You are on page 1of 10

Perceptual phenomena and cortical reorganization Neuroscience Vol. 102, No. 2, pp.

263±272, 2001263
q 2001 IBRO. Published by Elsevier Science Ltd
Pergamon Printed in Great Britain. All rights reserved
PII: S0306-4522(00)00491-7 0306-4522/01 $20.00+0.00
www.elsevier.com/locate/neuroscience

THE RELATIONSHIP OF PERCEPTUAL PHENOMENA AND CORTICAL


REORGANIZATION IN UPPER EXTREMITY AMPUTEES

È SSER, b* C. WINTER, b W. MU
S. M. GRU È HLNICKEL, b C. DENKE, b A. KARL, c K. VILLRINGER d and
H. FLOR a*
a
Department of Neuropsychology, Central Institute of Mental Health, Mannheim, Germany
b
Department of Psychology, Clinical Psychology and Behavioral Neuroscience Unit, Humboldt-University, Berlin, Germany
c
Department of Biopsychology, University of Technology, Dresden, Germany
d
Department of Radiology, University Clinical Center Benjamin Franklin, Free University, Berlin, Germany

AbstractÐIn this study 16 unilateral upper extremity amputees participated in a comprehensive psychophysiological
examination that included the assessment of painful and non-painful phantom and stump sensations, thermal and electric
perception as well as two-point discrimination thresholds, the detailed analysis of referred sensation and the measurement of
reorganizational changes in primary somatosensory cortex using neuroelectric source imaging. Reorganization of the
primary somatosensory cortex was associated with increased habitual phantom limb pain, telescoping, non-painful stump
sensations and painful referred sensation induced by painful stimulation. It was unrelated to non-painful phantom sensations,
non-painful referred sensation elicited by painful or non-painful stimulation, painful referred sensation elicited by non-
painful stimulation, perception thresholds and stump pain.
These data substantiate the hypothesis that painful and non-painful phantom phenomena are mediated by different neural
substrates. q 2001 IBRO. Published by Elsevier Science Ltd. All rights reserved.

Key words: cortical plasticity, phantom limb pain, phantom phenomena, referred sensations, neuroelectric source imaging, traumatic
amputation.

The amputation of a limb is usually accompanied by the Merzenich and colleagues 25 showed that the amputation
development of non-painful phantom sensations in the of a ®nger in an adult owl monkey led to an invasion of
now absent limb. In up to 85% of the cases there are 1±2 mm of adjacent areas into the cortical representation
also painful sensations in the phantom. 14,37 So far it is of the deafferented ®nger in primary somatosensory
not known which mechanisms contribute to the occur- cortex (SI; area 3b). An even more extensive reorganiza-
rence of these phantom phenomena. Both central and tion of about 1±2 cm was reported by Pons et al. 31 who
peripheral factors have been implicated. For example, studied adult macaques who had undergone deafferenta-
Melzack 24 postulated the existence of a neuromatrix tion of an upper extremity 12 years earlier. The area
involving several brain areas which are thought to under- below the mouth and the cheek now activated neurons
lie phantoms. Sherman 35 and others have pointed out that in the former arm and hand area. The magnitude of this
peripheral processes involving local psychophysiological reorganization suggested that the mechanism involved
changes at the stump may signi®cantly contribute to both might be more than just sprouting at the dendritic
non-painful phantom sensations and phantom limb pain. trees. 10,11 Ramachandran, Rogers-Ramachandran and
Decades ago it had already been suggested that plastic Stewart 33 found that stimulation of the face area of
alterations at the spinal, thalamic or cortical level might upper limb amputees led to referred sensations in the
be related to phantom phenomena. 3 In recent years, phantom hand with a one-to-one relationship of indivi-
animal experiments have shown that deafferentation of dual stimulation sites and areas of phantom sensations in
a nerve is followed by reorganization of the primary the hand (see also Refs. 3, 12, 20). Based on the Pons et
somatosensory area of the cortex. 15,25,31 For example, al. 31 data, they proposed that this phenomenon might be a
perceptual correlate of cortical reorganization.
Katz 17 postulated that cortical reorganization might be
*Corresponding authors. Tel.: 149-621-1703918; fax: 149-621-
1703932.
an adaptive and potentially phantom pain-relieving
E-mail address: gruesser@rz.hu-berlin.de (S. M. GruÈsser), mechanism and assumed that telescoping was a per-
¯or@as200.zi-mannheim.de (H. Flor). ceptual correlate of this reorganization which he believed
Abbreviations: D1, digit one; D5, digit ®ve; EEG, electroencephalo- to be negatively correlated with phantom limb pain. In
graphy; MPI, Multidimensional Pain Inventory; MRI, magnetic humans, electroencephalographic and magnetoence-
resonance image; RFS, referred sensation; SI, primary somatosen-
sory cortex; SVD, singular value decomposition; VAS, visual phalographic methods combined with magnetic reson-
analogue scale. ance imaging have revealed reorganizational changes
263
264 S. M. GruÈsser et al.

Table 1. Clinical characteristics of the study sample

Mean S.D. Range No. of subjects reporting


the phenomenon

Phantom limb pain 11


MPI (range 0±6) 2.06 1.81 0±5.33
VAS (range 0±100) 43.44 34.15 0±100
Non-painful phantom sensations 13
Index of frequency £ intensity (range 0±24) 5.04 5.00 0±16.07
Stump pain 6
MPI (range 0-6) 0.96 1.64 0±5.67
Non-painful stump sensations 5
Index of frequency £ intensity (range 0±24) 0.47 .90 0±2.74
Percentage of telescoping 31.38 44.36 0±100 5
Cortical reorganization (mm) 14.96 9.23 0.20±27.50 16

MPI, Multidimensional Pain Inventory; VAS, Visual Analogue Scale

following amputation that are comparable to those For the non-painful phantom and stump phenomena, the interview
observed in animals. 1,4±6,40 However, Elbert and collea- included a list of sensations taken from the relevant literature 14,36
that were rated with respect to intensity (0 ˆ ªnoneº to 3 ˆ ªvery
gues 4 were unable to ®nd a positive relationship between strongº) and frequency of occurrence (0 ˆ ªneverº to 8 ˆ
referred sensations and cortical reorganization in ®ve ªconstantº). The instrument also included an assessment of the
upper extremity amputees. Flor et al. 5 showed that corti- telescoping phenomenon. The interviewer marked in a template
cal reorganization in SI was signi®cantly related to the the location, position and size of the phantom limb as experienced
magnitude of phantom limb pain, and Knecht et al. 20 by the patient. The percentage of telescoping was determined by
measuring the distance from the phantom ®ngertips to the stump,
reported a close association between cortical reorganiza- subtracting it from the length of the intact arm and relating this
tion and the number of sites from which phantom sensa- difference to the maximum phantom length possible (length of the
tions could be elicited by painful stimulation. The intact arm subtracted by length of the stump). This yielded a
purpose of this study was to determine the relationship percentage score with 100% telescoping signifying a complete
shrinkage, i.e. a phantom not exceeding the stump length.
between cortical reorganization and the various percep- In addition, phantom and stump pain were assessed with the
tual sequelae of amputation using comprehensive, reli- Pain Intensity Scale of the West Haven±Yale Multidimensional
able and valid assessment instruments in order to better Pain Inventory (MPI), 18 a reliable and valid measure of the
understand the neural substrates of various phantom amount of pain which was modi®ed to separately assess phantom
phenomena. and stump pain. Handedness was determined by the Edinburgh
Handedness Inventory. 30
Sensations referred to the phantom limb were assessed by
EXPERIMENTAL PROCEDURES
applying painful and non-painful stimuli. Stimulation was
performed in randomized order in the four modalities pain,
Participants touch, vibration and heat on 234 sites that were more closely
spaced in areas near the amputation site and had wider spacing
Sixteen unilateral upper extremity amputees, 14 male and two in more distant areas. Touch stimuli were delivered with a cotton
female, were studied. Their mean age was 57.5 years applicator as described by Ramachandran and colleagues, 33 pain-
(S.D. ˆ 10.68, range 39±74). Thirteen participants were right- ful stimuli were applied with a safety needle. 20 Vibratory stimuli
handed, three had no clear hand preference, and none was left- were tested by a battery-operated vibrator and heat stimuli by a
handed. Mean age at the time of the amputation was 21.83 years thermode of 408C (thermal stimulator Medoc 2001). The duration
(S.D. ˆ 10.95, range 2±47). The average time since amputation of each stimulus was 1 s in all four modalities. The duration of the
was 50.33 years (S.D. ˆ 15.14, range 15±65). The mean stump entire assessment was about 120 min. The participants localized
length was 33.38 cm (S.D. ˆ 18.67, range 0±54). Forty-four per and evaluated the quality and intensity of stimulation they felt at
cent of the sample had been amputated on the right and 56% on the site of stimulation and reported and evaluated sensations else-
the left side of the body. In one case, the amputation was due to an where (e.g. in the phantom). Instructions to the subjects were
osteosarcoma; in all other 15 cases, it was caused by traumatic neutral with respect to the potential sites of mislocalization.
injury (mostly combat-related injuries). Fourteen of the 16 parti- The localization of sensations perceived at the site of stimulation
cipants experienced non-painful phantom sensations, and 11 as well as on the phantom was marked on the subject's body,
reported phantom limb pain (see Table 1). Only two subjects, drawn in a body template, videotaped and recorded on polaroid
both with amputations performed during childhood, experienced photographs. The intensity of the sensations was evaluated on a
no phantom limb whatsoever. Written informed consent of the seven-point rating scale ranging from no sensation to very strong
subjects was obtained and the study was approved by the local sensation, the quality was described by the subjects using the
Human Subjects Committee. same adjective lists that were employed in the interview. Referred
sensations (RFS) were measured in two ways: (i) the number of
sites from which (painful or non-painful) RFS could be elicited in
Methods the phantom, calculated for painful and non-painful stimulation,
Subsequent to a comprehensive neurological assessment, the and (ii) the percentage of sites at which the subjects experienced
subjects participated in a psychometric evaluation including an painful or non-painful RFS related to painful or non-painful
interview about the amputation and its consequences such as stimulation.
preamputation pain, phantom pain, non-painful phantom In a psychophysical examination, thermal and electric percep-
phenomena, telescoping, stump pain and stump sensations. 6,20 tion thresholds were assessed at the thenar eminence of the intact
The interview included Visual Analogue Scales (VAS, 0± hand, the stump 6 cm proximal to the amputation line and a
100 mm: ªnoneº to ªvery strongº) and, for the painful phantom homologous site on the intact arm, and the lower corners of the
and stump phenomena, adjective lists (0 ˆ ªnot at all trueº to mouth. Three ascending series of stimuli were used. In addition,
4 ˆ ªexactly trueº) based on the McGill Pain Questionnaire. 23 two-point discrimination thresholds were assessed at the same
Perceptual phenomena and cortical reorganization 265

sites, excluding the thenar eminence and instead including the phantom limb pain. They showed a signi®cantly larger
®rst and the ®fth digit of the intact hand. A caliper-probe which amount of reorganization of primary somatosensory
started at 0.5 mm was opened progressively wider in three ascend-
ing series. Electric stimuli were applied by a Toennies electric cortex as assessed with the Euclidean distance measure
stimulator which delivered unipolar impulses of 100 ms duration. (M ˆ 20.25 mm, S.D. ˆ 5.15) than the remaining ®ve
For determination of thermal thresholds, a Medoc thermal stimu- amputees without phantom limb pain [M ˆ 3.36 mm,
lator with a stimulation range from 0 to 508C was used. S.D. ˆ 2.87; t(14) ˆ 6.79, P , 0.001]. Phantom limb
Neuroelectric source imaging (64-channel EEG) was used to pain as assessed by the MPI Pain Intensity Scale was
assess cortical reorganization. The recordings were performed
with a 61-channel electrode matrix. The AgAgCl electrodes highly correlated with cortical reorganization (r16 ˆ
were mounted in an electrocap where the corners of the 10 £ 6 0.91, P , 0.001).
matrix were F7, T5, F8 and T6 according to the 10±20 EEG- Non-painful phantom sensations such as tickling or
system. The reference electrode was placed at Cz. The electrodes movements of the phantom limb were reported by 14
had a distance of 1.5 cm from center to center. The individual
matrix distribution was digitized with an infrared camera system of 16 subjects. These phenomena, as assessed by the
(Optotrak) including reference landmarks on the nasion, the left index of intensity and frequency of sensations, were posi-
and right preauricular points and Cz. In addition, horizontal and tively but not signi®cantly related to cortical reorganiza-
vertical electro-oculograms were recorded. One-thousand pneu- tion (r16 ˆ 0.38, n.s.). Group differences of the Euclidean
matic stimuli were delivered to the left and right corner of the distance measure between amputees with (M ˆ 15.92 mm,
lower lip and the ®rst and ®fth digits of the intact hand, respec-
tively. Two Synamps ampli®ers with Scan software (Neuroscan) S.D. ˆ 8.14) and without non-painful phantom phenom-
were used for data acquisition. The signals were continuously ena (M ˆ 10.90 mm, S.D. ˆ 14.53) were not signi®cant
sampled with a 1 kHz sampling rate and ®lter settings from d.c. [t(14) ˆ 20.84, n.s.]. To closer evaluate the relation
to 200 Hz. The data were then digitally ®ltered from 2 to 20 Hz. between painful and non-painful phantom phenomena
Magnetic resonance images (MRI) were recorded to obtain the
anatomic structure of the subjects' cortices (Siemens Vision MR and cortical reorganization, partial correlation coef®-
1.5 T scanner, T1-weighted, TR ˆ 22 ms, TE ˆ 10 ms, alpha ˆ cients were computed. The correlation between non-
30 degrees, slice thickness ˆ 1 mm). painful phantom sensations and cortical reorganization
The data were analysed with the MUSIC algorithm, which ®ts dropped to r13 ˆ 0.35 (n.s.) when phantom limb pain
probe dipoles to potential dipole locations in the region of inter- (MPI) was controlled for, and to r13 ˆ 20.04 when
est. 28 The region of interest here was a grid covering the indivi-
dual surface of the cortex of the patients. The time range was 0± VAS measures were used for both perceptual phenom-
100 ms with respect to trigger onset. The signal subspace con- ena. However, the association between phantom limb
tained the largest four components of the data singular value pain (MPI) and cortical reorganization remained stable
decomposition (SVD). The generator in SI was determined by with r13 ˆ 0.90 (P , 0.001) when non-painful phantom
selecting those dipole locations with maximum probability within
the somatosensory cortex. 29 The source localizations were super- sensations (index) were controlled for.
imposed onto the individual MR images. Six of the 16 subjects reported telescoping, that is, a
The measure of cortical reorganization was determined in the progressive regression of the phantom limb towards the
following manner: The Euclidean distance between the ®ngers stump. Three subjects had a complete telescope, i.e. the
(mean of d1 and d5) and the mouth representation on the intact phantom arm had shrunk to stump length. Three addi-
side was computed for the anterior±posterior, medial±lateral and
superior±inferior coordinates. This yielded a mouth±digit distance tional subjects showed a partial telescope with a shrink-
measure in millimetres. Based on the symmetry assumption of the age between 34 and 85%. All subjects with telescoping
two hemispheres, 9 the locations of the ®ngers of the intact side were also suffered from phantom limb pain, and both phenom-
transposed along the midsagittal plane to the hemisphere that repre- ena were positively correlated (r16 ˆ 0.43, P ˆ 0.05).
sents the amputation side and mirror locations of the digits were
determined. The same distance measure was now calculated for the Furthermore, telescoping was signi®cantly positively
amputated side, and the difference between the Euclidean distances associated with the amount of cortical reorganization
on the intact and the amputation side was then used as an indicator (r16 ˆ 0.65, P ˆ 0.003). There was a signi®cant group
of the amount of cortical reorganization. 4 This procedure yielded a difference in the amount of cortical reorganization
measure of the shift of the cortical representation of the mouth into between amputees with (M ˆ 22.50 mm, S.D. ˆ 2.74)
the representation of the deafferented area in millimetres.
and without telescoping [M ˆ 10.46 mm, S.D. ˆ 8.80;
t(12) ˆ 24.01, P ˆ 0.002]. When only subjects with
Data analysis
phantom limb pain were considered, the difference
Depending on scale characteristics, Spearman rank correlations between those with and without telescoping failed to
and Pearson correlation coef®cients were calculated to determine
the relationship between the amount of cortical reorganization and reach signi®cance [M ˆ 22.50, S.D. ˆ 2.74 vs M ˆ
phantom and stump sensations, thresholds, and referred sensations. 17.56, S.D. ˆ 6.34; t(9) ˆ ±1.74, n.s.] and the correlation
In addition, group differences between subjects with and without between telescoping and cortical reorganization was
phantom pain and non-painful phantom phenomena were tested lower (r11 ˆ 0.49, n.s.).
employing t-tests for unrelated samples. Since a priori hypotheses
about the direction of the association were present, one-tailed levels
of signi®cance were used, with the exception of stump phenomena Referred sensations and cortical reorganization
and stump pain where no previous reports on the association with
cortical reorganization existed. Six of the 16 subjects reported a referral of sensation
into the phantom limb when they were stimulated at
RESULTS various body sites (see Table 2). All six patients also
suffered from phantom limb pain. RFSs were elicited
Painful and non-painful phantom phenomena and corti-
mainly from the stump (n ˆ 5), but also bilaterally from
cal reorganization
the shoulder (n ˆ 5), the face (n ˆ 3), the back and neck
Eleven of the 16 amputees in the study suffered from areas (n ˆ 4), from the intact arm (n ˆ 4) and from the leg
266 S. M. GruÈsser et al.

Table 2. Number and percentage of sites from which referred sensations could be elicited

Stimulation modality

Painful Non-painful

M (S.D.) M (S.D.)

Painful n of sites 3.83 (4.07) 5.17 (8.06)


Referred % 63.89* (49.91) 53.85* (50.73)
Sensation Non-painful n 1.17 (1.60) 2.67 (3.27)
%* 36.11* (49.91) 46.15* (50.73)

*Percentage related to the number of sites where RFS were elicited in the respective stimulation modality.

(n ˆ 1). None of the subjects reported a topographic one- Other amputation-related variables and cortical
to-one relationship of individual stimulation sites and reorganization
areas of phantom sensations in the hand.
Other amputation-related variables such as time since
In four of the 16 subjects, painful stimulation elicited
amputation, age at the time of amputation or stump
painful RFSs in the phantom. There was a signi®cant
length were not signi®cantly correlated with cortical
group difference in the amount of cortical reorganization
reorganization (see Fig. 2).
between these subjects (M ˆ 23.3 mm, S.D. ˆ 3.93) and
the remaining subjects who did not report this phenom-
enon [M ˆ 12.2 mm, S.D. ˆ 8.86; t(14) ˆ 2.39, P ˆ 0.03; Stump phenomena and cortical reorganization
see Fig. 1]. No signi®cant differences emerged when Six subjects suffered from stump pain. The correlation
subjects with and without non-painful RFSs induced by between cortical reorganization and stump pain tended to
painful stimulation [M ˆ 21.77, S.D. ˆ 7.64 vs M ˆ be signi®cant for stump pain measured by the MPI Pain
13.41, S.D. ˆ 9.09; t(14) ˆ 1.47, n.s.], painful RFSs Intensity Scale (r16 ˆ 0.50, P ˆ 0.05), but became
elicited by non-painful stimulation [M ˆ 21.90, nonsigni®cant when phantom limb pain was controlled
S.D. ˆ 3.37 vs M ˆ 13.38, S.D. ˆ 9.48; t(14) ˆ 1.50, for (r13 ˆ 0.18, n.s.). There was a signi®cant positive
n.s.] or non-painful RFSs induced by non-painful stimu- correlation between the amount of phantom limb pain
lation [M ˆ 18.67, S.D. ˆ 5.81 vs M ˆ 14.12, S.D. ˆ 9.84; and the amount of stump pain, both measured by the
t(14) ˆ 0.76, n.s.] were compared. Likewise, only the MPI Pain Intensity Scale (r16 ˆ 0.48, P ˆ 0.03). When
percentage of sites where painful stimulation induced subjects with and without stump pain were compared,
painful RFS was signi®cantly associated with the amount no signi®cant differences in the amount of cortical
of cortical reorganization (r16 ˆ 0.57, P ˆ 0.01), whereas reorganization [with stump pain: M ˆ 17.38, S.D. ˆ
the remaining associations between cortical reorganiza- 9.69; without stump pain: M ˆ 13.53, S.D. ˆ 9.153;
tion and RFSs remained insigni®cant. t(14) ˆ 0.80, n.s.] or phantom limb pain [with stump
Subjects with painful RFSs elicited by painful stimu- pain: M ˆ 2.56, S.D. ˆ 1.80; without stump pain: M ˆ
lation also showed signi®cantly more phantom limb pain 1.7667, S.D. ˆ 1.853; t(14) ˆ 0.83, n.s.] emerged.
as measured by the MPI (M ˆ 3.58, S.D. ˆ 1.20) than Five of the 16 subjects reported spontaneous non-
subjects without this type of RFS [M ˆ 1.56, S.D. ˆ 1.73; painful stump sensations. A signi®cant correlation with
t(14) ˆ 2.16, P , 0.05]. No signi®cant differences in cortical reorganization was found when the adjective lists
phantom limb pain intensity emerged when patients (index of intensity and frequency) were used as stump
with and without non-painful RFSs induced by painful sensation measure (r16 ˆ 0.62, P ˆ 0.01). After control-
stimulation [M ˆ 3.33, S.D. ˆ 1.86 vs M ˆ 1.77, S.D. ˆ ling for phantom limb pain (MPI), this correlation
1.75; t(14) ˆ 1.39, n.s.], painful RFSs induced by non- dropped to r16 ˆ 0.53 (P ˆ 0.04), and even further to
painful stimulation [M ˆ 3.0, S.D. ˆ 0.33 vs M ˆ 1.85, r16 ˆ 0.12 (n.s.) when the VAS was used as measure of
S.D. ˆ 1.96; t(13.89) ˆ 2.0, P ˆ 0.07] or non-painful phantom pain intensity. There were no signi®cant group
RFSs induced by non-painful stimulation [M ˆ 2.67, differences in the magnitude of cortical reorganization
S.D. ˆ 0.88 vs M ˆ 1.92, S.D. ˆ 1.97; t(14) ˆ 0.63, n.s.] [M ˆ 19.36, S.D. ˆ 7.546 vs M ˆ 12.98, S.D. ˆ 9.55;
were compared. t(14) ˆ 1.31, n.s.] or the amount of phantom limb pain
[M ˆ 2.80, S.D. ˆ 1.80 vs M ˆ 1.73, S.D. ˆ 1.80;
t(14) ˆ 1.10, n.s.] between subjects with and without
Perception thresholds and cortical reorganization non-painful stump sensations.

Means and standard deviations for thermal, electric DISCUSSION


and two-point discrimination thresholds at the stump,
Correlates of cortical reorganization and mechanisms of
the homologous site at the intact arm and the lower
phantom limbs
corners of the mouth are shown in Table 3. There were
no signi®cant correlations between perception thresholds Painful and non-painful phantom sensations. Cortical
and the amount of cortical reorganization (see Table 4). reorganization in primary somatosensory cortex was
Fig. 1. (a) Averaged somatotopic source localizations. The coronal view shows the averaged localizations for amputees with painful
sensations referred from painful stimulation (n ˆ 4). In this group, the localization of the corner of the mouth in the hemisphere
contralateral to the amputation side has shifted far into the former hand region. (b) The coronal view shows the averaged localiza-
tions for amputees without painful sensations referred from painful stimulation (n ˆ 12). Compared to (a), there is a smaller shift of
the mouth localization contralateral to the amputation side into the former hand region.
268 S. M. GruÈsser et al.

Table 3. Perception thresholds

Two-point discrimination Electric thresholds Thermal thresholds

Mean (mm) S.D. Mean (mA) S.D. Mean (8C) S.D.

Stump 23.74 19.84 1.44 0.52 34.86 1.84


Intact arm (homologous site) 22.58 14.48 1.26 0.42 34.80 1.97
Corner of the mouth (amputation side) 7.47 3.74 1.33 0.37 33.34 0.50
Corner of the mouth (intact side) 6.88 3.22 1.32 0.20 33.64 2.16

Table 4. Perception thresholds and cortical reorganization

Two-point discrimination Electric thresholds Thermal thresholds

r P r p r p

Stump 20.002 0.497 20.040 0.888 0.302 0.274


Intact arm (homologous site) 20.044 0.436 0.289 0.296 20.054 0.855
Corner of mouth (amputation side) 0.299 0.130 0.160 0.569 0.414 0.125
Corner of mouth (intact side) 0.145 0.296 0.234 0.402 0.275 0.322

Statistical analyses (Pearson, one-tailed) and signi®cance levels.

positively associated with phantom limb pain, and this reorganization has been a matter of controversy. In
phenomenon seems to be very stable. 1,5,6,22,27 Non-painful contrast to Katz, 17 who assumed a negative relationship
phantom sensations were not consistently related to these between phantom pain and telescoping and postulated
plastic cortical changes. When a signi®cant correlation that this might be due to a bene®cial adaption through
was found, it was entirely mediated by phantom limb cortical reorganizational processes, we found a positive
pain. We assume that other cortical areas, for example, relationship between cortical reorganization and phan-
the posterior parietal cortex and SII, may be involved in tom limb pain as well as telescoping. Since all patients
the subjective awareness of a still existent, non-painful with telescoping also suffered from phantom limb pain,
limb. 7,19 the two variables could not be evaluated separately in
this study. However, when the relationship of cortical
Painful and non-painful stump sensations. There was a reorganization and telescoping was examined only in
signi®cant positive relationship between both painful and the phantom limb pain group, telescoping was no longer
non-painful stump phenomena and cortical reorganiza- positively related to SI reorganization suggesting that it
tion. In the case of stump pain, the association dis- is not primarily mediated by SI. It is possible that tele-
appeared after phantom limb pain was controlled for scoping is more related to changes in brain regions
while for non-painful stump sensations the relationship underlying non-painful phantom limb phenomena, such
was stable. Peripheral mechanisms such as neuroma, as posterior parietal cortex. Based on our data, the
vascular or muscular changes 32 may contribute to both putative bene®cial nature of telescoping with respect to
phantom limb and stump pain. Several studies, including phantom limb pain must be questioned.
our study (r16 ˆ 0.48, P , 0.05) reported a positive asso-
ciation between stump and phantom limb pain 26,34 and it Referred sensations. In contrast to the assumption of
has been suggested that there are common underlying Ramachandran and colleagues 33 that touch-induced
peripheral mechanisms triggering both phantom limb referred sensations are a perceptual correlate of cortical
and stump pain. 26,35 Random input from the periphery reorganization, we could not ®nd a signi®cant correlation
may, moreover, contribute to reorganizational changes. 38 between non-painful referred sensations induced by pain-
For example, the elimination of peripheral input by ful or non-painful stimulation and cortical reorganiza-
brachial plexus anesthesia led to concurrent reductions tion. Analogous to Knecht et al. 20,21 who demonstrated
in both phantom limb pain and cortical reorganization in a strong relationship between the amount of cortical
about half of the upper limb amputees studied. In the reorganization and the number of stimulated body sites
other half, both variables were unaffected. 1 In line with which elicited referred sensations based on painful (but
these ®ndings, non-painful stump sensations were also not non-painful) stimulation, we found a positive rela-
positively correlated with cortical reorganization. The tionship between the number of sites from which painful
peripheral factors that contribute to phantom limb pain, referred sensations could be elicited by painful stimula-
stump pain and cortical reorganization need to be further tion and the degree of cortical reorganization. This rela-
investigated. tionship re¯ects the close association of phantom limb
pain and SI reorganization. In the Knecht et al. 20,21 study
Telescoping. The development of telescoping and the type of referred sensation had not been evaluated. No
its relationship to phantom limb pain and cortical signi®cant differences in cortical reorganization were
5 10

n of nonpainful RFS by nonpainful stimulation


4 8
n of nonpainful RFS by painful stimulation
7

3 6

2 4

1 2

1
r = .36 (ns) r = .14 (ns)
0 0
0 10 20 30 0 10 20 30
cortical reorganization in mm cortical reorganization in mm

30 10

9
25
8
n of painful RFS by nonpainful stimulation

n of painful RFS by painful stimulation


7
20
6

15 5

4
10
3

2
5
1
r = .35 (ns) r = .51*
0 0
0 10 20 30 0 10 20 30
cortical reorganization in mm cortical reorganization in mm
3
10

8
nonpainful phantom sensations (index)
nonpainful stump sensations (index)

4
1

r = .62 r = .38 (ns)


0 0
0 10 20 30 0 10 20 30
cortical reorganization in mm cortical reorganization in mm
r(part) = .53 controlling for phantom pain r(part) = .35 (ns) controlling for phantom limb pain
6 6

5 5

4 4

3 3
phantom limb pain (MPI)

2 2
stump pain (MPI)

1 1

r = .50* r = .91**
0 0
0 10 20 30 0 10 20 30
cortical reorganization in mm cortical reorganization in mm
r(part) = .18 (ns) controlling for phantom limb pain r(part) = .88** controlling for stump pain

Fig. 2. (continued overleaf )


270 S. M. GruÈsser et al.

100 50

80 40

age at the time of amputation (years)


60 30

40 20
telescoping in percent

20 10

r = .65** r = .25 (ns)


0 0
0 10 20 30 0 10 20 30
cortical reorganization in mm cortical reorganization in mm

800 80

700
70

600

age at the time of assessment (in years)


60
500
time since amputation (in months)

400 50

300
40

200
30
100
r = –.03 (ns) r = .22 (ns)
0 20
0 10 20 30 0 10 20 30
cortical reorganization in mm cortical reorganization in mm
60 6

50 5

40 4

30 3
phantom limb pain (MPI)
stump length (in cm)

20 2

10 1

r = –.26 (ns) r = .48*


0 0
0 10 20 30 0 1 2 3 4 5 6
cortical reorganization in mm stump pain (MPI)
r(part) = .06 (ns) controlling for cortical reorganization

Fig. 2. Scatter plots demonstrating the relation between cortical reorganization and various amputation-related variables (n ˆ 16).
*P , 0.05, **P , 0.01; ns, non-signi®cant.

found for subjects with or without non-painful referred Perception thresholds. In contrast to reports by
sensations, especially those sensations that were elicited Haber 13 and Teuber et al., 39 we did not ®nd the stump
by non-painful stimulation. This suggests that other to be more sensitive than the homologous site at the
cortical areas contribute to the experience of these intact arm. This discrepancy could be due to differences
non-painful phantom phenomena. The ®nding that in inclusion criteria of the samples (e.g. we included
referred sensations could be elicited from both the intact patients with vascular disorders) and the exact site
and the amputation side as well as from distal areas such of the sensitivity assessment. In contrast to Haber 13
as the leg 2 suggests an involvement of the thalamus or the who measured thresholds 2.5 cm proximal from the
second somatosensory cortex where the leg and arm stump, we assessed thresholds 6 cm proximal to the
representation are adjacent. stump. Contrary to the hypothesis of Spitzer and
Perceptual phenomena and cortical reorganization 271

colleagues 38 that reduced stump thresholds are associated spontaneously occurring or induced phantom limb pain,
with an expanding cortical representation area, we could whereas no signi®cant relationship was found with non-
not ®nd a signi®cant relationship between cortical re- painful phantom phenomena and perception thresholds.
organization and stump perception thresholds. We The stable relationship between phantom limb pain and
also failed to observe this relationship for both two- cortical reorganization 1,5,6,16,22,27 and our present ®ndings
point discrimination and perception thresholds at the suggest that plastic changes in the primary somato-
mouth although we noted a signi®cant shift of the sensory cortex may be an important neural correlate of
cortical representation of the mouth into the region of phantom limb pain. The signi®cant positive relationship
the amputated hand. It is unlikely that an insuf®cient between painful phantom sensations induced by painful
reliability and validity of the threshold assessments stimulation and cortical reorganization con®rms this
caused this lack of signi®cant associations. Thresholds conclusion. Stump pain seems to be mediated by periph-
can be reliably determined and are stable over time eral mechanisms which may also trigger phantom pain.
in healthy controls (Denke C., Karl A., GruÈsser S., These data suggest that a distributed network involving
Birbaumer N. and Flor H., unpublished experiment). In various cortical and potentially subcortical areas under-
phantom limb pain patients, the same type of assessment lies painful and non-painful phantom phenomenaÐsimi-
yielded very heterogeneous results suggesting that these lar to the concept of a neuromatrix suggested by
perceptual phenomena may be variable and may also be Melzack. 24 Peripheral factors involved in stump pain
in¯uenced by factors such as concurrent stump pain. and stump sensations seem to contribute to both phantom
limb pain and cortical reorganization and need to be
further investigated. 8
CONCLUSIONS

The data of this study suggest that reorganizational AcknowledgementÐThis work was supported by the Deutsche
changes in SI appear to be mainly related to Forschungsgemeinschaft (Fl 156/16).

REFERENCES

1. Birbaumer N., Lutzenberger W., Montoya P., Larbig W., Unertl K., Grodd W. and Flor H. (1997) Effects of regional anesthesia on phantom
limb pain are mirrored in changes in cortical reorganization. J. Neurosci. 17, 5503±5508.
2. Borsook D., Becerra L., Fishman S., Edwards A., Jennings C. L., Stojanovic M., Papinicolas L., Ramachandran V. S., Gonzales R. G. and
Breiter H. (1998) Acute plasticity in the human somatosensory cortex following amputation. NeuroReport 9, 1013±1017.
3. Cronholm B. (1951) Phantom limbs in amputees. Acta Psychiatr. Scand. Suppl. 72, 1±310.
4. Elbert T., Flor H., Birbaumer N., Knecht S., Hampson S., Larbig W. and Taub E. (1994) Extensive reorganization of the somatosensory cortex
in adult humans after nervous system injury. NeuroReport 5, 2593±2597.
5. Flor H., Elbert T., Wienbruch C., Pantev C., Knecht S., Birbaumer N., Larbig W. and Taub E. (1995) Phantom limb pain as a perceptual
correlate of cortical reorganization. Nature 357, 482±484.
6. Flor H., Elbert T., MuÈhlnickel W., Pantev C., Wienbruch C. and Taub E. (1998) Cortical reorganization and phantom phenomena in congenital
and traumatic upper extremity amputees. Expl Brain Res. 119, 205±212.
7. Flor H., MuÈhlnickel W., Karl A., Denke C., GruÈsser S. and Taub E. (2000) A neural substrate for nonpainful phantom limb phenomena.
NeuroReport 11, 1407±1411.
8. Flor H. (2000) The functional organization of the brain in chronic pain. In Nervous System Plasticity and Pain: Progress in Brain Research, (eds
SandkuÈhler J., Bromm B. and Gebhardt G.). Elsevier, Amsterdam (in press).
9. Gallen C. C., Sobel D. F., Lewine J. D., Sanders J. A., Hart B. L., Davis L. E. and Orrison W. W. (1993) Neuromagnetic mapping of brain
function. Radiology 187, 863±867.
10. Garraghty P. E. and Muja N. (1996) NMDA Receptors and plasticity in adult primate somatosensory cortex. J. comp. Neurol. 367, 319±326.
11. Gilbert C. D. (1993) Rapid dynamic changes in adult cerebral cortex. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 3, 100±103.
12. Halligan P. W., Marshall J. C., Wade D. T., Davey J. and Morrison D. (1993) Thumb in cheek? Sensory reorganization and perceptual plasticity
after limb amputation. NeuroReport 4, 223±236.
13. Haber W. B. (1955) Effects of loss of limb on sensory functions. J. Psychol. 40, 115±123.
14. Jensen T. S., Krebs B., Nielsen J. and Rasmussen P. (1983) Immediate and long-term phantom limb pain in amputees: incidence, clinical
characteristics and relationship to pre-amputation limb pain. Pain 21, 267±278.
15. Kaas J. H. (1991) Plasticity of sensory and motor maps in adult mammals. A. Rev. Neurosci. 14, 137.
16. Karl A., Birbaumer N., Lutzenberger W., Cohen L. G. and Flor H. (2000) Reorganization of motor and somatosensory cortex in upper extremity
amputees with phantom limb pain. (Submitted for publication.)
17. Katz J. (1992) Psychophysiological contributions to phantom limbs. Can. J. Psychiat. 37, 282±298.
18. Kerns R. D., Turk D. C. and Rudy T. E. (1985) The West Haven Multidimensional Pain Inventory (MPI). Pain 23, 345±356.
19. Kew J. J. M., Ridding M. C., Rothwell J. C., Passingham R. E., Leigh P. N., Sooriakumeran S., Frackowiak R. S. J. and Brooks D. J. J. (1994)
Reorganization of cortical blood ¯ow and transcranial magnetic stimulation maps in human subjects after upper limb amputation. J. Neuro-
physiol. 72, 2517±2524.
20. Knecht S., Henningsen H., Elbert T., Flor H., HoÈhling C., Pantev C. and Taub E. (1996) Reorganizational and perceptual changes after
amputation. Brain 119, 1213±1219.
21. Knecht S., Hennigsen H., Flor H., Elbert T. and Taub E. (1998) Plasticity of plasticity? Changes in the pattern of perceptual correlates or
reorganization after amputation. Brain 121, 717±724.
22. Lotze M., Grodd W., Birbaumer N., Erb M., Huse E. and Flor H. (1999) Does use of a myoelectric prosthesis prevent cortical reorganization and
phantom limb pain? Nat. Neurosci. 2, 501±502.
23. Melzack R. A. (1975) The McGill Pain Questionnaire: major properties and scoring methods. Pain 1, 277±279.
24. Melzack R. (1990) Phantom limbs and the concept of a neuromatrix. Trends Neurosci. 13, 88±92.
25. Merzenich M. M., Nelson R. J., Stryker M. P., Cynader M. S., Shoppmann A. and Zook J. M. (1984) Somatosensory cortical map changes
following digit amputation in adult monkeys. J. comp. Neurol. 224, 591±605.
272 S. M. GruÈsser et al.

26. Montoya P., Larbig W., Grulke N., Flor H., Taub E. and Birbaumer N. (1997) The relationship of phantom limb pain to other phantom limb
phenomena in upper extremity amputees. Pain 72, 93±97.
27. Montoya P., Ritter K., Huse E., Larbig W., Braun C., ToÈpfner S., Lutzenberger W., Grodd W., Flor H. and Birbaumer N. (1998) The cortical
somatotopic map and phantom phenomena in subjects with congenital limb atrophy and traumatic amputees with phantom limb pain. Eur. J.
Neurosci. 10, 1095±1102.
28. Mosher J. C., Lewis P. S. and Leahy R. M. (1992) Multiple dipole modeling and localization from spatio-temporal MEG data. IEEE Trans.
Biomed. Engng 39, 541±557.
29. MuÈhlnickel W., Lutzenberger W. and Flor H. (1999) Localization of somatosensory evoked potentials in primary somatosensory cortex: a
comparison between PCA and MUSIC. Brain Topogr. 11, 1±7.
30. Old®eld R. C. (1971) The assessment and analysis of handedness: the Edinburgh Inventory. Neuropsychologia 9, 97±113.
31. Pons T. P., Garraghty P. E., Ommaya A. K., Kaas J. H., Taub E. and Mishkin M. M. (1991) Massive cortical reorganization after sensory
deafferentation in adult macaques. Science 252, 1857±1860.
32. Postone N. (1987) Phantom limb pain. A review. Int. J. Psychiat. Med. 17, 57±71.
33. Ramachandran V. S., Rogers-Ramachandran D. and Stewart M. (1992) Perceptual correlates of massive cortical reorganization. Science 258,
1159±1160.
34. Sherman R. A., Sherman C. J. and Parker L. (1984) Chronic phantom and stump pain among American veterans: results of a survey. Pain 18,
83±95.
35. Sherman R. A. (1989) Stump and phantom limb pain: mechanisms and syndromes. Neurol. Clin. 7, 249±264.
36. Sherman R. A. (1997) Physiological correlates. In Phantom Pain (ed. Sherman R. A.). Plenum, New York.
37. Sherman R. A., Katz J., Marbach J. J. and Heermann-Do K. (1997) Locations, characteristics, and descriptions. In Phantom Pain (ed. Sherman
R. A.). Plenum, New York.
38. Spitzer M., BoÈhler P., Weisbrod M. and Kischka U. (1995) A neural network model of phantom limbs. Biol. Cybern. 72, 197±206.
39. Teuber H. L., Krieger H. P. and Bender M. B. (1949) Reorganization of sensory function in amputation stumps: two-point discrimination. Fedn
Proc. 8, 156.
40. Yang T. T., Gallen C., Schwartz B. and Bloom F. E. (1994) Sensory maps in the human brain. Nature 368, 592±593.

(Accepted 17 October 2000)

You might also like