that define each domain (Costa and McCrae, 1992; Bagby
et al., 1999).
2. Methods

2.3. Cognitive assessment


2.1. Subject ascertainment

A customized version of the Penn Computerized Neuropsychological


Recruitment for this study was achieved through advertisements,
Battery (CNP) was used to assess specific cognitive abilities that may
physician referral, and the re-contact of previous research study par­
relate to creativity, some of which are novel in this regard (Brodzinsky
ticipants. To avoid selection bias, recruitment materials targeting bi­
and Rubien, 1976; Gur et al., 2010; Thorson and Powell, 1993; Furnham
polar and non-creative comparison participants made no mention of
et al., 2008; Kuncel et al., 2004). The Penn Humor Appreciation Test
creativity, whereas recruitment materials targeting creative individuals
made no mention of mental health. Structured clinical interviews were
conducted according to DSM-5 criteria for all participants to confirm Table 1
diagnoses of bipolar disorder and to ensure that the comparison par­ Sample description.
ticipants were free of a personal or family history of bipolar disorder, nCTL creCTL BP
psychosis, or recurrent major depression (Nurnberger et al., 1994). For N 103 102 111
those BP participants meeting criteria for a current manic, depressed or % Female 65.0% 62.7% 62.2%
mixed episode during the clinical interview, study participation was Age (years) 41.5 ± 13.2 40.7 ± 14.1 38.1 ± 12.8
Education (years) 15.4 ± 2.1 15.6 ± 1.9 15.0 ± 2.3
suspended and resumed, if possible, once active symptoms had abated.
As part of the initial interview, participants were also queried as to their All group comparison p-values are non-significant (>0.05).

150
T.A. Greenwood et al. Journal of Psychiatric Research 153 (2022) 149–158

presents pairs of verbal (newspaper headline gaffes) and figural (Mor­ from no talent whatsoever to public acknowledgment of one’s talent)
dillo cartoons) stimuli, with the original stimulus displayed next to one across 10 domains (visual arts, creative writing, performing arts, humor,
with the joke removed (e.g., “Red Tape Holds up New Bridge” versus inventions, etc.) and displays good test-retest reliability, internal con­
“Red Tape Delays New Bridge”), and asks the participant to determine sistency, and convergent validity with other measures of creativity
which one is funny (Bozikas et al., 2007; Kohn et al., 2011). Verbal and (Carson et al., 2005).
figural scores were averaged for a total humor appreciation score. The
Penn Verbal Reasoning Test consists of verbal analogy problems from the 2.5. Statistical analyses
Educational Testing Service factor-referenced test kit (Gur et al., 1982).
The Penn Matrix Reasoning Test evaluates nonverbal reasoning in a Univariate linear regression was used to adjust for the effects of age
manner analogous to Raven’s Standard Progressive Matrices, for which and gender, and the resultant standardized residuals were used to
the participant must conceptualize spatial, design, and numerical re­ calculate z-scores for each participant relative to the mean and standard
lations to choose the best option to complete increasingly complex ar­ deviation of the nCTL group. The relationships between individual traits
rays (Bilker et al., 2012). Verbal and nonverbal scores were averaged to were evaluated through correlational analyses using Pearson’s r. Anal­
provide a reasoning ability summary score as a measure of fluid intel­ ysis of variance (ANOVA) was used to assess global group differences
ligence, which is a component of general intellectual ability correlated with Tukey post hoc testing for specific group comparisons.
with other measures of IQ (Gur et al., 2012). The Penn Line Orientation Stepwise regression analyses were conducted to identify the traits
Test is a test of spatial processing ability in which participants are shown with the largest independent effects on high creative achievement in
a pair of lines with different orientations and asked to rotate one line so unaffected individuals. All traits that significantly distinguished the
that it is parallel to the other (Moore et al., 2015). The Penn Conditional creCTL group were included as potential predictor variables, including
Exclusion Test is a measure of mental flexibility that uses a set-shifting aspects of mood (hypomanic personality and the cyclothymic, hyper­
task analogous to the Wisconsin Card Sort Test (Kurtz et al., 2004). thymic, irritable, and anxious temperaments), personality and behavior
Tasks assessing general cognitive function in terms of working (openness, positive schizotypy, and impulsivity), and cognitive and
memory, attention, and memory were also included to provide a point of creative performance (humor appreciation, reasoning, spatial process­
comparison. The Letter N-Back is a working memory task presented in ing, memory, divergent thinking). Significant predictors with the
blocks of three conditions: 0-Back, 1-Back, and 2-Back (Ragland et al., strongest effects were selected through an iterative process to generate a
2002). The Penn Continuous Performance Test provides a measure visual final model including only predictors with unique contributions to cre­
attention and vigilance through the presentation of vertical and hori­ ative achievement. The standardized residual from the regression model
zonal lines that requires the participant to respond when a complete was then calculated across all individuals, and the area under the
number (or letter) is formed (Kurtz et al., 2001). Finally, three declar­ receiver operating characteristic (ROC) curve (AUC) was evaluated to
ative memory tasks, reflecting the main visual modalities, were assess its predictive utility. For these analyses, a standardized creative
administered with the scores averaged to produce a summary score (Gur achievement score of 3.0, reflecting outlying values ≥ 3 standard de­
et al., 1997; Glahn et al., 1997). The Penn Face Memory Test presents 20 viations from the nCTL mean, was used as a threshold for high creativity.
target faces that must be memorized and then identified amongst 20 While all bipolar individuals reporting creative involvement met this
distractors. The Penn Word Memory Test employs a similar paradigm threshold, only 65% of self-reported creative individuals qualified for
using words as a test of verbal memory, and the Visual Object Learning inclusion in the revised creative group (creCTL_rev). This threshold was
Test uses three-dimensional Euclidean shapes as a test of spatial also used to consider creative bipolar (creBP) and non-creative (nBP)
memory. bipolar subgroups in confirmatory analyses of the shared vulnerability
traits.
2.4. Creativity assessment
3. Results
We took a multifaceted approach to assessing creativity, as discussed
elsewhere and included here as a Supplement (Greenwood, 2017, 2020). As shown in Table 2, all hypothesized shared vulnerability traits
The Creative Personality Scale is derived from the Adjective Check List significantly discriminated among the groups and followed the expected
and consists of 30 self-descriptive items that predict high creativity pattern, with medium to large effects observed for the creCTL group
across diverse domains (Gough, 1979; Gough and Heilbrun, 1965). The compared to the nCTL group. The BP group exhibited very large effect
Barron-Welsh Art Scale evaluates figure preference through a series of sizes for all shared vulnerability traits except openness, which was in­
black and white figures that subjects rate as “like” or “dislike”, with termediate between the creCTL and nCTL groups. A graphical repre­
higher scores reflecting a preference for asymmetry and complexity sentation of these patterns is provided in Fig. 1. The anxious and
(Barron, 1963). The Torrance Test of Creative Thinking provides a classic irritable temperaments followed a similar pattern of medium and very
laboratory measure of divergent thinking ability (Torrance, 1974). large effect sizes in the creCTL and BP groups, respectively, whereas
Verbal divergent thinking was assessed using word-based exercises that hyperthymic temperament was moderately elevated in the creCTL group
required listing unusual uses for a common item, suggesting ideas for only. Other temperament and personality traits appeared specific to the
product improvement, and guessing consequences of a hypothetical BP group, with dysthymic temperament, negative schizotypy, and
situation. These exercises were scored for fluency (quantity of ideas), neuroticism being significantly higher and extraversion, agreeableness,
flexibility (categories of ideas, shift in focus), and originality (statistical and conscientiousness significantly lower. Creative personality, which
infrequency and unusualness of ideas). Figural divergent thinking was reflects an individual’s self-assessment of defining characteristics, was
assessed through drawing exercises involving picture construction and significantly higher in the creCTL group only, with the most commonly
completion from marked cues that were similarly scored for fluency and endorsed items suggesting that creative individuals view themselves as
originality, as well as elaboration (level of imagination and richness of unconventional, individualistic, original, inventive, and reflective (p <
detail), abstractness of titles (ability to capture the essence of the image 0.001 vs. nCTL).
and provide depth), and resistance to premature closure (the ability to A closer look at the trait facets provides additional insight into the
keep open long enough to make the mental leap that facilitates origi­ creative temperament and the shared vulnerability (Fig. 2). For
nality). A total divergent thinking score and summaries of fluency and example, the BP group was high across all aspects of impulsivity,
originality were computed across the verbal and figural domains. whereas the creCTL group was primarily characterized by cognitive
Finally, the Creative Achievement Questionnaire was administered, as it instability (racing thoughts, changing interests) and motor impulsivity
provides a real-life measure of involvement and achievement (ranging (spontaneous behavior), with more modest increases seen for attention

151
T.A. Greenwood et al. Journal of Psychiatric Research 153 (2022) 149–158

Table 2
Group differences in temperament, personality, creativity, and cognitive
function.
creCTL BP ANOVA

Mean Mean F P
± SD ± SD Value

Shared Hypomanic 1.46 ± 2.72 ± 104.56 <0.001


Vulnerability Personality ‡ 1.39** 1.67**
Traits Cyclothymic 1.21 ± 4.47 ± 180.64 <0.001
Temperament ‡ 1.76** 2.30**
Positive Schizotypy 1.37 ± 2.76 ± 64.04 <0.001
‡ 1.69** 2.36**
Openness † 0.99 ± 0.63 ± 24.41 <0.001
0.82** 1.21**
Impulsivity ‡ 0.69 ± 2.03 ± 67.96 <0.001
1.28** 1.56**
Other Irritable 0.52 ± 2.32 ± 79.94 <0.001
Temperament Temperament ‡ 1.13* 1.93**
& Personality Anxious 0.59 ± 2.23 ± 68.39 <0.001
Traits Temperament ‡ 1.35* 1.84**
Hyperthymic 0.44 ± 0.10 ± 6.09 0.002
Temperament † 0.75* 1.04 Fig. 1. Temperament and personality traits that distinguish both the creative
Dysthymic 0.29 ± 1.80 ± 54.20 <0.001 and bipolar groups and may represent a shared vulnerability. Mean standard
Temperament ‡ 1.24 1.75** scores for the creCTL and BP groups represent effect sizes with dashed lines
Negative 0.39 ± 1.71 ± 43.67 <0.001 indicating thresholds for small (±0.2), medium (±0.5) and large (±0.8) effects.
Schizotypy ‡ 1.15 1.87** The value for cyclothymic temperament in BP is truncated for ease of
Neuroticism ‡ 0.34 ± 1.84 ± 81.25 <0.001 presentation.
1.13 1.22**
Extraversion † 0.07 ± − 0.41 5.67 0.004
1.03 ± (restlessness) and the impulsiveness facet of neuroticism (impulsive
1.31* behavior, overindulgence). While the BP group was extreme across all
Agreeableness − 0.05 − 0.33 3.48 0.032 facets of schizotypy, the creCTL group revealed significant elevations
± 0.94 ±
1.00*
across all facets of positive schizotypy with only a small effect for the
Conscientiousness − 0.10 − 1.29 47.80 <0.001 negative schizotypy facet of constricted affect (aloof) and a trend toward
‡ ± 1.06 ± significance for social anxiety (p = 0.057), consistent with the low score
1.16** observed for the gregariousness facet of extraversion (preference for
Cognition Humor 0.40 ± − 0.06 5.80 0.003
solitude). The higher score for the creCTL group with regard to the
Appreciation ‡ 0.95* ± 1.20
Reasoning ‡ 0.45 ± − 0.07 8.83 <0.001 positive emotions facet of extraversion reflects an upbeat mood,
0.78* ± 1.06 consistent with the results for hyperthymic temperament. The BP group
Spatial Processing ‡ 0.43 ± − 0.05 9.16 <0.001 was significantly higher on all facets of neuroticism and lower on all
0.72* ± 0.93 facets of conscientiousness, consistent with the results for temperament
Mental Flexibility 0.16 ± − 0.13 2.65 0.072
0.86 ± 0.91
and impulsivity. The agreeableness domain was mixed, with the BP
Working Memory ‡ 0.22 ± − 0.41 9.46 <0.001 group scoring lower on facets of trust (suspicious) and compliance
0.70 ± (uncooperative) and the creCTL and BP groups scoring similarly low on
1.37* straightforwardness (manipulative) and high on tendermindedness
Attention † − 0.10 − 0.48 5.13 0.006
(sympathetic). Finally, as expected, the creCTL and BP groups scored
± 0.91 ±
1.42* similarly high across the openness facets of fantasy (imagination), aes­
Memory ‡ 0.40 ± − 0.21 11.40 0.000 thetics (appreciation of art and music), feelings (wide range of emotion,
0.79* ± 1.02 empathetic), and values (open-minded, tolerant). The creCTL group also
Creativity Creative 0.79 ± 0.21 ± 14.15 <0.001 scored significantly higher for the ideas facet (intellectual curiosity),
Personality ‡ 0.97** 1.28
Figure Preference 0.00 ± 0.25 ± 2.5 0.089
whereas the BP group was significantly lower for the action facet
0.95 1.04 (preference for routine).
Divergent Thinking 0.70 ± 0.57 ± 10.74 <0.001 As shown in Fig. 3, the creCTL group generally demonstrated
1.08** 1.33** enhanced cognitive and creative performance. In terms of cognitive
Originality 0.65 ± 0.72 ± 12.41 <0.001
ability, the creCTL group performed significantly better on tasks related
1.12** 1.28**
Fluency 0.45 ± 0.41 ± 4.94 0.008 to humor appreciation, reasoning ability (fluid intelligence, problem
1.08* 1.32* solving), spatial processing, and declarative memory compared with the
Achievement ‡ 5.24 ± 2.88 ± 82.71 <0.001 nCTL and BP groups, which did not differ in performance (Fig. 3a).
3.29** 3.63** Deficits in working memory and attention were particular to the BP
Means and standard deviations listed for the creCTL and BP groups are z-scores group, with no differences observed between the creCTL and nCTL
relative to the nCTL group and thus may be considered effect sizes. The nCTL groups. In terms of divergent thinking ability, the creCTL and BP groups
group by default has a mean of 0 and standard deviation of 1 for all traits and is both outperformed the nCTL group (Fig. 3a), demonstrating a greater
not listed. number of independent ideas (fluency) and more originality. The
Significance with respect to nCTL is indicated as * (p < 0.05) or ** (p < 0.001). divergent thinking summaries were primarily driven by the results of the
Group differences between creCTL and BP are indicated as † (p < 0.05) and ‡ (p
verbal tasks, on which both the creCTL and BP groups significantly
< 0.001).
outperformed the nCTL group across all domains (Fig. 3b). While per­
formance on the figural tasks was somewhat less impressive, the creCTL
and BP groups both demonstrated greater originality and abstractness of
titles, and the creCTL group revealed superior elaboration.

152
T.A. Greenwood et al. Journal of Psychiatric Research 153 (2022) 149–158

Fig. 2. Association of the personality facets across groups for impulsivity and schizotypy (a) and the NEO (b). Mean standard scores for the creCTL (blue) and BP
(red) groups represent effect sizes with dashed lines indicating thresholds for small (±0.2), medium (±0.5) and large (±0.8) effects. Significance with respect to nCTL
is indicated as * (p < 0.05) or ** (p < 0.001). Group differences between creCTL and BP are indicated as † (p < 0.05) and ‡ (p < 0.001).

We investigated the correlational patterns among all traits that affected individuals, while hypomanic personality and hyperthymic
distinguished creative from non-creative individuals (Table 3). All hy­ temperament were only associated with better creative performance in
pothesized shared vulnerability traits were significantly correlated the BP group. Despite following a similar pattern to the shared vulner­
among unaffected individuals and predicted higher creative achieve­ ability traits, the irritable and anxious temperaments were not associ­
ment. A similar correlational pattern was observed for the BP group, ated with creative achievement.
although, notably, there was no appreciable correlation between open­ Given the intercorrelations among traits distinguishing creative from
ness and cyclothymic temperament, and only openness was associated non-creative individuals (see Table 3), a stepwise regression model was
with higher achievement. Creative personality was strongly correlated applied to identify those traits exhibiting the strongest independent ef­
with hypomanic personality, openness, and hyperthymic temperament fects on high creative achievement among unaffected individuals.
among unaffected and affected individuals alike. Openness also pre­ Openness (b = 0.93, p < 0.001), hypomanic personality (b = 0.79, p <
dicted better cognitive and creative performance in both unaffected and 0.001), divergent thinking (b = 0.50, p = 0.013), and reasoning ability

153
T.A. Greenwood et al. Journal of Psychiatric Research 153 (2022) 149–158

Fig. 3. Enhanced cognitive and creative performance


among creCTL Mean standard scores for the creCTL
and BP groups represent effect sizes with dashed lines
indicating thresholds for small (±0.2), medium
(±0.5) and large (±0.8) effects. (a) Mean group per­
formance across individual cognitive tasks and sum­
maries of overall divergent thinking. (b) Details of the
verbal and figural divergent thinking performance
with creCTL in blue and BP in red. Significance with
respect to nCTL is indicated as * (p < 0.05) or ** (p <
0.001). Group differences between creCTL and BP are
indicated as † (p < 0.05) and ‡ (p < 0.001).

(b = 0.49, p = 0.043) emerged as the most significant, independent characteristics of the shared vulnerability that emerged through the
predictors, collectively explaining 35% of the variance in creative analyses are summarized in Fig. 4. These traits range from those asso­
achievement. The standardized residual from the regression model ciated with mood to aspects of personality and behavior, which may
correctly classified 84.8% (95% CI [78.9%, 90.8%], p < 0.001) of un­ provide richer experiences, greater emotional range, and perceptual
affected individuals with high creative achievement. Notably, this differences that may combine with enhanced cognitive abilities to foster
model also held for the BP group, correctly classifying 85.4% (95% CI creativity. Consistent with the proposed inverted-U relationship, only
[78.4%, 92.4%], p < 0.001) of individuals with high creative the more moderate expressions of hypomanic, cyclothymic, and
achievement. cognitive-perceptual schizotypal traits observed for creative individuals
A re-evaluation of the shared vulnerability traits with regard to high were significantly correlated with creative achievement, whereas more
creative achievement revealed no significant differences for the revised extreme expressions are maladaptive and associated with illness.
creCTL group (creCTL_rev) compared with the original (creCTL), sug­ Conversely, openness predicted higher achievement regardless of diag­
gesting that the patterns previously observed provide an accurate nosis, and the creative cohort demonstrated cognitive enhancements
reflection of creative traits and abilities regardless of the level of that may additionally foster creativity. Accounting for intercorrelations,
achievement to date. While the creative BP (creBP) and non-creative BP a combination of openness, hypomanic traits, divergent thinking, and
(nBP) subgroups were indistinguishable across most measures, key dif­ reasoning ability explained 35% of the variance in creative achievement
ferences emerged (Table 4). The creBP subgroup scored significantly and correctly classified 85% of individuals with high achievement
higher for hyperthymic temperament, openness, and creative person­ irrespective of diagnosis.
ality and demonstrated superior performance on the divergent thinking It is readily conceivable how the mild to moderate expressions of
tasks, comparable to the creCTL group. Still, even the nBP subgroup hypomania that define the hypomanic personality (high mood and en­
revealed higher openness scores, better creative performance and higher ergy, enhanced flow of ideas, self-confidence, social ease, etc.) could
creative achievement than the nCTL group, clearly demonstrating that provide an advantage for creativity and professional success, the “hy­
the initial results for the BP group were not entirely driven by creative pomanic edge” (Gartner, 2005). Indeed, hypomanic personality has
individuals who also happen to have bipolar disorder. previously shown associations with creative performance and achieve­
ment (Furnham et al., 2008; Schuldberg, 2001), and we have replicated
4. Discussion these findings here. Impulsivity is another key feature of bipolar disor­
der that is elevated in unaffected family members and correlated with
These results confirm and extend earlier observations of a shared both hypomanic traits and creativity (Schuldberg, 2001; Lombardo
vulnerability between bipolar disorder and creativity in the most et al., 2012; Peluso et al., 2007). While all facets of impulsivity were
comprehensive evaluation of such traits to date. The defining significantly elevated among the BP group, more moderate increases

154
T.A. Greenwood et al. Journal of Psychiatric Research 153 (2022) 149–158

Table 4

Only traits characteristic of creative individuals are shown for comparison among all unaffected individuals (CTL, i.e., nCTL and creCTL combined) and the bipolar group (BP). Correlations of at least small effect (r = 0.20)
0.61**

0.28**
Re-assessment of shared vulnerability traits and creativity in groups defined by

− 0.01
− 0.12
− 0.01
0.03

0.18
high creative achievement.

BP
Hyperthymic creCTL_rev creBP nBP ANOVA

Mean ± SD Mean ± Mean ± F P Value

− 0.12

− 0.04
0.42*

0.20*
0.07

0.00

0.11
CTL
SD SD

Hyperthymic 0.52 ± 0.38 ± − 0.02 ± 5.46 0.001


Correlational patterns between temperament and personality traits and their relationships to measures of creativity and cognition among unaffected individuals and those with bipolar disorder.

Temperament 0.71* 0.91* 1.05

− 0.05

− 0.06

0.17*
Openness 1.17 ± 1.17 ± 0.37 ± 23.99

0.01

0.01

0.03
0.01

0.13
<0.001
BP

0.76** 0.92** 1.25*


Creative 0.99 ± 0.76 ± − 0.05 ± 16.35 <0.001
Personality 0.98** 1.00** 1.33
Anxious

− 0.06
− 0.02 Divergent 0.76 ± 0.96 ± 0.34 ± 9.56

0.17*

0.15*
0.19*
<0.001
0.03

0.13
0.05
CTL

Thinking 1.02** 1.43** 1.22*


Fluency 0.41 ± 0.70 ± 0.23 ± 4.27 0.003
1.01* 1.44** 1.23
Originality 0.67 ± 1.07 ± 0.50 ± 10.02
0.68**

<0.001
− 0.06
0.22*

0.17* 1.11** 1.28** 1.24*


0.08
0.01

0.03
0.01

0.13
BP

Creative 5.90 ± 6.05 ± 0.63 ± 241.59 <0.001


Achievement 2.46** 2.53** 1.15*
Irritable

Means and standard deviations listed are z-scores relative to the nCTL group and
0.55**

0.17*

0.15*
0.16*
0.14

0.09
0.08
0.06
0.01

thus may be considered effect sizes. Key: creCTL_rev = individuals from the
CTL

creCTL group with achievement scores 3 standard deviations above the nCTL
mean; creBP = individuals from the BP group with achievement scores 3 stan­
dard deviations above the nCTL control mean; nBP = non-creative individuals
¡0.34**

¡0.21*
0.36**

− 0.10

− 0.14

− 0.01

from the BP group. Significance is indicated as * (p < 0.05) or ** (p < 0.001).


0.28*
0.15
0.01

0.01
BP

specific to racing thoughts and impulsive behavior were observed for the
Impulsivity

creCTL group, which may provide insight into the role of impulsivity in
0.33**
0.35**

0.20**

0.21*

creative generativity (Murray and Johnson, 2010).


0.03
0.08
0.13
0.10

0.13
0.03
CTL

Openness to experience is characteristic of those who are imagina­


tive, open-minded, intellectually curious, and attentive to their emo­
tions (Costa and McCrae, 1992). High scores reveal a fluid style of
0.33**
0.55**
0.38**
0.35**
0.41**
0.37**

0.36**
− 0.04

0.25*
0.07
0.03

consciousness that fosters novel associations among remotely connected


BP

ideas. Unsurprisingly, openness has shown strong correlates with


Openness

divergent thinking and is viewed as a central feature of creativity (Feist,


0.31**
0.26**

0.51**

0.32**

0.30**
0.26**
0.42**
0.20*

0.20*

1998; Dollinger et al., 2004; Ma, 2009; Parson et al., 1984; McCrae,
0.14*

0.19*
CTL

1987). Consistent with previous studies, we observed higher scores for


nearly all facets of openness in both the creCTL and BP groups (Srivas­
tava et al., 2010; Feist, 1998; Nowakowska et al., 2005; Barnett et al.,
0.39**

0.51**
0.48**
0.25*

0.27*
0.27*
0.19*
0.01
0.07
0.15
0.18
0.10

2011). Higher openness predicted better cognitive and creative perfor­


BP

mance in both unaffected and affected individuals and was one of the
Schizotypy

strongest predictors of creative achievement. This effect was largely


0.43**
0.38**
0.42**
0.36**

0.32**

independent of hypomanic personality, consistent with the suggestion


0.21*

0.18*

0.17*
0.08

0.01

0.13
0.06
CTL

that openness may mediate the link between hypomanic traits and
are shown in bold. Significance is indicated as * (p < 0.05) or ** (p < 0.001).

engagement in daily creativity activities (Furnham et al., 2008).


Much research on creativity has focused on aspects of schizotypy as a
0.52**

0.57**
0.60**
0.56**

− 0.01
− 0.08
− 0.09
− 0.13

subclinical expression of psychosis, and it has been suggested that both


0.09

0.08
0.01

0.18
0.12
BP

may result from reduced cognitive inhibition, which allows more stimuli
Cyclothymic

to filter into conscious awareness and may facilitate originality (Carson


et al., 2003; Green and Williams, 1999). Positive schizotypal traits
0.55**
0.39**
0.46**
0.63**
0.59**

0.24**
0.16*

0.15*
0.17*
0.07

0.05

0.06
0.13

reflecting cognitive disorganization and unusual perceptual experiences


CTL

are consistently elevated among individuals active in the creative arts


(Schuldberg, 2001; Schuldberg et al., 1988; Nelson and Rawlings, 2010;
0.53**
0.52**
0.45**
0.44**
0.51**
0.31**
0.62**
0.46**

0.36**

Sass, 2001; Tsakanikos and Claridge, 2005; Batey and Furnham, 2008;
− 0.06

− 0.07
0.06

0.03

0.12

Barrantes-Vidal, 2004; Burch et al., 2006). We observed similar eleva­


BP

tions in the creCTL group that significantly correlated with creative


Hypomanic

achievement. While the BP group also scored higher on measures of


0.54**
0.46**
0.47**
0.38**
0.43**
0.38**
0.44**
0.38**

0.42**
0.17*

0.17*

positive schizotypy, consistent with prior observations (Rybakowski and


0.10
0.06

0.09
CTL

Klonowska, 2011; Nettle, 2006; Mahon et al., 2013), these elevations


were more extreme and not predictive of creative achievement.
Expectedly, the creCTL group showed significant enhancements
Creative Achievement
Humor Appreciation
Creative Personality
Positive Schizotypy

Divergent Thinking

across multiple cognitive domains, including humor appreciation,


Spatial Processing

reasoning, and spatial processing ability. Humor appreciation has been


Hyperthymic
Cyclothymic

previously linked to creativity (Brodzinsky and Rubien, 1976; Thorson


Impulsivity

Reasoning
Openness

Memory

and Powell, 1993), and reasoning ability is a measure of fluid intelli­


Irritable
Anxious
Table 3

gence and creative problem solving that is highly correlated with

155
T.A. Greenwood et al. Journal of Psychiatric Research 153 (2022) 149–158

Fig. 4. Bipolar disorder, creativity, and the space


between. Traits identified through the analyses are
summarized by type, and the defining characteristics
are listed for each group. Shared traits are shown in
the space between. We note that many of these traits
are more extreme in the context of illness and less so
among creative individuals. These traits range from
those associated with mood or temperament to as­
pects of personality and behavior to cognitive attri­
butes that are associated either with dysfunction or
enhanced ability.

creativity (Furnham et al., 2008; Kuncel et al., 2004; Batey et al., 2009). evaluated aspects of ‘everyday’ creativity in groups defined by self-
Notably, the BP group did not show deficits on these tasks that have been report of creative occupation, which may represent a poor proxy for
linked to creativity as has been observed for other cognitive functions in creativity. While we were able to demonstrate the reproducibility of
bipolar disorder here and elsewhere (Bora et al., 2009; Van Rheenen these results using groups defined by high creative achievement, future
et al., 2017; Burdick et al., 2014). Contrary to previous studies that have studies could consider more objective indices of creative ability and
used figure preference tests to demonstrate increased creativity in bi­ performance. Additionally, it has been suggested that the over­
polar disorder, we found only a weak elevation in the BP group that was representation of bipolar disorder observed in population-based studies
driven by the dislike subscale (Simeonova et al., 2005; Srivastava et al., of creativity may reflect a preference for the unconventional lifestyle
2010; Soeiro-de-Souza et al., 2011; Soeiro-de-Souza et al., 2012; Strong provided by creative occupations, rather than a true association. Indeed,
et al., 2007; Fears et al., 2014). However, the creCTL and BP groups the personality traits most associated with choosing a creative occupa­
showed similarly superior divergent (“outside the box”) thinking and tion are openness and impulsivity (Feist, 1998), which are also associ­
originality, revealing large effect sizes comparable to previous reports ated with bipolar disorder as discussed above. However, our results
(Ma, 2009). It is important to note that educational attainment showed show that the association of these traits is not solely tethered to creative
similar distributions across the three groups and was not predictive of profession, as even the nBP subgroup scored higher on openness and
creative performance or achievement in any group. demonstrated more originality and higher creative achievement than
A positive mood has been suggested to provide a significant cognitive the nCTL group. These results thus support the notion that bipolar
advantage in terms of creative performance (Vosburg, 1998). Consistent spectrum traits offer advantages for creativity and provide motivation
with prior reports, we found hyperthymic temperament to be elevated for further research in this area (Greenwood, 2017, 2020).
specifically in the creCTL group and associated with creative achieve­ Bipolar disorder is somewhat unique among common medical ill­
ment in unaffected individuals (Schuldberg, 2001; Vellante et al., 2011). nesses, as many patients view their illness as an inherent part of their
While hyperthymic temperament was not characteristic of the BP group identity that imparts confidence, empathy, self-awareness, social ease,
as a whole, higher scores were associated with better creative perfor­ productivity, and creativity. Yet, it comes at a high cost in terms of
mance and were observed for the creBP subgroup. We also noted higher disability and suicide, and delayed treatment seeking and non-
cyclothymic, irritable, and anxious temperament scores in both the BP compliance, particularly among those who believe that their creative
and creCTL groups, consistent with previous findings (Nowakowska abilities are directly related to their mood fluctuations, pose significant
et al., 2005; Strong et al., 2007; Srivastava and Ketter, 2010). However, concerns. While creativity is far from a ubiquitous trait in bipolar dis­
unlike the changeable affect and emotional intensity that characterizes order, exploring this relationship may have important clinical and social
cyclothymic temperament, the negative affect reflected by the irritable implications. A better understanding of this complex illness will facili­
and anxious temperaments was not associated with creative achieve­ tate personalized treatment strategies and suggest methods to enhance
ment. This is consistent with the low signal observed for negative the positive attributes while limiting mood swings. Investigations of the
schizotypal traits and dysthymic temperament in the creCTL group. shared vulnerability may also shed light on the biological causes of
Similarly, we did not replicate previous reports of increased neuroticism illness, providing novel treatment targets. Finally, appreciating the
among creative individuals (Srivastava et al., 2010; Feist, 1998; Now­ positive traits associated with bipolar disorder will foster the celebration
akowska et al., 2005; Strong et al., 2007; Srivastava and Ketter, 2010; of neurodiversity and help reduce the stigma.
Gelade, 1997). These findings thus confirm and extend our hypotheses
regarding the relationship of positive affect and mood instability to Contributors
creative potential (Greenwood, 2017, 2020).
There are possible limitations to this study that should be considered. Ms. Chow was responsible for data collection and quality assurance
First, in order to promote comparison with existing literature, we have and assisted with manuscript revision. Drs. Gur and Kelsoe participated

156
T.A. Greenwood et al. Journal of Psychiatric Research 153 (2022) 149–158

in study design and manuscript revision. Dr. Greenwood is responsible Costa Jr., P.T., McCrae, R.R., 1992. NEO PI-R Professional Manual: Revised NEO
Personality Inventory (NEO PI-R) and NEO Five-Factor Inventory (NEO-FFI).
for the conception of the study, data collection and analysis, and inter­
Psychological Assessment Resources, Odessa, FL.
pretation of results, as well as drafting and critically revising the Dollinger, S.J., Urban, K.K., James, T.A., 2004. Creativity and openness: further
manuscript. All authors approved the final manuscript. validation of two creative product measures. Creativ. Res. J. 16 (1), 35–47.
Eckblad, M., Chapman, L.J., 1986. Development and validation of a scale for hypomanic
personality. J. Abnorm. Psychol. 95 (3), 214–222.
Declaration of competing interest Eysenck, H.J., 1995. Genius: the Natural History of Creativity. Cambridge University
Press, Cambridge, UK.
Fears, S.C., Service, S.K., Kremeyer, B., Araya, C., Araya, X., Bejarano, J., Ramirez, M.,
Ms. Chow and Drs. Greenwood, Gur, and Kelsoe have no competing Castrillon, G., Gomez-Franco, J., Lopez, M.C., Montoya, G., Montoya, P., Aldana, I.,
financial interests to report. Teshiba, T.M., Abaryan, Z., Al-Sharif, N.B., Ericson, M., Jalbrzikowski, M., Luykx, J.
J., Navarro, L., Tishler, T.A., Altshuler, L., Bartzokis, G., Escobar, J., Glahn, D.C.,
Ospina-Duque, J., Risch, N., Ruiz-Linares, A., Thompson, P.M., Cantor, R.M., Lopez-
Acknowledgements Jaramillo, C., Macaya, G., Molina, J., Reus, V.I., Sabatti, C., Freimer, N.B.,
Bearden, C.E., 2014. Multisystem component phenotypes of bipolar disorder for
The authors wish to thank all of the participants, without whom this genetic investigations of extended pedigrees. JAMA Psychiatr. 71 (4), 375–387.
Feist, G.J., 1998. A meta-analysis of personality in scientific and artistic creativity. Pers.
study would not have been possible. We also offer our sincerest gratitude Soc. Psychol. Rev. 2 (4), 290–309.
to the research volunteers who devoted their time and effort to this Furnham, A., Batey, M., Anand, K., Manfield, J., 2008. Personality, hypomania,
study: Augusta Wong, Frank Qiu, and Tiffany So. This study was sup­ intelligence and creativity. Pers. Indiv. Differ. 44 (5), 1060–1069.
Gartner, J.D., 2005. The Hypomanic Edge: the Link between (A Little) Craziness and (A
ported by a grant from the National Institute of Mental Health (R21- Lot of) Success in America. Simon & Schuster, New York.
MH106917). Gelade, G.A., 1997. Creativity in conflict: the personality of the commercial creative.
J. Genet. Psychol. 158 (1), 67–78.
Glahn, D.C., Gur, R.C., Ragland, J.D., Censits, D.M., Gur, R.E., 1997. Reliability,
Appendix A. Supplementary data performance characteristics, construct validity, and an initial clinical application of a
visual object learning test (VOLT). Neuropsychology 11 (4), 602–612.
Supplementary data to this article can be found online at https://doi. Goodwin, F.K., Jamison, K.R., 2007. Manic-depressive Illness: Bipolar Disorders and
Recurrent Depression. Oxford University Press, New York.
org/10.1016/j.jpsychires.2022.07.009. Gough, H.G., 1979. A creative personality scale for the Adjective Check List. J. Pers. Soc.
Psychol. 37 (8), 1398–1405.
References Gough, H.G., Heilbrun, A.B., 1965. The Adjective Checklist Manual. Consulting
Psychologists Press, Inc.
Green, M.J., Williams, L.M., 1999. Schizotypy and creativity as effects of reduced
Akiskal, H.S., 2002. The bipolar spectrum–the shaping of a new paradigm in psychiatry.
cognitive inhibition. Pers. Indiv. Differ. 27 (2), 263–276.
Curr. Psychiatr. Rep. 4 (1), 1–3.
Greenwood, T.A., 2017. Positive traits in the bipolar spectrum: the space between
Akiskal, H.S., Akiskal, K.K., 2007. In search of Aristotle: temperament, human nature,
Madness and Genius. Mol Neuropsychiatry 2 (4), 198–212.
melancholia, creativity and eminence. J. Affect. Disord. 100 (1–3), 1–6.
Greenwood, T.A., 2020. Creativity and bipolar disorder: a shared genetic vulnerability.
Akiskal, H.S., Akiskal, K.K., Haykal, R.F., Manning, J.S., Connor, P.D., 2005. TEMPS-A:
Annu. Rev. Clin. Psychol. 16, 239–264.
progress towards validation of a self-rated clinical version of the temperament
Gur, R.C., Gur, R.E., Obrist, W.D., Hungerbuhler, J.P., Younkin, D., Rosen, A.D.,
evaluation of the Memphis, Pisa, Paris, and san Diego autoquestionnaire. J. Affect.
Skolnick, B.E., Reivich, M., 1982. Sex and handedness differences in cerebral blood
Disord. 85 (1–2), 3–16.
flow during rest and cognitive activity. Science 217 (4560), 659–661.
Bagby, R.M., Costa, P.T., McCrae, R.R., Livesley, W.J., Kennedy, S.H., Levitan, R.D.,
Gur, R.C., Ragland, J.D., Mozley, L.H., Mozley, P.D., Smith, R., Alavi, A., Bilker, W.,
Levitt, A.J., Joffe, R.T., Young, L.T., 1999. Replicating the five factor model of
Gur, R.E., 1997. Lateralized changes in regional cerebral blood flow during
personality in a psychiatric sample. Pers. Indiv. Differ. 27 (6), 1135–1139.
performance of verbal and facial recognition tasks: correlations with performance
Barnett, J.H., Huang, J., Perlis, R.H., Young, M.M., Rosenbaum, J.F., Nierenberg, A.A.,
and "effort". Brain Cognit. 33 (3), 388–414.
Sachs, G., Nimgaonkar, V.L., Miklowitz, D.J., Smoller, J.W., 2011. Personality and
Gur, R.C., Richard, J., Hughett, P., Calkins, M.E., Macy, L., Bilker, W.B., Brensinger, C.,
bipolar disorder: dissecting state and trait associations between mood and
Gur, R.E., 2010. A cognitive neuroscience-based computerized battery for efficient
personality. Psychol. Med. 41 (8), 1593–1604.
measurement of individual differences: standardization and initial construct
Barrantes-Vidal, N., 2004. Creativity & Madness revisited from current psychological
validation. J. Neurosci. Methods 187 (2), 254–262.
perspectives. J. Conscious. Stud. 11 (3–4), 58–78.
Gur, R.C., Richard, J., Calkins, M.E., Chiavacci, R., Hansen, J.A., Bilker, W.B.,
Barron, F., 1963. The Barron–Welsh Art Scale, A Portion of the Welsh
Loughead, J., Connolly, J.J., Qiu, H., Mentch, F.D., Abou-Sleiman, P.M.,
Figure PreferenceTest. Consulting Psychologists Press, Palo Alto, CA.
Hakonarson, H., Gur, R.E., 2012. Age group and sex differences in performance on a
Batey, M., Furnham, A., 2008. The relationship between measures of creativity and
computerized neurocognitive battery in children age 8-21. Neuropsychology 26 (2),
schizotypy. Pers. Indiv. Differ. 45 (8), 816–821.
251–265.
Batey, M., Chamorro-Premuzic, T., Furnham, A., 2009. Intelligence and personality as
Higier, R.G., Jimenez, A.M., Hultman, C.M., Borg, J., Roman, C., Kizling, I., Larsson, H.,
predictors of divergent thinking: the role of general, fluid and crystallised
Cannon, T.D., 2014. Enhanced neurocognitive functioning and positive
intelligence. Think. Skills Creativ. 4 (1), 60–69.
temperament in twins discordant for bipolar disorder. Am. J. Psychiatr. 171 (11),
Bilker, W.B., Hansen, J.A., Brensinger, C.M., Richard, J., Gur, R.E., Gur, R.C., 2012.
1191–1198.
Development of abbreviated nine-item forms of the Raven’s standard progressive
Jamison, K.R., 1993. Touched with fire: manic-depressive illness and the artistic
matrices test. Assessment 19 (3), 354–369.
temperament. In: 1st Free Press Pbk. Free Press, New York.
Bora, E., Yucel, M., Pantelis, C., 2009. Cognitive endophenotypes of bipolar disorder: a
Jauk, E., Benedek, M., Dunst, B., Neubauer, A.C., 2013. The relationship between
meta-analysis of neuropsychological deficits in euthymic patients and their first-
intelligence and creativity: new support for the threshold hypothesis by means of
degree relatives. J. Affect. Disord. 113 (1–2), 1–20.
empirical breakpoint detection. Intelligence 41 (4), 212–221.
Bozikas, V.P., Kosmidis, M.H., Tonia, T., Garyfallos, G., Focas, K., Karavatos, A., 2007.
Kawamura, Y., Akiyama, T., Shimada, T., Minato, T., Umekage, T., Noda, Y., Ukawa, K.,
Humor appreciation in remitted patients with bipolar disorder. J. Nerv. Ment. Dis.
Hashidume, C., Sakai, Y., Otowa, T., Sasaki, T., Akiskal, H.S., 2010. Six-year stability
195 (9), 773–775.
of affective temperaments as measured by TEMPS-A. Psychopathology 43 (4),
Brodzinsky, D.M., Rubien, J., 1976. Humor production as a function of sex of subject,
240–247.
creativity, and cartoon content. J. Consult. Clin. Psychol. 44 (4), 597–600.
Kessler, R.C., Berglund, P., Demler, O., Jin, R., Merikangas, K.R., Walters, E.E., 2005.
Burch, G., Pavelis, C., Hemsley, D.R., Corr, P.J., 2006. Schizotypy and creativity in visual
Lifetime prevalence and age-of-onset distributions of DSM-IV disorders in the
artists. Br. J. Psychol. 97 (2), 177–190.
national comorbidity survey replication. Arch. Gen. Psychiatr. 62 (6), 593–602.
Burdick, K.E., Russo, M., Frangou, S., Mahon, K., Braga, R.J., Shanahan, M., Malhotra, A.
Kohn, N., Kellermann, T., Gur, R.C., Schneider, F., Habel, U., 2011. Gender differences in
K., 2014. Empirical evidence for discrete neurocognitive subgroups in bipolar
the neural correlates of humor processing: implications for different processing
disorder: clinical implications. Psychol. Med. 44 (14), 3083–3096.
modes. Neuropsychologia 49 (5), 888–897.
Burmeister, M., McInnis, M.G., Zollner, S., 2008. Psychiatric genetics: progress amid
Kuncel, N.R., Hezlett, S.A., Ones, D.S., 2004. Academic performance, career potential,
controversy. Nat. Rev. Genet. 9 (7), 527–540.
creativity, and job performance: can one construct predict them all? J. Pers. Soc.
Carson, S.H., 2011. Creativity and psychopathology: a shared vulnerability model. Can.
Psychol. 86 (1), 148–161.
J. Psychiatr. 56 (3), 144–153.
Kurtz, M.M., Ragland, J.D., Bilker, W., Gur, R.C., Gur, R.E., 2001. Comparison of the
Carson, S.H., Peterson, J.B., Higgins, D.M., 2003. Decreased latent inhibition is
continuous performance test with and without working memory demands in healthy
associated with increased creative achievement in high-functioning individuals.
controls and patients with schizophrenia. Schizophr. Res. 48 (2–3), 307–316.
J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 85 (3), 499–506.
Kurtz, M.M., Ragland, J.D., Moberg, P.J., Gur, R.C., 2004. The Penn Conditional
Carson, S.H., Peterson, J.B., Higgins, D.M., 2005. Reliability, validity, and factor
Exclusion Test: a new measure of executive-function with alternate forms of repeat
structure of the creative achievement questionnaire. Creativ. Res. J. 17 (1), 37–50.
administration. Arch. Clin. Neuropsychol. 19 (2), 191–201.
Coryell, W., Endicott, J., Keller, M., Andreasen, N., Grove, W., Hirschfeld, R.M.,
Scheftner, W., 1989. Bipolar affective disorder and high achievement: a familial
association. Am. J. Psychiatr. 146 (8), 983–988.

157
T.A. Greenwood et al. Journal of Psychiatric Research 153 (2022) 149–158

Kwapil, T.R., Miller, M.B., Zinser, M.C., Chapman, L.J., Chapman, J., Eckblad, M., 2000. Richards, R., Kinney, D.K., Lunde, I., Benet, M., Merzel, A.P., 1988. Creativity in manic-
A longitudinal study of high scorers on the hypomanic personality scale. J. Abnorm. depressives, cyclothymes, their normal relatives, and control subjects. J. Abnorm.
Psychol. 109 (2), 222–226. Psychol. 97 (3), 281–288.
Kyaga, S., Lichtenstein, P., Boman, M., Hultman, C., Langstrom, N., Landen, M., 2011. Russ, S., 1993. Affect and Creativity: the Role of Affect and Play in the Creative Process.
Creativity and mental disorder: family study of 300,000 people with severe mental Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Hillsdale, New Jersey.
disorder. Br. J. Psychiatry 199 (5), 373–379. Rybakowski, J.K., Klonowska, P., 2011. Bipolar mood disorder, creativity and
Kyaga, S., Landen, M., Boman, M., Hultman, C.M., Langstrom, N., Lichtenstein, P., 2013. schizotypy: an experimental study. Psychopathology 44 (5), 296–302.
Mental illness, suicide and creativity: 40-year prospective total population study. Sass, L.A., 2001. Schizophrenia, modernism, and the "creative imagination": on creativity
J. Psychiatr. Res. 47 (1), 83–90. and psychopathology. Creativ. Res. J. 13 (1), 55–74.
Lombardo, L.E., Bearden, C.E., Barrett, J., Brumbaugh, M.S., Pittman, B., Frangou, S., Schuldberg, D., 2001. Six subclinical spectrum traits in normal creativity. Creativ. Res. J.
Glahn, D.C., 2012. Trait impulsivity as an endophenotype for bipolar I disorder. 13 (1), 5–16.
Bipolar Disord. 14 (5), 565–570. Schuldberg, D., French, C., Stone, B.L., Heberle, J., 1988. Creativity and schizotypal
Ma, H., 2009. The effect size of variables associated with creativity: a meta-analysis. traits. Creativity test scores and perceptual aberration, magical ideation, and
Creativ. Res. J. 21 (1), 30–42. impulsive nonconformity. J. Nerv. Ment. Dis. 176 (11), 648–657.
Mahon, K., Perez-Rodriguez, M.M., Gunawardane, N., Burdick, K.E., 2013. Dimensional Simeonova, D.I., Chang, K.D., Strong, C., Ketter, T.A., 2005. Creativity in familial bipolar
endophenotypes in bipolar disorder: affective dysregulation and psychosis disorder. J. Psychiatr. Res. 39 (6), 623–631.
proneness. J. Affect. Disord. 151, 695–701. Soeiro-de-Souza, M.G., Dias, V.V., Bio, D.S., Post, R.M., Moreno, R.A., 2011. Creativity
McCrae, R.R., 1987. Creativity, divergent thinking, and openness to experience. J. Pers. and executive function across manic, mixed and depressive episodes in bipolar I
Soc. Psychol. 52 (6), 1258–1265. disorder. J. Affect. Disord. 135 (1–3), 292–297.
Moore, T.M., Scott, J.C., Reise, S.P., Port, A.M., Jackson, C.T., Ruparel, K., Savitt, A.P., Soeiro-de-Souza, M.G., Post, R.M., de Sousa, M.L., Missio, G., do Prado, C.M., Gattaz, W.
Gur, R.E., Gur, R.C., 2015. Development of an abbreviated form of the Penn Line F., Moreno, R.A., Machado-Vieira, R., 2012. Does BDNF genotype influence creative
Orientation Test using large samples and computerized adaptive test simulation. output in bipolar I manic patients? J. Affect. Disord. 139 (2), 181–186.
Psychol. Assess. 27 (3), 955–964. Srivastava, S., Ketter, T.A., 2010. The link between bipolar disorders and creativity:
Murray, G., Johnson, S.L., 2010. The clinical significance of creativity in bipolar evidence from personality and temperament studies. Curr. Psychiatr. Rep. 12 (6),
disorder. Clin. Psychol. Rev. 30 (6), 721–732. 522–530.
Nelson, B., Rawlings, D., 2010. Relating schizotypy and personality to the Srivastava, S., Childers, M.E., Baek, J.H., Strong, C.M., Hill, S.J., Warsett, K.S., Wang, P.
phenomenology of creativity. Schizophr. Bull. 36 (2), 388–399. W., Akiskal, H.S., Akiskal, K.K., Ketter, T.A., 2010. Toward interaction of affective
Nettle, D., 2001. Strong Imagination: Madness, Creativity and Human Nature. Oxford and cognitive contributors to creativity in bipolar disorders: a controlled study.
University Press, Oxford, England. J. Affect. Disord. 125 (1–3), 27–34.
Nettle, D., 2006. Schizotypy and mental health amongst poets, visual artists, and Strong, C.M., Nowakowska, C., Santosa, C.M., Wang, P.W., Kraemer, H.C., Ketter, T.A.,
mathematicians. J. Res. Pers. 40 (6), 876–890. 2007. Temperament-creativity relationships in mood disorder patients, healthy
Nowakowska, C., Strong, C.M., Santosa, C.M., Wang, P.W., Ketter, T.A., 2005. controls and highly creative individuals. J. Affect. Disord. 100 (1–3), 41–48.
Temperamental commonalities and differences in euthymic mood disorder patients, Thorson, J.A., Powell, F.C., 1993. Sense of humor and dimensions of personality. J. Clin.
creative controls, and healthy controls. J. Affect. Disord. 85 (1–2), 207–215. Psychol. 49 (6), 799–809.
Nurnberger Jr., J.I., Blehar, M.C., Kaufmann, C.A., York-Cooler, C., Simpson, S.G., Torrance, E.P., 1974. Torrance Tests of Creative Thinking, Verbal and Figural: Directions,
Harkavy-Friedman, J., Severe, J.B., Malaspina, D., Reich, T., 1994. Diagnostic Manual and Scoring Guide. Personal Press, New York.
interview for genetic studies. Rationale, unique features, and training. NIMH Tremblay, C.H., Grosskopf, S., Yang, K., 2010. Brainstorm: occupational choice, bipolar
Genetics Initiative. Arch. Gen. Psychiatr. 51 (11), 849–859. ; discussion 863-844. illness and creativity. Econ. Hum. Biol. 8 (2), 233–241.
Parson, R.J., Tittler, B.I., Cook, V.J., 1984. A multi-trait multi-method evaluation of Tsakanikos, E., Claridge, G., 2005. More words, less words: verbal fluency as a function
creativity and openness. Psychol. Rep. 54 (2), 403–410. of ’positive’ and ’negative’ schizotypy. Pers. Indiv. Differ. 39 (4), 705–713.
Patton, J.H., Stanford, M.S., Barratt, E.S., 1995. Factor structure of the Barratt Van Rheenen, T.E., Lewandowski, K.E., Tan, E.J., Ospina, L.H., Ongur, D., Neill, E.,
impulsiveness scale. J. Clin. Psychol. 51 (6), 768–774. Gurvich, C., Pantelis, C., Malhotra, A.K., Rossell, S.L., Burdick, K.E., 2017.
Peluso, M.A., Hatch, J.P., Glahn, D.C., Monkul, E.S., Sanches, M., Najt, P., Bowden, C.L., Characterizing cognitive heterogeneity on the schizophrenia-bipolar disorder
Barratt, E.S., Soares, J.C., 2007. Trait impulsivity in patients with mood disorders. spectrum. Psychol. Med. 47 (10), 1848–1864.
J. Affect. Disord. 100 (1–3), 227–231. Vellante, M., Zucca, G., Preti, A., Sisti, D., Rocchi, M.B., Akiskal, K.K., Akiskal, H.S.,
Ragland, J.D., Turetsky, B.I., Gur, R.C., Gunning-Dixon, F., Turner, T., Schroeder, L., 2011. Creativity and affective temperaments in non-clinical professional artists: an
Chan, R., Gur, R.E., 2002. Working memory for complex figures: an fMRI empirical psychometric investigation. J. Affect. Disord. 135 (1–3), 28–36.
comparison of letter and fractal n-back tasks. Neuropsychology 16 (3), 370–379. Verdoux, H., Bourgeois, M., 1995. [Comparative study on the occupational levels of
Raine, A., 1991. The SPQ: a scale for the assessment of schizotypal personality based on unipolar and bipolar probands and relatives]. Ann. Med.-Psychol. 153 (1), 67–70.
DSM-III-R criteria. Schizophr. Bull. 17 (4), 555–564. Vosburg, S.K., 1998. The effects of positive and negative mood on divergent-thinking
performance. Creativ. Res. J. 11 (2), 165–172.

158

You might also like