You are on page 1of 11

See discussions, stats, and author profiles for this publication at: https://www.researchgate.

net/publication/6179750

Perceived Effects of the Menstrual Cycle on Young Female Singers in the


Western Classical Tradition

Article in Journal of voice: official journal of the Voice Foundation · August 2007
DOI: 10.1016/j.jvoice.2007.05.004 · Source: PubMed

CITATIONS READS
15 556

2 authors:

Maree Ryan Dianna Theadora Kenny


The University of Sydney DK Consulting
6 PUBLICATIONS 59 CITATIONS 390 PUBLICATIONS 7,517 CITATIONS

SEE PROFILE SEE PROFILE

All content following this page was uploaded by Dianna Theadora Kenny on 18 May 2018.

The user has requested enhancement of the downloaded file.


Perceived Effects of the Menstrual Cycle on Young
Female Singers in the Western Classical Tradition
Maree Ryan and Dianna T. Kenny University of Sydney, New South Wales, Australia

Summary. This study investigated the perceived effects of the female hormonal cycle on young female classical
singers. All the singers, including male controls, were tertiary singing students from the Sydney Conservatorium of Mu-
sic, Australia, who were selected for entry into vocal study programs by competitive audition. Female participants com-
pleted a questionnaire and daily diary in the first and third months of the study. Male controls completed the diary for the
first month only. The questionnaire and diary focused on singers’ physical symptoms, their mood states, and vocal pro-
duction. Analysis of the diaries indicated that although 81% of female singers reported regular menstrual cycles and
43% reported using an oral contraceptive, neither of these factors was related to the voice quality variables as measured
on the first day of the cycle. Singers who were not taking a contraceptive pill rated their voice quality lower and their
mood higher than those on the pill. There was no relationship between temperature recording in the females and day of
cycle. Perceived voice quality for female singers was lower on days 1–3 compared to the remainder of the cycle and
there was a trend for ratings to improve through days 1–7. The voice parameters for male singers tended to be slightly
flatter over the cycle days than for females. Although voice quality in females indicated a tendency to be lower on av-
erage during days 24–4 of the cycle, voice quality for males tended to be more alike during the two phases, days 24–4
and days 5–23. Overall, reduced voice quality was associated with more negative mood experiences. The six most se-
verely affected females completed voice recordings of specific vocal tasks on the first day of the cycle and again in mid-
cycle. These recordings were randomly presented to both the participants and expert vocal pedagogues to ascertain
whether significant differences in vocal quality were perceptually identifiable. Singers, but not pedagogues, were
able to accurately identify the timing of the recordings. Although the singer recognized that greater effort is required
to produce the sound during menstruation, discernible differences were not detected by expert listeners.
Key Words: Menstrual cycle–Classical singing–Vocal quality–Perceptual effects.

INTRODUCTION ond duration using the vowels ‘‘ah,’’ ‘‘eu,’’ and ‘‘u.’’ Two
The relatively few studies to date that have investigated the ef- samples were taken, one at ovulation and the other at premen-
fect of the hormonal cycle on female singers have generally struation. Results indicated that participants showed no ten-
used subjects who were untrained singers and tasks that do dency to become hoarse at premenstruation and this was
not challenge the vocal instrument. The use of untrained singers replicated in a second group of women. The data did not sup-
does not provide information important to professional singers port the contention that hoarseness was a feature typical of pre-
who use their vocal instrument in a much more demanding way menstruation, as both series demonstrated no difference
with respect to pitch, dynamic range, and duration, and who between hoarseness at premenstruation and hoarseness at ovu-
perform more complex vocal motor tasks than speakers or un- lation. Among European opera singers, the belief that hoarse-
trained singers singing simple tunes. These methodological is- ness occurred at premenstruation had resulted in contracts
sues notwithstanding, all the studies to date support the containing clauses to exempt them from singing during that pe-
hypothesis that the voice is affected by the menstrual cycle riod of their monthly cycle.
via the hormonal and physiological changes that occur during Higgins and Saxman2 assessed the speech samples (six
this time. The results of most of these early studies, which are vowels) of 10 young women and five young men aged 19–26
summarized below, cannot be generalized to professional fe- years, all untrained singers, to assess hormonal effects on voice
male singers, hence the need for the current study. in females and to show that the vocal output alters because the
One of the earliest studies1 assessed the presence of premen- mass of the vocal folds changes. None of the female subjects
strual hoarseness in two groups of undergraduate majors, aged was taking the contraceptive pill. Basal temperature of the
19–36 years, with normal speaking voices, but no vocal train- women was recorded daily to reveal ovarian hormonal fluctua-
ing. None of the subjects was on the contraceptive pill and tion. Testing occurred every second day for 33 days. Results in-
all took their daily basal temperature to plot the hormonal dicated that at the premenstruum and during the onset of
changes. The vocal tasks used were speech samples of 3-sec- menstruation, the magnitude of frequency perturbation (distur-
bance) was not notably different from the average behavior of
Accepted for publication May 10, 2007.
individual subjects, in that it did not change, when the predic-
From the Australian Centre for Applied Research in Music Performance (ACARMP), tion was that the fundamental frequency would decrease. How-
Sydney Conservatorium of Music, University of Sydney, New South Wales, Australia.
Address correspondence and reprint requests to Professor Dianna T. Kenny, Director,
ever, ovulation was associated with notable changes, both
Australian Centre for Applied Research in Music Performance (ACARMP), Sydney Con- upward and downward, in the magnitude of frequency perturba-
servatorium of Music, The University of Sydney, New South Wales, Australia 2006.
E-mail: d.kenny@usyd.edu.au
tion for most subjects, supporting the idea that large fluctuations
Journal of Voice, Vol. 23, No. 1, pp. 99-108 in hormonal levels affect vocal fold stability. Conversely, larger
0892-1997/$36.00
Ó 2009 The Voice Foundation
frequency perturbations were found for young men than for
doi:10.1016/j.jvoice.2007.05.004 women in this investigation.
100 Journal of Voice, Vol. 23, No. 1, 2009

The results of neither of these earlier studies can be general- vocal intensity, vocal fatigue, decreased range with loss of
ized to the classical singing voice. Although vocal testing (ie, high notes, and loss of timbre in spoken and singing voice.
reading, singing chromatic scales and phrases, as well as low All these women had ceased menstruation and were not under-
and high sustained notes) used by Brown et al3,4 was appropri- going hormone replacement therapy. The vocal syndrome is
ate for assessment of professional singers, the study used progressive and is especially observed in voice professionals
a mixed sample of 16 women aged between 18 and 22 years, of all genres, who complain of loss of high notes, alteration
some of whom were trained singers and some of whom had in pianissimos, loss of harmonics, and reduced color of their
no singing training. No information as to which results be- voices. In addition, vocal fold and cervical smears showed
longed to which group was given so that no conclusions could a striking resemblance, indicating that the mucosal lining of
be drawn with respect to singers. A number of other methodo- both regions is similar and show similar changes such as muco-
logical problems with this study render the results uninterpret- sal atrophy from androgen effects.
able. For example, only participants with irregular menstrual Finally, in the study by Wicklund,8 a group of 20 women
cycles were included, so that results could not be generalized aged 20–57 years were selected to assess the relationship be-
to all women. Irregular menstrual cycles generally show more tween PMS and vocal dysphonia. The wide age range is prob-
hormonal fluctuations and greater negative impact on the lematic because different factors affect women at different
voice.3,4 Some of the women were taking the contraceptive ages, producing different symptoms. Many women in the inter-
pill but no subgroup analysis of this subsample was reported. mediate and older age groups may have borne children, which
The contraceptive pill eliminates hormonal fluctuations and affect hormones, and some of those approaching 50 years of age
hence vocal instability is either reduced or eradicated.5 may have been menopausal. The average age of menopause is
Chae et al6 evaluated whether the presence of premenstrual between 45 and 55 years and there are many symptoms that re-
syndrome (PMS) affects female singers and assessed 28 women flect hormonal changes up to 5 years before menopause, so it is
over a 2-month period. Participants were female nurses aged 21– plausible that several of these subjects may have been inappro-
30 years, all untrained singers. Their task involved the produc- priate subjects. However, there was an excellent questionnaire
tion of 5 seconds of sustained sound on the ‘‘ah’’ (as in ‘‘father’’) on PMS, in which several areas such as mood states, voice dif-
vowel. None of the participants took the contraceptive pill and ferences, and general health were addressed, as well as specific
none was consistent in taking their daily basal temperature to questions covering premenstrual dysphonia. Questions on gen-
plot their monthly hormonal changes. Chae et al6 hypothesized eral health included sleeping patterns, food cravings, head-
that PMS can predict the extent of voice change during the pre- aches, smoking habits, and exercise programs. In addition,
menstrual phase but found that the change in the voice was re- acoustic and glottographic testings were conducted. Results in-
lated to the presence of PMS, and not its severity. However, dicated that the severity levels of PMS and menstrual dysphonia
testing such as this is inadequate for singers and is more relevant may be linked.
to the speaking voice, although the methodological problems The effect of exercise on symptoms and dysphonia could not
were such that they made any conclusions difficult. be accurately assessed because of the small sample size.
Abitbol et al7 tested 97 women with vocal PMS, all vocal pro- Many observers have reflected on the mood states investi-
fessionals aged 23–36 years. None was taking the contraceptive gated in Wicklund’s research. Chae et al6 observed that one
pill and all presented with a premenstrual dysphonia. Estrogen or more of the four emotional components—sadness, tension,
and progesterone levels were measured and vocal fold smears anxiety, or conflict—are present along with the diagnostic cri-
and cervical smears were taken. The vocal stimulus was Fre`re teria for PMS. Davis and Davis9 stated that the larynx is espe-
Jacques, a French folk song, and although more challenging cially sensitive and vulnerable to fatigue and that an emotion
than speaking or sustaining vowels, it is still far removed in dif- such as anger will adversely affect the voice. Sommer,10 in
ficulty from the daily expectations of a professional singer, as it her paper on stress and menstrual disorder, described women
has limited pitch range, volume expectation, or technical chal- from many published papers reporting unpleasant symptoms
lenge. The results indicated that in the premenstrual phase, all such as stress, irritability, nervousness, and depression associ-
the subjects showed physical signs of change in the vocal folds ated with the premenstrual and menstrual phases of the cycle.
that disturbed vibratory and muscle function. Stroboscopy re- The aim of this study was to investigate, using appropriate
vealed asymmetric vocal fold vibrations, whereas associated sampling, design, and measures, the variability of the singing
spectrographic signs showed loss of certain harmonics and voice over the menstrual cycle and its perceived effects on
change in laryngeal kinetics. They concluded that the four clin- singers and expert listeners in order to add to the literature on
ical signs related to the voice—vocal fatigue, decreased range the effects of hormonal fluctuations on the young professional
(including loss of high tones and of pianissimo), loss of vocal female singer.
power, and loss of certain high harmonics (more metallic and Women are known to suffer from premenstrual symptomatol-
huskier voice)—apart from other signs such as irritability, ner- ogy to varying degrees as ascertained from a large body of liter-
vousness, and pelvic pain were part of the vocal PMS. ature.11 A diagnosis of PMS is only established if symptoms
Abitbol et al7 studied 100 menopausal women including 17 resolve for at least the days between the end of the menstrual pe-
who presented clinically with the vocal syndrome of meno- riod and ovulation, that is, approximately days 7–14 of the cycle.
pause, a condition wherein androgen action is virtually unop- Nader12 identified significant individual cyclical variation in
posed by estrogen effects. The signs noted were lowering of symptomatology surrounding the menstrual cycle, with
Maree Ryan and Dianna T. Kenny Does Menstrual Cycle Affect Female Classical Singers’ Voices? 101

premenstrual symptoms varying from mild to severe. About 36-page booklets. Participants were asked to complete one
30%–40% of women experience some form of premenstrual page per day for each day of the observation period.
symptoms, and 10% of women experience severe symptoms. The female participants were asked to record their daily tem-
Nader12 classified the signs and symptoms of the syndrome perature and their menstrual cycles over 2 noncontiguous
into somatic (body states), affective (mood states), pain and be- months of data, whereas male volunteers who were used as con-
havioral states, and other symptoms, such as food cravings, in- trols completed 1 month’s data. The females in our research
somnia, and fatigue. The participants recorded a daily diary were asked to record whether they were using hormonal contra-
related to the menstrual cycle, with physical symptoms based ception and other symptoms such as intercurrent illness, unre-
on those most commonly described in the literature.8,12 lated physical symptoms, for example, body pain, and alcohol
intake were also recorded, as such factors could influence voice
METHOD production and quality.
The study was conducted in two phases. In the first phase, the We selected 2 noncontiguous months’ data for diary data col-
whole sample recorded required information in daily diaries. lection because we did not want to burden busy participants, as
In the second phase, a subset of six female singers made voice most researchers acknowledge, the longer and harder the task,
recordings at different stages of the menstrual cycle for percep- the lower the compliance; secondly, the break of 1 month be-
tual assessment by expert judges. tween diary keeping would reduce recall and duplication effects.
Questionnaire on vocal quality. Items included in the question-
Phase 1 naire used to rate voice quality were selected on the basis that
Participants. A total of 27 singers, of whom six were male they are commonly used by vocal pedagogues and students to
and 21 were female, provided sufficient data to be included in describe the voice and which occurred in the literature as de-
longitudinal analyses. Of the six males, five completed the daily scriptors of vocal health and quality.7,13–15 Participants rated
diaries for 28 days and one completed the diary for 26 days; we the items on a 10-cm visual analog scale (VAS) with the anchors
used males in the study to assess variations in mood and self- strongly agree and strongly disagree (Figure 1). Negatively
perceived voice quality in the absence of obvious hormonal ef- worded items were reverse scored, so higher scores indicated
fects experienced by female singers. Of the 21 females, 14 com- more positive ratings of vocal quality.
pleted the diaries for 28 days, three for 27 days, three for 25 Profile of Mood States. Words used to describe mood states
days, and one for 18 days. were selected from each of the subscales of the Profile of Mood
No hormonal tests were conducted on any subject in the States (POMS).16 Other vocal researchers9,17,18 have taken a sim-
study because of the cost and intrusive nature of such tests. ilar approach to mood description in vocal research (Figure 2).
The male recording commenced on a random day 1, as opposed
to the physiological day 1 in the female cycle. To include Temperature chart. The temperature chart was based on the
chart used by the Family Planning Association of Australia
singers in longitudinal analyses, missing values were replaced
by the singer’s mean value for their cycle but missing data and modified to suit the current study. Recording of basal tem-
were not replaced past the last day of recording. perature and change, and ‘‘ovulation shift,’’ during the female
Other medical history were not taken into consideration in menstrual cycle followed protocols based on current practice
the student diaries, but the authors knew by direct association and previous studies.19–21
with the participants that there was one type 1 diabetic student,
one other with insulin resistance, and one female recently diag- Phase 2
nosed with hypothyroidism. Participants. Singers. The six female singers who reported
Measures. The daily diary and questionnaire were custom- being most affected by their hormonal fluctuations in the diary
designed for this study and covered questions on physical study participated in this phase of the study. Of those most af-
symptoms, voice, and mood states. Diaries were bound in fected by their cyclical symptoms, four were not taking the

Thinking about your singing today, to what extent do you agree or disagree with the following:
Strongly disagree Strongly agree
My voice felt in peak form ___________________________________
My voice felt fatigued ___________________________________
It took a lot of effort to sing ___________________________________
My ability to support my sound was affected ___________________________________
My voice seemed hoarse or husky ___________________________________
My voice felt muffled, weak or breathy ___________________________________
I could not extend to the top of my range ___________________________________
I was not able to control my voice as easily ___________________________________
My voice felt “sluggish” ___________________________________
My voice sounded flat in pitch ___________________________________

FIGURE 1. Questionnaire on vocal quality.


102 Journal of Voice, Vol. 23, No. 1, 2009

Below is a list of feelings that people have. For each feeling, cross ONE number for the answer
that best describes HOW YOU ARE FEELING RIGHT NOW. The numbers refer to these phrases

0 = Not at all, 1 = A little, 2 = Moderately, 3 = Quite a lot, 4 = Very

Relaxed (0) (1) (2) (3) (4)


Discouraged (0) (1) (2) (3) (4)
Annoyed / Irritable (0) (1) (2) (3) (4)
Sad / depressed (0) (1) (2) (3) (4)
Unable to Concentrate (0) (1) (2) (3) (4)
Energetic (0) (1) (2) (3) (4)
Forgetful / confused (0) (1) (2) (3) (4)
Tense / anxious (0) (1) (2) (3) (4)
Exhausted (0) (1) (2) (3) (4)
Cheerful (0) (1) (2) (3) (4)
Angry (0) (1) (2) (3) (4)
Fatigued (0) (1) (2) (3) (4)
Happy (0) (1) (2) (3) (4)

FIGURE 2. Items from the Profile of Mood States (POMS) used to assess mood.

pill and two were using oral contraception, yet only one in each RESULTS
subgroup recorded an obvious temperature rise. Their voices, Phase 1
using specific tasks, were recorded at two points in the monthly Statistical methods. Linear mixed models were used to
cycle (day 1 and midcycle, between days 10 and 14). The taped measure the independent effects of cycle (days 24–4 compared
samples were randomly aggregated onto a CD and both expert with days 5–23), gender, and contraceptive pill use on voice
pedagogues and the singers rated the vocal quality of the sam- quality and mood. The effect of contraceptive pill use was
ples on a vocal-quality rating scale to ascertain whether there tested in females only. An autoregressive covariance matrix
were any discernible differences in the voice production at these was used to allow for values closer together to be more corre-
times. The singers also indicated whether the sample was taken lated than values further apart. This covariance structure pro-
during menses or at midcycle. vided a lower 2 restricted log likelihood than either an
Perceptual judges. Six voice teachers who were expert vocal unstructured matrix or a compound symmetry matrix and was
pedagogues acted as judges of the vocal recordings. Their expe- therefore used in the mixed models. The residual values for
rience was wide-ranging and included significant international the models were normally distributed with no major outliers
performing careers and Australian- and overseas-acquired aca- and therefore assumptions for the models were met. Repeated
demic qualifications. measures general linear models were then used to examine
Vocal recordings. The musical task recorded was 20 sec- trends using a linear polynomial contrast in mood and voice
onds of an aria, O Mio Babbino Caro from the opera, Gianna quality over the first four days of the cycle. In addition, devia-
Schicchi by Puccini. This extract challenged each singer in tion contrasts were used in females to identify days where
range extension, sustained support of the voice, accuracy of in- values were significantly higher or lower than the mean value
tonation (pitch), flexibility of phrasing and movement through for all time points. There was insufficient data to undertake
the registers, and language facility. this analysis in males. Relationships between voice quality
We recorded arpeggios and other scale patterns and messa di measurements and mood were examined using Pearson’s corre-
voce at each session, but final analysis was confined to the aria lation coefficients.
extract as it revealed all the information appropriate to this Descriptive analyses. The characteristics of the 27 singers
study. Both musical tasks were chosen as specific to the singing are shown in Table 1. None of the female singers was pregnant
voice, in contrast to previous studies conducted on speaking and and four reported having an irregular menstrual cycle.
singing voices. This was a significant methodological advance Table 2 shows the effects of gender and cycle (days 24–4
in that vocal effects of the menstrual cycle might only be ob- compared to days 5–23) on voice quality and mood. The
served at the extreme range or technical challenge of the vocal mean scores for males tended to be higher than those for fe-
stimulus. The nearest tests to western classical singing in previ- males for all variables except range and mood and the standard
ous studies, that is, chromatic scales and phrases22 and Fre`re errors were also higher in males indicating a wider range used
Jacques,7 were both tasks that are too far removed in difficulty on each scale. There was a significant difference in peak form
from the daily expectations of a professional singer who re- between males and females (P < 0.0001) and a marginally sig-
quires wider range and greater volume in pieces of music of nificant difference in weak (P ¼ 0.07) and sluggish voice
much greater technical challenge. (P ¼ 0.07), with males scoring higher on all measurements.
Maree Ryan and Dianna T. Kenny Does Menstrual Cycle Affect Female Classical Singers’ Voices? 103

fatigued voice (P ¼ 0.07). The table also shows the estimated


TABLE 1.
Characteristics of Singers
mean values for cycle in females only after adjusting for the in-
fluence of contraceptive pill use. After adjusting for the effect of
Total Sample taking a contraceptive pill, the mean values for all voice mea-
Males Females surements again tended to be lower and mood tended to be
worse (higher score) in cycle days 24–4 than in cycle days 5–
Total number 6 21
23 with significant differences for fatigued voice (P ¼ 0.02)
Soprano — 19 (91%) and a marginally significant difference for effort (P ¼ 0.06)
Mezzo soprano — 2 (9%) and mood (P ¼ 0.07).
Baritone 4 (67%) — The mean values for voice quality for the first phase of the
Tenor 1 (17%) — cycle (days 24–4) and the remainder of the cycle (days 5–23)
Counter tenor 1 (17%) —
are shown in Figure 3 for females and in Figure 4 for males.
Age range (y) 19–29 18–27 The figure for females indicates a tendency for all measure-
Singing (y) 3–9 2–16 ments to be approximately one point lower on average during
Oral contraceptive pill — 9 (43%) days 24–4 of the cycle, whereas for males mean values tend
Regular cycles — 17 (81%) to be more alike during the two phases except for peak form.
Table 4 shows the mean values of the voice quality parame-
ters and mood for selected days over the cycle in males and fe-
males. Females tended to score lower on all voice quality
The difference in mood did not reach statistical significance at measurements, especially on days 1 and 4, and higher on
P ¼ 0.09. mood. For mood, female scores were on average 5–6 points
The estimated marginal mean values for cycle after adjusting higher than males on days 1 and 4 but similar on days 14 and 23.
for a gender influence are also shown in Table 2. The mean The F values and P values for the repeated measures devia-
scores for voice quality tended to be lower as indicated by the tion contrasts are shown in Table 4. Mean values for voice qual-
negative values for mean difference, and mood tended to be ity measurements were lower on day 1 of the cycle for females
higher as indicated by a positive mean difference, in cycle with significant differences for all measurements except hoarse
days 24–4 when compared with cycle days 5–23. There was or weak voice, range, and flat voice. In addition, the mean mood
a statistically significant difference for fatigued voice score was higher (worse) on day 1 (P ¼ 0.05). Also, mean
(P ¼ 0.004) and effort (P ¼ 0.03). values for all voice quality measurements except support
Table 3 shows the effects of taking a contraceptive pill and were significantly higher (better) in the center of the cycle, usu-
cycle in females. The negative mean differences for all voice ally at day 14 but at day 12 for hoarse or flat voice and at day 15
quality measurements except peak form indicate that singers for range. Mean mood score was significantly better (lower
who were not taking a contraceptive pill tended to rate lower score) at day 12 (P ¼ 0.001).
on the voice quality scores. They also rated almost two points Table 5 shows the trend test over days 1–4 for both males and
higher on mood. The difference in mean values was significant females. There was a significant trend for voice quality mea-
for hoarse voice (P ¼ 0.003) and marginally significant for surements to improve over days 1–4 in females for peak

TABLE 2.
Marginal Mean Values for Voice Quality and Mood in Males and Females and for Days 24–4 and 5–23 of Cycle Estimated
from Mixed Linear Models for Voice Quality and Mood Measurements
Gender Effect Cycle Effect
Voice
Quality Males Females Difference Days 24–4 Days 5–23 Difference
Measure Mean (SE) Mean (SE) (95% CI) P Value Mean (SE) Mean (SE) (95% CI) P Value
Peak form 6.8 (0.3) 5.4 (0.2) 1.4 (0.7,2.1) <0.0001 5.9 (0.2) 6.3 (0.2) 0.3 (0.8,0.2) 0.20
Fatigued 6.2 (0.4) 5.6 (0.2) 0.6 (0.2,1.4) 0.15 5.5 (0.3) 6.4 (0.2) 0.9 (1.4,0.3) 0.004
Effort 6.1 (0.4) 5.7 (0.2) 0.4 (0.4,1.2) 0.34 5.6 (0.3) 6.2 (0.2) 0.6 (1.2,0.1) 0.03
Support 6.3 (0.4) 6.1 (0.2) 0.2 (0.7,1.2) 0.62 6.2 (0.3) 6.2 (0.2) 0.1 (0.7,0.5) 0.83
Hoarse 7.1 (0.4) 6.5 (0.2) 0.6 (0.3,1.5) 0.21 6.6 (0.3) 7.0 (0.3) 0.3 (0.9,0.2) 0.26
Weak 7.0 (0.4) 6.2 (0.2) 0.8 (0.1,1.6) 0.07 6.6 (0.3) 6.7 (0.2) 0.1 (0.7,0.5) 0.74
Range 6.1 (0.4) 6.5 (0.2) 0.4 (1.3,0.5) 0.34 6.2 (0.3) 6.4 (0.3) 0.3 (0.9,0.3) 0.38
Control 6.8 (0.4) 6.1 (0.2) 0.7 (0.2,1.5) 0.13 6.3 (0.3) 6.6 (0.2) 0.3 (0.9,0.3) 0.25
Sluggish 7.0 (0.4) 6.2 (0.2) 0.8 (0.1,1.6) 0.07 6.4 (0.3) 6.7 (0.2) 0.3 (0.9,0.3) 0.34
Flat 7.6 (0.4) 7.0 (0.2) 0.7 (0.3,1.7) 0.17 7.3 (0.3) 7.3 (0.3) 0.1 (0.7,0.5) 0.85
Mood 14.9 (1.0) 16.9 (0.6) 2.0 (4.3,0.3) 0.09 16.6 (0.8) 15.3 (0.7) 1.3 (0.5,3.0) 0.16
Abbreviation: CI, confidence interval.
104 Journal of Voice, Vol. 23, No. 1, 2009

TABLE 3.
Marginal Mean Values for Voice Quality Measurements and Mood by Contraceptive Pill Use and Days 24–4 or Days 5–23 of
Cycle Estimated from Mixed Linear Models for Each Measurement in Females Only
Contraceptive Pill Effect Cycle Effect
Voice
Quality No Yes Difference Days 24–4 Days 5–23 Difference
Measure Mean (SE) Mean (SE) (95% CI) P Value Mean (SE) Mean (SE) (95% CI) P Value
Peak form 5.5 5.2 (0.3) 0.3 (0.4,1.1) 0.37 5.1 (0.3) 5.6 (0.2) 0.4 (1.0,0.1) 0.14
Fatigued 5.3 6.1 (0.3) 0.8 (1.6,0.1) 0.07 5.3 (0.3) 6.1 (0.2) 0.8 (1.5,0.1) 0.02
Effort 5.5 (0.3) 6.0 (0.3) 0.5 (1.3,0.3) 0.20 5.4 (0.3) 6.0 (0.2) 0.6 (1.3,0.1) 0.06
Support 5.9 (0.3) 6.3 (0.3) 0.5 (1.3,0.7) 0.49 6.1 (0.3) 6.1 (0.3) 0.1 (0.7,0.7) 0.98
Hoarse 5.9 (0.3) 7.3 (0.3) 1.3 (2.2,0.5) 0.003 6.4 (0.3) 6.8 (0.2) 0.4 (1.0,0.3) 0.24
Weak 6.1 (0.3) 6.5 (0.3) 0.4 (1.2,0.4) 0.31 6.2 (0.3) 6.4 (0.2) 0.2 (0.9,0.5) 0.57
Range 6.2 (0.3) 6.9 (0.3) 0.8 (1.6,0.1) 0.10 6.4 (0.3) 6.7 (0.2) 0.4 (1.1,0.3) 0.28
Control 5.8 (0.3) 6.4 (0.3) 0.6 (1.5,0.2) 0.15 5.9 (0.3) 6.3 (0.2) 0.4 (1.1,0.3) 0.24
Sluggish 6.1 (0.3) 6.2 (0.3) 0.1 (0.9,0.7) 0.85 6.0 (0.3) 6.4 (0.2) 0.4 (1.0,0.3) 0.30
Flat 6.8 (0.3) 7.1 (0.3) 0.3 (1.2,0.6) 0.49 7.0 (0.3) 7.0 (0.3) 0.1 (0.7,0.7) 0.87
Mood 17.8 (0.7) 16.0 (0.8) 1.8 (0.4,4.0) 0.10 17.8 (0.9) 15.9 (0.6) 1.9 (0.1,3.9) 0.07
Abbreviation: CI, confidence interval.

form, effort, support, and sluggishness but there were no trends Phase 2
for voice quality to improve over these days in males. Perceptual analysis of six female singers. Figure 5
Table 6 shows that there was a negative correlation between shows the pedagogue scores for each student during normal
voice quality and mood for all measurements on days 1, 4, 14, or menstruating phase. Variations in pedagogue scores were
and 23 indicating poor mood with reduced voice quality. Corre- wide with the smallest range of 4.7–7.1 for singer ID 21 and
lations greater than 0.43, which indicate that approximately the largest range of 1.7–5.9 for singer ID 22. There was no
40% of the variation in mood was explained by voice quality, consistent change in direction of scores during menstruating
were statistically significant. Most of the significant associa- and nonmenstruating phases with scores tending to decrease
tions occurred on days 1 and 14. for four singers and increase for one singer. As a check on con-
Mood had a stronger effect on the voice than the contracep- sistency of listener ratings, one singer provided data only dur-
tive pill, an interesting and somewhat unexpected finding. ing the nonmenstruating phase. The scores were equally
Mood improved somewhat in both males and females over variable.
days 1–7 although the trend was not statistically significant. Five of the six singers who were selected on the basis of the
The overall mood score was not significantly different between worst self-reported vocal symptoms during menstruation cor-
males and females, although males consistently rated their rectly identified their performance during menstruation when
mood as better than females. presented blind with both their recordings (one during

9 9
Days 24-4 Days 24-4
Days 5-23 Days 5-23
8 8
Mean voice quality score
Mean voice quality score

7 7

6 6

5 5

4 4
Fatigued

Effort

Support

Hoarse

Range

Control

Sluggish

Pitch
Fatigued

Effort

Support

Hoarse

Range

Control

Sluggish

Pitch

Peak

Weak
Peak

Weak

FIGURE 3. Mean unadjusted voice quality in females during days FIGURE 4. Mean unadjusted voice quality in males during days 24–
24–4 and 5–23. 4 and 5–23.
Maree Ryan and Dianna T. Kenny Does Menstrual Cycle Affect Female Classical Singers’ Voices? 105

menstruation and the other during nonmenstruation). Figure 6


TABLE 4.
Mean Values with Standard Deviations Shown in
shows the self-rated scores for each singer. Two singers (ID
Brackets for Voice Quality Measurements and Mood on 21 and 27) rated themselves consistently and highly but for
Days 1, 4, 12–15, and 23 of the Cycle the other four singers the ratings across voice quality were in-
consistent and variable.
Days
Perceptual analysis of six female singers demonstrated
Day 1 Day 4 12–15* Day 23
a wide variation in pedagogue scoring during both non-men-
Peak Females 4.1 (2.5) 5.2 (2.6) 6.1 (2.9) 5.7 (2.4) struating and menstruating phases. However, the students,
form Males 5.8 (1.8) 6.7 (1.7) 7.0 (1.4) 6.7 (1.0) who were selected based on the worst self-reported vocal symp-
Fatigued Females 4.1 (2.6) 5.2 (3.2) 6.9 (2.8) 6.7 (3.0) toms during menstruation, correctly identified their perfor-
Males 4.4 (2.6) 6.1 (2.4) 7.3 (2.8) 6.8 (1.9) mance during menstruation when presented blind with both
Effort Females 4.1 (2.8) 5.4 (3.0) 6.9 (2.6) 6.2 (2.8)
their recordings.
Males 4.9 (2.6) 6.7 (2.6) 6.7 (2.0) 4.9 (2.4) The participants were able to accurately determine the timing
of the recordings, whereas the pedagogues were unable to dis-
Support Females 3.9 (2.8) 5.7 (3.1) 6.8 (2.4) 6.4 (3.2) cern any difference in vocal quality between the two samples.
Males 4.2 (3.4) 6.1 (2.5) 6.5 (3.4) 6.3 (2.1)
Hoarse Females 5.3 (3.1) 5.8 (3.2) 7.5 (2.1) 7.3 (2.9)
Males 7.7 (1.3) 7.0 (2.9) 7.6 (2.9) 7.3 (2.0) DISCUSSION
Weak Females 5.2 (3.1) 5.9 (3.2) 7.0 (2.9) 6.2 (3.2) This study examined the effect of the menstrual cycle on the
Males 6.5 (2.5) 7.5 (1.5) 8.0 (1.4) 6.7 (2.1) western female classical singing voice. Participants were sing-
Range Females 4.9 (3.5) 5.8 (3.8) 7.6 (2.1) 7.1 (3.1) ing students studying at the Sydney Conservatorium of Music,
Males 6.3 (3.4) 5.4 (2.2) 6.1 (3.7) 6.3 (3.2) The University of Sydney. Twenty-one young western female
students and six young male singing students between 18 and
Control Females 4.5 (3.2) 5.8 (3.4) 7.2 (2.9) 5.6 (3.6)
28 years of age kept diaries of their perceived vocal quality
Males 5.9 (2.5) 6.5 (2.8) 6.6 (3.2) 6.7 (2.8)
and mood state every day for 1 (male) or 2 months (female).
Sluggish Females 4.6 (3.3) 6.2 (3.0) 7.1 (2.9) 5.7 (3.2) Analysis of the diaries showed large variations in female
Males 6.2 (3.0) 6.6 (3.0) 7.3 (2.8) 7.1 (2.3) symptomatology throughout the cycle with some singers re-
Pitch Females 6.3 (3.2) 6.7 (2.9) 7.1 (2.9) 6.6 (3.5) porting no significant adverse effects of their cycle, while others
Males 7.2 (2.5) 7.7 (2.1) 7.6 (3.6) 8.3 (2.3) reported a range of problems from mild to severe symptoms and
perceived reductions in their vocal quality, particularly during
Mood Females 22.0 (12.7) 19.1 (9.7) 13.5 (7.2) 16.9 (8.9)
Males 14.2 (7.9) 12.7 (5.4) 12.3 (5.9) 16.3 (7.2)
the first 7 days of the cycle. Similarly, Smith23 found that symp-
toms observed premenstrually also occur at other times in the
* The highest mean value over the 4 days is shown, usually day 14 as
shown in Table 5.
cycle but they do not necessarily occur in every cycle and do
not occur in all women.

TABLE 5.
F Value and P Value Statistics from General Linear Repeated Measures Models to Identify Lowest and Highest Days of Cycle
in Females Only and for Trends Over Days 1–4 of Cycle in Both Males and Females
Day 1* Days 12–15* Days 1–4 Trendy Days 1–4 Trendy

Females Females Females Males

Voice Quality F Value P Value Day F Value P Value F Value P Value F Value P Value
Peak form 4.7 0.06 14 6.8 0.03 5.2 0.03 3.9 0.11
Fatigued 7.5 0.02 14 25.5 0.001 2.0 0.17 3.9 0.11
Effort 9.7 0.01 14 12.7 0.01 4.4 0.05 0.3 0.64
Support 19.7 0.001 14 2.5 0.15 4.5 0.05 2.2 0.20
Hoarse 2.5 0.15 12 11.7 0.01 1.2 0.29 0.1 0.75
Weak 1.7 0.23 14 8.3 0.02 1.6 0.22 1.6 0.30
Range 2.4 0.15 15 6.1 0.03 1.1 0.30 0.7 0.43
Control 4.2 0.07 14 6.0 0.04 1.7 0.21 0.1 0.83
Sluggish 8.0 0.02 14 6.7 0.03 5.1 0.04 0.1 0.88
Flat 1.6 0.23 12 9.4 0.01 0.1 0.72 0.4 0.56
Mood 4.9 0.05 12 16.7 0.001 1.9 0.18 0.3 0.64
* Deviation contrast with remainder of cycle.
y
Polynomial linear contrast.
106 Journal of Voice, Vol. 23, No. 1, 2009

TABLE 6.
Correlation Between Mood and Voice Quality on Days 1, 4, 14, and 23 in Female Singers
Day 1 Day 4 Day 14 Day 23
Peak form 0.45 (0.04) 0.28 (0.22) 0.43 (0.05) 0.20 (0.41)
Fatigued voice 0.41 (0.07) 0.28 (0.21) 0.53 (0.01) 0.47 (0.04)
Effort 0.27 (0.24) 0.51 (0.02) 0.42 (0.06) 0.29 (0.22)
Support 0.50 (0.02) 0.24 (0.29) 0.37 (0.10) 0.14 (0.55)
Hoarse 0.45 (0.04) 0.39 (0.08) 0.49 (0.02) 0.37 (0.11)
Weak 0.47 (0.03) 0.29 (0.21) 0.51 (0.02) 0.32 (0.16)
Range 0.33 (0.14) 0.34 (0.14) 0.33 (0.15) 0.24 (0.31)
Control 0.49 (0.03) 0.48 (0.03) 0.50 (0.02) 0.33 (0.16)
Sluggish 0.67 (0.001) 0.40 (0.07) 0.49 (0.03) 0.23 (0.33)
Pitch 0.58 (0.01) 0.20 (0.38) 0.66 (0.001) 0.19 (0.41)
Notes: Pearson’s r value is shown with P value in brackets.

The effects of the cycle were assessed in a number of ways. voice stability through reductions in the extent of hormonal
We were interested in the variations over the first 7 days of the fluctuations over the cycle.
cycle, assessed from the first day of bleeding. A second set of We were surprised that of the six singers reporting the most
analyses comparing two phases of the cycle were also under- severe menstrual symptoms, two were taking the oral contra-
taken. The cycle was divided into the premenstrual phase and ceptive pill. Singers were not asked for the specific type of
menstrual phase (days 24–4) when symptoms tend to be at their oral contraceptive pill, although monophasic pills are more
maximum and these days were compared to the remainder of common in Australia than biphasic or triphasic pills. Future re-
the cycle (days 5–23). Results showed that those taking the search could explore which oral contraceptive pill is most effi-
oral contraceptive pill rated their voice quality higher than those cacious in reducing symptoms associated with voice-quality
not taking the pill, better their self-reported mood was than variations in female singers.
those not on the pill. This supports the research by Ledger After adjusting for the influence of the contraceptive pill, we
and Davidson (in Comins24) that the contraceptive pill has a sta- also demonstrated that voice was lower and mood was worse in
bilizing effect on the voice and may benefit female singers. The cycle days 24–4 than in cycle days 5–23 with significant differ-
association of better mood with contraceptive pill use needs fur- ences for fatigued voice. Abramson et al (in Emerich et al27)
ther exploration. state that altered metabolic activity by estrogen sensitive cells
Singers who were not taking a contraceptive pill tended to could have an effect on the quality of the voice, because vocal
rate lower on all voice quality scores, except peak form, a find- fold hormonal receptor sites are sensitive to estrogen, proges-
ing that supports Sataloff’s25 assertion that hormone imbal- terone, and thyroid hormone. More recently, these hormone re-
ances affect the voice and hormone therapies restore the ceptors have been found in the human vocal folds.28,29 This
voice. Similarly, Amir et al5,26 demonstrated that use of finding should lead to more defined clinical reasons for the var-
a low-dose monophasic contraceptive pill was associated with iations of the singing voice over the menstrual cycle. Through

FIGURE 5. Pedagogue scores for each student. FIGURE 6. Self-rated scores for each singer.
Maree Ryan and Dianna T. Kenny Does Menstrual Cycle Affect Female Classical Singers’ Voices? 107

the various physiological feedback pathways and the neuro- Mood improved somewhat in both males and females over
endocrine system, mood and body states of the singer will influ- days 1–7 although the trend was not statistically significant.
ence the level of hormone production, in turn triggering the The overall mood score was not significantly different between
receptor sites in the vocal folds. males and females, although males consistently rated their
Although male singers were included in the study as a control mood as better than females. Wicklund8 concluded that ‘‘the se-
group to ascertain to what extent mood and vocal quality fluctu- verity of PMS symptoms and menstrual dysphonia symptoms
ated from day to day without the influence of the female hor- may be linked, implying a similar wide range of mood and
mones, we were surprised to observe the degree to which the voice quality scores.’’ Abitbol et al7 defined vocal PMS that in-
male singers fluctuated over the 28 days of their diary record- cluded vocal fatigue, decreased range, loss of vocal power, and
ings. These fluctuations were less pronounced than for the fe- loss of certain high harmonics (huskier voice), with the symp-
males and showed no recognizable points of reference toms more pronounced in the premenstrual phase.
(‘‘fixed’’ days) to calendar days. The perceived effects of In females, most ratings tended to be lower on days 1–3 than
mood, effort, and physical symptoms were similar in both sexes. for the remainder of the cycle and there was a trend for some
Other recent research has also shown that males display varia- ratings to rise through days 1–7. Poor mood was directly asso-
tion over time in their mood and well-being. Earlier research ciated with perceived reduced voice quality for all days
by McFarlane and Williams (in Worsey and Aubeeluck30) dem- throughout the cycle and was significantly greater on days 1
onstrated that men may experience cyclical changes, whereas and 14. Similarly, there was a negative association between fa-
a more recent study by Worsey and Aubeeluck30 showed that tigue and mood at day 14, the peak in hormone secretion around
men and women who completed the Menstrual Distress Ques- ovulation. The effect of the menses, that is, withdrawal of hor-
tionnaire (MDQ—Moos, 1968) both complained of symptoms. mones, is well documented. Lacina (in Chae et al6) reported that
Men in fact rated significantly higher on the MDQ than women many female singers had difficulty with intricate phonation
for pain, concentration, behavioral change, arousal, and control. control before each menstruation. Lacina6 also reported that
The results indicated that men experience many of the symp- the changes to the voice appear proportional to the presenting
toms usually associated with menstrual distress in women but severity of the premenstrual symptoms, with which this re-
may describe them differently. The preliminary findings obvi- search is in agreement. On the other hand, the midcycle peak
ously have health inferences for women stereotyped as mani- of hormones also apparently affects the voice detrimentally in
festing PMS and also for men with cyclical changes that may association with body and mood states.
be unrecognized and may require treatment. Results supported the hypothesis that the voice is affected by
Although some cyclical variation was observed in both sexes, the menstrual cycle and its inherent physiological changes.
the results for the male singers showed more random fluctua- However, the effects were not uniform across all singers, a find-
tions than those observed in females. There was a trend for ing also observed by Brown and Rothman,22 who used trained
the voice-quality parameters to improve over the first 7 days singers, some of whom were taking the contraceptive pill and
of the cycle for both males and females, showing a similar pat- singing tasks such as chromatic scales and phrases where the
tern of improvement; however, the male pattern commenced on voices revealed the most difficulty in phonation.
a random day 1, as opposed to the physiological day 1 in the fe- There was wide variation in ratings of mood and voice qual-
male cycle. This finding suggests that recording symptoms in ity across the same days for the two cycles rated by the female
a diary format heightens awareness of the observer to their participants. Mood varied more obviously than voice quality,
voice quality, perhaps leading to initially more negative ratings. with worse moods associated with greater variation in vocal
However, we have no adequate explanation for this finding and quality.
further research is needed to clarify possible explanations for its Five of the six singers who were selected on the basis of
occurrence. having the worst self-reported vocal symptoms during menstru-
The young women in our study consistently rated their voice ation correctly identified their performance during menstrua-
quality lower than the men. Men did not rate their voices differ- tion when presented blind with both their recordings (one on
ently across the two cycle phases except for peak form and fa- the first day of menstruation and one at approximately mid-
tigued voice, whereas the females rated nearly all recorded cycle). Analysis of their diary data also showed that there was
symptoms differently between the two cycle phases. Males wide variation during both menstruating and nonmenstruating
scored higher than females for all qualities except range. phases, but with no consistent changes observed. Although
Male singers rated peak form significantly higher than female singers were able to identify perceptually their singing samples
singers, but there was only a marginal difference between taken at different times in their cycles, expert pedagogues did
men and women in ratings of weak voice and sluggish voice. not reliably discern differences in vocal quality. However,
Over the cycle, fatigued voice improved significantly in both singers’ awareness of vocal distress and low mood is not neces-
males and females but the women also had improved ratings sarily transmitted to their audience. The expert listeners were
for effort, support, control, and sluggishness, and to a lesser ex- unable to reliably discern differences in vocal quality between
tent peak form, whereas male ratings showed no differences in the two samples and did not rate the non-menstrual sample con-
these parameters. sistently higher than the menstrual sample. Recent commen-
Mood had a stronger effect on the voice than the contracep- tary31,32 has highlighted how singers develop the ability to
tive pill, an interesting and somewhat unexpected finding. discern very subtle differences in vocal quality that listeners
108 Journal of Voice, Vol. 23, No. 1, 2009

cannot. Consequently, singers’ education should include infor- 7. Abitbol J, Abitbol P, Abitbol B. Sex hormones and the female voice.
mation that their perceptions regarding their vocal quality may J Voice. 1999;13:424-446.
8. Wicklund K. A quantitative survey of premenstrual syndrome and men-
not adversely affect perceptual quality.
strual dysphonia symptoms experienced by singer/voice teachers. J Sing.
Future research could replicate this study with the addition of 1996;52:19-22.
blood samples assessing hormonal levels over the menstrual cy- 9. Davis CB, Davis ML. The effects of premenstrual syndrome (PMS) on the
cle, in an attempt to account for the wide variation in vocal and female singer. J Voice. 1993;7:337-353.
mood ratings between singers and within singers across two rat- 10. Sommer B. Stress and menstrual distress. J Human Stress. 1978;4:5-10.
41–47.
ing periods.
11. Abitbol J, de Brux J, Millot G, Masson M-F, Mimoun OL, Pau H, Abitbol B.
Hormonal vocal cord cycle: pre-menstrual syndrome. J Voice. 1989;3:
CONCLUSION 157-162.
This longitudinal study is important to the singing community, 12. Nader S. Premenstrual syndrome: tailoring treatment to symptoms. Post-
grad Med. 1991;90:173-180.
as it is a methodologically sound study using technically chal-
13. Rubin J. The ages of the voice. Aust Voice. 2001;7:17-22.
lenging musical tasks to examine the perceived effects on the 14. Sataloff RT. Laryngoscope: vocal ageing and its medical implications: what
female classical singing voice over the menstrual cycle. singing teachers should know. Part 1. J Sing. 2000;57:29-34.
Results indicated that the female hormonal cycle had a per- 15. Sataloff RT. Laryngoscope: vocal ageing and its medical implications: what
ceptual effect on the voice of the young female western classical singing teachers should know. Part 2. J Sing. 2000;57:23-28.
16. McNair DM, Droppleman LF. Profile of Mood States. San Diego, CA: Ed-
singer for the singers who reported being affected by menstrual
ucational and Industrial Service; 1981.
symptoms but no discernible perceptual effects on expert lis- 17. Abplanalp JM, Donnelly AF, Rose RM. Psychoendocrinology of the men-
teners. Not everyone reported symptoms and there was wide strual cycle: 1. Enjoyment of daily activities and moods. J Psychosom Med.
variation in symptomatology. However, the results indicated 1979;41:587-604.
that a small proportion of singers were severely affected during 18. Abplanalp JM, Donnelly AF, Rose RM. Psychoendocrinology of the men-
strual cycle: 2. The relationship between enjoyment of activities, moods
the menstrual phase of their cycles. These singers found the de-
and reproductive hormones. J Psychosom Med. 1979;41:605-615.
mands of coordinating their body more challenging, vocaliza- 19. Nix J. ‘‘Dear Diary.’’ body monitoring techniques for singers. J Sing.
tion more effortful, and singing less aurally and artistically 1998;54:25-27.
rewarding. 20. Rhoades R, Pflanzer R. Human Physiology. Orlando, FL: Saunders College
The study demonstrated that self-awareness of the artist is Publishing; 1992.
21. Williams PL, Wendell-Smith CP. Basic Human Embryology. London, UK:
much more sensitive and critical than the perception of a highly
Pitman Medical Publishing Co. Ltd.; 1966.
trained observer, implying that audience satisfaction is not nec- 22. Brown WS, Rothman H. The menstrual cycle and its relationship to the
essarily as difficult to achieve as self-satisfaction. highly stressed female singer. In: Transcript of Fourteenth Symposium on
Care of the Professional Voice. New York: The Voice Foundation; 1985:
Acknowledgments 114-123.
23. Smith SL. The menstrual cycle and mood disturbances. Clin Obstet Gynae-
We thank Dr Peter Lorenz, MBBS FRACGP DObstRCOG, for col. 1976;19:391-397.
expert advice on the medical components of the study and for 24. Comins J. Scientists say pill could be good for your voice [report on a re-
search project]. Singer 2002;22-23 [Voice Clinic].
support with the preparation of the manuscript. 25. Sataloff RT. Sex hormones and singers: endocrine effects on the voice
[C136]. NATS J. 1994;LI:31-34.
REFERENCES 26. Amir O, Biron-Shental T. The impact of hormonal fluctuations on female
1. Silverman EM, Zimmer CH. Effects of the menstrual cycle on voice quality. vocal folds. Curr Opin Otolaryngol Head Neck Surg. 2004;12:180-184.
Arch Otolaryngol Head Neck Surg. 1978;104:7-10. 27. Emerich K, Hoover C, Sataloff RT. The effects of menopause on the human
2. Higgins MB, Saxman JH. Variations in vocal frequency perturbation across voice: laryngoscope (‘‘meta-analysis’’). J Sing. 1996;52:39-42.
the menstrual cycle. J Voice. 1989;3:233-243. 28. Newman SR, Butler J, Hammond EH, Gray SD. Preliminary report on hor-
3. Brown WS, Hollien H. Effects of menstruation on the singing voice (1). mone receptors in the human vocal fold. J Voice. 2000;14:72-81.
Transcript of Twelfth Symposium on Care of the Professional Voice (1). 29. Altman KW, Haines GK III, Vakkalanka SK, Keni SP, Kopp PA,
New York: The Voice Foundation; 1982. 112–116. Radosevich JA. Identification of thyroid hormone receptors in the human
4. Brown WS, Isenberger H, Rothman H. Effects of menstruation on the sing- larynx. Laryngoscope. 2003;113:1931-1934.
ing voice (2). Transcript of Twelfth Symposium on Care of the Professional 30. Worsey J, Aubeeluck A. Are men suffering from ‘‘Menstrual Distress’’? A
Voice (1). New York: The Voice Foundation; 1982. 117–123. preliminary comparison of men and women’s self-reported symptoms using
5. Amir O, Biron-Shental T, Muchnik C, Kishon-Rabin L. Do oral contracep- Moos (1968) Menstrual Distress Questionnaire. In: Proceedings of the Brit-
tives improve voice quality? Limited trial on low-dose formulations. Obstet ish Psychological Society. 2005;13:91.
Gynecol. 2003;101:773-777. 31. Lã F, Davidson JW. Investigating the relationship between sexual hormones
6. Chae SW, Choi G, Choi JO, Jin SM, Kang HJ. Clinical analysis of voice and female western classical singing. Res Stud Music Educ. 2005;24:75-87.
change as a parameter of premenstrual syndrome. J Voice. 2001;15: 32. Sataloff RT, Spiegel JR, Hawkshaw MJ, Jones A. Laryngoscope: hormones
278-283. and the voice. J Voice. 1993;7:43-45.

View publication stats

You might also like