You are on page 1of 8

Current Psychology

https://doi.org/10.1007/s12144-017-9777-3

The effects of decisional and academic procrastination on students’


feelings toward academic procrastination
Meirav Hen 1 & Marina Goroshit 1

# Springer Science+Business Media, LLC, part of Springer Nature 2018

Abstract
Procrastination is often associated with feelings of discomfort. The current research examined the effects of decisional and
academic procrastination on students’ feeling of discomfort related to academic procrastination and the desire to change their
procrastination habits. We assumed that academic procrastination would mediate the relationship between decisional procrasti-
nation and students’ feelings toward academic procrastination. A total of 373 undergraduate social sciences students from
northern Israel participated in the study. The results challenge the traditional perception that procrastination is mostly associated
with feelings of discomfort and may imply that while decisional procrastinators experience discomfort associated with procras-
tination, academic procrastinators want to change their procrastination habits but do not report feelings of discomfort. The
findings also suggest a difference between the two types of procrastination and may contribute to the argument that for some
students, procrastination serves as an immediate emotional relief, followed by negative academic outcomes that enhance the
desire to change this habit.

Keywords Decisional procrastination . Academic procrastination . Feelings toward procrastination

Introduction findings indicate that decisional procrastinators recall fewer


happy memories across a lifetime and fewer details of those
Procrastination is typically defined as a voluntary delay of an memories and that prediction patterns differ between decision-
individual’s intended action toward some task despite foresee- al and general procrastinators (Tibbett and Ferrari 2015).
able negative consequences and a potentially overall worse Despite the wealth of literature on procrastination, only a
outcome (Ferrari 2010; Steel 2007). Some individuals delay few studies have directly asked students about their feelings
action as a maladaptive lifestyle across a variety of settings toward procrastination and about their desire to change this
(Steel and Ferrari 2013). These individuals are usually de- habit. These few studies explored feelings towards academic
scribed as chronic procrastinators, who purposefully delay procrastination only, which is usually perceived as a state-
the start or completion of tasks for irrational reasons and ex- related behavioral type of procrastination that differs from
perience anxiety over the delay (Ferrari 2010). One specific chronic types of procrastination. The findings of these studies
form of chronic procrastination is decisional procrastination suggested that upset feelings towards procrastination were
(Ferrari 1994). This type of procrastination is considered a only weakly associated with academic procrastination, while
cognitive type of procrastination and is believed to play a the desire to change the procrastination habit was strongly
unique role in an individual’s psychology (Tibbett and associated with the procrastinating behavior (Milgram et al.
Ferrari 2015). It is argued to be a strong predisposition to be 1992, 1995; Milgram and Naaman 1996). When these find-
unable to make decisions on time, related to a diffuse identity, ings were discussed it was suggested that for some students,
forgetfulness, cognitive processing failure, self-defeating be- state-related academic procrastination serves as a means to
haviors, and anger suppression (Di Fabio 2006). Recent regulate negative emotions that may accompany an aversive
task or as a strategic effort to improve a bad mood and there-
fore it is not associated with immediate discomfort, however it
* Meirav Hen is identified as a bad habit that needs to be changed
meiravhen2@gmail.com (Brinthaupt and Shin 2001; Sirois and Pychyl 2013).
Direct feelings towards procrastination and the desire to
1
Telhai Academic College, Qiryat Shemona, Israel change this habit have not yet been studied in relation to a
Curr Psychol

chronic type of procrastination, raising the question of wheth- procrastination, on the other hand, was related to failing task
er feeling patterns are similar across situational versus chronic completion, rejecting well-minded others, feeling guilty after
types of procrastination. a positive event, and choosing handicapping situations.
Interpersonal dependency was a significant predictor of deci-
sional procrastination, while self-esteem predicted behavioral
Decisional Procrastination procrastination. In a study that examined both behavioral and
decisional procrastination and self-discrepancies, only deci-
Decisional procrastination is believed to be a chronic cogni- sional procrastination significantly varied as a function of
tive type of procrastination, and it represents a stable individ- self-discrepancy (Orellana-Damacela et al. 2000).
ual difference variable characterized by delays in making de- Milgram and Tenne (2000) offered an explanation of the
cisions, particularly under stressful circumstances (Ferrari difference between decisional procrastination and other forms
1994). One possible explanation for this behavior is self- of behavioral procrastination. They suggested that decision-
esteem and social-esteem protection, with procrastinators be- making is logically and psychologically prior to the action that
ing afraid of revealing possible incompetence to themselves will be initiated once a decision has been made, and it may be
and others, so they make endless excuses (Ferrari et al. 2007). more ideational, deliberative, and consequently more vulner-
This avoidant behavior leads to deficits in many life domains, able to internal and external sources of ideational interference.
which may further reinforce chronic procrastinators’ negative Behavioral procrastination, by contrast, is action inhibited,
beliefs about their own abilities (Díaz-Morales et al. 2008). and it represents a difficulty in meeting one’s commitment to
Ferrari (2000) argues that decisional procrastination is as- behave in a certain manner as a consequence of the decision.
sociated with cognitive failure. The individual has a delay in Furthermore, van Hooft et al. (2005) discussed decisional pro-
processing and assessing information about alternative crastination in terms of a pre-decisional or deliberative phase
choices as a consequence of fragility, non-acquisition of com- in which people deliberate on what behavior to pursue and
plex information, or a memory deficit in relation to complex how to achieve their goals. They also argued that decisional
information. Milgram and Tenne (2000) argue that when faced procrastination appears to operate earlier in the action process
with the need to make a decision, people assess whether they and therefore differs from general and behavioral procrastina-
possess the resources to handle the situation, and if they per- tion, which are more proximal to the behavior. Overall the
ceive their resources as inadequate, they cope with the per- literature suggests that decisional procrastination differs from
ceived anxiety that ensues and avoid the situation by postpon- behavioral types of procrastination and therefor needs to be
ing the task or decision-making. Decisional procrastination further researched as a pre-decisional stage of procrastination
has also been found to be strongly associated with decisional that is potentially different from post-decisional or
worry and regret, discomfort and high anxiety (Fernie et al. implemental / behavioral types of procrastination (Steel
2015; Tibbett and Ferrari 2015). Additionally, decisional pro- 2010).
crastinators are reported to have greater difficulty making im-
portant life choices, and they exhibit negative health conse-
quences and lower life satisfaction (Patalano and LeClair Academic Procrastination
2011).
When compared with general and behavioral types of pro- Academic procrastination is a well-researched type of procras-
crastination, all types have been found to be associated with tination that has often revealed inconsistent findings regarding
similar negative factors, such as low self-esteem, fear of fail- its’ reasons and consequences (Kim and Seo 2015; Klingsieck
ure, sensation seeking, dependency on others, self- 2013). It is usually perceived as a state-related behavioral type
discrepancies and some self-defeating behaviors, though in a of procrastination, defined as the "voluntarily delay of an
different manner (Ferrari 1994; Orellana-Damacela et al. intended course of study-related action, despite expecting to
2000; Steel 2010). For example, decisional procrastination be worse off for the delay^ (Steel and Klingsieck 2016, p. 37).
was found to be strongly associated with neuroticism person- This type of procrastination affects over 70% of college stu-
ality factors, while behavioral procrastination was associated dents and is reportedly associated with unsatisfactory academ-
with conscientiousness (Morris and Fritz 2015). Díaz-Morales ic performance, lower levels of well-being, stress and anxiety.
et al. (2008) found that future time orientation negatively pre- Academic procrastination was found to be strongly associated
dicted avoidant procrastination, while decisional procrastina- with task aversiveness, task delay, self-efficacy, and impul-
tion was positively predicted by low present-hedonistic and siveness, as well as conscientiousness and its facets of self-
future time orientations. Ferrari (1994) found that among spe- control, distractibility, organization, and achievement motiva-
cific self-defeating behaviors, decisional procrastination was tion (Steel 2007). It affects graduate, undergraduate and high
related to failing to complete crucial tasks, inciting anger in school students (Onwuegbuzie and Jiao 2000), traditional and
others, and rejecting good-spirited others. Behavioral non-traditional students (Prohaska et al. 2000), high-achieving
Curr Psychol

and low-achieving college students (Kim and Seo 2015), stu- between procrastination and feelings of discomfort and the
dents in many different cultures (Klassen et al. 2010; Ozer desire to change this habit is unclear. Following the above
et al. 2009), and students in statistics, mathematics, writing, we hypothesized that:
English and communication courses (Fritzsche et al. 2003;
Olubusayo & Otedola, 2010; Onwuegbuzie 2004). 1. Decisional procrastination will be positively associated
Klingsieck (2013) identified four main theoretical perspec- with feelings of discomfort regarding academic procrasti-
tives to understand the reasons for this behavior. First, the nation, but not with the desire to change this habit.
differential psychology perspective that perceives it as a per- 2. Academic procrastination will be positively associated
sonality trait linking procrastination to other traits and trait- with the desire to change this habit, but negatively with
like variables such as consciousness, neuroticism and perfec- feelings of discomfort regarding academic procrastination.
tionism. Second, the motivational and volitional psychology 3. Academic procrastination will negatively mediate the re-
perspective that understands procrastination as a failure in lationship between decisional procrastination and feelings
motivation and/or volition relating it to motivational aspects of discomfort.
such as self-efficacy and goal orientation, or volitional aspects 4. Academic procrastination will positively mediate the re-
such as time management. Third is the clinical psychology lationship between decisional procrastination and the de-
perspective that focuses on the clinically relevant extent of sire to change this habit.
procrastination, linking it to anxiety, depression, stress, and
personality disorders. Finally, there is the situational perspec-
tive, which is recently becoming more popular and is focused
on investigating situational and contextual aspects of procras- Method
tination such as task and teacher characteristics. To date more
and more studies suggest that whatever reasons are revealed, Participants
the underlying theme in all perspectives is that academic pro-
crastination is a quintessential self-regulatory failure (Steel The research participants were 373 social sciences students at
and Klingsieck 2016).Interestingly although studied as a a three-year undergraduate college located in northern Israel
self-regulatory failure, studies indicated that in some cases (85% females). Their ages ranged from 19 to 58 years (M =
upset feelings and negative academic outcomes were only 25.44, SD = 4.19). Thirty-seven percent of the participants
weakly associated with academic procrastination (Kim & were first-year students; 37%, second-year; and 26%, third-
Seo, Milgram et al. 1992, Milgram et al. 1995; Milgram and year. Forty-four percent of the participants were education
Naaman 1996). These findings are somewhat surprising be- majors; 39%, social work; and 17%, psychology.
cause procrastination has mostly been found to be associated
with negative feelings such as guilt, regret, self-blame, anxiety Instruments
and despair (van Eerde 2003; Zeenath and Orcullo 2012). In
an attempt to address this paradox, we found several studies We measured decisional procrastination using the Decisional
which suggest that in some cases, academic procrastination is Procrastination Questionnaire (Mann 1982). This instrument
not necessarily dysfunctional and that the outcome of it may consists of 5 items measured on a 5-point Likert scale, from 1
motivate students to address boring and routine tasks more (never) to 5 (always). An example question is, "Even after I
effectively. In addition, it has been suggested that for some make a decision I delay acting upon it". It has been found to be
students, behavioral types of procrastination serve as a tempo- a valid measure of indecision (Mann et al. 1997) and has
rary relief from stress or as a strategic effort to improve a bad demonstrated acceptable criterion validity, internal reliability
mood (Brinthaupt and Shin 2001; Sirois and Pychyl 2013). and test-retest reliability (Beswick et al. 1988). For the present
These studies and others did find that students would like to study, we calculated a mean score of the five items (α = .85).
change this habit, but do not report feeling of discomfort re- We assessed academic procrastination using the Academic
garding academic procrastination (Milgram et al. 1992, 1995). Procrastination Scale - Student Form (Milgram, N. N., & Amir,
Taken together this literature review suggests that while N. Academic procrastination in pre-, early and late adolescence,
chronic and situational types of procrastination share some unpublished report). In this instrument, students rate the extent
similarities, they differ in nature and in consequences. The to which they procrastinate in three academic areas (7 items
chronic nature of decisional procrastination creates an ongo- each): (1) preparing homework for oral or written presentation
ing discomfort and may harm the hope to change this habit. in class or for keeping up with the class curriculum (e.g., BWhen
The situational nature of academic procrastination is associat- I have to sit down and do my homework, I put it off again and
ed with an immediate emotional relief, followed by negative again^); (2) writing and submitting papers of considerable
consequences, and a desire to change this habit. However length and comprehensiveness on specified topics (e.g.,
when both types are examined together the pattern of relations BWhen I have to sit down and prepare a written composition,
Curr Psychol

I put it off again and again^); and (3) preparing for and taking any stage. No incentive for participation was offered. We
written tests or examinations (e.g., BI find myself doing other received online questionnaires from 373 of 450 students
things (e.g., TV, music, book) when I am supposed to prepare for (83% participation rate). Ethics committee of the
an examination^). Responses are given on a 5-point Likert scale Psychology department of Tel-Hai Academic College,
ranging from 1 (never) to 5 (always). This scale was used with Israel, approved this study.
middle school and high school students (Milgram, N. N., &
Amir, N. Academic procrastination in pre-, early and late ado-
Statistical Analysis
lescence, unpublished report) and college students and their
parents (Milgrim et al. 1998). Recently, it has been used with
There are two predicted variables in our study – the feeling of
LD and non-LD college students (Hen and Goroshit 2014).
discomfort related to academic procrastination and a desire to
Furthermore, it has demonstrated high internal consistency
change the procrastination habit. To test our hypothesized
(α ≥ .90) and good construct validity (Milgram and Toubiana
mediation model, in which these two variables are predicted
1999). For the purpose of the present research, we calculated a
by decisional procrastination via academic procrastination, we
mean score of all 21 items (α = .85).
used ordinary least squares path analysis with the PROCESS
To measure students’ feelings toward procrastination, we
macro developed by Hayes (2013) in SPSS 21.0. A bootstrap
adopted items from Milgrim, Mey-Tal, and Levinson (1998).
procedure that resamples the data multiple times (5000 times
We used two batteries of items, with three items per battery. In
in our study, as per Hayes’ 2013 recommendations) and gives
the first battery, "feeling of discomfort as a result of procrastina-
an estimate of the entire sampling distribution for the indirect
tion", the students were asked to report how frequently they Bfelt^
effect was also performed. To test the null hypothesis regard-
bad when they delayed doing the tasks on each of the three sub-
ing mediation effects, the bias-corrected percentile method
scales of the Academic Procrastination Scale (e.g., BWhen I delay
generated 95% confidence intervals. The significance of the
doing my homework, I feel bad about it^). In the second battery,
indirect effect is the only requirement for mediation (Zhao
"desire to change a habit of procrastination", the respondents
et al. 2010). We can account for a significant indirect effect
indicated how often they Bwished to change their habits^ with
when the bias-corrected confidence intervals do not include
regard to the tasks discussed in the Academic Procrastination
zero.
Scale (e.g., BI would like to change my habits related to procras-
To assess the vulnerability of the regression coefficients to
tination in doing my homework^). The participants rated the items
the possibility of spurious associations, we entered a few con-
in both batteries on a 5-point Likert scale, from 1 (rarely) to 5
trol variables. In line with previous studies on study behaviors,
(very often). These scales revealed our study has good internal
these included gender (1 = female), age (Eggens et al. 2008;
reliability (α = .82 for Discomfort and α = .85 for Desire to
Nonis and Hudson 2010), and year of study (Onji and Kikuchi
change), as does the original study (α = .77 and α = .79, respec-
2011).
tively; See: Milgrim, Mey-Tal, & Levinson, 1998).

Procedure
Results
We designed an online questionnaire using Qualtrics soft-
ware (www.qualtrics.com). We sent a web link to the survey As a preliminary stage of our analysis, we ran Pearson bi-
via e-mail to students in all of our courses during the data variate correlations between the research variables (see
collection period. These included courses in quantitative Table 1). These correlations showed that (1) feeling of dis-
research methods and statistics, theories of personality, comfort related to academic procrastination and desire to
and a research seminar. At the first page of the question- change the procrastination habit are weakly and positively
naire, we briefly described the goal of the study: BThis sur- correlated (r = .18; p < .01), suggesting that these are two
vey aimed to learn about the way you usually make deci- distinct concepts; (2) decisional and academic procrastina-
sions, your study habits, and your feeling toward these tion are moderately and positively associated (r = .54;
habits. There are no right or wrong answers, you are asked p < .001); (3) decisional procrastination is moderately and
to pick the answer that reflects the most what do you think or positively correlated with the desire to change procrastina-
feel regarding each question or statement^. Afterwards, the tion habits (r = .39; p < .001) but not with feelings of dis-
participants were asked to sign a consent form prior to com- comfort about academic procrastination (r = .03; p = .56);
pleting the questionnaire; the form included assurance that and (4) academic procrastination is weakly and negatively
participation in the study was anonymous and that the data correlated with feelings of discomfort related to academic
would be kept confidential and used only for the purposes of procrastination (r = −.25; p < .001) and is moderately and
the current research. It was also explained that they were positively associated with the desire to change procrastina-
allowed to discontinue their participation in the study at tion habits (r = .63; p < .001). In addition, there was a
Curr Psychol

Table 1 Pearson correlation


coefficients between the research 1 2 3 4 5 6 7
variables
1 DP –
2 AP .54*** –
3 Discomfort .03 −.25*** –
4 Change .39*** .63*** .18** –
Controls
5 Gender (1-female) .01 −.07 .14* −.03 –
6 Age −.01 .01 −.03 .01 −.05 –
7 Year of study −.03 .09 −.08 −.05 −.13* .18** –
M 2.31 2.48 3.61 2.92 0.85 25.44 1.85
SD 0.88 0.71 1.00 1.13 0.35 4.19 0.77

DP- decisional procrastination, AP- academic procrastination, Discomfort- feeling of discomfort related to aca-
demic procrastination, Change- desire to change procrastination habit
N = 373
*
p < .05, ** p < .01, *** p < .001

negative and weak correlation between year of study and This mediation model explained approximately 10% of the
decisional procrastination (r = −.09; p = .07). variance in the dependent variable.
In the second stage of our analysis, we performed two
mediation analyses (for each dependent variable) using the
PROCESS macro (Hayes 2013). The results showed that de- Discussion
cisional procrastination positively predicted academic pro-
crastination and explained 29% of its variance (see Table 2 Decisional procrastination is often perceived as a unique
and Fig. 1). In line with our hypothesis, decisional procrasti- chronic type of procrastination that differs in nature and role
nation had a positive indirect effect on the desire to change from situational types of procrastination (Ferrari 1994; Tibbett
procrastination habits (b = 0.42, SE = 0.06; i.e., the higher the and Ferrari 2015). Recent studies have suggested that it is a
level of decisional procrastination, the stronger the desire to pre-decisional stage of procrastination that is potentially dif-
change procrastination habits) and a positive but insignificant ferent from post-decisional or implemental procrastination;
direct effect (b = 0.08, SE = 0.07; i.e.., the higher the level of therefore, it should be studied distinctively and in relation to
decisional procrastination, the stronger the desire to change other types of procrastination and should be addressed in the
procrastination habits). The total effect of decisional procras- proper manner (Steel 2010). The present study examined the
tination on the desire to change this habit was positive and effects of decisional and academic procrastination on feelings
significant, and the model explained approximately 41% of of discomfort related to academic procrastination and the de-
the variance in the dependent variable. sire to change this habit and the mediating role of academic
However, in contrast to our hypothesis, decisional pro- procrastination in the relationships between decisional pro-
crastination had a negative indirect effect on feelings of crastination and negative feelings. It was hypothesized that
discomfort related to academic procrastination (b = −0.22, while decisional procrastination will predict negative feelings
SE = 0.05; i.e., the higher the level of decisional procrasti- and not the desire to change this habit, situational procrastina-
nation, the weaker the feeling of discomfort related to aca- tion will serve as an immediate emotional relief and therefore
demic procrastination). It also had a positive direct effect on will not predict feelings of discomfort in the same manner.
feelings of discomfort (b = 0.26, SE = 0.07, i.e., the higher However when both types of procrastination will be
the level of decisional procrastination, the stronger the feel- associated,
ing of discomfort related to academic procrastination), sug- Our findings indicated that decisional procrastination and
gesting that negative feelings toward procrastination might academic procrastination were moderately and positively as-
arise partially because of indecisiveness. The total effect of sociated; strengthening the notion that these are distinct but
decisional procrastination on feeling of discomfort related related constructs (Morris and Fritz 2015). As hypothesized
to academic procrastination was close to zero and insignif- both types of procrastination were related to the feeling of
icant (b = 0.03, SE = 0.06), implying that this is a case of discomfort related to academic procrastination and to the de-
inconsistent mediation and that academic procrastination sire to change this habit but in different ways. While decision-
acts like a suppressor variable (MacKinnon et al. 2007). al procrastination was positively associated with feelings of
Curr Psychol

Table 2 Direct, indirect and total effects of decisional and academic procrastination on students’ feelings

IV DV Effect (unstandardized)

Direct Indirect Total

Coeff. p Coeff. Bootstrap 95% Coeff. p


(SE) (SE) CI (N = 5000) (SE)

DP AP 0.44 <.001 0.44 <.001


(0.04) (0.04)
Discomfort 0.26 <.001 −0.22 −0.32; −0.14 0.03 .64
(0.07) (0.05) (0.06)
Change 0.08 .20 0.42 0.32; 0.54 0.51 <.001
(0.07) (0.06) (0.07)
AP Discomfort −0.51 <.001
(0.09)
Change 0.96 <.001
(0.08)
Controls
Gender Discomfort 0.12 .48
(1 = female) (0.17)
Change −0.17 .26
(0.15)
Age Discomfort 0.01 .65
(0.01)
Change 0.01 .27
(0.01)
Year of study Discomfort −0.06 .39
(0.07)
Change −0.15 .02
(0.06)

DP- decisional procrastination, AP- academic procrastination, Discomfort- feeling of discomfort related to academic procrastination, Change- desire to
change procrastination habit
N = 373

discomfort related to academic procrastination and was unre- difficulties to decide (Milgram and Tenne 2000), academic
lated to the desire to change this habit, academic procrastina- procrastination seems to be more proximal to the action and
tion was negatively associated with feelings of discomfort maybe more affected by its’ immediate consequences of emo-
related to academic procrastination and positively associated tional relief (van Hooft et al. 2005). The desire to change this
with the desire to change it. These findings may suggest that habit indicated by academic procrastination but not by deci-
the difference in nature between the two types of procrastina- sional procrastination may reflect the hope academic procras-
tion may underline the difference in patterns. While decisional tinators experience in regards to changing this dysfunctional
procrastination is thought to be more vulnerable to internal act in comparison to the hopelessness expressed by the deci-
and external sources of ideational interference and more con- sional procrastinators. It may be suggested that decisional
sistently associated with negative feelings following the procrastinators do not believe they can change their

(a) 2
R =.29
(b) 2
R =.29

AP AP
2 2
R =.10 R =.40

DP .23 Discomfort DP .07 Change

Fig. 1 Mediation models for prediction of feeling of discomfort toward academic procrastination, Discomfort-feeling of discomfort toward
academic procrastination (a) and desire to change procrastination habit academic procrastination, Change-desire to change procrastination
(b) by decisional procrastination via academic procrastination. Notes: habit. Control variables are not presented in the figure to simplify the
Standardized estimates are shown. Coefficients higher than |.20| are graphic representation of relationships between the variables
statistically significant (p < .01). DP-decisional procrastination, AP-
Curr Psychol

decision-making process that precedes the academic procras- and how it can be addressed. Future studies can also exam-
tination action, and therefore do not indicate a desire to change ine the effects of decisional procrastination and other types
the procrastination act (Díaz-Morales et al. 2008). of behavioral procrastination (i.e., workplace procrastina-
Furthermore, when we examined academic procrastination tion) on feelings regarding procrastination and can contrib-
as a mediator in the relationships between decisional procras- ute to the overall understanding of and distinct intervention
tination, feelings of discomfort related to academic procrasti- in other situations and life-domains. Interventions may in-
nation and the desire to change this habit, we noticed a change clude strategies to help people better regulate their procras-
in patterns. Decisional procrastination had a negative indirect tinating behaviors (e.g., cognitive–behavioral strategies) or
effect on the feeling of discomfort related to academic pro- better deal with decision making (e.g., debiasing interven-
crastination and a positive indirect effect on the desire to tions) or increasing decision-making competence (Ceschi,
change this habit. This change in pattern may suggest that Costantini, Phillips & Sartori, 2017; Glick and Orsillo 2015;
when the decision –making processes is mediated by an actual Morewedge et al. 2015).
academic procrastination act, there is an immediate effect of
relief, strategic mood change (Sirois and Pychyl 2013) or pro- Compliance with Ethical Standards
tection of the fragile self through avoidance of the task (Ferrari
Conflict of Interests All authors declare that they have no conflict of
2010) that distance the effects of the negative feelings that
interest in pursuing this publication. This article does not contain any
were associated with decisional procrastination only. In addi- studies with animals performed by any of the authors. Informed consent
tion it may be suggested that the presence of the academic was obtained from all individual participants included in the study. All
procrastination action also increases the awareness to this dys- procedures performed in studies involving human participants were in
accordance with the ethical standards of the institutional and/or national
functional behavior and its’ consequences, way and beyond
research committee and with the 1964 Helsinki Declaration and its later
the hopelessness suggested in the decision-making process amendments or comparable ethical standards.
(Fernie et al. 2015).

Practical Implications, Limitations and Future


Research References

Our study may imply that when planning and conducting an Beswick, G., Rothblum, E. D., & Mann, L. (1988). Psychological ante-
cedents of student procrastination. Australian Psychologist, 23(2),
intervention for academic procrastinators, it is very impor- 207–217.
tant to understand the possible sources of this behavior and Brinthaupt, T. M., & Shin, C. M. (2001). The relationship of academic
how a specific student feels about it. If academic procrasti- cramming to flow experience. College Student Journal, 35(3), 457–
nation is a way to achieve immediate emotional relief, but 471 Retrieved from http://www.freepatentsonline.com/article/
College-Student-Journal/80744659.htm.
the results of this behavior make a student want to change
Ceschi, A., Costantini, A., Phillips, S. D., & Sartori , R.(2017). The career
this behavior, an intervention should include becoming decision-making competence: A new construct for the career realm.
aware of this process and developing other strategies for European Journal of Training and Development, 41(1), 8–27.
emotional relief. In addition, it may suggest that if a student Di Fabio, A. (2006). Decisional procrastination correlates: Personality
suffers from chronic decisional procrastination, he or she traits, self-esteem or perception of cognitive failure? International
Journal for Educational and Vocational Guidance, 6(2), 109–122.
might feel bad about academically procrastinating but not
Díaz-Morales, J. F., Ferrari, J. R., & Cohen, J. R. (2008). Indecision and
be able to make a decision regarding the desire to change avoidant procrastination: The role of morningness – Eveningness
this habit. This student might need initially to seek help for and time perspective in chronic delay lifestyles. The Journal of
his/her chronic dysfunctional decision – making processes Jeneral Psychology, 135(3), 228–240.
and then address their academic actions. Eggens, L., Van der Werf, M. P. C., & Bosker, R. J. (2008). The influence
of personal networks and social support on study attainment of stu-
This was an initial study that examined decisional pro- dents in university education. Higher Education: The International
crastination and academic procrastination in relation to feel- Journal of Higher Education and Educational Planning, 55(5),
ings of discomfort and the desire to change procrastinating 553–573.
behavior. The limitations of this study stem from the self- Fernie, B., McKenzie, A. M., Nikčević, A. V., Caselli, G., & Spada, M.
M. (2015). The contribution of metacognitions and attentional con-
report measures, the cross-sectional research design and the
trol to decisional procrastination. Journal of Rational-Emotive and
specific sample, which included mostly females who stud- Cognitive-Behavior Therapy, 34(1), 1–14.
ied social sciences. Future studies should address these lim- Ferrari, J. R. (1994). Dysfunctional procrastination and its relationship
itations to further understand these findings in other aca- with self-esteem, interpersonal dependency, and self-defeating be-
demic settings. In addition, future research should more haviors. Personality and Individual Differences, 17(5), 673–679.
Ferrari, J. R. (2000). Procrastination and attention: Factor analysis of
specifically seek to understand what underlies students’ ac- attention deficit, boredomness, intelligence, self-esteem, and task
ademic procrastination, how the tendency to delay decision- delay frequencies. Journal of Social Behavior and Personality,
making may affect the behavioral tendency to procrastinate, 15(5), 185–196.
Curr Psychol

Ferrari, J. R. (2010). Still procrastinating? The no regrets guide to getting Olubusayo, A., & Otedola, M. (2010) . Academic procrastination in
it done. Hoboken: Wiley. mathematics: Causes, dangers and implications of counselling for
Ferrari, J. R., Diaz-Morales, J. F., O’Callaghan, J., Diaz, K., & effective learning. International Education Studies, 3(3), 205–210.
Argumedo, D. (2007). Frequent behavioral delay tendencies by Onji, K., & Kikuchi, R. (2011). Procrastination, prompts, and prefer-
adults. International prevalence rates of chronic procrastination. ences: Evidence from daily records of self-directed learning activi-
Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 38(4), 458–464. ties. The Journal of Socio-Economics, 40(6), 929–941.
Fritzsche, B. A., Young, B. R., & Hickson, K. C. (2003). Individual Onwuegbuzie, A. J. (2004). Academic procrastination and statistics anx-
differences in academic procrastination tendency and writing suc- iety. Assessment and Evaluation in Higher Education, 29(1), 3–19.
cess. Personality and Individual Differences, 35(7), 1549–1557. Onwuegbuzie, A. J., & Jiao, Q. G. (2000). I’ll go to the library later: The
Glick, D. M., & Orsillo, S. M. (2015). An investigation of the efficacy of relationship between academic procrastination and library anxiety.
acceptance-based behavioral therapy for academic procrastination. College and Research Libraries, 61(1), 45–54.
Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 144(2), 400–409. Orellana-Damacela, L. E., Tindale, S. R., & Suárez-Balcázar, Y. (2000).
Hayes, A. (2013). Introduction to mediation, moderation, and conditional Decisional and behavioral procrastination: How they relate to self-
process analysis: A regression-based approach. New York: Guilford discrepancies. Journal of Social Behavior and Personality, 15(5),
Press. 225–238 Retrieved from http://www.library.umaine.edu/auth/
Hen, M., & Goroshit, M. (2014). Academic procrastination, emotional EZProxy/test/authej.asp?url=http://search.ebscohost.com/login.
intelligence, academic self-efficacy, and GPA: A comparison be- aspx?direct=true&db=aph&AN=10637241&site=ehost-live.
tween students with and without learning disabilities. Journal of
Ozer, B. U., Demir, A., & Ferrari, J. R. (2009). Exploring academic
Learning Disabilities, 47(2), 116–124.
procrastination among Turkish students: Possible gender differences
Kim, K. R., & Seo, E. H. (2015). The relationship between procrastina-
in prevalence and reasons. The Journal of Social Psychology,
tion and academic performance: A meta-analysis. Personality and
149(2), 241–257.
Individual Differences, 82, 26–33.
Klassen, R. M., Ang, R. P., Chong, W. H., Krawchuk, L. L., Huan, V. S., Patalano, A. L., & LeClair, Z. (2011). The influence of group decision
Wong, I. Y. F., & Yeo, L. S. (2010). Academic procrastination in two making on indecisiveness-related decisional confidence. Judgment
settings: Motivation correlates, behavioral patterns, and negative and Decision making, 6(1), 163–175 Retrieved from http://works.
impact of procrastination in Canada and Singapore. Applied bepress.com/andrea_patalano/1.
Psychology, 59(3), 361–379. Prohaska, V., Morrill, P., Atiles, I., & Perez, A. (2000). Academic pro-
Klingsieck, K. B. (2013). Procrastination. When good things don’t come crastination by nontraditional students. Journal of Social Behavior
to those who wait. European Psychologist, 18(1), 24–34. and Personality, 15(5), 125–134 Retrieved from http://prx.library.
MacKinnon, D. P., Fairchild, A. J., & Fritz, M. S. (2007). Mediation gatech.edu/login?url=http://search.ebscohost.com/login.aspx?
analysis. Annual Review of Psychology, 58(1), 593–614. direct=true&db=psyh&AN=2002-10572-010&site=ehost-live.
Mann, L. (1982). The decisional procrastination scale. Flinders Sirois, F., & Pychyl, T. (2013). Procrastination and the priority of short-
University of South Australia. term mood regulation: Consequences for future self. Social and
Mann, L., Burnett, P., Radford, M., & Ford, S. (1997). The Melbourne Personality Psychology Compass, 7(2), 115–127.
decision making questionnaire: An instrument for measuring pat- Steel, P., & Klingsieck, K. B. (2016). Academic procrastination:
terns for coping with decisional conflict. Journal of Behavioral Psychological antecedents revisited. Australian Psychologist,
Decision Making, 10(1), 1–19. 51(1), 36–46.
Milgram, N. N., Gehrman, T., & Keinan, G. (1992). Procrastination and Steel, P. (2007). The nature of procrastination: A meta-analytic and the-
emotional upset: A typological model. Personality and Individual oretical review of quintessential self-regulatory failure.
Differences, 13(12), 1307–1313. Psychological Bulletin, 133(1), 65–94.
Milgram, N. N., Marshevsky, S., & Sadeh, C. (1995). Correlates of aca- Steel, P. (2010). Arousal, avoidant and decisional procrastinators: Do they
demic procrastination: Discomfort, task aversiveness, and task ca- exist? Personality and Individual Differences, 48(8), 926–934.
pability. The Journal of Psychology, 129(2), 145–155. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2010.02.025.
Milgram, N. N., & Naaman, N. (1996). Typology in procrastination. Steel, P., & Ferrari, J. R. (2013). Sex, education and procrastination: An
Personality and Individual Differences, 20(6), 679–683. epidemiological study of procrastinators’ characteristics from a
Milgram, N. N., & Tenne, R. (2000). Personality correlates of decisional global sample. European Journal of Personality, 27(1), 51–58.
and task avoidant procrastination. European Journal of Personality, Tibbett, T. P., & Ferrari, J. R. (2015). The portrait of the procrastinator:
14(2), 141–156. Risk factors and results of an indecisive personality. Personality and
Milgram, N. N., & Toubiana, Y. (1999). Academic anxiety, academic Individual Differences, 82, 175–184.
procrastination, and parental involvement in students and their par- van Eerde, W. (2003). A meta-analytically derived nomological network
ents. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 69, 345–361. of procrastination. Personality and Individual Differences, 35(6),
Milgrim, N. N., Mey-Tal, G., & Levinson, Y. (1998). Procrastination, 1401–1418.
generalized or specific, in college students and their parents.
van Hooft, E. A. J., Born, M. P., Taris, T. W., Van der Flier, H., & Blonk,
Personality and Individual Differences, 25(2), 297–316.
R. W. B. (2005). Bridging the gap between intentions and behavior:
Morewedge, C. K., Yoon, H., Scopelliti, I., Symborski, C. W., Korris, J.
Implementation intentions, action control, and procrastination.
H., & Kassam, K. S. (2015). Debiasing decisions: Improved deci-
Journal of Vocational Behavior, 66(2), 238–256.
sion making with a single training intervention. Policy Insights from
the Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 2(1), 129–140. Zeenath, S., & Orcullo, D. J. C. (2012). Exploring academic procrastina-
Morris, P. E., & Fritz, C. O. (2015). Conscientiousness and procrastina- tion among undergraduates. International Proceedings of
tion predict academic coursework marks rather than examination Economics Development & Research, 47(9), 42–46.
performance. Learning and Individual Differences, 39, 193–198. Zhao, X., Lynch, J. G., & Chen, Q. (2010). Reconsidering Baron and
Nonis, S. A., & Hudson, G. I. (2010). Performance of college students: Kenny: Myths and truths about mediation analysis. Journal of
Impact of study time and study habits. Journal of Education for Consumer Research, 37(2), 197–206.
Business, 85(4), 229–238.

You might also like