You are on page 1of 15

Journal of Youth and Adolescence (2018) 47:2114–2128

https://doi.org/10.1007/s10964-017-0801-6

EMPIRICAL RESEARCH

Social Media Social Comparison of Ability (but not Opinion) Predicts


Lower Identity Clarity: Identity Processing Style as a Mediator
Chia-chen Yang1 Sean M. Holden2 Mollie D.K. Carter3
● ●

Received: 30 November 2017 / Accepted: 19 December 2017 / Published online: 11 January 2018
© Springer Science+Business Media, LLC, part of Springer Nature 2018

Abstract
Social comparison on social media has received increasing attention, but most research has focused on one type of social
comparison and its psycho-emotional implications. Little is known about how different types of social comparison influence
youth’s identity development. Drawing on the theories of identity processing styles and social comparison, we examined
how two different forms of social comparison on social media related to three identity processing styles, which in turn
predicted youth’s global self-esteem and identity clarity. We surveyed 219 college freshmen (Mage = 18.29; 74% female)
once in the Fall and once in the Spring. Social comparison of ability on social media was related to concurrent diffuse-
1234567890();,:

avoidant identity processing style, which predicted lower identity clarity months later. In contrast, social comparison of
opinion on social media did not influence college freshmen’s global self-esteem and identity clarity through identity
processing styles. The findings clarified the implications of online social comparison for youth’s identity development.
Keywords Social media Social comparison Identity processing styles Identity Self-esteem Emerging adult
● ● ● ● ●

Introduction but to learn about facts and social norms, construct or modify
one’s own value systems, and regulate behaviors (Festinger
Social comparison, the process of comparing oneself with 1954; Suls et al. 2000). For instance, when people face
others, takes two forms (Festinger 1954). Social comparison problems, they often engage in social comparison of opinion
of ability, which entails comparison of achievement and to explore what others would do in a similar situation.
performance broadly defined, is inherently judgmental and The process of social comparison is particularly relevant
competitive. It centers on determining how well one is doing, in the digital age as social media platforms (e.g., Facebook,
relative to others (Festinger 1954; Gibbons and Buunk 1999; Instagram, etc.) allow users to easily access others’ daily
Suls et al. 2002). A common example is students comparing updates, providing rich opportunities for social comparison.
their grades with others’ as a way to assess their own aca- Although scholars have started studying the implications of
demic performance. Social comparison of opinion, which social media social comparison, existing literature has
includes comparison of thoughts, attitudes, values, and several limitations. First, despite the growing literature on
beliefs, is usually free from the competitive and judgmental the negative relationship between online social comparison
characteristics embedded in social comparison of ability and various psycho-emotional outcomes (de Vries and
(Festinger 1954; Suls et al. 2000). The goal of opinion Kühne 2015; Feinstein et al. 2013; Lee 2014; Lim and Yang
comparison is not to distinguish the better from the worse, 2015; Vogel et al. 2014; Weinstein 2017), most research
focuses on the judgmental form of comparison, and thus
little is known about social comparison of opinion on social
* Chia-chen Yang media. Second, the implications of social media social
cyang2@memphis.edu comparison for youth’s identity development1 remain
1
Department of Counseling, Educational Psychology and Research,
1
University of Memphis, 303D Ball Hall, Memphis, TN 38152, USA Although some scholars emphasize the distinction between self and
2 identity, others suggest that such differentiation is artificial (see a
Department of Counseling, Educational Psychology and Research
review in Vignoles et al. 2011). In this study, we use the terms of “self”
& Center for Research in Educational Policy, University of
and “identity” interchangeably, both referring to a person’s organized
Memphis, 318 Browning Hall, Memphis, TN 38152, USA
knowledge related to himself or herself (e.g., traits, beliefs, values,
3
Department of Counseling, Educational Psychology and Research, worth, etc.; Berzonsky 2011; Campbell et al. 1996; Erikson 1968;
University of Memphis, 100 Ball Hall, Memphis, TN 38152, USA Rosenberg et al. 1989).
Journal of Youth and Adolescence (2018) 47:2114–2128 2115

largely unexamined. Identity development is a central information and traverse identity crises/conflicts through
developmental task for adolescents and emerging adults either information seeking and rational analysis, normative
(Arnett 2014; Erikson 1968), who are among the major assimilation, or avoidance and procrastination (Berzonsky
users of social media (Greenwood et al. 2016; Lenhart and Barclay 1981; Berzonsky 2011).
2015). Self-esteem and identity clarity represent two Corresponding with these differences in processing, three
important dimensions of identity development (Campbell identity processing styles have been noted: informational,
et al. 1996; Erikson 1968; Heppner and Kernis 2011), both normative, and diffuse-avoidant (Berzonsky 1990, 2011).
of which have crucial psychosocial implications, such as The informational processing style encompasses a deliberate,
better interpersonal relationships, better mental health, iterative, and integrative stance toward identity formation and
lower levels of rumination, and fewer depressive symptoms conflict, with one actively seeking out and analyzing relevant
(Campbell et al. 1996; Heppner and Kernis 2011; Luychx information as well as openly challenging and revising self-
et al. 2007; Trzesniewski et al. 2013). Although existing perceptions when faced with dissonant feedback (Berzonsky
literature has revealed the relationship between social et al. 2013). In contrast, normative identity processing
comparison and poor self-assessment (de Vries and Kühne involves automatized assimilation of identity information,
2015; Vogel et al. 2014), there is virtually no research on internalization of externally derived identity components
how social media social comparison associates with youth’s (e.g., goals and values from a certain group), and preserva-
identity clarity. Further, the identity processes through tion of self-conceptions despite dissonant information (Ber-
which social comparison may influence youth’s global self- zonsky et al. 2013). While both styles involve engaging with
esteem and identity clarity remain unclear. identity, the important difference is in the level of active,
Drawing on the theory of identity processing styles conscious engagement and internal versus external focus.
(Berzonsky 1990, 2011) and literature of social com- This divide is further displayed in the relations of each
parison, we aimed to shed light on these gaps through this construct, where informational processing is associated with
research. We sampled college freshmen because identity using intrinsic, self-developed values and goals to inform
development and social comparison may be particularly overall identity, while normative processing is associated
important to this group. Emerging adults transitioning to with using external, collective self-characteristics (Berzonsky
college are presented with opportunities to negotiate and (re) et al. 2011; Duriez et al. 2012).
construct identity. Part of this process may involve evaluation Finally, unlike the previous two styles, diffuse-avoidant
of one’s abilities and values, which can be conducted through processing involves distancing oneself from identity infor-
social comparison. We administered a short-term long- mation, conflicts, and decisions. Thus, one cognitively
itudinal survey study, in which we tested a path model where avoids conscious processing of identity matters, shifting
the associations between the two types of social media social attention away from meaningful life decisions, evading self-
comparison and two identity variables (global self-esteem feedback, and perpetually accommodating to situational
and identity clarity) were examined, with three identity pro- factors (Berzonsky et al. 2013). Further, this avoidance
cessing styles (Berzonsky 1990, 2011) being potential serves as a way to protect the self from negative feedback
mediators. while reducing individual contemplation in favor of relying
on social sources of positive feedback, such as popularity
Identity Processing Styles and reputation, to inform one’s sense of self (Berzonsky
et al. 2011; Duriez et al. 2012).
Identity processing theory (Berzonsky 1988) provides a By late adolescence, almost all individuals have the
social-cognitive reconception of Marcia’s (1966) identity capacity to utilize all three types of strategies, although they
status paradigm. Within this theory, individuals are viewed as vary in their preference toward each approach (Berzonsky
self-theorists who make sense of themselves and their 2011). The preference for and deployment of the identity
environment through the ongoing processing of information processing strategies are not static, but are influenced by
gained through experience (Berzonsky 1993). Inciting this both motivational and situational factors. Whereas scholars
framework is the notion that identity exploration and com- have explored how motivational variables (e.g., need for
mitment are often confounded within statuses, thereby pre- cognition, need for self-knowledge) contribute to one’s
venting clear understanding of how individuals cognitively identity processing styles (Berzonsky 2011), there remains
handle identity information (Berzonsky et al. 2013). By little information regarding how situational or contextual
looking beyond such statuses, attention is redirected onto the conditions and processes facilitate or hinder the use of these
processes underlying identity formation and revision to focus styles. In this study, we conceptualized social comparison
on the strategies employed to engage with or avoid identity on social media as a situational/contextual factor that would
information (Berzonsky 2011). Specifically, this framework associate with youth’s choice of identity processing style,
takes the stance that individuals process self-referent which in turn would influence their global self-esteem and
2116 Journal of Youth and Adolescence (2018) 47:2114–2128

identity clarity. Below, we review how identity processing Empirical findings generally support these propositions.
styles would/should relate to the identity outcomes and The informational processing style is more adaptive than the
online social comparison. diffuse-avoidant style (Phillips and Pittman 2007), with the
former often associated with higher and the latter with lower
Identity Processing Styles and Identity Development self-esteem (Crocetti et al. 2009; Eryigit and Kerpelman
2009; Soenens et al. 2011, 2016). The styles have also been
Self-esteem and identity construction have been theorized to examined in reference to identity clarity and other related
associate in different manners. Self-esteem can play the role constructs, such as self-concept clarity, identity integration,
of an identity motive (Heppner and Kernis 2011), an identity and identity commitment2. The informational processing
outcome (Heppner and Kernis 2011), or a parallel correlate style is often related to higher and the diffuse-avoidant style
of identity clarity (Campbell et al. 1996). In this study, we to lower levels of commitment (Berzonsky 2003; Crocetti
took the last, parallel perspective. Global self-esteem con- et al. 2009; Johnson and Nozick 2011; Luychx et al. 2007;
cerns evaluation of the identity content, reflecting an indivi- Soenens et al. 2011, 2016) and self-concept clarity (Eryigit
dual’s overarching sense of self-worth (Campbell et al. 1996; and Kerpelman 2009).
Rosenberg et al. 1989). Identity clarity regards organization The implications of the normative style for identity
of the identity structure, denoting the extent to which an development seem less conclusive. From the theoretical
individual has fashioned multiple identities, values, and goals perspective, people adopting the normative style “internalize
into a coordinated sense of self (Campbell et al. 1996; Harter and adhere to goals, values, and prescriptions appropriated
2012; Erikson 1968). from significant others and referent groups” (Berzonsky
For emerging adults, the life transition into college 2011, p. 59) and thus may be viewed positively by these
may bring particular salience to self-esteem and identity significant individuals or communities. The positive per-
clarity. Specifically, though individuals often experience ceptions from others can contribute to higher global self-
developmental trajectories of increasing self-esteem esteem (Harter 2012). Identity validation may be quite
(Erol and Orth 2011; Galambos et al. 2006; Orth et al. accessible to these people, which, along with the tendency
2015) and greater identity integration (Harter 2012) from toward identity preservation (Berzonsky 1999, 2011; Soe-
adolescence to adulthood, these positive trends may be nens et al. 2005), can lead to a clear and committed sense of
disrupted by the transition to college. For instance, findings self. However, empirical findings do not always support
show significant decreases in global self-esteem in the first these propositions. While the style had a positive associa-
semester at college (Chung et al. 2014). Likewise, identity tion with self-esteem, identity commitment, or identity
clarity appears in simultaneous disarray (Kroger et al. integration in some reports (Crocetti et al. 2009; Luychx
2010), with students often reporting feeling lost, unac- et al. 2007; Passmore et al. 2005; Soenens et al. 2011,
knowledged, and without place (Chow and Healey 2008; 2016), it did not relate to identity development in others
Scanlon et al. 2007). (Crocetti et al. 2009; Eryigit and Kerpelman 2009; Soenens
Given such salience of self-esteem and identity clarity at et al. 2016).
the college transition, it is crucial to investigate how they
are informed by identity processing styles. Theoretically, Social Media Social Comparison and Identity
individuals using the informational style should display Processing Styles
higher global self-esteem and identity clarity. These people
are openly and proactively engaged with identity concerns Festinger (1954) posits that individuals have the need to
and likely to engage in problem-focused reconciliation of evaluate themselves, and in the conditions where objective
self-conflicts that may otherwise undermine feelings of self- criteria for evaluation are unavailable, people compare
worth (Berzonsky 2011). Further, they are inclined toward themselves with others. This process is referred to as social
self-reflecting, learning about themselves, and adapting to comparison. In order to achieve accurate self-evaluation,
dissonant identity feedback, allowing them to develop a people often compare themselves with those who seem to
clear, integrated, and committed sense of self (Berzonsky be similar. Festinger’s theory focuses on two types of social
2011). In contrast, those adopting the diffuse-avoidant style comparison: ability and opinion.
tend to rely on unstable, situational identity components The nature of the two types of social comparison differs
(Berzonsky and Adams 1999), leaving a limited footing (Gibbons and Buunk 1999). Social comparison of ability is
from which to coordinate identity components and prohi-
2
biting identity integration. Lack of control over these Identity clarity, identity integration, and self-concept clarity can be
considered synonymous, as they primarily vary semantically in terms
external identity components can also lead to a low sense of
of the intended breadth of focus (Campbell et al. 1996). These con-
agency and thus low self-esteem (Erol and Orth 2011; structs also overlap with identity commitment to a large degree
Wojciszke et al. 2011). (Crocetti et al. 2016).
Journal of Youth and Adolescence (2018) 47:2114–2128 2117

competition-oriented, and the goal is to determine super- comparison of opinion on Facebook experienced less
iority versus inferiority (Festinger 1954). When individuals upward contrastive emotions, such as depression and envy,
see themselves fall behind others in an ability, the resolution and thus reported higher satisfaction with life. On the
is to improve their own ability, usually through non-social contrary, those inclined to engage in social comparison of
processes such as practicing (Festinger 1954). On the other ability on Facebook reported higher levels of these emo-
hand, social comparison of opinion focuses on information tions, and thus lower life satisfaction. The findings showed
collection as a way to learn about the context and the self that the two types of social comparison had opposite
(Suls et al. 2000). The goal of this information-oriented implications for psycho-emotional well-being.
comparison is to make informed decisions rather than The imbalanced attention to the two types of social
judgments. When individuals observe differences in opi- comparison is a missed opportunity, especially for emerging
nions, they take social approaches, such as discussion and adults who are transitioning into college and are eager to
persuasion, to reduce the opinion gaps (Festinger 1954). In reclaim their identity in the new environment. Social com-
their review of Festinger’s work, Park and Baek (2018) parison is a major mechanism to self-knowledge (Harter
argue that in social comparison of ability people view 2012). While most research focuses on social comparison of
comparison targets as competitors, whereas in social com- ability, social comparison of opinion may serve as an
parison of opinion individuals see comparison targets as important venue to clarify or challenge one’s own beliefs,
role models, consultants, or informants. attitudes, and values, all of which are crucial components of
Social media provide a unique context for social com- identity. In fact, both forms of social comparison may play a
parison. With hundreds of social media friends sharing their part in college youth’s identity negotiation, assisting one in
life updates, social media users have almost limitless knowing where to position herself/himself in the new
materials for social comparison. They can compare them- environment. Emerging adults are avid social media users
selves with a large number of people, including those who (Greenwood et al. 2016) and they are likely to perform both
are geographically distant, whenever they have access to the types of social comparison when accessing social media
platforms. However, information shared on social media platforms. It is thus important to explore the identity
does not reflect the full spectrum of users’ lives. Most users implications of both types of social comparison.
strategically present highly polished and socially desirable Specifically, the two types of social comparison may
images through selective self-presentation (Gardner and indirectly contribute to college youth’s global self-esteem
Davis 2013; Yang and Brown 2016). and identity clarity by prompting adoption of different
The easy access to other users’ selective self-presentation identity processing styles. For instance, comparing one’s
makes social media a convenient channel for social com- ability and opinion with others’ on social media may both
parison of ability, broadly defined. Scholars have explored relate to adoption of the informational style—the informa-
the judgmental form of social media social comparison by tion gleaned from online social comparison and the
studying comparison of competence, social skills, social accompanying self-assessment may prompt individuals to
connectedness, popularity, and whether others are doing actively explore, analyze, and make sense of their identity.
better or worse in general (e.g., de Vries and Kühne 2015; At the same time, social comparison (particularly that of
Feinstein et al. 2013; Stapleton et al. 2017; Vogel et al. ability) on social media may also relate to subscription to
2014). Alarmingly, the practice is associated with low self- the diffuse-avoidant style. Because judgmental social
esteem (Stapleton et al. 2017; Vogel et al. 2014), negative comparison on social media contributes to negative emo-
self-perceptions (de Vries and Kühne 2015), poor mental tional reactions and poor self-perceptions (de Vries and
health (Feinstein et al. 2013; Jang et al. 2016), and low Kühne 2015; Feinstein et al. 2013; Lee 2014; Lim and Yang
affective well-being (Lim and Yang 2015; Weinstein 2017). 2015; Vogel et al. 2014; Weinstein 2017), such online
The findings are concerning but unsurprising. After all, social comparison may urge youth to turn to the diffuse-
people perform more upward social comparison than avoidant style and delay consideration for identity-related
downward comparison on social media, meaning they often issues as a way to protect their self-images. In contrast, the
compare themselves with superior others rather than inferior normative style is related to following significant referent
others (Vogel et al. 2014). Such comparison can associate others’ expectations and values (Berzonsky 1990, 2011).
with poor well-being by triggering rumination and negative Given peer norms can be easily enacted and observed on
interpersonal emotions (Feinstein et al. 2013; Lim and Yang social media (Chua and Chang 2016; Manago et al. 2008;
2015). Sherman et al. 2016), online social comparison (particularly
In contrast to the rich literature on social media social that of opinion) may bring to users’ awareness salient norms
comparison of ability, there is only one study on social and thus activate the normative style. These possibilities,
media social comparison of opinion. Park and Baek (2018) however, have not been empirically examined.
found that people who were inclined to perform social
2118 Journal of Youth and Adolescence (2018) 47:2114–2128

media social comparison of ability, social media social


comparison of opinion, global self-esteem, identity clarity)
and four Time 2 outcome variables (social media social
comparison of ability, social media social comparison of
opinion, global self-esteem, identity clarity), with the three
Time 1 identity processing styles being the mediators (see
Fig. 2).

Methods
Fig. 1 Hypothesized model. SMSC-Ab = social media social com-
parison of ability, SMSC-Op = social media social comparison of
opinion
Participants

Current Study The research was conducted at a U.S. university in the mid-
south. At the beginning of the Fall semester (T1), research
Our major goal was to investigate how the two forms of information was announced via e-mail to a random sample
social comparison on social media related to the three of freshmen and face-to-face in various courses enrolling a
identity processing styles, which in turn predicted college high percentage of first-year students. Interested students
freshmen’s global self-esteem and identity clarity (Fig. 1). filled out an online survey after informed consent, and were
Although there is theoretical reason to believe that social invited to complete a follow-up survey in the following
comparison of ability and opinion may have differential Spring semester (T2). As part of the research requirement,
relations with identity processing styles, there is a lack of all participants had to use social media at least once a week.
empirical evidence from which to hypothesize these rela- The study had been approved by the institutional review
tions. Therefore, we posited the following research ques- board. Participants received a small amount of monetary
tions: how would social media social comparison of ability incentive or course credits for their participation.
(Research Question 1a) and that of opinion (Research A total of 219 college freshmen (Mage = 18.29, S.D.
Question 1b) each associate with the identity processing = .75; 74% female; 41% White, 38% Black) completed the
styles? Regarding the relationships between identity pro- first survey, and 136 of them also completed the second
cessing styles and identity outcomes, the consistency questionnaire (Mage = 18.24, S.D. = .65; 74% female; 45%
between empirical results and theoretical propositions for White, 37% Black). Compared with the university freshman
the informational and diffuse-avoidant styles allowed us to population (54% female; 47% White, 36% Black), female
formulate hypotheses for the paths involving them. Speci- students were overrepresented but the ethnic distributions
fically, we hypothesized that the informational style would
relate to higher global self-esteem and identity clarity
(Hypothesis 2a), while the diffuse-avoidant style would
relate to lower levels of both (Hypothesis 2b). For the
normative processing style, we refrained from hypothesiz-
ing any relationships due to inconsistent empirical evidence
in this area. Instead, we opted to raise the question: how
would the normative style relate to global self-esteem and
identity clarity (Research Question 2)?
Although we believed the hypothesized directionality
was theoretically sound, we also recognized the possibility
of the reverse paths. For instance, empirically, global self-
esteem can influence the identity processing styles (Soenens
et al. 2016), and the styles may predict youth’s engagement
in online social comparison (e.g., those who adopt the Fig. 2 The path model. SMSC-Ab = social media social comparison
informational style tend to actively seek out identity-related of ability, SMSC-Op = social media social comparison of opinion.
information, and thus they may perform more social com- The lines represent significant paths. Three indirect paths were sig-
parison of opinion as a way to explore their own thoughts nificant: T1 SMSC-Ab → T1 diffuse-avoidant → T2 identity clarity; T1
identity clarity → T1 diffuse-avoidant → T2 self-esteem, T1 identity
and values). To examine these directionalities and to control clarity → T1 diffuse-avoidant → T2 identity clarity. Numbers are stan-
for the Time 1 score of the outcome variables, we included dardized coefficients. For the clarity of presentation, controlled paths
in the model four Time 1 independent variables (social are excluded. *p < .05, **p < .01, ***p < .001
Journal of Youth and Adolescence (2018) 47:2114–2128 2119

were similar. When asked which social media platform they similar situation would do.” Gibbons and Buunk avoided
used most often, 35% (T1) and 37% (T2) of the participants the word “compare” in the Opinion subscale, likely because
selected Instagram from a pull-down menu; 27% (T1) and the term can prompt respondents to consider the judgmental
25% (T2) chose Facebook; 20% (T1) and 19% (T2) selected form of comparison. Instead, in the Opinion subscale,
Twitter. Among those who chose “Other,” 78% (or 14% of Gibbons and Buunk used the verbs such as “find out” and
the full sample at T1) and 69% (or 13% of the full sample at “know” to capture the information orientation of this form of
T2) nominated Snapchat as their most frequented social social comparison. See the Appendix for the final items,
media channel. Attrition of the 83 participants (38% of the factor loadings, and model fit indices. Both the SMSC-
T1 sample) was neither related to age, t(217) = 1.44, p Ability (5 items; αT1 = .84, αT2 = .84) and SMSC-Opinion
= .15, gender, χ2 (1) = .04, p = .85, race/ethnicity, χ2 (2) = (4 items; αT1 = .87, αT2 = .86) scales had good reliability.
2.53, p = .28, nor platform preference, χ2 (3) = 2.06, p The αs of our sample were between those reported by other
= .56. Independent t tests suggested no difference in the scholars (e.g., Park and Baek 2018; αability = .92, αopinion
focal variables between those who completed both surveys = .72). Higher mean scores reflected higher engagement in
and those who only participated at T1 (ps = .10 to .95). the given type of social comparison on social media.
Little’s (1988) missing completely at random test indicated
that the data were missing completely at random, χ2 (23) = Identity processing styles
23.04, p = .46.
The Revised Identity Style Inventory (ISI-5; Berzonsky
Measures et al. 2013) was administered at T1 to measure the three
identity processing styles: the informational, normative, and
The data of this report were collected as part of a long diffuse-avoidant styles. As indicated in other studies, the
survey study. To reduce participants’ burden, we shortened scale had good reliability: αinformational = .73–.86, αnormative
some scales by removing the items that had lower factor = .71–.82; αdiffuse-avoidant = .71–.89 (Berzonsky et al. 2013;
loadings as reported by the scale designers or based on the Soenens et al. 2016). Participants indicated how much they
results of our pilot study. agreed with the statements on a 5-point Likert scale (1 =
Strongly Disagree, 5 = Strongly Agree). Sample items
Social media social comparison (SMSC) included “When making important decisions, I like to have
as much information as possible” (Informational; 6 items; α
Nine items of the Iowa-Netherlands Comparison Orienta- = .90), “I automatically adopt and follow the values I was
tion Measure (INCOM; Gibbons and Buunk 1999) were brought up with” (Normative; 5 items; α = .79), and “When
adapted to measure social comparison activities on social personal problems arise, I try to delay acting as long as
media. Although the scale was originally designed to possible” (Diffuse-Avoidant; 6 items; α = .84).
measure an “orientation,” given the significant correlation
between the orientation and actual online social comparison Global self-esteem
behavior (Lee 2014), the scale can serve as a proper mea-
sure of online social comparison activities after modifica- We used the six-item version of the Rosenberg Self-Esteem
tion (see Cramer et al. 2016 for a similar approach). Scale (6-item RSES; Rosenberg et al. 1989, 1995).
We added “when using social media” or “on social media” Research using 6 similar (though not identical) items
to each item, and reworded the item “I’m not the type of reported good reliability (Richardson et al. 2009; α = .84).
person who compares often with others” to avoid the sole Participants responded to the questions on a 4-piont Likert
emphasis on social comparison as a personality trait. Par- scale (1 = Strongly Disagree, 4 = Strongly Agree). A higher
ticipants were instructed to consider the extent to which mean score reflected more self-acceptance, self-respect, and
they compared themselves with other people when using more positive self-evaluation (αT1 = .86, αT2 = .86). “I take
social media and indicate how well each statement applied a positive attitude toward myself” was a sample item.
to them on a 5-point Likert scale (1 = Not at All, 5 = Very
Well). Results of a confirmatory factor analysis confirmed Identity clarity
the two-factor structure identified by Gibbons and Buunk
(1999). A sample item of the social media social compar- Six items from the Identity subscale of the Erikson Psy-
ison of ability (SMSC-Ability) scale was “On social media, chosocial Stage Inventory (EPSI; Rosenthal et al. 1981)
I compare what I have done with others as a way to find out were used. Good reliability was reported for this 5-piont
how well I have done something.” A sample item of the Likert scale (1 = Strongly Disagree, 5 = Strongly Agree) in
social media social comparison of opinion (SMSC-Opinion) other research (e.g., Markstrom and Kalmanir 2001;
scale was “On social media, I try to know what others in a Schwartz et al. 2009; αs = .83–.89) as well as in our study
2120 Journal of Youth and Adolescence (2018) 47:2114–2128

Table 1 Descriptive statistics and correlations of major variables


Mean (S.D.) 1. 2. 3. 4. 5. 6. 7. 8. 9. 10.

1. SMSC-Ab T1 (5-pt) 2.91 (1.05)


2. SMSC-Ab T2 (5-pt) 2.89 (1.04) .60***
3. SMSC-Op T1 (5-pt) 2.80 (1.11) .38*** .35***
4. SMSC-Op T2 (5-pt) 2.89 (1.04) .32*** .48*** .60***
5. Self-esteem T1 (4-pt) 3.17 (.69) −.14* −.16 .01 .01
6. Self-esteem T2 (4-pt) 3.16 (.69) −.12 −.24** −.02 −.04 .70***
7. Identity clarity T1 (5-pt) 3.53 (.86) −.23*** −.23** −.08 −.03 .60*** .46***
8. Identity clarity T2 (5-pt) 3.53 (.83) −.32*** −.49*** −.16 −.19* .54*** .62*** .66***
9. Info T1 (5-pt) 4.00 (.74) .12 .19* .18** .19* .30*** .27** .19** .09
10. Normative T1 (5-pt) 2.55 (.81) .13 .13 .06 .23** .07 −.10 .11 .02 −.07
11. DA T1 (5-pt) 2.75 (.89) .33*** .29** .22** .20* −.37*** −.47*** −.52*** −.59*** −.15* .14*

SMSC-Ab = social media social comparison of ability, SMSC-Op = social media social comparison of opinion, Info = informational identity
processing style, DA = diffuse-avoidant identity processing style
*p < .05; **p < .01; ***p < .001

(αT1 = .82, αT2 = .83). A higher mean score reflected a associated with higher concurrent informational style, but
clearer sense of identity. “I know what kind of person I am” not the other two styles; T2 SMSC-Opinion was predicted
was a sample item. by T1 informational and normative styles, but not by T1
diffuse-avoidant style. H2a (the informational style would
Data Analysis relate to higher global self-esteem and identity clarity) was
partially supported: T1 global self-esteem, but not identity
We used Mplus to test our hypothesized model, with clarity, was related to higher concurrent informational style;
maximum likelihood robust (MLR) being the estimator. We T1 informational style did not predict T2 global self-esteem
controlled for the effects of gender, race/ethnicity, age, and nor identity clarity. H2b (the diffuse-avoidant style would
self-reported amount of social media use on all mediators relate to lower global self-esteem and identity clarity) was
and outcome variables. All the four T2 outcome variables also partially supported: T1 identity clarity, but not global
were set to allow the error terms to covary, and so were the self-esteem, was associated with lower concurrent diffuse-
three T1 mediators. We accepted models whose compara- avoidant style; T1 diffuse-avoidant style predicted lower T2
tive fit index (CFI) and Tucker-Lewis index (TLI) were global self-esteem and identity clarity. For RQ2 (how the
close to or greater than .95 (Hu and Bentler 1999), coupled normative style would relate to global self-esteem and
with a root mean square error of approximation (RMSEA) identity clarity), T1 normative style was not associated with
being smaller than .08 (Browne and Cudeck 1993). concurrent global self-esteem or identity clarity, nor did it
predict the two identity outcomes at T2. See Fig. 2.
For the indirect paths, the coefficients were calculated by
Results multiplying the coefficients of the direct paths involved in
the route. We were most interested in whether SMSC pre-
For the descriptive statistics and correlations of the major dicted the identity outcomes via identity processing styles
variables, see Table 1. and vice versa. Only one such path was significant: T1
The model fit was good: χ2 (12) = 14.32, p = .28; CFI SMSC-Ability predicted lower T2 identity clarity through
= .995; TLI = .968; RMSEA = .030, 90% CI [.000–.078]. its association with higher T1 diffuse-avoidant style. There
Path coefficients are available in Table 2. RQ1a explored were, however, three indirect paths having a p value
the relationships between SMSC-Ability and the identity between .05 and .10 (ps = .056 to .080), and two of them
processing styles. T1 SMSC-Ability was positively asso- involved the diffuse-avoidant style: T1 SMSC-Ability had a
ciated with T1 normative and diffuse-avoidant styles, but positive concurrent association with the diffuse-avoidant
not the informational style; T2 SMSC-Ability was predicted style, and the style predicted lower T2 self-esteem; T1
by T1 diffuse-avoidant style, but not the other two styles. identity clarity was associated with lower diffuse-avoidant
RQ1b explored the relationships between SMSC-Opinion style concurrently, and the style predicted higher T2 SMSC-
and the identity processing styles. T1 SMSC-Opinion was Ability. In contrast, T1 self-esteem had a positive
Journal of Youth and Adolescence (2018) 47:2114–2128 2121

relationship with T1 informational style, and the style pre- et al. 2000). In contrast, the defining feature of social
dicted higher T2 SMSC-Opinion. See Table 2 for all direct comparison of ability is competition (Festinger, 1954; Park
paths of interest, significant indirect paths, and significant and Baek 2018). Presenting oneself as popular, well-roun-
controlled paths. ded, and physically attractive is among the major goals of
youth’s social media self-presentation (Chua and Chang
2016; Manago et al. 2008; Zhao et al. 2008). As most users
Discussion strive to fulfill the goal by delivering a selectively positive
image (Gardner and Davis 2013; Yang and Brown 2016),
In this study, we explored the short-term longitudinal asso- social media are turned into a convenient channel for
ciations between social media social comparison and college upward social comparison (Vogel et al. 2014) where the
freshmen’s identity states, with the identity processing styles comparers easily experience negative emotions, such as
being potential mediators. Social comparison of ability and depression, envy, and shame (Feinstein et al. 2013; Lim and
social comparison of opinion are two forms of comparison Yang 2015; Park and Baek 2018). Individuals may adopt
serving different purposes; the former is a judgement of the diffuse-avoidant style after engagement in online social
performance and centers on competition, whereas the latter comparison of ability to escape from the self-threatening
provides information on how to manage values and behaviors information. At the same time, however, adoption of the
(Festinger 1954; Gibbons and Buunk 1999; Suls et al. 2002). diffuse-avoidant style, which relates to the desire for
As most existing research focused on the judgmental form of immediate rewards (Berzonsky 2011), may encourage
online social comparison and its association with psycho- individuals to perform more of such comparison to develop
emotional well-being (e.g., de Vries and Kühne 2015; a transient image that would impress others.
Feinstein et al. 2013; Vogel et al. 2014), our goal was to Social comparison of ability on social media was also
expand the literature by attending to both forms of compar- associated with higher concurrent engagement in the nor-
ison and their identity implications among young emerging mative style. This style, as the diffuse-avoidant style, may
adults. Overall, our results confirmed that the two types of serve as a coping mechanism when social comparison of
social comparison were distinguished processes, generating ability results in threats to the self. Adopting this style,
different direct and indirect associations with identity pro- people can ignore irreconcilable information (e.g., “I am
cessing styles, global self-esteem, and identity clarity, and inferior to my friends” versus “I am a person of worth”),
only social comparison of ability on social media indirectly hold on to the identity prescribed by significant others, and
predicted lower identity clarity. In the following sections, we thus preserve pre-existing self-conceptions (Berzonsky
start by introducing the direct paths between social media 2011; Berzonsky et al. 2013). Given the normative style is
social comparison and identity processing styles to reveal the associated with a foreclosed identity (Berzonsky 2011),
differences between the two types of social comparison. future scholars should explore whether online social com-
Then we comment on the indirect paths, followed by dis- parison of ability also contributes to rigid self-constructs
cussions of implications and limitations. through this style. Interestingly, despite its concurrent
relationship with social comparison of ability, the normative
Social Media Social Comparison and Identity style predicted higher engagement in social comparison of
Processing Styles opinion on social media. People adopting the normative
style place emphasis on the norms endorsed by the referent
Social media social comparison of ability was associated groups (Berzonsky 2011; Berzonsky et al. 2013), and social
with higher concurrent diffuse-avoidant style; the style then comparison of opinion facilitates acquirement of this
predicted higher social comparison of ability. On the other knowledge (Suls et al. 2000). Today, young people rely on
hand, social media social comparison of opinion was social media to enact and perceive peer norms (Sherman
associated with higher concurrent information style; the et al. 2016). For instance, teenage girls’ definition of beauty
style then predicted higher social comparison of opinion. is largely derived from observation of peers’ Instagram
Although neither indirect effect was significant, the direct pictures and feedback to the pictures (Chua and Chang
paths clearly revealed the differentiated implications of the 2016), and emerging adults observe their peers on social
two types of social comparison. Given the informational media to keep up with trends (Manago et al. 2008). At the
style is characterized by proactive search and analysis of transition to college, freshmen use social media to famil-
information that could inform one’s sense of self (Ber- iarize themselves with the conventions in the college
zonsky 2011; Berzonsky et al. 2013), it should not be sur- community; proper use of social media per se becomes part
prising that the adoption of this style would associate with of their identity work (Thomas et al. 2017). Normative-style
the type of social comparison involving learning about freshmen’s need for and attention to norms may have
others’ thoughts, values, and beliefs (Festinger 1954; Suls prompted them to engage in social comparison of opinion
2122 Journal of Youth and Adolescence (2018) 47:2114–2128

Table 2 Results of path analysis on social media, asking about or observing what life styles,
β S.E. p behaviors, and even ideologies would be well accepted by
the new group.
Direct paths of interest
It is noteworthy although both the informational and
T1 SMSC-Ab → T1 informational .10 .08 .207
normative styles predicted more social comparison of opi-
T1 SMSC-Ab → T1 normative .20* .08 .010
nion, the specific process may not be identical for the two
T1 SMSC-Ab → T1 diffuse-avoidant .17* .07 .010
styles. When engaging in the informational style, indivi-
T1 SMSC-Op → T1 informational .14* .07 .032
duals are eager to gain new self-knowledge and welcome
T1 SMSC-Op → T1 normative .02 .07 .781
information that challenges their self-schemas; when per-
T1 SMSC-Op → T1 diffuse-avoidant .11 .06 .081
T1 self-esteem → T1 informational .29*** .08 <.001
forming the normative style, individuals prefer information
T1 self-esteem → T1 normative −.02 .08 .767
that confirms their self-conceptions (Berzonsky 2011; Ber-
T1 self-esteem → T1 diffuse-avoidant −.10 .07 .155
zonsky et al. 2013). It suggests the two styles may urge
T1 identity clarity → T1 informational .06 .08 .479
people to seek different kinds of information on social
T1 identity clarity → T1 normative .13 .08 .097 media in the process of social comparison of opinion.
T1 identity clarity → T1 diffuse-avoidant −.41*** .06 <.001 Clarifying what information people seek in this process in
T1 informational → T2 SMSC-Ab .08 .07 .285 relation to the adopted identity processing style will further
T1 informational → T2 SMSC-Op .16* .08 .040 expand this research area.
T1 informational → T2 self-esteem .13 .07 .053
T1 informational → T2 identity clarity .01 .06 .914 Social Media Social Comparison Predicting Identity
T1 normative → T2 SMSC-Ab .05 .08 .514 Outcomes Through Identity Processing Styles
T1 normative → T2 SMSC-Op .14* .07 .034
T1 normative → T2 self-esteem −.08 .07 .241 Social comparison of ability on social media predicted
T1 normative → T2 identity clarity .03 .07 .615 lower identity clarity through its association with higher
T1 diffuse-avoidant → T2 SMSC-Ab .15* .07 .043 concurrent diffuse-avoidant style. It appeared that college
T1 diffuse-avoidant → T2 SMSC-Op .05 .07 .496 freshmen who compared their ability with others’ on social
T1 diffuse-avoidant → T2 self-esteem −.20** .07 .004 media were more likely to adopt the diffuse-avoidant style.
T1 diffuse-avoidant → T2 identity clarity −.32*** .07 <.001 According to the identity processing style model (Ber-
Indirect paths of interest zonsky 2011), individuals who subscribe to this style are
T1 SMSC-Ab → T1 diffuse-avoidant → T2 −.06* .03 .039 passive self-theorists. They avoid actively processing
identity clarity identity-related information and allow situational demands
T1 SMSC-Ab → T1 diffuse-avoidant → T2 −.03 .02 .064 (e.g., popularity, following the trend) to dictate their sense
self-esteem
of self. It is possible that the judgmental form of online
T1 identity clarity → T1 diffuse-avoidant →T2 −.06 .03 .056
SMSC-Ab social comparison provides rich contextual cues that pro-
T1 self-esteem → T1 informational → T2 .05 .03 .080 mote this approach to self-theorizing. Specifically, the
SMSC-Op negative psycho-emotional reactions coming along with
Significant controlled paths such social comparison (Feinstein et al. 2013; Lee 2014;
T1 SMSC-Ab → T2 SMSC-Ab .46*** .08 <.001 Lim and Yang 2015) may prompt individuals to adopt the
T1 SMSC-Op → T2 SMSC-Op .56*** .06 <.001 diffuse-avoidant style as a way to shelter themselves from
T1 self-esteem → T2 self-esteem .55*** .07 <.001 the sense of inferiority. When people approach their self in
T1 identity clarity → T2 identity clarity .43*** .07 <.001 the diffuse-avoidant manner, however, they tend to have a
Race/Ethnicity: black → T1 normative .29*** .07 <.001 fragmented identity, and their self-worth is more fragile and
Race/Ethnicity: other → T1 normative .22** .07 .002 less stable (Berzonsky 2011; Soenens et al. 2016). The
T1 identity clarity → T1 diffuse-avoidant → .08** .03 .009 diffuse-avoidant style is also related to defense maneuvers
T2 self-esteem
such as turning against oneself (Berzonsky and Kinney
T1 identity clarity → T1 diffuse-avoidant → .13*** .03 <.001
T2 identity clarity 2008), which could also do harm to one’s sense of self.
Although the indirect path from social comparison of ability
The reference group of race/ethnicity is White. “Race/Ethnicity: Other”
includes participants who identified as neither mono-racially/ethnically to prospective self-esteem via the diffuse-avoidant style was
White nor mono-racially/ethnically Black. Non-significant indirect only approaching significance (p = .064), the diffuse-
paths were excluded, except for the paths of interest whose p values avoidant style did predict lower self-esteem. Overall the
were below .10 paths identified in our model supported the profile of people
SMSC-Ab = social media social comparison of ability, SMSC-Op = adopting the diffuse-avoidant style.
social media social comparison of opinion
Whereas the indirect path from Time 1 online social
*p < .05; **p < .01; ***p < .001
comparison of ability to Time 2 identity clarity through the
Journal of Youth and Adolescence (2018) 47:2114–2128 2123

diffuse-avoidant style was significant, the opposite (Time 1 outcome. The relative significance of the diffuse-avoidant
identity clarity to the diffuse-avoidant style to Time 2 online style echoes research findings on the relationships between
social comparison of ability) was not. The p value of the the different styles and identity variables: the diffuse-
latter (.056), however, was close to the significance avoidant style has a robust association with low self-esteem
threshold. It suggests the possibility of a bi-directional and identity commitment (Crocetti et al. 2009; Luychx et al.
association: youth who perform more online social com- 2007; Passmore et al. 2005; Soenens et al. 2011, 2016),
parison of ability use the diffuse-avoidant style more often, whereas the normative style tends to produce mixed find-
which predicts lower identity clarity; feeling confused about ings, with some research reporting its positive role in
who they are, these youth are more likely to approach their identity development (Crocetti et al. 2009; Luychx et al.
sense of self in a diffuse-avoidant fashion, and the style in 2007; Passmore et al. 2005; Soenens et al. 2011, 2016) and
turn may predict further online social comparison of ability. some revealing a null effect (Crocetti et al. 2009; Eryigit
Because the diffuse-avoidant style is associated with efforts and Kerpelman 2009; Soenens et al. 2016). Although the
of impression management and sensitivity to evaluation informational style is usually related to higher self-esteem
(i.e., having high contingent self-esteem; Berzonsky 2011; and identity clarity (Crocetti et al. 2009; Eryigit and Ker-
Soenens et al. 2016), the rich cues for social comparison of pelman 2009; Luychx et al. 2007; Soenens et al. 2011,
ability on social media may be appealing to diffuse-avoidant 2016), some research presents non-significant results
individuals. These people may try to fathom how to present (Luychx et al. 2007; Passmore et al. 2005; Soenens et al.
a polished image that would win them immediate rewards 2016). One study even showed that the non-significant
(Berzonsky 2011) by comparing how well they are doing association was preceded by decreases in short-term self-
with others through online channels. Eventually, it may concept clarity (Johnson and Nozick 2011). These findings
become a vicious cycle that hinders, rather than benefits, suggest that the positive effect of the informational pro-
youth’s identity clarity. More research is needed to examine cessing style, which involves a high level of exploration and
whether the bi-directional associations are existent and self-reflection, may take more time to emerge. In the future,
robust. longitudinal research adopting a longer time span should
In contrast, social comparison of opinion on social media revisit this association.
did not predict the identity outcomes through identity pro-
cessing styles, although the positive concurrent correlation Implications
between social comparison of opinion and the diffuse-
avoidant style was approaching significance (p = .081). It Our findings have important theoretical implications. In the
suggests the possibility that social comparison of opinion, Introduction, we identified several limitations of existing
despite its less judgmental nature, can still overwhelm literature, and results of this study provide preliminary
college freshmen and thus activate the maladaptive identity information to fill these gaps. First, the findings present the
processing style. However, readers should not conclude that necessity of studying both types of online social compar-
the two types of social comparison had similar effects on ison. As most research still focuses on measuring or
identity development and did not need to be differentiated. manipulating social media users’ perception of whether
Results of factor analysis clearly showed the scale had a others are doing better or worse (e.g., de Vries and Kühne
two-factor structure (see Appendix), and, different from 2015; Feinstein et al. 2013; Vogel et al. 2014), the almost
social comparison of ability, none of the indirect paths exclusive emphasis on the judgmental form of social com-
involving social comparison of opinion was significant. parison obscures the full picture of this construct. It easily
Their diverse direct relationships with identity processing leads readers to assume that social comparison is detri-
styles summarized in earlier paragraphs also revealed how mental. As shown in this study, however, social comparison
the two types of social comparison could associate with the of opinion did not predict low global self-esteem and
same constructs in different ways, as suggested in other identity clarity. In fact, this type of comparison has been
research (Park and Baek 2018). All these findings call for found to associate with less negative emotion and thus
the distinction of one type of social comparison from the better psychological well-being (Park and Baek 2018).
other. Essentially, social comparison of ability and that of opinion
Findings of the indirect paths suggest the diffuse- are related but different processes (Suls et al. 2002), aiming
avoidant style was the most important mechanism at answering different questions about the self as a social
explaining how social comparison on social media may being. More research is needed to unravel how social
contribute to emerging adults’ identity development, parti- comparison of opinion is associated with behavioral, social,
cularly identity clarity. Whereas the diffuse-avoidant style and psychological variables. For instance, given its non-
linked several paths between the Time 1 and Time 2 vari- competitive and communicative nature (Festinger 1954),
ables, the other two styles failed to predict either identity social comparison of opinion may relate to better social
2124 Journal of Youth and Adolescence (2018) 47:2114–2128

outcomes, such as higher social capital or social con- these unpleasant psycho-emotional reactions resulting from
nectedness. To recognize these new research directions, it is online social comparison of ability, they should still avoid
crucial to start challenging the assumption that social retreating to the diffuse-avoidant style. Instead, they should
comparison is unidimensional and is inherently harmful. try to adopt the informational style, actively organizing and
Second, literature on social comparison has largely making sense of the information gleaned on social media,
overlooked its implications for identity development. While with the understanding that other users’ self-presentation
previous research has revealed the negative relationship only reflects a partial image. Although the information style
between online social comparison (mostly the judgmental did not predict positive identity outcomes in our model,
form) and self-esteem or self-perceptions (de Vries and theoretically it is still the most adaptive approach to identity
Kühne 2015; Vogel et al. 2014), our study is the first one development (Berzonsky 1990, 2011). Finally, the direct
illuminating the longitudinal relationship between online path between the diffuse-avoidant style and prospective
social comparison and identity clarity. As suggested in a online social comparison of ability suggests youth who
previous section, future research should explore whether approach identity issues in a maladaptive manner may in
lower identity clarity and social comparison of ability form turn rely on social media cues to develop a situational
a vicious feedback loop. identity, but this strategy can lead to even more fragmented
Third, our study identified one process through which and less coherent sense of self. Thus, emerging adults who
online social comparison of ability contributed to lower constantly avoid processing identity-related information at
identity clarity. We proposed identity processing styles as the college transition may need more attention and
possible mediators because this model captures the process assistance.
of identity exploration (Berzonsky 1990) and may link
social stimuli (e.g., social comparison) with identity out- Limitations
comes. The central role of the diffuse-avoidant style
revealed through this study suggests the possibility of this A few limitations of the study should be noted. First, our
style being a mechanism between other social media sample was not particularly large, given the number of paths
activities and identity outcomes. For instance, given that examined. In the future, larger samples should be adopted to
diffuse-avoidant individuals are more likely to prioritize revisit the paths approaching significance. The second
receiving immediate rewards over developing a stable self- limitation concerns the contexts of online social compar-
theory and to effortfully manage their self-images (Ber- ison. Research on social media and technology-mediated
zonsky 2011), the style could also be a mediator between communication can take two different approaches. The first
online self-presentation and youth’s identity development. one focuses on a specific social media platform, such as
This is another direction for future research. Facebook or Instagram (e.g., Park and Baek 2018; Vogel
At the practical level, professionals working with emer- et al. 2014; Weinstein 2017). It allows scholars to interpret
ging adults may consider informing youth of the identity the findings in light of the affordances provided by the
implications of online social comparison of ability. Emer- given platform. The second approach focuses on informa-
ging adults are among the most avid social media users tion of social media in general without making distinctions
(Greenwood et al. 2016). Given the positive correlation among platforms (e.g., Nesi et al. 2017; Pea et al. 2012;
between intensity of social media use and online social Yang et al. 2017). Findings from the latter approach are
comparison (Lee 2014), and given college freshmen’s needs interpreted by considering characteristics shared by most
to relocate themselves and learn about their positions rela- social media channels, such as easy access to large audi-
tive to others’ in the new environment, they may be parti- ences and their positive self-presentation. These results are
cularly vulnerable to the negative impact of social less vulnerable to the rapid change of the social media
comparison of ability on social media. One research finding landscape and are less likely to be dismissed for the
showed that bringing to adolescents’ attention the selec- declining popularity of a single platform. Our study took the
tively positive nature of social media self-presentation second, generalist avenue. Despite the strengths mentioned
mitigated the relationship between negative social compar- above, the lack of platform-specific information remains a
ison and low affective well-being (Weinstein 2017). It may limitation of the study. Instagram, for instance, is more
be helpful to also develop this awareness among emerging image-oriented than Facebook and Twitter. If “a picture is
adults and examine whether the awareness would reduce worth than a thousand words,” then the Instagram pictures
comparison-triggered emotions such as envy and shame showcasing vacations, achievements, and popularity may
(Lim and Yang 2015) and poor self-perceptions (de Vries activate stronger effects of social comparison of ability than
and Kühne 2015; Vogel et al. 2014). In addition, emerging other platforms. On the other hand, because opinion
adults should be reminded that even when they experience exchange relies more heavily on written or verbal
Journal of Youth and Adolescence (2018) 47:2114–2128 2125

communication, the text-based platforms may generate and negotiation (Chow and Healey 2008; Scanlon et al.
stronger effects of social comparison of opinion. These 2007; Thomas et al. 2017). Given the amount of time
hypotheses await future research to examine. emerging adults spend on social media and the rich
Finally, as the first study investigating the identity information available on these platforms for social
implications of social media social comparison, this project comparison, it is crucial to illuminate the identity implica-
focuses on a linear model with only three sets of focal tions of this practice. The differential patterns of how the
variables, but additional variables and alternative designs two types of social media social comparison associated with
should be considered in the future. For instance, factors identity processing styles and identity clarity revealed
predisposing individuals to social comparison could be exciting directions for future research and provided insights
investigated. Individuals who have a strong orientation for into guiding emerging adults to use social media in an
social comparison in general tend to be more self-conscious, adaptive manner. We believe more theory-driven research
less certain and more negative about the self, but also more as this one that attends to nuanced differences between/
social (Buunk and Gibbons 2006). Clarifying which char- among different dimensions of a given construct would
acteristics are more salient in which type of social com- allow us to further our understanding of youth development
parison would advance this line of research. Further, we in the digital age.
chose global self-esteem and identity clarity as our identity
Authors’ Contributions C-cY conceptualized the study, conceived of
outcomes because they reflect states of established identity the design, collected data, performed the statistical analyses, inter-
and have important psychosocial implications (Campbell preted the results, and composed the manuscript; SMH participated in
et al. 1996; Heppner and Kernis 2011; Luychx et al. 2007; developing hypotheses, interpreting results, and drafting the manu-
Trzesniewski et al. 2013). On the other hand, constructs script; MDKC participated in recruiting participants and drafting the
manuscript. All authors read and approved the final manuscript.
reflecting fluctuations or issues of identity development,
such as barometric self-esteem (Rosenberg 1986) and fra- Data Sharing Declaration The datasets generated and/or analyzed
gile self-esteem (Heppner and Kernis 2011) can also be during the current study are not publicly available but are available
informative and should be included in future research. from the corresponding author on reasonable request.
Lastly, although we intentionally chose college freshmen as
our target sample because the 1st year in college is a major Compliance with Ethical Standards
developmental transition and a prime time for online iden- Conflict of Interest The authors declare that they have no conflict of
tity work (Thomas et al. 2017), longitudinal research interest.
starting at early adolescence will further illuminate how
young people’s early experience contributes to later social Ethical Approval The Institutional Review Board at the University of
Memphis approved this research. All procedures performed in studies
comparison and identity formation. Data collection invol- involving human participants were in accordance with the ethical
ving more time points spanning across a longer period will standards of the institutional and/or national research committee and
allow for growth curve modeling and depiction of the with the 1964 Helsinki declaration and its later amendments or com-
dynamic processes of identity development. parable ethical standards.

Informed Consent Informed consent was obtained from all individual


participants included in the study.
Conclusion

The current study pioneers in exploring identity implica-


tions of two types of social media social comparison. Appendix
Despite the wide-spread assumption that social comparison
is detrimental to one’s well-being, our study showed that Social Media Social Comparison Scale
only the judgmental form was concerning. Specifically, Most people compare themselves from time to time with
comparing oneself against others on social media as a others. For example, they may compare the way they feel,
means to assess who was doing better was related to their opinions, their abilities, and/or their situation with
avoiding or delaying processing identity-related informa- those of other people. There is nothing particularly “good”
tion, which contributed to lower identity clarity. On the or “bad” about such comparison, and some people do it
other hand, social comparison of opinion on social media more than others. In this set of questions, we would like to
did not predict low identity clarity and global self-esteem find out the extent to which you compare yourself with
via the identity processing styles. The transition into college other people when using social media. How well do these
is a prime time for identity development, (re)construction, statements apply to you?
2126 Journal of Youth and Adolescence (2018) 47:2114–2128

References
T1 factor T2 factor
loading loading Arnett, J. J. (2014). Emerging adulthood: The winding road from the
late teens through the twenties. 2nd ed New York, NY: Oxford
Social media social comparison of ability University Press.
1. When using social media, I .70 .69 Berzonsky, M. D. (1988). Self-theorists, identity status, and social
cognition. In D. K. Lapsley & F. C. Power (Eds.), Self, ego, and
compare how my loved ones identity: Integrative approaches (pp. 243–262). New York, NY:
(romantic partner, family mem- Springer.
bers, etc.) are doing with how Berzonsky, M. D. (1990). Self-construction over the lifespan: A pro-
others are doing cess perspective on identity formation. In G. J. Neimeyer & R. A.
Neimeyer (Eds.), Advances in personal construct psychology,
2. When using social media, I .80 .86 Vol. 1 (pp. 155–186). Greenwich, CT: JAI Press.
compare how I do things with Berzonsky, M. D. (1993). A constructivist view of identity develop-
how others do things ment: People as post-positivist self-theorists. In J. Kroger (Ed.),
Discussions on ego identity (pp. 169–183). Hillsdale, NJ:
3. On social media, I compare what .85 .86 Erlbaum.
I have done with others as a way Berzonsky, M. D. (1999). Identity styles and hypothesis-testing stra-
tegies. The Journal of Social Psychology, 139(6), 784–789.
to find out how well I have done
https://doi.org/10.1080/00224549909598257.
something Berzonsky, M. D. (2003). Identity style and well-being: Does com-
4. On social media, I compare how .82 .78 mitment matter? Identity: An International Journal of Theory and
Research, 3(2), 131–142. https://doi.org/10.1207/
I am doing socially with other
s1532706xid030203.
people Berzonsky, M. D. (2011). A social-cognitive perspective on identity
5. I don’t really use social media to .43 .43 constuction. In S. J. Schwartz, K. Luychx & V. L. Vignoles
(Eds.), Handbook of identity theory and research. New York,
compare with others to see how NY: Sringer.
well I’m doing (reversely coded) Berzonsky, M. D., & Adams, G. R. (1999). Reevaluating the identity
Social media social comparison of status paradigm: Still useful after 35 years. Developmental
Review, 19(4), 557–590. https://doi.org/10.1006/drev.1999.0495.
opinion Berzonsky, M. D., & Barclay, C. R. (1981). Formal reasoning and
6. On social media, I talk with .72 .60 identity formation: A reconceptualization. In J. A. Meacham & N.
others about mutual opinions R. Santilli (Eds.), Social development in youth: Structure and
content (pp. 64–87). Basel: Karger.
and experiences Berzonsky, M., Cieciuch, J., Duriez, B., & Soenens, B. (2011). The
7. On social media, I try to find out .88 .83 how and what of identity formation: Associations between
identity styles and value orientations. Personality and Individual
what others think who face
Differences, 50, 295–299. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2010.10.
similar problems as I face 007.
8. On social media, I try to know .82 .90 Berzonsky, M. D., & Kinney, A. (2008). Identity processing style and
defense mechanisms. Polish Psychological Bulletin, 39(3),
what others in a similar situation
111–117. https://doi.org/10.2478/v10059-008-0022-7.
would do Berzonsky, M. D., Soenens, B., Luyckx, K., Smits, I., Papini, D. R., &
9. When using social media, I try to .75 .79 Goossens, L. (2013). Development and validation of the revised
identity style inventory (ISI-5): Factor structure, reliability, and
find out what others think about validity. Psychological Assessment, 25(3), 893–904. https://doi.
something that I want to learn org/10.1037/a0032642.
more about Browne, M. W., & Cudeck, R. (1993). Alternative ways of assessing
model fit. In K. A. Bollen & J. S. Long (Eds.), Testing structural
χ2 53.23** 51.15** equation models (pp. 136–162). Newbury Park, CA: Sage.
RMSEA .07 .08 Buunk, A. P., & Gibbons, F. X. (2006). Social comparison orientation:
A new perspective on those who do and those who don’t compare
CFI .96 .95 with others. In S. Guimond (Ed.), Social comparison and social
TLI .94 .93 psychology (pp. 15–32). New York, NY: Cambridge University
Press.
These are results of confirmatory factor analysis obtained from Mplus, Campbell, J. D., Trapnell, P. D., Heine, S. J., Katz, I. M., Lavallee, L.
with maximum likelihood robust being the estimator F., & Lehman, D. R. (1996). Self-concept clarity: Measurement,
**p < .01 personality correlates, and cultural boundaries. Journal of Per-
sonality and Social Psychology, 70(1), 141–156. https://doi.org/
10.1037/0022-3514.70.1.141.
Chow, K., & Healey, M. (2008). Place attachment and place identity:
First-year undergraduates making the transition from home to
university. Journal of Environmental Psychology, 28(4),
362–372. https://doi.org/10.1016/l.jenvp.2008.02.011.
Journal of Youth and Adolescence (2018) 47:2114–2128 2127

Chua, T. H. H., & Chang, L. (2016). Full length article: Follow me and Heppner, W. L., & Kernis, M. H. (2011). High self-esteem: Multiple
like my beautiful selfies: Singapore teenage girls engagement in forms and their outcomes. In S. J. Schwartz, K. Luyckx & V. L.
self-presentation and peer comparison on social media. Compu- Vignoles (Eds.), Handbook of identity theory and research (Vols.
ters in Human Behavior, 55, 190–197. https://doi.org/10.1016/j. 1 and 2), (pp. 329–355). New York, NY: Springer. https://doi.
chb.2015.09.011. org/10.1007/978-1-4419-7988-9_15.
Chung, J. M., Robins, R. W., Trzesniewski, K. H., Noftle, E. E., Hu, L., & Bentler, P. M. (1999). Cutoff criteria for fit indexes in
Roberts, B. W., & Widaman, K. F. (2014). Continuity and change covariance structure analysis: Conventional criteria versus new
in self-esteem during emerging adulthood. Journal of Personality alternatives. Structural Equation Modeling, 6(1), 1–55. https://
and Social Psychology, 106(3), 469–483. https://doi.org/10. doi.org/10.1080/10705519909540118.
1037/a0035135. Jang, K., Park, N., & Song, H. (2016). Social comparison on Face-
Cramer, E. M., Song, H., & Drent, A. M. (2016). Social comparison book: Its antecedents and psychological outcomes. Computers in
on Facebook: Motivation, affective consequences, self-esteem, Human Behavior, 62, 147–154. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.
and Facebook fatigue. Computers in Human Behavior, 64, 2016.03.082.
739–746. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2016.07.049s. Johnson, E. A., & Nozick, K. J. (2011). Personality, adjustment, and
Crocetti, E., Rubini, M., Berzonsky, M. D., & Meeus, W. (2009). Brief identity style influences on stability in identity and self-concept
report: The identity style inventory—validation in Italian ado- during the transition to university. Identity: An International
lescents and college students. Journal of Adolescence, 32, Journal of Theory and Research, 11(1), 25–46. https://doi.org/10.
425–433. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.adolescence.2008.04.002. 1080/15283488.2011.540737.
Crocetti, E., Rubini, M., Branje, S., Koot, H. M., & Meeus, W. (2016). Kroger, J., Martinussen, M., & Marcia, J. E. (2010). Identity status
Self-concept clarity in adolescents and parents: A six-wave change during adolescence and young adulthood: A meta-
longitudinal and multi-informant study on development and analysis. Journal of Adolescence, 33(5), 683–698. https://doi.
intergenerational transmission. Journal of Personality, 84(5), org/10.1016/j.adolescence.2009.11.002.
580–593. https://doi.org/10.1111/jopy.12181. Lee, S. (2014). How do people compare themselves with others on
de Vries, D. A., & Kühne, R. (2015). Facebook and self-perception: social network sites? The case of Facebook. Computers in Human
Individual susceptibility to negative social comparison on Face- Behavior, 32, 253–260. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2013.12.
book. Personality and Individual Differences, 85, 217–221. 009.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2015.05.029. Lenhart, A. (2015). Teens, social media and technology overview
Duriez, B., Luyckx, K., Soenens, B., & Berzonsky, M. (2012). A 2015. Pew Research Center. http://www.pewinternet.org/2015/
process-content approach to adolescent identity formation: 04/09/teens-social-media-technology-2015/.
Examining longitudinal associations between identity styles and Lim, M., & Yang, Y. (2015). Effects of users’ envy and shame on
goal pursuits. Journal of Personality, 80, 135–161. https://doi. social comparison that occurs on social network services. Com-
org/10.1111/j.1467-6494.2011.00729.x. puters in Human Behavior, 51, 300–311. https://doi.org/10.1016/
Erikson, E. (1968). Identity: Youth and crisis. Oxford: Norton & Co. j.chb.2015.05.013.
Erol, R. Y., & Orth, U. (2011). Self-esteem development from age 14 Little, R. J. A. (1988). A test of missing completely at random for
to 30 years: A longitudinal study. Journal of Personality and multivariate data with missing values. Journal of the American
Social Psychology, 101(3), 607–619. https://doi.org/10.1037/a Statistical Association, 83(404), 1198–1202. https://doi.org/10.
0024299. 1080/01621459.1988.10478722.
Eryigit, S., & Kerpelman, J. (2009). Using the identity processing style Luychx, K., Soenens, B., Berzonsky, M. D., Smits, I., Goossens, L., &
Q-sort to examine identity styles of Turkish young adults. Jour- Vansteenkiste, M. (2007). Information-oriented identity proces-
nal of Adolescence, 32(5), 1137–1158. https://doi.org/10.1016/j. sing, identity consolidation, and well-being: The moderating role
adolescence.2009.02.001. of autonomy, self-reflections, and self-rumination. Personality
Feinstein, B. A., Hershenberg, R., Bhatia, V., Latach, J. A., Meuwly, and Individual Differences, 43, 1099–1111. https://doi.org/10.
N., & Davila, J. (2013). Negative social comparison on Facebook 1016/j.paid.2007.03.003.
and depressive symptoms: Rumination as a mechanism. Psy- Manago, A. M., Graham, M. B., Greenfield, P. M., & Salimkhan, G.
chology of Popular Media Culture, 2(3), 121–170. https://doi. (2008). Self-presentation and gender on MySpace. Journal of
org/10.1037/a0033111. Applied Developmental Psychology, 29(6), 446–458. https://doi.
Festinger, L. (1954). A theory of social comparison processes. Human org/10.1016/j.appdev.2008.07.001.
Relations, 7, 117–140. https://doi.org/10.1177/ Marcia, J. E. (1966). Development and validation of ego identity
001872675400700202. status. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 3, 551–558.
Galambos, N. L., Barker, E. T., & Krahn, H. J. (2006). Depression, Markstrom, C. A., & Kalmanir, H. M. (2001). Linkages between the
self-esteem, and anger in emerging adulthood: Seven-year tra- psychosocial stages of identity and intimacy and the ego strengths
jectories. Developmental Psychology, 42(2), 350–365. https://doi. of fidelity and love. Identity: An International Journal of Theory
org/10.1037/0012-1649.42.2.350. and Research, 1(2), 179–196. https://doi.org/10.1207/
Gardner, H., & Davis, K. (2013). The app generation: How today’s S1532706XID0102_05.
youth navigate identity, intimacy, and imagination in a digital Nesi, J., Widman, L., Choukas-Bradley, S., & Prinstein, M. J. (2017).
world. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press. Technology-based communication and the development of
Gibbons, F. X., & Buunk, B. P. (1999). Individual differences in social interpersonal competencies within adolescent romantic relation-
comparison: Development of a scale of social comparison ships: A preliminary investigation. Journal of Research on
orientation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 76(1), Adolescence, 27(2), 471–477. https://doi.org/10.1111/jora.12274.
129–142. Orth, U., Maes, J., & Schmitt, M. (2015). Self-esteem development
Greenwood, S., Perrin, A., & Duggan, M. (2016). Social media update across the life span: A longitudinal study with a large sample
2016. Pew Research Center. http://www.pewinternet.org/2016/ from Germany. Developmental Psychology, 51(2), 248–259.
11/11/social-media-update-2016/. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0038481.
Harter, S. (2012). The construction of the self: Developmental and Park, S. Y., & Baek, Y. M. (2018). Two faces of social comparison on
sociocultural perspectives. New York, NY: Guilford Press. Facebook: The interplay between social comparison orientation,
2128 Journal of Youth and Adolescence (2018) 47:2114–2128

emotions, and psychological well-being. Computers in Human Suls, J., Martin, R., & Wheeler, L. (2000). Three kinds of opinion
Behavior, 79, 83–93. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2017.10.028. comparison: The triadic model. Personality & Social Psychology
Passmore, N. L., Fogarty, G. J., Bourke, C. J., & Baker-Evan, S. F. Review (Lawrence Erlbaum Associates), 4(3), 219–237.
(2005). Parental bonding and identity style as correlates of self- Suls, J., Martin, R., & Wheeler, L. (2002). Social comparison: Why,
esteem among adult adoptees and nonadoptees. Family Relations, with whom, and with what effect? Current Directions in Psy-
54, 523–534. chological Science, 11(5), 159–163.
Pea, R., Nass, C., Meheula, L., Rance, M., Kumar, A., Bamford, H., & Thomas, L., Briggs, P., Hart, A., & Kerrigan, F. (2017). Under-
Zhou, M. (2012). Media use, face-to-face communication, media standing social media and identity work in young people transi-
multitasking, and social well-being among 8- to 12-year-old girls. tioning to university. Computers in Human Behavior, 76,
Developmental Psychology, 48(2), 327–336. https://doi.org/10. 541–553. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2017.08.021.
1037/a0027030. Trzesniewski, K. H., Donnellan, M. B., & Robins, R. W. (2013).
Phillips, T. M., & Pittman, J. F. (2007). Adolescent psychological Development of self-esteem. In V. Zeigler-Hill (Ed.), Self-esteem
well-being by identity style. Journal of Adolescence, 30, (pp. 60–79). New York, NY: Psychology Press.
1021–1034. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.adolescence.2007.03.002. Vignoles, V. L., Schwartz, S. J., & Luyckx, K. (2011). Introduction:
Richardson, C. G., Ratner, P. A., & Zumbo, B. D. (2009). Further Toward and integrative view of identity. In S. J. Schwartz, K.
support for multidimensionality within the Rosenberg self-esteem Luychx & V. L. Vignoles (Eds.), Handbook of identity theory and
scale. Current Psychology: A Journal for Diverse Perspectives on research. New York, NY: Sringer.
Diverse Psychological Issues, 28(2), 98–114. https://doi.org/10. Vogel, E. A., Rose, J. P., Roberts, L. R., & Eckles, K. (2014). Social
1007/s12144-009-9052-3. comparison, social media, and self-esteem. Psychology of Pop-
Rosenberg, M. (1986). Self-concept from middle childhood through ular Media Culture, 3(4), 206–222. https://doi.org/10.1037/
adolescence. In J. Suls & A. G. Greenwald (Eds.), Psychological ppm0000047.
perspectives on the self, Vol. 3, (pp. 107–135). Hillsdale, NJ: Weinstein, E. (2017). Adolescents’ differential responses to social
Erlbaum. media browsing: Exploring causes and consequences of inter-
Rosenberg, M., Schooler, C., & Schoenbach, C. (1989). Self-esteem vention. Computers in Human Behavior, 76, 396–405. https://doi.
and adolescent problems: Modeling reciprocal effects. American org/10.1016/j.chb.2017.07.038.
Sociological Review, 54(6), 1004–1018. https://doi.org/10.2307/ Wojciszke, B., Baryla, W., Parzuchowski, M., Szymkow, A., & Abele,
2095720. A. E. (2011). Self-esteem is dominated by agentic over com-
Rosenberg, M., Schooler, C., Schoenbach, C., & Rosenberg, F. (1995). munal information. European Journal of Social Psychology, 41
Global self-esteem and specific self-esteem: Different concepts, (5), 617–627. https://doi.org/10.1002/ejsp.791.
different outcomes. American Sociological Review, 60(1), 141–156. Yang, C.-c, & Brown, B. B. (2016). Online self-presentation on
Rosenthal, D. A., Gurney, R. M., & Moore, S. M. (1981). From trust Facebook and self development during the college transition.
on intimacy: A new inventory for examining Erikson’s stages of Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 45(2), 402–416. https://doi.
psychosocial development. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 10 org/10.1007/s10964-015-0385-y.
(6), 525–537. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF02087944. Yang, C.-c., Holden, S. M., & Carter, M. D. K. (2017). Emerging
Scanlon, L., Rowling, L., & Weber, Z. (2007). ‘You don’t have like an adults’ social media self-presentation and identity development at
identity…you are just lost in a crowd’: Forming a student identity college transition: Mindfulness as a moderator. Journal of
in the first-year transition to university. Journal of Youth Studies, Applied Developmental Psychology, 52, 212–221. https://doi.org/
10(2), 223–241. https://doi.org/10.1080/13676260600983684. 10.1016/j.appdev.2017.08.006.
Schwartz, S. J., Zamboanga, B. L., Wang, W., & Olthuis, J. V. (2009). Zhao, S., Grasmuck, S., & Martin, J. (2008). Identity construction on
Measuring identity from an Eriksonian perspective: Two sides of Facebook: Digital empowerment in anchored relationships.
the same coin? Journal of Personality Assessment, 91(2), Computers in Human Behavior, 24(5), 1816–1836. https://doi.
143–154. https://doi.org/10.1080/00223890802634266. org/10.1016/j.chb.2008.02.012.
Sherman, L. E., Payton, A. A., Hernandez, L. M., Greenfield, P. M., &
Dapretto, M. (2016). The power of the like in adolescence: Chia-chen Yang is an Assistant Professor in the Department of
Effects of peer influence on neural and behavioral responses and Counseling, Educational Psychology and Research at the University of
social media. Psychological Science, 27(7), 1027–1035. https://
Memphis. She received her doctorate in Educational Psychology from
doi.org/10.1177/0956797616645673.
the University of Wisconsin-Madison. Her research interests focus on
Soenens, B., Berzonsky, M. D., Dunkel, C. S., Papini, D. R., &
adolescents’ and emerging adults’ use of communication technologies
Vansteenkiste, M. (2011). Are all identity commitments created
equally? The importance of motives for commitment for late and its association with young people’s social development, sense of
adolescents’ personal adjustment. International Journal of self, and psychological well-being.
Behavioral Development, 35(4), 358–369. https://doi.org/10.
1177/01650254111405954. Sean M. Holden is a doctoral candidate in the Department of
Soenens, B., Berzonsky, M. D., & Papini, D. R. (2016). Attending to Counseling, Educational Psychology and Research at the University of
the role of identity exploration in self-esteem: Longitudinal Memphis and full-time Research Associate at the Center for Research
associations between identity styles and two features of self- in Educational Policy. His primary research interests revolve around
esteem. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 40(5), supportive factors in adolescent and emerging adult motivation and
420–430. https://doi.org/10.1177/0165025415602560. identity processes, particularly the role of mindfulness in promoting
Soenens, B., Duriez, B., & Goossens, L. (2005). Social-psychological adaptive development and achievement of personally satisfying goals.
profiles of identity styles: Attitudinal and social-cognitive corre-
lates in late adolescence. Journal of Adolescence, 28, 107–125.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.adolescence.2004.07.001. Mollie D. K. Carter is a doctoral student in the Department of
Stapleton, P., Luiz, G., & Chatwin, H. (2017). Generation validation: Counseling, Educational Psychology and Research at the University of
The role of social comparison in use of instagram among emer- Memphis. Her research interests involve emerging adults’ social media
ging adults. Cyberpsychology, Behavior, and Social Networking. use, self-efficacy beliefs, and spiritual development during the college
https://doi.org/10.1089/cyber.2016.0444 transition.

You might also like