You are on page 1of 14

Clinical Gerontologist

ISSN: 0731-7115 (Print) 1545-2301 (Online) Journal homepage: www.tandfonline.com/journals/wcli20

Trajectories of Depressive Symptomatology and


Loneliness in Older Adult Sexual Minorities and
Heterosexual Groups

Christopher R. Beam & Emma M. Collins

To cite this article: Christopher R. Beam & Emma M. Collins (2019) Trajectories of Depressive
Symptomatology and Loneliness in Older Adult Sexual Minorities and Heterosexual Groups,
Clinical Gerontologist, 42:2, 172-184, DOI: 10.1080/07317115.2018.1518283

To link to this article: https://doi.org/10.1080/07317115.2018.1518283

Published online: 15 Oct 2018.

Submit your article to this journal

Article views: 713

View related articles

View Crossmark data

Citing articles: 10 View citing articles

Full Terms & Conditions of access and use can be found at


https://www.tandfonline.com/action/journalInformation?journalCode=wcli20
CLINICAL GERONTOLOGIST
2019, VOL. 42, NO. 2, 172–184
https://doi.org/10.1080/07317115.2018.1518283

Trajectories of Depressive Symptomatology and Loneliness in Older Adult


Sexual Minorities and Heterosexual Groups
Christopher R. Beam and Emma M. Collins
Psychology, University of Southern California Dana and David Dornsife College of Letters Arts and Sciences, Los Angeles, USA

ABSTRACT
Objectives: This article examines whether sexual minority men and women experience greater
increases in depressive symptoms and loneliness with age compared to heterosexual men and
women.
Methods: Using three waves of data from sexual minority (nMen = 87 and nWomen = 62) and
heterosexual (nMen = 1,297 and nWomen = 1,362) older adults in the National Social Life, Health, and
Aging Project, we used latent growth curve modeling to test whether change in depressive
symptoms and loneliness varies across sexual orientation and whether annual household income
and family support accounted for this change.
Results: Although differences in the growth trajectories of depressive symptoms and loneliness
across sexual orientation were not observed, gender differences were. Annual household income
and family support more strongly influenced initial depressive symptoms and loneliness in sexual
minority men and women than in heterosexual men and women.
Conclusions: Trajectories of depressive symptoms and loneliness in older adulthood do not vary
by sexual orientation. Economic and family resources may allow sexual minorities to cope
effectively with depressive symptoms and loneliness.
Clinical Implications: Clinicians should be cautious about assuming that older sexual minority
group members are more susceptible to depressive symptoms and loneliness than heterosexual
groups by virtue of their sexual preference.

Depressive symptoms and loneliness are highly study, thus, is to compare the change in depressive
correlated constructs that have not been studied symptoms and loneliness between older adult sex-
in sexual minority status groups. Older adult ual minority men and women and their hetero-
members of sexual minorities are at greater risk sexual counterparts.
for depressive symptomatology (Fredriksen- Rates of psychiatric disorders are higher in sex-
Goldsen, Kim, Barkan, Muraco, & Hoy-Ellis, ual minority groups compared to heterosexual
2013) and loneliness (Jacobs & Kane, 2012; Kim groups in the United States (U.S.) (Cochran &
& Fredriksen-Goldsen, 2016). Increased risk of Mays, 2000; Gilman et al., 2001) and the United
each is notable given that suicide risk, a potential Kingdom (Mercer et al., 2007). With respect to
consequence of depression and loneliness (Agerbo, major depressive disorder and depressive sympto-
Stack, & Petersen, 2011; Kalmar et al., 2008; matology (i.e., non-clinically diagnosed depres-
Stickley & Koyanagi, 2016), is significantly higher sion), men who have sex with men in the
among sexual minority adults compared to hetero- National Comorbidity Survey were nearly 1.5
sexual adults (Björkenstam, Andersson, Dalman, times more likely to be diagnosed with major
Cochran, & Kosidou, 2016). The majority of sexual depression compared to their heterosexual coun-
minority studies on depressive symptoms and terparts while women who have sex with women
loneliness are cross-sectional, which does not were nearly twice as likely to be diagnosed with
address whether longitudinal trajectories vary by major depression compared to heterosexual
sexual orientation. The purpose of the current women (Gilman et al., 2001). Using a broad

CONTACT Christopher R. Beam beamc@usc.edu Psychology, University of Southern California Dana and David Dornsife College of Letters Arts and
Sciences, 3620 McClintock Ave, Seeley G. Mudd Room 501, Los Angeles, 90089-4012
Color versions of one or more of the figures in the article can be found online at www.tandfonline.com/wcli.
© 2018 Taylor & Francis Group, LLC
CLINICAL GERONTOLOGIST 173

measure of sexual minority status where partici- rejection cues) out of the need for self-protection.
pants self-reported ever having had a sexual part- Sustained vigilance, in turn, leads to cognitive
ner of the same gender, adjusted odds ratios were biases that may reinforce negative evaluations,
higher for sexual minority men than sexual min- ineffective coping, automatic negative thinking
ority women (ORs 2.94 and 1.79, respectively) styles, and rumination that prevent reaffiliation
compared to their heterosexual counterparts in and perpetuate feelings of loneliness. In an ethno-
the National Household Survey of Drug Abuse graphic study of Dutch gay men (Aggarwal &
(Cochran & Mays). Rates of loneliness, similarly, Gerrets, 2014), a common reported theme was
are higher in Dutch sexual minority groups than diminished self-esteem and degraded self-worth
in heterosexual groups (Fokkema & Kuyper, that led to chronic feelings of increased vulner-
2009). Rates of loneliness in U.S. sexual minority ability to social rejection. These maladaptive cog-
populations are less clear, as studies on loneliness nitive biases can lead to increased levels of
have not included heterosexual samples (Jacobs & internalized disapproval of one’s own sexual orien-
Kane, 2012; Kim & Fredriksen-Goldsen, 2016). tation (Jacobs & Kane, 2012) and negative interac-
tions with others (Coyne, 1976) that may
perpetuate depressive symptoms and feelings of
Mechanisms of greater depressive symptoms and loneliness.
loneliness in sexual minority groups Loneliness moderately correlates with depres-
Minority stress theory (Meyer, 2003) and social sive symptoms in the general population of older
threat theory (Cacioppo, Cacioppo, & Boomsma, adults in cross-sectional (Adams, Sanders, & Auth,
2014) are used to explain why depressive symptoms 2004; Cacioppo, Hawkley, & Thisted, 2010) and
and loneliness may increase with age in sexual mino- longitudinal studies (Cacioppo, Hughes, Waite,
rities. Minority stress theory assumes that sexual Hawkley, & Thisted, 2006). Under the minority
minority groups experience unique stressors beyond stress model and social threat theory of loneliness,
general life stress experienced by all people, because depressive symptoms and loneliness may not only
of their belonging to a socially stigmatized group. increase over time in sexual minority groups but
Meyer (2003) proposed three processes unique to also be more strongly correlated with one another
sexual minority groups: a distal set of processes con- compared to heterosexual members.
sisting of external sources of stress; negative expecta-
tions formed from these stressful experiences; and
Predictors of depressive symptoms and
internalized negative social attitudes about their
loneliness
group. Negative expectations, social interactions,
and internalized negative attitudes may contribute We consider two protective factors against increas-
to feeling unsupported (Aggarwal & Gerrets, 2014; ing depressive symptoms and loneliness: annual
Kuyper & Fokkema, 2010), and ineffective coping household income and family support. Sexual
strategies in the face of discrimination (Feinstein, minority group members tend to live below the
Davila, & Dyar, 2017). As a result, sexual minority federal poverty line (Fredriksen-Goldsen et al.,
groups may experience increases in depressive 2013) compared to heterosexual group members.
symptoms and loneliness at a point in adulthood Annual household income, thus, may be a salient
when both decline in the general population (Fiske, protective mechanism against increases in depres-
Wetherell, & Gatz, 2009; Victor, Scambler, Bowling, sive symptoms and loneliness in sexual minority
& Bond, 2005; Victor et al., 2002). groups. Second, social (Domènech-Abella et al.,
Social threat theory (Cacioppo et al., 2014) 2017) and family support (Carstensen, 1992; Fiori
relatedly may explain potential rises in depressive & Jager, 2012) might also protect against depres-
symptoms and loneliness among sexual minorities. sive symptoms and loneliness in older adulthood.
Social threat assumes that people predisposed We, thus, examined whether greater family sup-
toward loneliness may feel intense desire to reaf- port also predicted lower depressive symptom and
filiate with others while simultaneously increase loneliness levels in sexual minority members com-
their vigilance for social threats of harm (e.g., pared to heterosexual members.
174 C. R. BEAM AND E. M. COLLINS

Potential gender differences greater increases in depressive symptoms and lone-


liness over time compared to their heterosexual coun-
Women are at greater risk for depressive symptoms
terparts. Second, given prior research, sexual minority
and risk of exposure to factors associated with
men are expected to have higher initial levels and
depressive symptoms (Sonnenberg, Beekman,
greater increases in depressive symptoms and lone-
Deeg, & Van Tilburg, 2000) than men. Rates of
liness over time compared to sexual minority women.
loneliness tend to be higher in women than men
Third, correlations between depressive symptom and
(Aartsen & Jylhä, 2011; Berg, Mellstrom, Persson,
loneliness individual growth parameters will be higher
& Svanborg, 1981), although null results also have
in sexual minority status men and women than in
been reported (Hawkley & Kocherginsky, 2018). Yet,
heterosexual men and women. Fourth, differences in
sexual minority men may have higher risk for
household income and family support will have stron-
depressive symptoms and loneliness than sexual
ger effects on growth of depressive symptoms and
minority women (Kim & Fredriksen-Goldsen,
loneliness in sexual minority men and women than
2016), although null findings have been reported,
in heterosexual men and women.
too (Björkenstam et al., 2016; Fredriksen-Goldsen
et al., 2013). We, thus, explored differences between
sexual minority status men and women.
Method
Participants
Problems studying sexual minority status groups
in aging populations The sample used for the current study was drawn
from the National Social Life, Health, and Aging
Historically, population-based studies of older Project (NSHAP), which is a population-based
adults in the United States have not collected longitudinal study of social, physical, and psycho-
data on sexual orientation, gender identity, or logical health in Americans born between 1920
sexual history (Brown & Grossman, 2014). and 1947 (Waite et al., 2007). NSHAP consists of
Classification of sexual minority groups typically 3 waves of data, with the first wave collected in
has been based on self-reported sexual histories 2005–2006 using probability sampling (N = 3,005),
(Cochran & Mays, 2000; Gilman et al., 2001) the second wave collected in 2010–2011
rather than directly assessing sexual orientation. (N = 3,377), and the third wave collected in
In the current sample, a similar approach was 2015–2016 (N = 4,777). At wave 2, a subsample
taken. We acknowledge that using sexual histories of spouses was added (n = 955); and at wave 3, a
to classify older participants as members of min- new cohort (born between 1948 and 1965) was
ority groups does not necessarily imply partici- added (n = 2,368). Participants who provided
pants currently self-identify as gay, lesbian, or complete responses to all depressive symptom
bisexual, but could indicate previous sexual items or loneliness items at any wave were
exploration decades prior. included in the analytic sample, yielding a total
sample of 2,808 participants. Sexual identity was
determined using two items that assessed sexual
The current study
history: “In your entire life so far, how many men
We used latent growth modeling to investigate differ- have you had sex with, even if only one time?” and
ences in the longitudinal trajectories of depressive “In your entire life so far, how many women have
symptoms and loneliness between sexual minority you had sex with, even if only one time?” (Waite
men and women and their heterosexual counterparts. et al., 2007). Men who responded in the affirma-
Additionally, we treated the individual growth para- tive to the former were coded as men who have
meters of depressive symptoms and loneliness as out- had sex with men (MSM; n = 87) while women
comes to test whether the effects of annual household who responded in the affirmative to the latter were
income and family support vary as a function of sexual coded as women who have had sex with women
orientation. We made four hypotheses. First, sexual (WSW; n = 62). Men who denied ever having had
minority status members have higher initial levels and sex with another man were coded as heterosexual
CLINICAL GERONTOLOGIST 175

(MSW; n = 1,297) while women who denied ever Household income was measured with a single
having had sex with another woman were coded as item that asked participants to report their house-
heterosexual (WSM; n = 1,362). The self-identified hold income (in U.S. dollars) over the last year.
ethnic composition is: 77.24% white/Caucasian; Family support is an ordinal variable measured
16.20% black/African American; 0.75% Asian or with a single item (“How many family/relatives
Pacific Islander; 1.21% American Indian or do you feel close too?”). Response categories
Alaskan Native; 4.34% other; and 0.25% did not included “none” (0), “one” (1), “2–3” (2), “4–9”
report. The current study was approved by the (3), “10–20” (4), and “more than 20” (5), with
Institutional Review Board at the University of higher scores indicating greater family support.
Southern California (ID: UP-17–00067).
Data analysis
Measures
Descriptive statistics are presented to illustrate dif-
Depressive symptoms were measured using 10 ferences within and between waves of measure-
items from the Center for Epidemiological- ment in depressive symptoms and loneliness
Depression scale (CESD; Radloff, 1977). The between sexual minority and heterosexual men
subset of items included measures that assessed and women. As participants provided up to three
feelings of depression, loss of appetite, whether repeated measurements, with intervals of 5–6 years
everything felt effortful, restless sleep, feeling between measurements, individual longitudinal
happy, whether people were unfriendly, enjoyment trajectory plots are presented to illustrate trends
of life, feelings of sadness, feeling disliked by in depressive symptoms and loneliness for all sex-
others, and could not get going. One item measur- ual minority and heterosexual men and women.
ing feelings of loneliness was excluded. Items were Smoothing lines and 95% confidence intervals
rated on a 4-point Likert scale (“rarely or none of (CIs) were generated using a local polynomial
the time”, “some of the time”, “occasionally”, regression (loess) for each group.
“most of the time”), with all items scaled so that We estimated latent growth curve models
higher values indicate greater depressive symp- (LGM) to test for differences in initial and linear
toms. Scale means were constructed for all parti- change in depressive symptoms and loneliness
cipants. Scale reliabilities (McDonald’s ω; scores between sexual minority status and hetero-
McDonald, 1999) across waves 1–3 were .78, .77, sexual men and women. LGMs estimate individual
.78, respectively. trajectories of change in complex research designs,
Loneliness was measured using three items from particularly longitudinal designs where missing
the UCLA Loneliness Scale (Russell, 1996). The data (e.g., attrition) are expected (Bryk &
items assessed lack of companionship (“How Raudenbush, 1987; Finkel, Reynolds, McArdle,
often do you feel that you lack companionship?”); Gatz, & Pedersen, 2003; McArdle & Anderson,
feeling left out (“How often do you feel left out?”); 1990; McArdle & Hamagami, 1992). The inclusion
and feeling isolated from others (“How often do of incomplete data stabilizes both mean and var-
you feel isolated from others?”). At wave 1, three iance estimates. LGMs assume that missing data
response options were given (“hardly ever (or are missing at random (MAR) and give more
never)”, “some of the time”, and “often”) whereas weight in model estimation to participants provid-
at waves 2 and 3, four response options were given ing greater numbers of measurements.
(“never”, “hardly ever”, “some of the time”, and As shown in Figure 1, LGMs include the following
“often”). In order to put items on the same scale parameters: a random intercept (I), which is the mean
across waves, “never” and “hardly ever” were col- estimate of scores at the first wave of measurement
lapsed into a single category at waves 2 and 3. across the sample or group, and a random linear slope
Items were summed, with scores ranging from 3 (S), which is the mean linear rate of change across the
to 9 (higher scores indicate greater loneliness). entire sample or group. Intercepts and slopes are
Scale reliabilities across waves 1–3 were .69, .67, estimated for each participant and then aggregated
.65, respectively. across groups. Fixed (means) and random (variances
176 C. R. BEAM AND E. M. COLLINS

u1 u2 u3 individual factor loadings so that the time metric is


common across individuals (Mehta & West, 2000).
The intercepts, thus, represent the group-level mean
CESD1 CESD2 CESD3
depressive symptom and loneliness scores at age 65
while the linear slope represents the unit change per
year. Significant random effects indicate individual
differences in intraindividual change (Singer &
Willett, 2003). For depressive symptoms (D) and
loneliness (L), the LGMs are defined as:
1 1 1 a1 a2 a3  
Dij ¼ γD00 þ γD10 AGEij 65 þ ζ D0i
  (1)
þ ζ D1i AGEij 65 þ εDij
1
DI DS
 
* Lij ¼ γL00 þ γL10 AGEij 65 þ ζ L0i
* *   (2)
* * þ ζ L1i AGEij 65 þ εLij
* *
* *
* Dij and Lij represent depressive symptom and lone-
LI LS liness scores, respectively, for the ith individual at the
1
jth measurement; AGEij is the ith individual’s age at the
jth measurement centered at an origin of 65 years; ζD00
1 1 1 a1 a2 a3
and ζL00 reflect mean scores at age 65; ζD10 and ζL10
represent linear rates of change; ζD0i and ζL0i are the ith
individual’s deviation from the mean of the intercepts
while ζD1i and ζL1i are the ith individual’s deviation
from the mean of the linear slopes; and residuals eDij
and eLij are independent and identically distributed
UCLA1 UCLA2 UCLA3 deviations of the ith individual’s scores at the jth mea-
surement from the expected linear trajectory.
Covariance structure analysis enables estimation
v1 v2 v3 of the covariances between intercepts and slopes
within and between latent constructs:
Figure 1. Correlated latent growth curve model. CESD = depressive
symptoms scores; UCLA = loneliness scores; D1 = latent intercept for σ 2ζD0 σ ζD0 ζD1 σ ζD0 ζL0 σ ζD0 ζL1
depressive symptoms; Ds = latent slope for depressive symptoms
σ ζD0 ζD1 σ 2ζD1 σ ζD1 ζL0 σ ζD1 ζL1
LI = latent intercept for loneliness; LS = latent slope for loneliness; Cov½ζ  ¼
u = residual score for depressive symptoms; v = residual score for σ ζD0 ζL0 σ ζD1 ζL0 σ 2ζL0 σ ζL0 ζL1
loneliness; a1, a2, and a3 = individual varying ages of measurement σ ζD0 ζL1 σ ζD1 ζL1 σ ζL0 ζL1 σ 2ζL1
centered at age 65; 1 indicate fixed paths; * indicate parameter
estimates. (3)
Under the current hypothesis that minority stress and
and covariances) effects describe the distributions of
social threat operate in tandem for sexual minority
each LGM parameter based on chronological age
status groups, the expectation is that the correlation
(McArdle, Ferrer-Caja, Hamagami, & Woodcock,  qffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffi
2002). As age at first measurement varied across indi- between intercepts rζD0 ζL0 ¼ σ ζD0 ζL0 = σ 2ζD0 σ 2ζL0
viduals, participants’ ages at each measurement were  qffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffi
scaled relative to an origin of 65 years and treated as and slopes rζD1 ζL1 ¼ σ ζD1 ζL1 = σ 2ζD1 σ 2ζL1 are stron-
definition variables, which are variables that fix model ger for sexual minority men and women compared to
parameters to individual data values by estimating heterosexual men and women, respectively.
CLINICAL GERONTOLOGIST 177

Next, we regressed the intercepts and slopes on within and between measurements, depressive
annual household income and family support to symptoms scores (labeled CES-D) in the MSM
test the hypothesis that effects of income and group were slightly higher than the MSW group
family support on initial and linear slopes are and were nearly equal in magnitude as the WSW
stronger in sexual minority groups than in hetero- group. CES-D scores were nearly identical across
sexual groups. both female (WSM and WSW) groups. CES-D
All models were fit in Mplus 8.0 (Muthén & scores declined over waves in the MSW and the
Muthén, 1998–2017) using full-information max- WSW groups while scores were nonlinear across
imum likelihood with robust standard errors to waves in the MSM and WSM groups.
account for missing data under the MAR assump- For loneliness (labeled UCLA), scores were
tion (Enders, 2010; Little, 1995), nonindependence nonlinear across groups, with WSM and WSW
of observations (i.e., clustering of respondents and groups reporting higher scores at wave 1, but
their partners) and violations of multivariate nor- WSW reporting lower scores than WSM at wave
mality. Given the complex survey data design of 3. In the male groups, MSM reported higher scores
NSHAP, standard errors, and model fit statistics at wave 2 and wave 3 compared to MSW, on
(e.g., chi-square estimates) take into account the average.
non-independence of clustered observations. The Finally, the MSM group reported slightly less
Wald χ2 test was used to compare parameter esti- family support than all other groups, and men
mates. Given the relatively large number of statis- reported higher annual household incomes than
tical tests performed, we used a conservative alpha women, with MSW reporting the highest annual
level of .01. income and WSM the lowest annual income.
Figure 2 presents the longitudinal trajectory
plots of depressive symptoms (a) and loneliness
Results (b) scores by age. The thin, colored lines represent
individual participants’ trajectories while the thick
Descriptive results
colored lines are smoothing lines for each sexual
Table 1 presents the means and standard devia- orientation group. CES-D scores are relatively
tions of depressive symptoms and loneliness across constant across ages, with the exception that
the three waves. Unadjusted for age differences MSM participants reported increases in symptoms

Table 1. Descriptive statistics.


Men Women
n M SD Min, Max n M SD Min, Max
Sexual Minority Status Groups
CES-D T1 80 1.49 0.39 1, 2.6 58 1.50 0.50 1, 3.1
CES-D T2 59 1.43 0.38 1, 2.6 43 1.45 0.48 1, 3.7
CES-D T3 42 1.48 0.50 1, 3.1 34 1.44 0.37 1, 2.5
UCLA T1 75 3.91 1.36 3, 9 50 4.20 1.39 3, 8
UCLA T2 59 4.27 1.62 3, 8 41 4.34 1.39 3, 7
UCLA T3 40 4.05 1.55 3, 8 35 3.97 1.50 3, 9
Family Support T1 84 2.64 1.16 0, 5 60 2.83 0.99 0, 5
Household Income T1 77 54,481.39 53,479.48 6000, 350,000 48 47,922.92 43,135.76 6000, 250,000
Heteronormative Groups
CES-D T1 1180 1.41 0.44 1, 3.9 1208 1.50 0.47 1, 3.7
CES-D T2 918 1.37 0.39 1, 3.2 940 1.46 0.44 1, 3.2
CES-D T3 604 1.37 0.37 1, 2.9 673 1.48 0.45 1, 3.4
UCLA T1 1035 3.89 1.30 3, 9 1110 4.18 1.48 3, 9
UCLA T2 794 4.07 1.47 3, 9 833 4.26 1.49 3, 9
UCLA T3 575 3.85 1.29 3, 9 654 4.19 1.45 3, 9
Family Support T1 1214 2.79 1.12 0, 5 1300 2.95 1.04 0, 5
Household Income T1 1009 60,724.36 85,496.97 0, 1,800,000 904 42,656.42 70,352.09 60, 1,000,000
Notes. CES-D = 10-item center for epidemiological depression scale; UCLA = 3-item UCLA loneliness scale; T1 = intake measurement; T2 and T3
equal wave 2 and wave 3 follow-up scores, respectively; household income reported in USD.
178 C. R. BEAM AND E. M. COLLINS

Figure 2. Longitudinal trajectories of CES-D depression (a) and UCLA loneliness (b) scores for each group. MSW = men who have sex
with women; WSM = women who have sex with men; MSM = men who have had sex with men; WSW = women who have had sex
with women.

beginning around age 80 while WSM reported low – around the same values as MSW – and increases
decreases in symptoms at about the same age. after age 80. For WSW, the trajectories decline with
MSW and WSW show slight linear increases age, starting higher than all other groups and ending
with age. in late adulthood with the lowest level of loneliness.
For loneliness, scores tend to be low, with partici- The trajectory of MSW is relatively flat.
pants’ clustering at the minimum value of three, sug-
gesting a large group of participants who do not
report feelings of loneliness. On average, however, Multivariate results
the trajectory of WSM tended to be higher than all Table 2 presents the growth parameters for depres-
other groups and monotonically increases starting sive symptoms and loneliness for each sexual
around the mid-70s. The trajectory of MSM begins orientation group. Intercepts (γD0 and γL0) were
CLINICAL GERONTOLOGIST 179

Table 2. Bivariate linear growth curve model parameter inadmissible – standard error of 2.32 of the latter.
estimates. For WSM and WSW, the correlations between
WSM MSW MSM WSW
intercepts were .73 and .81 while the correlations
Fixed Effects Est SE Est SE Est SE Est SE
γD0 1.47 0.02 1.40 0.01 1.48 0.05 1.48 0.07
between slopes were .80 and .97, respectively. For
γD1 0.04 0.01 0.03 0.01 -0.02 0.04 0.02 0.06 the women, the pattern of differences, however,
γL0 4.17 0.05 3.97 0.04 3.89 0.17 4.35 0.19 was as predicted by both models.
γL1 0.07 0.04 0.04 0.04 0.25 0.16 -0.12 0.15
Variance Components Finally, we tested whether annual household
Level-1 income and family support accounted for indivi-
σ2εL 0.93 0.04 0.93 0.04 0.93 0.04 0.93 0.04 dual differences in the individual growth para-
σ2εD 0.08 0.00 0.08 0.00 0.08 0.00 0.08 0.00
Level-2 meters of depressive symptoms and loneliness.
ζ D0 0.14 0.01 0.11 0.01 0.09 0.03 0.14 0.08 Parameter estimates are presented in Table 3.
ζ D1 0.04 0.01 0.01 0.01 0.04 0.08 0.07 0.06
ζ L0 1.26 0.11 1.03 0.11 1.25 0.46 1.00 0.30 Annual household income did not predict differ-
ζ L1 0.37 0.08 0.04 0.09 0.51 0.51 0.32 0.65 ences in the intercepts and (Wald χ2 = 9.46, df = 4,
σζD0 ζD1 -0.03 0.01 -0.01 0.01 -0.02 0.04 -0.05 0.06 p = .051) or slopes (Wald χ2 = 2.96, df = 4,
σζD0 ζL0 0.31 0.03 0.25 0.03 0.25 0.11 0.30 0.10
σζD0 ζL1 -0.08 0.03 -0.02 0.03 -0.10 0.14 -0.13 0.15 p = .565), although the former was marginally
σζD1 ζL0 -0.08 0.03 -0.04 0.03 -0.08 0.10 0.02 0.12 significant. Family support did not predict differ-
σζD1 ζL1 0.09 0.02 0.01 0.02 0.13 0.16 0.15 0.20
σζL0 ζL1 -0.29 0.09 -0.05 0.08 -0.29 0.48 -0.07 0.34
ences in the intercepts (Wald χ2 = 7.50, df = 4,
Notes. γ = fixed effects; ζ = random effects; D0 = depressive symptom p = .112) or slopes (Wald χ2 = 8.91, df = 4,
intercept; D1 = depressive symptom linear slope; L0 = loneliness p = .062), although the latter was marginally sig-
intercept; L1 = loneliness linear slope; nificant. A few regression effects in Table 3, how-
ever, are worthy of comment despite marginal
statistically significant; however, these values did statistical significance. First, annual household
not significantly differ when set equal within gen- income had the largest influence on the intercepts
der, that is MSW equals MSM and WSM equals of depressive symptoms and loneliness in the
WSW (Wald χ2 = 5.62, df = 4, p = .229). The MSM group, particularly for depressive symptoms.
depressive symptom slope for WSM was statisti- Second, greater family support predicted lower
cally significant while the slope for MSW trended initial scores for depressive symptom in MSW
toward significant (p = .025). The loneliness slope and WSM, but not in the MSM or WSW groups.
effects for WSM and MSM also were marginally Third, family support had the largest effect on the
significant (p = .077 and .109, respectively). Given intercept of loneliness in the WSW group, as the
the relatively small magnitudes of the slopes across effect was nearly 2.2 times larger in this group
groups, significant differences were not observed compared to MSW and WSM.
when set equal within gender (Wald χ2 = 6.77,
df = 4, p = .149). Furthermore, MSM individual
growth parameters did not differ from WSW Discussion
(Wald χ2 = 6.08, df = 4, p = .193), suggesting no Trajectories of depressive symptoms and loneliness
differences between sexual minority men and in sexual minority men and women do not signifi-
women. There was, however, a statistically signifi- cantly differ from their heterosexual counterparts,
cant difference between male and female growth contrasting with previous cross-sectional studies
parameters for depressive symptoms and loneli- (Cochran & Mays, 2000; Gilman et al., 2001;
ness (Wald χ2 = 42.03, df = 12, p < .001). Mercer et al., 2007). Differences that were observed
Next, we tested the correlations between the emerged along gender lines. Effects of family sup-
intercepts and slopes of depressive symptoms and port and annual household income on initial scores
loneliness across sexual orientation and observed suggest sexual minority status groups have better
nonsignificant group differences (Wald χ2 = 0.24, outcomes when social and economic support
df = 4, p = .993). For MSW and MSM, the correla- mechanisms are in place.
tions between intercepts were .74 and .75, respec- We posited that a combination of negative
tively, while the correlations between slopes were expectations associated with minority stress and
.60 and .97, respectively, with a large – and likely sustained vigilance toward social threats of harm
180 C. R. BEAM AND E. M. COLLINS

Table 3. Intercepts and slopes as outcomes parameter There is little doubt that sexual minority
estimates. groups experience forms of stress not experi-
WSM MSW MSM WSW
enced by heterosexual groups in the form of
Fixed Effects Est SE Est SE Est SE Est SE
bias, discrimination, and harassment (Meyer,
γD0 1.63 0.05 1.54 0.04 1.39 0.10 1.76 0.23
γD1 -0.01 0.04 -0.04 0.03 0.26 0.17 0.24 0.18 2003). The similarity between growth parameters
γL0 4.78 0.16 4.44 0.12 4.14 0.44 5.56 0.54 across sexual orientations may suggest that older
γL1 -0.09 0.13 -0.06 0.10 0.76 0.52 0.01 0.53
γ2D0 -0.13 0.02 -0.10 0.03 -0.25 0.07 -0.13 0.11
adult sexual minority group members marshal
γ2D1 0.03 0.02 0.02 0.02 0.13 0.07 0.11 0.10 resources to cope with minority stress in ways
γ2L0 -0.34 0.05 -0.21 0.07 -0.84 0.23 -0.33 0.26 that cause them to resemble heterosexual
γ2L1 0.09 0.05 0.01 0.04 0.12 0.23 0.21 0.22
γ3D0 -0.05 0.02 -0.05 0.01 0.05 0.04 0.09 0.06 groups – at least with respect to depressive
γ3D1 0.01 0.01 0.02 0.02 -0.10 0.06 -0.08 0.05 symptoms and loneliness. The current findings
γ3L0 -0.19 0.05 -0.16 0.04 -0.07 0.15 -0.42 0.17
γ3L1 0.04 0.04 0.03 0.03 -0.19 0.18 -0.03 0.16
differ from differences in loneliness found
Variance Components between Dutch sexual minority status and het-
Level-1 erosexual groups (Fokkema & Kuyper, 2009), a
σ2εL 0.93 0.04 0.93 0.04 0.93 0.04 0.93 0.04
σ2εD 0.08 0.00 0.08 0.00 0.08 0.00 0.08 0.00 difference possibly attributed to clearer definition
Level-2 of sexual minority status than in the current
ζ D0 0.12 0.01 0.10 0.01 0.07 0.02 0.13 0.07
ζ D1 0.04 0.01 0.01 0.01 0.01 0.02 0.02 0.05
study. Further research in U.S. sexual minority
ζ L0 1.12 0.11 0.95 0.10 1.01 0.25 0.71 0.25 samples may elucidate how sexual minority
ζ L1 0.37 0.08 0.06 0.09 0.23 0.27 0.03 0.09 groups manage bias and discrimination to
σζD0 ζD1 -0.03 0.01 -0.01 0.01 0.01 0.02 -0.04 0.03
σζD0 ζL0 0.26 0.03 0.22 0.03 0.22 0.03 0.22 0.03 further understand the null results observed here.
σζD0 ζL1 -0.07 0.03 -0.03 0.02 -0.03 0.02 -0.03 0.02 Depressive symptom levels are stable across
σζD1 ζL0 -0.07 0.03 -0.02 0.02 -0.02 0.02 -0.02 0.02 older adulthood if only slightly increasing
σζD1 ζL1 0.09 0.02 0.02 0.02 0.02 0.02 0.02 0.02
σζL0 ζL1 -0.26 0.08 -0.05 0.08 -0.15 0.22 -0.02 0.54 (Beekman et al., 2002; Fiske et al., 2009; Kessler,
Notes. γ = fixed effects; ζ = random effects; D0 = depressive symptom Foster, Webster, & House, 1992), which we
intercept; D1 = depressive symptom linear slope; L0 = loneliness inter- observed in sexual minorities, too. Despite slight
cept; L1 = loneliness linear slope; γ2 = within-person regression weight
for annual household earnings (grand mean-centered log units); positive linear slopes in heterosexual men and
γ3 = within-person regression weight for self-reported family support. women, the trajectory plots illustrate similarity in
depressive symptom trends across all groups.
would cause sexual minorities to experience Further, lack of differences in the trajectories of
increases in depressive symptoms and loneliness depressive symptoms does not necessarily imply
with increasing age. The null differences observed that clinical treatment is the same. Symptom struc-
across sexual status in the current study contrast tures may differ in sexual minority groups from
with findings in three epidemiological studies heterosexual groups (Nesselroade & Molenaar,
(Cochran & Mays, 2000; Gilman et al., 2001; 2016), and future research should consider etiolo-
Mercer et al., 2007). A key difference between gical differences in depressive symptoms and lone-
these studies and NSHAP is the age of measure- liness between sexual minority status groups and
ment. All three studies focused on early middle age heterosexual groups.
(30s and 40s) whereas the NSHAP focuses on late There were similar loneliness trends between
adulthood. One possibility is that all older adults, sexual minority men and heterosexual women,
regardless of sexual status, experience fewer potentially suggesting that loss of male partners
depressive symptoms and loneliness overall and among older adults may explain the rise in their
little fluctuation of each in late adulthood. loneliness levels with age. As men have greater
Indeed, the current findings fit well with findings mortality risk in general (Austad, 2006), and sex-
that indicate few group differences in depressive ual minority men in particular (Cochran & Mays,
symptoms (Blazer, 2003; Fiske et al., 2009) 2011), heterosexual women and sexual minority
and little to no increase in both depressive symp- men who live to old-old age may be at greater
toms (Fiske et al., 2009) and loneliness (Victor risk of loneliness as they lose partners and friends
et al., 2005). to death.
CLINICAL GERONTOLOGIST 181

Post-hoc probing of the data suggests that gen- (Kockler & Heun, 2002) or possibly experience
der differences occurred in the initial levels of symptoms more intensely (Fujita, Diener, &
depressive symptoms and loneliness, but not the Sandvik, 1991). Each of these explanations, how-
linear slopes. The rank order differences between ever, is speculative, as none could be directly tested
men and women fall along gender lines (Fiske, in the current report.
Gatz, & Pedersen, 2003; Fiske et al., 2009; Finally, the current findings help to clarify how
Jansson et al., 2004; Nolen-Hoeksema, Larson, & financial and family support might reduce stress in
Grayson, 1999) and loneliness (Aartsen & Jylhä, the lives of sexual minority men and women.
2011; Berg et al., 1981; Borys & Perlman, 1985; Higher levels of annual income and family support
Shankar, Hamer, & Steptoe, 2017). Notably, how- predicted lowest levels of depressive symptoms
ever, similarity in rates of change of depressive and loneliness in sexual minority status men and
symptoms and loneliness were observed across women. Annual income had the strongest effects
gender, consistent with findings in other large on initial levels of depressive symptoms and lone-
population-based samples (Cacioppo et al., 2006; liness in sexual minority men while higher family
Fiske et al.; Hawkley & Kocherginsky, 2018; support predicted lowest initial levels of loneliness
Kessler et al., 1992). in sexual minority women. For sexual minority
Minority stress and social threat may explain groups, greater earning power and stronger family
why women tended to report higher levels of support may counterbalance stress incurred via
depressive symptoms and loneliness. Women discrimination and prejudice based on sexual
may experience bias, prejudice, and stigma asso- orientation (Meyer, 2003), providing needed cop-
ciated with femininity, which could be sources of ing resources.
chronic stress and heightened vigilance toward Aging studies that include sexual minority sta-
social threats not experienced by men (Fiske, tus groups are rare, with longitudinal studies even
Bersoff, Borgida, Deaux, & Heilman, 1991; rarer. The primary strength of the current study,
Rudman & Glick, 2001). The accumulation of thus, is examining trajectories of depressive symp-
stress and sustained vigilance toward threats across toms and loneliness of older adult sexual minority
decades of experience may not cause men and men and women against their heterosexual coun-
women to diverge in depressive symptoms and terparts. To our knowledge, this is the first study
loneliness in older adulthood, but rather could be to evaluate longitudinal change in depressive
a source of stable differences between them symptoms and loneliness in older adult sexual
(Bromberger & Matthews, 1996). In support of minorities. An additional strength of the current
this hypothesis is that the correlations between study is the representativeness of the NSHAP sam-
true initial levels of depressive symptoms and ple, as the results can be safely generalized to the
loneliness are nearly equal, suggesting only differ- U.S. population of older adult sexual minority men
ences in the means. and women.
Several alternative explanations may account for There are several limitations to the current
why women experience greater initial levels of study. First, the assessment of participants’ history
depressive symptoms and loneliness. First, the of same-sex sexual partners used to operationalize
stress and strain hypotheses posit that women sexual minority groups is inadequate in general,
experience greater stress and strain over their but may be especially inadequate in older adult
lives that, in turn, causes greater symptom reports samples. The items may select on decades old
(Nolen-Hoeksema et al., 1999), consistent with sexual exploration in participants who later iden-
minority stress models. Second, the recurrence of tify as heterosexual. Future studies should directly
depression hypothesis posits that men and women assess sexual orientation.
are equally likely to develop depressive symptoms A second limitation is the specification of the
initially, but women are more likely to experience model selected. While latent growth curve models
subsequent symptoms (Hankin et al., 1998). Third, are useful for evaluating linear change, an autore-
women may be more comfortable disclosing true gressive model specifying time constant and time-
levels of depressive symptoms compared to men varying effects also may be appropriate (Steyer,
182 C. R. BEAM AND E. M. COLLINS

Schmitt, & Eid, 1999). Additionally, there may be Funding


two different trends in loneliness – one for young-
This work was supported by the National Institute on Aging
old age and one for old-old age. The benefit of a [T32AG000037].
latent growth curve modeling, however, is the
ability to handle heterogeneity in ages and num-
bers of measurement, although we note that the References
assumption that the same growth pattern across
older adulthood may not hold. Aartsen, M., & Jylhä, M. (2011). Onset of loneliness in older
adults: Results of a 28 year prospective study. European
A third limitation is that measuring depressive
Journal of Ageing, 8(1), 31–38. doi:10.1007/s10433-011-
symptomatology may miss important differences 0175-7
between sexual minority and heterosexual groups, as Adams, K. B., Sanders, S., & Auth, E. A. (2004). Loneliness
group differences in late adulthood often are minimal and depression in independent living retirement commu-
(Blazer, 2003). Further, use of the CES-D is limited by nities: Risk and resilience factors. Aging & Mental Health,
its emphasis on depressed mood as opposed to psy- 8(6), 475–485. doi:10.1080/13607860410001725054
Agerbo, E., Stack, S., & Petersen, L. (2011). Social integration
chomotor symptoms (Gay, Kottorp, Lerdal, & Lee,
and suicide: Denmark, 1906–2006. The Social Science
2016). Journal, 48(4), 630–640. doi:10.1016/j.soscij.2011.06.004
A final limitation is that we limited our study to Aggarwal, S., & Gerrets, R. (2014). Exploring a Dutch para-
two covariates, annual household income and dox: An ethnographic investigation of gay men’s mental
family support. These variables were chosen health. Culture, Health and Sexuality, 16, 105–119. Taylor
because of their salience in the lives of sexual & Francis. doi:10.1080/13691058.2013.841290
Austad, S. N. (2006). Why Women Live Longer Than Men:
minorities (Meyer, 2003). Other covariates also
SexDifferences in Longevity. Gender Medicine, 3(2), 79–92.
may matter, like education, friend support, partner doi:10.1016/S1550-8579(06)80198-1
(living alone) status, and physical health variables. Beekman, A. T. F., Geerlings, S. W., Deeg, D. J., Smit, J. H.,
Schoevers, R. S., De Beurs, E., . . . Van Tilburg, W. (2002).
The natural history of late-life depression. Archives of
General Psychiatry, 59(2002), 605–611. doi:10.1001/
Clinical implications archpsyc.59.7.605
Berg, S., Mellstrom, D., Persson, G., & Svanborg, A. (1981).
(1) Clinicians should be cautious about Loneliness in the Swedish aged. Journals of Gerontology, 36
assuming that sexual minority groups’ (3), 342–349. doi:10.1093/geronj/36.3.342
clinical presentation of depressive symp- Björkenstam, C., Andersson, G., Dalman, C., Cochran, S., &
toms and loneliness is different by virtue Kosidou, K. (2016). Suicide in married couples in Sweden:
of their sexual preference. Is the risk greater in same-sex couples? European Journal
of Epidemiology, 31(7), 685–690. doi:10.1007/s10654-016-
(2) Women may experience unique forms
0154-6
of stress that explain higher self- Blazer, D. G. (2003). Depression in late life: Review and
reported depressive symptoms and commentary. The Journals of Gerontology Series A:
loneliness. Biological Sciences and Medical Sciences, 58, 249–265.
(3) Support characteristics, like financial doi:10.1093/gerona/58.3.M249
resources and family support, should Borys, S., & Perlman, D. (1985). Gender differences in lone-
liness. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 11(1),
be considered when conducting case
63–74. doi:10.1177/0146167285111006
conceptualizations of sexual minority Bromberger, J. T., & Matthews, K. A. (1996). A “feminine”
men and women. model of vulnerability to depressive symptoms: A long-
itudinal investigation of middle aged women. Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology, 70, 591–598.
doi:10.1037/0022-3514.70.3.591
Brown, M. T., & Grossman, B. R. (2014). Same-sex sexual
relationships in the national social life, health and aging
Disclosure statement project: Making a case for data collection. Journal of
Gerontological Social Work, 57(2–4), 108–129.
No potential conflict of interest was reported by the authors. doi:10.1080/01634372.2013.865695
CLINICAL GERONTOLOGIST 183

Bryk, A. S., & Raudenbush, S. W. (1987). Application of B: Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences, 58(6), P320–
hierarchical linear models to assessing change. P328. doi:10.1093/geronb/58.6.P320
Psychological Bulletin, 101(1), 147–158. doi:10.1037/0033- Fiske, A., Wetherell, J. L., & Gatz, M. (2009). Depression in
2909.101.1.147 older adults. Annual Review of Clinical Psychology, 5, 363–
Cacioppo, J. T., Cacioppo, S., & Boomsma, D. I. (2014). 389. doi:10.1146/annurev.clinpsy.032408.153621
Evolutionary mechanisms for loneliness. Cognition & Fiske, S. T., Bersoff, D. N., Borgida, E., Deaux, K., & Heilman,
Emotion, 28(1), 3–21. doi:10.1080/02699931.2013.837379 M. E. (1991). Social science research on trial: Use of sex
Cacioppo, J. T., Hawkley, L. C., & Thisted, R. A. (2010). Perceived stereotyping research in Price Waterhouse v. Hopkins.
social isolation makes me sad: 5-year cross-lagged analyses of American Psychologist, 46, 1049–1060. doi:10.1037/0003-
loneliness and depressive symptomatology in the Chicago 066X.46.10.1049
Health, Aging, and Social Relations Study. Psychology and Fokkema, T., & Kuyper, L. (2009). The relation between social
Aging, 25(2), 453–463. doi:10.1037/a0017216 embeddedness and loneliness among older lesbian, gay, and
Cacioppo, J. T., Hughes, M. E., Waite, L. J., Hawkley, L. C., & bisexual adults in the Netherlands. Archives Sexual Behavioral,
Thisted, R. A. (2006). Loneliness as a specific risk factor for 38, 264–275. doi:10.1007/s10508-007-9252-6
depressive symptoms: Cross-sectional and longitudinal Fredriksen-Goldsen, K. I., Emlet, C. A., Kim, H. J., Muraco,
analyses. Psychology and Aging, 21(1), 140–151. A., Erosheva, E. A., Goldsen, J., & Hoy-Ellis, C. P. (2013).
doi:10.1037/0882-7974.21.1.140 The physical and mental health of lesbian, gay male, and
Carstensen, L. L. (1992). Social and emotional patterns in bisexual (LGB) older adults: The role of key health indica-
adulthood. Psychology and Aging, 7(3), 331–338. tors and risk and protective factors. Gerontologist, 53(4),
doi:10.1037/0882-7974.7.3.331 664–675. doi:10.1093/geront/gns123
Cochran, S. D., & Mays, V. M. (2000). Relation between Fredriksen-Goldsen, K. I., Kim, H.-J., Barkan, S. E., Muraco, A., &
psychiatric syndromes and behaviorally defined sexual Hoy-Ellis, C. P. (2013). Health disparities among Lesbian, Gay,
orientation in a sample of the US population. American and Bisexual older adults: Results from a population-based
Journal of Epidemiology, 151(5), 516–523. doi:10.1093/ study. American Journal of Public Health, 103(10), 1802–
oxfordjournals.aje.a010238 1809. doi:10.2105/AJPH.2012.301110
Cochran, S. D., & Mays, V. M. (2011). Sexual orientation and Fujita, F., Diener, E., & Sandvik, E. (1991). Gender differences in
mortality among US men aged 17 to 59 years: Results from negative affect and well-being: The case for emotional intensity.
the National Health and Nutrition Examination Survey III. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 61(3), 427–434.
American Journal of Public Health, 101(6), 1133–1138. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.61.3.427
doi:10.2105/AJPH.2010.300013 Gay, C., Kottorp, A., Lerdal, A., & Lee, K. (2016).
Coyne, J. C. (1976). Toward an interactional description of Psychometric limitations of the Center for Epidemiologic
depression. Psychiatry, 39(1), 28–40. doi:10.1080/ Studies-Depression Scale for assessing depressive symp-
00332747.1976.11023874 toms among adults with HIV/AIDS: A Rasch analysis.
Domènech-Abella, J., Elvira, L., Rubio-Valera, M., Olaya, B., Depress Researcher Treatment, 1–11.
Moneta, M. V., Rico-Uribe, L. A., . . . Haro, J. M. (2017). Gilman, S. E., Cochran, S. D., Mays, V. M., Hughes, M., Ostrow,
Loneliness and depression in the elderly: The role of social D., & Kessler, R. C. (2001). Risk of psychiatric disorders among
network. Social Psychiatry and Psychiatric Epidemiology, 52 individuals reporting same-sex sexual partners in the national
(4), 381–390. doi:10.1007/s00127-017-1339-3 comorbidity survey. American Journal of Public Health, 91(6),
Enders, C. K. (2010). Applied missing data analysis. New 933–939. doi:10.2105/AJPH.91.6.933
York, NY: The Guilford Press. Hankin, B. L., Abramson, L. Y., Moffitt, T. E., Silva, P. A.,
Feinstein, B. A., Davila, J., & Dyar, C. (2017). A weekly diary McGee, R., & Angell, K. E. (1998). Development of depres-
study of minority stress, coping, and internalizing symp- sion from preadoles- cence to young adulthood:
toms among gay men. Journal of Consulting and Clinical {Emerging} gender differences in a 10-year longitudinal
Psychology, 85(12), 1144–1157. doi:10.1037/ccp0000236 study. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 107(1), 128–140.
Finkel, D., Reynolds, C. A., McArdle, J. J., Gatz, M., & doi:10.1037/0021-843x.107.1.128
Pedersen, N. L. (2003). Latent growth curve analyses of Hawkley, L. C., & Kocherginsky, M. (2018). Transitions in
accelerating decline in cognitive abilities in late adulthood. loneliness among older adults. Research on Aging, 40(4),
Developmental Psychology, 39(3), 535–550. doi:10.1037/ 265–387. doi:10.1177/0164027517698965
0012-1649.39.3.535 Jacobs, R. J., & Kane, M. N. (2012). Correlates of loneliness in
Fiori, K. L., & Jager, J. (2012). The impact of social support midlife and older gay and bisexual men. Journal of Gay
networks on mental and physical health in the transition to and Lesbian Social Services, 24(1), 40–61. doi:10.1080/
older adulthood: A longitudinal, pattern-centered approach. 10538720.2012.643217
International Journal of Behavioral Development, 36(2), 117– Jansson, M., Gatz, M., Berg, S., Johansson, B., Malmberg, B.,
129. doi:10.1177/0165025411424089 McClearn, G. E., . . . Pedersen, N. L. (2004). Gender differ-
Fiske, A., Gatz, M., & Pedersen, N. L. (2003). Depressive ences in heritability of depressive symptoms in the elderly.
symptoms and aging: The effects of illness and non- Psychological Medicine, 34, 471–479. doi:10.1017/
health-related events. The Journals of Gerontology, Series S0033291703001375
184 C. R. BEAM AND E. M. COLLINS

Kalmar, S., Szanto, K., Rihmer, Z., Mazumdar, S., Harrison, Nesselroade, J. R., & Molenaar, P. C. M. (2016). Some beha-
K., & Mann, J. J. (2008). Antidepressant prescription and viorial science measurement concerns and proposals.
suicide rates: Effect of age and gender. Suicide and Life- Multivariate Behavioral Research, 51(2–3), 396–412.
Threatening Behavior, 38(4), 363–374. doi:10.1521/ doi:10.1080/00273171.2015.1050481
suli.2008.38.4.363 Nolen-Hoeksema, S., Larson, J., & Grayson, C. (1999).
Kessler, R. C., Foster, C., Webster, P. S., & House, J. S. (1992). The Explaining the gender difference in depressive symptoms.
relationship between age and depressive symptoms in two Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 77(5), 1061–
national surveys. Psychology and Aging, 7(1), 119–126. 1072. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.77.5.1061
Retrieved from http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/ Radloff, L. S. (1977). The CES-D scale: A self-report depres-
1558696 sion scale for research in the general population. Applied
Kim, H. J., & Fredriksen-Goldsen, K. I. (2016). Living Psychological Measurement, 1(3), 385–401. doi:10.1177/
arrangement and loneliness among lesbian, gay, and bisex- 014662167700100306
ual older adults. Gerontologist, 56(3), 548–558. Rudman, L. A., & Glick, P. (2001). Prescriptive gender
doi:10.1093/geront/gnu083 stereotypes and backlash toward agentic women.
Kockler, M., & Heun, R. (2002). Gender differences of Journal of Social Issues, 57(4), 743–762. doi:10.1111/
depressive symptoms in depressed and nondepressed 0022-4537.00239
elderly persons. International Journal of Geriatric Russell, D. W. (1996). UCLA loneliness scale (version 3):
Psychiatry, 17(1), 65–72. doi:10.1002/gps.521 Relability, validity, and factor structure. Journal of
Kuyper, L., & Fokkema, T. (2010). Loneliness among older Personality Assessment, 66(1), 20–40. doi:10.1207/
lesbian, gay, and bisexual adults: The role of minority s15327752jpa6601
stress. Archives Sexual Behavioral, 39, 1171–1180. Shankar, A., Hamer, M., & Steptoe, A. (2017). Social isolation
doi:10.1007/s10508-009-9513-7 and loneliness : Prospective associations with functional
Little, R. J. (1995). Modeling the drop-out mechanism in status in older adults. Health Psychology, 36(2), 179–187.
repeated-measures studies. Journal of the American doi:10.1037/hea0000437
Statistical Association, 90(431), 1112–1121. doi:10.1080/ Singer, J. D., & Willett, J. B. (2003). Applied longitudinal data
01621459.1995.10476615 analysis: Modeling change and event occurrence (1st ed.).
McArdle, J. J., & Anderson, E. (1990). Latent variable growth Oxford, NY: Oxford University Press.
models for research on aging. In J. E. Birren & K. W. Schaie Sonnenberg, C. M., Beekman, A. T., Deeg, D. J., & Van
(Eds.), Handbook of the psychology of aging (3rd ed., pp. 21–44). Tilburg, W. (2000). Sex differences in late-life depres-
San Diego, CA: Academic Press. sion. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica, 101(4), 286–292.
McArdle, J. J., Ferrer-Caja, E., Hamagami, F., & Woodcock, Retrieved from http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/
R. W. (2002). Comparative longitudinal structural analyses 10782548
of the growth and decline of multiple intellectual abilities Steyer, R., Schmitt, M., & Eid, M. (1999). Latent state-trait
over the life span. Developmental Psychology, 38(1), 115– theory and research in personality and individual differ-
142. doi:10.1037/0012-1649.38.1.115 ences. European Journal of Personality, 13, 389–409.
McArdle, J. J., & Hamagami, F. (1992). Modeling incomplete doi:10.1002/(SICI)1099-0984(199909/10)13:5<389::AID-
longitudinal and cross-sectional data using latent growth PER361>3.0.CO;2-A
structural models. Experimental Aging Research, 18(3), Stickley, A., & Koyanagi, A. (2016). Loneliness, common
145–166. doi:10.1080/03610739208253917 mental disorders and suicidal behavior: Findings from a
McDonald, R. P. (1999). Test theory: A unified treatment. general population survey. Journal of Affective Disorders,
Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. 197, 81–87. doi:10.1016/j.jad.2016.02.054
Mehta, P. D., & West, S. G. (2000). Putting the individual Victor, C. R., Scambler, S. J., Bowling, A. N. N., & Bond, J.
back into individual growth curves. Psychological Methods, (2005). The prevalence of, and risk factors for, loneliness
5(1), 23–43. Retrieved from http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/ in later life: A survey of older people in Great Britain.
pubmed/10937321 Ageing and Society, 25(6), 357–375. doi:10.1017/
Mercer, C. H., Bailey, J. V., Johnson, A. M., Erens, B., S0144686X04003332
Wellings, K., Fenton, K. A., & Copas, A. J. (2007). Victor, C. R., Scambler, S. J., Shah, S., Cook, D. G., Harris, T.,
Women who report having sex with women: British Rink, E., & De Wilde, S. (2002). Has loneliness amongst
national probability data on prevalence, sexual behaviors, older people increased? An investigation into variations
and health outcomes. American Journal of Public Health, between cohorts. Ageing & Society, 22(5), 585–597.
97(6), 1126–1133. doi:10.2105/AJPH.2006.086439 doi:10.1017/S0144686X02008784
Meyer, I. H. (2003). Prejudice, social stress, and mental health Waite, L. J., Laumann, E. O., Levinson, W., Lindau, S. T.,
in lesbian, gay, and bisexual populations: Conceptual McClintock, M. K., & O’Muircheartaigh, C. A. (2007). A
issues and research evidence. Psychological Bulletin, 129 national social life, health, and aging project (NSHAP):
(5), 674–697. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.129.5.674 Wave 1, July 2005–March 2006. Ann Arbor, MI: Inter-
Muthén, L. K., & Muthén, B. O. (1998–2017). Mplus user’s University Consortium for Political and Social Research.
guide. Los Angeles, CA: Muthén & Muthén. doi:10.3886/ICPSR20541.v8.

You might also like