You are on page 1of 31

See discussions, stats, and author profiles for this publication at: https://www.researchgate.

net/publication/247857275

The token economy: An evaluative review

Article in Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis · October 1972


DOI: 10.1901/jaba.1972.5-343

CITATIONS READS

576 7,137

2 authors, including:

Richard R Bootzin
The University of Arizona
144 PUBLICATIONS 13,229 CITATIONS

SEE PROFILE

All content following this page was uploaded by Richard R Bootzin on 22 May 2014.

The user has requested enhancement of the downloaded file.


JOURNAL OF APPLIED BEHAVIOR ANALYSIS 19 '2 5 A3 .`72 NUMBER 3 (FALL 1972)

THE TOKEN ECONOMY: AN EVALUATIVE REVIEW'


ALAN E. KAZDIN AND RICHARD R. BOOTZIN
THE PENNSYLVANIA STATE UNIVERSITY AND NORTHWESTERN UNIVERSITY

Token economies have been applied in a wide range of settings. While there are several
advantages to the use of this procedure, there are obstacles that may impede its imple-
mentation and therapeutic efficacy. These include: staff training, client resistance, cir-
cumvention of the contingencies, and non-responsiveness of subjects. Studies employing
token programs with psychiatric patients, retardates, children in classroom settings,
delinquents, and autistic children are reviewed. Although token economies are successful
while in operation, the issue of generalization of behavior gains or resistance to extinc-
tion has not been given careful consideration. Inasmuch as generalization is perhaps the
most crucial issue, several procedures are presented that are designed to facilitate main-
tenance of performance when reinforcement is withdrawn. Methodological suggestions
for investigations on token reinforcement in applied settings are presented.

Operant approaches to behavior change have when the back-up reinforcer cannot be parcelled
become increasingly popular in recent years out; (4) allow sequences of responses to be
(see Bandura, 1969; Sherman and Baer, 1969; reinforced without interruption; (5) maintain
Ullmann and Krasner, 1965, 1969). An ex- their reinforcing properties because of their
traordinarily wide range of deviant and mal- relative independence of deprivation states; (6)
adaptive behavior has been treated, ranging from are less subject to satiation effects; (7) provide
decreasing the frequency of thumbsucking the same reinforcement for individuals who
(Baer, 1962) to teaching mute autistic children have different preferences in back-up rein-
to speak (Lovaas, 1968). The success of these forcers; and (8) may take on greater incentive
techniques encouraged investigators to develop value than a single primary reinforcer since,
systems to modify the behavior of groups of according to Ferster and DeMyer (1962), the
individuals with maladaptive behaviors. This effects resulting from association with each pri-
was made possible by using generalized con- mary reinforcer may summate.
ditioned reinforcers (see Kelleher and Gollub, There are additional advantages in using
1962). Such reinforcers are interchangeable for tangible conditioned reinforcers, such as tokens,
a wide variety of primary and back-up rein- instead of other generalized conditioned rein-
forcers. forcers, such as approval. Some of these as listed
There are a number of advantages in using by Ayllon and Azrin (1968a) are: "(1) The
generalized conditioned reinforcers. Specifically, number of tokens can bear a simple quantita-
conditioned reinforcers: (1) bridge the delay tive relation to the amount of reinforcement;
between the target response and back-up rein- (2) the tokens are portable and can be in the
forcement; (2) permit the reinforcement of a subject's possession even when he is in a situa-
response at any time; (3) may be used to main- tion far removed from that in which the tokens
tain performance over extended periods of time were earned; (3) no maximum exists in the
number of tokens a subject may possess . . .;
'Reprints max- be obtained from Alan E. Kazdin, (4)
tokens can be used directly to operate de-
Department of Psychology, The Pennsylvania State vices for the automatic delivery of reinforcers;
University, University Park, Pennsylvania, 16802. (5) tokens are durable and can be continuously
343
344 ALAN E. KAZDIN and RICHARD R. BOOTZIN

present during the delay . .. ; (6) the physical explanations are often enough. When instruc-
characteristics of the tokens can be easily stan- tions are not sufficient, tokens are established
dardized; (7) the tokens can be made fairly as conditioned reinforcers by making them dis-
indestructible so they will not deteriorate during criminative stimuli for the back-up reinforcers.
the delay; (8) the tokens can be made unique This is typically accomplished by giving out a
and nonduplicable so that the experimenter can few tokens immediately before the opportunity
be assured that they are received only in the to "spend" them. After the value of tokens is
authorized manner." (p. 77). In addition, to- established, clients are informed that they may
kens provide a visible record of improvement. earn tokens by performing various behaviors.
This may facilitate social reinforcement from Finally, the rules of the token system are pro-
staff members, as well as self-reinforcement. vided, i.e., how tokens may be earned, spent, and
A brief explanation of the steps required to lost; and the system is in operation.
implement a reinforcement program will fa- Although this model generally holds, pro-
cilitate the presentation of the token programs grams often differ in a number of procedural
in this review. Initially, identification of those details. For example, in some programs, rein-
behaviors to be altered is required. Investigators forcement contingencies are the same for all
have emphasized the importance of specifying people in the economy; in other programs, the
the responses of interest in descriptive terms in contingencies are individualized. In the former
order that reinforcement can be delivered for an case, a particular behavior is reinforced for all
unambiguous response, and so that evaluation clients with the same magnitude of reinforce-
of progress through behavioral assessment can ment (i.e., number of tokens). In the latter
be made (Ayllon and Azrin, 1968a, p. 36; case, performance of a particular behavior (e.g.,
Schaefer and Martin, 1969). reading) may be reinforced for some individuals
After target behaviors are determined, the but not for others, or the magnitude of rein-
available reinforcers in the environment must forcement may vary. Individualized contingen-
be defined and enumerated. Back-up reinforcers cies have the obvious advantage of focusing on
for tokens may be selected on the basis of a particular idiosyncratic problems. A number of
principle elucidated by Premack (1965). The token programs have combined the two types
principle states that a more-probable or higher- of contingencies. That is, use is made of group
frequency behavior may be used to reinforce contingencies, and added to these are ways in
a less-probable behavior. Thus, to select effec- which each individual can earn tokens depend-
tive reinforcers, it is only necessary to note ing upon his particular problems.
which responses are frequently emitted. Privi- Programs also differ considerably in the
leges such as "sitting around", leaving the ward, amount of staff training provided and in pro-
going on walks, and watching television can be cedures initiated to: (1) minimize resistance of
used as reinforcers if their performance is of a clients to the economy, (2) prevent circumven-
higher relative frequency than the other behav- tion of the contingencies, and (3) deal with
iors that they are to reinforce. Other reinforcers nonresponsiveness. The way in which these
included as back-up reinforcers in virtually problems are resolved can make the difference
every token program include canteen items between an effective and ineffective program.
usually displayed in a "store". At various periods Because of this, a detailed review of these
of the day, clients may exchange tokens for food, problems follows.
cigarettes, toys, toiletries, and so on.
After target behaviors are agreed upon and Staf Training2
reinforcers are selected, it is necessary to estab- The training of staff to administer a token
lish the tokens as secondary reinforcers. Verbal economy represents a formidable task for the
THE TOKEN ECONOMY: AN EVALUATIVE REVIEW 345
effective use of reinforcement procedures. Per- when in contact with the clients. In fact, Gard-
haps more than with other procedures carried ner (1972) found that rehearsing appropriate
out in treatment facilities, the nonprofessional behavior in training led to superior performance
staff must be adept at employing the procedures than did lectures alone.
of an operant program. Nonprofessional staff, Even when staff are trained adequately, pos-
as behavioral engineers, are responsible for what itive consequences must be associated with de-
behaviors are reinforced, extinguished, shaped, sirable performance. For example, Panyan,
punished, and so on. Boozer, and Morris (1970) showed that sub-
Training is a formidable task because attend- sequent to their training program, the staff be-
ants and teachers often maintain inappropriate came increasingly lax in their use of skills. Once
behavior by reinforcing deviant responses feedback was reinstated, the staff improved.
(Buehler, Furniss, and Patterson, 1966; Dobson, Some programs have been able to reinforce
Gelfand, and Gelfand, 1967; Ebner, unpub- the staff with tangible reinforcers, such as salary
lished). In mental hospitals, in particular, con- increases, vacations, and workshift preferences
tingencies are frequently arranged for the com- (e.g., Ayllon and Azrin, 1968a). A less-dramatic
fort and convenience of the attendants, rather procedure was employed by McNamara (1971).
than the treatment and training of the patients He dispensed tokens to teachers that could be
(Dunham and Weinberg, 1960; Goffman, exchanged at the end of the day for beer.
1961; Ullmann and Krasner, 1969). Many re- The training of staff into effective behavioral
searchers have emphasized the importance of engineers remains a crucial obstacle that must
adequate staff training programs (e.g., Becker, be successfully confronted for a maximally ef-
Kuypers, and O'Leary, 1968; Krasner, 1968; fective program. Effective implementation of
Miron, unpublished). Ross (unpublished) sug- reinforcement procedures with qualified staff is
gested that the staff remains the "Achilles' heel" a prior condition that must be met before the
of operant programs. Although there is no theoretical questions regarding the application
doubt that staff training is important, behavior of various principles can be considered. Even
change has sometimes been accomplished by when staff behaviors are effectively altered while
staffs having only minimal levels of training the program is in effect, there has been little
(Kuypers et al., 1968; Meichenbaum, Bowers, follow-up of staff behavior to ensure that these
and Ross, 1968). Even so, a highly trained staff changes are maintained. Although the evidence
would appear to be an advantage. Only quite for resistance to extinction is not encouraging
recently has there been any research evaluating (Kazdin, 1972a; Panyan et al., 1970) one in-
training programs. Most training programs rely teresting study has been reported. Baldwin
upon the usual academic procedures of lectures, (1967) found that attendants, trained in rein-
reading, and examination. Such procedures im- forcement techniques on a token economy ward
part knowledge of behavior principles without with retarded children, performed significantly
guarantees that staff perform appropriately more positive patient-oriented responses and
less "custodial" responses when working on
other wards than attendants without training.
2The significance of staff training in operant con-
ditioning programs is attested to by the appearance of Resistance of Clients to the
various training manuals for the ward personnel of Token-Reinforcement System
psychiatric patients (Schaefer and Martin, 1969) and
retardates (Bensberg, 1965), and for the parents and Client resistance is expressed in the form of
teachers of school children (Becker, Engelmann, and anger, complaints, disruptive behavior, impul-
Thomas, 1971; Homme, Csanyi, Gonzales, and
Rechs, 1969; Meacham and Wiesen, 1970; Mink, sive acts, rule-breaking, and requests for transfer
1970; Patterson and Guillon, 1968). to other wards or hospitals. Although there have
346 ALAN E. KAZDIN and RICHARD R. BOOTZIN

been only a few reports of adverse reactions to are obtained in ways contrary to the goals of
the initial establishment of token reinforcement the administration and staff. One report has
procedures (e.g., Guyett, unpublished; Lachen- been presented of a female schizophrenic obtain-
meyer, 1969), it is probably a more frequent ing back-up reinforcers from male patients in
problem than has been reported. However, from exchange for sexual favors (Liberman, 1968).
the numerous reports that have failed to note Stealing tokens has also enabled some individ-
patient resistance, and the few reports that men- uals to escape reinforcement contingencies.
tion favorable patient reaction, it would appear Typically, to overcome stealing, tokens are in
that client resistance is not intrinsic to the token some way marked to individualize them, thus
reinforcement procedures. It may be reasonable making theft unprofitable (Lachenmeyer,
to expect minor resistances to follow implemen- 1969).
tation of contingent reinforcement procedures. Contingencies may also be escaped when be-
Token programs often use reinforcers (e.g., haviors are performed without staff surveillance.
privacy, recreational privileges, free-time, meals, This presents a twofold problem for token pro-
and so on) that were previously freely available. grams. When staff members are not present,
A general method employed that seems to desirable behaviors performed by a client are
mitigate resistance involves planning the pro- unreinforced. Similarly, undesirable responses
gram in conjunction with the client, thus em- performed in the absence of staff may be rein-
phasizing his responsibility for his behavior. For forced (by peers, or the reinforcers gained from
example, Steffy (unpublished) reported the the behavior). To resolve this dilemma, inves-
planning of token reinforcement programs with tigators have tried to design programs such that
aggressive psychotic patients. Contractual agree- desirable responses (as well as undesirable re-
ments were successfully made with patients on sponses) are detected by some change in the
an individual basis in order to link reinforcers physical environment (Burchard, 1969). For
with the performance of critical behaviors. example, in examining whether or not an in-
Patients have also been employed in the dividual performed various self-care behaviors,
execution of routine duties connected with the specific checks are made to notice if the patient's
token system (e.g., banking of tokens, checking bed is made, if he is showered, and so on (Ayl-
attendance of patients to assignments) (At- lon and Azrin, 1965, 1968a). The number of
thowe, unpublished, c); and have had important behaviors that may be defined by distinct
roles in developing contingencies (Lovitt and changes in the physical environment is limited.
Curtiss, 1969). Subtle behaviors are often not handled by the
Procedures exemplified by Steffy (unpub- contingencies, perhaps, because their effects are
lished) and Atthowe (unpublished a, unpub- difficult to define. Thus, escape from reinforce-
lished c) appear to be effective in overcoming ment contingencies remains a problem in all
the patient resistance reported by other authors. token programs. Even so, as Burchard (1969)
Emphasis on patient responsibility for his own noted, considerable changes have been obtained
behavior, lack of coercion, contractual arrange- by means of token reinforcement. It is an em-
ments of programs, and client voice in matters pirical question as to whether complete control
relating to the program, all seem to mitigate of complex contingencies is a prerequisite for
against patient rebellion and the possibility of effective treatment.
an unjust and oppressive system.
Nonresponsiveness to Reinforcement
Circumvention of Contingencies Procedures
Often, specific contingencies for clients can Some populations with which reinforcement
be circumvented if tokens or back-up reinforcers techniques have been used may present limiting
THE TOKEN ECONOMY: AN EVALUATIVE REVIEW 347
conditions for the effective application of oper- three of seven retardates studied were unaffected
ant principles. For example, Lindsley (1956, by the token-reinforcement contingencies. Ray
1960) and Sidman (1960, 1962) found that and Shelton (unpublished) reported that 13%
psychotics in a free-operant situation responded of the disturbed adolescent retardates studied did
quite differently than normals. Response rates not show significant reductions in inappropriate
were much slower and there were many un- behaviors. It was noted that all of these subjects
predictable pauses in responding. Similar re- exhibited psychotic symptoms or "complex"
sults have been found with the mentally re- behavioral problems. An examination of the
tarded (Barrett and Lindsley, 1962; Ellis, 1962; data reported by Hunt and Zimmerman (1969)
Spradlin, 1962) and with autistic children (Fer- indicates that two of .14 subjects did not in-
ster and DeMyer, 1961, 1962; Metz, 1965). crease performance over baseline.
These studies suggest that nonresponsiveness The data are rather convincing that a small
could be expected to be a problem in token percentage of patients in almost all token pro-
economies with these patient populations. In- grams remain unaffected. The question that re-
deed, this has been the case. mains is whether this is due to the practical
Ayllon and Azrin (1965), in their series of problems related to implementing and carrying
studies on the effect of contingent reinforce- out the procedures or whether it is due to the
ment in altering job preferences, found that applicability and appropriateness of the operant
18% of the chronic schizophrenic patients paradigm with some individuals (see Davison,
"were relatively unaffected by the reinforce- 1969, for possible limitations of the operant
ment procedure" (p. 318). The patients who paradigm with psychiatric patients). Although
did not respond were not distinguished by psy- the research to answer this question has not yet
chiatric diagnosis, age, IQ, or length of hospit- been done, some interesting solutions for non-
alization. The authors suggested that a failure responsiveness have been proposed.
to use sufficiently reinforcing behaviors, such as Many researchers individualize the contin-
sleeping and eating, in the management of the gencies of the economy to maximize the prob-
contingencies accounted for the unresponsive- ability of responsiveness. Atthowe and Krasner
ness of some subjects. (1968) occasionally devaluated the tokens as
Atthowe (unpublished), and Atthowe and a way of discouraging hoarding and encourag-
Krasner, (1968) reported that 10% of the pa- ing utilization of available reinforcers. Ayllon
tients (those most severely withdrawn) did not and Azrin (1968b) used what they call "rein-
gain from the token program. Of the individuals forcer sampling" to increase responsiveness.
who did respond to the token economy, the Patients are first encouraged to sample potential
"most active" patients gained the least. Non- reinforcers in settings that would maximize their
responsiveness was attributed to a failure to use. Only after the reinforcer has acquired value
use strong reinforcers, and to the use of a gen- for the patient is it associated with tokens.
eralized ward program in lieu of individualized Having provided an overview of the pro-
contingencies. Other studies with psychiatric pa- cedures employed in token economies, a review
tients have also found that long-term withdrawn of specific programs is now presented.
patients show the least over-all improvement
(e.g., Curran, Jourd, and Whitman, unpub- Psychiatric Inpatients
lished; Golub, unpublished; Steffy, unpub- The major impetus for token economies with
lished). psychiatric inpatients has come from the crea-
Reports of nonresponsiveness have not been tive and systematic work of Ayllon and Azrin
restricted to psychiatric settings. Zimmerman, (1965, 1968a). They employed a token econ-
Zimmerman, and Russell (1969) reported that omy for "backward", female, chronic schizo-
348 ALAN E. KAZDIN and RICHARD R. BOOTZIN

phrenics at Anna State Hospital in Illinois. The that prolongs the chronic schizophrenic's hos-
patients had a median age of about 50 yrs and pitalization. Other studies have also indicated
median years of hospitalization of about 16. that the self-care behaviors of chronic schizo-
Because patients exhibited severe behavioral def- phrenics can be modified within the framework
icits, the authors had very modest goals. The of a token economy (Atthowe and Krasner,
behaviors reinforced were primarily work activ- 1968; Ellsworth, 1969, Golub, unpublished;
ities and self-care behaviors. To evaluate the Lloyd and Garlington, 1968; Steffy, Hart, Craw,
effectiveness of the reinforcement procedures, Torney, and Marlett, 1969). Self-care behav-
Ayllon and Azrin used the within-subject ABAB iors that have been successfully increased by
design in which the frequency of the behavior such programs include continence, self-feeding,
of interest is alternately reinforced and not rein- getting out of bed on time, and personal ap-
forced in consecutive phases of the experiment. pearance routines such as shaving and wearing
The studies indicated that reinforcement was clean clothes.
effective in maintaining job performance in the Even when the token economy is focused
hospital, as compared with noncontingent token primarily upon self-care behaviors, some authors
reinforcement. The reinforcement contingencies have reported beneficial effects in social be-
were effective in altering voluntary job prefer- haviors. Atthowe and Krasner (1968), for ex-
ences by manipulation of the number of tokens ample, rated the "social responsiveness" of pa-
that could be earned. Similar effects were dem- tients in a weekly group meeting. They found
onstrated with on-ward assignments. Addition- that the social interaction was significantly in-
ally, the distribution of tokens noncontingently creased after the introduction of a token econ-
led to a decrease in such behaviors as job at- omy, even though it was not one of the target
tendance, thus demonstrating the salience of the behaviors. They also report a general "widening
response-reinforcement relationship. This ini- of interest and a lessening of apathy" (p. 40).
tial research program was successful in demon- Some programs have attempted to modify
strating the efficacy of a token economy. social behavior and apathy directly, rather than
Following the lead of Ayllon and Azrin, the depend upon generalized effects. A notable at-
use of token economies within psychiatric facil- tempt in this direction has been the work of
ities has increased dramatically. In a recent re- Henderson and Scoles (1970; Scoles and Hen-
port of the Veterans Administration (Chase, derson, unpublished; Henderson, unpublished,
unpublished) it was noted that as of December, 1969). They developed a program for psychotic
1969 there were 27 on-going token economy men that focused on vocational habits, counter-
programs within VA hospitals alone, involving symptom behavior, and social adjustment. The
937 patients. Although token economies have social activities that were reinforced included en-
proliferated at an accelerated rate, there are gaging in superficial conversation, initiating so-
only a few systematic outcome studies. cial interactions, conversing with visitors from
The previously mentioned work of Ayllon the community, and showing leadership and
and Azrin (1965, 1968a) indicated that con- social approach responses. Introduction, removal,
tingent reinforcement could increase the fre- and reintroduction of contingent token reinforce-
quency of work activities and self-care behaviors ment indicated that social responsivenness was
of long-term chronic schizophrenics. The im- clearly under the control of the reinforcement
mediate goal of the program was improved be- contingencies. However, independent measures
havior within the hospital, and this was accom- of social performance taken throughout the
plished. This is not a trivial demonstration, study failed to demonstrate behavioral changes
because it is often the lack of these behaviors beyond the specific responses reinforced (Scoles
THE TOKEN ECONOMY: AN EVALUATIVE REVIEW 349
and Henderson, unpublished). The specificity of for work performance and self-care behaviors.
the effects of reinforcement were noted also by Positive (token) and negative reinforcers were
Henderson (unpublished). In this latter study, employed. Negative reinforcers included time-
improvements in social and vocational skills out from reinforcement, seclusion, and physical
were reported. However, in spite of these im- restraint. Periods of controlled behavior received
provements, symptomatic behaviors remained positive reinforcement even if seclusion or phys-
for some patients. ical restraint had been needed to facilitate con-
Schaefer and Martin (1966) also attempted to trol. Evaluation of patient progress consisted of
modify social interaction and apathy. Their op- ratings of the patients' self-control during the
erational definition of apathy was engaging program compared with retrospective ratings
in only one behavior (e.g., standing) without of the patients' level of control before the pro-
the simultaneous performance of a concomi- gram. Hospital records were used to make ret-
tant behavior (e.g., talking). Behavioral check- rospective ratings. Improvement was noted for
list data were gathered on chronic patients. Half 64 of the 73 patients in the ratings of controlled
of the patients received token reinforcement for behavior. However, the manner in which assess-
the performance of various responses related ment was made, the use of retrospective ratings
to personal hygiene, social interaction, and ade- without the raters being "blind" as to the pa-
quate work performance; the remaining patients tients who were treated, and the lack of control
received tokens noncontingently. The results periods (within subjects) or of a control group
over a three-month period indicated that pa- all make the specific therapeutic agent of the
tients on contingent token reinforcement sig- program unclear.
nificantly decreased on apathy ratings over time Few studies have compared reinforcement
and were significantly more improved than con- procedures with other therapeutic treatments.
trol subjects at the termination of the study. In one such study, Marks, Sonoda, and Schalock
This study is particularly noteworthy because it (1968) compared relationship therapy (indi-
is one of the few in which a randomly assigned vidual sessions five days per week for 1 hr) with
control group was included. contingent token reinforcement on social be-
In addition to changing social behavior, de- havior, work competence, and communication
creasing the frequency of aggressive behavior skills. Each patient received both treatments
has been an important goal. Steffy and his as- consecutively (with the order being balanced
sociates (Fenz and Steffy, 1968; Steffy, unpub- for half of the patients). Both treatments were
lished; Steffy et al., 1969) described a program effective, as indicated by improvement on per-
to deal with both aggressive and regressed fe- sonality and behavioral measures. There were no
male in-patients. As part of this program (Steffy, differences between the treatments. The authors
unpublished), individual contracts were made noted some difficulties in keeping the treatments
and negotiated with each patient to help bring distinct (i.e., administering individual therapy
her aggressive behavior under control. Seven of without reinforcing desirable behaviors, and
nine aggressive patients showed increased job vice versa). Hartlage (1970) also compared
productivity and fewer acts of violence. How- contingent reinforcement with individual ther-
ever, the behavior of six of the eight socially apy. He found that contingent reinforcement
withdrawn patients was not altered. was the more effective treatment for chronic
The primary focus of a program developed schizophrenics, as indicated by measures of
by Curran, Jourd, and Whitman (unpublished) hospital adjustment and interpersonal relations.
was on behaviors related to self-control (e.g., no In summary, the effectiveness of token rein-
physical assaults). Reinforcement was also given forcement to increase the frequency of a target
350 ALAN E. KAZDIN and RICHARD R. BOOTZIN

response (whether it be self-care of social be- continued, social reinforcement maintained or


havior) seems well established. However, it is increased the initial improvements. Analysis of
also necessary to know whether the gains made a 1-yr follow-up indicated significant group im-
will be maintained in settings outside of the provements in self-care, personal appearance,
hospital. This aspect is discussed more fully in walking, and sitting behaviors. Social skills and
the section on generalization. verbal behavior did not improve over baseline.
Hunt, Fitzhugh, and Fitzhugh (1968) also
The Mentally Retarded described a token program designed to improve
Ward and self-care behaviors. The necessity personal appearance of retardates (mean IQ
of teaching severely retarded children basic self- 73). Those retardates who were most likely to
care behaviors has made that response class an graduate to the community were brought to-
important focus for many token programs es- gether in their own ward and participated in
tablished in institutional settings. There have the program. Behaviors related to personal ap-
been many reports describing ongoing token pearance while performing work activities were
programs in which little if any outcome data the focus of the program. An initial period (14
are presented (e.g., Anderson, Morrow, and days) of continuous reinforcement was followed
Schleisinger, unpublished; Bourgeois, 1968; by intermittent reinforcement (10 days). Dur-
Rooney, 1966). What follows is a brief presen- ing intermittent reinforcement, subjects more
tation of those few studies attempting some frequently met the personal appearance criteria
systematic evaluation. than during continuous reinforcement. Examina-
In a token program developed by Girardeau tion of the individual data indicated that rein-
and Spradlin (1964) for retarded girls in a forcement was effective in temporarily improv-
cottage residential setting, reinforcement was ing four of the 12 subjects. These subjects
contingent on good grooming, work tasks, and exhibited appropriate behavior during the rein-
cooperative play. Individualized criteria for forcement phases and met the criterion less
performance were set to reward improvement. frequently when reinforcement was discontinued.
Individualized contingencies were also used for It is unclear what the results indicate about the
behavior problems of particular individuals. The remaining subjects who maintained appropriate
authors report marked gains four-and-one-half behavior during the extinction phase. However,
months after beginning the program. Lent since an individual baseline was not taken, there
(unpublished, unpublished, 1968) continued is no way of differentiating subjects who im-
and expanded the program started by Girardeau proved during reinforcement phases from sub-
and Spradlin. Residents were rewarded for per- jects who met the criterion before the program
sonal appearance, occupational skills, social be- began.
haviors, and functional academic skills. They re- Ray and Shelton (unpublished) reported a
ceived check marks for their. performance of token program with 42 institutionalized retard-
clearly specified appropriate behaviors. Group ates in which the focus of contingent reinforce-
contingencies were used, together with individ- ment was on behaviors related to self-care, din-
ualized contingencies for specific problems. The ing-room activities, and meal routines. The ma-
highly specific behavioral criteria ensured high jority of the subjects in the program (77%) were
interrater reliability (Lent, unpublished). reported as showing significant reductions in
Lent (unpublished) reported that several be- several inappropriate behaviors, such as eating
haviors within each of the previously mentioned at an inappropriate speed, and failing to meet
categories were modified by token reinforcement. standards of personal cleanliness. Of the 62 re-
Moreover, when token reinforcement was dis- tardates who were in the program at some point
THE TOKEN ECONOMY: AN EVALUATIVE REVIEW 351
during its 2-yr duration, eight (13%) were number and letter identification, cursive writing,
regarded as program failures. There were no and completing art work. The authors note that
experimental control periods in which the re- generalized positive effects were evident beyond
inforcement contingencies were reversed or the particular situations in which tokens were
omitted. given and that individualized reinforcement
Classroom behaviors. One of the earliest pro- contingencies appeared to be superior to group
grams was developed by Birnbrauer and Lawler contingencies.
(1964) for severely retarded children (IQs Zimmerman, Zimmerman, and Russell
below 40). Behaviors reinforced were entering (unpublished, 1969) tested the effectiveness of a
the classroom quietly, hanging coats, sitting at token program in developing instruction-follow-
desk attentively, and working persistently on a ing behavior. The class was initially praised
task. Initially, candy was made contingent upon when instructions were followed, then given
the performance of appropriate behaviors; sub- token reinforcement (with praise), then again
sequently, tokens were used and could be ex- praise alone, and, finally token reinforcement
changed for candy and trinkets. Token reinforce- (ABAB design). The duration of the experiment
ment procedures were effective in improving 37 was seven weeks.
of 41 pupils on behavioral criteria. The specific For four of seven subjects, token reinforce-
aspects of the program that led to these im- ment generated and maintained higher frequen-
provements, as the authors note, are unclear cies of instruction-following behavior than did
because control periods were not included. contingent praise. Two subjects were not differ-
Birnbrauer, Wolf, Kidder, and Tague (1965) entially affected by alterations of the contingen-
expanded the program of Birnbrauer and Law- cies, and one subject showed improvement
ler (1964) and attempted systematic evaluation throughout the study. It is important to note
of the token reinforcement procedures. The that this study compared the effects of contin-
dependent measures were the percentage of gent praise with and without the benefits of
errors in assignments, productivity (the number token reinforcement. Thus, the "failures," i.e.,
of items completed), and the amount of disrup- subjects who did not respond differentially to
tive behavior. Token reinforcement was dis- social and token reinforcement, perhaps demon-
pensed according to individualized performance strated the equality of the reinforcers for these
criteria. An ABAB design was used in which subjects, rather than reflected the ineffectiveness
token reinforcement was followed by no token of the operant procedures.
reinforcement and then by token reinforcement Workshop. Zimmerman, Stuckey, Garlick,
again. Contingent social approval was given for and Miller (1969) evaluated the effectiveness
appropriate behavior throughout the study. The of a token program for retardates in a sheltered
results confirmed the importance of token rein- workshop. After a baseline was established, sub-
forcement because the majority of subjects (10 jects were told that although they would not
of 15) showed decreased performance on at earn tokens for a while, they could "practice"
least one of the three criteria when tokens were (by working well) -so that they would know
not given. However, only four subjects showed how to earn tokens. During this practice period,
decreases in performance on all criteria, and subjects were told how many tokens they would
five subjects were not adversely affected at all. have earned if tokens had been given. Thus, the
Orlando, Schoelkopf, and Tobias (unpub- effect of feedback could be assessed indepen-
lished) described a similar classroom program dently of token reinforcement. In the next
for trainable and educable retardates. Token re- phase, contingent reinforcement was delivered
inforcement was dispensed for behaviors such as for production. This was followed by a phase
352 ALAN E. KAZDIN and RICHARD R. BOOTZIN

consisting of alternating days of practice and Madsen, Becker, and Thomas, 1968; Schmidt
token reinforcement. Then, baseline data were and Ulrich, 1969; Thomas, Becker, and Arm-
again collected with all contingencies removed. strong, 1968; Ward and Baker, 1968). How-
The results were that token reinforcement ever, in some settings and for some children,
elicited the highest production rates, followed praise may not be sufficiently reinforcing. In
by practice and then baseline. There was no dif- these instances, a token system may be more
ference between the first and second baseline. effective. Since token programs in the classroom
This study indicates clearly that feedback alone have been recently reviewed (O'Leary and Drab-
can improve performance, but that token rein- man, 1971), only a sample of relevant studies
forcement increases performance even further. is included here.
This study is particularly noteworthy for at- Walker and Buckley (1968) used token re-
tempting to separate the information value of inforcement with a child who had particular
tokens from their incentive and reinforcement difficulty in paying attention to classroom tasks.
value. Academic skills and social responsiveness had
Hunt and Zimmerman (1969) evaluated been previously altered with contingent social
token reinforcement for institutionalized re- reinforcement but attending behaviors remained
tardates in a simulated workshop. After a base- unaffected. A special treatment session was
line period in which productivity was assessed, scheduled in an isolated room for 40 min daily,
14 subjects were informed that increases in pro- during which the subject could earn points. The
duction (over individualized criterion levels) points could be exchanged for various tangible
would be rewarded with coupons redeemable objects. The subject was rewarded for paying
for items in a canteen. During the experimental attention for increasing durations of time (to a
sessions, this bonus for production was alter- maximum of 10 min). During a baseline period,
nated with no coupon payment. Following this the subject attended an average of 33% of the
period, all reinforcement was removed. The re- time. During contingent reinforcement, this in-
sults were that productivity was significantly creased to an average of 93%. Finally, when
higher in those periods in which reinforcement reinforcement was withdrawn, the average per-
was given than in periods in which no "bonus" centage of attending behavior fell to 44%. After
was given. Performance in the postexperimental the experiment, attending behaviors were main-
baseline was significantly higher than in the pre- tained in the classroom at a high level with
experimental baseline. A closer examination of token reinforcement delivered on a variable-
the data presented indicates that a number of interval schedule.
individuals did not increase in productivity in A series of studies using entire classrooms has
the experimental sessions, and of those who did, been done by O'Leary and Becker and their as-
some did not differentially respond to the token- sociates. In the first study, O'Leary and Becker
reinforcement and nonreinforcement periods (1967) described the use of token reinforce-
within the experimental sessions. ment in an elementary school adjustment class.
Students were placed in the special class because
Children in Classroom Settings of a history of undesirable classroom behaviors.
Operant principles have also been applied in Observations were made on a 20-sec observe,
elementary schools. Usually, classroom problems 10-sec record basis for a 2.5 hr period each day.
may be alleviated by instructing teachers to use During baseline, teachers conducted the class as
attention, praise, and approval as social rein- usual. During the token-reinforcement period,
forcers (Becker, Madsen, Arnold, and Thomas, students were told which behaviors received
1967; Hall, Panyan, Rabon, and Broden, 1968; points. These included paying attention, remain-
THE TOKEN ECONOMY: AN EVALUATIVE REVIEW 353
ing seated, and facing forward. Points were ex- of classroom rules, structured educational les-
changeable for small prizes. sons, teacher praise, and token reinforcement
The average deviant behavior for all children were examined on disruptive behaviors in an
was 76% during the baseline period and 10% elementary school classroom.
during token reinforcement. Although the pro- Eight experimental phases were presented to
cedures led to dramatic improvement in class- the class in the following order: (1) baseline
room behaviors, the effects of increases in teacher period; (2) classroom rules, i.e., instructions;
attention and daily instructions were confounded (3) planned lessons; (4) praising of appropri-
with the token reinforcement contingencies. As ate and ignoring of inappropriate behaviors;
mentioned previously, contingent teacher atten- (5) token reinforcement; (6) praising and
tion without token reinforcement is often effec- ignoring; (7) token reinforcement; and (8)
tive in reducing deviant behaviors. follow-up. The follow-up period was in fact
Kuypers et al., (1968) set up another token another token reinforcement period in which
economy in an elementary school classroom. there were fewer back-up reinforcers and in
However, in this study, the teachers received which group competition was introduced.
less training. They were not given background Rules, lesson structure, and praise-and-ignore
training in operant procedures, but rather were conditions did not have any reliable effect on
just given written instructions in how to carry deviant behaviors. Token reinforcement de-
out the study. Observations of behavior during creased disruptive behavior for six of the seven
baseline indicated deviant behavior for 54% of subjects. The "follow-up" period was somewhat
the observational periods. The percentage de- less effective than previous token periods. Gen-
creased to 28% when token reinforcement was eralization effects, such as increased class at-
given for instruction-following behaviors. An in- tendance during token-reinforcement periods
crease in percentage of deviant behaviors to and gains in achievement test scores, were also
41% was noted when token reinforcement was noted. The authors concluded that token rein-
removed. Individual data revealed that four of forcement was effective in reducing disruptive
the six children studied were reliably affected by behavior, whereas rules, lesson structure, and
the token reinforcement procedures. Little gen- social reinforcement were not. However, the re-
eralization of beneficial effects was noted from sults were less effective than those reported by
the afternoon class in which tokens were de- O'Leary and Becker (1967) in which rules, prais-
livered to the morning sessions in which token ing of appropriate and ignoring of inappropriate
procedures were not in effect. behaviors, and token reinforcement were intro-
The authors regarded this token program as duced simultaneously.
being only marginally effective, particularly Although analysis of the separate treatments
when compared to the results reported by was the goal of this investigation, treatment
O'Leary and Becker (1967). The discrepancy in conditions were cumulative. Rules were included
the efficacy of the two programs was attributed in the planned lesson period, and these condi-
to several factors in the replication. Primarily, tions were both included in the praise-ignore
there was a failure of teachers in the second condition. Hence, evaluation of the separate
program to use contingent teacher attention and effect of each condition is not possible. Even if
praise. the conditions were not combined in this man-
O'Leary, Becker, Evans, and Saudargas ner, the order of conditions was fixed. Thus, the
(1969) analyzed the various procedures that effect of any condition cannot be separated from
were confounded with the token reinforcement the order in which it appeared or the effect of a
contingencies in the previous studies. The effects history of prior conditions. In contrast, Kazdin
354 ALAN E. KAZDIN and RICHARD R. BOOTZIN

( 1972b) was able to analyze the separate effects within-subject design (n 2) that varying the
of instructions and token reinforcement. Four magnitude of reinforcement decreased or in-
different token economies were implemented in creased the number of reading assignments com-
elementary school classes. They represented all pleted.
combinations of the two variables: instructions In a second experiment, tokens were given
(either given or not) and reinforcement (either for language and arithmetic, as well as reading.
contingent or noncontingent). The results indi- After responses stabilized, the magnitude of
cated that contingent reinforcement, irrespec- reinforcement was increased for an individual's
tive of instructions, increased on-task and de- area of lowest performance. The results indicated
creased deviant behaviors. There was evidence that varying the number of tokens for a partic-
of generalization because improvement was ular behavior altered performance on that be-
noted in both morning and afternoon sessions, havior without affecting the performance of
even though reinforcement was given only in other behaviors for which reinforcement had not
the morning. changed. For every subject (n= 11), shifts in
Token procedures have not only been ap- contingencies led to shifts in performance of
plied to classroom management problems, they assignments.
have also been applied to help increase academic A number of additional token reinforcement
activities. Bushell, Wrobel, and Michaelis contingencies were included in this program.
(1968) focused upon such classroom behaviors Reinforcement was administered for extra work
as writing, reciting, participating in activities, assignments, attendance, grade averages, "good"
and completing projects with preschool chil- behavior, and cooperation. The effects of these
dren. Tokens could be exchanged for a special contingencies were not systematically evaluated.
event each day such as a short trip, movie, or The over-all effect of the program was evaluated
story. In a within-subject design, a token-rein- by comparing the subjects in the remedial pro-
forcement period was followed by a period in gram with a control group selected for, but not
which tokens were given contingently, but all assigned to, the program. At the end of a 1-yr
subjects received the special privilege. After period, the subjects who received the remedial
this period, the contingencies were reinstituted program were significantly higher in public
and tokens were again required for the special school grades and in gains on an achievement
event. The results indicated that study behavior test than the subjects not exposed to the pro-
scores varied with the contingent delivery of gram.
the back-up reinforcer. Performance declined In another study described by Wolf and Ris-
when tokens had no special purchasing power. ley (unpublished), points were given for com-
Wolf, Giles, and Hall (1968) examined the pleting assignments correctly and for being in
effectiveness of token reinforcement procedures one's seat when a bell sounded. The bell was
in a remedial education program for low achiev- sounded on a variable-interval schedule (VI
ing elementary school students (mean IQ = 88). 20-min). The results of the-e procedures showed
Reinforcement (points redeemable for candy, an increase in academic behavior and a decrease
novelties, clothing, food, field trips, movies, in disruptive behavior. These behaviors did not
privileges, money, and numerous other items) show improvement when token reinforcement
was given for completing classroom assignments was administered noncontingently or when sub-
correctly. Points were given differentially de- jects were only instructed to behave in the de-
pending upon the grade received for a particular sired manner.
remedial assignment as well as report-card A special program was developed for one
grades for regular class assignments. In an subject whose out-of-seat behavior had not been
initial experiment, it was demonstrated in a substantially affected by the previous contin-
THE TOKEN ECONOMY: AN EVALUATIVE REVIEW 355

gencies. Her behavior was brought under con- received token reinforcement in the final 17-
trol when it was used to determine whether or week period, showed marked improvement in
not her classmates would receive reinforcement. the final period. The E-C groups, contrary to the
The use of an individual's performance as a prediction, did not show a decline in perform-
criterion for delivering reinforcement to one's ance when token reinforcement was withdrawn
peers, along with concomitant peer pressure and after the first 17-week period. For this latter
social reinforcement, appears to be effective in group, a moderate gain was noted when token
modifying behavior (e.g., Patterson, 1965). reinforcement was removed. The reason for the
Barrish, Saunders, and Wolf (1969) devel- stability of the gain in behaviors in the control
oped this idea further within a token system in period is not clear from this study. 'Although
an elementary classroom. Students were divided token reinforcement improved attending be-
into two separate teams or groups. Disruptive haviors, reading and arithmetic achievement
behaviors (talking out or being out of seat) of levels were not reliably affected. Several addi-
any individual resulted in a point for his team. tional studies (Clark, Lachowic'z, and Wolf,
The group with the fewest points over various 1968; Graubard, 1969; Haring, Hayden, and
periods of time received a number of privileges Nolan, 1969; McKenzie, Clark, Wolf, Kothera,
(e.g., extra recess, time for special activities). and Benson, 1968; Miller and Schneider, 1970)
The contingencies significantly and reliably have also indicated that token reinforcement is
modified the target behaviors. Inappropriate be- an effective means of altering classroom be-
haviors increased when contingencies were re- havior.
moved.
A study by Hewett, Taylor, and Artuso Delinquents
(1969) is interesting by virtue of its design. One of the major difficulties in treating de-
Token reinforcement was employed in ele- linquents is that much of their deviant behavior
mentary school classrooms to reinforce the atten- may be maintained by peer support. In fact,
tiveness of students and to improve reading and deviant behaviors in institutional settings are
arithmetic skills. In the experimental (E) con- often reinforced by peers more times than they
dition, the teacher administered tangible and are punished by staff, and socially conforming
token reinforcement for appropriate behaviors. responses are punished more often by peers than
In the control (C) condition, the teacher was they are reinforced by staff (Buehler et al.,
instructed to rely on her usual method of teach- 1966). In addition, peer reinforcement of anti-
ing, including social reinforcement, but token social behavior is often more immediate than
reinforcement was not employed. The experi- contingent social or token reinforcement from
ment was conducted over a 34-week period. For staff members (Ross, unpublished b). An addi-
two consecutive periods of 17 weeks, four se- tional difficulty is that delinquents often have an
quences were employed as follows: E-E, C-C, extensive repertoire of inappropriate responses.
E-C, and C-E. The subjects were assigned to one The suppression of a particular response may
of these groups in a manner that achieved com- lead to the performance of other inappropriate
parable class groupings with respect to IQ, age, responses (Meichenbaum et al., 1968). The re-
and academic achievement. (The precise man- sponse frequency of delinquent behaviors for a
ner of matching and assigning subjects to con- given individual is usually low, making another
ditions was not presented.) difficulty for the application of operant tech-
The results indicated, as predicted, that the niques (Burchard and Tyler, 1965). Finally,
E-E group (token reinforcement over the entire Ross (unpublished a) noted that institutional-
34 weeks) was higher than the C-C group in ized delinquents were adept at finding loopholes
attentive behaviors. The C-E groups, which only in a token reinforcement program, modifying
356 ALAN E. KAZDIN and RICHARD R. BOOTZIN

the program to suit their own ends, and con- not reinforced, was not modified. Subsequently,
stantly testing the limits of the system. Perhaps when reinforcement was given for behavior dur-
for the above reasons, the application of operant ing the morning (VI schedule), classroom be-
procedures to delinquents has clearly been less haviors improved to a level equal to the after-
frequent than to other populations. noon performance. In a final condition, fining of
Because delinquents frequently are behind in tokens (i.e., punishment) for inappropriate be-
school, academic performance has been an im- havior was combined with the existing reinforce-
portant target response for token programs. In ment for both morning and afternoon periods.
a case study, Tyler (1967) described a program A combination of these procedures led to a de-
to alter the academic performance of an institu- crease in inappropriate behaviors compared to
tionalized delinquent. Tokens were contingent the effect of reinforcement alone. The schedules
upon daily and weekly evaluations of perform- of reinforcement, fixed and variable, did not dif-
ance. They were exchangeable for noninstitu- ferentially affect performance. At the end of
tional clothes, use of a comfortable bed, and treatment, the class showed a mean level of ap-
canteen items. Grade-point average increased propriate behavior that was significantly higher
during the three-week period in which token than the pretreatment level. The final level of
reinforcement was employed. Performance de- appropriate behavior was found to be about the
clined when the subject was taken off the pro- same as that of noninstitutionalized students in
gram. a nearby school.
A token program with two groups of adoles- Cohen (1968) also employed token rein-
cent delinquents was reported by Tyler and forcement techniques for developing academic
Brown (1968). In this study, a quiz was given skills. His subjects were institutionalized de-
each day based on the televised news of the linquents. For approximately 3 hr each day,
preceding day. Members of one group received each subject had the opportunity to work on
tokens contingent upon their quiz scores, whereas educational materials that were individually pre-
members of the other group received tokens programmed. Individuals were not coerced to
independently of their performance. After ap- work on assignments or remain in the classroom.
proximately four weeks, the experimental con- However, points (exchangeable for consumable
ditions for the groups were reversed. The results items, privileges, private facilities, and money)
indicated that quiz performance was greater dur- were given for correct completion of assign-
ing contingent reinforcement for both groups. ments, test performance, and studying. Points
Meichenbaum et al. (1968) employed token were given once a week for the entire week's
reinforcement to modify classroom behaviors of work. Measures of time spent studying indicated
institutionalized female delinquents. Classroom that the behavior was controlled by reinforce-
behaviors were categorized as inappropriate (un- ment, and that students studied more frequently
related to the task set forth by the teacher) or as "pay day" approached. After eight months,
appropriate (related to class activity). Subjects subjects who previously had little interest in
were given feedback notes by observers in the academic pursuits and had dropped out of school
classroom when their behavior was appropriate. had gained more than two grade levels on stan-
These notes could be exchanged for money. dard achievement tests.
The results indicated that after a baseline Even though academic deficits are important,
period, the introduction of reinforcement (FI the primary target response for delinquents is
schedule) in the afternoon led to a sudden im- usually antisocial behavior. Burchard and Tyler
provement in behavior. However, only the be- (1965) reported such a case study. Timeout
havior in the afternoon session was altered; be- from reinforcement was used for antisocial and
havior during the morning session, which was disruptive behavior, and tokens were used to
THE TOKEN ECONOMY: AN EVALUATIVE REVIEW 357

reinforce amount of time not in isolation. Tokens two, the resident was charged a constant fee
were backed up by canteen privileges. The time- that was not contingent upon his performance.
out and reinforcement procedures were effective The final phase was a reinstatement of phase
in reducing the severity and frequency of un- one. The results indicated that the total number
acceptable behaviors. of timeout and seclusion periods varied with
Burchard (1967) described a program for contingent and noncontingent fining. When
delinquent adolescents who were mildly retarded residents were fined according to their inappro-
(IQ ranging from 50 to 70). The population priate behaviors, the number of timeout and
was characterized by a variety of antisocial seclusion periods were less than when there was
behaviors such as property damage, theft, arson, no contingent relationship. It should be noted
truancy, and so on. A general ward program that the overlap in distributions for the ABA
was developed for training in practical skills periods indicates that the differential effect of
(personal, social, recreational, educational, and the punishment procedures was not of great
vocational). This program is one that has effec- magnitude.
tively employed a number of punishment pro- Phillips (1968) implemented a token pro-
cedures. Behaviors that were punished were gram for three delinquent males assigned to a
those that were considered likely to be met with foster care program. Tokens (points), which
punishment in the community. Reinforcement could be exchanged for several privileges, were
was delivered for those behaviors that were con- given out for a variety of behaviors relating to
sidered as most likely to be met with reward in academic achievement, cooperative behavior,
the community. self-care skills, household chores, and being in-
Two experiments were reported by Burchard. formed of current events. The boys could lose
In the initial study, tokens were given for the tokens for such things as being late and getting
amount of time spent sitting at assigned seats in poor grades. Five experiments were conducted
a classroom and workshop, and for the number within the general context of the token program.
of problems or tasks completed. The effect of The results of these five experiments indicated
reinforcement was examined in an ABAB design that aggressive statements and poor grammar
(contingent, noncontingent, and contingent re- decreased, and tidiness, punctuality, and amount
inforcement periods). Each experimental period of academic tasks completed increased. Fining
lasted for five consecutive days. The results indi- tokens for various inappropriate behaviors in
cated that the target behaviors varied with the these studies was shown to decrease the target
reinforcement contingencies. There was no over- high-frequency behavior. In altering "poor"
lap in the response distributions for contingent grammar (use of the word "ain't"), changes
and noncontingent periods. effected by fining were maintained when fines
In a second experiment, the effect of punish- were removed. The effect of fining appeared to
ment was examined on a variety of inappropri- be specific to the situation in which it was em-
ate behaviors such as stealing, lying, cheating, ployed. For example, tardiness for one activity,
and fighting. Timeout from reinforcement (loss if fined, did not appear to alter tardiness in
of tokens and required time spent in "timeout" other situations. Instruction alone in which the
area for a short period of time) and seclusion subjects were told to behave in a certain manner
(loss of a greater number of tokens than for (e.g., "do not talk aggressively", or "speak cor-
timeout, and isolation in a special room for a rectly") was considerably less effective than con-
longer period of time than above) were used. tingent punishment or reward.
In phase one of the experiment, the number of Stayer and Jones (unpublished) reported the
tokens a resident was fined depended upon his use of a token reinforcement program for sol-
frequency of timeout and seclusion. In phase diers who were labeled conduct disorders. For
358 ALAN E. KAZDIN and RICHARD R. BOOTZIN

these soldiers, a special hospital ward was estab- briefly studies effecting behavioral changes with
lished in which they could earn points for par- token reinforcement.
ticipation in various scheduled activities, and Ferster and DeMyer (1961, 1962) are respon-
work performance. Points could be exchanged sible for the initial use of conditioned reinforce-
for canteen items, recreational activities, privi- ment with autistic children. A simple response
leges, and money. Although no data are reported (key pressing) was employed to examine sus-
for the specific effects of reinforcement contin- tained performance and responsiveness to var-
gencies, follow-up assessment conducted at three, ious reinforcers. Generalized conditioned rein-
six, and nine months revealed a higher percent- forcers (coins) were used, which the subject
age (69%) of treated soldiers having completed could deposit in a vending machine to obtain
a tour of duty or serving in good standing than food, candy, and toys. The task requirements for
a nontreated comparison group (28%). A recent reinforcement were altered gradually to develop
report on the token program at Walter Reed complex responses. The results indicated that it
Hospital (Boren and Colman, 1970) evaluated was possible to bring the behavior of autistic
the effects of various procedures (alterations of children under control of conditioned rein-
reward magnitude, modeling, response-chaining, forcers, and gradually widen their behavioral
response-cost, and group versus individual con- repertoire. As the authors pointed out, the use
tingencies). Positive token reinforcement was of operant techniques in these studies was an
shown to increase attendance to meetings, ver- attempt at experimental analysis of behavior
bal behavior, and discussions of personal as op- rather than to develop complex responses for the
posed to impersonal problems. Interestingly, the purpose of treatment.
removal of tokens (fining) did not decrease Hingtgen, Sanders, and DeMyer (1965) em-
inappropriate behavior as intended, but increased ployed a lever-pressing task with childhood
it. schizophrenics. Lever pressing was trained in
two individuals, simultaneously, who were work-
Autistic Children ing independently. Gradually, cooperative re-
The use of operant conditioning procedures sponses were required for reinforcement; i.e.,
with autistic and schizophrenic children has token reinforcement for one individual de-
produced dramatic results. Self-destructive be- pended on the prior response of the other. Co-
haviors have been extinguished (Lovaas, Freitag, operative behaviors were shaped in three pairs
Gold, and Kassorla, 1965; Wolf, Risley, and of subjects in this manner.
Mees, 1964), speech has been developed Other investigators have explored responses of
(Lovaas, Berberich, Perloff, and Schaeffer, greater complexity. Metz (1965, unpublished)
1966), and stuttering has been decreased used token and primary reinforcement to train
(Browning, 1967). The efficacy of positive and autistic children to perform imitative behaviors.
negative reinforcement, as well as punishment, Initial training involved demonstration of vari-
has been demonstrated for a number of be- ous nonverbal tasks by the experimenter (e.g.,
havioral problems (Lovaas, 1968). In the hugging a doll, blowing a horn, moving blocks)
majority of work, there is a predominant reli- and rewarding with praise and tokens for imita-
ance on primary reinforcement, such as food tive responses. The tokens were exchangeable
or avoidance of pain. Although no general token for food. Subsequent to training, additional test-
programs for autistic children have been pre- ing on similar tasks revealed that the imitative
sented in the literature, token reinforcement has response set generalized to other tasks than
been employed in a few studies. Because of the those in which the subjects were trained. More-
problems associated with treating this popula- over, the imitative response set was maintained
tion, it is particularly interesting to mention by praise alone without token reinforcement.
THE TOKEN ECONOMY: AN EVALUATIVE REVIEW 359
Thus, the training procedure was effective in fails to generalize to conditions in which those
developing generalized imitative behavior. This conditions are not in effect.
achievement is significant inasmuch as imitative However, it may be premature to identify
response patterns represent a particular response token economies as only prosthetic environ-
deficit in autistic children. ments. The relevant experiments have not been
done. Most researchers have used the within-
GENERALIZATION
subject design with a reversal of effects to indi-
cate that the reinforcement procedures were
Generalization may be divided into stimulus functionally related to the dependent variable.
and response generalization. Stimulus generali- The goal of the research was not maintenance
zation refers to the transfer of effects to other of desired behavior. In fact, Bijou, Peterson,
stimulus conditions or situations. In other words, Harris, Allen, and Johnston (1969) cautioned
the concern is whether behavior change is main- researchers using an ABAB design not to wait
tained when there is no token economy. Re- too long before reversing, lest the behavior
sponse generalization here refers to the spread come under the control of new conditioned rein-
of effects to behaviors or responses that were forcers and thus not reverse.
not of initial focus. That is, generalization oc- It is evident that the meaning of stimulus
curs from the responses upon which treatment generalization changes somewhat depending
focused to other responses that may be related upon the treatment setting discussed. In the
but were not specifically dealt with. psychiatric hospital, generalization refers to the
transfer of behavior from within the hospital to
Stimulus Generalization extratreatment settings (the community, home,
The generalization of treatment effects to and place of employment). Remarkably little
stimulus conditions in which token reinforce- research has assessed generalization directly.
ment is not given might be expected to be the Generalization is usually inferred from increased
raison d'etre of token economies. An examina- discharge and decreased readmission rates (At-
tion of the literature leads to a different conclu- thowe and Krasner, 1968; Curran et al., unpub-
sion. There are numerous reports of token pro- lished, Ellsworth and Foster, 1969; Henderson
grams showing behavior change only while and Scoles, 1970; Schaefer and Martin, 1966,
contingent token reinforcement is being de- 1969; Steffy et al., 1969). Since discharge and
livered. Generally, removal of token reinforce- readmission rates depend upon administrative
ment results in decrements in desirable responses decisions, increases and decreases can be accom-
and a return to baseline or near-baseline levels plished without concomitant changes in the psy-
of performance. Such a state of affairs led chological status of the patients. In this regard,
Zimmerman, Zimmerman, and Russell (unpub- recent programs are likely to benefit from the
lished) to conclude that token economies are community psychology emphasis that has swept
prosthetic rather than therapeutic, a distinction the mental health field in recent years. Hospital
made by Lindsley (1964). Prosthetic environ- staffs have been encouraged to discharge patients
ments show changes only during treatment con- and to develop community resources so that
ditions, whereas removal of these conditions re- patients could be treated without requiring pro-
sults in a loss of treatment effects. Therapeutic longed hospitalization. Any program that con-
environments show changes that are maintained trasts present discharge and readmission rates
beyond the treatment conditions themselves. with those before the .program started is likely
Identification of token economies with prosthetic to find favorable statistics. Thus, in many re-
environments emphasizes the fact that behavior ports, it is not clear whether token economies are
controlled by token reinforcement contingencies in fact more successful at keeping people in the
360 ALAN E. KAZDIN and RICHARD R. BOOTZIN

community or have benefited from a change in the parents were trained to use contingent rein-
the orientation of the hospital staffs. Even in forcement at home. These procedures were effec-
studies that have included control groups, ward tive in maintaining improved behavior both at
staff and treatment have been frequently con- school and at home. In light of studies where be-
founded. Differential discharge and readmission havior changes are not maintained, it appears
rates may have been due to different staff policy that generalization should be planned, rather
rather than to the generalized effects of token than depended upon as an inadvertent conse-
economies. In summary, although token econ- quence of the token program. However, few di-
omies have been dramatically effective at chang- rect attempts to program generalization have
ing behavior within the psychiatric hospital, been made.
there is little evidence that improvement is
maintained outside the institution. Response Generalization
For token programs implemented in a class- There has been a paucity of reports of re-
room setting, generalization refers primarily to sponse generalization in the literature on token
the transfer of performance within the same reinforcement. Primarily this is due to the fact
setting. With some exceptions (Wahler, 1969), that both treatment and the assessment of treat-
behavior in nonschool settings is not monitored ment effects focus directly on the target
for evidence of generalization. Instead, the goal behavior. Usually, concomitant changes in non-
is to maintain improved classroom behavior target behaviors are not measured. Some evi-
when the token economy is withdrawn and in dence for generalized effects of the token system
classes not associated with the token economy. relates to the cluster of behaviors usually called
As in the case of mental hospitals, generaliza- "institutionalization". Several authors have com-
tion in classroom settings is not usually found mented on the deleterious effects of the "total"
unless it is programmed as part of the procedures. institution (Goffman, 1961; Scheff, 1966; Ull-
Reinforcement programs that have been imple- mann and Krasner, 1969). Changes in specific
mented in either the mornings or afternoons target responses have led to a decrease in institu-
(Becker et al., 1967; Broden, Hall, Dunlap, tionalized behaviors. Atthowe (unpublished b)
and Clark, 1970; Kuypers et al., 1968; Meich- and Atthowe and Krasner (1968) focused on
enbaum et al., 1968; O'Leary et al., 1969), target behaviors such as attendance to group ac-
with few exceptions (Kazdin, 1972b; Walker, tivities, self-care behaviors, social interaction,
Mattson, and Buckley, unpublished), have not and participation in activities (Atthowe, unpub-
found evidence of generalization to the part of lished a). Some generalized effects were noted,
the day in which tokens were not dispensed. such as greater utilization of day passes and an
Also, studies that have examined resistance to increase in discharge rates. Although social in-
extinction have generally found that behavior teraction among patients was reinforced, At-
changes are not maintained (Barrish et al., 1969; thowe (unpublished b) reported that some
Kuypers et al., 1968; O'Leary et al., 1969; forms of social interaction not specifically rein-
Walker and Buckley, 1968). However, few di- forced showed marked improvement. Similarly,
rect attempts to program generalization have Schaefer and Martin (1966) reported that hos-
been made. An exception to this is a study by pitalized schizophrenics, at the termination of
Patterson and Brodsky (1966) in which a 5-yr- token reinforcement procedures, were signifi-
old boy was treated for multiple problems at cantly less apathetic, as measured by clearly dis-
home and in school. To ensure generalization, cernible behaviors, than control patients given
the child's environment was programmed to "normal" ward treatment. The findings of these
support adaptive behaviors. Peers were rewarded studies conducted with mental patients support
for reciprocating positive social interaction, and the notion that token programs increase the gen-
THE TOKEN ECONOMY: AN EVALUATIVE REVIEW 361
eral interests and participation of inmates in an and Saslow, 1969), multiple-response measures
institutional setting. and measures of a more global or general nature
Aside from decreasing apathy and increasing might be meaningful additions.
activity of patients, a few studies present more
specific data on response generalization. Winkler
(1970) noted that episodes of both violence and Procedures to Increase Generalization
noise decreased on a psychiatric ward while a Although most research with token economies
token program was in effect for behaviors not has not focused upon generalization, a number
directly related. Other authors working with of procedures have been used to enhance main-
psychiatric patients have reported anecdotal ac- tenance of behavioral gains. Perhaps the most
counts of response generalization. Steffy (1968) frequently used procedure is to follow Ayllon
reported mood changes and improvement in and Azrin's (1968a, pp. 49-56) Relevance of
social behavior, and Curran et al., (unpublished) Behavior Rule which states: "Teach only those
reported improved "maturation and personality behaviors that will continue to be reinforced
development." after training." Thus, behaviors should be se-
Burchard and Tyler (1965) reported the de- lected that can come under the control of
crease in both frequency and severity of disrup- naturally occurring reinforcers in the person's
tive behavior as a result of a token program with environment. Target behaviors that have typi-
an institutionalized delinquent. The concurrent cally been selected in token economies (self-care
change of frequency and topography of the re- behaviors, work skills, academic behaviors) do
sponse would seem to be evidence of response meet this criterion. These are behaviors that will
generalization. continue to be reinforced (e.g., by social ap-
The clearest evidence for response generaliza- proval) after training.
tion has been reported by Meichenbaum (1969). Because social approval is not reinforcing for
This study investigated the differential effects of everyone (e.g., delinquents; Quay and Hunt,
instructions and reinforcement on the language 1965), it may be important to increase the rein-
behavior of schizophrenics. Subjects were rein- forcement value of verbal statements and praise.
forced for either "healthy" talk or proverb For example, in a study reported by Wahler
abstractions. Subjects receiving reinforcement (unpublished), parental approval of cooperative
for only one of these two response classes, behavior was initially ineffective in modifying
showed improvement in both. In addition, the uncooperative behavior of their children. A
treated subjects showed improved behavior on reinforcement program was then developed in
a word-association test and a similarities subtest which tokens (exchangeable for toys) and ap-
of the Wechsler-Bellevue Adult Intelligence proval were given for cooperative responses.
Scale. Similar improvement was not shown by Gradually, tokens were eliminated, and coopera-
no-contact control and attention-contact control tive behavior was successfully maintained by
subjects. social approval. Pairing verbal praise and token
In general, response generalization has re- reinforcement has been used by a number of
ceived little empirical investigation in token researchers in the hope of facilitating subsequent
reinforcement programs. The references to generalization (Atthowe and Krasner, 1968;
beneficial effects of reinforcement contingencies Lent, unpublished; O'Leary and Becker, 1967).
are generally restricted to the target behaviors The efficacy of this procedure has been demon-
of interest. While the specificity of assessment is strated recently (Locke, 1969).
desirable and remains a singular advantage of Another technique employed to facilitate gen-
operant or learning programs over treatment eralization involves the gradual removal of
programs based on other models (see Kanfer token reinforcement. Schaefer and Martin
362 ALAN E. KAZDIN and RICHARD R. BOOTZIN

(1969) discussed how token reinforcement was quickly extinguished. In addition, appropriate
gradually faded out in a hospital setting for psy- behavior was maintained after the termination
chotic patients. Praise, extra privileges, and of training.
staff approval were all given contingently and Stimulus variation has not been attempted on
were gradually substituted for token reinforce- a large-scale basis in a token program. Even
ment as the individuals improved. Atthowe and programs using exposure to community activities
Krasner (1968) described an "elite" group of or employment as part of generalization training
patients who, because of their prolonged per- do not usually reinforce behavioral performance
formance of appropriate behavior, were given a in the community (an exception is Kelley and
carte blanche for privileges, and were free from Henderson, 1971). In one of the few attempts to
specific reinforcement contingencies. do so, Lent (unpublished b, 1968) trained ado-
Another way to fade token reinforcement is lescent retardates in various household activities
to have the subjects spend increasingly longer in which they might participate upon discharge.
parts of their day out of the program. Hender- A model home, built on the grounds of the in-
son and Scoles (1970) employed this technique stitution, had facilities in which household train-
while training psychotic males in vocational, ing could be conducted. This represents one of
social, and countersymptom behaviors. To fa- the few attempts to reinforce behavior under
cilitate generalization, individuals participated stimulus conditions similar to extratreatment
in social activities in the community where non- conditions.
token reinforcers presumably would be opera- Stepwise or leveled token systems have been
tive. Kelley and Henderson (1971) reported that employed to maintain desirable behaviors and
individuals in their program are reinforced for the beneficial effects of treatment, and hence,
looking for jobs in the community, obtaining constitute another technique to enhance gen-
interviews, and making phone calls to prospec- eralization. Several token programs require in-
tive employers. Once employment is secured, dividuals to begin at an initial level and, de-
additional privileges are given (e.g., move to the pending on their improvement and sustained
"tpenthouse" in the facility). Exposing an insti- performance, allow them to progress to higher
tutionalized patient to the community not only levels. Initial levels require the performance of
removes the specific token reinforcement con- few behaviors and offer few reinforcers; After
tingencies that might be controlling his behavior the individual is able to meet the particular re-
but also places him under stimulus conditions quirements of a given level for a certain period
similar to those he will experience subsequent of time, he is able to progress to a level that will
to discharge. entitle him to receive added privileges. The
Requiring that the subject spends time in the notion of levels is not new in, for example, men-
community is one way of varying the stimulus tal hospital procedures, and bears similarity to
conditions for appropriate behavior. Stimulus the reasoning behind the halfway house move-
variation can also be directly programmed. ment. In levelled token programs, it is the goal
Goocher and Ebner (unpublished) trained a de- to have the patients at the highest level perform
viant child in appropriate classroom behavior, desirable behaviors without token reinforce-
initially, in the presence of only the experi- ment. In leveled token programs, it is the goal
menter. Gradually, planned distractions were planning of programs for other patients, assume
introduced (e.g., a television set operating) . Fi- responsibilities for other patients on the ward,
nally, the training was continued in the class- and so on (Garlington and Lloyd, unpublished;
room itself in the presence of other children. Al- Guyett, unpublished; Schaefer and Martin,
though deviant behavior often increased when 1969). Terminal treatment behaviors in a lev-
distracting stimuli were introduced, it was eled system are assumed to be closely related to
THE TOKEN ECONOMY: AN EVALUATIVE REVIEW 363
extrainstitutional performance demands. Albeit forcement procedures (e.g., retardates), such
the leveled system has prima facie appeal, its use limitations cannot be determined on a priori
remains to be justified by follow-up data. grounds.
Another technique employed to facilitate Although there is an abundant literature on
generalization involves training relatives in be- the effects of schedules of reinforcement on ex-
havioral principles so that important contin- tinction, schedules are seldom varied in token
gencies can be continued. This procedure is not economies. Partially, this is due to the fact that
new to the operant approach, but has been used it would be uneconomical to monitor the sched-
only in a few instances with token-reinforce- ules so closely. In addition, intermittent sched-
ment procedures. O'Leary, O'Leary, and Becker ules may only delay extinction, rather than
(1967) trained an aggressive, hyperactive child prevent it. Nevertheless, a few studies have in-
to cooperate at home. Training was conducted vestigated the effects of different schedules. Some-
by the experimenters in the child's home. Rein- what inconsistent results have been obtained
forcers for cooperative behaviors consisted of (Haring and Hauck, 1969; Meichenbaum et al.,
candy, social, and token reinforcement (ex- 1968). So little is now known about the effects
changeable for small toys). The child's mother of schedules of reinforcement in token econo-
was trained to take over the entire token pro- mies, that it is an obvious next step for research
gram with the child. Although some deviant in the area. This is particularly so since reinforce-
behaviors remained, the operant procedures car- ment is seldom dispensed according to a 1:1
ried out by the parent were clearly effective in ratio schedule. In the typical token economy,
increasing cooperative behaviors. Other investi- much behavior, both appropriate and inappropri-
gators have reported similar success with parents ate, goes undetected. In addition, the staff is by
of psychiatric patients (Henderson and Scoles, no means the only dispenser of reinforcers.
1970) and of brain-damaged children (Salzinger, Delay of reinforcement is another variable
Feldman, and Portnoy, 1970). that would seem to have implications for in-
Self-reinforcement (Kanfer, 1970) represents creasing resistance to extinction. Two separate
a technique of considerable potential. This tech- procedures have been employed in delaying
nique relies on the individual giving himself a reinforcement. One procedure used is to increase
reinforcer contingent upon the performance of the delay between the response and token rein-
an appropriate response. If an individual can be forcement. For example, in the token economy
trained to reward himself, or develop his own presented by Atthowe and Krasner (1968), a
contingencies (Homme, Csanyi, Gonzales, and number of behaviors earned tokens that were
Rechs, 1969; O'Leary, unpublished), it is more paid at the end of the week, rather than upon
likely that he will be able to monitor his behav- each performance of the response. Atthowe and
ior in a number of settings. Although self-devel- Krasner utilized a number of different delay
oped contingencies may be more effective than periods for various behaviors.
externally imposed ones (Lovitt and Curtiss, Another delay of reinforcement procedure
1969), self-reinforcement has not been more involves the manipulation of the delay between
effective than externally administered contingen- token reinforcement and the exchange of tokens
cies (Johnson, 1969). Self-reinforcement should for back-up reinforcers. The delay here is in the
be more fully investigated. Self-regulation (Kan- exchange of back-up reinforcers, rather than in
fer, 1970) and self-control procedures (see Ban- the presentation of conditioned reinforcers.
dura, 1969 for a review) appear to be useful for O'Leary and Becker (1967) employed this tech-
maintaining a variety of behaviors. Although nique for elementary school students. Points
such techniques might have some limitations were given for instruction-following behaviors.
with populations that often receive token rein- Gradually, the number of reinforcement periods
364 ALAN E. KAZDIN and RICHARD R. BOOTZIN

decreased and the delay between token rein- used is the ABAB design (where A refers to the
forcement and exchange of tokens increased up baseline period and B refers to the treatment).
to a four-day delay period. This design has been referred to as the intra-
Manipulation of delay of reinforcement subject replication design (Sidman, 1960), the
(token or back-up) is designed to increase re- reversal technique (Baer, Wolf, and Risley,
sistance to extinction, presumably because of the1968), and the equivalent time-samples design
resemblance of delayed reinforcement in treat- (Campbell and Stanley, 1963). If the behavior
ment and nontreatment settings. Numerous re- of the subject(s) improves whenever treatment
is presented and declines whenever treatment is
wards in the natural setting (e.g., grades, money)
are delayed. Thus, it seems desirable to train withdrawn, and this occurs repeatedly, a func-
subjects so that they could perform without tional control of the target behavior has been
receiving rewards immediately for performance. powerfully demonstrated. The four stages of the
It is assumed that training under delayed rein- ABAB design (alternations of base and treat-
forcement in a treatment setting will generalize ment conditions) appear to represent the basic
to performance in nontreatment settings. It is essential in demonstrating a functional relation-
also hoped that in the treatment setting when ex-ship. There are, of course, several variations of
trinsic reinforcement is delayed, behaviors will the design. For example, in later phases, after
come under the control of naturally occurring functional control has been demonstrated, pro-
reinforcers, such as praise and attention. Evi- cedures to enhance stimulus generalization or
dence supporting these assumptions is not resistance to extinction may be implemented and
available. assessed. A major advantage of this design is
The manipulation of other parameters of re- that it rules out maturation and chance as alter-
inforcement (e.g., varied reinforcement; Kimble, native hypotheses for the effectiveness of the ex-
perimental variable. Nevertheless, there are a
1961) could also be used to increase resistance to
extinction. With varied reinforcement, a number variety of problems in using this design to which
of aspects of reinforcement are varied simul- the researcher should be alerted.
taneously (e.g., magnitude, delay, and place of The first problem involves reversing or with-
reinforcement). drawing the experimental contingencies. The
Although there are a number of procedures ABAB design is suitable only if the behavior
for potentially increasing generalization, it isbeing studied is transient and reversible. How-
our guess that the most fruitful techniques will ever, it is by no means clear that the effects of
be the ones that emphasize programming the token economies are transient and reversible.
natural environment (e.g., Kazdin, 1971; Patter- Although in most cases the behavior does reverse
son and Brodsky, 1966). The investigation of with the reversal of conditions, there are in-
reinforcement parameters during acquisition stances in which this does not happen (e.g.,
may help refine the token economy methodol- Kazdin, 1972b; Walker et al., unpublished). Of-
ogy, but it seems unlikely that it will provide ten, this may be taken as evidence that the be-
a means for dramatically increasing generaliza- havior has come under control of other variables
tion. in the environment or as evidence for resistance
to extinction. However, if resistance to extinction
is being tested (and a reversal is not expected),
this within-subject design is inadequate. In this
METHODOLOGY instance, the design does not control for the
The majority of investigations of token econ- effects of maturation, regression to the mean,
omies employs designs in which the subject is or extraneous change-producing events. To con-
his own control. Of these, the most frequently trol for these alternative hypotheses, other de-
THE TOKEN ECONOMY: AN EVALUATIVE REVIEW 365

signs, such as multiple-baseline or between- in behavior may be due to the instructions to


subject designs, are essential. These designs are the subjects at the onset of each phase, rather
discussed later in this section. than to the changes in contingencies. Thus, in
Difficulty in reversing the behavior often one of the studies reported by Ayllon and Azrin
occurs because baseline stimulus conditions are (1965), patients were instructed at the beginning
not reinstated. Other aspects of the environment of the reversal that they were in a sense receiving
(e.g., social approval) may have changed con- a "vacation with pay". It is not surprising that
comitantly with the introduction of tokens, but work performance fell dramatically during this
then were not reversed when tokens were with- "noncontingent reinforcement" phase because
drawn. This problem frequently can be avoided people seldom work during vacations. The effect
if, in addition to the records of response fre- of instructions, or of what Orne (1962) calls
quency, detailed records of stimulus conditions the demand characteristics of the experiment, re-
are kept. mains a plausible alternative hypothesis of the
Another potential confound that may vary effectiveness of token reinforcement in many
with the presentation and withdrawal of token studies. Yet, the effect of instructions in the
reinforcement is staff behavior. During the rein- absence of reinforcement has been shown to be
forcement phase of the experiment, the staff may transient (e.g., Packard, 1970).
dispense considerable attention, encouragement, A third problem area for the ABAB design
and contact with the subjects, which they may is the generalizability of the results to other
withdraw during the reversal phase. For exam- settings. The results may not generalize to set-
ple, in one study it was suggested that staff may tings in which the experimental variable is
have more contact with subjects when contin- available continuously and does not alternate
gent reinforcement is given than when noncon- with a baseline period. Although this seems
tingent reinforcement is given (Mandelker, implausible, often response frequency reaches
Brigham, and Bushell, 1970). This may have the new heights immediately after a reversal, and
effect of ensuring the expected reversal. How- it is possible that the reversal was a prerequisite
ever, it does not demonstrate the functional con- for this degree of effectiveness. In cases where
trol of token reinforcement over the target re- the effectiveness of one phase may depend upon
sponse. One solution to this is to have baseline the experience with the preceding phases, be-
and reversals include social approval and atten- tween-subject designs would provide convergent
tion, thus evaluating the effectiveness of token validation of the effectiveness of the techniques
reinforcement as a supplement to approval. without confounding them with sequence effects.
Even so, the staff may alter their behavior subtly In spite of these limitations to the usefulness of
to ensure the reversal. Potential solutions to this the ABAB design, it still provides the most prac-
problem that should be investigated are: tical evaluative tool for evaluating ongoing pro-
(1) alerting staff of this problem; (2) reducing grams. No token economy should be instituted
their commitment to the reversal by structuring without providing for systematic evaluation.
it as an exploratory alteration of the contingen- Although in most instances, this design will be
cies; and (3) detailing changes in staff behavior quite sufficient, other designs may be useful.
across experimental phases. One such design is the multiple-baseline de-
In addition to difficulties associated with re- sign (Baer, Wolf, and Risley, 1968) or the
versal, a second, and related, problem of the multi-element baseline design (Sidman, 1960).
ABAB design is that some variable other than In this design, a functional relationship between
token reinforcement that covaries with its pre- controlling conditions and behavior is demon-
sentation and withdrawal, may have functional strated somewhat differently. This is particularly
control over the response. For example, changes well suited to situations in which a behavioral
366 ALAN E. KAZDIN and RICHARD R. BOOTZIN

reversal is unanticipated or undesirable. There that altering the behavior of one subject may
are at least three variations of this design de- affect the performance of other subjects, even
pending upon whether multiple-baseline data though baseline has been continued for the other
are collected across behaviors, across individuals, subjects (see Broden, Bruce, Mitchell, Carter,
or across situations. (For an excellent example of and Hall, 1970).
the use of multiple baseline designs as discussed A final version of the multiple-baseline de-
here, refer to Hall, Cristler, Cranston, and sign examines baseline data across several situ-
Tucker, 1970.) ations for one or more individuals. In this ver-
When multiple-baseline data are collected sion, the contingencies are introduced in one
across behaviors, several (two or more) behaviors situation while baseline data are collected on
of a subject (or subjects) are monitored. After all the behavior(s) in other situations. Sequentially,
behaviors have stabilized, reinforcement is made the contingencies are introduced into different
contingent upon the occurrence of only one of situations. Here, it is demonstrated that the be-
the responses. Ideally, as the first target response havior does not change in a given situation until
changes, the other behaviors remain at baseline the contingencies are extended to include these
levels. After the initial target response has sta- situations.
bilized at its new level, the contingency is ex- As a final type of design to be discussed, be-
tended to include an additional target behavior. tween-subject designs may be used in which
This procedure is continued until all behaviors subjects are randomly assigned to experimental
have been included in the contingency. Al- and control groups. This design is also appropri-
though, there are no reversals or returns to base- ate for situations when reversals in behavior are
line in this design, extraneous events are ruled not expected. If the behavior of control subjects
out as alternative hypotheses by the demonstra- does not change, while that of the treatment sub-
tion that the baseline response frequencies of the jects does, experimental control of the behavior
specific behaviors remain stable until the con- has been demonstrated. There is obvious resem-
tingencies are applied to each consecutively. blance between this design and multiple-baseline
A major difficulty in using this version of the design across individuals. However, in the be-
design is the possibility of response generaliza- tween-subject design subject selection to groups
tion. Some responses may remain independent as is random. In addition, it is possible to evaluate
contingencies are applied to one of them, but components of a treatment (without confound-
other responses will change as the target re- ing with sequence) by assigning different com-
sponse changes. Clearly, before this design can ponents to different treatment groups. Although
be effectively utilized, more information about this is a potentially powerful design, it is usually
the nature and extent of response generalization rarely found in the token economy literature,
in token settings will be required. primarily because practical considerations within
Another version of this design employs mul- the applied settings make random assignment
tiple-baseline data on a single response across difficult, if not impossible. In order for this de-
several individuals. Each subject receives only sign to be used, random assignment is essential.
baseline and treatment phases with no reversals. Otherwise, the treatment and control groups
However, the subjects receive the baseline phase may differ because of variables not related to the
for differing lengths of time, thus ruling out one being investigated. Campbell and Stanley
extraneous events as a plausible hypothesis for (1963) described a number of between-subject
the effectiveness of the contingencies. The ad- experimental designs that might be used when
vantage of this design over the previous one is random assignment is not, possible. However,
that response generalization presents no special the within-subject designs already described ap-
difficulties. However, it is sometimes possible pear preferable to these in most situations.
THE TOKEN ECONOMY: AN EVALUATIVE REVIEW 367
REFERENCES
CONCLUSION
Anderson, D., Morrow, J. E., and Schleisinger, R.
The extensive literature that has developed The effects of token reinforcers on the behavior
on token economies indicates clearly that a wide problems of institutionalized female retardates.
variety of behaviors can be changed in many Unpublished paper presented at Western Psycho-
logical Association Convention, San Francisco,
different populations of subjects using con- May, 1967.
ditioned reinforcers. However, there are three Atthowe, J. M. Ward 113: Incentives and costs-A
areas in particular that have been insufficiently manual for patients. Unpublished manuscript,
Veterans Administration Hospital, Palo Alto,
investigated and in which future studies could California, 1964. (a)
profitably concentrate. Atthowe, J. M. The token economy: Its utility and
First, studies investigating programmed gen- limitations. Unpublished paper presented to the
eralization are badly needed. Inasmuch as main- Western Psychological Association, Long Beach,
California, April, 1966. (b)
tenance of target behaviors beyond the token Atthove, J. M. Ward 203 C1 token program:
reinforcement conditions is an important goal of Manual for ward staff. Unpublished manuscript,
token programs, the examination of variables Veterans Administration Hospital, Palo Alto,
California, 1967. (c)
that contribute to response maintenance appears Atthowe, J. M. and Krasner, L. Preliminary report
to be especially important. As reviewed earlier, on the application of contingent reinforcement
numerous procedures for augmenting generali- procedures (token economy) on a "chronic"
zation and resistance to extinction are available. psychiatric ward. Journal of Abnormal Psychology,
1968, 73, 37-43.
The application of these techniques for use in Ayllon, T. and Azrin, N. H. The measurement and
token economies should be evaluated. reinforcement of behavior of psychotics. Journal
Second, few investigators have tried to bring of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 1965,
8, 357-383.
complex behaviors such as language and social Ayllon, T. and Azrin, N. H. The token economy: a
behavior under control of token reinforcement. motivational system for therapy and rehabilitation.
This is probably because the practical problems New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts, 1968. (a)
Ayllon, T. and Azrin, N. H. Reinforcer sampling: a
associated with monitoring and escaping contin- technique for increasing the behavior of mental
gencies are difficult to solve. However, any gen- patients. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis,
eral methodology of behavior change must ad- 1968, 1, 13-20. (b)
Baer, D. M. Laboratory control of thumbsucking by
dress itself to these complex behaviors as well. withdrawal and re-presentation of reinforcement.
Third, almost all studies report that the be- Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior,
havior of some subjects was not altered. At this 1962, 5, 525-528.
Baer, D. M., Wolf, M. M., and Risley, T. R. Some
stage in our knowledge it is not clear whether current dimensions of applied behavior analysis.
this was due to not applying sufficiently in- Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 1968, 1,
dividualized contingencies or whether there are 91-97.
important individual-difference variables that Baldwin, V. L. Development of social skills in re-
tardates as a function of three types of reinforce-
interact with the token procedures. Both possi- ment programs. Unpublished dissertation, Disser-
bilities require further investigation. tation Abstracts, 1967, 27(9-A), 2865.
That token programs are effective in altering Bandura, A. Principles of behavior modification.
New York: Holt, Rinehart, & Winston, 1969.
behaviors, and offer numerous advantages as Barrett, B. H. and Lindsley, 0. R. Deficits in acqui-
treatment programs, cannot be disputed from an sition of operant discrimination and differentiation
examination of the literature. The stability of shown by institutionalized retarded children.
American Journal of Mental Deficiency, 1962,
changes effected, and the generalization of im- 67, 424-436.
provements to nontreatment settings, if demon- Barrish, H. H., Saunders, M., and Wolf, M. M.
strated, will strengthen the thrust of the trend Good behavior game: effects of individual con-
to establish token economies in numerous treat- tingencies for group consequences on disruptive
behavior in a classroom. Journal of Applied Be-
ment, rehabilitation, and educational settings. havior Analysis, 1969, 2, 119-124.
368 ALAN E. KAZDIN and RICHARD R. BOOTZIN

Becker, W. C., Engelmann, S., & Thomas, D. R. havior of preschool children. Journal of Applied
Teaching: A basic course in applied psychology. Behavior Analysis, 1968, 1, 55-62.
Chicago: Science Research Associates, 1971. Campbell, D. T. and Stanley, J. C. Experimental
Becker, W. C., Madsen, C. H., Arnold, C. R., and and quasi-experimental designs for research on
Thomas, D. R. The contingent use of teacher teaching. In N. L. Gage (Ed.), Handbook of
attention and praising in reducing classroom be- research on teaching. Chicago: Rand McNally,
havior problems. Journal of Special Education, 1963. Pp. 171-246.
1967, 1, 287-307. Chase, J. D. Token economy programs in the Vet-
Bensberg, G. J. (Ed.) Teaching the mentally re- erans Administration. Unpublished manuscript,
tarded: A handbook for ward personnel. Atlanta: VA Department of Medicine and Surgery, Wash-
Southern Regional Education Board, 1965. ington, D.C., 1970.
Bijou, S. W., Peterson, R. F., Harris, F. R., Allen, Clark, M., Lachowicz, J., and Wolf, M. A pilot
K. E., and Johnston, M. S. Methodology for ex- basic education program for school dropouts in-
perimental studies of young children in natural corporating a token reinforcement system. Be-
settings. Psychological Record, 1969, 19, 177-210. haviour Research and Therapy, 1968, 6, 183-188.
Birnbrauer, J. S. and Lawler, J. Token reinforce- Cohen, H. L. Educational therapy: The design of
ment for learning. Mental Retardation, 1964, 2, learning improvements. In J. M. Shlien (Ed.) Re-
275-279. search in psychotherapy. Vol. III. Washington,
Birnbrauer, J. S., Wolf, M. M., Kidder, J., and Tague, D. C.: American Psychological Association,
C. E. Classroom behavior of retarded pupils 1968. Pp. 21-53.
with token reinforcement. Journal of Experi- Curran, J. P., Jourd, S., and Whitman, N. Behavior
mental Child Psychology, 1965, 2, 219-235. change techniques on a closed psychiatric ward.
Boren, J. J. and Colman, A. D. Some experiments Unpublished manuscript, Anoka State Hospital,
on reinforcement principles within a psychiatric Anoka, Minnesota, 1968.
ward for delinquent soldiers. Journal of Applied Davison, G. C. Appraisal of behavior modification
Behavior Analysis, 1970, 3, 29-37. techniques with adults in institutional settings.
Bourgeois, T. L. Reinforcement theory in teaching In C. M. Franks (Ed.), Behavior therapy: Ap-
the mentally retarded: A token economy program. praisal and status. New York: McGraw-Hill,
Perspectives in Psychiatric Care, 1968, 6. 1969. Pp. 220-278.
Broden, M., Bruce, C., Mitchell, M., Carter, V., and Dunham, H. W. and Weinberg, S. K. The culture
Hall, R. V. Effects of teacher attention on at- of the state mental hospital. Detroit: Wayne State
tending behavior of two boys at adjacent desks. University Press, 1960.
Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 1970, 3, Ebner, M. An investigation of the role of the social
199-203. environment in the generalization and persistence
Broden, M., Hall, R. V., Dunlap, A., and Clark, R. of the effect of a behavior modification program.
Effects of teacher attention and a token reinforce- Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of
ment system in a junior high school special edu- Oregon, 1967.
cation class. Exceptional Children, 1970, 36, Ellis, N. R. Amount of reward and operant be-
341-349. havior in mental defectives. American Journal of
Browning, R. M. Behaviour therapy for stuttering Mental Deficiency, 1962, 66, 595-599.
in a schizophrenic child. Behaviour Research and Ellsworth, J. R. Reinforcement therapy with
Therapy, 1967, 5, 27-35. chronic patients. Hospital and Community Psy-
Buehler, R. E., Patterson, G. R., and Furniss, J. M. chiatry, 1969, 20, 36-38.
The reinforcement of behaviour in institutional Ellsworth, J. R. and Foster, L. D. The rehabilitation
settings. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 1966, of chronically hospitalized patients through a
4, 157-167. token economy system conducted by non-profes-
Burchard, J. D. Systematic socialization: A pro- sionals. Unpublished manuscript, Veterans Ad-
grammed environment for the habilitation of ministration Hospital, Roseburg, Oregon, 1969.
antisocial retardates. Psychological Record, 1967, Fenz, W. D. and Steffy, R. S. Electrodermal arousal
17, 461-476. of chronically ill psychiatric patients undergoing
Burchard, J. D. Residential behavior modification intensive behavioral treatment. Psychosomatic
programs and the problem of uncontrolled con- Medicine, 1968, 30, 423-436.
tingencies: A reply to Lachenmeyer. Psychologi- Ferster, C. B. and DeMyer, M. K. The development
cal Record, 1969, 19, 259-261. of performances in autistic children in an auto-
Burchard, J. D. and Tyler, V. 0. The modification matically controlled environment. Journal of
of delinquent behaviour through operant con- Chronic Diseases, 1961, 13, 312-345.
ditioning. Behaviour Research and Therapy, Ferster, C. B. and DeMyer, M. K. A method for the
1965, 2, 245-250. experimental analysis of the behavior of autistic
Bushell, D., Wrobel, P., and Michaelis, M. Applying children. American Journal of Orthopsychiatry,
"group" contingencies to the classroom study be- 1962, 1, 87-110.
THE TOKEN ECONOMY: AN EVALUATIVE REVIEW 369
Garlington, W. K. and Lloyd, K. E. The establish- script. Spruce House, Horizon House, Inc., Phila-
ment of a token economy ward at the State Hos- delphia, 1968.
pital North in Orofino, Idaho. Unpublished paper Henderson, J. D. The use of dual reinforcement in
presented at research meeting, Fort Steilacoom, an extensive treatment system. In R. D. Rubin
Washington, November, 1966. and C. M. Franks (Eds.), Advances in behavior
Gardner, J. M. Teaching behavior modification to therapy, 1968. New York: Academic Press, 1969.
nonprofessionals. Journal of Applied Behavior Henderson, J. D. and Scoles, P. E. A community-
Analysis, 1972, 5, in press. based behavioral operant environment for psy-
Gelfand, D. M., Gelfand, S., and Dobson, W. R. chotic men. Behavior Therapy, 1970, 1, 245-251.
Unprogrammed reinforcement of patients' be- Hewett, F. M., Taylor, F. D. and Artuso, A. A. The
haviour in a mental hospital. Behaviour Research Santa Monica Project: Evaluation of an engineered
and Therapy, 1967, 5, 201-207. classroom design with emotionally disturbed
Girardeau, F. L. and Spradlin, J. E. Token rewards children. Exceptional Children, 1969, 35, 523-
in a cottage program. Mental Retardation, 1964, 529.
2, 345-351. Hingtgen, J. N., Sanders, B. J., and DeMyer, M. K.
Goffman, E. Asylums. New York: Anchor, 1961. Shaping cooperative responses in early childhood
Golub, C. M. The influence of various demographic schizophrenics. In L. P. Ullmann and L. Krasner
variables on the participation of Veterans Admin- (Eds.), Case studies in behavior modification.
istration Day Treatment Center patients in a New York: Holt, Rinehart, and Winston, 1965.
token economy. Unpublished doctoral disserta- Homme, L., Csanyi, A., Gonzales, M., and Rechs, J.
tion, University of Minnesota, 1969. How to use contingency contracting in the class-
Goocher, B. E. and Ebner, M. A behavior modifica- room. Champaign, Illinois: Research Press, 1969.
tion approach utilizing sequential response tar- Hunt, J. G., Fitzhugh, L C., and Fitzhugh, K. B.
gets in multiple settings. Unpublished paper pre- Teaching "exit-ward" patients appropriate per-
sented at Midwestern Psychological Association, sonal appearance by using reinforcement tech-
Chicago, May, 1968. niques. American Journal of Mental Deficiency,
Graubard, P. S. Utilizing the group in teaching dis- 1968, 73, 41-45.
turbed delinquents to learn. Exceptional Children, Hunt, J. G. and Zimmerman, J. Stimulating pro-
1969, 35, 267-272. ductivity in a simulated sheltered workshop
Guyett, I. Behavior modification using resocializa- setting. American Journal of Mental Deficiency,
tion and token economy for the rehabilitation of 1969, 74, 43-49.
chronic schizophrenic patients. Unpublished pa- Johnson, S. M. Effects of self-reinforcement and
per presented at Quarterly Meeting of State Psy- external reinforcement in behavior modification.
chologists of Pennsylvania, Dixmont State Hos- Proceedings, 77th Annual Convention, American
pital, October, 1968. Psychological Association, 1969, 4, 535-536.
Hall, R. V., Cristler, C., Cranston, S., and Tucker, B. Kanfer, F. H. Self-regulation: Research, issues, and
Teachers and parents as researchers using multiple speculations. In C. Neuringer and J. L. Michael
baseline designs. Journal of Applied Behavior (Eds.), Behavior Modification in Clinical Psy-
Analysis, 1970, 3, 247-255. chology, New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts,
Hall, R. V., Panyan, M., Rabon, D., and Broden, M. 1970. Pp. 178-220.
Instructing beginning teachers in reinforcement Kanfer, F. H. and Saslow, G. Behavioral diagnosis.
procedures which improve classroom control. In C M. Franks (Ed.), Behavior therapy: Ap-
Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 1968, 1, praisal and status. New York: McGraw-Hill,
3 15-322. 1969. Pp. 417-444.
Haring, N. G. and Hauck, M. A. Improved learning Kazdin, A. E. Toward a client-administered token
conditions in the establishment of reading skills reinforcement program. Education and Training
with disabled readers. Exceptional Children, 1969, of the Mentally Retarded, 1971, 6, 52-5 5.
35, 341-352. Kazdin, A. E. Assessment of teacher training in a
Haring, N. G., Hayden, A., and Nolen, P. A. Ac- reinforcement program. Journal of Teacher Edu-
celerating appropriate behaviors of children in a cation, 1972, in press. (a)
Head Start program. Exceptional Children, 1969, Kazdin, A. E. The role of instructions and rein-
35, 773-784. forcement in behavior changes in token reinforce-
Hartlage, L. C. Subprofessional therapists' use of ment programs. Journal of Educational Psychol-
reinforcement versus traditional psychotherapeu- ogy, 1972, in press. (b)
tic techniques with schizophrenics. journal of Kelleher, R. T. and Gollub, L. R. A review of posi-
Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 1970, 34, tive conditioned reinforcement. Journal of the
181-183. Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 1962, 5,
Henderson, J. D. Conditioning techniques in a com- 543-597.
munity-based residential treatment facility for Kelley, K. M. and Henderson, J. D. A community-
emotionally disturbed men. Unpublished manu- based operant learning environment II: Systems
370 ALAN E. KAZDIN and RICHARD R. BOOTZIN

and procedures. In R. Rubin, H. Fensterheim, schizophrenic children. Science, 1966, 51, 705-
A. Lazarus, and C. M. Franks (Eds.), Advances 707.
in behavior therapy. New York: Academic Press, Lovaas, 0. I., Freitag, G., Gold, V. J., and Kassorla,
1971. I. C. Experimental studies in childhood schizo-
Kimble, G. A. Hilgard and Marquis' conditioning phrenia: Analysis of self-destructive behavior.
and learning. New York: Appleton-Century- journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 1965,
Crofts, 196 1. 2, 67-84.
Krasner, L. Assessment of token economy in chronic Lovitt, T. C. and Curtiss, K. A. Academic response
populations. Paper presented at American Psy- rate as a function of teacher and self-imposed con-
chological Association, San Francisco, September, tingencies. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis,
1968. 1969, 3, 49-53.
Kuypers, D. S., Becker, W. C., and O'Leary, K. D. Madsen, C. H., Becker, W. C., and Thomas, D. R.
How to make a token system fail. Exceptional Rules, praise, and ignoring: elements of ele-
Children, 1968, 113, 101-108. mentary classroom control. Journal of Applied
Lachenmeyer, C. W. Systematic socialization: Ob- Behavior Analysis, 1968, 1, 139-150.
servations on a programmed environment for the Mandelker, A. V., Brigham, T. A., and Bushell, D.
habilitation of antisocial retardates. Psychological The effects of token procedures on a teacher's
Record, 1969, 19, 247-257. social contacts with her students. Journal of Ap-
Lent, J. R. A demonstration program for intensive plied Behavior Analysis, 1970, 3, 169-174.
training of institutionalized mentally retarded McNamara, J. R. Teacher and students as sources
girls. Unpublished Progress Report, U. S. Depart- for behavior modification in the classroom. Be-
ment of Health, Education, and Welfare, Feb- havior Therapy, 1971, 2, 205-213.
ruary, 1966. Marks, J., Sonoda, B., and Schalock, R. Reinforce-
Lent, J. R. A demonstration program for intensive ment vs. relationship therapy for schizophrenics.
training of institutionalized mentally retarded Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 1968, 73, 397-
girls. Unpublished Progress Report, U. S. Depart- 402.
ment of Health, Education, and Welfare, January, McKenzie, H. S., Clark, M., Wolf, M. Kothera, R.,
1967. (b) and Benson, C. Behavior modification of chil-
Lent, J. R. Mimosa Cottage: Experiment in hope. dren with learning disabilities using grades as
Psychology Today, 1968, 2, 50-58. tokens and allowances as back up reinforcers. Ex-
Liberman, R. A view of behavior modification ceptional Children, 1968, 34, 745-752.
projects in California. Behaviour Research and Meacham, M. and Wiesen, A. Changing Classroom
Therapy, 1968, 6, 331-341. behavior: A manual for precision teaching. Scran-
Textbook Co., 1970.
Lindsley, 0. R. Operant conditioning methods ap- Meichenbaum, D.International
ton, Penn.:
H. The effects of instruction and
plied to research in chronic schizophrenia. Psy- reinforcement on thinking and language be-
chiatric Research Reports, 1956, 5, 118-139. haviour of schizophrenics. Behaviour Research
Lindsley, 0. R. Characteristics of the behavior of and Therapy, 1969, 7, 101-114.
chronic psychotics as revealed by free-operant Meichenbaum, D. H., Bowers, K., and Ross, R. R.
conditioning methods. Diseases of the Nervous Modification of classroom behavior of institution-
System, (Monograph Supplement), 1960, 21, alized female adolescent offenders. Behaviour Re-
66-78. search and Thearpy, 1968, 6, 343-353.
Lindsley, 0. R. Direct measurement and prothesis Metz, J. R. Conditioning generalized imitation in
of retarded behavior. Journal of Education, 1964, autistic children. Journal of Experimental Child
147, 62-81. Psychology, 1965, 2, 389-399.
Lloyd, K. E. and Garlington, W. K. Weekly varia- Metz, J. R. Conditioning social and intellectual
tions in performance on a token economy psychi- skills in autistic children. In J. Fisher and R. Har-
atric ward. Behaviour Research and Therapy, ris (Eds.), Reinforcement theory in psychological
1968, 6, 407-410. treatment: A symposium. Unpublished Research
Locke, B. Verbal conditioning with retarded sub- Monograph No. 8, California Department of
jects: Establishment or reinstatement effective re- mental Hygiene, 1966. Pp. 25-39.
inforcing consequences. American Journal of Mink, 0. The behavior change process. Chicago:
Mental Deficiency, 1969, 73, 621-626. Harper & Row, 1970.
Lovaas, 0. I. Some studies on the treatment of Miller, L. K. and Schneider, R. The use of a token
childhood schizophrenia. In J. M. Shlien (Ed.), system in project Head Start. Journal of Applied
Research in psychotherapy. Vol. III. Washington, Behavior Analysis, 1970, 3, 213-220.
D. C.: American Psychological Association, 1968. Miron, N. B. Behavior shaping and group nursing
Pp. 103-129. with severely retarded patients. In J. Fisher and
Lovaas, 0. I., Berberich, J. P., Perloff, B. F., and R. Harris (Eds.), Reinforcement theory in psy-
Schaeffer, B. Acquisition of imitative speech in chological treatment: A symposium. Unpublished
THE TOKEN ECONOMY: AN EVALUATIVE REVIEW 371

Research Monograph No. 8, California Depart- icism and verbal conditioning: A replication and
ment of Mental Hygiene, 1966. Pp. 1-14. extension. Journal of Consulting Psychology,
O'Leary, K. D. Establishing token programs in 1965, 29, 283.
schools: Issues and problems. Unpublished paper Ray, E. T. and Shelton, J. T. The use of operant
presented at American Psychological Association, conditioning with disturbed adolescent retarded
Washington, D. C., August, 1969. boys. Unpublished paper presented at the 20th
O'Leary, K. D. and Becker, W. C. Behavior modi- Mental Hospital Institute, Washington, D. C., Oc-
fication of an adjustment class: A token reinforce- tober, 1968.
ment program. Exceptional Children, 1967, 9, Rooney, J. R. Paxton program: Orientation and pro-
637-642. cedure manual for a token economy. Unpublished
O'Leary, K. D., Becker, W. C., Evans, M. B. and manuscript, Sonoma State Hospital, Eldridge,
Saudargas, R. A. A token reinforcement pro- California, 1966.
gram in a public school: A replication and sys- Ross, R. R. Application of operant conditioning
tematic analysis. Journal of Applied Behavior procedures to the behavioral modification of insti-
Analysis, 1969, 2, 3-13. tutionalized adolescent offenders. Unpublished
O'Leary, K. D. and Drabman, R. Token reinforce- manuscript, University of Waterloo, 1967. (a)
ment programs in the classroom: A review. Psy- Ross, R. R. Application of operant conditioning
chological Bulletin, 1971, 75, 379-398. procedures to the behavioral modification of in-
O'Leary, K. D., O'Leary, S., and Becker, W. C. Mod- stitutionalized adolescent offenders. Unpublished
ification of a deviant sibling interaction pattern progress report, University of Waterloo, 1968. (b)
in the home. Behaviour Research and Therapy, Salzinger, K., Feldman, R. S., and Portnoy, S. Train-
1967,5, 113-120. ing parents of brain-injured children in the use
Orlando, R., Schoelkopf, A., and Tobias, L. Tokens of operant conditioning procedures. Behavior
as reinforcers: Classroom applications by teachers Therapy, 1970, 1, 4-32.
of the retarded. Unpublished manuscript. Institute Schaefer, H. H. and Martin, P. L. Behavioral therapy
of Mental Retardation and Intellectual Develop- for "apathy" of hospitalized schizophrenics. Psy-
ment, Nashville, Tennessee, 1968. chological Report, 1966, 19, 1147-1158.
Orne, M. T. On the social psychology of the psy- Schaefer, H. H. and Martin, P. L. Behavioral
chological experiment: with particular reference therapy. New York: McGraw-Hill, 1969.
to demand characteristics and their implications. Scheff, T. J. Being mentally ill. Chicago: Aldine,
American Psychologist, 1962, 17, 776-783. 1966.
Packard, R. G. The control of "classroom attention": Schmidt, G. W. and Ulrich, R. E. Effects of group
a group contingency for complex behavior. Jour- contingent events upon classroom noise. journal
nal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 1970,3, 13-28. of Applied Behavior Analysis, 1969, 2, 171-179.
Panyan, M., Boozer, H., and Morris, N. Feedback to Scoles, P. E. and Henderson, J. D. Effects of token
attendants as a reinforcer for applying operant reinforcement on the social performance of psy-
techniques. Journal of Applied Analysis, 1970, chotic men. Unpublished manuscript, Spruce
3, 1-4. House, Horizon House, Inc., Philadelphia, 1968.
Patterson, G. R. An application of conditioning Sherman, J. A. and Baer, D. M. Appraisal of oper-
techniques to the control of a hyperactive child. ant therapy techniques with children and adults.
In L. P. Ullmann and L. Krasner (Eds.), Case In C. M. Franks (Ed.), Behavior therapy: Ap-
Studies in Behavior Modification. New York: praisal and status. New York: McGraw-Hill,
Holt, Rinehart and Winston, 1965. Pp. 370- 1969. Pp. 192-219.
375. Sidman, M. Tactics of scientific research. New York:
Patterson, G. R. and Brodsky, G. A behaviour mod- Basic Books, 1960.
ification programme for a child with multiple Sidman, M. Operant techniques. In A. Bachrach
problem behaviours. Journal of Child Psychology (Ed.), Experimental foundations of clinical psy-
and Psychiatry, 1966, 7, 277-295. chology. New York: Basic Books, 1962. Pp. 170-
Patterson, G. R. and Gullion, M. Living with chil- 210.
dren: New methods for parents and teachers. Spradlin, J. E. Effects of reinforcement schedules on
Champaign, Illinois: Research Press, 1968. extinction in severely mentally retarded children.
Phillips, E. L. Achievement place: Token reinforce- American Journal of Mental Deficiency, 1962,
ment procedures in a home-style rehabilitation 66, 634-640.
setting for "predelinquent" boys. Journal of Stayer, S. J. and Jones, F. Ward 108: Behavior mod-
Applied Behavior Analysis, 1968, 1, 213-223. ification and the delinquent soldier. Unpublished
Premack, D. Reinforcement theory. In D. Levine paper presented at Behavioral Engineering Con-
(Ed.), Nebraska symposium on motivation: 1965. ference, Walter Reed General Hospital, 1969.
Lincoln: University of Nebraska Press, 1965. Steffy, R. A. Service applications: Psychotic adoles-
Pp. 123-180. cents and adults. Treatment of aggression. Un-
Quay, H. C. and Hunt, W. A. Psychopathy, neurot- published paper presented to American Psycho-
372 ALAN E. KAZDIN and RICHARD R. BOOTZIN

logical Association, San Francisco, September, for deviant behavior in children. In unpublished
1968. Monograph 1, Department of Special Education,
Steffy, R. A., Hart, J., Craw, M., Torney, D., and University of Oregon, 1969.
Marlett, N. Operant behaviour modification tech- Ward, M. H. and Baker, B. L. Reinforcement
niques applied to severely regressed and aggressive therapy in the classroom. Journal of Applied Be-
patients. Canadian Psychiatric Association Journal, havior Analysis, 1968, 1, 323-328.
1969, 14, 59-67. Winkler, R. C. Management of chronic psychiatric
Thomas, D. R., Becker, W. C., and Armstrong, M. patients by a token reinforcement system. Journal
Production and elimination of disruptive class- of Applied Behavior Analysis, 1970, 3, 47-55.
room behavior by systematically varying teacher's Wolf, M. M., Giles, D. K., and Hall, R. V. Experi-
behavior. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, ments with token reinforcement in a remedial
1968, 1, 35-46. classroom. Behaviour Research and Therapy,
Tyler, V. 0. Application of operant token reinforce- 1968, 6, 51-64.
ment of academic performance of an institution- Wolf, M. M. and Risley, T. Analysis and modifica-
alized delinquent. Psychological Reports, 1967, tion of deviant child behavior. Unpublished pa-
21, 249-260. per presented at American Psychological Associa-
Tyler, V. 0. and Brown, G. D. Token reinforce- tion, Washington, D. C., 1967.
ment of academic performance with institutional- Wolf, M. M., Risley, T., and Mees, H. Application
ized delinquent boys. Journal of Educational of operant conditioning procedures to the be-
Psychology, 1968, 59, 164-168. haviour problems of an autistic child. Behaviour
Ullmann, L. P. and Krasner, L. (Eds.) Case studies Research and Therapy, 1964, 1, 305-312.
in behavior modification. New York: Holt, Rine- Zimmerman, E. H., Zimmerman, J., and Russell,
hart, & Winston, 1965. C. D. Differential effects of token reinforcement
Ullmann, L. P. and Krasner, L. A psychological ap- on "attention" in retarded students instructed as a
proach to abnormal behavior. Englewood Cliffs, group. Unpublished paper presented at American
New Jersey: Prentice-Hall, 1969. Psychological Association, San Francisco, Septem-
Wahler, R. G. Behavior therapy for oppositional ber, 1968.
children: Love is not enough. Unpublished paper Zimmerman, E. H., Zimmerman, J., and Russell,
read at Eastern Psychological Association Meet- C. D. Differential effects of token reinforcement
ing, Washington, D. C., April, 1968. on instruction-following behavior in retarded
Wahler, R. G. Setting generality: some specific and students instructed as a group. Journal of Applied
general effects of child behavior therapy. Journal Behavior Analysis, 1969, 2, 101-112.
of Applied Behavior Analysis, 1969, 2, 239-249. Zimmerman, J., Stuckey, T. E., Garlick, B. J., and
Walker, H. and Buckley, N. The use of positive re- Miller, M. Effects of token reinforcement on
inforcement in conditioning attending behavior. productivity in multiply handicapped clients in a
Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 1968, 1, sheltered workshop. Rehabilitation Literature,
245-252. 1969, 30, 34-41.
Walker, H. M., Mattson, R. H., and Buckley, N. K.
Special class placement as a treatment alternative Received 17 December 1970.

View publication stats

You might also like