You are on page 1of 11

Veterinary Record: first published as 10.1136/vr.104619 on 14 February 2018. Downloaded from http://veterinaryrecord.bmj.com/ on 20 May 2018 by guest. Protected by copyright.

Paper

Ethical dilemmas encountered by small


animal veterinarians: characterisation,
responses, consequences and beliefs
regarding euthanasia
Barry Kipperman,1 Patricia Morris,2 Bernard Rollin3

Small animal veterinarians’ opinions were investigated regarding the frequency and nature of ethical dilemmas
encountered, beliefs regarding euthanasia and balancing client and animal interests, prevalence and value of
ethics training and proposals to mitigate the stressful effects of ethical dilemmas. The majority (52 per cent)
of 484 respondents in the USA indicated via an online survey experiencing an ethical dilemma regarding the
interests of clients and those of their patients at least weekly. Scenarios involving client financial concerns were
commonly reported causes of ethical conflicts. While only 20 per cent of respondents indicated that other prac-
titioners prioritise patient interests, 50 per cent of respondents characterised their own behaviour as prioritising
patients. Most respondents (52 per cent) reported that ethical dilemmas are the leading cause, or are one of many
equal causes, of work-related stress. Less experienced practitioners, general practitioners and associate veteri-
narians were more likely to encounter situations they defined as ethical dilemmas, and female respondents were
more likely to find ethical dilemmas stressful. Most small animal veterinarians experience ethical dilemmas regu-
larly, which contribute to moral stress. Results suggested that most small animal practitioners believe that greater
awareness of moral stress and providing training in ethical theories and tools for coping with ethical dilemmas
can ameliorate moral stress.

Introduction Ethics4 are also ambiguous regarding professional advo-


Small animal veterinarians in practice encounter cacies asserting as the first principle that, 'The choice
conflicts between satisfying the interests of the pet of treatments or animal care shall not be influenced by
owner and those of the animal.1 2 The American Veter- considerations other than the welfare of the patient,
inary Medical Association (AVMA) Veterinarian’s Oath3 the needs of the client, and the safety of the public’, yet
provides no guidance in prioritisation of a veterinar- declaring under supporting annotations that ‘Veteri-
ian’s varied obligations: “…I… swear to use my scien- narians should first consider the needs of the patient’
tific knowledge … for the benefit of society through the (American Veterinary Medical Association Principles).
protection of animal health and welfare,…the promo- In contrast to these US codes of conduct, the UK code
tion of public health, and the advancement of medical of conduct more explicitly advises an animal advo-
knowledge" (American Veterinary Medical Associa- cacy position: 'On occasions, the professional respon-
tion oath). The AVMA Principles of Veterinary Medical sibilities in the Code may conflict with each other and
veterinary surgeons may be presented with a dilemma.
In such situations, veterinary surgeons should balance
Veterinary Record (2018) doi: 10.1136/vr.104619 the professional responsibilities, having regard first to
animal welfare’ (Royal College of Veterinary Surgeons
1
Department of Veterinary Medicine E-mail for correspondence:
and Surgery, College of Veterinary ​kippermanb@​missouri.​edu (RCVS) Code of Professional Conduct for Veterinary
Medicine, University of Missouri, Provenance and peer review Not
Surgeons and Supporting Guidance).5
Columbia, Missouri, USA commissioned; externally peer Ethical dilemma arise when there are competing
2
Department of Sociology, California reviewed. interests of equal moral weight, and there is no obvi-
State University, Sacramento,
ous way to prioritise one responsibility over others.6 In
California, USA Received July 18, 2017
3
Department of Philosophy, Colorado a study of ethical dilemmas, 57 per cent of veterinari-
Revised December 17, 2017
State University, Fort Collins, Colorado, Accepted January 28, 2018 ans in the UK reported facing 1–2 ethical dilemmas per
USA week, and 34 per cent of practitioners encountered an

Vet RecorD |  1
ethical dilemma 3–5 times per week.7 Moreover, 78 per views of practitioners regarding euthanasia decisions;
cent of the veterinarians in that study felt that they were determine the methods veterinarians use to resolve eth-

Veterinary Record: first published as 10.1136/vr.104619 on 14 February 2018. Downloaded from http://veterinaryrecord.bmj.com/ on 20 May 2018 by guest. Protected by copyright.
not given enough, or in many cases any, ethics edu- ical dilemmas; determine which interest veterinarians
cation during their training. Results of another study8 prioritise when client and patient interests conflict; as-
indicate that only 30 per cent of AVMA Counsel On Ed- sess measures that might reduce the impact of ethical
ucation accredited veterinary schools in the USA that dilemmas; document the prevalence of ethics training
responded to a survey, reported offering a formal course and the influence of such training on managing ethical
in ethics. The non-specific attributions within the US or- dilemmas and evaluate differences among veterinari-
ganisational principles (AVMA oath, AVMA principles) ans regarding ethical dilemmas on the basis of gender,
regarding how to prioritise satisfying the interests of an- practice role (owner vs associate), practice type (refer-
imals, pet owners and the veterinarian, and the global ral vs other), years of practice experience, age or num-
dearth of ethics instruction in training programmes,7–11 ber of veterinarians in the practice.
provide little direction to veterinarians in navigating
ethical conflicts. Materials and methods
Bernard Rollin12 13 defines a unique type of angst Survey design
experienced by veterinarians as a result of ethical di- A pilot survey was developed and administered to
lemmas as moral stress. Moral stress arises from the a select group of 12 small animal practitioners who
tension between what one feels one ought to be doing evaluated the survey for clarity and completeness,
or the ideal of the professional, and reality. Kipperman and the survey was revised and refined on the basis of
and others14 documented that 49 per cent of small ani- their feedback. The Human Ethical Review Committee
mal practitioners in the USA reported moderate-to-sub- at the University of Edinburgh approved the survey
stantial burnout. In a survey in the UK, 83 per cent of in May 2016. The final survey consisted of 5 sections
veterinarians agreed that veterinary medicine was a and 29 questions and took approximately 20 min to
stressful occupation.15 Decisions regarding euthanasia complete. The first section provided a number of clin-
are common causes of moral stress for veterinarians, ical scenarios which were selected because they may be
especially if economic considerations are perceived to associated with ethical dilemmas, and focused on how
be the basis for these determinations.14 16–23 A veterinar- often these situations are encountered; the percentage
ian may choose to comply with euthanasia requests, of these encounters that would be classified as ethical
dissuade the client or decline such requests.17 21 Yeates dilemmas; the degree of moral stress these cause and
and Main24 identified that 97 per cent of veterinarians how often any type of ethical dilemma between satis-
in the UK reported refusing euthanasia requests less fying the interests of clients and patients is encoun-
often than yearly, suggesting that such decisions are tered. The second section posed questions regarding
uncommon. use of euthanasia as an aid or method to resolve ethical
With regard to the responsibility of the veterinarian dilemmas or difficult cases when this may not be in
when faced with ethical conflicts, Main25 has argued the best interest of the patient; whether euthanasia of
for a patient advocacy position: "… The obligation to animals using acceptable methods is always an ethical
the animal’s best interest is greater than a concern for procedure regardless of reason or circumstance; the
the psychological well-being of the client. If we placed frequency with which client requests for euthanasia
a higher value on clients’ sensitivities, then our duty are declined and reasons for reluctance to decline
as animal advocates would not be fulfilled". However, euthanasia requests. The third section focused on the
results of a US survey26 demonstrate that as veterinar- values of respondents via ranking of varied interests
ians age, willingness to comply with client’s wishes when making decisions that involve ethical dilemmas,
increased relative to the interests of patients. Given the and which interests are prioritised when animal and
importance of decisions associated with ethical dilem- client interests are at odds. The fourth section posed
mas to animal welfare, animal owners and veterinari- questions regarding methods used to address ethical
ans, there is limited scientific literature examining how dilemmas; the prevalence of approaches to address
veterinarians balance or prioritise client and animal in- ethical dilemmas in training programmes and the value
terests when these are at odds, the relationship between of such training in addressing ethical dilemmas. The
specific clinical situations veterinarians encounter and final section focused on the impact of ethical dilemmas
stress7 27 or the opinions of small animal veterinarians on work-related stress, and the potential of varied
regarding the ethical context of euthanasia.22 24 28 29 proposals to ameliorate the consequences of such
The purpose of the small animal practitioner survey stress. The questionnaire is available on request to the
reported here was to assess the frequency with which corresponding author.
ethical dilemmas between client and patient interests
occur; identify some of the common circumstances that Survey distribution
cause ethical dilemmas; determine the degree of mor- An email containing an invitation to participate in
al stress elicited by these conditions; characterise the the survey and a link to the survey was distributed to

2 | Vet Record
4100 members of the California Veterinary Medical TABLE 1: Frequency with which small animal veterinarians encounter an
Association, 4400 members of the Humane Society

Veterinary Record: first published as 10.1136/vr.104619 on 14 February 2018. Downloaded from http://veterinaryrecord.bmj.com/ on 20 May 2018 by guest. Protected by copyright.
ethical dilemma between satisfying the interests of their clients and those
Veterinary Medical Association, 93 members of the of their patients
Society for Veterinary Medical Ethics and 1309 small Frequency Percentage

animal specialists of the American College of Veteri- Multiple times daily 13.1
nary Internal Medicine. Those who belonged to more Once a day 5.8

than one organisation received multiple invitations to A few times a week 25.6
Once a week 7.7
the survey. Veterinarians without small animal experi-
ence were excluded from participation. The survey was A few times a month 23.9
Once a month 6.0
conducted from September 30, 2016 to November 3,
A few times a year 14.1
2016, and participation in the survey was voluntary
Once a year 2.1
and anonymous.
Every few years 1.2
Never 0.4
Data analysis
Responses to survey questions were tabulated and cent), mixed animal (1.2 per cent) and academic prac-
descriptive statistics (number and percentage) were tice (1 per cent); 4.5 per cent of respondents reported
generated. Responses were compared on the basis of work in a non-listed category.
year of graduation from veterinary school, practice role Of the 484 respondents, 31 per cent were categorised
(owner vs associate), respondent gender, practice type as referral practitioners (RP: defined as practitioners
(referral vs non-referral), year of birth, number of veter- in emergency, specialty or academic type), and 69 per
inarians in the practice and self-reported patient advo- cent were categorised as general practitioners (GP). Of
cacy. The Wilcoxon rank-sum statistical test and Mann- 481 respondents, 33 per cent identified themselves as
Whitney U test were used for comparisons between two practice owners and 67 per cent identified themselves
groups in which the dependent variable was ordinal, as associate or relief veterinarians (ie, non-owners),
and Spearman's correlation coefficient was used to and 80 per cent were female and 20 per cent were male.
quantify the magnitude of the relationship between The median number of years in practice for the re-
ordinal variables. Independent samples t tests were spondents was 20 (range, 0–53 years). The median age
used for comparisons between two groups in which the of respondents was 50 years (range, 25–86 years). The
dependent variable was continuous, and the Pearson's median number of full-time practitioners affiliated with
correlation coefficient was used to quantify the magni- each respondent’s practice was 3 (mean=8).
tude of the relationship between continuous variables.
Logistic regression was performed when multiple inde- Frequency of ethical dilemmas
pendent variables were used to predict the value of a The frequency with which veterinarians encountered
dichotomous dependent variable. All analyses were any type of ethical dilemma between satisfying the
performed with statistical software (IBM SPSS Statistics interests of their clients and those of their patients is
for Windows, V.22) and values of P<0.05 were consid- shown in Table 1. The median frequency with which
ered to be statistically significant. practitioners encountered ethical dilemmas was
once a week, and 19 per cent of respondents reported
Results encountering such an ethical dilemma at least once a
Study participants day. Female respondents (21.4 per cent) were more
Responses were received from 600 veterinarians; 116 than twice as likely as males (9.3 per cent) to report
respondents were excluded because they failed to experiencing frequent (at least once a day) ethical
answer at least 50 per cent of the survey questions. dilemmas. A logistical analysis (Table 2) was performed
Thus, responses from 484 veterinarians were evalu-
ated. One hundred and eighty (37 per cent) respond- TABLE 2: Logistic regression model on the likelihood of a respondent
ents were members of the Humane Society Veterinary experiencing ethical dilemmas at least once per day.

Medical Association, 135 (28 per cent) were members OR


Variable B se Sig. Exp(B)
of the California Veterinary Medical Association, 127
  (Intercept) −0.805 0.343 0.019 0.447
(26 per cent) were members of the American College of
Gender (ref: male) 0.078 0.332 0.814 1.082
Veterinary Internal Medicine and 23 (5 per cent) were Female
members of the Society for Veterinary Medical Ethics. Ownership (ref: yes) 0.618 0.299 0.039 1.855
The largest proportion (45 per cent) of respondents No

reported working in small animal general practice, while Practice type (ref: RP) 1.057 0.304 0.001 2.882
General practitioners
27.5 per cent reported working in specialty or referral Years of experience (ref: >15) 0.911 0.301 0.002 2.487
practice; other practice types included shelter medi- <15 years
cine (6.2 per cent), relief (5.6 per cent), mobile (3.9 per Model χ2=32.576, df=4, P<0.001.

cent), emergency (2.9 per cent), feline only (1.9 per RP, referral practitioner.

Vet RecorD |  3
TABLE 3: Frequency with which the respondents encounter clinical scenarios associated with ethical dilemmas

Veterinary Record: first published as 10.1136/vr.104619 on 14 February 2018. Downloaded from http://veterinaryrecord.bmj.com/ on 20 May 2018 by guest. Protected by copyright.
Financial limitations compromise Euthanasia based on lack of Euthanasia based on Therapeutic trial elected instead
Frequency patient care (%) means (%) unwillingness to treat (%) of testing (%)
Multiple times a day 36.4 1.0 0 23.4
Once a day 11.8 2.5 1.2 9.5
A few times a week 27.7 7.9 2.1 28.6
Once a week 6.6 8.3 2.9 8.1
A few times a month 9.7 17.8 12.4 18.0
Once a month 2.9 13.2 10.1 4.5
A few times a year 3.3 33.5 40.1 4.8
Once a year 0.0 4.8 11.0 1.0
Every few years 0.4 7.4 12.6 0.2
Never 1.0 3.7 7.6 1.9

to determine whether gender or career trajectory varia- limitations at least once or multiple times per day.
bles best predict the probability of reporting frequent Euthanasia requests perceived to be based on lack
ethical dilemmas. Associates, GP and those with less of financial means occurred with a reported median
veterinary work experience were each associated with frequency of once a month, followed by euthanasia
an increased probability of reporting frequent ethical requests where the respondent believed this decision
dilemmas. Maintaining all other covariants constant, was due to client unwillingness to pay for treatment,
the odds of reporting frequent ethical dilemmas were with a median frequency of a few times a year.
1.855 times greater for associates as compared with The percentage of clinical scenarios which veterinar-
owners (P=0.04), 2.882 times greater for GP relative ians classified as ethical dilemmas is shown in Table 4.
to RP (P=0.00) and 2.487 times greater for less experi- While 29 per cent of respondents reported that econom-
enced (under 15 years) veterinarians as compared with ic limitations compromising patient care were consid-
their more experienced counterparts (P=0.00). Gender ered an ethical dilemma at least 50 per cent of the time,
was analytically and statistically insignificant after the 39 per cent considered having to perform an empirical
inclusion of these other variables. therapeutic trial instead of diagnostic testing because
of costs or owner preference was considered an ethical
Commonly encountered ethical dilemmas dilemma at least 50 per cent of the time, 51 per cent of
The frequencies with which veterinarians encounter respondents felt that euthanasia requests which were
specific clinical scenarios, which may be associ- perceived to be based on lack of financial means were
ated with ethical dilemmas are presented in Table 3. considered an ethical dilemma at least 50 per cent of
Scenarios involving owner financial concerns were the the time and 66 per cent of respondents reported that
most commonly reported causes of potential ethical euthanasia requests believed to be due to client unwill-
conflicts. Client financial limitations compromising ingness to pay for treatment were considered an ethical
patient care and having to perform empirical ther- dilemma at least 50 per cent of the time.
apeutic trials instead of diagnostic testing because
of costs or owner preference, were reported to occur Comparisons between RP and GP regarding ethical dilem-
most often, both with a median reported frequency of mas
a few times a week. Approximately three-quarters of Regarding client financial limitations compromising
respondents reported encountering economic limita- patient care, GP reported significantly (P=0.011) greater
tions compromising patient care at least a few times median frequency of such encounters (once a day) than
per week, and 48 per cent reported experiencing these RP (a few times a week), GP indicated significantly

TABLE 4: The percentage of clinical scenarios which respondents classified as ethical dilemmas.
Percentage classified as ethical Financial limitations compromise Euthanasia based on lack of Euthanasia based on Therapeutic trial elected instead
dilemmas patient care (%) means (%) unwillingness to treat (%) of testing (%)
90%–100% 5.8 19.3 36.5 3.6
80%–89% 3.8 8.3 6.4 2.9
70%–79% 6.5 10.0 9.8 7.4
60%–69% 3.5 4.0 4.0 6.5
50%–59% 9.2 9.1 8.9 18.7
40%–49% 5.2 4.4 3.8 7.2
30%–39% 10.0 5.5 5.3 11.4
20%–29% 12.1 7.6 4.7 14.3
<20% 39.0 26.9 14.6 23.4
Never 4.8 4.9 5.9 4.6

4 | Vet Record
(P=0.001) more encounters of this nature as an ethical the interests of clients and animals conflict, 57 per
dilemma (median=20 per cent–29 per cent) than did cent of respondents characterised the client as being

Veterinary Record: first published as 10.1136/vr.104619 on 14 February 2018. Downloaded from http://veterinaryrecord.bmj.com/ on 20 May 2018 by guest. Protected by copyright.
RP (median=<20 per cent) and moral stress ratings for prioritised, whereas 20 per cent reported clients and
this circumstance for GP were significantly (P=0.001) patients were equally prioritised, 20 per cent reported
greater than the corresponding ratings for RP (median the interests of the patient as being prioritised and 3 per
response for both groups was ‘moderate’). In general, cent felt the veterinarian’s interest was prioritised. Half
GPs were more likely to report greater moral stress of the respondents characterised their own behaviour
from the presented scenarios causing ethical dilemmas as prioritising the interests of the patient when the
compared with RP. interests of clients and animals conflict, whereas 32 per
cent reported prioritising clients and patients equally,
Beliefs regarding euthanasia 16 per cent reported prioritising the client’s interest,
When asked to indicate their level of agreement on while only 2 per cent reported to prioritise their own
a 5-point Likert scale with the statement, 'Veterinar- interests.
ians sometimes use euthanasia as an aid or method There was no significant (P=0.15) difference between
to resolve difficult cases when this may not be in the RP (patient=24 per cent) and GP (patient=17 per cent)
best interest of the patient’, 45 per cent of respondents regarding the perception of which interest is prioritised
agreed and 37 per cent disagreed. The level of agree- by colleagues when the interests of clients and animals
ment did not differ significantly between male (agree) conflict. There were no significant differences regard-
and female (neutral; P=0.16) respondents, based on ing which interest respondents prioritised when the
years of experience (P=0.45, Spearman’s r=0.035), or interests of clients and animals conflict between male
between RP (agree) and GP (neutral; P=0.08). When (patient=46 per cent) and female (patient=52 per cent;
respondents were asked whether they had ever used P=0.34) respondents, between owners (patient=52 per
euthanasia as an aid or method to resolve difficult cent) and associates (patient=50 per cent; P=0.77), be-
cases when it may not be in the best interest of the tween RP (patient=52 per cent) and GP (patient=50 per
patient, 42 per cent of practitioners reported that they cent; P=0.82) or based on years of experience (P=0.44,
had done this at least once in their career. Most (83 per Spearman’s r=0.035). Respondents who characterised
cent) respondents did not agree that the euthanasia of their own behaviour as prioritising patient interests
animals using acceptable methods is always an ethical were significantly (P=0.03) more likely to report eutha-
procedure regardless of reason or circumstance. Opin- nasia requests perceived to be based on lack of finan-
ions regarding whether the euthanasia of animals cial means as an ethical dilemma (median response was
using acceptable methods is always an ethical proce- 50 per cent–59 per cent) and were significantly (P=0.01)
dure regardless of reason or circumstance did not differ more likely to rate these situations as stressful (median
significantly (P=0.20) between male (agree=22 per response was ‘high’), compared with respondents se-
cent) and female (agree=16 per cent) respondents, lecting all other advocacy behaviours (corresponding
between RP (agree=15 per cent) and GP (agree=18 per median responses were 40 per cent–49 per cent, and
cent; P=0.42) or based on years of experience (P=0.13, ‘moderate’), respectively.
Pearson’s r=0.079).
The majority (80 per cent) of respondents reported Resolution of ethical dilemmas and ethics training
to have declined a euthanasia request at some point in Using a 6-point ordinal scale from 1=never to
their career; of these, 52 per cent reported that they de- 6=very frequently, respondents reported how often
cline euthanasia requests every few years, while 32 per they used the following methods to address ethical
cent reported to decline such requests at least a few dilemmas; my gut instinct based on my personal value
times a year. The most common reasons respondents system (median rating was 6), consultation with friends
provided for reluctance to decline euthanasia requests or colleagues (median rating was 5), guidance from
were fear that the client may seek other options which my training (median rating was 5), examples set by
could worsen the animal’s welfare (65.3 per cent), diffi- my colleagues (median rating was 4), guidance from
culty declining such requests once a client had reached written policies of state or national veterinary organ-
this decision (50.8 per cent) and concern that declining isations (median rating was 4) and consideration of
euthanasia requests may jeopardise respondent rela- varied ethical theories (median rating was 3) was
tionship with the client (15.1 per cent). Respondents used least often. The proportion of respondents who
who reported prioritising patient interests relative to reported having received training in their curriculum
client interests were significantly (P<0.00) more likely regarding approaches to address ethical dilemmas
to decline euthanasia requests. was nearly evenly split, with 51 per cent of respond-
ents receiving ethics training. Those respondents who
Balancing client against animal interests reported receiving ethics training were asked whether
When asked for their opinion regarding which interest they believed such training adequately prepared them
is prioritised by other small animal practitioners when to address ethical dilemmas in practice. On a 5-point

Vet RecorD |  5
TABLE 5: Moral stress ratings of respondents regarding clinical scenarios associated with ethical dilemmas.

Veterinary Record: first published as 10.1136/vr.104619 on 14 February 2018. Downloaded from http://veterinaryrecord.bmj.com/ on 20 May 2018 by guest. Protected by copyright.
Financial limitations compromise Euthanasia based on lack of Euthanasia based on Therapeutic trial elected instead
Moral stress rating patient care (%) means (%) unwillingness to treat (%) of testing (%)
Very high 9.0 18.9 35.2 2.6
High 14.9 26.0 20.4 7.2
Moderate 37.2 28.3 24.2 34.8
Low 23.9 13.0 7.3 33.7
Very low 11.6 9.6 5.8 15.6
None 3.4 4.3 7.1 6.2

ordinal scale ranging from strongly agree to strongly the scenario where euthanasia is requested because the
disagree, 39 per cent of respondents agreed that respondent believes that the client is unwilling to pay
such instruction prepared them to address the ethical for treatment.
dilemmas they encounter, 38 per cent were neutral Opinions regarding the effect of ethical dilemmas
regarding the benefits of ethics instruction in addressing on work-related stress included attributing ethical di-
ethical dilemmas and 23 per cent of respondents disa- lemmas as ‘the leading cause’ (9.1 per cent), as ‘one of
greed that ethics training prepared them to address many equal causes’ (42.4 per cent), ‘moderate contribu-
ethical dilemmas. tion, but other things affect stress more’ (32.3 per cent),
‘minimal contribution, many other things affect stress
Effect of ethical dilemmas on moral stress more’ (16.0 per cent) and ‘no effect’ (0.2 per cent).
Respondents were asked to rate the degree of moral Logistic regression was used (Table 6) to examine the
stress elicited by varied clinical scenarios associated factors that predict when ethical dilemmas were consid-
with ethical dilemmas (see Table 5). Most respond- ered a leading source of stress, that is, when respond-
ents characterised the clinical scenarios associated ents considered ethical dilemmas as either ‘the leading
with client economic concerns as causing at least a cause’ or ‘one of many equal causes’. Variables includ-
moderate degree of moral stress, except for the scenario ed respondent gender, career-related variables such as
of having to perform therapeutic trials based on cost or ownership status, work experience and whether the
client preference. Slightly less than half (45 per cent) of respondent is RP or GP, whether or not the respondent
the respondents reported moderate to very high stress reports to experience ethical dilemmas at least once a
scores (median response was ‘low’) when having to day and whether or not the respondent prioritises the
perform empirical therapeutic trials instead of diag- patient when client and patient interests conflict. Re-
nostic testing based on cost or client preference; 61 per sults were statistically significant for gender (P<0.00),
cent of respondents reported moderate to very high years of experience (P=0.04) and frequency of ethical
stress scores (median response was ‘moderate’) when dilemmas (P<0.00). Maintaining all other covariants
encountering economic limitations compromising constant, the odds of considering ethical dilemmas a
patient care; 73 per cent of respondents reported leading source of stress were 3.041 times greater for
moderate to very high stress scores (median response females relative to males, 1.565 times greater for less
was ‘moderate’) when encountering euthanasia experienced veterinarians relative to more experienced
requests believed to be due to lack of financial means and 2.87 times greater for those who report to expe-
and 80 per cent of respondents reported moderate to rience ethical dilemmas at least once a day relative to
very high stress scores (median response was ‘high’) for those who report experiencing them less often.

TABLE 6: Logistic regression model on the likelihood of respondents defining ethical dilemmas as a leading cause of work-related stress
OR
Variable B se Sig. Exp(B)
 (Intercept) −0.889 0.318 0.005 0.411
Gender (ref: male) 1.112 0.260 0.000 3.041
Female
Ownership (ref: yes) −0.228 0.233 0.216 0.750
No
Practice type (ref: RP) 0.178 0.216 0.408 1.195
General practitioners
Years of experience (ref: >15) 0.448 0.223 0.045 1.565
<15 years
Frequency of dilemma (ref: <1/day) 1.054 0.263 0.000 2.870
Greater or=once per day
Conflicting interest (ref: client) 0.228 0.195 0.243 1.256
Prioritising patient
RP, referral practitioner.

6 | Vet Record
TABLE 7: Percentages of small animal practitioners who indicated the respondent believed this decision was due to client
unwillingness to pay for treatment, was a few times a

Veterinary Record: first published as 10.1136/vr.104619 on 14 February 2018. Downloaded from http://veterinaryrecord.bmj.com/ on 20 May 2018 by guest. Protected by copyright.
effective or very effective opinions regarding various proposals for
reducing moral stress from ethical dilemmas. year. However, 22 per cent of respondents reported ex-
Proposal Percentage periencing one or both types of economic euthanasia at
Training and tools for coping with ethical dilemmas 83.9 least once a week.
Greater awareness of moral stress and burnout 83.9 Female respondents were more likely than males to
Increased access to support services 77.8 report experiencing ethical dilemmas, and were much
Greater awareness of costs of veterinary care among pet owners 74.9
more likely to experience them at least once a day.
Greater acceptance that it is ethical to decline euthanasia requests 66.2
However, almost all of this relationship was explained
Greater acceptance that veterinarians are limited by wishes of pet owner 57.7
by the lower rate of practice ownership, fewer years of
Ethical guidelines from veterinary organisations 53.9
work experience and decreased probability of being a
referral veterinarian, among females. In the present
Proposals for mitigation of moral stress survey, more experienced practitioners were less likely
Opinions regarding various proposals for reducing to report the scenarios we provided as an ethical dilem-
moral stress are presented in Table 7. The proposals that ma. It is reasonable to presume that with experience a
most respondents indicated would be effective or very practitioner would develop improved moral reasoning
effective in reducing moral stress among veterinarians skills, which is the process by which people determine
were greater awareness within the profession of moral that one course of action is morally right and another
stress and burnout (83.9 per cent), modify veterinary course of action is morally wrong. However, moral rea-
school curricula to include specific training in ethical soning scores among veterinarians in the UK did not
theories and tools for coping with ethical dilemmas improve with years of experience, which disputes this
(83.9 per cent), increased access to counselling and assumption.30 Alternatively, experienced practitioners
support services (77.8 per cent) and greater aware- over time may become desensitised to circumstances
ness of the costs of veterinary care among pet owners that novice colleagues may find ethically problematic.
(74.9 per cent); 38 respondents cited other options. Contrary to the findings of previous research of small
animal veterinarians in the USA14 where RP were more
Discussion likely to encounter financial limitations than GP, refer-
The majority (52 per cent) of the small animal practi- ral practitioners in this survey encountered financial
tioners in the USA that responded to the present survey limitations affecting patient care less frequently and
reported experiencing an ethical dilemma between were less likely to consider these situations an ethical
satisfying the interests of their clients and those of their dilemma when encountered, compared with GP. One
patients at least once a week, with 19 per cent reporting explanation for this finding is that RP may encounter
encountering ethical dilemmas at least daily. Among fewer financial limitations because they may see a se-
the clinical scenarios provided, the two most common lect subset of clients who are more motivated, and may
dilemmas respondents encountered included client be more capable of committing financial resources to-
financial limitations compromising patient care and wards veterinary care. GPs were more likely to report
having to perform empirical therapeutic trials instead greater moral stress from the presented ethical dilem-
of diagnostic testing because of costs or owner prefer- mas compared with RP. As GP often have long-standing
ence, with a median response for both of a few times a relationships with their clients, it is possible that they
week. These results concur with studies that found that empathise more with the plight of the client compared
Danish and the US small animal veterinarians encoun- with RP.
tered economic limitations regularly.14 23 Although More respondents in the present survey agreed
only 29 per cent of respondents considered financial (45 per cent) than disagreed (37 per cent) that veteri-
limitations to patient care to be an ethical dilemma narians use euthanasia as an aid or method to resolve
the majority of the time, the absolute number of these difficult cases when this may not be in the best interest
conflicts is still high given the frequency with which of the patient, and 42 per cent of practitioners report-
this type of encounter was reported. Moreover, financial ed that they had done this at least once in their career.
limitations to care may lead to economic euthanasia at Rollin13 suggests that euthanasia is a double-edged
a later time, which most respondents considered to be sword, referring to its potential benefit in ending termi-
an ethical dilemma at least 50 per cent of the time these nal suffering, but noting that it can also be performed in
situations are encountered. circumstances that when scrutinised, appear less com-
Regarding the frequency with which small animal pulsory. The AVMA Guidelines for the Euthanasia of
practitioners encounter economic euthanasia, results Animals31 are ambiguous regarding the basis of eutha-
were mixed. The median frequency of requests for eu- nasia decisions, noting, 'Impacts on animals may not
thanasia where the respondent perceived this to be always be the center of the valuation process, and there
based on lack of financial means was once a month, is disagreement on how to account for conflicting inter-
and the median response for euthanasia requests where specific interests'. The RCVS Code of Conduct5 is more

Vet RecorD |  7
specific on this issue: 'The veterinary surgeon's primary may change their attitudes towards euthanasia over
obligation is to relieve the suffering of an animal, but time, coming to perceive it as more acceptable.20 21 35

Veterinary Record: first published as 10.1136/vr.104619 on 14 February 2018. Downloaded from http://veterinaryrecord.bmj.com/ on 20 May 2018 by guest. Protected by copyright.
account must be taken not only of the animal's condi- Corroborating this theory, a study by Ogden and oth-
tion, but also the owner's wishes and circumstances’ ers28 in the UK suggests that tolerance of convenience
(Royal College of Veterinary Surgeons Code of Profes- euthanasia may increase over time as students in their
sional Conduct for Veterinary Surgeons and Supporting early stages of study were found to score lower on an
Guidance). acceptance scale of convenience euthanasia compared
Convenience euthanasia refers to circumstances with students in their clinical years and recently gradu-
where the primary rationale for this request is based on ated veterinarians.
human needs such as economics, changes in personal The second most common reason cited by the re-
circumstances, lack of time, capacity or desire to care spondents in this survey for reluctance to decline eu-
for the animal, rather than animal welfare.7 28 32 Results thanasia requests was the difficulty in doing so once a
of a study of Canadian practitioners by Rathwell-Deault client had reached this decision. The AVMA Guidelines
and others32 indicate that decisions regarding conveni- for the Euthanasia of Animals31 encourage deliberation
ence euthanasia were made mostly by considering the and discussion rather than acquiescence: 'There may
veterinarian's and the client's/owner's interests. Our be instances in which the decision to kill an animal is
findings suggest that small animal practitioners some- questionable, especially if the animal is predicted to
times perform euthanasia when this is not justified have a life worth living…In this case, the veterinarian,
based on animal welfare considerations. as…animal advocate, should be able to speak frankly
Although practitioners admitted performing eutha- about the animal’s condition and suggest alternatives
nasia when this may not serve animal interests, only to euthanasia’. In accord with other research on US
17 per cent of respondents believed that euthanasia of veterinarians,21 multiple respondents commented that
animals using acceptable methods is always an ethical they felt discouraged in exercising their discretion re-
procedure regardless of reason or circumstance. Eutha- garding refusal of euthanasia requests and sensed peer
nasia derives from the Greek roots of ‘a good death’ and pressure from employers or hospital administrators to
in human semantics is restricted to circumstances of comply with euthanasia requests that conflicted with
mercy killing, in which death is viewed as a respite from their personal values. These findings suggest there may
inevitable suffering.33 The AVMA Guidelines on Eutha- be benefit to training in veterinary curricula emphasis-
nasia in 200734 defined euthanasia solely by method ing navigating challenging end-of-life decisions, and in
and outcome: ‘…The act of inducing humane death in particular, encouraging students to feel comfortable ad-
an animal’. In 2013, the AVMA Guidelines for the Eu- vocating for alternatives to euthanasia when this may
thanasia of Animals31 expanded this limited designa- not be in an animal’s interest.
tion and define euthanasia by method (‘A good death is A fundamental ethical problem is whether veterinar-
… the humane termination of an animal’s life’), and by ians should give primary consideration to the animal or
intention (‘… ending the life of an … animal in a way that to the client.18 36–38 However, a barrier discouraging the
… eliminates pain and distress'). Our finding that most animal advocacy position is economic; the veterinarian
respondents reject the notion that euthanasia is always is dependent on the pet owner who pays for veterinary
an ethical procedure regardless of rationale, suggests services.25 Results of a survey39 of Australian veterinary
that small animal veterinarians in the USA repudiate students supported an animal advocacy posture, as
method-based, value-free definitions of euthanasia. An 96 per cent agreed that, "My primary focus as a veteri-
intention-based definition of euthanasia that centres on narian should be the interests of all animals in my care".
benefiting the recipient may provide clearer guidance to In the present survey, only 20 per cent of respondents
veterinarians as they confront euthanasia decisions. indicated that they believed other practitioners priori-
While 80 per cent of practitioners in this survey in- tise the interests of the patient, and only 50 per cent of
dicated having declined a euthanasia request, these respondents described their own behaviour as prioritis-
decisions were uncommon (median frequency was ing patient interests. These findings raise questions re-
every few years). The most common reason cited by re- garding whether the majority of small animal veterinar-
spondents for reluctance to decline euthanasia requests ians in the USA see their professional role as primarily
was fear that the client may seek other options, which advocates for animals.
could worsen the animal’s welfare. Studies by Yeates In attempting to explain the disparate findings be-
and Main24 and Morris21 confirm our finding that fear tween self-reported advocacy behaviours in contrast
may be the main reason why some veterinarians are re- to the perception of the same behaviours of their col-
luctant to decline euthanasia requests, and suggest this leagues, research by Bazerman and Gino40 has docu-
concern acts as a rationalisation to help practitioners mented an inconsistency in the way we judge our own
cope with difficult end-of-life decisions in which they dubious behaviours relative to those of others, a ten-
may feel guilt or another negative emotion. A potential dency known as moral hypocrisy. A study by Batson
consequence of such reasoning is that veterinarians and others41 on moral hypocrisy demonstrated that

8 | Vet Record
individuals perceived their own transgressions to be very high stress ratings were reported by 45 per cent of
less objectionable than the same transgression enact- respondents who encountered having to perform em-

Veterinary Record: first published as 10.1136/vr.104619 on 14 February 2018. Downloaded from http://veterinaryrecord.bmj.com/ on 20 May 2018 by guest. Protected by copyright.
ed by another. Research also suggests that although we pirical therapeutic trials instead of diagnostic testing
recognise others’ conflicts of interest, we fail to recog- based on cost or client preference, and 61 per cent of re-
nise conflicts of interest that we face that may lead to spondents indicated moderate to very high stress when
unethical behaviour.42 In addition, Joseph and others43 faced with economic limitations compromising patient
found that self-reporting of one’s own ethical behav- care. Higher levels of stress were reported to be asso-
iour is significantly correlated with exaggeration of re- ciated with economic euthanasia, as 73 per cent of re-
sponse. These theories may apply to our findings if our spondents reported moderate to very high stress associ-
respondents perceived animal advocacy as inherently ated with euthanasia requests believed to be due to lack
more ethical than client advocacy. Our findings raise of financial means, and 80 per cent of respondents in-
the need for additional study on this subject perhaps dicated moderate to very high stress associated with cir-
requiring ethnographic investigative techniques.21 37 We cumstances where euthanasia was requested because
were surprised to find no statistically significant differ- the practitioner believed that the client was unwilling
ence in self-reported advocacy pattern based on years to pay for treatment.
of experience; we had surmised that patient advocacy Although respondents reported encountering eco-
would decline with experience, as studies by Paul and nomic euthanasia infrequently (once a month to a few
Podberscek,44 Schoenfeld-Tacher and others45 and Co- times a year), our findings suggest that the moral con-
lombo and others46 have indicated declining empathy text of euthanasia requests as perceived by the veteri-
for animals as veterinary students progress through narian may be associated with degree of reported stress,
training programmes. as practitioner belief that the client can afford treatment
When asked to consider methods they rely on when but instead chooses euthanasia generated more moral
encountering ethical dilemmas, respondents indicated stress than the perception that the client is unable to
that gut instinct based on their personal value system afford treatment. The comparatively lower prevalence
was the most common method used, while guidance of reported stress when confronting economic con-
from written policies of state or national veterinary straints not associated with euthanasia may reflect an
organisations, and consideration of varied ethical the- acquiescence that such circumstances are accepted or
ories, were least often used. These findings are inter- considered as normal within small animal practice,
esting in that they tell us something about how veteri- economic limitations may be perceived as the sole re-
narians approach ethical dilemmas, but how much one sponsibility of the pet owner rather than a consequence
technique influences another is unknown. For example, of the veterinarian’s actions, fees or hospital policies,7
neither the researchers nor the respondents can easily or stress may simply be perceived as more significant
separate out how much ethics training influences a vet- when economic limitations are directly associated with
erinarian’s ‘gut instinct’ or personal approach to resolv- a patient’s death.21
ing ethical dilemmas. The majority (52 per cent) of respondents in this
We were surprised to find that only 39 per cent of survey reported that ethical dilemmas were the leading
respondents who had received ethics instruction in vet- cause, or were one of many equal causes of work-relat-
erinary school agreed that such training prepared them ed stress. Female respondents were significantly more
to address ethical dilemmas. These findings suggest a likely than males to report ethical dilemmas as a source
need for future studies to further explore veterinarians’ of stress. The reported impact of ethical dilemmas on
opinions about ethics training, as it seems reasonable stress decreased with increased experience. These find-
to assume that veterinarians are influenced by such in- ings are consistent with those of other studies,7 47–53
struction, but our data suggest that the training is not which indicated that female and less experienced vet-
necessarily seen as especially salient to ethical deci- erinarians had a higher prevalence of serious psycho-
sion-making. While the majority of our respondents logical distress or found ethical dilemmas more stress-
who received ethics instruction were skeptical of its ful compared with all veterinarians. Given that the vast
effectiveness for resolving dilemmas, it is important to majority of those entering the veterinary profession in
note that we did not inquire about the nature and ex- the USA and the UK are female,54 55 the findings that
tent of ethics training respondents received, why our women were more likely to experience ethical dilem-
respondents found their training ineffective at this task mas and were more likely to report such dilemmas as
or how they would evaluate the merits of their ethics stressful, warrants efforts to mitigate these detrimental
training. experiences, especially early in their careers. Most re-
Most respondents in the present survey character- spondents indicated that they support modifications to
ised all clinical scenarios associated with client eco- veterinary school curricula to include specific training
nomic limitations as causing at least a moderate degree in ethical theories and tools for coping with ethical di-
of moral stress, except for having to perform therapeu- lemmas, and efforts to increase awareness within the
tic trials based on cost or client preference. Moderate to profession of moral stress and burnout. This finding is

Vet RecorD |  9
interesting in that it represents a paradox, when most 7 Batchelor CE, McKeegan DE. Survey of the frequency and perceived stressfulness
of ethical dilemmas encountered in UK veterinary practice. Vet Rec 2012;170:19.
respondents also felt that their ethics training had not

Veterinary Record: first published as 10.1136/vr.104619 on 14 February 2018. Downloaded from http://veterinaryrecord.bmj.com/ on 20 May 2018 by guest. Protected by copyright.
8 Shivley CB, Garry FB, Kogan LR, et al. Survey of animal welfare, animal behavior,
prepared them well to address ethical dilemmas. and animal ethics courses in the curricula of AVMA Council on Education-accredited
veterinary colleges and schools. J Am Vet Med Assoc 2016;248:1165–70.
One of the limitations of this study was that the vol- 9 Self DJ, Pierce AB, Shadduck JA. A survey of the teaching of ethics in veterinary
untary nature of the survey might have biased respond- education. J Am Vet Med Assoc 1994;204:944–5.
10 Colonius T, Swoboda J. Student perspectives on animal-welfare education in
ents towards those feeling more affected by ethical American veterinary medical curricula. J Vet Med Educ 2010;37:56–60.
11 Magalhães-Sant'Ana M. Ethics teaching in European veterinary schools: a qualita-
dilemmas. The proportion of female respondents was tive case study. Vet Rec 2014;175:592–7.
higher than that in the US veterinary population.56Fe- 12 Rollin BE. Putting the Horse before Descartes. 58. Pennsylvania, USA: Temple
males in the general population report a higher prev- University Press, 2011.
13 Rollin BE. Euthanasia, moral stress, and chronic illness in veterinary medicine. Vet
alence of psychological distress,57 so our moral stress Clin North Am Small Anim Pract 2011;41:651–9.
14 Kipperman BS, Kass PH, Rishniw M. Factors that influence small animal veterinar-
results might have been skewed. There are two potential ians' opinions and actions regarding cost of care and effects of economic limitations
sources of misleading information in behavioural stud- on patient care and outcome and professional career satisfaction and burnout. J Am
Vet Med Assoc 2017;250:785–94.
ies: conscious lying and self-deception.58 We attempted 15. Robinson D, Hooker H. The UK Veterinary Profession in 2006: the findings of a
to minimise the former by assuring respondents that survey of the profession conducted by the Royal College of Veterinary Surgeons.
London: Royal College of Veterinary Surgeons, 2006. www.​google.​com/​search?​
their responses would remain confidential and that client=​safari&​rls=​en&​q=​The+​UK+​Veterinary+​Profession+​in+​2006:+​the+​findings+​
their participation was voluntary. The possibility of re- of+​a+​survey+​of+​the+​profession+​conducted+​by+​the+​Royal+​College+​of+​Veterinary+​
Surgeons&​ie=​UTF-​8&​oe=​UTF-​8. (Accessed 23 Jun 2017).
sponse bias through self-deception can be inherent in 16 Fogle B, Abrahamson D. Pet loss: a survey of the attitudes and feelings of practicing
any research in which people are asked to discuss their veterinarians. Anthrozoos 1990;3:143–50.
17 Sanders CR. Killing with kindness: veterinary euthanasia and the social construction
values. of personhood. Sociol Forum 1995;10:195–214.
Results of the present study confirmed that small an- 18 Tannenbaum J. Veterinary medical ethics: animal welfare, client relations, competi-
tion and collegiality. 2nd ed. Mosby, 1995.
imal practitioners in the USA regularly experience ethi- 19. Swabe J. Veterinary dilemmas: ambiguity and ambivalence in human–animal inter-
cal dilemmas involving client and patient interests, and action. In: Podberscek AL, Paul ES, Serpell JA, eds. Companion animals and us:
Exploring the relationships between people and pets: Cambridge University Press,
these contribute to moral stress. Results revealed that 2000:292–312.
small animal veterinarians perform euthanasia when 20 Bartram DJ, Baldwin DS. Veterinary surgeons and suicide: a structured review of
possible influences on increased risk. Vet Rec 2010;166:388–97.
this is not justified based on animal welfare considera- 21 Morris P. Blue juice: Euthanasia in veterinary medicine. Pennsylvania, USA: Temple
tions. While the majority of respondents indicated that University Press, 2012.
22 Hartnack S, Springer S, Pittavino M, et al. Attitudes of Austrian veterinarians
other practitioners prioritise client interests, 50 per towards euthanasia in small animal practice: impacts of age and gender on views on
cent of respondents characterised their own behaviour euthanasia. BMC Vet Res 2016;12:26.
23 Kondrup SV, Anhøj KP, Rødsgaard-Rosenbeck C, et al. Veterinarian's dilemma: a
as prioritising patient interests. Moral stress from eth- study of how Danish small animal practitioners handle financially limited clients. Vet
ical fatigue and lack of consensus regarding animal Rec 2016;179:596.
24 Yeates JW, Main DC. Veterinary opinions on refusing euthanasia: justifications and
advocacy among small animal veterinarians may have philosophical frameworks. Vet Rec 2011;168:263.
detrimental consequences for animal welfare. On the 25 Main DC. Offering the best to patients: ethical issues associated with the provision of
veterinary services. Vet Rec 2006;158:62–6.
basis of the results of this study, concerted efforts to ed- 26 Verdon D. Triennial report: generational differences: DVM Newsmagazine, 2009.
ucate veterinary students and veterinarians about ethi- http://​veterinarynews.​dvm360.​com/​2009-​triennial-​report-​generational-​differences
(accessed 23 Jun 2017).
cal dilemmas unique to the profession and their roles as 27 Graaf G. Veterinarians’ discourses on animals and clients. J Agric Environ Ethics
animal advocates are warranted, and may help to miti- 2005;18:557–78.
28 Ogden U, Kinnison T, May SA. Attitudes to animal euthanasia do not correlate with
gate one of the major causes of moral stress. acceptance of human euthanasia or suicide. Vet Rec 2012;171:174.
29 Sugita H, Irimajiri M. A survey of veterinarians’ attitudes toward euthanasia of
Acknowledgements The authors thank Dr Jim Clark, Dr Christine Kim and Pam companion animals in Japan. Anthrozoos 2016;29:297–310.
Runquist for their contributions towards shaping the survey; all of the respondents 30 Batchelor CE, Creed A, McKeegan DE. A preliminary investigation into the moral
and anonymous referees for their useful comments. This manuscript represents a reasoning abilities of UK veterinarians. Vet Rec 2015;177:124.
portion of a dissertation submitted by Dr Kipperman to the University of Edinburgh as 31 American Veterinary Medical Association (AVMA). Guidelines for the euthanasia
partial fulfillment for a Master’s degree. of animals. 2013 https://www.​avma.​org/​KB/​Policies/​Documents/​euthanasia.​pdf
(accessed 23 Jun 2017).
Funding None declared. 32 Rathwell-Deault D, Godard B, Frank D, et al. Conceptualization of conven-
Competing interests None declared. ience euthanasia as an ethical dilemma for veterinarians in Quebec. Can Vet J
2017;58:255–60.
© British Veterinary Association (unless otherwise stated in the text of the article) 33 McMillan FD. Rethinking euthanasia: death as an unintentional outcome. J Am Vet
2018. All rights reserved. No commercial use is permitted unless otherwise expressly Med Assoc 2001;219:1204–6.
granted. 34 American Veterinary Medical Association (AVMA). Guidelines on euthanasia. 2007
https://​grants.​nih.​gov/​grants/​olaw/​Euthanasia2007.​pdf (accessed 23 Jun 2017).
35 Harmon-Jones E, Harmon-Jones C. Cognitive dissonance theory after 50 years of
development. Zeitschrift für Sozialpsychologie 2007;38:7–16.
References 36 Rollin BE. An introduction to veterinary medical ethics. 2nd ed. New Jersey, USA:
1 Rollin BE. Veterinary and animal ethics. In: Wilson J, ed. Law, ethics and the veteri-
Blackwell Publishing, 2006.
nary profession. Indiana: Priority Press, 1988:24–49.
37 Ashall V, Millar KM, Hobson-West P. Informed consent in veterinary medi-
2 Tannenbaum J. Veterinary medical ethics: a focus of conflicting interests. J Soc Issues
cine: ethical implications for the profession and the animal ‘patient’. Food Ethics
1993;49:143–56.
2017;37:1–12.
3 American Veterinary Medical Association (AVMA). Veterinarian’s oath. https://www.​
38 Weich K, Grimm H. Meeting the patient’s interest in veterinary clinics. Ethical dimen-
avma.​org/​KB/​Policies/​Pages/​veterinarians-​oath.​aspx (accessed 23 Jun 2017).
sions of the 21st century animal patient. Food Ethics 2017;229.
4 American Veterinary Medical Association (AVMA). Principles of veterinary medical
ethics. https://www.​avma.​org/​KB/​Policies/​Pages/​Principles-​of-​Veterinary-​Medical-​ 39 Verrinder JM, Phillips CJ. Identifying veterinary students' capacity for moral
Ethics-​of-​the-​AVMA.​aspx (accessed 23 Jun 2017). behavior concerning animal ethics issues. J Vet Med Educ 2014;41:358–70.
5 Royal College of Veterinary Surgeons. Code of professional conduct for veterinary 40 Bazerman MH, Gino F. Behavioral ethics: toward a deeper understanding
surgeons and supporting guidance. http://www.​rcvs.​org.​uk/​advice-​and-​guidance/​ of moral judgment and dishonesty. Annual Review of Law and Social Science
code-​of-​professional-​conduct-​for-​veterinary-​surgeons/​pdf (accessed 23 Jun 2017). 2012;8:85–104.
6 Morgan CA, McDonald M. Ethical dilemmas in veterinary medicine. Vet Clin North 41 Batson CD, Kobrynowicz D, Dinnerstein JL, et al. In a very different voice:
Am Small Anim Pract 2007;37:165–79. unmasking moral hypocrisy. J Pers Soc Psychol 1997;72:1335–48.

10 | Vet Record
42 Moore DA, Tetlock PE, Tanlu L, et al. Conflicts of interest and the case of auditor 52 Mastenbroek NJ, Jaarsma AD, Demerouti E, et al. Burnout and engagement,
independence: moral seduction and strategic issue cycling. Acad Manage Rev and its predictors in young veterinary professionals: the influence of gender. Vet Rec

Veterinary Record: first published as 10.1136/vr.104619 on 14 February 2018. Downloaded from http://veterinaryrecord.bmj.com/ on 20 May 2018 by guest. Protected by copyright.
2006;31:10–29. 2014;174:144.
43 Joseph J, Berry K, Deshpande SP. Impact of emotional intelligence and other 53 Nett RJ, Witte TK, Holzbauer SM, et al. Risk factors for suicide, attitudes toward
factors on perception of ethical behavior of peers. Journal of Business Ethics mental illness, and practice-related stressors among US veterinarians. J Am Vet Med
2009;89:539–46. Assoc 2015;247:945–55.
44 Paul ES, Podberscek AL. Veterinary education and students' attitudes towards 54 American Association of Veterinary Medical Colleges (AAVMC. Detailed look at admis-
animal welfare. Vet Rec 2000;146:269–72. sions cycle demonstrates demand, process improvement. 2017 ​http​://a​avmc​.org/​
45 Schoenfeld-Tacher RM, Kogan LR, Meyer-Parsons B, et al. Educational research VetMedEducatorJa​n2017.​​asp​x#​Progress (accessed 23 Jun 2017).
report: changes in students' levels of empathy during the didactic portion of a veteri- 55 British Veterinary Association and the Association of Veterinary Students. Membership
nary program. J Vet Med Educ 2015;42:194–205. and benefits BVA-AVS research report. 2016 https://www.​bva.​co.​uk/​uploadedFiles/​
46 Colombo ES, Pelosi A, Prato-Previde E. Empathy towards animals and belief in Content/​Membership_​and_​benefits/​BVA-​AVS-​Research-​Report-​2016.​pdf (accessed
animal-human-continuity in Italian veterinary students. Anim Welf 2016;25:275–86. 25 Jun 2017).
47 Elkins AD, Kearney M. Professional burnout among female veterinarians in the 56 American Veterinary Medical Association (AVMA). Market research statistics: U.S.
United States. J Am Vet Med Assoc 1992;200:604–8. veterinarians. 2016 https://www.​avma.​org/​KB/​Resources/​Statistics/​Pages/​Market-​
48 Reijula K, Räsänen K, Hämäläinen M, et al. Work environment and occupational research-​statistics-​US-​veterinarians.​aspx (accessed 23 Jun 2017).
health of Finnish veterinarians. Am J Ind Med 2003;44:46–57. 57 Reeves WC, Strine TW, Pratt LA, et al. Mental illness surveillance among adults in
49 Gardner DH, Hini D. Work-related stress in the veterinary profession in New Zealand. the United States. Morbidity and Mortality Weekly Report 2011;60:1–32.
N Z Vet J 2006;54:119–24. 58 Herzog HA, Vore TL, New JC. Conversations with veterinary students: attitudes,
50 Fritschi L, Morrison D, Shirangi A, et al. Psychological well-being of Australian ethics, and animals. Anthrozoos 1989;2:181–8.
veterinarians. Aust Vet J 2009;87:76–81.
51 Hatch PH, Winefield HR, Christie BA, et al. Workplace stress, mental health, and
burnout of veterinarians in Australia. Aust Vet J 2011;89:460–8.

Vet RecorD |  11

You might also like