You are on page 1of 62

The Neurology of Consciousness:

Cognitive Neuroscience and


Neuropathology 2nd Edition ■ Ebook
PDF Version
Visit to download the full and correct content document:
https://ebookmass.com/product/the-neurology-of-consciousness-cognitive-neuroscien
ce-and-neuropathology-2nd-edition-ebook-pdf-version/
THE NEUROLOGY OF CONSCIOUSNESS
SECOND EDITION
xii FOREWORD

Observations such as those described above have to classify and categorize the different phenomena
also led to a conflation of the phenomenon of attention associated with consciousness and conscious access
with that of consciousness. Some suggest that aware- and at least make an attempt to link them together.
ness and perception must interact to provide a The difficulty is that the more the problem of con-
reportable sensory phenomenon or motor action. For sciousness is investigated, the more one realizes that it
instance, there are those who believe that the defining is a difficult problem indeed, involving many func-
characteristic or evidence that can be obtained for a tions and many levels of brain organization, each inter-
conscious event is that it is reportable to the self or acting with others in unexplored and unexpected
others. ways. There is a view—which is not shared by all—
The notion of conscious level has proved useful in that no global synthesis will be possible without an
clinical medicine, largely because of its prognostic understanding of the molecular, cellular, neural, and
value. Much recent work with comatose patients has physiological principles that govern the various spatial
led to elaboration of schemata of different levels in the and temporal levels of brain organization. In the study
continuum between complete loss of consciousness of consciousness we may be living at a time analogous
and wakefulness. These constructs deal with the state to that which, with the rise of molecular biology,
of awareness in addition to states of access to the con- started the process of understanding how living organ-
tents of consciousness and responses to them. The isms are constituted some 50 years ago. “Life” was dif-
interaction between them remains one of the mysteries ficult to define then and “vitalist” theories were still
of the complex set of brain functions we continue to common. This is no longer the case today. The intro-
call consciousness. These recent advances in classifying duction of a diversity of novel methods and theoretical
states of impaired consciousness also take into account paradigms produced new knowledge about bacterial
the notion of local and global impairments of con- cells and subsequently about eukaryotic cells too,
sciousness. They have used a novel type of report that despite the continuing absence of a satisfactory a priori
itself rests on recordings of brain physiology. The general definition of what “life” is. A similar process
reproduction of patterns of regionally distributed brain may be in the process of illuminating what conscious-
activity when people imagine complex behavior is ness and conscious access are.
compared to that found in people who cannot commu- The editors and contributors of this excellent text
nicate by normal means, when they are invited to combine most of the knowledge and data that are cur-
imagine the same behavior. Correspondence has been rently available—from normality through disease. For
found in sufficient cases to suggest that there are con- that they are to be congratulated. Federating this infor-
scious processes at work in some partially damaged mation is a prerequisite to integrating it into more gen-
brains that are capable of integrating various types of eral processes, which is one of many steps we need to
information and relating them to memories. These provide a better understanding of biological mechan-
observations and their clinical consequences turn con- isms engaged in conscious processing.
sciousness, at least in part, into a social phenomenon
with strong links to communication.
Richard Frackowiak
It is becoming clear to the reader that taking the
Centre Hospitalier Universitaire Vaudois,
human neuroscience perspective, normal or pathologi-
University of Lausanne and Ecole Polytechnique
cal, the word consciousness remains ambiguous and
Fédérale de Lausanne, Lausanne, Switzerland
so a book that dissects phenomena related to con-
sciousness into individual components is welcome. Jean-Pierre Changeux
What will it require to put the facts about each element Pasteur Institute and Collège de France,
together? It is true there are already models that help Paris, France
Preface

Thinking must never submit itself, neither to a dogma, nor years; and the apparition of the earth’s first simple ani-
to a party, nor to a passion, nor to an interest, nor to a precon-
mals to about 600 million years. Natural selection, as
ceived idea, nor to anything whatsoever, except to the facts
themselves, because for it to submit to anything else would be
revealed by Charles Darwin (1809 1882) then gave
the end of its existence. Henri Poincaré (1854 1912) rise to nervous systems as complex as the human
brain, arguably the most complex object in the uni-
‘Truth is sought for its own sake. And those who verse. And, somehow, through the interactions among
are engaged upon the quest for anything for its own its 100 billion neurons, connected by trillions of synap-
sake are not interested in other things. Finding the ses, emerges our conscious experience of the world
truth is difficult, and the road to it is rough.’ wrote Ibn and of ourselves.
al-Haytham (965 1039), a pioneer of the scientific The study of consciousness has remained within the
method. This book addresses one of the biggest chal- scope of philosophy for millennia. Recent empirical
lenges of science; understanding the biological basis of evidence from functional neuroimaging offers a new
human consciousness. It does so through observation way to investigate the mind body conundrum. It also
and experimentation in neurological patients, formu- gives new opportunities to the neurological commu-
lating hypotheses about the neural correlates of nity to improve our understanding and management
consciousness and employing an objective and repro- of patients with disorders of consciousness. This sec-
ducible methodology. This scientific method, as first ond edition of The Neurology of Consciousness aims at
proposed by Isaac Newton (1643 1727), has proven revising our understanding of the anatomical and
utterly successful in replacing Dark Age ‘magical functional underpinnings of human consciousness by
thinking’ with an intelligent, rational understanding of emphasizing a lesion approach through the study of
nature. Scientific methodology, however, also requires neurological patients. This second edition seems criti-
imagination and creativity. For instance, methodologi- cal to us as numerous recent findings and seminal arti-
cally well-described experiments allowed Louis cles have been published since the first edition of the
Pasteur (1822 1895) to reject the millennia-old book in 2009. The different chapters review the map-
Aristotelian (384 322 BC) view that living organisms ping of conscious perception and cognition in health
could spontaneously arise from non-living matter. (e.g., wakefulness, sleep, dreaming, sleepwalking and
Pasteur’s observations and genius gave rise to the anaesthesia) and in disease (e.g., post-comatose states,
germ theory of disease, which would lead to the use of seizures, split-brains, neglect, amnesia, dementia, and
antiseptics and antibiotics, saving innumerable lives. so on).
The progress of science also largely depends upon ‘A genuine glimpse into what consciousness is
the invention and improvement of technology and would be the scientific achievement, before which all
instruments. For example, the big breakthroughs of past achievements would pale’ wrote William James in
Galileo Galilei (1564 1642) were made possible thanks 1899. Testable hypotheses on consciousness, even if
to eyeglass makers’ improvements in lens-grinding still far away from solving all problems related to the
techniques, which permitted the construction of his neural substrate of consciousness, give us such a
telescopes. Similarly, advances in engineering led to glimpse. In our view, scientific and technological
space observatories such as the Hubble Telescope advances complemented by an adequate theoretical
shedding light on where we come from. Rigorous sci- framework will ultimately lead to an understanding of
entific measurements permitted to trace back the the neural substrate of consciousness.
beginning of the universe to nearly 14 billion years; the We thank our funding agencies including the
age of the earth to more than 4.5 billion years; the ori- National Institutes of Health, the European Commission,
gin of life on earth to (very) approximately 3.5 billion the McDonnell Foundation, the Mind Science

xiii
xiv PREFACE

Foundation Texas, the Belgian National Funds for School of Medicine and Public Health. We learned a lot
Scientific Research (FNRS), the French Speaking while working on this second edition of The Neurology of
Community Concerted Research Action, the Queen Consciousness and we hope you do too while reading it.
Elizabeth Medical Foundation, the Belgian American
Education Foundation, the Wallonie-Bruxelles Steven Laureys (Liège),
International, Liège Sart Tilman University Hospital, the Olivia Gosseries (Liège and Madison),
University of Liège and the University of Wisconsin and Giulio Tononi (Madison)
List of Contributors

Selma Aybek Laboratory for Behavioral Neurology and Nathan Faivre Laboratory of Cognitive Neuroscience, Brain
Imaging of Cognition, Department of Neurosciences and Mind Institute, School of Life Sciences, École Polytechnique
Clinic of Neurology, University Medical Center, Geneva, Fédérale de Lausanne, Lausanne, Switzerland; Center
Switzerland for Neuroprosthetics, École Polytechnique Fédérale de
Claudio L. Bassetti Department of Neurology, University Lausanne, Lausanne, Switzerland
Hospital, Bern, Switzerland Joseph J. Fins Weill Cornell Medical College and the
Olaf Blanke Laboratory of Cognitive Neuroscience, Brain Rockefeller University, New York, NY, and Yale Law
Mind Institute, School of Life Sciences, École School, New Haven, CT, USA
Polytechnique Fédérale de Lausanne, Lausanne, Pascal Fries Ernst Strüngmann Institute for Neuroscience
Switzerland; Center for Neuroprosthetics, École in Cooperation with Max-Planck-Society, Frankfurt,
Polytechnique Fédérale de Lausanne, Lausanne, Germany; Donders Institute for Brain, Cognition and
Switzerland; Department of Neurology, University Behaviour, Radboud University Nijmegen, Nijmegen, The
Hospital, Geneva, Switzerland Netherlands
Hal Blumenfeld Departments of Neurology, Neurobiology, Michael S. Gazzaniga Dynamical Neuroscience, University
and Neurosurgery, Yale University School of Medicine, of California, Santa Barbara, CA, USA; Psychological and
New Haven, CT, USA Brain Sciences, University of California, Santa Barbara,
Melanie Boly Department of Psychiatry, University of CA, USA
Wisconsin-Madison, Madison, WI, USA; Department of Joseph T. Giacino Department of Physical Medicine and
Neurology, University of Wisconsin-Madison, Madison, Rehabilitation, Spaulding Rehabilitation Hospital and
WI, USA Harvard Medical School, Boston, MA, USA
Marie-Aurélie Bruno Coma Science Group, Neurology Olivia Gosseries Department of Psychiatry, University of
Department and GIGA, University of Liège, Liège, Wisconsin-Madison, Madison, WI, USA; Coma Science
Belgium Group, Neurology Department and GIGA, University of
Liège, Liège, Belgium
Chris Butler Nuffield Department of Clinical
Neurosciences, University of Oxford, John Radcliffe Christof Koch Allen Institute for Brain Science, Seattle,
Hospital, Oxford, UK WA, USA
Camille Chatelle Department of Neurology, Massachusetts Andrea Kübler Institute of Psychology, University of
General Hospital, Boston, MA, USA; Department of Würzburg, Würzburg, Germany
Physical Medicine and Rehabilitation, Spaulding Ron Kupers Department of Neuroscience and Pharmacology,
Rehabilitation Hospital and Harvard Medical School, University of Copenhagen, Copenhagen, Denmark
Boston, MA, USA Steven Laureys Coma Science Group, Neurology
Athena Demertzi Coma Science Group, Neurology Department and GIGA, University of Liège, Liège,
Department and GIGA, University of Liège, Liège, Belgium Belgium
Sebastian Dieguez Laboratory for Cognitive and Nicole L. Marinsek Dynamical Neuroscience, University of
Neurological Sciences, Department of Medicine, Hôpital California, Santa Barbara, CA, USA
de Fribourg, Université de Fribourg, Fribourg, George A. Mashour Department of Anesthesiology,
Switzerland Neuroscience Graduate Program, Center for
Brian L. Edlow Department of Neurology, Massachusetts Consciousness Science, University of Michigan Medical
General Hospital, Boston, MA, USA; Athinoula A. School, Ann Arbor, MI, USA
Martinos Center for Biomedical Imaging, Massachusetts Marcello Massimini Department of Biomedical and
General Hospital, Charlestown, MA, USA Clinical Sciences “Luigi Sacco,” University of Milan,
Andreas K. Engel Department of Neurophysiology and Milan, Italy
Pathophysiology, University Medical Center Hamburg- Donatella Mattia Fondazione Santa Lucia, IRCCS, Rome,
Eppendorf, Hamburg, Germany Italy

xv
xvi LIST OF CONTRIBUTORS

Michael B. Miller Dynamical Neuroscience, University of Mario Rosanova Department of Biomedical and Clinical
California, Santa Barbara, CA, USA; Psychological and Sciences “Luigi Sacco,” University of Milan, Milan, Italy;
Brain Sciences, University of California, Santa Barbara, Fondazione Europea di Ricerca Biomedica, FERB Onlus,
CA, USA Milan, Italy
Lionel Naccache INSERM U1127, Institut du Cerveau et de Eric Salmon Cyclotron Research Centre, University of
la Moelle Epinière, PICNIC Lab, Paris, France, Faculté de Liege, Belgium
Médecine Pitié-Salpêtrière, Université Pierre et Marie Nicholas D. Schiff Department of Neurology and
Curie, Paris, France; Departments of Neurology and of Neuroscience, Weill Medical College of Cornell
Clinical Neurophysiology, Assistance Publique Hôpitaux University, New York, NY, USA
de Paris, Groupe hospitalier Pitié-Salpêtrière Charles Foix,
Caroline Schnakers Department of Psychology and
Paris, France
Neurosurgery, University of California, Los Angeles, CA,
Paolo Nichelli Department of Biomedical, Metabolical and USA
Neural Sciences, University of Modena and Reggio
Francesca Siclari Center for Investigation and Research on
Emilia, Emilia-Romagna, Italy
Sleep, University Hospital and University of Lausanne,
Marie-Christine Nizzi Psychology Department, Harvard Switzerland
University, Cambridge, MA, USA Giulio Tononi Department of Psychiatry, University of
Adrian M. Owen The Brain and Mind Institute, The Wisconsin-Madison, Madison, WI, USA
University of Western Ontario, London, ON, Canada Naotsugu Tsuchiya School of Psychological Sciences,
Pietro Pietrini Clinical Psychology Branch, Department of Faculty of Biomedical and Psychological Sciences, Monash
Neuroscience, University of Pisa Medical School, Pisa, University, VIC, Australia
Italy Patrik Vuilleumier Laboratory for Behavioral Neurology
Bradley R. Postle Departments of Psychology and and Imaging of Cognition, Department of Neurosciences
Psychiatry, University of Wisconsin-Madison, Madison, and Clinic of Neurology, University Medical Center,
WI, USA Geneva, Switzerland
Maurice Ptito Department of Neuroscience and Susan Whitfield-Gabrieli Martinos Imaging Center at
Pharmacology, University of Copenhagen, Copenhagen, McGovern Institute for Brain Research, Massachusetts
Denmark Institute for Technology, Cambridge, MA, USA
Geraint Rees Faculty of Life Sciences, School of Life and Adam Zeman Department of Neurology, University of
Medical Sciences, University College London, London, UK Exeter Medical School, Exeter, UK
C H A P T E R

1
Neuroanatomical Basis of Consciousness
Hal Blumenfeld
Departments of Neurology, Neurobiology, and Neurosurgery, Yale University School of Medicine,
New Haven, CT, USA

O U T L I N E

Introduction 3 Cortical Networks and Consciousness 16


The Cortex and Arousal 16
The Consciousness System 5
Attention and Consciousness 16
Subcortical Networks and Consciousness 6 Hemispheric Dominance of Attention 17
The Thalamus and Consciousness 8 Affect, Motivation, and Attention 18
Glutamatergic and Related Arousal Systems 9 The Binding Problem 18
Cholinergic Arousal Systems 9 Top-Down and Bottom-Up Attention Networks 19
GABAergic Arousal Systems 12 Task-Positive and Task-Negative Networks 19
Noradrenergic Arousal Systems 12 Memory Systems and Consciousness 20
Serotoninergic Arousal Systems 13 Volition and Conscious Free Will 21
Dopaminergic Arousal Systems 13 Self-Awareness and Embodiment 22
Histaminergic Arousal Systems 14 Awareness: Conscious Report and
Orexinergic Arousal Systems 15 Contrastive Analysis 22
Adenosine and Arousal 15
Acknowledgments 23
Amygdala and Arousal 15
Attention and Awareness: Roles of Subcortical References 23
Arousal Systems, Tectal Region, Basal Ganglia,
Claustrum, and Cerebellum 15

INTRODUCTION all the various types of information processed by


hierarchically organized sensory, motor, emotional, and
Consciousness is of great importance to normal memory systems in the brain (Figure 1.1). Much of
human quality of life. The nature of consciousness and neuroscience is dedicated to understanding the normal
the best way to understand and define it have long functioning of these systems. Selective deficits in con-
generated lively debate among scientists, philosophers, tents of consciousness, such as loss of a portion of one’s
clinicians, and the general public. From a neurological visual field, or sudden impairment in spoken language,
perspective, consciousness is classically described as are also the main subject matter of clinical neurology.
emerging from brain systems that make up the content of However, level of consciousness can affect all of
consciousness, regulated by distinct systems that control these specific functions. The level of consciousness is
the level of consciousness (Plum and Posner, 1982). controlled by specialized cortical and subcortical sys-
The content of consciousness is the substrate upon tems that determine the amount of alertness, attention,
which levels of consciousness act. This content includes and awareness (mnemonic, AAA) (Blumenfeld, 2002).

S. Laureys, O. Gosseries & G. Tononi (Eds) DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/B978-0-12-800948-2.00001-7


The Neurology of Consciousness, Second edition. 3 © 2016 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.
4 1. NEUROANATOMICAL BASIS OF CONSCIOUSNESS

FIGURE 1.1 The content of consciousness. Parallel interconnected and hierarchically organized sensory and motor systems receive inputs,
generate outputs, and perform internal processing on multiple levels, from relatively simple to highly abstract. Three additional special
systems—mediating memory, emotions and drives, and consciousness itself—act on the other systems in a widely distributed manner,
especially at the highest levels of processing. Source: Modified with permission from Blumenfeld (2010).

Basic alertness (arousal, wakefulness) is necessary for discussed in turn, including the thalamus and sub-
any meaningful responses to occur. Attention enables cortical arousal nuclei acting through multiple neuro-
selective or sustained information to be processed. transmitters (glutamate, acetylcholine, gamma amino
Finally, awareness is the ability to form experiences butyric acid (GABA), norepinephrine, serotonin, dopa-
that can potentially be reported later. This chapter will mine, histamine, orexin) that arise from the upper
review the neuroanatomical basis of brain systems that brainstem, basal forebrain, and hypothalamus. The
control the level of consciousness. In analogy with second half of the chapter reviews important cortical
other cortical-subcortical systems such as the sensory, networks for controlling the level of alertness, atten-
motor or limbic systems, the brain networks dedicated tion, and awareness, including systems that select and
to regulating the level of consciousness can be referred encode conscious experiences into memories for sub-
to as the “consciousness system” (Blumenfeld, 2010, sequent report. This neuroanatomical review of the
2012). This chapter begins with an overview of the cortical and subcortical systems that control level of
main cortical and subcortical structures that constitute consciousness will serve as a general introduction to the
the consciousness system. Next, the major subcortical normal functions as well as disorders of consciousness
networks that regulate level of consciousness are each discussed in the remaining chapters in this book.

THE NEUROLOGY OF CONSCIOUSNESS


THE CONSCIOUSNESS SYSTEM 5

FIGURE 1.2 The consciousness system. Anatomical structures involved in regulating the level of consciousness, specifically controlling the
level of alertness, attention and awareness. (A) Medial view showing cortical (blue) and subcortical (red) components of the consciousness sys-
tem. (B) Lateral cortical components of the consciousness system. Note that other circuits not pictured here, such as the anterior insula, claus-
trum, basal ganglia, amygdala, and cerebellum, may also play a role in attention and other aspects of consciousness. Source: Reproduced with
permission from Blumenfeld (2010).

THE CONSCIOUSNESS SYSTEM literature (Heilman and Valenstein, 2003; Mesulam,


2000). Recently these same association cortex regions
The specialized brain networks controlling the level have also been described as participating in either so-
of consciousness can be referred to as the “conscious- called task-positive networks based on their activation
ness system” (Blumenfeld, 2009, 2010) (Figure 1.2). during externally oriented attention (Asplund et al.,
It has long been recognized through studies based 2010; Buschman and Miller, 2007; Dosenbach et al.,
on human brain disorders (Penfield, 1950; Plum and 2007; Vanhaudenhuyse et al., 2011) or task-negative
Posner, 1972; Von Economo, 1930) as well as experimen- networks, also known as the “default mode” based on
tal animal models (Bremer, 1955; Moruzzi and Magoun, activity at rest (Fox et al., 2005; Raichle et al., 2001).
1949; Steriade and McCarley, 2010) that the level of Regardless of the heterogeneous functions of individual
consciousness depends critically on both cortical and regions or networks, it is the collective activity of wide-
subcortical structures. Here we provide a brief overview spread areas of bilateral association cortex that deter-
of the cortical and subcortical networks comprising mines the level of consciousness. Taken as a whole, the
the consciousness system, which will be discussed in higher-order association cortex interacts with subcorti-
greater detail in the remaining sections of the chapter. cal arousal systems (Steriade and McCarley, 2010) to
Cortical components of the consciousness system exert powerful control over the overall level of arousal,
include the major regions of the higher-order “heteromo- attention, and awareness.
dal” (Mesulam, 2000) association cortex (Figure 1.2; see Subcortical components of the consciousness system
also Figure 1.11). On the medial brain surface, important include the upper brainstem activating systems, thala-
components are the medial frontal, anterior cingulate, mus, hypothalamus, and basal forebrain (Figure 1.2A).
posterior cingulate, and medial parietal (precuneus, It is likely that other subcortical structures (not shown)
retrosplenial) cortex (Figure 1.2A). On the lateral also participate, including portions of the basal gan-
surface, major consciousness system networks include glia, cerebellum, amygdala, and claustrum. Multiple
the lateral frontal, anterior insula, orbital frontal, and parallel neurotransmitter systems participate in
lateral temporal-parietal association cortex (Figure 1.2B). subcortical arousal including acetylcholine, glutamate,
It is important to recognize that individual gamma amino butyric acid (GABA), norepinephrine,
components of the higher-order association cortex play serotonin, dopamine, histamine, and orexin (Cooper
important and well-studied roles in specific cognitive et al., 2003; Saper et al., 2005; Steriade et al., 1997;
functions in the dominant and non-dominant hemi- Steriade and McCarley, 2010). Like the diverse cortical
spheres as described in the behavioral neurology regions already discussed, these subcortical pathways

THE NEUROLOGY OF CONSCIOUSNESS


6 1. NEUROANATOMICAL BASIS OF CONSCIOUSNESS

FIGURE 1.3 Arousal circuits of the pontomesencephalic reticular formation, thalamus, hypothalamus and basal forebrain. (A) Midsagittal
view; (B) coronal view. Widespread projections to the cortex arise from outputs of the pontomesencephalic reticular formation relayed via the
thalamic intralaminar nuclei, basal forebrain, and hypothalamus. Source: Reproduced with permission from Blumenfeld (2010).

each carry out individual roles, but it is the collective alertness and arousal. Attention and awareness are also
and parallel actions of all of these systems that facilitated by the same midline arousal systems, as well as
together control the level of consciousness. by other subcortical networks including the superior colli-
Understanding consciousness depends not only on culi, cerebellum, amygdala, basal ganglia, claustrum, and
neuroanatomy but also on neurophysiology. Although thalamic reticular nucleus (Crick and Koch, 2005; Dreher
this chapter will focus on the “where” of consciousness, and Grafman, 2002; Krauzlis et al., 2013; O’Halloran et al.,
equally important is “how” these networks interact to 2012; Zikopoulos and Barbas, 2012).
form consciousness. Recent proposed physiological In terms of alertness and arousal, much has been
mechanisms for consciousness include synchronized learned about the basic anatomy of consciousness by
oscillations (Buzsaki and Wang, 2012; Llinás and Paré, understanding which brain lesions can cause coma.
1997; Singer, 1998), slow cortical potentials (Li et al., Coma is as a state of unarousable unresponsiveness in
2014), connectivity (Boly et al., 2011; Rosanova et al., which the eyes are closed and no purposeful responses
2012; Rubinov and Sporns, 2010), information can be elicited (Fisher, 1969; Plum and Posner, 1972).
integration (Tononi, 2005; Tononi and Koch, 2008), pop- Coma occurs either through bilateral damage to wide-
ulation neuroenergetics (Shulman et al., 2003), and spread cortical areas, or via lesions in a core set of struc-
recurrent or global neuronal processing (Dehaene et al., tures lying in upper brainstem and medial diencephalon.
1998; Lamme and Roelfsema, 2000; Sergent and Dehaene, These critical subcortical arousal structures were initially
2004) among others. Much additional work is needed identified based on strokes and other localized disorders
before the physiological mechanisms of consciousness in human patients (Penfield, 1950; Plum and Posner,
are more definitely known. By contrast, when it comes to 1972; Von Economo, 1930) as well as lesion, disconnec-
neuroanatomy, the past century of research has at least tion, and stimulation experiments performed in animal
led to a basic understanding of the most important brain models (Bremer, 1955; Moruzzi and Magoun, 1949;
structures contributing to consciousness. We now turn in Steriade and McCarley, 2010). In the brainstem, the
greater detail to these major cortical and subcortical net- subcortical arousal systems begin in the upper pons
works that constitute the consciousness system. and extend to the midbrain. Lesions in this small but
critical region of the upper pons and midbrain pro-
duce profound coma, whereas lesions in the lower
SUBCORTICAL NETWORKS pons or medulla do not typically disrupt conscious-
AND CONSCIOUSNESS ness (Figure 1.3). The core brainstem arousal systems
lie in the tegmentum and include a variety of nuclei
The main subcortical components of the conscious- embedded within the brainstem reticular formation.
ness system include the midbrain and upper pons, thal- The tegmentum is sandwiched between the more
amus, hypothalamus, and basal forebrain (Figure 1.2). ventral brainstem basis—containing ascending and
These structures contribute importantly to maintaining descending white matter pathways; and the more

THE NEUROLOGY OF CONSCIOUSNESS


SUBCORTICAL NETWORKS AND CONSCIOUSNESS 7
TABLE 1.1 Widespread Projection Systems in the Nervous System
Projection Neurotransmitter
system Location(s) of cell bodies Main target(s) receptor(s)a,b Function(s)c

Reticular Midbrain and rostral pons Thalamic intralaminar nuclei, Unknown Alertness
formation hypothalamus, basal forebrain (glutamate?)d
Intralaminar Thalamic intralaminar nuclei Cortex, striatum (Glutamate?) Alertness
nuclei
Midline Midline thalamic nuclei Cortex (Glutamate?) Alertness
thalamic nuclei
Norepinephrine Pons: locus ceruleus and lateral Entire CNS α1A D, α2A D, β1 3 Alertness, attention, mood
tegmental area elevation
Dopamine Midbrain: substantia nigra Striatum, limbic cortex, D1 5 Movements, initiative,
pars compacta and ventral amygdala, nucleus accumbens, working memory
tegmental area prefrontal cortex

Serotonin Midbrain, pons, and medulla: Entire CNS 5-HT1A F, 5-HT2A C, Alertness, mood elevation,
raphe nuclei 5-HT3 7 breathing control
Histamine Hypothalamus: tubero-mammillary Entire brain H1 4 Alertness
nucleus; midbrain: reticular
formation
Orexin Posterior lateral hypothalamus Entire brain OX1, OX2 Alertness, food intake
(hypocretin)
Acetylcholine Basal forebrain: nucleus basalis, Cerebral cortex including Muscarinic (M1 5), Alertness, memory
medial septal nucleus, and nucleus hippocampus nicotinic subtypes
of diagonal band

Pontomesencephalic region: Thalamus, cerebellum, Muscarinic (M1 5), Alertness, memory


pedunculopontine nucleus and pons, medulla nicotinic subtypes
laterodorsal tegmental nucleus
a
Many of the neurons releasing the neuromodulatory transmitters listed here also release a variety of peptides, which are likely to play a neuromodulatory role as well.
b
Additional receptor subtypes are constantly being added.
c
Functions listed are highly simplified here; see references at the end of this chapter for additional details.
d
Entries in parenthesis with question mark are uncertain.
Source: Modified with permission from Blumenfeld (2010).

dorsal tectum—lying dorsal to the cerebral aqueduct originally called the “ascending reticular activating sys-
or fourth ventricle. Lesions outside the brainstem tem” (ARAS) (Moruzzi and Magoun, 1949) recognizing
tegmentum in the basis or tectum do not produce their important role in arousal. Although the term
coma. Bilateral lesions of the thalamus, particularly “ARAS” is still occasionally used, in reality these
in the intralaminar and midline thalamic nuclei, can arousal systems arise from a variety of specific nuclei
also produce profound suppression of arousal. (Table 1.1) rather than from what was formerly consid-
Subsequent work has revealed that the subcortical ered a single diffusely organized system.
arousal systems consist of multiple parallel neurotrans- The subcortical arousal systems in the midbrain and
mitter systems and pathways (Figure 1.3; Table 1.1). upper pons have three main targets (Figure 1.3):
Unlike most pathways in the nervous system which (i) Putative glutamatergic neurons from the reticular
project to relatively narrow target regions, the sub- formation and cholinergic neurons from the pedun-
cortical arousal systems belong to a set of widespread culopontine tegmental nucleus and laterodorsal
projecting systems (Table 1.1) that reach many struc- tegmental nucleus (LDT) project mainly to the thala-
tures or even the entire nervous system. Interestingly mus, particularly to the thalamic intralaminar nuclei
the projection systems arising from the upper brain- which, in turn, increase cortical arousal. (ii) Other
stem including the midbrain and upper pons (ponto- neurons project to the nucleus basalis and hypothala-
mesencephalic reticular formation; Figure 1.3) tend mus, which again relay arousal influences to the cortex.
to project upward to the cortex, diencephalon, and basal (iii) Finally, the monaminergic neurotransmitter
forebrain while those in the lower pons and med- systems (norepinephrine, dopamine, serotonin; not
ulla project downward to the brainstem, cerebellum, shown in Figure 1.3) project directly to the entire fore-
and spinal cord. The upward projecting systems were brain including the cortex and subcortical structures.

THE NEUROLOGY OF CONSCIOUSNESS


8 1. NEUROANATOMICAL BASIS OF CONSCIOUSNESS

The Thalamus and Consciousness


Nearly all information destined for the cortex first
reaches the thalamus. The thalamus transmits this
information and then receives an even greater number
of reciprocal connections back from the cortex.
Therefore the thalamus plays a key role in all aspects
of forebrain function including consciousness. The
thalamus relays the content of consciousness, and also
controls the level of consciousness through specialized
circuits that regulate the level of arousal and are
crucial for selective attention.
Organization of the thalamus can be described
based on regions or based on projections. The regional
organization of the thalamus divides the thalamic sub-
nuclei proceeding from lateral to medial (Figure 1.5)
FIGURE 1.4 Major inputs to the pontomesencephalic reticular into the thalamic reticular nucleus located most
formation and related structures. Source: Reproduced with permission laterally, followed by the lateral nuclear group which
from Blumenfeld (2010). contains the largest number of thalamic relay nuclei
In addition to these ascending connections, the subcor- (Table 1.2). Continuing medially, next comes the
tical arousal systems are also highly interconnected Y-shaped white matter internal medullary lamina
and strongly influence each other’s function through which separates the lateral, anterior, and medial
multiple connections within and between the brain- nuclear groups from each other (Figure 1.5). Embedded
stem, thalamus, hypothalamus, and basal forebrain. within the internal medullary lamina lie the intra-
The upper brainstem arousal systems are influenced laminar nuclei. Finally a thin layer of midline thalamic
by a variety of inputs including numerous regions of the nuclei are located most medially, adjacent to the third
association cortex and limbic cortex, as well as sensory ventricle (Figure 1.5).
pathways such as the anterolateral pain transmission Projection patterns can also be used to classify the
pathways (Figure 1.4). Inhibitory influences arise from thalamic subnuclei (Table 1.2). Some, such as the
the thalamic reticular nucleus (not to be confused with ventral posterior lateral nucleus, a somatosensory
the reticular formation) as well as other GABAergic relay in the lateral thalamus, project to a relatively
inputs (Parent and Steriade, 1981, 1984; Ropert and localized region of cortex, and are referred to as
Steriade, 1981). The arousal systems are also strongly specific relay nuclei. Others, such as the thalamic intra-
regulated by brainstem and hypothalamic circuits con- laminar nuclei, have more widespread projections to
trolling circadian sleep rhythms (Saper et al., 2005, 2010). many cortical areas, and are called diffusely or widely
The subcortical arousal systems will now each be projecting (“nonspecific”) nuclei.
discussed in greater detail to more fully appreciate The specific thalamic relay nuclei communicate with
the functional anatomy of these complex parallel the cortex regarding each sensory and motor function,
arousal systems and their contributions to conscious- and are therefore responsible for all the individual
ness. It should be noted that, unlike gross lesions of contents of consciousness. On the other hand, the
the brainstem-diencephalic arousal systems, lesions widely projecting thalamic nuclei influence the overall
or pharmacological blockade of the individual pro- level of cortical arousal, and therefore control the
jecting neurotransmitter systems do not cause coma. level of consciousness. The rostral intralaminar nuclei
Blockade of some neurotransmitters, especially ace- (central lateral, paracentral, central medial nuclei;
tylcholine or histamine, produces severe confusion Table 1.2) and midline thalamic nuclei are thought to
and drowsiness, but not coma. Thus, the normal be particularly important for activating the cortex
awake, conscious state does not depend on a single (Figure 1.3). As was already discussed, the intralaminar
projection system, but rather on the parallel action of thalamus plays a key role in transmitting arousal influ-
multiple anatomical and neurotransmitter systems ences from the midbrain and upper pontine cholinergic
acting together (Table 1.1). After discussing each of and glutamatergic systems to the cortex.
the major subcortical arousal systems, emphasizing The thalamic reticular nucleus forms a thin shell of
their role in maintaining the alert state, we will then predominantly GABAergic inhibitory neurons on the
briefly discuss their role together with other subcorti- lateral thalamus (Figure 1.5). As axons traverse this
cal structures (tectal region, basal ganglia, claustrum, nucleus traveling from thalamus to cortex or from
cerebellum) in attention and awareness. cortex back to thalamus they give off collateral

THE NEUROLOGY OF CONSCIOUSNESS


SUBCORTICAL NETWORKS AND CONSCIOUSNESS 9

FIGURE 1.5 The thalamus. Main nuclear divisions and nuclei are shown (see also Table 1.2). The posterior portion of the reticular nucleus
has been removed. Source: Reproduced with permission from Blumenfeld (2010).

branches to the thalamic reticular neurons. The include those arising from the midbrain and upper
thalamic reticular neurons, in turn, project to the thala- pontine reticular formation that project to the thalamus
mus and inhibit the specific thalamocortical neurons and basal forebrain (Steriade, 2004; Steriade et al.,
corresponding to individual corticothalamic loops. 1993a), as well as the widespread projections from the
The reciprocal connections between thalamic relay thalamic intralaminar nuclei to the cortex (Figure 1.3).
nuclei and the thalamic reticular nucleus are thought It is not known whether other excitatory amino acid
to play an important role in generating corticotha- neurotransmitters such as aspartate might also play a
lamic rhythms during normal sleep and waking significant role in arousal.
activity, as well as in pathological rhythms such as
epilepsy (McCormick, 2002; McCormick and Bal, 1997;
McCormick and Contreras, 2001; Steriade et al., 1993b). Cholinergic Arousal Systems
These physiological rhythms are crucial for regulating Acetylcholine is the major neurotransmitter of the
the level of consciousness. In addition, the thalamic peripheral nervous system, but in the central nervous sys-
reticular nucleus influences arousal through long-range tem it has a more neuromodulatory function, where its
inhibitory projections to the pontomesencephalic retic- role in arousal has been studied extensively. The two
ular formation (Parent and Steriade, 1984). Selective main sources of cholinergic projections neurons in the
attention may also be mediated through the particular central nervous system lie in the brainstem pontomesen-
arrangement of reticular thalamic neurons and their cephalic reticular formation and in the basal forebrain
directed inhibitory projections to the thalamus, which (Figure 1.6; Table 1.1). At the junction of the midbrain and
are capable of generating an inhibitory surround pons, the pedunculopontine nucleus is located in the lat-
around a “searchlight” of focused attention in a narrow eral reticular formation, while the laterodorsal tegmental
band of thalamocortical channels (Crick, 1984; Mayo, nucleus lies in the periaqueductal gray (Mesulam et al.,
2009; Pinault, 2004). 1983). The pedunculopontine nucleus stretches from the
caudal midbrain substantia nigra pars reticulata into the
rostral pons towards the superior cerebellar peduncle
Glutamatergic and Related Arousal Systems (Mena-Segovia et al., 2009; Rye et al., 1987). The nucleus
Glutamate is the most prevalent excitatory neuro- has a gradient of increasing cholinergic and decreasing
transmitter of the central nervous system. For many of GABAergic neurons as it extends caudally, and also con-
the arousal systems the most likely neurotransmitter is tains glutamatergic neurons (Wang and Morales, 2009).
glutamate, although it has not been identified with Cholinergic neurons from these brainstem nuclei project
certainty (Steriade and McCarley, 2010). Arousal to the thalamus, including the intralaminar nuclei, play-
system pathways probably mediated by glutamate ing an important role in arousal. Brainstem cholinergic

THE NEUROLOGY OF CONSCIOUSNESS


TABLE 1.2 Major Thalamic Nuclei
Diffuseness of
projections to
Nucleia Main inputsb Main outputs cortexc Proposed functions

RELAY NUCLEI
Lateral nuclear group
Ventral posterior lateral Medial lemniscus, Somatosensory cortex 1 Relays somatosensory spinal
nucleus spinothalamic tract inputs to cortex

Ventral posteromedial Trigeminal lemniscus, Somatosensory and taste 1 Relays somatosensory cranial
nucleus trigeminothalamic tract, cortex nerve inputs and taste to
taste inputs cortex

Lateral geniculate nucleus Retina Primary visual cortex 1 Relays visual inputs to cortex

Medial geniculate nucleus Inferior colliculus Primary auditory cortex 1 Relays auditory inputs to
cortex

Ventral lateral nucleus Internal globus pallidus, deep Motor, premotor, and 1 Relays basal ganglia and
cerebellar nuclei, substantia supplementary motor cerebellar inputs to cortex
nigra pars reticulata cortex

Ventral anterior nucleus Substantia nigra pars reticulata, Widespread to frontal 11 1 Relays basal ganglia and
internal globus pallidus, deep lobe, including cerebellar inputs to cortex
cerebellar nuclei prefrontal, premotor,
and supplementary
motor cortex

Pulvinar Tectum (extrageniculate visual Parietotemporo-occipital 11 Behavioral orientation toward


pathway), other sensory inputs association cortex relevant visual and other
stimuli

Lateral dorsal nucleus See anterior nucleus 11 Functions with anterior nuclei

Lateral posterior nucleus See pulvinar 11 Functions with pulvinar

Ventral medial nucleus Midbrain reticular formation Widespread to cortex 11 1 May help maintain alert,
conscious state

Medial nuclear group


Mediodorsal nucleus (MD) Amygdala, olfactory cortex, Frontal cortex 11 Limbic pathways, major relay
limbic basal ganglia to frontal cortex

Anterior nuclear group


Anterior nucleus Mammillary body, hippocampal Cingulate gyrus 1 Limbic pathways
formation

Midline thalamic nuclei


Paraventricular, parataenial, Hypothalamus, basal forebrain, Amygdala, hippocampus, 11 Limbic pathways
interanteromedial, inter- amygdala, hippocampus limbic cortex
mediodorsal, rhomboid,
reuniens (medial ventral)

INTRALAMINAR NUCLEI
Rostral intralaminar nuclei
Central medial nucleus Deep cerebellar nuclei, globus Cerebral cortex, striatum 11 1 Maintain alert consciousness;
pallidus, brainstem ascending motor relay for basal ganglia
Paracentral nucleus reticular activating systems and cerebellum
Central lateral nucleus (ARAS), sensory pathways

Caudal intralaminar nuclei


Centromedian nucleus Globus pallidus, ARAS, sensory Striatum, cerebral cortex 11 1 Motor relay for basal ganglia
pathways

Parafascicular nucleus

RETICULAR NUCLEUS
Reticular nucleus Cerebral cortex, thalamic relay Thalamic relay and None Regulates state of other
and intralaminar nuclei, ARAS intralaminar nuclei, ARAS thalamic nuclei
a
Some additional, smaller nuclei have not been included here.
b
In addition to the inputs listed, all thalamic nuclei receive reciprocal inputs from the cortex and from the thalamic reticular nucleus. Modulatory cholinergic, noradrenergic, serotonergic,
and histaminergic inputs also reach most thalamic nuclei.
c
1 represents least diffuse (specific thalamic relay nuclei);11 represents moderately diffuse; 11 1 represents most diffuse.
Source: Modified with permission from Blumenfeld (2010).
SUBCORTICAL NETWORKS AND CONSCIOUSNESS 11

FIGURE 1.6 Cholinergic projection systems. (A) Overview and inset showing axial section through caudal midbrain. (B) Coronal section
through basal forebrain. See also Table 1.1. Source: Reproduced with permission from Blumenfeld (2010).

arousal is thought to act synergistically with non- surrounding regions (substantia innominata, globus
cholinergic putative glutamatergic pontomesencephalic pallidus, and preoptic magnocellular nucleus) not only
neurons that project to intralaminar thalamus and basal project to almost the entire neocortex (Mesulam et al.,
forebrain (Figure 1.3) (Rasmusson et al., 1994, 1996; 1983; Rye et al., 1984) but also innervate some nuclei in
Steriade, 2004; Steriade et al., 1993a). In sleep, pontogen- the thalamus (reticular thalamic, mediodorsal, antero-
iculate waves arise from cholinergic brainstem neurons ventral/anteromedial, and ventromedial nuclei)
projecting to thalamocortical neurons in the lateral genic- (Heckers et al., 1992; Parent et al., 1988; Steriade et al.,
ulate nucleus (Steriade et al., 1989, 1990). The pedunculo- 1987). The hippocampal archicortex, however,
pontine nucleus also has numerous ascending and receives cholinergic inputs mainly from the medial
descending motor projections and is involved in septal nuclei and nucleus of the diagonal band of
controlling locomotion (Inglis and Winn, 1995). Broca (Rye et al., 1984). Additional cholinergic neu-
Interestingly, the brainstem has very few direct rons lie in the medial habenula, and short-range cho-
cholinergic projections to the cortex and nearly all linergic neurons are present in the striatum and to a
facilitatory effects of the brainstem cholinergic systems more limited extent within the cortex itself. Like the
on cortex are mediated via the thalamus (Beninato and brainstem cholinergic nuclei, the basal forebrain con-
Spencer, 1987; Cornwall et al., 1990; Hallanger et al., tains both cholinergic and non-cholinergic neurons,
1987; Hallanger and Wainer, 1988; Heckers et al., 1992; including GABA and glutamate as transmitters
Jones and Webster, 1988; Satoh and Fibiger, 1986). The among others (Brashear et al., 1986; Alvaro Duque
major source of cholinergic input to the cortex is the et al., 2007).
basal forebrain (Figure 1.6; Table 1.1). Cholinergic The brainstem and basal forebrain cholinergic
neurons in the nucleus basalis of Meynert and systems work together to abolish cortical slow wave

THE NEUROLOGY OF CONSCIOUSNESS


12 1. NEUROANATOMICAL BASIS OF CONSCIOUSNESS

FIGURE 1.7 Noradrenergic projection systems. See also Table 1.1. Source: Reproduced with permission from Blumenfeld (2010).

activity and promote an alert state (Dringenberg and ventral lateral preoptic nucleus, which have wide-
Olmstead, 2003; Steriade, 2004). Cholinergic arousal in spread inhibitory projections to virtually all subcortical
the central nervous system is mediated predominantly arousal systems (Saper et al., 2010); lateral septal
by muscarinic acetylcholine receptors, although nico- GABAergic neurons thought to inhibit the basal fore-
tinic receptors may also play an important role in brain and hypothalamus (Mesulam and Mufson, 1984;
arousal and attention (Bloem et al., 2014). As would be Varoqueaux and Poulain, 1999); and the thalamic
expected from the connections of cholinergic neurons reticular nucleus which contains GABAergic neurons
described here, pharmacological blockade of central projecting both to the remainder of the thalamus and
cholinergic neurotransmission produces an acute state to the brainstem reticular formation (Parent and
of delirium and memory loss. However, despite the Steriade, 1984; Steriade et al., 1984). In addition,
importance of acetylcholine in consciousness, selective GABAergic neurons in the globus pallidus internal
damage to cholinergic neurotransmission does not pro- segment inhibit regions of the thalamus including the
duce coma (Blanco-Centurion et al., 2006, 2007; Buzsaki intralaminar nuclei. It has been proposed that paradox-
et al., 1988; Fuller et al., 2011), presumably because ical arousal effects of GABA agonists such as zolpidem
of the multiple parallel neurotransmitter systems in minimally conscious state, or benzodiazepines in
participating in consciousness as already discussed. catatonia may occur when these agents inhibit the glo-
bus pallidus, thereby removing tonic inhibition of the
intralaminar thalamus (Brown et al., 2010; Giacino
GABAergic Arousal Systems
et al., 2014). Activation of these multiple GABAergic
Found in local inhibitory interneurons throughout inhibitory projections converging on the subcortical
the cortex and subcortical structures, GABA is the arousal systems has also been proposed as the mecha-
most prevalent inhibitory neurotransmitter in the cen- nism for loss of consciousness in partial seizures
tral nervous system and plays a major role in regulat- (Blumenfeld, 2012; Englot and Blumenfeld, 2009).
ing arousal. Several long-range GABAergic projection
systems also contribute to controlling arousal. Some
GABAergic neurons in the basal forebrain are thought
Noradrenergic Arousal Systems
to promote arousal because these inhibitory neurons in Norepinephrine (noradrenaline)-containing neurons
turn project to cortical inhibitory interneurons (Freund are located in the locus ceruleus in the rostral pons
and Meskenaite, 1992). However, the overall effects of adjacent to the fourth ventricle, as well as in the nearby
basal forebrain GABAergic neurons on arousal may lateral tegmental area extending into the more caudal
be heterogeneous because of variable firing patterns pons and medulla (Figure 1.7; Table 1.1). Ascending
with respect to cortical activation and sleep-wake noradrenergic projections reach the cortex, thalamus
cycles (Hassani et al., 2009; Jones, 2004; Manns et al., and hypothalamus (Foote et al., 1983; Morrison et al.,
2000); and because parvalbumin-containing GABAergic 1981; Pickel et al., 1974) to regulate sleep-wake cycles,
neurons are related to cortical desynchrony whereas attention, and mood, while descending projections to
neuropeptide Y-containing neurons may have the the brainstem, cerebellum, and spinal cord modulate
opposite effect (Alvaro Duque et al., 2000, 2007). autonomic function and gating of pain. Many attention-
Other important long-range GABAergic projections enhancing drugs such as amphetamines augment
mainly inhibit arousal. These include neurons in the noradrenergic function. Norepinephrine is thought to

THE NEUROLOGY OF CONSCIOUSNESS


SUBCORTICAL NETWORKS AND CONSCIOUSNESS 13

FIGURE 1.8 Serotonergic projections systems. See also Table 1.1. Source: Reproduced with permission from Blumenfeld (2010).

play an important role in promoting arousal (Berridge, promote or inhibit arousal in different brain regions
2008; Berridge et al., 2012; Constantinople and Bruno, (Dugovic et al., 1989; Dzoljic et al., 1992; Kumar et al.,
2011). For example, selective α-2 agonists such as 2007; Lemoine et al., 2007; Leonard and Llinás, 1994;
clonidine or the anesthetic agent dexmedetomidine Luebke et al., 1992; Monckton and McCormick, 2002;
markedly depress arousal possibly by inhibiting locus Muraki et al., 2004; Rogawski and Aghajanian, 1980).
ceruleus neurons (Correa-Sales et al., 1992; De Sarro Rostral brainstem serotonergic neurons have been pro-
et al., 1987; Scheinin and Schwinn, 1992). However, posed to promote arousal in response to hypoventilation
selective removal or blockade of noradrenergic neurons and increased carbon dioxide levels, perhaps playing an
affects arousal but does not produce coma (Berridge important role in preventing sudden infant death syn-
et al., 1993; Blanco-Centurion et al., 2004, 2007; Cirelli drome and sudden unexplained death in epilepsy
and Tononi, 2004; Hunsley and Palmiter, 2003) again (Buchanan and Richerson, 2010; Kinney et al., 2009;
reinforcing the notion of multiple parallel systems Richerson and Buchanan, 2011; Sowers et al., 2013).
promoting consciousness.
Dopaminergic Arousal Systems
Serotoninergic Arousal Systems
Most dopaminergic neurons are located in the
Serotonergic neurons are found predominantly in the ventral midbrain, either in the substantia nigra pars
midline raphe nuclei of the midbrain, pons, and medulla compacta or in the adjacent ventral tegmental area
(Figure 1.8; Table 1.1). The more rostral serotonergic (Figure 1.9; Table 1.1). These mesencephalic nuclei give
neurons in the midbrain and upper pontine raphe rise to the following three ascending dopaminergic
nuclei project to the entire forebrain, participating in projection systems: (i) the mesostriatal (nigrostriatal)
sleep-wake regulation; dysfunction of serotonergic pathway projects from the substantia nigra to the
systems is thought to play a role in a number of caudate and putamen (Matsuda et al., 2009); (ii) the
psychiatric disorders including depression, anxiety, mesolimbic pathway arises mainly from the ventral
obsessive-compulsive disorder, aggressive behavior, and tegmental area and projects to limbic structures
eating disorders. More caudal serotonergic neurons in the including the medial temporal lobe, amygdala, cingu-
pons and medulla are important for modulation of late gyrus, septal nuclei, and nucleus accumbens
breathing, pain, temperature control, cardiovascular, and (Fallon, 1981; Oades and Halliday, 1987); (iii) the
motor function. mesocortical pathway arises mainly from the ventral
The most important rostral raphe nuclei participating tegmental area as well as scattered neurons in the
in arousal are the dorsal raphe and median raphe vicinity of the substantia nigra and ventral periaque-
(Jacobs and Azmitia, 1992; Wiklund et al., 1981). The role ductal gray, projecting to the prefrontal cortex
of serotonergic neurons in arousal is complex, possibly (Figure 1.9) as well as to the thalamus (Garcia-Cabezas
because the large diversity of serotonin receptors et al., 2009; Groenewegen, 1988; Lu et al., 2006;
(Hannon and Hoyer, 2008) leads to effects that either Sanchez-Gonzalez et al., 2005). Dopaminergic neurons

THE NEUROLOGY OF CONSCIOUSNESS


14 1. NEUROANATOMICAL BASIS OF CONSCIOUSNESS

FIGURE 1.9 Dopaminergic projections systems. See also Table 1.1. Source: Reproduced with permission from Blumenfeld (2010).

in the ventral tegmental area also project caudally to neurotransmission or effects of this medication on
various brainstem nuclei and to the spinal cord (Oades other neurotransmitter systems (Giacino et al., 2012).
and Halliday, 1987).
Dopamine may contribute to maintaining the
waking state at least in part through effects on other
Histaminergic Arousal Systems
subcortical arousal circuits (Deutch et al., 1986; Histamine-containing neurons are found mainly in
Lu et al., 2006; Neylan et al., 1992; Qu et al., 2008; the tuberomamillary nucleus (Panula et al., 1984) of
Volkow et al., 2009). Effects of dopamine on the the posterior hypothalamus (Figure 1.10; Table 1.1),
thalamus and cortex can be either excitatory or inhibi- although a few scattered histaminergic neurons are
tory (Bandyopadhyay and Hablitz, 2007; Govindaiah also found in the midbrain reticular formation.
et al., 2010; Lavin and Grace, 1998; Penit-Soria et al., Widespread ascending projections of histaminergic
1987). Impaired dopaminergic transmission to the pre- neurons from the tuberomamillary nucleus reach
frontal cortex has been proposed to be important for nearly the entire forebrain including cortex and thala-
the apathetic negative symptoms of schizophrenia, and mus, while descending projections target the brainstem
may also contribute to states of markedly reduced and spinal cord (Brown et al., 2001; Hong and Lee,
motivation, initiative and action/intention seen in 2011; Lin et al., 1996; Panula et al., 1989).
frontal lobe disorders, abulia, and akinetic mutism Anti-histamine medications are intended to act on
(Combarros et al., 2000; Kim et al., 2007; Yang et al., peripheral histamine release from mast cells, but are
2007). Amantadine improves arousal in chronic disor- well-known to induce drowsiness presumably through
ders of consciousness, although it is unclear whether central actions (White and Rumbold, 1988). Histamine
the mechanism is through enhanced dopaminergic can produce arousal effects on cortex (Dringenberg

THE NEUROLOGY OF CONSCIOUSNESS


SUBCORTICAL NETWORKS AND CONSCIOUSNESS 15
Peyron et al., 2000; Thannickal et al., 2000). The beneficial
effects of modafenil in preventing the symptoms of
narcolepsy may in part be through activation of orexin
neurons (Chemelli et al., 1999).

Adenosine and Arousal


Although the neuroanatomical sources of adenosine
are not well characterized, this neuromodulator may
be important in mechanisms of conscious arousal
(Huang et al., 2014; Liu and Gao, 2007). The effects of
adenosine on arousal are generally inhibitory, and cir-
cadian fluctuations in adenosine levels peak just prior
to the initiation of sleep. Adenosine receptors are
found in both cortex and thalamus, where they have
an overall inhibitory function on arousal. Caffeine
blocks adenosine receptors and this may be one impor-
FIGURE 1.10 Histaminergic projections systems. See also tant mechanism whereby coffee promotes alertness
Table 1.1. Source: Reproduced with permission from Blumenfeld (2010). (Huang et al., 2011; Lazarus et al., 2011).

and Kuo, 2003; McCormick and Williamson, 1989) and Amygdala and Arousal
thalamus (McCormick and Williamson, 1991). In addi-
Because the amygdala has widespread and reciprocal
tion to the cortex and thalamus, arousal actions of
cortical-subcortical connections that contribute to aro-
histamine may be mediated by activation of other sub-
usal particularly in response to emotions, it is appro-
cortical arousal systems including other hypothalamic
priate to include this complex of nuclei located in the
nuclei (Brown et al., 2001; Lin et al., 1994), the basal
anteromedial temporal lobe as an important subcortical
forebrain (Dringenberg and Kuo, 2003; Khateb et al.,
component of the consciousness system (Steriade and
1995; Zant et al., 2012), brainstem cholinergic (Khateb
McCarley, 2010). The main components of the amygda-
et al., 1990; Lin et al., 1996), and noradrenergic nuclei
loid nuclear complex are the corticomedial, basolateral,
(Korotkova et al., 2005). Effects of histamine are
and central nuclei, as well as the bed nucleus of the
receptor-dependent as activation of H1 receptors pro-
stria terminalis. The basolateral nucleus is largest in
motes wakefulness, whereas H3-receptors appear to
humans and has widespread direct and indirect connec-
have the opposite role (Huang et al., 2006; Khateb
tions to the cortex, basal forebrain, and medial thalamus
et al., 1990; Lin et al., 1990, 1996; Valjakka et al., 1996).
(Aggleton, 2000; LeDoux, 2007). The smaller cortico-
medial nucleus participates in appetitive states via the
hypothalamus, as well as in olfaction. The central
Orexinergic Arousal Systems nucleus, although smallest, has important connections
Orexin (hypocretin) is a peptide produced in with the hypothalamus and brainstem participating in
neurons of the perifornical, lateral, and posterior arousal and autonomic control (LeDoux, 2007).
hypothalamus (Peyron et al., 1998; Sakurai et al., 1998),
which project to both cortex and virtually all sub- Attention and Awareness: Roles of Subcortical
cortical arousal systems (Chemelli et al., 1999; Peyron
Arousal Systems, Tectal Region, Basal Ganglia,
et al., 1998) to promote the awake state. The arousal
Claustrum, and Cerebellum
effects of orexin likely arise from both cortical and
subcortical mechanisms (Bourgin et al., 2000; Eriksson To complete our discussion of subcortical networks
et al., 2001; España et al., 2001; Hagan et al., 1999; regulating the level of consciousness, it is important to
Horvath et al., 1999; Kiyashchenko et al., 2002; again emphasize the functions of the consciousness
Tsunematsu et al., 2011; van den Pol et al., 2002). system in controlling alertness, attention, and aware-
Abnormalities of the orexin system are thought ness, and to briefly mention several additional sub-
to play a role in narcolepsy, a disorder characterized cortical structures participating in these functions. As
by excessive daytime sleepiness and pathological we have already discussed, the thalamus and other
transitions into rapid eye movement sleep (Anaclet et al., multiple parallel subcortical arousal systems in the
2009; Chemelli et al., 1999; Gerashchenko et al., upper brainstem, hypothalamus, and basal forebrain
2003; Hara et al., 2001; Lin et al., 1999; Nishino et al., 2000; (Table 1.1) are essential for maintaining the alert state.

THE NEUROLOGY OF CONSCIOUSNESS


16 1. NEUROANATOMICAL BASIS OF CONSCIOUSNESS

These same systems also play a key role in controlling systems and consciousness will then be considered, as
attention and awareness not only in a permissive sense well as the role of cortical networks in self-awareness,
(e.g., being in a coma is not compatible with attention planning voluntary action and free will. Finally, we
and awareness), but also by facilitating the addi- will review cortical networks revealed by contrastive
tional processing in cortical and subcortical networks analysis (perceived vs non-perceived stimuli) to play
necessary for attention and for awareness. an important role in conscious awareness.
Several additional subcortical structures also play
a role. Components of the tectal region, specifically
the superior colliculi and pretectal area form an impor-
The Cortex and Arousal
tant circuit along with the pulvinar of the thalamus The most important input to subcortical arousal
to direct saccadic eye movements towards salient systems, including the thalamus, hypothalamus,
stimuli, and the same circuits also participate in basal forebrain, and the multiple brainstem arousal
directed attention (Krauzlis et al., 2013). The basal systems is the cerebral cortex itself. It has long been
ganglia have major reciprocal connections with the known that stimulation of the higher-order heteromo-
thalamic intralaminar nuclei and this circuit as well as dal frontoparietal association cortex increases arousal
other basal ganglia connections may contribute to (Figure 1.11) (Segundo et al., 1955). Conversely, abla-
arousal and attention functions (Dreher and Grafman, tion of these same regions of the higher-order associa-
2002; Hager et al., 1998; Ring and Serra-Mestres, 2002). tion cortex markedly decreases arousal (Ropert and
The claustrum is a thin layer of neurons located in the Steriade, 1981; Steriade and McCarley, 2010; Watson,
white matter between the putamen and insula, with et al., 1977), although the subcortical arousal systems
widespread cortical connections that have been pro- also receive inputs from primary sensorimotor cortices
posed to play an important role in the attention and (Catsman-Berrevoets and Kuypers, 1981; Ropert and
awareness aspects of consciousness (Crick and Koch, Steriade, 1981; Rossi and Brodal, 1956). In further sup-
2005). Finally, the cerebellum has major reciprocal port of the importance of the cerebral cortex in main-
connections with the prefrontal cortex and has also taining consciousness it was recognized by clinicians
been proposed to participate in attention, although that unilateral cortical lesions usually do not markedly
this remains somewhat controversial (Baumann and depress level of consciousness, but bilateral lesions of
Mattingley, 2014; Bischoff-Grethe et al., 2002; Dreher the association cortex can produce coma (Plum and
and Grafman, 2002; O’Halloran et al., 2012). Posner, 1972; Posner et al., 2007). The parietal cortex of
the non-dominant (usually right) hemisphere appears
to play a particularly important role in arousal where
CORTICAL NETWORKS large lesions—although not producing coma—do often
AND CONSCIOUSNESS produce a markedly drowsy clinical state with forced
eye closure. Thus, in addition to its important role in
The cortical components of the consciousness system producing the specific individual contents of con-
include widespread regions of association cortex in sciousness, the cerebral cortex is also a major driver in
the bilateral cerebral hemispheres, particularly in the regulating the overall level of conscious arousal.
lateral frontal, anterior insula, lateral parietal (and adja-
cent temporal-occipital cortex), medial frontal, medial
parietal (precuneus) and cingulate cortex (Figure 1.2).
Attention and Consciousness
As has already been discussed, individual cortical There has been recent debate on the relationship
components of the consciousness system have specific between attention and consciousness. Some view atten-
well-studied functions in behavioral neurology which tion and consciousness as orthogonal functions that
contribute to the various contents of consciousness along can be fully dissociated and operate independently
with specific primary and secondary sensorimotor and (Koch and Tsuchiya, 2007). Others consider attention
limbic cortical areas (Figure 1.11). However, it is the and consciousness to be essentially identical, constitut-
collective action of widespread bilateral association ing different names for the same set of functions
cortex regions that gives rise to regulation of the level of (Prinz, 2000, 2012). Still others posit that attention is
alertness, attention, and conscious awareness. necessary for but not identical to consciousness
In this section we will review the contributions of because additional functions are needed for conscious
the cerebral cortex to arousal and the generation of an awareness (Dehaene et al., 2006; Kouider and Dehaene,
alert, awake state. We will next discuss attention in 2007). These very different understandings of the rela-
somewhat greater detail, describing several formula- tionship between attention and consciousness may
tions of cortical systems that control different aspects arise at least in part from different models for defining
of attention. The relationship between memory and understanding attention. There are a large number

THE NEUROLOGY OF CONSCIOUSNESS


CORTICAL NETWORKS AND CONSCIOUSNESS 17

FIGURE 1.11 Cerebral cortex. (A) Lateral view. (B) Medial view. Main primary cortical and association cortical areas are indicated.
Unimodal association cortex is modality-specific, whereas higher-order heteromodal association cortex combines information across modali-
ties. Regions labeled here as limbic cortex include allocortical regions such as the hippocampus (archicortex) and pyriform cortex (paleocortex)
as well as transitional paralimbic and neocortical areas such as the parahippocampal gyrus, cingulate gyrus, anterior insula, orbitofrontal cor-
tex, and temporal pole. Source: Reproduced with permission from Blumenfeld (2010).

of models of attention and a complete discussion of Hemispheric Dominance of Attention


the anatomy of attention is far beyond the scope of this Even more than the laterality of language, spatial
chapter. However, here we will at least discuss a few attention is strongly lateralized and the large majority
of the major current formulations of attention espe- of individuals have dominant spatial attention in the
cially in how their cortical anatomical underpinnings right hemisphere (Heilman and Valenstein, 2003;
relate to understanding consciousness. Heilman et al., 2000; Mesulam, 2000). Contralateral

THE NEUROLOGY OF CONSCIOUSNESS


18 1. NEUROANATOMICAL BASIS OF CONSCIOUSNESS

FIGURE 1.12 Top-down, bottom-up attention networks and hemineglect. (A) Areas commonly damaged in the right hemisphere
that cause the contralateral hemineglect syndrome. (B) Dorsal and ventral frontoparietal attention. Areas in blue indicate the dorsal fronto-
parietal “top-down” attention network. FEF, frontal eye field; IPs/SPL, intraparietal sulcus/superior parietal lobule. Areas in orange indicate
the stimulus-driven ventral frontoparietal “bottom-up” attention network. TPJ, temporoparietal junction (IPL/STG, inferior parietal lobule/
superior temporal gyrus); VFC, ventral frontal cortex (IFg/MFg, inferior frontal gyrus/middle frontal gyrus). The areas damaged in neglect
(A) better match the ventral network. Source: Modified with permission from Corbetta and Shulman (2002).

attentional neglect is more common and usually far level of consciousness. In some studies impaired
more severe with lesions of the right hemisphere, consciousness is more common when the left hemi-
especially with damage to the right lateral parietal sphere is impaired for example, due to stroke, or dur-
cortex, although hemineglect can also be seen with ing selective administration of barbiturate anesthetics
right frontal lesions (Figure 1.12A). Unilateral neglect to one hemisphere at a time in the angiogram Wada
profoundly impairs the affected individual’s ability to test (Albert et al., 1976; Franczek et al., 1997; Schwartz,
attend to visual, auditory, and tactile stimuli, to motor 1967), or with focal seizures originating in the left
intention and indeed even to the very existence of the hemisphere (Englot et al., 2010; Gabr et al., 1989; Inoue
entire contralateral half of their body and the external and Mihara, 1998; Lux et al., 2002). However, it is diffi-
world opposite to the side of their lesion. Although the cult to determine whether a bias of testing methods
impaired attention is usually most profound contra- which are heavily weighted towards verbal rather than
lateral to right hemisphere lesions, there is also some non-verbal questions and responses might strongly
attentional impairment which extends to the ipsi- influence these finding.
lateral side so that overall attention is globally
decreased (Heilman et al., 2000). Milder degrees of Affect, Motivation, and Attention
unilateral contralateral neglect are usually seen with One important aspect of cortical attention networks
left lateral parietal or left frontal lesions. Overall, these that should not be overlooked is the major role of
features of the neglect syndrome support a model in affect and motivational drives (Damasio and Carvalho,
which attention functions are distributed between the 2013; Heilman and Valenstein, 2003; Heilman et al.,
association cortices in both hemispheres, but with a 2000; Satz and Heilman, 1983). Subjects who are
dominant role played by the right lateral parietal and emotionally motivated, for example, by seeking a
frontal association cortex. reward, are clearly more successful in attention tasks
Some, even as early as Descartes, have argued that (Heilman and Valenstein, 2003). The orbital frontal cor-
language is necessary for conscious thought (Descartes, tex and other limbic circuits have been implicated and
1637/1988, 1649/1970) and because the left hemisphere are likely to be crucial for the motivational aspects of
is usually dominant for language it could be viewed attention (Faw, 2003).
as more important for consciousness than the right.
However, clearly non-verbal thoughts can still gener- The Binding Problem
ate conscious experiences (Devinsky, 2000). Therefore, A classical quandary in understanding attention
language might best be viewed as an important com- and consciousness is the question of where and how
ponent of the content of consciousness (along with the diverse aspects of any particular percept come
other specific functions such as visual perception, together to form a unified conscious experience. How
mathematical ability, judgment of distance, and so on), do the look, smell, and sound of your friend come
rather than as a necessary regulator of the overall together in your brain as “Adam.” Even within a

THE NEUROLOGY OF CONSCIOUSNESS


CORTICAL NETWORKS AND CONSCIOUSNESS 19
single modality, it is challenging to understand how more ventrally in the temporal-parietal junction and the
for example, the different parts of a visual scene are ventral frontal cortex (including the frontal operculum),
unified as whole image. This question has been and is strongly lateralized to the right hemisphere. One
referred to as the binding problem (Singer, 2001). important area of ongoing research is how the two
Many potential solutions have been proposed for how systems interact, for example, during stimulus-driven
different components of sensory input are bound reorienting in visual search where sensory inputs affect
together into a single percept, including coherent high- goal-oriented attention and vice-versa.
frequency oscillations, lateral connections between
neurons in particular cortical layers, among others Task-Positive and Task-Negative Networks
(Singer, 2001; Zmigrod and Hommel, 2013). In terms of Another important way of describing cortical
cortical anatomy some insights into how visual bind- attention networks has arisen from functional neuro-
ing occurs may be gleaned from a particular disorder imaging studies during tasks compared to rest, as well
referred to as Balint syndrome (Barton, 2011; Heilman as the analysis of resting functional connectivity. In a
and Valenstein, 2003; Michel and Henaff, 2004). series of observations by Raichle et al. (2001), Shulman
Patients with Balint syndrome experience simultag- et al. (1997) and subsequently confirmed by other
nosia, meaning that they perceive individual fragments groups it was noted that, regardless of the specific
or components of a visual scene one at a time without task, a particular set of regions tends to show reduced
assembling them into a coherent whole. Balint syn- activity during task blocks when functional neuro-
drome is caused by bilateral lesions at the juncture imaging data are analyzed by conventional block-
between the dorsolateral parietal and occipital cortices design analyses contrasting task versus rest. These
(often due to strokes in the watershed territory brain regions were dubbed the “default-mode net-
between the posterior and middle cerebral arteries). It work” (hypothesized to be relatively active at rest by
has been proposed that simultagnosia occurs because default) or “task-negative networks.” On the other
these lesions disconnect visual input from parietal and hand, regions showing relatively increased activity
other association cortices needed to stitch together the during task blocks show greater variability depending
individual components into a unified image. This on the specific task, but do show some general similar-
suggests that one mechanism for attentional binding ities between studies—particularly those involving
may be the interaction of primary cortices or unimodal attention—and have been referred to as “task-positive
association cortex with higher-order (heteromodal) networks.” Of interest, like many other important
parietal or other association cortices (Figure 1.11). In brain networks (Biswal et al., 1995, 1997), the default-
this case, one would predict that simultagnosia should mode and task-positive networks can be demonstrated
occur not just in visual but in other (e.g., auditory or in functional neuroimaging studies based on their high
tactile) modalities when unimodal cortices are dis- within-network connectivity using resting data in the
connected from the higher-order association cortex absence of task (Fox et al., 2005; Fransson, 2005;
bilaterally, a topic for potential additional investigation Greicius et al., 2003). At rest, the default-mode and
in future work. task-positive networks demonstrate high within-
network functional connectivity (correlations over time)
Top-Down and Bottom-Up Attention Networks and these two networks are overall negatively corre-
Emerging from other recent formulations of lated with each other (Figure 1.13). Based on both task-
attention, including those proposed by Mesulam, related analyses, and resting connectivity analyses, the
Heilman, and Posner (Heilman and Valenstein, 2003; default-mode network generally includes the following
Mesulam, 2000; Posner and Dehaene, 1994; Posner and cortical regions bilaterally: precuneus/posterior cingu-
Petersen, 1990), the model of Corbetta and Shulman late, posterior-inferior parietal lobule (angular gyrus),
(2002) emphasizes two separate but interacting sys- ventral-anterior medial frontal, middle temporal gyrus,
tems mediating the top-down and bottom-up aspects and medial temporal cortex (Figure 1.13) (Fox et al.,
of attention (Figure 1.12B). In this scheme, goal- 2005). The task-positive networks include the following
oriented selection of stimuli and responses is con- cortical regions bilaterally: anterior insula/frontal oper-
trolled by dorsal regions of the frontal and parietal culum, supplementary motor/dorsal medial frontal
association cortex, including the frontal eye fields and lobe, lateral premotor cortex (includes frontal eye
intraparietal sulcus. This top-down attention system is fields), anterior middle frontal gyrus, superior parietal
bilaterally distributed in both hemispheres. In contrast, lobule/anterior inferior parietal lobule, lateral inferior
a second attention system serves a stimulus-driven posterior temporal gyrus (lateral area 37) (Figure 1.13).
“circuit-breaking” role for grabbing and reorienting Because the concept of two large anti-correlated
attention in response to salient or changing sensory sti- networks mediating attention switching in the brain
muli. This bottom-up attention system is localized may be over-simplified, a number of groups have

THE NEUROLOGY OF CONSCIOUSNESS


20 1. NEUROANATOMICAL BASIS OF CONSCIOUSNESS

FIGURE 1.13 Task-positive and default-mode (task-negative) networks by resting functional connectivity analysis. A conjunction analysis
was performed by including voxels significantly positively or negatively correlated with three seed regions in the task-positive network (intra-
parietal sulcus; frontal eye fields; lateral posterior-inferior temporal region) and three in the default-mode network (ventral medial prefrontal
cortex; posterior cingulate/precuneus; lateral parietal cortex). Warm colors show regions correlated with the task-positive seeds or negatively
correlated with default-mode seeds. Cool colors show regions correlated with the default-mode seeds or negatively correlated with task-
positive seeds. Source: Reproduced with permission from Fox et al. (2005).

attempted to further refine these ideas and the field claustrum, a subcortical structure discussed above that
is still very much in evolution. It has been proposed might participate in attention and consciousness. In
that certain components of the task-positive net- additional analyses based on resting functional con-
work, referred to as the frontal parietal control nectivity and graph theory, the same group again
network, might play a role in switching between emphasized the importance of what they refer to as
externally directed attention mediated by the dorsal the cingulo-opercular network, as distinct from the
attention network (similar to top-down attention areas other task-positive and default-mode network nodes
of Corbetta and Shulman discussed above) and (Dosenbach et al., 2007). Another very interesting
internally directed attention mediated by the default- approach has been model-free analysis of functional
mode network (Gao and Lin, 2012; Vincent et al., neuroimaging data by simply using massive averaging
2008). There is evidence that the more lateral of large data sets during a simple attention task
components of the task-positive network, particularly (Gonzalez-Castillo et al., 2012). This type of analysis
the dorsolateral frontal and parietal cortices are more revealed that virtually the entire brain shows signals
important for externally directed attention, while the that vary over time in relation to the task, with a com-
medial components of the default-mode network bination of transient or sustained increases and/or
participate in internally directed attention (Demertzi decreases depending on the specific brain region.
et al., 2013; Vanhaudenhuyse et al., 2011). In another In summary, a large number of cortical networks
series of studies, Dosenbach et al. (2006, 2007, 2008) have been shown to participate in different aspects of
identified certain bilateral core components of the task- attention and to modulate their activity in relation to
positive network, namely the dorsal anterior cingu- onset and end of attention tasks. The detailed roles
late/medial superior frontal cortex (and adjacent that different components play in attention and
supplementary motor area), and anterior insula/frontal consciousness, including anterior insula/frontal oper-
operculum which constitute a common network culum, dorsal attention, default-mode, task-positive,
involved in: (i) task initiation, (ii) sustained activity, bottom-up, top-down and other networks, will be
and (iii) error detection (Figure 1.14). In contrast, they important subjects for further investigation.
found that other areas in the task-positive and default-
mode networks are activated or inactivated less con-
sistently in these three different conditions. Again it
Memory Systems and Consciousness
is unclear if the functional imaging signals in anterior As we have discussed, the level of consciousness
insula, which play a prominent role in this network, depends on regulation of alertness, attention, and
could also have a contribution from the nearby awareness. Alertness (arousal, wakefulness) can be

THE NEUROLOGY OF CONSCIOUSNESS


CORTICAL NETWORKS AND CONSCIOUSNESS 21

FIGURE 1.14 Core components of human task-related networks. Conjunction analysis of the fixed-effects maps for positive sustained,
start-cue and error-related activity. The dorsal anterior cingulate/medial superior frontal cortex (and adjacent supplementary motor area), and
anterior insula/frontal operculum carried positive sustained, start-cue and error-related activity across most of the tasks included in the analy-
sis (conjunction regions shown in black). Source: Reproduced with permission from Dosenbach et al. (2006).

tested based on responses to simple questions or subsequent report, investigating the “gateway” leading
commands, and attention based on tasks requiring into these systems may be a productive approach for
selective responses. Awareness, on the other hand, is understanding the mechanisms of conscious awareness.
demonstrated when a subject reports being aware of a
particular stimulus or event. Because this report
inevitably occurs after the event, testing of conscious
Volition and Conscious Free Will
awareness requires memory. Memory therefore plays a Although much research has been done in the field
crucial role in the ability to report conscious experi- of conscious perception particularly emphasizing the
ences. In fact it may be useful to define awareness as visual system, an equally important area of inves-
the attentive and other processes necessary for events tigation is the mechanisms of conscious action includ-
to be selected, handed off and encoded into memory ing those governing planning and initiation of
for subsequent report. voluntary movement (Roskies, 2010). Understanding
Depending on whether report of conscious aware- conscious action has important implications for deter-
ness occurs following a short or long delay, different mining moral and legal responsibility, as well as philo-
memory systems may participate. Immediate recall or sophical relevance including the definition of free
iconic memory most likely involves activity in pri- will. Conscious action can be divided into planning,
mary and unimodal cortices and where events are “premeditation” or initiation of future activities, and
initially processed (Coltheart, 1980; Loftus et al., 1992). awareness of ongoing or completed deeds. The latter is
Slightly more enduring memories lasting several more similar to conscious sensory perception, in the
seconds can enter working memory systems involving sense that it involves subsequent report of events that
the dorsolateral frontal and parietal association cortices have already taken place. Conscious planning and
(Funahashi et al., 1993; Ikkai and Curtis, 2011). Some initiation of voluntary movement as well as deci-
have defined consciousness based on the transfer of sion making on the other hand, likely involve other
attended events into working memory (Prinz, 2012). neuroanatomical circuits, as we will briefly discuss
Long-term memory storage of facts and autobiogra- further below.
phical events requires the action of medial temporal Motor planning depends on a distributed network
and medial diencephalic memory circuits (Squire and including the premotor, supplementary motor and
Wixted, 2011; Tulving, 2002). Episodic or declarative other frontal cortical circuits, interacting with parietal
memory, which depends on these anatomical systems, association cortex and subcortical networks especially
can be considered conscious memory in contrast to in the basal ganglia (Roskies, 2010). Decision making is
unconscious procedural memory that involves learning likewise a growing field in neuroscience, and recent
skills or unconscious priming. Because medial temporal work has again implicated distributed frontoparietal
and medial diencephalic memory systems are so specifi- and subcortical circuits (Glimcher, 2003; Lau and
cally linked to the encoding of conscious experiences for Glimcher, 2008; Levy et al., 2010; Platt and Glimcher,

THE NEUROLOGY OF CONSCIOUSNESS


22 1. NEUROANATOMICAL BASIS OF CONSCIOUSNESS

1999; Roskies, 2010). Based on stimulation mapping, an important topic of investigation that may shed
some interesting dissociations have been found. insights into what many consider the highest forms of
Stimulation of the parietal cortex is accompanied by an consciousness (Zeman and Coebergh, 2013).
awareness of voluntary movement initiation or urge to Important advances have recently been made in
move even if no actual motion takes place (Desmurget understanding the mechanisms that provide a first-
et al., 1999, 2009; Desmurget and Sirigu, 2009). On the person perspective by the innovative studies of Olaf
other hand, stimulation of the premotor cortex of the Blanke and colleagues (Blanke, 2004; Blanke et al., 2002;
frontal lobe can produce actual movement even when Easton et al., 2009; Heydrich et al., 2010; Lenggenhager
the subject is unaware of the movement (Desmurget et al., 2009; Lopez et al., 2010). A series of investigations
et al., 2009; Roskies, 2010). Finally, stimulation of the sup- using patients with out-of-body experience, or functional
plementary motor area produces an urge to move that neuroimaging and behavioral interventions to create an
may feel compulsory or involuntary (Fried et al., 1991). out-of-body experience in normal subjects, have revealed
Spontaneous voluntary movements are preceded that the right temporal-parietal junction plays a crucial
1 2 s earlier by a “readiness potential” or “Bereitschafts role in this unusual condition. Additional insights may
potential” that can be recorded from the scalp near the come from ongoing investigation of other situations
midline and has subsequently been localized to the where the first-person perspective is altered, for exam-
supplementary motor area based on intracranial mea- ple, during early development or in psychiatric disor-
surements (Fried et al., 2011). In a thought-provoking ders where depersonalization can occur (Bunning and
study, Benjamin Libet famously examined the timing of Blanke, 2005; Lewis, 2011; Sierra and David, 2011).
the readiness potential and provided evidence that it
may precede the moment when individuals become
Awareness: Conscious Report and
consciously aware of their decision or will to move by
several hundred milliseconds (Libet et al., 1982, 1983a,b).
Contrastive Analysis
The interpretation of this study and its relation to free Philosophers and scientists have long enjoyed a
will has been hotly debated in later work (Herrmann debate about whether or not consciousness can be
et al., 2008; Jo et al., 2013; Schurger et al., 2012). understood through scientific investigation. The crux of
the argument rests on the definitions of “consciousness”
and “understanding.” Some define the qualitative phe-
Self-Awareness and Embodiment
nomenal aspects of consciousness or qualia, as internal
Although some consider self-awareness to be the subjective feelings of awareness accessible only through
defining sine qua non of consciousness, others view first-person experience, and define understanding
awareness of self to be just one example of the many consciousness as having that first-person experience
things that an individual can be aware of, and there- (Chalmers, 1996). These definitions automatically
fore not necessary or sufficient for consciousness exclude the possibility of understanding consciousness
(Zeman, 2005). A closely related topic is the ability to through scientific means because science requires exter-
have a first-person perspective or sense of embodiment nal second-person observations. On the other hand, if
arising from one’s own individual point of view, one defines conscious awareness as an experience that
which clearly makes an important contribution to the can be described or reported to others, and if one
subjective feeling of awareness. defines understanding consciousness as identifying the
Awareness of self can be drastically and selectively necessary and sufficient physiological mechanisms for
impaired in certain neurological disorders, which may such an experience, then consciousness falls clearly in
provide some insight into the neuroanatomical basis of the realm of scientific investigation. The key to this
self-awareness. For example, patients with lesions in the second approach is that external report of conscious-
non-dominant (usually right) hemisphere particularly in ness is allowed as a method for investigating conscious-
the lateral parietal cortex are often agnostic to the very ness. Report of consciousness has its limitations of
existence of the entire left side of their own bodies, and course, but so do all scientific methods and as long as
when specifically asked sometimes consider their left the limitations are recognized and results interpreted
limbs to belong to someone else (Heilman et al., 2000). cautiously, much progress can be made.
Frontal lobe dysfunction commonly leads to impaired Taking this approach, a large number of studies have
self-monitoring or self-awareness, and is thought to investigated the contrast between brain activity when a
underlie patient’s inability to recognize their own defi- conscious event is reported or is not reported under
cits, for example when some patients confabulate instead very similar circumstances. This contrastive analysis
of admitting to amnesia (Kopelman, 2014). One special- has been performed in a variety of situations where
ized form of self-awareness is awareness of one’s own events that are or are not reported can be obtained
conscious awareness. This form of meta-cognition is also (Dehaene and Changeux, 2011; Lamme, 2006; Tononi

THE NEUROLOGY OF CONSCIOUSNESS


REFERENCES 23
and Koch, 2008). For example, presentation of visual, References
tactile, or other stimuli at the threshold of detection
Aggleton, J.P.E., 2000. The Amygdala: A Functional Analysis, second
yields approximately 50% that are reported. These stud- ed. Oxford University Press, Oxford, UK.
ies have shown that consciously reported stimuli are Albert, M.L., Silverberg, R., Reches, A., Berman, M., 1976. Cerebral
accompanied by significantly greater activity in the dominance for consciousness. Arch. Neurol. 33 (6), 453 454.
bilateral frontal and parietal association cortex than Anaclet, C., Parmentier, R., Ouk, K., Guidon, G., Buda, C., Sastre, J.-
P., et al., 2009. Orexin/hypocretin and histamine: distinct roles in
with the unreported stimuli (Li et al., 2014; Palva et al.,
the control of wakefulness demonstrated using knock-out mouse
2005; Wyart and Tallon-Baudry, 2008). Similarly, post- models. J. Neurosci. 29 (46), 14423 14438.
hoc analysis of items presented during memory Asplund, C.L., Todd, J.J., Snyder, A.P., Marois, R., 2010. A central
encoding has demonstrated increased activity in fronto- role for the lateral prefrontal cortex in goal-directed and
parietal cortex during presentations of items that are stimulus-driven attention. Nat. Neurosci. 13 (4), 507 512.
Available from: http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/nn.2509.
later successfully reported (Burke et al., 2014). Another
Bandyopadhyay, S., Hablitz, J.J., 2007. Dopaminergic modulation of
paradigm has been binocular rivalry, in which one local network activity in rat prefrontal cortex. J. Neurophysiol. 97
image is presented to the left eye and a different image (6), 4120 4128.
to the right eye, causing the perceived image to ran- Barton, J.J., 2011. Disorders of higher visual processing. Handb. Clin.
domly alternate every few seconds between the two Neurol. 102, 223 261.
Baumann, O., Mattingley, J.B., 2014. Effects of attention and
images (Blake and Logothetis, 2002; Polonsky et al.,
perceptual uncertainty on cerebellar activity during visual motion
2000). It was found that these alternating experiences perception. Cerebellum. 13 (1), 46 54.
can be related to alternating activity between different Beninato, M., Spencer, R.F., 1987. A cholinergic projection to the rat
neurons in the temporal and parietal association cortex, substantia nigra from the pedunculopontine tegmental nucleus.
particularly in a visual association cortex region in the Brain Res. 412 (1), 169 174.
Berridge, C.W., 2008. Noradrenergic modulation of arousal. Brain
inferior temporal lobe. Other paradigms including
Res. Rev. 58 (1), 1 17.
masking of visual stimuli by flanking stimuli presented Berridge, C.W., Page, M.E., Valentino, R.J., Foote, S.L., 1993. Effects
immediately before or after the target, or use of the of locus coeruleus inactivation on electroencephalographic activ-
“attentional blink” have also been employed to control ity in neocortex and hippocampus. Neuroscience. 55 (2),
whether or not stimuli are perceived and consciously 381 393.
Berridge, C.W., Schmeichel, B.E., España, R.A., 2012. Noradrenergic
reported, again showing that consciously perceived sti-
modulation of wakefulness/arousal. Sleep Med. Rev. 16 (2),
muli have greater activations including larger neuroim- 187 197.
aging signals, event-related potentials, or synchronized Bischoff-Grethe, A., Ivry, R.B., Grafton, S.T., 2002. Cerebellar
oscillations in the frontal and parietal association cortex involvement in response reassignment rather than attention.
(Dehaene and Changeux, 2011; Del Cul et al., 2009; J. Neurosci. 22 (2), 546 553.
Biswal, B., Yetkin, F.Z., Haughton, V.M., Hyde, J.S., 1995. Functional
Kouider and Dehaene, 2007; Marois et al., 2004).
connectivity in the motor cortex of resting human brain using
In summary, multiple studies using contrastive echo-planar MRI. Magn. Reson. Med. 34 (4), 537 541.
analysis have demonstrated that events reported as Biswal, B.B., Van Kylen, J., Hyde, J.S., 1997. Simultaneous assessment
consciously perceived give rise to greater activity in of flow and BOLD signals in resting-state functional connectivity
the bilateral frontal and parietal association cortex. maps. NMR Biomed. 10 (4 5), 165 170.
Blake, R., Logothetis, N., 2002. Visual competition. Nat. Rev.
These findings support the idea that the anato-
Neurosci. 3 (1), 13 21.
mical regions constituting the consciousness system Blanco-Centurion, C., Gerashchenko, D., Salin-Pascual, R.J., Shiromani,
(Figure 1.2) play an important role in regulating con- P.J., 2004. Effects of hypocretin2-saporin and antidopamine-beta-
scious awareness. Further work is needed to more hydroxylase-saporin neurotoxic lesions of the dorsolateral pons on
firmly establish that these areas are necessary and suffi- sleep and muscle tone. Eur. J. Neurosci. 19 (10), 2741 2752.
Blanco-Centurion, C., Gerashchenko, D., Shiromani, P.J., 2007. Effects
cient for conscious events, for example through lesion/
of saporin-induced lesions of three arousal populations on daily
inactivation and activation studies. In addition, levels of sleep and wake. J. Neurosci. 27 (51), 14041 14048.
although much has been learned about the neuroana- Blanco-Centurion, C.A., Shiromani, A., Winston, E., Shiromani, P.J.,
tomical structures participating in consciousness, 2006. Effects of hypocretin-1 in 192-IgG-saporin-lesioned rats.
further investigation will be crucial to firmly establish Eur. J. Neurosci. 24 (7), 2084 2088.
Blanke, O., 2004. Out of body experiences and their neural basis.
the physiological mechanisms through which con-
BMJ. 329 (7480), 1414 1415.
sciousness is generated in these networks. Blanke, O., Ortigue, S., Landis, T., Seeck, M., 2002. Stimulating
illusory own-body perceptions. Nature. 419 (6904), 269 270.
Bloem, B., Poorthuis, R.B., Mansvelder, H.D., 2014. Cholinergic
Acknowledgments modulation of the medial prefrontal cortex: the role of nicotinic
This work was supported by NIH R01NS055829, R01NS066974, receptors in attention and regulation of neuronal activity. Front.
R21 NS083783, R01MH67528, P30NS052519, U01NS045911, UL1 Neural Circuits. 8, 17.
RR0249139, the Swebilius Foundation, the Loughridge Williams Blumenfeld, H., 2002. Neuroanatomy Through Clinical Cases.
Foundation, and the Betsy and Jonathan Blattmachr Family. Sinauer Assoc. Publ., Inc., Sunderland, MA.

THE NEUROLOGY OF CONSCIOUSNESS


24 1. NEUROANATOMICAL BASIS OF CONSCIOUSNESS

Blumenfeld, H., 2009. Epilepsy and consciousness. In: Laureys, S., Corbetta, M., Shulman, G.L., 2002. Control of goal-directed and
Tononi, G. (Eds.), The Neurology of Consciousness: Cognitive stimulus-driven attention in the brain. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 3 (3),
Neuroscience and Neuropathology. Academic Press, New York, 201 215.
NY, pp. 15 30. (Chapter 2). Cornwall, J., Cooper, J.D., Phillipson, O.T., 1990. Afferent and
Blumenfeld, H., 2010. Neuroanatomy Through Clinical Cases, second efferent connections of the laterodorsal tegmental nucleus in the
ed. Sinauer Assoc Publ Co., Sunderland, MA. rat. Brain Res. Bull. 25 (2), 271 284.
Blumenfeld, H., 2012. Impaired consciousness in epilepsy. Lancet Correa-Sales, C., Rabin, B.C., Maze, M., 1992. A hypnotic response to
Neurol. 11, 814 826. dexmedetomidine, an alpha 2 agonist, is mediated in the locus
Boly, M., Garrido, M.I., Gosseries, O., Bruno, M.A., Boveroux, P., coeruleus in rats. Anesthesiology. 76 (6), 948 952.
Schnakers, C., et al., 2011. Preserved feedforward but impaired top- Crick, F., 1984. Function of the thalamic reticular complex: the
down processes in the vegetative state. Science. 332 (6031), 858 862. searchlight hypothesis. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA. 81 (14),
Bourgin, P., Huitrón-Reséndiz, S., Spier, A.D., Fabre, V., Morte, B., 4586 4590.
Criado, J.R., et al., 2000. Hypocretin-1 modulates rapid eye move- Crick, F.C., Koch, C., 2005. What is the function of the claustrum?
ment sleep through activation of locus coeruleus neurons. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B Biol. Sci. 360 (1458), 1271 1279.
J. Neurosci. 20 (20), 7760 7765. Damasio, A., Carvalho, G.B., 2013. The nature of feelings: evolu-
Brashear, H.R., Zaborszky, L., Heimer, L., 1986. Distribution of tionary and neurobiological origins. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 14 (2),
gabaergic and cholinergic neurons in the rat diagonal band. 143 152.
Neuroscience. 17 (2), 439 451. Dehaene, S., Changeux, J.P., 2011. Experimental and theoretical
Bremer, F., 1955. Interrelationships between cortex and subcortical approaches to conscious processing. Neuron. 70 (2), 200 227.
structures; introductory remarks. Electroencephalogr. Clin. Dehaene, S., Kerszberg, M., Changeux, J.P., 1998. A neuronal model
Neurophysiol. Suppl.(Suppl. 4), 145 148. of a global workspace in effortful cognitive tasks. Proc. Natl.
Brown, E.N., Lydic, R., Schiff, N.D., 2010. General anesthesia, sleep, Acad. Sci. USA. 95 (24), 14529 14534.
and coma. N. Engl. J. Med. 363 (27), 2638 2650. Dehaene, S., Changeux, J.P., Naccache, L., Sackur, J., Sergent, C.,
Brown, R.E., Stevens, D.R., Haas, H.L., 2001. The physiology of brain 2006. Conscious, preconscious, and subliminal processing: a
histamine. Prog. Neurobiol. 63 (6), 637 672. testable taxonomy. Trends Cogn. Sci. 10 (5), 204 211.
Buchanan, G.F., Richerson, G.B., 2010. Central serotonin neurons are Del Cul, A., Dehaene, S., Reyes, P., Bravo, E., Slachevsky, A., 2009.
required for arousal to CO2. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA. 107 (37), Causal role of prefrontal cortex in the threshold for access to
16354 16359. consciousness. Brain. 132 (Pt 9), 2531 2540.
Bunning, S., Blanke, O., 2005. The out-of body experience: precipitating Demertzi, A., Soddu, A., Laureys, S., 2013. Consciousness supporting
factors and neural correlates. Prog. Brain Res. 150, 331 350. networks. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 23 (2), 239 244.
Burke, J.F., Long, N.M., Zaghloul, K.A., Sharan, A.D., Sperling, M.R., De Sarro, G.B., Ascioti, C., Froio, F., Libri, V., Nistico, G., 1987.
Kahana, M.J., 2014. Human intracranial high-frequency activity Evidence that locus coeruleus is the site where clonidine and
maps episodic memory formation in space and time. Neuroimage. drugs acting at alpha 1- and alpha 2-adrenoceptors affect sleep
85 (Pt 2), 834 843. and arousal mechanisms. Br. J. Pharmacol. 90 (4), 675 685.
Buschman, T.J., Miller, E.K., 2007. Top-down versus bottom-up Descartes, R., 1637/1988. Discourse on the method. In: Descartes:
control of attention in the prefrontal and posterior parietal Selected Philosophical Writings (Cottingham, Stoothoff, Murdoch,
cortices. Science. 315 (5820), 1860 1862. Trans.). Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, p. 45.
Buzsaki, G., Wang, X.J., 2012. Mechanisms of gamma oscillations. Descartes, R., 1649/1970. Letter to Henry More (February 5, 1649). In:
Annu. Rev. Neurosci. Available from: http://dx.doi.org/10.1146/ Philosophical Letters (A. Kenny, Trans.). Clarendon Press, Oxford,
annurev-neuro-062111-150444. pp. 244 245.
Buzsaki, G., Bickford, R.G., Ponomareff, G., Thal, L.J., Mandel, R., Desmurget, M., Sirigu, A., 2009. A parietal-premotor network for
Gage, F.H., 1988. Nucleus basalis and thalamic control of neocor- movement intention and motor awareness. Trends Cogn. Sci. 13
tical activity in the freely moving rat. J. Neurosci. 8 (11), (10), 411 419.
4007 4026. Desmurget, M., Epstein, C.M., Turner, R.S., Prablanc, C., Alexander,
Catsman-Berrevoets, C.E., Kuypers, H.G., 1981. A search for corti- G.E., Grafton, S.T., 1999. Role of the posterior parietal cortex in
cospinal collaterals to thalamus and mesencephalon by means of updating reaching movements to a visual target. Nat. Neurosci. 2
multiple retrograde fluorescent tracers in cat and rat. Brain Res. (6), 563 567.
218 (1 2), 15 33. Desmurget, M., Reilly, K.T., Richard, N., Szathmari, A., Mottolese,
Chalmers, D.J., 1996. The Conscious Mind: In Search of a C., Sirigu, A., 2009. Movement intention after parietal cortex
Fundamental Theory. Oxford University Press, Oxford, UK. stimulation in humans. Science. 324 (5928), 811 813.
Chemelli, R.M., Willie, J.T., Sinton, C.M., Elmquist, J.K., Scammell, Deutch, A.Y., Goldstein, M., Roth, R.H., 1986. Activation of the
T., Lee, C., et al., 1999. Narcolepsy in orexin knockout mice: locus coeruleus induced by selective stimulation of the ventral
molecular genetics of sleep regulation. Cell. 98 (4), 437 451. tegmental area. Brain Res. 363 (2), 307 314.
Cirelli, C., Tononi, G., 2004. Locus ceruleus control of state- Devinsky, O., 2000. Right cerebral hemisphere dominance for a sense
dependent gene expression. J. Neurosci. 24 (23), 5410 5419. of corporeal and emotional self. Epilepsy Behav. 1, 60 73.
Coltheart, M., 1980. Iconic memory and visible persistence. Percept. Dosenbach, N.U., Visscher, K.M., Palmer, E.D., Miezin, F.M.,
Psychophys. 27 (3), 183 228. Wenger, K.K., Kang, H.C., et al., 2006. A core system for the
Combarros, O., Infante, J., Berciano, J., 2000. Akinetic mutism from implementation of task sets. Neuron. 50 (5), 799 812.
frontal lobe damage responding to levodopa. J. Neurol. 247 (7), Dosenbach, N.U., Fair, D.A., Miezin, F.M., Cohen, A.L., Wenger, K.
568 569. K., Dosenbach, R.A., et al., 2007. Distinct brain networks for
Constantinople, C.M., Bruno, R.M., 2011. Effects and mechanisms of adaptive and stable task control in humans. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci.
wakefulness on local cortical networks. Neuron. 69 (6), 1061 1068. USA. 104 (26), 11073 11078.
Cooper, J.R., Bloom, F.E., Roth, R.H., 2003. The Biochemical Basis Dosenbach, N.U., Fair, D.A., Cohen, A.L., Schlaggar, B.L., Petersen,
of Neuropharmacology, eighth ed. Oxford University Press, S.E., 2008. A dual-networks architecture of top-down control.
Oxford, New York. Trends Cogn. Sci. 12 (3), 99 105.

THE NEUROLOGY OF CONSCIOUSNESS


REFERENCES 25
Dreher, J.C., Grafman, J., 2002. The roles of the cerebellum and basal Fried, I., Katz, A., McCarthy, G., Sass, K.J., Williamson, P., Spencer,
ganglia in timing and error prediction. Eur. J. Neurosci. 16 (8), S.S., et al., 1991. Functional organization of human supplementary
1609 1619. motor cortex studied by electrical stimulation. J. Neurosci. 11
Dringenberg, H.C., Kuo, M.C., 2003. Histaminergic facilitation of (11), 3656 3666.
electrocorticographic activation: role of basal forebrain, thalamus, Fried, I., Mukamel, R., Kreiman, G., 2011. Internally generated pre-
and neocortex. Eur. J. Neurosci. 18 (8), 2285 2291. activation of single neurons in human medial frontal cortex pre-
Dringenberg, H.C., Olmstead, M.C., 2003. Integrated contributions dicts volition. Neuron. 69 (3), 548 562.
of basal forebrain and thalamus to neocortical activation Fuller, P.M., Sherman, D., Pedersen, N.P., Saper, C.B., Lu, J., 2011.
elicited by pedunculopontine tegmental stimulation in urethane- Reassessment of the structural basis of the ascending arousal sys-
anesthetized rats. Neuroscience. 119 (3), 839 853. tem. J. Comp. Neurol. 519 (5), 933 956.
Dugovic, C., Wauquier, A., Leysen, J.E., Marrannes, R., Janssen, P.A., Funahashi, S., Bruce, C.J., Goldman-Rakic, P.S., 1993. Dorsolateral
1989. Functional role of 5-HT2 receptors in the regulation of sleep prefrontal lesions and oculomotor delayed-response perfor-
and wakefulness in the rat. Psychopharmacology (Berl). 97 (4), mance: evidence for mnemonic “scotomas”. J. Neurosci. 13 (4),
436 442. 1479 1497.
Duque, A., Balatoni, B., Detari, L., Zaborszky, L., 2000. EEG correla- Gabr, M., Luders, H., Dinner, D., Morris, H., Wyllie, E., 1989. Speech
tion of the discharge properties of identified neurons in the basal manifestations in lateralization of temporal lobe seizures. Ann.
forebrain. J. Neurophysiol. 84 (3), 1627 1635. Neurol. 25, 82 87.
Duque, A., Tepper, J.M., Detari, L., Ascoli, G.A., Zaborszky, L., 2007. Gao, W., Lin, W., 2012. Frontal parietal control network regulates the
Morphological characterization of electrophysiologically and anti-correlated default and dorsal attention networks. Hum. Brain
immunohistochemically identified basal forebrain cholinergic Mapp. 33 (1), 192 202.
and neuropeptide Y-containing neurons. Brain Struct. Funct. 212 Garcia-Cabezas, M.A., Martinez-Sanchez, P., Sanchez-Gonzalez,
(1), 55 73. M.A., Garzon, M., Cavada, C., 2009. Dopamine innervation in the
Dzoljic, M.R., Ukponmwan, O.E., Saxena, P.R., 1992. 5-HT1-like thalamus: monkey versus rat. Cereb. Cortex. 19 (2), 424 434.
receptor agonists enhance wakefulness. Neuropharmacology. 31 Gerashchenko, D., Blanco-Centurion, C., Greco, M.A., Shiromani,
(7), 623 633. P.J., 2003. Effects of lateral hypothalamic lesion with the neuro-
Easton, S., Blanke, O., Mohr, C., 2009. A putative implication toxin hypocretin-2-saporin on sleep in Long-Evans rats.
for fronto-parietal connectivity in out-of-body experiences. Neuroscience. 116 (1), 223 235.
Cortex. 45 (2), 216 227. Giacino, J.T., Whyte, J., Bagiella, E., Kalmar, K., Childs, N., Khademi,
Englot, D.J., Blumenfeld, H., 2009. Consciousness and epilepsy: why A., et al., 2012. Placebo-controlled trial of amantadine for severe
are complex-partial seizures complex? Prog. Brain Res. 177, traumatic brain injury. N. Engl. J. Med. 366 (9), 819 826.
147 170. Giacino, J.T., Fins, J.J., Laureys, S., Schiff, N.D., 2014. Disorders of
Englot, D.J., Yang, L., Hamid, H., Danielson, N., Bai, X., Marfeo, A., consciousness after acquired brain injury: the state of the science.
et al., 2010. Impaired consciousness in temporal lobe seizures: Nat. Rev. Neurol. 10 (2), 99 114.
role of cortical slow activity. Brain. 133 (Pt 12), 3764 3777. Glimcher, P.W., 2003. The neurobiology of visual-saccadic decision
Eriksson, K.S., Sergeeva, O., Brown, R.E., Haas, H.L., 2001. Orexin/ making. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 26, 133 179.
hypocretin excites the histaminergic neurons of the tubero- Gonzalez-Castillo, J., Saad, Z.S., Handwerker, D.A., Inati, S.J., Brenowitz,
mammillary nucleus. J. Neurosci. 21 (23), 9273 9279. N., Bandettini, P.A., 2012. Whole-brain, time-locked activation with
España, R.A., Baldo, B.A., Kelley, A.E., Berridge, C.W., 2001. Wake- simple tasks revealed using massive averaging and model-free
promoting and sleep-suppressing actions of hypocretin (orexin): analysis. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA. 109 (14), 5487 5492.
basal forebrain sites of action. Neuroscience. 106 (4), 699 715. Govindaiah, G., Wang, Y., Cox, C.L., 2010. Dopamine enhances
Fallon, J.H., 1981. Collateralization of monoamine neurons: mesote- the excitability of somatosensory thalamocortical neurons.
lencephalic dopamine projections to caudate, septum, and frontal Neuroscience. 170 (4), 981 991.
cortex. J. Neurosci. 1 (12), 1361 1368. Greicius, M.D., Krasnow, B., Reiss, A.L., Menon, V., 2003. Functional
Faw, B., 2003. Pre-frontal executive committee for perception, work- connectivity in the resting brain: a network analysis of the default
ing memory, attention, long-term memory, motor control, and mode hypothesis. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA. 100 (1), 253 258.
thinking: a tutorial review. Conscious. Cogn. 12 (1), 83 139. Groenewegen, H.J., 1988. Organization of the afferent connections of
Fisher, C.M., 1969. The neurological examination of the comatose the mediodorsal thalamic nucleus in the rat, related to the
patient. Acta Neurol. Scand. 45 (Suppl. 36), 31 56. mediodorsal-prefrontal topography. Neuroscience. 24 (2),
Foote, S.L., Bloom, F.E., Aston-Jones, G., 1983. Nucleus locus ceru- 379 431.
leus: new evidence of anatomical and physiological specificity. Hagan, J.J., Leslie, R.A., Patel, S., Evans, M.L., Wattam, T.A., Holmes,
Physiol. Rev. 63 (3), 844 914. S., et al., 1999. Orexin A activates locus coeruleus cell firing and
Fox, M.D., Snyder, A.Z., Vincent, J.L., Corbetta, M., Van Essen, D.C., increases arousal in the rat. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA. 96 (19),
Raichle, M.E., 2005. The human brain is intrinsically organized 10911 10916.
into dynamic, anticorrelated functional networks. Proc. Natl. Hager, F., Volz, H.P., Gaser, C., Mentzel, H.J., Kaiser, W.A., Sauer, H.,
Acad. Sci. USA. 102 (27), 9673 9678. 1998. Challenging the anterior attentional system with a continuous
Franczek, S., Demakis, G.J., Pennell, E.B., DeBose, C., Gilmore, R.L., performance task: a functional magnetic resonance imaging
1997. Revisited: cerebral dominance for consciousness. approach. Eur. Arch. Psychiatry Clin. Neurosci. 248 (4), 161 170.
Electroencephalogr. Clin. Neurophysiol. 102, 24P 25P. Hallanger, A.E., Wainer, B.H., 1988. Ascending projections from the
Fransson, P., 2005. Spontaneous low-frequency BOLD signal fluctua- pedunculopontine tegmental nucleus and the adjacent mesopon-
tions: an fMRI investigation of the resting-state default mode of tine tegmentum in the rat. J. Comp. Neurol. 274 (4), 483 515.
brain function hypothesis. Hum. Brain Mapp. 26 (1), 15 29. Hallanger, A.E., Levey, A.I., Lee, H.J., Rye, D.B., Wainer, B.H., 1987.
Freund, T.F., Meskenaite, V., 1992. gamma-Aminobutyric acid- The origins of cholinergic and other subcortical afferents to the
containing basal forebrain neurons innervate inhibitory interneur- thalamus in the rat. J. Comp. Neurol. 262 (1), 105 124.
ons in the neocortex. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA. 89 (2), Hannon, J., Hoyer, D., 2008. Molecular biology of 5-HT receptors.
738 742. In: Monti, J.M., Pandi-Perumal, S.R., Jacobs, B.L., Nutt, D.J. (Eds.),

THE NEUROLOGY OF CONSCIOUSNESS


Another random document with
no related content on Scribd:
DANCE ON STILTS AT THE GIRLS’ UNYAGO, NIUCHI

Newala, too, suffers from the distance of its water-supply—at least


the Newala of to-day does; there was once another Newala in a lovely
valley at the foot of the plateau. I visited it and found scarcely a trace
of houses, only a Christian cemetery, with the graves of several
missionaries and their converts, remaining as a monument of its
former glories. But the surroundings are wonderfully beautiful. A
thick grove of splendid mango-trees closes in the weather-worn
crosses and headstones; behind them, combining the useful and the
agreeable, is a whole plantation of lemon-trees covered with ripe
fruit; not the small African kind, but a much larger and also juicier
imported variety, which drops into the hands of the passing traveller,
without calling for any exertion on his part. Old Newala is now under
the jurisdiction of the native pastor, Daudi, at Chingulungulu, who,
as I am on very friendly terms with him, allows me, as a matter of
course, the use of this lemon-grove during my stay at Newala.
FEET MUTILATED BY THE RAVAGES OF THE “JIGGER”
(Sarcopsylla penetrans)

The water-supply of New Newala is in the bottom of the valley,


some 1,600 feet lower down. The way is not only long and fatiguing,
but the water, when we get it, is thoroughly bad. We are suffering not
only from this, but from the fact that the arrangements at Newala are
nothing short of luxurious. We have a separate kitchen—a hut built
against the boma palisade on the right of the baraza, the interior of
which is not visible from our usual position. Our two cooks were not
long in finding this out, and they consequently do—or rather neglect
to do—what they please. In any case they do not seem to be very
particular about the boiling of our drinking-water—at least I can
attribute to no other cause certain attacks of a dysenteric nature,
from which both Knudsen and I have suffered for some time. If a
man like Omari has to be left unwatched for a moment, he is capable
of anything. Besides this complaint, we are inconvenienced by the
state of our nails, which have become as hard as glass, and crack on
the slightest provocation, and I have the additional infliction of
pimples all over me. As if all this were not enough, we have also, for
the last week been waging war against the jigger, who has found his
Eldorado in the hot sand of the Makonde plateau. Our men are seen
all day long—whenever their chronic colds and the dysentery likewise
raging among them permit—occupied in removing this scourge of
Africa from their feet and trying to prevent the disastrous
consequences of its presence. It is quite common to see natives of
this place with one or two toes missing; many have lost all their toes,
or even the whole front part of the foot, so that a well-formed leg
ends in a shapeless stump. These ravages are caused by the female of
Sarcopsylla penetrans, which bores its way under the skin and there
develops an egg-sac the size of a pea. In all books on the subject, it is
stated that one’s attention is called to the presence of this parasite by
an intolerable itching. This agrees very well with my experience, so
far as the softer parts of the sole, the spaces between and under the
toes, and the side of the foot are concerned, but if the creature
penetrates through the harder parts of the heel or ball of the foot, it
may escape even the most careful search till it has reached maturity.
Then there is no time to be lost, if the horrible ulceration, of which
we see cases by the dozen every day, is to be prevented. It is much
easier, by the way, to discover the insect on the white skin of a
European than on that of a native, on which the dark speck scarcely
shows. The four or five jiggers which, in spite of the fact that I
constantly wore high laced boots, chose my feet to settle in, were
taken out for me by the all-accomplished Knudsen, after which I
thought it advisable to wash out the cavities with corrosive
sublimate. The natives have a different sort of disinfectant—they fill
the hole with scraped roots. In a tiny Makua village on the slope of
the plateau south of Newala, we saw an old woman who had filled all
the spaces under her toe-nails with powdered roots by way of
prophylactic treatment. What will be the result, if any, who can say?
The rest of the many trifling ills which trouble our existence are
really more comic than serious. In the absence of anything else to
smoke, Knudsen and I at last opened a box of cigars procured from
the Indian store-keeper at Lindi, and tried them, with the most
distressing results. Whether they contain opium or some other
narcotic, neither of us can say, but after the tenth puff we were both
“off,” three-quarters stupefied and unspeakably wretched. Slowly we
recovered—and what happened next? Half-an-hour later we were
once more smoking these poisonous concoctions—so insatiable is the
craving for tobacco in the tropics.
Even my present attacks of fever scarcely deserve to be taken
seriously. I have had no less than three here at Newala, all of which
have run their course in an incredibly short time. In the early
afternoon, I am busy with my old natives, asking questions and
making notes. The strong midday coffee has stimulated my spirits to
an extraordinary degree, the brain is active and vigorous, and work
progresses rapidly, while a pleasant warmth pervades the whole
body. Suddenly this gives place to a violent chill, forcing me to put on
my overcoat, though it is only half-past three and the afternoon sun
is at its hottest. Now the brain no longer works with such acuteness
and logical precision; more especially does it fail me in trying to
establish the syntax of the difficult Makua language on which I have
ventured, as if I had not enough to do without it. Under the
circumstances it seems advisable to take my temperature, and I do
so, to save trouble, without leaving my seat, and while going on with
my work. On examination, I find it to be 101·48°. My tutors are
abruptly dismissed and my bed set up in the baraza; a few minutes
later I am in it and treating myself internally with hot water and
lemon-juice.
Three hours later, the thermometer marks nearly 104°, and I make
them carry me back into the tent, bed and all, as I am now perspiring
heavily, and exposure to the cold wind just beginning to blow might
mean a fatal chill. I lie still for a little while, and then find, to my
great relief, that the temperature is not rising, but rather falling. This
is about 7.30 p.m. At 8 p.m. I find, to my unbounded astonishment,
that it has fallen below 98·6°, and I feel perfectly well. I read for an
hour or two, and could very well enjoy a smoke, if I had the
wherewithal—Indian cigars being out of the question.
Having no medical training, I am at a loss to account for this state
of things. It is impossible that these transitory attacks of high fever
should be malarial; it seems more probable that they are due to a
kind of sunstroke. On consulting my note-book, I become more and
more inclined to think this is the case, for these attacks regularly
follow extreme fatigue and long exposure to strong sunshine. They at
least have the advantage of being only short interruptions to my
work, as on the following morning I am always quite fresh and fit.
My treasure of a cook is suffering from an enormous hydrocele which
makes it difficult for him to get up, and Moritz is obliged to keep in
the dark on account of his inflamed eyes. Knudsen’s cook, a raw boy
from somewhere in the bush, knows still less of cooking than Omari;
consequently Nils Knudsen himself has been promoted to the vacant
post. Finding that we had come to the end of our supplies, he began
by sending to Chingulungulu for the four sucking-pigs which we had
bought from Matola and temporarily left in his charge; and when
they came up, neatly packed in a large crate, he callously slaughtered
the biggest of them. The first joint we were thoughtless enough to
entrust for roasting to Knudsen’s mshenzi cook, and it was
consequently uneatable; but we made the rest of the animal into a
jelly which we ate with great relish after weeks of underfeeding,
consuming incredible helpings of it at both midday and evening
meals. The only drawback is a certain want of variety in the tinned
vegetables. Dr. Jäger, to whom the Geographical Commission
entrusted the provisioning of the expeditions—mine as well as his
own—because he had more time on his hands than the rest of us,
seems to have laid in a huge stock of Teltow turnips,[46] an article of
food which is all very well for occasional use, but which quickly palls
when set before one every day; and we seem to have no other tins
left. There is no help for it—we must put up with the turnips; but I
am certain that, once I am home again, I shall not touch them for ten
years to come.
Amid all these minor evils, which, after all, go to make up the
genuine flavour of Africa, there is at least one cheering touch:
Knudsen has, with the dexterity of a skilled mechanic, repaired my 9
× 12 cm. camera, at least so far that I can use it with a little care.
How, in the absence of finger-nails, he was able to accomplish such a
ticklish piece of work, having no tool but a clumsy screw-driver for
taking to pieces and putting together again the complicated
mechanism of the instantaneous shutter, is still a mystery to me; but
he did it successfully. The loss of his finger-nails shows him in a light
contrasting curiously enough with the intelligence evinced by the
above operation; though, after all, it is scarcely surprising after his
ten years’ residence in the bush. One day, at Lindi, he had occasion
to wash a dog, which must have been in need of very thorough
cleansing, for the bottle handed to our friend for the purpose had an
extremely strong smell. Having performed his task in the most
conscientious manner, he perceived with some surprise that the dog
did not appear much the better for it, and was further surprised by
finding his own nails ulcerating away in the course of the next few
days. “How was I to know that carbolic acid has to be diluted?” he
mutters indignantly, from time to time, with a troubled gaze at his
mutilated finger-tips.
Since we came to Newala we have been making excursions in all
directions through the surrounding country, in accordance with old
habit, and also because the akida Sefu did not get together the tribal
elders from whom I wanted information so speedily as he had
promised. There is, however, no harm done, as, even if seen only
from the outside, the country and people are interesting enough.
The Makonde plateau is like a large rectangular table rounded off
at the corners. Measured from the Indian Ocean to Newala, it is
about seventy-five miles long, and between the Rovuma and the
Lukuledi it averages fifty miles in breadth, so that its superficial area
is about two-thirds of that of the kingdom of Saxony. The surface,
however, is not level, but uniformly inclined from its south-western
edge to the ocean. From the upper edge, on which Newala lies, the
eye ranges for many miles east and north-east, without encountering
any obstacle, over the Makonde bush. It is a green sea, from which
here and there thick clouds of smoke rise, to show that it, too, is
inhabited by men who carry on their tillage like so many other
primitive peoples, by cutting down and burning the bush, and
manuring with the ashes. Even in the radiant light of a tropical day
such a fire is a grand sight.
Much less effective is the impression produced just now by the
great western plain as seen from the edge of the plateau. As often as
time permits, I stroll along this edge, sometimes in one direction,
sometimes in another, in the hope of finding the air clear enough to
let me enjoy the view; but I have always been disappointed.
Wherever one looks, clouds of smoke rise from the burning bush,
and the air is full of smoke and vapour. It is a pity, for under more
favourable circumstances the panorama of the whole country up to
the distant Majeje hills must be truly magnificent. It is of little use
taking photographs now, and an outline sketch gives a very poor idea
of the scenery. In one of these excursions I went out of my way to
make a personal attempt on the Makonde bush. The present edge of
the plateau is the result of a far-reaching process of destruction
through erosion and denudation. The Makonde strata are
everywhere cut into by ravines, which, though short, are hundreds of
yards in depth. In consequence of the loose stratification of these
beds, not only are the walls of these ravines nearly vertical, but their
upper end is closed by an equally steep escarpment, so that the
western edge of the Makonde plateau is hemmed in by a series of
deep, basin-like valleys. In order to get from one side of such a ravine
to the other, I cut my way through the bush with a dozen of my men.
It was a very open part, with more grass than scrub, but even so the
short stretch of less than two hundred yards was very hard work; at
the end of it the men’s calicoes were in rags and they themselves
bleeding from hundreds of scratches, while even our strong khaki
suits had not escaped scatheless.

NATIVE PATH THROUGH THE MAKONDE BUSH, NEAR


MAHUTA

I see increasing reason to believe that the view formed some time
back as to the origin of the Makonde bush is the correct one. I have
no doubt that it is not a natural product, but the result of human
occupation. Those parts of the high country where man—as a very
slight amount of practice enables the eye to perceive at once—has not
yet penetrated with axe and hoe, are still occupied by a splendid
timber forest quite able to sustain a comparison with our mixed
forests in Germany. But wherever man has once built his hut or tilled
his field, this horrible bush springs up. Every phase of this process
may be seen in the course of a couple of hours’ walk along the main
road. From the bush to right or left, one hears the sound of the axe—
not from one spot only, but from several directions at once. A few
steps further on, we can see what is taking place. The brush has been
cut down and piled up in heaps to the height of a yard or more,
between which the trunks of the large trees stand up like the last
pillars of a magnificent ruined building. These, too, present a
melancholy spectacle: the destructive Makonde have ringed them—
cut a broad strip of bark all round to ensure their dying off—and also
piled up pyramids of brush round them. Father and son, mother and
son-in-law, are chopping away perseveringly in the background—too
busy, almost, to look round at the white stranger, who usually excites
so much interest. If you pass by the same place a week later, the piles
of brushwood have disappeared and a thick layer of ashes has taken
the place of the green forest. The large trees stretch their
smouldering trunks and branches in dumb accusation to heaven—if
they have not already fallen and been more or less reduced to ashes,
perhaps only showing as a white stripe on the dark ground.
This work of destruction is carried out by the Makonde alike on the
virgin forest and on the bush which has sprung up on sites already
cultivated and deserted. In the second case they are saved the trouble
of burning the large trees, these being entirely absent in the
secondary bush.
After burning this piece of forest ground and loosening it with the
hoe, the native sows his corn and plants his vegetables. All over the
country, he goes in for bed-culture, which requires, and, in fact,
receives, the most careful attention. Weeds are nowhere tolerated in
the south of German East Africa. The crops may fail on the plains,
where droughts are frequent, but never on the plateau with its
abundant rains and heavy dews. Its fortunate inhabitants even have
the satisfaction of seeing the proud Wayao and Wamakua working
for them as labourers, driven by hunger to serve where they were
accustomed to rule.
But the light, sandy soil is soon exhausted, and would yield no
harvest the second year if cultivated twice running. This fact has
been familiar to the native for ages; consequently he provides in
time, and, while his crop is growing, prepares the next plot with axe
and firebrand. Next year he plants this with his various crops and
lets the first piece lie fallow. For a short time it remains waste and
desolate; then nature steps in to repair the destruction wrought by
man; a thousand new growths spring out of the exhausted soil, and
even the old stumps put forth fresh shoots. Next year the new growth
is up to one’s knees, and in a few years more it is that terrible,
impenetrable bush, which maintains its position till the black
occupier of the land has made the round of all the available sites and
come back to his starting point.
The Makonde are, body and soul, so to speak, one with this bush.
According to my Yao informants, indeed, their name means nothing
else but “bush people.” Their own tradition says that they have been
settled up here for a very long time, but to my surprise they laid great
stress on an original immigration. Their old homes were in the
south-east, near Mikindani and the mouth of the Rovuma, whence
their peaceful forefathers were driven by the continual raids of the
Sakalavas from Madagascar and the warlike Shirazis[47] of the coast,
to take refuge on the almost inaccessible plateau. I have studied
African ethnology for twenty years, but the fact that changes of
population in this apparently quiet and peaceable corner of the earth
could have been occasioned by outside enterprises taking place on
the high seas, was completely new to me. It is, no doubt, however,
correct.
The charming tribal legend of the Makonde—besides informing us
of other interesting matters—explains why they have to live in the
thickest of the bush and a long way from the edge of the plateau,
instead of making their permanent homes beside the purling brooks
and springs of the low country.
“The place where the tribe originated is Mahuta, on the southern
side of the plateau towards the Rovuma, where of old time there was
nothing but thick bush. Out of this bush came a man who never
washed himself or shaved his head, and who ate and drank but little.
He went out and made a human figure from the wood of a tree
growing in the open country, which he took home to his abode in the
bush and there set it upright. In the night this image came to life and
was a woman. The man and woman went down together to the
Rovuma to wash themselves. Here the woman gave birth to a still-
born child. They left that place and passed over the high land into the
valley of the Mbemkuru, where the woman had another child, which
was also born dead. Then they returned to the high bush country of
Mahuta, where the third child was born, which lived and grew up. In
course of time, the couple had many more children, and called
themselves Wamatanda. These were the ancestral stock of the
Makonde, also called Wamakonde,[48] i.e., aborigines. Their
forefather, the man from the bush, gave his children the command to
bury their dead upright, in memory of the mother of their race who
was cut out of wood and awoke to life when standing upright. He also
warned them against settling in the valleys and near large streams,
for sickness and death dwelt there. They were to make it a rule to
have their huts at least an hour’s walk from the nearest watering-
place; then their children would thrive and escape illness.”
The explanation of the name Makonde given by my informants is
somewhat different from that contained in the above legend, which I
extract from a little book (small, but packed with information), by
Pater Adams, entitled Lindi und sein Hinterland. Otherwise, my
results agree exactly with the statements of the legend. Washing?
Hapana—there is no such thing. Why should they do so? As it is, the
supply of water scarcely suffices for cooking and drinking; other
people do not wash, so why should the Makonde distinguish himself
by such needless eccentricity? As for shaving the head, the short,
woolly crop scarcely needs it,[49] so the second ancestral precept is
likewise easy enough to follow. Beyond this, however, there is
nothing ridiculous in the ancestor’s advice. I have obtained from
various local artists a fairly large number of figures carved in wood,
ranging from fifteen to twenty-three inches in height, and
representing women belonging to the great group of the Mavia,
Makonde, and Matambwe tribes. The carving is remarkably well
done and renders the female type with great accuracy, especially the
keloid ornamentation, to be described later on. As to the object and
meaning of their works the sculptors either could or (more probably)
would tell me nothing, and I was forced to content myself with the
scanty information vouchsafed by one man, who said that the figures
were merely intended to represent the nembo—the artificial
deformations of pelele, ear-discs, and keloids. The legend recorded
by Pater Adams places these figures in a new light. They must surely
be more than mere dolls; and we may even venture to assume that
they are—though the majority of present-day Makonde are probably
unaware of the fact—representations of the tribal ancestress.
The references in the legend to the descent from Mahuta to the
Rovuma, and to a journey across the highlands into the Mbekuru
valley, undoubtedly indicate the previous history of the tribe, the
travels of the ancestral pair typifying the migrations of their
descendants. The descent to the neighbouring Rovuma valley, with
its extraordinary fertility and great abundance of game, is intelligible
at a glance—but the crossing of the Lukuledi depression, the ascent
to the Rondo Plateau and the descent to the Mbemkuru, also lie
within the bounds of probability, for all these districts have exactly
the same character as the extreme south. Now, however, comes a
point of especial interest for our bacteriological age. The primitive
Makonde did not enjoy their lives in the marshy river-valleys.
Disease raged among them, and many died. It was only after they
had returned to their original home near Mahuta, that the health
conditions of these people improved. We are very apt to think of the
African as a stupid person whose ignorance of nature is only equalled
by his fear of it, and who looks on all mishaps as caused by evil
spirits and malignant natural powers. It is much more correct to
assume in this case that the people very early learnt to distinguish
districts infested with malaria from those where it is absent.
This knowledge is crystallized in the
ancestral warning against settling in the
valleys and near the great waters, the
dwelling-places of disease and death. At the
same time, for security against the hostile
Mavia south of the Rovuma, it was enacted
that every settlement must be not less than a
certain distance from the southern edge of the
plateau. Such in fact is their mode of life at the
present day. It is not such a bad one, and
certainly they are both safer and more
comfortable than the Makua, the recent
intruders from the south, who have made USUAL METHOD OF
good their footing on the western edge of the CLOSING HUT-DOOR
plateau, extending over a fairly wide belt of
country. Neither Makua nor Makonde show in their dwellings
anything of the size and comeliness of the Yao houses in the plain,
especially at Masasi, Chingulungulu and Zuza’s. Jumbe Chauro, a
Makonde hamlet not far from Newala, on the road to Mahuta, is the
most important settlement of the tribe I have yet seen, and has fairly
spacious huts. But how slovenly is their construction compared with
the palatial residences of the elephant-hunters living in the plain.
The roofs are still more untidy than in the general run of huts during
the dry season, the walls show here and there the scanty beginnings
or the lamentable remains of the mud plastering, and the interior is a
veritable dog-kennel; dirt, dust and disorder everywhere. A few huts
only show any attempt at division into rooms, and this consists
merely of very roughly-made bamboo partitions. In one point alone
have I noticed any indication of progress—in the method of fastening
the door. Houses all over the south are secured in a simple but
ingenious manner. The door consists of a set of stout pieces of wood
or bamboo, tied with bark-string to two cross-pieces, and moving in
two grooves round one of the door-posts, so as to open inwards. If
the owner wishes to leave home, he takes two logs as thick as a man’s
upper arm and about a yard long. One of these is placed obliquely
against the middle of the door from the inside, so as to form an angle
of from 60° to 75° with the ground. He then places the second piece
horizontally across the first, pressing it downward with all his might.
It is kept in place by two strong posts planted in the ground a few
inches inside the door. This fastening is absolutely safe, but of course
cannot be applied to both doors at once, otherwise how could the
owner leave or enter his house? I have not yet succeeded in finding
out how the back door is fastened.

MAKONDE LOCK AND KEY AT JUMBE CHAURO


This is the general way of closing a house. The Makonde at Jumbe
Chauro, however, have a much more complicated, solid and original
one. Here, too, the door is as already described, except that there is
only one post on the inside, standing by itself about six inches from
one side of the doorway. Opposite this post is a hole in the wall just
large enough to admit a man’s arm. The door is closed inside by a
large wooden bolt passing through a hole in this post and pressing
with its free end against the door. The other end has three holes into
which fit three pegs running in vertical grooves inside the post. The
door is opened with a wooden key about a foot long, somewhat
curved and sloped off at the butt; the other end has three pegs
corresponding to the holes, in the bolt, so that, when it is thrust
through the hole in the wall and inserted into the rectangular
opening in the post, the pegs can be lifted and the bolt drawn out.[50]

MODE OF INSERTING THE KEY

With no small pride first one householder and then a second


showed me on the spot the action of this greatest invention of the
Makonde Highlands. To both with an admiring exclamation of
“Vizuri sana!” (“Very fine!”). I expressed the wish to take back these
marvels with me to Ulaya, to show the Wazungu what clever fellows
the Makonde are. Scarcely five minutes after my return to camp at
Newala, the two men came up sweating under the weight of two
heavy logs which they laid down at my feet, handing over at the same
time the keys of the fallen fortress. Arguing, logically enough, that if
the key was wanted, the lock would be wanted with it, they had taken
their axes and chopped down the posts—as it never occurred to them
to dig them out of the ground and so bring them intact. Thus I have
two badly damaged specimens, and the owners, instead of praise,
come in for a blowing-up.
The Makua huts in the environs of Newala are especially
miserable; their more than slovenly construction reminds one of the
temporary erections of the Makua at Hatia’s, though the people here
have not been concerned in a war. It must therefore be due to
congenital idleness, or else to the absence of a powerful chief. Even
the baraza at Mlipa’s, a short hour’s walk south-east of Newala,
shares in this general neglect. While public buildings in this country
are usually looked after more or less carefully, this is in evident
danger of being blown over by the first strong easterly gale. The only
attractive object in this whole district is the grave of the late chief
Mlipa. I visited it in the morning, while the sun was still trying with
partial success to break through the rolling mists, and the circular
grove of tall euphorbias, which, with a broken pot, is all that marks
the old king’s resting-place, impressed one with a touch of pathos.
Even my very materially-minded carriers seemed to feel something
of the sort, for instead of their usual ribald songs, they chanted
solemnly, as we marched on through the dense green of the Makonde
bush:—
“We shall arrive with the great master; we stand in a row and have
no fear about getting our food and our money from the Serkali (the
Government). We are not afraid; we are going along with the great
master, the lion; we are going down to the coast and back.”
With regard to the characteristic features of the various tribes here
on the western edge of the plateau, I can arrive at no other
conclusion than the one already come to in the plain, viz., that it is
impossible for anyone but a trained anthropologist to assign any
given individual at once to his proper tribe. In fact, I think that even
an anthropological specialist, after the most careful examination,
might find it a difficult task to decide. The whole congeries of peoples
collected in the region bounded on the west by the great Central
African rift, Tanganyika and Nyasa, and on the east by the Indian
Ocean, are closely related to each other—some of their languages are
only distinguished from one another as dialects of the same speech,
and no doubt all the tribes present the same shape of skull and
structure of skeleton. Thus, surely, there can be no very striking
differences in outward appearance.
Even did such exist, I should have no time
to concern myself with them, for day after day,
I have to see or hear, as the case may be—in
any case to grasp and record—an
extraordinary number of ethnographic
phenomena. I am almost disposed to think it
fortunate that some departments of inquiry, at
least, are barred by external circumstances.
Chief among these is the subject of iron-
working. We are apt to think of Africa as a
country where iron ore is everywhere, so to
speak, to be picked up by the roadside, and
where it would be quite surprising if the
inhabitants had not learnt to smelt the
material ready to their hand. In fact, the
knowledge of this art ranges all over the
continent, from the Kabyles in the north to the
Kafirs in the south. Here between the Rovuma
and the Lukuledi the conditions are not so
favourable. According to the statements of the
Makonde, neither ironstone nor any other
form of iron ore is known to them. They have
not therefore advanced to the art of smelting
the metal, but have hitherto bought all their
THE ANCESTRESS OF
THE MAKONDE
iron implements from neighbouring tribes.
Even in the plain the inhabitants are not much
better off. Only one man now living is said to
understand the art of smelting iron. This old fundi lives close to
Huwe, that isolated, steep-sided block of granite which rises out of
the green solitude between Masasi and Chingulungulu, and whose
jagged and splintered top meets the traveller’s eye everywhere. While
still at Masasi I wished to see this man at work, but was told that,
frightened by the rising, he had retired across the Rovuma, though
he would soon return. All subsequent inquiries as to whether the
fundi had come back met with the genuine African answer, “Bado”
(“Not yet”).
BRAZIER

Some consolation was afforded me by a brassfounder, whom I


came across in the bush near Akundonde’s. This man is the favourite
of women, and therefore no doubt of the gods; he welds the glittering
brass rods purchased at the coast into those massive, heavy rings
which, on the wrists and ankles of the local fair ones, continually give
me fresh food for admiration. Like every decent master-craftsman he
had all his tools with him, consisting of a pair of bellows, three
crucibles and a hammer—nothing more, apparently. He was quite
willing to show his skill, and in a twinkling had fixed his bellows on
the ground. They are simply two goat-skins, taken off whole, the four
legs being closed by knots, while the upper opening, intended to
admit the air, is kept stretched by two pieces of wood. At the lower
end of the skin a smaller opening is left into which a wooden tube is
stuck. The fundi has quickly borrowed a heap of wood-embers from
the nearest hut; he then fixes the free ends of the two tubes into an
earthen pipe, and clamps them to the ground by means of a bent
piece of wood. Now he fills one of his small clay crucibles, the dross
on which shows that they have been long in use, with the yellow
material, places it in the midst of the embers, which, at present are
only faintly glimmering, and begins his work. In quick alternation
the smith’s two hands move up and down with the open ends of the
bellows; as he raises his hand he holds the slit wide open, so as to let
the air enter the skin bag unhindered. In pressing it down he closes
the bag, and the air puffs through the bamboo tube and clay pipe into
the fire, which quickly burns up. The smith, however, does not keep
on with this work, but beckons to another man, who relieves him at
the bellows, while he takes some more tools out of a large skin pouch
carried on his back. I look on in wonder as, with a smooth round
stick about the thickness of a finger, he bores a few vertical holes into
the clean sand of the soil. This should not be difficult, yet the man
seems to be taking great pains over it. Then he fastens down to the
ground, with a couple of wooden clamps, a neat little trough made by
splitting a joint of bamboo in half, so that the ends are closed by the
two knots. At last the yellow metal has attained the right consistency,
and the fundi lifts the crucible from the fire by means of two sticks
split at the end to serve as tongs. A short swift turn to the left—a
tilting of the crucible—and the molten brass, hissing and giving forth
clouds of smoke, flows first into the bamboo mould and then into the
holes in the ground.
The technique of this backwoods craftsman may not be very far
advanced, but it cannot be denied that he knows how to obtain an
adequate result by the simplest means. The ladies of highest rank in
this country—that is to say, those who can afford it, wear two kinds
of these massive brass rings, one cylindrical, the other semicircular
in section. The latter are cast in the most ingenious way in the
bamboo mould, the former in the circular hole in the sand. It is quite
a simple matter for the fundi to fit these bars to the limbs of his fair
customers; with a few light strokes of his hammer he bends the
pliable brass round arm or ankle without further inconvenience to
the wearer.
SHAPING THE POT

SMOOTHING WITH MAIZE-COB

CUTTING THE EDGE


FINISHING THE BOTTOM

LAST SMOOTHING BEFORE


BURNING

FIRING THE BRUSH-PILE


LIGHTING THE FARTHER SIDE OF
THE PILE

TURNING THE RED-HOT VESSEL

NYASA WOMAN MAKING POTS AT MASASI


Pottery is an art which must always and everywhere excite the
interest of the student, just because it is so intimately connected with
the development of human culture, and because its relics are one of
the principal factors in the reconstruction of our own condition in
prehistoric times. I shall always remember with pleasure the two or
three afternoons at Masasi when Salim Matola’s mother, a slightly-
built, graceful, pleasant-looking woman, explained to me with
touching patience, by means of concrete illustrations, the ceramic art
of her people. The only implements for this primitive process were a
lump of clay in her left hand, and in the right a calabash containing
the following valuables: the fragment of a maize-cob stripped of all
its grains, a smooth, oval pebble, about the size of a pigeon’s egg, a
few chips of gourd-shell, a bamboo splinter about the length of one’s
hand, a small shell, and a bunch of some herb resembling spinach.
Nothing more. The woman scraped with the
shell a round, shallow hole in the soft, fine
sand of the soil, and, when an active young
girl had filled the calabash with water for her,
she began to knead the clay. As if by magic it
gradually assumed the shape of a rough but
already well-shaped vessel, which only wanted
a little touching up with the instruments
before mentioned. I looked out with the
MAKUA WOMAN closest attention for any indication of the use
MAKING A POT. of the potter’s wheel, in however rudimentary
SHOWS THE a form, but no—hapana (there is none). The
BEGINNINGS OF THE embryo pot stood firmly in its little
POTTER’S WHEEL
depression, and the woman walked round it in
a stooping posture, whether she was removing
small stones or similar foreign bodies with the maize-cob, smoothing
the inner or outer surface with the splinter of bamboo, or later, after
letting it dry for a day, pricking in the ornamentation with a pointed
bit of gourd-shell, or working out the bottom, or cutting the edge
with a sharp bamboo knife, or giving the last touches to the finished
vessel. This occupation of the women is infinitely toilsome, but it is
without doubt an accurate reproduction of the process in use among
our ancestors of the Neolithic and Bronze ages.
There is no doubt that the invention of pottery, an item in human
progress whose importance cannot be over-estimated, is due to
women. Rough, coarse and unfeeling, the men of the horde range
over the countryside. When the united cunning of the hunters has
succeeded in killing the game; not one of them thinks of carrying
home the spoil. A bright fire, kindled by a vigorous wielding of the
drill, is crackling beside them; the animal has been cleaned and cut
up secundum artem, and, after a slight singeing, will soon disappear
under their sharp teeth; no one all this time giving a single thought
to wife or child.
To what shifts, on the other hand, the primitive wife, and still more
the primitive mother, was put! Not even prehistoric stomachs could
endure an unvarying diet of raw food. Something or other suggested
the beneficial effect of hot water on the majority of approved but
indigestible dishes. Perhaps a neighbour had tried holding the hard
roots or tubers over the fire in a calabash filled with water—or maybe
an ostrich-egg-shell, or a hastily improvised vessel of bark. They
became much softer and more palatable than they had previously
been; but, unfortunately, the vessel could not stand the fire and got
charred on the outside. That can be remedied, thought our
ancestress, and plastered a layer of wet clay round a similar vessel.
This is an improvement; the cooking utensil remains uninjured, but
the heat of the fire has shrunk it, so that it is loose in its shell. The
next step is to detach it, so, with a firm grip and a jerk, shell and
kernel are separated, and pottery is invented. Perhaps, however, the
discovery which led to an intelligent use of the burnt-clay shell, was
made in a slightly different way. Ostrich-eggs and calabashes are not
to be found in every part of the world, but everywhere mankind has
arrived at the art of making baskets out of pliant materials, such as
bark, bast, strips of palm-leaf, supple twigs, etc. Our inventor has no
water-tight vessel provided by nature. “Never mind, let us line the
basket with clay.” This answers the purpose, but alas! the basket gets
burnt over the blazing fire, the woman watches the process of
cooking with increasing uneasiness, fearing a leak, but no leak
appears. The food, done to a turn, is eaten with peculiar relish; and
the cooking-vessel is examined, half in curiosity, half in satisfaction
at the result. The plastic clay is now hard as stone, and at the same
time looks exceedingly well, for the neat plaiting of the burnt basket
is traced all over it in a pretty pattern. Thus, simultaneously with
pottery, its ornamentation was invented.
Primitive woman has another claim to respect. It was the man,
roving abroad, who invented the art of producing fire at will, but the
woman, unable to imitate him in this, has been a Vestal from the
earliest times. Nothing gives so much trouble as the keeping alight of
the smouldering brand, and, above all, when all the men are absent
from the camp. Heavy rain-clouds gather, already the first large
drops are falling, the first gusts of the storm rage over the plain. The
little flame, a greater anxiety to the woman than her own children,
flickers unsteadily in the blast. What is to be done? A sudden thought
occurs to her, and in an instant she has constructed a primitive hut
out of strips of bark, to protect the flame against rain and wind.
This, or something very like it, was the way in which the principle
of the house was discovered; and even the most hardened misogynist
cannot fairly refuse a woman the credit of it. The protection of the
hearth-fire from the weather is the germ from which the human
dwelling was evolved. Men had little, if any share, in this forward
step, and that only at a late stage. Even at the present day, the
plastering of the housewall with clay and the manufacture of pottery
are exclusively the women’s business. These are two very significant
survivals. Our European kitchen-garden, too, is originally a woman’s
invention, and the hoe, the primitive instrument of agriculture, is,
characteristically enough, still used in this department. But the
noblest achievement which we owe to the other sex is unquestionably
the art of cookery. Roasting alone—the oldest process—is one for
which men took the hint (a very obvious one) from nature. It must
have been suggested by the scorched carcase of some animal
overtaken by the destructive forest-fires. But boiling—the process of
improving organic substances by the help of water heated to boiling-
point—is a much later discovery. It is so recent that it has not even
yet penetrated to all parts of the world. The Polynesians understand
how to steam food, that is, to cook it, neatly wrapped in leaves, in a
hole in the earth between hot stones, the air being excluded, and
(sometimes) a few drops of water sprinkled on the stones; but they
do not understand boiling.
To come back from this digression, we find that the slender Nyasa
woman has, after once more carefully examining the finished pot,
put it aside in the shade to dry. On the following day she sends me
word by her son, Salim Matola, who is always on hand, that she is
going to do the burning, and, on coming out of my house, I find her
already hard at work. She has spread on the ground a layer of very
dry sticks, about as thick as one’s thumb, has laid the pot (now of a
yellowish-grey colour) on them, and is piling brushwood round it.
My faithful Pesa mbili, the mnyampara, who has been standing by,
most obligingly, with a lighted stick, now hands it to her. Both of
them, blowing steadily, light the pile on the lee side, and, when the
flame begins to catch, on the weather side also. Soon the whole is in a
blaze, but the dry fuel is quickly consumed and the fire dies down, so
that we see the red-hot vessel rising from the ashes. The woman
turns it continually with a long stick, sometimes one way and
sometimes another, so that it may be evenly heated all over. In
twenty minutes she rolls it out of the ash-heap, takes up the bundle
of spinach, which has been lying for two days in a jar of water, and
sprinkles the red-hot clay with it. The places where the drops fall are
marked by black spots on the uniform reddish-brown surface. With a
sigh of relief, and with visible satisfaction, the woman rises to an
erect position; she is standing just in a line between me and the fire,
from which a cloud of smoke is just rising: I press the ball of my
camera, the shutter clicks—the apotheosis is achieved! Like a
priestess, representative of her inventive sex, the graceful woman
stands: at her feet the hearth-fire she has given us beside her the
invention she has devised for us, in the background the home she has
built for us.
At Newala, also, I have had the manufacture of pottery carried on
in my presence. Technically the process is better than that already
described, for here we find the beginnings of the potter’s wheel,
which does not seem to exist in the plains; at least I have seen
nothing of the sort. The artist, a frightfully stupid Makua woman, did
not make a depression in the ground to receive the pot she was about
to shape, but used instead a large potsherd. Otherwise, she went to
work in much the same way as Salim’s mother, except that she saved
herself the trouble of walking round and round her work by squatting
at her ease and letting the pot and potsherd rotate round her; this is
surely the first step towards a machine. But it does not follow that
the pot was improved by the process. It is true that it was beautifully
rounded and presented a very creditable appearance when finished,
but the numerous large and small vessels which I have seen, and, in
part, collected, in the “less advanced” districts, are no less so. We
moderns imagine that instruments of precision are necessary to
produce excellent results. Go to the prehistoric collections of our
museums and look at the pots, urns and bowls of our ancestors in the
dim ages of the past, and you will at once perceive your error.
MAKING LONGITUDINAL CUT IN
BARK

DRAWING THE BARK OFF THE LOG

REMOVING THE OUTER BARK


BEATING THE BARK

WORKING THE BARK-CLOTH AFTER BEATING, TO MAKE IT


SOFT

MANUFACTURE OF BARK-CLOTH AT NEWALA


To-day, nearly the whole population of German East Africa is
clothed in imported calico. This was not always the case; even now in
some parts of the north dressed skins are still the prevailing wear,
and in the north-western districts—east and north of Lake
Tanganyika—lies a zone where bark-cloth has not yet been
superseded. Probably not many generations have passed since such
bark fabrics and kilts of skins were the only clothing even in the
south. Even to-day, large quantities of this bright-red or drab
material are still to be found; but if we wish to see it, we must look in
the granaries and on the drying stages inside the native huts, where
it serves less ambitious uses as wrappings for those seeds and fruits
which require to be packed with special care. The salt produced at
Masasi, too, is packed for transport to a distance in large sheets of
bark-cloth. Wherever I found it in any degree possible, I studied the
process of making this cloth. The native requisitioned for the
purpose arrived, carrying a log between two and three yards long and
as thick as his thigh, and nothing else except a curiously-shaped
mallet and the usual long, sharp and pointed knife which all men and
boys wear in a belt at their backs without a sheath—horribile dictu!
[51]
Silently he squats down before me, and with two rapid cuts has
drawn a couple of circles round the log some two yards apart, and
slits the bark lengthwise between them with the point of his knife.
With evident care, he then scrapes off the outer rind all round the
log, so that in a quarter of an hour the inner red layer of the bark
shows up brightly-coloured between the two untouched ends. With
some trouble and much caution, he now loosens the bark at one end,
and opens the cylinder. He then stands up, takes hold of the free
edge with both hands, and turning it inside out, slowly but steadily
pulls it off in one piece. Now comes the troublesome work of
scraping all superfluous particles of outer bark from the outside of
the long, narrow piece of material, while the inner side is carefully
scrutinised for defective spots. At last it is ready for beating. Having
signalled to a friend, who immediately places a bowl of water beside
him, the artificer damps his sheet of bark all over, seizes his mallet,
lays one end of the stuff on the smoothest spot of the log, and
hammers away slowly but continuously. “Very simple!” I think to
myself. “Why, I could do that, too!”—but I am forced to change my
opinions a little later on; for the beating is quite an art, if the fabric is
not to be beaten to pieces. To prevent the breaking of the fibres, the
stuff is several times folded across, so as to interpose several
thicknesses between the mallet and the block. At last the required
state is reached, and the fundi seizes the sheet, still folded, by both
ends, and wrings it out, or calls an assistant to take one end while he
holds the other. The cloth produced in this way is not nearly so fine
and uniform in texture as the famous Uganda bark-cloth, but it is
quite soft, and, above all, cheap.
Now, too, I examine the mallet. My craftsman has been using the
simpler but better form of this implement, a conical block of some
hard wood, its base—the striking surface—being scored across and
across with more or less deeply-cut grooves, and the handle stuck
into a hole in the middle. The other and earlier form of mallet is
shaped in the same way, but the head is fastened by an ingenious
network of bark strips into the split bamboo serving as a handle. The
observation so often made, that ancient customs persist longest in
connection with religious ceremonies and in the life of children, here
finds confirmation. As we shall soon see, bark-cloth is still worn
during the unyago,[52] having been prepared with special solemn
ceremonies; and many a mother, if she has no other garment handy,
will still put her little one into a kilt of bark-cloth, which, after all,
looks better, besides being more in keeping with its African
surroundings, than the ridiculous bit of print from Ulaya.
MAKUA WOMEN

You might also like