You are on page 1of 6

Discuss the effects of sleep deprivation on memory formation.

Sleep is an essential aspect of human life and is necessary for the proper functioning of the
body and brain. It has been well established that sleep plays a crucial role in memory
consolidation and learning. While sleep is undoubtedly crucial for optimal learning and
performance of tasks, there is still uncertainty regarding its role in memory consolidation
(Siegal, 2001). Sleep deprivation is the condition of not getting enough sleep to meet the
body's needs. Sleep deprivation can be acute, which is a short-term condition, or chronic,
which is a long-term condition. Acute sleep deprivation is caused by staying up late or
waking up early, while chronic sleep deprivation can result from lifestyle factors, such as
shift work or insomnia. Every day, millions of individuals all over the world suffer from a lack
of sleep (Hublin et al., 2001). More and more people are being affected by the societal
expectation to stay awake for longer periods of time in our modern, constantly active world
(Bonnet and Arand, 1995). Sleep deprivation is a prevalent problem in modern society that
can lead to cognitive impairment. While it is generally acknowledged that lack of sleep
affects learning and memory, recent research has focused on identifying the specific
molecular signalling pathways that are affected by sleep loss. Understanding these
pathways is crucial for developing interventions that can reverse the memory impairments
caused by sleep deprivation (Havekes et al., 2012). An intriguing idea is that sleep plays a
significant role in the processes of learning, memory, and the brain's ability to change and
adapt, also known as plasticity (Walker and Stickgold, 2006). In this essay, we will discuss
the effects of sleep deprivation on memory formation and the mechanisms behind these
effects.

Sleep deprivation can also lead to changes in brain wave activity. During sleep, the brain
undergoes different stages of activity, including slow-wave sleep and rapid eye movement
(REM) sleep. Slow-wave sleep is associated with memory consolidation, while REM sleep is
linked to the consolidation of emotional memories. Sleep deprivation can lead to a decrease
in slow-wave sleep and REM sleep, impairing memory consolidation and recall (Walker,
2009).

For more than 200 years, there has been an understanding that sleep is crucial for memory
and learning. As far back as 1801, David Hartley suggested that dreams could impact the
strength of neural connections that contribute to memory formation (Nicolas and Murray,
1999). Nearly a century after the initial hypothesis, the first comprehensive evaluation of
the relationship between sleep and memory occurred in 1924. This study revealed that
memory retention was superior after a night of sleep compared to being awake during the
day (Yaroush et al., 1971). The concept of sleep-dependent memory processing is a recent
theory that has gained support from numerous studies in the past decade across various
fields of neuroscience (Walker and Stickgold, 2006, Marshall and Born, 2007). Defining
sleep-dependent memory processing is challenging due to the intricate nature of sleep and
memory. Sleep is generally categorized into two broad states: rapid eye movement (REM)
and non-REM (NREM) sleep, with NREM sleep further subdivided into stages of varying
depth.
Figure 01. When we encounter an experience, certain elements of it are stored in our
memory. This storage process is called "encoding" and triggers a series of processes known
as "consolidation". These consolidation processes take time and ultimately result in the
formation of a long-lasting memory (Nadel et al., 2012).

Memory, on the other hand, encompasses multiple types, such as declarative (conscious
memories of factual information) and nondeclarative (procedural memories of skills and
habits). Memory also goes through various stages, including acquisition, consolidation,
integration, recall, and erasure. Therefore, sleep-dependent memory processing cannot be
easily described in a single sentence due to the complexity of sleep and memory as separate
phenomena (Walker and Stickgold, 2004). The interaction between sleep stages and
memory stages is complex, as there are multiple variations of both. This complexity creates
numerous possibilities for how different sleep stages may affect various aspects of memory
(Walker and Stickgold, 2004).

One of the most significant impacts of sleep deprivation is its effect on memory formation.
Research has shown that sleep deprivation impairs the consolidation of memories, which
can lead to problems with learning and memory recall. Studies have shown that people who
are sleep-deprived have a harder time recalling information they have learned, making it
more difficult to retain new information. Studies have shown that sleep-deprived individuals
often have difficulty with both short-term and long-term memory. For example, they may
have trouble remembering details from a recent conversation or recalling information from
a lecture they attended earlier in the day. Sleep deprivation can also impact the ability to
form new memories. During sleep, the brain transfers information from short-term to long-
term memory. When sleep is disrupted, this process can be compromised, leading to
difficulties in retaining new information. (Blissitt, 2001, Acosta, 2019).

There are several mechanisms that explain the effect of sleep deprivation on memory
formation. One of these mechanisms is the impact on the hippocampus, a brain region
crucial for memory consolidation. The hippocampus plays a crucial role in the formation of
new memories and their integration into long-term memory. Experiments conducted later
on discovered that sleep deprivation has a significant impact on memory consolidation that
relies on the hippocampus. This was demonstrated using the Morris water maze, which is a
task that can be customized to evaluate learning and memory formation that depends on
the hippocampus (hidden platform version of the task) or does not depend on the
hippocampus (visible platform version of the task) (Morris et al., 1982). During sleep, the
hippocampus processes and consolidates memories formed during the day. Sleep
deprivation can lead to decreased hippocampal activity, resulting in impaired memory
consolidation and recall (Maquet, 2001).
Another mechanism is the effect on the prefrontal cortex, a brain region responsible for
executive function, including attention and decision-making. The prefrontal cortex is also
involved in working memory, which is the ability to temporarily hold and manipulate
information in the mind. Sleep deprivation can also impact cortical areas that play a crucial
role in the process of memory consolidation (Vyazovskiy et al., 2008). Sleep deprivation
impairs the process of consolidating memory related to objects, specifically recognition
memory (Palchykova et al., 2006). The ability to recognize objects may not necessarily
require the involvement of the hippocampus, and whether or not it does may depend on
the way in which the recognition skills were learned or acquired (Oliveira et al., 2010). The
process in question does not solely rely on the hippocampus, but also involves other cortical
regions outside of the hippocampus, such as the perirhinal cortex and insular cortex
(Winters et al., 2004). Lack of sleep has an impact on the brain's cortical systems that are
responsible for a range of functions, such as executive attention, working memory, and
advanced cognitive abilities which can impair memory formation and recall (Wang et al.,
2011).

Sleep deprivation can also impact the levels of neurotransmitters, such as dopamine and
serotonin, which play a critical role in memory formation. Studies have shown that sleep
deprivation can decrease the levels of these neurotransmitters, impairing memory
consolidation and recall. Additionally, sleep deprivation can also cause an increase in the
stress hormone cortisol, which can have a negative impact on memory consolidation and
recall (Siddique et al., 2018).

Figure 02.
The figure displays a graphical representation of the molecular signalling pathways in the
hippocampus that are involved in memory formation. Under normal conditions,
glutamatergic and cholinergic signalling are active, while adenosine and GABAergic
signalling are low. In addition, cAMP signalling and CREB-mediated gene transcription are
also active. However, sleep deprivation has been shown to decrease glutamatergic and
cholinergic signalling, increase adenosine and GABAergic signalling, and attenuate cAMP
signalling and CREB-mediated gene transcription. These molecular changes may interact to
affect learning and memory formation. The figure illustrates all these pathways in a single
connected pathway to demonstrate how the effects of sleep deprivation could potentially
impact memory. The dashed black lines and downward arrows indicate a reduction in
signalling, while the red lines and upward arrows indicate an increase in signalling (Havekes
et al., 2012).

However, sleep deprivation can interfere with this process, leading to a decline in memory
performance. Studies have shown that sleep-deprived individuals have a harder time
remembering information they have learned, particularly information that requires
concentration, attention, and focus. In addition to the impact on memory formation, sleep
deprivation can also have other negative effects on the body and brain. Sleep deprivation
can impair immune function, leading to an increased risk of infections and illnesses. Chronic
sleep deprivation has also been linked to an increased risk of developing mental health
disorders, such as depression and anxiety (Blissitt, 2001).

In summary, Recent research indicates that during sleep, our brain is highly active and
engaged in processing information acquired during the day. Adequate sleep is crucial for
proper memory consolidation and is also significant for brain development and plasticity
during different stages of growth. The ultimate goal is to achieve a coordinated equilibrium
in mental, emotional, and physiological well-being. To prevent the negative effects of sleep
deprivation on memory formation, it is essential to prioritize getting enough sleep. Adults
should aim to get 6 hours of sleep per night, while children and teenagers require more.
Establishing a regular sleep schedule, avoiding caffeine and alcohol before bedtime, and
creating a comfortable sleep environment can all help to promote healthy sleep habits
(Ferrara and De Gennaro, 2001).
Reference

ACOSTA, M. T. 2019. Sleep, memory and learning. Medicina, 79, 29-32.


BLISSITT, P. A. 2001. Sleep, memory, and learning. Journal of Neuroscience Nursing, 33, 208.
BONNET, M. H. & ARAND, D. L. 1995. We are chronically sleep deprived. Sleep, 18, 908-911.
FERRARA, M. & DE GENNARO, L. 2001. How much sleep do we need? Sleep medicine
reviews, 5, 155-179.
HAVEKES, R., VECSEY, C. G. & ABEL, T. 2012. The impact of sleep deprivation on neuronal
and glial signaling pathways important for memory and synaptic plasticity. Cellular
signalling, 24, 1251-1260.
HUBLIN, C., KAPRIO, J., PARTINEN, M. & KOSKENVUO, M. 2001. Insufficient sleep—a
population-based study in adults. Sleep, 24, 392-400.
MAQUET, P. 2001. The role of sleep in learning and memory. science, 294, 1048-1052.
MARSHALL, L. & BORN, J. 2007. The contribution of sleep to hippocampus-dependent
memory consolidation. Trends in cognitive sciences, 11, 442-450.
MORRIS, R. G., GARRUD, P., RAWLINS, J. A. & O'KEEFE, J. 1982. Place navigation impaired in
rats with hippocampal lesions. Nature, 297, 681-683.
NADEL, L., HUPBACH, A., GOMEZ, R. & NEWMAN-SMITH, K. 2012. Memory formation,
consolidation and transformation. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 36, 1640-1645.
NICOLAS, S. & MURRAY, D. J. 1999. Théodule Ribot (1839–1916), founder of French
psychology: A biographical introduction. History of Psychology, 2, 277.
OLIVEIRA, A. M., HAWK, J. D., ABEL, T. & HAVEKES, R. 2010. Post-training reversible
inactivation of the hippocampus enhances novel object recognition memory. Learning &
memory, 17, 155-160.
PALCHYKOVA, S., WINSKY-SOMMERER, R., MEERLO, P., DÜRR, R. & TOBLER, I. 2006. Sleep
deprivation impairs object recognition in mice. Neurobiology of learning and memory, 85,
263-271.
PALMER, N. D., LANGEFELD, C. D., CAMPBELL, J. K., WILLIAMS, A. H., SAAD, M., NORRIS, J.
M., HAFFNER, S. M., ROTTER, J. I., WAGENKNECHT, L. E. & BERGMAN, R. N. 2006. Genetic
mapping of disposition index and acute insulin response loci on chromosome 11q: the
Insulin Resistance Atherosclerosis Study (IRAS) Family Study. Diabetes, 55, 911-918.
SIDDIQUE, S. A., TAMILSELVAN, T., VISHNUPRIYA, M. & BALAMURUGAN, E. 2018. Evaluation
of neurotransmitter alterations in four distinct brain regions after rapid eye movement sleep
deprivation (REMSD) induced mania-like behaviour in Swiss albino mice. Neurochemical
research, 43, 1171-1181.
SIEGEL, J. M. 2001. The REM sleep-memory consolidation hypothesis. Science, 294, 1058-
1063.
SPIEGEL, K., LEPROULT, R. & VAN CAUTER, E. 1999. Impact of sleep debt on metabolic and
endocrine function. The lancet, 354, 1435-1439.
VYAZOVSKIY, V. V., CIRELLI, C., PFISTER-GENSKOW, M., FARAGUNA, U. & TONONI, G. 2008.
Molecular and electrophysiological evidence for net synaptic potentiation in wake and
depression in sleep. Nature neuroscience, 11, 200-208.
WALKER, M. P. 2009. The role of sleep in cognition and emotion. Annals of the New York
Academy of Sciences, 1156, 168-197.
WALKER, M. P. & STICKGOLD, R. 2004. Sleep-dependent learning and memory consolidation.
Neuron, 44, 121-133.
WALKER, M. P. & STICKGOLD, R. 2006. Sleep, memory, and plasticity. Annu. Rev. Psychol.,
57, 139-166.
WANG, G., GRONE, B., COLAS, D., APPELBAUM, L. & MOURRAIN, P. 2011. Synaptic plasticity
in sleep: learning, homeostasis and disease. Trends in neurosciences, 34, 452-463.
WINTERS, B. D., FORWOOD, S. E., COWELL, R. A., SAKSIDA, L. M. & BUSSEY, T. J. 2004.
Double dissociation between the effects of peri-postrhinal cortex and hippocampal lesions
on tests of object recognition and spatial memory: heterogeneity of function within the
temporal lobe. Journal of Neuroscience, 24, 5901-5908.
YAROUSH, R., SULLIVAN, M. J. & EKSTRAND, B. R. 1971. Effect of sleep on memory: II.
Differential effect of the first and second half of the night. Journal of experimental
psychology, 88, 361.

You might also like