You are on page 1of 17

See discussions, stats, and author profiles for this publication at: https://www.researchgate.

net/publication/258144059

Career Indecision Versus Indecisiveness

Article in Journal of Career Assessment · February 2013


DOI: 10.1177/1069072712454698

CITATIONS READS
88 4,120

4 authors, including:

Annamaria Di Fabio Letizia Palazzeschi


University of Florence University of Florence
396 PUBLICATIONS 7,254 CITATIONS 56 PUBLICATIONS 1,624 CITATIONS

SEE PROFILE SEE PROFILE

Itamar Gati
Hebrew University of Jerusalem
143 PUBLICATIONS 8,204 CITATIONS

SEE PROFILE

All content following this page was uploaded by Annamaria Di Fabio on 25 July 2016.

The user has requested enhancement of the downloaded file.


Journal ofhttp://jca.sagepub.com/
Career Assessment

Career Indecision Versus Indecisiveness: Associations With Personality Traits and


Emotional Intelligence
Annamaria Di Fabio, Letizia Palazzeschi, Lisa Asulin-Peretz and Itamar Gati
Journal of Career Assessment 2013 21: 42 originally published online 6 August 2012
DOI: 10.1177/1069072712454698

The online version of this article can be found at:


http://jca.sagepub.com/content/21/1/42

Published by:

http://www.sagepublications.com

Additional services and information for Journal of Career Assessment can be found at:

Email Alerts: http://jca.sagepub.com/cgi/alerts

Subscriptions: http://jca.sagepub.com/subscriptions

Reprints: http://www.sagepub.com/journalsReprints.nav

Permissions: http://www.sagepub.com/journalsPermissions.nav

Citations: http://jca.sagepub.com/content/21/1/42.refs.html

>> Version of Record - Feb 6, 2013


OnlineFirst Version of Record - Aug 6, 2012

What is This?

Downloaded from jca.sagepub.com at Central Library Alzahra Univ on October 26, 2013
Article
Journal of Career Assessment
21(1) 42-56
ª The Author(s) 2013
Career Indecision Versus Reprints and permission:
sagepub.com/journalsPermissions.nav
Indecisiveness: Associations DOI: 10.1177/1069072712454698
jca.sagepub.com
With Personality Traits and
Emotional Intelligence

Annamaria Di Fabio1, Letizia Palazzeschi1,


Lisa Asulin-Peretz2, and Itamar Gati3

Abstract
The goal of the present study was to investigate the distinctions between career indecision
and indecisiveness. The different patterns of the associations between career indecision and
indecisiveness, on one hand, and personality traits, career decision-making self-efficacy, perceived
social support, and emotional intelligence, on the other, were studied in a sample of 361 university
students. The results showed that career indecision, as measured by the Career Decision-making
Difficulties Questionnaire, is most highly associated with emotional intelligence, whereas career
indecisiveness, as measured by the Indecisiveness scale, is most highly associated with personality
traits, and in particular with emotional stability. This pattern of results was obtained for both women
and men; however, the prediction was stronger for indecision (R2 ¼ .76 and .55, for women and
men, respectively) than indecisiveness (R2 ¼ .35 and .28, for women than for men, respectively).
Possible explanations of these differences are discussed.

Keywords
career indecision, career decision-making difficulties, indecisiveness, personality traits, emotional
intelligence, career decision-making self-efficacy, perceived social support

Career decisions are one of the most complex and difficult decisions one has to make in the course of
life. They involve many factors and aspects that need to be taken into consideration, such as skills
and abilities, life goals, career preferences, dealing with one’s own expectations and often with those
of significant others, as well as uncertainty and the frequent necessity of making compromises.

1
Department of Psychology, University of Florence, Florence, Italy
2
School of Education, Hebrew University of Jerusalem, Jerusalem, Israel
3
Department of Psychology, Hebrew University, Israel

Corresponding Author:
Annamaria Di Fabio, Dipartimento di Psicologia, Università degli Studi di Firenze, via di San Salvi, 12, Complesso di San Salvi,
Padiglione 26, 50135 Firenze, Italy.
Email: adifabio@psico.unifi.it

42
Fabio et al. 43

Therefore, it is not surprising that difficulties in the career decision-making process are the general
rule rather than the exception (Gati, 1986; Osipow, 1999).
Career indecision is a construct that refers to emergence of problems during the career
decision-making process (Brown & Rector, 2008; Gati, Krausz, & Osipow, 1996; Osipow, 1999;
Phillips & Pazienza, 1988). Indecision explains why some individuals are uncertain about their edu-
cational and vocational future, while others are more confident in making their choices (Wanberg &
Muchinsky, 1992). Indecision is generally regarded as a normal stage through which almost every-
body passes during their lifetime (Germeijs & Verschueren, 2007; Osipow, 1999; Saka & Gati,
2007; Skorikov & Patton, 2007).
In contrast to indecision, indecisiveness refers to a chronic inability to make decisions in various
contexts and situations (Frost & Shows, 1993; Saka, Gati, & Kelly, 2008). It is therefore important to
distinguish between indecision, which refers to a normal phase of life in which careers are chosen
and pursued, and indecisiveness, which is a personality characteristic displayed in the individual’s
difficulty in making decisions in various life contexts. Using a different framing, Savickas (2004)
distinguished between undecided individuals—those with temporary indecision, a passing inability
to choose, but with the potential make a decision in the near future—and indecisive individuals, who
suffer from chronic indecisiveness, attributable to high anxiety and low problem-solving abilities.
Thus, indecisiveness can be defined as a chronic state that stems from pervasive emotional and
personality-related difficulties (Gati, Asulin-Peretz, & Fisher, 2012; Germeijs & Verschueren,
2007; Saka et al., 2008).

Career Indecision
Few other decisions influence individuals’ lives as much as career decisions (Hackett & Betz, 1995).
Therefore, it is not surprising that understanding career indecision continues to be one of the major
issues of vocational psychology (Brown & Rector, 2008; Phillips & Pazienza, 1988; Savickas, 1995;
Skorikov, 2007). Identifying specific difficulties impeding individuals’ career decision-making
process is one of the first steps in career counseling (Osipow, 1999). To achieve this goal, Gati et
al. (1996) proposed and validated a taxonomy of difficulties in career decision making. According
to the proposed taxonomy, there are 10 types of difficulties, which are divided into two groups,
indicating a temporal distinction between the difficulties that often arise before one begins the
decision-making process (Lack of Readiness) and the difficulties that typically arise only after the
process has begun (Lack of Information and Inconsistent Information). To test this theoretical
model, Gati et al. (1996) developed the Career Decision-making Difficulties Questionnaire
(CDDQ), which was used in the present study to assess career indecision, and is described in detail
in the Method section.
Finally, career decision difficulties have also been found to be associated with personality and
emotional intelligence (Di Fabio & Palazzeschi, 2009). With regard to personality traits, the total
CDDQ score and the three major difficulty clusters were found to correlate inversely with Extraver-
sion and positively with Neuroticism; for emotional intelligence, it was found that the total CDDQ
score and the three major difficulty clusters correlate inversely with emotional intelligence, partic-
ularly with the intrapersonal dimension.

Indecisiveness
Indecisiveness has been defined as the inability to make decisions in various contexts and situations
(Frost & Shows, 1993; Gaffner & Hazler, 2002; Patalano & Wengrovitz, 2006; Saka & Gati, 2007).
Individuals with high indecisiveness take more time to choose among different alternatives (Frost &
Shows, 1993), use less effective decisional strategies (Ferrari & Dovidio, 2000, 2001), require more

43
44 Journal of Career Assessment 21(1)

cognitive effort in decision making (Ferrari & Dovidio, 2001), feel more threatened by ambiguous
situations (Rassin & Muris, 2005a), and are more likely to postpone decisions (Rassin & Muris,
2005b). Indeed, indecisive individuals have more difficulty choosing college majors (Gayton,
Clavin, Clavin, & Broida, 1994) and making other life decisions (Germeijs & De Boeck, 2002).
Moreover, indecisiveness has been associated with individual characteristics such as neuroticism
(Jackson, Furnham, & Lawty-Jones, 1999), low self-esteem (Burka & Yuen, 1983; Ferrari, 1991),
procrastination (Beswick, Rothblum, & Mann, 1988; Effert & Ferrari, 1989; Ferrari, 1992), obses-
sive–compulsive tendencies (Frost & Shows, 1993; Gayton et al., 1994), and perfectionism (Frost &
Shows, 1993; Gayton et al., 1994). More recently, Rassin, Muris, Franken, Smit, and Wong (2007)
showed that indecisiveness, as measured by the Indecisiveness scale (IS; Frost & Shows, 1993), is
negatively correlated with decision-making self-efficacy. Because of the typically unfavorable
consequences of indecisiveness, locating factors associated with it is important for designing inter-
ventions aimed at reducing them.

Personality Traits
The role of personality in vocational choices has long been recognized (e.g., Borges & Savickas,
2002; Holland, 1997; Tokar, Fischer, & Subich, 1998). The study of personality traits in the context
of career decision making has received increased attention in recent years (e.g., Gati et al., 2011;
Nilsson et al., 2007). Guichard and Huteau (2001) showed that decision-making strategies may
be a function of the context, but could nonetheless correspond to deeper personal tendencies as well.
Previous studies found career decision-making difficulties to be associated with general personality
factors, such as the Big-Five model of personality factors (e.g., Lounsbury, Hutchens, & Loveland,
2005). When personality traits are assessed in terms of Costa and McCrae’s Five-Factor Model
(1992), Extraversion and Neuroticism seem to be more associated with the difficulties involving
career decision making than with Agreeableness, Consciousness, or Openness to experience (Feld-
man, 2003; Kelly & Shin, 2009; Lounsbury et al., 2005). These results were replicated in a research
that used the CDDQ (Gati et al., 1996) to measure career decision-making difficulties; negative cor-
relations with Extraversion and positive correlations with Neuroticism emerged for the three clusters
of the CDDQ (Di Fabio & Palazzeschi, 2009).

Career Decision-Making Self-Efficacy


Career decision-making self-efficacy is regarded as one of the significant factors that affect the
career decision-making process. It is defined as the level to which the individual feels confident
in his or her ability to successfully perform tasks in the career decision-making process, such as gath-
ering occupational information, selecting goals, making plans for the future, and problem solving
(Betz, Klein, & Taylor, 1996). Inverse associations between career decision-making self-efficacy
and career indecision have consistently emerged (Betz et al., 1996; Creed, Patton, & Bartrum,
2004; Guay, Ratelle, Senécal, Larose, & Deschênes, 2006; Nota, Ferrari, Solberg, & Soresi,
2007; Osipow & Gati, 1998).

Perceived Social Support


Perceived social support may come from three groups (family, friends, and significant others; Zimet,
Dahlem, Zimet, & Farley, 1988) who can facilitate or impede the individual’s career decision-
making process. Indeed, the role of perceived social support in the career decision-making process
has been widely acknowledged (Blustein, Prezioso, & Schultheiss, 1995; Blustein, Walbridge,
Friedlander, & Palladino, 1991; Kenny & Bledsoe, 2005; Wolfe & Betz 2004; Young et al.,

44
Fabio et al. 45

2001). In the context of career indecision, familial support emerged as particularly important (Blus-
tein et al., 1995, 1991; Constantine, Wallace, & Kindaichi, 2005), and therefore it was also included
in the present research.

Emotional Intelligence
Emotional intelligence has been defined as a blend of emotional and social competences that deter-
mine the way in which one relates to oneself and to others and deals with environmental pressures
and demands (Bar-On, 2002). Because individuals with high emotional intelligence have a greater
awareness of their emotions and a greater capacity to integrate emotional experience with thoughts
and actions, emotional intelligence may have a role in career exploration and career decision making
(Brown, George-Curran, & Smith, 2003; Di Fabio & Palazzeschi, 2009; Emmerling & Cherniss,
2003). Indeed, emotional intelligence has been recognized in the past decade as an additional critical
variable in career choice (Di Fabio & Kenny, 2011); lower emotional intelligence has been associ-
ated with greater difficulties in each of the three clusters of the CDDQ (Di Fabio & Palazzeschi,
2008). Furthermore, emotional intelligence also appears to have a greater influence on each of three
clusters of career decision-making difficulties than either personality traits, career decision-making
self-efficacy, or perceived social support (Palazzeschi & Di Fabio, in press).

Gender Differences in Career Decision Making


Previous studies revealed gender differences in certain career decision-making variables. Gender
differences in the levels of individuals’ career decision-making difficulties were fairly small (Gati,
Osipow, Krausz, & Saka, 2000; Gati & Saka 2001), but nevertheless women were consistently found
to be more indecisive than men (Hijazi, Tatar, & Gati, 2004; Rassin & Muris, 2005; Serling & Betz,
1990). In addition, a small gender difference was reported by Saka, Gati, and Kelly (2008) in anxiety
about the process, one of the factors that are assumed to underlie general indecisiveness. Finally, a
few consistent gender differences also emerged in the way men and women tend to make career
decisions. Specifically, women tend to consult with others and to use intuition more than men (Gati,
Gadassi, & Mashiah-Cohen, 2012; Gati & Levin, 2011) and they also tend to be more dependent
than men (Mau, 2000).

The Present Research


The goal of the present research was to explore the differences between career indecision and career
indecisiveness by studying their differential patterns of associations with personality traits, career
decision-making self-efficacy, perceived social support, and emotional intelligence. Specifically,
we hypothesized that different variables would be most closely associated with indecision and
indecisiveness:

Hypothesis 1: Emotional intelligence will be more associated with career indecision than with
indecisiveness (Palazzeschi & Di Fabio, in press).
Hypothesis 2: Personality traits will be more associated with indecisiveness than with indecision
(Frost & Shows, 1993; Savickas, 2004).
Hypothesis 3: Career decision-making self-efficacy and perceived social support will be more
associated with indecision than with indecisiveness (Osipow & Gati, 1998; Palazzeschi, &
Di Fabio, in press).

In addition, in light of previous research that revealed gender differences in aspects of career deci-
sion making, we will investigate differences in the pattern of associations between career indecision

45
46 Journal of Career Assessment 21(1)

and indecisiveness, on one hand, and personality traits, career decision-making self-efficacy, per-
ceived social support, and emotional intelligence, on the other.

Method
Participants
The participants were 361 students (187 [52%] women and 174 men) from the University of
Florence. Their age ranged from 23 to 27 (M ¼ 24.04, SD ¼ 1.91). Their majors were Agricultural
Sciences (8.3%); Architecture (7.8%); Economics (8.9%); Pharmacy (7.2%); Law (9.1%); Engineer-
ing (7.5%); Humanities and Philosophy (8.3%); Medicine and Surgery (8.6%); Psychology (9.4%);
Educational Science (7.8%); Mathematical, Physical, and Natural Sciences (9.1%); Political Science
(8%); other (7.2%). They were sophomores (36.6%), juniors (29.4%), and seniors (34.1%). The
participants were predominantly White Italians from middle-class backgrounds.

Instruments
The CDDQ. We used the Italian version of the 34-item CDDQ (Gati et al., 1996), as developed by
Di Fabio and Palazzeschi (2010). As described in the Introduction, the CDDQ comprises 10 scales
representing 10 possible difficulties, organized in three major difficulty clusters. Respondents are
asked to report the degree to which each statement describes them on a 9-point Likert-type scale
(1—Does not describe me to 9—Describes me well). The CDDQ has been found reliable and its
structure is stable across cultures (Creed & Yin, 2006; Di Fabio & Kenny, 2011; Gati, Osipow,
Krausz, & Saka, 2000; Lancaster, Rudolph, Perkins, & Patten, 1999; Mau, 2001). The positive
correlations between the three clusters and 10 scales of the CDDQ, on one hand, and the Career
Decision scale (Osipow, Carney, Winer, Yanico, & Koschier, 1976), on the other, as well as their
negative correlation with the Career Decision Self-Efficacy scale–Short Form (CDSES-SF; Betz
& Taylor, 2000; Di Fabio & Palazzeschi, 2010; Osipow & Gati, 1998), support the concurrent valid-
ity of the CDDQ. The concurrent validity of the CDDQ was also supported by the similarity between
the conclusions drawn from it on the basis of career counselees’ responses to this questionnaire and
the judgments of their career counselors (Gati, Osipow, Krausz, & Saka, 2000). The CDDQ was
found to distinguish between decided and undecided students (Lancaster et al., 1999; Mau, 2001)
and to evaluate the effectiveness of career interventions (Fouad, Cotter, & Kantamneni, 2009; Gati,
Saka, & Krausz, 2001).
The Italian version of the CDDQ has been found to have adequate psychometric properties, with
Cronbach’s a internal consistency reliability estimates of .86, .90, and .92, for Lack of Readiness,
Lack of Information, and Inconsistent Information, respectively (Di Fabio & Palazzeschi, 2010).
Furthermore, the structure of the 10 scales and the three major clusters was also supported by
Exploratory and Confirmatory factor analyses (Di Fabio & Palazzeschi, 2010; Gati et al., 1996). The
Cronbach’s a internal consistency reliability estimate of the total CDDQ score was .85 in the present
sample.

The Indecisiveness scale (Frost & Shows, 1993). To evaluate indecisiveness, we used the Italian ver-
sion of the IS (Di Fabio, Busoni, & Palazzeschi, 2011). The scale measures indecisiveness using 15
items, with a 5-point Likert-type response scale (1—strongly disagree to 5—strongly agree). Exam-
ples are When ordering from a menu, I usually find it difficult to decide what to get; It seems that
deciding on the most trivial thing takes me a long time. Exploratory factor analysis revealed a uni-
dimensional structure (Di Fabio et al., 2011). The Cronbach’s a internal consistency reliability was
.85, and its concurrent validity was adequate, having correlations ranging from .44 to .68 with
dimensions of the Melbourne Decision Making Questionnaire (Mann, Burnet, Radford, & Ford,

46
Table 1. Correlations Among the CDDQ Total Score, Indecisiveness Scale, BFQ Personality Traits, CDSES–SF, the MSPSS Dimensions, and the Bar-On EQ-i
Total Score (N ¼ 361).

1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12

1. CDDQ _
2. IS .51** _
3. BFQ Ex .43** .54** _
4. BFQ A .16** .20** .27** _
5. BFQ C .19** .18** .08 .19** _
6. BFQ ES .47** .57** .39** .32** .35** _
7. BFQ O .14** .16** .18** .41** .21** .35** _
8. CDSES–SF .55** .20** .26** .25** .14** .35** .14** _
9. MSPSS SO .43** .10 .22** .04 .04 .05 .03 .32** _
10. MSPSS Fa .52** .19** .28** .05 .05 .20** .01 .32** .66** _
11. MSPSS Fr .44** .12** .30** .06 .01 .13* .01 .34** .73** .69** _
12. EQ Total .70** .47** .29** .26** .27** .41** .14** .48** .32** .36** .33** _
M 4.57 41.42 71.38 77.28 73.45 71.14 73.45 60.45 14.12 13.89 14.30 317.51
SD 1.18 8.90 17.45 9.69 15.58 16.66 16.18 12.93 6.50 5.22 5.14 51.56
A .85 .82 .83 .76 .84 .89 .78 .74 .89 .89 .90 .92
Note. BFQ A ¼ agreeableness; BFQ Ex ¼ extraversion; BFQ C ¼ conscientiousness; BFQ ES ¼ emotional stability; BFQ O ¼ openness; CDSES–SF ¼ Career Decision-Making Self
Efficacy scale–Short Form; MSPSS SO ¼ MSPSS Significant Others; MSPSS Fa ¼ MSPSS Family; MSPSS Fr ¼ MSPSS Friends; QE Total ¼ Bar-On Emotional Quotient Inventory Total
Score.
*p < .05. **p < .01.

47
48 Journal of Career Assessment 21(1)

1997) and a correlation of .67 with the general IS of the CDDQ in the present study. In the present
sample, the Cronbach’s a internal consistency reliability estimate was .82.

The Big Five Questionnaire (BFQ). The BFQ is composed of 132 items, with a 5-point Likert
response scale (1—absolutely false to 5—absolutely true; Caprara, Barbaranelli, & Borgogni,
1993). The BFQ distinguishes among five fundamental personality dimensions (with two
subdimensions for each of the five). The Cronbach’s a internal consistency reliability was .81 for
Extraversion, .73 for Agreeableness, .81 for Conscientiousness, .90 for Emotional Stability, and
.75 for Openness to experience (Caprara et al., 1993). The correlations with the Wechsler Adult
Intelligence scale (Wechsler, 1981) were negligible (Caprara et al., 1993). The correlations with the
State-Trait Anxiety Inventory, Form X (STAI, Form X, Spielberger, Gorsuch, & Lushene, 1968)
were .38 for Extraversion, .15 for Agreeableness, .23 for Conscientiousness, .62 for
Emotional Stability, and .15 for Openness. The reliabilities of the five factors in the present study
ranged from .76 to .89 (see Table 1, bottom row).

CDSES-SF. We used the Italian version of the CDSES-SF (Betz et al., 1996; Betz & Taylor, 2000;
Nota, Pace, & Ferrari, 2008). The Italian version comprises 20 items, with a 5-point Likert-type
scale (1—I have no confidence to 5—I have complete confidence). The Cronbach’s a reliability
of the total CDSE in the present study was .74.

The Multidimensional scale of Perceived Social Support (MSPSS). We used the Italian version of the
MSPSS (Di Fabio & Busoni, 2008; Zimet, Dahlem, Zimet, & Farley, 1988). The scale is composed
of 12 items, with a 7-point Likert-type response scale (1—strongly disagree to 7—strongly agree).
The instrument measures perceived support from family, friends, and significant others. Item exam-
ples are My family works very hard to help me, I can speak to my friends about my problems, and
When I need someone, there is always a special person who stands by me. The Cronbach’s a of the
Italian version is. 94, .93, and .94, for the dimensions of Family, Friends, and Significant others,
respectively (Di Fabio & Busoni, 2008). A confirmatory factor analysis supported the original
three-dimensional structure (Di Fabio & Busoni, 2008). In the present sample, the Cronbach’s a
internal consistency reliability estimates were .89, .90, and .89, for the dimensions of Family,
Friends, and Significant others, respectively.

The Bar-On Emotional Quotient Inventory (Bar-On EQ-I). To assess emotional intelligence, we used
the Italian version of the Bar-On EQ-I (Bar-On, 1997), which was developed by Franco and
Tappatà (2009). It comprises 133 items, with a 5-point Likert-type response scale (1—not at
all true for me to 5—absolutely true for me). The questionnaire measures five principal emo-
tional intelligence dimensions: (a) intrapersonal, associated with awareness of one’s own emo-
tions, strengths and weaknesses and the ability to express one’s feelings; (b) interpersonal,
linked to social awareness and interpersonal relationships, and involving the ability to recog-
nize others’ emotions, feelings, and needs and to establish and maintain cooperative, construc-
tive, and satisfactory relationships; (c) adaptability, connected with the ability to cope flexibly
with everyday problems; (d) stress management, the ability to cope with stressful situations
adaptively and to manage one’s emotions beneficially; and (e) general mood, which is the
capacity to be optimistic, express positive feelings, and enjoy the presence of other people. The
Cronbach’s a internal consistency reliability estimates were .91, .84, .81 .87, and .83 for the
five dimensions, respectively; Cronbach’s a was .95 for the EQ-i total score (Franco &
Tappatà, 2009). The five-dimensional structure of the EQ-i was supported by a Confirmatory
Factor Analysis (Franco & Tappatà, 2009). In the present sample, the Cronbach’s a internal
consistency reliability estimate of the total EQ score was .92.

48
Fabio et al. 49

Procedure
All the questionnaires were included in a single booklet and filled out by participants in small
groups of about 20–25 students, administered by specialized personnel and conforming to the
Privacy Law. All the students filled out the questionnaires in the same order (CDDQ, IS, BFQ,
CDSES-SF, MSPSS, Bar-On EQ-i), as part of a career intervention. Filling out the booklet took
about 45–60 min.

Results
The means, standard deviations, Cronbach’s a internal consistency reliability estimates, and
Pearson correlations among the CDDQ Total Score, IS, the five BFQ Dimensions (Extraver-
sion, Agreeableness, Conscientiousness, Emotional Stability, and Openness), CDSES-SF Total
Score, MSPSS dimensions (Significant others, Family, Friends), and the Bar-On EQ-i Total
Score are presented in Table 1. As hypothesized, indecision, as measured by the CDDQ, was
highly negatively correlated with the Emotional Intelligence scale (r ¼ .70); it was also
negatively correlated with the CDSES (r ¼ .55). Noticeable negative correlations also
emerged with perceived social support (r ¼ .52, .44, and .43, for Family, Friends, and
Significant others, respectively), and with two dimensions of the Big Five—Emotional Stability
(r ¼ .47) and Extraversion (r ¼ .43).
Although indecision and indecisiveness were moderately correlated (r ¼ .51), the pattern of
their correlations with the other variables were different. Indecisiveness was associated more
strongly than indecision with Emotional Stability, r ¼ .57 vs. .47, t(358) ¼ 2.40, p < .05,
and Extraversion, r ¼ .54 vs. 43, t(358) ¼ 2.93, p < .01. In contrast, indecision correlated
more strongly than indecisiveness with emotional intelligence, r ¼ .70 vs. .47, t(358) ¼ 6.26,
p < .001).
To test the relative contribution of the different variables to the prediction of indecision and
indecisiveness, we carried out a series of stepwise multiple regression analyses; these analyses were
carried out separately for women and for men in order to test the consistency of the results. The
results, presented in Tables 2 and 3, support the hypothesis that different research variables are
associated with indecision and with indecisiveness. Specifically, as can be seen in Table 2, career
indecision is best explained by emotional intelligence for both women and men (b ¼ .45 and
.39, respectively), less by perceived social support from family (b ¼ .24 and .26, for women
and men, respectively), even less by career decision-making self-efficacy (b ¼ .19 and .22, for
women and men), and Extraversion (b ¼ .19 only for women), and least by Emotional Stability
(b ¼ .11 and .16, for women and men, respectively). A few findings are noteworthy. First, all
the bs are negative, as predicted. Second, except for the Extraversion dimension of the BFQ, the
order of the variables contributing to the prediction of indecision was similar for women and men.
Third, the prediction of indecision for women (R2 ¼ .76) was higher than that for men (R2 ¼ .55);
part of this difference is attributable to the contribution of the Extraversion dimension of the BFQ for
women, but not for men.
Table 3 presents the results of the analyses carried out to predict indecisiveness, again
separately for women and for men. As can be seen in Table 3, indecisiveness is best explained
by Emotional Stability (b ¼ .20 and .17, for women and men, respectively), less by
Extraversion (b ¼ .17 and .14, for women and men, respectively), and least by emotional
intelligence (b ¼ .14 and .11, for women and men, respectively). Again, as predicted, all
b are negative. Second, the order of variables contributing to the prediction of indecisiveness
was similar for women and men. Third, the prediction of indecisiveness for women (R2 ¼ .35)
was higher than that for men (R2 ¼ .28).

49
50 Journal of Career Assessment 21(1)

Table 2. Predicting the CDDQ Score: Results of the Multiple Regression Analyses for Women and Men.

Women (n ¼ 187) Men (n ¼ 174)

Predictor B SEB b R B SEB b R

1. QE total .01 .01 .45** .75 .01 .01 .39** .64


2. MSPSS FA .05 .01 .24** .81 .06 .01 .26** .69
3. CDSES–SF .01 .01 .19** .84 .02 .01 .22** .72
4. BFQ Ex .01 .01 .19** .86
5. BFQ ES .02 .01 .11** .87 .01 .01 .16** .74
Note. BFQ ES ¼ Emotional Stability; CDSES-SF ¼ Career Decision-Making Self Efficacy-scale–Short Form; MSPSS FA ¼ MSPSS
Family; QE Total ¼ Bar-On Emotional Quotient Inventory Total Score.
**p < .01., R2 ¼ .76, p < .001 for women and R2 ¼ .55, p < .001 for men.

Table 3. Predicting the Indecisiveness Score: Results of the Multiple Regression Analyses for Women and Men.

Women (n ¼ 187) Men (n ¼ 174)

Predictor B SEB b R B SEB b R

1. BFQ ES .53 .13 .20** .48 .50 .11 .17** .42


2. BFQ Ex .32 .11 .17** .55 .28 .07 .14** .46
3. QE Total .52 .11 .14** .59 .48 .09 .11** .53

Note. BFQ ES ¼ Emotional Stability; BFQ Ex ¼ Extraversion; QE Total ¼ Bar-On Emotional Quotient Inventory Total Score.
**p < .01. R2 ¼ .35, p < .001 for women and R2 ¼ .28, p < .001 for men.

Discussion
The present research studied the distinctions between career indecision and career indecisiveness by
investigating factors that were hypothesized as being associated with them, including personality
traits, career decision-making self-efficacy, perceived social support, and emotional intelligence.
As hypothesized, we found that different sets of variables were associated with indecision and inde-
cisiveness. Specifically, high emotional intelligence was the best predictor of low indecision, for
both women and men; in fact, it accounted for more of the variance in indecision than all of the other
variables put together. This result replicated the association recently reported between career
indecision and emotional intelligence (Palazzeschi & Di Fabio, in press), further underlining the
association of the latter with aspects of career decision-making process.
In addition, and in line with previous findings, negative correlations were found between career
indecision and (a) Extraversion and Emotional Stability (Di Fabio & Palazzeschi, 2008; Feldman,
2003; Palazzeschi & Di Fabio, in press), (b) perceived social support (especially by the family; Blus-
tein et al., 1991, 1995; Constantine et al., 2003), and (c) career decision self-efficacy (Creed et al.,
2004; Gianakos, 1999; Luzzo, 1993). These results show that in addition to within-individual
personality and cognitive characteristics, indecision is associated with external factors, specifically,
perceived social support.
The second hypothesis was also confirmed. Indecisiveness was better explained by personality
traits, especially low Emotional Stability, than by low emotional intelligence. This finding supports
the claim made by Savickas (2004) that chronic indecisiveness is associated with difficulties in man-
aging anxiety. It is also compatible with the findings of Gati et al. (2011) that, among the five dimen-
sions of the BFQ, the dimension of Emotional Stability was most highly correlated with all three
clusters of the Emotional and Personality related Career Difficulties Questionnaire (Saka & Gati,
2007), which measure career indecisiveness (Gati, Asulin-Peretz, & Fisher, 2012).

50
Fabio et al. 51

However, emotional intelligence was also associated with indecisiveness—its inclusion in the regres-
sion as an additional predictor of indecisiveness increased R2 by 5% and 7% (for women and men,
respectively), over the variance accounted for by the Emotional Stability and the Extraversion factors
of the BFQ (30% and 21%, for women and men, respectively). It has been reported that emotional intel-
ligence can be increased with specific training (Di Fabio & Kenny, 2011). The present findings suggest
that such an increase in emotional intelligence could reduce not only indecision but also indecisiveness.
Emotional Stability was found to predict indecisiveness (R2 ¼ 23% and 18%, for women and
men, respectively); however, although its contribution to the prediction of indecision was also sta-
tistically significant, it was negligible in terms of variance explained (DR2 was only 2% and 3%, for
women and men, respectively). This distinction supports the claim that indecisiveness has its roots in
personality, supporting previous studies on the factors underlying indecisiveness (Gati et al., 2011;
Saka & Gati, 2007; Saka, Gati, & Kelly, 2008).
Because of previous findings about gender differences in career decision making (e.g., Gati et al.,
2010, 2011; Hijazi, Tatar, & Gati, 2004; Mau, 2000), the analyses were carried out separately for
women and men. The similarity of the results for women and men, for predicting both indecision
and indecisiveness, supports the generalizability of the results. However, an unpredicted pattern
emerged: the variance accounted for both in indecision and in indecisiveness was consistently higher
for women than for men. This unexpected result cannot be explained by the larger within-group
differences in both indecision and indecisiveness for women as compared to men, because the SD
was in fact slightly higher for men than for women in both measures. This difference, however,
is compatible with previous reports about the inconsistency of the conclusions about gender
differences in the associations between emotional intelligence and other measures (e.g., Brown et
al., 2003). Thus, further research is required to test the consistency of the difference between women
and men in the strength of associations between indecision and indecisiveness, on one hand, and the
various measures used to predict them, on the other, and discover its source.
The other unexpected result—that the prediction of indecisiveness was much lower than that of
indecision—reflects that indecisiveness is a more complex phenomenon, and therefore harder to pre-
dict. This account is supported by the fact that only 3 of the 10 possible predictors included in the
regression analysis contributed to the prediction of indecisiveness; thus, other variables, for exam-
ple, the individual’s career decision-making profile (Gati et al., 2010) or style (Harren, 1979), should
also be considered in future research.

Limitations
Before discussing the implications, it is important to acknowledge the limitations of this research.
The first limitation is that all data were collected in a single session; although the CDDQ and the
IS were filled out before the other measures, future studies should use a longitudinal instead of a
correlational design. Second, because the participants were students at the University of Florence,
future research should test the associations between indecision and indecisiveness and the other
measures in a more heterogeneous Italian sample, as well as samples from other countries. Another
limitation involves the use of self-report measurements only; in particular, regarding the assessment
of emotional intelligence, future research should use other instruments to assess ability-based emo-
tional intelligence, such as the Mayer–Salovey–Caruso (2002) Emotional Intelligence Test.

Implications
The results of the present research contribute to the study of the constructs of career indecision and
indecisiveness, highlighting similarities and differences. The distinction between indecision and
indecisiveness is of interest because they differ in their severity and hence in their implications for

51
52 Journal of Career Assessment 21(1)

the career counseling process (Gati, Amir, & Landman, 2010). The differential strength of the asso-
ciations of career indecision and indecisiveness with emotional intelligence supports the theoretical
distinction between the two, a distinction that has been repeatedly reported in previous studies (e.g.,
Gaffner & Hazler, 2002; Germeijs & De Boeck, 2002; Saka & Gati, 2007). The associations of these
two constructs with emotional intelligence open novel intervention options on both career indecision
and indecisiveness. Indeed, Di Fabio and Kenny (2011) demonstrated how a training program for
enhancing emotional intelligence increased emotional intelligence (both ability-based and self-
reported) according to the ability-based model (Mayer et al., 2002), and moreover reduced not only
career decision-making difficulties but also general indecisiveness.
The development of emotional intelligence seems to be a strong factor (Hage et al., 2007) pre-
paring individuals to cope adaptively with environmental demands by improving their social skills
and facilitating their ability to succeed in life. It may therefore be suggested that, in a preventive
perspective (Kenny, Horne, Orpinas, & Reese, 2009), emotional intelligence might also be a key
ingredient in innovative interventions in career decision making.

Acknowledgments
The authors thank Reuma Gadassi, Naomi Goldblum, and Nimrod Levin for their comments on a previous
version of this article. This research was supported in part by the Research Laboratory of Psychology for
Vocational Guidance and Career Counseling at the University of Florence and the Samuel and Esther Melton
chair of the last author.

Declaration of Conflicting Interests


The authors declared no potential conflicts of interest with respect to the research, authorship, and/or
publication of this article.

Funding
The authors received no financial support for the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article.

References
Bar-On, R. (1997). The Emotional Intelligence Inventory (Eq-I): Technical manual. Toronto, Canada:
Multi-Health Systems.
Beswick, G., Rothblum, E. D., & Mann, L. (1988). Psychological antecedents to student procrastination.
Australian Psychologist, 23, 207–217. doi:10.1080/00050068808255605
Betz, N. E., Klein, K. L., & Taylor, K. M. (1996). Evaluation of a short form of the Career Decision-Making
Self-Efficacy scale. Journal of Career Assessment, 4, 47–57. doi: 10.1177/106907279600400103
Betz, N. E., & Taylor, K. M. (2000). Manual for the Career Decision Making Self-Efficacy scale (CDMSES)
and CDMSES–Short Form. Unpublished manuscript, The Ohio State University, Columbus, OH.
Blustein, D. L., Prezioso, M. S., & Schultheiss, D. P. (1995). Attachment theory and career development: Current
status and future directions. The Counseling Psychologist, 23, 416–432. doi: 10.1177/0011000095233002
Blustein, D. L., Walbridge, M. M., Friedlander, M. L., & Palladino, D. E. (1991). Contributions of psycholo-
gical separation and parental attachment to the career development process. Journal of Counseling
Psychology, 38, 39–50. doi:10.1037/0022-0167.38.1.39
Borges, N. J., & Savickas, M. L. (2002). Personality and medical specialty choice: A literature review and inte-
gration. Journal of Career Assessment, 10, 362–380. doi:10.1177/10672702010003006
Brown, C., George-Curran, R., & Smith, M. L. (2003). The role of emotional intelligence in the career commitment
and decision-making process. Journal of Career Assessment, 11, 379–392. doi: 10.1177/1069072703255834
Brown, S. D., & Rector, C. C. (2008). Conceptualizing and diagnosing problems in vocational decision-making. In
S.D. Brown & R. W. Lent (Eds.), Handbook of counseling psychology (4th ed., pp. 392–407). New York, NY:
Wiley.

52
Fabio et al. 53

Burka, J. B., & Yuen, L. M. (1983). Procrastination: Why do you do it and what to do about it. Reading, MA:
Addison-Wesley.
Caprara, G. V., Barbaranelli, C., & Borgogni, L. (1993). BFQ: Big five questionnaire. Manual (2nd ed.).
Firenze, Italy: O.S. Organizzazioni Speciali.
Constantine, M. G., Wallace, B. C., & Kindaichi, M. M. (2005). Examining contextual factors in the career
decision status of African American adolescents. Journal of Career Assessment, 13, 307–319. doi: 10.
1177/1069072705274960
Costa, P. T., & McCrae, R. R. (1992). NEO PI-R professional manual. Odessa, FL: Psychological Assessment
Resources.
Creed, P., Patton, W., & Bartrum, D. (2004). Internal and external barriers, cognitive style, and the career devel-
opment variables of focus and indecision. Journal of Career Development, 30, 277–294. doi:10.1023/B:
JOCD.0000025116.17855.ea
Creed, P. A., & Yin, W. O. (2006). Reliability and validity of a Chinese version of the career decision-making
difficulties questionnaire. International Journal for Educational and Vocational Guidance, 6, 47–63. doi:
10.1007/s10775-006-0003-3
Di Fabio, A., & Busoni, L. (2008). Misurare il supporto sociale percepito: Proprietà psicometriche della
Multidimensional Scale of Perceived Social Support (MSPSS) in un campione di studenti universitari
[Measuring perceived social support: Psychometric properties of the Multidimensional Scale of
Perceived Social Support (MSPSS) in a sample of university students]. Risorsa Uomo, 14, 339–350.
Di Fabio, A., Busoni, L., & Palazzeschi, L. (2011). Indecisiveness scale (IS): Proprietà psicometriche della ver-
sione italiana [Indecisiveness Scale (IS): Psychometric properties of the Italian version]. Counseling.
Giornale Italiano di Ricerca e Applicazioni, 4, 13–24.
Di Fabio, A., & Kenny, M. E. (2011). Promoting emotional intelligence and career decision making among
Italian high school students. Journal of Career Assessment, 19, 21–34. doi:10.1177/1069072710382530
Di Fabio, A., & Palazzeschi, L. (2008). Indecision vocationelle et intelligence émotionnelle: quelques données
empiriques sur un échantillon d’apprentis italiens [Career decision difficulties and emotional intelligence:
Some empirical facts on a sample of Italian apprentices]. Pratiques Psychologiques, 14, 213–222. doi:10.
1016/j.prps.2007.11.006
Di Fabio, A., & Palazzeschi, L. (2009). Emotional intelligence, personality traits, and career decision
difficulties. International Journal for Educational and Vocational Guidance, 9, 135–146. doi:10.1007/
s10775-009-9162-3
Di Fabio, A., & Palazzeschi, L. (2010). Career decision-making difficulties questionnaire: Proprietà psicome-
triche nel contesto italiano [Career Decision-Making Difficulties Questionnaire: Psychometric properties in
the Italian context]. Counseling: Giornale Italiano di Ricerca e Applicazioni, 3, 351–364.
Effert, B., & Ferrari, J. R. (1989). Decisional procrastination: Examining personality correlates. Journal of
Social Behavior and Personality, 4, 151–156.
Emmerling, R. J., & Cherniss, C. (2003). Emotional intelligence and career choice process. Journal of Career
Assessment, 11, 153–167. doi:10.1177/1069072703011002003
Feldman, D. C. (2003). The antecedents and consequences of early career indecision among young adults.
Human Resource Management Review, 13, 499–531. doi:10.1016/S1053-4822%2803%2900048-2
Ferrari, J. R. (1991). Compulsive procrastination: Some self-reported characteristics. Psychological Reports,
68, 455–458. doi:10.2466/PR0.68.2.455-458
Ferrari, J. R. (1992). Psychometric validation of two procrastination inventories: Arousal and avoidance
measures. Journal of Psychopathology and Behavioral Assessment, 14, 97–110. doi:10.1007/BF00965170
Ferrari, J. R., & Dovidio, J. F. (2000). Examining behavioral processes in indecision: Decisional procras-
tination and decision-making style. Journal of Research in Personality, 34, 127–137. doi:10.1006/jrpe.
1999.2247
Ferrari, J. R., & Dovidio, J. F. (2001). Behavioral information search by indecisiveness. Personality and
Individual Differences, 30, 1113–1123. doi:10.1016/S0191-8869(00)00094-5

53
54 Journal of Career Assessment 21(1)

Fouad, N., Cotter, W. E., & Kantamneni, N. (2009). The effectiveness of a career decision-making course.
Journal of Career Assessment, 17, 338–347. doi: 10.1177/1069072708330678
Franco, M., & Tappatà, L. (2009). EQ-iTM Emotional Quotient Inventory. Validazione italiana [ EQ-iTM
Emotional Quotient Inventory. Italian validation]. Firenze, Italy: Giunti O. S.
Frost, R. O., & Shows, D. L. (1993). The nature and measurement of compulsive indecisiveness. Behavior
Research Therapy, 31, 683–692. doi:10.1016/0005-7967(93)90121-A
Gaffner, D. C., & Hazler, R. J. (2002). Factors related to indecisiveness and career indecision in undecided col-
lege students. Journal of College Student Development, 43, 317–326.
Gati, I. (1986). Making career decisions: A sequential elimination approach. Journal of Counseling Psychology,
33, 408–417. doi: 10.1037/0022-0167.33.4.408
Gati, I., Asulin-Peretz, L., & Fisher, A. (2012). Emotional and personality-related career decision-making dif-
ficulties: A three-year follow-up. The Counseling Psychologist, 40(1), 6–27.
Gati, I., Gadassi, R., & Mashiah-Cohen, R. (2012). Career Decision-Making Profiles vs. Styles: Convergent and
incremental validity. Journal of Vocational Behavior, 81, 2–16.
Gati, I., Gadassi, R., Saka, N., Hadadi, Y., Ansenberg, N., Friedman, R., & Asulin-Peretz, L. (2011). Emotional
and personality-related aspects of career decision-making difficulties: Facets of career indecisiveness.
Journal of Career Assessment, 19, 3–20. doi:10.1177/1069072710382525
Gati, I., Krausz, M., & Osipow, S. H. (1996). A taxonomy of difficulties in career decision making. Journal of
Counseling Psychology, 43, 510–526. doi:10.1037/0022-0167.43.4.510
Gati, I., Landman, S., Davidovitch, S., Asulin-Peretz, L., & Gadassi, R. (2010). From career decision-making
styles to career decision-making profiles: A multidimensional approach. Journal of Vocational Behavior,
76, 277–291. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jvb.2009.11.001
Gati, I., & Levin, N. (2012). The stability and structure of Career Decision-Making Profiles: A one-year follow-
up. Journal of Career Assessment, 20(4), 390–403.
Gati, I., Osipow, S. H., Krausz, M., & Saka, N. (2000). Validity of the career decision-making difficulties ques-
tionnaire: Counselees’ versus career counselors’ perceptions. Journal of Vocational Behavior, 56, 99–113.
doi:10.1006/jvbe.1999.1710
Gati, I., & Saka, N. (2001). High school students’ career-related decision-making difficulties. Journal of
Counseling and Development, 79, 331–340.
Gati, I., Saka, N., & Krausz, M. (2001). ‘‘Should I use a computer-assisted career guidance system?’’ It depends
on where our career decision-making difficulties lie. British Journal of Guidance and Counselling, 29,
301–321. doi:10.1080/03069880120073021
Gayton, W. F., Clavin, R. H., Clavin, S. L., & Broida, J. (1994). Further validation of the indecisiveness scale.
Psychological Reports, 75, 1631–1634. doi:10.2466/pr0.1994.75.3f.1631
Germeijs, V., & De Boeck, P. (2002). A measurement scale for indecisiveness and its relationship to career
indecision and other types of indecision. European Journal of Psychological Assessment, 18, 113–122.
doi:10.1027//1015-5759.18.2.113
Germeijs, V., & Verschueren, K. (2007). Educational choices in adolescence: The decision-making process,
antecedents, and consequences. In V. B. Skorikov & W. Patton (Eds.), Career development in childhood
and adolescence (pp. 203–219). Rotterdam, the Netherlands: Sense.
Gianakos, I. (1999). Patterns of career choice and career decision-making self-efficacy. Journal of Vocational
Behavior, 54, 244–258. doi:10.1006/jvbe.1998.1668
Guay, F., Ratelle, C. F., Senécal, C., Larose, S., & Deschěnes, A. (2006). Distinguishing developmental from
chronic career indecision: Self-efficacy, autonomy, and social support. Journal of Career Assessment, 42(2),
235–251. doi:10.1177/1069072705283975
Guichard, J., & Huteau, M. (2001). Psychologie de l’orientation [Vocational psychology]. Paris, France:
Dunod.
Hackett, G., & Betz, N. E. (1995). Self-efficacy and career choice and development. In J. E. Maddux (Ed.), Self-
efficacy, adaptation, and adjustment (pp. 249–280). New York, NY: Plenum Press.

54
Fabio et al. 55

Hage, S. M., Romano, J. L., Conayne, R. K., Kenny, M., Mattews, C., Schwartz, J. P., & Waldo, M. (2007). Best
practice guidelines on prevention practice, research, training, and social advocacy for psychologists. The
Counseling Psychologist, 35, 493–566. doi:10.1177/0011000006291411
Harren, V. A. (1979). A model of career decision making for college students. Journal of Vocational Behavior,
14, 119–133. doi:10.1016/0001-8791(79)90065-4
Hijazi, Y., Tatar, M., & Gati, I. (2004). Career decision-making difficulties among Israeli and Palestinian Arab
high-school seniors. Professional School Counseling, 8, 64–72.
Holland, J. L. (1997). Making vocational choices (3rd ed.). Odessa, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources.
Jackson, C. J., Furnham, A., & Lawty-Jones, M. (1999). Relationship between indecisiveness and neuroticism:
The moderating effect of a tough-minded culture. Personality and Individual Differences, 27, 789–800. doi:
10.1016/S0191-8869(99)00027-6
Kelly, K. R., & Shin, Y. J. (2009). Relation of neuroticism and negative career thoughts and feelings to lack of
information. Journal of Career Assessment, 17, 201–213. doi: 10.1177/1069072708329029
Kenny, M. E., & Bledsoe, M. (2005). Contributions of the relational context to career adaptability among urban
adolescents. Journal of Vocational Behavior, 66, 257–272. doi:10.1016/j.jvb.2004.10.002
Kenny, M. E., Horne, A. M., Orpinas, P., & Reese, L. E. (2009). Social justice and the challenge of preventive
interventions: An introduction. In M. E. Kenny, A. M. Horne, P. Orpinas, & L. E. Reese (Eds.), Realizing
social justice: The challenge of preventive interventions (pp. 3–14). Washington, DC: American
Psychological Association.
Lancaster, P. L., Rudolph, C., Perkins, S., & Patten, T. (1999). The reliability and validity of the career
decision difficulties questionnaire. Journal of Career Assessment, 7, 393–413. doi:10.1177/
106907279900700405
Lounsbury, J. W., Hutchens, T., & Loveland, J. M. (2005). An investigation of big five personality traits and
career decidedness among early and middle adolescents. Journal of Career Assessment, 13, 25–39. doi:10.
1177/106907270427027
Luzzo, D. A. (1993). Value of career-decision-making self-efficacy in predicting career-decision-making
attitudes and skills. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 40, 194–199. doi:10.1037/0022-0167.40.2.
194
Mann, L., Burnett, P., Radford, M., & Ford, S. (1997). The Melbourne Decision Making Inventory: An instru-
ment for measuring patterns for coping with decisional conflict. Journal of Behavioral Decision Making, 10,
1–19. doi:10.1002/(SICI)1099-0771(199703)10:1<1:: AID-BDM242>3.0.CO;2-X
Mau, W. C. (2000). Cultural differences in career decision-making styles and self-efficacy. Journal of
Vocational Behavior, 57, 365–378. doi:10.1006/jvbe.1999.1745.
Mau, W. C. (2001). Assessing career decision-making difficulties: A cross-cultural study. Journal of Career
Assessment, 9, 353–364. doi:10.1177/106907270100900403
Mayer, J. D., Salovey, P., & Caruso, D. R. (2002). Mayer-Salovey-Caruso Emotional Intelligence Test
(MSCEIT): User’s manual Toronto, Canada: Multi-Health Systems.
Nilsson, J. E., Flores, L. Y., Berkel, L. V., Schale, C. L., Linnemeyer, R. M., & Summer, I. (2007). International
career articles: A content analysis of four journals across 34 years. Journal of Vocational Behavior, 70,
602–613. doi:10.1016/j.jvb.2007.01.003
Nota, L., Pace, F., & Ferrari, L. (2008). Career Decision Self-Efficacy Scale–Short Form: Uno studio per l’adat-
tamento italiano [Career Decision Self-Efficacy Scale–Short Form: A study for Italian adaptation]. GIPO
Giornale Italiano di Psicologia dell’Orientamento, 9, 23–35.
Osipow, S. H. (1999). Assessing career indecision. Journal of Vocational Behavior, 55, 147–154. doi:10.1006/
jvbe.1999.1704
Osipow, S. H., Carney, C. G., Winer, J., Yanico, B., & Koschier, M. (1976). The Career Decision scale.
Columbus: OH: Marathon Consulting Press.
Osipow, S. H., & Gati, I. (1998). Construct and concurrent validity of the career decision-making difficulties
questionnaire. Journal of Career Assessment, 6, 345–364. doi:10.1177/106907279800600305

55
56 Journal of Career Assessment 21(1)

Palazzeschi, L., & Di Fabio, A. (in press). Difficultés à prendre des décisions de carrière et variables indivi-
duelles: au-delà de la personnalité, du sentiment d’efficacité et du soutien social perçu, le rôle de l’intelli-
gence émotionnelle [Career decision-making difficulties and individual variables: Behind personality,
self-efficacy and perceived social support, the role of emotional intelligence]. Pratiques Psychologiques.
Patalano, A. L., & Wengrovitz, S. M. (2006). Cross-cultural exploration of the Indecisiveness scale: A compar-
ison of Chinese and American men and women. Personality and Individual Differences, 41, 813–824. doi:
10.1016/j.paid.2006.03.023
Phillips, S. D., & Pazienza, N. J. (1988). History and theory of the assessment of career development and deci-
sion making. In W. B. Walsh & S. H. Osipow (Eds.), Career decision making (pp. 1-31). Hillsdale, NJ:
Erlbaum.
Rassin, E., & Muris, P. (2005a). Indecisiveness and the interpretation of ambiguous situations. Personality and
Individual Differences, 39, 1285–1291. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2005.06.006
Rassin, E., & Muris, P. (2005b). To be or not to be . . . indecisive: Gender differences, correlations with
obsessive-compulsive complaints, and behavioral manifestation. Personality and Individual Differences,
38, 1175–1181. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2004.07.014
Rassin, E., Muris, P., Franken, I., Smit, S., & Wong, M. (2007). Measuring general indecisiveness. Journal of
Psychopathology and Behavioral Assessment, 29, 61–68. doi:10.1007/s10862-006-9023-z
Saka, N., & Gati, I. (2007). Applying decision theory to facilitating adolescent career choices. In V. B. Skorikov
& W. Patton (Eds.), Career development in childhood and adolescence (pp. 181–202). Rotterdam, the
Netherlands: Sense.
Saka, N., Gati, I., & Kelly, K. R. (2008). Emotional and personality-related aspects of career decision-making
difficulties. Journal of Career Assessment, 16, 403–424. doi:10.1177/1069072708318900.
Savickas, M. L. (1995). Constructivist counseling for career indecision. The Career Development Quarterly,
43, 363–373.
Savickas, M. L. (2004). Vocational psychology, overview. In C. Spielberger (Ed.), Encyclopedia of applied
psychology (pp. 655–667). Amsterdam, the Netherlands: Elsevier.
Serling, D. A., & Betz, N. E. (1990). Development and evaluation of a measure of fear of commitment. Journal
of Counseling Psychology, 37(1), 91–97.
Skorikov, V. B. (2007). Adolescent career development and adjustment. In V. B. Skorikov & W. Patton (Eds.),
Career development in childhood and adolescence (pp. 237–254). Rotterdam, the Netherlands: Sense.
Skorikov, V. B., & Patton, W. (2007). Future directions in research on career development during childhood
and adolescence, In V. B. Skorikov & W. Patton (Eds.), Career development in childhood and adolescence
(pp. 325–336). Rotterdam, the Netherlands: Sense.
Spielberger, C. D., Gorsuch, R. L., & Lushene, R. E. (1968). State-trait anxiety inventory, Form x. Palo Alto,
CA: Consulting Psychology Press.
Tokar, D., Fischer, A., & Subich, L. M. (1998). Personality and vocational behavior: A selective review of the
literature, 1993-1997. Journal of Vocational Behavior, 53, 147–179. doi:10.1006/jvbe.1998.1660
Wanberg, C. R., & Muchinsky, P. M. (1992). A typology of career decision status: Validity extension of the
vocational decision status model. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 39, 71–80. doi: 10.1037/0022-0167.
39.1.71
Wechsler, D. (1981). Manual for the Wechsler Adult Intelligence scale–Revisited. New York, NY:
Psychological Corporation.
Wolfe, J. B., & Betz, N. E. (2004). The relationship of attachment variables to career decision-making self-
efficacy and fear commitment. The Career Development Quarterly, 52, 363–369.
Young, R. A., Valach, L., Ball, J., Paseluikho, M. A., Wong, Y. S., DeVries, R. J., & Turkel, H. (2001). Career
development in adolescence as a family project. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 48, 190–202. doi:10.
1037/0022-0167.48.2.190
Zimet, G. D., Dahlem, N. W., Zimet, S. G., & Farley, G. K. (1988). The multidimensional scale of perceived
social support. Journal of Personality Assessment, 52, 30–41. doi: 10.1207/s15327752jpa5201_2

56

View publication stats

You might also like