You are on page 1of 14

Journal of Product & Brand Management

Gender segmentation to increase brand preference? The role of product involvement


Enav Friedmann, Oded Lowengart,
Article information:
To cite this document:
Enav Friedmann, Oded Lowengart, (2019) "Gender segmentation to increase brand preference? The role of product
involvement", Journal of Product & Brand Management, https://doi.org/10.1108/JPBM-06-2018-1917
Permanent link to this document:
https://doi.org/10.1108/JPBM-06-2018-1917
Downloaded on: 01 March 2019, At: 02:43 (PT)
References: this document contains references to 77 other documents.
To copy this document: permissions@emeraldinsight.com
Downloaded by ECU Libraries At 02:43 01 March 2019 (PT)

Access to this document was granted through an Emerald subscription provided by emerald-srm:161304 []
For Authors
If you would like to write for this, or any other Emerald publication, then please use our Emerald for Authors service
information about how to choose which publication to write for and submission guidelines are available for all. Please visit
www.emeraldinsight.com/authors for more information.
About Emerald www.emeraldinsight.com
Emerald is a global publisher linking research and practice to the benefit of society. The company manages a portfolio of
more than 290 journals and over 2,350 books and book series volumes, as well as providing an extensive range of online
products and additional customer resources and services.
Emerald is both COUNTER 4 and TRANSFER compliant. The organization is a partner of the Committee on Publication Ethics
(COPE) and also works with Portico and the LOCKSS initiative for digital archive preservation.

*Related content and download information correct at time of download.


Gender segmentation to increase brand
preference? The role of product involvement
Enav Friedmann
Bar Ilan University, Ramat Gan, Israel, and
Oded Lowengart
Ben Gurion University, Beer Sheva, Israel

Abstract
Purpose – This paper aims to address the role of product involvement in the brand preference formation of men and women. Product involvement
can be defined as a consumer’s motivation for product purchase that affects their information processing strategies when forming a brand
preference (e.g. more automatic at low levels vs more deliberative at high levels). Given that gender differences are found to be context-dependent,
it was expected that, when forming a single brand preference, men would emphasize instrumental aspects (functional and socially conspicuous
utilities) and women the experiential utility of the brand only with high-involvement-level products.
Design/methodology/approach – A descriptive survey (n = 459) using structural equation modeling (SEM) analysis was used following an online
experiment where involvement level was manipulated (n = 255) to validate the results.
Findings – Stereotypical gender differences appeared at high, but not low-involvement levels. Theoretically, these findings question the
evolutionary basis of gender differences, as differences were not consistent at both levels.
Downloaded by ECU Libraries At 02:43 01 March 2019 (PT)

Practical implications – The findings raise questions about the efficacy of segmenting by gender when aiming to increase brand preference of low-
involvement products, whereas stereotypical targeting seem to be effective for increasing preference for high-involvement ones.
Originality/value – For the first time, the role of product involvement and gender was examined in brand preference formation. This can
theoretically clarify whether gender differences are consistent or dependent on the level of involvement. This information can help in designing
efficient marketing strategies for products with different involvement levels.
Keywords Gender differences, Brand preference, Level of product involvement
Paper type Research paper

Introduction proxy for psychological characteristics (Ye and Robertson,


2012).
Product involvement is regarded as a general attitude concerning The evolutionary theory explains why physical descriptors are
the importance of a product to a consumer (Ahtola, 1985), which believed to be related to psychological differences.
is mainly a function of its frequency of purchase, social and Psychological gender differences based on biological physical
economic risk and cost (Nkwocha et al., 2005). Understanding descriptors originated as a function of the division of labor
the role of product involvement in the brand formation process between the genders millions of years ago (Saad and Gill,
by gender is theoretically important as an answer to whether men 2000). For men, survival was dependent on becoming good
and women are inherently different in their brand preference
hunters and finding a fertile mate, whereas women needed to
considerations (Dennis and McCall, 2005).
excel at gathering food for the family, which allowed them to
Gender refers to the psychological attributes associated with
raise offspring into adulthood. This difference in goals led
physiology that are socially constructed in an individual’s identity
women to be more caring and men to be more target oriented
(Bem, 1974, 1981; Spence and Helmreich, 1978). There is
(Brown, 2004). The dominant theory of gender is the classical
evidence suggesting that it is a more robust concept than
differences model, based on the idea that men and women are
convention conveys (Campbell et al., 2004). Although this
vastly different psychologically because of sociobiological
construct is broader than a solely physiological categorization,
processes (Swani et al., 2013). The effect of their different
marketers commonly use demographic gender information as a
biological programming as evolutionary gatherers and hunters
singular biological descriptor (Spence and Sawin, 1985; Ye and
has been extended to the context of purchasing behavior
Robertson, 2012). In effect, a physical characteristic is used as a
(Dennis and McCall, 2005). For example, women were found
to have more positive attitudes toward shopping than men
(Kuruvilla et al., 2009). Furthermore, according to this theory,
to demonstrate their symbolic economic power, men purchase
The current issue and full text archive of this journal is available on
Emerald Insight at: www.emeraldinsight.com/1061-0421.htm
Received 28 June 2018
Revised 7 October 2018
7 December 2018
Journal of Product & Brand Management 8 December 2018
© Emerald Publishing Limited [ISSN 1061-0421] 11 December 2018
[DOI 10.1108/JPBM-06-2018-1917] Accepted 12 December 2018
The role of product involvement Journal of Product & Brand Management
Enav Friedmann and Oded Lowengart

items that position them favorably in competition with other crucial for the decision of whether to segment by gender and
men and reflect their prowess as hunters (Dennis and McCall, how to design the right appeal is understanding if there are
2005). Empirical research to date has examined how men and valued utilities differentiated by gender, and in which specific
women may differ in their utility considerations when forming situations these occur.
brand preferences of a single brand, mainly claiming that men To conclude, the objective of this research was to examine
care more about instrumental utilities, whereas women are which utilities are important for men and women when they
more experientially oriented (Jackson et al., 2011; Dennis and form brand preferences at different levels of product
McCall, 2005; Falk and Campbell, 1997; Fischer and Arnold, involvement. Our findings suggest that when forming such
1994). Instrumental utilities of a product include its functional preferences, product involvement serves as a boundary
core attributes as well as socially conspicuous utilities that condition for gender differences. In fact, men and women use a
satisfy the desire for recognition from others by enhancing one’s different set of considerations only in high-involvement
social well-being (Friedmann and Lowengart, 2018; situations. In two studies, when forming high-involvement
Friedmann and Brueller, 2018). These two utilities were brand preference, we found that women exhibited more
generally found to be more important for men (Griskevicius concern about the experiential utility of the product, whereas
et al.,2007; Friedmann and Lowengart, 2018; Friedmann and men were more concerned about its functional utility. Based on
Brueller, 2018). However, Meyers-Levy and Loken (2015) these findings, our recommendation is that marketers who aim
doubted this evolutionary claim, suggesting that the context to increase brand preference of high-involvement products
serves as a boundary condition for gender differences. should tailor their claims based on gender, whereas marketers
Similarly, Friedmann and Lowengart (2018) showed that of low-involvement products should carefully consider
gender differences in utility importance are context dependent investing resources in gender targeting.
and are clearer when single brands are evaluated separately
Downloaded by ECU Libraries At 02:43 01 March 2019 (PT)

(such as when forming a brand preference), rather than in Theoretical background


contexts where there is a choice between alternatives.
Following this contextual claim, in this research, we explored Gender differences
whether such stereotypical utilities (experiential utility for Research in this area describes biological (Fisher, 1999) and
women and instrumental utility for men) indeed are relevant in sociological factors (Barnett and Hyde, 2001) as the source of
different involvement levels of a product. psychological differences. These factors lead to perceived
Product involvement is related to purchase motivation and personality differences between the genders (Brackett et al.,
affects the cognitive effort a consumer will invest before making 2004), where women are more expressive, emotional and
a purchase decision (Punj and Moore, 2009). Thus, it is contextual thinkers; and men are more task-oriented analytical
believed that different information processing strategies are thinkers focused on demonstrating competency and prize
used by consumers in low and high involvement (e.g. more instrumentality (Trofimova, 2015).
automatic in low vs more deliberative in high involvement)
(Bettman et al., 1998; Ferreira, and Coelho, 2015). So, we Psychological differences
questioned whether different information processing strategies Research has established that men and women differ in their
used in low and high involvement levels, will affect the personality traits, e.g. women are described as more emotional
stereotypical considerations men and women use when forming (Miller, 2012). This is expressed by greater emotional
brand preference. intelligence, as they are better than men at perceiving and
This question has meaningful managerial applications. In expressing emotions, and they use emotions more to facilitate
low- or high-involvement products, it is crucial that marketers thought (Brackett et al., 2004). Men are described as more
understand the importance of utilities designed for each gender competitive than women (Niederle and Vesterlund, 2007),
segment to increase brand preference. For example, over the with a stronger drive for success (Reis, 1991).
years, many commercials for cars were directed at men using The basis of these psychological differences derives from the
hedonistic, experiential slogans such as: “Oh, what a feeling – interaction of biology and sociology (Cosmides and Tooby,
Toyota,” “Corolla – feels good inside,” “BMW – The feeling 2013). Barnett and Hyde (2001) focused on the sociological
remains” and “Peugot – There are sensations you will never differences between men and women where these differences
forget.” Recently, Apple used a female singer (Yael Naim) to stem mainly from different environments, expectations and
pitch “a new soul” to sell their Airbook, replete with emotional norms. In a similar fashion, Eagly (1987) contended that the
and sensatory appeal, using a male hand and a utilitarian division of labor between men and women shapes gender roles
envelope to demonstrate the thinness of the laptop. But are in such a way that women are expected to behave with a
these unstereotypical experiential claims effective in increasing communal focus (e.g. showing concern for the welfare of other
brand preference of men when considering high-involvement people), whereas men focus on agency (e.g. showing
products? In this research, we examine what appeals are competency, task orientation, and instrumentality). Other
effective in increasing brand preference when targeting men or research has mainly focused on biology. For example, men and
women with both low- and high-involvement products. women have different physiologies, including different
Marketers aiming to increase brand preference should hormones (Buser, 2012) and brain structures (Fisher, 1999).
understand a particular segment’s needs and what utilities they The sociobiology–evolutionary theory posits that the differences
value. Product positioning should be based on this identified between the sexes originated in human evolution where
value, and the ad campaign structured accordingly (Kotler, environment shaped biology, and sex-related reproductive
1997; Kohli and Leuthesser, 1993; Knox, 2004). Hence, function caused the environment to differ for each sex (Brown,
The role of product involvement Journal of Product & Brand Management
Enav Friedmann and Oded Lowengart

2004). The hunter–gatherer theory (Cosmides and Tooby, 2013) been explored. Specifically, the importance of different utilities
states that evolution shaped men as hunters and women as for each gender was not examined at different involvement
gatherers and home-based caregivers of children as an levels.
explanation for their different cognitive abilities. A modern The three following utilities are described as central when
variation of this approach is the application of the Savannah forming brand preference (Sweeney and Soutar, 2001; Park
hypothesis (Dennis and McCall, 2005) to explain variance in et al., 1986):
how men and women shop. For example, when shopping, 1 Experiential utility: This is the perceived enjoyment or hedonic
women are more expressive and men are more instrumental benefit of having the brand. It increases arousal and promotes
(Falk and Campbell, 1997; Jackson et al., 2011; Kuruvilla et al., fantasy, escapism, perceived freedom and change in mood
2009; Tifferet and Herstein, 2012; Yang and Lee, 2010). (Babin et al., 1994; Bloch and Richins, 1983a; Tynan and
However, to date, no one has examined whether these McKechnie, 2009; Kuikka and Laukkanen, 2012). Some
instrumental–expressive gender differences are indeed consumers have reported potential entertainment and
consistent and inherent across products and situations with emotional value (Babin et al., 1994).
varying levels of product involvement. 2 Functional utility: Sheth et al. (1991) defined the
This question is important for better designing the marketing functional value of a brand as the perceived instrumental
offer to satisfy a segment’s real needs and to save resources usefulness that is derived from its performance, which
when segmentation is not needed. The social implications of comprises the core value of the product.
this investigation can provide an answer as to whether 3 Socially conspicuous utility: (sometimes termed in the
marketing communications (such as media images or literature as conspicuous utility). This means that owning
positioning statements) often exaggerate differences between a specific brand item might serve as an instrumental way
men and women that may not actually exist when trying to to improve one’s standing by eliciting positive reactions
Downloaded by ECU Libraries At 02:43 01 March 2019 (PT)

increase brand preference (Schroeder and Borgerson, 2015). from significant others about the purchase (Walsh et al.,
2008). This benefit can symbolize the individual’s status
The role of product involvement and brand utilities’ for others and can fulfill needs of self-enhancement, role
importance position, group membership or ego-identification (Park
The term “involvement” has several definitions and uses in the et al., 1986).
literature. It has been defined as the degree of arousal, interest or
drive evoked by a particular stimulus or situation (Bloch and The importance of the above utilities is likely to change in
Richins, 1983b; Buchholz and Smith, 1991). Involvement is also different involvement levels. For example, in high-involvement
regarded as an overall attitude construct that comprises the situations, will both men and women focus on the functional–
thoughts and feelings about the importance of a product to the core attributes of a product, and in low-involvement ones on
consumer (Ahtola, 1985). Mittal (1989) noted that involvement experiential utilities? Or will differences exist as is
is one’s perceived importance and relevance of a product or the stereotypically expected of them (i.e. beyond the level of
decision to buy it – not only at the time of purchase. He also involvement, men will be more functional and women more
noted that products may differ in their ability to arouse experiential)? Another possibility could be that gender
involvement at the point of purchase. For instance, usually the differences will appear only in one of the levels of involvement:
level of involvement in purchasing a car is high, whereas the level in high involvement, when cognitive resources are invested, and
of involvement in purchasing bread is low (Ahmed et al., 2004). the stereotypical utility might be considered as maximizing the
According to Laurent and Kapferer (1985), a central indicator of consumer’s overall utility; or in low involvement, where
involvement may be price. When the price is high, the risks of a stereotypical utilities will be automatic, as they are inherent.
mispurchase are high, so consumers are likely to be more The answer to the question of the effect of product involvement
involved in such decisions (Rothschild, 1979). Certain product on gender differences in the brand preference process is not
categories are strongly related to the level of consumer trivial and needs further examination.
involvement; hence, Nkwocha et al. (2005) used durable and In consideration consumption of low-involvement products
non-durable goods to create conditions of high and low such as retail products and those bought without planning and
involvement. deliberation, gender differences were found to exist
Petty and Cacioppo (1986) postulated in the elaboration (Verplanken and Herabadi, 2001; Wardle et al., 2004;
likelihood model that different utilities are suggested to be Yen, 2005; Prättälä et al., 2006; Tifferet and Herstein, 2012),
persuasive at different involvement levels: in high-involvement as well as documented differences when purchasing high-
cases, because consumers elaborated ad messages more involvement products (Limtanakool et al., 2006; Gilbert et al.,
extensively, the content of a message had greater impact. In 2003; Lee, 2009). Although these studies did not investigate the
contrast, in low-involvement cases, messages that focused on importance of experiential, functional and socially conspicuous
peripheral cues were more effective in changing attitudes. This utility considerations when forming preference, as the majority
might imply that different utilities (e.g. functional utility as a of academic literature and popular media claim (Falk and
core element of the product) or peripheral utilities (as other Campbell, 1997; Fischer and Arnold, 1994; Shields, 2013),
experiential or socially conspicuous utilities that provide they do highlight consistent gender differences in women’s and
benefit, but are not part of the product itself) might have men’s purchase considerations.
different importance at low- or high-involvement levels. On the other hand, several researchers have suggested that
However, in this context, the importance of utilities in brand consumer involvement may be an important link between
preference formation at different involvement levels had not yet gender and consumer perception or behavior (Fischer and
The role of product involvement Journal of Product & Brand Management
Enav Friedmann and Oded Lowengart

Arnold, 1994; McCabe, 2001; Sirgy, 1982). Ye and Robertson in Study 2, it was operationalized by the claim: “Best
(2012) found that gender is connected to brand loyalty through quality, best performance.”
product involvement, which might suggest that gender has a 3 Socially conspicuous utility: The ability of a brand to enhance
unique relationship with product involvement leading to one’s sense of self and socially conspicuous status. In Study
consumer behavior. In addition, several researchers highlighted 1, we measured this using three items from Sweeney and
the role of context in gender differences (Meyers-Levy and Soutar’s (2001) PERVAL scale. For example, participants
Loken, 2015; Friedmann and Lowengart, 2018; Friedmann responded to the statement, “This brand will improve the
and Brueller, 2018), claiming that there are no evolutionary- way people look at me” on a scale of 1 to 7, with 1 meaning
based differences, rather that gender differences appear only in do not agree at all and 7 meaning totally agree. In Study 1, the
specific situations. In accordance with these contextual claims, Cronbach’s a for this construct was 0.92, whereas in Study
and the notion of Punj and Moore (2009) that with high- 2, it was operationalized by the claim: “impressing others,
involvement products, consumers expend a great deal of improving the way people are looking at you.”
cognitive thought on purchasing decisions relative to low-
In addition, we looked at:
involvement ones, we expected that gender-related utilities  Involvement level: Defined as an internal state of the
would be reflected in brand preference formation only when
consumer that results in arousal, interest or drive induced
purchasing high-involvement products. Thus, we hypothesized
by a stimulus or purchase occasion. Based on the work of
that:
Buchholz and Smith (1991) and Mittal (1989), three
H1. Only with high-involvement products will women questions assessed consumers’ overall attitude toward the
demonstrate a stronger relationship between preference product, as well as their cognitive and emotional
for a brand and its experiential utility than men. tendencies toward it: “In selecting from the many types
Downloaded by ECU Libraries At 02:43 01 March 2019 (PT)

and brands of this product available in the market, could


H2. Only with high-involvement products will men you say that, from 1 (I would not care at all) to 7 (I would
demonstrate a stronger relationship between preference care a great deal), how much thought you put into
for a brand and its functional utility than women. evaluating the product?”; “From 1 (not at all) to 7 (very
much), how much do you feel that the product is
H3. Only with high-involvement products will men important to you?” The Cronbach’s a for this construct in
demonstrate a stronger relationship between preference Study 1 was 0.78, and in Study 2, it was 0.878.
for a brand and its socially conspicuous utility than  Brand preference: The precedence a brand receives when
women. the participants evaluate its benefits. In Study 1, we
measured this using one item in which the participants
were asked about their “overall rating preference” on a
Methods semantic differentiation scale ranging from 1 (lowest) to 7
To examine the effect of product involvement on the brand (highest), based on Ghose and Lowengart (2001b).
preference formation of men and women, two studies were  Demographic and segmentation questions: Collected details
designed. The first was a descriptive study, and the second was such as gender (birth-assigned gender), income level, age
an online experiment. The measures of interest will be and frequency of item purchases.
presented following an elaboration of the samples, procedures
and results of each study.
Study 1
Measures Sample and procedure
In both studies, the main variables were these three utilities: We administered a survey to 459 undergraduate students (212
1 Experiential utility: The emotional satisfaction that arises men and 247 women) in three university departments:
from the purchase of a product. In Study 1, we measured Engineering, Psychology and Business Administration. For
this using three items from Sweeney and Soutar’s (2001) their participation, the participants received a chocolate bar.
PERVAL scale. For example, participants responded to They were randomly asked to fill out one of two questionnaires
the statement, “I will enjoy this brand” on a Likert scale of about high- and low-involvement products: computer brands
1 to 7, with 1 meaning do not agree at all and 7 meaning and soap purchases or car brands and sausage purchases.
totally agree. In Study 1, the Cronbach’s a for this Appendix 1 presents the descriptive statistics of the sample. As
dimension was 0.88, whereas in Study 2, it was the table indicates, with regard to the demographic categories,
operationalized by the claim: “Get addicted to life’s there were no significant differences between men and women.
pleasures.” Prior to this, as suggested by Quester and Lin Lim (2003) for
2 Functional utility: The rational instrumental benefits of a better survey design, we convened an exploratory research
product that are evident in its quality and performance. In focus group of 23 students to help identify the product
Study 1, we assessed this based on two items from the categories and involvement levels, the relevant utilities
quality/performance dimension of Sweeney and Soutar’s (functional, experiential and socially conspicuous, and three
(2001) PERVAL scale. For example, we asked consumers alternative dominant brands in each product category. Using
to evaluate the performance of a brand on a differential these results, two surveys were created containing two product
semantic scale ranging from 1 = low to 7 = high. In Study categories for each level of involvement that included the brand
1, the Cronbach’s a for this dimension was 0.67, whereas utilities, alternatives, preferences and segmentation questions.
The role of product involvement Journal of Product & Brand Management
Enav Friedmann and Oded Lowengart

For example, we asked respondents how they evaluated the level). To assess the moderating effect of gender in predicting
quality of a Mazda, Renault and Ford automobile or an HP, the preference for a brand for high- (but not low-) involvement
Dell and Sony-Vaio computer (see Appendix 2 for all variables products, we followed Schumacker and Marcoulides (1998)
collected for each brand). Then the average of each utility was suggestion to use a multi-sample approach when one of the
found and the average of the preferences, and these were used independent variables is categorical. In this approach,
in an structural equation modeling (SEM) analysis to represent the interaction effect is examined by evaluating the fit of the
each category. Querying the participants about their constrained model that forces the estimates in the men’s and
perceptions about real brands and asking them to state their women’s models to be equal, meaning that no gender
preferences for each brand helped to indirectly assess the difference exists, with the fit of the free model in which men and
saliency of the various utilities of the brand. Using this women have different estimates. Significant x 2 differences
technique, we could better expose the tradeoffs people were between the constrained and free models mean that the
making in the considered utilities compared to asking them equality constraint does not fit the data, and there is an
directly which utility was important to them when forming interaction effect (Rigdon et al., 1998). We studied the
brand preference (Wilkie and Pessemier, 1973). possibility of this interactive effect separately for low- and high-
involvement products.
Data analysis
After preliminary research that detected the level of Results
involvement of several product categories, and in accordance
with Nkwocha et al. (2005) suggesting that high and low Confirmatory factor analysis
involvement can be represented by expensive durable and First, to support the distinction of the factors from one another,
inexpensive non-durable categories, the high-involvement level we conducted a confirmatory factor analysis to ensure an
Downloaded by ECU Libraries At 02:43 01 March 2019 (PT)

was represented by car and computer categories and the low- acceptable fit of the measurement models and to examine the
involvement level by soap and sausage categories. reliability of the constructs. Only after the theoretical
First, for each product category, we measured the level of constructs had a good fit, did we examine the fit of the model
involvement and found that the difference between the high- with all hypothesized relationships between these constructs
involvement products and the low-involvement ones was (the theoretical structural model). The parameters for
significant (see Appendix 3 for means and standard deviations). estimating the fit of the measurement and structural models for
The “average categories” representing high- and low-involvement each involvement level and for each gender were acceptable
levels enabled us to examine overall models to be constructed that and met the fit criteria (CMIN/DF < 5; comparative fit index,
were relevant for low- and high-involvement categories. normed fit index > 0.9; root mean square error of
We also ensured that involvement was indeed an intrinsic approximation  0.08). We evaluated the coefficient of each
property of the product categories by examining a two-way utility in predicting product preferences for men and women
analysis of variance with involvement evaluation as a dependent separately for each level of involvement. Figures 1 and 2
variable, and gender and product categories (low–high) and their present the results of these analyses.
interaction as independent variables. Involvement did not vary as As the figures demonstrate, it seems that the experiential
a function of gender between the categories as the interaction consideration is related to brand preference in high-involvement
variable was insignificant, f(3,865) = 0.538, sig = 0.656. This categories only for women, and the socially conspicuous
examination ensured that certain categories can be regarded as consideration is related to brand preference in low-involvement
manifesting a certain level of involvement from both genders. categories only for men. To examine whether these differences
We analyzed the data using AMOS software. As was were significant, we conducted the next analyses.
suggested by Anderson and Gerbing (1988), this analysis took
place in two phases. In the first phase, we studied four Examining the gender difference for each level of
measurement models to confirm the existence of the constructs involvement
(women and men  high and low levels of involvement). In the To investigate the interaction between the utilities and gender
final measurement model, we included the three utilities and in predicting preferences for high- and low-involvement levels,
preference for a brand. We also examined and confirmed the we compared a free model of men and women to a constrained
significance of the involvement dimension. Furthermore, we model in which the various parameters were forced to be equal.
tested the model using a dummy variable for the product Next, we looked at betas of each utility predicting the
category (1 = cars/0 = computers for high involvement and 1 = preferences of men and women. Finally, we determined
soap/0 = sausage for low involvement) because the whether the constrained model with the betas forced to be
nonsignificance of this variable at any level of involvement for equal between the genders better fit the data than the free
men and women enabled us to examine the model with the two model that allowed the betas to differ between the genders.
product categories combined as representative of a high or low Table I presents the results of this analysis.
level of involvement. Adding the dummy variable for a category From Table I, it can be seen that differences in the
somewhat reduced the fit indices. Therefore, we selected the measurement weights between the models of men and women
model without the dummy variable for product category as the were evident only in the high-involvement product categories
final one for the high- and low-involvement brands. In and not in the low-involvement ones.
the second stage, we used the four structural models to examine As Table II illustrates, a difference between all three betas
the relationships between the utilities and brand preference exists only in high-involvement categories. Looking at
(separate models for women and men for each involvement estimates of the regression from Figures 1 and 2 for a further
The role of product involvement Journal of Product & Brand Management
Enav Friedmann and Oded Lowengart

Figure 1 Structural models for women and men – high-involvement products


Downloaded by ECU Libraries At 02:43 01 March 2019 (PT)

Figure 2 Structural models for women and men – low-involvement products

understanding of the direction of difference reveals that the Study 2


functional and socially conspicuous utilities are more
Sample and procedure
prominent in determining brand preference for men, and the
experiential aspect is more of a factor among women. In this study, we used Prolific, an online survey company (www.
To validate these results, we designed Study 2, where prolific.ac) to recruit 255 UK participants, 125 for the low-
involvement was manipulated into high and low levels. involvement condition (56 men, 69 women) and 130 for high
The role of product involvement Journal of Product & Brand Management
Enav Friedmann and Oded Lowengart

Table I Examining gender differences for each level of involvement


Gender difference for Gender difference for
high-involvement categories low-involvement categories
Model Dx 2 Ddf p Dx 2 Ddf p
Measurement weight 12.099 4 0.017* 4.889 4 0.299
(a model where the betas and indicators are forced to be equal)
Measurement intercept 37.172 13 0.001* 26.415 13 0.015*
(all the above and the intercepts in the equations for predicting preferences are
forced to be equal)
Structural covariances 45.476 19 0.001* 31.555 19 0.035*
(all the above and the factor variances and covariances are forced to be equal)
Measurement residuals 66.242 28 0.001* 44.058 28 0.027*
(all the parameters are equal across groups, including the variance of the
residuals)
Notes: *p  0.05; italic values: men’s and women’s measurement weights (estimates in the models) are different only for high involvement categories

Table II Examining the differences in betas between men and women for each involvement level
Downloaded by ECU Libraries At 02:43 01 March 2019 (PT)

Low involvement High involvement


Standardized estimate Dx 2 DDF p Dx 2 Ddf p
b1 (for experiential) 0.000 1 0.998 4.095 1 0.043*
b2 (for functional) 1.507 1 0.220 4.627 1 0.031*
b3 (for socially conspicuous) 1.231 1 0.267 8.030 1 0.005*
Note: *p  0.05

involvement (56 men, 74 women) (see further details on the high and low levels (in men: Mlow involvement = £5.23, SD =
sample in Appendix 4). These participants were paid £1 for 2.93; Mhigh involvement = £814.64, SD = 1,196.47, t(55) =
their participation and were randomly assigned to a low- or 5.062, sig = 0.000; in women: Mlow involvement = £5.31,
high-involvement scenario. They completed a questionnaire SD = 3.133; Mhigh involvement = £1998.76, SD = 5166.77,
about their evaluation of advertising claims for a certain t(73) = 3.319, sig = 0.001). Participants were also asked to
product. For the high-involvement scenario, respondents were state what product they had in mind and the frequency of their
asked to “Think about a product that you have an immediate buying such a product (every several days/several weeks/several
need to buy. At this time, this product is very important to you, months/several years). The significant difference in the price of
and if you are not able to purchase it, it will be a significant high-involvement products was attributed to the percentage of
problem for you. Assume the product is quite costly and you women who designated purchasing a car as an option. Cars
rarely buy it. It will be very useful in the near future, and you are were far more expensive than the other products that were also
financially capable of purchasing it.” For the low-involvement ranked at this involvement level.
scenario, respondents were asked to “Think about a product After the description of the scenarios, in both conditions,
that you do not have an immediate need to buy. At this time, respondents were told to consider the purchase of that product.
this product is not very important to you, and if you don’t Then three advertising claims were presented (see Appendix 5
purchase it, you will not have any problem. Assume the product for the claims). In each ad claim, we highlighted one main
is quite inexpensive, and you frequently buy it. It will probably attribute of the product (functional, socially conspicuous or
be of some usefulness in the near future, and you are financially experiential). Participants were asked to read all three claims
capable of purchasing it.” before answering and then to rank the claims by the likelihood
The mean levels of involvement as measured by the three of their leading them to prefer one brand over the others; the
involvement measures were significantly different between the most convincing ad was to be ranked first, the next was second
two conditions (in men: Mlow involvement = 4.43, SD = 1.16; and the least convincing was third.
Mhigh involvement = 5.83, SD = 1.05, t(110) = 6.691, sig = We also conducted manipulation checks for the utility
0.000; in women: Mlow involvement = 4.39, SD = 1.50; manipulation on 130 participants to show that the utilities were
Mhigh involvement = 6.18, SD = 0.81, t(141) = 8.928, sig = indeed perceived as different. Each ad claim was presented, and
0.000). participants were asked to state what utility they believed the
For each scenario, participants were asked to state the price specific claim signaled. The options were: functional,
of the product in pounds. According to Laurent and Kapferer experiential and socially conspicuous. Indeed, the majority of
(1985), price is a central indicator of involvement, and indeed participants were able to differentiate between claims (95.4 per
significant differences in product prices were found between cent identified the functional claim, 77.7 per cent identified the
The role of product involvement Journal of Product & Brand Management
Enav Friedmann and Oded Lowengart

socially conspicuous claim and 73.1 per cent the experiential selections are likely made with little cognitive effort relative to
claim). high-involvement purchases (Brock and Brannon, 1992;
Bettman et al., 1998). It might be that, in these situations, men
Results and women do not expend a great deal of cognitive thought on
such decisions (Bettman et al., 1998; Punj and Moore, 2009);
We used a Mann–Whitney test to examine the differences
hence, their brand preference process does not reflect specific
between the sum of ranks and mean rank of the two
gender-related needs. Such needs are more evident when the
independent samples (men and women) for examining possible
process is more conscious and deliberative. This finding is
gender differences in the ranking of the utilities. Table III lists
similar to that of Friedmann and Brueller (2018), where fewer
the results. We found that differences in utility importance were
stereotypical gender differences were found in automatic
not found at low-involvement levels, but were found at high-
processes (in a brand selection context, BSC), whereas in more
involvement ones. Specifically, in agreement with Study 1, in
careful and conscious decision-making (in a single choice
high involvement, functional utility was more related to men’s
evaluation of one brand, SCE), more stereotypical gender
brand preference, whereas the experiential utility was more
differences were found. In the BSC, cognitive resources were
related to women’s preference. In contrast to Study 1, the
limited, and as low involvement situations revealed a more
socially conspicuous utility was ranked equally between the
automatic process that showed no gender differences. In
genders.
contrast, the SCE, similar to the high-involvement situations,
revealed a more contemplative and calculated process where
General discussion stereotypical gender differences were shown.
Although gender differences in purchasing behavior are widely Men’s and women’s utility considerations for forming
Downloaded by ECU Libraries At 02:43 01 March 2019 (PT)

documented and claimed to be inherent, no research has explored preferences for high-involvement products are consistent with
this claim across varying levels of product involvement. For the social expectations. Thus, using experimental claims in car
first time, the role of product involvement in the brand preference commercials (a high-involvement product) is not recommended
formation of men and women was examined. Findings reveal that for increasing men’s brand preference. But these stereotypical
involvement serves as a boundary condition for gender differences gender differences that were found in high involvement are not
in the preference formation process, whereby such differences inherent, as significant differences were not found in the low
appear only with high-involvement products. level of involvement.
H1 and H2 were confirmed in both studies, as the In the past, gender was found to contribute to behavioral
experiential and functional utilities were found to be more predictions, especially in a consumer behavior context
dominant considerations for women and men accordingly only (Friedmann and Lowengart, 2018), as was found in this
at high-involvement levels. H3 was inconclusive, as the socially research. Self-expression is increasingly becoming a motivating
conspicuous utility was found to be a more dominant factor in product and brand choices (Ye and Robertson, 2012),
consideration in high involvement for men only in Study 1, but thus it is reasonable that only with high-involvement products
not in Study 2. The results of this research point to the fact that where consumers put effort into the purchasing decision are
gender differences are not inherent, as suggested in the they more able to express their gender identities by
literature by several researchers (Dennis and McCall, 2005; demonstrating a higher importance of stereotypical utilities.
Falk and Campbell, 1997; Fischer and Arnold, 1994). This research augments prior studies on gender stereotypes
Involvement has an important role in affecting information as the common classical differences model between the genders
processing (Celsi and Olson, 1988; Richins and Bloch, 1986) (Swani et al., 2013) was not found to be valid in low-
and is relevant when making a purchasing decision. In low- involvement situations; thus, evolutionary theory cannot
involvement purchases, such as of frequently purchased account for consistent gender differences.
undifferentiated commodities (e.g. salt and mineral water), As most empirical studies claim psychological gender
differences exist (Sharma et al., 2012), leading advertisers to
Table III Gender differences in ranking utilities frequently use gender stereotypes (Grau and Zotos, 2016), this
research adds an important dimension. The role of product
Category Attribute Gender Mean rank p-value
involvement is central in the decision of whether the use of
Low involvement Functional Men 51.02 0.276 gender stereotypes will effectively increase brand preference.
Women 55.47
Socially conspicuous Men 55.89 0.396 Theoretical and managerial implications
Women 51.59
Experiential Men 54.46 0.747 These results add theoretical understanding of how gender
Women 52.74 plays out in forming a preference for a brand at different levels
High involvement Functional Men 58.73 0.029* of involvement with a product. For the first time, it was
Women 68.99 demonstrated that such differences between the sexes exist at
Socially conspicuous Men 63.61 0.642 high-involvement levels only. These findings do not accord
Women 65.66 with claims of structural constant differences between men and
Experiential Men 73.02 0.012* women.
Women 57.88 The experiential utility, which the literature has tended to
regard as a peripheral consideration in general, emerged as a
Notes: *p < 0.05; a lower rank means a more convincing ad claim
major factor for women when forming a preference in high-
The role of product involvement Journal of Product & Brand Management
Enav Friedmann and Oded Lowengart

involvement products. In contrast, for men, the functional feminine/masculine color choices), stereotypical gender
utility was a more dominant consideration in high-involvement differences will be apparent. Similarly, it would be interesting
purchases. to focus on gender not as a dichotomous variable (male/
These results have marketing implications for those who seek female), but as a continuum. For example, exploring how
more efficient segmentation schemes when aiming to increase gender non-conformists (i.e. people who identify as neither
their brand preference. At high-involvement levels, emotional fully male nor fully female) react to utility function primes.
appeals that underscore pleasurable, positive experiences might Thus, it would be interesting to investigate gender
be more effective in increasing brand preference when stereotypes and utility functions from a non-binary gender
appealing to women. For men, campaigns that focus on the perspective.
functional benefits of the product seem to be more effective.
However, marketers trying to sell low-involvement products
might reconsider when investing resources in differentiating the References
message for women and men. Ahmed, Z.U., Johnson, J.P., Yang, X., Fatt, C.K., Teng, H.S.
and Boon, L.C. (2004), “Does country of origin matter for
Limitations and future research low-involvement products?”, International Marketing Review,
This research has several limitations. First, product Vol. 21 No. 1, pp. 102-120.
involvement has dozens of definitions in the literature, and the Ahtola, O.T. (1985), “Hedonic and utilitarian aspects of
examination of gender differences in the importance of these consumer behavior: an attitudinal perspective”, Advances in
utilities can be made when involvement is defined and Consumer Research, Vol. 12 No. 1, pp. 7-10.
measured differently (Mittal, 1989). Even though a relative Anderson, J.C. and Gerbing, D.W. (1988), “Structural
Downloaded by ECU Libraries At 02:43 01 March 2019 (PT)

homogeneity of involvement for each product category was equation modeling in practice: a review and recommended
found, confirming that it is tied to the category as suggested by two-step approach”, Psychological Bulletin, Vol. 103 No. 3,
other studies (Laurent and Kapferer, 1985; Nkwocha et al., p. 411.
2005), we suggest that further research examine whether these Babin, B.J., Darden, W.R. and Griffin, M. (1994), “Work and/
findings hold in product categories where the level of or fun: measuring hedonic and utilitarian shopping value”,
involvement has greater variance (such as with clothing). This Journal of Consumer Research, Vol. 20 No. 4, pp. 644-656.
might enable the contrast between consumers who differ in Barnett, R.C. and Hyde, J.S. (2001), “Women, men, work, and
their level of involvement in purchasing the same product to be family: an expansionist theory”, The American Psychologist,
studied. Vol. 56 No. 10, pp. 781-796.
Another limitation of this research is that gender was Bem, S.L. (1974), “The measurement of psychological
measured by biological (birth) sex alone (as most marketers androgyny”, Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology,
measure this concept). Future examination might explore the Vol. 42 No. 2, pp. 155-162.
importance of the utilities of different gender segments, based Bem, S.L. (1981), “Gender schema theory: a cognitive account of
on preferred gender identity and not only on physical gender as sex typing”, Psychological Review, Vol. 88 No. 4, pp. 354-364.
Palan (2001) suggested. This is important in understanding Bettman, J.R., Luce, M.F. and Payne, J.W. (1998), “Constructive
how “gender” is conceptualized and measured in determining consumer choice processes”, Journal of Consumer Research,
whether there is a difference between the “genders.” Vol. 25 No. 3, pp. 187-217.
Furthermore, socially conspicuous utility differed between Bloch, P.H. and Richins, M.L. (1983a), “Shopping without
Studies 1 and 2. In Study 1, it was more important for men in purchase: an investigation of consumer browsing behavior”,
high-involvement products, whereas in Study 2, it was equally Advances in Consumer Research, Vol. 10 No. 1, pp. 389-393.
important to both genders. This might be because of the nature Bloch, P.H. and Richins, M.L. (1983b), “A theoretical model
of this complex dimension of this utility, which may involve for the study of product importance perceptions”, Journal of
different benefits, related to both personal self and social self. Marketing, Vol. 47 No. 3, pp. 69-81.
Some researchers have claimed that women search for social Brackett, M.A., Mayer, J.D. and Warner, R.M. (2004),
confirmation and care more about others’ opinions than men “Emotional intelligence and its relation to everyday
(Venkatesh et al., 2000; Wardle et al., 2004). Future research behaviour”, Personality and Individual Differences, Vol. 36
should deconstruct this variable into its different elements and No. 6, pp. 1387-1402.
examine this question to gain more consistent results. In Brock, T.C. and Brannon, L.A. (1992), “Liberalization of
addition, the model proposed in this research is based on commodity theory”, Basic and Applied Social Psychology,
participants solely from individualistic, Western cultures (e.g. Vol. 13 No. 1, pp. 135-144.
Israel and the UK). Further research should explore whether Brown, D.E. (2004), “Human universals, human nature and
the model proposed in the study remains the same when human culture”, Daedalus, Vol. 133 No. 4, pp. 47-54.
applied in collectivist cultures that may have different gender Buchholz, L.M. and Smith, R.E. (1991), “The role of consumer
stereotypes. involvement in determining cognitive response to broadcast
Another important direction of future work might focus on advertising”, Journal of Advertising, Vol. 20 No. 1, pp. 4-17.
possible moderators that may change the results when Buser, T. (2012), “The impact of the menstrual cycle and
examining gender differences in the importance of a utility in hormonal contraceptives on competitiveness”, Journal of
low-involvement products. It might be that in low- Economic Behavior and Organization, Vol. 83 No. 1,
involvement products with gender-related sensory cues (e.g. pp. 1-10.
The role of product involvement Journal of Product & Brand Management
Enav Friedmann and Oded Lowengart

Campbell, A., Shirley, L. and Candy, J. (2004), “A longitudinal Kuikka, A. and Laukkanen, T. (2012), “Brand loyalty and the
study of gender-related cognition and behavior”, Developmental role of hedonic value”, Journal of Product & Brand
Science, Vol. 7 No. 1, pp. 1-9. Management, Vol. 21 No. 7, pp. 529-537.
Celsi, R.L. and Olson, J.C. (1988), “The role of involvement in Kuruvilla, S.J., Joshi, N. and Shah, N. (2009), “Do men and
attention and comprehension processes”, Journal of women really shop differently? An exploration of gender
Consumer Research, Vol. 15 No. 2, pp. 210-224. differences in mall shopping in India”, International Journal of
Cosmides, L. and Tooby, J. (2013), “Evolutionary psychology: Consumer Studies, Vol. 33 No. 6, pp. 715-723.
new perspectives on cognition and motivation”, Annual Laurent, G. and Kapferer, J.N. (1985), “Measuring consumer
Review of Psychology, Vol. 64, pp. 201-229. involvement profiles”, Journal of Marketing Research, Vol. 22
Dennis, C. and McCall, A. (2005), “The savannah hypothesis of No. 1, pp. 41-53.
shopping”, Business Strategy Review, Vol. 16 No. 3, pp. 12-16. Lee, K. (2009), “Gender differences in Hong Kong adolescent
Eagly, A.H. (1987), Sex Differences in Social Behavior: A Social- consumers’ green purchasing behavior”, Journal of Consumer
Role Interpretation, Erlbaum, Hillsdale, NJ. Marketing, Vol. 26 No. 2, pp. 87-96.
Falk, P. and Campbell, C. (1997), The Shopping Experience, Limtanakool, N., Dijst, M. and Schwanen, T. (2006), “The
Sage Publications, London. influence of socioeconomic characteristics, land use and
Ferreira, A.G. and Coelho, F.J. (2015), “Product involvement, travel time considerations on mode choice for medium-and
price perceptions, and brand loyalty”, Journal of Product & longer-distance trips”, Journal of Transport Geography,
Brand Management, Vol. 24 No. 4, pp. 349-364. Vol. 14 No. 5, pp. 327-341.
Fischer, E. and Arnold, S.J. (1994), “Sex, gender identity, McCabe, C.A. (2001), “Gender effects on attitude toward
gender role attitudes, and consumer behavior”, Psychology sports consumption”, doctorial dissertation, University of
and Marketing, Vol. 11 No. 2, pp. 163-182. Kentucky Press, Lexington.
Downloaded by ECU Libraries At 02:43 01 March 2019 (PT)

Fisher, H.E. (1999), The First Sex: The Natural Talents of Meyers-Levy, J. and Loken, B. (2015), “Revisiting gender
Women and How They Will Change the World, Random differences: what we know and what lies ahead”, Journal of
House, New York, NY. Consumer Psychology, Vol. 25 No. 1, pp. 129-149.
Friedmann, E. and Brueller, D. (2018), “Is stereotypical Miller, J.B. (2012), Toward a New Psychology of Women, Beacon
gender targeting effective for increasing service choice?”, Press, Boston, MA.
Journal of Retailing and Consumer Services, Vol. 44, pp. 35-44. Mittal, B. (1989), “Measuring purchase-decision involvement”,
Friedmann, E. and Lowengart, O. (2018), “The context of Psychology and Marketing, Vol. 6 No. 2, pp. 147-162.
choice as boundary condition for gender differences in brand Niederle, M. and Vesterlund, L. (2007), “Do women shy away
choice considerations”, European Journal of Marketing, from competition? Do men compete too much?”, Quarterly
Vol. 52 Nos 5/6, pp. 1280-1304. Journal of Economics, Vol. 122 No. 3, pp. 1067-1101.
Gilbert, D., Lee-Kelley, L. and Barton, M. (2003), Nkwocha, I., Bao, Y., Johnson, W.C. and Brotspies, H.V.
“Technophobia, gender influences and consumer decision- (2005), “Product fit and consumer attitude toward brand
making for technology-related products”, European Journal of extensions: the moderating role of product involvement”,
Innovation Management, , Vol. 6 No. 4, pp. 253-263. Journal of Marketing Theory and Practice, Vol. 13 No. 3,
Ghose, S. and Lowengart, O. (2001b), “Taste tests: impacts of pp. 49-61.
consumer perceptions and preferences on brand positioning Palan, K.M. (2001), “Gender identity in consumer behavior
strategies”, Journal of Targeting, Measurement and Analysis for research: a literature review and research agenda”, Academy
Marketing, Vol. 10 No. 1, pp. 26-41. of Marketing Science Review, Vol. 2001 No. 10, pp. 1-31.
Grau, S.L. and Zotos, Y.C. (2016), “Gender stereotypes in Park, C.W., Jaworski, B.J. and Maclnnis, D.J. (1986),
advertising: a review of current research”, International “Strategic brand concept-image management”, Journal of
Journal of Advertising, Vol. 35 No. 5, pp. 761-770. Marketing, Vol. 50 No. 4, pp. 135-145.
Griskevicius, V., Tybur, J.M., Sundie, J.M., Cialdini, R.B., Petty, R.E. and Cacioppo, J.T. (1986), “The elaboration
Miller, G.F. and Kenrick, D.T. (2007), “Blatant likelihood model of persuasion”, Advances in Experimental
benevolence and conspicuous consumption: when romantic Social Psychology, Vol. 19 No. 1, pp. 123-205.
motives elicit strategic costly signals”, Journal of Personality Prättälä, R., Paalanen, L., Grinberga, D., Helasoja, V.,
and Social Psychology, Vol. 93 No. 1, pp. 85-102. Kasmel, A. and Petkeviciene, J. (2006), “Gender differences
Jackson, V., Stoel, L. and Brantley, A. (2011), “Mall attributes in the consumption of meat, fruit and vegetables are similar
and shopping value: differences by gender and generational in Finland and the Baltic countries”, European Journal of
cohort”, Journal of Retailing and Consumer Services, Vol. 18 Public Health, Vol. 17 No. 5, pp. 520-525.
No. 1, pp. 1-9. Punj, G. and Moore, R. (2009), “Information search and
Knox, S. (2004), “Positioning and branding your organization”, consideration set formation in a web-based store environment”,
Journal of Product & Brand Management, Vol. 13 No. 2, Journal of Business Research, Vol. 62 No. 6, pp. 644-650.
pp. 105-115. Quester, P. and Lin Lim, A. (2003), “Product involvement/
Kohli, C.S. and Leuthesser, L. (1993), “Product positioning: a brand loyalty: is there a link?”, Journal of Product & Brand
comparison of perceptual mapping techniques”, Journal of Management, Vol. 12 No. 1, pp. 22-38.
Product & Brand Management, Vol. 2 No. 4, pp. 10-19. Reis, S.M. (1991), “The need for clarification in research
Kotler, P. (1997), Marketing Management: Analysis, Planning, designed to examine gender differences in achievement and
Implementation and Control, Prentice Hall, Upper Saddle accomplishment”, Roeper Review, Vol. 13 No. 4,
River, NJ. pp. 193-198.
The role of product involvement Journal of Product & Brand Management
Enav Friedmann and Oded Lowengart

Richins, M.L. and Bloch, P.H. (1986), “After the new wears Tifferet, S. and Herstein, R. (2012), “Gender differences in
off: the temporal context of product involvement”, Journal of brand commitment, impulse buying, and hedonic
Consumer Research, Vol. 13 No. 2, pp. 280-285. consumption”, Journal of Product & Brand Management,
Rigdon, E.E., Schumacker, R.E. and Wothke, W. (1998), “A Vol. 21 No. 3, pp. 176-182.
comparative review of interaction and nonlinear modeling”, Trofimova, I. (2015), “Do psychological sex differences reflect
Interaction and Nonlinear Effects in Structural Equation evolutionary bisexual partitioning?”, The American Journal of
Modeling, Erlbaum, Mahwah, NJ. Psychology, Vol. 128 No. 4, pp. 485-514.
Rothschild, M.L. (1979), “Marketing communications in Tynan, C. and McKechnie, S. (2009), “Experience marketing:
nonbusiness situations or why it’s so hard to sell brotherhood a review and reassessment”, Journal of Marketing
like soap”, Journal of Marketing, Vol. 43, pp. 11-20. Management, Vol. 25 Nos 5/6, pp. 501-517.
Saad, G. and Gill, T. (2000), “Applications of evolutionary Walsh, G., Kilian, T. and Buxel, H. (2008), “Assessing the
psychology in marketing”, Psychology and Marketing, Vol. 17 consumer perceived value scale”, Advances in Consumer
No. 12, pp. 1005-1034. Research, Vol. 35, pp. 688-689.
Schroeder, J.E. and Borgerson, J.L. (2015), “Critical visual Wardle, J., Haase, A.M., Steptoe, A., Nillapun, M.,
analysis of gender: reactions and reflections”, Journal of Jonwutiwes, K. and Bellisie, F. (2004), “Gender
Marketing Management, Vol. 31 Nos 15/16, pp. 1723-1731. differences in food choice: the contribution of health beliefs
Schumacker, R.E. and Marcoulides, G.A. (1998), Interaction and dieting”, Annals of Behavioral Medicine: A Publication of
and Nonlinear Effects in Structural Equation Modeling, the Society of Behavioral Medicine, Vol. 27 No. 2,
Erlbaum, Mahwah, NJ. pp. 107-116.
Sharma, P., Chen, I. and Luk, S. (2012), “Gender and age as Venkatesh, V., Morris, M.G. and Ackerman, P.L. (2000), “A
moderators in the service evaluation process”, Journal of longitudinal field investigation of gender differences in
Downloaded by ECU Libraries At 02:43 01 March 2019 (PT)

Services Marketing, Vol. 26 No. 2, pp. 102-114. individual technology adoption decision-making processes”,
Sheth, J.N., Newman, B.I. and Gross, B.L. (1991), “Why we Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes,
buy what we buy: a theory of consumption values”, Journal of Vol. 83 No. 1, pp. 33-60.
Business Research, Vol. 22 No. 2, pp. 159-170. Verplanken, B. and Herabadi, A. (2001), “Individual differences
Shields, S.A. (2013), “Gender and emotion: what we think in impulse buying tendency: feeling and no thinking”, European
we know, what we need to know, and why it matters”, Journal of Personality, Vol. 15 No. S1, pp. s71-s83.
Psychology of Women Quarterly, Vol. 37 No. 4, Wilkie, W.L. and Pessemier, E.A. (1973), “Issues in
pp. 423-435. marketing’s use of multi-attribute attitude models”, Journal
Sirgy, J.M. (1982), “Self-concept in consumer behavior: a critical of Marketing Research, Vol. 10 No. 4, pp. 428-441.
review”, Journal of Consumer Research, Vol. 9 No. 3, pp. 287-299. Yang, K. and Lee, H. (2010), “Gender differences in using
Spence, J.T. and Helmreich, R. (1978), Masculinity and mobile data services: utilitarian and hedonic value
Femininity, University of TX Press, Austin, TX. approaches”, Journal of Research in Interactive Marketing,
Spence, J.T. and Sawin, L.L. (1985), “Images of masculinity Vol. 4 No. 2, pp. 142-156.
and femininity: a reconceptualization”, in O’Leary, V.E., Ye, L. and Robertson, T.M. (2012), “Gender identity: does it
Unger, R.K. and Wallston, B.S. (Eds), Women, Gender, and matter for consumers’ perceptions?”, Journal of Business
Psychology, Erlbaum, Hillsdale, NJ, pp. 35-66. Diversity, Vol. 12 No. 3, pp. 81-92.
Swani, K., Weinberger, M.G. and Gulas, C.S. (2013), “The Yen, S.T. (2005), “Zero observations and gender differences in
impact of violent humor on advertising success: a gender cigarette consumption”, Applied Economics, , Vol. 37 No. 16,
perspective”, Journal of Advertising, Vol. 42 No. 4, pp. 1839-1849.
pp. 308-319.
Sweeney, J.C. and Soutar, G.N. (2001), “Consumer perceived Corresponding author
value: the development of a multiple item scale”, Journal of Enav Friedmann can be contacted at: enav.friedmann@
Retailing, Vol. 77 No. 2, pp. 203-220. gmail.com
The role of product involvement Journal of Product & Brand Management
Enav Friedmann and Oded Lowengart

Appendix 1. Demographics of the field sample


(Study 1)

Table AI
Total (n = 459) Cars and sausage (n = 221) Computers and soap (n = 238)
Demographics Women (n = 247) Men (n = 212) Women (n = 120) Men (n = 101) Women (n = 137) Men (n = 101)
Mean SD Mean SD Mean SD Mean SD Mean SD Mean SD
Age in years 26.66 4.82 27.42 4.29 26.84 5.03 27.46 4.71 26.5 4.64 27.39 3.88
Frequencies women Frequencies men Frequencies women Frequencies men Frequencies women Frequencies men
Income
Substantially below average (%) 51.6 43.6 48.3 44.6 54.8 42.6
Somewhat below average (%) 20.5 23.3 19.2 24.8 21.8 21.8
Average (%) 11.4 12.8 13.3 9.9 8.9 15.8
Somewhat above average (%) 11.0 12.9 10 13.9 11.3 11.9
Substantially above average (%) 3.4 5.4 3.3 5.9 3.2 5
Notes: Frequency of purchasing high-involvement products: mean frequency of men 21.78 months, SD = 22.59; mean frequency of women 20.66 months,
SD = 21.47; frequency of purchasing low-involvement products: mean frequency of men 1.87 months, SD = 2.38; mean frequency of women 1.62 months,
SD = 2.15
Downloaded by ECU Libraries At 02:43 01 March 2019 (PT)

Appendix 2. Questions in questionnaire. Please


answer the questions below about Brands 1/2/3 as
you perceive them on a scale of 1 to 7. (These
questions were asked for three real brands in
each product category)

Func1 Performance of the brand


(poor performance) 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 (great performance)
Func2 Quality of components
(low quality) 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 (high quality)
Exp1 I will enjoy using the brand
(don’t agree at all) 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 (totally agree)
Exp2 This brand will make me want to use it
(don’t agree at all) 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 (totally agree)
Exp3 This brand gives me great pleasure
(don’t agree at all) 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 (totally agree)
Scon1 This brand will improve the way others look at me
(don’t agree at all) 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 (totally agree)
Scon2 This brand will make a good impression on others
(don’t agree at all) 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 (totally agree)
Scon3 People will compliment me when I use this brand
(don’t agree at all) 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 (totally agree)
The role of product involvement Journal of Product & Brand Management
Enav Friedmann and Oded Lowengart

Appendix 3

Table AII Means and standard deviations of involvement in four categories


High involvement Low involvement
Cars Computer Liquid soap Sausage
Gender Mean SD Mean SD Mean SD Mean SD
Men 6.32 0.95 6.21 0.94 3.26 1.43 2.88 1.31
Women 6.51 0.87 6.48 0.65 2.88 1.31 3.02 1.47
All 6.26 0.95 3.07 1.38
Notes: Differences between high- and low-involvement levels were significant [in men: t(395) = −26.83, sig = 0.000; in women: t(474) = −29.44,
sig = 0.000]

Appendix 4

Table AIII Demographics of the experimental sample (Study 2)


Low involvement (n = 125) High involvement (n = 130)
Demographics Women (n = 69) Men (n = 56) Women (n = 74) Men (n = 56)
Downloaded by ECU Libraries At 02:43 01 March 2019 (PT)

Demographics Mean SD Mean SD Mean SD Mean SD


Age in years 32.99 `8.90 34.52 `10.55 36.47 8.91 33.55 9.47
Frequencies women Frequencies men Frequencies women Frequencies men
Income
Substantially below average (%) 29 33.9 14.9 23.2
Somewhat below average (%) 30.4 21.4 33.8 16.1
Average (%) 14.5 16.1 20.3 21.4
Somewhat above average (%) 18.8 17.9 23.0 30.4
Substantially above average (%) 7.2 10.7 8.1 8.9
Notes: Differences did not exist between gender and income, in low involvement: x 2(df = 4) = 1.681, sig = 0.794; in high involvement: x 2(df = 4) =5.738,
sig = 0.220; nor were any significant age differences found between men and women: in low involvement: t(123) = 0.880, sig = 0.380; and in high
involvement: t(128) = 1.800, sig = 0.074. A chi-square test between frequency of purchase (scale of 1 – several days, 2 – several weeks, 3 – several months
and 4 – several years), and gender was not significant in the low-involvement level: x 2(df = 3) = 3.965, sig = 0.265, but it was significant in the high-
involvement level: x 2(df = 1) =7.185, sig = 0.028 (as 16 per cent of the men said they buy high-involvement products once every several months compared to
4 per cent of women, which means 96 per cent of the women said they buy them once every several years)

Appendix 5. Functional, socially conspicuous and


experiential claims presented in Study 2
BEST QUALITY.
BEST PERFORMANCE.

IMPRESSING OTHERS.
IMPROVING THE WAY PEOPLE
ARE LOOKING AT YOU.

GET ADDICTED
TO LIFE’S PLEASURES.

For instructions on how to order reprints of this article, please visit our website:
www.emeraldgrouppublishing.com/licensing/reprints.htm
Or contact us for further details: permissions@emeraldinsight.com

You might also like