You are on page 1of 13

P1: GAD

Journal of Psychopathology and Behavioral Assessment (JOBA) pp1116-joba-480578 January 28, 2004 10:51 Style file version June 25th, 2002

Journal of Psychopathology and Behavioral Assessment, Vol. 26, No. 2, June 2004 (°
C 2004)

Evaluation of Two New Scales Assessing Driving Anger:


The Driving Anger Expression Inventory and the Driver’s
Angry Thoughts Questionnaire

Jerry L. Deffenbacher,1,3 Gail S. White,1 and Rebekah S. Lynch2

Accepted September 9, 2003

This study assessed the validity of the Driving Anger Expression Inventory (DAX) and Driver’s Angry
Thoughts Questionnaire (DATQ). Scales within the DAX and DATQ appeared to assess separate,
correlated constructs. Aggressive forms of thinking and anger expression correlated positively with
each other, trait driving anger, aggressive and risky behavior, some crash-related conditions, general
trait anger, and general forms of anger expression. Positive, constructive forms of thinking and anger
expression correlated positively with each other, but minimally or negatively with other variables.
Specific aggressive forms of thinking and anger expression formed stronger links with each other than
other forms of expression, supporting the discriminant validity of the DATQ and DAX. Hierarchical
regressions with DATQ and DAX scales entered after other variables showed that they added explained
variance above and beyond other measures, supporting the discriminant and incremental validity of
these scales. It was concluded that the DAX and DATQ have utility for researchers and clinicians
interested in angry drivers.

KEY WORDS: angry drivers; anger expression; anger-related thoughts.

Angry, aggressive drivers have received considerable higher incidence of intermittent explosive disorder and
media attention in the past few years, and public con- other psychopathology (Galovski, Blanchard, & Veazey,
cern about the psychological and health hazard posed by 2002), some of which gets played out on the road, and
these drivers has led to increased governmental and police drivers who physically assault another driver experience
attention. It would appear that the public’s concerns are more crashes and traffic tickets (Hemenway & Solnick,
not misplaced. “Road rage,” incidents involving assault 1993).
or attempted assault (e.g., firing a gun at another driver Highly angry drivers are also at elevated risk
or attempting to run someone off the road) appeared to (Deffenbacher, Deffenbacher, Lynch, & Richards, 2003;
increase approximately 7% a year in the United States Deffenbacher, Filetti, Richards, Lynch, & Oetting, 2003;
during the early 1990s (American Automobile Associa- Deffenbacher, Huff, Lynch, Oetting, & Salvatore, 2000;
tion, 1997). Some public officials have estimated that from Deffenbacher, Lynch, Filetti, Dahlen, & Oetting, 2003).
one third to two thirds of injury accidents involve aggres- High and low anger drivers drive the same number of times
sive driving such as speeding and rapid lane changing in and miles per week, suggesting that they are not exposed to
heavy traffic (Martinez, 1997; Snyder, 1997). Addition- qualitatively different driving environments which might
ally, court- and self-referred aggressive drivers experience account for differences observed. However, more things
on the road provoke high anger driver, they become angry
1 Department of Psychology, Colorado State University, Fort Collins, 2.5–3.0 times more often than low anger drivers, and they
Colorado. become more intensely angry when provoked. High anger
2 Nursing Program, Front Range Community College, Fort Collins,
drivers also tend to express their anger in more dysfunc-
Colorado.
3 To whom correspondence should be addressed at Department of Psy- tional ways. They are more verbally and physically aggres-
chology, Colorado State University, Fort Collins, Colorado 80523- sive and are more likely to use their vehicle as a means of
1876; e-mail: jld6871@colostate.edu. expressing their anger and are less likely to express anger

87
0882-2689/04/0600-0087/0 °
C 2004 Plenum Publishing Corporation
P1: GAD
Journal of Psychopathology and Behavioral Assessment (JOBA) pp1116-joba-480578 January 28, 2004 10:51 Style file version June 25th, 2002

88 Deffenbacher, White, and Lynch

behind the wheel in adaptive and constructive ways. High (b) Personal Physical Aggressive Expression in which the
anger drivers also engage in 3.5–4.0 times more aggres- drivers uses his/her physical being to express anger and
sion and 1.5–2.0 times more risky behavior while driv- intimidate through things such as hostile gestures and get-
ing and, in some studies, report more close calls, moving ting out of the vehicle to fight with an offending driver; (c)
violations, and minor accidents than low anger drivers. Using the Vehicle to Express Anger involving behaviors
In studies involving driving simulations (Deffenbacher, such as flashing lights, slowing down or speeding up to
Deffenbacher, et al., 2003), high anger drivers, compared frustrate or retaliate upon another driver, or purposefully
to low anger drivers, engaged in excessive speeding (e.g., tailgating another driver; and (d) Adaptive/Constructive
40% vs. 10% at 10 mph over the limit and 12% vs. 0% Expression involves the driver’s attempts to focus on
at 20 mph over the limit) in low impedance simulations, positive coping and safe driving through things such as
and in high impedance simulations reported more anger cognitive reframing and relaxing and distracting behav-
and verbal and physical aggression, engaged in more er- iors. The Driver’s Angry Thoughts Questionnaire (DATQ;
ratic driving, experienced shorter times and distances to Deffenbacher, Petrilli, Lynch, Oetting, & Swaim, 2003)
crashes, and had double the number of crashes. Moreover, provides five, factor analytically derived scales assess-
studies of British drivers show that driving anger corre- ing the following cognitive characteristics: (a) Judgmental
lates positively with both aggressive and nonaggressive and Disbelieving Thinking involves thoughts of mild dero-
driving offenses (Lajunen & Parker, 2001; Lajunen, Park, gation, questioning of other driver’s skills, abilities, and
& Stradling, 1998; Underwood, Chapman, Wright, & motivations, statements implying others should not be al-
Crundall, 1999). Other studies also show that high anger lowed to drive, and rhetorical questions indicating that
drivers also tend to be more generally angry, impulsive, they cannot believe how others are driving; (b) Pejora-
and aggressive (Deffenbacher, Deffenbacher, et al., 2003; tive Labeling and Verbally Aggressive Thinking involves
Deffenbacher, Filetti, et al., 2003; Lajunen & Parker, highly negative thoughts about the other driver (e.g., name
2001). In summary, high anger drivers are more easily, fre- calling) and thinking about how the person would like to
quently, and intensely angered on the road, tend to express engage in verbally aggressive behavior (e.g., telling the
that anger in aggressive ways, engage in more aggressive person off); (c) Physically Aggressive Thinking involv-
and risky behavior, and experience greater incidence of ing thoughts of wanting to hurt others and behaviors they
some crash-related conditions such as moving violations, would engage in to do so (e.g., beating the hell out of
close calls, and the like. the offending driver); (d) Revenge and Retaliatory Think-
Recently, angry, aggressive drivers have begun to ing involves thoughts of getting back at others and the
receive clinical attention (Deffenbacher et al., 2000; behaviors needed to exact revenge (e.g., boxing the other
Deffenbacher, Filetti, Lynch, Dahlen, & Oetting, 2002; driver in to show him/her); and (e) Coping Self-instruction
Galovski & Blanchard, 2002). As research and practice addresses instructing one’s self to engage in positive
move to clinical applications, clinicians and treatment re- coping activities such as relaxing and accepting bad
searchers need reliable and valid instruments with which situations.
to assess important characteristics of angry drivers. One To be of value as clinical assessment devices, new
reasonably well validated measure is the Driving Anger scales such as the DAX and DATQ must meet at least two
Scale (DAS; Deffenbacher, Oetting, & Lynch, 1994) basic criteria. First, they must have clinical face validity.
which provides a measure of anger intensity across a num- That is, they must appear to assess important parameters
ber of driving situations. However, other instruments are and processes related to the client and presenting prob-
needed to screen for and assess issues such as how anger lems. Both scales appear to meet this test. They address
is expressed behind the wheel and the cognitive elements the cognitive and expressive dimensions of anger while
of the angry driver’s experience. driving. At least on their face, the DAX and DATQ could
Recently, two new self-report instruments, one as- extend the clinical interview and other assessment strate-
sessing ways of expressing anger and the other assessing gies such as self-monitoring to flesh out the cognitive and
types of angry cognitions when angered behind the wheel, expressive elements of anger behind the wheel. Second,
have been reported. The Driving Anger Expression In- they must correlate in logical and reasonable ways with
ventory (DAX; Deffenbacher, Lynch, Oetting, & Swaim, other clinically important elements of client experience,
2002) provides four, factor analytically derived scales such as intensity of anger, aggression, and risky behavior
measuring: (a) Verbal Aggressive Expression involving while driving. Initial validity work (see Method section
expressing anger through things such cursing at or calling for review) suggests that both scales meet this criterion,
other drivers name, asking negative rhetorical questions but additional work is needed to strengthen confidence
about the person’s driving and character, and the like; in findings. The value of the DAX and DATQ would be
P1: GAD
Journal of Psychopathology and Behavioral Assessment (JOBA) pp1116-joba-480578 January 28, 2004 10:51 Style file version June 25th, 2002

Two New Scales for Driving Anger 89

strengthened further if it could be shown that they provide 1.4% African American, 1.1% Native American, and 4.6%
information and added predictive power above and beyond of mixed or other background. Participants received one
that provided by instruments that are readily available such of four research credits for participation.
as the Driving Anger Scale (DAS; Deffenbacher et al.,
1994) or the Trait Anger Scale (TAS; Spielberger, 1988).
Instruments
If, for example, the DAX and DATQ provide little infor-
mation beyond that provided by the DAS, then they might
Demographic Information
not add much clinical information and would have little
to recommend them as additional outcome measures. If,
On the first page of the packet, participants indicated
on the other hand, the DAX and DATQ add information
their age, gender, and year in school, endorsed all ethnic-
and predictive power beyond that provided by other in-
ity categories that applied (i.e., Native American, African
struments, this would support added value to clinicians
American, Asian American, Hispanic/Latino, White non-
and outcome researchers.
Hispanic, or multiple ethnic background/other), and
This research addressed these issues in three ways.
checked the size of town from which they graduated
First, it provided a partial replication of correlational find-
high school (i.e., under 1,000, 1,000–2,000, 2,000–3,000,
ings from the Deffenbacher, Lynch, et al. (2002) and
3,000–5,000, 5,000–10,000, and over 10,000).
Deffenbacher et al. (2003) studies and included the DAX
and DATQ in the same study. Second, it explored the added
value of the DAX and DATQ in hierarchical regression Driving Anger Scale (DAS)
models in which other variables such as gender and the
DAS were entered first so that the contribution of the DAX The 14-item DAS (Deffenbacher et al., 1994) mea-
or DATQ above beyond these variables could be explored. sures the propensity to anger when driving. Respondents
Evidence of incremental validity would support the value rated on a 5-point scale (1 = not at all, 5 = very much)
and potential utility of these measures. Third, evidence of the intensity of anger experienced when they encounter
discriminant validity was explored. For example, if spe- the situation described (e.g., someone is weaving in and
cific scales of the DAX or DATQ were differentially re- out of traffic or someone runs a red light or stop sign).
lated to each other or other phenomena such as aggression Alpha reliabilities range from .80 to .93 and 10-week
or risky behavior on the road, this would suggest spe- test–retest reliability was .84 (Deffenbacher, 2000).
cific relationships which would aid in the understanding of General and clinical samples have shown that driving
clients and might provide clinically useful relationships to anger as measured by the DAS correlates positively
target in treatment. For example, if revengeful/retaliatory with anger in both the individual’s most provocative
thinking was most strongly related to using the vehicle to situations and in commonly occurring driving events,
express anger and to aggression, then it would suggest with state anger and aggressiveness during visualization
that this cognitive–expressive-behavioral link be targeted of provocative driving situations and high impedance
for individuals assessed high on these variables. Discrim- driving simulations, with the frequency and intensity of
inant validity was explored through tests of differences of anger in day-to-day driving, with aggressive forms of ex-
strengths of correlations and by weightings of variables in pressing anger behind the wheel, and with the frequency
regression models. of aggressive and risky behavior and some crash-related
outcomes such as moving violations (Deffenbacher et al.,
METHOD 2000; Deffenbacher, Deffenbacher, et al., 2003; Deffen-
bacher, Filetti, et al., 2003; Deffenbacher, Lynch, et al.,
Paticipants 2003; Deffenbacher, Lynch, Deffenbacher, & Oetting,
2001). Driving anger also correlates positively with
Participants were 436 (218 male, 218 female) intro- general trait anger, aggressive and less controlled forms
ductory psychology students (M age = 18.75, SD = 2.53) of general anger expression, impulsivity, and trait anxiety
at a Western State University of over 25,000 enrollment. (Deffenbacher et al., 2000; Deffenbacher, Deffenbacher,
Of these, 70.2% were freshmen, 20.0% sophomores, and et al., 2003; Deffenbacher, Lynch, et al., 2003).
9.8% juniors and seniors, and 68.3% graduated from
high schools in towns over 10,000 in population, 16.3% Driving Anger Expression Inventory (DAX)
in towns from 5,000–10,000, and 15.3% in towns un-
der 5,000. Ethnic breakdown was 82.6% non-Hispanic The 49-item DAX is a self-report of how people ex-
White, 4.8% Hispanic/Latino, 1.8% Asian American, press their anger behind the wheel (Deffenbacher et al.,
P1: GAD
Journal of Psychopathology and Behavioral Assessment (JOBA) pp1116-joba-480578 January 28, 2004 10:51 Style file version June 25th, 2002

90 Deffenbacher, White, and Lynch

2001; Deffenbacher, Lynch, et al., 2002). Respondents engaging in verbally aggressive behavior (e.g., What an
rated on a 4-point scale (1 = almost never, 4 = almost ass or I am so pissed.); (c) the 14-item Revenge and Retal-
always) how often they express their anger while driving iatory Thinking (α = .93) assesses the tendency to think
in the manner described in the item. The DAX yields four about getting back at others and the behaviors necessary to
reliable forms of anger expression. The 12-item Verbal do so (e.g., I am not going to let them do that to me or I am
Aggressive Expression Scale (αs = .88–.90) assesses peo- going to teach them a lesson.); (d) the 8-item Physically
ple’s tendency to express their driving anger through ver- Aggressive Thinking assesses thoughts of wanting to hurt
bally aggressive means (e.g., yelling or swearing at an- others and thinking about the behaviors necessary to do so
other driver). The 11-item Personal Physical Aggressive (e.g., I want to run them off the road or I would like to beat
Expression Scale (αs = .80–.84) assesses the person’s the hell out of them.); and (e) the 9-item (α = .83) Cop-
propensity to use his/her physical presence to express ing Self-instruction assesses thoughts of positive, adaptive
anger and intimidate (e.g., giving the other driver the finger coping (e.g., Cope with it, sometimes you just have to live
or getting out of the car to engage in a fist fight). The 11- with bad drivers or Just pay attention to my driving, oth-
item Use of the Vehicle to Express Anger Scale (αs = .86– ers can be crazy if they want.). Judgmental/disbelieving
.89) measures the tendency to use the vehicle in aggressive thinking correlated positively with other forms of think-
expression of anger (e.g., speeding up to frustrate another ing, whereas pejorative labeling/verbally aggressive, re-
driver or driving up close to another driver’s bumper). The vengeful/retaliatory, and physically aggressive thinking
fourth scale, the 15-item Adaptive/Constructive Expres- correlated highly and positively with each other and neg-
sion Scale (αs = .89–.90), assesses the person’s tendency atively with coping self-instructions. Forms of angry cog-
toward positive coping in the face of anger on the road nitions behind the wheel generally correlated in expected
(e.g., engaging in relaxation or thinking about things to directions with trait driving anger, anger in specific driv-
distract one from provocation). Verbal, personal physical, ing situations, aggressive forms of driving anger expres-
and vehicular aggressive forms of driving anger expres- sion, aggressive and risky behavior on the road, and some
sion correlate positively with each other (rs = .40–.50), crash-related conditions. Cognitions in the DATQ were
the DAS, and risky and aggressive behavior on the road, better predictors of driving-related variables than a gen-
but are uncorrelated with or form small negative correla- eral measure of hostile cognitions (Deffenbacher et al.,
tions with the adaptive/constructive form of expression. 2003).
Adaptive/constructive expression tends to form smaller
negative correlations with driving-related variables. Ver-
bal, personal physical, and vehicular aggressive forms of Driving Survey
anger expression also tend to correlate positively with
aggressive cognitions. Adaptive/constructive expression The Driving Survey (Deffenbacher et al., 2000;
also correlated positively with coping self-instructions, Deffenbacher, Deffenbacher, et al., 2003) assessed the
but was less correlated with other cognitions behind the three-month frequencies of 13 aggressive (e.g., mak-
wheel. ing an angry gesture at another driver) and 15 risky
(e.g., speeding 20+ mph over the speed limit) behav-
iors and three crash-related conditions (i.e., loss of con-
Driver’s Angry Thoughts Questionnaire (DATQ) centration, minor loss of vehicular control, and close
calls while driving) and last year frequencies of mov-
On the 65-item DATQ respondents rated on a 5-point ing violations and minor and major accidents. Partici-
scale (1 = not at all, 5 = all the time) how often they have pants circled a number from 0 to 5+ (with 5+ being
the thought listed or one similar to it when they are angry treated as 5 in analyses) to indicate the frequency of
behind the wheel (Deffenbacher et al., 2003). The DATQ the behavior or condition. Three-month reports of Ag-
yields five scales of driving-related cognitions: (a) the 21- gression (αs = .85–.89) and Risky Behavior (αs = .83–
item Judgmental and Disbelieving Thinking (α = .94) as- .86) form reliable indices, whereas the six crash-related
sesses mild to moderate derogation of other drivers, state- conditions do not (αs = .41–.55). Crash-related condi-
ments or questions indicating that they cannot believe that tions, therefore, were analyzed individually. Compared to
others are driving the way that they are or that they should low anger drivers, high anger drivers report more aggres-
not be allowed to drive (e.g., Who do they think they are sive and risky behavior and more of some crash-related
or They are clueless.); (b) the 13-item Pejorative Label- conditions (Deffenbacher et al., 2000; Deffenbacher,
ing and Verbally Aggressive Thinking (α = .92) involves Deffenbacher, et al., 2003; Deffenbacher, Lynch, et al.,
much more negative labels and judgments and thoughts of 2002).
P1: GAD
Journal of Psychopathology and Behavioral Assessment (JOBA) pp1116-joba-480578 January 28, 2004 10:51 Style file version June 25th, 2002

Two New Scales for Driving Anger 91

Trait Anger Scale (TAS) received two informed consent forms, one of which they
read, signed, and returned, the other of which they kept
The 10-item TAS assessed general feelings of anger for their records. After signing consent forms, they com-
(Spielberger, 1988). Participants rated on a 4-point scale pleted one of three counterbalanced orders of the DAS,
(1 = almost never, 4 = almost always) the degree to which DAX, DATQ, Driving Survey, TAS, and AX. However,
the statement reflects how he/she typically feels or re- the TAS and AX always followed driving measures, be-
acts. TAS α reliabilities range from .81 to .91, and 2-week cause pilot work suggested this reduced confusion about
to 2-month test–retest range reliabilities from .70 to .77 whether general measures referred to driving or not. When
(Jacobs, Latham, & Brown, 1988; Morris, Deffenbacher, students finished surveys, they received a written debrief-
Lynch, & Oetting, 1996). The TAS is a well validated mea- ing statement.
sure correlating positively with measures of anger and ag-
gression (Deffenbacher et al., 1996; Spielberger, 1988,
1999) and anger consequences (Deffenbacher, Oetting, RESULTS
Lynch, & Morris, 1996) and correlates more with anger
variables than with other cognitive, behavioral, emotional, Preliminary analyses showed that demographics,
and personality variables (Deffenbacher, Oetting, Lynch, save gender, were not related to findings.
et al., 1996; Deffenbacher, Oetting, Thwaites, et al., 1996,
Spielberger, 1999). Gender Differences

Anger Expression Inventory (AX) Gender differences (Table I) on the DAS were as-
sessed by a one-way ANOVA, whereas the DAX, DATQ,
The 24-item AX was included as a general mea- Driving Survey, and general anger measures (TAS and
sure of anger expression (Spielberger, 1988). The 24-item AX) were analyzed by one-way MANOVAs employing
AX asked respondents to rate how they generally express the Wilks’s λ statistic. Effect sizes are in terms of η2 and are
themselves when angry or furious. They rated on a 4-point interpreted according to Cohen’s criteria (Cohen, 1988)
scale (1 = almost never, 4 = almost always) how descrip- where an effect size of .01 to .04 is considered small, .05
tive the item was of the person’s way of expressing anger to .14 moderate, and greater than .14 as large. The DAS did
generally. The AX is composed of three 8-item scales: not show gender effects (Table I), but the DAX revealed a
(a) Anger-In assessing the tendency to suppress anger moderate multivariate gender effect, F(4, 427) = 10.68,
and harbor grudges; (b) Anger-Out assessing the ten- p < .001, η2 = 0.09. Men and women did not differ on
dency to express anger outwardly and negatively; and (c) verbal expression of anger while driving, but men re-
Anger-Control assessing attempts to calm down and con- ported greater physical and vehicular expression and less
trol anger. AX scale α reliabilities range from .73 to .84. adaptive/constructive means of expressing anger (Table I).
Anger-In tends to be relatively uncorrelated with Anger- Significant gender effects were small to moderate. The
Out and Anger-In, whereas Anger-Out and Anger-Control DATQ demonstrated a large multivariate gender effect,
are negatively correlated. Anger-In and Anger-Out cor- F(5, 426) = 18.84, p < .001, η2 = 0.18, due to men re-
relate positively with the TAS, whereas Anger-Control porting more revengeful/retaliatory and physically aggres-
correlates negatively with the TAS. Validity for the AX sive thinking than women (Table I). Effect sizes were
scales is reflected in different patterns of correlations with moderate and large, respectively. Men and women did not
measures of anger, anger consequences, personality, and differ on other anger-related patterns of thinking. Aggres-
physiological reactions (Deffenbacher, Oetting, Thwaites, sive and Risky Behavior from the Driving Survey revealed
et al., 1996; Spielberger, 1988). a small multivariate gender effect, F(2, 429) = 5.83, p <
.01, η2 = 0.03, with men reporting more aggressive and
Procedure risky behavior. Effect sizes for these two variables were
small. The six crash-related conditions demonstrated a
Students read about the study on the departmental small multivariate gender effect, F(6, 425) = 2.30, p <
website which described research opportunities and which .05, η2 = 0.03. Men and women differed only on mov-
described this research as a one-credit study involving ing violations, due to men reporting more tickets. General
emotion, especially anger, and driving behavior. Inter- anger measures also revealed a small multivariate gender
ested students signed up for a day and time fitting their effect, F(4, 427) = 3.71, p < .01, η2 = 0.03, due to men
schedules. The study was conducted in university class- reporting more general trait anger and Anger-Out than
rooms holding 100–200 students. Upon arriving, students women. Effects sizes for both were small.
P1: GAD
Journal of Psychopathology and Behavioral Assessment (JOBA) pp1116-joba-480578 January 28, 2004 10:51 Style file version June 25th, 2002

92 Deffenbacher, White, and Lynch

Table I. Gender Differences on the DAS, DAX, DATQ, Driving Survey, TAS, and AX

Men Women
Measures M SD M SD Univariate gender, F(1, 431) Gender effect, η2

DAS 46.94 9.23 45.80 9.45 1.60 0.00


DAX-verbal 27.12 7.73 26.31 8.25 1.10 0.00
DAX-personal physical 14.83 3.79 13.40 2.26 22.94∗∗∗ 0.05
DAX-use of vehicle 20.43 6.49 18.08 5.58 16.34∗∗∗ 0.04
DAX-adaptive/constructive 31.94 7.93 35.36 8.91 17.80∗∗∗ 0.04
DATQ-JD 54.00 17.85 55.11 20.56 0.35 0.00
DATQ-PLVA 41.15 12.46 38.84 12.87 3.58 0.01
DATQ-RR 29.55 13.64 22.49 9.50 38.90∗∗∗ 0.08
DATQ-PA 15.08 8.46 9.47 3.54 80.72∗∗∗ 0.16
DATQ-CSI 18.10 5.86 18.94 6.82 1.90 0.00
Aggression (3 months) 12.22 11.40 9.85 9.02 5.73∗ 0.01
Risky behavior (3 months) 24.59 16.28 19.85 12.47 11.56∗∗∗ 0.03
Loss of concentration (3 months) 2.58 1.73 2.66 1.70 0.23 0.00
Minor loss of control (3 months) 1.16 1.25 1.10 1.30 0.27 0.00
Close call (3 months) 1.05 1.28 1.21 1.19 2.00 0.01
Moving tickets (last year) 1.47 1.46 1.05 1.29 9.79∗∗ 0.02
Minor accidents (last year) 0.92 1.00 0.87 0.98 0.30 0.00
Major accidents (last year) 0.31 0.64 0.25 0.53 1.04 0.00
TAS 19.61 5.56 18.20 5.09 7.58∗∗ 0.02
Anger-in 16.81 4.47 16.23 4.43 1.80 0.00
Anger-out 16.90 4.00 15.87 3.75 7.74∗∗ 0.02
Anger-control 23.02 4.85 22.87 5.11 0.09 0.00

Note. DAS = Driving Anger Scale; DAX = Driving Anger Expression; DATQ = Driver’s Angry Thoughts Questionnaire; JD =
Judgmental/Disbelieving thinking; PLVA = Pejorative Label/Verbally Aggressive thinking; RR = Revenge and Retaliatory thinking;
PA = Physically Aggressive thinking; CSI = Coping Self-Instruction; and TAS = Trait Anger Scale.
∗ p < .05. ∗∗ p < .01. ∗∗∗ p < .001.

Correlations Between Measures by Gender or correlated negatively with other forms of driving anger
expression and the DAS. Relationships were similar for
Because some gender differences were found in the men and women, but the correlation between verbal
distributions of variables, potential gender differences in and physical forms of anger expression was stronger
correlations were explored by tests for differences in for women, whereas the correlation between physical
independent correlations (Bruning & Kintz, 1997). Be- and vehicular forms of expression was stronger for
cause of the number of correlations involved here and in men.
other analyses α was set a p < .01 to reduce Type I error. Judgmental/disbelieving thinking correlated posi-
The three aggressive forms of expressing driving tively with other forms of driving anger-related thoughts
anger (verbal, physical, and use of the vehicle) correlated (Table III), and pejorative labeling/verbally aggres-
positively with each other and with the DAS (Table II). sive, revengeful/retaliatory, and physically aggressive
Adaptive/constructive expression was uncorrelated with thoughts correlated positively with each other. Coping

Table II. Correlations Between Trait Driving Anger (DAS) and Forms of Anger Expression (DAX)

Measure DAX-verbal DAX-physical DAX-vehicular DAX-adaptive/constructive

DAS .49(.35) .40(.30) .47(.37) −.25(−.02)


DAX-verbal .57a (.73)b .53(.54) −.21(−.27)
DAX-physical .67a (.45)b −.27(−.14)
DAX-vehicle −.42(−.38)

Note. Correlations for men are outside the parentheses, correlations for women are inside parentheses.
Different subscripts indicate a significant difference between genders in strengths of correlations ( p < .01).
DAS = Driving Anger Scale; and DAX = Driving Anger Expression. r > .18, p < .01.r > .22, p < .001.
P1: GAD
Journal of Psychopathology and Behavioral Assessment (JOBA) pp1116-joba-480578 January 28, 2004 10:51 Style file version June 25th, 2002

Two New Scales for Driving Anger 93

Table III. Correlation Between Trait Driving Anger (DAS) and Angry Thoughts (DATQ)

Measure DATQ-JD DATQ-PLVA DATQ-RR DATQ-PA DATQ-CSI

DAS .32(.36) .52(.43) .46(.35) .40(.19) −.08(.09)


DATQ-JD .62(.70) .43(.48) .34(.25) .35(.47)
DATQ-PLVA .64(.61) .61a (.37)b −.05(.15)
DATQ-RR .75(.63) −.14(.03)
DATQ-PA −.17(.11)

Note. Correlations for men are outside the parentheses, correlations for women are inside parenthe-
ses. Different subscripts indicate a significant difference between genders in strengths of correlations
( p < .01). DAS = Driving Anger Scale; DATQ = Driver’s Angry Thoughts Questionnaire; JD = Judg-
mental/Disbelieving thinking; PLVA = Pejorative Label/Verbally Aggressive thinking; RR = Revenge
and Retaliatory thinking; PA = Physically Aggressive thinking; and CSI = Coping Self-instruction.
r > .18, p < .01. r > .22, p < .001.

self-instructional thoughts correlated only with judgmen- of revengeful/retaliatory and physically aggressive think-
tal/disbelieving cognitions. Trait driving anger (DAS) cor- ing with personal physical aggressive driving anger ex-
related positively with all forms of anger-related thinking, pression were stronger for men than women. Men also
except for coping self-instructions. The pattern of corre- showed a stronger negative correlation between adap-
lations for men and women were similar, but for men the tive/constructive anger expression and physically aggres-
correlation of physically aggressive thinking with pejora- sive thinking than women did.
tive labeling/verbally aggressive thinking was larger than A series of tests for differences in strengths of de-
for women. pendent correlations (Bruning & Kintz, 1997) were con-
The relationships between angry thinking (DATQ) ducted on correlations in Table IV to assess whether cer-
and anger expression (DAX) are reported in Table IV. tain forms of thinking might be more strongly related
Judgmental/disbelieving, pejorative labeling/verbally ag- to specific forms of anger expression. Alpha level was
gressive, revengeful/retaliatory, and physically aggressive set at p < .05. Such differences, if they were found,
thinking correlated positively with verbal, personal phys- would provide evidence of discriminant validity for the
ical, and vehicular forms of anger expression. Pejora- DAX and DATQ. Pejorative labeling/verbally aggres-
tive labeling/verbally aggressive, revengeful/retaliatory, sive and judgmental/disbelieving thinking formed signif-
and physically aggressive thinking correlated nega- icantly larger correlations with verbally aggressive anger
tively with adaptive/constructive anger expression. Cop- expression than with other forms of anger expression,
ing self-instructions correlated negatively with using whereas revengeful/retaliatory formed stronger correla-
the vehicle to express anger and positively with adap- tions with using the vehicle as the means of express-
tive/constructive expression of anger. The general pattern ing anger and coping self-instructions correlated stronger
was the same for both sexes; however, the correlations with adaptive/constructive expression than with other

Table IV. Correlation Between Driving Anger Expression (DAX) and Angry Thoughts (DATQ)

Measure DAX-verbal DAX-physical DAX-vehicle DAX-adapt/cons

DATQ-JD .55(.54) .39(.43) .30(.36) .10(.15)


DATQ-PLVA .75(.77) .55(.59) .55(.53) −.28(−.20)
DATQ-RR .48(.53) .67a (.50)b .81(.80) −.43(−.29)
DATQ-PA .44(.28) .67a (.50)b .56(.42) −.39a (−.09)b
DATQ-CSI −.07(.03) −.10(.06) −.19(−.11) .65(.69)

Note. Correlations for men are outside the parentheses, correlations for women are inside paren-
theses. Different subscripts indicate a significant difference between genders in strengths of cor-
relations ( p < .01). DAX = Driving Anger Expression; Adapt/Cons = Adaptive/Constructive
Expression; DATQ = Driver’s Angry Thoughts Questionnaire; JD = Judgmental/Disbelieving
thinking; PLVA = Pejorative Label/Verbally Aggressive thinking; RR = Revenge and Retalia-
tory thinking; PA = Physically Aggressive thinking; and CSI = Coping self-instruction. r > .18,
p < .01. r > .22, p < .001.
P1: GAD
Journal of Psychopathology and Behavioral Assessment (JOBA) pp1116-joba-480578 January 28, 2004 10:51 Style file version June 25th, 2002

94 Deffenbacher, White, and Lynch

Table V. Correlations of Trait Anger and General Anger Expression With sive anger expression with Anger-Control were larger than
Driving Anger Measures the parallel correlations for women. In summary, general
anger and driving anger measures correlated significantly
Measure TAS Anger-in Anger-out Anger-control
and shared, in many cases, considerable common variance.
TAS .17(.31) .74(.64) −.51(−.43) Table VI presents the correlations of trait driving
Anger-in .02(.13) .02(−.07) anger, driving anger expression, angry cognitions, trait
Anger-out −.49(−.57)
anger, and general anger expression with responses from
DAS .50(.41) .19(.18) .39(.28) −.31(−.11)
DAX-VAE .56(.53) .25(.24) .54(.45) −.33(−.22) the Driving Survey. Adaptive/constructive expression of
DAX-PPE .64(.49) .05(.16) .61(.48) −.46a (−.20)b driving anger, controlled expression of anger generally,
DAX-UVE .64(.57) .06(.12) .52(.43) −.43(−.34) and coping-self instructions correlated negatively with ag-
DAX-AC −.36(−.28) .08(.06) −.31(−.27) .47(.52) gressive behavior on the road. All other variables, except
DATQ-JD .35(.33) .20(.20) .36(.26) −.19(.01)
Anger-In with which there was a zero order correlation,
DATQ-PLVA .63(.55) .17(.27) .57(.48) −.31(−.21)
DATQ-RR .66(.54) −.10(.12) .50(.47) −.40(−.34) correlated positively with aggression. The same general
DATQ-PA .65a (.43)b .10(.06) .52a (.27)b −.40(−.19) pattern of correlations was found for risky behavior, albeit
DATQ-CSI −.13(−.09) .16(.02) −.15(−.16) .22(.34) with smaller correlations generally and a few nonsignifi-
cant relationships. Exploration of correlations of variables
Note. Correlations for men are outside the parentheses, correlations for
with crash-related conditions showed that these variables
women are inside parentheses. Different subscripts indicate a signifi-
cant difference between genders in strengths of correlations ( p < .01). were, in general, not highly correlated with anger vari-
DAX = Driving Anger Expression; DATQ = Driver’s Angry Thoughts ables; many correlations were not significant, and ones
Questionnaire; VAE = Verbally Aggressive Expression; PPE = Per- that were significant tended to be small in size. Overall,
sonal Physical Expression; UVE = Use of Vehicle to Express anger; the pattern of correlations was similar for men and women.
JD = Judgmental/Disbelieving thinking; PLVA = Pejorative Label-
However, the correlation of men’s trait anger with aggres-
ing/Verbally Aggressive thinking; RR = Revenge and Retaliatory think-
ing; PA = Physically Aggressive thinking; and CSI = Coping Self- sion was stronger than that correlation for women.
instruction. r > .18, p < .01. r > .22, p < .001.

Regression Analyses
forms of expression. The pattern for physically aggressive
thinking was less clear. Physically aggressive thinking Correlations showed that forms of driving anger ex-
formed larger correlations with personal physical and ve- pression and angry cognitions were meaningfully corre-
hicular forms of anger expression than verbal or adap- lated with each other and with trait driving anger, aggres-
tive/constructive forms, but the strengths of relationships sion and risky behavior on the road, and some crash-related
with personal physical and vehicular forms of expression conditions. Many of these relationships were moderate
did not differ. to large in size. Moreover, some forms of angry think-
Table V presents the correlations of trait anger and ing were more highly correlated with some forms of ex-
general anger expression with trait driving anger, driving pressing driving anger than others. These findings suggest
anger expression, and angry cognitions. As found in prior that the DAX and DATQ could be useful assessment de-
literature, trait anger correlated positively with Anger- vices with drivers. However, correlations also showed that
In and Anger-Out and negatively with Anger-Control. trait driving anger, general trait anger, and general forms
Anger-In was unrelated to Anger-Out or Anger-Control, of anger expression were correlated with important vari-
but Anger-Out and Anger-Control correlated negatively. ables such as aggressive and risky behavior, suggesting
General anger measures showed many significant cor- that assessment instruments already available might serve
relations with driving anger measures. Trait anger and as well as the DAX and DATQ. The value of the DAX and
Anger-Out were moderately to strongly correlated with DATQ would be strengthened if it could be shown that
trait driving anger, all forms of expressing anger behind they added to the prediction of important driving-related
the wheel, and all angry cognitions, except for coping variables above and beyond that provided by other mea-
self-instructions. Anger-Control showed the reverse pat- sures.
terns of correlations, albeit with somewhat smaller corre- To address these issues, a series of hierarchical re-
lations in most cases. Anger-In showed small or nonsignif- gressions were conducted. Because some gender differ-
icant correlations with driving anger measures. Strengths ences were noted in distributions and in strengths of some
of correlations were similar for men and women, but the correlations, a series of preliminary hierarchical regres-
correlations of men’s physically aggressive thinking with sions were conducted. Gender was entered on Step 1, the
trait anger and Anger-Out and of men’s physically aggres- DAS, TAS, or AX on Step 2, either the DAX or DATQ on
P1: GAD
Journal of Psychopathology and Behavioral Assessment (JOBA) pp1116-joba-480578 January 28, 2004 10:51 Style file version June 25th, 2002

Two New Scales for Driving Anger 95

Table VI. Correlations Between Driving Anger Scale, Driver’s Angry Thoughts Questionnaire, Driving Anger Expression, Trait Anger Scale, Anger
Expression Inventory, Aggression, Risky Behavior, and Crash-Related Outcomes

Risky Loss of Loss of Close Moving Minor Major


Measures Aggression behavior concentration control calls violations accidents accidents

DAS .45(.36) .34(.35) .16(.18) .11(.18) .24(.17) .09(.09) .12(.09) .08(.01)


DAX-verbal .49(.56) .29(.31) .20(.13) .16(.06) .15(.17) .01(.09) .07(−.02) .11(.15)
DAX-physical .64(.48) .42(.31) .08(.12) .11(.06) .07(.16) .02(.11) .07(.03) .23(.12)
DAX-vehicle .64(.61) .54(.48) .16(.18) .10(.19) .15(.21) .02(.13) .02(.05) .13(.04)
DAX-adapt/cons −.40(−.40) −.37(−.21) −.19(−.15) −.09(−.12) −.07(−.06) −.01(−.09) .00(.05) .07(−.02)
DATQ-JD .23(.36) .05(.21) .12(.10) .04(.06) .07(.10) −.10(.04) −.04(−.06) .10(.04)
DATQ-PLVA .58(.55) .35(.35) .21(.23) .15(.14) .20(.22) .02(.12) .07(−.01) .10(.10)
DATQ-RR .64(.63) .53(.42) .18(.26) .13(.17) .16(.19) .01(.09) .06(.03) .06(.06)
DATQ-PA .55(.40) .35(.36) .13(.17) .09(.09) .11(.22) −.04(.08) .04(.09) .01(.04)
DATQ-CSI −.20(−.07) −.21(−.01) −.06(.01) −.02(.01) −.03(.09) −.04(−.03) −.05(.00) .06(.05)
TAS .61a (.31)b .46(.42) .15(.15) .11(.16) .19(.23) .03(.15) .11(.13) .11(.08)
Anger-in .02(.11) .06(.18) .18(.16) .20(.19) .20(.25) .08(.07) .14(.07) .00(−.07)
Anger-out .57(.43) .40(.35) .13(.15) .09(.17) .12(.19) −.01(.11) .14(.07) .10(.07)
Anger-control −.36(−.39) −.22(−.25) −.06(−.24) −.03(−.21) −.10(−.19) .08(−.15) −.03(−.10) −.05(−.02)

Note. Correlations for men are outside the parentheses, correlations for women are inside parentheses. Different subscripts indicate a significant
difference between genders in strengths of correlations ( p < .01). DAS = Driving Anger Scale; DATQ = Driver’s Angry Thoughts Questionnaire;
JD = Judgmental/Disbelieving thinking; PLVA = Pejorative Label/Verbally Aggressive thinking; RR = Revenge and Retaliatory thinking; PA =
Physically Aggressive thinking; CSI = Coping Self-instruction; DAX = Driving Anger Expression; Adapt/Cons = Adaptive/Constructive; and TAS =
Trait Anger Scale. r > .18, p < .01. r > .22, p < .001.

Step 3, and All Gender × Step 3 variable interactions on ther the DAS or the TAS on Step 2 and DAX scales on
Step 4. Because none of the interactions were significant Step 3 (Table VII). Gender did not contribute to regres-
and accounted for at least 1% of the variance, the lower sions on aggression; however, the DAS and TAS contribute
limit of a small effect size (Cohen, 1988), interactions with large amounts of variance in aggression. Three DAX
gender were not considered meaningful. A second set of scales, however, contributed an additional 33.5 and 17.6%
hierarchical regressions were then run. Gender was en- additional variance, respectively. Use of the Vehicle to Ex-
tered on Step 1, the DAS, TAS, or AX on Step 2, and the press Anger contributed large and moderate amounts of
DAX or DATQ scales were allowed to enter stepwise on variance, Personal Physical Expression contributed mod-
Step 3. Stepwise entry on Step 3 provided a clearer sense erate and small amounts, and Adaptive/Constructive Ex-
of which DAX or DATQ scales contributed most to added pression an additional small amount. Risky behavior fol-
prediction of the variable of interest. To be retained in the lowed a somewhat similar pattern. Gender contributed a
regression model, DAX or DATQ scales had to be signifi- small amount of variance, whereas the DAS and TAS con-
cant at p < .01 to control for Type I error and contribute at tributed moderate and large amounts of variance. DAX
least 1% added variance, the lower limit of Cohen’s small scales, however, contributed an additional 17.8 and 9.7%
effect size. The latter criterion was designed to limit sta- of explained variance. Use of the Vehicle to Express Anger
tistically significant, but potentially trivial contributions was the primary significant contributor to both equations.
from being considered as evidence of incremental valid- The added value and incremental validity of the DAX
ity of the DAX or DATQ. Finally, since correlations in would also be enhanced if DAX scales contributed mean-
Table VI showed that crash-related conditions were not ingful variance above and beyond that provided by a mea-
well predicted by any variable, many regression models sure of general anger expression (the AX). To address this
for these variables were highly similar and redundant. To issue, another set of hierarchical regressions were con-
reduce redundancy, contributions to regressions on crash- ducted in which gender was entered on Step 1, AX scales
related conditions will be summarized in the text, whereas on Step 2, and DAX scales stepwise on Step 3. General
Table VII summarizes regression models for aggressive anger expression (AX) contributed large amounts of vari-
and risky behavior. ance to the prediction of aggressive and risky behavior,
To assess whether driving anger expression (DAX but DAX scales contributed additional variance beyond
scales) added to the prediction of aggressive and risky be- the AX. Use of the Vehicle to Express Anger contributed
havior beyond that provided by level of driving anger or additional large amounts of variance to the prediction
general anger, hierarchical regression analyses entered ei- of aggressive (17.2%) and risky behavior (14.0%), and
P1: GAD
Journal of Psychopathology and Behavioral Assessment (JOBA) pp1116-joba-480578 January 28, 2004 10:51 Style file version June 25th, 2002

96 Deffenbacher, White, and Lynch

Table VII. Hierarchical Regressions With Gender on Step 1; DAS, TAS, or AX on Step 2; and DAX or DATQ Stepwise on Step 3

Measure Variables in the equation R2

Equations with DAX on Step 3


Aggressive behavior Gender (β = −.12, 1.3%), DAS (β = .41, 16.3%∗∗∗ ), UVE (β = .56, .511∗∗∗
24.7%∗∗∗ ), PPE (β = .31, 5.8%∗∗∗ ), and AC (β = −.19, 3.0%∗∗∗ )
Risky behavior Gender (β = −.16, 2.6%∗∗∗ ), DAS (β = .33, 11.0%∗∗∗ ), and UVE (β =.46, 16.8%∗∗∗ ) .304∗∗∗
Aggressive behavior Gender (β = −.12, 1.3%), TAS (β =.58, 32.5%∗∗∗ ), UVE (β =.44, .515∗∗∗
11.8%∗∗∗ ), PPE (β = .26, 3.6%∗∗∗ ), and AC (β = −.17, 2.2%∗∗∗ )
Risky behavior Gender (β = −.16, 2.6∗∗∗ ), TAS (β =.44, 18.8%∗∗∗ ), and UVE (β =.40, 9.7%∗∗∗ ) .311∗∗∗
Aggressive behavior Gender (β = −.12, 1.3%), AX (27.0%∗∗∗ ), UVE (β = .49, 17.2%∗∗∗ ) .509∗∗∗
PPE (β =.26, 3.6%∗∗∗ ), and AC (β = −.17, 1.8%∗∗ )
Risky behavior Gender (β = −.16, 2.6%∗∗∗ ), AX (14.8%∗∗∗ ), and UVE (β = .44, 14.0%∗∗∗ ) .312∗∗∗
Equations with DATQ on Step 3
Aggressive behavior Gender (β = −.12, 1.3%), DAS (β =.41, 16.3%∗∗∗ ), RR (β =.58, .484∗∗∗
25.9%∗∗∗ ), PLVA (β = .23, 3.0%∗∗∗ ), and JD (β = −.18, 1.8%∗∗ )
Risky behavior Gender (β = −.16, 2.6%∗∗∗ ), DAS (β = .33, 11.0%∗∗∗ ), and RR (β = .44, 14.5%∗∗∗ ) .291∗∗∗
Aggressive behavior Gender (β = −.12, 1.3%), TAS (β =.58, 32.5%∗∗∗ ), RR (β = .47, .533∗∗∗
12.7%∗∗∗ ), and PLVA (β = .17, 2.0%∗∗ )
Risky behavior Gender (β = −.16, 2.6∗∗∗ ), TAS (β = .36, 18.8%∗∗∗ ), and RR (β =.36, 7.7%∗∗∗ ) .470∗∗∗

Note. The first number within the parentheses is the standardized β weight for the variable entered on that step. The second number within the parentheses
is the percent added variance or 1R 2 provided by the variable when entered on that step. Asterisks within parentheses reflect the significance of the F
to enter; asterisks after the R 2 reflect the significance of the total variance accounted for by the equation. DAS = Driving Anger Scale; TAS = Trait
Anger Scale; DAX = Driving Anger Expression Inventory; UVE = Use of the Vehicle to Express anger; PPE = Personal Physical Expression of anger;
AC = Adaptive/Constructive Expression of anger; DATQ = Driver’s Angry Thoughts Questionnaire; RR = Revenge and Retaliatory thinking; PLVA
= Pejorative Labeling/Verbally Aggressive thinking; and JD = Judgmental and Disbelieving thinking.
∗∗ p < .01. ∗∗∗ p < .001.

Personal Physical Expression and Adaptive/Constructive additional variance) showed that they contributed small to
Expression contributed additional small amounts to the moderate amounts of variance to equations for losses of
prediction of aggression (3.6 and 1.8%) for a total of concentration, minor losses of vehicular control, and close
22.6% additional predicted variance of aggression for the calls, and minor accidents, but did not contribute to the
DAX. prediction of moving violations or major accidents. DAX
The validity and utility of the DATQ would be scales also contributed to some of these regressions. Adap-
strengthened further if this cognitive measure added to tive/Constructive Expression contributed an additional 1–
the prediction of driving-related variables above and be- 2% to the prediction of losses of concentration, and Per-
yond that provided by measures of anger such as the DAS sonal Physical Expression was the sole significant predic-
and TAS. As in earlier analyses, hierarchical regressions tor of major accidents (3–3.5% of variance). Pejorative
(Table VII) were conducted with gender entered on Step 1, Labeling/Verbally Aggressive thinking contributed an ad-
the DAS or TAS on Step 2, and DATQ scales stepwise on ditional 1–3% predicted variance for losses of concentra-
Step 3. Gender and anger measures accounted for variance tion and close calls.
as in earlier regression models, but revengeful/retaliatory
thinking accounted for large amounts of additional vari-
ance in aggressive and risky behavior on the road (25.9 and DISCUSSION
12.7% to aggression and 14.5 and 7.7% to risky behavior,
respectively). Pejorative labeling and verbally aggressive Limitations of the Study
thinking also added variance for aggressive behavior (3.0
and 2.0%). This study has two major limitations. First, it em-
Regarding regressions on crash-related conditions, ployed a college sample. The degree of generalizability to
gender contributed significantly only to the regression on older, younger, more diverse drivers, or specialized pop-
moving violations (2.2%), where it was the only signifi- ulations such as court-referred drivers is unknown. How-
cant predictor. Men averaged more moving violations than ever, college students are a meaningful group in their own
women. Regressions involving the DAS and TAS (1–5% right. They represent large group of young adults who, as
additional variance) and the AX scales together (2–8% a group, experience considerable anger while driving and
P1: GAD
Journal of Psychopathology and Behavioral Assessment (JOBA) pp1116-joba-480578 January 28, 2004 10:51 Style file version June 25th, 2002

Two New Scales for Driving Anger 97

for whom auto accidents are a leading source of injury measures were independent enough to suggest that they
and death. Thus, they are a meaningful group upon which assess different, but correlated elements of driving anger
to develop and validate assessment strategies that could expression. Second, aggression and positive coping with
be of value in other populations. Second, the study re- anger are not the ends of a continuum. Although they
lied entirely on self-report measures. Self-report is most were not completely unrelated, the zero order or small
appropriate for phenomena such as angry feelings and degree of correlation between adaptive/constructive and
cognitions, because these are internal conditions, often more aggressive forms of expression suggests that they
best reported by the individual. However, other parameters are somewhat different constructs. Clinically, this suggests
could be bolstered by the use of behavioral observation, that the aggressive and constructive expression should be
reports of others, and archival records (e.g., records of assessed and targeted separately and that it cannot be as-
traffic citations and accidents). However, within the limits sumed that the reduction of aggressive forms of expression
of self-report measures, results provided validation of two will automatically lead to positive means of dealing with
instruments that could enhance other information about anger and frustration on the road. Third, the relatively low
angry drivers. degree of correlation of adaptive/constructive expression
with aggression and risky behavior suggests that safe driv-
ing practices, both generally and when angry, should also
Gender Differences
be assessed and correlated to see if adaptive/constructive
forms of handling anger correlate with safe driving.
This study found a greater number of gender differ-
The relationship among scales on the DATQ also
ences than prior studies. For example, men reported more
replicated those found earlier (Deffenbacher et al., in
personal physical and vehicular anger expression and
press). Judgmental/disbelieving thinking correlated posi-
less adaptive/constructive anger expression than women,
tively with all forms of angry thinking. The three most ag-
whereas in a prior study (Deffenbacher, Lynch, et al.,
gressive forms of angry cognitions (revengeful/retaliatory,
2002) men and women differed only on personal physical
physically aggressive, and pejorative labeling/verbally ag-
expression. Gender differences on revengeful/retaliatory
gressive) correlated positively with each other, but once
and physically aggressive thinking replicated the earlier
again the degree of correlation suggested that they were as-
study (Deffenbacher et al., in press), and gender was also
sessing related, but separable dimensions of angry thought
the sole predictor of moving violations, with men receiv-
processes. Coping self-instructional thinking correlated
ing more tickets than women. Additionally, some gen-
minimally with or formed small negative correlations with
der differences were noted in the strength of correlations,
the three more aggressive patterns of thinking. Much
where such differences were not found in prior studies
like the correlation of adaptive/constructive expression
(Deffenbacher et al., in press; Deffenbacher, Lynch, et al.,
with aggressive forms of anger expression, this pattern
2002). Although there were many nonsignificant correla-
of correlation suggested that positive, adaptive cognitions
tions and occasional inconsistencies, correlations for men
are somewhat orthogonal to more hostile and aggressive
and women generally followed the same pattern, but a
thinking and that changing one will not necessarily lead
small number of correlations between aggressive anger
to changes in the other. Both types of thinking may need
expression, aggressive thinking, and aggression tended to
separate attention in assessment and treatment.
be larger for men than women. However, regression anal-
Correlations of DAX and DATQ scales with other
yses in which interactions of variables with gender were
variables replicated earlier findings (Deffenbacher et al.,
included showed that these differences did not influence
2001; Deffenbacher, Lynch, et al., 2002) and provided
major findings.
evidence of convergent validity. Specifically, aggressive
forms of anger expression (verbal, physical, and vehic-
Correlational Findings: Relationships Among Scales ular) and angry cognitions (pejorative labeling/verbally
for Each Measure and Convergent Validity aggressive, revengeful/retaliatory, and physically aggres-
sive) correlated positively with each other, trait driving
Relationships among scales on the DAX repli- anger, aggressive and risky behavior on the road, gen-
cated those found previously (Deffenbacher et al., 2001, eral trait anger, and aggressive general anger expression
Deffenbacher, Lynch, et al., 2002). Specifically, verbal, (Anger-Out). These measures also tended to correlate neg-
physical, and vehicular forms of anger expression corre- atively with adaptive/constructive driving anger expres-
lated positively with each other and minimally or nega- sion, coping self-instruction, and controlled anger expres-
tively with adaptive/constructive expression. The degree sion generally (Anger-Control), whereas the latter three
and pattern of correlations suggested three things. First, measures tended to correlate positively with each other.
P1: GAD
Journal of Psychopathology and Behavioral Assessment (JOBA) pp1116-joba-480578 January 28, 2004 10:51 Style file version June 25th, 2002

98 Deffenbacher, White, and Lynch

The overall pattern of correlations supported convergent In summary, both the DAX and DATQ hold promise
validity for the DAX and DATQ, as they correlated logi- for researchers and clinicians working with driving anger.
cally and meaningfully with other measures. They assess important elements of an angry driver’s expe-
rience, namely what he/she may think and how he/she may
Added Value: Evidence of Discriminant and express anger. The strength and pattern of correlations sup-
Incremental Validity port their validity in relation to anger, aggression, and risky
behavior on the road and, in some cases, to crash-related
The three most aggressive types of angry cogni- events. Evidence of discriminant and incremental validity
tions (pejorative labeling/verbally aggressive, revenge- further strengthens their value as instruments for assessing
ful/retaliatory, and physically aggressive) correlated angry cognitions and forms of expression of anger while
strongly with driving anger and aggressive anger ex- driving.
pression. This suggests that these types of thoughts are
associated with anger arousal when driving and may chan-
nel behavior toward aggression. However, it was the pat- ACKNOWLEDGMENTS
tern of differential strengths of correlations of forms of
thinking with different types of anger expression that This study was supported, in part, by Grant
suggests an even stronger link. Specifically, pejorative R49/CCR811509 from the Centers for Disease Control
labeling/verbally aggressive thinking was most strongly and Prevention and by 5 P50 DA07074-10 from the Na-
associated with verbally aggressive expression, revenge- tional Institute on Drug Abuse. Its contents are solely the
ful/retaliatory thinking most strongly associated with us- responsibility of the authors and do not necessarily repre-
ing the vehicle to express anger, and revengeful/retaliatory sent the official views of the Centers for Disease Control
and physically aggressive thinking more with physically and Prevention.
aggressive expression. On the more positive side, coping
self-instructional thinking and adaptive/constructive ex-
pression were also most strongly linked. Somewhat sim- REFERENCES
ilar item content on the DATQ and DAX may account
for some of this association, but the patterns of corre- American Automobile Association. (1997). Aggressive driving: Three
lations suggest that specific types of angry thinking are studies. Washington, DC: American Automobile Association Foun-
linked differentially with different forms of anger expres- dation for Traffic Safety.
Bruning, J. L., & Kintz, B. L. (1977). Computational handbook of statis-
sion, which, in turn, may focus and direct angry feelings tics. Glenview, IL: Scott, Foresman and Company.
and aggressive urges toward specific forms of expression. Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences
These patterns of differential strengths of correlations sup- (2nd ed.). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Deffenbacher, J. L. (2000). The Driving Anger Scale (DAS). In J.
port the discriminant validity of the DATQ and DAX and Maltyby, C. A. Lewis, & A. Hill (Eds.), Comissioned reviews of 250
suggest that specific thought-action sequences should be psychological tests (pp. 287–292). Lampeter, UK: Edwin Mellen.
assessed and treated together. Deffenbacher, J. L., Deffenbacher, D. M., Lynch, R. S., & Richards,
T. L. (2003). Anger, aggression, and risky behavior: A comparison
Regression analyses supported the incremental va- of high and low anger drivers. Behaviour Research and Therapy,
lidity of the DAX and DATQ. Forms of anger expression, 41, 701–718.
especially using the vehicle to express anger, personal Deffenbacher, J. L., Filetti, L. B., Lynch, R. S., Dahlen, E. R., & Oetting,
E. R. (2002). Cognitive-behavioral treatment of high anger drivers.
physical aggressive expression, and adaptive/constructive Behaviour Research and Therapy, 40, 717–737.
expression, consistently added explained variance to re- Deffenbacher, J. L., Filetti, L. B., Richards, T. L., Lynch, R. S., & Oetting,
gressions on aggressive and risky behavior, above and be- E. R. (2003). Characteristics of two groups of angry drivers. Journal
of Counseling Psychology, 50, 123–132.
yond that contributed by trait driving anger (DAS), general Deffenbacher, J. L., Huff, M. E., Lynch, R. S., Oetting, E. R., & Salvatore,
trait anger (TAS), and general forms of anger expression N. F. (2000). Characteristics and treatment of high anger drivers.
(AX). Angry thinking also contributed to the prediction Journal of Counseling Psychology, 47, 5–17.
Deffenbacher, J. L., Lynch, R. S., Deffenbacher, D. M., & Oetting, E. R.
of risky and aggressive behavior above and beyond that (2001). Further evidence of reliability and validity for the Driving
provided by the DAS and TAS, with revengeful/retaliatory Anger Expression Inventory. Psychological Reports, 89, 535–540.
thinking and pejorative labeling/verbally aggressive think- Deffenbacher, J. L., Lynch, R. S., Filetti, L. B., Dahlen, E. R., & Oetting,
E. R. (2003). Anger, aggression, risky behavior, and crash-related
ing making the largest contributions. These findings sug- outcomes in three groups of drivers. Behaviour Research and Ther-
gest that the DAX and DATQ added additional, predic- apy, 41, 333–349.
tive information not readily provided by other established Deffenbacher, J. L., Lynch, R. S., Oetting, E. R., & Swaim, R. C. (2002).
The Driving Anger Expression Inventory: A measure of how people
measures, which supports their value as an assessment express their anger on the road. Behaviour Research and Therapy,
instruments for driving anger. 40, 717–737.
P1: GAD
Journal of Psychopathology and Behavioral Assessment (JOBA) pp1116-joba-480578 January 28, 2004 10:51 Style file version June 25th, 2002

Two New Scales for Driving Anger 99

Deffenbacher, J. L., Oetting, E. R., & Lynch, R. S. (1994). Devel- Lajunen, T., & Parker, D. (2001). Are aggressive people aggressive
opment of a driving anger scale. Psychological Reports, 74, 83– drivers? A study of the relationship between self-reported general
91. aggressiveness, driver anger and aggressive driving. Accident Anal-
Deffenbacher, J. L., Oetting, E. R., Lynch, R. S., & Morris, C. D. (1996). ysis and Prevention, 33, 243–255.
The expression of anger and its consequences. Behaviour Research Lajunen, T., Parker, D., & Stradling, S. G. (1998). Dimensions of driver
and Therapy, 34, 575–590. anger, aggressive and highway code violations and their mediation
Deffenbacher, J. L., Oetting, E. R., Thwaites, G. A., Lynch, R. S., Baker, by safety orientation in UK drivers. Transportation Research, Part
D. A., Stark, P. S., et al., (1996). State-trait anger theory and the F 1,107–121.
utility of the Trait Anger Scale. Journal of Counseling Psychology, Martinez, R. (1997, July). Statement of the Honorable Recardo Martinez,
43, 131–148. M. D., of the National Highway Traffic Safety Administration. Sub-
Deffenbacher, J. L., Petrilli, R. T., Lynch, R. S., Oetting, E. R., & Swaim, committee on Surface Transportation, Committee on Transporta-
R. C. (2003). The Driver’s Angry Thoughts Questionnaire: A mea- tion and Infrastructure, U.S. House of Representatives, Washington,
sure of angry cognitions when driving. Cognitive Therapy and Re- D.C.
search, 27, 383–402. Morris, C. D., Deffenbacher, J. L., Lynch, R. S., & Oetting, E. R. (1996,
Galovski, T. E., & Blanchard, E. B. (2002). The effectiveness of a August). Anger expression and its consequences. Paper presented
brief psychological intervention on court-referred and self-referred at the 104th Annual Convention of the American Psychological
aggressive drivers. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 40, 1385– Association, Toronto, Ontario, Canada.
1402. Snyder, D. S. (1997, July). Statement of David S. Snyder, Assistant
Galovski, T., Blanchard, E. B., & Veazey, C. (2002). Intermittent ex- General Counsel of the American Insurance Association. Sub-
plosive disorder and other psychiatric comorbidity among court- committee on Surface Transportation, Committee on Transporta-
referred and self-referred aggressive drivers. Behaviour Research tion and Infrastructure, U.S. House of Representatives, Washington,
and Therapy, 40, 641–651. D.C.
Hemenway, D., & Solnick, S. (1993). Fuzzy dice, dream cars and inde- Spielberger, C. D. (1988). State-Trait Anger Expression Inventory (1st
cent gestures: Correlates of driving behaviour? Accident Analysis ed.). Odessa, FL:Psychological Assessment Resources.
and Prevention, 25, 161–170. Spielberger, C. D. (1999). State-Trait Anger Expression Inventory (2nd
Jacobs, G. A., Latham, L. E., & Brown, M. S. (1988). Test–retest reli- ed.). Odessa, FL:Psychological Assessment Resources.
ability of the State-Trait Personality Inventory and the Anger Ex- Underwood, G., Chapman, P., Wright, S., & Crundall, D. (1999). Anger
pression Scale. Anxiety Research, 1, 363–365. while driving. Transportation Research, Part F 2, 55–68.

You might also like