You are on page 1of 8

Journal of Business Research 59 (2006) 46 – 53

The impact of perceived corporate social responsibility


on consumer behavior
Karen L. Becker-Olsena, B. Andrew Cudmoreb, Ronald Paul Hillc,T
a
Lehigh University, United States
b
Florida Institute of Technology, United States
c
University of South Florida St. Petersburg, United States

Received 23 May 2004; accepted 5 January 2005

Abstract

The use of corporate social responsibility (CSR) initiatives to influence consumers and differentiate product offerings has become quite
common. This research builds on the growing body of marketing literature through two investigations that manipulate consumers’
perceptions of fit, motivation, and timing of corporate social initiatives embedded within promotions. We find that low-fit initiatives
negatively impact consumer beliefs, attitudes, and intentions no matter what the firm’s motivation, and that high-fit initiatives that are profit-
motivated have the same impact. Furthermore, consumers consider the timing (proactive versus reactive) of the social initiative as an
informational cue, and only the high-fit, proactive initiatives led to an improvement in consumer beliefs, attitudes, and intentions.
D 2005 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved.

Keywords: Perceived corporate social responsibility; Consumer belief; Marketing

1. Introduction simply the act itself. Through two studies we investigate the
role of perceived fit (e.g., similarity between corporate
Based on the assumption that consumers will reward mission and social initiative), perceived corporate motive
firms for their support of social programs, many organ- (other-centered versus profit-centered), and timing of an
izations have adopted social causes (Levy, 1999). However, announcement (reactive versus proactive) on consumers’
it is unlikely that consumers will blindly accept these social responses to corporate social initiatives.
initiatives as sincere actions and thus may or may not This research makes several contributions. First, in the
reward the firm. Prior research confirms this notion (Barone initial study, we replicate the findings of previous scholar-
et al., 2000; Brown and Dacin, 1997; Creyer and Ross, ship on sponsorship (Simmons and Becker-Olsen, 2004;
1997; Ellen et al., 2000; Sen and Bhattacharya, 2001). In Speed and Thompson, 2000) and social marketing (Sen and
fact, research suggests consumers will punish firms that are Bhattacharya, 2001), which show that low-fit alignments
perceived as insincere in their social involvement (Sen and with intrinsically positive initiatives can lead to negative
Bhattacharya, 2001; Simmons and Becker-Olsen, 2004). assessments of firms by consumers. Second, we build on
Therefore, we propose that potential positive associations previous social marketing literature by explicitly evaluating
stemming from a social initiative depend on the consumer’s additional firm/program conditions that may influence
evaluation of that initiative in relation to the firm, rather than consumers’ responses towards firms and their products,
such as perceived motivation of the firm. Third, in the
second study, we show that high-fit initiatives, which are
T Corresponding author. 4681 Short Leaf Lane, St. Petersburg, FL 33703,
United States. Tel.: +1 727 553 4505; fax: +1 727 525 8432.
perceived as reactive rather than proactive, can also
E-mail addresses: ronaldpaulhill@msn.com, rphill@stpt.usf.edu negatively impact consumer behavior. Finally, in both
(R.P. Hill). studies, we evaluate not only global assessments of the
0148-2963/$ - see front matter D 2005 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved.
doi:10.1016/j.jbusres.2005.01.001
K.L. Becker-Olsen et al. / Journal of Business Research 59 (2006) 46–53 47

firm (e.g., overall attitude), but also specific beliefs such as market position (cf. Keller, 1993; Erdem and Swait,
corporate ability and corporate credibility. 1998a,b; Park et al., 1986). Clear positive market positions
are important because they help consumers understand how
firms fit into the competitive landscape, provide a point of
2. Theoretical foundation differentiation, reduce uncertainty about firms and their
products, and increase purchase intentions (Brown and
There have been numerous studies on corporate social Dacin, 1997).
responsibility (CSR), corporate ethics, and social sponsor- Alternatively, a low-fit initiative is likely to be perceived
ship that suggest a link between social initiatives and as inconsistent with prior expectations and actions, making
improved financial performance (McGuire et al., 1988; Pava it more difficult to integrate new knowledge into existing
and Krause, 1996; Stanwick and Stanwick, 1998), as well as memory structures. Research has shown that consumers
studies that demonstrate the link between social initiatives who elaborate an incongruity have diminished attitudes
and positive affective, cognitive, and behavioral responses toward the firm and its initiatives (Forehand and Grier,
by consumers (Brown and Dacin, 1997; Creyer and Ross, 2003; Menon and Kahn, 2003). Furthermore, the lack of
1997; Ellen et al., 2000; Folkes and Kamins, 1999; Murray congruity is likely to reduce the clarity of the firm’s market
and Vogel, 1997; Sen and Bhattacharya, 2001). Specifically, position and call into question the firm’s motives. For
previous research has looked at the relationship between example, the lack of consistency between prior expectations
social initiatives and price (Creyer and Ross, 1997), and new information has been shown to trigger skepticism
perceived quality (Folkes and Kamins, 1999), corporate and lead to negative attitudes (Boush et al., 1994; Folkes,
attitudes (Brown and Dacin, 1997), and purchase intentions 1999; Ford et al., 1990). Given this discussion, Hypothesis 1
(Murray and Vogel, 1997). The section now turns to a is:
discussion of the key variables of fit and motivation.
H1. Low fit between firms and social initiatives relative to
high fit will result in a greater number of thoughts (H1a),
2.1. Fit
thoughts that are less favorable (H1b), thoughts that are
more focused on firm motive (H1c), more negative attitudes
Fit is defined in a social marketing context as the
toward the firm (H1d), beliefs about the firm as less credible
perceived link between a cause and the firm’s product line,
(H1e), and lower likelihood of purchase intention (H1f).
brand image, position, and/or target market (Varadarajan
and Menon, 1988). Fit is important because it influences: (1)
how much thought people give to a relationship (e.g., 2.2. Motivation
increased elaboration about the firm, the social initiative,
and/or the relationship itself when perceived inconsistencies Consistent with the earlier discussion, perceived corpo-
with prior expectations and information exist; Forehand and rate motivation is likely to influence consumers’ attitudes
Grier, 2003; Meyers-Levy and Tybout, 1994; Meyers-Levy toward firms and their social initiatives. Although the act of
et al., 1994); (2) the specific types of thoughts generated supporting a social initiative may seem to be a public
(e.g., low fit generates negative thoughts and low fit itself is serving action, consumers’ perceptions of the underlying
considered negative) (Forehand and Grier, 2003); and (3) motivations for the act may drive their evaluations of the
evaluations of the two objects (Johar and Pham, 1999; Sen firm and impact beliefs, attitudes, and intentions. Consider
and Bhattacharya, 2001; Speed and Thompson, 2000). Nike, who in conjunction with the London Department of
Findings in much of the sponsorship, branding, and Education, is tackling issues of racism and school bullying
endorsement literatures are consistent with associative through a community-based, after-school sports program.
network theory. Specifically, high levels of perceived Together they have publicly stated that the program will
relatedness enhance consumer attitudes towards firms/ have positive benefits for young people, the community, and
brands because they view the actions of firms as appropriate ultimately the firm; although Nike claims it is not intended
(Aaker, 1990; Keller and Aaker, 1993; John et al., 1998; to sell more sportswear (Mason, 2001). Clearly this program
Mandler, 1982; Simonin and Ruth, 1998; Speed and has a high degree of fit, but another relevant factor is
Thompson, 2000; Till and Busler, 2000). Thus, a good fit consumers’ perceptions of Nike’s motivation. If consumers
between prior expectations, knowledge, associations, question a firm’s motivation, they may elicit more persua-
actions, and competencies of a firm and a given social sion knowledge (Friestad and Wright, 1994, 1995), which
initiative (e.g., Home Depot and Habitat for Humanity) can results in greater cognitive elaboration in the evaluation of
be more easily integrated into the consumer’s existing these motivations.
cognitive structure, strengthening the connection between Attribution theory (Jones and Davis, 1965; Kelley, 1967,
the firm and the social initiative (Fiske and Taylor, 1991; 1972) and the persuasion knowledge model provide a basis
Wojciske et al., 1993). Such high-fit initiatives are a for the argument that consumers will attempt to understand
function of perceived consistency with prior expectations firms’ motives embedded within marketing communica-
and actions between firms and causes, reinforcing the firm’s tions. Thus when presented with evidence of a firm’s social
48 K.L. Becker-Olsen et al. / Journal of Business Research 59 (2006) 46–53

involvement, consumers are likely to elaborate on the a=.96, for liking; and 1=unfamiliar/7=familiar, 1=did not
message and assign one of two primary types of motives recognize/7=recognized, and 1=had not heard of/7=had
to the firm-self serving (e.g., to increase profits, sales or heard of, Cronbach’s a=.96, for familiarity). Further,
boost a specific brand) or public serving (e.g., help needy homelessness and domestic violence were perceived as
citizens, assist with community development or raise equally important (Ms=6.32 and 6.39, F 1,25b1) and equally
awareness for a specific cause). In this research we look relevant (Ms=5.92 and 6.11, F 1,25b1), when measured by
at motivations that are characterized as profit-motivated or one 7-point scale each (1=not important/7=very important
socially motivated. We know that the specific attributions and 1=not relevant/7=relevant).
that underlie perceived motivations are likely to influence A second set of pretests evaluated the stimuli to ensure
evaluations of firms (Boush et al., 1994; Campbell and that firm/initiative pairings were representative of the
Kirmani, 2000; Ellen et al., 2000). Thus, when motivations intended fit and motivation manipulations. Similar to earlier
are considered firm serving or profit-related, attitudes investigations, subjects were exposed to the social initiative
toward firms are likely to diminish; when motivations are information in the form of a USA Today newspaper article.
considered socially motivated, attitudes toward firms are Fit between the firm and social initiative was measured
likely to be enhanced. using four scaled items (1=low fit/7=strong fit, 1=dissimilar/
Furthermore, increased elaboration improves the like- 7=similar, 1=inconsistent/7=consistent, and 1=not comple-
lihood that persuasion knowledge will be used as a coping mentary/7=complementary, Cronbach’s a=.94). Fit varied as
strategy to update or bcorrectQ initially favorable beliefs expected (Ms high fit=5.42 and Ms low fit 2.02;
about firms with less favorable beliefs that firms are F 1,25=145.25, pb.001), and this effect does not differ by
motivated by self-interest (cf. Campbell and Kirmani, firm. Motivation was manipulated by changing the stated
2000; Friestad and Wright, 1994, 1995). Additionally, this goals of the program (e.g., Home Depot hopes this and other
elaboration may engender thoughts about efficacy, leading new programs will boost sales or Home Depot hopes this
consumers to question what benefits are likely to accrue to and other programs will help those at risk). Motivation also
firms or the initiatives (i.e., profit or social change). This varies as expected (Ms profit-motivated=6.11 and Ms
skepticism occurs because consumers hold intuitive beliefs socially driven=3.16; F 1,25 =140.13, pb.001; where
that social initiatives are primarily motivated by corporate 1=socially driven/7=profit-motivated), and it does not differ
self-interest (Speed and Thompson, 2000; Webb and Mohr, by firm.
1998). In this first study, the effects of fit and motivation were
Thus, our discussion leads us to Hypothesis 2: tested using a 2 (high fit vs. low fit)!2 (profit-motivated vs.
social interest) randomized, between-subjects, ANOVA
H2. Profit-motivated versus socially motivated CSR initia-
design. These effects were evaluated with the firms and
tives will result in a greater number of thoughts (H2a),
issue pairings as presented in Table 1. One hundred and
thoughts that are less favorable (H2b), thoughts that are
eight subjects from an adult continuing education program
more focused on firm motive (H2c), more negative attitudes
participated in the study and received no compensation.
toward the firm (H2d), beliefs about the firm as less credible
Their average age was 46 and 58% were females. Subjects
(H2e), and lower likelihood of purchase intention (H2f).
were told they were participating in a study regarding
perceptions of various firms and asked to: (1) evaluate their
feelings about the firm listed on the top of the page using
3. Study 1: Effects of fit and motivation three seven-point scales (1=negative/7=positive, 1=unfav-
orable/7=favorable, and 1=bad/7=good); (2) list what they
3.1. Method knew about the firm including their product line, services
offered, employee policies, corporate value system, and
In a pretest we sought to identify a pair of equally well- social initiatives; and (3) determine how likely they were to
liked and highly familiar firms that could be paired with a recommend or purchase from the firm using two seven-
set of equally important and relevant social initiatives. A point scales (1=not recommend/7=strongly recommend and
group of 28 respondents was given a list of eight companies 1=never purchase/7=always purchase).
(Disney, Toys R Us, Home Depot, Ford Motor Company,
Citigroup, Dell, Lockheed Martin, and Revlon) and six
different social initiatives (environmental preservation, early Table 1
childhood education, domestic violence, homelessness, Study design
cancer screening, and voting). The results showed that Profit-motivated Socially motivated
Home Depot and Revlon were equally well-liked firms High fit Home Depot—Homelessness Home Depot—Homelessness
(Ms=6.23 and 6.14, F 1,25b1) and equally familiar (Ms=6.24 Revlon—Domestic violence Revlon—Domestic violence
and 6.26, F 1,27b1), when liking and familiarity are measured Low fit Home Depot—Domestic Home Depot—Domestic
using three 7-point scales each (1=negative/7=positive, violence violence
Revlon—Homelessness Revlon—Homelessness
1=unfavorable/7=favorable, and 1=bad/7=good, Cronbach’s
K.L. Becker-Olsen et al. / Journal of Business Research 59 (2006) 46–53 49

They subsequently were instructed to turn the page and were counted and categorized as firm-related, fit-related,
read a newspaper clipping containing three business news motive-related, cause-related, and other by two coders.
items: one about a new service from VISA.COM, one about Additionally positive, negative, and neutral valences were
the experimental firm and its social initiative, and one about assigned. Inter-coder agreement was 79% and discrepancies
Wendy’s founder’s struggle with liver cancer. After reading were resolved by discussion. Both H1a and H2a are
the articles, subjects were asked once again to: (1) rate their supported since subjects provided a greater number of
feelings about the experimental firm using the same three thoughts when fit is low rather than high (Ms low fit=4.64,
items; (2) list any thoughts about the firm and the social Ms high fit=2.13; F 1,107=15.82, pb.05), and a greater
initiative; and (3) rate their willingness to support the firm. number of thoughts when the motivation is profit-driven
Next subjects were requested to turn the page and answer a rather than socially driven (Ms profit motivation=3.57, Ms
series of scaled belief measures about the firm. These belief social motivation=2.93; F 1,107=7.52, pb.05). Within the
measures were collapsed based on Cronbach’s alpha high-fit condition, profit motivation elicited more thoughts
reliability estimate to form three relevant dimensions: (1) than social motivation (Ms profit-motivated=2.48, Ms
corporate ability (makes good products, is an innovative socially motivated=1.37; F 1,53=9.48, pb.05). However, in
firm, has reliable products, is a well-managed firm, and is the low-fit conditions, there are no significant differences in
financially strong; Cronbach’s a=.86), (2) corporate credi- number of thoughts across types of motivation ( F 1,53=1.26,
bility (is a firm I can trust, is a firm that cares about its pN.05).
customers, has a strong value system, and is a firm I believe Overall favorability of thoughts was measured by taking
in; Cronbach’s a=.85), and (3) corporate positioning (clearly the total number of positive thoughts minus the total number
communicates what it stands for, conveys a consistent of negative thoughts. We find support for H1b and H2b
image, I understand what the firm does, and clearly given overall favorability of thoughts is reduced when fit is
represents who they are; Cronbach’s a=.74). low ( F 1,107=29.65, pb.001), and overall favorability of
Then subjects were instructed to: (1) rate the fit between thoughts is reduced when the motivation is profit-driven
the firm and social initiative using the four scaled items ( F 1,107=16.34, pb.001). Interestingly, the thoughts gener-
from the pretest; (2) determine the perceived motivation for ated in the high-fit, socially motivated condition tend to
participation in the program via three scaled items (1=self- focus more on general positive affect toward the firm (e.g.,
interested/7=community interested, 1=firm-focused/7=cus- Home Depot is a good firm, and I like Revlon), whereas
tomer-focused, and 1=profit-motivated/7=socially moti- thoughts in the high-fit, profit-motivated condition tend to
vated); (3) note the personal relevance and probability of be more specific (e.g., Home Depot hopes people will buy
receiving a direct benefit from the cause, importance of the more products, I am not sure if this will really help homeless
cause, and how important it is for firms to support social people, Revlon products are not going to help these people,
initiatives; and (4) list any other thoughts they have on and this is a good project for Revlon) indicating a higher
corporate social responsibility. level of elaboration.
Across the low-fit conditions we find support for H1c,
3.2. Results which predicts a greater focus on spontaneous thoughts
about firm motivation ( F 1,107=9.11, pb.05; e.g., why would
The results generally support our predictions (see they do this and they want to make a quick buck).
Table 2). A manipulation check shows that perceived fit Additionally we find thoughts related to fit (e.g., this is
varies as expected (Ms high fit=6.24 and Ms low fit=2.38, not a good program for Home Depot, it does not make sense
F 1, 107=98.51, pb.001), as does motive (Ms profit-moti- that Revlon would be involved in this, and these do not fit
vated=3.33 and Ms socially motivated=5.58, F 1, 107=73.66, together), and specific negative firm beliefs (e.g., this will
pb.001). not get people interested in the firm, I will not buy more,
The first three sub-hypotheses are related to the thought and this will not help the bottom line), revealing that
listings respondents did after reading the articles. Thoughts respondents did use their persuasion knowledge to evaluate

Table 2
Study 1 results
Low fit High fit Profit motivation Social motivation
a b c
Number of thoughts (H1a and H2a) 4.64 2.13 3.57 2.93d
Favorability of thoughts (H1b and H2b) .96a 1.45b 1.21c 1.73d
Thoughts related to motive (H1c and H2c) .89a .53b 1.36c .72d
Overall attitude (H1d and H2d) ".79a .38b ".34c .21d
Purchase intention (H1f and H2f) ".9a .39b .05c .41d
Firm credibility (H1e and H2e) 4.62a 5.86b 4.88c 5.04c
Firm position (no specific hypotheses) 4.02a 5.99b 5.01c 4.99c
Firm ability (no specific hypotheses) 5.93a 6.09a 5.97c 6.06c
Means with different superscripts differ, pb.05.
50 K.L. Becker-Olsen et al. / Journal of Business Research 59 (2006) 46–53

firms’ actions. We also find support for H2c since thoughts these good deeds will not always lead to positive thoughts,
across the profit-motivated conditions tend to question firm beliefs, attitudes, and intentions. Low-fit initiatives are
motivation more than in the socially-motivated conditions likely to diminish overall attitude as well as perceptions of
( F 1,107=14.25, pb.001). corporate credibility, corporate position, and purchase
The last three sub-hypotheses are related to closed end intention. Interestingly, when the firm is viewed as
measures that reflect respondent perception of overall motivated by firm centered interests (i.e., profit), there is
attitude, specific beliefs and behavioral intentions. Overall not a reduction in perceived corporate credibility. This is
attitude is the difference between firm assessment pre and consistent with Forehand and Grier’s (2003) notion that
post reading of the stimuli. We find both H1d and H2d are skepticism is not driven simply by a firm being profit-
supported since overall attitude is significantly elevated in motivated, but rather by a discrepancy between stated
the high-fit conditions relative to the low-fit conditions (Ms objectives and firm actions (e.g., if the objectives are stated
high fit=.38, Ms low fit=".79; F 1,107=12.05, pb.05), and in as purely social and firm actions appear to be self-serving).
the socially motivated condition relative to the profit- Thus, marketers need to be acutely aware of the negative
motivated condition (Ms social=.21, Ms profit=".34; impact of choosing low-fit CSR initiatives and how the firm
F 1,107=19.56, pb.05). Further, overall attitude is signifi- communicates its motivation for getting involved with a
cantly greater in the socially motivated, high-fit condition given initiative. Finally, we found no changes in perceived
than in the profit-motivated, high-fit condition (Ms socially corporate ability across fit or motivation, suggesting that
motivated=.57, Ms profit-motivated=.21, F 1,53 =8.36, companies cannot use social initiatives in place of strong
pb.05), with no differences across the low-fit conditions brand management and high-quality products. In categories
( F 1,53=.98, pN.05). where there is intense competition among similar goods and
Although we only made predictions regarding beliefs services, social initiatives may be used to differentiate
about credibility, we examined two other belief measures— offerings but are unlikely to influence consumers’ assess-
corporate ability and corporate positioning. We find support ments of desired functionality.
for H1e, which predicts that low fit will lead to beliefs that
the firm is less credible, ( F 1,107=11.24, pb.05), but H2e,
which predicts that profit motivation will lead to beliefs that 4. Study 2: The effects of information timing
the firm is less credible, is not supported ( F 1,107=1.56,
pN.05). However, we find that within the low-fit conditions, In the first study, we evaluated fit and motivation and
socially motivated initiatives reduce some of the negative found that promotion of high-fit, socially motivated
effects of fit ( F 1, 53=21.46, pb.001). Furthermore, we find initiatives improves consumers’ responses to firms, while
that perceived corporate ability is unaffected by fit ( F 1, promotion of low-fit, profit-motivated initiatives has the
107=.73, pN.05) or motivation ( F 1,107=1.02, pN.05). None- opposite relationship. The firms described in Study 1
theless, clarity of corporate positioning is enhanced in both engaged proactively with the initiative, rather than as a
high-fit conditions but reduced in the low-fit conditions ( F 1, response to some environmental catalyst or disaster. In
107=56.34, pb.001), while positioning remains unaffected Study 2 we examine the case in which firms react to events
by type of motivation ( F 1, 107=2.31, pN.05). that prompt them to behave in a socially responsible
Purchase intention, similar to overall attitude, is a manner. We believe that this additional variable, referred
measure of change from pre to post reading of the stimuli. to in this work as timing and characterized on two levels as
With regard to H1f and H2f, which predict a lowering of proactive and reactive, will influence perceptions of firms.
purchase intentions across low-fit and profit-motivated
conditions, our results are supportive ( F 1, 107=16.32, 4.1. Timing
pb.001 and F 1, 107=7.45, pb.05, respectively). Within the
low-fit conditions, socially motivated initiatives reduce the Firms often engage in social initiatives as a reaction to
negative effects of low fit; while in the high-fit, socially natural disasters, consumer boycotts, NGO pressures, or a
motivated conditions, purchase intentions increased ( F 1, number of other corporate crises. It seems intuitive that
53=1.46, pb.05 and F 1, 53=2.48, pb.05, respectively). There consumers’ responses to reactive CSR initiatives (e.g.,
is no significant change in purchase intentions within the McDonalds and its use of recyclable packaging material
high-fit condition ( F 1, 53=. 05, pN.05). or Starbucks’ consumer boycott for free-trade coffee) will be
different than responses to proactive initiatives (e.g., Yoplait
3.3. Discussion and Breast Cancer). Ellen et al. (2000), through a variable
they call donation manipulation, demonstrate that consumer
We find that firms with the desire to be perceived as response to firms’ support for natural disasters is more
bdoing goodQ within their target markets may be able to do positive than support for ongoing causes. They suggest that
so through promotion of carefully selected social initiatives. support for ongoing causes may arouse skepticism about
They may even enhance some corporate associations and firms’ potentially self-interested motives, while support for
overall brand equity with appropriate marketing. However, a disaster is likely to spark attributions that firms are
K.L. Becker-Olsen et al. / Journal of Business Research 59 (2006) 46–53 51

altruistic. In this work, we look at events for which firms ipates with local shelters to help homeless people find jobs.
may have culpability, such as widespread layoffs or child Ford initiates a child seat safety campaign after 14 children
labor. In these cases, we expect skepticism to drive were severely injured in Ford Windstar vans where child
consumer responses. Kernisky (1997) argues that during seats were not installed properly. Toys R Us initiates several
times of corporate crisis it is increasingly difficult, yet programs for missing children after a 7-year-old girl is taken
increasingly important, for firms to maintain legitimacy in from a store and found murdered in a nearby field. The
order to reduce consumer skepticism. proactive conditions show no tie other than to corporate
We know from the marketing literature that when mission and social domain.
expectations are exceeded by actions, firms are rewarded Similar to the first experiment, the three firms are equally
with more positive attitudes and an increase in behavioral well-liked (Ms=6.41, 6.46, and 6.35, F 1,25b1) and familiar
intentions (Creyer and Ross, 1997; Pyu, 1998; Zeithmal, (Ms=6.04, 6.13, and 6.27, F 1,27b1), and the three social
1998). However, research by Griffin et al. (1996) demon- initiatives are perceived as equally important (Ms=6.02,
strates the importance of the anticipation of critical negative 6.18, and 6.09, F 1,25b1) and relevant (Ms=5.88, 5.84, and
events on consumers’ post-purchase evaluations. Therefore 5.94, F 1,25b1). One hundred and fifty subjects from a
if consumers are inherently skeptical of firms’ actions (e.g., continuing education program participated in this study
Exxon’s involvement in environmental issues after a major without compensation. The average age was 36 and 47% of
oil spill), then only proactive social initiatives (e.g., Home the respondents were female.
Depot’s support of Habitat for Humanity) may exceed
baseline expectations and lead to more positive beliefs, 4.3. Results
attitudes, and intentions (Creyer and Ross, 1997).
We expect that consumers will interpret firms’ behaviors Once again results are generally consistent with our
and draw conclusions about motives depending upon the predictions and do not vary by firm, so their reporting is
context in which they occur (Jones and Davis, 1965; Ellen et collapsed across organizational exemplars. Our first three
al., 2000). In the case of reactive initiatives, context sub-hypotheses were regarding amount and content of
provides cues as to firms’ motivations, prompting consum- elaboration. Two researchers coded thoughts with inter-
ers to elaborate more on their actions; this greater elabo- coder reliability of 86%. Consistent with H3a and b, more
ration is likely to lower CSR evaluations (Menon and Kahn, thoughts were generated when the initiatives were reactive
2003). In contrast, under conditions of proactive initiatives, than proactive (Ms reactive=4.56, Ms proactive=2.36;
the context and motivations are ambiguous; thus, it is less F 1,149=18.82, pb. 05), and these thoughts were generally
likely that consumers will elaborate on their actions. more positive when the initiatives were proactive. In
Corporate credibility also may be impacted since commu- contrast, H3c was not supported since there was no
nications that are perceived as reactive may decrease significant difference in the total number of motive thoughts
corporate legitimacy, increase feelings of corporate self- (Ms proactive=1.19, Ms reactive=.96; F 1, 149=2.68, pN.05).
interest, and decrease feelings of honesty and trust However, the valence of these thoughts varied, and within
(Kernisky, 1997), all of which are likely to increase the proactive conditions respondents characterized motive
skepticism and decrease attitudes and beliefs towards firms. as bhelpingQ and benefiting both the firm and the cause,
This discussion suggests Hypothesis 3: while in the reactive conditions motivation was described as
selfish and profit-driven.
H3. Reactive versus proactive CSR initiatives will result in
Although not hypothesized specifically, we find a
a greater number of thoughts (H3a), thoughts that are less
significant difference in the number and content of
favorable (H3b), thoughts that are more focused on firm
attribution thoughts (Ms proactive=.14, Ms reactive=1.56;
motive (H3c), more negative attitudes toward the firm
F 1, 149=11.49, pb.05), suggesting that respondents blamed
(H3d), beliefs about the firm as less credible (H3e), and
the companies for problems in the reactive conditions.
lower likelihood of purchase intention (H3f).
Finally, both the content and number of thoughts related to
efficacy varied across conditions (Ms proactive=.78, Ms
4.2. Method reactive=1.89; F 1, 149=9.13, pb.05). Thoughts in the
proactive conditions were equally split between companies
In this experiment we examined three firms (Home and program efficacy and were entirely positive, whereas
Depot, Ford Motor Company, and Toys R Us) and three thoughts in the reactive conditions were skewed towards
social issues (homelessness, vehicle safety, and missing companies rather than program efficacy and were stated
children), which are paired to reflect the high-fit conditions negatively.
described in Study 1. Each of the reactive conditions Our final three sub-hypotheses reflect objective questions
presents the firm and social initiative as a response to some rather than spontaneous inferences. Using these measures,
previously taken action. For example, Home Depot lays off we find support for H3d since attitudes toward the firms
1200 employees, 20 of whom cannot find alternative jobs were enhanced when the programs were perceived as
and end up homeless. As a consequence the firm partic- proactive ( F 1, 149=34.19, pb.01) and reduced when the
52 K.L. Becker-Olsen et al. / Journal of Business Research 59 (2006) 46–53

programs were perceived as reactive ( F 1, 149=15.18, pb.05). scale consumer boycotts, reductions in brand images, or
Consistent with the first study, we find support for H3e/f. temporary drops in sales) when their negative records
Specifically, corporate credibility ( F 1, 149=29.18, pb.05), become public (Sen and Bhattacharya, 2001). Consumer
corporate positioning ( F 1, 149=9.46, pb.05), and purchase watchdog groups such as CorpWatch have successfully
intentions ( F 1, 149=11.37, pb.05) are all significantly exploited this relationship with their name-and-shame
enhanced when the initiatives are proactive. A unique result publicity programs for irresponsible firms.
of this investigation is that corporate ability also was Interestingly, across our two studies we found that
enhanced when the initiatives are proactive ( F 1, 149=8.79, greater than 80% of respondents believed firms should
pb.05). If this variable is split into product and management engage in social initiatives and 76% felt those initiatives
dimensions, the management factor is revealed as the would benefit firms. In our second study we also asked
driving force and there are no differences in product respondents if they would boycott firms that acted irrespon-
assessments across conditions ( F 1, 149=1.89, pN.05). sibly, and we found that 52% stated that they would boycott
if reasonable alternatives were available. Thus, our results
4.4. Discussion suggest that consumers expect firms to be involved in social
initiatives and may reward them for their efforts through
We were able to replicate the findings of the first purchase behavior. Additional investigations that focus on
experiment with regard to high-fit, socially motivated the interaction between the inherent virtue of the CSR action
initiatives. Furthermore, we extend these findings and and the perceived tangible benefit to the firm on consumers’
demonstrate that some high-fit, socially motivated programs buying behavior are warranted in order to determine the
may negatively impact consumer behavior responses to external validity of our findings.
promotions. The thought listings suggest that when con- Although these studies generally present a consistent set
sumers assign blame for acts to firms, they strongly question of results, scholars may wish to extend other findings as
organizational motives and negatively evaluate organiza- well. First, investigations focused on several firm-specific
tional actions. The findings involving corporate ability may factors may advance our understanding of consumers’
be the result of using social initiatives that are more closely perceptions of fit and motivation. Second, research con-
aligned to product/service offerings than in Study 1, ducted in marketplace settings with actual initiatives and
suggesting to respondents that managers could have targeted consumers will expand the external validity of our
prevented the acts that sparked the initiatives. results. Third, research that considers a wider variety of
industries and program initiatives will increase the general-
izability of findings and relevant contexts for marketing
5. Consumer behavior implications decision-making. Fourth, some of our variables such as
timing may be confounded with other factors such as locus
In his book Give and Take, Levy (1999) espouses the of control and require refinement. Finally, investigations of
belief that corporate philanthropy and social initiatives are the persistence of effects over time may enhance our
the heart and soul of business. He stresses that social understanding since consumers typically process informa-
endeavors must be consistent with firms’ operating objec- tion and form opinions during longer time horizons than
tives (heart) and must be an expression of their values (soul). these studies allowed.
Our research provides empirical support for this contention
and shows that when social initiatives are not aligned with
corporate objectives (low fit), CSR can actually become a References
liability and diminish previously held beliefs about firms.
Thus, marketers should select social programs carefully and Aaker David A. Brand extensions: the good, the bad, and the ugly. Sloan
ensure that their communications make the connection Manage Rev 1990;31(Summer):47 – 56.
Barone Michael J, Miyazaki Anthony D, Taylor Kimberly A. The influence
between the social domain and the firm so that consumers of cause-related marketing on consumer choice: does one good turn
perceive initiatives as proactive and socially motivated. deserve another? J Acad Mark Sci 2000;28(Spring):248 – 62.
While some CSR research suggests divergent results, this Boush David M, Freistad Marion, Rose Gregory M. Adolescent skepticism
variation may have been a function of their historical toward TV advertising and knowledge of advertiser tactics. J Consum
Res 1994;21(June):165 – 75.
context (see Carroll, 1999; Hill et al., 2003). Most likely
Brown Tom J, Dacin Peter A. The company and the product: corporate
consumer expectations related to CSR have increased over associations and consumer product responses. J Mark 1997;61(1):68 – 85.
the past 5 to 10 years as: (1) the number of firms with social Campbell Margaret C, Kirmani Amna. Consumer’s use of persuasion
responsibility programs grew, (2) more firms communicated knowledge: the effects of accessibility and cognitive capacity on
their efforts with the public, and (3) consumer groups perceptions of an influence agent. J Consum Res 2000;27(June):69 – 83.
promoted firm wrongdoings and called for large-scale Carroll Archie. Corporate social responsibility: evolution of a definitional
construct. Bus Soc 1999;38(3):268 – 95.
boycotts (Snider et al., 2003). There is managerial and Creyer Elizabeth, Ross William T. The influence of firm behavior on
empirical evidence that corporations with poor CSR records purchase intention: do consumers really care about business ethics?
experience significant negative consequences (e.g., large- J Consum Mark 1997;14(6):421 – 8.
K.L. Becker-Olsen et al. / Journal of Business Research 59 (2006) 46–53 53

Ellen Pam Schoder, Mohr Lois A, Web Deborah J. Charitable programs and Meyers-Levy Joan, Tybout Alice. Schema congruity as a basis for product
the retailer: do they mix? J Retail 2000;76(3):393 – 406. evaluation. J Consum Res 1989;16(1):39 – 54.
Erdem Tqlin, Swait Joffre. Brand equity as a signaling phenomenon. Meyers-Levy Joan, Louie Therese, Curren Mary T. How does the congruity
J Consum Psychol 1998a;7(2):131 – 57. of brand names affect evaluations of brand name extensions? J Appl
Erdem Tqlin, Swait Joffre. An empirical analysis of umbrella branding. Psychol 1994;79(1):46 – 53.
J Mark Res 1998b;35(August):339 – 51. Murray Keith, Vogel Christine M. Using a hierarchy-of-effects approach to
Fiske Susan T, Taylor Shelley A. Social cognition. New York7 McGraw Hill gauge the effectiveness of corporate social responsibility to generate
Book Company; 1991. goodwill toward the firm: financial versus nonfinancial impacts. J Bus
Folkes Valerie S. Recent attribution research in consumer behavior: a Res 1997;38(2):141 – 60.
review and new directions. J Consum Res 1999;14(March):548 – 65. Park C Whan, Jaworski Bernard J, MacInnis Deborah J. Strategic brand
Folkes Valerie S, Kamins Michael A. Effects of information about firms’ concept-image management. J Mark 1986;50(October):135 – 45.
ethical and unethical actions on consumer’s attitudes. J Consum Pava Moses L, Krause Joshua. Corporate social responsibility and financial
Psychol 1999;8(3):243 – 59. performance: the paradox of social cost. Westport (CT)7 Quorum
Ford Gary T, Smith Darlene B, Swasy John L. Consumer skepticism of Books; 1996.
advertising claims: testing hypotheses from economics of information. Pyu Sung-soo. Effects of service quality perceptions on expectations. J Int
J Consum Res 1990;16(March):433 – 41. Mark Mark Res 1998;23(2):81 – 90.
Forehand Mark R, Grier Sonya. When is honesty the best policy? The effect Sen Sankar, Bhattacharya CB. Does doing good always lead to doing
of stated company intent on consumer skepticism. J Consum Psychol better? Consumer reactions to corporate social responsibility. J Mark
2003;13(3):349 – 56. Res 2001;38(2):225 – 43.
Friestad Marian, Wright Peter. The persuasion knowledge model: how people Simmons Carolyn, Becker-Olsen Karen. When do Social Sponsorship
cope with persuasion attempts. J Consum Res 1994;21(June):1 – 31. Enhance or Dilute Equity: Fit, Message Source and the Persistence of
Friestad Marian, Wright Peter. Persuasion knowledge: lay people’s and Effect. Working Paper. 2004.
researcher’s beliefs about the psychology of advertising. J Consum Res Simonin Bernard, Ruth Julie A. Is a firm known by the firm it keeps?
1995;22(June):62 – 74. Assessing the spillover effects of brand alliances on consumer attitudes.
Griffin Mitch, Babin Barry J, Attaway Jill S. Anticipation of injurious J Mark Res 1998;35(February):30 – 42.
consumption outcomes and its impact on consumer attributions of Snider Jamie, Hill Ronald Paul, Martin Diane. Corporate social responsi-
blame. J Acad Mark Sci 1996;24(Fall):314 – 27. bility in the 21st century: a view from the world’s most successful firms.
Hill Ronald Paul, Stephens Debra, Smith Iain. Corporate social responsi- J Bus Ethics 2003;48(December):175 – 87.
bility: an examination of individual firm behavior. Bus Soc Rev Speed Richard, Thompson Peter. Determinants of sports sponsorship
2003;108(September):339 – 62. response. J Acad Mark Sci 2000;28(Spring):226 – 38.
Johar Gita Venkataramani, Pham Michael Tuan. Relatedness, prominence, Stanwick Peter A, Stanwick Sarah D. The relationship between corporate
and constructive sponsor identification. J Mark Res 1999;36(August): social performance and organizational size, financial performance, and
249 – 312. environmental performance: an empirical examination. J Bus Ethics
John Deborah Roedder, Loken Barbara, Joiner Christopher. The negative 1998;17(2):195 – 204.
impact of extensions: can flagship products be diluted? J Mark 1998; Till Brian D, Busler Michael. The match-up hypothesis: physical
62(January):19 – 31. attractiveness, expertise and the role of fit on brand attitude, purchase
Jones Edwards, Davis Keith. The attribution process in person perception. intent and brand beliefs. J Advert 2000;29(3):1 – 13.
In: Berkowitz L editor. Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, Varadarajan P Rajan, Menon Anil. Cause related marketing: a co-alignment
vol. 2. New York7 Academic Press; 1965. p. 220 – 66. of marketing strategy and corporate philanthropy. J Mark 1988;
Keller Kevin Lane. Conceptualizing, measuring, and managing consumer- 52(July):58 – 74.
based brand equity. J Mark 1993;57(January):1 – 10. Webb Deborah J, Mohr Lois A. A typology of consumer responses to
Keller Kevin Lane, Aaker David A. The effects of sequential introduction cause-related marketing: from skeptics to socially concerned. J Public
of brand extensions. J Mark Res 1992;24(February):35 – 50. Policy Mark 1998;17(Fall):226 – 38.
Kelley Harold H. Attribution theory in social psychology. In: Levine D Wojciske Bogdan, Bazinska Roza, Jaworski Marcin. On the dominance of
editor. Nebraska Symposium on Motivation, vol. 15. Lincoln7 moral categories in impression formation. Pers Soc Psychol Bull
University of Nebraska Press; 1967. 1993;24(12):1251 – 64.
Kelley Harold H. Attribution theory in social interaction. In: Jones EE, Zeithmal Valerie. The nature and determinants of customer expectations of
Kanouse DE, Kelley HH, Nisbett RE, Valins S, Weiner B, editors. service. Acad Mark Stud J 1998;21(1):1 – 12.
Attribution: perceiving the causes of behavior. Morristown (NJ)7
General Learning Press; 1972. p. 1 – 26.
Kernisky Debra. Proactive crisis management and ethical discourse: Dow
chemical’s issues management bulletins 1979–1990. J Bus Ethics 1997;
Further reading
16(8):843 – 54.
Levy Reynold. Give and take. Cambridge (MA)7 Harvard Business School Barone Michael J, Shimp Terence A, Sprott David E. Product ownership as
Press; 1999. a moderator of self-congruity effects. Mark Lett 1999;10(1):75 – 85.
Mandler George. The structure of value: accounting for taste. In: Clark Dolich Ira. Congruence relationships between self images and product
Margaret S, Fiske Susan T, editors. Affect and Cognition: The 17th brands. J Mark Res 1969;6(1):80 – 5.
Annual Carnegie Symposium. Hillsdale (NJ)7 Lawrence Erlbaum Drumwright Minnette E. Company advertising with a social dimension: the
Associates; 1982. p. 3 – 36. role of non-economic criteria. J Mark 1996;60;71 – 87.
Mason Tania. Nike and the DoE tackle school bullying. Marketing News Ellen Pam Schoder, Mohr Lois A, Web Deborah J. Pure or mixed motives:
[July 12, 2001, pp. 1]. consumer attributions about cause marketing offers. Proceedings
McGuire Jean B, Sundgren A, Schneeweis Thomas. Corporate social association for consumer research, 2002 [Atlanta, GA].
responsibility and firm financial performance. Acad Manage J 1988; Sirgy Joseph. Self concept in consumer behavior: a critical review. J
31(6):854 – 72. Consum Res 1982;9(3):287 – 301.
Menon Satya, Kahn Barbara. Corporate sponsorships of philanthropic Sirgy Joseph, Samli Coskun. A path analytic model of store loyalty
activities: when do they impact perception of sponsor brand? J Consum involving self concept, store image, geographic location and socio-
Psychol 2003;13(3):316 – 27. economic status. J Acad Mark Sci 1985;13(3):265 – 92.

You might also like