You are on page 1of 10

Applied Neuropsychology: Child

ISSN: (Print) (Online) Journal homepage: https://www.tandfonline.com/loi/hapc20

Phonological awareness and rapid automatized


naming as predictors of early literacy skills among
children with mild to borderline intellectual
functioning

Mohammed Alhwaiti

To cite this article: Mohammed Alhwaiti (2024) Phonological awareness and rapid automatized
naming as predictors of early literacy skills among children with mild to borderline intellectual
functioning, Applied Neuropsychology: Child, 13:1, 8-16, DOI: 10.1080/21622965.2022.2106863

To link to this article: https://doi.org/10.1080/21622965.2022.2106863

Published online: 17 Aug 2022.

Submit your article to this journal

Article views: 182

View related articles

View Crossmark data

Full Terms & Conditions of access and use can be found at


https://www.tandfonline.com/action/journalInformation?journalCode=hapc20
APPLIED NEUROPSYCHOLOGY: CHILD
2024, VOL. 13, NO. 1, 8–16
https://doi.org/10.1080/21622965.2022.2106863

Phonological awareness and rapid automatized naming as predictors of early


literacy skills among children with mild to borderline intellectual functioning
Mohammed Alhwaiti
Faculty of Education, Umm Al Qura University, Saudi Arabia

ABSTRACT KEYWORDS
The aim was to investigate the predicting role of phonological awareness (PA) and rapid automat- Early literacy skills; mild to
ized naming (RAN) on early literacy skills (e.g., word reading [WR] and spelling [SP]) among chil- borderline intellectual
dren with mild to borderline intellectual functioning. A total of 68 children in inclusive schools in functioning; phonological
awareness; rapid
grades 1 and 2, from 12 primary schools from Makka, classified as having intellectual disabilities automatized naming;
(ID) of unspecified origin were chosen to participate in this study. Measures of PA and RAN were spelling; word reading
correlated with measures of WR and SP in children with mild to borderline intellectual functioning.
This study advanced knowledge about the predictors (PA and RAN) of early literacy skills (WR and
SP) among children with mild to borderline intellectual functioning.

Introduction of language related to speech sounds (Eissa, 2014, 2017;


Sawi, 2013).
Children are taught letter-sound correspondence, spelling,
In typically developing children, PA is an essential skill
and how to read words from an early age in Kindergarten
for learning to read (Eissa, 2017). Many cases of reading dis-
(Vander Stappen & Van Reybroeck, 2018). However, this
abilities in children, adolescents, and adults can be traced
process may be more difficult for children with intellectual
back to poor PA skills (Dessemontet et al., 2021). In chil-
disabilities (ID). Compared to their normally developed dren with ID, there is increasing evidence that PA also plays
peers, children with ID may have considerable difficulties an important role in reading. It is strongly related to reading
with some early literacy skills such as word reading (WR) acquisition, and weaknesses in these skills are implicated in
and spelling (SP) (Eissa & Huseini, 2013; Soltani & Roslan, learning disorders (Adlof et al., 2015; Barker et al., 2013;
2013), as their ability to perform these skills often lags Sermier & de Chambrier, 2015; Soltani & Roslan, 2013).
behind their chronological and mental age. However, findings from several studies suggest that PA is
Mastering WR and SP skills is a vital ability for the an area of impairment in children with Down syndrome,
social, educational, and professional development of all chil- Williams syndrome, or Fragile X syndrome compared to
dren. Those with ID are no exception (de Chambrier et al., children who typically develop with mental age, verbal skills,
2021). Research on the precursors of reading is of great language skills, and reading (Adlof et al., 2015; Laing et al.,
importance in order to understand the nature of the under- 2001; Lemons & Fuchs, 2010; Menghini et al., 2004; Næss,
lying linguistic and cognitive processes in early literacy skills 2016; Steele et al., 2013). The results of the meta-analysis
(Mostafa, 2017; van Tilborg et al., 2014). (Næss, 2016) indicate that rhyme awareness (an important
Phonological awareness (PA) and rapid automatized PA task that refers to the awareness of words that rhyme
naming (RAN), although less studied among individuals and words that do not rhyme) in particular presents a
with ID, are among the most robust correlates of difficul- marked impairment in children with Down syndrome
ties in early literacy skills (e.g., WR and SP) (Soltani & (Najjar, 2014).
Roslan, 2013; Vander Stappen & Van Reybroeck, 2018). Furthermore, the evolution of their PA skills over time
PA involves knowledge of a particular type of word, as it remains unclear. It is well known (e.g., Eissa, 2017) that
is composed of speech elements and can be broken down children become increasingly sensitive to smaller and
into parts of sounds smaller than syllables. This is a mani- smaller parts of words as they grow older. Studies with chil-
festation of the important spoken language system in early dren with undetermined etiology within a narrow age range
reading. PA means awareness of the spoken language, as are not available. Studies examining the PA skills of children
words consist of a series of small sounds called micropho- with ID with unspecified or mixed etiologies are scarce.
nemic units, and PA is linguistic awareness that enables Borderline intellectual functioning has always been a dif-
the individual to employ information about the structure ficult concept (Wieland & Zitman, 2016). The term

CONTACT Mohammed Alhwaiti mmhwaiti@uqu.edu.sa Faculty of Education, Umm Al-Qura University, Makkah 21421, Saudi Arabia.
ß 2022 Taylor & Francis Group, LLC
APPLIED NEUROPSYCHOLOGY: CHILD 9

borderline intellectual functioning describes a group of peo- underlying the RAN–reading relation is still a subject of
ple who function on the border between normal intellectual debate. According to the double-deficit hypothesis, PA and
functioning and intellectual disability, between 1 and 2 RAN are two separable sources of reading dysfunction (de
standard deviations below the mean on the normal curve Chambrier et al., 2021). Some authors (e.g., Adlof et al.,
of the distribution of intelligence, roughly an IQ between 2015; Eissa, 2013, 2017; Sermier et al., 2017) indicate that
70 and 85 (Eissa & Adel, 2019; Hasanin, 2015). Children PA is an area of weakness in children with ID of different
with ID usually display a certain degree of phonological etiologies. In spite of the well-established link between RAN
deficit (Eissa, 2013) that may interfere with their perception and reading, key remaining questions exist regarding the
of the meaning of print (Al Said, 2014). Despite this, some nature of this relationship (Powell & Atkinson, 2021).
authors have listed successful PA interventions for children
with ID. For example, Eissa (2013) explored the effective-
Present study
ness of a PA training intervention on pre-reading skills for
children with ID. The results of this study indicated the The current study investigated the predicting role of PA
effectiveness of the program used in improving the pre- and RAN on early literacy skills (e.g., WR and SP)
reading skills of the target children and that the use of PA among children with mild to borderline intellectual func-
intervention programs in the classroom for children with tioning. Previous research (e.g., Eissa, 2013, 2017, Khalik,
speech and/or language problems can be effective in train- 2014, Mohammed & Amaal, 2012) has shown that IDs
ing in PA activities. may adversely affect the reading skills of children; how-
PA is seen as “the ability to define and manipulate the ever, a minimal amount is known about how PA and
phonemic structure of oral language” (Layton & Deeny, RAN contribute to and predict early literacy skills (WR
2002, p. 38), and the acquisition of PA involves learning two and SP).
things: first, that PA involves knowledge, and learning that The important role of PA in learning to read for ID chil-
Words can be divided into parts of sounds smaller than the dren was evidenced mostly for reading in languages other
syllable, and, second, PA involves learning about the pho- than Arabic language, which is likely to be qualified as a
nemes themselves (Al Said, 2014) and awareness of the mixed-bag for orthographic depth (Abu-Rabia & Hanna,
phonological structure of the word helps the student to 2022; Eissa, 2017; Taha & Saiegh-Haddad, 2017). Arabic has
identify the relationships between the spoken form of the two coexisting orthographies, voweled and unvoweled. The
word and its written representation. former follows phonographic principles, mapping the phon-
Moreover, RAN can be used as a useful precursor when emic structure of words, whereas the unvoweled orthog-
diagnosing reading and attentional problems, particularly in raphy uses a consonantal alphabet and follows
children aged from 5 to 9 (Areces et al., 2018). It has been morphographic principles, focusing on morphological struc-
highlighted that children with ID often have difficulties in ture (Mahmoud et al., 2019). RAN and PA are independent
reading, display a skewed developmental pattern and weak- deficits that impact differently on spelling and reading out-
nesses in PA (Channell et al., 2013; Eissa, 2013; Lemons comes (Torppa et al., 2013).
et al., 2013). Implicit phonological tasks are those in which The aforementioned review of literature makes clear that
phonological processing is automatically performed, such as both PA and RAN may influence early literacy skills (e.g.,
verbal short-term memory tasks or RAN (Stampoltzis et al., WR and SP) among children with mild to borderline intel-
2020). PA and letter-sound knowledge were found to be sig- lectual functioning. However, until now, to my knowledge,
nificant predictors of word identification and reading com- these two constructs have not yet been investigated within
prehension in different profiles of children with ID one design, particularly with children with mild to border-
(Dessemontet et al., 2021). line intellectual functioning.
The contribution of RAN to reading acquisition and
reading difficulties has also been extensively studied among
normal and learning disabled children (Ara ujo et al., 2015;
Hypotheses
Georgiou et al., 2013; Landerl et al., 2019). Bowers et al. In this study, to gain insight into the predictive role of PA
(1999) proposed that RAN predicts reading because it con- and RAN to early literacy skills (e.g., WR and SP) for chil-
tributes to the development of orthographic knowledge. Kail dren with mild to borderline intellectual functioning, the
et al. (1999) argued that RAN and reading are related following hypotheses were tested.
because they both rely on general processing speed, which is
Hypothesis 1: There is a positive correlation among PA,
critical in tasks that require integration of information
RAN, WR, and SP for children with borderline intellectual
within and between sub-processes. Students with dyslexia
functioning.
scored worse in most PA, RAN, and reading fluency meas-
ures in comparison to their non-dyslexic peers (Stampoltzis Hypothesis 2: There are combined effects of PA and RAN
et al., 2020). However, RAN of individuals with ID have on WR and SP for children with borderline intellectual
been rarely studied (de Chambrier et al., 2021). A study by functioning.
van Tilborg et al. (2014) found that the ID group (7.6 years)
was more than two times slower than the typically develop- Hypothesis 3: PA and RAN contribute to WR and SP for
ing (TD) group (6 years). However, the mechanism children with borderline intellectual functioning.
10 M. ALHWAITI

Table 1. Participant demographics.


PA N ¼ 68 children
IQ ranged from 16.57 to 18.59 (M ¼ 17.02, SD ¼ 1.02)
Age ranges from 7 to 8 years old (M ¼ 7.8, SD ¼ 1.033)
Boys 53 (77.9%)
word reading
Girls 15 (22.1%)
Socioeconomic status low to medium socioeconomic status
RAN
there is no score per scale. The total score is 36. The items
spelling
are organized in ascending difficulty over the course of three
Figure 1. Research Model. sets (A, Ab, and B); the B set is more difficult than Ab on
average, which in turn is more difficult than the A set, on
average. The items consist in a drawing with a missing part,
which the individual needs to complete by choosing one
Method option among six alternative responses. There is only one
correct answer for each item. The respondents score one for
Quantitative research was carried out and questionnaires each correct response and zero for each wrong response.
were used to obtain data from the target children. The minimum score is zero and the maximum score is 36.
The instrument can be applied individually or in groups and
Design there are no time limitations (Eissa, 2020). Raw scores were
used for analysis. Good reliability (split-half reliability coeffi-
A survey method was used. The independent variables were cient ¼ .87) was determined for this task.
PA and RAN, while the dependent variables were WR
and SP.
The research model is shown in Figure 1. PA scale
Six PA tests from a study by Eissa (2017) included Phoneme
Deletion (PD), Phoneme substitution—Beginning Sounds
Participants (BS), Phoneme Substitution—Ending Sounds (ES), Phoneme
A total of 68 children in inclusive schools in grades 1 and 2, Substitution—Middle Sounds (MS), Rhyme Recognition
from 12 primary schools from Makka, classified as having
(RR), and Blending Phonemes (BP).
ID of unspecified origin were chosen to participate in this
study. Each child participant met the criteria for mild to
Phoneme deletion (PD). In the PD test, the initial sound
borderline intellectual functioning (MBIF). ID scores ranged
location is deleted and, as such, the resultant answer is a
from 50 to 84, which was consistent with MBIF (Eissa &
non-word. For example, listen to the word book. If you take
Hasanein, 2020). Criteria for these children to be included
away the/b/sound, only/ook/is left (3 items).
were: (a) they are below-average intellectual functioning and
limitations in social and adaptive behavior (American
Psychiatric Association, 2013 ); (b) they have normal or cor- Phoneme substitution—beginning sounds (BS). In the BS
rected normal vision and sufficient hearing; (c) they come test, the child replaces the first phoneme sound of a given
from families with low to medium socioeconomic status; word with a new sound. This task requires adding the new
and (d) they come from an Arabic-speaking family. All chil- sound to the beginning. For example, change the first sound
dren attended inclusion primary schools. All children were in cat to h. What is the new word?” (3 items).
right-handed. Participants were 53 boys and 15 girls. Their
chronological age ranged from 7 to 8 years old with a mean Phoneme substitution—ending sounds (ES). In the ES test,
of 7.8. Their IQ scores using Raven’s Colored Progressive the child is prompted to replace one phonemic unit in this
Matrices Test (CPM; Raven et al., 1986) ranged from 16.57 task. The child is asked to change the last sound in that
to 18.59 (M ¼ 17.02, SD ¼ 1.02). Table 1 shows demographic word to make a new word. For example, if I say the word
and diagnostic characteristics of the participants. can and change the last sound to/p/, the new word is cap
(3 items).

Measures Phoneme substitution—Middle sounds (MS). In the MS


IQ measure test, the child is focusing on the middle sound in the task.
Raven’s CPM is a nonverbal intelligence test, representative For example, if I say the word pan, change the middle sound
of general intellectual capacity or the “g” factor proposed by to/i/, the new word is pin (3 items).
Spearman (Eissa, 2020; Eissa & Al Farahati, 2012). CPM was
developed to assess children aged from 5 to 11 years old, Rhyme recognition (RR). In the RR test, two words rhyme
mentally disabled individuals, and the elderly. The test is when they have the same sound at the end. The child listens
composed of three sets/scales (A, Ab, and B) with 12 items to two words and is asked whether they are rhyme or not
each. The sum of these sets composes the general score and and responds yes or no (3 items).
APPLIED NEUROPSYCHOLOGY: CHILD 11

Figure 2. Items in Word Reading Test.

Blending phonemes (BP). In the BP test, the task requires the 20 words. This was administered individually. The cor-
the child to synthesize or blend each sound in the word. For rected pronounced word was given one point. While the
example, /k//i//te/ (3 items). incorrect was given zero. Raw scores were used for analysis.
The internal consistency of the survey was measured Good reliability (split-half reliability coefficient ¼ .79) was
through Cronbach’s alpha estimated at 0.87, 0.85, 0.83, 0.82, determined for this task.
0.84, and 0.86 for Phoneme Deletion (PD), Phoneme substi-
tution—Beginning Sounds (BS), Phoneme Substitution—
Ending Sounds (ES), Phoneme Substitution—Middle Sounds SP test
(MS), Rhyme Recognition (RR), and Blending Phonemes A list of words selected from the students’ book to ensure
(BP), respectively. A group of 5 experts examined the con- that those words are familiar to children was designed par-
tent validity. They indicated whether questions were irrele- ticularly for the SP test in this study. A card with 8 words
vant or highly relevant. All items were highly relevant. The was presented. The child was asked to spell the 8 words (see
content validity index at the item level was (I-CVI) ¼ 0.90. Figure 3). The children were tested individually. The cor-
rected response was given a point, while the wrong one was
RAN scale given a zero.
The author designed RAN measures of numbers, letters, col- Raw scores were used for analysis. Good reliability (split-
ors, and objects based on Denckla and Rudel’s scale (1974) half reliability coefficient ¼ .80) was determined for
and on the Arabic Language Book taught in inclusive this task.
schools in grades 1 and 2. The four tests of RAN consist of Data were collected through structured self-administered
a chart containing 60 different items. The items used in questionnaires. After obtaining written consents from
RAN digit were numbers from 1 to 15. The items used in parents and the school principal, children were tested in
RAN letter (15 letters) were /s/,/n/,/k/o/s/w/i/n/m/t/e/a/c/d/ their schools. Each round of the questionnaire was adminis-
b. The items used in RAN color (15 colors) were blue, red, tered by the researcher with the support of the school’s
yellow, purple, white, black, gray, pink, silver, brown, gold, teachers and conducted in a room other than their regu-
mauve, cream, olive, and orange. The items used in RAN lar classroom.
object (15 objects) were the pictures of book, plane, kite, Invalid questionnaires (n ¼ 6), including those that were
car, house, ladder, spoon, pencil, pen, desk, table, shoes, incomplete or provided the same response for all items or
cup, glass, and computer. The children were tested individu- with many missing values, were eliminated. Pearson correl-
ally. The corrected response was given a point, while the ation (R) and multiple regression (MRA) were used to ana-
wrong one was given zero. lyze data.
The internal consistency of the survey was measured Children were informed about their role in the study, the
through Cronbach’s alpha estimated at 0.83, 0.82, 0.84, and purpose of the study, and the data collection methods. Each
0.86 for RAN digit, RAN letter, RAN color, and RAN object, child was encouraged to continue through the end of the
respectively. A group of 5 experts examined the content val- study but were informed that they were free to discontinue
idity. They indicated whether questions were irrelevant or at any time.
highly relevant. All items were highly relevant. A content
validity index at the item level was (I-CVI) ¼ 0.90.
Results
WR test Measurement scores of PA, RAN, WR, and SP
A reading a list of words selected from the students’ book to
ensure that those words are familiar to children was Mean, standard deviation, skewness, and kurtosis of PA,
designed particularly for this study. A card with four col- RAN, WR, and SP measures are provided in Table 2.
umns, where there are 20 words, eight in each column was Skewness and kurtosis values for all measures indicated nor-
presented (see Figure 2). The child is asked to pronounce mal distributions of scores (Dorothy & Brian, 2018).
12 M. ALHWAITI

Figure 3. Items in Spelling Test.

individual contributions to the prediction of SP. The beta


Table 2. Mean, standard deviation, range, skewness, and kurtosis of weight that represented the relative contribution of the PA
study variables. to the prediction was observed.
Variable Mean SD Range Skewness Kurtosis
PA 10.00 .91 6–12 –.342 .178
RAN 36.22 .98 32–39 –.428 .195 Discussion
WR 11.12 .95 6–13 .132 –.865
SP 3.24 .94 2–5 .376 –.645 As has been shown, measures of PA and RAN were corre-
Notes. PA ¼ phonological awareness; RAN ¼ rapid automatized naming; lated with measures of WR and SP in children with mild to
WR ¼ word reading; SP ¼ spelling.
borderline intellectual functioning. The results imply that
instruction in PA and RAN should facilitate the acquisition
Table 3. Inter-correlations of study variables. of early WR and SP in such children. Results of this study
Variable PA RAN WR SP. also extend the literature on PA and RAN to children with
PA mild to borderline intellectual functioning of unspecified
RAN .622 origin; as noted earlier, participants in most studies in this
WR .636 .661
SP .642 .655 .624
literature have had either Down syndrome or Williams syn-
Notes. PA ¼ phonological awareness; RAN ¼ rapid automatized naming;
drome (Saunders & DeFulio, 2007).
WR ¼ word reading; SP ¼ spelling. PA has been identified as strong correlates of WR and SP
in both typically developing and participants with ID
Inter-correlations (Saunders & DeFulio, 2007; Soltani & Roslan, 2013).
Although some authors in the literature (e.g., Peterson et al.,
Table 3 shows the means and inter-correlations of PA, 2018) found only modest prediction from kindergarten and
RAN, WR, and SP. Table 3 shows that there are significant first-grade PA to the subsequent reading assessments, on the
correlations among PA, RAN, WR, and SP. PA correlates contrary, this study found that PA was a good predictor of
positively with RAN, and SP. RAN correlates positively with WR and SP.
WR and SP. The results of this study are aligned with other research
studies in the literature (e.g., M€
uller & Brady, 2001; Muter
Prediction et al., 2004; Vellutino et al., 2004; Wagner et al., 1994;
Wimmer et al., 1991) which found that PA is consistently
As shown in Table 4, PA yielded an R coefficient of 0.301 found to predict reading and spelling development in the
and Adjusted R Square of 0.311. This shows that 31.1% of normal range; although some other studies conducted within
the total variance in WR of those who participated in the transparent orthographies report that PA is either marginally
study is accounted for by the combination of PA. The table related to early literacy development (Aarnoutse et al., 2005;
also indicates that the analysis of variance of the multiple Holopainen et al., 2001) or diminishes as a reliable predictor
regression data produced an F-ratio value significant at 0.01 after the first two grades (Lervåg et al., 2009; Verhagen
level. The table also shows that PA made significant individ- et al., 2008). Moreover, RAN exerts the greatest influence in
ual contributions to the prediction of WR. The beta weight both WR and SP at this age, which is in alignment with the
that represented the relative contribution of the PA to the findings of Stainthorp et al. (2013) who found that the par-
prediction was observed. ticipants with low naming performance were significantly
As shown in Table 5, PA yielded an R coefficient of poorer spellers overall and had specific difficulty in spelling
0.340 and Adjusted R Square of 0.345. This shows that irregular words.
34.5% of the total variance in SP of those who participated Although findings within and across orthographies are
in the study is accounted for by the combination of PA. The mixed, the general pattern seems to be that the association
table also indicates that the analysis of variance of the mul- between PA and reading is more pronounced in opaque
tiple regression data produced an F-ratio value significant at orthographies than in several transparent European orthog-
0.01 level. The table also shows that PA made significant raphies. One possible explanation of our findings is that the
APPLIED NEUROPSYCHOLOGY: CHILD 13

Table 4. The regression results (model summary).


Change statistics
Model R R Square Adjusted R Square Std. Error of the Estimate R Square change F Change df1 df2 Sig. F change
1 0.301a 0.307 0.311 4.12034 0. 311 3.441 1 65 0.000
Model Sum of Squares df Mean Square F Sig.
Regression 5.297030 1 5.29.703 4.231 0.000
Residual 9.213733 63 143.1964
Total 14.865430 64
Model Unstandardized coefficients Standardized coefficients t P
B Std error Beta 3.786 0.000
I (constant) 3.460 1.002 0.382
PA 0.346 0.077 3.697 0.000
Notes. aPredictors (constant), PA ¼ phonological awareness.

Table 5. The regression results (model summary).


Change statistics
Model R R Square Adjusted R Square Std. Error of the Estimate R Square change F Change df1 df2 Sig. F change
1 0.343a 0.340 0.345 4.56382 0. 345 3.601 1 65 0.000
Model Sum of Squares df Mean Square F Sig.
Regression 6.647030 1 6.647030 4.446 0.000
Residual 10.813766 63 171.647079
Total 16.865030 64
Model Unstandardized coefficients Standardized coefficients t p
B Std error Beta 3.889 0.000
I (constant) 3.840 1.002 0.398
PA 0.366 0.070 3.711 0.000
Notes. Predictors (constant), PA ¼ phonological awareness, SP ¼ spelling.
a

Arabic language is a combination of both opacity and trans- be implemented, it will positively impact early literacy skills
parency. As Abu-Rabia and Hanna (2022) stated, “Arabic (WR and SP) among children with mild to borderline intel-
belongs to the cluster of languages known as ‘Semitic lan- lectual functioning. In theory, the findings will provide a
guages.’ Unlike other Semitic languages, spoken Arabic dif- theoretical reference for future research on PA and RAN
fers from one geographic location to another. This utilization as predictors and better understand these influ-
difference is expressed and reflected in practically all of the encing factors on early literacy skills (WR and SP). In prac-
linguistic fields: phonological, morphological, syntactic and tice, this is conducive for the promotion of PA and RAN for
semantic.” (p. 301). the improvement of early literacy skills (WR and SP) among
Again, results of this study align with other research children with mild to borderline intellectual functioning.
studies within the literature that have shown that RAN pre- Finally, the assessment and better understanding of reading
dicts later literacy performance in both transparent and opa- precursors in children with ID may contribute to the devel-
que orthographies (e.g., de Jong & van der Leij, 2002; opment of more effective intervention.
Georgiou et al., 2008; Kirby et al., 2003; Lervåg et al., 2009;
Wimmer et al., 2000)
Thus, the overall impression from the present analyses is Limitations and future research
that there are more similarities than dissimilarities in the
prediction of reading from PA and RAN in different lan- There are some limitations of this study that need to be
guages. Much less is known about the association between addressed in future studies. First, because of the nature of
RAN and SP. The few studies that have investigated this this study, children were tested only once on all variables.
relationship in English report that RAN predicts SP concur- As a result, it is not likely to tap into the development of
rently (Savage et al., 2008). The findings of this study, early literacy skills. Future research needs to follow a longi-
although in the Arabic language, concur with these findings, tudinal perspective, in order to strengthen the relationships
which proves the flexibility of the Arabic language. between the variables and the reliability of the hypotheses,
and for the possibility to investigate the development of
early literacy skills in children. Second, the number of chil-
Conclusions dren was small. Therefore, findings of the present study
This study advanced knowledge about the predictors (PA need therefore be interpreted with caution.
and RAN) of early literacy skills (WR and SP) among chil-
dren with mild to borderline intellectual functioning. In this
regard, if the suggestive measures proposed in this study for Institutional review board statement
children with mild to borderline intellectual functioning can Not applicable.
14 M. ALHWAITI

Informed consent statement children. Cortex, 10(2), 186–202. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0010-


9452(74)80009-2
Not applicable. Dessemontet, R., Chambrier, A., Martinet, C., Natalina, M., & Linder,
A. (2021). Effects of a phonics-based intervention on the reading
skills of students with intellectual disability. Research in
Disclosure statement Developmental Disabilities, 111, 103883. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.
ridd.2021.103883
The author declares no conflict of interest. Dorothy, J., & Brian, C. (2018). Normal Distribution. In Bruce B. Frey
(Ed.), The SAGE encyclopedia of educational research, measurement,
and evaluation. SAGE Publications, Inc.
ORCID Eissa, M. (2013). The effectiveness of a phonological awareness training
intervention on pre-reading skills of children with mental retard-
Mohammed Alhwaiti http://orcid.org/0000-0002-0048-7387
ation. Psycho-Educational Research Reviews, 2(2), 11–21. https://
www.journals.lapub.co.uk/index.php/perr/article/view/211.
Eissa, M. (2014). The effect of a phonological awareness intervention
Data availability statement program on phonological memory, phonological sensitivity, and
Data sharing not applicable. The data are not publicly available due to metaphonological abilities of preschool children at-risk for reading
participants’ privacy. disabilities. Psycho-Educational Research Reviews, 3(2), 71–83.
Retrieved from https://www.perrjournal.com/index.php/perrjournal/
article/view/359
References Eissa, M. (2017). Are phonological awareness intervention programs
effective for children with disabilities? A systematic review. Psycho-
Aarnoutse, C., van Leeuwe, J., & Verhoeven, L. (2005). Early literacy Educational Research Reviews, 6(3), 11–22. https://www.journals.
from a longitudinal perspective. Educational Research and lapub.co.uk/index.php/perr/article/view/177.
Evaluation, 11(3), 253–275. https://doi.org/10.1080/08993400 Eissa, M. (2020). Manual: Raven’s Coloured Progressive Matrices
500101054 (MPC). Dar El-Fikr.
Abu-Rabia, S., & Hanna, M. (2022). The impact of digital books on Eissa, M., & Adel, M. (2019). Defining and determining intellectual dis-
reading in Arabic orthography. Creative Education, 13(01), 296–319. ability (intellectual developmental disorder): Insights from DSM-5.
https://doi.org/10.4236/ce.2022.131018 Psycho-Educational Research Reviews, 8(1), 51–54. https://www.jour-
Adlof, S. M., Klusek, J., Shinkareva, S. V., Robinson, M. L., & Roberts, nals.lapub.co.uk/index.php/perr/article/view/352.
J. E. (2015). Phonological awareness and reading in boys with fragile Eissa, M., & Al Farahati, A. (2012). The Raven’s colored progressive
X syndrome. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, and Allied matrices test: A normative data for gifted students in Egypt Aged
Disciplines, 56(1), 30–39. https://doi.org/10.1111/jcpp.12267 10–17. Psycho-Educational Research Reviews, 1(1), 86–92. https://
Al Said, A. (2014). The Effectiveness of a Phonological Awareness www.perrjournal.com/index.php/perrjournal/article/view/403
Training Intervention on Phonological Working Memory of Eissa, M. A., & Huseini, H. H. (2013). Effect of multisensory approach
Children with Intellectual Disabilities. Psycho-Educational Research on increasing math skills children with mild intellectual disabilities.
Reviews, 3(1), 50–55. Retrieved from https://www.perrjournal.com/ Psycho-Educational Research Reviews, 2(3), 73–82. https://www.perr-
index.php/perrjournal/article/view/368 journal.com/index.php/perrjournal/article/view/375
American Psychiatric Association (2013). Diagnostic and Statistical Eissa, M., & Hasanein, H. (2020). The effect of social information proc-
manual of mental disorders. Washington DC: APA. essing (SIP) model intervention on reducing challenging behavior
Araujo, J. (2015). La formacion de mediadores para la promocion de la among children with mild to borderline intellectual functioning.
lectura. Centro de estudios literarios. Universidad de Complutense, Electronic Journal of Research in Educational Psychology, 18(2),
327–344. https://doi.org/10.25115/ejrep.v18i51.2986
12.
Georgiou, G. K., Parrila, R., Cui, Y., & Papadopoulos, T. C. (2013).
Areces, D., Garcıa, T., Gonzalez-Castro, P., Alvarez-Garcıa, D., &
Why is rapid automatized naming related to reading? Journal of
Rodrıguez, C. (2018). Naming speed as a predictive diagnostic meas-
Experimental Child Psychology, 115(1), 218–225. https://doi.org/10.
ure in reading and attentional problems. Child Neuropsychology,
1016/j.jecp.2012.10.015
24(8), 1115–1128. https://doi.org/10.1080/09297049.2017.1391191
Georgiou, G. K., Parrila, R., & Papadopoulos, T. C. (2008). Predictors
Barker, R. M., Sevcik, R. A., Morris, R. D., & Romski, M. (2013). A
of word decoding and reading fluency across languages varying in
model of phonological processing, language, and reading for stu-
orthographic consistency. Journal of Educational Psychology, 100(3),
dents with mild intellectual disability. American Journal on 566–580. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-0663.100.3.566
Intellectual and Developmental Disabilities, 118(5), 365–380. http:// Hasanin, H. (2015). The effect of social information processing model
dx.doi.org/10.1352/1944-7558-118.5.365 training on improving social behaviour of with intellectual disabil-
Bowers, P. G., Sunseth, K., & Golden, J. (1999). The route between ities. Psycho-Educational Research Reviews, 4(1), 10–15. Retrieved
rapid naming and reading progress. Scientific Studies of Reading, from https://www.perrjournal.com/index.php/perrjournal/article/
3(1), 31–53. https://doi.org/10.1207/s1532799xssr0301_2 view/334
Channell, M. M., Loveall, S. J., & Conners, F. A. (2013). Strengths and Holopainen, L., Ahonen, T., & Lyytinen, H. (2001). Predicting delay in
weaknesses in reading skills of youth with intellectual disabilities. reading achievement in a highly transparent language. Journal of
Research in Developmental Disabilities, 34(2), 776–787. https://doi. Learning Disabilities, 34(5), 401–413. https://doi.org/10.1177/
org/10.1016/j.ridd.2012.10.010 002221940103400502
de Chambrier, A. F., Sermier Dessemontet, R., Martinet, C., & Fayol, Kail, R., Hall, L. K., & Caskey, B. J. (1999). Processing speed, exposure
M. (2021). Rapid automatized naming skills of children with intel- to print, and naming speed. Applied Psycholinguistics, 20(2),
lectual disability. Heliyon, 7(5), e06944. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.heli- 303–314. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0142716499002076
yon.2021.e06944. Khalik, A. (2014). The effectiveness of a phonological awareness train-
de Jong, P. F., & van der Leij, A. (2002). Effects of phonological abil- ing intervention on phonological working memory of children with
ities and linguistic comprehension on the development of reading. intellectual disabilities. Psycho-Educational Research Reviews, 3(1),
Scientific Studies of Reading, 6(1), 51–77. https://doi.org/10.1207/ 50–55. https://www.journals.lapub.co.uk/index.php/perr/article/view/
S1532799XSSR0601_03 230.
Denckla, M. B., & Rudel, R. (1974). Rapid ‘‘automatized’’ Kirby, J. R., Parrila, R., & Pfeiffer, S. L. (2003). Naming speed and
naming of pictures, objects, colors, letters, and numbers by normal phonological awareness as predictors of reading development.
APPLIED NEUROPSYCHOLOGY: CHILD 15

Journal of Educational Psychology, 95(3), 453–464. https://doi.org/10. with mild mental retardation. American Journal of Mental
1037/0022-0663.95.3.453 Retardation, 112(3), 155–166. https://doi.org/10.1352/0895-8017
Laing, E., Hulme, C., Grant, J., & Karmiloff-Smith, A. (2001). Learning Savage, R., Pillay, V., & Melidona, S. (2008). Rapid serial naming is a
to read in Williams syndrome: Looking beneath the surface of atyp- unique predictor of spelling in children. Journal of Learning
ical reading development. Journal of Child Psychology and Disabilities, 41(3), 235–250. https://doi.org/10.1177/00222194083
Psychiatry, and Allied Disciplines, 42(6), 729–739. https://doi.org/10. 15814
1111/1469-7610.00769 Sawi, R. A. S. (2013). Preventing early reading disabilities in preschool
Landerl, K., Freudenthaler, H. H., Heene, M., De Jong, P. F., children at-risk for reading failure: A phonological awareness-based
Desrochers, A., Manolitsis, G., Parrila, R., & Georgiou, G. K. (2019). program. Psycho-Educational Research Reviews, 2(1), 13–22. https://
Phonological awareness and rapid automatized naming as longitu- www.perrjournal.com/index.php/perrjournal/article/view/383
dinal predictors of reading in five alphabetic orthographies with Sermier, R., & de Chambrier, A. F. (2015). The role of phonological
varying degrees of consistency. Scientific Studies of Reading, 23(3), awareness and letter-sound knowledge in the reading development
220–234. https://doi.org/10.1080/10888438.2018.1510936 of children with intellectual disabilities. Research in Developmental
Layton, L. & Deeny, K (2002). Sound practice: phonological awareness Disabilities, 41, 1–12. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.ridd.2015.04.001
in the classroom. London: D. Fulton Publishers. Sermier, R., de Chambrier, A.-F., Martinet, C., Moser, U., & Bayer, N.
Lemons, C. J., & Fuchs, D. (2010). Phonological awareness of children (2017). Exploring strengths and weaknesses in phonological aware-
with Down syndrome: Its role in learning to read and the effective- ness in primary pupils with intellectual disability. American Journal
ness of related interventions. Research in Developmental Disabilities, on Intellectual and Developmental Disabilities, 122(6), 476–491.
31(2), 316–330. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ridd.2009.11.002 https://doi.org/10.1352/1944-7558-122.6.476
Lemons, C. J., Mrachko, A. A., Kostewicz, D. E., Paterra, M. F. (2013). Soltani, A., & Roslan, S. (2013). Contributions of phonological aware-
Effectiveness of decoding and phonological awareness interventions ness, phonological short-term memory, and rapid automated nam-
for children with Down Syndrome. Exceptional Children, 79, 67–90. ing, toward decoding ability in students with mild intellectual
Lervåg, A., Bråten, I., & Hulme, C. (2009). The cognitive and linguistic disability. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 34(3), 1090–1099.
foundations of early reading development: A Norwegian latent vari- https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ridd.2012.12.005
able longitudinal study. Developmental Psychology, 45(3), 764–781. Stainthorp, R., Powell, D., & Stuart, M. (2013). The relationship
https://doi.org/10.1037/a0014132. between rapid naming and word spelling in English. Journal of
Mahmoud, G., Abdel, A., Hamzeh, D., & Mohammed, A. (2019). Research in Reading, 36(4), n/a–388. https://doi.org/10.1111/JRIR.
Effects of rapid automatized naming and phonological awareness 12002
deficits on the reading ability of Arabic-speaking elementary stu- Stampoltzis, A., Plakida, E., & Peristeri, E. (2020). Rapid Automatized
dents. Applied Neuropsychology: Child, 10(1), 1–13. https://doi.org/ Naming (RAN) and its relationship to phonological awareness and
10.1080/21622965.2019.1585247 reading: A pilot study in a greek sample of students with Dyslexia.
Menghini, D., Verucci, L., & Vicari, S. (2004). Reading and phono- Open Journal of Modern Linguistics, 10(03), 174–194. https://doi.org/
logical awareness in Williams syndrome. Neuropsychology, 18(1), 10.4236/ojml.2020.103011
29–37. https://doi.org/10.1037/0894-4105.18.1.29 Steele, A., Scerif, G., Cornish, K., & Karmiloff-Smith, A. (2013).
Mohammed, A., & Amaal, A. (2012). Effectiveness of a phonological Learning to read in Williams syndrome and Down syndrome:
awareness training intervention on word recognition ability of chil- Syndrome specific precursors and developmental trajectories.
dren with. Autism Spectrum Disorder, 1(1), 2–10. https://www.jour- Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, and Allied Disciplines,
nals.lapub.co.uk/index.php/perr/article/view/83. 54(7), 754–762. https://doi.org/10.1111/jcpp.12070
Mostafa, A. A. (2017). The effect of using multiple intelligences on Taha, H., & Saiegh-Haddad, E. (2017). Morphology and spelling in
some basic reading skills of first graders at-risk for reading disabil- Arabic: Development and interface. Journal of Psycholinguistic
ities. Psycho-Educational Research Reviews, 6(3), 109–116. https:// Research, 46(1), 27–38. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10936-016-9425-3.
www.perrjournal.com/index.php/perrjournal/article/view/267 Torppa, M., Parrila, R., Niemi, P., Lerkkanen, M.-K., Poikkeus, A.-M.,
M€uller, K., & Brady, S. (2001). Correlates of early reading perfomance & Nurmi, J.-E. (2013). The double-deficit hypothesis in the transpar-
in a transparent orthography. Reading and Writing, 14(7/8), ent Finnish orthography: A longitudinal study from kindergarten to
757–799. https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1012217704834 Grade 2. Reading and Writing, 26(8), 1353–1380. https://doi.org/10.
Muter, V., Hulme, C., Snowling, M. J., & Stevenson, J. (2004). 1007/s11145-012-9423-2
Phonemes, rimes, vocabulary, and grammatical skills as foundations van Tilborg, A., Segers, E., van Balkom, H., & Verhoeven, L. (2014).
of early reading development: Evidence from a longitudinal study. Predictors of early literacy skills in children with intellectual disabil-
Developmental Psychology, 40(5), 665–681. https://doi.org/10.1037/ ities: A clinical perspective. Research in Developmental Disabilities,
0012-1649.40.5.665 35(7), 1674–1685. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ridd.2014.03.025
Næss, K. (2016). Development of phonological awareness in Down syn- Vander Stappen, C., & Van Reybroeck, M. (2018). Phonological aware-
drome: A meta analysis and empirical study. Developmental ness and rapid automatized naming are independent phonological
Psychology, 52(2), 177–190. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0039840 competencies with specific impacts on word reading and spelling:
Najjar, K. R. (2014). Differential diagnosis of autism and asperger’s An intervention study. Frontiers in Psychology, 9, 320. https://doi.
syndrome. Psycho-Educational Research Reviews, 3(2), 42–55. org/10.3389/fpsyg.2018.00320
https://www.perrjournal.com/index.php/perrjournal/article/view/357 Vellutino, F. R., Fletcher, J. M., Snowling, M. J., & Scanlon, D. M.
Peterson, R. L., Arnett, A. B., Pennington, B. F., Byrne, B., Samuelsson, (2004). Specific reading disability (dyslexia): What have we learned
S., & Olson, R. K. (2018). Literacy acquisition influences children’s in the past four decades? Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry,
rapid automatized naming. Developmental Science, 21(3), 1–9. and Allied Disciplines, 45(1), 2–40. https://doi.org/10.1046/j.0021-
https://doi.org/10.1111/desc.12589 9630.2003.00305.x.
Powell, D., & Atkinson, L. (2021). Unraveling the links between rapid Verhagen, W., Aarnoutse, C., & van Leeuwe, J. (2008). Phonological
automatized naming (RAN), phonological awareness, and reading. awareness and naming speed in the prediction of Dutch children’s
Journal of Educational Psychology, 113 (4), 706–718. https://doi.org/ word recognition. Scientific Studies of Reading, 12(4), 301–324.
10.1037/edu0000625 https://doi.org/10.1080/10888430802132030
Raven, J. C., Court, J. H., & Raven, J. (1986). Manual for Raven’s pro- Wagner, R. K., Torgesen, J. K., & Rashotte, C. A. (1994). Development
gressive matrices and vocabulary scales, Research Supplement No. 3. of reading-related phonological processing abilities: New evidence of
London: Lewis. bidirectional causality from a latent variable longitudinal study.
Saunders, K. J., & DeFulio, A. (2007). Phonological awareness and Developmental Psychology, 30(1), 73–87. https://doi.org/10.1037/
rapid naming predict word attack and word identification in adults 0012-1649.30.1.73
16 M. ALHWAITI

Wieland, J., & Zitman, F. G. (2016). It is time to bring borderline intel- consequence than prediction but still important. Cognition, 40(3),
lectual functioning back into the main fold of classification systems. 219–249. https://doi.org/10.1016/0010-0277(91)90026-Z
BJPsych Bulletin, 40(4), 204–206. https://doi.org/10.1192/pb.bp.115. Wimmer, H., Mayringer, H., & Landerl, K. (2000). The double-deficit
051490. hypothesis and difficulties in learning to read a regular orthography.
Wimmer, H., Landerl, K., Linortner, R., & Hummer, P. (1991). The Journal of Educational Psychology, 92(4), 668–680. https://doi.org/10.
relationship of phonemic awareness to reading acquisition: More 1037/0022-0663.92.4.668

You might also like