You are on page 1of 7

Aggression and Violent Behavior 51 (2020) 101380

Contents lists available at ScienceDirect

Aggression and Violent Behavior


journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/aggviobeh

Executive dysfunction, violence and aggression T


a,⁎ b,c a
Ana Rita Cruz , Andreia de Castro-Rodrigues , Fernando Barbosa
a
Laboratory of Neuropsychophysiology, University of Porto, Rua Alfredo Allen, 4200-135 Porto, Portugal
b
Instituto Superior de Psicologia Aplicada (ISPA), Universitary Institute, Rua Jardim do Tabaco, nº341149-041 Lisboa, Portugal
c
Faculty of Law, University Lusíada North (Porto), Rua Dr. Lopo de Carvalho, 4369-006 Porto, Portugal

A R T I C LE I N FO A B S T R A C T

Keywords: Executive Functions (EFs) is an umbrella expression used for several processes and subprocesses encompassing
Executive functions goal-oriented behavior and decision-making strategies. However, the extent to which impaired EFs relate to and
Impulsivity characterize different types of criminal behavior and aggressiveness is not clear. Similarly, research on the
Aggression association between EFs and psychopathy has also produced mixed findings. The failure to disaggregate between
Violence
different types of crime and antisocial individuals can impede the identification of specific neurocognitive
Crime
Psychopathy
mechanisms associated with crime. We reviewed the extent to which EFs (according to Miyake latent variable
analysis) are associated with violent and non-violent crime, impulsive and premeditated aggression as well as
psychopathic traits.
Findings, although nonconsensual, suggest a more nuanced view on the association between EFs and crime,
suggesting that different executive domains might be more compromised in violent and impulsive individuals,
and specific psychopathy facets are more associated with executive dysfunction and criminality. Studies in line
with this approach will allow offenders to benefit from intervention strategies that will address their specific
deficits, optimizing their EFs and contributing to better cognitive management in demanding and complex si-
tuations, such as criminal and violent deterrence.

1. Introduction complications), (b) general upbringing (e.g., significant exposure to


violence, socioeconomic status and cultural background), (c) being
Aggressive behaviors affect social, economic, legal, and political physically and sexually abused, (d) parenting style and childhood
areas, with enormous costs associated with violence and incarceration. maltreatment (e.g., parental absence or early maternal rejection), (e)
From the countries in the Organization for Economic Cooperation genetic predisposition, (f) brain lesions, (g) early health risk factors, (h)
and Development (OECD), in 2018, Portugal occupied the middle po- peer groups, and (i) living in a low-income neighborhood (Bergeron &
sition regarding incarceration rates with 129 inmates per 100.000 in- Valliant, 2001; Broomhall, 2005; Friedman, Rhee, Ross, Corley, &
habitants (OECD, 2019). Statistics from the Portuguese prison system Hewitt, 2018; Hancock, Tapscott, & Hoaken, 2010; Ishikawa, Raine,
(2017) reported a total of 11,335 Portuguese convicted inmates (81.5% Lencz, Bihrle, & Lacasse, 2001; Pennuto, 2007; Raine, 2019; Volavka,
males) by the end of 2017. In Portugal, the crimes with higher pre- 1999). The former factors are influenced by variables such as age,
valence are property crimes (29.3%), but crimes against people come in gender differences, cognitive functioning, psychopathic facets, hyper-
a close second place (26%). Even though there is a considerable impact activity symptoms, psychopathology, social skills, attitudes, personality
of criminal behaviors on society, there is no consensus regarding the traits, and intelligence (Hancock, 2014; Mol, Van Den Bos, Derks, &
directionality of the relationship between aggressive behavior, crime, Egger, 2009; Ogilvie, Stewart, Chan, & Shumd, 2011; Paschall &
and related variables, such as aggressive patterns or psychopathy. Fishbein, 2002). The numerous factors related to aggression and po-
Throughout history, aggressive behavior has been analyzed as a tential criminality require a multiple factor approach based on the
behavioral expression, triggered in response to a perceived threat heterogeneity of criminal acts and human behaviors (Bergeron &
(Tirapu-Ustárroz, García Molina, Ríos-Lago, & Ardila, 2012). There are Valliant, 2001). Among those factors are the psychobiological ones, in
psychosocial explanations and intergenerational factors that may pre- which executive functions (EFs) are comprised.
dispose the individual to later (adult) aggression and violence, such as EFs can be defined as cognitive processes (although motivationally
(a) prenatal risk factors (e.g., fetal exposure to alcohol, obstetrical affected) that integrate ideas, thoughts, movements, and actions during


Corresponding author at: Faculty of Psychology and Educational Sciences of the University of Porto, Rua Alfredo Allen, 4200-135 Porto, Portugal.
E-mail addresses: anaritacruz@fpce.up.pt (A.R. Cruz), arodrigues@ispa.pt (A. de Castro-Rodrigues), fbarbosa@fpce.up.pt (F. Barbosa).

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.avb.2020.101380
Received 30 January 2019; Received in revised form 25 July 2019; Accepted 27 January 2020
Available online 15 February 2020
1359-1789/ © 2020 Published by Elsevier Ltd.
A.R. Cruz, et al. Aggression and Violent Behavior 51 (2020) 101380

complex situations that require solving problems or inhibiting in- Evidence of the relationship between executive dysfunction and
appropriate responses. Impairments in executive functioning are related subtypes of aggression comes from data that support significant asso-
to self-regulation problems and regulation of socially appropriate be- ciations between prefrontal dysfunction, as measured by neu-
havior, and so, individuals with executive deficits are more prone to ropsychological tests, and antisocial and aggressive behavior (Brower &
show aggressiveness and impulsive behavior (Ogilvie et al., 2011). Price, 2001; Paschall & Fishbein, 2002). Offenders tend to demonstrate
Executive dysfunction can, therefore, be associated with difficulties in lower executive skills, deficits in social abilities, lack of foresight, poor
attention control, impulsivity, rigidity, carelessness, planning goal-or- judgment, impulsivity, and immaturity. Therefore, their executive
iented behaviors (e.g., difficulty on setting goals), reduced capacity for functioning distinguishes them from non-offenders (Bergeron &
self-control, emotional lability, cognitive flexibility or adaptive orga- Valliant, 2001). Offenders also reveal marked deficits in their ability to
nization of information (Jackson, Loxton, Harnett, Ciarrochi, & Gullo, interpret social cues, empathic behavior, and problem-solving abilities
2014; Morgan & Lilienfeld, 2000; Ogilvie et al., 2011; Paschall & (Bergeron & Valliant, 2001). Nonetheless, according to Miyake's EFs
Fishbein, 2002). When in the presence of executive dysfunction, the model (2000), instead of a general executive dysfunction, offenders
individual tends to perseverate in one single strategy to solve problems could be impaired on a single executive domain, and not necessarily on
(Pino & Werlang, 2006), showing difficulties to self-regulate or monitor the other domains (e.g., updating deficits appear to be more prominent
the behavior in an appropriate manner (Paschall & Fishbein, 2002) in impulsive offenders). Additionally, executive functioning deficits
which can result in aggressive and violent actions. seem to be more noticeable on groups of individuals with persistent and
Our review aims to provide an overview of the research in the field severe behavioral problems (Barker et al., 2007; Greenfield & Valliant,
of EFs and crime by describing the extent to which EFs are associated to 2007; Morgan & Lilienfeld, 2000; Ogilvie et al., 2011). Offenders who
antisocial and criminal behavior, explicitly exploring whether this re- show executive impairments, perform worse on executive measures,
lationship might vary according to aggressiveness or psychopathy. and act more aggressively and violently (Dolan & Anderson, 2002;
Hoaken, Shaughnessy, & Pihl, 2003). Thus, it is plausible that this be-
2. Executive functioning, aggression, and violent criminality havior pattern is due, at least partly, to behavioral disinhibition, cog-
nitive rigidity, and difficulties with generating alternative non-ag-
Although the first references to executive functioning and ag- gressive responses (Giancola, Roth, & Parrott, 2006).
gressive and violent behavior were associated with the description of There is also evidence that recidivist inmates, regardless of offense
lesions and impairments in the prefrontal cortex, that is not the ob- levels, perform worse on EFs measures, and one possible explanation is
jective of this research. One of the aims of the current work is to revise a defective inhibition mechanism (cf. Ross & Hoaken, 2011). This may
the role of executive functioning in antisocial behaviors, particularly in be because first time convicted inmates were not exposed to the same
violent criminality, from a neuropsychological perspective (Deu, 1998; amount of risk factors, such as traumatic brain injury, persistent deviant
Dolan & Anderson, 2002; Hancock et al., 2010; Morgan & Lilienfeld, trajectory, substance abuse or detrimental prison impact (Ross &
2000; Ogilvie et al., 2011; Ross & Hoaken, 2011). Since these concepts Hoaken, 2011). Other explanation might be aging, since EFs are vul-
may be overlapped, it is important to distinguish them and clarify its nerable to the aging process. Because of that it might be more difficult
use in the literature. to prevent recidivism in older individuals. For example, the speed of
Humans have a predetermined ability to plan and imagine future processing decreases with age, increasing reaction times. Likewise,
situations and forecast the consequences of their conduct. This ability is lower IQ along with lower educational background, which often char-
a direct manifestation of intact executive functioning. EFs emerge from acterizes offenders' samples, are associated with poor executive per-
the association between situations that require a solution, and our ca- formance (Baker & Ireland, 2007).
pacity to plan and preview the outcome of our actions (Tirapu-Ustárroz Baker and Ireland (2007) collected longitudinal self-report data of
et al., 2012). EFs can be defined as cognitive processes (although mo- physical violence and theft on a sample of men and described that
tivationally affected) that integrate ideas, thoughts, movements, and physical violence and theft show different developmental trajectories
actions during complex situations that require solving problems or in- during adolescence and early adulthood. Particularly, individuals that
hibiting inappropriate responses. committed physical violence performed worse on tasks of verbal and
Executive functioning can be analyzed according to three domains, executive functioning, whereas those who committed theft showed
namely (a) Inhibition –deliberate inhibition of a dominant or prepotent better performance on both verbal and executive tasks. Additionally,
response; (b) Shifting – defined as the ability to perform a new opera- the developmental trajectories of offenders who committed physical
tion in the face of proactive interference; and, (c) Updating - char- violence and theft reflected different neurocognitive functioning. Ex-
acterized as the ability to actively hold information in mind, replacing ecutive dysfunction seems to have a stronger relationship with violent
old, no longer relevant information with newer, more relevant in- crimes, than with non-violent crimes (Ross & Hoaken, 2011). These
formation (Miyake et al., 2000). Accordingly, EFs are responsible not results suggest that those who engage in violent behaviors are more
only for regulating thoughts and actions, but also for promoting an impaired than non-violent offenders (Baker & Ireland, 2007; Barker
adaptive behavior, and consequently, have a crucial role in under- et al., 2007; Hancock, 2014; Miura, 2009). Therefore, across studies, it
standing violent behaviors. Executive processes are responsible for has been found that there is a consistent and robust association between
goal-directed behaviors, decision making, activation, and inhibition of performance on executive measures, and offensive and violent, or
actions (Ogilvie et al., 2011). Executive deficits are associated with simply aggressive behaviors (Baker & Ireland, 2007; Bergeron &
antisocial behavior (ASB), such as criminality, delinquency, physical Valliant, 2001; Broomhall, 2005; Ogilvie et al., 2011). Hence, executive
aggression, conduct disorders, as well as psychopathy and other per- dysfunction might be varying according to the type of offense and its
sonality disorders (Morgan & Lilienfeld, 2000). severity. Therefore, research comprising participants of forensic set-
Aggressive behavior is a multifaceted construct, defined as overt tings started to consider them also in terms of the severity of their
and intentional against another person, which may be expressed be- aggressive acts (violent or non-violent). (Hancock, 2014; Hancock
haviorally in different ways (Parrott & Giancola, 2007; Volavka, 1999). et al., 2010).
Aggression and violence, although used interchangeably, should also be However, other studies have compared the executive performance
accounted as two constructs, even if they are part of the same con- of violent and non-violent offenders and reported the existence of
tinuum (Allen & Anderson, 2015). Aggression requires the individual's considerable overlap between them (Greenfield & Valliant, 2007;
ability to act with the intent to cause harm to a second or third party. Hoaken, Allaby, & Earle, 2007; Pennuto, 2007; Robertson, Taylor, &
Violence is considered a severe form of aggression, aiming to cause Gunn, 1987). Hoaken et al. (2007) reported that both violent and non-
severe harm. violent offenders performed poorly on EF measures compared to

2
A.R. Cruz, et al. Aggression and Violent Behavior 51 (2020) 101380

controls, and violent offenders did not perform significantly worse than 3. Impulsive and premeditated aggression
non-violent ones. Furthermore, Pennuto (2007) only found minimal
significant differences in the inhibition/divided attention task when Regardless of the heterogeneity of aggressive behaviors (either
violent offenders were compared to non-violent offenders on measures violent or non-violent), these may be primarily impulsive or pre-
of EFs. meditated, each differing in their origins, as well as in their behavioral
Keeping the focus on violent crimes, although some offenders have a expression and underlying physiological mechanisms (Kockler,
markedly planned and predatory criminal trajectory, the majority have Stanford, Nelson, Meloy, & Sanford, 2006; e.g., Volavka, 1999). Some
a single act of unplanned, reactive violence (Hoaken et al., 2007). aggressive acts are rapid and unplanned reactions to external or in-
Therefore, different neurocognitive functions can contribute differently ternal stimuli, which result in aggressive outbursts, without regulation;
to the type of crime committed and the pattern of aggression re- some are directed toward acquiring power over others, oriented by
presented. Still, it is not consensual which criteria to adopt to identify goals with previous planning, displayed by an opportunistic offense, not
and classify offenders, because the focus could be on their attitudes requiring anger nor provocation (Cornell, 1996; Meloy, 2006).
(e.g., hostility), emotions, the impact of psychotropic substances, or the As impulsivity and premeditation are commonly considered the two
relationship with the victim (Hancock et al., 2010). Under these cir- main types of primary aggression, and since these two types of ag-
cumstances, in addition to the current crime, the classification of the gression have a different nature and may imply distinct levels of dan-
offenders must also consider the history of previous convictions (e.g., gerousness and arousal as well as responsivity to penal interventions,
Hoaken et al., 2007) and their aggressive patterns, namely if it is im- violent perpetrators can be classified as predominantly impulsive or
pulsive or premeditated, at least. predominantly premeditated (Houston, Stanford, Villemarette Pittman,
Executive dysfunction is a risk factor for committing violent crimes, Conklin, & Helfritz, 2003; Stanford et al., 2003; Volavka, 1999). There
which are characterized by higher impulsivity, tendencies to act out, are constraints regarding the definition of aggression patterns. Im-
violation of social norms, and disregard for others (Bergeron & Valliant, pulsive and premeditated aggression differ, for example, in the level of
2001; Crowell, Kieffer, Kugeares, & Vanderploeg, 2003; Hoaken et al., planning. Impulsive individuals tend to react in response to intense
2007). The inability to modify behavior in response to environmental emotional stimuli, fail to control their impulses and correctly analyze
changes combined with inhibition deficits could contribute to sig- contextual cues, and do not account for possible negative consequences
nificant difficulties in social situations, leading to increased violent of their actions on themselves or others (Moeller, Barratt, Dougherty,
responses. EFs determine foresight capability, planning, previewing Schmitz, & Swann, 2001). Typically, their aggressive outbursts are
ability, and impulse regulation that, when impaired, constitute a risk emotionally charged (Villemarette-Pittman, Stanford, & Greve, 2003),
factor for criminality. There is a high correlation between impulsivity highly spontaneous (Broomhall, 2005), uncontrolled (Raine et al.,
and aggression among offenders' samples (e.g., impulsivity correlates 1998), unpredictable, and occur in response to a perceived threat or
with measures of response inhibition). Additionally, individuals from danger (Meloy, 2006). Hence, impulsive aggression is also known as
those samples also demonstrate deficits in executive memory (Dolan & reactive, affective, emotional, expressive, or not planned (Meloy,
Anderson, 2002). Also, there is a high prevalence of traumatic and 2006). The aggressiveness shown is usually out of proportion regarding
acquired brain injuries in prisoners' samples, which often result in the psychosocial stimuli that caused it and constituted a risk factor for
worse executive performance by those individuals (Hancock et al., involvement in ASB (Villemarette-Pittman et al., 2003).
2010; Marsh & Martinovich, 2006; Ross & Hoaken, 2011). In fact, “impulsive individuals are at greater risk for dropping out of
As EFs coordinate higher-order control mechanisms of thought, school, engaging in criminal activities, and seeking out more stimu-
action, and emotion, deficits in these domains may express a biological lating environments” (Villemarette-Pittman et al., 2003, p. 1534). Im-
vulnerability for aggression (Bufkin & Luttrell, 2005; Fabian, 2010). It pulsive offenders demonstrate poor cognitive control and flexibility,
is not surprising that EFs often appear to be compromised in prisoners' which in turn impairs their ability to understand possible alternative
samples. Inmates tend to show deficits in cognitive supervisory capacity behaviors, increasing the probability of recurring actions (Broomhall,
(Dores et al., 2014), and in the maintenance of appropriate social and 2005; Deu, 1998). As Bergeron and Valliant (2001) explain, the “ten-
intimate relationships, fundamental in the management of daily life dency toward action coupled with impulsivity implies that offenders do
activities, such as administering finances, maintaining a job or dealing not think through conceivable outcomes of a given situation or consider
adequately with provocative interpersonal situations (Hancock et al., the ramifications of their actions” (p. 39). Under these circumstances,
2010). their ability to plan is compromised. Additionally, their impulsivity
Regardless of methodological limitations, namely (a) poor specifi- level may correlate with neuropsychological impairments, resulting in a
cation of EFs, (b) small sample size, (c) lack of prospective data, (d) worse performance on complex, speeded tests of planning and decision
absence of a control group or difficulties in the selection of a compar- making (Deu, 1998; Pietrzak, Sprague, & Snyder, 2008; Raine et al.,
ison group, (e) failure to control for confounding factors, (f) the evi- 1998), along with interpersonal aggression and violent crime (Hoaken
dence that not all individuals characterized as antisocial or with con- et al., 2003).
duct disorder show executive impairments, and (g) the possibility that According to some authors (e.g., Dolan & Anderson, 2002; Kockler &
there may be EFs differences even within criminals that committed the Stanford, 2008), working memory may play a significant role in im-
same type of crime (Ogilvie et al., 2011; e.g., Ross & Hoaken, 2011), pulsivity among all executive domains. Memory is fundamental for
executive impairments are a plausible and well documented explana- searching for regularities and similarities between situations; it links
tion for criminal conducts. past and future, and is decisive for the course of action to follow
In summary, available evidence seems enough to suggest that ex- (Kockler & Stanford, 2008). Poor inhibitory control accounts for the
ecutive deficits are more common in individuals who engage in anti- decrease in the ability to store and maintain the information, leading to
social behavior, particularly in violent criminal behavior when com- poor emotional and impulse regulation that in turn would also con-
pared to non-violent offenders. Such deficits also seems to characterize tribute to a defective working memory functioning (Hoppenbrouwers
male teenagers and young adults with persistent antisocial behavior et al., 2013). Impulsive individuals are more prone to memory im-
during their lives (Raine et al., 2005). However, more research is ne- pairments, lacking the ability to follow an action-oriented course to-
cessary on the executive functioning differences between violent and ward the future and take appropriate decisions based on previous ex-
non-violent offenders, as well as between violent offenders of distinct periences, unable to learn from their mistakes (Bergeron & Valliant,
types. 2001; Deu, 1998; Dolan & Anderson, 2002; Kockler & Stanford, 2008;
Syngelaki, Moore, Savage, Fairchild, & Van Goozen, 2009). On the
contrary, lower levels of impulsivity are correlated to better working

3
A.R. Cruz, et al. Aggression and Violent Behavior 51 (2020) 101380

Fig. 1. Interrelations among phenotypic constructs of psychopathy (adapted from Barbosa, unpublished work; Patrick et al., 2009).

memory (Kockler & Stanford, 2008). However, various studies (e.g., (Sellbom & Phillips, 2013; Viding, 2004). Psychopathic individuals are
Hoaken et al., 2007; Meijers, Harte, Jonker, & Meynen, 2015) also re- more susceptible to adopt violent, antisocial and risk behaviors, as well
port that other EFs besides working memory, such as set-shifting, ap- as to relapse in such behaviors (Woodworth & Porter, 2002), and are
pear to be impaired in both violent and non-violent offenders. responsible for a large number of violent crimes (Patrick, Fowles, &
On the contrary, premeditation requires impulse control, planning, Krueger, 2009; Reidy, Shelley-Tremblay, & Lilienfeld, 2011). In-
forethought, and orienting behavior toward a goal (e.g., bullying, ob- dividuals with high levels of psychopathy present a distinct profile of
taining money, restoring one's self-image, securing power and dom- offending, and their neurocognitive features indicate problems in pro-
inance over others or controlling criminal activities), and is in- cessing distress in others and punishment directed to oneself (Viding,
tentionally executed (Brower & Price, 2001; Cooke, Michie, De Brito, 2004). Moreover, in addition to the differences between psychopathic
Hodgins, & Sparkes, 2011; Villemarette-Pittman et al., 2003). Pre- and non-psychopathic offenders regarding their criminal behavior,
meditated aggression is also mentioned as instrumental, proactive, important distinctions occur inside the former group.
predatory, and cold-blooded (Cornell, 1996; Mathias et al., 2007; Psychopathy has been recently conceptualized as a multi-
Meloy, 2006). Instrumental aggression is goal-driven and is usually dimensional construct, thus describing a heterogeneous group (Mol
directed toward a stranger (Cornell, 1996). It becomes clear that intact et al., 2009), with different offending patterns. According to the
EFs in this group of offenders is essential to correctly respond to a triarchic model (Patrick et al., 2009), psychopathy is considered a
problematic situation or making appropriate changes based on con- multivariate construct and comprises three distinct phenotypes:
textual and social contingencies (Giancola et al., 2006). For example, meanness, boldness, and disinhibition.
intact ability to switch flexibly between tasks or mental sets (Miyake & Meanness comprises lack of empathy, callousness, emotional de-
Friedman, 2012), i.e., intact cognitive and behavioral flexibility, might tachment, active exploitativeness, excitement seeking, rebelliousness,
be significant features that describe premeditated individuals. These instrumental or predatory aggression, abuse of others, destructiveness,
individuals seem to be more focused on their goals and are more mal- and empowerment through cruelty (Patrick, 2010; Patrick et al., 2009;
leable and better equipped to adapt to new contextual cues or situa- Venables, Hall, & Patrick, 2014). Boldness includes the capacity to re-
tions. main calm and concentrated in stressful situations and to recover from
When compared to impulsive offenders, who show executive defi- those events quickly. Finally, disinhibition reflects an externalizing
cits (Villemarette-Pittman et al., 2003), previous studies suggest that component of psychopathy, related to deviant behavioral problems in
premeditated individuals do not demonstrate EFs difficulties, at least in child and adult populations, and substance addictions (Patrick et al.,
activities that require planning or abstract thinking (Broomhall, 2005; 2009).
Haberle, 2011). However, more research is necessary to explore how Although interrelated, these phenotypes can be measured sepa-
different types of aggression relate to different executive capabilities rately, and exhibit distinct psychological and neurobiological referents
since these two types of aggression have different nature and may imply (Patrick et al., 2009), as illustrated by Figure 1.
distinct levels of dangerousness, as well as distinct levels of responsivity For example, disinhibition might have a deterrent effect on an in-
to penal interventions. dividuals ability to switch its cognitive mindset, resulting in a risk
factor for responding inappropriately in complex social interactions.
Disinhibition is more related to impulsive, violent, callous, reactive
4. EFs and psychopathy
forms of aggression, and is associated with the abnormal functioning of
the prefrontal cortex and anterior cingulate cortex, which coordinates
Commonly defined as a personality disorder, psychopathy has been
behavior and regulates emotional responses (Patrick, 2010). As a result,
inherently associated with antisocial and criminal behaviors. Deviant
poor executive functioning mostly maps onto the phenotype of disin-
traits, such as callousness, lack of remorse, egocentricity, pathological
hibition (Patrick et al., 2009). Therefore, higher disinhibition scores
lying and deception, lack of empathy and concern for others, absence of
might be associated with an increased individual predisposition to en-
guilt, impulsiveness, and ASB are characteristics of psychopathic in-
gage in antisocial behaviors in general and aggressive behaviors in
dividuals (Glenn, Kurzban, & Raine, 2011). These individuals are often
particular, either violent or non-violent.
motivated by a personal gain to manipulate, harm, and exploit others

4
A.R. Cruz, et al. Aggression and Violent Behavior 51 (2020) 101380

However, externalizing tendencies are not synonymous of psycho- 1. differences in the definition of the concept of psychopathy (e.g., a
pathy. The “diagnosis” of psychopathy would be suitable only when single broad syndrome vs. lower-level trait dimensions);
paired with other phenotypic features. I.e., higher scores on these 2. the tasks adopted may not be tapping the same EFs domains;
groups might reflect different outcomes and psychopathic paths 3. variations in group composition and comparison groups used (e.g.,
(Patrick et al., 2009): unsuccessful psychopaths tend to score higher on sampling characteristics, including the use of antisocial versus
both meanness and disinhibition facets (Patrick & Drislane, 2015). healthy comparison groups);
However, successful psychopaths might result from the combination of 4. no control of confounding variables (e.g., age, IQ, substance abuse,
higher scores on meanness and boldness (Venables et al., 2014; Wall, comorbid psychiatric disorders, socioeconomic status);
Wygant, & Sellbom, 2015). Additionally, each phenotypic construct 5. measures of psychopathy used (e.g., reduced sensitivity and/or
integrates diverse manifestations of the psychopathic personality and specificity of the tests).
correlates with other self-report approaches of the psychopathic per-
sonality (Sellbom & Phillips, 2013). For example, disinhibition and So far, it has discussed the inconsistencies in the relationship be-
meanness are depicted as moderately interrelated, and the combination tween psychopathy and EFs. Those could be due, as referred in point
of the two would result in an increased vulnerability to the criminal five above, to the use of different and/or broad measures of psycho-
psychopathy; higher scores in disinhibition may play a critical con- pathy, and even to distinct EFs assessed. Nevertheless, the association
tribution to an unsuccessful pattern. In turn, the occurrence of suc- between psychopathic facets and executive functioning could help to
cessful psychopathy would be better explained by higher scores on explain the differences between individuals and their criminal offenses.
boldness and meanness, and fewer externalization features (Patrick For example, it is expected that unsuccessful psychopaths demonstrate
et al., 2009). lower performance on executive functions and different aggression
Psychopathy has been presented as a robust predictor of criminal patterns. Notably, it is expected to find a predominant pattern of im-
violence (Salekin, Rogers, & Sewell, 1996) and recidivism (Andrade, pulsive aggression in the externalizing type, in comparison to a pre-
2008; Mol et al., 2009; Salekin et al., 1996; Woodworth & Porter, dominant pattern of premeditated aggression in the fearless type.
2002), associated with more contacts with the criminal justice system, Existing evidence on these predictions is still scarce, demanding further
and more violent incidents throughout incarceration (Andrade, 2008; studies to test them.
Hakkanen-Nyholm & Hare, 2009). Psychopathic individuals present Besides the links between the variables mentioned above, which are
higher rates of criminal and violent recidivism and are more likely to be currently being demonstrated, this revision also calls attention to the
convicted of violent offenses than their non-psychopathic counterparts lack of knowledge that we still have, in terms of understanding the
(Andrade, 2008). multiple relations between the distinct types of psychopathy, different
Also, the literature suggests a link between psychopathic traits, patterns of aggression, and the resulting criminal offenses. To deepen
aggression, and executive dysfunction (Blair et al., 2006; Woodworth & this knowledge, using the EFs as a core element, is essential to develop
Porter, 2002). Regardless, it is not consensual whether psychopathic more adequate assessment procedures, and to better intervene with
individuals, or which psychopathic individuals, perform poorly on these individuals in their particularities, in order to prevent crime and
(which) measures of EF, display minimal impairments, or present def- criminal recidivism.
icits in particular EFs components, such as inhibition (cf. Bagshaw,
Gray, & Snowden, 2014; Baskin-Sommers et al., 2015; Ogilvie et al., 5. Discussion and conclusions
2011). Neuropsychological approaches demonstrate that executive
dysfunction may be considered a marker for criminal behavior in The variables contributing to criminality in general, and aggressive
general, and this would explain why violent and non-violent offenders, behavior, are multi-deterministic (Bergeron & Valliant, 2001). There is
with or without psychopathy, might not reveal EFs differences (Barbosa not a single approach nor any dichotomic relation between variables
& Monteiro, 2008). Still, criminal psychopaths show interference and that might explain aggressive behaviors (Broomhall, 2005). Therefore,
inhibitory deficits in neuropsychological measures (Pham, a more sophisticated approach to violent criminality should account for
Vanderstukken, Philippot, & Vanderlinden, 2003). Accordingly, im- numerous predisposing factors for committing a crime.
pulsive violence was found to be significantly related to antisocial fa- Executive functions are essential for behavior regulation, but are an
cets of psychopathy, including executive impairments, while pre- overarching and complex construct, which implies several cognitive
meditated violence was significantly related to interpersonal-affective processes and subprocesses. Hence, addressing executive functions in
facets of psychopathy, and conversely, these last individuals may show their relation with aggression features, such as impulsivity and pre-
better executive performance (Broomhall, 2005; Mol et al., 2009). meditation, or psychopathic traits, in offenders is essential for planning
In recent years, studies on psychopathic features have been ex- appropriate assessment, treatment, and management strategies (Haden
tended beyond individuals who repeatedly engage in antisocial or & Shiva, 2008).
violent acts. Current approaches consider that the psychopathic fea- Offenders are commonly characterized by a pattern of executive
tures would account for more than a behavioral expression of aggres- dysfunction in the available literature (Meijers et al., 2015; Ross &
sion/violence (Patrick et al., 2009). “Successful” criminal psychopaths Hoaken, 2011), even when the approach to the EFs is not replicable.
seem to escape from a conviction in comparison to unsuccessful ones The difficulty that offenders demonstrate in tasks taping working
(Ishikawa et al., 2001). Despite demonstrating ASB, successful criminal memory, cognitive flexibility, and inhibition of inappropriate responses
psychopaths exhibit intact prefrontal functioning, including planning may have relevant implications in their ability to adapt to a changing
abilities and adaptive behavior regulation (Mol et al., 2009) performing environment, and consequently influence their scholar, work, and
better on EFs measures than unsuccessful convicted psychopaths, and personal achievements throughout a lifetime (Hancock et al., 2010).
even controls (Crowell et al., 2003; Ishikawa et al., 2001). Due to such executive impairments, as rigid behavior and impulsive
Despite the existence of commonalities across studies with prisoners' outbursts, it is not surprising that offenders, at least to some degree and
samples, divergent findings have been reported within these groups regardless of their crimes, would show executive deficits (Hoaken et al.,
(e.g., Pham et al., 2003). Such divergences are seemingly due to 2007). However, heterogeneity regarding offenders' characteristics and
methodological inconsistencies across studies (Bagshaw et al., 2014; executive deficits makes challenging to identify a pattern of executive
Baskin-Sommers et al., 2015; Dolan, 2012; Dolan & Anderson, 2002; impairments among different types of criminals.
Ishikawa et al., 2001; Mol et al., 2009; Ogilvie et al., 2011; Pham et al., Thus, although executive dysfunction may be associated with
2003), namely: criminality in general, it seems to show a stronger link to violent of-
fenses (cf., Hoaken et al., 2007; Ogilvie et al., 2011). It has been

5
A.R. Cruz, et al. Aggression and Violent Behavior 51 (2020) 101380

previously demonstrated that violent offenders exhibit executive dys- first author. The study was also supported by Grant SFRH/BPD/
function (Cooke et al., 2011; Hancock et al., 2010), mainly if the offense 108602/2015 from the Portuguese Foundation for Science and
is also characterized by an impulsive pattern of aggression (Broomhall, Technology awarded to Andreia de Castro-Rodrigues.
2005). Violent offenders show deficits in identifying, pursuing, and
evaluating their progress on tasks, and consequently achieving desired Declaration of competing interest
outcomes, revealing self-regulation and self-control difficulties. Speci-
fically, this class of offenders seems to have significantly impaired their None.
ability to monitor, code and manipulate incoming information (Miyake
et al., 2000), and to disregard no longer relevant cues, replacing it by References
more recent and relevant ones (Fisk & Sharp, 2004). Aggressive in-
dividuals also demonstrate the inability to use feedback cues to regulate Allen, J. J., & Anderson, C. A. (2015). The Wiley handbook of violence and aggression.
Wiley-Blackwell.
their behavior and make quick decisions, which in turn increases the
Andrade, J. T. (2008). The inclusion of antisocial behavior in the construct of psycho-
likelihood of an aggressive disproportionate response to provocation pathy: A review of the research. Aggression and Violent Behavior, 13(4), 328–335.
(Hoaken et al., 2003). Their inaccurate interpretation of environmental https://doi.org/10.1016/j.avb.2008.05.001.
Bagshaw, R., Gray, N. S., & Snowden, R. J. (2014). Executive function in psychopathy:
cues is typically associated with hostility (Hoaken et al., 2007). Con- The tower of London, Brixton spatial anticipation and the Hayling sentence com-
sequently, in the presence of impairments in executive domains, the pletion tests. Psychiatry Research, 220(1–2), 483–489. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.
behavior becomes deregulated and disinhibited, increasing the pro- psychres.2014.07.031.
Baker, S. F., & Ireland, J. L. (2007). The link between dyslexic traits, executive func-
pensity to risk-taking behaviors. On the other hand, intact executive tioning, impulsivity and social self-esteem among an offender and non-offender
functioning, and consequently better impulse control may act as a de- sample. International Journal of Law and Psychiatry, 30(6), 492–503. https://doi.org/
10.1016/j.ijlp.2007.09.010.
terrent factor on offenders, thus leading to a potential decrease in Barbosa, M. F. S., & Monteiro, L. M. C. (2008). Recurrent criminal behavior and executive
violent criminal activity, and consequently less incarceration and re- dysfunction. The Spanish Journal of Psychology, 11(1), 259–265. https://doi.org/10.
cidivism rates. 1017/S1138741600004297.
Barker, E. D., Séguin, J. R., White, H. R., Bates, M. E., Lacourse, E., Carbonneau, R., &
Additionally, the relation between EFs, violent crime, and im- Tremblay, R. E. (2007). Developmental trajectories of male physical violence and
pulsivity have been extensively examined, with studies investigating theft: Relations to neurocognitive performance. Archives of General Psychiatry, 64(5),
specific executive domains in violent and non-violent criminality, and 592–599. https://doi.org/10.1001/archpsyc.64.5.592.
Baskin-Sommers, A. R., Brazil, I. A., Ryan, J., Kohlenberg, N. J., Neumann, C. S., &
their potential effects on impulsive aggression. However, executive Newman, J. P. (2015). Mapping the association of global executive functioning onto
dysfunction might not adequately explain other forms of aggressive diverse measures of psychopathic traits. Personality Disorders: Theory, Research, and
Treatment, 6(4), 336–346. https://doi.org/10.1037/per0000125.
behavior (specifically premeditated aggression). A pattern of impaired Bergeron, T. K., & Valliant, P. M. (2001). Executive function and personality in adolescent
or normal functioning of certain executive domains may constitute an and adult offenders vs. non-offenders. Journal of Offender Rehabilitation, 33(3), 27–45.
important psychobiological correlate of distinct personality structures, https://doi.org/10.1300/J076v33n03_02.
Blair, K. S., Newman, C., Mitchell, D. G. V., Richell, R. A., Leonard, A., Morton, J., & Blair,
namely psychopathic facets of personality, which in turn predispose R. J. R. (2006). Differentiating among prefrontal substrates in psychopathy:
either to dominantly reactive or instrumental forms of aggression. It Neuropsychological test findings. Neuropsychology, 20(2), 153–165. https://doi.org/
10.1037/0894-4105.20.2.153.
appears that executive dysfunction depends differentially on diverse
Broomhall, L. (2005). Acquired sociopathy: A neuropsychological study of executive
manifestations of criminal and aggressive behaviors, and it is suggested dysfunction in violent offenders. Psychiatry, Psychology and Law, 12(2), 367–387.
that offenders demonstrate an executive performance distinct to in- https://doi.org/10.1375/pplt.12.2.367.
Brower, M. C., & Price, B. H. (2001). Neuropsychiatry of frontal lobe dysfunction in
dividuals from community samples. The previous findings highlight the violent and criminal behaviour: A critical review. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery,
importance of more homogeneity in this field of research. Particularly, and Psychiatry, 71(6), 720–726. https://doi.org/10.1136/jnnp.71.6.720.
to cover some of the identified shortcomings, studies should include Bufkin, J. L., & Luttrell, V. R. (2005). Neuroimaging studies of aggressive and violent
behavior: Current findings and implications for criminology and criminal justice.
control samples, specify groups of offenders, and distinguish these ac- Trauma, Violence & Abuse, 6(2), 176–191. https://doi.org/10.1177/
cording to their aggressive pattern. Besides, authors should include 1524838005275089.
Cooke, D., Michie, C., De Brito, S., Hodgins, S., & Sparkes, L. (2011). Measuring life-long
well-validated measures for both EFs and aggression and replicate the patterns of instrumental aggression: A methodological note. Psychology, Crime & Law,
experimental designs with different types of offenders, especially in 17(4), 37–41. https://doi.org/10.1080/10683160903203953.
what concerns psychopathic traits. In order to do so it is required a new Cornell, D. G. (1996). Coding guide for violent incidents: Instrumental versus hostile/reactive
aggression.
meta-analysis based on the Ogilvie and collaborators work of 2011. Crowell, T. A., Kieffer, K. M., Kugeares, S., & Vanderploeg, R. D. (2003). Executive and
This future work should expand previous findings by addressing the nonexecutive neuropsychological functioning in antisocial personality disorder.
relationship between the different dimensions of aggressive and violent Cognitive and Behavioral Neurology, 16(2), 100–109. Retrieved from http://
europepmc.org/abstract/med/12799596.
behaviors with EFs. Deu, N. (1998). Executive function and criminal fantasy in the premeditation of criminal
In conclusion, the inconsistency of results between studies reveals behaviour. Criminal Behaviour and Mental Health, 8(S1), 41–50. https://doi.org/10.
1002/cbm.283.
that a unique neuropsychological explanation for the relationship be- Dolan, M. (2012). The neuropsychology of prefrontal function in antisocial personality
tween aggression and EFs is improbable. Even though aggressive of- disordered offenders with varying degrees of psychopathy. Psychological Medicine,
fenders tend to evidence a global executive deficit, the presence of 42(08), 1715–1725. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291711002686.
Dolan, M., & Anderson, I. M. (2002). Executive and memory function and its relationship
impulsivity or other factors, such as low education, might also explain to trait impulsivity and aggression in personality disordered offenders. The Journal of
and influence these poor EFs results. Here we tried to clarify the EFs Forensic Psychiatry, 13(3), 503–526. https://doi.org/10.1080/
0958518021000019452.
domains that are possibly more impaired in different types of offenders.
Dores, A. R., Carvalho, I. P., Barbosa, F., Martins, C., de Sousa, L., & Castro-Caldas, A.
It is crucial to improve our understanding on the phenomenology of (2014). Conceptualization and rehabilitation of executive functions. European
executive deficits in the offenders' populations, which will help form Psychologist, 19(4), 269–277. https://doi.org/10.1027/1016-9040/a000196.
Fabian, J. M. (2010). Neuropsychological and neurological correlates in violent and ho-
better diagnostic and treatment policies. Hopefully, in the future, these micidal offenders: A legal and neuroscience perspective. Aggression and Violent
individuals may benefit from intervention strategies that will address Behavior, 15(3), 209–223. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.avb.2009.12.004.
their personality features and specific deficits, optimizing their EFs and Fisk, J. E., & Sharp, C. A. (2004). Age-related impairment in executive functioning:
Updating, inhibition, shifting, and access. Journal of Clinical and Experimental
contributing to better cognitive control in demanding and complex si- Neuropsychology, 26(7), 874–890. https://doi.org/10.1080/13803390490510680.
tuations (Strobach, Salminen, Karbach, & Schubert, 2014). Friedman, N. P., Rhee, S. H., Ross, J. M., Corley, R. P., & Hewitt, J. K. (2018). Genetic and
environmental relations of executive functions to antisocial personality disorder
symptoms and psychopathy. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 48(6),
Funding 470–471. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2018.12.007.
Giancola, P. R., Roth, R. M., & Parrott, D. J. (2006). The mediating role of executive
functioning in the relation between difficult temperament and physical aggression.
This work was supported by The Poruguese Foundation for Science Journal of Psychopathology and Behavioral Assessment, 28(4), 211–221. https://doi.
and Technology (Grant number SFRH/BD/76062/2011) awarded to the org/10.1007/s10862-005-9015-4.

6
A.R. Cruz, et al. Aggression and Violent Behavior 51 (2020) 101380

Glenn, A. L., Kurzban, R., & Raine, A. (2011). Evolutionary theory and psychopathy. Parrott, D. J., & Giancola, P. R. (2007). Addressing “the criterion problem” in the as-
Aggression and Violent Behavior, 16(5), 371–380. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.avb.2011. sessment of aggressive behavior: Development of a new taxonomic system. Aggression
03.009. and Violent Behavior, 12(3), 280–299. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.avb.2006.08.002.
Greenfield, R., & Valliant, P. M. (2007). Moral reasoning, executive function, and per- Paschall, M. J., & Fishbein, D. H. (2002). Executive cognitive functioning and aggression:
sonality in violent and nonviolent adult offenders. Psychological Reports, 101(1), A public health perspective. Aggression and Violent Behavior, 7(3), 215–235. https://
323–333. https://doi.org/10.2466/pr0.101.1.323-333. doi.org/10.1016/S1359-1789(00)00044-6.
Haberle, S. (2011). The role of the prefrontal cortex in the expression of impulsive- and pre- Patrick, C. J. (2010). Operationalizing the triarchic conceptualization of psychopathy:
meditated - aggression. University of Tasmania. Preliminary description of brief scales for assessment of boldness, meanness, and
Haden, S. C., & Shiva, A. (2008). Trait impulsivity in a forensic inpatient sample: An disinhibition. Tallahassee, FL. Retrieved from https://www.phenxtoolkit.org/toolkit_
evaluation of the barratt impulsiveness scale. Behavioral Sciences & the Law, 26(6), content/supplemental_info/psychiatric/measures/Triarchic_Psychopathy_Measure_
675–690. https://doi.org/10.1002/bsl.820. Manual.pdf.
Hakkanen-Nyholm, H., & Hare, R. D. (2009). Psychopathy, homicide, and the courts: Patrick, C. J., & Drislane, L. E. (2015). Triarchic model of psychopathy: Origins,
Working the system. Criminal Justice and Behavior, 36(8), 761–777. https://doi.org/ Operationalizations, and observed linkages with personality and general psycho-
10.1177/0093854809336946. pathology. Journal of Personality, 83(6), 627–643. https://doi.org/10.1111/jopy.
Hancock, M. (2014). Executive dysfunction: A contributor to subtypes of violence or general 12119.
criminality? London, Ontario, Canada: University of Western Ontario. Retrieved from Patrick, C. J., Fowles, D. C., & Krueger, R. F. (2009). Triarchic conceptualization of
http://ir.lib.uwo.ca/etd. psychopathy: Developmental origins of disinhibition, boldness, and meanness.
Hancock, M., Tapscott, J. L., & Hoaken, P. N. S. (2010). Role of executive dysfunction in Development and Psychopathology, 21(3), 913. https://doi.org/10.1017/
predicting frequency and severity of violence. Aggressive Behavior, 36(5), https://doi. S0954579409000492.
org/10.1002/ab.20353 n/a-n/a. Pennuto, T. O. (2007). Executive functioning, psychopathy, and moral reasoning among male
Hoaken, P. N. S., Allaby, D. B., & Earle, J. (2007). Executive cognitive functioning and the offenders. Faculty of Pacific Graduate.
recognition of facial expressions of emotion in incarcerated violent offenders, non- Pham, T. H., Vanderstukken, O., Philippot, P., & Vanderlinden, M. (2003). Selective at-
violent offenders, and controls. Aggressive Behavior, 33(5), 412–421. https://doi.org/ tention and executive functions deficits among criminal psychopaths. Aggressive
10.1002/ab.20194. Behavior, 29(5), 393–405. https://doi.org/10.1002/ab.10051.
Hoaken, P. N. S., Shaughnessy, V. K., & Pihl, R. O. (2003). Executive cognitive functioning Pietrzak, R. H., Sprague, A., & Snyder, P. J. (2008). Trait impulsiveness and executive
and aggression: Is it an issue of impulsivity? Aggressive Behavior, 29(1), 15–30. function in healthy young adults. Journal of Research in Personality, 42(5),
https://doi.org/10.1002/ab.10023. 1347–1351. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jrp.2008.03.004.
Hoppenbrouwers, S. S., De Jesus, D. R., Stirpe, T., Fitzgerald, P. B., Voineskos, A. N., Pino, V. D., & Werlang, G. (2006). Homicídio e lobo frontal: revisão da literatura.
Schutter, D. J. L. G., & Daskalakis, Z. J. (2013). Inhibitory deficits in the dorsolateral Interação Em Psicologia, 10(1), 127–137.
prefrontal cortex in psychopathic offenders. Cortex, 49(5), 1377–1385. https://doi. Raine, A. (2019). A neurodevelopmental perspective on male violence. Infant Mental
org/10.1016/j.cortex.2012.06.003. Health Journal, 40(1), 84–97. https://doi.org/10.1002/imhj.21761.
Houston, R. J., Stanford, M. S., Villemarette Pittman, N. R., Conklin, S. M., & Helfritz, L. E. Raine, A., Meloy, J. R., Bihrle, S., Stoddard, J., LaCasse, L., & Buchsbaum, M. S. (1998).
(2003). Neurobiological correlates and clinical implications of aggressive subtypes. Reduced prefrontal and increased subcortical brain functioning assessed using posi-
Journal of Forensic Neuropsychology, 3(4), 67–87. https://doi.org/10.1300/ tron emission tomography in predatory and affective murderers. Behavioral Sciences &
J151v03n04_05. the Law, 16(3), 319–332. Retrieved from http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/
Ishikawa, S. S., Raine, A., Lencz, T., Bihrle, S., & Lacasse, L. (2001). Autonomic stress 9768464.
reactivity and executive functions in successful and unsuccessful criminal psycho- Raine, A., Moffitt, T. E., Caspi, A., Loeber, R., Stouthamer-Loeber, M., & Lynam, D.
paths from the community. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 110(3), 423–432. https:// (2005). Neurocognitive impairments in boys on the life-course persistent antisocial
doi.org/10.1037//0021-843X.I10.3.423. path. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 114(1), 38–49. https://doi.org/10.1037/0021-
Jackson, C. J., Loxton, N. J., Harnett, P., Ciarrochi, J., & Gullo, M. J. (2014). Original and 843X.114.1.38.
revised reinforcement sensitivity theory in the prediction of executive functioning: A Reidy, D. E., Shelley-Tremblay, J. F., & Lilienfeld, S. O. (2011). Psychopathy, reactive
test of relationships between dual systems. Personality and Individual Differences, 56, aggression, and precarious proclamations: A review of behavioral, cognitive, and
83–88. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2013.08.024. biological research. Aggression and Violent Behavior, 16(6), 512–524. https://doi.org/
Kockler, T. R., & Stanford, M. S. (2008). Using a clinically aggressive sample to examine 10.1016/j.avb.2011.06.002.
the association between impulsivity, executive functioning, and verbal learning and Robertson, G., Taylor, P. J., & Gunn, J. C. (1987). Does violence have cognitive corre-
memory. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology : The Official Journal of the National lates? The British Journal of Psychiatry : The Journal of Mental Science, 151(1), 63–68.
Academy of Neuropsychologists, 23(2), 165–173. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.acn.2007. https://doi.org/10.1192/bjp.151.1.63.
10.006. Ross, E. H., & Hoaken, P. N. S. (2011). Executive cognitive functioning abilities of male
Kockler, T. R., Stanford, M. S., Nelson, C. E., Meloy, J. R., & Sanford, K. (2006). first time and return Canadian federal inmates. Canadian Journal of Criminology and
Characterizing aggressive behavior in a forensic population. The American Journal of Criminal Justice, 53(4), 377–403. https://doi.org/10.3138/cjccj.53.4.377.
Orthopsychiatry, 76(1), 80–85. https://doi.org/10.1037/0002-9432.76.1.80. Salekin, R. T., Rogers, R., & Sewell, K. W. (1996). A review and meta-analysis of the
Marsh, N. V., & Martinovich, W. M. (2006). Executive dysfunction and domestic violence. psychopathy checklist and psychopathy checklist-revised: Predictive validity of
Brain Injury, 20(1), 61–66. https://doi.org/10.1080/02699050500110645. dangerousness. Clinical Psychology: Science and Practice, 3(3), 203–215. https://doi.
Mathias, C. W., Stanford, M. S., Marsh, D. M., Frick, P. J., Moeller, F. G., Swann, A. C., & org/10.1111/j.1468-2850.1996.tb00071.x.
Dougherty, D. M. (2007). Characterizing aggressive behavior with the impulsive/ Sellbom, M., & Phillips, T. R. (2013). An examination of the triarchic conceptualization of
premeditated aggression scale among adolescents with conduct disorder. Psychiatry psychopathy in incarcerated and nonincarcerated samples. Journal of Abnormal
Research, 151(3), 231–242. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psychres.2006.11.001. Psychology, 122(1), 208–214. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0029306.
Meijers, J., Harte, J. M., Jonker, F. A., & Meynen, G. (2015). Prison brain? Executive Stanford, M. S., Houston, R. J., Mathias, C. W., Villemarette-Pittman, N. R., Helfritz, L. E.,
dysfunction in prisoners. Frontiers in Psychology, 6. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg. & Conklin, S. M. (2003). Characterizing aggressive behavior. Assessment, 10(2),
2015.00043. 183–190. Retrieved from http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/12801190.
Meloy, J. R. (2006). Empirical basis and forensic application of affective and predatory Strobach, T., Salminen, T., Karbach, J., & Schubert, T. (2014). Practice-related optimi-
violence. The Australian and New Zealand Journal of Psychiatry, 40(6–7), 539–547. zation and transfer of executive functions: A general review and a specific realization
https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1440-1614.2006.01837.x. of their mechanisms in dual tasks. Psychological Research, 78(6), 836–851. https://
Miura, H. (2009). Differences in frontal lobe function between violent and nonviolent doi.org/10.1007/s00426-014-0563-7.
conduct disorder in male adolescents. Psychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences, 63(2), Syngelaki, E. M., Moore, S. C., Savage, J. C., Fairchild, G., & Van Goozen, S. H. M. (2009).
161–166. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1440-1819.2009.01935.x. Executive functioning and risky decision making in young male offenders. Criminal
Miyake, A., & Friedman, N. P. (2012). The nature and organization of individual differ- Justice and Behavior, 36(11), 1213–1227. https://doi.org/10.1177/
ences in executive functions: Four general conclusions. Current Directions in 0093854809343095.
Psychological Science, 21(1), 8–14. https://doi.org/10.1177/0963721411429458. Tirapu-Ustárroz, J., García Molina, A., Ríos-Lago, M., & Ardila, A. (2012). In J. Tirapu-
Miyake, A., Friedman, N. P., Emerson, M. J., Witzki, A. H., Howerter, A., & Wager, T. D. Ustárroz, A. G. Molina, M. Ríos-Lago, & A. Ardila (Eds.). Corteza prefrontal, funciones
(2000). The unity and diversity of executive functions and their contributions to ejecutivas y regulación de la conducta. Barcelona: Viguera.
complex “frontal lobe” tasks: A latent variable analysis. Cognitive Psychology, 41(1), Venables, N. C., Hall, J. R., & Patrick, C. J. (2014). Differentiating psychopathy from
49–100. https://doi.org/10.1006/cogp.1999.0734. antisocial personality disorder: A triarchic model perspective. Psychological Medicine,
Moeller, F. G., Barratt, E. S., Dougherty, D. M., Schmitz, J. M., & Swann, A. C. (2001). 44(5), 1005–1013. https://doi.org/10.1017/S003329171300161X.
Psychiatric aspects of impulsivity. American Journal of Psychiatry, 158(11), Viding, E. (2004). Annotation: Understanding the development of psychopathy. Journal of
1783–1793. https://doi.org/10.1176/appi.ajp.158.11.1783. Child Psychology and Psychiatry, and Allied Disciplines, 45(8), 1329–1337. https://doi.
Mol, B., Van Den Bos, P., Derks, Y., & Egger, J. (2009). Executive functioning and the two- org/10.1111/j.1469-7610.2004.00840.x.
factor model of psychopathy: No differential relation? The International Journal of Villemarette-Pittman, N. R., Stanford, M. S., & Greve, K. W. (2003). Language and ex-
Neuroscience, 119(1), 124–140. https://doi.org/10.1080/00207450802324861. ecutive function in self-reported impulsive aggression. Personality and Individual
Morgan, A. B., & Lilienfeld, S. O. (2000). A meta-analytic review of the relation between Differences, 34(8), 1533–1544. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0191-8869(02)00136-8.
antisocial behavior and neuropsychological measures of executive function. Clinical Volavka, J. (1999). The neurobiology of violence: An update. The Journal of
Psychology Review, 20(1), 113–136. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0272-7358(98) Neuropsychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences, 11(3), 307–314.
00096-8. Wall, T. D., Wygant, D. B., & Sellbom, M. (2015). Boldness explains a key difference
Ogilvie, J. M., Stewart, A. L., Chan, R. C. K., & Shumd, D. (2011). Neuropsychological between psychopathy and antisocial personality disorder. Psychiatry, Psychology and
measures of executive function and antisocial behavior: A meta-analysis. Criminology, Law, 22(1), 94–105. https://doi.org/10.1080/13218719.2014.919627.
49(4), 1063–1107. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1745-9125.2011.00252.x. Woodworth, M., & Porter, S. (2002). In cold blood: Characteristics of criminal homicides
OECD (2019). https://www.statista.com/statistics/300986/incarceration-rates-in-oecd- as a function of psychopathy. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 111(3), 436–445.
countries/. https://doi.org/10.1037/0021-843X.111.3.436.

You might also like