You are on page 1of 21

Scholarly Article

Journal of Service Research


2023, Vol. 0(0) 1–21
Being Alone or Together: How Frontline © The Author(s) 2023
Article reuse guidelines:
Anthropomorphized Robots Affect Solo sagepub.com/journals-permissions
DOI: 10.1177/10946705231218405
journals.sagepub.com/home/jsr
(vs. Joint) Service Consumption

Do The Khoa1,*  and Kimmy Wa Chan2,* 

Abstract
Solo consumption has become an emerging trend in recent years. However, the service experiences of solo customers with the
growing adoption of frontline humanlike robots remain unclear, particularly in direct comparison with joint customers. Building on
the literature of anthropomorphism and information processing theory, this study examines whether and how frontline an-
thropomorphized robots (FAR) might improve the service experiences of solo customers relative to their joint counterparts. Data
from four studies, including field and online experiments, reveal that solo customers are more likely than joint customers to
perceive FAR as offering rapport but also as being eerie, leading to different service evaluations (both attitudinal and behavioral
outcomes). Nevertheless, as parallel mechanisms, these levels of social rapport and eeriness are contingent on features of the FAR,
the service delivery process, and customers’ consumption goals. The rapport (eeriness) mechanism is strengthened (weakened)
when the robot is of in-group favoritism, the service process deprives customers of control, and customers have a hedonic
consumption goal. With the boom in adopting frontline humanlike robots in hospitality services, this study offers managerially
relevant implications for serving solo customers as an emerging segment along with the traditional segment of joint customers.

Keywords
solo and joint consumption, frontline anthropomorphized robots, information processing, social rapport, eeriness

While customer experience is considered social in nature boundary conditions are mostly not explored (see Table 1 for a
(Lemon and Verhoef 2016), recent years have seen a boom in review). Notably, in the field of information systems, though a
solo customers who consume services alone, representing a few studies have examined how the social presence of others
promising market segment (Pfalz 2021). Solo travelers and solo affects one’s technology usage (e.g., Goel et al. 2013; Schultze
diners are two prime examples. In the United States, sales of and Brooks 2018), they primarily explore the role of “remote”
single roundtrip travel tickets increased by 200% during others in virtual environments, such as 3D virtual world, and do
summer 2021, compared with the same period in 2020 (Diakite not specifically examine (anthropomorphized) service robots in
2021); single bookings in the first three quarters of 2021 in- physical frontline environment, not to mention their focus on
creased by 300% compared with reservations made for families non-marketing outcome variables. These gaps are crucial given
or groups of friends (Kamin 2021). Restaurants also note the the recent post-pandemic proliferation of service robotics (Wan,
sweeping prevalence of the “table for one” trend; single diners Chan, and Luo 2021), especially those frontline anthropo-
represented up to 35% of US restaurants’ market share in morphized robots (FAR)—service robots with humanlike fea-
2020 and became the largest restaurant visitor segment (Cheng tures (e.g., name, embodiment, and voice) serving at the
2020). In this sense, solo customers are no longer the exception frontlines. The use of FAR is rising in service settings,
but represent a growing segment that hospitality providers must
consider.
While research on solo or joint consumption is gaining more 1
Royal Holloway-University of London, UK
2
attention, it predominately tackles these two trends in isolation. Hong Kong Baptist University, Hong Kong
More importantly, questions on solo customers’ views of Received: 3 January 2022
frontline service robots, in direct comparison with those of joint *Two authors contribute equally
customers, remain unaddressed. Although a few prior studies
Corresponding Author:
(e.g., Fraune, Šabanović, and Kanda 2019; Preusse et al. 2021) Do The Khoa, Royal Holloway-University of London Egham Hill, Egham Surrey
have attempted to compare individuals with groups when en- TW20 0EX, UK
countering robots, the underlying mechanisms and the Email: dothekhoa@iss.nthu.edu.tw
2 Journal of Service Research 0(0)

Table 1. Previous Studies of Solo and Joint Consumption.

Context (Solo or
Paper Purpose Robot Joint) Mediators/Moderators Outcome(s)

Bhargave and Comparison of solo with joint No Hedonic activities Mediator: evaluations of final Global, retrospective
Montgomery experience for temporal sequence of (Art gallery) (solo episode in a sequence evaluations
(2013) episodes (e.g., series of paintings) and joint) Moderator: social context Moment-to-moment
rating
Bianchi (2015) Drivers for (dis)satisfaction among solo No Traveling (solo) N/A (qualitative interview, (Dis)satisfaction
holiday travelers CIT method)
Brick et al. (2021) Impact of shared decision making No Shopping (car, Mediators: self-influence, Relationship
(shared decision vs. self-decision vs. couch, household partner engagement, satisfaction
partner-made decision) on items) (solo vs. perceived power
relationship satisfaction joint) Moderator: n/a
Brown, Buhalis, Investigate feelings of dining alone when No Traveling (solo) N/A (qualitative narrative Discomfort
and Beer traveling interview)
(2020)
Etkin (2016) Impacts of relationship time perspective No Committed Mediator: value of Variety-seeking
on variety preference for joint relationship excitement
consumption activities (joint) Moderator: physical
presence of relationship
partner
Garcia-Rada, Study the choice between experience No Hedonic activity Mediator: desire to create Preference for
Norton and quality and physical togetherness in sharing (joint) shared memories togetherness (vs.
Ratner (2023) sharing activity with close (vs. distant) Moderators: outcome experience quality)
relationship partner asymmetry of self and
partner, experience type,
reminder of ability to share
memories
Hart and Dale Impacts of jointness (companions) on No Restaurant and Mediator: n/a Satisfaction
(2014) service consumption retail shopping Moderator: gender Attitude
(solo and joint) Time and money spent
Her and Seo Impacts of other diners on focal solo No Restaurant (solo) Mediators: loneliness, Intention to eat alone
(2018) diner’s intention negative evaluation from
others
Moderator: crowding level
Hwang, Shin and Roles of spatial distance and social No Restaurant (solo) Mediator: in-group bias Enjoyment
Mattila (2018) distance in affecting solo dining Moderator: power
experience
Kim et al. (2022) Effect of no-preference communication No Restaurant, movie, Mediator: perception of Decision difficulty
on joint decision making and and game (joint) undisclosed preferences
consumption experience Moderator: decision role Reduced liking choice
(decision maker vs co- of preferred options
consumer)
Liu and Min Impact of decision role (requestor vs. No Restaurant, museum Mediators: mitigated Preference expression
(2020) responder) in joint consumption (joint) decision burden, likability
decision via easygoingness
Moderators: category
similarity, group size
Lteif et al. (2023) Impact of sharing product with others No Product Mediator: identification with Perceived product
(strangers as sharing-out vs. close consumption the product efficacy
people as sharing-in) on product sharing (joint) Moderator: self-brand Behavioral intentions
efficacy perception connection (purchasing,
recommending)
Luo (2005) Role of others’ presence in influencing No Shopping (joint) Mediator: n/a Impulsive purchasing
focal consumer’s impulsive Moderators: group choice
purchasing cohesiveness, susceptibility
to influence

(continued)
Khoa and Chan 3

Table 1. (continued)

Context (Solo or
Paper Purpose Robot Joint) Mediators/Moderators Outcome(s)

Moon, Bonn, and Impacts of key physical and No Restaurant (solo) Mediator: perceived Satisfaction
Cho (2020) psychological factors on solo dining territoriality
experience Moderator: solo diners’ Revisit intention
motivations
Nikolova and Effect of joint goal progress (high vs. No Financing plan, team Mediator: relational self- Goal-consistent
Nenkov (2021) low/no) on subsequent individual work (joint) concept boost behavior
goal-consistent decisions Moderator: relationship
power
Raghunathan and Impact of exposure to others’ opinions No TV advertising, Mediators: sense of Enjoyment of shared
Corfman about hedonic experiences orange juice belonging, confidence in experience
(2006) (congruent vs. incongruent) on the tasting (joint) accuracy
enjoyment of such shared Moderators: need to belong,
experiences need for accuracy
Ramanathan and Influence of the presence of others on No Video watching Mediator: n/a Video evaluation
McGill (2007) one’s moment-to-moment and (solo and joint) Moderator: Type of Rewatch intention
retrospective evaluations of an presence
experience
Ratner and Role of accompanying partners’ No Hedonic public Mediator: Inference about Interest in activity
Hamilton presence in solo activities’ experience consumption number of others
(2015) (solo) Moderators: culture, activity Enjoyment
type
Shin, Hwang, and Effects of self-esteem on solo diners’ No Restaurant (solo) Mediator: perceived fit Satisfaction
Mattila (2018) experience Moderator: incidental
similarity cue
Su, Cheng, and Effects of tourism activity type No Traveling (joint) Mediator: emotional arousal Storytelling intention
Swanson (experiential vs. material) on Moderators: presence and
(2020) storytelling ability of travel companion
Wu et al. (2021) Impacts of clarity about partner’s No Leisure activities Mediators: ability to focus on Enjoyment
interest in activity on enjoyment in (solo and joint) activity, distraction
shared experiences Moderators: need for
navigation, relevance of
partner’s interest
Fraune, Impacts of group and group traits (i.e., Yes Retail shopping Mediator: n/a Interaction with robot
Šabanović, and entitativity and social norms) on (solo and joint) Moderator: gender Duration of interaction
Kanda (2019) interacting with robots Social gesture toward
robot
Preusse et al. How individuals (alone) and group Yes Restaurant (solo and Mediator: n/a Interactions (both
(2021) members (with others) interact with joint) verbal and non-
service robots verbal) with robot
Moderator: n/a Acceptance of robot
Current research Impacts of solo context (solo vs. joint) Yes Airline, restaurant, Mediators: social rapport Satisfaction
on customers’ experiences with and hotel (Solo and eeriness WOM
frontline anthropomorphized robots and Joint) Moderators: in-group Recommendation
(FAR) favoritism, control Revisit intention
deprivation, consumption Pay-Per-Person
goals WeChat Posting

particularly in the hospitality sector (Choi and Wan 2021), customers perceive that they have a personal, emotional con-
where companies adopt them as concierges in hotels or servers nection or bond with the FAR (Gremler and Gwinner 2000),
in restaurants (McLeay et al. 2020). Some service businesses subsequently exerting positive effects on service evaluations,
(e.g., Dadawan restaurant in the Netherlands) even rely almost such as satisfaction, word-of-mouth (WOM) (Becker, Mahr, and
entirely on FAR, with minimal or no human service presence Odekerken-Schröder 2022), and loyalty (Gremler and Gwinner
(Kim, Choe, and Hwang 2020). Such service robots that evoke 2000). However, anthropomorphism might not always be ben-
strong anthropomorphism can create and elicit social rapport eficial. For instance, Akdim, Belanche, and Flavián (2021) show
(Qiu et al. 2019). Social rapport means the extent to which that customers develop negative attitudes towards service robots
4 Journal of Service Research 0(0)

with high humanlikeness and are inclined to reject them. Spe- strengthened (weakened) if (a) the robot evokes strong in-group
cifically, when a humanoid robot imitates human characteristics favoritism (Study 2), (b) the service process deprives customers
but falls short of achieving full humanness, it can elicit the of a sense of control (Study 3), and (c) customers have a hedonic
feelings of discomfort (e.g., eeriness). This is because customers consumption goal (Study 4). Such insightful findings thus bring
perceive a discrepancy between the robot’s expected human several key contributions to existing literature. First, we add
features and its actual imperfect humanlike qualities (i.e., the knowledge to existing studies that predominately examine solo
uncanny valley; Mende et al. 2019). This perceived eeriness, in or joint consumption in isolation (see Table 1) by providing
turn, leads to poor service outcomes, such as undermining comparative insights that help differentiate these two forms of
customers’ satisfaction and loyalty (Mende et al. 2019) or shaping consumption in the wave of the rapid rise of service robots in
negative attitudes toward robots that potentially evoke adverse hospitality. Second, we extend the literature on robotics an-
WOM (Kim, Schmitt, and Thalmann 2019). thropomorphism by challenging the conventional assumption of
Considering these disparate findings about the effectiveness the decontextualized positive effect of anthropomorphism on
of FAR together with the growing expansion of the solo cus- the customer-robot frontline interactions and hence expanding
tomer segment in hospitality, the current research, therefore, our holistic understanding of the effectiveness of robot an-
builds on the literature of anthropomorphism and information thropomorphism. Notably, we unveil that anthropomorphism
processing theory to explicitly investigate how the social can simultaneously evoke both social rapport and eeriness
context (i.e., solo vs. joint consumption) might affect service mechanisms, with opposite effects for solo (vs. joint) customers.
evaluations when encountering FAR. With the contention that This investigation thus responds to the recent call for identifying
solo (joint) customers adopt a more analytic (holistic) thinking new, theoretically meaningful mediators of robot anthropo-
style (Bhargave and Montgomery 2013; Krishna, Zhou, and morphism (Blut et al. 2021). To this end, we also enrich the
Zhang 2008; Smith and Redden 2020), we posit that solo stream of human user-technology interactions by explicating the
customers’ “zoom-in” approach and discrete thinking heighten underlying mechanisms driving the effect of users’ social
their focal attention on FAR. On the one hand, because solo context on their experience with new technologies (e.g., robots).
customers, unlike their joint counterparts, lack any companions Third, from a contingency approach, our findings offer more
and FAR act as the key frontline agents that socialize with them, nuanced insights into the activation processes of social rapport
they might thus perceive a stronger social rapport with FAR and eeriness mechanisms by identifying a set of managerially
during the frontline interactions, which would positively affect relevant moderators and capturing objective service outcomes
their service evaluations (Gremler and Gwinner 2000). We that are often overlooked in prior literature. Accordingly, our
name this effect the positive social rapport mechanism. On the study provides timely and relevant implications for service firms
other hand, with their heightened attention on FAR and their in relation to adopting FAR. When implementing FAR, these
analytic thinking, those solo customers would also perceive service providers must be aware of the customers’ social context
FAR as more distinctive and dissimilar than their joint coun- (solo vs. joint consumption). For instance, to better serve the
terparts would (e.g., Krishna, Lwin, and Morrin 2010), which emerging and promising solo segment of guests, hotels might
could heighten their perceptions of FAR eeriness and thus add local cues to their FAR (e.g., national flag and mother
negatively affect their service evaluations (Kim, Schmitt, and language) to evoke in-group favoritism and/or adopt a highly
Thalmann 2019; Mende et al. 2019). We name this effect the automated process that is fully managed by FAR. Meanwhile,
negative eeriness mechanism. As these two opposing mecha- restaurants might use ambient cues (e.g., lighting, scent, and
nisms might nullify the direct effect of FAR on customers’ music) to encourage a sensory hedonic consumption experience
ultimate service evaluations, it is thus essential to put forth for solo diners.
conditions that will likely influence both mechanisms simul-
taneously. Hence, we further identify features of the FAR (in-
Theoretical Background and
group favoritism), service delivery process (degree of control
deprivation), and customers’ consumption goals (hedonic vs. Hypotheses Development
utilitarian) as three key managerially relevant boundary con- Solo (vs. Joint) Consumption and Information
ditions that likely influence customers’ information processing
style and thus activate levels of social rapport and eeriness
Processing Style
differently, with distinct ultimate influences on solo (vs. joint) We define solo consumption as doing things alone in the
customers’ service experiences. To comprehensively capture marketplace (Leary et al. 2003) and solo customers as anyone
customers’ overall service evaluations and enhance our find- participating in consumption behaviors on his or her own
ings’ robustness, we include attitudinal (i.e., satisfaction, without any companions (Goodwin and Lockshin 1992). In
WOM, and revisit intention) and behavioral (i.e., pay-per- contrast, joint customers are those who consume with at least
person and WeChat posting) service outcomes. one companion. While solo consumption has started growing
With data from one field and three online experiments, we considerably, particularly in hospitality services, extant research
reveal the existence of the two opposing (i.e., positive social primarily examines the drivers of one’s solo consumption and
rapport and negative eeriness) mechanisms (Study 1). Impor- related experiences (e.g., Hwang, Shin, and Mattila 2018).
tantly, we show that the social rapport (eeriness) mechanism is Studies largely lack investigations into their interactive
Khoa and Chan 5

experience with frontline service robots or make a direct new product with incongruent attributes, they are more attentive
comparison between solo and joint customers (e.g., Her and Seo to those specific attributes and perceive substantial and bother-
2018; Moon, Bonn, and Cho 2020). The only exceptions are the some dissimilarity, whereas holistic thinkers find similarities by
works of Fraune, Šabanović, and Kanda (2019) and Preusse focusing on their relatedness to the base product (Fӧrster 2009;
et al. (2021), which examine how individuals vs. groups in- Lee and Chu 2021).
teracted differently with service robots. However, both the While individuals generally have a culturally dominant
underlying mechanisms that drive how solo vs. joint group processing style (Nisbett et al. 2001), recent research has shown
interacts with robots differently and the boundary conditions that thinking styles also can vary within an individual across
that might alter this difference are not considered and addressed situations (Benoit and Miller 2017; Choi, Koo, and Choi 2007).
in these two studies. Also, existing research on solo con- For instance, in their study on the temporal sequence of episodes
sumption is predominately focused on only a single service in art galleries, Bhargave and Montgomery (2013) show that
context (e.g., solo restaurant, Her and Seo 2018; solo traveling, one’s social context affects information processing styles, such
Su, Cheng, and Swanson 2020) and only captures subjective or that solo visitors, who experience less social connection than
attitudinal service outcomes, a broader study context covering joint visitors, engage in analytic (less holistic) information
diverse types of services and the inclusion of more objective or processing, which then diminishes the contextual dependence of
behavioral service outcomes are imperative in enhancing the solo visitors’ judgments of the episodes throughout the expe-
validity of research along this stream (see Table 1). rience. Likewise, Krishna, Zhou, and Zhang (2008) find that
Against these backdrops, we aim to offer a more nuanced priming interdependence with others triggers more holistic (less
understanding of the interplay of customers’ social context analytic) processing on subsequent tasks.
(i.e., solo vs. joint consumption) with the adoption of FAR in These lines of reasonings together suggest that solo customers
hospitality services. In particular, we rely on information pro- tend to exhibit an analytic thinking style while joint customers
cessing theory as the key theoretical lens for our propositions. In might hold a holistic thinking style. Accordingly, we propose that
general, individuals embrace two types of information processing when solo (vs. joint) customers engage in more analytic (vs.
styles, holistic and analytic, that differ in their attention and depth holistic) information processing during service encounters, it
of information processing (Hossain 2018). Holistic thinkers influences their perceptions of FAR and subsequent attitudinal
adopt a top-down, “zoom-out” information integration style, so and behavioral service evaluations (i.e., satisfaction,1 WOM,
they consider the context of information that they assimilate when revisit intention, and actual purchases). More importantly, we
making judgments. Analytic thinkers instead rely on a bottom-up propose social rapport and eeriness as the two opposing un-
style and make judgments based on individual elements, separate derlying mechanisms and further put forth with boundary con-
from the context and with a “zoom-in” approach (Nisbett 2003). ditions. Please refer to Figure 1 as our conceptual framework.
For example, to evaluate a new product, customers with a holistic
and concrete thinking style exhibit flexibility in categorizing and Frontline Anthropomorphized Robots (FAR), Social
emphasizing relationships across categories. They are willing to
Rapport, and Eeriness
accept stimuli that deviate from categorization norms. In contrast,
analytic thinkers, with their discrete thinking orientation, seek to Anthropomorphism refers to attributing humanlike properties
impose a well-defined structure and embrace categorization and characteristics to a nonhuman entity, such as a robot (Epley,
norms (Hossain 2018). Suppose analytic thinkers encounter a Waytz, and Cacioppo 2007). Certain features can evoke

Figure 1. Conceptual framework.


6 Journal of Service Research 0(0)

anthropomorphism, such that adding a face, arms, or a voice to a H1: (a) Solo (vs. joint) customers perceive greater social rapport
robot could activate people’s sense that the robot resembles a with FAR, (b) which in turn positively affects their service
human (Blut et al. 2021). Anthropomorphized robots provoke evaluations. [Positive social rapport mechanism]
enhanced social rapport with customers, including a sense of
interpersonal and emotional connection between customers and Eeriness Mechanism. In solo consumption, customers tend to
robots (Biedenbach et al. 2011; Gremler and Gwinner 2000). adopt an analytic processing style and are more context-
Such connections are found to ultimately increase customer independent with discrete thinking (Bhargave and Mont-
service evaluations (e.g., satisfaction and WOM, Becker et al. gomery 2013; Smith and Redden 2020); they would thus find
2022; loyalty, Gremler and Gwinner 2000). FAR more distinctive and dissimilar relative to the surrounding
Nevertheless, the association between anthropomorphism environment. Research on distinctiveness suggests that a
and customers’ acceptance and evaluations of service robots stimulus can be distinctive if it differs from its immediate
is not always positive (Blut et al. 2021). Robots that highly surrounding context or is unexpected, unusual, or contextually
resemble human beings can evoke negative feelings inappropriate (e.g., Krishna et al. 2010). Such stimuli can
(i.e., uncanny valley effect, Kim et al. 2019; Mende et al. capture people’s attention and be particularly perceived as
2019; Mori 1970). People might find them eerie, with a distinctive for those who embrace an analytic thinking style
feeling of creepiness and strangeness that leads to reduced (Herz 1997). As such, we extrapolate that solo customers would
likability (Kätsyri et al. 2015) and increased rejection (Akdim perceive FAR as detached from the context and more likely to
et al. 2021). As Mori (1970) notes, the degree of affinity attend to its dissimilarity (Lee 2018), thus perceiving FAR as
(likeability) of humanlike robots may depend on positive eerier. Since robots’ eeriness or creepiness would lead to re-
shinwakan (i.e., social rapport) and negative bukimi duced customers’ liking and acceptance of robots (Kätsyri et al.
(i.e., eeriness) (MacDorman et al. 2009). Taken together, 2015), we expect that solo customers will ultimately feel less
because service robot anthropomorphism might have varying satisfied and less likely to revisit (Mende et al. 2019) or rec-
effects on customers’ ultimate service evaluations, we ommend the services to others (Kim, Schmitt, and Thalmann
therefore seek to advance our understanding of how the two 2019). Conversely, joint customers tend to exhibit holistic
opposing mechanisms (i.e., social rapport and eeriness) above processing and are more context-dependent and have connected
might be activated differently by one’s social consumption thinking (Bhargave and Montgomery 2013). They are more
contexts (i.e., solo vs. joint), as well as identify relevant likely to take an integrative view and perceive FAR as less
boundary conditions. Specifically, we draw on the informa- unusual and more acceptable. Lee and Chu (2021) propose that
tion processing styles embraced by solo (vs. joint) customers holistic thinkers tolerate the addition of incongruent attributes to
and the literature on anthropomorphism to develop our a base product. However, analytic thinkers exhibit narrow,
hypotheses. inflexible categorizations and deem such additions to violate
their categorization norms, resulting in negative evaluations. In
Social Rapport Mechanism. Given their analytic thinking style in sum, because the perceived distinctiveness of FAR could be
solo consumption, those solo customers might be relatively more pronounced for solo customers due to their analytic
more attentive to FAR, as they view the robot as the key social processing, they would perceive FAR as eerier, which in turn
agent with which they can socialize and interact. Customers in dampens their service evaluations. Formally, we propose:
solo consumption are both physically alone and less socially H2: (a) Solo (vs. joint) customers perceive greater eeriness of
connected (Goodwin and Lockshin 1992), they might therefore FAR, (b) which in turn negatively affects their service evalu-
experience a greater situational need for belonging relative to ations. [Negative eeriness mechanism]
their joint counterparts (Hwang, Su, and Mattila 2020). As
noted by Baumeister and Leary (1995), a strong desire to belong
might lead customers to look for companionship for their Boundary Conditions for Social Rapport and
consumption, by seeking more social interactions and devoting
more thoughts to relationship partners. As such, solo customers
Eeriness Mechanisms
will perceive a stronger social rapport with FAR. Because social With the concurrent existence of the two opposing mechanisms,
rapport has been shown to enhance satisfaction, WOM, and namely social rapport and eeriness, the effects on service
loyalty (Gremler and Gwinner 2000), those solo customers will outcomes counterbalance and could be canceled out (e.g., Li,
consequently be more satisfied with the services and engage Chan, and Kim 2019). Hence, it is imperative to investigate
more in positive WOM and revisiting. In contrast, joint cus- relevant boundary conditions that might activate different levels
tomers, as they adopt a more holistic thinking style and make of these two parallel mechanisms, thereby improving service
judgments by assimilating with the context, including their outcomes for solo (vs. joint) customers. As thinking styles are
companions, appear to focus relatively less on FAR and are less malleable and contextual (Bhargave and Montgomery 2013;
likely to regard it as their key social agent. Overall, relative to Nisbett et al. 2001), we, therefore, capture the features of FAR
joint counterparts, solo customers might seek out and find more (i.e., in-group favoritism), service delivery process (i.e., control
social rapport from FAR, which subsequently enhances their deprivation), and customers’ consumption goals (i.e., hedonic
service evaluations. In turn, we posit: vs. utilitarian) that are closely linked to one’s information
Khoa and Chan 7

processing style and also managerially relevant to firms’ H3a: The positive social rapport mechanism is strengthened
practices (Table W-A1, in Web Appendix A, provides a sum- when in-group favoritism toward FAR is present (vs. absent).
mary of theoretical reasonings for the moderation effects). This H3b: The negative eeriness mechanism is weakened when in-
enables us to offer a more nuanced understanding of the in- group favoritism toward FAR is present (vs. absent).
terplay of customers’ social context with FAR.
Control Deprivation. Human beings have an innate desire for
In-Group Favoritism. People categorize themselves and others control over their environment (Chen, Lee, and Yap 2017).
into in-group members who are similar or out-group members While the adoption of FAR in services is booming, there are
who are dissimilar to them (Hogg and Terry 2000; Turner 1987). rising concerns about the loss of control over FAR, too (Choi
This categorization relies on comparisons of the self with others and Wan 2021; Puntoni et al. 2020). Fast and Horvitz (2017)
on various factors, including arbitrary ones (e.g., birth date, demonstrate in their analysis of articles published between
gender, and surname), especially if the categorization involves 1986 and 2016 that the risk of losing control following the
unknown others (Kuchenbrandt et al. 2013). Depending on deployment of novel technological agents consistently ranks
whether an in-group or out-group perception forms, in-group among people’s top concerns.
favoritism might arise (Hwang, Shin, and Mattila 2018; Tajfel Prior research states that a sense of control could influence
and Billic 1974). Such favorable attitudes toward in-group (vs. one’s information processing style, such that when individuals
out-group) members have primarily been documented among are deprived of control, their thinking style becomes more
human social groups, but they can be extended to robots (Eyssel analytic, and they are in a motivational state to regain their lost
and Kuchenbrandt 2011). Notably, in-group favoritism is not a control (Chen, Lee, and Yap 2017; Zhou et al. 2012). Applying
fixed trait but can be evoked by situational cues, such as the to our context, when sensing a lack of control over the service
salience of the categorization cues (Her and Seo 2018; Hwang, process, which becomes more prevalent in hospitality when the
Shin, and Mattila 2018). fully automated service encounter with only FAR is increasingly
In-group favoritism might influence one’s information preferred in post-pandemic (Wan, Chan, and Luo 2021), those
processing style, such that when individuals perceive others joint customers who used to adopt a holistic thinking style might
with high in-group favoritism, their social connectedness and now shift to become more analytic, resulting in increased at-
interdependence are enhanced, leading to a more holistic tention to FAR. As such, they might seek to reassert control by
thinking approach. This effect is especially evident among solo regaining their identity, such as upholding their uniqueness and
customers, who, upon sensing a high level of in-group favor- superiority (as humans) over the robot by drawing a clear
itism toward FAR, are more inclined to view FAR as an in-group boundary or distinction between humans and robots (Lu, Zhang,
member which is similar and closely connected to them. As a and Zhang 2021). Such a boundary would reduce joint cus-
result, they experience a stronger sense of connection and tomers’ perceived social rapport with FAR while intensifying
perceive a closer relational bond with FAR (i.e., enhanced social the distinctiveness of FAR. That is, they would perceive FAR as
rapport). On the other hand, given that such enhanced inter- more distinctive, creepier and less acceptable under control
dependence caused by in-group favoritism also evokes more deprivation. In sum, deprived control appears to further hamper
holistic thinking (Krishna et al. 2008) among solo customers, the already low level of social rapport with FAR and heighten
such that they, who used to adopt an analytic thinking style with eeriness for joint customers, reducing their service evaluations
rigid categorization norms, would now perceive FAR as more (Gremler and Gwinner 2000; Mende et al. 2019).
acceptable, less distinctive and less creepy (i.e., reduced eeri- In contrast, under control deprivation, solo customers also
ness). Consequently, solo customers would be more satisfied hold an analytic thinking style. However, they might now seek
with services provided by FAR and increase their re-patronage to regain their control by coordinating with the robot instead, as
and WOM due to enhanced social rapport and reduced eeriness which is the only agent that can help them get the task done. As
driven by in-group favoritism perceptions. Meanwhile, as joint Chen, Lee, and Yap (2017) note, control deprivation elicits
customers have already established connections with their problem-solving tendencies that can reaffirm a sense of control
companions (e.g., friends and family) prior to their interactions over the environment. Therefore, when relying on FAR and
with FAR, in-group favoritism, as a categorization cue, might be treating it like a partner to deal with deprived control, solo
less salient and receive less attention from those joint customers customers might experience greater social rapport. Indeed,
(Hornsey 2008; Hwang, Shin, and Mattila 2018). In other according to Swann, Stephenson, and Pittman (1981), control
words, in-group favoritism is less likely to improve social deprivation could trigger a search for social information; as the
rapport or further reduce the perceived eeriness of FAR among key social agent available for interactions with solo customers,
joint customers. Taken together, we predict that in-group fa- FAR should then prompt a stronger sense of social rapport for
voritism might strengthen the positive social rapport mechanism solo ones (i.e., enhanced social rapport). Also, they would now
while also weakening the negative eeriness mechanism for solo be prone to perceive FAR as less distinctive and less creepy
(vs. joint) customers, which in turn enhances solo (vs. joint) (i.e., reduced eeriness). Thus, unlike joint customers, we expect
customers’ service evaluations (e.g., satisfaction and WOM). that control deprivation improves rapport with but lowers ee-
Formally stated: riness of FAR for solo customers, which in turn enhances their
8 Journal of Service Research 0(0)

service evaluations (Gremler and Gwinner 2000; Mende et al. hedonic (vs. utilitarian) condition that consequently influence
2019). Overall, we posit that: service evaluations, we posit that:
H4a: The positive social rapport mechanism is strengthened H5a: The positive social rapport mechanism is strengthened
when control is deprived (vs. not). when the consumption goal is hedonic (vs. utilitarian).
H4b: The negative eeriness mechanism is weakened when H5b: The negative eeriness mechanism is weakened when the
control is deprived (vs. not). consumption goal is hedonic (vs. utilitarian).
We conducted four empirical studies, using diverse samples
Hedonic and Utilitarian Consumption Goals. Hedonic consump- (field data, MTurk, Prolific, and Clickworker) and across
tion is focused on affective, sensory experiences, and emotional different hospitality settings (restaurant, airport, and hotel) to
feelings (Botti and McGill 2011), while utilitarian consumption establish the parallel mechanisms (Study 1) and determine the
is cognitively driven and goal-oriented, designed to fulfill basic influences of the three boundary conditions: features of the
needs (e.g., hunger). These consumption goals influence cus- FAR (in-group favoritism, Study 2), the service delivery
tomers’ information processing (Melnyk, Klein, and Vӧlckner process (control deprivation, Study 3), and the customers’
2012), such that hedonic (utilitarian) consumption is more consumption goals (hedonic vs. utilitarian, Study 4). We also
associated with an emotional (rational) approach and facilitates controlled for the effects of gender and consumption fre-
more holistic (analytic) thinking (Hossain 2018). quency, together with some context-specific covariates to
When the consumption goal is hedonic, solo customers enhance the validity of our findings. We summarized our
become less analytic and take a more holistic approach to FAR studies in Table 2 and reported the descriptive statistics and
and the service experience. They focus more on the affective, measurement items of constructs in Web Appendices A (Table
sensorial, and experiential pleasure, as well as enjoyable feel- W-A2) and C, respectively.
ings of the whole service experience (Botti and McGill 2011),
which may prompt their stronger desire for companionship as a
crucial social element that can enhance the experiential com-
Study 1: Field Experiment (Restaurant Dining)
ponent of their hedonic consumption (Kim and Ratner 2018).
As such, they would perceive a stronger social rapport from We first contrasted solo customers with joint customers re-
FAR, which subsequently drives their satisfaction, WOM and garding their perceptions of the social rapport and eeriness
revisit intention (Gremler and Gwinner 2000). Regarding the toward actual FAR used at a restaurant, as well as their service
perceptions of eeriness in hedonic consumption, due to the solo evaluations, including both attitudinal (satisfaction) and be-
customers’ diminished attentiveness and sensitivity to the havioral service outcomes (pay-per-person and WeChat post-
dissimilarity of FAR as a result of their shift to holistic thinking ing) (H1 and H2). We also ruled out alternative explanations
(Hossain 2018), the perceived eeriness likely decreases too, as (e.g., warmth and competence).
they would now find FAR less distinctive, less creepy, and With a pretest, we confirmed that the solo consumption
therefore more acceptable. On the other hand, while joint condition induced less holistic (more analytic) processing and a
customers are also motivated to fulfill this hedonic need (e.g., greater need to belong compared to the joint consumption
enjoyment), they can turn to their companions who share the condition (Bhargave and Montgomery 2013). Diners at a
consumption experiences with them and so are unlikely to rely Beijing-based restaurant that implemented real service robots
on FAR for seeking social rapport. In other words, FAR is (see Web Appendix D) participated in this pretest (52.7% fe-
unlikely to improve those joint customers’ service evaluations male; 45 solo diners, 48 joint diners). FAR at this restaurant not
via social rapport. Also, since joint customers already adopt a only greeted customers but also moved around tables to take
holistic, “zoom-out” thinking approach, hedonic consumption orders and serve food and drink to diners. The pretest procedure
is less likely to alter their attention to and perceived eeriness relied on a survey link embedded into a QR code, which res-
of FAR. taurant staff presented to each diner after they completed their
If the consumption goal is utilitarian though, we predict no meal, with a request to complete a short survey in exchange for a
differences between solo and joint customers on perceptions of free dish (maximum of ¥50 RMB). In each joint group, only one
either social rapport or eeriness. According to Kim and Kim person could take the survey. Data were collected at different
(2014), in utilitarian consumption settings, people make deci- times (e.g., lunch and dinner) and on both weekdays and
sions based on value calculations rather than feelings. They are weekends to minimize any time effects.
unlikely to care whether they are alone or with others as they Participants first rated five items that gauged their holistic
only hope to fulfill the utilitarian needs, such that their main thinking style (e.g., “The whole is greater than the sum of its
focus is just getting the task done to reach their functional goal parts,” ω = 0.95; Choi et al. 2003). As expected, solo diners
(Ratner and Hamilton 2015). Simply put, they might not care indicated less holistic thinking than joint diners (Msolo = 4.96 vs.
about what FAR looks like (e.g., creepy-looking) and whether Mjoint = 5.94, t(91) = 4.589, p < .001).2 We further measured
they can build a social rapport with it when their consumption loneliness, social exclusion, and mood on 7-point Likert scales
goal is utilitarian. Overall, given the enhanced social rapport (1 = not at all, 7 = very much) (Web Appendix C). We found no
and reduced eeriness of FAR for solo (vs. joint) customers under differences between solo and joint groups for these factors.
Khoa and Chan 9

Table 2. Summary of Four Studies.

Outcome
Study Key Purpose Context Variable(s) Key Findings

Study 1 (Social context: solo Test the social rapport and Restaurant Satisfaction Solo customers perceive a stronger
vs. joint) eeriness mechanisms by dining (field Actual behavioral social rapport with FAR than joint
N = 223 actual diners contrasting solo vs. joint setting) outcomes customers, which in turn positively
customers’ experiences when -Pay-Per-Person affects service evaluations [positive
encountering FAR (H1 and H2) -WeChat posting social rapport mechanism]
Solo customers perceive greater
eeriness of FAR than joint
customers, which in turn negatively
affects service evaluations [negative
eeriness mechanism]
Study 2 (Social context: solo Examine the boundary condition of Airport check- Satisfaction The social rapport and eeriness
vs. joint) x (In-group in-group favoritism (H3a and in Recommendation mechanisms are strengthened and
favoritism: present vs. H3b) intention weakened, respectively, when in-
absent) group favoritism is present
N = 145 MTurk workers Consequently, in-group favoritism
improves service outcomes for
solo travelers, relative to joint
travelers
Study 3 (Social context: solo Investigate the boundary condition Hotel check-in Satisfaction The rapport mechanism is enhanced
vs. joint) x (Control of control deprivation (H4a and WOM and the eeriness mechanism is
deprivation: yes vs. no) H4b) attenuated, when control is
N = 218 Prolific panelists deprived
Consequently, service outcomes are
improved for solo travelers,
relative to joint counterparts,
under control deprivation
Study 4 (Social context: solo Study the boundary condition of Restaurant Satisfaction The rapport mechanism is
vs. joint) x (Consumption consumption goals (H5a and H5b) dining Revisit intention strengthened in hedonic (vs.
goals: hedonic vs. utilitarian) condition, whereas the
utilitarian vs. baseline) eeriness mechanism is
N = 316 Clickworker unexpectedly not weakened in
participants hedonic (vs. utilitarian) condition
Service outcomes are improved for
both groups (being more salient for
solo customers) when the
consumption goal is hedonic

Design and Procedure Results


In our main study, we collected field data from the same res- Social Rapport and Eeriness. An analysis of variance (ANOVA)
taurant, using the same procedure as in the pretest. Servers indicated significant main effects for social rapport (Msolo =
approached 248 customers, and we obtained 223 valid re- 5.17 vs. Mjoint = 4.44; F(1, 221) = 15.368, p < .001, η2partial =
spondents (57.8% female; Mage = 28.49 years; 104 solo diners, 0.065) and eeriness (Msolo = 3.30 vs. Mjoint = 2.44; F(1, 221) =
119 joint diners). These participants rated two items, measuring 14.026, p < .001, η2partial = 0.060)—in line with our expecta-
their satisfaction with the dining experience involving FAR on tions that solo diners would perceive higher levels of both social
7-point scales (“very dissatisfied/very satisfied,” “very rapport and eeriness for FAR than joint diners. We thus found
displeased/very pleased”; rSpearman–Brown = 0.78; Spreng et al. support for H1a and H2a (see Figure W-A1, Web Appendix B).
1996). They also responded to four social rapport items (e.g., Consistent with the pretest, we also found no significant dif-
“The service robot in the restaurant related well to me,” ω = ferences between solo and joint diners in terms of their lone-
0.91; Biedenbach et al. 2011; Gremler and Gwinner 2000) and liness (Msolo = 2.46 vs. Mjoint = 2.35, p = .613), social exclusion
three eeriness items (eerie, unnatural, creepy; ω = 0.92, Mende (Msolo = 2.32 vs. Mjoint = 2.16, p = .447), or mood5 (Msolo =
et al. 2019).3 Lastly, we obtained the actual behavioral data of 5.41 vs. Mjoint = 5.61, p = .315). Our results remained robust
pay-per-person and WeChat posting after the meal (0 = no, 1 = when we controlled for gender, monthly frequency of dining
yes, recorded by the staff on site4). out, number of diners, number of dishes (per person), dining
10 Journal of Service Research 0(0)

duration, and dining time (1 = lunch, 2 = dinner). We thus did and Mattila 2015) and found no statistically significant dif-
not discuss these variables in our further analyses. ference between solo and joint customers for either warmth
(Msolo = 5.08 vs. Mjoint = 5.18; t(221) = 0.469, p = .640) or
Mediation. To formally verify the parallel mechanisms of social competence (Msolo = 5.44 vs. Mjoint = 5.20; t(221) = 1.372, p =
rapport and eeriness, we separately conducted a parallel me- .172). In another parallel mediation test (PROCESS Model 4),
diation test, using PROCESS Model 4 with 10,000 boot- the results further indicated non-significant indirect effects for
strapping iterations (Hayes 2017) on satisfaction, pay-per- both warmth (95% CI = [ 0.19, 0.10]) and competence (95%
person (PPP), and WeChat posting. We dummy-coded the CI = [ 0.02, 0.15]) on satisfaction, pay-per-person (warmth:
social context (0 = joint; 1 = solo) as the independent variable 95% CI = [ 2.18, 1.00]; competence: 95% CI = [ 0.83, 2.37])
and included social rapport and eeriness as parallel mediators. and WeChat posting (warmth: 95% CI = [ 0.31, 0.18]; com-
The relationships from social rapport to satisfaction (b = 0.25, petence: 95% CI = [ 0.05, 0.15]). Thus, we could rule out
SE = 0.06, 95% CI = [0.13, 0.36]), ppp (b = 3.83, SE = 1.91, warmth and competence as alternative underlying mediators.
95% CI = [0.06, 7.60]), and WeChat posting (b = 0.30, SE = Our results remained robust when including them as covariates.
0.11, 95% CI = [0.08, 0.51]), were all significant with positive
coefficients. H1b was thus supported. Similarly, the relationships
from eeriness to satisfaction (b = 0.16, SE = 0.05, 95% CI = Discussion
[ 0.26, 0.07]), ppp (b = 3.37, SE = 1.55, 95% CI = This field experiment in a natural restaurant setting confirms that
[ 6.43, 0.32]), and WeChat posting (b = 0.30, SE = 0.09, a solo consumption context triggers less holistic (more analytic)
95% CI = [ 0.47, 0.12]), were all significant with negative thinking. We also offer insights that solo diners perceive FAR as
coefficients. H2b was then supported. Consistently, the indirect greater in terms of social rapport but also eerier than their joint
effects from social context to satisfaction, via social rapport counterparts do, with concomitant influences on both attitudinal
(ab = 0.15, SE = 0.07, 95% CI = [0.04, 0.31]) and eeriness (i.e., satisfaction) and behavioral service outcomes (i.e., pay-
(ab = 0.18, SE = 0.07, 95% CI = [ 0.33, 0.07]), were both per-person and WeChat posting), in opposing directions. Hence,
significant, but in opposite directions. We found similar result we next consider boundary conditions in which social rapport
patterns for the indirect effects on pay-per-person and WeChat and eeriness mechanisms might be activated differently, thereby
posting through social rapport (ppp: ab = 2.33, SE = 1.29, 95% improving service outcomes for solo (vs. joint) customers.
CI = [0.13, 5.07]; posting: ab = 0.18, SE = 0.11, 95% CI = [0.02,
0.44]) and eeriness (ppp: ab = 3.71, SE = 2.03, 95% CI =
[ 8.30, 0.39]; posting: ab = 0.33, SE = 0.12, 95% [CI] = Study 2: Moderation of In-Group Favoritism
[ 0.60, 0.13]). Overall, social rapport and eeriness simulta-
neously mediate the effect of solo (vs. joint) context on service
(Airport Check-In)
evaluations, in further support of H1 (positive social rapport To investigate the boundary condition of in-group favoritism
mechanism) and H2 (negative eeriness mechanism). We re- (H3a and H3b), we studied an airport check-in context with FAR,
ported all the direct, indirect, and total effects of this study in using a 2 (social context: solo vs. joint) × 2 (in-group favoritism:
Table W-A3 (Web Appendix A). present vs. absent) between-subjects experimental design. We
primed robot anthropomorphism with four elements: appear-
Outcomes. Meanwhile, regarding the effect of the social context ance (with an image of an anthropomorphized robot), a name
(solo vs. joint consumption) on downstream outcomes, as ex- (Amezen), a first-person pronoun, and a humanlike voice
pected, one-way ANOVAs on satisfaction (Msolo = 5.63 vs. generated by Naver Papago (Li and Sung 2021).6 In a pretest
Mjoint = 5.76; F(1, 221) = 0.711, p = .400), pay-per-person (in with 43 MTurk workers (53.5% female; Mage = 41.42 years), we
RMB) (Msolo = 128.92 vs. Mjoint = 123.37; F(1, 221) = 1.145, validated this anthropomorphism manipulation with the two
p = .286), and WeChat posting (β = .05, SE = 0.27, Wald = 0.04, items from Study 1 (rSpearman Brown = 0.73). The pretest par-
p = .850) indicated no differences across conditions. These null ticipants perceived the service robot in the scenario as an-
effects imply the opposing effects of social rapport and eeriness thropomorphic (Maverage = 4.99 vs. 4.00 midpoint, t(42) = 6.210,
mechanisms that seemingly counterbalance each other, which p < .001). To manipulate in-group favoritism, we followed prior
thus unveils the importance of identifying the relevant boundary literature to manipulate it through home country similarity
conditions in activating the mechanisms in our subsequent (i.e., “Robot is developed in your home country,” Eyssel and
studies. Kuchenbrandt 2011) and incidental similarity (i.e., “Robot is
manufactured on the same date and month as your birthday,”
Wan and Wyer 2018).
Alternative Explanations
In the main study, participants first wrote down their home
Previous research identifies warmth and competence as psy- country, which created an initial prime of home country (dis)
chological states that might account for the effects of robot similarity with the robot. They then had to imagine that they
anthropomorphism (e.g., Choi, Mattila, and Bolton 2020; Kim, were traveling on their own (vs. with friends) and had already
Schmitt, and Thalmann 2019). To rule out these alternative booked a flight. The scenario indicated that when they arrived
explanations, we measured warmth and competence (Bolton alone (vs. with their friends) at the airport, they approached a
Khoa and Chan 11

frontline service robot named Amezen at the check-in counter. we predicted, when we evoked in-group favoritism, solo
They noticed that Amezen featured stickers on its left arm and travelers displayed even more heightened social rapport than
waist, indicating it was made in their home country and joint travelers (Msolo present = 6.11 vs. Mjoint present = 4.76, p <
manufactured on the same date and month as their birthday. The .001), consistent with H3a (Figure 2, Panel A). On the other
detailed experimental stimuli are available in Web Appendix D. hand, a two-way ANOVA on eeriness indicated a non-
significant main effect of social context (F(1, 141) = 0.215,
p = .644, η2partial = 0.002) but a significant main effect of in-
Design
group favoritism (F(1, 141) = 13.521, p < .001, η2partial = 0.088).
We recruited 145 qualified MTurk workers (53.8% female; Notably, there was a significant interaction effect (F(1, 141) =
Mage = 42.72 years) through the CloudResearch platform. They 12.841, p < .001, η2partial = 0.083). According to the planned
were first randomly assigned to one of the four experimental contrasts, when in-group favoritism was absent, solo travelers
conditions. Then they rated the same items as in Study 1 to reported higher eeriness perceptions than joint travelers did
gauge their satisfaction with the check-in experience (rSpearman– (Msolo absent = 3.20 vs. Mjoint absent = 2.57, p = .037). In contrast,
Brown = 0.91), sense of social rapport (ω = 0.90), and perceived solo travelers expressed lower eeriness perceptions than their
eeriness (ω = 0.85). They also responded to one manipulation joint counterparts when in-group favoritism was present (Msolo
check question on social context (i.e., In the scenario above, present = 1.74 vs. Mjoint present = 2.55, p = .003), consistent with
your check-in at the airport is a (a) solo experience (alone) or (b) H3b (see Figure 2, Panel A)
joint experience (with your friends)) and three manipulation
check items for in-group favoritism (e.g., “I feel favorable to Mediation. To formally test the parallel mechanisms, we con-
have this service robot as part of my group”; ω = 0.82; Hwang, ducted a moderated mediation test using PROCESS Model
Shin, and Mattila 2018, Web Appendix C). 8 with 10,000 bootstrapping iterations (Hayes 2017). We first
dummy-coded social context (0 = joint; 1 = solo) as the in-
Manipulation Checks. All participants’ responses matched their dependent variable and in-group favoritism (0 = absent; 1 =
assigned social context.7 Also, participants in the presence of in- present) as the moderator. Then, we specified satisfaction as the
group favoritism perceived FAR more favorably (i.e., in-group dependent variable and included social rapport and eeriness as
member) than those in the absence of in-group favoritism mediators. When in-group favoritism was absent (baseline), the
(Mpresence = 4.65 vs. Mabsence = 4.10, t(143) = 2.595, p = .01). We indirect effects through both social rapport (ab = 0.43, SE =
further assessed the scenario realism with two items (e.g., “How 0.17, 95% CI = [0.10, 0.77]) and eeriness (ab = 0.14, SE =
realistic is the scenario?” 1 = not realistic at all, 7 = very re- 0.09, 95% CI = [ 0.34, 0.01]) were significant, but in op-
alistic; rSpearman Brown = 0.81). Participants regarded the scenario posite directions, replicating Study 1’s results. When in-group
as realistic (Maverage = 5.60 vs. 4.00 midpoint, t(144) = 18.734, favoritism was present, the indirect effect via social rapport
p < .001). (ab = 0.89, SE = 0.15, 95% CI = [0.61, 1.19]) was still sig-
nificant but stronger (i.e., due to the strengthened positive social
Control Variables. To rule out potential confounding of the rapport mechanism, formally supporting H3a). In contrast, the
country-of-origin effect with our in-group favoritism manipu- indirect effect via eeriness (ab = 0.19, SE = 0.09, 95% CI =
lation, we measured patriotism (ω = 0.96) and ethnocentrism [0.05, 0.38]) was also significant but in a positive direction
(ω = 0.94) (Web Appendix C). The results revealed no dif- (i.e., due to the weakened negative eeriness mechanism, for-
ferences between the absence and presence of in-group fa- mally supporting H3b). Similar results emerged for recom-
voritism conditions on patriotism (Mabsence = 5.06 vs. Mpresence = mendation intention (Web Appendix B). Details of direct and
5.21, t(143) = 0.564, p = .573) or ethnocentrism (Mabsence = indirect effects and moderated mediation indexes are presented
4.10 vs. Mpresence = 4.36, t(143) = 0.965, p = .336). Our results of in Table W-A3, Web Appendix A.
ANOVA and mediation tests remained robust when controlling
for them. Outcomes. A two-way ANOVA on satisfaction exhibited a
marginally significant interaction effect (F(1, 141) = 3.591, p =
.06, η2partial = 0.025). According to the planned contrasts, there
Results was no difference in satisfaction between solo and joint travelers
Social Rapport and Eeriness. For the 2 (social context: solo vs. in the absence of in-group favoritism condition (Msolo absent =
joint) × 2 (in-group favoritism: present vs. absent) design, a two- 5.59 vs. Mjoint absent = 5.32, p = .217), confirming the canceling
way ANOVA on social rapport yielded a significant main effect out effects of the two opposing mechanisms. However, when we
of social context (F(1, 141) = 39.541, p < .001, η2partial = 0.219) induced in-group favoritism, solo travelers reported higher
and a significant main effect of in-group favoritism (F(1, 141) = satisfaction than joint travelers did (Msolo present = 6.20 vs. Mjoint
11.289, p = .001, η2partial = 0.074). The results showed a present = 5.36, p < .001). We also found a statistically significant
marginally significant interaction effect (F(1, 141) = 2.980, p = increase in satisfaction among solo travelers in the absence
.087, η2partial = 0.021). Thereby, without in-group favoritism, versus presence of in-group favoritism (Msolo absent = 5.59 vs.
solo travelers reported higher social rapport with FAR than joint Msolo present = 6.20, p = .003) but a non-significant difference for
travelers (Msolo absent = 5.25 vs. Mjoint absent = 4.48, p = .004). As joint travelers across the two conditions (Mjoint absent = 5.32 vs.
12 Journal of Service Research 0(0)

Figure 2. Graphical plots for Studies 2, 3, and 4.


Khoa and Chan 13

Mjoint present = 5.36, p = .875; Figure W-A2, Web Appendix B). in the baseline condition (Mcontrol deprivation = 5.43 vs. Mbaseline =
In-group favoritism thus helps improve service outcomes for 4.35, t(36) = 3.004, p = .005); the manipulations were
solo travelers, but not joint ones. Our results were robust with successful.
the outcome of recommendation intention (Figure W-A3, Web
Appendix B).
Design
In the main study, we recruited 218 qualified Prolific panelists
Discussion (51.4% female; Mage = 32.80 years) and randomly assigned
We establish in-group favoritism as a boundary condition, such them to one of the four experimental conditions. We asked them
that its presence improves service outcomes for solo customers to rate their satisfaction with the check-in experience (rSpearman
relative to joint ones. Specifically, in-group favoritism Brown = 0.92), sense of social rapport (ω = 0.93), and eeriness

strengthens the positive social rapport mechanism and weakens (ω = 0.85). We also included two items to measure WOM (e.g.,
the negative eeriness mechanism, consequently evoking greater “I will encourage my friends and relatives to stay at this hotel,”
satisfaction and recommendation intention among solo trav- rSpearman Brown = 0.96; Han et al. 2011, Web Appendix C).
elers. While prior research predominately established the
benefits of fostering in-group favoritism among humans (e.g., Results
Hwang, Shin, and Mattila 2018), we offer additional insights to
service providers that they can also leverage in-group favoritism Social Rapport and Eeriness. The results of a two-way ANOVA
toward FAR, mainly when serving solo customers. For instance, on social rapport revealed a significant main effect of social
airline carriers can promote in-group favoritism by adding context (F(1, 214) = 37.834, p < .001, η2partial = 0.150) but a
“local” cues to anthropomorphized robots (e.g., national flags) non-significant main effect of control deprivation (F(1, 214) =
to help enhance the service experience for solo domestic pas- 0.003, p = .957, η2partial = 0.000). Importantly, there was a
sengers during check-in. significant interaction effect (F(1, 214) = 7.124, p = .008,
η2partial = 0.032). According to the planned contrasts, solo
customers reported higher social rapport in the baseline con-
Study 3: Moderation of Control Deprivation dition than joint customers (Msolo baseline = 4.73 vs. Mjoint
baseline = 4.04, p = .047). When deprived of control, solo
(Hotel Check-In) customers manifested much more social rapport than joint
Fully automated services without human staff have become a customers (Msolo deprivation = 5.28 vs. Mjoint deprivation = 3.51, p <
rising trend, yet concerns about loss of control are accelerating .001). The social rapport mechanism thus became strengthened
too (Fast and Horvitz 2017). In this study, we examined de- when deprived of control, consistent with H4a (see Figure 2,
prived control during the service delivery process by consid- Panel B). Another two-way ANOVA for eeriness also indicated
ering a hotel check-in setting using FAR, with a 2 (social a significant main effect of social context (F(1, 214) = 6.056, p =
context: solo vs. joint) × 2 (control deprivation: yes vs. no) .015, η2partial = 0.028) and a marginally significant main effect of
between-subjects experiment (H4a and H4b). We manipulated control deprivation (F(1, 214) = 2.762, p = .098, η2partial =
anthropomorphism with a different humanlike robot image but 0.013). Notably, the interaction effect was significant (F(1,
primed it with the same human name as in Study 1 (Amezen), a 214) = 34.548, p < .001, η2partial = 0.139). In the planned
first-person pronoun, and a humanlike voice.8 Participants had contrasts, we found that in the baseline condition, solo cus-
to imagine traveling on their own (vs. with their friends) and that tomers reported higher eeriness than joint customers (Msolo
they had already booked the hotel. When they arrived alone (vs. baseline = 4.38 vs. Mjoint baseline = 3.74, p = .045). When deprived
with their friends) at the hotel, they came to the front desk and of control, however, solo customers now perceived less eeriness
encountered FAR. In the control deprivation condition, they than joint ones (Msolo deprivation = 3.59 vs. Mjoint deprivation = 5.16,
read that there were no frontline human staff around and no p < .001); the eeriness mechanism was thus weakened under
signs for accessing them, so they had to use the robot to check- deprived control, consistent with H4b (Figure 2, Panel B).
in. In the baseline control, we did not offer such information; the
scenario described how they approached FAR to check into their Mediation. To formally verify the parallel mechanisms, we
room (Web Appendix D). performed a moderated mediation test using PROCESS Model
We conducted a pretest with 38 Prolific panelists (44.7% 8 with 10,000 bootstrapping iterations (Hayes 2017). We
female; Mage = 33.34 years) to confirm the effectiveness of dummy-coded social context (0 = joint; 1 = solo) as the in-
anthropomorphism features as well as the manipulation check of dependent variable and control deprivation (0 = baseline; 1 =
control deprivation with four items (e.g., “I feel not in good control deprivation) as the moderator, specified satisfaction as
control when dealing with the check-in process at the hotel”; the dependent variable, and included social rapport and eeriness
ω = 0.86, Web Appendix C). Participants perceived the service as mediators. In the baseline condition, the indirect effects
robot as anthropomorphic (Maverage = 4.39 vs. 4.00 midpoint, through both social rapport (ab = 0.54, SE = 0.27, 95% CI =
t(37) = 2.634, p = .012). Also, those in the control deprivation [0.01, 1.09]) and eeriness (ab = 0.34, SE = 0.17, 95% CI =
condition reported a higher perceived loss of control than those [ 0.67, 0.01]) were significant, in opposite directions,
14 Journal of Service Research 0(0)

replicating the mediation effect results from Studies 1 and 2. enjoyment, joy, and relaxation (vs. felt very hungry and merely
However, in the control deprivation condition, while the indirect wanted to find a place to have lunch quickly before getting back
effect via social rapport (ab = 1.36, SE = 0.17, 95% CI = [1.04, to work in the afternoon). The baseline condition did not de-
1.71]) remained significant and stronger (i.e., due to the scribe any such consumption goals. The scenario indicated they
strengthened positive social rapport mechanism, formally went solo (vs. with their friends) to a restaurant for lunch. When
supporting H4a); the indirect effect via eeriness (ab = 0.82, SE = they arrived alone (vs. with their friends), they noticed a
0.17, 95% CI = [0.52, 1.17]) was significant but in the positive frontline service robot named Amizen, which came to greet
direction (i.e., due to the weakened negative eeriness mecha- them and instruct them on how to place an order. After com-
nism, formally supporting H4b). We also obtained similar pleting the order, Amizen delivered the food items, returned to
mediation results for WOM (Web Appendix B). Details of direct refill their water glass, and finally dropped off their bill (Web
and indirect effects and moderated mediation indexes are re- Appendix D).
ported in Table W-A3, Web Appendix A. We carried out a pretest with 85 Clickworker participants
(56.5% female; Mage = 31.71 years) to check anthropomorphism
Outcomes. For the two-way ANOVA on satisfaction, the in- with the same two items as in previous studies (rSpearman Brown =
teraction effect was significant (F(1, 214) = 10.198, p = .002, 0.90). Participants perceived the service robot as anthropo-
η2partial = 0.045). The planned contrasts indicated, in the morphic (Maverage = 4.86 vs. 4.00 midpoint, t(84) = 4.759, p <
baseline condition, no difference in satisfaction between solo .001). We included two manipulation check items for con-
and joint customers (Msolo baseline = 5.22 vs. Mjoint baseline = 4.76, sumption goals (e.g., “How would you perceive your dining at
p = .160) due to the opposing mechanisms. However, in the this restaurant?” 1 = definitely utilitarian, 7 = definitely hedonic;
control deprivation condition, solo customers expressed higher rSpearman–Brown = 0.63, Kim and Kim 2014). A one-way AN-
satisfaction with the hotel check-in process than joint customers OVA (consumption goals: hedonic vs. utilitarian vs. baseline)
(Msolo deprivation = 5.80 vs. Mjoint deprivation = 4.12, p < .001) yielded a significant main effect (F(2, 82) = 21.187, p < .001,
(Figure W-A4, Web Appendix B). The results pertaining to η2partial = 0.341), and the planned comparison indicated that
WOM were similar (Figure W-A5, Web Appendix B). participants in the hedonic condition expressed stronger per-
ceptions of hedonic consumption goal than those in either the
utilitarian (Mhedonic = 5.38 vs. Mutilitarian = 3.05, p < .001) or
Discussion baseline (Mhedonic = 5.38 vs. Mbaseline = 4.02, p = .001) con-
This study reveals that deprived control is not necessarily dition. Respondents in the utilitarian condition also reported
damaging; the effect depends on customers’ social context. lower perceived hedonism than those in the baseline condition
While deprived control (e.g., without the presence of human (Mutilitarian = 3.05 vs. Mbaseline = 4.02 as close to the midpoint of
staff when in need) is unfavorable for joint customers as it 4.00, p = .016). Our manipulation was thus successful. The three
heightens their perceived eeriness of FAR, it improves solo conditions did not differ in terms of perceptions of the res-
customers’ service experiences due to the enhanced social taurant’s luxury (i.e., “Overall, the restaurant looks…,” 1 = very
rapport and the reduced eeriness toward FAR. Practically, our casual, 7 = very luxurious; Maverage = 4.06 vs. 4.00 midpoint,
results thus suggest that a fully automated service process t(84) = 0.347, p = .729).
managed by anthropomorphized robots could be adopted to
serve the solo segment but not the joint segment. Design
We recruited 316 qualified subjects from the Clickworker online
Study 4: Moderation of Consumption Goals panel (57.3% female; Mage = 34.46 years) and randomly as-
signed them to one of the six conditions. Participants responded
(Restaurant Dining)
to the same items as in prior studies to capture their satisfaction
Customers differ in their consumption goals, which could in- with the dining experience (rSpearman–Brown = 0.85), social
fluence their information processing styles when encountering rapport (ω = 0.92), and eeriness 2 (ω = 0.75). We also used three
FAR. In this study, we thus examined the boundary condition of items to gauge their revisit intention (e.g., “I intend to revisit this
consumption goals (hedonic vs. utilitarian; H5a and H5b) in a restaurant in the near future,” ω = 0.95; Kim et al. 2013).
restaurant dining context using FAR. We used a 2 (social
context: solo vs. joint) × 3 (consumption goals: hedonic vs.
Results
utilitarian vs. baseline) between-subjects factorial design. We
provided the participants with another anthropomorphized robot Social Rapport. For the 2 (social context: solo vs. joint) × 3
image while priming other anthropomorphic components,9 the (consumption goals: hedonic vs. utilitarian vs. baseline) design,
same as Studies 2 and 3. the two-way ANOVA on social rapport exhibited a significant
We followed Botti and McGill’s (2011) work to manipulate main effect of social context (F(1, 310) = 15.919, p < .001,
hedonic vs. utilitarian goals. Specifically, participants were told η2partial = 0.049) and a significant main effect of consumption
to imagine that they had just finished their morning work, were goals (F(2, 310) = 22.257, p < .001, η2partial = 0.126). There was
free, and wanted to find a place for lunch while seeking a marginally significant interaction effect (F(2, 310) = 2.376,
Khoa and Chan 15

p = .095, η2partial = 0.015). Planned contrasts showed that in the (ab = 0.48, SE = 0.14, 95% CI = [0.22, 0.78]) became sig-
baseline condition, solo diners sensed higher social rapport nificant, whereas such indirect effect via eeriness (95% CI =
toward FAR than joint diners (Msolo baseline = 5.48 vs. Mjoint [ 0.07, 0.09]) became non-significant. Together, these findings
baseline = 4.67, p = .002); in the utilitarian condition, we found no indicate that the positive social rapport mechanism became
significant differences in the social rapport between solo and stronger in the hedonic condition, in formal support of H5a.
joint groups (Msolo utilitarian = 4.18 vs. Mjoint utilitarian = 4.04, p = However, the negative eeriness mechanism did not get much
.656). Meanwhile, as expected, in the hedonic condition, solo weaker in the hedonic condition compared to the utilitarian
diners reported much greater social rapport than joint diners condition, H5b was thus not supported. Similar results were
(Msolo hedonic = 5.82 vs. Mjoint hedonic = 4.88, p < .001). That is, obtained for revisit intention (Web Appendix B).
the social rapport mechanism became strengthened in hedonic
(vs. utilitarian) consumption, consistent with H5a (Figure 2; Outcomes. In a two-way ANOVA for satisfaction, the baseline
Panel C). condition indicated no significant difference between solo and
joint groups (Msolo baseline = 5.93 vs. Mjoint baseline = 5.59, p =
Eeriness. Another two-way ANOVA on eeriness indicated a .133). While we also uncovered no significant differences in the
non-significant main effect of social context (F(1, 310) = 0.102, utilitarian condition (Msolo utilitarian = 5.09 vs. Mjoint utilitarian =
p = .749, η2partial = 0.000) but a significant main effect of 4.91, p = .579), solo diners reported higher satisfaction than
consumption goals (F(2, 310) = 10.092, p < .001, η2partial = their joint counterparts in the hedonic condition (Msolo hedonic =
0.061). Notably, the interaction effect was significant (F(2, 6.27 vs. Mjoint hedonic = 5.60, p = .001) (Figure W-A6, Web
310) = 4.648, p = .010, η2partial = 0.029). According to the Appendix B). An ANOVA on revisit intention yielded similar
planned contrasts, the solo diners’ group reported higher ee- results (Figure W-A7, Web Appendix B).
riness perceptions in the baseline condition than the joint diners’
group (Msolo baseline = 3.47 vs. Mjoint baseline = 2.89, p = .024).
Surprisingly, in the utilitarian condition, joint diners instead
Discussion
manifested a stronger eeriness than solo ones (Msolo utilitarian = Study 4 partially supports the moderation of consumption goals
3.56 vs. Mjoint utilitarian = 4.22, p = .042). When the consumption (hedonic vs. utilitarian). That is, the adoption of FAR is par-
goal was hedonic, there were, however, no significant differ- ticularly beneficial for solo customers who would perceive
ences in eeriness across two groups (Msolo hedonic = 2.99 vs. greater social rapport and lower eeriness toward FAR in pursuit
Mjoint hedonic = 3.07, p = .775). In other words, though the of a hedonic consumption goal. Nevertheless, firms must be
eeriness mechanism was still attenuated in the hedonic con- cautious of adopting FAR for joint customers who pursue a
dition compared to the baseline condition, the eeriness mech- utilitarian consumption goal as they would perceive FAR as far
anism was unexpectedly not weakened in the hedonic condition, eerier, consequently hampering their satisfaction and revisit
relative to the utilitarian condition (Figure 2; Panel C). intention. This might be due to their dramatic shift to more
analytic thinking with a utilitarian goal. Given the presence of
Mediation. To formally test the parallel mechanisms, we un- companions, those joint customers might feel more compelled
dertook a moderated mediation test using PROCESS Model to draw a clear boundary to differentiate FAR from their peer
8 with 10,000 bootstrapping iterations (Hayes 2017). We group to secure their human identity (Lu et al. 2021). This
dummy-coded social context (0 = joint; 1 = solo) as the in- motivation might thus further heighten their perceived eeriness
dependent variable, specified satisfaction as the dependent of FAR. To potentially alleviate this problem, service providers
variable, and included social rapport and eeriness as two parallel can manipulate ambient cues (e.g., lighting, scent, and layout) to
mediators. The consumption goals are a multi-categorical factor create more hedonic consumption experiences for their
with three levels; hence, we adopted the indicator coding customers.
method (Hayes and Montoya 2017) to establish two dummy
variables: D1 (0 = baseline, 1 = utilitarian, 0 = hedonic) and D2
(0 = baseline, 0 = hedonic, 1 = utilitarian). In the baseline
General Discussion
condition, the indirect effects through both social rapport (ab = The trend of solo consumption (e.g., traveling alone and dining
0.42, SE = 0.14, 95% CI = [0.16, 0.69]) and eeriness out alone) has proliferated in recent years, representing a
(ab = 0.07, SE = 0.04, 95% CI = [ 0.17, 0.01]) were promising service segment for the hospitality sector. Mean-
significant and in opposite directions, as in our previous studies. while, the post-pandemic era has also witnessed the broader
In the utilitarian condition, the indirect effect through social implementation of frontline anthropomorphized robots (FAR)
rapport (95% CI = [ 0.25, 0.40]) was non-significant across services, particularly in the hospitality industry, to
(i.e., social rapport level was equivalently low for both solo enhance customers’ experiences (Lemon and Verhoef 2016).
and joint groups under the utilitarian goal). In contrast, the Surprisingly, despite these two growing trends, they have
indirect effect through eeriness (ab = 0.08, SE = 0.05, 95% CI = mostly been studied separately, without convergent consid-
[0.002, 0.19]) was significant but “flipped” in direction due to eration of how FAR might be deployed more effectively to
the unexpected upsurge of eeriness for joint diners. In the serve solo customers than their joint counterparts. Drawing on
hedonic condition, the indirect effect via social rapport the literature of anthropomorphism and information
16 Journal of Service Research 0(0)

processing theory, the current study reveals how and in which Third, we enrich existing literature with a contingency view
conditions FAR might facilitate service experiences for solo on the conditions in which the positive social rapport and the
(vs. joint) customers. To that end, we collect both field and negative eeriness mechanisms get activated differently. To offer
online data, using diverse samples (MTurk, Prolific, and a more comprehensive view of the interplay between FAR and
Clickworker) and across different hospitality settings (airport, solo (vs. joint) consumption, we identify three critical and
restaurant, and hotel) to propose and empirically examine the managerially relevant boundary conditions that capture features
dual social rapport–eeriness mechanisms that drive the in- of the FAR (in-group favoritism), the service delivery process
teractions of FAR with solo (vs. joint) customers (Study 1) on (control deprivation), and the customers’ consumption goals
both attitudinal and behavioral service outcomes. We also (hedonic vs. utilitarian). Our findings reveal a facilitating role of
identify three relevant boundary conditions (in-group favor- in-group favoritism, deprived control during the service de-
itism, Study 2; control deprivation, Study 3; consumption livery process, and a hedonic consumption goal to enhance
goals, Study 4) in which social rapport and eeriness mecha- social rapport and reduce eeriness among solo (vs. joint) cus-
nisms get activated differently, with varying ultimate influ- tomers, which in turn improves solo service experiences. These
ences on various service outcomes for solo (vs. joint) results hence offer a deeper understanding of the conditional
customers. effect of the interplay between FAR and social context on solo
(vs. joint) experiences. In addition, we also cultivate the extant
literature on solo vs. joint consumption by including a diverse
Theoretical Contributions
scope of service contexts (i.e., hotel, restaurant, and airport) as
The current research makes several contributions to extant well as capturing both subjective and objective (e.g., pay-per-
literature. First, although a few previous attempts have tapped person) service outcomes for a more holistic assessment of the
into solo and joint consumption in isolation, we lack rooted proposed effects and their robustness, a crucial attempt to es-
research evidence that directly compares or differentiates these tablish external validity of our findings.
two consumption patterns concurrently and systematically,
particularly in relation to service robots (see Table 1). This
Managerial Implications
research offers an initial empirical direct comparison of solo
with joint consumptions, and we situate this explicit comparison This research provides managerially relevant implications for
within the service robotics context. We thus establish a strong service providers pursuing or considering adopting FAR in the
theoretical foundation for contrasting solo with joint con- wave of the upward segment of solo customers.
sumptions and add a more nuanced view of the interplay be-
tween frontline service robots and social context (solo vs. joint), Social Context Matters. Our work uncovers that using FAR can
which in turn expands a broader understanding of customer be a double-edged sword in that it can trigger both social rapport
experience in the service robotics era (Lemon and Verhoef and eeriness, depending on customers’ social context (solo or
2016). To this end, we also enrich the recent IS literature joint). Specifically, relative to joint customers, solo customers
that examined the impacts of social presence of others on in- sense more social rapport with FAR but also perceive them as
dividuals’ technology usage, though they primarily explored the eerier, which concurrently improves and dampens service
role of “remote” others in virtual environments and with non- outcomes (e.g., satisfaction and pay-per-person), respectively.
marketing outcomes. Thus, we go beyond the common belief that a humanlike service
Second, recent service research has made impressive strides robot is always desirable by suggesting that there is no one-size-
in exploring robot anthropomorphism, primarily in studies that fits-all formula for predicting the outcomes of robot anthro-
advocated its positive effects (e.g., via enhanced social rapport) pomorphism in services. Service providers should carefully
but largely ignored its potential dark sides. Even in research that consider the service context surrounding their target customers
acknowledged the negative eeriness effect (e.g., Kim, Schmitt, when deploying FAR to serve their customers.
and Thalmann 2019; Mende et al. 2019), we hardly find si-
multaneous considerations of the two opposing mechanisms, Promoting In-Group Favoritism of FAR. While in-group favoritism
nor do we have sufficient knowledge about the conditions that has been shown to exert favorable effects among human groups
might amplify or attenuate these effects. In response, we le- (Hwang, Shin, and Mattila 2018), our results show that service
verage Blut et al.’s (2021) call to contextualize the complex, firms can also enjoy such advantages by eliciting solo cus-
multifaceted essence of robot anthropomorphism. As we affirm, tomers’ in-group favoritism toward FAR. This may be possible
robot anthropomorphism evokes both social rapport and eeri- with easy-to-implement interventions, such as signaling close
ness mechanisms, with opposing effects on both attitudinal and similarities between solo customers and robots. Taking the
behavioral service outcomes for solo (vs. joint) customers. That tourism sector, for example, many tourists now still prefer to
is, relative to joint customers, solo customers perceive greater travel domestically rather than internationally due to ongoing
social rapport yet higher eeriness toward FAR, thereby affecting concerns about health safety in post-pandemic, so hotel and
their service evaluations differently. We, therefore, shed light on restaurant managers could add “local” cues to their anthropo-
the two-sided nature of robot anthropomorphism in (solo) morphized robots (e.g., national flags,10 traditional costumes,
consumption context. and other home country signs), which could help provoke
Khoa and Chan 17

greater in-group favoritism among solo domestic tourists. browses rooms with the best deal, indicating utilitarian goal.
Functioning FARs using local language to communicate with Such information would be instrumental in helping service
domestic customers (e.g., greeting customers in their native providers that rely on FAR to adjust their service provisions.
tongue) is another way to evoke in-group favoritism for the local Because both solo and joint customers perceive FAR more
solo group. If companies (e.g., hotels) have customers’ data positively (more social rapport, less eeriness) under hedonic
prior to their arrival, FARs can also rely on such information to consumption, especially for solo customers, marketers might
build closer communications with customers (e.g., conversing create hedonic consumption experiences that encourage cus-
with customers by their name during the check-in process). tomers to focus more on and enjoy the holistic consumption
Such implications could also be extended to digital service process. Hospitality operators might create ambient cues (e.g.,
encounters. Adopting anthropomorphized intelligent agents is layout, lighting, scent, and music) and servicescape to facilitate
prevalent in online services (Schanke, Burtch, and Ray 2021). positive sensory experiences. For instance, Five Guys fast-food
For instance, Air New Zealand implements an anthropomor- restaurant is renowned for its surprising and delightful expe-
phized chatbot named Oscar for online booking. Hence, we rience. Its kitchen is in plain view, and customers can watch
recommend that online marketing communications use inci- their burgers being made in an authentic way to order. Similarly,
dental similarity cues, such as highlighting anthropomorphized L’Hotel in Paris is known for its luxurious design and unique
robots’ names or other demographic features similar to those of interiors. Specifically, its ambient elements like chic furnish-
their customers, to trigger customers’ favoritism toward these ings, dim lighting, and soothing background music create a
anthropomorphized intelligent agents. With advanced tech- relaxing atmosphere for guests. In addition, restaurants can
nologies and generative AI, firms should also be able to tailor consider the visual presentation of food (sight), the sizzle of a
FAR’s responses to align with customers’ personal needs and dish (sound), the texture of a dish (touch), and the aroma from
preferences, which thereby enhances customers’ favoritism the kitchen (smell) to engage diners with their senses for better
toward FAR during their interactions. hedonic dining experience. In his case study, Ponsignon (2023)
also demonstrates the effectiveness of making the customer
Deprived Control is Not Always Bad. Service providers like hotels experience journey more hedonic in a traditionally utilitarian
or airlines should determine what constitutes an appropriate service context by fostering experiential involvement with fun
amount of control for their customers over the service delivery activities to evoke feelings of pleasure or excitement. In this
process. People have innate needs to control their tasks, but regard, technologies like virtual reality (VR) and augmented
sometimes, a certain degree of control deprivation (e.g., cus- reality (AR) would help to enhance such experiential element of
tomers must interact with the robot because no human staff are the consumption. One example is Marriott Hotels that offers
available) could improve service outcomes for solo customers. “VRoom Service” by allowing guests to request VR headsets to
Hence, marketers might adopt a fully automated service process their room for virtual travel tours. This is further accompanied
managed by FAR to serve solo customers. Nevertheless, they by “VR Postcards” with 3D immersive travel stories about the
should be cautious about joint customers, for whom deprived journey to different destinations.11 This Vroom service, by
control will drive them to perceive FAR as creepier and be less incorporating technology and storytelling together, creates
satisfied with this fully automated service. As such, marketers authentic and hedonic experiences for guests. In sum, if FAR is
might delegate more control to those joint customers by letting in place, firms would have a handful of feasible approaches to
them know they can get help from human employees when in elicit more hedonic consumption experiences for their cus-
need. For instance, hotels might provide simple cues, like a tomers. However, if the customers’ consumption goal is of
“help” button on the cashier or counter, that would be pertinent utilitarian, aiming to just get their service tasks done, firms
in this effort. Overall, firms should be careful when deciding or might consider deploying alternatives, such as self-service ki-
assigning the service provider (i.e., FAR or human staff) to serve osks, as interacting with FAR would increase its perceived
their customers, as a fully automated service delivery process eeriness and hamper customers’ service evaluations, particu-
managed by FAR might be beneficial for solo customers but larly for joint customers.
backfire for joint ones.

Hedonic Consumption Experience Can Be Fostered. Service pro-


Limitations and Further Research
viders often can determine in advance whether customers’ We acknowledge several limitations that warrant further re-
intended consumption goal is hedonic or utilitarian (e.g., “Are search. First, considering the emergence of service robotics and
you traveling for business or leisure?” “Are you celebrating any solo consumption in the hospitality sector, we examine the
special events at dinner tonight?”), as well as whether they are interplay of FAR with social context (solo vs. joint) in this
traveling alone or with others with a short questionnaire. Al- specific context. However, continued research might explore
ternatively, they might also leverage analytical tools to analyze whether our findings can be generalized to other service con-
customers’ purchasing history and browsing behaviors to infer texts, such as financial service, where both solo customers and
their motivations. For instance, a hotel might notice if a cus- service robots are also prevalent. We also call for studies that
tomer frequently browses high-end hotel rooms, suggesting extend the investigation into transformative services, such as
hedonic consumption goal, whereas another customer only healthcare and education, which promise to contribute
18 Journal of Service Research 0(0)

meaningfully to consumers’ well-being (Ge and Schleimer Supplemental Material


2023). Relatedly, future research could also explore our pro- Supplemental material for this article is available online.
posed effects in the service failure context as which might
constitute a negative experience, whereby customers’ service
expectations might vary and cause different perceptions of FAR. Notes
Second, while our central focus is on solo customers, future 1. We refer satisfaction in this research as one’s transaction-specific
research could deepen the comparative group of joint customers satisfaction. That is, customers’ experience after a particular
by considering their composition such as the number of joint service encounter with the service robot (Jones and Suh 2000).
companions (Mora and González 2016), their cohesiveness with 2. The respondents also answered four items that captured their
companions (Luo 2005), or shared identity (Kovacheva and current need to belong (e.g., “I am feeling a strong ‘need to be-
Lamberton 2018), as which might affect joint consumption long,’” ω = .95; Web Appendix C). As anticipated, solo diners
experiences. For instance, a highly cohesive group might be less expressed a higher need for belonging than joint diners (Msolo =
responsive to external cues and more focused on the group’s 4.97 vs. Mjoint = 4.14, t(91) = 2.951, p = .004).
activity. This could consequently reduce their attention towards 3. All diners also completed two checks, related to robot anthro-
FAR. Third, emerging research has begun to distinguish among pomorphism (1 = “very machinelike,” 7 = “very humanlike”; 1 =
different types of artificial intelligence (AI), namely mechanical, “more like an object,” 7 = “more like a person”; rSpearman Brown =
thinking, and feeling AI (Schepers et al. 2022). Future research .91, Choi, Mattila, and Bolton 2020). They perceived the restaurant
could elucidate whether these different types of AI influence service robots as anthropomorphic (Maverage = 4.69 vs. 4.00 as the
solo customers’ experience with FAR. For example, it is worth midpoint, t(222) = 6.173, p < .001).
investigating whether feeling AI might be more valued by solo 4. WeChat is the most popular social media platform in China and
customers. Fourth, another fruitful direction is to explore other WeChat Moments allows users to create postings and share their
relevant moderators, such as personality traits (e.g., self- status update with friends who can then like and comment, a
construal) or cultural background, that might alter people’s platform similar to Facebook Newsfeed. Postings on WeChat
information processing styles and thereby influence their about diners’ experiences at the restaurant could thus serve as an
evaluations of FAR. Specifically, past research contends that actual behavior capturing recommendations.
people with an independent (vs. interdependent) self-construal 5. Per an anonymous reviewer’s suggestion, we additionally per-
tend to prefer humanoid robot over mechanoid robot (Chang formed mediation analysis on mood and the indirect effects via
et al. 2023). Prior research also suggests that individuals in mood were non-significant for all three outcome variables (sat-
Western cultures tend to construe themselves as independent isfaction, CI = [ .23, .06]; PPP, CI = [ 2.92, .61]; WeChat
and be more analytic in thinking, whereas people in Eastern posting, CI = [ .43, .11]). We can thus rule out mood as an al-
cultures are likely to associate themselves with others, em- ternative explanation.
phasize social needs and with holistic thinking (Hwang, Shin, 6. This study presents a Pepper robot that has been deployed in
and Mattila 2018). This suggests the potential moderating roles airports in reality (https://thepointsguy.com/2018/02/munich-
of self-construal and culture in solo customer-FAR interaction. airport-humanoid-robot-josie-pepper/). In a pretest, we deter-
Finally, future research might also want to capture other be- mined that Amezen was perceived as a gender-neutral, culturally
havioral outcomes, particularly negative ones such as cus- non-sensitive name, which was unlikely to affect customers’
tomers’ switching or destructive behaviors, to provide a more perceived in-group favoritism. Its humanlike voice is available
comprehensive view of the processes we identify in this here: https://soundcloud.com/d-k-696080300/study2_aiport.
research. 7. We also included a similar manipulation check on social context
for Studies 3 and 4, and the results revealed that all participants’
Declaration of Conflicting Interests responses matched their assigned condition (solo or joint). Hence,
our manipulation was successful.
The author(s) declared no potential conflicts of interest with respect to
8. The image showed an actual anthropomorphized robot used at
the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article.
Henn-na, the world’s first robot-run hotel in Japan. The humanlike
voice can be found here: https://soundcloud.com/d-k-696080300/
Funding study3_hotel.
The author(s) disclosed receipt of the following financial support for 9. This robot appears at the Dadawan restaurant in Maastricht, the
the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article: This research Netherlands (https://www.trouw.nl/binnenland/in-dit-maastrichtse-
is supported by a grant from the Hong Kong SAR GRF research grants restaurant-neemt-robot-james-voortaan-uw-bestelling-op∼b52b5e49/).
awarded to the second author (HKBU 12501222). The pretest indicated that Amizen was perceived as a gender-neutral
name. Its humanlike voice is available here: https://soundcloud.com/
d-k-696080300/study4_restaurant.
ORCID iDs 10. For instance, the anthropomorphized robot featured in this link
Do The Khoa  https://orcid.org/0000-0002-7093-770X (https://www.bbc.com/pidgin/tori-53792797) displays a national
Kimmy Wa Chan  https://orcid.org/0000-0003-3604-1384 flag on its chest and left arm to indicate its origin.
Khoa and Chan 19

11. https://news.marriott.com/news/2015/09/09/marriott-hotels- Choi, S. and L. Wan (2021). The Rise of Service Robots in the
introduces-the-first-ever-in-room-virtual-reality-travel-experience. Hospitality Industry: Some Actionable Insights. Boston Hospi-
tality Review, 2–11.
Diakite, P. (2021), “Solo Travel Is on the Rise, Here’s Where Travelers
References Are Headed This Labor Day,” August 20, 2021, https://
Akdim, K., D. Belanche, and M. Flavián (2021). Attitudes Toward travelnoire.com/solo-travel-on-the-rise-labor-day-2021
Service Robots: Analyses of Explicit and Implicit Attitudes Based Epley, N., A. Waytz, and J. T. Cacioppo (2007), “On Seeing Human: A
on Anthropomorphism and Construal Level Theory. International Three-Factor Theory of Anthropomorphism,” Psychological
Journal of Contemporary Hospitality Management, 35 (8), 1–22. Review, 114 (4), 864-886.
Baumeister, R. F. and M. R. Leary (1995), “The Need to Belong: Desire Eyssel, F. and D. Kuchenbrandt (2011), “Social Categorization of
for Interpersonal Attachments as a Fundamental Human Moti- Social Robots: Anthropomorphism as a Function of Robot Group
vation,” Psychological Bulletin, 117 (3), 497-529. Membership,” British Journal of Social Psychology, 51 (4),
Becker, M., D. Mahr, and G. Odekerken-Schröder (2022), “Customer 724-731.
Comfort During Service Robot Interactions,” Service Business, Fast, E. and E. Horvitz (2017), “Long-Term Trends in the Public
17(1), 137-165. Perception of Artificial Intelligence,” Proceedings of The AAAI
Benoit, I. D. and E. G. Miller (2017), “The Mitigating Role of Holistic Conference On Artificial Intelligence, 31 (1), Article 1.
Thinking on Choice Overload,” Journal of Consumer Research, Fӧrster, J. (2009), “Relations Between Perceptual and Conceptual
34 (3), 181-190. Scope: How Global Versus Local Processing Fits a Focus on
Biedenbach, G., M. Bengtsson, and J. Wincent (2011), “Brand Equity Similarity Versus Dissimilarity,” Journal of Experimental Psy-
in the Professional Service Context: Analyzing the Impact of chology: General, 138 (1), 88-111.
Employee Role Behavior and Customer–Employee Rapport,” Ge, G. L. and S. C. Schleimer (2023), “Robotic Technologies and Well-
Industrial Marketing Management, 40 (7), 1093-1102. Being for Older Adults Living at Home,” Journal of Services
Blut, M., C. Wang, N. V. Wünderlich, and C. Brock (2021), “Un- Marketing, 37 (3), 340-350.
derstanding Anthropomorphism in Service Provision: A Meta- Goel, L., S. Prokopec, and I. Junglas (2013), “Coram Populo-In the
Analysis of Physical Robots, Chatbots, and Other AI,” Journal of Presence of People: The Effect of Others in Virtual Worlds,”
the Academy of Marketing Science, 49 (4), 632-658. Journal of Computer-Mediated Communication, 18 (3), 265-282.
Bolton, L. E. and A. Mattila (2015). How Does Corporate Social Goodwin, C. and L. Lockshin (1992), “The Solo Consumer: Unique
Responsibility Affect Consumer Response to Service Failure in Opportunity for the Service Marketer,” Journal of Services
Buyer–Seller Relationships? Journal of Retailing, 91 (1), Marketing, 6 (3), 27-36.
140–153. Gremler, D. D. and K. P. Gwinner (2000), “Customer-Employee
Botti, S. and A. L. McGill (2011), “The Locus of Choice: Personal Rapport in Service Relationships,” Journal of Service Re-
Causality and Satisfaction with Hedonic and Utilitarian search, 3 (1), 82-104.
Decisions,” Journal of Consumer Research, 37 (6), Han, H., L.-T. J. Hsu, J.-S. Lee, and C. Sheu (2011), “Are Lodging
1065-1078. Customers Ready to Go Green? An Examination of Attitudes,
Chang, Y., Y. Gao, D. Zhu, and A. A. Safeer (2023). Social Robots: Demographics, And Eco-Friendly Intentions,” International
Partner or Intruder in the Home? The Roles of Self-Construal, Journal of Hospitality Management, 30 (2), 345-355.
Social Support, and Relationship Intrusion in Consumer Prefer- Hayes, A. (2017), Introduction to Mediation, Moderation, and Con-
ence. Technological Forecasting and Social Change, 197, ditional Process Analysis: A Regression Based Approach. New
122914. York, NY: Guilford Press.
Chen, C. Y., L. Lee, and A. J. Yap (2017). Control Deprivation Hayes, A. F. and A. K. Montoya (2017), “A Tutorial on Testing,
Motivates Acquisition of Utilitarian Products. Journal of Con- Visualizing, and Probing an Interaction Involving a Multi-
sumer Research, 43, 1031–1047. categorical Variable in Linear Regression Analysis,” Communi-
Cheng, A. (2020), “New Study is a Sign Restaurants Need to Pamper cation Methods and Measures, 11 (1), 1-30.
Solo Diners More Than Ever,” accessed April 21, 2021,https:// Herz, R. S. (1997), “The Effects of Cue Distinctiveness on Odor-Based
www.forbes.com/sites/andriacheng/2020/03/03/one-more-sign- Context-Dependent Memory,” Memory and Cognition, 25 (3),
restaurants-need-to-pamper-solo-diners-more-than-ever 375-380.
Choi, I., R. Dalal, C. Kim-Prieto, and H. Park (2003), “Culture and Hogg, M. A. and D. J. Terry (2000), “Social Identity and Self-
Judgement of Causal Relevance,” Journal of Personality and Categorization Processes in Organizational Contexts,” The
Social Psychology, 84 (1), 46-59. Academy of Management Review, 25 (1), 121-140.
Choi, I., M. Koo, and J. A. Choi (2007), “Individual Differences in Hornsey, M. J. (2008), “Social Identity Theory and Self-Categorization
Analytic Versus Holistic Thinking,” Personality and Social Theory: A Historical Review,” Social and Personality Psychology
Psychology Bulletin, 33 (5), 691-705. Compass, 2 (1), 204-222.
Choi, S., A. S. Mattila, and L. E. Bolton (2020), “To Err Is Human(- Hossain, M. T. (2018), “How Cognitive Style Influences the Mental
oid): How Do Consumers React to Robot Service Failure and Accounting System: Role of Analytic Versus Holistic Thinking,”
Recovery?” Journal of Service Research, 24 (3), 354-371. Journal of Consumer Research, 45 (3), 615-632.
20 Journal of Service Research 0(0)

Hwang, Y., N. Su, and A. Mattila (2020), “The Interplay Between Lemon, K. N. and P.C. Verhoef (2016), “Understanding Customer
Social Crowding and Power on Solo Diners’ Attitudes Toward Experience Throughout the Customer Journey,” Journal of
Menus with Popularity and Scarcity Cues,” International Journal Marketing, 80 (November), 69-96.
of Contemporary Hospitality Management, 32 (3), 1227-1246. Li, X. and Y. Sung (2021). Anthropomorphism Brings Us Closer:
Jones, M. A. and J. Suh (2000), “Transaction-Specific Satisfaction and Mediating Role of Psychological Distance in User–AI Assistant
Overall Satisfaction: An Empirical Analysis,” Journal of Services Interactions, Computers in. Human Behavior, 118, 1–9.
Marketing, 14 (2), 147-159. Li, X., K. W. Chan, and S. Kim (2019), “Service with Emoticons: How
Kamin, D. (2021), “Traveling Alone, in Groups,” accessed August 20, Customers Interpret Employee Use of Emoticons in Online
2021,https://www.nytimes.com/2021/10/14/travel/solo-travel- Service Encounters,” Journal of Consumer Research, 45 (5),
group-tours.html 973-987.
Kätsyri, J., K. Förger, M. Mäkäräinen, and T. Takala (2015). A Review Lu, L., P. Zhang, and T. Zhang (2021). Leveraging “Human-Likeness”
of Empirical Evidence on Different Uncanny Valley Hypotheses: of Robotic Service at Restaurant. International Journal of Hos-
Support for Perceptual Mismatch as One Road to the Valley of pitality Management, 94, 102823.
Eeriness. Frontiers in Psychology, 6, 1–16. MacDorman, K., R. D. Green, C. C. Ho, and C. T. Koch (2009), “Too
Kim, H. J., J. Park, M.-J. Kim, and K. Ryu (2013). Does Perceived Real for Comfort? Uncanny Responses to Computer Generated
Restaurant Food Healthiness Matter? Its Influence on Value, Faces,” Computers in Human Behavior, 25 (3), 695-710.
Satisfaction and Revisit Intentions In Restaurant Operations in McLeay, F., V. S. Osburg, V. Yoganathan, and A. Patterson (2020),
South Korea. International Journal of Hospitality Management, “Replaced By a Robot: Service Implications in the Age of the
33, 397–405. Machine,” Journal of Service Research, 24 (1), 104-121.
Kim, J., J. Choe, and J. Hwang (2020). Application of Consumer Melnyk, V., K. Klein, and F. Vӧlckner (2012), “The Double-Edged
Innovativeness to the Context of Robotic Restaurants. Interna- Sword of Foreign Brand Names for Companies from Emerging
tional Journal of Contemporary Hospitality Management, 33 (1), Countries,” Journal of Marketing, 76 (6), 21-37.
224–242. Mende, M., M. L. Scott, J. van Doorn, D. Grewal, and I. Shanks
Kim, S. and J. Kim (2014), “The Influence of Hedonic Versus Utili- (2019), “Service Robots Rising: How Humanoid Robots In-
tarian Consumption Situations on the Compromise Effect,” fluence Service Experiences and Elicit Compensatory Con-
Marketing Letters, 27 (2), 387-401. sumer Responses,” Journal of Marketing Research, 56 (4),
Kim, N. and R. Ratner (2018), “Signaling Fun: Anticipated Sharing 535-556.
Leads to Hedonic Choice,” in NA - Advances in Consumer Re- Mora, J. and E. González (2016). Do Companions Really Enhance
search Volume 46, A. Gershoff, R. Kozinets, and T. White, eds. Shopping? Assessing Social Lift over Forms of Shopper Value
Duluth, MN: Association for Consumer Research, 209-213. in Mexico. Journal of Retailing & Consumer Services, 28,
Kim, S. Y., B. H. Schmitt, and N. M. Thalmann (2019), “Eliza in the 228–239.
Uncanny Valley: Anthropomorphizing Consumer Robots In- Mori, M. (1970), “The Uncanny Valley,” Energy, 7 (4), 33-35.
creases Their Perceived Warmth but Decreases Liking,” Mar- Nisbett, R. E. (2003), The Geography of Thought. New York: Free Press.
keting Letters, 30 (1), 1-12. Nisbett, R. E., R. K. Peng, I. Choi, and A. Norenzayan (2001), “Culture
Kovacheva, A. and C. Lamberton (2018), “Whose Experience is it, and Systems of Thought: Holistic Versus Analytic Cognition,”
Anyway? Psychological ownership and Enjoyment of Shared Psychological Review, 108 (2), 291-310.
Experiences,” Psychological ownership and consumer behavior, Pfalz, L. (2021), “Are Solo Trips Growing in Popularity?” accessed
195-210. August 20, 2021,https://www.travelpulse.com/news/features/are-
Krishna, A., R. Zhou, and S. Zhang (2008), “The Effect of Self- solo-trips-growing-in-popularity.html
Construal on Spatial Judgments,” Journal of Consumer Research, Ponsignon, F. (2023), “Making the Customer Experience Journey
35 (2), 337-348. More Hedonic in a Traditionally Utilitarian Service Context:
Krishna, A., M. O. Lwin, and M. Morrin (2010), “Product Scent and A Case Study,” Journal of Service Management, 34 (2),
Memory,” Journal of Consumer Research, 37 (1), 57-67. 294-315.
Kuchenbrandt, D., F. Eyssel, S. Bobinger, and M. Neufeld (2013). Puntoni, S., R. W. Reczek, M. Giesler, and S. Botti (2020), “Consumers
When a Robot’s Group Membership Matters: Anthropomorphi- and Artificial Intelligence: An Experiential Perspective,” Journal
zation of Robots as a Function of Social Categorization. Inter- of Marketing, 85 (1), 131-151.
national Journal of Social Robot, 5 (3), 409–417. Qiu, H., M. Li, B. Shu, and B. Bai (2019). Enhancing Hospitality
Leary, M. R., K. C. Herbst, and F. McCrary (2003). Finding Pleasure in Experience with Service Robots: Mediating of Rapport
Solitary Activities: Desire for Aloneness or Disinterest in Social Building. Journal of Hospitality Marketing & Management, 29
Contact? Personal and Individual Difference, 35 (1), 59–68. (3), 247–268.
Lee, J. (2018), “Can a Rude Waiter Make Your Food Less Tasty? Social Schanke, S., G. Burtch, and G. Ray (2021), “Estimating the Impact of
Class Differences in Thinking Style and Carryover in Consumer “Humanizing” Customer Service Chatbots,” Information Systems
Judgments,” Journal of Consumer Psychology, 28 (3), 450-465. Research, 32 (3), 736-751.
Lee, J. and W. Chu (2021), “The Effect of Adding Novel Attributes to Schepers, J., D. Belanche, L. V. Casaló, and C. Flavián (2022), “How
Hedonic vs. Utilitarian Base: Role of Holistic vs. Analytic Smart Should a Service Robot Be?” Journal of Service Research,
Thinking Style,” Asia Marketing Journal, 23 (2), 1-29. 25 (4), 565-582.
Khoa and Chan 21

Schultze, U. and J. A. M. Brooks (2018), “An Interactional View of Wan, L. C., E. Chan, and X. Luo (2021). Robots Come to Rescue: How
Social Presence: Making the Virtual Other “Real”,” Information to Reduce Perceived Risk of Infectious Disease in Covid19-
Systems Journal, 29 (3), 707-737. Stricken Consumers? Annals of Tourism Research, 88, 1–4.
Smith, R. W. and J. P. Redden (2020). The Role of Holistic Processing Zhou, X., L. He, Q. Yang, J. Lao, and R. F. Baumeister (2012), “Control
in Simultaneous Consumption. Journal of Experimental Social Deprivation and Styles of Thinking,” Journal of Personality and
Psychology, 91, 104023. Social Psychology, 102 (3), 460-478.
Spreng, R., S. MacKenzie, and R. Olshavsky (1996), “A Re-
examination of the Determinants of Consumer Satisfaction,” Author Biographies
Journal of Marketing, 60 (3), 15-32.
Do The Khoa is an Assistant Professor of Marketing in the
Swann, W. B., B. Stephenson, and T. Pittman (1981). Curiosity and
School of Business & Management, Royal Holloway-
Control: On the Determinants of the Search for Social Knowledge.
University of London, UK. His main research areas are service
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 40(4), 635–642.
robotics and transformative consumer research. He has pub-
Tajfel, H. and M. Billic (1974), “Familiarity and Categorization in
lished in Psychology & Marketing, Journal of Service Man-
Intergroup Behavior,” Journal of Experimental Social Psychol-
agement, The Service Industries Journal, among others.
ogy, 10 (2), 159-170.
Turner, J. C. (1987), “A Self-Categorization Theory,” in Rediscovering Kimmy Wa Chan is a Professor of Marketing in Hong Kong
the Social Group: A Self-Categorization Theory, J. Turner, et al. Baptist University. Her research interest is services marketing.
Wetherell, eds. New York: Blackwell, 42-67. She has published in premier journals, including Journal of
Wan, L. and R. Wyer (2018). The Influence of Incidental Similarity on Marketing Research, Journal of Marketing, Journal of Con-
Observers’ Causal Attributions and Reactions to a Service Failure. sumer Research, Journal of the Academy of Marketing Science,
Journal of Consumer Research, 45 (6), 1350–1368. Journal of Service Research, among others.

You might also like