You are on page 1of 10

This article was downloaded by: [University of Arizona]

On: 23 August 2013, At: 08:47


Publisher: Routledge
Informa Ltd Registered in England and Wales Registered Number: 1072954 Registered office: Mortimer
House, 37-41 Mortimer Street, London W1T 3JH, UK

Journal of Clinical and Experimental


Neuropsychology
Publication details, including instructions for authors and subscription information:
http://www.tandfonline.com/loi/ncen20

The Boston Qualitative Scoring System as a Measure


of Executive Functioning in Rey-Osterrieth Complex
Figure Performance
Jessica Somerville , Geoffrey Tremont & Robert A. Stern
Published online: 09 Aug 2010.

To cite this article: Jessica Somerville , Geoffrey Tremont & Robert A. Stern (2000) The Boston Qualitative Scoring
System as a Measure of Executive Functioning in Rey-Osterrieth Complex Figure Performance, Journal of Clinical and
Experimental Neuropsychology, 22:5, 613-621

To link to this article: http://dx.doi.org/10.1076/1380-3395(200010)22:5;1-9;FT613

PLEASE SCROLL DOWN FOR ARTICLE

Taylor & Francis makes every effort to ensure the accuracy of all the information (the “Content”) contained
in the publications on our platform. However, Taylor & Francis, our agents, and our licensors make no
representations or warranties whatsoever as to the accuracy, completeness, or suitability for any purpose of
the Content. Any opinions and views expressed in this publication are the opinions and views of the authors,
and are not the views of or endorsed by Taylor & Francis. The accuracy of the Content should not be relied
upon and should be independently verified with primary sources of information. Taylor and Francis shall
not be liable for any losses, actions, claims, proceedings, demands, costs, expenses, damages, and other
liabilities whatsoever or howsoever caused arising directly or indirectly in connection with, in relation to or
arising out of the use of the Content.

This article may be used for research, teaching, and private study purposes. Any substantial or systematic
reproduction, redistribution, reselling, loan, sub-licensing, systematic supply, or distribution in any
form to anyone is expressly forbidden. Terms & Conditions of access and use can be found at http://
www.tandfonline.com/page/terms-and-conditions
Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology 1380-3395/00/2205-613$15.00
2000, Vol. 22, No. 5, pp. 613-621 © Swets & Zeitlinger

The Boston Qualitative Scoring System as a Measure of


Executive Functioning in Rey-Osterrieth Complex Figure
Performance*
Jessica Somerville1, Geoffrey Tremont2, and Robert A. Stern3
1
University of Rhode Island, 2Department of Psychiatry and Human Behavior, Brown University School of
Medicine, 3Department of Psychiatry and Human Behavior and Department of Clinical Neurosciences, Brown
University School of Medicine
Downloaded by [University of Arizona] at 08:47 23 August 2013

ABSTRACT

The Boston Qualitative Scoring System (BQSS) for the Rey-Osterrieth Complex Figure (ROCF) includes
five scores (Planning, Fragmentation, Neatness, Perseveration, and Organization) developed to measure the
executive aspects of ROCF productions. To assess the convergent and discriminant validity of the BQSS,
these five scores were compared to scores on four traditional tests of executive functioning, as well as to
three non-executive measures, in 141 adult patients. BQSS executive variables significantly correlated with
the traditional executive measures and were less correlated with discriminant measures. The BQSS Organi-
zation summary score also significantly differentiated patients with either no, mild, or severe executive
dysfunction. These results support the construct validity of the BQSS and demonstrate that the ROCF may
be a useful measure of executive functioning.

The Rey-Osterrieth Complex Figure (ROCF) that patients with Parkinson’s disease used a
(Osterrieth, 1944; Rey, 1941) has become one of more disorganized approach when copying the
the most widely used neuropsychological tasks ROCF compared to controls. Similarly, poor
in both clinical and research settings (Knight & organizational strategies have been found in pa-
Kaplan, in press). The most commonly em- tients with obsessive-compulsive disorder (Sav-
ployed scoring methods for the ROCF, including age et al., 1999), and, in patients with frontal
the original 36-point scoring system (Osterrieth, lesions, ROCF recall was improved when orga-
1944) and the more recent system by Meyers nizational strategies were provided (Diamond,
and Meyers (1995), provide a single, quantita- DeLuca, & Kelley, 1997; Eslinger & Gratten,
tive summary score based on the presence and 1990).
accuracy of 18 units of the figure. The construc- Several qualitative approaches to scoring the
tion strategy, planning, and organizational ap- ROCF have been proposed in recent years (e.g.,
proach used to draw the figure are not assessed Bennett-Levy, 1984; Bernstein & Waber, 1996;
with this single-score method. However, this Binder, 1982; Chervinsky, Mitrushina, & Satz,
more qualitative information may be valuable in 1992; Chiulli, Halland, LaRue, & Garry, 1995;
identifying executive dysfunction, as well as Hamby, Wilkins, & Barry, 1993; Loring, Lee, &
understanding the reason for poor recall. For Meador, 1988; Rapport, Farchione, Dutra, Web-
example, Grossman and colleagues (1993) found ster, & Charter, 1996; Shorr, Delis, & Massman,

*
The authors would like to acknowledge the assistance of Sarah M. Halpert.
Address correspondence to: Dr. Robert Stern, Neuropsychology Program, Rhode Island Hospital, 110 Lockwood
Street, Suite 430, Providence, RI 02903. Tel.: ++1 401 444-4500. Fax: ++1 401 444-6643. E-mail: rstern@life-
span.org
Accepted for publication: April 7, 2000.
614 JESSICA SOMERVILLE ET AL.

1992; Visser, 1973). However, these systems adult patients with Attention Deficit Hyperactiv-
focus on only one or two qualitative features of ity Disorder (ADHD) and matched controls. Ad-
performance, such as organization or symmetry, ditionally, the executive scores from the BQSS
or are not appropriate for use with adults. Many have been found to be useful in discriminating
of these qualitative scoring systems have dem- children with ADHD from controls (Cahn et al.,
onstrated the importance of executive function- 1996). BQSS executive scores have also differ-
ing in ROCF performance (Troyer & Wishart, entiated controls from other groups with pre-
1997), though they do not provide a comprehen- sumed executive dysfunction, such as patients
sive system for the evaluation of executive skills with traumatic brain injury (Javorsky, Rosen-
contributing to the ROCF. baum, & Stern, 1999) and recently detoxified
To date, the only commercially available alcoholics (Dawson & Grant, 2000).
qualitative scoring system available for adults is To examine convergent and discriminant va-
the Boston Qualitative Scoring System (BQSS) lidity, the present study investigated the hypoth-
Downloaded by [University of Arizona] at 08:47 23 August 2013

(Stern et al., 1994; Stern et al., 1999). This sys- esis that BQSS scores designed to measure exec-
tem provides a comprehensive set of qualitative utive functioning are more highly correlated
ratings, as well as salient quantitative summary with traditional measures of executive function-
scores, with extensive normative data. In addi- ing than with measures that are purportedly less
tion to measuring other qualitative features of sensitive to executive functioning. In addition,
visuoconstructive skills (e.g., Accuracy, Place- this study evaluated the usefulness of the BQSS
ment, Rotation, and Asymmetry), four of the 17 Organization summary score in differentiating
scores (Planning, Fragmentation, Neatness, and amongst groups of patients with differing levels
Perseveration), as well as the Organization sum- of executive dysfunction, as determined by stan-
mary score, were developed to be sensitive to dard measures of executive functioning.
executive dysfunction. These scores quantify the
approach used by the examinee when reproduc-
ing the figure. A poor organizational strategy METHOD
may result in a very fragmented approach, poor
placement of the figure on the page or elements Participants
within the figure, compromised integrity of the The sample consisted of 141 outpatients (66
figure, and a haphazard order in which the ele- women and 75 men) referred for neuropsychologi-
cal evaluation at the neuropsychology service of a
ments are drawn. The ROCF can also include
large urban university-based medical center. The
repetitions of elements within the figure, reflect- mean age of the sample was 44.1 years (SD = 14.2,
ing perseverative tendencies (Stern & Prohaska, range = 18-81), the mean number of years of edu-
1996). cation was 13.7 (SD = 3.0, range = 6-21), and 94%
Research on the BQSS has provided evidence of the sample was Caucasian. Patients represented
for its reliability and validity (Stern et al., 1994; a variety of diagnostic categories, including: Trau-
Stern et al., 1999). Preliminary support for the matic brain injury (25%), psychiatric illness
(12%), stroke/cerebrovascular disease (10%), de-
validity of the BQSS executive scores has also mentia (9%), seizure disorder (6%), chronic fa-
been established in studies of patient popula- tigue syndrome/fibromyalgia (5%), toxic/meta-
tions with known executive dysfunction. For bolic disorder (5%), brain neoplasm (4%), devel-
example, Mahurin, Eckert, Velligan, Hazelton, opmental disorder (3%), and a variety of other
and Miller (1997) found that specific BQSS ex- neurological/medical conditions (21%).
ecutive scores were significantly associated with
measures of functional abilities and cognitive Procedures
Demographic and medical information was ob-
executive skills among patients with schizophre- tained from medical records, and when possible,
nia. A recent study by Schreiber, Javorsky, Rob- directly confirmed by the patient and/or family
inson, and Stern (2000) found that BQSS execu- member(s). As part of a comprehensive neuropsy-
tive scores (i.e., Neatness, Planning, and Perse- chological evaluation, patients were examined us-
veration) successfully discriminated between ing the standardized procedures for the following
BQSS AS A MEASURE OF EXECUTIVE FUNCTIONING 615

executive tasks: Trail Making Test (TMT; Reitan Finally, the Digit Span Forward raw score from the
& Wolfson, 1985), Wisconsin Card Sorting Test Digit Span Subtest of the WMS-R (Wechsler,
(WCST; Heaton, 1981), Controlled Oral Word As- 1987) was also used as a discriminant validity
sociation Test (COWA; Benton, 1968), and the measure.
Similarities subtest from the WAIS-R (Wechsler, Participants were administered the Rey-Oster-
1981). Executive functioning tasks were selected rieth Complex Figure (ROCF) according to the
based on their common use in neuropsychological instructions in the BQSS test manual, using col-
assessment (Lezak, 1995) and their sensitivity for ored markers to track the order in which the pro-
detecting executive impairment (Stern & Prohaska, duction was drawn (Stern et al., 1999). Although
1996). One primary score was selected from each each participant received copy, immediate, and
task for use in this specific study. These included: delayed recall conditions, only the copy condition
TMT Part B Total Time, WCST Perseverative Re- was used for the purpose of this study because of
sponses, COWA Total Words, and Similarities the focus on executive aspects of visuoconstruc-
Raw Score. tion, and not on memory. All productions were
In order to examine the discriminant validity of scored by two trained research assistants using the
Downloaded by [University of Arizona] at 08:47 23 August 2013

the BQSS executive scores, as well as to assure comprehensive scoring criteria provided in the
that we were not merely examining generalized BQSS manual. The rater was blind to the scores on
severity of impairment, three additional variables the other neuropsychological measures. Interrater
were selected. The first was a retention score from reliability in our lab has been established and is
the Logical Memory Subtest of the Wechsler Me- generally excellent; for the scores examined in this
mory Scale Revised (WMS-R; Wechsler, 1987). study, the mean " was .81. The executive variables
Because organization can facilitate learning, the of interest for this study were the Planning, Frag-
percent of information retained from immediate to mentation, Neatness, and Perseveration qualitative
delayed recall (retention score) was used instead of scores, and the Organization summary score (see
the Logical Memory free recall scores. In addition, Table 1 for descriptions). The means, standard de-
the retention score (and not free recall score) from viations, and ranges for the measures used in this
the Visual Reproduction Subtest of the WMS-R study are presented in Table 2.
(Wechsler, 1987) was used for this same reason.

Table 1. Description of the Boston Qualitative Scoring System (BQSS) Executive Functioning Scores.

Score Description

Fragmentation Measures integration of information (i.e., whether or not the individual ele-
ments are drawn as whole units). Range of scores: 0 (extreme fragmentation)
to 4 (no fragmentation).
Planning Measures overall planning ability based on the order in which elements are
drawn, placement on the page, placement within the figure, and overall integ-
rity of the production. Range of scores: 0 (extremely poor planning) to 4 (good
planning).
Neatness Rates how neatly the figure was drawn as evidenced by the number of wavy
lines, gaps and overshoots, cross-outs, rounded corners, etc. Range of scores:
0 (extremely messy) to 4 (very neat).
Perseveration Measures the extent of recognizably inappropriate repetition. May take one of
two forms: Repetition of components within a cluster or replication of an ele-
ment of the figure (Configural, Cluster, or Detail). Range of scores: 0 (extreme
perseveration) to 4 (no perseveration).
Organization Summary Score Arithmetic sum of the copy condition Fragmentation and Planning scores, pro-
viding a more omnibus measure of organizational skills. Range of scores: 0
(extremely poor organization) to 8 (good organization).

Note. Reproduced by special permission of the Publisher, Psychological Assessment Resources, Inc., 16204
North Florida Avenue, Lutz, Florida, 33549, from the Boston Qualitative Scoring System for the Rey-Osterrieth
Complex Figure Professional Manual, by Robert A. Stern, Ph.D., et al, Copyright 1994, 1996, 1998 by PAR, Inc.
Further reproduction is prohibited without permission of PAR, Inc.
616 JESSICA SOMERVILLE ET AL.

Table 2. Descriptive Data for the BQSS Executive Scores and Other Neuropsychological Measures Used in the
Present Study.

N M (SD) Range
BQSS Scores
Fragmentation 141 2.75 (0.96) 1-4
Planning 141 2.26 (1.02) 0-4
Neatness 141 2.00 (0.91) 0-4
Perseveration 141 3.20 (1.21) 0-4
Organization 141 5.01 (1.70) 1-8
Other Measures
WCST-PR 141 21.12 (19.85) 3-114
TMT-B 141 101.91 (72.30) 34-360
COWA 141 35.00 (12.76) 5-83
Similarities 141 18.74 (4.88) 1-28
Downloaded by [University of Arizona] at 08:47 23 August 2013

Digits Forward 128 7.66 (2.19) 2-12


LM Retention 130 76.36 (24.63) 0-125
VR Retention 132 73.53 (28.66) 0-117

Note. BQSS = Boston Qualitative Scoring System. The BQSS Organization summary score ranges from 0 (very
poor) to 8 (very good). WCST-PR = Wisconsin Card Sorting Task-Perseverative Responses; TMT-B = Trail
Making Test-Part B; COWA = Controlled Oral Word Association-Total Words; Similarities = Similarities subtest
of the WAIS-R; Digits Forward = Digit Span subtest of the WMS-R, forward score; LM Retention = Logical
Memory Retention (Percent Retention from Immediate to Delayed Recall on the WMS-R Logical Memory
Subtest [Logical Memory II/Logical Memory I]); VR Retention = Visual Reproduction Retention (Percent Reten-
tion from Immediate to Delayed Recall on the WMS-R Visual Reproduction Memory Subtest [Logical Memory
II/Logical Memory I]).

RESULTS tween the BQSS variables and the executive


tests (mean correlation = .26) (note that the Or-
To investigate the convergent validity of the ganization score was excluded from the calcu-
BQSS scores, Pearson correlation coefficients lated mean correlations due to redundancy).
were derived between the five BQSS scores and Table 4 presents correlations between the ex-
the four executive test scores. Discriminant va- ecutive BQSS variables and the discriminant
lidity was assessed by comparing the BQSS ex- validity measures. The executive BQSS vari-
ecutive scores with the three variables presumed ables were less correlated with the discriminant
not to be direct measures of executive function- validity measures (average correlation = .16)
ing. In addition, the intracorrelations among the than with the executive measures (average cor-
BQSS scores and among the executive tests relation = .26). In fact, the executive BQSS
were calculated. measures were less correlated with the discri-
As depicted in Table 3, all five BQSS mea- minant measures than the other executive mea-
sures were significantly correlated with WCST sures were with the same discriminant measures
Perseverative Responses and with COWA Total (average correlation = .24).
Words. All BQSS scores, except the Fragmenta- To examine the clinical utility of the BQSS
tion score, were also significantly correlated for identifying patients with executive dysfunc-
with Trails B Time and with Similarities Raw tion, participants were placed into one of three
Score. In addition, the strengths of the intra- groups based on their performance on the execu-
correlations among the executive tests (mean tive measures used in the convergent validity
correlation = .37) were similar to the intra- analyses. The three groups were determined as
correlations among the BQSS variables (mean follows: (1) No Executive Impairment (no im-
correlation = .36), as well as those found be- paired executive tests; n = 45), (2) Mild Execu-
Downloaded by [University of Arizona] at 08:47 23 August 2013

Table 3. Correlations Between BQSS Scores and Executive Tests and Intracorrelations Among BQSS Variables and Executive Tests.

BQSS Executive Tests

Fragment Planning Neatness Persev Organiz1 WCST-PR TMT-B COWA Similar


BQSS Fragment –– .46*** .20* .34*** .85*** –.19* –.16 .31*** .16
Planning –– .24** .54*** .86*** –.30*** –.18* .44*** .25**
Neatness –– .38*** .23** –.26** –.29*** .26** .20*
Persev –– .52*** –.31*** –.21* .39*** .17*
Organiz –– –.28*** –.20* .44*** .24**
Executive Tests WCST-PR –– .19* –.46*** –.44***
TMT-B –– –.30*** –.37***
COWA –– –.44***
Similar ––

Note. BQSS = Boston Qualitative Scoring System. Fragment = Fragmentation; Persev = Perseveration; Organiz = Organization; WCST-PR = Wisconsin Card
Sorting Test-Perseverative Responses; TMT-B = Trail Making Test-Part B-Time; COWA = Controlled Oral Word Association-Total Words; Similar = Similarities
subtest of the WAIS-R.
1
The Organization score is the arithmetic sum of the Fragmentation score and the Planning score, thus accounting for the high correlations between Organization
and Fragmentation and Planning.
* p < .05; ** p < .01; *** p < .001.
BQSS AS A MEASURE OF EXECUTIVE FUNCTIONING
617
618 JESSICA SOMERVILLE ET AL.

Table 4. Correlations Between Discriminant Variables and BQSS Executive Variables and Between Discriminant
Variables and Other Executive Functioning Variables.

Digit Span Forward LM Retention VR Retention


BQSS Fragmentation .10 .05 .09
Planning .27** .19 .26**
Neatness .18* .14 .17
Perseveration .16 .21* .15
Organization .22* .15 .21*
Executive Measures WCST-PR –.22* –.25** –.33***
TMT-B –.19* –.08 –.23**
COWA .42*** .22* .25**
Similarities .25** .20* .23**

Note. BQSS = Boston Qualitative Scoring System; LM Retention = Logical Memory Retention (Percent Reten-
Downloaded by [University of Arizona] at 08:47 23 August 2013

tion from Immediate to Delayed Recall on the WMS-R Logical Memory Subtest [Logical Memory II/Logical
Memory I]). VR Retention = Visual Reproduction Retention (Percent Retention from Immediate to Delayed
Recall on the WMS-R Visual Reproduction Subtest (Visual Reproduction II/Visual Reproduction I). WCST-PR
= Wisconsin Card Sorting Test-Perseverative Responses. TMT-B = Trail Making Test-Part B-Time. COWA =
Controlled Oral Word Association-Total Words. Similarities = Similarities subtest of the WAIS-R.
* p < .05; ** p < .01; *** p < .001.

tive Impairment (1-2 impaired executive tests; n various aspects of executive functioning, includ-
= 73), and (3) Significant Executive Impairment ing the WCST, Trail Making Test Part B, and
(3-4 impaired executive tests; n = 23). A score the WAIS-R Similarities subtest. The BQSS ex-
was considered impaired if it fell one standard ecutive scores were generally unrelated to mea-
deviation or more below the published age sures of verbal and visual memory retention and
and/or education normative group. A one-way basic attentional skills, as measured by Logical
Analysis of Variance (ANOVA) for the BQSS Memory Retention, Visual Reproduction Reten-
Organization summary score, using the three tion, and the Digit Span Forward score, respec-
groups of executive impairment as the indepen- tively. These results provide preliminary support
dent variable, was statistically significant, for the convergent and discriminant validity of
F(2,138) = 11.0, p < .001. Figure 1 graphically the BQSS as a measure of the executive aspects
presents the results for each group. Post-hoc of ROCF performance.
analyses, using the Tukey HSD, revealed that The current results also demonstrate that the
patients in the No Executive Impairment group BQSS Organization summary score may be a
performed significantly better on the Organiza- clinically useful measure of executive dysfunc-
tion score (M = 5.8, SD = 1.6) than patients in tion. Patients with no executive impairment had
the Mild Executive Impairment group (M = 4.8, significantly better organization (i.e., good plan-
SD = 1.6), who in turn, performed significantly ning and less fragmentation) on the ROCF than
better than patients in the Significant Executive patients with mild executive dysfunction, who in
Impairment group (M = 3.9, SD = 1.6). turn, performed better than patients with severe
executive dysfunction, as measured by perfor-
mance on commonly used measures of executive
DISCUSSION functioning. To examine whether this finding
was merely a function of overall cognitive im-
The present study found that BQSS scores de- pairment (i.e., not specifically executive impair-
veloped to be sensitive to executive functioning ment), we performed the same analysis using the
were significantly related to performance on discriminant measures chosen for this study.
traditional neuropsychological tests measuring However, none of these measures (i.e., Digit
BQSS AS A MEASURE OF EXECUTIVE FUNCTIONING 619

6.0

Mean BQSS Organization Score


5.0

4.0

3.0

2.0

1.0
Downloaded by [University of Arizona] at 08:47 23 August 2013

0 1-2 3-4

Number of Impaired Executive Tests

Fig. 1. Mean Boston Qualitative Scoring System Organization Score (+SE) for the No Executive Impairment
group (no impaired executive tests; n = 45), the Mild Executive Impairment group (1-2 impaired execu-
tive tests; n = 73), and the Significant Executive Impairment group (3-4 impaired executive tests; n =
23). All group comparisons are significant (p < .05).

Span Forward, Logical Memory Retention, Vi- organizational strategies could differentiate pa-
sual Reproduction Retention) resulted in signifi- tients from healthy controls (Diamond et al.,
cant between-group differences, separating all 1997; Grossman et al., 1993; Savage et al.,
three groups (i.e., no, mild, and severe impair- 1999). The present results also confirm a previ-
ment). ous examination of ROCF qualitative scoring
These results, along with other previous re- systems (Troyer & Wishart, 1997) which dem-
search, empirically demonstrate that the ROCF onstrated the importance of assessing executive
can yield information about multiple aspects of functioning with the ROCF.
individuals’ neuropsychological functioning. Although the relationships between the BQSS
Given that the administration of the ROCF (us- scores and the executive measures were signifi-
ing BQSS scoring) can yield independent infor- cantly related, the correlations were not as
mation about visuoconstruction, visuospatial strong as typically desired for validity coeffi-
memory, and executive functioning, it appears cients and did not account for a large percentage
to be a valuable component of any neuropsycho- of shared variance. However, the correlations
logical evaluation, particularly with the time among the traditional tests of executive func-
limitations imposed by the current healthcare tioning were of similar magnitude, a finding also
fiscal environment. These findings also high- seen in other correlational studies of executive
light the importance of examining qualitative functioning (Boone, Ponton, Gorsuch, Gonzalez,
features of the ROCF and suggest that systems & Miller, 1998; Franzen & Petrick, 1995;
using only one quantitative visuoconstruction Humes, Welsh, Retzlaff, & Cookson, 1997; Per-
summary score may miss valuable information, riene, 1993; Troyer & Wishart, 1997).
such as the organizational strategy employed by In conclusion, results of the present study
the patient. The current findings are consistent provide preliminary evidence for the construct
with previous research indicating that ROCF validity of the BQSS executive scores. Previous
620 JESSICA SOMERVILLE ET AL.

research with the BQSS has also indicated the Binder, L.M. (1982). Constructional strategies on
utility of the BQSS as a measure of executive Complex Figure drawings after unilateral brain
damage. Journal of Clinical Neuropsychology, 4,
functioning by its ability to predict group mem-
51-58.
bership in patients with a variety of disorders Boone, K.B., Ponton, M.O., Gorsuch, R.L., Gonzalez,
associated with executive impairment, including J.J., & Miller, B.L. (1998). Factor analysis of four
ADHD (Cahn et al., 1996; Schreiber et al., measures of prefrontal lobe functioning. Archives
2000), schizophrenia (Mahurin et al., 1997), al- of Clinical Neuropsychology, 13, 585-595.
Cahn, D.A., Marcotte, A.C., Stern, R.A., Arruda, J.A.,
cohol abuse (Dawson & Grant, 2000), and trau- Akshoomoff, N.A., & Leshko, I.C. (1996). The
matic brain injury (Javorsky et al., 1999). Addi- Boston qualitative scoring system for the Rey-
tional research, however, is needed. For exam- Osterrieth Complex Figure: A study of children
ple, it would be particularly interesting to exam- with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. The
ine patients with focal frontal lesions compared Clinical Neuropsychologist, 10, 397-406.
Chervinsky, A.B., Mitrushina, M., & Satz, P. (1992).
to those with nonfrontal lesions. Additionally, Comparison of four methods of scoring the Rey-
Downloaded by [University of Arizona] at 08:47 23 August 2013

performance patterns of the various BQSS exec- Osterrieth Complex Figure Drawing Test on four
utive variables could be examined with various age groups of normal elderly. Brain Dysfunction,
patient groups. That is, it is possible that differ- 5, 267-287.
ent patient populations may exhibit specific pat- Chiulli, S.J., Halland, K.Y., LaRue, A., & Garry, P.J.
(1995). Impact of age on drawing the Rey-Oster-
terns of executive dysfunction (e.g., high perse- rieth figure. The Clinical Neuropsychologist, 9,
veration and poor organization, or extreme mes- 219-224.
siness and fragmentation, etc.). Other future re- Dawson, L.K., & Grant, I. (2000). Alcoholics’ initial
search could include the relationship between organizational and problem-solving skills predict
BQSS executive variables and other variables of learning and memory performance on the Rey-
Osterrieth Complex Figure. Journal of the Interna-
ROCF performance (i.e., visuoconstruction or tional Neuropsychological Society, 6, 12-19.
memory) and how they interact to result in the Diamond, B.J., DeLuca, J., & Kelley, S.M. (1997).
final production, and in subsequent recall. Addi- Memory and executive functions in amnesic and
tionally, the validity of each specific executive non-amnesic patients with aneurysms of the ante-
BQSS variable should be examined in future rior communicating artery. Brain, 120, 1015-1025.
Eslinger, P.J., & Grattan, L.M. (1990). Influence of
research. Overall, based on the current findings, organizational strategy on neuropsychological per-
the ROCF appears to be a useful clinical tool in formance in frontal lobe patients. Journal of Clini-
the assessment of executive dysfunction, though cal and Experimental Neuropsychology, 12, 54.
further studies are recommended to enhance the Franzen, M.D. & Petrick, J.D. (1995). Preliminary
qualitative interpretation of specific executive norms for Design Fluency. Paper presented to the
103rd meeting of the American Psychological As-
deficits. sociation, NY.
Grossman, M., Carvell, S., Peltzer, L., Stern, M.B.,
Gollump, S., & Hurtig, H.I. (1993). Visual con-
REFERENCES struction of impairments in Parkinson’s disease.
Neuropsychology, 7, 536-547.
Hamby, S.L., Wilkins, J.W., & Barry, N.S. (1993).
Bennett-Levy, J. (1984). Determinants of performance Organizational quality on the Rey-Osterrieth and
on the Rey-Osterrieth Complex Figure Test: An Taylor Complex Figure tests: A new scoring sys-
analysis, and a new technique for single-case as- tem. Psychological Assessment, 5, 27-33.
sessment. British Journal of Clinical Psychology, Heaton, R.K. (1981). Wisconsin Card Sorting Test
23, 109-119. Manual. Odessa, FL: Psychological Assessment
Benton, A.L. (1968). Differential behavioral effects in Resources.
frontal lobe disease. Neuropsychologia, 6, 53-60. Humes, G.E., Welsh, M.C., Retzlaff, P. & Cookson,
Bernstein, J.H. & Waber, D.P. (1996). Developmental N. (1997). Towers of Hanoi and London: Reliabil-
Scoring System for the Rey-Osterrieth Complex ity of two executive function tasks. Assessment, 4,
Figure. Odessa, FL: Psychological Assessment 249-257.
Resources.
BQSS AS A MEASURE OF EXECUTIVE FUNCTIONING 621

Javorsky, D.J., Rosenbaum, J., & Stern, R.A. (1999). formance in adults with attention deficit hyperac-
Utility of the Boston Qualitative Scoring System tivity disorder: A Validation study of the Boston
(BQSS) for the Rey-Osterrieth Complex Figure in Qualitative Scoring System. The Clinical Neuro-
the evaluation of traumatic brain injury [abstract]. psychologist, 13, 509-520.
Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 14, 789- Shorr, J.S., Delis, D.C., & Massman, P.J. (1992).
790. Memory for the Rey-Osterrieth Figure: Perceptual
Knight, J. & Kaplan, E. (in press). Rey-Osterrieth clustering, encoding, and storage. Neuropsychol-
Complex Figure Handbook. Odessa, FL: Psycho- ogy, 6, 43-50.
logical Assessment Resources, Inc. Silva, S.G, Stern, R.A., Chaisson, N., Singer, E.A.,
Lezak, M.D. (1995). Neuropsychological assessment Gaver, V., Watson, J.B., Golden, R.N., & Evan,
(3rd ed.). New York: Oxford University Press. D.L. (1995). Evidence of mild visuoconstructive
Loring, D.W., Lee, G.P., & Meador, K.J. (1988). Re- impairments in HIV infection using the Boston
vising the Rey-Osterrieth: Rating right hemisphere Qualitative Scoring System for the Rey-Osterrieth
recall. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 3, Complex Figure [Abstract]. Journal of the Interna-
239-247. tional Neuropsychological Society, 1, 138.
Mahurin, R., Eckert, S., Velligan, D., Hazelton, B., & Starkstein, S.E., & Robinson, R.G. (1997). Mecha-
Downloaded by [University of Arizona] at 08:47 23 August 2013

Miller. (1997). Rey figure as a predictor of every- nism of Disinhibition after brain lesions. The Jour-
day functional ability in schizophrenia [Abstract]. nal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 185, 108-114.
Journal of Neuropsychiatry and Clinical Neuro- Stern, R.A., Javorsky, D.J., Singer, E.A., Singer Har-
sciences, 9, 36-37. ris, N.G., Somerville, J.A., Duke, L.M., Thomp-
Meyers, J.E. & Meyers, K.R. (1995). Rey Complex son, J., & Kaplan, E. (1999). The Boston Qualita-
Figure Test and Recognition Trial. Odessa, FL: tive Scoring System for the Rey-Osterrieth Com-
Psychological Assessment Resources. plex Figure. Odessa, FL: Psychological Assess-
Osterrieth, P.A. (1944). Le test de copie d’une figure ment Resources.
complexe. Archives de Psychologie, 30, 206-356. Stern, R.A. & Prohaska, M.L. (1996). Neuropsycho-
Perrine, K. (1993). Differential aspects of conceptual logical evaluation of executive functioning. In
processing in the Category Test and Wisconsin Dickstein L.L., Riba, M.B., Oldham, J.M. (Eds.),
Card Sorting Test. Journal of Clinical and Experi- Review of Psychiatry, Vol.15 (pp. 243-266). Wash-
mental Neuropsychology, 15, 461-473. ington: American Psychiatric Press,.
Rapport, L.J., Farchione, T.J., Dutra, R.L., Webster, Stern, R.A., Singer, E.A., Duke, L.M., Singer, N.G.,
J.S., & Charter, R. (1996). Measures of hemi-inat- Morey, C.E., Daughtrey, E.W., & Kaplan, E.
tention on the Rey Figure copy by the Lezak- (1994). The Boston qualitative scoring system for
Osterrieth scoring method. The Clinical Neuropsy- the Rey-Osterrieth Complex Figure: Description
chologist, 10, 450-454. and interrater reliability. The Clinical Neuropsy-
Reitan, R.M., & Wolfson, D. (1985). The Halstead- chologist, 8, 309-322.
Reitan Neuropsychological Test Battery. Tucson, Troyer, A.K., & Wishart, H.A. (1997). A comparison
AZ: Neuropsychological Press. of qualitative scoring systems for the Rey-
Rey, A. (1941). L’examin psychologique dans les cas Osterrieth Complex Figure test. The Clinical Neu-
d’escephalopathie traumatique. Archives de ropsychologist, 11, 381-390.
Psychologie, 28, 286-340. Visser, R.S.H. (1973). Manual of the Complex Figure
Savage, C.R., Baer, L., Keuthen, N.J., Brown, H.D., Test. Lisse, Netherlands: Swets & Zeitlinger.
Rauch, S.L., & Jenike, M.A. (1999). Organiza- Wechsler, D. (1981). Manual for the Wechsler Adult
tional strategies mediate nonverbal memory im- Intelligence Scale – Revised. New York: Psycho-
pairment in obsessive-compulsive disorder. Bio- logical Corporation.
logical Psychiatry, 45, 905-916. Wechsler, D. (1987). Manual for the Wechsler Mem-
Schreiber, H.E., Javorsky, D.J., Robinson, J., & Stern, ory Scale – Revised. San Antonio, TX: Psychologi-
R.A. (1999). Rey-Osterrieth Complex Figure per- cal Corporation.

You might also like