You are on page 1of 11

NeuroImage 143 (2016) 15–25

Contents lists available at ScienceDirect

NeuroImage
journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/neuroimage

Functional activity and white matter microstructure reveal


the independent effects of age of acquisition and proficiency
on second-language learning
Emily S. Nichols n, Marc F. Joanisse
The University of Western Ontario, Canada

art ic l e i nf o a b s t r a c t

Article history: Two key factors govern how bilingual speakers neurally maintain two languages: the speakers’ second
Received 22 May 2016 language age of acquisition (AoA) and their subsequent proficiency. However, the relative roles of these
Received in revised form two factors have been difficult to disentangle given that the two can be closely correlated, and most prior
23 August 2016
studies have examined the two factors in isolation. Here, we combine functional magnetic resonance
Accepted 24 August 2016
Available online 25 August 2016
imaging with diffusion tensor imaging to identify specific brain areas that are independently modulated
by AoA and proficiency in second language speakers. First-language Mandarin Chinese speakers who are
Keywords: second language speakers of English were scanned as they performed a picture-word matching task in
Bilingualism either language. In the same session we also acquired diffusion-weighted scans to assess white matter
fMRI
microstructure, along with behavioural measures of language proficiency prior to entering the scanner.
DTI
Results reveal gray- and white-matter networks involving both the left and right hemisphere that in-
Proficiency
Age of acquisition dependently vary as a function of a second-language speaker's AoA and proficiency, focused on the
superior temporal gyrus, middle and inferior frontal gyrus, parahippocampal gyrus, and the basal
ganglia. These results indicate that proficiency and AoA explain separate functional and structural net-
works in the bilingual brain, which we interpret as suggesting distinct types of plasticity for age-de-
pendent effects (i.e., AoA) versus experience and/or predisposition (i.e., proficiency).
& 2016 The Authors. Published by Elsevier Inc. This is an open access article under the CC BY license
(http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).

1. Introduction than L1 learning. On this account L2 learning requires increased


neural resources due to maturational changes in neural plasticity
Prior studies of bilingualism have identified ways in which the within regions and pathways supporting first language learning
neural representation of a second language (L2) differs from that of (Abutalebi, 2008; Klein et al., 2013; Li et al., 2014; Mechelli et al.,
an individual's first language (L1). Specifically, there are several 2004; Pakulak and Neville, 2011; Perani et al., 1996; Wartenburger
differences in activation between L2 and L1, both in terms of degree et al., 2003; Weber-Fox and Neville, 1996). Concordant with this
and extent. L2s tend to not only show more activity within tradi- view, studies have found that age-of-acquisition (AoA) modulates
tional left-hemisphere language areas, but also tend to activate more these effects; individuals who are early L2 learners show patterns of
regions outside the traditional language network (Chee et al., 2004; brain activity that are more similar for L1 compared to late L2
Dehaene et al., 1997; Kim et al., 1997; Perani et al., 1998; Warten- learners (Perani and Abutalebi, 2005; Wartenburger et al., 2003).
burger et al., 2003). Much of the current data come from studies Additionally, structural connectivity, as measured using DTI, appears
using fMRI to compare cortical activity, although an emerging lit- to vary as a function of AoA. A common measure of white matter
erature also reveals differences between L2 and L1 in white matter microstructure is fractional anisotropy (FA), which ranges from zero
connectivity as examined with diffusion tensor imaging (DTI). to one and represents the cohesiveness of white matter tracts. High
There are two predominant theories as to why neural signatures FA suggests that water diffusion is restricted to a single direction and
of L2 differ from those of L1. The first is that these differences reflect thus the white matter tract is cohesive, while low FA suggests that
reduced neuroplasticity during L2 learning that occurs at a later age water diffusion is unrestricted and the tract is less cohesive. FA varies
with AoA such that children who learned two languages from birth
n (simultaneous bilinguals) show higher white matter integrity in the
Correspondence to: The Brain and Mind Institute, Department of Psychology,
The University of Western Ontario, London, Ontario, Canada N6C 5B7. left inferior fronto-occipital fasciculus (IFOF), the tract connecting
E-mail address: enicho4@uwo.ca (E.S. Nichols). anterior frontal regions with posterior temporal regions when

http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2016.08.053
1053-8119/& 2016 The Authors. Published by Elsevier Inc. This is an open access article under the CC BY license (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
16 E.S. Nichols, M.F. Joanisse / NeuroImage 143 (2016) 15–25

compared to children who learned their two languages sequentially Table 1


(Mohades et al., 2012). However, lower integrity was also found in Subject demographics.
the tracts projecting from the anterior portion of the corpus callo-
Measure Mean (SD) Min Max
sum to orbitofrontal cortex compared to late L2 learners. These re-
sults highlight how differences in brain connectivity may be related Age (years) 22.18 (4.24) 18 35
to L2 AoA. Research suggests that there are separate L1 and L2 Sex f 17
networks that are complementary in their importance as a function m 5
Years of schooling 14.8 (2.26) 12 20
of AoA (Mohades et al., 2012). The differing influence of AoA on Proficiency (% correct) L1 85.98 (6.34) 72.92 97.92
separate tracts may reflect their relative importance in L1 versus L2 L2 68.28 (6.62) 54.17 77.08
processing. Age of Acquisition 13.82 (7.12) 4 30
The alternative theory is that neural differences in L1 versus L2 Duration of exposure to L2 (years) 8.36 (4.47) 0 15
are instead driven by the fact that individuals' L2 is generally lower
Note: Proficiency score reflects percent correct on each language's proficiency test.
in proficiency than their L1. On this account, second language
processing involves increased processing demands, and therefore
University of Western Ontario and from the London, Ontario
greater neural resources. While greater processing demands may
community. All subjects were English L2 speakers, aged 18–35
induce experience-based plasticity, these changes should be se-
(M¼ 22.18, SD ¼4.24), and ranged in age of acquisition from 4–30
parable from age-induced plasticity. Indeed, prior neuroimaging
years (M¼13.82, SD ¼7.12). Two additional subjects were recruited
studies have observed that higher-proficiency L2 users do show
but excluded from analyses, one due to an incidental finding and
patterns of neural activity that more closely resemble those of L1
one due to chance-level performance on both the English and
users, compared to lower-proficiency L2 users, even when AoA is
Mandarin fMRI tasks. Additional subject demographics are pre-
controlled for (Chee et al., 1999; Newman et al., 2012; Pakulak and
sented in Table 1.
Neville, 2011; Perani et al., 1998; Wartenburger et al., 2003).
While both AoA and proficiency explanations are appealing, it
2.2. Behavioural materials
is difficult to adjudicate among them given that the two are gen-
erally confounded: early L2 learners also tend to have higher
L1 Mandarin and L2 English proficiency levels were assessed
proficiency than late learners, making it difficult to tease apart the
prior to scanning using a subset of 48 questions from the Hanyu
relative contribution of both factors. Indeed, it could be argued
Shuiping Kaoshi (HSK Centre, Beijing, China) and the Test of Eng-
that the two are simply reflections of each other, and cannot be
disentangled. However, early AoA does not always mean high lish as a Second Language (ETS, Princeton, NJ) respectively, with
proficiency, and high proficiency does not always mean early AoA. both tests consisting of three sections: Grammar, reading com-
Although it is well established that children acquire an L2 more prehension, and vocabulary. The three sections were combined to
easily than adults, adults are still capable of becoming highly give a final mark out of 48 for each language, representing overall
proficient (Perani et al., 1998; Wartenburger et al., 2003), and proficiency in these three domains. As the fMRI task was an au-
some children fail to fully acquire a second language in spite of ditory lexical-semantic one, this general proficiency created from
adequate opportunity to do so (Frost et al., 2013; Sahinkarakas, not just lexical knowledge score helped avoid circularity in cor-
2011). Factors such as motivation and environment also play a key relating lexical knowledge with itself. Despite the written form of
role in successful L2 acquisition (Bernaus and Gardner, 2008; the proficiency test and the auditory form of the fMRI task, lexical
Gardner et al., 2004) unlike in L1, where failure to acquire lan- knowledge is amodal (Coccia et al., 2004; Lambon Ralph and
guage is extremely rare outside situations involving severe sensory Patterson, 2008; Patterson et al., 2007) and thus is not confounded
deprivation (Fromkin et al., 1974). The present study addressed by the modality in which each task was administered.
individual differences in L2 learning success by examining neural AoA was defined as the age at which subjects first began
signatures of L2 processing in adult native speakers of Mandarin learning English. To verify right-handedness, subjects completed
who are second language English speakers. These speakers an abridged version of the Edinburgh Handedness Inventory
showed significant variation in both AoA and proficiency, allowing (Oldfield, 1971). All behavioural measures were completed in
us to examine the two factors in parallel. Additionally, this ap- Mandarin aside from the English proficiency test. Letters of in-
proach allowed us to examine effects of L2 learning using a within- formation, informed consent and task instructions were likewise
subjects design, rather than creating (potentially arbitrary) groups administered in Mandarin by a native speaker.
of high versus low proficiency and early versus late L2 learners.
We first used fMRI to isolate areas involved in L2 processing, 2.3. f MRI task
and examined how variation in patterns of activation can be ex-
plained either by AoA or proficiency. Next, we used DTI to identify Subjects completed a picture-word matching task during
regions in which tract coherence correlated with either AoA or L2 scanning. This task has been used extensively in the past to study
proficiency, and which white matter tracts projected from these lexical knowledge (Breining et al., 2014; Dräger et al., 2004; We-
regions. We identified networks of both functional and structural niger et al., 2000), and was chosen to examine lexical-semantic
organisation that were independently explained by proficiency processing. Because this task does not engage reading or syntax, it
and AoA, suggesting the L2 speaker's brain organisation is not provided an ideal task to investigate lexical-semantic processing.
wholly influenced by age-dependent learning, but is also suscep- Pictures appeared centred on a screen mounted at the head of
tible to L2 language ability levels. the scanner bore, which subjects viewed through a mirror placed
above the head coil. At the same time, a word was played binau-
rally through insert earphones (Sensimetrics Corporation, Malden,
2. Methods MA). Both match and mismatch trials were presented in order to
maximise participants’ engagement during the task, and to en-
2.1. Subjects courage greater depth of processing than what might be expected
during passive listening or lexical decision. Subjects were required
Twenty-two (17 female) neurologically healthy right-handed to indicate as quickly as possible with a button press whether the
native speakers of Mandarin were recruited via posters at the picture and word matched. Each picture was visible for 2.5 s.
E.S. Nichols, M.F. Joanisse / NeuroImage 143 (2016) 15–25 17

Table 2 3D pulse sequence weighted for T1 contrast (MPRAGE; TR¼ 2.3 s;


Contrasts of word measures between languages. TE¼2.98 ms; FOV¼256  256 mm; voxel size¼1 mm3; 176 slices;
GRAPPA acceleration factor¼2).
Measure Mean (SD) t p
Data analysis was performed using the AFNI software package
English Mandarin (Cox, 1996). Functional scans were first de-obliqued (transformed
to the same cardinal orientation as the anatomical scan, AFNI
Frequency 213.83 (258.19) 129.69 (100.66)  1.92 .058 3dWarp), then motion corrected by registering each volume to the
Imageability 5.23 (0.81) 5.78 (.87)  2.89 .004
Familiarity 5.48 (0.66) 5.78 (.62)  1.02 .310
last functional volume of the session, which was acquired im-
Picture/word match 6.03 (0.57) 6.21 (.57)  1.43 .157 mediately preceding the anatomical scan, using a 3d rigid body
transformation (AFNI 3dvolreg). Outlier volumes caused by hard-
ware artifacts were identified as ones significantly deviating from
Stimulus presentation and response recording was controlled with average image intensity using AFNI 3dToutcount using a threshold
E-Prime software (Psychology Software Tools, Inc., Sharpsburg, PA) of 10,000 outlier voxels, and subsequently removed from statis-
and a Windows laptop. tical analyses using the CENSORTR option in AFNI 3dDeconvolve. A
The scanning session was divided into 8 runs of 20 trials each single motion parameter was created by averaging the absolute
for a total of 160 trials. A short break was provided between each values of six motion parameters (roll, pitch, yaw, superior, left,
3.5-minute scanning run. Each image appeared twice during the posterior directions), and this was included in the subject-level
experiment, once in a matching pair and once in a semantically statistical model as a variable of no interest.
unrelated mismatching pair. Subjects completed four English runs Single-subject statistical maps were formed using a general
alternating with four Mandarin runs, with starting language linear model (GLM) with the following six predictor functions: one
counterbalanced. Each run began with an image reminding sub- each for matching and mismatch trials of English runs, one each
jects of which buttons to respond with, and subjects viewed a for matching and mismatch trials of Mandarin runs, one re-
fixation crosshair between trials. Subjects were also informed at presenting reaction times and one representing motion. Only
this time of the language in which the next run would be per- correct trials were included in the analysis, with accuracy ranging
formed. Each trial was 2.5 s in duration and presented at the onset from 71.25% to 100% correct on the Mandarin task, and ranging
of an image acquisition, with inter-trial interval jittered between from 82.5% to 100% correct on the English task. Subjectwise sta-
2.5–12.5 s in 2.5 s increments, allowing us to more accurately de- tistical maps were then resampled to 1 mm3, and transformed to
convolve the blood oxygen level dependent signal. stereotaxic space of Talairach and Tournoux (1988) via an auto-
Stimulus word properties are described in Table 2. We selected matic registration procedure (least-squares cost function) and a
40 items in each language, which would each appear twice, once standard atlas brain. A 5 mm FWHM Gaussian spatial filter was
in a matching pair and once in a mismatching pair. Items denoted then applied (AFNI 3dmerge). Finally, a brain mask was calculated
common single-word concepts in English and Mandarin, which and applied to each subject's GLM using an automatic procedure
represented two-syllable words in both English and Mandarin, and (AFNI 3dSkullStrip). Paired samples t-statistical maps were ob-
had frequencies greater than 40 per million in both languages tained for differences in activation of L1 and L2 trials, in order to
(English: CELEX Lexical Database, Baayen et al., 1995; Mandarin: identify regions that were differentially involved in the processing
SUBTLEX-CH, Cai and Brysbaert, 2010). In a separate pilot study of each language. Subsequent analyses examined the association
involving different subjects we asked groups of native speakers of between subjectwise regional brain activity and behavioural
English or Mandarin to rate the imageability and familiarity of the measures obtained prior to scanning. First, difference in L1/L2
stimulus words, as well as the correspondence of the pictures to brain activity, using an L2 versus L1 contrast, was calculated.
target words. As noted in Table 2, both groups showed equally Multiple linear regression was then performed on this subtraction
high ratings on all three sets of measures. Both groups of raters (AFNI 3dRegAna), using AoA and proficiency as independent pre-
showed highly similar ratings of familiarity and picture/word dictor variables. This identified clusters of voxels in which the
correspondence, although imageability was higher for Mandarin difference between L1 and L2 activation was predicted by AoA
words than English words (t(39) ¼  2.89, p ¼.004), and frequency independent of L2 proficiency scores, and by L2 proficiency scores
approached significance. Although much effort was put into independent of AoA. Specifically, this analysis revealed areas pre-
matching languages on these measures, differences between lan- dicted by one behavioural variable while holding constant the
guages should not affect interpretation of the results, as our focus effects of the other variable. Region of Interest (ROI) masks were
was on subject-wise variability in neural responses to L1 versus L2 then drawn using these areas, and by-subject beta values for the
rather than how neural responses to the two lists differ on a L2 versus L1 contrast were extracted and plotted.
whole-group basis. For contrasts, correction for multiple comparisons at po.001 was
achieved by setting a minimum cluster size of 195 voxels, obtained
2.4. fMRI acquisition and processing using a 10,000 iteration Monte Carlo simulation (AFNI 3dClustSim) at
a voxelwise alpha level of po.001. Minimum cluster size after cor-
Imaging was conducted on a Siemens Magnetom TIM Trio rection for multiple comparisons at po.05 for the multiple regres-
whole-body 3 T scanner with a 32-channel head coil. T2*-weighted sion analyses was achieved by setting a minimum cluster size of
functional scans were acquired in a transverse plane with 45 slices 1318 voxels with a voxelwise alpha level of po.01.
per volume (TR¼ 2.5 s; TE¼38 ms; flip angle¼80°; FOV¼
192  192 mm; voxel size 3 mm3) using an iPAT parallel acquisition 2.5. DTI acquisition and processing
sequence (generalised auto-calibrating partially parallel acquisition
[GRAPPA]; acceleration factor¼ 2), providing full coverage of the Diffusion-weighted images were acquired following the ana-
cerebrum with partial loss of the cerebellum. A total of 576 func- tomical scan. Images were acquired in the axial plane using an EPI
tional scans were acquired for each participant over 8 runs (3.5 min sequence (68 slices with 2 mm slice thickness, voxel size ¼
per run). To ensure stability of the MR signal four functional volumes 2.083  2.083 mm in-plane, matrix¼ 96  96  68, field of view¼
that were run but then discarded prior to the start of image acqui- 200 mm2, 64 diffusion directions with b ¼1000 s/mm2, TR ¼7.6 s,
sition. After the final functional run, a whole-head high-resolution TE ¼79 ms; GRAPPA acceleration factor¼3).
3D anatomical scan was acquired within the sagittal plane, using a DTI scans were processed using the AFNI and FSL (FMRIB
18 E.S. Nichols, M.F. Joanisse / NeuroImage 143 (2016) 15–25

Software Library, FMRIB, Oxford, UK; Smith et al., 2004) software overlaying on a stereotaxic brain atlas. Note that tractography is
packages. First, eddy current correction (eddy_correct in FSL) and used here for illustrative purposes, showing how significant FA
motion correction (AFNI 3dvolreg) were performed. Next, voxel- clusters relate to long-distance tracts; thus only sub-regions of
wise statistical analysis of the fractional anisotropy (FA) data was these tracts actually showed FA values that were significantly re-
carried out using FSL TBSS (Tract-Based Spatial Statistics; Smith lated by our behavioural measures.
et al., 2006). FA images were created by fitting a tensor model to
the raw diffusion (FDT in FSL), and then brain-extracted using (BET
in FSL; Smith, 2002). All subjects' FA data were then aligned into a 3. Results
common space using the FSL nonlinear registration tool FNIRT
(Andersson et al., 2010), which uses a b-spline representation of 3.1. Behavioural results
the registration warp field (Rueckert et al., 1999). Next, the mean
FA image was skeletonised with a threshold of 0.2, which re- Analyses of the behavioural data acquired prior to scanning
presents the centres of all tracts common to the group, and each confirmed L2 proficiency was significantly weaker than L1 profi-
subject's aligned FA data was then projected onto this skeleton. ciency (t(21)¼3.24, p¼ .003). We also observed a relative decoupling
This skeletonised data was submitted to multiple linear regression of AoA and proficiency measures, marked by a null correlation in the
(AFNI 3dRegAna), again using AoA and proficiency as independent two measures (r¼.02, p¼.940). Corresponding analyses using age of
predictor variables. This identified clusters of voxels in which the arrival in North America or number of years since arrival yielded
FA was predicted by AoA independent of L2 proficiency scores, and similarly null effects, suggesting that proficiency and AoA were
by L2 proficiency scores independent of AoA. Minimum cluster generally unrelated in our sample.
size after correction for multiple comparisons at p o .001 for the For the picture-word matching task performed during f MRI
multiple linear regression analyses was achieved by setting a scanning, there was no significant difference between Mandarin
minimum cluster size of 8 voxels with a voxelwise alpha level of and English trials in terms of RT (M¼ 1.27 s, SD ¼.32, M¼1.28 s,
p o.01, again using a 10,000 iteration Monte Carlo simulation SD ¼.62, respectively; t(21) ¼0.36, p o.941) or accuracy (M ¼94%,
(AFNI 3dClustSim). (Note the minimum cluster size is much smaller SD ¼8.79, M¼95%, SD ¼4.03; t(21) ¼ .61, p ¼.517).
for DTI than f MRI analyses. This is due to the use of a much
smaller search space in the DTI analyses, which represented a 3.2. f MRI results
white matter skeleton versus a spatially-smoothed whole-brain in
the f MRI analyses). For each significant cluster, probabilistic trac- We first identified regions showing differences in activation for
tography was performed subject-wise in native space using BED- L1 (Mandarin) versus L2 (English) trials. As listed in Table 3, results
POSTX and FDT diffusion, with ROI masks used as waypoints. The revealed greater activity in the right middle frontal gyrus and right
resulting tractography was then transformed to Talairach space paracentral lobule in L1 compared to L2, and greater activity in
and averaged to form a thresholded map. To aid visualisation, bilateral superior temporal gyri (STG) in L2 compared to L1.
2 mm Gaussian blurring was applied to ROI masks before being Multiple regression analysis was next performed to identify
how AoA and L2 proficiency predicted differences in activation for
Table 3 L2 versus L1 trials. Note these regions might not overlap with
Clusters of significant activation in L2–L1 contrast at a voxel-wise alpha of p o .001. those identified in the direct L2 versus L1 contrast above because
Contrast Region Talairach coordinates Size
the degree of difference could differ across subjects as a function
(mm3) of either the AoA or proficiency variables. Indeed, as shown in
x y z Table 4, AoA uniquely predicted activity differences in seven areas,
notably the right parahippocampal gyrus, bilateral STG, and the
L1 Mandarin4 L2 R Middle fron- 34 35 26 464
English tal gyrus
bilateral inferior frontal gyrus (Fig. 1A). Conversely, proficiency
R Paracentral 2  41 50 331 uniquely predicted activity in the left parahippocampal gyrus and
lobule the right cingulate gyrus (Table 4 and Fig. 1B). The relationship
L2 English 4L1 L Superior  46  29 9 5499 between AoA and key regions identified by the above analyses is
Mandarin temporal
plotted in Fig. 2A; for all regions except the bilateral STG, early L2
gyrus
R Superior 51  15 7 5005 learners showed greater activation when performing the task in L1
temporal compared to when performing the task in L2. As AoA increased,
gyrus the direction of the difference in activation reversed, and late L2
learners showed greater activation when performing the task in L2
Note. Coordinates denote the location of peak activation. L/R¼ left/right.
compared to when performing the task in L1. Interestingly, in the

Table 4
Clusters of significant activation in multiple regression of AoA and proficiency in L2 English when compared to L1 Mandarin.

Predictor Region Talairach coordinates Size (mm3)

x y z

Age of acquisition L Superior temporal gyrus  37  60 18 4875


R Parahippocampal gyrus  64  14 8 3711
R Superior temporal gyrus 57  13 5 3097
L Uncus 45  54 15 2735
L Inferior frontal gyrus  30 37 6 2420
R Inferior frontal gyrus 28 3  10 1808
R Middle occipital gyrus  24 4 9 1325

Proficiency L Parahippocampal gyrus 41  13 9 1605


R Cingulate gyrus 52  38 19 1498
E.S. Nichols, M.F. Joanisse / NeuroImage 143 (2016) 15–25 19

bilateral STG the difference in activation between L2 and L1 was


largest at early AoAs, with L1 activating more than L2, and ap-
proached zero with increasing AoA.
The relationship between proficiency and key regions identified
by the above analyses is plotted in Fig. 2B. For both areas, low pro-
ficiency L2 speakers showed greater activation when performing the
task in L1 compared to when performing the task in L2. However, as
proficiency increased, the reverse pattern appeared, with high pro-
ficiency L2 speakers showing greater activation when performing
the task in L2 than when performing the task in L1.
It is important to note that when examining proficiency, raw L2
proficiency score was used as a regressor, as opposed to the dif-
ference between L1 and L2 proficiency. However, L2 proficiency
and the L1–L2 proficiency difference are tightly correlated
(r ¼  .81, p o.001). Due to the nature of the values of the differ-
ence between L1 and L2 proficiency, a regression model using the
difference as a regressor was unable to converge, however plotting
the L1–L2 proficiency difference against betas extracted from the
left parahippocampal gyrus and right cingulate confirmed that
smaller differences between L1 and L2 proficiency, i.e., higher L2
Fig. 1. Multiple regression of the difference in activation between the L2 English
versus L1 Mandarin tasks as a function of either (A) AoA or (B) L2 proficiency. proficiency, corresponded to greater activation levels for L2 versus
Statistical maps are thresholded at F ¼4.38, p¼ .05, overlaid on a stereotaxic brain L1 in both areas.
atlas, L ¼ left. Cluster sizes are reported in Table 4.

Fig. 2. Scatterplots indicating A) The relationship between AoA, controlling for proficiency, and beta values for the difference in activation between the L2 English and L1
Mandarin tasks (L2 versus L1 contrast), in each of the regions predicted by AoA. B) The relationship between proficiency, controlling for AoA, and beta values for the L2
versus L1 contrast, in both regions predicted by proficiency. Positive beta values reflects L2 4L1, and negative beta values reflects L14 L2.
20 E.S. Nichols, M.F. Joanisse / NeuroImage 143 (2016) 15–25

3.3. DTI results individuals began learning English at an early age, the level of
proficiency achieved by those individuals was not uniformly high.
To examine how white matter integrity is related to L2 beha- If the relationship were stronger, the independent contribution of
vioural measures, we performed multiple regression on the skele- either variable would have been statistically harder to identify. The
tonised FA data, allowing us to identify regions of white matter weak relationship between the two variables provided us with the
whose FA was uniquely predicted by AoA or proficiency. Results of opportunity to examine proficiency and AoA variables in a way
the multiple regression are shown in Table 5 and Fig. 3. AoA was that was not confounded.
positively associated with FA in four sections of tracts: two in the left During f MRI scanning, individuals performed a lexical proces-
inferior longitudinal fasciculus (ILF), one in the fibres emerging from sing task in either Mandarin (L1) or English (L2). Groupwise ac-
the anterior midbody of the corpus callosum, and one in the arcuate tivity was significantly greater in L1 than in L2 in the right middle
fasciculus. Proficiency was also positively associated with FA in three frontal gyrus (MFG) and the right paracentral lobule. The right
sections of tracts: one in the right ILF, one in the right arcuate fas- MFG has been implicated in a number of processes, including re-
ciculus, and one in the forceps minor of the corpus callosum. cognition memory (Ranganath et al., 2003; Rugg et al., 1996),
We next used probabilistic tractography to compute volumes of which may be higher in L1 than L2. The paracentral lobule is a key
white matter fibres extending from each ROI identified in the above part of the default mode network (Margulies et al., 2009), which
regression analysis. The group averages of the computed volumes for may be more engaged during a language task in which the subject
each ROI are shown in Fig. 4. Two separate networks of white matter is more comfortable. Conversely, groupwise activity was sig-
were identified, with AoA being associated with the bilateral ILF, left nificantly greater in L2 than in L1 in both the left and right su-
corpus callosum, and left arcuate fasciculus. This network was perior temporal gyri, in line with previous studies of spoken word
composed of complimentary right and left hemisphere tracts, with processing in a second language (Dehaene et al., 1997; Kim et al.,
connections between hemispheres. In contrast, proficiency showed 1997; Perani et al., 1998). However, due to the large amount of
an extensive right-lateralised network composed of the inferior variability in our sample, L2–L1 contrasts should be interpreted
longitudinal fasciculus, arcuate fasciculus, and forceps minor, with with caution.
limited projections to the left hemisphere. We next examined individual differences in second language
learning by performing a linear regression analysis on subjectwise
differences in L2 versus L1 activation in which we tested the in-
4. Discussion dependent effects of AoA and proficiency on neural activation. This
approach identified multiple brain regions that were independently
Previous studies disagree as to whether the neural substrates of modulated by either AoA or L2 proficiency. We found that AoA un-
first- versus second-language processing differ due to age of ac- iquely predicted the degree of L2 involvement in the classic lan-
quisition (AoA) or proficiency. In the past, both factors have typi- guage-processing network, namely the bilateral STG and IFG, such
cally been studied independently of one another. Likewise, both that L2 activity relative to L1 activity in these regions increased as a
have typically been treated as discrete rather than continuous function of later AoA. Both the STG and IFG are known to be involved
variables. As such, there has been much inconsistency in the lit- in speech comprehension (Friederici et al., 2003; Grindrod et al.,
erature with respect to the cutoff points for early versus late age of 2008; Myers et al., 2009; Obleser et al., 2006; Scott and Johnsrude,
acquisition, and to low and high proficiency. Thus, results tend to 2003; Warren et al., 2006), with degree of activity in both areas
be difficult to interpret across studies. found to increase as a function of processing demands (Fridriksson
The present study sought to isolate the contribution of both and Morrow, 2005; Noppeney et al., 2004; Obleser et al., 2007;
variables to neural function and white matter microstructure Sharp et al., 2010). Additionally, our results support the finding of
supporting second-language English in native speakers of Man- increased bilateral activation in L2 versus L1 (Centeno et al., 2014;
darin. Of note, individuals in our sample showed a relative de- Park et al., 2012). This was demonstrated by the positive relationship
coupling of the two variables of interest such that their AoA did between AoA and activation in all areas identified in the regression,
not significantly correlate with English proficiency level. This is suggesting that as AoA increases, there is additional recruitment of
likely due to how English as a second language is taught in right-hemisphere areas in addition to the typical left-hemisphere
mainland China, which provides minimal opportunity to use areas. Thus, the increased activity observed in the present study may
English outside of the educational curriculum. Indeed, the popu- reflect more effortful processing within basic speech perception
lation studied here was not fully immersed in their L2 from the mechanisms as AoA increases.
time of first exposure (and perhaps not ever) and consequently AoA also modulated activity in the right parahippocampal gyrus.
their proficiency outcomes varied significantly compared to po- The left parahippocampal gyrus has been implicated in semantic
pulations where this is the case. Thus while many of these memory and retrieval (Binder et al., 1997), and it is possible that

Table 5
Clusters of white matter in which FA was predicted by AoA and proficiency in Mandarin–English L2 speakers.

Predictor Region Talairach coordinates Size (mm3)

x y z

Age of acquisition L Corpus callosum  12 20 49 21


L Arcuate fasciculus  38 8 36 18
L Inferior longitudinal fasciculus  34  63 25 9
R Inferior longitudinal fasciculus 28  77 30 9

Proficiency R Inferior longitudinal fasciculus 44  24  15 10


R Arcuate fasciculus 8  13 13 10
R Forceps minor 36 48 1 8
E.S. Nichols, M.F. Joanisse / NeuroImage 143 (2016) 15–25 21

Fig. 3. Multiple regression of the skeletonised FA values as a function of either (A) AoA or (B) L2 proficiency. Regions explaining significant variance are shown in red. To aid
visualisation, ROI masks were smoothed to 2 mm before being overlaid on the averaged FA skeleton. Statistical maps are thresholded at F¼ 14.92, p¼ .001 (corrected),
overlaid on a stereotaxic brain atlas, L¼ left. Cluster sizes are reported in Table 5.

AoA-related maturational constraints lead to increased recruitment index of subsequent proficiency differences between early and late
of right parahippocampal areas for semantic retrieval of L2. Indeed, L2 learners.
the right parahippocampal gyrus has been implicated in L2 proces- Our design also allowed us to similarly examine the influence
sing, and found to be present in high proficiency late L2 learners of proficiency independent of AoA, yielding a complementary
while processing their L2 but not their L1 (Perani et al., 1998). pattern of results in the left parahippocampal gyrus and right
The positive relationship between AoA and level of activation in cingulate gyrus. At lower proficiencies, activity in L2 was less than
the language-processing network also supports previous research activity in L1; however, as proficiency increased, activity in L2
showing increased activation in late learners (Isel et al., 2010; Kim became greater than activity in L1. One interpretation is that
et al., 1997; Hernandez and Li, 2007; Wartenburger et al., 2003). lower-proficiency L2 speakers will tend to rely less on brain re-
FMRI effects that are attributed to AoA could reflect proficiency gions involved L1 language processing. Indeed, Chee et al. (2004)
which is often confounded with AoA. Because proficiency was found increased left insula activation in high proficiency L2
explicitly controlled for in our analyses of AoA, we argue that ac- speakers compared to low proficiency speakers, even though both
tivation differences predicted by AoA reflect age-dependent effects groups performed equally on a phonological working memory
of plasticity for spoken word processing, rather than simply an task. Thus, greater engagement of language-related areas may
22 E.S. Nichols, M.F. Joanisse / NeuroImage 143 (2016) 15–25

Fig. 4. Probabilistic tractography based on white matter seeds identified by multiple regression (see Fig. 3 and Table 5), representing the independent contribution of AoA
(A) and L2 proficiency (B). Tractography is overlaid on a stereotaxic brain atlas, L ¼ left.

support enhanced, rather than more effortful, L2 processing, al- To address this, we located regions of white matter where the FA
lowing individuals to become more proficient (Callan et al., 2003, values were independently predicted by AoA or proficiency. We
2004). Although it is difficult to determine whether greater ac- also computed white matter tracts extending from these ROIs as
tivity reflects effort or effectiveness, the positive correlation be- an indication of the structural pathways involved in second lan-
tween proficiency and L2–L1 difference shown in Fig. 2B supports guage processing. The results revealed that FA was positively
this conclusion, showing that increasing proficiency predicts correlated with AoA in one section of the left corpus callosum,
greater L2 activation than L1 activation in both the right cingulate which has been previously found to be larger in bilinguals than
gyrus and the left parahippocampal gyrus. monolinguals (Coggins et al., 2004), although Mohades et al.
Much less is known about how L2 ability is reflected in the (2012) failed to find differences in FA in this tract. FA was also
microstructure of white matter tracts supporting language. While positively correlated with AoA in the left arcuate fasciculus, which
some studies have observed differences in white matter for contains bidirectional connections between IFG and STG (i.e.,
monolinguals versus bilinguals (Coggins et al., 2004; Cummine Broca's area and Wernicke's area), and in the bilateral ILF, which
and Boliek, 2013; Luk et al., 2011; Mohades et al., 2012), it remains connects occipital regions of cortex to inferior temporal regions of
an open question whether these differences are influenced by the cortex through ventral pathways. This supports the present f MRI
age at which a L2 was learned, or the relative proficiency that an results, which found increased activation with increasing AoA that
individual has achieved in their dominant versus second language. included the left IFG and STG, demonstrating that both local
E.S. Nichols, M.F. Joanisse / NeuroImage 143 (2016) 15–25 23

activity and between-region connectivity are involved in L2 ac- independent of AoA. These results indicate that neuroanatomical
quisition and processing. The present results also support the models of L2 processing must account for both AoA and profi-
presence of a dual route in language connectivity containing both ciency, with age-dependent and experience-dependent plasticity
dorsal and ventral regions (Duffau et al., 2005; Frey et al., 2008), affecting the L2 processing network and driving differences be-
demonstrating that these pathways are affected by the age at tween L1 and L2.
which a second language is learned. The present study currently leaves open the nature of the re-
FA positively correlated with proficiency in sections of the right lationship between proficiency and white matter integrity. While
ILF, arcuate fasciculus, and forceps minor of the corpus callosum, we only identified tract regions showing a positive association
consistent with f MRI findings of greater right-hemisphere in- between FA and proficiency, the direction of causation remains
volvement linked to L2 proficiency (Chee et al., 2001; Dehaene unclear. The association may relate to the reinforcement of white
et al., 1997; Wang et al., 2011). All three tracts have previously matter tracts as a result of more frequent practice and/or greater
been tied to bilingualism (Mohades et al., 2012; Luk et al., 2011), L2 exposure – that is, all individuals might possess a similar initial
although results have been variable. While a cross-sectional study state of white matter coherence, but those who are generally
of bilingual and monolingual children found higher FA in the predisposed to being better second language learners will subse-
anterior portion of the corpus callosum in monolinguals (Mohades quently develop more coherent white matter tracts over time.
et al., 2012), these group differences were no longer present two Alternatively, it might be that higher proficiency is itself a direct
years later (Mohades et al., 2015). In contrast, Luk et al. (2011) consequence of superior pre-existing white matter integrity in
found higher FA in the corpus callosum in lifelong bilinguals areas required for learning an L2, such that higher white matter
compared to monolinguals. Similarly, Mohades et al. (2012), (2015) integrity individuals are predisposed to become more successful
found no differences between monolinguals and bilinguals in the L2 learners. We expect that longitudinal designs that better ac-
arcuate fasciculus, while Luk and colleagues found increased FA in count for the independent contributions of AoA and proficiency
both the SLF and ILF of lifelong bilinguals relative to monolinguals. will shed more light on this distinction.
Although these studies did not examine proficiency and AoA, the One potential concern about our design is also worth noting
present study suggests that differences in white matter micro- here: because the two languages were presented in interleaved
structure between bilinguals and monolinguals may be in part due alternating runs during scanning, it is possible that the differences
to both age effects and proficiency levels in L2. in proficiency are inflated due to language switching. For instance
We propose that these positive correlations reflect the differing individuals with greater language proficiency might also have had
and independent contributions of AoA and proficiency to L2 pro- greater facility in alternating between English and Mandarin from
cessing. The most intuitive finding is of a positive relationship one run to the next, thereby influencing the results. However, this
between proficiency and tract FA, such that higher proficiency in a was not a rapid language-switching paradigm, rather each run
second language promotes higher white matter integrity among consisted of 3.5 min of trials in a single language. Likewise, sub-
brain regions supporting second-language learning. In contrast, jects were given breaks between languages and informed of which
the positive relationship between AoA and FA is perhaps less in- language was upcoming. Based on this, we argue that the alter-
tuitive, however it suggests that older learners rely more heavily nating runs paradigm was likely not problematic, although we
on the bilateral ILF, left corpus callosum, and left arcuate fasciculus cannot rule out its influence on the results. In addition, it should
to compensate for a late AoA. Indeed, higher FA values have been be noted that due to the limited nature of the f MRI task, the
found in the corpus callosum of bilingual individuals who acquired functional conclusions cannot be extended beyond the current
their two languages sequentially relative to simultaneously (Mo- paradigm.
hades et al., 2012). This positive relationship between AoA and FA
is in agreement with the present f MRI findings in which later AoA 4.1. Conclusions
led to an increase in activity, again suggesting increased effort or
usage of areas related to L2 processing. The goal of the present study was to examine both functional
Probabilistic tractography allowed us to determine the network and structural correlates of second language processing as a
of tracts supporting the bilingual brain. Two separate sets white function of age of acquisition and proficiency. Two separate net-
matter tracts emerged, with those underlying AoA showing com- works of brain regions and white matter tracts predicted by these
plimentary tracts extending anterior-posterior in each hemisphere two variables emerged, confirming that both play a role in how the
and connecting inter-hemispherically. In contrast, areas of FA that brain supports two languages.
correlated with proficiency were right-lateralised, thus the net- These findings help to integrate previous results concerning the
work revealed by probabilistic tractography was also generally functional and anatomical correlates of L2 learning. This is the first
right-lateralised, although the tracks also projected contralaterally study to map how AoA and proficiency independently explain
through anterior portions of the corpus callosum. We propose that neural differences in second language. Of note, treating both AoA
the maturational constraints reflected here by AoA modulate and proficiency as continuous variables also overcomes the con-
whole-brain white matter connections involved in L2 acquisition cern with experimental designs that attempt to divide individuals
and processing, while proficiency instead primarily modulates into binary groups such as ‘early’ versus ‘late’ L2 learners, and
connections in the right hemisphere. This is supported by previous ‘high’ versus ‘low’ proficiencies. Inconsistencies in the literature
research showing that AoA predicts more widespread modulations regarding where to draw the divide between early and late AoA
in the brain than proficiency, while proficiency tends to result in and high and low proficiency have made it difficult to compare
the recruitment of right-hemisphere areas (Chee et al., 2001; De- between studies. The current approach provides a clearer de-
haene et al., 1997; Indefrey, 2006; Kim et al., 1997; Wang et al., scription as to how the brain is affected by these two variables.
2011). This is also the first study we know of to merge measures of
The results help to inform present theories of why neural sig- grey and white matter organisation to address this important
natures of L2 differ from those of L1. We identified a network of question. Additionally, there are few studies using DTI to compare
regions in which differences in L1–L2 processing are predicted by monolinguals and bilinguals, and the results appear to be con-
AoA independently of proficiency. However, we also identified two flicting, in that they have yielded both positive and negative as-
regions, the right cingulate gyrus and the left parahippocampal sociations between white matter FA and L2 ability. The results of
gyrus, whose L1–L2 differences were predicted by proficiency the current study indicate that both functional activity and white
24 E.S. Nichols, M.F. Joanisse / NeuroImage 143 (2016) 15–25

matter integrity play a role in L2 learning, that the association org/10.1006/cbmr.1996.0014.


might be either positive or negative depending on the brain region Cummine, J., Boliek, C.A., 2013. Understanding white matter integrity stability for
bilinguals on language status and reading performance. Brain Struct. Funct. 218,
in question, and that ultimate success in L2 learning hinges both 595–601. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s00429-012-0466-6.
on age-dependent and age-independent factors. Dehaene, S., Dupoux, E., Mehler, J., Cohen, L., Paulesu, E., Perani, D., van de Moor-
tele, P.F., Lehéricy, S., Le Bihan, D., 1997. Anatomical variability in the cortical
representation of first and second language. Neuroreport 8, 3809–3815. http:
//dx.doi.org/10.1097/00001756-199712010-00030.
Funding Dräger, B., Breitenstein, C., Helmke, U., Kamping, S., Knecht, S., 2004. Specific and
nonspecific effects of transcranial magnetic stimulation on picture-word ver-
ification. E. J. Neurosci. 20 (6), 1681–1687. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/
This work was supported by a Discovery Grant and Accelerator j.1460-9568.2004.03623.x.
Award from the Natural Sciences and Engineering Research Council. Duffau, H., Gatignol, P., Mandonnet, E., Peruzzi, P., Tzourio-Mazoyer, N., Capelle, L.,
2005. New insights into the anatomo-functional connectivity of the semantic
system: a study using cortico-subcortical electrostimulations. Brain 128,
797–810. http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/brain/awh423.
Acknowledgements Frey, S., Campbell, J.S., Pike, G.B., Petrides, M., 2008. Dissociating the human lan-
guage pathways with high angular resolution diffusion fiber tractography. J.
Neurosci. 28, 11435–11444, doi:28/45/11435 [pii]/r10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2388-
We are grateful to Yi Xie for providing help with Mandarin 08.2008.
translations. Fridriksson, J., Morrow, L., 2005. Cortical activation and language task difficulty in
aphasia. Aphasiology 19, 239–250. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/
02687030444000714.
Friederici, A.D., Rüschemeyer, S.-A., Hahne, A., Fiebach, C.J., 2003. The role of left
Appendix A. Supporting information inferior frontal and superior temporal cortex in sentence comprehension: lo-
calizing syntactic and semantic processes. Cereb. Cortex 13, 170–177. http://dx.
doi.org/10.1093/cercor/13.2.170.
Supplementary data associated with this article can be found in the Fromkin, V., Krashen, S., Curtiss, S., Rigler, D., Rigler, M., 1974. The development of
online version at http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2016.08.053. language in genie: a case of language acquisition beyond the “critical period.”.
Brain Lang. 1, 81–107.
Frost, R., Siegelman, N., Narkiss, A., Afek, L., 2013. What predicts successful literacy
acquisition in a second language? Psychol. Sci. 24, 1243–1252. http://dx.doi.org/
References 10.1177/0956797612472207.
Gardner, R.C., Masgoret, A.M., Tennant, J., Mihic, L., 2004. Integrative motivation:
changes during a year-long intermediate-level language course. Lang. Learn. 54,
Abutalebi, J., 2008. Neural aspects of second language representation and language 1–34. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9922.2004.00247.x.
control. Acta Psychol. 128, 466–478. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j. Grindrod, C.M., Bilenko, N.Y., Myers, E.B., Blumstein, S.E., 2008. The role of the left
actpsy.2008.03.014. inferior frontal gyrus in implicit semantic competition and selection: an event-
Andersson, J.L.R., Jenkinson, M., Smith, S., 2010. Non-linear registration, aka spatial related f MRI study. Brain Res. 1229, 167–178. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.
normalisation. FMRIB Technical Report TR07JA2. brainres.2008.07.017.
Baayen, R., Piepenbrock, R., Gulikers, L., 1995. CELEX2 LDC96L14. Web Download. Hernandez, A.E., Li, P., 2007. Age of acquisition: Its neural and computational me-
Linguistic Data Consortium, Philadelphia. chanisms. Psychol. Bull. 133, 638–650. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/
Bernaus, M., Gardner, R.C., 2008. Teacher motivation strategies, student percep- 0033-2909.133.4.638.
tions, student motivation, and english achievement. Mod. Lang. J. 92, 388–401. Indefrey, P., 2006. A meta-analysis of hemodynamic studies on first and second
http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1540-4781.2008.00753.x. language processing: which suggested differences can we trust and what do
Binder, J.R., Frost, J.A., Hammeke, T.A., Cox, R.W., Rao, S.M., Prieto, T., 1997. Human they mean? Lang. Learn. 56, 279–304. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/
brain language areas identified by functional magnetic resonance imaging. J. j.1467-9922.2006.00365.x.
Neurosci. 17, 353–362. Isel, F., Baumgaertner, A., Thran, J., Meisel, J.M., Buchel, C., 2010. Neural circuitry of
Breining, B.L., Faria, A.V., Hillis, A.E., 2014. Neural regions supporting lexical pro- the bilingual mental lexicon: effect of age of second language acquisition. Brain
cessing of objects and actions: a case series analysis. In: Conference Abstract: Cognit. 72, 169–180. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.bandc.2009.07.008.
Academy of Aphasia – 52nd Annual Meeting. Front. Psychol. http://dx.doi.org/ Kim, K.H., Relkin, N.R., Lee, K.M., Hirsch, J., 1997. Distinct cortical areas associated
10.3389/conf.fpsyg.2014.64.00030. with native and second languages. Nature 388, 171–174. http://dx.doi.org/
Cai, Q., Brysbaert, M., 2010. SUBTLEX-CH: Chinese word and character frequencies 10.1038/40623.
based on film subtitles. PLoS One 5, e10729. http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal. Klein, D., Mok, K., Chen, J.K., Watkins, K.E., 2013. Age of language learning shapes
pone.0010729. brain structure: a cortical thickness study of bilingual and monolingual in-
Callan, D.E., Jones, J.A., Callan, A.M., Akahane-Yamada, R., 2004. Phonetic perceptual dividuals. Brain Lang. 131, 20–24. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.bandl.2013.05.014.
identification by native- and second-language speakers differentially activates Lambon Ralph, M.A., Patterson, K., 2008. Generalization and differentiation in se-
brain regions involved with acoustic phonetic processing and those involved mantic memory. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 1124, 61–76. http://dx.doi.org/10.1196/
with articulatory-auditory/orosensory internal models. Neuroimage 22, annals.1440.006.
1182–1194. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.03.006. Li, P., Legault, J., Litcofsky, K.A., 2014. Neuroplasticity as a function of second lan-
Callan, D.E., Tajima, K., Callan, A.M., Kubo, R., Masaki, S., Akahane-Yamada, R., 2003. guage learning: anatomical changes in the human brain. Cortex 58, 301–324.
Learning-induced neural plasticity associated with improved identification http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cortex.2014.05.001.
performance after training of a difficult second-language phonetic contrast. Luk, G., Bialystok, E., Craik, F.I.M., Grady, C.L., 2011. Lifelong bilingualism maintains
Neuroimage 19, 113–124. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S1053-8119(03)00020-X. white matter integrity in older adults. J. Neurosci. 31, 16808–16813. http://dx.
Centeno, M., Koepp, M.J., Vollmar, C., Stretton, J., Sidhu, M., Michallef, C., Symms, M. doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4563-11.2011.
R., Thompson, P.J., Duncan, J.S., 2014. Language dominance assessment in a Margulies, D.S., Vincent, J.L., Kelly, C., Lohmann, G., Uddin, L.Q., Biswal, B.B., Vill-
bilingual population: validity of f MRI in the second language. Epilepsia 55, ringer, A., Castellanos, F.X., Milham, M.P., Petrides, M., 2009. Precuneus shares
1504–1511. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/epi.12757. intrinsic functional architecture in humans and monkeys. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci.
Chee, M.W., Hon, N., Lee, H.L., Soon, C.S., 2001. Relative language proficiency USA 106, 20069–20074. http://dx.doi.org/10.1073/pnas.0905314106.
modulates BOLD signal change when bilinguals perform semantic judgments. Mechelli, A., Crinion, J.T., Noppeney, U., O’Doherty, J., Ashburner, J., Frackowiak, R.S.,
Neuroimage 13, 1155–1163. http://dx.doi.org/10.1006/nimg.2001.0781. Price, C.J., 2004. Structural plasticity in the bilingual brain. Nature 431, 757.
Chee, M.W., Tan, E.W., Thiel, T., 1999. Mandarin and English single word processing http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/nature03017.
studied with functional magnetic resonance imaging. J. Neurosci. 19, Mohades, S.G., Struys, E., Van Schuerbeek, P., Mondt, K., Van De Craen, P., Luypaert,
3050–3056. R., 2012. DTI reveals structural differences in white matter tracts between bi-
Chee, M.W.L., Soon, C.S., Lee, H.L., Pallier, C., 2004. Left insula activation: a marker lingual and monolingual children. Brain Res. 1435, 72–80. http://dx.doi.org/
for language attainment in bilinguals. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 101, 10.1016/j.brainres.2011.12.005.
15265–15270. http://dx.doi.org/10.1073/pnas.0403703101. Mohades, S.G., Van Schuerbeek, P., Rosseel, Y., Van De Craen, P., Luypaert, R., Bae-
Coccia, M., Bartolini, M., Luzzi, S., Provinciali, L., Ralph, M. a L., 2004. Semantic ken, C., 2015. White-matter development is different in bilingual and mono-
memory is an amodal, dynamic system: Evidence from the interaction of lingual children: a longitudinal DTI study. PLoS One 10, 1–17. http://dx.doi.org/
naming and object use in semantic dementia. Cognit. Neuropsychol. 21, 10.1371/journal.pone.0117968.
513–527. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/02643290342000113. Myers, E.B., Blumstein, S.E., Walsh, E., Eliassen, J., 2009. Inferior frontal regions
Coggins, P.E., Kennedy, T.J., Armstrong, T.A., 2004. Bilingual corpus callosum underlie the perception of phonetic category invariance. Psychol. Sci. 20,
variability. Brain Lang. 89, 69–75. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0093-934X(03) 895–903. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.2009.02380.x.
00299-2. Newman, A.J., Tremblay, A., Nichols, E.S., Neville, H.J., Ullman, M.T., 2012. The in-
Cox, R.W., 1996. AFNI: software for analysis and visualization of functional mag- fluence of language proficiency on lexical semantic processing in native and
netic resonance neuroimages. Comput. Biomed. Res. 29, 162–173. http://dx.doi. late learners of English. J. Cognit. Neurosci. 24, 1205–1223.
E.S. Nichols, M.F. Joanisse / NeuroImage 143 (2016) 15–25 25

Noppeney, U., Phillips, J., Price, C., 2004. The neural areas that control the retrieval Sahinkarakas, S., 2011. Young students' success and failure attributions in language
and selection of semantics. Neuropsychologia 42, 1269–1280. http://dx.doi.org/ learning. Soc. Behav. Personal.: Int. J. 39, 879–885. http://dx.doi.org/10.2224/
10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2003.12.014. sbp.2011.39.7.879.
Obleser, J., Boecker, H., Drzezga, A., Haslinger, B., Hennenlotter, A., Roettinger, M., Scott, S.K., Johnsrude, I.S., 2003. The neuroanatomical and functional organization
Eulitz, C., Rauschecker, J.P., 2006. Vowel sound extraction in anterior superior of speech perception. Trends Neurosci. 26, 100–107. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/
temporal cortex. Hum. Brain Mapp. 27, 562–571. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/ S0166-2236(02)00037-1.
hbm.20201. Sharp, D.J., Awad, M., Warren, J.E., Wise, R.J.S., Vigliocco, G., Scott, S.K., 2010. The
Obleser, J., Wise, R.J.S., Alex Dresner, M., Scott, S.K., 2007. Functional integration neural response to changing semantic and perceptual complexity during lan-
across brain regions improves speech perception under adverse listening guage processing. Hum. Brain Mapp. 31, 365–377. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/
conditions. J. Neurosci. 27, 2283–2289. http://dx.doi.org/10.1523/ hbm.20871.
JNEUROSCI.4663-06.2007. Smith, S.M., Jenkinson, M., Woolrich, M.W., Beckmann, C.F., Behrens, T.E.J., Jo-
Oldfield, R.C., 1971. The assessment and analysis of handedness: the Edinburgh hansen-Berg, H., Bannister, P.R., De Luca, M., Drobnjak, I., Flitney, D.E., Niazy, R.
inventory. Neuropsychologia 9, 97–113. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0028-3932 K., Saunders, J., Vickers, J., Zhang, Y., De Stefano, N., Brady, J.M., Matthews, P.M.,
(71)90067-4. 2004. Advances in functional and structural MR image analysis and im-
Pakulak, E., Neville, H.J., 2011. Maturational constraints on the recruitment of early plementation as FSL. Neuroimage 23 (Suppl 1), S208–19. http://dx.doi.org/
processes for syntactic processing. J. Cognit. Neurosci. 23, 2752–2765. http://dx.
10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.07.051.
doi.org/10.1162/jocn.2010.21586.
Smith, S.M., 2002. Fast robust automated brain extraction. Hum. Brain Mapp. 17,
Park, H.R.P., Badzakova-Trajkov, G., Waldie, K.E., 2012. Language lateralisation in
143–155. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/hbm.10062.
late proficient bilinguals: a lexical decision f MRI study. Neuropsychologia 50,
Smith, S.M., Jenkinson, M., Johansen-Berg, H., Rueckert, D., Nichols, T.E., Mackay, C.
688–695. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2012.01.005.
E., Watkins, K.E., Ciccarelli, O., Cader, M.Z., Matthews, P.M., Behrens, T.E.J., 2006.
Patterson, K., Nestor, P.J., Rogers, T.T., 2007. Where do you know what you know?
Tract-based spatial statistics: voxelwise analysis of multi-subject diffusion data.
The representation of semantic knowledge in the human brain. Nat. Rev.
Neuroimage 31, 1487–1505. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.
Neurosci. 8, 976–987. http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/nrn2277.
neuroimage.2006.02.024.
Perani, D., Abutalebi, J., 2005. The neural basis of first and second language pro-
Talairach, J., Tournoux, P., 1988. Co-planar stereotaxic atlas of the human brain. In:
cessing. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 15, 202–206. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.
conb.2005.03.007. 3-Dimensional Proportional System: An Approach to Cerebral Imaging.
Perani, D., Dehaene, S., Grassi, F., Cohen, L., Cappa, S.F., Dupoux, E., Fazio, F., Mehler, Wang, Y., Xiang, J., Vannest, J., Holroyd, T., Narmoneva, D., Horn, P., Liu, Y., Rose, D.,
J., 1996. Brain processing of native and foreign languages. Neuroreport 7, deGrauw, T., Holland, S., 2011. Neuromagnetic measures of word processing in
2439–2444. bilinguals and monolinguals. Clin. Neurophysiol. 122, 1706–1717. http://dx.doi.
Perani, D., Paulesu, E., Galles, N.S., Dupoux, E., Dehaene, S., Bettinardi, V., Cappa, S.F., org/10.1016/j.clinph.2011.02.008.
Fazio, F., Mehler, J., 1998. The bilingual brain proficiency and age of acquisition Warren, J.D., Scott, S.K., Price, C.J., Griffiths, T.D., 2006. Human brain mechanisms for
of the second language. Brain 121, 1841–1852. the early analysis of voices. Neuroimage 31, 1389–1397. http://dx.doi.org/
Ranganath, C., Johnson, M.K., D’Esposito, M., 2003. Prefrontal activity associated 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.01.034.
with working memory and episodic long-term memory. Neuropsychologia 41, Wartenburger, I., Heekeren, H.R., Abutalebi, J., Cappa, S.F., Villringer, A., Perani, D.,
378–389. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0028-3932(02)00169-0. Olgettina, V., 2003. Early setting of grammatical processing in the bilingual
Rueckert, D., Sonoda, L.I., Hayes, C., Hill, D.L., Leach, M.O., Hawkes, D.J., 1999. brain. Neuron 37, 159–170.
Nonrigid registration using free-form deformations: application to breast MR Weber-Fox, C.M., Neville, H.J., 1996. Maturational constraints on functional spe-
images. IEEE Trans. Med. Imaging 18, 712–721. http://dx.doi.org/10.1109/ cializations for language processing: ERP and behavioral evidence in bilingual
42.796284. speakers. J. Cognit. Neurosci. http://dxdoi.org/10.1162/jocn.1996.8.3.231.
Rugg, M.D., Fletcher, P.C., Frith, C.D., Frackowiak, R.S., Dolan, R.J., 1996. Differential Weniger, D., Crelier, G.R., Alkadhi, H., Kollias, S.S., 2000. Picture-word matching as a
activation of the prefrontal cortex in successful and unsuccessful memory re- paradigm in determining regions of language processing: an f MRI study.
trieval. Brain 119, 2073–2083. http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/brain/119.6.2073. Neuroimage 11, S314. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S1053-8119(00)91246-1.

You might also like