You are on page 1of 12

Associations Between Physical Performance and

Executive Function in Older Adults With Mild


Cognitive Impairment: Gait Speed and the Timed ''Up
& Go'' Test
Ellen L. McGough, Valerie E. Kelly, Rebecca G.
Logsdon, Susan M. McCurry, Barbara B. Cochrane,
Joyce M. Engel and Linda Teri
PHYS THER. 2011; 91:1198-1207.
Originally published online May 26, 2011
doi: 10.2522/ptj.20100372

The online version of this article, along with updated information and services, can be
found online at: http://ptjournal.apta.org/content/91/8/1198

Collections This article, along with others on similar topics, appears


in the following collection(s):
Alzheimer Disease
Gait Disorders
Geriatrics: Other
Neurology/Neuromuscular System: Other
Tests and Measurements
e-Letters To submit an e-Letter on this article, click here or click on
"Submit a response" in the right-hand menu under
"Responses" in the online version of this article.
E-mail alerts Sign up here to receive free e-mail alerts

Downloaded from http://ptjournal.apta.org/ by guest on June 1, 2014


Research Report
Associations Between Physical
Performance and Executive Function
in Older Adults With Mild Cognitive
Impairment: Gait Speed and the
Timed “Up & Go” Test
Ellen L. McGough, Valerie E. Kelly, Rebecca G. Logsdon, Susan M. McCurry,
Barbara B. Cochrane, Joyce M. Engel, Linda Teri
E.L. McGough, PT, PhD, Depart-
ment of Rehabilitation Medicine,
University of Washington, 1959
Background. Older adults with amnestic mild cognitive impairment (aMCI) are
NE Pacific St, Box 356490, Seattle, at higher risk for developing Alzheimer disease. Physical performance decline on gait
WA 98195 (USA). Address all cor- and mobility tasks in conjunction with executive dysfunction has implications for
respondence to Dr McGough at: accelerated functional decline, disability, and institutionalization in sedentary older
emcg@uw.edu. adults with aMCI.
V.E. Kelly, PT, PhD, Department
of Rehabilitation Medicine, Uni- Objectives. The purpose of this study was to examine whether performance on
versity of Washington. 2 tests commonly used by physical therapists (usual gait speed and Timed “Up & Go”
R.G. Logsdon, PhD, School of Test [TUG]) are associated with performance on 2 neuropsychological tests of
Nursing, University of Washington. executive function (Trail Making Test, part B [TMT-B], and Stroop-Interference,
S.M. McCurry, PhD, School of calculated from the Stroop Word Color Test) in sedentary older adults with aMCI.
Nursing, University of Washington.
Design. The study was a cross-sectional analysis of 201 sedentary older adults with
B.B. Cochrane, PhD, RN, FAAN,
School of Nursing, University of memory impairment participating in a longitudinal intervention study of cognitive
Washington. function, aging, exercise, and health promotion.
J.M. Engel, OT, PhD, FAOTA,
Department of Occupational Sci-
Methods. Physical performance speed on gait and mobility tasks was measured
ences & Technology, University via usual gait speed and the TUG (at fast pace). Executive function was measured with
of Wisconsin–Milwaukee, Milwau- the TMT-B and Stroop-Interference measures.
kee, Wisconsin.

L. Teri, PhD, School of Nursing, Results. Applying multiple linear regression, usual gait speed was associated with
University of Washington. executive function on both the TMT-B (␤⫽⫺0.215, P⫽.003) and Stroop-Interference
(␤ ⫽⫺0.195, P⫽.01) measures, indicating that slower usual gait speed was associated
[McGough EL, Kelly VE, Logsdon
RG, et al. Associations between with lower executive function performance. Timed “Up & Go” Test scores (in
physical performance and exec- logarithmic transformation) also were associated with executive function on both the
utive function in older adults TMT-B (␤⫽0.256, P⬍.001) and Stroop-Interference (␤⫽0.228, P⫽.002) measures,
with mild cognitive impairment: indicating that a longer time on the TUG was associated with lower executive
gait speed and the Timed “Up &
function performance. All associations remained statistically significant after adjusting
Go” Test. Phys Ther. 2011;91:
1198 –1207.] for age, sex, depressive symptoms, medical comorbidity, and body mass index.
© 2011 American Physical Therapy
Association
Limitations. The cross-sectional nature of this study does not allow for inferences
of causation.
Published Ahead of Print: May 26,
2011 Conclusions. Physical performance speed was associated with executive func-
Accepted: March 22, 2011
Submitted: November 5, 2010 tion after adjusting for age, sex, and age-related factors in sedentary older adults with
aMCI. Further research is needed to determine mechanisms and early intervention
strategies to slow functional decline.
Post a Rapid Response to
this article at:
ptjournal.apta.org

1198 f Physical Therapy Volume 91 Number 8 August 2011


Downloaded from http://ptjournal.apta.org/ by guest on June 1, 2014
Physical Performance and Executive Function in Older Adults With Mild Cognitive Impairment

M
ild cognitive impairment difficulty of motor tasks,13,14 espe- healthy, slower self-selected gait
(MCI) is considered a transi- cially in novel or demanding situa- speed was associated with cognitive
tional state that is less severe tions.15 Medication adherence, cook- impairment at the 6-year follow-up.20
than dementia, but beyond that ing, housekeeping, and motor tasks In the Sydney Older Persons Study of
of typical age-related cognitive performed in a complex environ- people who did not have dementia
changes.1 Mild cognitive impair- ment are examples of goal-directed at baseline, the presence of slowed
ment is defined as impairment activities that are vulnerable to gait speed in combination with
(adjusted for age and education) in decline in executive function.12 cognitive deficits was associated
one or more domains of cognition, Executive function is thought to rely with increased odds of progression
with relative sparing of global cogni- strongly on the prefrontal cortex and to dementia.19 The combination of
tive functions.2– 4 Although MCI is includes multiple cognitive pro- impaired physical performance and
associated with only mild decline in cesses such as planning, tracking, executive dysfunction may be more
cognition, the onset of dementia is judgment, initiation, scanning, predictive of dementia risk; there-
characterized by overt difficulties in sequencing, problem solving, and fore, it has implications for acceler-
multiple domains of cognitive func- cognitive flexibility.12,16 The notion ated functional decline, disability,
tion as well as performance of daily that executive function is multifac- and institutionalization in older
activities.2 Even in the presence of eted in nature is supported by evi- adults with aMCI.
MCI, reduced function has been dence from functional magnetic res-
identified in executive function onance studies indicating that Studies of physical performance in
tasks,5,6 instrumental activities of different aspects of executive func- individuals with MCI support the
daily living7,8 and physical perfor- tion rely on different parts of the notion that physical performance
mance tasks.9,10 There are 2 major prefrontal cortex.17 impairment is present prior to the
subclassifications of MCI—amnestic onset of dementia,21,22 especially in
MCI (aMCI) and nonamnestic MCI Declining physical performance in older adults who demonstrate exec-
(naMCI)—the more common of conjunction with cognitive decline utive dysfunction.9,23 Executive dys-
which is aMCI.4,11 Older adults with has been associated with increased function is predictive of functional
aMCI, involving early memory loss, risk for dementia and disability in decline and increased risk for
are at higher risk for Alzheimer dis- population-based studies of older dementia in community-dwelling
ease (AD),4,11 and reduced executive adults.18,19 In a prospective, longitu- older adults.24,25 Early pathology,
function may be associated with dinal study of older adults who were consistent with AD, may contribute
early physical decline in people with
aMCI. Identifying whether physical
performance decline is associated
with reduced executive function is
The Bottom Line
important for developing physical
therapy management strategies What do we already know about this topic?
aimed at slowing the progression of
Older adults with mild cognitive impairment (MCI) are at higher risk for
functional decline and associated dis-
ability in older adults with aMCI. dementia and associated disability. Functional decline often is accelerated
in the presence of both physical and cognitive impairments.
The worsening of executive function What new information does this study offer?
in older adults with aMCI is associ-
ated with the conversion to AD.5 The In this study of sedentary older adults with amnestic MCI (memory loss),
degenerative processes in aMCI slower physical performance on gait and mobility tasks was associated
involve medial temporal lobe struc- with lower performance on executive function tasks, such as those
tures, as observed in early stages of involving planning and judgment.
AD, but also may include the frontal
lobe, the part of the brain involved in If you’re a patient or caregiver, what might these
executive function.4,5 Executive findings mean for you?
function involves higher-order cogni-
tive processes necessary for imple- Comprehensive prevention and rehabilitation strategies that enhance
mentation of goal-directed behav- both cognitive and physical function are important in reducing functional
iors,12 and reliance on executive decline and disability in older adults.
function is elevated with increasing

August 2011 Volume 91 Number 8 Physical Therapy f 1199


Downloaded from http://ptjournal.apta.org/ by guest on June 1, 2014
Physical Performance and Executive Function in Older Adults With Mild Cognitive Impairment

sex, depressive symptoms, medical


28 presentations at independent retirement residences
comorbidity, and body mass index
(BMI). We hypothesized that slower
physical performance speed would
890 telephone screening calls be associated with lower execu-
tive function after adjusting for fac-
tors that are known to affect both
physical performance and executive
Not eligible (n=343): function.
Too active=117
Health problem=93 Eligible for in-person Method
Too young=27 screening
No intervention group=60 (n=547)
Participants
Unavailable=46 This study involved analysis of base-
line data from the Resources and
Activities for Life-Long Indepen-
dence (RALLI) Study, a longitudinal
Completed in-person intervention study of cognitive func-
screening Eligible, not tion, aging, exercise, and health pro-
(n=359) interested (n=188) motion in sedentary older adults
with aMCI. Participants were volun-
teers living in independent retire-
ment residences who reported mild
Eligible for Not eligible after in-person
memory problems. Study flyers were
study screening (n=97):
High cognition=37
distributed, and a presentation was
(n=262)
Possible dementia=50 given to residents of 28 independent
Health problem=10 retirement living centers in the
Seattle, Washington, metropolitan
region. Residents who were inter-
ested in volunteering for the RALLI
Completed baseline assessment Study contacted the study coordina-
(n=201)
tor (Figure). The sample size was
Figure. determined based on a power analy-
Flow chart of participant recruitment and screening. sis conducted for the randomized
controlled trial.

to physical performance impairment higher risk for dementia and disabil- Participants enrolled in the study
through alterations in memory, ity,28 identifying whether physical were aged 70 years and older, were
attention, and executive function performance decline is associated sedentary, and were classified as
networks.26,27 Alternatively, age and with executive dysfunction is impor- having aMCI based on screening
age-related comorbid conditions may tant for developing physical therapy interviews and a consensus meeting.
be responsible for declining physical early intervention strategies for older Study recruitment and screening
performance and executive dysfunc- adults with aMCI. The purpose of consisted of: (1) a telephone screen-
tion in older adults with memory this study was to determine whether ing interview, (2) an in-home screen-
impairment. It is unclear whether an performance on 2 tests that are com- ing evaluation that consisted of a
association between physical perfor- monly used by physical therapists semistructured interview and neuro-
mance and executive function (usual gait speed and the Timed “Up psychological screening tests, and
remains after adjusting for age and & Go” Test [TUG]) are associated (3) an expert consensus panel to
age-related factors that are known to with performance on 2 neuropsy- review screening data. Petersen cri-
affect both physical performance chological tests of executive func- teria1,4 were applied using a combi-
and executive function in older tion (the Trail Making Test, part B nation of cognitive test scores,
adults with aMCI. [TMT-B], and Stroop-Interference, screening interview data, and con-
calculated from the Stroop Word sensus case review to identify peo-
Because older adults with both phys- Color Test) in sedentary older adults ple with memory problems that
ical and cognitive impairment are at with aMCI after adjusting for age, would be consistent with a clinical

1200 f Physical Therapy Volume 91 Number 8 August 2011


Downloaded from http://ptjournal.apta.org/ by guest on June 1, 2014
Physical Performance and Executive Function in Older Adults With Mild Cognitive Impairment

subtype of aMCI (single or multiple for dementia.3 Sedentary lifestyle Physical Performance Measures
domain). Petersen criteria included: was defined as performance of less Usual gait speed was calculated from
(1) memory complaint, (2) impaired than 150 minutes of moderate- an 8-foot (approximately 2.4 m) walk
memory for age and education, (3) intensity exercise per week (over test in which participants walked at
preserved general cognitive function, the previous month), as recom- their comfortable pace. The 8-foot
(4) essentially preserved activities of mended by the American College walk test was completed inside the
daily living, and (5) not already diag- of Sports Medicine and the Ameri- participant’s apartment or in a
nosed with dementia. Participants can Heart Association.33 nearby hallway on a level surface
were enrolled in the study from July with low-pile or indoor/outdoor car-
2007 through December 2009. Potential participants were excluded pet. The time to walk 8 feet was
from the study if they: (1) did not averaged over 2 trials and converted
Cognitive function tests and clinical meet aMCI criteria; (2) were unable to gait speed (meters per second).
criteria used to determine whether to walk independently with an assis- Comfortable walking speed measure-
participants met the aMCI classifica- tive device; (3) were expecting to ments have been reported to be
tion criteria included: (1) the Mini- move away from the area; (4) had a highly reliable (r⫽.903) in individu-
Mental State Examination (MMSE) known terminal illness; (5) were als who were healthy and ranging in
for global cognition,29 (2) the actively suicidal, hallucinating, or age from 20 to 79 years.36 Usual gait
Wechsler Memory Scale–Revised delusional; (6) had been hospitalized speed is comparable to the entire
(WMS-R) Logical Memory I and II within the previous 12 months; (7) Short Physical Performance Battery
subtests for immediate and delayed had an uncontrolled chronic medical in predicting disability in older
recall,30 and (3) the Clinical Demen- condition; (8) were blind or deaf; or adults.37
tia Rating Scale for severity rating (9) had a known central nervous
of cognitive impairment.31 Memory system condition associated with The TUG38 was performed at a fast
impairment was determined by a dementia. Upon enrollment in the pace to measure mobility speed.39
Clinical Dementia Rating Scale score study, participants completed 2 Participants were asked to move as
of 0.5 (consistent with MCI), a score in-home baseline evaluations admin- quickly but as safely as possible to
on the WMS-R Logical Memory sub- istered by trained research assistants. rise from an armchair (45.72-cm [18-
tests that was 1 standard deviation During these evaluations, testing was in] seat height), walk 3 m, turn
below age- and education-adjusted completed for demographic and around a cone, walk back to the
norms,32 problems on the memory health-related information, physical chair, and sit down. Time to com-
recall items of the MMSE, or performance measures, and execu- plete the TUG was averaged over 2
observed difficulty with everyday tive function measures as described trials. When performed at a comfort-
recall during the assessment inter- below. Each participant gave con- able pace, TUG scores have good
view. Because the classification of sent prior to the screening process. interrater and intrarater reliability as
aMCI involves a synthesis of informa- well as a high correlation with the
tion obtained through neuropsycho- Demographic and Health- Berg Balance Scale scores (r⫽⫺.81),
logical assessment, observations of Related Information gait speed (r⫽⫺.61) and Barthel
daily activities, and clinical judg- Demographic and health-related infor- Index of Activities of Daily Living
ment,2,3 each participant was mation was collected via self-report scores (r⫽⫺.78), and normative val-
reviewed through a consensus pro- responses. Medical comorbidity, ues have been reported.36,40 When
cess to determine eligibility for the assessed with the Self-Administered performed as quickly and as safely as
study. The above neuropsychologi- Comorbidity Questionnaire,34 was possible, the TUG has demonstrated
cal test scores, performance on spe- defined as having any of the following high sensitivity and specificity in
cific memory tasks, and evidence conditions: heart disease, hyperten- identifying older adults who are
indicating intact ability to perform sion, diabetes, pulmonary disease, prone to falling.39
activities of daily living were exam- kidney disease, peripheral vascular
ined by 2 clinical psychologists at a disease, osteoarthritis, rheumatoid Executive Function Measures
consensus meeting. Because aMCI arthritis, or back pain. Symptoms of The TMT-B was used to evaluate the
is a clinical classification for which depression were assessed using the components of executive function
there is no single, definitive diagnos- Geriatric Depression Scale (range of that represent complex visual scan-
tic test, a series of neuropsychologi- scores⫽0 –15).35 Body mass index ning, speed, attention, and ability to
cal tests as well as an expert clini- (kg/m2) was calculated using height shift sets.41,42 To complete this test,
cian’s observations and judgment are and weight measured at baseline. participants used a pencil to connect
critical in identifying people at risk 25 encircled numbers and letters in

August 2011 Volume 91 Number 8 Physical Therapy f 1201


Downloaded from http://ptjournal.apta.org/ by guest on June 1, 2014
Physical Performance and Executive Function in Older Adults With Mild Cognitive Impairment

numerical and alphabetical order, Data Analysis Stroop-Interference, was associated


alternating between numbers and We used SPSS statistical software, with the TUG after adjusting for
letters.43 The maximum amount of version 16.0,* for descriptive statis- covariates, 2 models were created
time allowed to complete the TMT-B tics and data analysis. To examine using log(TUG) as the outcome. The
is 300 seconds; longer times indicate the association between physical same covariates as above were
worse performance in executive performance and components of entered into each model because
function. The TMT-B has been executive function, linear regression they are known to influence both
widely used in studies of older was applied and model fit was eval- mobility speed and cognitive func-
adults, and normative data have been uated. A curvilinear relationship was tions. The covariate variables were
reported.44,45 The TMT-B was used in present between the TUG and exec- added first to each TUG model, fol-
this study because it is considered to utive function (both TMT-B and lowed by the executive function
be specific to executive function Stroop-Interference measures). With variable.
processes due to its requirements for the understanding that the model is
switching sets and mental tracking not intended for prediction, but A dichotomous variable was created
throughout the task.46 rather to determine whether a rela- for comorbidity (none versus one or
tionship exists, we made the deci- more medical conditions). Sex was
The Stroop Word Color Test was sion to log transform TUG scores. coded 0 (male) or 1 (female). Corre-
used to assess components of exec- Upon transformation, we found that lations and the variance inflation fac-
utive function representing a per- a linear relationship was present tor for multicollinearity were used to
son’s ability to deal with conflicting between log(TUG) and each execu- identify whether covariates were
stimuli.47 This test involves pairs of tive function variable. strongly correlated. The contribu-
conflicting stimuli that are presented tion of the executive function vari-
simultaneously, that is, the name of To assess whether executive func- able in each model was assessed by
one color printed in another color. tion, as measured by the TMT-B and the change in R2 values from the
There are 3 portions to the Stroop Stroop-Interference, was associated model with the covariates only to
Word Color Test: word naming (W), with usual gait speed after adjusting the model with the covariates and
color naming (C), and color interfer- for age, sex, depressive symptoms, the executive function variable.
ence (CW). Although there are vari- medical comorbidity, and BMI, we Residual analysis for each multiple
ations in test length and scoring created 2 multiple linear regression linear regression model included
methods,48,49 the version selected models. Covariates known to influ- normal probability plots and scatter
for this study involved recording the ence both walking speed and cogni- plots of standardized residuals.
number of correct responses in 45 tive functions, including age, sex,
seconds for each portion of the depressive symptoms, medical comor- Role of the Funding Source
test.50 A difference in the number of bidity, and BMI, were entered into Dr McGough received support
words printed in black ink compared each model. The covariate variables through a National Institutes of
with colors named correctly for were added first to each usual gait Health Rehabilitation Sciences pre-
words printed in a different color (ie, speed model, followed by the execu- doctoral fellowship (grant 2T32-HD-
blue ink for the word “red”) is inter- tive function variable. Although per- 00742416A1), a National Institute of
preted as interference of color stim- formance on the TMT-B and the Nursing Research/National Institutes
uli. An overall Stroop-Interference Stroop Word Color Test have been of Health post-doctoral fellowship
score, as introduced by Golden,51 associated with age and years of edu- (grant T32 NR007106), and the de
was calculated for this study using cation in older adults,45,53 education Tornyay Healthy Aging Doctoral
the formula: [CW ⫺ (W ⫻ C)/(W ⫹ was not included as a covariate in the Scholarship (School of Nursing, Uni-
C)]. In a previous study comparing multiple regression analysis because versity of Washington). This work
older adults with aMCI with older the majority of our sample had 12 was supported by the National Insti-
adults with noncognitive impair- years of more of education (97% had tute on Aging/National Institutes
ments and mild AD, those with aMCI ⬎12 years of education, and 79.6% of Health (grant 2RO1 AG14777-
performed less well than those who had ⬎13 years of education). 06A2).
were noncognitively impaired and
better than the AD group on the To assess whether executive func- Results
color interference condition.52 Nor- tion, as measured by the TMT-B and Data for demographic and health-
mative values for the raw scores from related variables are summarized in
the 3 portions of the Stroop Word * SPSS Inc, 233 S Wacker Dr, Chicago, IL Table 1. Participants had a mean age
Color Test have been reported.44,53 60606. of 84.6 years (SD⫽5.7), were 80.1%

1202 f Physical Therapy Volume 91 Number 8 August 2011


Downloaded from http://ptjournal.apta.org/ by guest on June 1, 2014
Physical Performance and Executive Function in Older Adults With Mild Cognitive Impairment

female, and were 91% Caucasian. Table 1.


The initial sample was composed of Descriptive Statisticsa
201 participants; however, 19 partic- Mean (SD) or
ipants did not complete the TMT-B Characteristic n Percentage Minimum Maximum
(16 due to vision problems and 3 due Demographic
to missing data), and 25 participants Age (y) 201 84.6 (5.7) 69.7 104.3
did not complete the Stroop Word
Sex, % female 201 80.1
Color Test (22 due to vision prob-
lems or color blindness and 3 due to Ethnicity, % Caucasian 201 91.0

missing data). There also were miss- % living alone 201 68.7
ing data on the GDS (n⫽2), TUG % high school education 201 97.5
(n⫽5), usual gait speed (n⫽2), Physical performance and
MMSE (n⫽1), and logical memory executive function
(n⫽1). After accounting for all data Gait speed (m/s) 199 0.61 (0.18) 0.24 1.08
entered into the multiple linear TUG (s) 196 11.96 (5.54) 5.20 35.70
regression models, 179 cases were
TUG (log) 196 1.041 (0.17) 0.716 1.553
analyzed for associations between
Trail Making Test, part B 182 148.04 (70.35) 47.0 300.0b
physical performance and the
TMT-B, and 173 cases were analyzed Stroop-Interference 176 ⫺81.09 (20.78) ⫺139.00 ⫺23.00
for associations between physical Clinical
performance and the Stroop- Geriatric Depression Scale 199 2.48 (2.37) 0 12
Interference measure. Sixteen par- WMS-R Logical Memory I 200 19.9 (7.5) 5.0 42.0
ticipants (8.0% of the entire sample
WMS-R Logical Memory II 200 14.1 (7.6) 0 35.0
and 10.8% of those in the final anal-
MMSE 200 26.47 (2.56) 18.00 30.00
ysis) reached the maximum time
(300 seconds) on the TMT-B. % CDR 0.5 200 100.0

% BMI ⱖ25 kg/m2 201 59.9


Usual gait speed was statistically sig- % medical comorbidity 201 77.6
nificantly associated with executive a
TUG⫽Timed “Up & Go” Test; TMT-B⫽Trail Making Test, part B; WMS-R⫽Wechsler Memory Scale–
function in both the unadjusted anal- Revised; MMSE⫽Mini-Mental State Examination; CDR⫽Clinical Dementia Rating Scale; BMI⫽body
ysis (Tab. 2) and after adjusting for mass index.
b
10.8% of participants (n⫽16) reached the maximum TMT-B time of 300 seconds.
covariates (Tab. 3). In the unadjusted
analysis, usual gait speed was associ-
ated with the TMT-B (␤⫽⫺.267, more variance than the unadjusted statistically significant when the
P⬍.001) and Stroop-Interference model. The change in R2 attributed TMT-B and Stroop-Interference mea-
(␤⫽⫺.214, P⫽.004) measures. The to the addition of the Stroop- sures were in the models, with
change in R2 values attributed to Interference measure to the model slower usual gait speed associated
executive function was .07 for the was .034. The overall change in R2 with older age and depressive
TMT-B and .05 for the Stroop- values was .102; therefore, the full symptoms.
Interference measure. After adjust- model explained 67.1% more of the
ing for covariates, the TMT-B variance than the unadjusted model. Log(TUG) was statistically signifi-
(␤⫽⫺.215, P⫽.003) and Stroop- In the full model for usual gait speed, cantly associated with executive
Interference (␤⫽⫺.195, P⫽.01) age and depressive symptoms were function in both the unadjusted anal-
findings were statistically significant,
indicating that slower usual gait
speed was associated with lower Table 2.
executive function performance on Linear Regression for Usual Gait Speed and Timed “Up & Go” Test (TUG) (Log
both measures. The change in R2 val- Transformed)
ues attributed to the addition of the Executive Function
TMT-B (the difference between the
Physical Trail Making Test, Stroop-Interference
full model and the model with cova- Performance Part B (nⴝ180) Measure (nⴝ174)
riates only) was .044. The overall Usual gait speed (m/s) ␤⫽⫺.267, P⬍.001 (R2⫽.07) ␤⫽⫺.214, P⫽.004 (R2⫽.05)
change in R2 values was .084; there-
Log(TUG) ␤⫽.290, P⬍.001 (R ⫽.08)
2
␤⫽.251, P⫽.001 (R2⫽.06)
fore, the full model explained 54.5%

August 2011 Volume 91 Number 8 Physical Therapy f 1203


Downloaded from http://ptjournal.apta.org/ by guest on June 1, 2014
Physical Performance and Executive Function in Older Adults With Mild Cognitive Impairment

Table 3. (␤⫽.228, P⫽.002) findings were sta-


Linear Regression for Gait Speed (m/s) tistically significant after adjusting
Standardized
for the other variables, indicating
Explanatory Variable Coefficient that slower TUG times were associ-
and Covariates (␤) P R 2a F ated with lower executive function
Model 1 (n⫽179) Age ⫺.199 .007 performance on both measures.
Sex ⫺.08 .27

Depressive symptoms ⫺.182 .01


The results indicate that a longer
time to complete the TUG was asso-
Medical comorbidity .057 .42
ciated with lower executive func-
Body mass index ⫺.09 .22 .110
tion, that is, a longer time to perform
Trail Making Test, part B ⫺.215 .003 .154b 5.25 (P⬍.001) the TMT-B and higher Stroop-
Model 2 (n⫽173) Age ⫺.173 .03 Interference scores. The change in
Sex ⫺.124 .09 R2 values attributed to the addition
Depressive symptoms ⫺.232 .002
of the TMT-B to the model was .063
(the difference between the full
Medical comorbidity .081 .26
model and the model with covariates
Body mass index ⫺.071 .35 .118
only). The overall change in R2 val-
Stroop-Interference ⫺.195 .01 .152b 5.00 (P⬍.001) ues was .13; therefore, the full model
a 2
The R value for the model not including the executive function variable. explained 61.6% more variance than
b
Change in R2 value was statistically significant at the .05 level when adding the executive function the unadjusted model. The change in
variable to the model.
R2 values attributed to the addition
of the Stroop-Interference measure
to the model was .043. The overall
ysis (Tab. 2) and after adjusting for function variable was .08 for the change in R2 values was .087; there-
covariates (Tab. 4). In the unadjusted TMT-B and .06 for the Stroop- fore, the full model explained 59.2%
analysis, log(TUG) was associated Interference measure. Log(TUG) was more of the variance than the unad-
with the TMT-B (␤⫽.290, P⫽⬍ .001) associated with both executive func- justed model. In the full models
and Stroop-Interference (␤⫽.251, tion measures after adjusting for for log(TUG), age, depressive symp-
P⫽.001) measures. The change in R2 covariates. The TMT-B (␤⫽.256, toms, and BMI were statistically sig-
values attributed to the executive P⬍.001) and Stroop-Interference nificant covariates, with higher val-
ues of log(TUG) (and, therefore,
slower performance on the TUG)
Table 4. associated with higher values of BMI
Linear Regression for Timed “Up & Go” Test (Log Transformed)
and depressive symptoms.
Standardized
Explanatory Variable Coefficient
(␤)
Examination of multicollinearity
and Covariates P R 2a F
among the explanatory variables
Model 1 (n⫽178) Age .173 .02
using the variance inflation factor
Sex .051 .45 resulted in values close to 1, indicat-
Depressive symptoms .217 .002 ing no collinearity. Analysis of resid-
Medical comorbidity ⫺.009 .90 uals for each model using normal
Body mass index .264 ⬍.001 .148 q-plots and scatter plots of residuals
by the estimated values showed that
Trail Making Test, part B .256 ⬍.001 .211b 7.66 (P⬍.001)
the model fit the data appropriately.
Model 2 (n⫽173) Age .156 .05

Sex .097 .18 Discussion


Depressive symptoms .245 .001 In this study of sedentary older
Medical comorbidity ⫺.036 .61 adults with aMCI, an association
Body mass index .198 .008 .104 between physical performance
speed and executive function on the
Stroop-Interference .228 .002 .147b 5.96 (P⬍.001)
TMT-B and Stroop-Interference mea-
a
The R2 value for the model not including the executive function variable.
b
Change in R2 value was statistically significant at the .05 level when adding the executive function
sures was demonstrated after adjust-
variable to the model. ing for age, sex, depressive symp-

1204 f Physical Therapy Volume 91 Number 8 August 2011


Downloaded from http://ptjournal.apta.org/ by guest on June 1, 2014
Physical Performance and Executive Function in Older Adults With Mild Cognitive Impairment

toms, and BMI. Slower usual walking older adults with aMCI may be partic- contribute to physical performance
speed was associated with lower ularly vulnerable to executive function impairment through alterations in
performance on a test of mental flex- and mobility impairment and, there- memory, attention, and executive
ibility (TMT-B) and with reduced fore, at higher risk for subsequent function networks.26,27 Alternatively,
ability to manage conflicting stimuli functional decline and falls. in older adults with aMCI, patholog-
(Stroop-Interference). Similarly, per- ical mechanisms associated with
formance on a functional mobility Slowed physical performance may declining physical performance may
task (TUG at fast pace) was associ- be a compensatory strategy to main- result from pathology not typically
ated with both measures of execu- tain accuracy in older adults with associated with AD, but instead
tive function. The results of this aMCI.59 People with MCI performed with other dementia syndromes
study demonstrate a consistent rela- daily activities at slower speeds, but (eg, Parkinson disease, vascular dis-
tionship between 2 commonly used maintained accuracy on a series of ease) that interfere with frontal-
physical therapy assessment tools daily activities.60 Older adults with subcortical circuits.27,64 Therefore,
and 2 measures of executive func- probable AD who were asked to per- further research is needed to iden-
tion. This finding is clinically rele- form a cognitive task (repeating ran- tify neuropathological mechanisms
vant in older adults with memory dom digits) while walking demon- involved in the association between
impairment because impairments in strated slower walking and greater physical performance speed and
physical and cognitive domains variability in their walking pattern, executive dysfunction in older adults
increase the risk for accelerated possibly due to reduced ability to with aMCI.
functional decline and disability, divide or prioritize attention.55 A
especially in the presence of execu- similar phenomenon may be occur- This study had a defined sample of
tive dysfunction.24 ring in older adults with aMCI, with sedentary older adults with aMCI and
a slowing of task speed in an effort to valid and reliable measures of physi-
The prevalence of slowed gait speed maintain accuracy even under condi- cal performance and executive func-
is evident when working memory is tions of relatively low cognitive or tion. There were, however, several
challenged in older adults with environmental challenge, as imple- limitations. A ceiling effect on the
MCI,54 thus supporting the notion mented in our study. Therefore, TMT-B occurred with 8.0% of par-
that gait is not entirely automatic, older adults with aMCI may be par- ticipants (final analysis) reaching the
but instead requires attentional ticularly vulnerable to physical per- 300-second maximum, so we lack an
resources.13,55 Physical performance formance decline and fall risk on estimate of the slowest performance
is particularly challenged when older tasks that require attention and learn- possible on the TMT-B. The cross-
adults are asked to concurrently per- ing, such as attending to a new walk- sectional nature of this study does
form a cognitive task, suggesting that ing route or other nonroutine activ- not allow for inferences of causation.
allocation of attention is necessary ities. Although age and age-related Nevertheless, consistent associations
in older adults with and without comorbid conditions may contribute were demonstrated, suggesting that
cognitive impairment.56 Associations to declining physical performance combining physical performance
between physical performance and and executive dysfunction in older and executive function assessments
cognitive function have been adults with memory impairment, the may be clinically useful in detecting
reported in previous studies in the statistically significant associations early functional decline in older
areas of gait speed, balance, and fall that remain after adjusting for these adults with MCI. Although efforts
risk in older adults with MCI,9,57 and factors in our study suggest that were made to minimize bias through
they are especially robust in the pres- other mechanisms, such as brain the selection of valid tests, consider-
ence of executive dysfunction.23 pathology, may be contributing to ation of potential confounders, and
Declining executive function may be this relationship. recruitment practices,65 a potential
an early indicator of overall func- source of bias remains because this
tional decline in older adults. For Medial temporal lobe structures, sample of older adults was recruited
example, in a prospective study of which are responsible for memory from independent retirement living
older women with intact cognition and learning, are the first brain centers. Future longitudinal studies
at baseline, executive function decline regions affected by AD pathology, to assess the predictive value of
occurred 3 years prior to memory followed by other cortical and sub- executive function measures on
decline over a 9-year follow-up period, cortical regions with disease progres- physical performance in people with
and executive function decline sion.61,62 Pathology consistent with aMCI are needed.
occurred more often than any other AD has been reported in the brains
cognitive impairment.58 Sedentary of older adults with aMCI63 and may

August 2011 Volume 91 Number 8 Physical Therapy f 1205


Downloaded from http://ptjournal.apta.org/ by guest on June 1, 2014
Physical Performance and Executive Function in Older Adults With Mild Cognitive Impairment

Conclusions Health post-doctoral fellowship (grant T32 14 Yogev-Seligmann G, Hausdorff JM, Giladi
NR007106), and the de Tornyay Healthy N. The role of executive function and
Slower physical performance was attention in gait. Mov Disord. 2008;23:
Aging Doctoral Scholarship (School of Nurs-
associated with lower executive 329 –342.
ing, University of Washington). This work
function in our sample of sedentary was supported by the National Institute on
15 Stuss DT. Biological and psychological
development of executive functions.
older adults with aMCI, and associa- Aging/National Institutes of Health (grant Brain Cogn. 1992;20:8 –23.
tions remained statistically signifi- 2RO1 AG14777-06A2). 16 Lezak MD. Domains of behavior from a
cant after adjusting for age, sex, DOI: 10.2522/ptj.20100372
neuropsychological perspective: the
whole story. Nebr Symp Motiv. 1994;41:
depressive symptoms, medical comor- 23–55.
bidity, and BMI. Slower gait and mobil- 17 Huizinga M, Dolan CV, van der Molen MW.
ity associated with reduced executive References Age-related change in executive function:
1 Petersen RC, Smith GE, Waring SC, et al. developmental trends and a latent variable
function in sedentary older adults Mild cognitive impairment: clinical char- analysis. Neuropsychologia. 2006;44:
with aMCI have implications for acterization and outcome [erratum in: 2017–2036.
Arch Neurol. 1999;56:760]. Arch Neurol.
accelerated functional decline, dis- 1999;56:303–308. 18 Aggarwal NT, Wilson RS, Beck TL, et al.
Motor dysfunction in mild cognitive
ability, and institutionalization. Fur- 2 Gauthier S, Reisberg B, Zaudig M, et al. impairment and the risk of incident Alz-
ther research is needed to determine Mild cognitive impairment. Lancet. 2006; heimer disease. Arch Neurol. 2006;63:
367:1262–1270. 1763–1769.
mechanisms for this association and
3 Petersen RC, Negash S. Mild cognitive 19 Waite LM, Grayson DA, Piguet O, et al.
whether early intervention strategies impairment: an overview. CNS Spectr. Gait slowing as a predictor of incident
are effective in slowing functional 2008;13:45–53. dementia: 6-year longitudinal data from
4 Petersen RC. Mild cognitive impairment as the Sydney Older Persons Study. J Neurol
decline and disability in sedentary Sci. 2005;229 –230:89 –93.
a diagnostic entity. J Intern Med. 2004;
older adults with aMCI. Early inter- 256:183–194. 20 Marquis S, Moore MM, Howieson DB, et al.
vention strategies that focus on 5 Rozzini L, Chilovi BV, Conti M, et al. Con- Independent predictors of cognitive
version of amnestic mild cognitive impair- decline in healthy elderly persons. Arch
enhancing executive function as Neurol. 2002;59:601– 606.
ment to dementia of Alzheimer type is
well as physical performance (eg, independent to memory deterioration. Int 21 Kluger A, Gianutsos JG, Golomb J, et al.
exercise) should be studied in seden- J Geriatr Psychiatry. 2007;22:1217–1222. Patterns of motor impairement in normal
6 Rosenberg PB, Mielke MM, Appleby B, aging, mild cognitive decline, and early
tary older adults with aMCI. Alzheimer’s disease. J Gerontol B Psychol
et al. Neuropsychiatric symptoms in MCI
subtypes: the importance of executive Sci Soc Sci. 1997;52:P28 –P39.
dysfunction. Int J Geriatr Psychiatry. 22 Kluger A, Gianutsos JG, Golomb J, et al.
Dr McGough, Dr Kelly, Dr Logsdon, Dr 2011;26:364 –372. Clinical features of MCI: motor changes.
McCurry, and Dr Teri provided concept/ 7 Perneczky R, Pohl C, Sorg C, et al. Com- Int Psychogeriatr. 2008;20:32–39.
idea/research design. All authors provided plex activities of daily living in mild cog- 23 Persad CC, Jones JL, Ashton-Miller JA, et al.
writing. Dr McGough, Dr Logsdon, Dr nitive impairment: conceptual and diag- Executive function and gait in older adults
McCurry, and Dr Teri provided data collec- nostic issues. Age Ageing. 2006;35: with cognitive impairment. J Gerontol A
240 –245. Biol Sci Med Sci. 2008;63:1350 –1355.
tion. Dr McGough, Dr McCurry, and Dr Teri
provided data analysis. Dr Logsdon, Dr 8 Mariani E, Monastero R, Ercolani S, et al. 24 Johnson JK, Lui LY, Yaffe K. Executive
Influence of comorbidity and cognitive function, more than global cognition, pre-
McCurry, and Dr Teri provided project status on instrumental activities of daily dicts functional decline and mortality in
management, fund procurement, partici- living in amnestic mild cognitive impair- elderly women. J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med
pants, and facilities/equipment. Dr Teri ment: results from the ReGAI project. Int Sci. 2007;62:1134 –1141.
provided institutional liaisons. Dr Kelly, J Geriatr Psychiatry. 2008;23:523–530.
25 Blacker D, Lee H, Muzikansky A, et al. Neu-
Dr Logsdon, Dr McCurry, Dr Cochrane, 9 Liu-Ambrose TY, Ashe MC, Graf P, et al. ropsychological measures in normal indi-
Dr Engel, and Dr Teri provided consulta- Increased risk of falling in older viduals that predict subsequent cognitive
community-dwelling women with mild decline. Arch Neurol. 2007;64:862– 871.
tion (including review of manuscript cognitive impairment. Phys Ther. 2008;
before submission). The authors thank the 88:1482–1491. 26 Buckner RL. Memory and executive func-
tion in aging and AD: multiple factors that
Northwest Research Group on Aging, Ken 10 Verghese J, Robbins M, Holtzer R, et al. cause decline and reserve factors that
Pike, PhD, for statistical support, and June Gait dysfunction in mild cognitive impair- compensate. Neuron. 2004;44:195–208.
van Leynseele, MA, for study coordination. ment syndromes. J Am Geriatr Soc. 2008;
27 Tekin S, Cummings JL. Frontal-subcortical
56:1244 –1251.
neuronal circuits and clinical neuropsychi-
The University of Washington Institutional 11 Reisberg B, Ferris SH, Kluger A, et al. Mild atry: an update. J Psychosom Res. 2002;
Review Board approved the study cognitive impairment (MCI): a historical 53:647– 654.
procedures. perspective. Int Psychogeriatr. 2008;20:
28 von Bonsdorff M, Rantanen T, Laukkanen
18 –31.
P, et al. Mobility limitations and cognitive
A poster presentation of this research was 12 Royall DR, Lauterbach EC, Cummings JL, deficits as predictors of institutionalization
given at the Combined Sections Meeting et al. Executive control function; a review among community-dwelling older people.
of the American Physical Therapy Associa- of its promise and challenges for clinical Gerontology. 2006;52:359 –365.
tion; February 17–20, 2010; San Diego, research: a report from the Committee on
Research of the American Neuropsychiat- 29 Folstein MF, Folstein SE, McHughs PR.
California. ric Association. J Neuropsychiatry Clin “Mini-mental state”: a practical method for
Neurosci. 2002;14:377– 405. grading the cognitive state of patients for
Dr McGough received support through a the clinician. J Psychiatr Res. 1975;12:
National Institutes of Health Rehabilitation 13 Sheridan PL, Hausdorff JM. The role of 189 –198.
higher-level cognitive function in gait:
Sciences predoctoral fellowship (grant 2T32- executive dysfunction contributes to fall 30 Weschler D. Weschler Memory Scale. 3rd
HD-00742416A1), a National Institute of risk in Alzheimer’s disease. Dement Geri- ed. In: Corporation TP (ed.). San Antonio,
TX: The Psychological Corporation; 1997.
Nursing Research/National Institutes of atr Cogn Disord. 2007;24:125–137.

1206 f Physical Therapy Volume 91 Number 8 August 2011


Downloaded from http://ptjournal.apta.org/ by guest on June 1, 2014
Physical Performance and Executive Function in Older Adults With Mild Cognitive Impairment

31 Hughes CP, Berg L, Danziger WL, et al. A 43 Bowie CR, Harvey PD. Administration and 55 Sheridan PL, Solomont J, Kowall N, Haus-
new clinical scale for the staging of interpretation of the Trail Making Test. dorff JM. Influence of executive function
dementia. Br J Psychiatry. 1982;140: Nat Protoc. 2006;1:2277–2281. on locomotor function: divided attention
566 –572. increases gait variability in Alzheimer’s
44 Steinberg BA, Bieliauskas LA, Smith GE, disease. J Am Geriatr Soc. 2003;51:
32 Steinberg BA, Bieliauskas LA, Smith GE, Ivnik RJ. Mayo’s older Americans norma- 1633–1637.
Ivnik RJ. Mayo’s older Americans norma- tive studies: age- and IQ-adjusted norms
tive studies: age- and IQ-adjusted norms for the Trail Making Test, the Stroop Test, 56 Kelly VE, Schrager MA, Price R, et al. Age-
for the Wechsler memory scale—revised. and MAE Controlled Oral Word Associa- associated effects of a concurrent cogni-
Clin Neuropsychol. 2005;19:378 – 463. tion Test. Clin Neuropsychol. 2005;19: tive task on gait speed and stability during
329 –377. narrow-base walking. J Gerontol A Biol Sci
33 Nelson ME, Rejeski WJ, Blair SN, et al. Med Sci. 2008;63:1329 –1334.
Physical activity and public health in older 45 Zalonis I, Kararizou E, Triantafyllou NI, et
adults: recommendation from the Ameri- al. A normative study of the Trail Making 57 Eggermont LH, Gavett BE, Volkers KM, et
can College of Sports Medicine and the Test A and B in Greek adults. Clin Neuro- al. Lower-extremity function in cognitively
American Heart Association. Med Sci psychol. 2008;22:842– 850. healthy aging, mild cognitive impairment,
Sports Exerc. 2007;39:1435–1445. and Alzheimer’s disease. Arch Phys Med
46 Arbuthnott K, Frank J. Trail Making Test, Rehabil. 2010;91:584 –588.
34 Sangha O, Stucki G, Liang MH, et al. The part B as a measure of executive control:
Self-administered Comorbidity Question- validation using a set-switching paradigm. 58 Carlson MC, Xue QL, Zhou J, Fried LP.
naire: a new method to assess comorbidity J Clin Exp Neuropsychol. 2000;22: Executive decline and dysfunction pre-
for clinical and health services research. 518 –528. cedes declines in memory: the Women’s
Arthritis Rheum. 2003;49:156 –163. Health and Aging Study II. J Gerontol A
47 Stroop JR. Studies of interference in serial Biol Sci Med Sci. 2009;64:110 –117.
35 Yesavage JA, Brink TL, Rose TL, et al. verbal reaction. J Exp Psychol. 1936;18:
Development and validation of a geriatric 634 – 662. 59 Scherder E, Eggermont L, Swaab D, et al.
depression screening scale: a preliminary Gait in ageing and associated dementias;
48 Van der Elst W, Van Boxtel MP, Van
report. J Psychiatr Res. 1982;17:37– 49. its relationship with cognition. Neurosci
Breukelen GJ, Jolles J. The Stroop Color- Biobehav Rev. 2007;31:485– 497.
36 Bohannon RW. Comfortable and maxi- Word Test: influence of age, sex, and
mum walking speed of adults aged 20 –79 education; and normative data for a large 60 Wadley VG, Okonkwo O, Crowe M, Ross-
years: reference values and determinants. sample across the adult age range. Assess- Meadows LA. Mild cognitive impairment
Age Ageing. 1997;26:15–19. ment. 2006;13:62–79. and everyday function: evidence of
reduced speed in performing instrumental
37 Guralnik JM, Ferrucci L, Pieper CF, et al. 49 Valentijn SA, van Boxtel MP, van Hooren activities of daily living. Am J Geriatr Psy-
Lower extremity function and subsequent SA, et al. Change in sensory functioning chiatry. 2008;16:416 – 424.
disability: consistency across studies, pre- predicts change in cognitive functioning:
dictive models, and value of gait speed results from a 6-year follow-up in the Maas- 61 Braak H, Braak E. Neuropathological stage-
alone compared with the short physical tricht Aging Study. J Am Geriatr Soc. ing of Alzheimer-related changes. Acta
performance battery. J Gerontol A Biol Sci 2005;53:374 –380. Neuropathol. 1991;82:239 –259.
Med Sci. 2000;55:M221–M231. 50 Golden CJ, Freshwater SM. Stroop Color 62 Braak H, Alafuzoff I, Arzberger T, et al.
38 Podsiadlo D, Richardson S. The timed “Up and Word Test: A Manual for Clinical Staging of Alzheimer disease-associated
& Go”: a test of basic functional mobility and Experimental Uses. Wood Dale, IL: neurofibrillary pathology using paraffin
for frail elderly persons. J Am Geriatr Soc. Stoelting Co; 2002. sections and immunocytochemistry. Acta
1991;39:142–148. Neuropathol. 2006;112:389 – 404.
51 Golden CJ. Identification of brain disor-
39 Shumway-Cook A, Brauer S, Woollacott M. ders by the Stroop Color and Word Test. 63 Petersen RC, Parisi JE, Dickson DW, et al.
Predicting the probability for falls in J Clin Psychol. 1976;32:654 – 658. Neuropathologic features of amnestic mild
community-dwelling older adults using cognitive impairment. Arch Neurol. 2006;
52 Kramer JH, Nelson A, Johnson JK, et al.
the Timed Up & Go Test. Phys Ther. 2000; 63:665– 672.
Multiple cognitive deficits in amnestic
80:896 –903. mild cognitive impairment. Dement Geri- 64 Blahak C, Baezner H, Pantoni L, et al. Deep
40 Steffen TM, Hacker TA, Mollinger L. Age- atr Cogn Disord. 2006;22:306 –311. frontal and periventricular age related
and gender-related test performance in white matter changes but not basal ganglia
53 Zalonis I, Christidi F, Bonakis A, et al. The
community-dwelling elderly people: Six- and infratentorial hyperintensities are
stroop effect in Greek healthy population:
Minute Walk Test, Berg Balance Scale, associated with falls: cross-sectional
normative data for the Stroop Neuropsy-
Timed Up & Go Test, and gait speeds. results from the LADIS study. J Neurol
chological Screening Test. Arch Clin Neu-
Phys Ther. 2002;82:128 –137. Neurosurg Psychiatry. 2009;80:608 – 613.
ropsychol. 2009;24:81– 88.
41 Reitan RM, Herring S. A short screening 65 Portney LG, Watkins WM. Foundations of
54 Montero-Odasso M, Bergman H, Phillips
device for identification of cerebral dys- Clinical Research Applications to Prac-
NA. Dual-tasking and gait in people with
function in children. J Clin Psychol. 1985; tice. 3rd ed. Upper Saddle River, NJ: Pear-
mild cognitive impairment: the effect of
41:643– 650. son Education Inc; 2009.
working memory. BMC Geriatr. 2009;
42 Greenlief CL, Margolis RB, Erker GJ. Appli- 9:41.
cation of the Trail Making Test in differen-
tiating neuropsychological impairment of
elderly persons. Percept Mot Skills. 1985;
61(3 pt 2):1283–1289.

August 2011 Volume 91 Number 8 Physical Therapy f 1207


Downloaded from http://ptjournal.apta.org/ by guest on June 1, 2014
Associations Between Physical Performance and
Executive Function in Older Adults With Mild
Cognitive Impairment: Gait Speed and the Timed ''Up
& Go'' Test
Ellen L. McGough, Valerie E. Kelly, Rebecca G.
Logsdon, Susan M. McCurry, Barbara B. Cochrane,
Joyce M. Engel and Linda Teri
PHYS THER. 2011; 91:1198-1207.
Originally published online May 26, 2011
doi: 10.2522/ptj.20100372

References This article cites 62 articles, 12 of which you can access


for free at:
http://ptjournal.apta.org/content/91/8/1198#BIBL
Cited by This article has been cited by 6 HighWire-hosted articles:

http://ptjournal.apta.org/content/91/8/1198#otherarticles
Subscription http://ptjournal.apta.org/subscriptions/
Information
Permissions and Reprints http://ptjournal.apta.org/site/misc/terms.xhtml
Information for Authors http://ptjournal.apta.org/site/misc/ifora.xhtml

Downloaded from http://ptjournal.apta.org/ by guest on June 1, 2014

You might also like