You are on page 1of 9

See discussions, stats, and author profiles for this publication at: https://www.researchgate.

net/publication/15442341

What happens during vocal warm-up?

Article in Journal of Voice · April 1995


DOI: 10.1016/S0892-1997(05)80221-8 · Source: PubMed

CITATIONS READS

88 1,395

3 authors, including:

Johan Sundberg
KTH Royal Institute of Technology
454 PUBLICATIONS 15,966 CITATIONS

SEE PROFILE

All content following this page was uploaded by Johan Sundberg on 08 September 2018.

The user has requested enhancement of the downloaded file.


Journalof Voice
Vol.9, No. 1, pp. 37--44
1995RavenPress, Ltd., New York

What Happens During Vocal Warm-Up?

Ninni Elliot, Johan Sundberg, and *Patricia Gramming


Department of Speech Communication and Music Acoustics, KTH, and *Department of Phoniatrics, Karolinska
Hospital, Stockholm, Sweden

Summary: Most singers prefer to warm up their voices before performing.


Although the subjective effect is often considerable, the underlying physiolog-
ical effects are largely unknown. Because warm-up tends to increase blood
flow in muscles, it seems likely that vocal warm-up might induce decreased
viscosity in the vocal folds. According to the theory of vocal-Told vibration,
such a decrease should lead to a lower phonation threshold pressure. In this
investigation the effect of vocal warm-up on the phonation threshold pressure
was examined in a group of male and female singers. The effect varied con-
siderably between subjects, presumably because the vocal-fold viscosity was
not a dominating factor for the phonation-threshold pressure. Key Words: Sub-
glottal pressure--Warm-up--Phonation threshold pressure--Vocal folds--
Singing.

Phonetograms, or voice-range profiles, are fre- glottal pressure coefficient, the viscous damping of
quently used in phoniatric practice to assess vocal the vocal folds, the surface wave velocity, the pre-
function (1-8). Gramming (8) o b s e r v e d that the phonatory glottal width, and the inverse of the vo-
lower phonetogram contour was significantly ele- cal-fold thickness. In other words, PTP can be ex-
vated in male patients who had nonorganic dyspho- pected to rise if the vocal folds b e c o m e less mobile,
~lia, and returned to normal after recovery. As these if the surface wave velocity or the glottal adduction
differences agreed with differences in the associ- are increased, and if the vocal fold thickness is de-
ated subglottai pressures, Gramming speculated creased.
that they reflect changes in the mobility in the mem- As the vocal-fold thickness decreases with voice
branous portion of the vocal folds. fundamental f r e q u e n c y , the PTP increases with
Because vocal loudness is regulated by means of pitch. This prediction has been repeatedly corrob-
subglottal pressure, the lower contour in a phone- orated by measurements, e.g., by Gramming (16).
togram is obviously related to the lowest subglottal Titze (14) derived a theoretically based function for
pressure capable of bringing the vocal folds to os- this dependence:
cillation, the so-called phonation threshold pressure
(PTP). This pressure has been the object of several Pth = ktBc~o/T (1)
studies (9-15). Based on a theoretical model of the
vocal fold oscillator, Ishizaka and Matsudaira (12) where Pth is the lung pressure at phonation thresh-
predicted that the PTP is proportional to the trans- old, k t is a transglottal pressure coefficient, B is the
mean damping coefficient for mechanical vibration
in the tissue, c is the mucosal wave velocity in the
Accepted January 4, 1994.
Address correspondence and reprint requests to J. Sundberg vocal-fold cover, ~0 is the prephonatory glottal half-
at Department of Speech Communication and Music Acoustics, width, and T is the vocal fold thickness. Applying
KTH, Box 70014, S-10044 Stockholm, Sweden. this relation to results from models, he found that
This investigation was first presented at the 22nd Annual Sym-
posium Care of the Professional Voice, Philadelphia, Pennsylva- the best fit was obtained using the equation with the
nia, June 1993. following constants:

37
38 N. E L L I O T E T A L .

Pth = 0.14 + 0.60 (F0/F0)2 (2) EXPERIMENT

where Pth is measured in kPa and F0 is the mean F 0 Ten subjects participated in the experiment:
for conversational speech, or, 120 Hz for males and seven female and three male, with age range of 22-
190 Hz for females in Titze's applications. 44 and 32-57, respectively. They all had a consid-
Finkelhor and collaborators (I0) studied the de- erable amount of voice training but were all amateur
pendence of the PTP on the vocal-fold viscosity. By singers in the sense that they did not earn their liv-
bathing the vocal folds and adjacent tissues in var- ing from singing and typically practiced singing
ious osmotic solutions, they manipulated this vis- once or twice per week, mostly in choirs. The rea-
cosity in excised canine larynges and found changes son we chose amateurs was that the warm-up ap-
of the PTP that were in accordance with the theo- pears to typically have a greater effect on the voice
retical prediction. Thus, when they reduced the vis- in such subjects compared with professional sing-
cosity, the PTP tended to drop, and vice versa. Ver- ers, who seem to be more or less constantly warmed
dolini-Marston et al. (17) carried out a related ex- up, perhaps because of the frequent use of their
periment on living adult subjects. By changing the professional voice.
relative humidity of the air, by ingestion of decon- Data were collected for all subjects in an anecho-
gestant mucolytics, and by varying the amount of ic chamber, and, in addition, also in a sound-treated
liquid intake, they induced changes of hydration in booth for three of the subjects. In ascending order
their six subjects. They found that the average PTP they sang the pitches C, E, G, and A in each octave
dropped significantly in the " w e t " conditions of throughout their ranges, starting from the lowest
their six subjects, although the effect varied with pitch they could produce. Each pitch was given to
pitch and between subjects. The greatest effect of the subjects from a synthesizer. On each pitch the
decreased viscosity was observed at the highest subjects repeated the syllable [pa] at least six times,
pitch. as softly as possible. They held a plastic tube, ID 4
In neither of these experiments the precise effects mm, connected to a calibrated pressure transducer
of the manipulations on the tissue viscosity were (Glottal Enterprises) in the corner of their mouth,
analyzed. In particular, it is hard to predict to what and the subglottai pressure was captured as the oral
extent the different layers of the vocal folds were pressure during the [p] occlusion. The pressure data
affected. It seems reasonable to assume that the were recorded on a TEAC multichannel FM DAT
bathing of the vocal folds in the first mentioned ex- recorder with the audio signal picked up by a high-
periment caused a more superficial effect than the fidelity microphone at a constant distance of 30 cm
more global manipulations used in the second ex- on a second channel.
periment. It can be assumed that the glottal mucosa To ensure a constant subglottal pressure for all
was affected in both experiments, whereas the ef- syllables sung at each pitch, the subjects were in-
fects on the viscosity of the vocalis muscle is hard structed to sustain a tone of constant loudness in-
to predict. Consequently, it seems unclear whether terleaved with the [p] consonant, avoiding emphasis
the observed changes of the PTP were caused by a on the consonant. The subjects first sang a com-
decrease of the vocalis muscle viscosity or merely plete series of pitches as softly as possible. When-
by a change in the characteristics of the mucosa, or ever needed, the subjects were reminded to pro-
both. duce the softest possible phonation. Then the same
According to Safran et al. (18), the viscosity in a procedure was repeated using a degree of vocal
muscle can be decreased by means of a warm-up. loudness that the subjects perceived as "comfort-
After warm-up, the muscle temperature is raised able."
and hence the viscosity of the muscle tissue is de- After the first recording, the subjects had a warm-
creased. It seems reasonable to assume that vocal up session with a professional singing teacher (co-
warm-up has the same effects on laryngeal muscu- author NE). The warm-up exercises started with a
lature as on other muscles. Hence, a vocal warm-up descending melodic pattern, sung softly on the syl-
should reduce the viscosity of the vocalis muscle. lable [mu:] and commencing in the middle of the
An interesting question then is whether a warm-up subject's pitch range. The procedure continued
induces a lowering of the PTP. The purpose of the with various exercises involving pitch changes, dif-
present investigation was to investigate this ques- ferent vowels, and different degrees of vocal loud-
tion in a pilot study of a group of amateur singers. ness, consistently avoiding extremely loud singing.

Journal of Voice, Vol. 9, No. 1, 1995


WHAT HAPPENS DURING VOCAL WARM-UP? 39

The entire warm-up program took 30 min, approx- periment. The subjects felt the voice timbre to be
imately. Thereafter, the subjects repeated the re- better, that it was easier to sing, particularly at high
cording procedure. pitches, and that the voice appeared as a more obe-
dient instrument. However, one of the subjects
ANALYSIS (HH) was clearly disturbed by the unusual acous-
tics in the anechoic room, particularly after the
The analysis of the pressure data was carried out warm-up. One of the subjects (coauthor JS) made
by means of an oscillograph. Mostly, the pressure the experiment about a week after recovery from
data appeared as a series of peaks of similar ampli- laryngitis. In this case, the warm-up session did not
tudes and with flat tops. This suggested that the bring his voice to what he conceived as typical for a
subjects managed to keep a constant subglottal warmed-up condition, his pitch range being ex-
pressure for each pitch, typically varying within tended a few semitones below his normal lower
10% of the mean. However, some of the soprano limit after the warm-up, thus suggesting an atypical
subjects tended to produce edged peaks, particu- vocal-fold status. He repeated the experiment three
larly at their top pitches, possibly because of a par- times, thus providing comparative data.
ticular articulatory behavior acquired during train- Figure 1 shows the PTP data from his three rep-
ing. All such data were discarded, as they could be etitions of the experiment. Also shown are Titze's
assumed not to reflect subglottal pressure accu- (14) theoretically derived PTP values, computed
rately (19). For this reason, the data from two of the with the values in Eq. 2 and using 120 Hz for the
female subjects had to be eliminated from further mean F0 in conversational speech. Two of the sub-
analysis. The mean over the repeated attempts to ject's curves are rather similar showing a mean ab-
produce a given pitch was accepted as the pressure solute difference from the theoretically predicted
value for that pitch. values of ---0.5 and ---0.8 cm of H20 before warm-up
and ±0.5 and ---0.8 cm after the warm-up. The de-
RESULTS viations from the theoretical curve were greatest in
the lowest and highest parts of the pitch range. For
All subjects reported that the warm-up session the third curve the corresponding mean difference
was successful, so that they felt the voice to be in is 1.6 and 1.2 cm of H20, respectively, mainly be-
better condition during the second part of the ex- cause of great discrepancies at intermediate and at

BEFORE AFTER

;/
I 0.00 / jf I o.o0
JS

B.OO . . ..oo

6.00 g 6.00

,.oo ,.oo

9
N 2.00 ~ 2.00

0.00 0.00
0 I0 20 30 40 0 10 20 30 40
FOre C2 (semitones) FOre C2 (semitones)
FIG. 1. Phonation threshold pressures for male subject JS before and after vocal warm-up (left and fight panels) as function of voice
fundamental frequency F 0 expressed in semitones relative to the indicated pitch reference C2 (65 Hz). Symbols refer to different
experimental sessions; filled diamonds refer to the occasion when the subject was recovering from an acute laryngitis. The heavy curve
represents Titze's theoretically derived prediction of the phonation threshold pressure.

Journal of Voice, Vol. 9, No. 1, 1995


40 N. ELLIOT ET AL.

high fundamental frequencies. This curve, which TABLE 1. Absolute values of the difference between
constantly showed higher values both before and the individual subjects' phonation threshold pressure
(PTP) and Titze's theoretically derived PTP, assuming
after the warm-up, pertains to the subject's post- a mean fimdamentalfi'equency of 120 and 190 Hz for
cold session. By and large, these results suggest the male and female subjects, respectively (the numbers
that the data obtained were reasonably reproducible represent the means overall pitches; the group means
and support the assumption that they reflect vocal- and standard deviations are given below the values for
fold status. the individual subjects)
Figure 2 presents the corresponding PTP values Before After
for all subjects before the warm-up. Again, Titze's Mean SD Mean SD
theoretically predicted curve is shown for compar- Subject (cm H:O) (cm H20) (cm H20 ) (cm H20)
ison, calculated with an assumed mean fundamental Females
frequency for conversational speech of 190 and 120 GM 1.7 1.7 2.2 1.9
Hz for the female and male subjects, respectively. EL 0.6 0.6 0.8 0.5
GB 1.8 1.7 0.8 0.4
The data for the female subjects show a great inter- PG 1.9 1.6 2.0 1.6
individual scatter. One of the subjects (HH) had HH 4.3 2.5 3.5 2.3
PTP values that were mostly at least - 2 cm H20 AMHB 1.2 0.9 1.4 1.1
J1 1.0 0.7 2.0 1.1
above the group average and another subject (EL) Mean 1.9 1.7
showed exceedingly low values, apparently void of SD 2.0 !.5
a pitch dependence. The mean absolute value of the Males
ST 1.5 1.1 1.3 1.1
difference b e t w e e n the individual curves and SH 0.9 0.6 2.2 1.3
Titze's curve are listed in Table 1. Before warm-up, JS 1.6 1.2 1.7 0.9
this difference amounted to 1.9 cm H20, SD 2.0 cm Mean 1.4 1.8
SD 1.1 i.I
H20, for the female subjects and to 1.4 cm H20, SD
1.1 cm H20, for the male subjects. The differences
among the male subjects were smaller, the data ap- To examine the effect of vocal warm-up on the
proximating the theoretical curve rather closely. PTP, the ratio was computed between PTP after
Similar values were observed after the warm-up. and before the warm-up procedure. The data for the
We conclude that our experiment yielded reason- entire group of subjects are shown in the two panels
ably realistic estimates of the PTP of our subjects. of Fig. 3, the left showing data for female and the

PTP FEMALE SUBJECTS, PRAE PTP MALE SUBJECT5, PRAE

I0.(10 10.00

8.00 D 8.00

6
g
=E
6.0o 6.00

~ 4.0[3 4.00

2110 o 2.00

0.00 I I I I 1 ] 0.00 ] I [ l I I I
0 5 ~0 15 2o 25 3o 35 s zo 15 20 25 3o 35 40
Fo re c3 (wmltones) FOre C2 (zemllones)

FIG. 2. Phonation threshold pressures (PTP) before vocal warm-up for the female and male subjects (left and tight panels) as function
of voice fundamental frequency F 0 expressed in semitones relative to the indicated pitch references (C3 i 30 Hz, C2 65 Hz). Symbols refer
to subjects as follows. Left panel: HH open triangles; GB open diamonds; AH filled circles: PG filled triangles; GM filled diamonds; EL
open squares. Right panel: SH filled triangles; ST open squares; JS open diamonds. The open circles with heavy dashed curve in the left
panel represent the group mean and the heavy solid curves in both graphs represent Titze's theoretically derived prediction.

Journal of Voice, Vol. 9, No. 1, 1995


W H A T H A P P E N S D U R I N G VOCAL WARM-UP? 41

2.00 2.00
MALE
1.80

1.60
1.50
1.40
M E A ~
1.20

1.00 1.00
~ MEAN 0.80

~2 0.60
0.50
0.40

0.20

0.0o ! I I I 0.00 I I I
0 10 20 30 40 0 10 20 30 40
PITCH (st re C3) PITCH (st re C2)
FIG. 3. Subglottal pressure ratio after and before vocal warm-up in female and male subjects as function of voice fundamental frequency
expressed in semitones relative to the indicated pitch references (left and right panels, respectively). Symbols refer to subjects. Left
panel: HH open triangles; GB open diamonds; AH filled circles; PG filled triangles; EL filled diamonds; GM open squares. Right panel:
JS open triangles; ST filled triangles; SH open diamonds. The heavy curves represent the group averages.

right for male subjects. The heavy curve represents cal loudness remained at slightly below twice the
the average. The interindividual variability in the PTP throughout the pitch range both before and af-
female group is considerable, the values varying be- ter warm-up. The same applies to the male subjects.
tween 0.5 and 1.5. However, for most subjects the
ratio showed a clear although individual pitch de- DISCUSSION
pendence. For example, for male subject JS, the
ratio tended to increase with pitch, whereas for fe- The reproducibility of our PTP data seemed ac-
male subject GB, it tended to decrease with pitch. ceptable for many reasons. First, when subject JS
In female subject GM, it remained in the vicinity of repeated the experiment, quite similar values were
1.3 throughout most of her pitch range, approaching obtained. Furthermore, his PTP values seemed con-
1.0 for her top pitches. The heavy average curve is sistently higher than normal during his postcold
close to the horizontal unity line for the female sub- condition. Also, for most subjects the shift in PTP
jects and mostly somewhat above this line for the after warm-up showed a systematic pitch depen-
male subjects. dence, and Ps observed for neutral loudness ap-
Summarizing, we found a great variability with proximated a constant ratio to the PTP. All these
respect to the effect of vocal warm-up on the PTP. observations suggest that the random variation in
This pressure increased with pitch in some subjects, our PTP data was reasonably small.
decreased in others, and remained basically unaf- In many subjects the warm-up effect varied with
fected by pitch in yet other subjects. No clear dif- pitch. In only a few subjects we found a PTP effect
ference was found between female and male sub- that remained similar in extent throughout the sub-
jects. ject's entire pitch range. A comparable variability in
Adopting the idea of Titze, the subjects' pres- pitch dependence was found by Verdolini-Marston
sures at the loudness level, which they perceived as et al. (17) in their investigation of PTP shifts in-
"comfortable," were related to the PTP. The re- duced by hydration and dehydration. Only in rare
suiting pressure ratios are shown in Fig. 4. Among cases did the results for intermediate pitches in that
the female subjects one showed exceptionally high investigation show a coherence with those observed
ratios. The reason was that her PTP remained low at low and high pitches.
and constant throughout her range, whereas at her The aim of the present investigation was to test
comfortable voice level she increased her subglottal the hypothesis that vocal warm-up induces a low-
pressure with pitch. Disregarding this subject, the ering of the PTP. We assumed that vocal warm-up
typical pressure ratio used for the comfortable vo- has the same effects on laryngeal muscles as on

Journal of Voice, Vol. 9, No. 1, 1995


42 N. E L L I O T E T A L .

FEMALE MALE
10.00 10.00

9.00 9.00

BEFORE BEFORE
8.00 8.00

7,00 7.00

6.00 6.00

5.00

4.00 .--.
/ .=..
z
~
5.00

4.00

3.00 3.00

2.00 2,00

1.00 1.00

0.00 0.00 I I I

0 10 20 30 40 0 10 20 30 40

10.00 I0,00

9.00 AFTER 9.00 AFTER

8.00 8.00

7.00 7.00

6.00 6.00
._./'~
,,=.
z
5.00

4.00
/ \ .=..
Z
5.00

4,00

3.00 3.00

2.00 2.00

1.00 1.00

0.00 I J ! 0.00 I [ I

0 I0 20 30 40 0 10 20 30 40
PITCH (st re C3) PITCH (st re C2)
FIG. 4. Ratio of the subglottal pressure between softest and "comfortable" vocal loudness as function of voice fundamental frequency
expressed in semitones relative to the indicated pitch references for female and male subjects (left and right panels, respectively) before
and after vocal warm-up (upper and lower panels, respectively). Symbols refer to subjects. Left panels: AH plus signs; GM open
triangles; GB open circles; EL filled circles; PG crosses; HH stars. Right panels: ST open triangles; JS open circles; SH filled circles.
The heavy curves represent the group averages.

other muscles, i.e., a rise of the temperature result- up effect similar to that in other muscles. In other
ing in a lowering of the viscosity. The results words, we see no reason to doubt that the vocalis
showed a great interindividual variability. What muscle possessed a lower viscosity after vocal
may have caused this variability? warm-up in our experiment.
One possibility is that the effects of vocal warm- Our results suggest that the reduced viscosity in
up are not equivalent to that of a warm-up of other the vocalis muscle did not consistently induce a
muscles. However, this does not seem likely. First, lower PTP. The PTP is, however, dependent on a
all subjects reported that they felt in good and im- number of other factors as well, which might be
proved vocal shape after the warm-up session. Sec- influenced by a vocal warm-up. A decrease of the
ond, the vocalis muscle is participating in the vocal- vocal-fold thickness leads to an increase of PTP,
fold vibrations and is active in control of pitch. but it is not obvious why a vocal warm-up should
Hence, it is vibrated and also it alternately con- result in a reduced vocal-fold thickness. Other fac-
tracts and is stretched during singing exercises. It is tors are the surface wave velocity and the prepho-
hard to doubt that this muscle is subject to a warm- natory glottal width. Also in these respects, an ob-

Journal of Voice, Vol. 9, No. I, 1995


W H A T H A P P E N S D U R I N G VOCAL WARM-UP? 43

vious effect of vocal warm-up is hard to see. Still, vibrations may increase. Also, a vocal warm-up
because there are several factors of relevance to the may affect the nervous control system of phonation
PTP, our results seem to demonstrate that the re- and optimize the vibratory conditions, e.g., the mu-
duced viscosity was not a dominating factor in all tual tuning to subglottal pressure, vocal-fold longi-
our subjects. It may be of relevance that the vibra- tudinal tension, and glottal adduction (E. Knutsson,
tional involvement of the vocalis muscle is minute personal communication). Such effects would be
in the softest possible phonation; rather, the vibra- worthwhile to examine in future research.
tions seem to be limited mainly to the surface layer.
According to Hirano (20), the various layers of the CONCLUSIONS
vocal folds have different mechanical properties.
Our PTP data were in good agreement with and Clear effects of vocal warm-up on the PTP were
thus supported Titze's theoretically derived predic- observed. However, they differed from subject to
tion, even though the PTP seems to typically in- subject. This suggests that the decrease of the vo-
crease slightly toward the lowest part of the pitch cal-fold viscosity, which presumably accompanies a
range. This increase was probably the reason why vocal warm-up, is not a dominating factor for the
Holmberg et al. (11) found that subglottal pressure PTP in all subjects.
in their subjects was higher not only for high pitches
but also for low pitches as compared with the indi- Acknowledgment: Valuable comments on the manu-
vidual subject's neutral speaking pitch. script were provided by Dr. Ron Scherer. The kind as-
sistance of Gunilla Berndtson and Sten Ternstr6m as well
Many subjects experience the effects of vocal as of the subjects of the experiments is gratefully ac-
warm-up as rather physical. The instrument is felt knowledged. The research was supported by a grant from
to function in a better way. In view of this, it is quite the Swedish Research Council for Engineering Sciences.
interesting to note the great interindividual variabil-
ity that seems to exist with respect to the physical REFERENCES
correlates of a warm-up. The variability suggests
that the mechanical properties of the vocal fold dif- I. Luchsinger R. Physiologie der Stimme. Folia Phoniatr
(Basel) 1953;5:58-127.
fer significantly between individuals. Such a vari- 2. Vogelsanger G. Experimentelle Priifung der Stimmeleistung
ability is likely to produce voice timbre differences. beim Singen. Folia Phoniatr (Basel) 1954;6:!93-227.
The tendency of our subjects to use a constant 3. Damst~ H. The phonetogram. Practica Oto-Rhino-Latyngol
1970;32:185-7.
factor for the PTP when singing at a comfortable 4. Komiyama S, Watanabe H, Ryu S. Phonetographic relation-
vocal loudness strongly supports the idea of Titze ship between pitch and intensity of the human voice. Folia
Phoniatr (Basel) 1984;36:1-7.
(14) that subglottal pressure should be related to the 5. Klingholz F, Martin F, Jolk A. Die Bestimmung der Regis-
PTP. In future experiments we plan to induce terbrfiche aus dem Stimmfeld. Sprache Stimme Geh6r 1985;
changes of the mechanical properties of the mucosa 9:109-11.
by medication of patients with acute laryngitis. It 6. Schutte H, Seidner W. Recommendation by the Union of
European Phoniatricians: standardizing voice area measure-
would also be interesting to analyze the shifts in the ment--phonetography. Folia Phoniatr (Basel) 1983;35:
PTP caused by starting the voice with a warm-up as 286--8.
compared with a vocally demanding song. It may be 7. Coleman R, Mabis J. Fundamental frequency--sound pres-
sure level profiles of adult male and female voices. J Speech
assumed that in the latter case, remaining muscular Hear Res 1977;20:197-204.
tonus will be created in the vocal folds inflicting on 8. Gramming P, The phonetogram, an experimental and clinical
the vocal-fold viscosity and, thus, perhaps prevent- study [Dissertation]. Lurid, Sweden: Lund University, 1988.
9. Baer T. Investigation of phonation using excised larynges
ing the singer's access to pitches that demand a [Dissertation]. Cambridge, Massachusetts: Massachusetts
great vibrating mass. Institute of Technology, 1975.
Because all subjects felt a clear difference in their 10. Finkelhor B, Titze I, Durham P. The effect of viscosity
changes in the vocal folds on the range of oscillation. J Voice
voice function after the warm-up, and because the 1988;1:320-5.
concomitant changes in PTP differed between sub- I !. Holmberg E, Hillman R, Perkell S. Glottal airflow and trans-
glottal air pressure measurements for male and female
jects, the physical essence of the warm-up does not speakers in low, normal, and high pitch. J. Voice 1989;3:
seem to dwell in the effects on the PTP. A warm-up 294-305.
can be assumed to be associated with many other 12. Ishizaka K, Matsudaira M. Fluid mechanical considerations
effects. For example, the amplitudes of the higher o f vocal cord vibration, monograph 8. Santa Barbara, Cali-
fornia: Speech Communication Research Laboratory, 1972.
voice source partials may increase for a given sub- 13. Titze I. Regulation of vocal power and efficiency by subglot-
glottal pressure, or the periodicity of the vocal-fold tal pressure and glottal width. In: Fujimura O, ed. Vocal fold

Journal of Voice, Vol. 9, No. 1, 1995


44 N. ELLIOT ET AL.

physiology: voice production, mechanisms, and fi~nctions. tion threshold pressure with induced conditions of hydra-
New York: Raven Press, 1988: 227-38. tion. J Voice 1990;4:142-51.
14. Titze I. Phonation threshold pressure: a missing link in glot- 18. Safran M, Seaber A, Garrett W. Warm-up and muscular
tal aerodynamics. J Acoust Soc Am 1992;91:2926-35. injury prevention: an update. Sports Med 1989;8:239--49.
15. Titze I. Acoustic interpretation of the voice range profile 19. Holmberg E. Laryngeal airway resistance as a function of
(phonetogram). J Speech Hear Res 1992;35:21-34. phonation type. In: Lubker J, ed. Phonetic experimental
research, institute of linguistics (PERIL US I1). Stockholm:
16. Gramming P. Vocal loudness and frequency capabilities of Stockholm University, 1980:44-57.
the voice. J Voice 1991;5:144-57. 20. Hirano M. Clinical examination of voice. New York:
17. Verdolini-Marston K, Titze I, Druker D. Change in phona- Springer Verlag, 1981.

Journal of Voice, Vol. 9, No. 1, 1995

View publication stats

You might also like