You are on page 1of 8

J Physiol (2003), 553.3, pp. 967–974 DOI: 10.1113/jphysiol.2003.

047597
© The Physiological Society 2003 www.jphysiol.org
Physiology in Press

A learned component of the ventilatory response to exercise


in man
Helen E. Wood, Marzieh Fatemian and Peter A. Robbins
University Laboratory of Physiology, University of Oxford, Parks Road, Oxford OX1 3PT, UK

The ventilatory response to mild-to-moderate exercise in humans is isocapnic, or ‘error-free’. It has


been suggested that this response is learned over many repetitions of exercise through the process of
minimising any deviations from normal in the blood gas tensions, as sensed by the chemoreceptors.
However, relatively limited training programmes have failed to produce any convincing evidence in
humans that forcibly altering the blood gas tensions during repeated periods of exercise alters the
subsequent steady-state ventilatory response to exercise. In this study, eight healthy young subjects
were exposed, over a 7 day training period, to a total of 70 repeated bouts of exercise paired with a
simultaneous airway CO2 load to stimulate the chemoreceptors (protocol EX + CO2). The
ventilatory response to exercise was measured before and after training to determine whether it had
been modified. Two further training protocols were undertaken as controls. One employed
repeated exercise without an airway CO2 load, and the other employed repeated airway CO2 loading
without exercise. On the 1st and 2nd days following training with protocol EX + CO2, end-tidal PCJ
was regulated at a lower level during steady-state exercise than following training with the control
protocols and than before training (mean ± S.E.M. reduction in end-tidal PCJ = 1.32 ± 0.36 Torr,
ANOVA, P < 0.05). In contrast to previous studies, this finding demonstrates that the steady-state
ventilatory response to exercise can be modified by a prior period of altered chemoreception during
exercise. This suggests that ventilation is matched to metabolic rate during exercise by a mechanism
that involves learning and memory.
(Resubmitted 19 May 2003; accepted after revision 24 September 2003; first published online 26 September 2003)
Corresponding author P. A. Robbins: University Laboratory of Physiology, University of Oxford, Parks Road, Oxford OX1 3PT,
UK. Email: peter.robbins@physiol.ox.ac.uk

As first demonstrated by Haldane & Priestley (1905), the mechanisms have been calibrated to ensure that ◊E rises in a
increase in metabolism that occurs as a result of muscular manner so precisely matched to ◊CJ.
exercise is matched exactly by an increase in pulmonary
In an attempt to overcome this problem, Somjen (1992)
ventilation (◊E). They proposed that during exercise, an
has hypothesised that feedforward control is a learned
increase in alveolar PCJ (PA,CJ), and hence arterial PCJ
response. He proposed that the brain ‘knows’ exactly how
(Pa,CJ), would stimulate the chemoreceptors, and that this
much O2 is demanded and CO2 produced, by the level of
would increase ◊E, thus providing the link between ◊E and
exercise being undertaken and has learned to anticipate
metabolic CO2 production (◊CJ). However, subsequent
Journal of Physiology

the corresponding increase in ◊E that is necessary to avoid


studies have demonstrated that the matching of ◊E to ◊CJ is
any changes in arterial blood gases. He hypothesised that
so precise that during mild-to-moderate exercise, Pa,CJ
the brain has learned how to do this over a period of many
does not change and hence there is no error signal to drive
years, from early in infancy, by a process of ‘trial and error’.
this feedback mechanism (Wasserman et al. 1967). How
◊E would initially be driven by chemoreceptor feedback
this exact matching of ◊E to ◊CJ occurs is not understood.
signals during increased levels of activity, but gradually the
A variety of mechanisms have been proposed to drive the individual would learn to anticipate these errors in blood
increase in ◊E during exercise, including ‘irradiation’ of gases and thus avoid them. Hence, control of ◊E becomes
signals from the motor cortex (feedforward control), possible without the need for any error signal.
stimulation of neural receptors in the exercising muscles,
Somjen indicated that control of ◊E in the absence of error
stimulation of chemoreceptors by a humoral factor released
signals is only possible while the internal and external
from the exercising muscles, and stimulation of chemo-
environments remain constant, because if these change,
receptors in the lungs by mixed venous PCJ (Zuntz &
error signals will result and a process of re-learning will be
Geppert, 1886). All of these mechanisms may, to a greater or
required. As such, it should be possible to test this theory
lesser extent, serve to increase ◊E during muscular exercise.
by repeatedly generating error signals experimentally (i.e.
However, none provides an explanation of how such
disturbances in arterial blood gases) during exercise to
968 H. E. Wood, M. Fatemian and P. A. Robbins J Physiol 553.3
Physiology in Press

induce a process of re-learning. Subsequent removal of METHODS


these error signals should reveal whether learning has
Subjects
occurred, because ◊E and ◊CJ will no longer be exactly The subjects were eight young healthy males with no history of
matched, the brain having learned to anticipate the level of respiratory or cardiovascular disease. All subjects were non-
◊E required under different conditions. smokers. All experiments conformed to the Declaration of
Helsinki, and received the approval of the local ethics committee
There is some evidence in support of this theory using just (COREC). The nature and requirements of the experiments were
such an experimental approach in goats (Martin & explained to the subjects without revealing the purpose of the
Mitchell, 1993). After repeated trials of exercise paired study, and subjects gave their informed written consent prior to
with an added external dead space to induce hypercapnia, each experiment.
steady-state ◊E was increased during subsequent trials of Protocols
exercise without added dead space. However, similar Prior to the main experiments, a set of preliminary measurements
experiments in human subjects have failed to demonstrate was made. These included measurements of end-tidal PCJ
convincing evidence in favour of Somjen’s theory (Helbling (PET,CJ) and PJ (PET,J) with the subject at rest, determination of
et al. 1997; Moosavi et al. 2002; Turner & Sumners, 2002). maximal work rate using an incremental exercise test, and
measurements of PET,CJ and PET,J at a work rate equivalent to
With one exception, a potential problem with the design of 50 % of maximal (chosen to be below the anaerobic threshold but
these previous studies is that the overall period within above a level associated with slow walking/cycling on level
which training has taken place has been relatively short. ground). This subject-specific work rate was used throughout the
remainder of the study.
The exception is the study by Helbling et al. (1997), where
an 8 day training programme was employed. However, in For the main experiments, the subjects undertook three different
their training programme there were only two rest-to- protocols in a random order. The randomisation was undertaken
exercise transitions on each training day. Should it be the by noting that there were six possible orders in which these
protocols could be undertaken; these were chosen at random to
case that the change in chemoreceptor feedback occurring
provide the order for subjects 1–6, with subject 7 and 8 repeating
from rest to exercise is important in the learning process, the order used for subjects 1 and 2. Each protocol comprised a
then it is possible that the total number of transitions 7 day training period, which was separated from the training
employed by Helbling et al was too few for effective period of any other protocol by a minimum of 4 weeks (see Fig. 1).
modification of the steady-state ventilatory response to Measurements of the ventilatory response to exercise were made
exercise to occur. In general, it seems unlikely that a before and after these training periods to determine whether the
control system that has been calibrated over years would response had been modified by training. In protocol EX + CO2,
training involved repeated bouts of exercise paired with a
alter its properties markedly over a period lasting only a
simultaneous airway CO2 load to stimulate the chemoreceptors;
few hours or perhaps with a training protocol involving in protocol EX, training involved repeated bouts of air-breathing
relatively few rest-to-exercise transitions. The present exercise; and in protocol CO2, training involved repeated bouts of
study sought to address these issues by employing a airway CO2 loading at rest. During each 7 day training period
training protocol lasting for 7 consecutive days, with 10 subjects came to the laboratory twice a day, and on each occasion
rest-to-exercise transitions on each day. undertook five bouts of training, each lasting 4 min. These bouts
were separated from one another by 4 min periods of air-
Journal of Physiology

Figure 1. Summary of protocols and


measurements
Subjects undertook three different 7 day training
protocols with identical measurements of the
ventilatory response to exercise, under closed-
loop and open-loop conditions, made before and
after each training period.
J Physiol 553.3 Learned ventilatory response to exercise 969
Physiology in Press

breathing rest. Outside the laboratory, subjects were requested to was able to maintain for a full minute. Heart rate was monitored
refrain from taking exercise other than the minimum required to throughout the test to check that the expected maximal value was
travel around (slow walking and cycling on level ground). Subjects reached, and plots of PET,CJ vs. work rate were inspected visually
were equipped with a pulse meter to warn them if they were to check that anaerobic threshold had been achieved several
exerting themselves to too great a degree. Subjects tended to only minutes prior to the end of the test (i.e. plots became alinear).
wear the monitor for the first couple of days of each protocol, until
During training, the inspired gas composition was controlled
they became accustomed to maintaining this minimal level of
manually using rotameters (Roxspur Measurement & Control,
exertion.
Bramley, UK). For protocol EX + CO2, values for inspiratory PCJ
In order to determine whether the subjects’ perception of and PJ were chosen that would raise PET,CJ by 8 Torr above the
breathlessness differed between the three training protocols they normal level during exercise and lower PET,J to 8 Torr below the
were asked to rate their sensations of breathlessness during the normal level during exercise. PET,J was lowered in addition to
final minute of each bout of training, according to a modified raising PET,CJ in order that the stimulus would mimic the effects of
Borg scale (Borg, 1982). This scale associates numerical ‘scores’ hypoventilation as closely as possible. With a background flow of
with verbal expressions; in this case, descriptions of the strength of air, the flows of CO2 and N2 were adjusted until the desired end-
sensations of breathlessness, which ranged from zero (‘none at tidal gas values were achieved. For protocol EX, subjects breathed
all’) to 10 (‘very, very heavy’). A card displaying the modified Borg air throughout the training periods. For protocol CO2, values for
scale was held in front of the subject, who was asked to point at the inspiratory PCJ and PJ were chosen that would raise PET,CJ by
score associated with the expression that best described their 8 Torr above the normal level at rest and lower PET,J to 8 Torr
sensations of breathlessness at that moment. below the normal level at rest.
The respiratory response to exercise before and after training was Measurements of the ventilatory response to exercise on days
assessed in two ways. In the first, subjects undertook an 8 min before and after the training periods were made using a turbine
period of exercise breathing air, which leaves the biological volume transducer (Cardiokinetics) to measure respiratory
feedback loop between CO2 and ventilation intact (closed-loop volumes (Howson et al. 1986). All data were logged to a personal
measurements). In the second, subjects undertook an identical computer. Two sets of measurements were made under closed-
period of exercise, but with their PET,CJ regulated at a fixed level, loop conditions followed by two sets of measurements under
which opens the biological feedback loop to allow measurement open-loop conditions. For each measurement, there was an initial
of ◊E at a fixed level of PCJ (open-loop measurements). Measure- period of 5 min at rest, followed by 8 min of exercise, and then a
ments were undertaken the day before training (day _1), on the final 5 min of rest, throughout which the subject was seated on the
1st and 2nd days after training (days +1 and +2) and 2 weeks after cycle ergometer. For the closed-loop measurements, subjects
training (day +14). breathed air throughout. For the open-loop measurements, the
end-tidal gases were regulated using an end-tidal forcing system
Experimental technique
(Robbins et al. 1982). PET,CJ was held constant at 2 Torr above the
Subjects sat upright in a chair for the procedures exclusively at
subject’s normal air-breathing value in exercise during the 8 min
rest, and on a cycle ergometer for those involving any exercise. For
of exercise and at 2 Torr above the subject’s normal air-breathing
the preliminary measurements of the end-tidal gases at rest, gas
value at rest during the 5 min periods of rest before and after
was sampled from a catheter taped just below the subject’s nostril
exercise. PET,J was held at the subject’s normal level during
and analysed by mass spectrometry (Airspec MGA300, UK). For
exercise for the 8 min of exercise, and at the subject’s normal level
all other experiments, subjects breathed through a mouthpiece
at rest for the 5 min periods of rest before and after exercise.
with the nose occluded by a clip. Gas was sampled from a port in
the mouthpiece. Data analysis
In order to assess visually the effects of the different protocols,
All exercise in this study was performed on the same cycle
breath-by-breath data from the pre- and post-training measure-
ergometer (Mijnhardt KEM-3, Cardiokinetics, Salford, UK). This
ments were first averaged over 20 s intervals. This enabled the
ergometer was electromagnetically braked and provided a
responses to be averaged across subjects for both the closed- and
constant work rate, independent of pedal frequency. A coloured
open-loop measurements. These average responses were then
Journal of Physiology

light display mounted on the handlebars indicated pedal rate, and


plotted.
subjects were instructed to maintain a rate of 60 revs min_1 (the
‘green range’) at all times. Subjects were assisted by verbal To provide a statistical analysis of the significance of any changes
instructions whenever they had problems maintaining this rate observed, rest-to-exercise differences were calculated for PET,CJ
(‘slow down’ or ‘speed up’, as required). and ◊E for each exercise response. These were obtained by
subtracting the average ◊E or PET,CJ for the last 3 min of rest prior
The procedure used for the incremental exercise test was as
to the onset of exercise from the average ◊E or PET,CJ for the final
follows: subjects pedalled at 0 W for 4 min, after which the load
3 min of exercise. For each subject, the pairs of values obtained on
was increased by 25–30 W (the chosen increment depended on
the same day were averaged. Differences between these responses
how fit the subject reported himself to be) every minute until
for days _1, +1 and +2 for the different protocols were then
exhaustion. Subjects were instructed to maintain a pedal speed of
assessed for statistical significance using ANOVA. In the ANOVA,
60 revs min_1. Exhaustion was taken as the point when either the
subjects were treated as a random factor, and the fixed factors were
subject stopped voluntarily or was unable to maintain a pedal rate
set in one of the two ways, depending on the hypothesis under test.
of 60 revs min_1 following an increment, and was told to stop.
Verbal encouragement was given throughout the test to ensure the To determine whether the different protocols had different
subject reached exhaustion. Maximal O2 uptake capacity was effects, the data for days +1 and +2 were compared between
taken as the maximal single-breath value achieved during the test protocols and with the control (day _1) data. To determine
and was calculated using the method of Pandit & Robbins (1992). specifically whether the effects of protocol EX + CO2 for example,
Maximal work rate was taken as the highest load that the subject differed from all else, the days +1 and +2 data for protocol
970 H. E. Wood, M. Fatemian and P. A. Robbins J Physiol 553.3
Physiology in Press

EX + CO2 were compared with pooled data from the day _1 RESULTS
measurements and the days +1 and +2 data from the other two
protocols. Subjects
The individual physical characteristics of the subjects are
Daily average ratings of breathlessness (Borg scores) were calculated
for each subject and these were averaged overall to give a mean given in Table 1, along with values for maximal O2 uptake
score for each protocol. Differences between these scores were (◊O2,max) and the work rate equivalent to 50 % of maximal.
assessed statistically using ANOVA. There may have been some bias away from regular
sportsmen in the selection of subjects due to the
All statistical analysis was performed using the SPSS statistical
software package. Significance was accepted at P < 0.05 and unless limitations that the protocol placed on sporting activities.
indicated otherwise, data are presented as the mean ± S.E.M. However, of the eight subjects selected, only one usually
took no regular exercise.
Closed-loop responses
The closed-loop responses to (air-breathing) exercise
before and after training with the three protocols are
shown in Figs 2 and 3. Average values for PET,CJ, ◊E,
respiratory frequency (fR) and tidal volume (VT) over the
final 3 min of both rest and exercise are given in Table 2.
Inspection of the figures suggests that there was no
training effect of the repeated inspiratory CO2 load at rest
(protocol CO2) in the sense that the regulation neither of
PET,CJ during exercise nor ◊E during exercise was affected
by the training. Following training with repeated exercise
(protocol EX), there was again no change in the level at
which PET,CJ was regulated during exercise compared with
Journal of Physiology

before training. However, ◊E during exercise was lower


after the 70 training bouts of exercise than before,
suggesting a physical training effect. In contrast to the
other two training protocols, training with paired exercise
and inspiratory CO2 load (protocol EX + CO2) resulted in
the PET,CJ during air-breathing exercise being regulated at
a lower level than before training. ◊E during exercise
following protocol EX + CO2 training was maintained at
Figure 2 pre-training levels, instead of falling, as occurred in
PET,CJ measured in normal closed-loop (air-breathing) conditions
response to the physical training effect of protocol EX.
during exercise (0–8 min) before training (day _1) and after These observations were confirmed by the statistical
training (days +1, +2 and +14). Pre-training (day _1)
measurements (also shown in post-training panels for analysis. Following training with protocol EX, ◊E increased
comparison), open symbols; post-training measurements, filled less with exercise on days +1 and +2 compared with the
symbols. Values are breath-by-breath data averaged over 20 s other two protocols and with pre-training responses
intervals, averaged across subjects. (reduction in ◊E = 4.1 ± 1.3 l min_1, ANOVA, P < 0.05).
J Physiol 553.3 Learned ventilatory response to exercise 971
Physiology in Press

Following training with protocol EX + CO2, the increase No significant difference was detected between protocols
in PET,CJ with exercise was less on days +1 and +2 in the changes in VT from rest to exercise. ANOVA
compared with the two control protocols and with pre- conducted on the changes in fR from rest to exercise did not
training responses (reduction in PET,CJ = 1.3 ± 0.4 Torr, quite reach significance (P = 0.07).
ANOVA, P < 0.05). By day +14 the responses of both
Sensations of breathlessness
PET,CJ and ◊E to exercise had returned to normal.
The mean ± S.D. ratings of breathlessness for each of the
Examining the pattern of breathing during the closed-loop training protocols were 1.7 ± 1.1 for protocol EX + CO2,
response (see Table 2), there was no significant difference 1.6 ± 1.0 for protocol EX and 1.3 ± 1.1 for protocol CO2.
in changes in fR from rest to exercise between protocols. ANOVA revealed no significant difference between these
However, after training with protocol EX, VT increased less values.
with exercise on days +1 and +2 compared with the other
two protocols and with pre-training responses (P < 0.05). DISCUSSION
The response of VT had returned to normal by day +14.
The major finding of this study is that repeated
Open-loop responses disturbances in chemoreception during exercise in
The open-loop responses to exercise at a regulated PET,CJ humans modify the normal ventilatory response to
before and after training are shown in Fig. 4. Average exercise. In particular, repetitive occurrences of increased
values for PET,CJ, ◊E, fR and VT over the final 3 min of both chemoreceptor activity during exercise result in an
rest and exercise are given in Table 2. Consistent with the augmentation of subsequent ventilatory responses to
expectations from the closed-loop responses, ◊E during exercise, which in a naturally occurring situation would
exercise was higher after training with protocol EX + CO2 act to strengthen the proportionality between ◊E and ◊CJ
(increase in ◊E = 11.5 ± 3.8 l min_1), lower after training and tighten control over Pa,CJ.
with protocol EX (reduction in ◊E = 13.2 ± 4.5 l min_1)
and unchanged after training with protocol CO2. The Critique of the study
increases in ◊E from rest to exercise were significantly In this study, PET,CJ has been used as a proxy measurement
different between day _1 and days +1 and +2 between for Pa,CJ, whereas ideally, direct measurements of Pa,CJ
protocols (ANOVA, P < 0.05). By day +14 the responses would have been made. However, with a study design where
had returned to normal. the same subjects undertook all three protocols, this would
Journal of Physiology

Figure 3
◊E measured in normal closed-loop (air-breathing) conditions Figure 4
during exercise (0–8 min) before training (day _1) and after ◊E measured in open-loop conditions (at regulated PET,CJ) during
training (days +1, +2 and +14). Pre-training (day _1) exercise (0–8 min) before training (day _1) and after training (days
measurements (also shown in post-training panels for +1, +2 and +14). Pre-training (day _1) measurements (also shown
comparison), open symbols; post-training measurements, filled in post-training panels for comparison), open symbols; post-
symbols. Values are breath-by-breath data averaged over 20 s training measurements, filled symbols. Values are breath-by-
intervals, averaged across subjects. breath data averaged over 20 s intervals, averaged across subjects.
972 H. E. Wood, M. Fatemian and P. A. Robbins J Physiol 553.3
Physiology in Press

have required arterial catheterisation of our volunteers on at or a reduction in the respiratory dead space. The latter
least six occasions, which we felt was difficult to justify. would seem less likely given that there were no significant
Although PET,CJ is a fairly good approximation of Pa,CJ, changes in fR. Importantly, from our perspective the
there is nevertheless a difference between the two variables, change that occurred with training was a change in ◊E
and this difference changes with the level of exercise without a change in PET,CJ. Such a change would be
(Robbins et al. 1990). At rest, PET,CJ is lower than Pa,CJ, but entirely in keeping with the notion that the respiratory
during exercise PET,CJ rises to a value above Pa,CJ. Thus, if response to exercise slowly adapts to ensure isocapnia
Pa,CJ remains constant with exercise, PET,CJ will rise. when internal factors change.
Robbins et al. (1990) reported that the difference between
In contrast to protocol EX, protocol CO2 had no effects on
Journal of Physiology

PET,CJ and Pa,CJ changed on average by ~ 4 Torr from rest


the ventilatory response to exercise. Thus, if the null
to 100 W exercise, which would seem to be in keeping with
hypothesis of this study were to hold true (i.e. that there is no
the average difference of ~ 6 Torr from rest to the average
learning associated with repeated episodes of chemoreceptor
work rate of 134 W in the present study.
stimulation paired with exercise), then the effects observed
A second complication of the present study was the finding with protocol EX + CO2 would need to be the same as those
that repeated bouts of exercise without an airway CO2 load for protocol EX. This is clearly not the case. First, following
(protocol EX) resulted in a reduction in the increment in ◊E training with exercise and CO2, the normal response to
with exercise in the days immediately following training. exercise has become hypocapnic, which is not the case after
This was the result of a significant reduction in the training with exercise alone. Secondly, at regulated PET,CJ
increment of VT from rest to exercise after protocol EX. (open-loop responses), the ventilatory response to exercise
Although we did not expect this result, previous studies of following training with exercise and CO2 is increased,
exercise training, somewhat dissimilar from our own, have whereas it is decreased following training with exercise alone.
also reported a reduction in ◊E (Rasmussen et al. 1975;
Comparison with other studies
Casaburi et al. 1987). Theoretically, this could result from
Long-lasting changes in the control of ◊E during exercise,
an increase in efficiency with which the exercise is
which are indicative of mechanisms involving learning,
undertaken, an alteration of metabolic substrate employed
J Physiol 553.3 Learned ventilatory response to exercise 973
Physiology in Press

have been demonstrated in goats following surgery to component to the ventilatory response to exercise in
denervate proprioceptors in the chest wall (Mitchell et al. humans relates to the training protocol employed. In the
1988). After surgery, the normal ventilatory response to present study there were 70 rest-to-exercise transitions in
exercise was abolished, with goats becoming severely the training period, whereas for previous studies the
hypercapnic during exercise. Over repeated trials of maximum number has been 16. In general, it is well-
exercise, a normal response was re-established. This can recognised that the learning of a motor skill often requires
not be explained by functional recovery, and hence the many repetitions over several training sessions, and in
authors concluded that it was due to adaptive control addition a period of consolidation may be required in
mechanisms that had modified components of the order for performance gains to be fully expressed (Karni et
respiratory control system (Mitchell et al. 1990). They al. 1998). There is little reason to suppose that learning the
proposed that this mechanism was elicited by repeated respiratory response to exercise should be any different in
associations of chemoreceptor feedback with exercise, and this respect. Indeed, given the length of time over which
indeed, subsequently demonstrated that repeated exercise learning has occurred, it may well be the case that longer
paired with hypercapnia (induced by increasing the protocols than that employed in the present study would
respiratory dead space) augmented the subsequent have greater effects on the ventilatory response to exercise.
ventilatory response to exercise alone (Martin et al. 1990). Clearly, much more experimental work is required before
the components that are important for learning to occur
In humans, results from similar studies pairing exercise
are properly identified.
with dead space have been far less convincing. It does
appear that pairing exercise with dead space may have A further difference between our study and those
some effect on the time course of the rapid transient in ◊E at conducted previously lies in the nature of the respiratory
the start of exercise (Helbling et al. 1997; Turner & stimulus that was paired with exercise. Previous studies
Sumners, 2002). Furthermore, there are two preliminary (Helbling et al. 1997; Cathcart et al. 2000; Moosavi et al.
reports (Adams et al. 1992; Turner et al. 1996) that 2002; Turner & Sumners, 2002) have all paired added dead
repeated pairings of dead space and exercise alter the space with exercise. However, it has been observed that
subsequent steady-state ventilatory response to exercise. dead space may have mechanical as well as chemical
However, neither of these findings has been substantiated influences on ◊E, such as changes in the endogenous
within the context of a full publication, and furthermore resistance that may affect the proprioceptors (Martin &
the preliminary findings of Adams et al. (1992) have been Mitchell, 1993). We have avoided any possible mechanical
attributed to habituation to the experience of performing influence on ◊E by inducing hypercapnia by adding CO2 to
exercise in the laboratory in a later full publication by the the inspired air. The dead space used by Turner & Sumners
same group of authors (Moosavi et al. 2002). (2002) induced hypercapnia of 6–8 Torr, which is similar
to the level used in the present study. We can assume that
Three full publications and one preliminary publication
the degree of hypercapnia would have been similar in the
have reported no effect of repeated pairings of dead space
study by Helbling et al. (1997) as they used a very similar
and exercise on the subsequent steady-state response to
dead space volume (1.5 l) to Turner & Sumners (2002).
exercise (Helbling et al. 1997; Cathcart et al. 2000; Moosavi
Moosavi et al. (2002) have stated that their protocol may
et al. 2002; Turner & Sumners, 2002). In all but one of
have been adversely affected by using a small dead space
these studies (Helbling et al. 1997), the period of
volume (0.6 l), which induced a lesser degree of
conditioning has been undertaken within a single day. In
hypercapnia.
the study by Helbling et al. (1997), the conditioning
Journal of Physiology

consisted of 16 sessions of arm-cranking exercise paired Congenital central hypoventilation syndrome


with added dead space over an 8 day period. In this study Congenital central hypoventilation syndrome (CCHS) is a
there was a 2 day gap in the middle of the training period, syndrome in which patients have ineffective chemoreceptor
whereas the present study employed a training protocol of regulation of breathing and severe hypoventilation during
7 consecutive days. sleep. If Somjen’s (1992) hypothesis is correct, such patients
might be expected to have a ventilatory response to exercise
Unlike the studies by Helbling et al. (1997) and Turner & that is poorly controlled in relation to metabolism because
Sumners (2002), our primary focus was on the steady-state they may not have the chemoreceptor feedback necessary
response to exercise. As such, a detailed analysis of the with which to learn the calibration of their exercise
transient in ventilation before a steady state was achieved response. Shea et al. (1993) investigated the respiratory
has not been undertaken. For this reason, we are unable to response to exercise in five children with CCHS together
comment on how the transient might have been modified with five controls. On average, they found that there was a
after the training protocols. similar small increase in PET,CJ in both groups. They
We consider that the principal difference between the concluded that chemoreceptors are not necessary for an
present study and previous studies investigating a learned appropriate ventilatory response to aerobic exercise.
974 H. E. Wood, M. Fatemian and P. A. Robbins J Physiol 553.3
Physiology in Press

However, we do not consider that their data really support Martin PA & Mitchell GS (1993). Long-term modulation of the
this conclusion. In particular, from the individual data exercise ventilatory response in goats. J Physiol 470, 601–617.
plotted in their figures, we estimate that the variance of the Martin PA, Mitchell GS, Brown KL & Kaarakka P (1990). Paired
exercise and chemoreceptor stimulation alter subsequent
change in PET,CJ from rest to exercise is some five- to sixfold
ventilatory responses to exercise. FASEB J 4, A540.
greater in the children with CCHS than in their normal Mitchell GS, Douse MA & Foley KT (1990). Receptor interactions in
controls. Finally, although it is clear that patients with modulating ventilatory activity. Am J Physiol 259, R911–920.
CCHS lack a reflex stimulation of ◊E by CO2, it is not Mitchell GS, Foley K, McGuirk S, Selby BD, Schaefer SA & Lange KJ
necessarily clear that the defect is in the afferent limb of the (1988). Effects of thoracic dorsal rhizotomy (TDR) on ventilatory
reflex loop and that these patients also lack chemoreception control during mild exercise in goats. FASEB J 2, A1508.
through which the ventilatory response to exercise could be Moosavi SH, Guz A & Adams L (2002). Repeated exercise paired
with ‘imperceptible’ dead space loading does not alter the VE of
calibrated.
subsequent exercise in humans. J Appl Physiol 92, 1159–1168.
Concluding remarks Pandit JJ & Robbins PA (1992). Ventilation and gas exchange during
In conclusion, the results of the present study demonstrate sustained exercise at normal and raised CO2 in man. Respir Physiol
88, 101–112.
that humans, like goats, can learn the appropriate
Rasmussen B, Klausen K, Clausen JP & Trap-Jensen J (1975).
calibration of the feedforward mechanisms that regulate Pulmonary ventilation, blood gases, and blood pH after training of
breathing in exercise, with the chemoreceptors providing the arms or the legs. J Appl Physiol 38, 250–256.
the sensory feedback that is necessary for such learning to Robbins PA, Conway J, Cunningham DA, Khamnei S & Paterson DJ
occur. Such a mechanism provides a solution to the long- (1990). A comparison of indirect methods for continuous
standing problem of how, during exercise, feedforward estimation of arterial PCO2 in men. J Appl Physiol 68, 1727–1731.
stimuli can provide a respiratory response that is so tightly Robbins PA, Swanson GD & Howson MG (1982). A prediction-
correction scheme for forcing alveolar gases along certain time
coupled to metabolism.
courses. J Appl Physiol 52, 1353–1357.
Shea SA, Andres LP, Shannon DC & Banzett RB (1993). Ventilatory
REFERENCES responses to exercise in humans lacking ventilatory
chemosensitivity. J Physiol 468, 623–640.
Adams L, Moosavi S & Guz A (1992). Ventilatory response to
Somjen GG (1992). The missing error signal-regulation beyond
exercise in man increases by prior conditioning of breathing with
negative feedback. News Physiol Sci 7, 184–185.
added dead space. Am Rev Respir Dis 145, A882.
Turner DL, Greenway JR, Lawrence H, Lyons P, Taylor MR & Iqbal
Borg GAV (1982). Psychophysical bases of perceived exertion. Med
ZM (1996). Long-term modulation of ventilatory control in
Sci Sports Exerc 14, 377–381.
exercising humans (Abstract). Neurosci Abstr 22, 1602.
Casaburi R, Storer TW & Wasserman K (1987). Mediation of
Turner DL & Sumners DP (2002). Associative conditioning of the
reduced ventilatory response to exercise after endurance training. J
exercise ventilatory response in humans. Respir Physiol Neurobiol
Appl Physiol 63, 1533–1538.
132, 159–168.
Cathcart AJ, Herrold N, Turner AP, Wilson J & Ward SA (2000).
Wasserman K, Van Kessel AL & Burton GG (1967). Interaction of
Absence of long-term modulation in response to external dead
physiological mechanisms during exercise. J Appl Physiol 22,
space loading during moderate exercise in humans. J Physiol 44P,
71–85.
528P.
Zuntz N & Geppert J (1886). Ueber die Natur der normalen
Haldane JS & Priestley JG (1905). The regulation of the lung-
Atemreize und den Ort ihrer Wirkung. Pflügers Arch Gesamte
ventilation. J Physiol 32, 225–266.
Physiol Menschen Tiere 38, 337–338.
Helbling D, Boutellier U & Spengler CM (1997). Modulation of the
ventilatory increase at the onset of exercise in humans. Respir
Physiol 109, 219–229. Acknowledgements
We thank Mr David O’Connor for his skilled technical assistance.
Journal of Physiology

Howson MG, Khamnei S, O’Connor DF & Robbins PA (1986). The


properties of a turbine device for measuring respiratory volumes This study was supported by the Wellcome Trust. Helen E. Wood
in man. J Physiol 382, 12P. held a Wellcome Prize Studentship.
Karni A, Meyer G, Rey-Hipolito C, Jezzard P, Adams MM, Turner R
& Ungerleider LG (1998). The acquisition of skilled motor
performance: fast and slow experience-driven changes in primary
motor cortex. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 95, 861–868.

You might also like